Tourist Preferences Influence of Unconscious Needs: Doi:10.1016/j.annals.2005.10.014
Tourist Preferences Influence of Unconscious Needs: Doi:10.1016/j.annals.2005.10.014
Tourist Preferences Influence of Unconscious Needs: Doi:10.1016/j.annals.2005.10.014
424–441, 2006
Ó 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Printed in Great Britain
0160-7383/$32.00
www.elsevier.com/locate/atoures
doi:10.1016/j.annals.2005.10.014
TOURIST PREFERENCES
Influence of Unconscious Needs
Xuan Tran
Linda Ralston
University of Utah, USA
Abstract: This study examines the relationships among tourists’ unconscious needs for
achievement, affiliation and power and their preferences for adventure, cultural, and eco-
related tourism. Study data were collected from students at the University of Utah via an
online questionnaire. The unconscious needs were scored from their stories created based
on thematic apperception test pictures. The tourist preferences were measured through six
packages. The relationships among the needs and preferences were examined through
canonical variate analysis. Two significant relationships were detected: one between the need
for achievement and the preference for adventure tourism; and the other between the need
for affiliation and the preference for cultural tourism. Keywords: unconscious needs for
achievement, affiliation, power, preferences. Ó 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Résumé: Préférences touristiques: influence des besoins inconscients. Cette étude examine
les rapports entre les besoins inconscients des touristes pour la réussite, l’affiliation et le pou-
voir et leurs préférences pour le tourisme aventure, culturel ou écologique. Les données ont
été cueillies des étudiants à l’University of Utah par moyen d’un questionnaire en ligne. Les
besoins inconscients sont mesurés dans les narrations créées à partir d’images d’aperception
thématique de six exemples de voyages organisés. Les rapports besoin-préférence sont exam-
inés par analyse canonique. On détecte deux rapports significatifs: l’un entre le besoin de la
réussite et la préférence pour le tourisme aventure; et l’autre entre le besoin d’affiliation et la
préférence pour le tourisme culturel. Mots-clés: besoins inconscients pour la réussite, affilia-
tion, pouvoir, préférences. Ó 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
INTRODUCTION
Failure to adequately explain why United States tourists hesitate to
engage in international tourism is an important problem for the
increasingly competing businesses in this field. According to the World
Tourism Organization, the number of outbound Americans decreased
14% in the period 2001–2002 (WTO 2003). The number of inbound
American tourists also decreased 3% in the following season (US
Department of Commerce 2004). US tourists are important because
they represent the world’s top spenders. For example, in 2002 all inter-
national arrivals to Asia increased by 8%, except American tourists,
Xuan Tran is Lecturer in the Department of Parks, Recreation, and Tourism at the
University of Utah (Salt Lake City, Utah 84112, USA. Email <Xuan.Tran@health.utah.edu>).
His research interests include cultural tourism development, motivations, and satisfaction.
The research interest of Linda Ralston, Associate Professor in the same department, includes
sustainable tourism development, motivations, and customer service.
424
TRAN AND RALSTON 425
N-Ach
Tourist
Tourist
Motives /
N-Aff Experience
Preferences
N-Pow
N-Ach
Life
N-Aff
Satisfaction
N-Pow
Ha1: A significant relationship will exist between the need for achievement and the
preference for adventure tourism.
Ha2: A significant relationship will exist between the need for affiliation and the
preference for cultural tourism.
Ha3: A significant relationship will exist between the need for power and the
preference for eco-related tourism.
Study Methods
In order to explore the relationships of motives and preferences, the
variables must be measured with appropriate methods. A sample was
randomly selected from a list of email addresses for all University of
Utah undergraduate and graduate students. The 200 students in the
resulting sample were sent an electronic invitation to participate in
the online questionnaire. The study achieved a 70% response rate with-
in two weeks of the initial invitation.
Unconscious motives were measured with the TAT and tourist pref-
erences with the self-report questionnaire method. The TAT, an exer-
cise in which participants create brief imaginative stories in response to
pictures, is a test designed to measure the human needs or motivation
that people are either unwilling or unable to verbalize because of a lack
of conscious awareness (Murray 1943). The test has frequently been
used to create measures of implicit motivation based on the assump-
tion that such motives are best measured by spontaneous fantasy as op-
posed to conscious statements of goals or motives (McClelland 1989;
Weinberger and McClelland 1990). The seven Murray (1943) TAT pic-
tures were chosen due to four reasons. First, the pictures represented
common situations and thus could better elicit content of interest to all
432 NEEDS AND TOURIST PREFERENCES
three needs and motive combinations. Second, the pictures with few
subjects represented sufficiently low ambiguous levels for a respondent
to effectively produce a particular type of motive separate from others
(Smith 1992). Third, the number of seven pictures was large enough
(>6) to be reliable (Smith 1992). Four, the seven pictures included
males and females who were older than the subjects to avoid those
people eliciting recollections of past events rather than thoughts that
reflect current concerns (McClelland, Atkinson, Clark and Lowell
1953). The two TAT trained raters reviewed the coding manual and
received clarification regarding the meaning of code categories. The
raters then scored practice materials, compared their scoring with each
other, and discussed differences between theirs and an expert’s scoring
sample to further clarify their understanding of the coding categories
based on the scoring of the TAT manual (Smith 1992:632).
The scoring of n-aff and n-pow are not presented here, but is avail-
able through the authors. The n-ach is the need for success in com-
petition with some standard of excellence involving long-term
achievement, competing to a performance standard, or doing some-
thing unique. Based on the behavioral sequence model of McClelland
and Franz (1992), using the TAT, the goal or achievement imaginary
(AI) is scored +1, 0 or 1 when the following situations are evidenced
in the story. Competition with a standard of excellence, scored AI = +1
(such as when one wants to win an essay contest). Unique accomplish-
ment, scored AI = +1 (for example, Tom and his assistant were working
on the publication of his famous booklet advocating freedom for
America). Long-term involvement, scored AI = +1 (one is going to be
a doctor). Doubtful achievement imagery (TI), scored TI = 0 (they
are making the bolt). Unrelated imagery (UI), scored UI = 1 (he ap-
pears to be thinking of something). It should be noted that if a respon-
dent’s story is scored TI = 0 or UI = 1, then there is no achievement
imagery and the analysis is concluded. It can not be scored on the
remaining 10 subcategories. Stated need (N) for achievement is scored
+1 when the story evidenced a desire to reach an achievement goal (he
wants to finish the painting).
Instrumental activity is scored +1 when a successful outcome is de-
scribed (I = +1) (he will try his best and hopes to become the best doc-
tor in the country) or in progress (I? = +1) or unsuccessful (I = +1)
(they have worked diligently night and day in the past with repeated
trials yielding only failures). Anticipatory goal state (Ga) either positive
(Ga+) or negative (Ga ) is scored +1 when respondents are thinking
about the outcomes the characters would achieve (they will attempt
to sell their new discovery with confidence of becoming rich or the
boy thinks he just can’t make it through college). Stories are scored
+1 for blocks within their control (Bp) or outside their control (Bw)
when the progress of goal-directed activity is hindered in some way
(the son has been faced with some trying situation or problem). Nur-
turant press (Nu) is scored +1 when the character receives assistance
from a third party in the story (the experienced machinist is trying
to straighten things out for the apprentice and is encouraging him).
Affective state is scored +1 when the story evidences a positive feeling
TRAN AND RALSTON 433
Study Findings
The first canonical correlation function relating two sets of variables
(motives and preferences) was calculated and explained 17.1% of the
variance in the preference variables. Wilks’ Lamda was significant for
the combined function (K = .73, p < .01). It indicates how well the
groups of motive factors could predict each of the four sets of tourist
tours, with its structured coefficients (Rc = .38) showing the highest
possible correlation between any linear combination of the motive vari-
ables and the preference variables. One pair shared significant varia-
tion (Table 1). The structure matrix reveals that high n-ach and low
n-aff are predictive of adventure preferences and lack of preference
for one of the cultural tours (structure coefficient = .586). There
was one pair of significant canonical variates (p < .05). Canonical corre-
lations and loadings exceeded 0.3 for the variates. The structure matrix
was examined to interpret the variate pair. The pair of variates revealed
a very strong relationship between two of the motive variables [n-ach
(.708) and n-aff ( .639)] and two of the tourist experience preference
variables (adventure [.650] and culture [ .586]). Essentially, the vari-
ate suggests that individuals with a high n-ach will prefer an adventure
experience and individuals possessing a high n-aff will be interested in
a cultural one.
As a result, there are three conclusions for the test of the three re-
search hypotheses. First the n-ach was correlated positively with the
preference for an adventure and for escape tourism. The research
hypothesis Ha1, that a significant relationship would exist between
the n-ach and the preference for adventure, was not rejected. The n-
ach was significantly associated with the preference for this experience.
TRAN AND RALSTON 435
Motives
N-Affiliation .683 .639.
N-Achievement 773 .708
N- Power .159 .103
Tourism Types
Cultural Tourism 1 .524 .323
Cultural Tourism 2 .280 .586
Adventure Tourism 1 .655 .650
Adventure Tourism 2 .350 .548
Eco-related Tourism 1 .164 .161
Eco-related Tourism 2 .062 .024
the stories respondents submitted for the less ambiguous pictures per-
mitted the expression of achievement, power, or affiliation motives. In
this case, it would be best to select a picture low in ambiguity in order
to identity each motive.
Finally, external factors that affect motives and preferences—such as
finance, education, sex, and time—were assumed to be constant in the
study. Change of a respondent’s finances, differences in education,
marital status, and gender could all affect motives and preferences,
but were beyond the scope of this study. Time is the last constraint that
influences motives and preferences since ‘‘behavior in the same situa-
tion may vary during a period of time’’ (Smith 1992:15). It would also
be useful to identify customers with homogeneous unconscious motive
profiles (Dubois 2002) and relate those profiles to preferences.
Although the results cannot be generalized to a specific destination
or the entire population, these findings contribute to a greater under-
standing of the relationship between unconscious needs, motivations,
and preferences. They also demonstrate that further research is
needed in this area. It is evident that the insights gleaned from this re-
search should be utilized when replicating the study using a random
sample of tourists to a particular destination that provides a variety
of potential activities. An analysis of the unconscious motives and the
planned activities within a given tourism package would be better able
to predict the preferences of the general population to this
destination.
In order to apply the results of this study to a contemporary tourism
situation, it is appropriate to examine the position of the World of
Tourism Organization which often mentions the role of tourism in
generating income to alleviate the poverty experienced in many devel-
oping countries. Once a country or destination has identified the
potential market, they can research the target market to better under-
stand their unconscious needs and subsequently plan appropriate
packages to attract tourists to their destination. Furthermore, since
the United States tourism market is particularly lucrative, an examina-
tion of American tourists’ unconscious motives might be helpful in
identifying packages that appeal specifically to them (McClelland
1985). Although the analysis of unconscious motives will not replace
current market profiles and market effectiveness research, it does pro-
vide a potentially effective means of better understanding the motiva-
tions and preferences of specific markets. This study did not
examine the role of unconscious motives in influencing the preference
of one destination over another. Further research will be required to
isolate the influence of unconscious motives in selecting one among
similar destinations. It did demonstrate that unconscious motives have
an influence in respondents’ preferences for specific activities, trans-
portation choices, and lodging options. Since most destinations have
the potential to modify activities, transportation, and lodging choices
within a package, this paper presents a potentially significant applica-
tion for vacation destinations.
Theory and research regarding achievement and affiliation motives
were found to be related to adventure and culture preferences. These
438 NEEDS AND TOURIST PREFERENCES
CONCLUSION
This study featured three contributions that have not been previ-
ously examined in the literature. It demonstrates the potential applica-
tion of one type of measurement of unconscious needs and
preferences. There are numerous other measures of unconscious
needs that merit testing to determine if a less time-consuming yet
equally effective means of examining the relationship exists. This re-
search also contributes to a further understanding of the relationships
between the theory of motivation, pioneered by McClelland’s research
within human psychology, and motivation/preferences, explored by a
limited number of researchers (Ralston 1993a). Thanks to the achieve-
ments of psychology, the success of this line of investigation will stim-
ulate future researchers to deepen their knowledge concerning the
practical application of psychology to the literature. Finally, the study
detected two strong relationships: one between the n-ach and adven-
ture tourism; and one between the n-aff and cultural tourism. Despite
the complexities related to projective techniques and possible contex-
tual effects, the results suggest that awareness of the needs structure
can help researchers and marketers to accomplish their goals and
satisfy tourists’ preferences.
In conclusion, the types of packages American tourists prefer
depends on their types of motives. The finding of the positive relation-
ship between achievement motivation and preference for an adventur-
ous type of experiences provides helpful information for managers,
marketers, or anyone who would like to attract American tourists to
their destinations. In the promotional materials intended for this mar-
ket, it is recommended to include activities such as exploring nature,
sailing from island to island, and whitewater canoeing or more chal-
lenging activities such as climbing to the top of a mountain, and
TRAN AND RALSTON 439
REFERENCES
Abbey, J.
2002 Hospitality Sales and Marketing. Lansing: EI-AHLA.
Allport, G.
1938 Personality: A Psychological Interpretation. New York: Holt, Rinehart
and Winston.
1973 The Trend of Motivational Theory. In Human Motivation: A Book of
Readings, D. McClelland and R. Steele, eds., pp. 89–102. Morristown: General
Learning Press.
Ashworth, G., and B. Goodall
1990 Marketing Tourism Places. London: Routledge.
Bello, D., and M. Etzel
1985 The Role of Novelty in the Pleasure Travel Experience. Journal of Travel
Research 9(1):20–26.
Capozzoli, M., L. McSweeney, and D. Sinha
1999 Beyond Kappa: A Review of Inter-rater Agreement Measures. The
Canadian Journal of Statistics 27(1):3–23.
Cohen, E.
1972 Towards a Sociology of International Tourism. Social Research
39(1):64–82.
Crompton, J.
1979 Motivations for Pleasure Vacation. Annals of Tourism Research
6:408–424.
Crotts, J.
1993 Personality Correlates of the Novelty Seeking Drive. Journal of Hospi-
tality and Leisure Marketing 1:3–29.
Dann, G.
1977 Anomie, Ego-enhancement and Tourism. Annals of Tourism Research
4:184–194.
Deci, E.
1975 Intrinsic Motivation. New York: Plenum.
Dubois, R.
2002 Multilevel Motivational Personality Structure, Well-being, and Coping
Style in Graduate Business Students. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Chicago
School of Professional Psychology, United States.
Freud, S.
1915 Instincts and their Vicissitudes. New York: Collier.
Gerrig, J., and G. Zimbardo
2002 Psychology and Life. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
440 NEEDS AND TOURIST PREFERENCES
Gray, H.
1970 International Travel: International Trade. Lexington: Health Lexington.
Goodall, B.
1988 How Tourists Choose Their Holidays: An Analytical Framework. In
Marketing in the Tourism Industry. The Promotion of Destination Regions, B.
Goodall and G. Ashworth, eds., pp. 41–60. London: Routledge.
Heyns, R., J. Veroff, and J. Atkinson
1958 A Scoring Manual for the Affiliation Motive. In Motivation and
Personality, P. Smith, ed., pp. 213–224. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Iso-Ahola, S.
1980 The Social Psychology of Leisure and Recreation. Dubuque: WC Brown.
Langens, T., and D. McClelland.
1997 Implicit Motives, Explicit Motives, and Emotional Well-being. Poster
presented at the 105th convention of the American Psychological Association,
Chicago.
Maslow, A.
1943 Theory of Human Motivation. Psychological Review 50:370–396.
1954 Motivation and Personality. New York: Harper and Row.
McClelland, D.
1953 The Achievement Motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
1985 Human Motivation. New York: Cambridge University Press.
1989 Motivational Factors in Health and Disease. American Psychologist
44:675–683.
1992 Motivational Configurations. In Motivation and Personality: Handbook
of Thematic Content Analysis, P. Smith, ed., pp. 87–99. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
McClelland, D., J. Atkinson, R. Clark, and E. Lowell
1953 The Achievement Motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
McClelland, D., and C. Franz
1992 Motivational and Other Sources of Work Accomplishment in Mid-life: A
Longitudinal Study. Journal of Personality 60:679–707.
McGuiggan, R.
2000 The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and Leisure Attribute Preference. In
Consumer Psychology of Tourism, Hospitality and Leisure, A. Woodside, G.
Crouch, J. Mazanec, M. Oppermann and M. Sakai, eds., pp. 245–267.
Wallingford: CABI Publishing.
McIntosh, R., and C. Goeldner
1990 Tourism. Principles, Practices, and Philosophies (sixth ed.). Columbus:
Grid Publishing.
Mehrabian, A.
1968 An Analysis of Personality Theories. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
Monash University Canonical Correlation <http://www.med.monash.edu.au/
psych/research/rda/Canonical%20correlation.htm> (7 February 2005).
Murray, H.
1938 Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press.
1943 Thematic Apperception Test Manual. Cambridge: Harvard University
Press.
Nickerson, N., and G. Ellis
1991 Traveler Types and Activation Theory: A Comparison of Two Models.
Journal of Travel Research 35(3):26–31.
Oppermann, M.
2000 Where Psychology and Geography Interface in Tourism Research and
Theory. In Consumer Psychology of Tourism, Hospitality and Leisure, A.
Woodside, G. Crouch, J. Mazanec, M. Oppermann and M. Sakai, eds., pp.
19–38. Wallingford: CABI Publishing.
Pearce, P.
1988 The Ulysses Factor. New York: Springer-Verlag.
1995 Pleasure Travel Motivation. In Tourism Principles, Practices, Philoso-
phies, R. McIntosh, C. Goeldner and J. Ritchie, eds., (seventh ed.) pp.
167–178. New York: Wiley.
TRAN AND RALSTON 441
Plog, S.
1974 Why Destination Areas Rise and Fall in Popularity. The Cornell Hotel
and Restaurant Administration Quarterly 14(4):55–58.
Ralston, L.
1993a The Association between the Need for Affiliation and Traveler Type
with the Motivation for Travel. Vision in Leisure and Business 12(3):24–41.
1993b The Relationship between the Expressed Need for Affiliation and
Motivation for Travel. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas A&M
University.
Ross, G.
1994 The Psychology of Tourism. Melbourne: Hospitality Press.
Smith, C.
1992 Motivation and Personality: Handbook of Thematic Content Analysis.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Smith, V.
1989 Hosts and Guests. The Anthropology of Tourism (second ed.). Phila-
delphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Taylor, G.
1994 Travel Styles. In Global Tourism, the Next Decade, W. Theobald, ed., pp.
188–198. London: Butterworth-Heinermann.
Tinsley, H., and D. Tinsley
1986 A Theory of the Attributes, Benefits and Causes of Leisure Experience.
Leisure Sciences 8(1):1–45.
US Department of Commerce
2004 Forecast for International Travelers too and from the United States
<http://tinet.ita.doc.gov/view/f-2004-99-001/intlforecastvolume.html> (15
November 2004).
Weinberger, J., and D. McClelland
1990 Cognitive Versus Traditional Motivational Models. In Handbook of
Motivation and Cognition, E. Higgins and R. Sorrentino eds. (vol. 2) pp.
562–597. New York: The Guilford Press.
WTO
2003 WTO statistics: Basic Set 2003. Madrid, Spain: WTO.
2004 Sustainable Development of Tourism Conceptual Definition <http://
www.worldtourism.org/frameset/frame_sustainable.html> (12 November
2004).
2004 WTO and ETC Study Looks at the Future Challenges for City Tourism
& Culture in Europe <http://www.world-tourism.org/newsroom/Releases/
2004/november/city.htm> (12 November 2004).
Woodside, A., and R. MacDonald
1994 General Systems Framework of Customer Choice Processes for Tourism
services. In Spoilt for Choice. Decision Making Processes and Preference
Changes of Tourists Intertemporal and Intercountry Perspectives, R. Gasser
and K. Weiermair, eds., pp. 30–59. Vienna: Kultur Verlag.
Submitted 19 January 2005. Resubmitted 22 April 2005. Resubmitted 21 May 2005. Final
version16 July. Accepted 10 October 2005. Refereed anonymously. Coordinating Editor:
Regina G. Schl€u ter