Cam 13 Reading Test 3
Cam 13 Reading Test 3
Cam 13 Reading Test 3
For millennia, the coconut has been central to the lives of Polynesian and Asian peoples. In the western
world, on the other hand, coconuts have always been exotic and unusual, sometimes rare. The Italian
merchant traveller Marco Polo apparently saw coconuts in South Asia in the late 13th century, and
among the mid-14th-century travel writings of Sir John Mandeville there is mention of ‘great Notes of
Ynde’ (great Nuts of India). Today, images of palm-fringed tropical beaches are clichés in the west to sell
holidays, chocolate bars, fizzy drinks and even romance.
Typically, we envisage coconuts as brown cannonballs that, when opened, provide sweet white flesh.
But we see only part of the fruit and none of the plant from which they come. The coconut palm has a
smooth, slender, grey trunk, up to 30 metres tall. This is an important source of timber for building
houses, and is increasingly being used as a replacement for endangered hardwoods in the furniture
construction industry. The trunk is surmounted (cover) by a rosette of leaves, each of which may be up
to six metres long. The leaves have hard veins in their centres which, in many parts of the world, are
used as brushes (bàn chải) after the green part of the leaf has been stripped away (loại bỏ). Immature
coconut flowers are tightly clustered together among the leaves at the top of the trunk. The flower
stems may be tapped (make use of )for their sap to produce a drink, and the sap can also be reduced by
boiling to produce a type of sugar used for cooking.
Coconut palms produce as many as seventy fruits per year, weighing more than a kilogram each. The
wall of the fruit has three layers: a waterproof outer layer, a fibrous middle layer and a hard, inner layer.
The thick fibrous middle layer produces coconut fibre, ‘coir’, which has numerous uses and is particularly
important in manufacturing ropes. The woody innermost layer, the shell, with its three prominent
‘eyes’, surrounds the seed. An important product obtained from the shell is charcoal, which is widely
used in various industries as well as in the home as a cooking fuel. When broken in half, the shells are
also used as bowls in many parts of Asia.
Inside the shell are the nutrients (endosperm) needed by the developing seed. Initially, the endosperm
is a sweetish liquid, coconut water, which is enjoyed as a drink, but also provides the hormones which
encourage other plants to grow more rapidly and produce higher yields. As the fruit matures (trưởng
thành, ripe), the coconut water gradually solidifies (đặc lại, cô đọng lại)to form the brilliant white, fat-
rich, edible flesh or meat. Dried coconut flesh, ‘copra’, is made into coconut oil and coconut milk, which
are widely used in cooking in different parts of the world, as well as in cosmetics. A derivative (sự chiếc
xuất )of coconut fat, glycerine, acquired strategic importance in a quite different sphere (sector,
domain), as Alfred Nobel introduced the world to his nitroglycerine-based invention: dynamite. (thuốc
nỗ)
Their biology would appear to make coconuts the great maritime voyagers and coastal colonizers of the
plant world. The large, energy-rich fruits are able to float in water and tolerate salt, but cannot remain
viable indefinitely; studies suggest after about 110 days at sea they are no longer able to germinate.
Literally cast onto desert island shores, with little more than sand to grow in and exposed to the full
glare of the tropical sun, coconut seeds are able to germinate and root. The air pocket in the seed,
created as the endosperm solidifies, protects the embryo. In addition, the fibrous fruit wall that helped
it to float during the voyage stores moisture that can be taken up by the roots of the coconut seedling as
it starts to grow.
There have been centuries of academic debate over the origins of the coconut. There were no coconut
palms in West Africa, the Caribbean or the east coast of the Americans before the voyages of the
European explorers Vasco da Gama and Columbus in the late 15th and early 16th centuries. 16th
century trade and human migration patterns reveal that Arab traders and European sailors are likely to
have moved coconuts from South and Southeast Asia to Africa and then across the Atlantic to the east
coast of America. But the origin of coconuts discovered along the west coast of America by 16th century
sailors has been the subject of centuries of discussion. Two diametrically opposed origins have been
proposed: that they came from Asia, or that they were native to America. Both suggestions have
problems. In Asia, there is a large degree of coconut diversity and evidence of millennia of human use –
but there are no relatives growing in the wild. In America, there are close coconut relatives, but no
evidence that coconuts are indigenous. These problems have led to the intriguing suggestion that
coconuts originated (có nguồn gốc) on coral islands in the Pacific and were dispersed (scatter) from
there.
Passage 2
The typical way of talking to a baby – high-pitched, exaggerated and repetitious – is a source of
fascination for linguists who hope to understand how ‘baby talk’ impacts on learning. Most babies start
developing their hearing while still in the womb, prompting some hopeful parents to play classical music
to their pregnant bellies. Some research even suggests that infants are listening to adult speech as early
as 10 weeks before being born, gathering the basic building blocks of their family’s native tongue.
Early language exposure seems to have benefits to the brain – for instance, studies suggest that babies
raised in bilingual homes are better at learning how to mentally prioritize information. So how does the
sweet if sometimes absurd sound of infant-directed speech influence a baby’s development? Here are
some recent studies that explore the science behind baby talk.
Fathers don’t use baby talk as often or in the same ways as mothers – and that’s perfectly OK, according
to a new study. Mark VanDam of Washington State University at Spokane and colleagues equipped
parents with recording devices and speech-recognition software to study the way they interacted with
their youngsters during a normal day. ‘We found that moms do exactly what you’d expect and what’s
been described many times over,’ VanDam explains. ‘But we found that dads aren’t doing the same
thing. Dads didn’t raise their pitch or fundamental frequency when they talked to kids.’ Their role may
be rooted in what is called the bridge hypothesis, which dates back to 1975. It suggests that fathers use
less familial language to provide their children with a bridge to the kind of speech they’ll hear in public.
‘The idea is that a kid gets to practice a certain kind of speech with mom and another kind of speech
with dad, so the kid then has a wider repertoire of kinds of speech to practice,’ says VanDam.
Scientists from the University of Washington and the University of Connecticut collected thousands of
30-second conversations between parents and their babies, fitting 26 children with audio-recording
vests that captured language and sound during a typical eight-hour day. The study found that the more
baby talk parents used, the more their youngsters began to babble. And when researchers saw the same
babies at age two, they found that frequent baby talk had dramatically boosted vocabulary, regardless
of socioeconomic status. ‘Those children who listened to a lot of baby talk were talking more than the
babies that listened to more adult talk or standard speech,’ says Nairán Ramirez-Esparza of the
University of Connecticut. ‘We also found that it really matters whether you use baby talk in a one-on-
one context,’ she adds. ‘The more parents use baby talk one-on-one, the more babies babble, and the
more they babble, the more words they produce later in life.’
Another study suggests that parents might want to pair their youngsters up so they can babble more
with their own kind. Researchers from McGill University and Université du Québec à Montréal found
that babies seem to like listening to each other rather than to adults – which may be why baby talk is
such a universal tool among parents. They played repeating vowel sounds made by a special
synthesizing device that mimicked sounds made by either an adult woman or another baby. This way,
only the impact of the auditory cues was observed. The team then measured how long each type of
sound held the infants’ attention. They found that the ‘infant’ sounds held babies’ attention nearly 40
percent longer. The baby noises also induced more reactions in the listening infants, like smiling or lip
moving, which approximates sound making. The team theorizes that this attraction to other infant
sounds could help launch the learning process that leads to speech. ‘It may be some property of the
sound that is just drawing their attention,’ says study co-author Linda Polka. ‘Or maybe they are really
interested in that particular type of sound because they are starting to focus on their own ability to
make sounds. We are speculating here but it might catch their attention because they recognize it as a
sound they could possibly make.’
In a study published in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, a total of 57 babies from two
slightly different age groups – seven months and eleven and a half months – were played a number of
syllables (âm tiết) from both their native language (English) and a non-native tongue (Spanish). The
infants were placed in a brain-activation scanner that recorded activity in a brain region known to guide
the motor movements that produce speech. The results suggest that listening to baby talk prompts
infant brains to start practicing their language skills. ‘Finding activation in the motor areas of the brain
when infants are simply listening is significant, because it means the baby brain is engaged in trying to
talk back right from the start, and suggests that seven-month-olds’ brains are already trying to figure out
how to make the right movements that will produce words,’ says co-author Patricia Kuhl. Another
interesting finding was that while the seven-month-olds responded to all speech sounds regardless of
language, the brains of the older infants worked harder at the motor activations of non-native sounds
compared to native sounds. The study may have also uncovered a process by which babies recognize
differences between their native language and other tongues.
Passage 3
The Harappan Civilisation of ancient Pakistan and India flourished 5,000 years ago, but a thousand years
later their cities were abandoned. The Harappan Civilisation was a sophisticated Bronze Age society who
built ‘megacities’ and traded internationally in luxury craft products, and yet seemed to have left almost
no depictions of themselves. But their lack of self-imagery – at a time when the Egyptians were carving
and painting representations of themselves all over their temples – is only part of the mystery.
B
‘There is plenty of archaeological evidence to tell us about the rise of the Harappan Civilisation, but
relatively little about its fall,’ explains archaeologist Dr Cameron Petrie of the University of Cambridge.
‘As populations increased, cities were built that had great baths, craft workshops, palaces and halls laid
out in distinct sectors. Houses were arranged in blocks, with wide main streets and narrow alleyways,
and many had their own wells and drainage systems. It was very much a “thriving” civilisation.’ Then
around 2100 BC, a transformation began. Streets went uncleaned, buildings started to be abandoned,
and ritual structures fell out of use. After their final demise, a millennium passed before really large-
scale cities appeared once more in South Asia.
Some have claimed that major glacier-fed rivers changed their course, dramatically affecting the water
supply and agriculture; or that the cities could not cope with an increasing population, they exhausted
their resource base, the trading economy broke down or they succumbed to invasion and conflict; and
yet others that climate change caused an environmental change that affected food and water provision.
‘It is unlikely that there was a single cause for the decline of the civilisation. But the fact is, until now, we
have had little solid evidence from the area for most of the key elements,’ said Petrie. ‘A lot of the
archaeological debate has really only been well-argued speculation.’
A research team led by Petrie, together with Dr Ravindanath Singh of Banaras Hindu University in India,
found early in their investigations that many of the archaeological sites were not where they were
supposed to be, completely altering understanding of the way that this region was inhabited in the past.
When they carried out a survey of how the larger area was settled in relation to sources of water, they
found inaccuracies in the published geographic locations of ancient settlements ranging from several
hundred metres to many kilometres. They realised that any attempts to use the existing data were likely
to be fundamentally flawed. Over the course of several seasons of fieldwork they carried out new
surveys, finding an astonishing 198 settlement sites that were previously unknown.
Now, research published by Dr Yama Dixit and Professor David Hodell, both from Cambridge’s
Department of Earth Sciences, has provided the first definitive evidence for climate change affecting the
plains of north-western India, where hundreds of Harappan sites are known to have been situated. The
researchers gathered shells of Melanoides tuberculata snails from the sediments of an ancient lake and
used geochemical analysis as a means of tracing the climate history of the region. ‘As today, the major
source of water into the lake is likely to have been the summer monsoon,’ says Dixit. ‘But we have
observed that there was an abrupt change about 4,100 years ago, when the amount of evaporation
from the lake exceeded the rainfall – indicative of a drought.’ Hodell adds: ‘We estimate that the
weakening of the Indian summer monsoon climate lasted about 200 years before recovering to the
previous conditions, which we still see today.’
It has long been thought that other great Bronze Age civilisations also declined at a similar time, with a
global-scale climate event being seen as the cause. While it is possible that these local-scale processes
were linked, the real archaeological interest lies in understanding the impact of these larger-scale events
on different environments and different populations. ‘Considering the vast area of the Harappan
Civilisation with its variable weather systems,’ explains Singh, ‘it is essential that we obtain more climate
data from areas close to the two great cities at Mohenjodaro and Harappa and also from the Indian
Punjab.’
Petrie and Singh’s team is now examining archaeological records and trying to understand details of how
people led their lives in the region five millennia ago. They are analysing grains cultivated at the time,
and trying to work out whether they were grown under extreme conditions of water stress, and
whether they were adjusting the combinations of crops they were growing for different weather
systems. They are also looking at whether the types of pottery used, and other aspects of their material
culture, were distinctive to specific regions or were more similar across larger areas. This gives us insight
into the types of interactive networks that the population was involved in, and whether those changed.
Petrie believes that archaeologists are in a unique position to investigate how past societies responded
to environmental and climatic change. ‘By investigating responses to environmental pressures and
threats, we can learn from the past to engage with the public, and the relevant governmental and
administrative bodies, to be more proactive in issues such as the management and administration of
water supply, the balance of urban and rural development, and the importance of preserving cultural
heritage in the future.’