Mind Shift How Culture Transformed The Human Brain
Mind Shift How Culture Transformed The Human Brain
Mind Shift How Culture Transformed The Human Brain
•
•
•
•• • HOW
CULTURE
• TRANSFORMED
THE HUMAN
• • BRAIN
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 27/01/21, SPi
M I ND SH I F T
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 27/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 27/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 27/01/21, SPi
1
Great Clarendon Street, Oxford, ox2 6dp,
United Kingdom
Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford.
It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship,
and education by publishing worldwide. Oxford is a registered trade mark of
Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries
© John Parrington 2021
The moral rights of the author have been asserted
First Edition published in 2021
Impression: 1
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in
a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the
prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted
by law, by licence or under terms agreed with the appropriate reprographics
rights organization. Enquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of the
above should be sent to the Rights Department, Oxford University Press, at the
address above
You must not circulate this work in any other form
and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer
Published in the United States of America by Oxford University Press
198 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016, United States of America
British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data
Data available
Library of Congress Control Number: 2020948532
ISBN 978–0–19–880163–4
Printed and bound in Great Britain by
Clays Ltd, Elcograf S.p.A.
Links to third party websites are provided by Oxford in good faith and
for information only. Oxford disclaims any responsibility for the materials
contained in any third party website referenced in this work.
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 27/01/21, SPi
AC K NOW L E DGE M E N T S
I would like to thank a number of people who have helped bring this,
my third book, to fruition. I owe a particular debt to Latha Menon, my
editor at Oxford University Press, who had a great deal of input into the
book. Latha’s critical comments had a major impact in pushing me to go
that ‘extra mile’ in the writing and her encouragement at all stages made a
huge difference to me during the five years that I have been working on
this project. I would also like to thank Jenny Nugee of the OUP editorial
team, for her help on a multitude of practical matters, and Charles Lauder
and Saraswathi Ethiraju of SPi Global, for meticulous copy-editing of the
book. Quite a number of people helped immensely with the writing of
the book through their critical comments; I would like to thank Jonathan
Bate, David Crane, Jane Hardy, Marnie Holborow, George Paizis, Naomi
Rokotnitz, John Rose, Margarida Ruas, and Jozsef Somogyi, as well as an
anonymous reviewer, for their comments in this regard. I would also like
to thank Margarida Ruas for producing the superb set of line drawings for
the book. I also owe thanks to Anthony Morgan for producing the author
photo for the book cover. For their expert assistance with marketing and
publicity, and answers to many questions on this front, I would like to
thank Anna Gell and Kate Roche of OUP. I owe thanks also to friends and
colleagues who have indulged me in my speculations about some of the
themes of this book. I also owe a great debt of thanks is to my family, for
providing such a warm and lovely home environment that meant so
much to me during the long hours spent on researching and writing, and
for putting up with the time I spent on this project. Finally, I would like to
dedicate this book to the memory of the family members who are
mentioned in it, but are no longer here to provide me with their love,
v
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 27/01/21, SPi
vi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 27/01/21, SPi
CON T E N T S
Introduction 1
con t e n ts
Glossary 437
Endnotes 439
Index of Names 499
Index of Subjects 507
viii
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 27/01/21, SPi
William Golding
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 27/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
INTRODUCTION
z
in t roduc t ion
2
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
3
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
4
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
there is increasing evidence that more positive life events can beneficially
affect the brain through epigenetic mechanisms.17
Yet while focusing on the molecular and cellular biology of the human
brain is a crucial aspect of understanding how our minds work, it is
unlikely to be enough on its own, for our brains are shaped by our social
environment to a degree unique to our species. In this book, I argue that
society radically restructures the human brain within an individual per-
son’s lifetime, and that it has also played a central role in the past history
of our species, by shaping brain evolution. So if we are truly to under-
stand the human mind we must explain how the biological object that is
the brain has become infused with this social influence. But in so doing,
we must also steer a path between two overly polarized views of the
human mind.
The first viewpoint was expressed by Jim Watson, co-discoverer of the
famous double helix structure of DNA, who said of the Human Genome
Project that ‘we used to think that our fate was in our stars, but now we
know that, in large measure, our fate is in our genes.’18 This viewpoint can
be applied to humans both as a species and as individuals. At the species
level, it is linked with the idea that human behaviour and society are direct
readouts of our genetic code, which is why the genome is sometimes
referred to as the ‘blueprint’ of the species. Such a view may also be asso-
ciated with the belief that supposedly universal human characteristics
like competition, selfishness, sexism, homophobia, racism, and even a
willingness to make war on other nations are intimately linked to our
biology, and not just tendencies specific to the particular type of society
in which we currently live.
At the individual level, this viewpoint seeks to explain differences
between people—expressed via their personality, intellectual capacity, or
sexuality—primarily at the level of genetics. For Jim Watson, ‘If some-
one’s liver doesn’t work, we blame it on the genes; if someone’s brain
doesn’t work properly, we blame the school. It’s actually more humane to
think of the condition as genetic. For instance, you don’t want to say
that someone is born unpleasant, but sometimes that might be true.’19
5
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
Showing how widespread such views are among some sections of the
scientific establishment, Daniel Koshland, former editor of Science magazine,
when asked in 1989 why so much money was being invested in the
Human Genome Project rather than used to help the homeless, replied,
‘What these people don’t realize is that the homeless are [mentally]
impaired . . . Indeed, no group will benefit more from the application of
human genetics.’20 Here, current social problems are reduced to a prob-
lem of the individual, rooted in a defective biology. But Koshland’s state-
ment also reflects the optimistic view that it will be possible to identify a
clear link between genes and mental disorders, thereby leading to revolu-
tionary new drug treatments for such disorders.
The second viewpoint rejects the idea that human behaviour and soci-
ety are primarily a matter of biology, but makes the equally bold assump-
tion that genetics has little or no influence on how an individual turns out
in life. Such a view was expressed forcibly by John Watson, founder of the
behaviourist movement in psychology, who in 1930 said, ‘Give me a dozen
healthy infants, well-formed, and my own specified world to bring them
up in and I'll guarantee to take any one at random and train him to become
any type of specialist I might select—doctor, lawyer, artist, merchant-chief
and, yes, even beggar-man and thief, regardless of his talents, penchants,
tendencies, abilities, vocations and the race of his ancestors.’21
More recently, in 2013, the British Psychology Society (BPS) issued a
statement attacking what it calls the ‘biomedical model’ of mental disorders,
which many BPS members see as the dominant standpoint of most
psychiatrists and one that views such disorders primarily as illnesses to
be treated using drugs. Instead Lucy Johnstone, a clinical psychologist
who helped draw up the statement, rejects the idea that mental disorders
have any biological basis, saying that ‘On the contrary, there is now
overwhelming evidence that people suffer breakdown as a result of a
complex mix of social and psychological circumstances—bereavement
and loss, poverty and discrimination, trauma and abuse.’22
This statement is interesting because it suggests that psychiatrists and
clinical psychologists—the two main sets of health professionals that
6
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
7
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
8
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
9
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
10
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
11
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
12
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
how to help her.’40 Of course it is easy with hindsight to imagine what might
have been, but there seems little doubt that the erosion of NHS services
has had a negative impact on mental health diagnosis and treatment.
In the USA, the lack of a publicly funded health service like the NHS
means that the situation for people with mental disorders is even worse.41
Until recently, even those with a private health insurance plan were not
covered for mental illness. In November 2013 President Barack Obama’s
health reforms forced insurance companies to cover mental disorders in
the same way as other medical conditions.42 Yet the legacy of this lack of
cover means that the infrastructure to deal with mental disorders remains
woefully inadequate. Paul Appelbaum, a psychiatrist and expert on legal
issues in medicine at Columbia University, New York, has noted that ‘right
to care does not mean access to treatment. Tens of millions of people who
did not have insurance coverage may now be prompted to seek mental
health treatment. And the capacity just isn’t there to treat them. There
really is no mental health system in the US.’43 As a consequence, more
than half a million US citizens with serious mental illness are falling
through the cracks of society, leading Tim Murphy, a child psychologist
and Republican congressman, to remark that ‘we have replaced the hos
pital bed with the jail cell, the homeless shelter and the coffin. How is that
compassionate?’44
While US states have been reducing hospital bed places for decades, the
worst cuts occurred following the recession that began in 2008, with $5
billion disappearing from mental health services budgets from 2009 to
2012.45 In the same period, 4,500 public psychiatric hospital beds—nearly
10 per cent of the total supply—were lost in the USA. According to the
2012 US National Survey on Drug Use and Health, nearly 40 per cent of
adults with ‘severe’ mental illness received no treatment at all.46 One pos-
sible consequence of this situation is mass shooting incidents, reports of
which now regularly punctuate the US news. Yet a less reported but
ultimately far greater loss is the number of Americans who commit sui-
cide each year, with, for instance, almost 45,000 people taking their own
13
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
lives in the USA in 2016—a fate that claims the lives of more citizens than
car accidents, prostate cancer, or homicides, according to the US Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention.47 Worryingly, the US suicide rate
increased 33 per cent from 1999 to 2017, with rates rising particularly
sharply since 2006.48 So while the US suicide rate increased by about 1 per
cent a year from 2000 to 2006, that rate doubled from 2006 to 2016.49
And although suicide is the starkest indicator of mental distress, there are
others; so drug overdoses claimed 70,000 US lives in 2017. Because of
this, US life expectancy, perhaps the broadest measure of a nation’s health,
fell from 2016 to 2019, in part because of the rise in drug overdoses and
suicides.50 This was the biggest three-year drop since the years 1915 to 1918.
Mental health problems cause misery for sufferers and their families,
but also have an economic impact; for instance they cost the British econ-
omy £110 billion a year, according to a 2009 report by the Royal College of
Psychiatrists, the London School of Economics, and the NHS Mental
Health Network.51 Obviously, a better resourced health service would be a
key step to helping people with mental disorders, but improving the treat-
ments—based on both drugs and psychotherapy—available for such
people is also critical. Ultimately, the possibility of such an improvement
rests on a better understanding of how the human mind works, and how
this relates to human brain function, topics with which this book will be
concerned. Here though we face a problem, for while there are many dif-
ferent viewpoints about these subjects, what is lacking is a unifying
framework that binds them together.
This problem is not new. In 1934, comparing viewpoints about the
mind at that time, the Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky stated that ‘there
exist many psychologies, but [no] unified psychology.’52 At the time, three
particularly influential views were introspective psychology, behaviourism,
and psychoanalysis. Introspective psychology, pioneered by individuals
like William James (brother of novelist Henry James) sought to understand
the human mind by questioning individuals about their innermost thoughts.
Behaviourism, as already mentioned, was stimulated by Ivan Pavlov’s dis-
covery of conditional reflexes. The third viewpoint, psychoanalysis,
14
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
15
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
in t roduc t ion
16
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
Part I
ORIGINS OF MIND
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 1
mind a nd m at t e r
Humans
Animals
Plants
Vegetative Sensitive Rational
Soul Reproduction Soul Soul
& Mobility
Growth & Thought
Sensation &
Reflection
20
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
are asleep, and also such waking actions as walking, singing, and the like’.6
This was bold thinking, for it suggested that not only bodily functions but
also aspects of human behaviour can be viewed as analogous to the work-
ings of a machine. But Descartes was careful to note that higher mental
functions such as conscious awareness, free will, and personality cannot
be explained in this manner, saying that ‘on the one hand I have a clear
and distinct idea of myself, in so far as I am simply a thinking, non-extended
thing [that is, a mind], and on the other hand I have a distinct idea of body,
in so far as this is simply an extended, non-thinking thing.’7 This led to his
famous statement, ‘I think, therefore I am.’8
Although Descartes believed that the soul can influence the body
through a point of contact he arbitrarily located in the pineal gland, he
argued that its supernatural form made it impossible to understand by
scientific methods. This separation between body and mind left open the
possibility that the mind has an immortal nature, as the soul, in keeping
with the idea of an afterlife. Descartes’ standpoint made some sense given
the limited understanding of brain function at that time. It may also have
been a pragmatic position given the Church’s persecution of individuals
for putting forward scientific views about the physical universe, let alone
questioning the existence of an immortal soul. But the effects of this
separation have clouded understanding of the human mind—and how it
works—to the present day.
One of the first people to challenge the idea that the mind could not be
studied scientifically like the rest of the human body was the philosopher
John Locke. Born in Somerset in 1632, Locke grew up during the English
Civil War of 1642–9;9 indeed his father was a captain in the parliamentary
army, and as a student at Westminster School in London, Locke was
deeply affected by hearing the groan of the crowd at Charles I’s execution,
which took place at nearby Whitehall in January 1649.10 The social revolu-
tion that accompanied the civil war transformed British society by
overturning the old feudal order, allowing new ideas about science and
society to blossom.11 As a scholar at Christ Church in Oxford, Locke
turned his attention to the human mind; his solution to the problem of
21
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
22
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
23
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
24
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
DNA
D
A T G C C T G T T C A T C G T three bases
code for
particular amino acid, the basic units that when linked together form
proteins (Figure 2). For instance, GTT codes for the amino acid valine,
CCT for proline, and so on for all twenty amino acids. Working out the
genetic code was a key scientific achievement of the 1960s.21 Finally in
1977 Fred Sanger developed a method for ‘sequencing’—that is, reading—
all the letters of a genome. Following a colossal effort involving scientists
across the globe, the human genome was fully sequenced in April 2003.22
The central importance of DNA to life—and the fact that differences in
genomes ultimately define to which species an individual organism
belongs—led to claims that possession of the human genome sequence
would have a major impact on our understanding of what it means to be
human. The late Sir John Sulston, who led the Human Genome Project in
Britain, said at the end of the project that ‘we’ve now got to the point in
human history where for the first time we are going to hold in our hands
the set of instructions to make a human being.’23 So given that a defining
feature of being human is our conscious minds, how have advances in
genetics affected our ability to understand the way our minds work? To
properly address this question, it is worth restating a few basic principles
of genetics.
Genetics was established by Gregor Mendel, a monk working in a
monastery in Brno, once part of the Austro-Hungarian Empire but now in
the Czech Republic. In 1865 Mendel showed that particular characteristics
25
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
NN Nn Nn nn Nn nn Nn nn
Unaffected Carrier Carrier Affected Affected Unaffected Affected Unaffected
26
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
27
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
28
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
xy xx
Unaffected Carrier
father mother
xy xy xx xx
Unaffected Affected Carrier Unaffected
son son daughter daughter
this genetic defect to their children before they realize they have the
disease and can transmit it.31
It has been possible to link disorders like cystic fibrosis, DMD, and
Huntington’s to single genes because the genetic changes associated with
them have such dramatic effects. For instance, with DMD, no functional
dystrophin protein is produced, while in Huntington’s, the negative
effects of the mutant huntingtin protein are so severe that a sufferer even-
tually dies from the condition. Yet there is growing evidence that more
common conditions, whether bodily disorders like diabetes or heart dis-
ease, or those that affect the mind like depression or schizophrenia, have
no such simple genetic link.
Why this is so is something that I will explore in greater detail in later
chapters of this book, but it does suggest that claims about the genetic
basis of human behaviour, and differences between human individuals,
that are based on a simple extrapolation from rare single-gene disorders
have overexaggerated the similarities between these disorders and the far
more common ailments that can affect both the mind and body.
29
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
This also seems to be the case for other human characteristics often
claimed to have a clear genetic basis, ranging from intelligence to sexual
inclination. For instance, a recent study investigated nearly 80,000 chil-
dren and adults in order to identify differences in DNA sequence across
the human genome associated with ‘general intelligence’—defined by an
individual’s IQ score and ability to give answers to brief touchscreen puz-
zles. The study identified 52 genomic regions that showed such an asso-
ciation.32 However, these only account for about 5 per cent of the IQ
scores among different people. Given that IQ tests themselves have been
criticized for their class and cultural bias, that is hardly a ringing endorse-
ment for a genetic basis for intelligence. Media reports in 1993 that a ‘gay
gene’ predisposes men to becoming homosexual were misleading given
that no gene had been discovered, only an association with a slight genetic
difference in the X chromosome.33 In fact, subsequent attempts to find an
actual gene failed to identify such an entity.34 None of this means genes
are not involved in human behaviour—an aspect I will explore later in
this book—but it does mean we need to move beyond simplistic views
about their involvement.
Linked to the attempt to identify simple links between genes and
human behaviour has been the claim that such genes map directly on to
structures in the brain. Harvard University psychologist Steven Pinker
has proposed that the human brain is an interacting community of dis-
tinct modules, each specialized for a particular function. Conveniently,
each module is suggested to have a specific genetic basis.35 Explaining
how such genetically coded brain modules arose, Pinker has argued that
each evolved as a response to problems faced by our hunter-gatherer
ancestors as they sought to survive and prosper in a hostile environment.
Yet neuroscientist Steven Rose of the British Open University has criti-
cized Pinker on the grounds that the latter seems ‘not very interested in
actual brains, since his mental “modules” may or may not map onto
actual neuronal ensembles.’36 In fact recent studies of the brain confirm
that the brain is modular in some respects, yet increasingly point to con-
sciousness as a feature of the whole brain. This raises the question of how
30
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m at t e r
a brain that is partly modular in its structure might at the same time only
function properly as an interconnected whole—something we will con-
sider later in this book.
In summary, a major problem with much popular speculation about
the biological roots of consciousness is that those who advocate a
gene-based view of consciousness often appear to have little understand-
ing of modern genetics, while speculation about how brain structures
shape that consciousness often bears little resemblance to emerging
knowledge about the complexity of an actual human brain. There is a
common thread here, which is that idealized genes and brains have been
substituted for real ones. Unfortunately, because of this tendency, it is not
clear how much we have really advanced forwards from René Descartes
and his belief that the human mind was an unknowable entity or, for that
matter, the behaviourists with their view that the human mind could be
treated as a black box.
In contrast, to understand human consciousness we need to under-
stand real genes, real brains, and how these have evolved in humans com-
pared to other species. And to do this means first of all re-examining the
unique features that distinguish us from such species.
31
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 2
tool a nd sy mbol
33
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
34
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
35
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
36
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
Homo
H
heidelbergensis
Homo HHomo
1 antecessor eerectus P.
? robustus
P.
Homo boisei
ergaster Homo
2 habilis
Homo species
Paranthropus
Australopithecus aethiopicus
africanus
3
Australopithecus
afarensis
‘Lucy’ Australopithecus
4 anamensis
Ardipithecus
ramidus
5 ‘Ardi’
37
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
38
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
In addition, the tools they created were probably still quite ad hoc and largely
disposable. Thomas Wynn, a palaeoanthropologist of the University of
Colorado, believes that the new findings suggest that ‘when you needed a
stone tool and you didn’t have one, you just made one, then dropped it.’23
A breakthrough in tool-making seems to have taken place in a later
proto-human species—Homo erectus. According to Wynn, ‘the technol-
ogy [of this species] is really different, more sophisticated in a cognitive
way than anything earlier hominids or chimpanzees could do—some see
cognitive abilities to coordinate spatial and shape information that chim-
panzees don’t have.’24 Homo erectus was far less based in the treetops, and
the tools it created were much less disposable, pointing to a greater invest-
ment of labour in these objects, and also perhaps more of a sense of their
use in different, future situations.
If sustained tool use was the initial defining feature of humanity, how
did this translate into a capacity for language and accelerated brain devel-
opment? Engels believed that increasing use and design of tools made
possible by the bipedalism of our proto-human ancestors was a key step
in the development of language. As he put it, communal tool use ‘helped
to bring the members of society together by increasing the cases of
mutual support and joint activity . . . Men-in-the-making arrived at the
point where they had something to say to each other.’25 During this pro-
cess, ‘the reaction of labour and speech on the development of the brain
and its attendant senses, of the increasing clarity of consciousness, power
of abstraction and of conclusion, gave both labour and speech an ever
renewed impulse to further development.’26 So what is the evidence for
such a process of positive feedback?
One problem in exploring this question is that, unlike fossils and stone
tools, language leaves no direct imprint to be discovered by archaeologists,
although it does make its presence known in the human body through
changes in the larynx and in certain brain regions. Nevertheless, these are
subtle changes that are difficult to detect in a fossilized skeleton. Instead, we
must guess the likely development of language evolution by indirect means.
39
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
40
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
found that blood flow changes during the first ten seconds of each
experimental period—when the volunteers were working out how to
shape the stone or thinking up their first words—were very similar.
Uomini and Meyer think that their findings suggest that language and
tool-making coevolved as early as 1.75 million years ago. Yet since they
used a tool-making method known as the Late Acheulean, dating from
half a million years ago, that put much greater emphasis on symmetry and
aesthetic considerations than the Early Acheulean method of 1.75 million
years ago, Michael Corballis, of the University of Auckland, New Zealand,
believes that while the study does suggest coevolution between language
and toolmaking, ‘language itself emerged much later, but was built on
circuits established during the Acheulean period.’30
Exactly how language first arose in humans may always remain a mat-
ter of speculation in the absence of direct evidence. What we can investi-
gate directly is what makes language capacity unique to human beings. In
fact not everyone accepts this uniqueness. One area of dispute is the
extent to which humanity’s extraordinary language capability is innate,
rather than something learned. I have already mentioned the view of
behaviourists like B. F. Skinner that all human behaviour is a series of con-
ditioned reflexes. Skinner viewed language as no different in this respect.
He believed that if a child says ‘milk’ and her mother subsequently gives
her some, the child finds this rewarding, enhancing her language develop-
ment. Evidence for this view came from studies like one by Doug Guess of
Kansas University, who in 1968 claimed he had taught a girl with severe
learning disabilities to make correct grammatical utterances using posi-
tive reinforcement of praise and food.31 And, indeed, studies of chimpan-
zees in the 1960s and 1970s suggested that the ability to be taught language
by such reinforcement might not even be unique to humans.
Although chimps have no vocal apparatus for making the sounds of
human speech, Allen and Beatrix Gardner, psychologists at the University
of Nevada, claimed to have taught a chimp, Washoe, to use American sign
language for the deaf.32 The Gardners raised Washoe like their own child;
she wore clothes and sat with them at the dinner table. During her life
41
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
Washoe learned over 250 symbols and was even said to have coined
completely novel words. For instance, after seeing a swan, Washoe made
the two signs for ‘water’ and ‘bird’, suggesting she had grasped the link
between the two. Hearing about this incident, Harvard University psych
ologist Roger Brown viewed it ‘like getting an S.O.S. from outer space’.33
Washoe was not the only ape to display an apparent flair for learning
sign language. A bonobo chimp, Kanzi, learned over 400 symbols, and
was said to have invented novel words, referred to past versus present
events, and understood others’ points of view, all implying a more com-
plex understanding of language than simply associating words with their
objects.34 And a gorilla, Koko, was reported to be able to understand
grammar, something only humans were thought capable of doing. On
being informed of the death of Robin Williams in 2014, it was said that
Koko made the sign for ‘cry’, and ‘looked very thoughtful’, despite having
only met the actor once in 2001, when Williams spent an afternoon with
the celebrity ape.35
Despite the potency of such stories, major criticisms have been raised
regarding the idea that language acquisition in humans is simply a matter
of reinforcement. Notably in the 1960s Noam Chomsky of the
Massachusetts Institute of Technology identified key problems with this
view.36 Chomsky highlighted a central feature of human language, namely
its capacity for expressing an infinite number of ideas using a limited
set of symbols. Such creativity is made possible not only by the size of a
person’s vocabulary, but also by how words are strung together in a pre-
cise order. Chomsky argued that the speed with which children learn new
words, but also how to combine them in a grammatically precise fash-
ion, means our species must have an innate biological capacity for
complex language.37
But what about the claims that other primates can learn human lan-
guage? In fact there is now evidence that experimenters who claimed
amazing linguistic abilities for Washoe the chimp or Koko the gorilla may
have overinterpreted their responses, and even unconsciously given the
apes visual cues about what to say. Herbert Terrace, who studied the
42
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
43
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
research methods, but eventually gave up on the debate, saying that her
time would be ‘much better spent conversing with the gorillas’.42
Since taught apes may often be merely mimicking the gestures of
human researchers, and researchers may be showing selective bias in
drawing conclusions about ape language capacity, another approach
would be to study whether apes show a capacity for sophisticated forms
of communication in the wild. Recently Catherine Hobaiter and col-
leagues at the University of St Andrews in Scotland investigated wild
chimpanzees and claimed to have found that they can communicate 19
specific messages to one another with a ‘lexicon’ of 66 gestures. Hobaiter
believes the chimps are similar to humans in having a communication
system in which they deliberately sent a message to another individual.
For her, ‘the big message . . . is that there is another species out there that is
meaningful in its communication, so that's not unique to humans.’43 Yet
while Hobaiter claims that some gestures are only used to convey one
meaning, like leaf clipping, where a chimp takes small bites from leaves to
elicit sexual attention, undermining this claim, other gestures were more
ambiguous; for instance, a grab can apparently signify either ‘Stop that’,
‘Climb on me’, or ‘Move away.’44
Susanne Shultz, a biologist at the University of Manchester, believes
the researchers’ attempts to fill in the gaps in our knowledge of the evolu-
tion of human language are commendable, but ‘the vagueness of the ges-
ture meanings suggest either that the chimps have little to communicate,
or we are still missing a lot of the information contained in their gestures
and actions. Moreover, the meanings seem to not go beyond what other
less sophisticated animals convey with non-verbal communication.’45 It
therefore seems that fundamental differences remain in terms of language
capacity between humans and apes.
Yet while the behaviourist view that human language acquisition is
simply an accumulation of conditioned reflexes now looks incorrect,
recent studies have also challenged Chomsky’s view of a biological basis
for a ‘universal grammar’ shared by all humans.46 Instead, increasing evi-
dence points to both human biology and the process of growing up in a
44
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
tool a nd sy mbol
45
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 3
saw every ‘man [as] born free and master of himself’,4 were important
proponents of this individualist viewpoint.
The importance of the individual is taken for granted in modern capit
alist society. Indeed, former British Prime Minister Margaret Thatcher
even notoriously claimed in 1987 that ‘there’s no such thing as society.
There are individual men and women and there are families.’5 While many
would disagree with this extreme position, the view that society is merely
a collection of individuals driven by self-interest is a very common one.
Yet recent studies have questioned the idea that we are naturally soli-
tary individuals. Instead they suggest that socializing with others is so
central to our species that rejection—whether by a lover, friend, or even
while playing a game with total strangers—is registered in the same brain
regions as respond to physical pain. One such study, led by Matthew
Lieberman at the University of California, Los Angeles, imaged the brains
of volunteers playing an invented online game called Cyberball.6 When
invisible online ‘players’ deliberately stopped passing the e-ball to the vol-
unteer, the brain region that lit up in the scanner was the one also acti-
vated by physical pain. Looking at scans from two studies side by side,
Lieberman noted that ‘without knowing which was an analysis of phys
ical pain and which was an analysis of social pain, you wouldn’t have been
able to tell the difference.’7
Other studies have undermined the idea that human beings are inher-
ently selfish, indicating instead that altruistic acts trigger activity in the
‘reward’ region of the brain that is stimulated when a person experiences
pleasure. For instance, Jorge Moll and colleagues at the National Institutes
of Health, near Washington, DC, showed that this region became more
active when people gave $10 to charity than when they received the same
amount.8 And Tristen Inagaki and Naomi Eisenberger of the University of
California, Los Angeles, have shown that comforting someone in distress
is a powerful stimulus for this brain region.9 They performed brain scans
on women holding their boyfriend’s hand, both when the couples were
sitting together normally and when the man was given an electric shock.
The brain’s reward region was activated on both occasions, but even
47
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
48
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
49
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
50
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
Culture
DEV LOPM NT
DEVELOPMENT
Social Language
interactions
ons
51
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
and thin blocks; and ‘cev’—small and thin—on the reverse of each block.
The children were not told that these four words represented such com-
plex concepts, nor did they know that the words referred to a combin
ation of the height and the size of the object. The child was asked to group
together all blocks that might belong to the same kind. After this sorting
exercise, the experimenter picked a wrongly selected block, read its name,
and encouraged the child to keep trying. Following each attempt, another
wrongly selected block was picked up, and its name revealed. In this way,
the child could discover to which characteristics the words referred.
Vygotsky called his test the ‘double stimulation’ method, since the child
was stimulated both by the physical properties of the blocks and by the
nonsense names.25 The test was supposed to indicate the level of concep-
tual problem solving that the child had reached in their mental develop-
ment. The overall finding of the study was that true conceptual problem
solving was only achieved by older adolescents. But also interesting was
what was revealed about the different stages of preconceptual thinking
in children.
Vygotsky named the preconceptual stages syncretic, complex, and
pseudo-conceptual. The youngest children grouped blocks syncretically,
meaning they collected blocks at random, based on a vague idea that they
belonged together.26 Complex thinking was more advanced in that blocks
were grouped by real similarity, but inconsistently. So in a ‘collection
complex’, blocks were paired by complementarity: red with blue, tall with
flat, and so on. In a ‘chain complex’, if the first block was red and round,
the second one could be red and triangular, the third triangular and green.
This type of thinking was distinguished from true conceptual thought by
showing no hierarchical organization in the selections.
An intriguing phenomenon uncovered by the study was ‘pseudo-
conceptual’ thinking.27 Here, at first glance children appeared to be
sorting blocks on a purely conceptual basis, selecting them for instance by
a shared feature, like colour. The preconceptual nature of such a selection
was only revealed when the experimenter turned over a block and
exposed its inaccuracy according to the nonsense word describing it.
52
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
53
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
work whose content was drawn from the community from which the
children came. Moll and Greenberg’s approach involved creating ‘mean-
ingful connections between academic and social life through the concrete
learning activities of the students’.32 Spelling and grammar were not
ignored in the study, but the emphasis was on treating reading and writ-
ing as communicative and meaningful. With such an approach even the
most uninterested or apparently incapable children made great improve-
ments in their literary skills.33
Vygotsky was not the only thinker with such a view of the key role of
language in mental development working in Russia in the 1920s and
1930s. Valentin Voloshinov, who also died young, in 1935, aged only 40, in
this case after incarceration in a Stalinist ‘Gulag’ prison-camp,34 came to
similar conclusions during this period. Although Voloshinov and
Vygotsky are not recorded as having met or read each other’s work, cer-
tain themes ‘in the air’ in Russia at this time may have contributed to the
large degree of overlap and complementarity in their approaches to the
study of human consciousness.35
Voloshinov was more concerned with adult thought patterns than
those occurring in children. He argued that people ‘think and feel and
desire with the help of words; without inner speech we would not become
conscious of anything in ourselves. This process of inner speech is just as
material as is outward speech.’36 But he also believed that thought and
language share the same transmission medium and have the same source,
that is, society. He proposed that each person engages in ‘horizontal’
social relationships with other individuals in specific speech acts, and
simultaneously in ‘vertical’ internal relationships between the outer
world and their own psyche.37 The mind is thus not an internal but a
boundary phenomenon. Or as Voloshinov put it, ‘individual conscious-
ness is not the architect of the ideological superstructure, but only a ten-
ant lodging in the social edifice of ideological signs.’38
Expressed in this way, it might appear that Voloshinov saw language,
and therefore thought, as something imposed upon the individual, like
the ‘blank slate’ view of the mind of the behaviourists. But this was far
54
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
55
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
56
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
57
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
58
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
59
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
60
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
Part II
CH A P T ER 4
ne rv e a nd br a in
Dendrites
Terminal bulb
Nucleus
Cell body
Axon Myelin sheath
eat
Transmitted signal
Incoming
i signal
i
Axon
Myelin layers
Neural signal
64
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
Axon terminal
Sending
neuron
Synaptic vesicles
carrying neurotransmitter
Neurotransmitter
Receiving
ng released into synapse
neuron
Neurotransmitter
attached to receptor
Nerve iimpulse
re-initiated
A crucial property of the brain and nervous system is that neurons are
connected together in chains. There is no direct electrical contact between
adjacent neurons; instead, chemical messengers known as neurotrans-
mitters are released into the narrow space between the axon of one
neuron and the dendrite of the next—the synapse (Figure 9). By a process
similar to a key fitting into a lock, neurotransmitters interact with
‘receptor’ regions of particular ion channels on the surface of the dendrite
to open or close particular channels, altering the ionic composition of the
neuron as a consequence.
How neurons conduct electrical impulses was shown by Alan Hodgkin
and Andrew Huxley at the University of Cambridge in 1939.6 They studied
axons from giant squids, which were large enough that the researchers
could impale them with electrodes, recording both electrical current and
voltage. These studies showed that when unstimulated, the neuron has a
negative electric charge. Activating neurotransmitters cause sodium ions
to flow into the cell. Since sodium is positively charged, the neuron
becomes more positive. At a certain point, it can become so positive that
this triggers an explosive rise and fall of electrical charge called an action
potential, the fall being due to positive potassium ions rushing out of
the cell.7 The action potential sweeps like a wave along the axon, and,
due to the conducting effect of the myelin sheath, it can reach speeds of
65
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
250 miles per hour in some human neurons. At the axon’s end, the action
potential causes the release of neurotransmitter into a synapse, and this in
turn activates or inhibits a neighbouring neuron via its dendrites. Once a
neuron has conducted an action potential, protein pumps on its surface
return its ionic composition to a resting level, at which point it can begin
the process of electrical conduction once more.
Since an axon may have hundreds of branches, it can activate or inhibit
the dendrites of many other neurons. As each neuron has many den-
drites, whether the activation of a dendrite in a neighbouring neuron
leads to an action potential depends on the information flowing into that
neuron from other axons, and whether the neurotransmitters being
released into each synapse are excitatory or inhibitory. In this way the
behaviour of each neuron is influenced by the sum of the information
flowing into it via all its dendrites.
This is the situation for all neurons, but there is much diversity in size
and shape between these cells.8 Multipolar neurons with the form
described earlier—a cell body from which emerges a single long axon and
a crown of shorter branching dendrites—are the most common type of
neuron in our nervous system. But one type of multipolar neuron only
has a single primary projection that functions as both axon and dendrites.
And neurons involved in sensing pressure, touch, and pain have axons
that split in two opposite directions, with one end heading for the skin,
joints, and muscle, while the other connects with the spinal cord. The
latter is a bundle of nerves running up and down the spine, and can be
viewed as akin to an information superhighway, transmitting messages to
and from the brain at rapid speed.
Neurons can be classified according to their functional role (Figure 10).9
Sensory neurons supply information to the brain from sensory organs
like the eyes, ears, nose, tongue, and skin. Motor neurons spread out from
the brain and spinal cord to regulate the action of muscles, which they
activate via a ‘neuromuscular junction’, a structure that works in a similar
way to a synapse. Interneurons connect neurons in the brain; while some
66
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
Rece
Receptor
cells
Musc
Muscle
link distant brain regions, others group neurons into smaller circuits of
neighbouring cells. But descriptions of such major classes only give a fla-
vour of the true complexity of the brain, which contains hundreds of
neuronal types. Even a glance at some of the names assigned to nerve
cells—cone, climbing fibre, crab-like, medium spiny, pyramidal, chande-
lier, and tripolar—gives a sense of the multitude of the variety of cell
shapes found in the brain.
One class of neurons that have excited much interest recently gets their
name not from their shape, but from their function. They were discovered
by Giacomo Rizzolatti and colleagues at the University of Parma in Italy
in 1992 when they implanted microelectrodes in the brains of monkeys to
study their brain activity during different actions, such as the clutching of
food.10 One day, as a researcher reached for his own food, he noticed
neurons activating in the monkeys’ premotor cortex—the same region
that showed activity when the animals made their own similar hand
movement. Following up this intriguing observation, the group found
that this activity was due to a class of neurons which they dubbed ‘mirror
neurons’ that fire when a monkey performs an action, but also when it
67
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
Oligodendrocyte
Microglia
Astrocyte
68
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
changing. For it turns out that glial cells can play a highly active role in
brain cell communication, and perhaps in the development of human
intelligence.
If asked to name the most intelligent person of the twentieth century,
many people would probably identify Albert Einstein as their chosen fig-
ure of genius. When Einstein died in 1955 and left his body to be examined
for the purposes of medical research, some people expected that his brain
would turn out to be a lot bigger than those of less distinguished individ
uals. Yet a post-mortem showed that not only was Einstein’s brain of aver-
age size, but also there was nothing unusual about the number or size of
its neurons. But in the late 1980s, some scientists investigating Einstein’s
brain claimed that it had an unusually large number of neuroglia, espe-
cially in the association cortex, an area of the brain linked to imagination
and complex thinking.14 Quite what the significance of this finding about
one, albeit very gifted, individual might be for our understanding of brain
function is far from clear, but it is one of the discoveries that have chal-
lenged the idea of glial cells as merely supporting actors.
One reason why neuroglia were underestimated for so long was that
they seemed to have no way of transmitting information in the brain.
Neurons, as we have seen, carry information along their length via action
potentials, and communicate with other neurons by releasing chemical
messengers across connecting synapses. In contrast, neuroglia cannot
generate action potentials. However, recent studies have shown that
chemical communication is just as important for them. They turn out to
have many of the same cell surface receptors as neurons, allowing them to
communicate with both neurons and other neuroglia.15
One class of neuroglia, called astrocytes, play an important role in the
formation of synapses.16 If rodent neurons are cultured outside the body
in the absence of astrocytes, they form few synaptic connections. But if
astrocytes are added to the culture, the number of synapses rapidly rises,
and synaptic activity increases tenfold. Astrocytes secrete chemicals that
enhance synapse formation and directly regulate synaptic activity. The
close physical association between synapses and astrocytes ensures that
69
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
these are the first cells to respond to changes in synaptic activity during
embryo development, but also in the adult brain.17 Astrocytes are highly
dynamic, constantly modulating their association with synapses—and
thereby their influence—with the dynamic pattern being dependent on
the state of the brain.
Another class of glial cells called microglia make up about 10 per cent
of the cells in the brain.18 For some years now these cells have been recog-
nized as the brain’s primary defenders against disease; they identify
injured neurons and destroy them, and strip away defective synapses,
thereby removing diseased cells that have a negative impact on the rest of
the brain.19 For decades, scientists thought of microglia only as immune
cells and believed that they were quiet and passive in the absence of an
infectious invader. Recently though, that idea has begun to change. One
clue that microglia might have a wider role was the discovery that these
are the fastest-moving cells in a healthy brain. For instance, recent studies
have revealed that microglia can reach out to surrounding neurons and
contact synapses in the absence of disease. These cells regulate the num-
ber of synaptic connections just as a gardener prunes a plant. This is
important because, for reasons that are not fully understood, the brain
begins with more synapses than it needs. Cornelius Gross, of the European
Molecular Biology Laboratory in Heidelberg, who studies this process,
has shown that microglia help sculpt the brain by eliminating unwanted
synapses.20 But how do the microglia know which synapses to remove
and which to leave?
In fact, microglia can receive two types of message from neurons—one
that identifies synapses that play an important role and should be pre-
served, another that highlights weaker ones that need pruning. Pruning is
important, as shown by the demonstration by Gross’s team that remov-
ing the receptor for a chemical named fractalkine from microglia leads to
weak synaptic contacts caused by defective synaptic pruning in the
hippocampus, a brain region involved in learning and memory. Gross
believes this shows that during embryo development neurons ‘call out’ to
microglia for assistance with pruning.21 Yet it appears that there are also
70
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
71
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
Cortex
Thalamus
Hypothalamus
Amygdala
Hippocampus Cerebellum
Pons
Medulla
Let us now move from the cellular structure of the brain, with its many
types of neurons and glia, to its large-scale structure. The brain has three
main parts: the forebrain, midbrain, and hindbrain (Figure 12).27 The fore-
brain includes the cerebrum, which has two hemispheres and a highly
folded surface ‘cortex’—derived from the Latin word for bark, for indeed
the cortex wraps around the brain like bark on a tree. The cortex is par-
ticularly enlarged in humans, in places having double the area it should
for a primate of our size.28 This is the part of the human brain thought to
be involved in reasoning, planning, and problem solving. But, demon-
strating how specific brain regions are involved in multiple functions, it
also plays key roles in the regulation of movement, perception, visual
processing, recognition of sounds, and speech.
Also buried within the forebrain are the various parts of the limbic sys-
tem, which are more ancient from an evolutionary viewpoint and often
called the ‘emotional brain’, reflecting their role in mediating different
emotions, or even the ‘lizard brain’, to convey the fact that we share these
brain regions with ‘lower’ organisms.29 The limbic system contains the
72
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
73
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
74
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
that size is not the only factor to consider, australopithecine brains show
subtle changes in structure and shape compared with those of apes.42 For
instance in this species, the neocortex—the part of the brain involved in
higher-order brain functions in modern humans—had begun to expand.
The most dramatic changes in brain size and structure occurred in the
final phase of human evolutionary change.43 Homo habilis, which appeared
on Earth 1.9 million years ago and is particularly associated with stone-
tool production and use, saw a modest increase in brain size, but also an
apparent expansion of a region of the frontal cortex linked to language
called Broca’s area. The first fossil skulls of Homo erectus, which appeared
1.8 million years ago, had brains averaging just over 600 ml, in contrast to
Homo sapiens, which has an average brain volume of around 1,400 ml.44
Chet Sherwood, an anthropologist at George Washington University in
Washington, DC, believes that there must have been a clear functional
benefit for such a change. As he notes, ‘brains don’t just do that for no
reason in evolution. Evolution is frugal and cost-effective, and brain tissue
has extraordinary metabolic expense. There must have been some adap-
tive value to brain size increase.’45
Neanderthals, who lived between 400,000 and 40,000 years ago, had
brains similar in size to those of modern humans. But Robin Dunbar and
colleagues at the University of Oxford have suggested that Neanderthals
devoted more of their brains to controlling their bodies and regulating
their vision.46 This was because these proto-humans had significantly
wider shoulders, thicker bones, and larger eyes than early humans. The
researchers believe that this latter difference could be because
Neanderthals underwent their key stages of evolution in Europe, at a
higher latitude, and therefore poorer light conditions, than Africa, where
Homo sapiens originated. After ‘correcting’ for these differences, the
amount of brain volume left for other tasks is significantly smaller for
Neanderthals than for modern humans. So while the average brain
volumes of the two groups is the same, at around 1,400 ml, the corrected
average Neanderthal brain volume is just 1,134 ml, compared to 1,332 ml
for humans.47
75
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
76
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
77
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
78
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
ne rv e a nd br a in
79
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 5
understanding of how the human brain works, and of some of the factors
that might make it unique compared to those of other species. But first, let
us review what a gene is, and how it is regulated.
Traditionally, a gene has been defined as a stretch of DNA that codes for
a specific protein. Proteins are the cell’s building blocks, but also perform
many other functions, such as transporting substances in and out of the
cell, turning food into energy, and producing the chemical signals that
regulate cell function. DNA acts as a linear code of four letters, the chem-
ical ‘bases’ A, C, G, and T,5 but proteins too can be viewed as a linear code,
with their letters being amino acids. The connection between the two
molecules is that a triplet of DNA bases codes for each amino acid. In fact
the DNA information in each gene is copied into an intermediate form—
a type of RNA called messenger RNA (Figure 13). This copying process,
known as ‘transcription’, is carried out by a catalytic protein, or enzyme,
called RNA polymerase, and it occurs in the cell nucleus. The messenger
RNA is then transported out of the nucleus and into the cell’s cytoplasm.
Here, the information within it is ‘translated’ by a cellular machine called
a ribosome into a linear chain of amino acids, which then folds into a 3D
structure—a protein. Because each of the 20 amino acids has a specific
RNA polymerase
DNA Transcription
mRNA
Ribosome
Translation
Polypeptide
81
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
size, shape, and chemical property, each type of protein will have a unique
set of properties. Haemoglobin, for instance, is a globular protein exquis
itely suited for its role carrying oxygen around the body, while collagen is
a long, highly tensile protein that provides support for bones and tendons.
Yet both are produced by information provided by the linear DNA code.
Genes are turned on or off by proteins called transcription factors,
which attach themselves to regulatory DNA regions associated with a
gene. They then interact with RNA polymerase, enhancing or suppress-
ing its ability to transcribe the DNA information into messenger RNA.6
Such is the situation in all organisms, from bacteria to humans. But the
human genome is very different from the compact genetic entity in a bac-
terium, in that our genes make up less than 2 per cent of our genome. The
proportion of the remaining 98 per cent of DNA that has some function
is currently uncertain: it has been estimated at between 8 and 80 per cent
of the genome, with continuing debate about the exact figure.7
A major source of such new insights is the Encyclopaedia of DNA
Elements (ENCODE) project, a follow-up to the Human Genome Project
and the subject of my book The Deeper Genome.8 For geneticist Bing Ren of
the University of California, San Diego, ‘it’s not overstating to say that
ENCODE is as significant for our understanding of the human genome as
the original DNA sequencing of the human genome.’9 ENCODE sought to
map all the functional activity in the human genome. One of its revela-
tions was that the regulatory regions that switch genes on or off may
number as many as four million, in contrast to the approximately 20,000
genes in our genome. Rather than being on/off regulators these regions
are more like the many switches a sound engineer uses to control a band’s
live performance, since they control the expression of the messenger
RNA in a graded fashion.
Another revelation was that such switches are often far away—at
least in linear terms on the chromosome—from the genes they control.
At first, this discovery made little sense; however, recent studies have shown
that each chromosome in the living cell has an intricate 3D structure.10
Consequently, switches that appear distant if we think of a c hromosome
82
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
83
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
84
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
85
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
86
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
after 22 weeks, chimp brain growth levels off while human development
continues. Meanwhile, another recent study has shown that specific dif-
ferences in a gene called NOTCH2 increase the human brain’s production
of neural stem cells and delay their maturation into cortical neurons,
which may also help to explain why our brains keep growing far longer
than those of other primates.27
Size alone, however, is unlikely to explain the unique characteristics of
the human brain. Scientists are also trying to identify the genetic basis
of structural and functional differences between our brains and those of
other mammalian species, particularly primates. Studies such as those
above have identified novel human genes, as well as human-specific pat-
terns of expression of genes present in both humans and chimpanzees,
that may regulate unique human characteristics such as conscious aware-
ness. Yet, other findings suggest that a key difference between humans
and chimps is that our brains are less rigidly controlled by our genes,
allowing the environment to play a greater role in our brain development.
One such study led by Aida Gómez-Robles at George Washington
University in Washington, DC, compared hundreds of human and chim-
panzee brains using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), which provides
a detailed image of the living brain.28 The researchers obtained family
trees for both humans and chimps, which meant they could measure
similarity in the brains of genetically related individuals, including identi-
cal twins. The study showed that related humans vary far more in the
shape and location of their neocortical folds than chimpanzees, with two
chimp brothers being far more similar than two human brothers in the
shape and location of these folds.29 This suggests that chimps are more
limited in the ways their brains can develop and their ability to learn new
behaviours or skills than humans. And with such a loosening of the
genetic ties in humans, our brains are probably more susceptible to exter-
nal influences; in other words, they are more ‘plastic’. This allows envir
onment, experience, and social interactions with other individuals to play
a more dramatic role in organizing the cerebral cortex. Mary Jane West-
Eberhard of the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute in Panama
87
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
88
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
unexpected finding, for if transposon activity can interfere with the struc-
tural integrity of the genomes of each new neuron, then each nerve cell in
the brain might be genetically different, conflicting with over a century of
scientific dogma that has generally viewed each cell in the human body as
having the same genome.
To investigate this possibility, Gage’s team obtained brains from
deceased people who had donated their bodies to medical research, and
sequenced the genomes of hundreds of individual neurons.35 Remarkably,
they found that cells in the same brain are indeed genetically distinct. So
what purpose might such genomic instability serve? Gage believes that
transposons may generate novel responses in neurons in ways that the
20,000 or so genes in the human genome alone cannot. One interesting
implication of his team’s discovery is that identical twins might differ far
more in brain function than suspected, since transposon events in their
neurons will increasingly diverge as they develop, and so may their
behaviour.
A puzzling aspect of the findings is how transposon movement could
play a positive role given that such activity is thought to be mainly disrup-
tive. One possible answer comes from a new type of transposon identi-
fied by Gage’s team that has the potential to produce hundreds or even
thousands of previously unknown proteins.36 This transposon, called
ORF0 and found in humans and other primates, can blend with the
protein-coding sequences of existing genes, and as a consequence these
can then form completely new proteins, with novel cellular roles. For
Ahmet Denli, a researcher on the study, ‘this discovery redraws the blue-
print of an important piece of genetic machinery in primates, adding a
completely new gear.’37 Yet whether ORF0 plays specific, unique roles in
humans remains to be shown.
Transposon activity is just one way that alteration of the genomes of
different neurons can occur. There is also increasing evidence that brain
cells can be altered genetically in a more subtle manner, via changes in the
individual letters of the DNA code. Such changes, which occur in brain
cells but also in cells in other organs, are termed ‘somatic mutations’ to
89
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
distinguish them from the ‘germline mutations’ that affect the ‘germ cells’,
that is the eggs and sperm.38 Somatic mutations are caused by errors dur-
ing DNA replication, chemical damage to the DNA, or inefficient DNA
repair mechanisms. Recent studies indicate that such events occur in
brain cells much more frequently than previously thought, which means
that individual neurons may take on different properties than their neigh-
bours as a consequence.
Mutations in neurons may be a factor in a variety of brain disorders. A
study led by Christopher Walsh of Mount Sinai University, New York,
found somatic mutations in the brains of children with hemimegalen-
cephaly, a disorder in which one hemisphere is enlarged, causing epilepsy
and intellectual disability.39 These mutations cause specific cells in the
brain cortex to proliferate abnormally, which raises the question of
whether somatic mutations play a role in other brain disorders, including
psychiatric conditions. An even more intriguing question is whether
somatic mutations contribute to differences between individuals.
Neuroscientist Alysson Muotri of the University of California, San Diego,
believes this ‘might explain why everybody’s different—it’s not all about
the environment or genome. There’s something else. As we understand
more about [the role of somatic mutations], I think the contribution to
individuality . . . will become clear.’40
Another revelation of recent years is the discovery that RNA can travel
around the brain, transferring information. We have already seen how
genes work by being transcribed into a messenger RNA copy; this then
moves out of the cell nucleus and is translated into a protein in the cyto-
plasm. Initially it was assumed that both DNA and RNA remain firmly
within the cell. However, recent studies suggest that certain types of RNA
can leave one cell and end up in a different one. Such transport involves
structures called exosomes, which are particles secreted from many types
of cells (Figure 14).41 In the brain and peripheral nervous system, there is
growing recognition that exosomes can guide the direction of nerve
growth, control nerve connections, and help regenerate nerves. According
to Eva-Maria Krämer-Albers of the Johannes Gutenberg University in
90
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
Synaptic plasticity
& strength
neuron
Neuro-
transmission
Support
Degradation,
Neuroprotection immune function
micro
gllia
astrocyte oligodendrocyte
91
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 6
E ach human life begins with the union of a sperm and an egg.
Embryogenesis is the process by which the fertilized egg divides
repeatedly to produce the 37 trillion cells that make up a person.1 But
embryo development is about far more than just this dramatic increase in
cell number. It also involves the formation of all the specialized cell types
of the body, and their organization into tissues and organs. Initially, the
embryo is just a ball of cells called a blastocyst. But then a dramatic trans-
formation takes place called gastrulation. The biologist Lewis Wolpert, of
University College London, has said that ‘it is not birth, marriage, or
death, but gastrulation which is truly the most important time in your
life.’2 This may be overstating things, but gastrulation is certainly a key
event in the formation of a human being, for at this stage the embryo
acquires the distinct cell layers that later give rise to different organs.
During gastrulation, three cell layers form—the ectoderm, mesoderm,
and endoderm.
From the ectoderm comes the skin and nervous system, including the
brain. The guts develop from the endoderm, and the other organs from
the mesoderm. The nerves and brain arise from a structure within the
ectoderm called the neural tube that forms three weeks after conception.3
The lower part of this becomes the spinal cord, while the brain develops
from the upper part (Figure 15). At four weeks, the part of the neural tube
that forms the brain is already divided into three regions which will
become the hindbrain, midbrain, and forebrain.
While these are the gross structural changes underlying brain develop-
ment, equally important are the cellular changes. The development of the
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
Neural tube
Cross section
Ectoderm
closing
Mesoderm
Endoderm
day 19 day 21
Midbrain Hindbrain
H
Forebrain
Spinal cord
day 28
human brain occurs at such a rapid rate that, at times, 250,000 neurons
are added every minute!4 A newborn baby’s brain has almost the same
number of neurons as that of an adult. However, brain growth continues
after birth due to the creation of new glial cells and connections between
neurons.
Both neurons and glial cells originate from neural stem cells.5 The pro-
cess by which a stem cell gives rise to a more specialized cell type is called
differentiation, and is driven by chemical messengers such as hormones
and growth factors attaching themselves to receptors on the surfaces of
stem cells. These receptors then transmit signals inside the cell that turn
particular genes on or off, leading to changes in cellular proteins.
Differentiation is a multistep process, with neural stem cells first giving
rise to the prototypes of either neurons or glial cells, which then act as
precursors to more specialized cell types within these different classes, to
eventually form the hundreds of different cell types in the brain.6
A key feature of the brain is that incoming signals from the rest of the
body also shape its development. This was graphically shown by experi-
ments in the 1950s by David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel of Johns Hopkins
93
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
94
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
kittens with one eye deprived of vision for only the first three months
remain blind in that eye for their whole life. This showed that visual inputs
at this stage of life are critical to development of this part of the brain. This
feature of brain development was further explored by Colin Blakemore
and Grahame Cooper of the University of Oxford in the 1970s.12 They
placed kittens in enclosures whose walls were painted with only vertical,
or horizontal, lines. Kittens only exposed to horizontal lines could see the
seats of chairs and would jump on to them to sleep, but could not see the
chair’s legs, and were constantly banging into them. In contrast, kittens
only exposed to vertical lines were fully aware of the chair legs but could
never identify a spot to snooze above these legs.
Not surprisingly, such experiments are controversial. Indeed,
Blakemore endured years of attacks by animal rights supporters, despite,
or perhaps because of, his willingness to discuss what might seem like
barbaric experiments with the general public. According to him, ‘there
were times I was shocked by what happened to me—razor blades in enve-
lopes, bombs, threats against my kids—but I never doubted the principle
of public engagement.’13 His justification for the experiments was that
they revealed fundamental aspects of brain development and had poten-
tial relevance for treatment of human visual disorders.
Such studies show that the environment can shape the development of
the brain, but do not explain how this occurs. However, further investiga-
tions have begun to reveal the neural basis for this environmental influ-
ence. For instance, recently Blakemore and his colleagues investigated
how mice develop sensitivity in their whiskers.14 Such whiskers are con-
nected to clumps of neurons in the brain called ‘barrel structures’—
so-called because of their barrel-like shape. The researchers showed that
if a clump of whiskers is removed at an early age, the brain region linked
to that area never develops the barrel structure.15 Such findings might
suggest that the brain is merely a blank slate upon which the environment
imposes itself, but Blakemore believes that a more accurate way of under-
standing the interaction between brain and environment is to view it as a
complex feedback loop.
95
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
96
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
generated using X-rays, which mutate the DNA. By studying fly embryos
produced in this way, it is possible to identify mutants in which nerve and
brain development is disrupted. Studies of the genetic basis of such
mutants can then identify the genes involved.
Investigations of such fly mutants have identified genes that control
how neurons connect with each other.19 Recall that neurons possess a cell
body, but also dendrites and an axon. They receive incoming signals from
other neurons via their dendrites and in turn transmit messages to the
dendrites of other neurons through their axon. In the embryonic brain,
neurons must identify their correct partners in the brain’s wiring circuit.
Studies of flies have shown that neurons do this by expressing genes that
code for proteins on the surface of the developing axon which recognize
cues in the brain environment and guide neurons along molecular
pathways—like train tracks—to their correct connections.20 Other genes
control the formation of synapses. Such findings can increase our under-
standing of human brain development since related genes often exist in the
human genome and can play similar roles in the development of connec-
tions between human neurons cultured outside the body.
Because of this overlap in functional roles, genes that are linked to
brain disorders in humans and are also present in the fly genome have
been identified. In such cases, the fly can be used to study the functional
role of such genes in brain development. Such an approach was used to
study the gene associated with a condition called Fragile X, which is the
most common cause of inherited mental disability in humans and is asso-
ciated with a mutation in the FMR1 gene.21 Studies of the brains of people
affected by this disorder had indicated that they have slight differences in
the structure of the dendrites of their neurons, but the functional signifi-
cance of this difference was not clear. But investigations in flies showed
that this gene is involved in the formation of the neuron’s internal cellular
‘skeleton’, the cytoskeleton, and that defects in FMR1 affect the formation
of synapses, which may be one reason for the learning disabilities of
people with the disorder.22 Further studies may help identify drugs that
can offset some of the mental problems caused by this gene defect.
97
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
98
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
99
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
100
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
that such embryos should not be cultured for more than 14 days.36 But this
is a fairly arbitrary limit and it could be argued that to better understand
the biological factors that lead to miscarriage and developmental dis
orders, the limit should be extended. However, whether such an approach
could ever be used to study human embryonic brain and nervous system
development would clearly be a highly controversial ethical topic.37
Another area of stem cell research that poses both ethical and techno-
logical challenges is the study of human brain organoids, or ‘mini-brains’.
These can be generated from embryonic stem cells but also from induced
pluripotent stem (iPS) cells—ordinary skin cells transformed into a stem-
cell-like state. By growing such cells in a 3D matrix, it is possible to create
embryonic brain-like structures. An unexpected discovery of such stud-
ies has been the complex nature of such organoids. For instance, Paola
Arlotta at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology has shown that
mini-brains grown from iPS cells derived from healthy volunteers and
cultured for nine months contain many types of neurons ranging from
those that normally make up the cerebral cortex to ones that link the right
and left hemispheres of the brain.38 Glial cells were present as well as
neurons. The neurons created in this way even formed electrically active
neuronal networks. According to Arlotta, the neurons ‘connect with each
other, forming circuits, and once they’re connected, they can synchronize
their activity’, thereby potentially mimicking higher-order functions of
the human brain.39 Mini-brain studies have also revealed important dif-
ferences between those derived from individuals with specific brain dis
orders and those from unaffected people.40
Some scientists doubt the extent to which such mini-brains can ever
mimic the complexity of brain structure in a living human. But neurosci-
entists are working hard to improve the power of this approach via a
series of innovations. One involves growing relatively simple organoids
representative of different brain regions and allowing them to connect
with one another. For instance, In-hyun Park and colleagues at Yale
University grew two separate organoids, one based on the brain cortex,
the other on a structure called the medial ganglionic eminence (MGE) that
101
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
102
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
103
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
104
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
grow t h a nd de v e lopme n t
memory aids [which] all demonstrate that even at early stages of historical
development humans went beyond the limits of the psychological func-
tions given to them by nature and proceeded to a new culturally elabor
ated organization of their behaviour.’52
During a child’s development, such mediated forms of memory come
to dominate over natural forms of remembering, as Vygotsky demon-
strated in a study carried out with children and adults of different ages.53
Participants were presented with a list of fifteen words to be memorized.
In the first stage of the study, remembering had to be done without any
help, but in the second stage, participants were also offered fifteen picture
cards that could be used as memory aids. The major finding of the study
was that the degree to which the picture cards helped memorization
depended on a participant’s age.
In preschool children the use of memory aids did not lead to significant
differences in remembering. However, the situation changed dramatically
with seven- to eight-year-old children, who recalled about 75 per cent of
the words with the help of the cards, but only 30 per cent without them.
Interestingly, adults recalled 93 per cent of the words with the help of the
cards compared to 60 per cent without them, showing that although the
cards were useful in memorization, their overall influence was less.
Vygotsky interpreted the findings as showing that external memory aids
can help children at a certain stage of development, and also adults, to
remember things, even in ‘unmediated’ conditions.54 However, adults can
additionally draw on memorization techniques that are internalized in a
way that young children cannot. What does such a transformation of
memory involve in terms of neuronal processes? To explore this ques-
tion, it is time to look at what modern research is uncovering about the
process of learning and memory in the animal, and human, brain.
105
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
Part III
CH A P T ER 7
l e a r ning a nd me mory
showed that the process by which Aplysia learns to withdraw its sensitive
gills in response to a stimulus involves production of a chemical messen-
ger called cyclic AMP. This then activates the enzyme PKA, and PKA acts
to modify the synapses between connecting neurons. Longer- term
changes are regulated by the gene regulatory protein CREB, which is acti-
vated by PKA. The switching on of specific genes that trigger the produc-
tion of new proteins leads to cellular changes such as an increase in
synaptic connections.5 This suggested that learning involves a physical
change in nerve connections.
Another important step forward in our understanding of the cellular
mechanism of memory formation was made in 1973 by Tim Bliss and
Terje Lømo, at the University of Oslo in Norway. They showed that
repeated stimulation of connected neurons in slices of the hippocampus
region of a rabbit brain increased the efficiency of signalling between the
cells by strengthening the synapses at which they communicate with each
other. They called this process long-term potentiation (LTP) (Figure 16).6
LTP leads to the long-term strengthening of the synapses between two
neurons activated simultaneously. When axons that connect with
neurons of the hippocampus are exposed to a high-frequency stimulus,
the neurons become super-sensitized for up to several weeks. Subsequent
work has shown that a strong input leads to more receptors for neuro
transmitters being directed to the surface of the receiving neuron beyond
the synapse, amplifying the signal it receives.7 And it turns out that this
movement of receptors to the cell surface is catalyzed by the calcium-
sensitive enzyme CaMKII, which we have met before. It is activated by an
increase in calcium ions in the receiving, or postsynaptic, neuron which
occurs when the neuron is stimulated repeatedly.
One potential problem with the idea that LTP is the sole basis of mem-
ory is that synaptic components are short-lived and yet memories can last
lifetimes. This suggests that synaptic information is encoded at a deeper,
finer-grained molecular scale. And indeed, in support of this idea, Ryohei
Yasuda and colleagues at Duke University in North Carolina have shown
that such hard-wiring of memory may involve changes to the neuron’s
110
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
111
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
112
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
One class of regulatory RNAs called micro RNAs regulate the translation
of messenger RNAs into proteins.15 This is important because unlike
transcription, which occurs in the central cell nucleus, translation can
happen locally in different parts of a neuron, not only in the main cell
body but also in other cellular regions. And there is evidence that such
local translational activity is important in memory formation: the ribo-
somes on which messenger RNAs are translated accumulate at synapses
following strong neuronal activity.16 Other studies have shown that spe-
cific micro RNAs are generated at postsynaptic regions on dendrites in
response to signals at the synapse. Slices of hippocampus taken from a
mouse brain can undergo long-term potentiation in response to stimula-
tion, and slices that undergo such LTP also show an increase in the amount
of neuronal micro RNAs.17
Changes in gene expression mediated by regulatory RNAs in neurons
persist for weeks, suggesting a potential role in long-term memory for-
mation. An important feature of regulatory RNAs is that they can interact
with other such RNAs. This interaction affects their function and means
that a given regulatory RNA can either inhibit, have no effect upon, or
even enhance the activity of a messenger RNA, depending on which other
regulatory RNAs are present in its vicinity. This means that gene expres-
sion at individual synapses may be very sensitive to subtle changes in the
chemical signals at such synapses.18
While regulatory RNAs can be generated at synapses and act to control
the production of particular proteins at these sites, there is also emerging
evidence that some proteins produced at synapses are gene regulatory
proteins that can conversely travel to the cell nucleus and influence
expression of genes there.19 This could be a way in which local activity at
different synapses has an impact on the more long-term genetic behav-
iour of a neuron, which could be one basis for lasting memory formation.
As I mentioned previously, a surprising recent discovery about regula-
tory RNAs is that they can be secreted from the cell in membrane-bound
structures called ‘exosomes’ and then carry information to other cells.
There is evidence that such exosomes can travel from the postsynaptic
113
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
neuron to the presynaptic one, and help enhance the connection between
the two as part of the learning process.20 Exosomes are also able to travel
to other cells in the brain besides their synaptic partner neurons. They
may provide a way for neurons to influence the activity of not only other
neurons, but also of glial cells in their vicinity.
Neurons in the brain cortex are organized into functionally related
minicolumns, which tend to show highly correlated input and output
activity.21 Neil Smalheiser, a neuroscientist at the University of Illinois,
believes that ‘if neighbouring neurons that are activated together release
exosomes together, this may provide a means to “synchronize” their gene
expression.’22 This, in turn, may help establish or reinforce a circuit-level
memory representation that is retained by the minicolumn as a whole.
Communication via exosomes could allow cells to activate their neigh-
bours, but also inhibit them. This could be important in fine-tuning a
learned response by enhancing valuable synaptic connections while
eliminating inactive ones that have ceased to contribute to the learning
process.
If studies like these provide insights into the molecular and cellular
mechanisms by which memories can be stored, where in the brain does
such activity take place? One brain region that plays a particularly import
ant role is the hippocampus.23 The first recognition that the hippocampus
has a role in memory came in the 1950s because of the unfortunate conse-
quences of an operation on a man called Henry Molaison, of Hartford,
Connecticut.24 He suffered from a severe form of epilepsy—the brain dis-
order in which seizures occur that are caused by intense bursts of elec
trical activity across the brain. Molaison’s seizures were so frequent and
violent that as a teenager he had to leave school for several years, and
when he finally finished his studies in his early twenties, the only job that
he could manage with his disability was winding copper coils on electric
motors at a local factory.25
In 1953, a neurosurgeon called William Scoville proposed to drill into
Molaison’s skull, insert a metal tube, and suck out the hippocampal
region from which his seizures seemed to originate.26 At that time, almost
114
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
nothing was known about the functional role of the hippocampus, apart
from a suspicion that it was involved in mediating our sense of smell.
Losing such a sense seemed a small price to pay if Molaison’s violent seiz
ures could be stopped, and the operation was given the go-ahead. Initially,
the operation was judged a success since the seizures were greatly reduced.
But it soon became clear that Molaison—then aged 27—had been left
without the ability to store or recall any experiences after his operation.
From that point on, for the remaining 55 years of his life until his death in
2008, he was never able to remember anything for more than half a
minute after it had occurred.
While a personal tragedy for Molaison, his condition was a revelation
for scientists seeking to understand the biological basis of memory.27
Until this point, it was assumed that memory was a property of the whole
brain. But only Molaison’s capacity to store and recall new memories
appeared defective. In contrast, his memories of life before the operation
seemed to have been retained, but only in a very general fashion. So
Molaison could remember that his father was born in Louisiana and that
he had accompanied his parents on family holidays in Massachusetts. But
if asked about specific episodes in such holidays, he had no idea. This led
to the proposal that different parts of the brain play distinct roles in mem-
ory formation, with the hippocampus being the place where new mem
ories are formed, while long-term memory storage takes place in other
parts of the brain, particularly the prefrontal cortex, the region of the
brain involved in planning and problem-solving.
Intriguingly, Molaison seemed perfectly capable of learning new prac-
tical skills. For instance, in one study he was told to trace the image of a
star on a piece of paper while looking only at the image of the hand doing
the drawing in a mirror. Such a task is difficult to perform at first, but gets
easier with practice. And Molaison improved with each time that he
carried out the task, even though he had no conscious memory of ever
having tried it before.
Molaison’s memory defects suggested that the hippocampus acts to
knit together our experiences—which can encompass sights, sounds,
115
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
116
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
117
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
118
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
remain ‘silent’ for several weeks, as shown by the fact that the neurons in
the prefrontal cortex showed clear signs of remembrance of a painful
stimulus when artificially activated by optogenetics, but they did not show
any activation during natural memory recall. These findings suggest that
the hippocampus and the cortex act in a complementary manner. What
seems to be happening is that at first only a silent copy of a memory is
made in the prefrontal cortex, and then gradually this becomes cemented
as a long-term memory while the hippocampal one is erased. However,
the nature of this long-term cement still remains to be determined. And
the devastating effects of loss of hippocampal function in Henry Molaison
and Lonni Sue Johnson show that, at least in human beings, the hippocam-
pus can clearly not be bypassed for normal memory formation.
Other recent studies have suggested that the hippocampus acts as a
‘convergence zone’ that pulls together different bits of information stored
in the cortex that relate to distinct elements of an event into a coherent
whole. One such study was led by Ed Wu Xuekui of the University of
Hong Kong.41 His team used optogenetics to trigger low-frequency stimu-
lation of neurons in a part of the hippocampus known as the dorsal den-
tate gyrus and found that this increased functional connectivity between
different regions of the cortex.
Such a dynamic interaction of the hippocampus and the cortex may be
key to the ability of the brain to form new associations between people,
places, and objects. For instance, if you were to chance upon Halle Berry
at the Eiffel Tower, your brain would form an association between the two
different elements of this encounter, along the lines of the studies of the
epileptic patients mentioned earlier.42 Yet what still remains unclear is
how the brain manages to create an association between two separate
objects, like Halle Berry and the Eiffel Tower, yet still maintain a separ
ation between the two individual objects, so that if you visited this Paris
landmark on another occasion, but this time happened to bump into
Brad Pitt, your memory could nevertheless create a new association. And
it may be that the interaction between the hippocampus and cortex is
somehow key to this.
119
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
Slow oscillat
ion from th
e cortex
us
camp
the hippo
from
Ripple
120
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
121
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
Deep, Delta
dreamless sleep (up to 4Hz)
1 second
122
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
access to working memory. They clear out working memory, and can act
as a switch from one thought or item to another.49
So far in our consideration of the biological mechanisms underpinning
memory, we have been focusing on the individual brain. But the main
focus of this book is the importance of social interaction in the develop-
ment of the human mind. An indication of the importance of social inter-
action in the formation of human memory capacity came from a study in
the late 1980s led by Hallam Hurt, a specialist in child development at the
University of Pennsylvania in Philadelphia.50 She was concerned about
the effect that the crack cocaine epidemic devastating US inner cities at
that time was having upon the development of children of addicted
mothers. To investigate this, Hurt and her colleagues studied four-year-
old children from low-income families and compared the IQ of infants
who had been exposed to the drug with those who had not. Somewhat to
Hurt’s surprise, the study found no significant difference between the two
sets of children. Instead, the IQ of both sets of children was much lower
than average. According to Hurt, ‘these little children were coming in cute
as buttons, and yet their IQs were like 82 and 83. Average IQ is 100. It was
shocking.’51
This discovery led Hurt and her colleagues to investigate how a factor
that the two sets of children had in common—their poverty—might have
affected their intellectual development; for instance was it possible that a
lack of time, resources, and education, which could be characteristic of
parents of children in such a state of poverty, might have had an impact?
The researchers began to monitor how frequently the parents of children
in such families spoke to them affectionately, spent time answering their
questions, and hugged, kissed, and praised them.52 They also asked
whether the parents had at least ten books at home for the children, a
record player with songs for them, and toys to help them learn numbers.
They found that children who had received more attention and nurtur
ing at home tended to have a higher IQ. And the same children also did
much better at memory tasks. These included being asked to remember
people’s faces or a list of words, and to find a token hidden in one of a
123
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
l e a r ning a nd me mory
124
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 8
t hough t a nd l a nguage
126
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
127
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
Quite what we can learn from the mice studies mentioned above is also
far from clear, since in this species both the FOXP2 and SRPX2 genes may
be playing quite different roles to the situation in humans. Because of
such potential limitations of using the mouse as a model, scientists have
begun to look to other species to study the role of FOXP2 in language.
One interesting line of investigation is being pursued by Stephanie
White of the University of California, Los Angeles. She has focused on
songbirds, since these are one of the few animals besides humans that can
produce a wide variety of complex vocal sounds.9 In addition, much like
the situation in humans, songbirds have a critical period in youth when
they are best at learning vocal communication skills. In birds, this is when
they learn a song they will use later in life as a courtship song. After this
critical period ends, just as it is more difficult for human beings to learn
languages, so it is for songbirds to learn their songs.
White and her colleagues have shown that changes in the level of
FOXP2 protein in a part of the songbird brain involved in song led to
changes in the activity of thousands of other genes.10 White believes this
shows that the activities of these genes are being manipulated by FOXP2,
much like an orchestra is led by a conductor. According to her, ‘it’s not
that all the genes (or instrumentalists) became loud or became quiet; it’s
that they change in a coordinated way. We refer to these as “suites of
genes”, and one of these suites of genes is highly correlated to learning in
young birds.’11 Intriguingly, preventing such changes in FOXP2 disrupted
learning, which may mimic what happens in human beings with a FOXP2
defect. And, indeed, White hopes that studying the relationship between
FOXP2 and the genes that it controls in birds may lead to new insights
about FOXP2’s role in humans.
In another line of investigation, scientists are seeking to study the role
of FOXP2 in non-human primates. Although such primates cannot speak,
we saw previously how apes can be taught sign language; however, they
also seem to have natural limits to this capacity, with no indication that it
is comparable to the complex sophistication of human language. Yet given
that it is becoming increasingly possible, through the new technology of
128
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
129
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
Primary
visual cortex
later, Karl Wernicke studied other patients with language defects and
identified another region on the left side of the brain associated with lan-
guage, now known as ‘Wernicke’s area’.15 People with damage to Broca’s
area generally understand language but cannot generate intelligible
speech, whereas those with damage to Wernicke’s area cannot under-
stand language but can produce it. Findings such as these led to the idea
that discrete brain regions play defined roles in the language process.16
However, more recent studies of brain-damaged patients, experimen-
tal treatments that stimulate or inhibit particular regions of the brain in
patients, and imaging studies of the human brain have all undermined the
idea of such a simple division of language production and comprehen-
sion.17 Rather, they suggest that language is a dynamic process distributed
across much wider areas of the brain than had been suspected. Moreover,
while there is no doubt that regions such as Broca’s and Wernicke’s play
important roles in language, there is increasing evidence that they are also
involved in other aspects of brain function.18
Indeed, the idea that either a particular gene or a specific region of the
brain is responsible for human language capacity betrays an overly sim-
plistic view of how evolution works. One feature of evolutionary change
is its conservative character. Driven by trial and error, rather than a pre-
formed plan, evolution tends to co-opt existing processes and structures,
rather than generate completely novel ones.19 And there is good reason to
130
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
believe that a similar process may have occurred during the evolution of
human language.
One interesting recent finding is that Broca’s region—and its equiva-
lent in an ape brain—seems to be the main location of the ‘mirror neu-
rons’ that I mentioned previously. Mirror neurons, recall, become
activated when an individual performs an action, but also when the indi-
vidual observes someone else making the same movement. Their associ
ation with Broca’s region has led to the suggestion that these neurons
may play a role in language in humans.20 This would fit with the fact that
imitation plays an important role in the way that children learn lan-
guage. More controversial is the possibility that human language may
have its origins in the gestures that other primates use to communicate
with other.
While an attractive idea, allowing for a way for language to emerge
from an existing biological mechanism in our ape ancestors, one objec-
tion, voiced by linguist Robbins Burling of the University of Michigan, is
that such a transition would have required a ‘move from a visual lan-
guage to an audible one.’21 Yet since in monkeys mirror neurons can be
activated by sounds—for instance, the tearing of paper, or cracking of a
nut—the transition from a gestural to a sound-based language system
might still be feasible. Another factor in favour of such a transition is that
deaf people have developed sign language systems as sophisticated as
any other form of human language, without using sound. Of course
there is a world of difference between the language systems used by our
species—whether sign-based or verbal—and the gestures used by apes,
which as we have seen seem incapable of conveying abstract symbolic
information as human language does. But by providing an initial bio-
logical framework in the brain for language to develop, it is easier to
imagine how our amazing language capacity might have gradually
evolved by commandeering existing neural circuits, rather than appear-
ing, fully formed, out of nowhere.
Another interesting recent discovery about Broca’s region has come
from a brain imaging study of individuals with mutations in the FOXP2
131
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
132
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
133
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
134
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
makes eye contact with a parent or teacher and then follows their gaze to
an object—have enhanced brain responses to the sounds of a new lan-
guage. One such study by Rechele Brooks and colleagues at the University
of Washington in Seattle has led Brooks to conclude that ‘young babies’
social engagement contributes to their own language learning—they’re
not just passive listeners of language. They’re paying attention, and show-
ing parents they’re ready to learn when they’re looking back and forth.
That’s when the most learning happens.’35
A curious finding of such imaging studies is that babies first exposed to
their native language show activation not only in the brain areas involved
in hearing, but also in the superior temporal gyrus, Broca’s area, and the
cerebellum—‘motor’ regions associated with speech production. This
suggests that an infant’s brain starts laying down the neural connections
necessary for the formation of words long before it actually begins to
speak. According to Patricia Kuhl, who led the above study at the
University of Washington, ‘most babies babble by 7 months, but don’t
utter their first words until after their first birthdays. Finding activation in
motor areas of the brain when infants are simply listening is significant,
because it means the baby brain is engaged in trying to talk back right
from the start and suggests that 7-month-olds’ brains are already trying to
figure out how to make the right movements that will produce words.’36
The findings confirm the importance of adults speaking to babies during
social interactions even if the latter cannot yet respond in kind.
The consequences for language development of not being exposed to
such social interaction in the first years of life were tragically shown by
the example of children in orphanages in Romania.37 From the 1960s
onwards, the regime led by dictator Nicolae Ceausescu implemented a
drastic process of forced industrialization. To increase the working popu-
lation, contraception and abortion were banned and childless couples
taxed, while people from the countryside were pressurized to find work
in the cities. During these drives many newborn children were aban-
doned and subsequently ended up in state-r un orphanages. Only when a
revolution overthrew Ceausescu in 1989 did it become apparent how
135
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
136
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
137
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
138
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
t hough t a nd l a nguage
139
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 9
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
141
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
142
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
143
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
144
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
where we met her, the Eiffel Tower, might also allow us to create completely
imaginary visions. In other words, by employing such mechanisms of
memory, the imagination allows us to juxtapose images that we have seen
separately in other contexts in completely novel ways.
In fact, most of us are far more likely to be able to conjure up an imagin
ary vision of Halle Berry at the Eiffel Tower than actually remember meet
ing her there. Yet from a neuronal point of view, it is potentially much
harder to conjure up the imaginary vision, for the simple reason that here
we have two juxtaposed images, like seeing a dolphin balancing a pine
apple on its snout, that are highly unlikely to have occurred at the same
time. And while an original memory of two juxtaposed objects could rely
on their physical association in the real world, a purely imaginative vision
has no such help from reality. Instead, it is left to the brain to make this
association. So how does it do this, and which brain regions are involved?
Traditionally the area of the brain assumed to be involved in mediating
a ‘higher’ function such as imagination has been the cerebral cortex, par
ticularly the prefrontal cortex, the section of the cortex that lies at the
very front of the brain, just under the forehead.21 This brain region is often
described as responsible for the ‘executive’ functions of human con
sciousness, with its role being to manage complex processes like reason,
logic, problem solving, planning, and memory, just as the chief executive
of a company is meant to guide and control the operations of the organ
ization. To do so, the prefrontal cortex is believed to direct attention,
develop and pursue goals, and inhibit counterproductive impulses.
A historical example often used to support this idea of the dominant
coordinating role played by the prefrontal cortex in human conscious
ness is the famous case of Phineas Gage, who was involved in a tragic
accident in the mid-nineteenth-century.22 In September 1848, Gage, a
25-year-old construction foreman, was in charge of men building a rail
way in Vermont, USA. The team employed explosives to clear a route for
the railway, and at one point Gage was using an iron rod to compact gun
powder in a borehole. Unfortunately, the iron produced a spark that
prematurely ignited the explosive, and the resulting blast propelled the
145
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
146
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
147
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
the brain to railway tracks, with the switch operator activating some parts
of the track and taking others offline.29 This model would explain how
attention works—for instance, a person can focus on a particular picture
while ignoring a noise in the background. It may also explain how differ
ent pieces of information might be juggled around in the brain while we
try to solve a problem creatively. It suggests a directing role for the
prefrontal cortex in this process, but argues against the idea that this
region is all-powerful, for the fact that this brain region requires incoming
signals from these other regions shows it is also directed by them.
Another important aspect of Alex Schlegel’s study mentioned earlier is
that its findings contradict a common idea about creativity: that it is pri
marily concentrated on one side of the brain. According to this idea, the
right brain is the site of creative thinking and the left brain that of logical
thinking. Yet Schlegel’s team found evidence that both the right and left
parts of the cerebral cortex are involved in the imaginative process.30 But
perhaps the most intriguing aspect of this study was that it unexpectedly
highlighted an important role for a region of the brain called the cerebel
lum in imagination and the creative process.
Previously the cerebellum has been more linked to control of balance
and coordinated movement of the body’s muscles.31 Yet recognition of a
role for this brain region in higher mental functions should maybe not be
a surprise, given emerging facts about its contribution to brain function,
particularly in humans. For although it occupies only 20 per cent of
human brain volume, the cerebellum contains 70 per cent of its neurons.
And a sign that the cerebellum may have specific roles to play in our spe
cies is that it has increased three- to fourfold in size compared to that in
other primate species in just the past million years of human evolution.32
So how might this brain region influence imagination and creativity?
Surprisingly, it seems to do so in ways that are similar to its role in coord
inating movement.
Research indicates that the human cerebellum can enhance the func
tions of the cerebral cortex in four different ways.33 First, it increases the
speed, efficiency, and appropriateness of processes in the cortex by
148
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
149
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
150
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
151
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
152
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
Eureka moment. This is the emerging evidence that there may be a thin
line between genius and mental disorder, which provides a demonstra
tion of the potential problems in trying to identify a clear biological
source for genius. In fact, the belief that creativity and madness are closely
linked goes back at least to ancient Greek times, when Plato drew atten
tion to the eccentricities of poets and playwrights, and Aristotle noted
that ‘those who have been eminent in philosophy, politics, poetry, and the
arts have all had tendencies toward melancholia.’51 The seventeenth-century
English poet and critic John Dryden observed that ‘great wits are sure to
madness near allied, and thin partitions do their bounds divide.’52 Recent
studies have shown that such a link between creativity and mental
disorder may be based on actual neural mechanisms.
For instance, Andreas Fink and colleagues at the University of Graz in
Austria used fMRI of the brain to study a variety of human individuals,
including some that were prone to schizophrenia.53 While inside the brain
scanner, individuals were asked to come up with original uses for every
day objects—a common assessment of creativity. The researchers found
important similarities in the brain images of people who showed evi
dence of high creativity and those prone to schizophrenia, with both
showing higher activity in the right precuneus, an area of the brain that
helps us gather information. This suggests that creative individuals and
people prone to schizophrenia may both share an inability to filter out
irrelevant material. And the findings support a proposal by Shelley
Carson, a Harvard University psychologist, who believes that creativity
and mental illness are both characterized by a process that she calls
‘cognitive disinhibition’ (Figure 20).54 This involves a failure to keep
off-the-wall images or ideas out of conscious awareness. This failure may
make people with a tendency towards mental disorders like schizophre
nia or bipolar disorder more prone to delusional thoughts or mental
confusion, but it could also be a fertile source for the creative mind.
According to Carson, ‘you have more information in conscious aware
ness that could be combined and recombined in novel and original ways
to come up with creative ideas.’55 An individual with such tendencies
153
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
cr e at i v i t y a nd im aginat ion
IUS PSYCHO
GEN PAT
IVE HO
E AT LO
C R
Cognitive
GY
disinhibition
Working memory skills Enhanced Working memory deficits
W
novelty salience
Hyperconnectivity
Cognitive flexibility Perseveration
Emotional
liability
154
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 10
R el e a s i n g h
m
or
on
es Pituitary
P gland
Th id
Thyroid Adrenal
Ovaries Testes gland glands
nerve connections to the organs, but also by brain regions like the hypo
thalamus, and glands linked to it such as the pituitary that secrete hor
mones into the blood (Figure 21).4 The pituitary gland in turn can influence
the activity of the adrenal gland, the thryoid gland, the gonads, and vari
ous other organs.
A revelation of recent years has been that many bodily regions both
receive signals from the brain and send reciprocal messages back to it.5
Research shows that the gonads, stomach, intestines, liver, kidney, heart,
and even fat tissue not only receive signals from the brain but also secrete
hormones that can influence it and, as a consequence, our mental state.
So when someone says that they have a ‘gut feeling’ about something, it
may have a real biological basis. In addition, we are beginning to realize
that these different parts of the body can communicate with each other in
a far more interactive fashion than had been realized.
For this reason, some scientists now argue that even human conscious
ness cannot be thought of as a product solely of the brain, but also of the
body. In what has become known as the ‘4Es’ theory of consciousness,
the human mind is said to be ‘embodied’, because of the reciprocal
156
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
i nteraction of the brain and the other bodily organs.6 For instance, Antonio
Damasio, a neuroscientist at the University of Southern California, has
argued that a crucial area of investigation for psychology should be ‘why
and how we emote, feel, use feelings to construct our selves . . . and how
brains interact with the body to support such functions.’7 The three other
E’s stand for ‘embedded’, meaning that the human brain/body also has an
interaction with the world around it; ‘enacted’, because this interaction is
an active process; and ‘extended’, because humans employ tools as part of
that interaction with the world.8 I will return later to this theory of con
sciousness, and how it relates to the theme of this book.
To complicate the picture further, a chemical defined as a hormone
because it is released into the bloodstream by a gland and subsequently
fulfils particular functions in the body may also be secreted directly into
the brain, where it can play quite different roles. One example of such a
hormone is oxytocin. For some years this hormone has been known to
play an important role both before and after a baby is born, by inducing
labour, and stimulating milk production by the mother’s breasts. But
more recently, oxytocin has also been shown to be released by the hypo
thalamus directly into the brain.9 There it influences behaviours including
social recognition, bonding, and parental behaviour.10 In addition, this
chemical plays an important role in orgasm, and its concentration in the
brain increases when a person falls in love, and decreases as a couple
become accustomed to each other. For this reason oxytocin is often
referred to as the ‘love hormone’. But this term is misleading, since the
chemical can also intensify memories of bonding gone wrong, such as in
men who have poor relationships with their mothers. It can also make
people less accepting of individuals seen as outsiders. In other words,
whether oxytocin makes you feel warm and trusting towards other
human beings or suspicious of them depends on the particular context of
the interaction.
One intriguing possibility recently put forward by Erich Jarvis of the
Rockefeller University in New York is that oxytocin may facilitate the
acquisition of language.11 I have mentioned that views about how chil
157
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
dren acquire the capacity for language have tended to be split between
behaviourists who see this capacity as a learned response and those, like
Noam Chomsky, who believe it is an innate ‘instinct’ in humans. However,
Jarvis believes the real situation is somewhere in between and that oxy
tocin may play a crucial role.12 He points to evidence that oxytocin is
active not only in the brains of mammals, but also in those of songbirds,
which, as we have seen, have been used as a model for the study of the
genetic basis of language in humans. Noting that oxytocin is particularly
active in the brain regions involved in song acquisition in songbirds, Jarvis
proposes that oxytocin might control the social mechanism of vocal
learning in both songbirds and human beings. As he puts it, ‘when a child
says “Daddy” and receives a pat on the back or a smile, that gives the child
a feeling of reward. That feeling may release oxytocin into the vocal-
learning circuits, to strengthen the memory in the vocal-learning path
way of how to say that sound.’13
Another chemical that plays dual roles in the brain and the rest of the
body is dopamine.14 Like the more commonly known adrenaline, this
chemical is a modified amino acid, the molecules that also serve as the
building blocks for proteins. Dopamine is known to be involved in regu
lating the activities of the kidney, pancreas, and immune system. However,
it is in the brain that this chemical has been shown to play its most import
ant roles to date.
Dopamine is vital for our control of movement, as shown by its deple
tion in people with Parkinson’s disease, a disorder associated with mobil
ity problems, tremors, and speech impairments.15 But it also regulates
many higher mental functions in humans, such as learning, concentra
tion, planning ahead, and pleasure-seeking behaviour. The role of this
chemical in the pleasure response was demonstrated in the 1980s by
Wolfram Schultz of the University of Fribourg in Switzerland.16 Studying
monkeys, he developed methods for recording activity from neurons that
use dopamine to transmit information to other neurons.
Schultz found that in an untrained monkey, the dopamine neurons
responded whenever a reward was given to the animal, but if it was shown
158
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
159
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
those with analogues in animals and human infants, such as joy or fear;
and those with no such analogies, such as guilt or pride. Yet for the cultur
ally mediated view of human consciousness I have been proposing in this
book, this is a false division; rather, all emotions have a social dimension
and develop as an individual human’s mind matures into adulthood.
A feeling so apparently basic as hunger can clearly be very different in
an adult human compared to an animal or for that matter a human child.
Imagine also the difference between a person lost in the wilderness des
perate for food compared to the hunger of someone about to eat at an
expensive restaurant with a partner or close friend. Clearly here context is
everything. This is also true of other feelings, like sexual desire. Far from
sex being merely a physical act, in humans it is closely entwined with the
idea of love, traditionally seen as a higher emotion. At the same time sex
in humans can take on a dazzling—and sometimes quite bizarre—num
ber of different forms. In both these cases, a biological desire becomes
transformed by the social context, but also by the intellect. And it seems
therefore possible that the chemistry of the brain may be subtly different
in humans.
While emotions are radically altered in humans because of the influence
of higher mental functions, equally higher functions cannot be separated
from emotional responses. Lev Vygotsky made this point in arguing that
‘when associated with a task that is important to the individual [and] that
somehow has its roots in the centre of the individual’s personality, realistic
thinking calls to life much more significant emotional experience than
imagination or daydreaming.’22 This may seem counterintuitive, as rational
thought is often counterposed to emotional responses, but this ignores
the importance of emotion for rational thought.
One way of examining how rational thought and emotional responses
are intertwined is to study how these two factors develop from childhood
to adulthood. Giovana Reis Mesquita of the University of Bahia in Brazil
has recently argued that, ‘in the field of emotions, one can draw a parallel
reasoning between thought and language, as Vygotsky proposed . . . the
child, when born, is endowed with thought, but a practical thought, very
160
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
Out of reach
oximal develo
of pr pm
ne
en
Zo
nt abilit
rre
Cu
ies
scaffold
161
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
he believed that learning is most effective when the child gets assistance
from a teacher, carer, or older child in such a way that the child can accom
plish tasks that would be outside their level of competence, if left unaided.
The ZPD is sometimes described as a kind of ‘scaffolding’ in aiding a
child’s learning.26
The ZPD is often discussed purely in terms of intellectual support.
However, Holbrook Mahn and Vera John-Steiner of the University of New
Mexico have argued that Vygotsky also saw emotional support of the
child as critical to successful learning.27 Such support includes encour
aging confidence and the sharing of risk in presenting new ideas, con
structive criticism, and creating a safety zone.
To facilitate such interactions, in studies carried out in schools in New
Mexico in the mid-1990s, Mahn and John-Steiner encouraged teachers to
use ‘dialogue journals’.28 These involved students writing for 10 to 15 min
utes at the beginning of class on whatever topic they chose. The teacher
then made their own comments in the journals. Students were encour
aged to focus on communication, not to worry about mistakes, and were
also free to jump topic and draw on their own interests and experiences.
As students became less anxious about their writing, they became more
fluent. A recurring theme in the students’ reflections about such journals
was that the responses they received from the teacher played an import
ant role in motivating them and giving them the confidence to take risks
with their writing.29
The example above shows that a caring school environment and
emphasis on meaningful communication between teacher and student
can help create positive emotional responses that facilitate learning. But
in some circumstances, emotional responses may become negative and
even dangerous to health and well-being. This is the situation with addic
tion.30 Earlier in this chapter, we saw how different chemicals play import
ant roles in brain function. These chemicals not only are crucial to life,
but also modulate key emotional responses. However, such responses are
also central to how recreational drugs work. While these drugs are an
important feature of human society, they also carry the risk of addiction,
162
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
which can lead to much misery, and even death. It is therefore worth
looking at how such drugs elicit the responses they do, and how individ
uals can become addicted to them.
Human beings have probably used naturally occurring chemicals for
both medicinal and recreational purposes since long before our species
developed writing and could thereby record our experiences. Indeed, Elisa
Guerra-Doce of the University of Valladolid in Spain has recently com
piled evidence from around the world which indicates that ancient human
societies were using recreational drugs at least as early as 13,000 years
ago.31 For instance, it seems that human societies were cultivating opium
poppies by at least 6000 bc. Discussing the cultivation of such poppies,
Guerra-Doce notes that ‘apart from its use as a food plant, there is also
uncontested evidence for the exploitation of its narcotic properties.’32
While opium dulls the senses, other drugs stimulate them, leading to
psychedelic effects. Guerra-Doce found evidence that between 8600 bc
and 5600 bc, inhabitants of caves in Peru’s Callejon de Huaylas valley
were using Echinopsis pachanoi—a cactus that contains the psychedelic
substance mescaline. The most ancient signs that Guerra-Doce uncovered
of recreational drug use were reddish stains on 13,000-year-old human
teeth found in a burial pit on Palawan Island in the Philippines, thought to
be caused by chewing the leaves of the betel plant, which is still used in
much of Asia as a stimulant. Other drugs that ancient societies appear to
have used include tobacco, cannabis, and hallucinogenic ‘magic’ mush
rooms, alongside the more commonplace alcohol.33
Such drugs are well known, in an often notorious way, in modern soci
ety. However, it is important that we do not assume that modern use of
narcotic and psychedelic drugs can necessarily be equated to the way
these drugs were used in the past. For instance, Elisa Guerra-Doce points
out that most traces of the drugs identified by this study were in tombs
and sites with ritual or religious significance.34 Because of this, she thinks
the substances were used either during mortuary rites, to provide susten
ance for the deceased in their journey into the afterlife, or as a tribute to
underworld deities, with use of such substances perhaps being restricted
163
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
due to their sacred role.35 However, given the often illicit way drugs are
used in contemporary society, it is also important not to disregard the
possibility that in ancient times, recreational drugs were taken more gen
erally for pleasure, even if religious rules meant they were only supposed
to be consumed as part of ritual ceremonies.
Recently, scientists have begun to realize that the brain mechanisms
that underlie the pleasure we can get from recreational drugs have roots
in the same ones that give pleasure in the rest of our lives.36 Essentially, the
brain’s reward system learns the actions that produce positive outcomes,
such as obtaining food or sex. It then reinforces the desire to initiate those
behaviours by inducing pleasure in anticipation of the relevant action.
But in some circumstances this system becomes oversensitized to pleas
urable but harmful behaviours, producing pathological impulses like
addiction, a term derived from the Latin for ‘enslaved by’ or ‘bound to’.37
Addiction exerts a long and powerful influence on the brain in three dis
tinct ways: craving the object of addiction; loss of control over its use; and
continuing involvement despite adverse consequences.38 In the 1930s,
when researchers first began investigating addictive behaviour, they
believed that people with addictions were morally flawed or lacking in
willpower. However, today addiction is recognized as a chronic condition
that changes both brain structure and function. Just as cardiovascular dis
ease damages the heart and diabetes impairs the pancreas, addiction
hijacks the brain. Recovery from addiction involves willpower, but it is
often not enough to ‘just say no’. Instead, multiple strategies—including
psychotherapy, medication, and self- care—may be required to treat
addiction. 39
Addiction can prove fatal. In the case of tobacco, an addict’s life can be
shortened by the increased likelihood of getting lung cancer or other
respiratory problems.40 A chronic alcoholic may die of liver cirrhosis or
cancer of the liver, although drinking far too much alcohol in one go—as
in binge drinking—can also prove fatal.41 In the case of an opioid like her
oin, although the drug is taken because of the pleasurable sensations it
produces, it has other effects, one being to supress the brain regions that
164
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
165
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
166
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
hooked on social media that it begins to dominate their life and a person
who uses it occasionally. In addition, a major theme of this book is that
there is a danger of reducing human behaviour to simple biology, and this
applies to Internet addiction as much as any other form of human behav
iour. Instead, we should be sensitive to the negative social influences that
can fuel addiction in its many forms—whether to alcohol, heroin, or the
latest computer game or social media site. In particular, we need to con
sider whether excess use of a drug or excessive time spent in front of a
computer screen is a cause of social dysfunction, or rather a consequence
of estrangement from society.
Moreover, it is important that drugs themselves are not only considered
from a purely negative point of view. This is particularly true in a book
devoted to exploring the unique aspects of human consciousness com
pared to that of other species, given interesting new ideas about how
drugs may have played a role in the development of that consciousness.
Until recently it was assumed that consumption of alcohol by our species
on a regular basis only occurred with the agricultural revolution that
took place about 12,000 years ago, because only then would it have been
possible to stay sufficiently long in a place to grow and ferment the raw
materials for making an alcoholic beverage. But evidence has been emer
ging that production and consumption of alcohol not only may have pre
dated the agricultural revolution, but also may even have been a stimulus
for this key event in human history.52
Brian Hayden, an archaeologist at Simon Fraser University in Canada,
has recently found evidence of beer- making activities among the
Natufians—sedentary hunter-gatherers who inhabited an area of the
Middle East that is now part of Syria, Jordan, and Lebanon, and who are
thought to have acted as forerunners of the agricultural revolution—as
early as 13,000 years ago.53 Archaeological remains found in this region
include stones for grinding barley and brewing vessels that could have
potentially been used to make beer. Hayden believes that cultural factors,
not economic ones, stimulated the domestication of grains like barley, in
that once people understood the effects of alcohol on the mind, it became
167
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
a central part of feasts and other social gatherings that forged bonds
between people and inspired creativity. According to Hayden, ‘it’s not
that drinking and brewing by itself helped start cultivation, it’s this con
text of feasts that links beer and the emergence of complex societies.’54
Such consumption of alcohol in a social setting could have been import
ant in the development of human consciousness by allowing our ances
tors to become more expansive in their thinking, as well as more
collaborative and creative. A night of drinking may have ushered in these
feelings of freedom—although the morning after, instincts to conform
and submit may have kicked back in to restore the social order.55
According to this view, alcohol played an important role in the develop
ment of human consciousness, but only late in our evolution, at the dawn
of civilization. However, there have also been claims that drugs helped
shape our species’ consciousness much earlier in prehistory. For instance,
the ‘stoned ape’ hypothesis, put forward by Terence McKenna in his 1992
book Food of the Gods, proposes that the proto-human species Homo erectus
came across the ‘magic mushrooms’ that contain the psychedelic drug
psilocybin because of the tendency of this fungus to grow in the dung of
animals that they tracked for food, and that this chemical induced the
Homo erectus brain to reorganize and take on new mental properties; this in
turn kick-started an evolution of cognition that eventually led to the devel
opment of the sophisticated art, culture, and technology of Homo sapiens.56
Like all highly speculative theories of human evolution, a major prob
lem with the ‘stoned ape’ theory is the lack of direct supporting evidence.
It is also far from clear how such a behavioural change in a proto-human
species like Homo erectus would then manifest itself in the evolution of our
own much later species. Nevertheless, the evidence mentioned previously
that has been emerging about the important role of different frequency
brain waves in coordinating different regions of the human brain means
that it would be unwise to be totally dismissive about the idea that a drug
might have had radical effects on brain function—including such import
ant attributes as creativity and imagination—and therefore might have
played an important role in the evolution of human consciousness.
168
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
169
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
170
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 11
U ntil now this book has focused mainly on the conscious human
mind. It is now time to delve deeper and look at the unconscious
aspects of our minds. The unconscious consists of those mental processes
that occur automatically and are not available to introspection. In fact,
increasingly, studies of brain function are revealing that a surprising
amount of that function is automatic and, therefore, unconscious.1
Obvious instances are breathing and control of the rate of our heart beat
and the functions of other organs, but as I have said, not only sporting
ability but also a certain amount of creativity and imagination may be a
partially unconscious process. Recent studies in this area are throwing up
some interesting, and unexpected, findings. For example, sophisticated
new imaging methods have shown that the processes that underlie the
decision to press a right or left button are, in fact, activated milliseconds
to seconds before we are actually consciously aware of making the
decision.2
Although such findings might appear to challenge the idea of ‘free will’,
maybe we should not be surprised to find that so much of the function of
the brain is automatic, given the many tasks that it must undertake in a
typical day. Take, for instance, just a small portion of that day when some-
one is making their way to work. As much of the journey—whether on
foot or by bike, car, or public transport—is part of a well-rehearsed rou-
tine, many aspects of it may be automatic. Even the thoughts we have on
that journey, whether about the work we expect to do that day, or some
argument we had the previous evening with a partner, friend, or c olleague,
will be on our mind, but perhaps at different levels of conscious experience.
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
172
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
173
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
book, Psyche: On the Development of the Soul, in which he said that ‘the key
to an understanding of the nature of the conscious life of the soul lies in
the sphere of the unconscious’.10 Carus’s view fused—often rather
haphazardly—ideas about unconscious memory with the question of
unconscious biological and natural processes governing the development
of life, enveloping both in an aura of mysticism. ‘Can the free activity of
the conscious soul’, he asked, ‘ever match the perfection and abundance
of the creations of the unconscious soul?’11
Despite such important antecedents, it is Sigmund Freud who has
become the principal figure associated with the notion of an unconscious
mind. Freud was born in 1856, in Freiberg, Moravia—then part of the
Austro-Hungarian Empire, now in the Czech Republic; he originally
planned to study law at the University of Vienna, but opted instead for
medicine, with a particular focus on the new field of psychiatry. Freud
became interested in ‘hysteria’, a disorder commonly diagnosed in women
at this time, characterized by anxiety and irritability. With his colleague
Joseph Breuer, he began exploring the life histories of clients with hysteria,
which led them to the view that talking to such patients was a ‘cathartic’
way of releasing ‘pent up emotion’.12 They published Studies on Hysteria in
1895, and began developing the ideas that led to psychoanalysis.13
Breuer and Freud viewed the unconscious as the epicentre of people’s
repressed thoughts, traumatic memories, and fundamental drives of sex
and aggression. Freud saw it as a storage facility for hidden sexual desires,
resulting in ‘neuroses’—or what we would now call anxiety disorders. He
considered the human mind to consist of the id, the ego, and the superego
(Figure 23).14 He believed that the id forms our unconscious drives and is
not bound by morality but seeks only to satisfy pleasure; the ego is our
conscious perceptions, memories, and thoughts that enable us to deal
effectively with reality; and the superego attempts to mediate the drives of
the id through socially acceptable behaviours.15
Freud believed that we can learn much about an individual through the
interpretation of their dreams.16 He argued that when we are awake our
deepest desires are not acted upon because they are inhibited by
174
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
Conscious
EGO Preconscious
SUPEREGO
ID
Unconscious
c onsiderations of reality (through the ego) and morality (via the super-
ego). But during sleep these restraining forces are weakened and we may
experience our desires through our dreams. Freud also believed that our
dreams contain repressed or anxiety-provoking thoughts that cannot be
acknowledged directly even while sleeping for fear of anxiety and embar-
rassment, but which can enter our dreams in a disguised, symbolic form.
Although dreams may indicate supressed unconscious thoughts, there
are limits to how much we can learn from them, mainly because of the
difficulties in trying to recall dreams without at the same time supressing
their content. So Freud developed a method of analysis that he called ‘free
association’.17 The idea behind this was to get the patient to express any-
thing they might be thinking, however odd. The psychoanalyst notes
instances when the patient fails to free associate, because for Freudians,
this represents the unconscious disrupting the capacity for an individual
to express their feelings. And this can provide clues about the nature of
unconscious desires in the individual.
So what are the unconscious desires that an individual is driven to
supress? One of the most controversial aspects of Freud’s view of the
mind is the emphasis he put on sexuality. He felt that suppression of sex-
ual desires was the biggest motivating force behind human behaviour,
and particularly the cause of mental disorders. This is clear from his
175
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
176
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
stuck at a latent phase. Such ideas now seem a product of a much more
backward era in terms of views of women and sexuality.
Criticisms like these have led to Freud’s view of the mind being often
disparaged in academic psychology and neuroscience. So does this mean
that Freud’s ideas about the mind—and particularly his proposals about
the unconscious—have nothing to offer the view of human conscious-
ness that I have been developing in this book? In fact, I believe we have
much to learn from Freud, but only if we go beyond the superficial frame-
work of his ideas, which were heavily influenced by the prejudices of his
time, and instead focus on what I consider the most important aspect of
his view of the mind: that it is a highly dynamic entity.
Previously I mentioned Valentin Voloshinov and his view that language
restructures the developing mind, in a dialogue with society. Voloshinov
acknowledged the importance of Freud’s discoveries about the uncon-
scious mind; however, he provided a very different explanation of the
material basis of the unconscious.23 Voloshinov believed that rather than
representing our repressed animal instincts, the complex and subtle
behaviour displayed by the unconscious suggests it is better understood
as that portion of the conscious which is not yet articulate, and also as an
ongoing struggle among various motives and voices within the con-
scious. And while Freud thought that the id and ego which emerge during
psychoanalysis are repressed inner realities in the process of discharge,
for Voloshinov they are rather reflections of real social tensions, including
potentially those between analyst and patient.
If the unconscious represents the least articulate part of a continuous
dialogue taking place within our minds, this is because the themes with
which it is concerned are those where the ‘official’ ideology of society is
most at variance with an ‘unofficial’ one based on a person’s actual experi
ence. For Voloshinov, the levels of consciousness ‘corresponding to
Freud's unconscious lie at a great distance from the stable system of the
ruling ideology. They bespeak the disintegration of the unity and integ-
rity of the system, the vulnerability of the usual ideological motivations.’24
Viewed in this light, it is perhaps no surprise that Freud discovered the
177
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
178
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
179
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
180
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
181
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
182
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
183
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
conscious a nd u nconscious
184
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
Part IV
MIND IN TROUBLE
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 12
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
188
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
189
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
networks to influence the activity of ‘core’ genes that are more directly
connected to an illness.
GWAS proponents have countered that by identifying the components
of such networks, scientists can build up a clearer picture of how the cell
normally works, and how it can go wrong during the development of a
particular disease.19 But this does not address the possibility that GWAS
may be failing to identify the core genes that might be playing a much
greater role in a disorder than those so far identified.
One potential flaw of GWAS in this respect is its focus on ‘common’
variations—that is, single-letter differences in the human DNA sequence
found in at least 5 per cent of individuals.20 When this type of analysis
was first initiated, there was a good rationale for such a focus. For
reasons of cost, the only method of analysis for studying a large number
of individuals was not to sequence the whole genome but rather to iden-
tify the presence, or absence, of common variations in the genomes of
those individuals that were being assessed. But what if the common
variations being assessed have no role in the disorder? This then raises
the question of why they would show any association in the first place.
To understand this we need to look in a little more detail at how gene
association studies work.
GWAS make use of the fact that genes are linked to other parts of the
genome by virtue of belonging to the same chromosome, there being 23
pairs of chromosomes in a typical human cell. Each gene also comes as a
pair, with subtle variations in sequence between the two members of such
a pair, which is why scientists often refer to gene ‘variants’. Now it might
be expected that a gene on any particular chromosome would always be
linked to other genes on that chromosome. However, a process called
‘crossing over’ that occurs during the formation of egg and sperm cells
shuffles genetic information between any particular chromosome pair—
that is, between the chromosome derived from the mother and that which
comes from the father.21 This exchange of information swaps sections of
one gene variant with that of its pair, which is why no two eggs or sperm
from a particular individual are genetically the same, and why siblings
190
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
191
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
on the fact that particular CNVs are associated with a greatly enhanced
risk of an individual succumbing to schizophrenia.
For instance, one CNV, in which approximately one and a half million
letters is lost from a region of chromosome 3, leads to a 40-fold increase
in the chance of an affected person becoming schizophrenic.26 This region
of the genome contains 22 protein-coding genes and there is now interest
in investigating whether any of these are involved in the increased risk of
schizophrenia. One particularly interesting discovery is that some of the
genes eliminated by CNVs associated with this disorder seem to play
important roles in the way that glial cells regulate inflammation—one of
the ways the body responds to infection—in the brain. This has led to the
idea that one factor that might trigger schizophrenia could be an inappro
priate response to trauma, resulting in damage to regions of the brain.
Other recent findings have suggested that schizophrenia may be trig-
gered by an inappropriate immune response. For instance Oliver Howes
and colleagues at the London Institute of Medical Sciences have shown
that the earliest stages of schizophrenia are associated with a surge in the
number and activity of glial cells in the brain.27 We observed earlier that
glial cells help to fight infection, so a surge in their numbers could typic
ally occur in response to an infective agent in the brain, but they also act
in a ‘gardening’ role, pruning unwanted connections between neurons.
But in schizophrenics, the pruning seems to become overly aggressive,
leading to vital connections between neurons being lost.
The most extensive pruning in schizophrenics occurs in the prefrontal
cortex, a region that as we have seen plays an important coordinating
role in the brain, as well as in regions involved in hearing, which may
explain why people suffering from this disorder often hear voices.28 The
prefrontal cortex also indirectly controls the brain’s levels of dopamine,
which could be important since, as we noted earlier, imbalances in dopa-
mine may underlie some of the symptoms experienced by schizophren-
ics. Most drugs used to treat schizophrenia work by blocking dopamine
release, which can bring psychotic symptoms under control, but
probably does little to address the underlying problem in the brain.
192
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
According to Howes, ‘the current drugs . . . all still work in exactly the
same way. They are only able to target the delusion side of things. It’s like
getting a sledgehammer and squashing it down.’29 In contrast, he hopes
that studying how an inappropriate immune response might play a
major role in the development of schizophrenia could lead to new drug
treatments in the future.
Another emerging idea about why certain individuals succumb to
schizophrenia is that while the symptoms of this disorder generally only
become obvious in early adulthood, the condition itself may be set in train
by changes in the brain at a much earlier stage of development. Evidence
for such a view comes from studies of the link between schizophrenia and
CNVs mentioned above. One intriguing aspect of such studies is not only
the new insights that they are generating about the underlying mechan
isms of this disorder, but also the revelation that quite different mental
disorders—as defined by their symptoms—may be more closely related in
terms of their developmental origins than had been suspected.
For instance, an interesting feature of many CNVs is that they can both
increase the chance of an individual becoming schizophrenic and make
them more likely to succumb to conditions like autism or attention def
icit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD), which first show symptoms in chil-
dren.30 And this raises an important question: what might have caused
such a change in the brain during its development—is it primarily due to
a genetic difference, or is the environment also an important factor?
In addressing this question, it is useful to look at a very different way of
explaining schizophrenia, one based on the idea that it is not genetics but
the social environment that determines whether an individual succumbs
to this disorder. One version of this hypothesis sees schizophrenia as pri-
marily the result of defective family relations, and the disorder as a conse-
quence of those distorted relations, but also as a response to wider
pressures in society. Such a view of schizophrenia is particularly associ-
ated with British psychiatrist R. D. Laing.
Ronald David Laing was born in Glasgow in 1927. In the 1960s he began
to define psychosis in terms of its relation to society, and psychotics as
193
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
individuals who in their own way are making sense of their social circum-
stances.31 One theory particularly associated with Laing is that of the
‘double- bind’. Although first put forward by British anthropologist
Gregory Bateson in the 1950s, it was Laing who popularized this theory.32
It proposes that a key factor in the development of schizophrenia is major
problems of communication within the patient’s family. The basis of the
theory is that the person who becomes mentally unwell is caught in a
situation in which messages from other family members are deeply
contradictory; however, the contradiction is never brought into the open,
and the unwell person is unable to leave the field of interaction with such
family members.33
The mechanics of the double-bind are best conveyed by real-life
examples, as in Laing’s 1964 book Sanity, Madness and the Family.34 The
book was based on studies by Laing and his colleague Aaron Esterson of
patients, their parents, and siblings, individually, in pairs, and as groups.
One case that shows some of the dysfunctional behaviour that can exist
in a schizophrenic’s family is that of a patient code-named ‘Maya Abbott’,
who believed her parents were trying to influence her by telepathy and
thought-control.35 While that may sound like a classic case of paranoid
schizophrenia, Laing and Esterson discovered on speaking to the girl’s
parents that for years they had indeed been trying to influence her
thoughts, believing her to be telepathic. Long before she became ill, the
parents had been sending each other signals which ‘Maya’ was not sup-
posed to perceive, in order to test their hypothesis. Yet when confronted
by ‘Maya’, the parents denied having made the gestures.36 More generally,
numerous studies carried out over the past few decades suggest that sex-
ual abuse, physical abuse, emotional abuse, and a variety of other types of
adverse childhood experiences may be important factors in the develop-
ment of a schizophrenic.37
If schizophrenia can be a product of both differences in individual biol-
ogy and a dysfunctional or harmful social environment, how does it actu-
ally manifest itself in the brain? One confusing issue is that two people
can be diagnosed with schizophrenia with completely different sets of
194
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
195
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
196
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
197
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
imagine that by buying and eating this particular foodstuff they are also
participating in the performance, and perhaps enjoying the attentions of
the woman herself. In this example the viewer is being asked to take part
in what Vygotsky called ‘complex’ thinking, not true conceptual thought.
Yet they are doing so on the clear understanding that what they are watch-
ing is a commercial, and they are not literally either participating in the
piano playing or engaging sexually with the attractive young woman.
In contrast, if a schizophrenic teenager says she is the Virgin Mary, it
is assumed that her logic is that ‘the Virgin Mary was a virgin; I am a
virgin; therefore, I am the Virgin Mary.’47 Here complex thinking is dis-
played, but the schizophrenic seems incapable of recognizing that the
conflation of herself with a religious figure is an illusion. Yet psychosis
can often coexist in schizophrenics with phases of apparent rationality.
One explanation is that the schizophrenic is still relying on a framework
of language established before the onset of their condition, so that even
their rationality may hide the fact that word meaning may be quite
altered in such an individual, but be partially hidden because of the
common language that they share with ‘normal’ individuals in the
population. But of course it may also be the case that the degree of
regression from conceptual thinking in a schizophrenic individual will
vary greatly depending on changes, for instance stressful incidents, in
their environment.
If different biological origins for schizophrenia might manifest them-
selves in common regressive behaviour at the conceptual level, can
Vygotsky’s views about this mental disorder help us better understand
the social triggers for schizophrenia? A study of schizophrenics and their
families by Bjorn Rund of the University of Oslo, published in 1985, found
that non-paranoid schizophrenics, but notably not paranoid schizo-
phrenics, generally came from families that displayed abnormalities in
their patterns of communication.48 For instance, the parents of such
schizophrenics were often highly egocentric in their behaviour, and they
tended to ignore what their children were saying, interrupt them, or send
mixed messages.
198
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
Rund related his findings to Vygotsky’s claim ‘that higher mental func-
tions are internalized social relations.’49 Rund claimed that this implies
that ‘many of the processes involved in the individual’s cognitive activity
are direct reflections of processes and patterns which characterize
interpsychological activity’ and particularly focused on attention, the
process whereby an individual’s awareness becomes focused, like a spot-
light, on a subset of what is going on in their head or their environment.50
Vygotsky thought that voluntary forms of attention emerge as ‘people
who surround the child begin to use various stimuli and means to direct
the child’s attention and subordinate it to their control.’51 However, in a
family situation in which normal communication has broken down,
Rund argued that the child has ‘no opportunity to establish a firm way of
focusing attention on the relevant stimuli in a given situation, because it
will never know which are the relevant stimuli. Instead, the attentional
styles that are internalized will be characterized by a steady wandering
from one stimulus to another, in a search for the most relevant one.’52
And in line with what I said previously about the importance of emo-
tions, Rund also believed that the ‘emotional climate’ within a family
could affect the development of schizophrenia within it.53 This is surely
even more the case if actual physical and sexual abuse are a part of family
life. So anxiety, insecurity, and instability tend to be a feature of the
families in which schizophrenics grew up.
Rund left open the possibility that if biological factors play a role in the
genesis of schizophrenia, this could explain not only why the condition
may run in families, as shown by the fact that unaffected family members
can still display abnormal forms of behaviour, but also why such behav-
iour may help precipitate the condition in a vulnerable individual in the
family.54 However, this also means that other factors outside the family—
racism, sexism, homophobia, problems at school or work, or simply the
pressure of living in a modern capitalist society—might combine to pre-
cipitate the disorder in a specific individual within that family. Finally, it is
also worth considering Laing’s proposal that the apparent irrationality of
schizophrenic behaviour might be an attempt to try and make sense, in
199
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
200
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
ophrenia’, because the clinical definition of the disorder ‘is already begin-
ning to break down, for example, into those cases caused by copy number
[genetic] variations, drug abuse, social adversity . . . presumably this process
will accelerate, and the term schizophrenia will be confined to history,
like “dropsy” [the former term for complications of heart disease].’57
Coupled to this acknowledgement of the potential diversity of causes
is increasing evidence that the severity of schizophrenia may differ in
particular individuals. This is important because some health profes-
sionals have tended to only view this condition as a ‘hopeless chronic
brain disease’ and ignore individuals who make a full recovery, by argu-
ing that this ‘mustn’t have been schizophrenia after all’.58 Yet a recogni-
tion of schizophrenia’s heterogeneity suggests that individuals with a
milder form of the disorder may not show the same total breakdown of
conscious awareness found in someone with a severe form. It may also
be the case that the extent of such a breakdown is affected by the varying
levels of stress in an individual’s life, explaining how some people can
‘recover’ from such a disorder.
Importantly, not only might different types of schizophrenia show dis-
tinct responses to different types of drugs, but also some may be more
amenable to psychotherapy than to drug treatment. Indeed, recent studies
have suggested that people with a history of childhood trauma diagnosed
with schizophrenia are less likely to be helped by antipsychotic drugs.
One psychotherapy approach claimed to have a very good level of suc-
cess in the treatment of schizophrenia is called ‘Open Dialogue’.59 This
approach, first pioneered in Finland—which in the 1980s had one of the
worst incidences of schizophrenia in Europe—has over the past 30 years
of application in this country led to 74 per cent of patients with psychosis
being back at work within two years, compared with just 9 per cent in
Britain.60
An important feature of the Open Dialogue approach is that as well as
the patient, other members of their family are also involved in the therapy
sessions.61 Such an input could be important given the potential role of
dysfunctional family relations in the generation of the condition, but also
201
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
202
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
sa ni t y a nd m a dness
human conscious awareness, and its role in our unique capacities of cre
ativity and imagination, it is maybe not surprising that such enhanced
social interactions can have a therapeutic impact. Intriguingly, there is
also increasing evidence that psychotherapy can lead to positive changes
in the neural chemistry of the brain,64 suggesting that it is a mistake to
only equate such changes with pharmacological interventions.
Here though we currently face a problem. As a consequence of the
ongoing economic crisis that has had such a devastating negative impact
on healthcare and other welfare services, provision of high-quality psy-
chotherapy for those with mental disorders is getting worse, at the same
time as societal pressures such as job insecurity, stagnating salaries, and
attacks on pensions are increasing. The result has been a dramatic rise in
the number of individuals suffering from such disorders. And this com
bination of rising social pressures leading to increased mental distress,
and a healthcare system increasingly unable to cope with the effects of
such distress, is having particularly tragic consequences with regard to
the next type of mental disorder I want to discuss—clinical depression
and anxiety.
203
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 13
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
from the wreckage, the shocking actual unfolding of events on the plane
emerged.3
The recorder showed that at 10.30 a.m. the pilot, Patrick Sonderheimer,
left the cockpit to use the toilet, leaving co-pilot Andreas Lubitz in charge.
But when Sonderheimer returned he was unable to get back into the
cockpit, and Lubitz failed to respond to pleas to open the door. In the
minutes before the crash, Sonderheimer became increasingly frantic,
even using an emergency axe to try and break down the door, but to no
avail. Ironically, after the 9/11 atrocity, cockpit doors were strengthened to
prevent terrorists storming a plane’s cockpit and using it as a weapon of
mass destruction. Mercifully, most of the plane’s occupants seem to have
only become aware of the gravity of the situation just before the crash, for
only then did the cockpit recorder register sounds of passengers’ screams.4
The most obvious initial possibility was that Lubitz had somehow
fallen unconscious and could not therefore open the door. However,
while searching his apartment, detectives found a note from Lubitz’s doc-
tor declaring him unfit to work. Further investigations revealed that the
co-pilot was taking anti-depressant drugs and had been diagnosed with
suicidal tendencies. Meanwhile a study of his computer found web
searches that included ‘ways to commit suicide’ and ‘cockpit doors and
their security provisions’.5 It was looking like Lubitz had become so
depressed that he set out to kill not only himself but also everyone else on
the flight. One question was why Lubitz’s doctors had not informed his
airline of his condition. It transpired that under German patient confiden-
tiality laws, although doctors could advise the co-pilot not to fly, they
could not pass on their concerns to his employer.6
For campaigners against the negative stigma often associated with
mental disorders, the disaster represented a nightmare situation. Masuma
Rahim, a British psychologist, noted that coverage of the disaster was
associated with ‘a barrage of stigmatizing, fear-mongering media reports,
both in the UK and internationally.’7 As Rahim pointed out, ‘there is virtu-
ally no evidence to suggest that the depressed [generally] pose a danger to
others as a result of their illness. [Indeed], this is true of the full range of
205
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
mental health problems: the scientific literature is clear that people with
schizophrenia, long demonized and reviled by the press, are far more
likely to be harmed by others or themselves than to enact violence.’8
This is an important point given that media reports tend to focus on
atrocities like mass shootings, which particularly in the USA are now
almost commonplace and are often ascribed to mental disorder. Yet con-
centrating on the murderous actions of one individual against other
human beings obscures the fact that a far greater cause of death in the
USA is suicide, with the numbers of those deciding to end their lives this
way increasing over recent years. More than 100 US citizens commit sui-
cide each day, this being the tenth leading cause of death overall—third
among 15 to 24 year olds and fourth among 25 to 44 year olds.9 As well as
the tragic loss of the individual, there is the pain and heartbreak for rela-
tives and friends of that individual, with potential effects reverberating for
years to come.
I know this from personal experience. As I mentioned before, in 2015,
my sister Kay committed suicide, after a rapid decline into a severe depres-
sion that proved impervious to either drug treatments or cognitive behav-
ioural therapy. Yet she was the last person one might have expected to
take her own life. Sociable and outgoing, she was devoted to family and
friends, and capable of extraordinary generosity to others. A highlight of
my teenage years was when Kay received her first pay cheque from a
part-time job, and took me, her younger brother, for dinner at the Box
Tree restaurant in Ilkley, Yorkshire, then in possession of its first Michelin
star.10 Later as a health visitor in Bradford, my sister’s caseload included
heroin addicts who worked as prostitutes to fund their habit. It was typ
ical of Kay that she helped one prostitute to both kick her drug habit and
start a new life at university.
In 2013 my sister was diagnosed with skin cancer. As well as the cancerous
cells being removed surgically, Kay was given low doses of interferon—a
chemical produced naturally by the body, but also used as a drug to
treat viral infections and some types of cancer. For a while it seemed
that the surgery had been successful, but then my sister found a lump
206
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
207
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
208
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
209
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
Serotonin
Serotonin
Serotonin transporter SSRI
receptor
210
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
211
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
Another worrying aspect of SSRIs is that while these drugs are only
meant to be taken for a limited period, in practice many patients remain
on them for years, and for some it can be very difficult to stop taking
them, such are the severe effects of withdrawal, which for one patient
included ‘severe shakes, suicidal thoughts, a feeling of too much caffeine
in my brain, electric shocks, hallucinations, insane mood swings.’24
Another patient noted that ‘while there is no doubt I am better on this
medication, the adverse effects have been devastating when I have tried to
withdraw—with “head zaps”, agitation, insomnia and mood changes.
This means that I do not have the option of managing the depression any
other way.’25
One problem in determining the true effectiveness of SSRIs for the
treatment of depression is how little is understood about the biological
basis of this disorder and how drugs like SSRIs alleviate it. In particular,
doubts have been raised about whether the primary defect in depression
is really low levels of serotonin in the brain. So Alan Gelenberg, a depres-
sion and psychiatric researcher at Pennsylvania State University, believes
that ‘there’s really no evidence that depression is a serotonin-deficiency
syndrome. It’s like saying that a headache is an aspirin-deficiency syn-
drome.’26 The idea that reduced serotonin levels are the primary cause of
depression has been undermined in a number of ways.
For instance, a study in 2003 suggested that people with a variant of the
gene that controls serotonin uptake were not as well equipped to deal
with stressful life events and more likely to develop depression.27 Since its
publication, this study has been cited more than 4,000 times, and around
a hundred other studies have been published about links between this
gene, stressful life events, and depression risk, with some confirming a
link, others finding no such evidence. Yet a ‘meta-analysis’ of these
different studies failed to confirm the findings of the original study.
According to Laura Jean Bierut, who led the meta-analysis, this means
that ‘we still know that stress is related to depression, and we know that
genetics is related to depression, but we now know that this particular
gene is not.’28
212
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
213
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
after a trauma or illness, and triggering a severe version of the low mood
people can experience when fighting a virus like flu. Indeed, recent stud-
ies suggest that treating inflammation can alleviate depression, while
treatments that boost the immune system to fight illness can be accom
panied by a depressive mood—for instance, many people feel down after
a vaccination.33
Based on such findings, Ed Bullmore, a psychiatrist at the University of
Cambridge, believes a new field of ‘immuno-neurology’ is on the hori-
zon. For him, it is clear that inflammation can cause depression: ‘We give
people a vaccination and they will become depressed . . . In experimental
medicine studies if you treat a healthy individual with an inflammatory
drug, like interferon, a substantial percentage of those people will become
depressed. So we think there is good enough evidence for a causal effect.’34
If such a viewpoint is correct, it comes tragically too late for my sister,
whose ultimately fatal depression may have been triggered by the inter-
feron that was prescribed to protect her from skin cancer. Yet hopefully,
such new insights will mean that we should not only be able to devise
better treatments for depression, but also identify potential problems
with drugs that may have dangerous effects in certain individuals.
Other studies have supported the idea that in a depressive brain, the
ability of neurons to connect appropriately becomes abnormal, creating
or enhancing low mood, destructive thinking, and other symptoms.
Increasingly, neuroscientists believe that the key to understanding depres-
sion lies in identifying how the disorder affects neural circuitry and the
way that widely separated brain regions communicate through the
long-range projection of nerve fibres.35 According to this view, mental
disorders result from the disruption of the larger circuit wiring of the
brain. And indeed recent studies have shown that certain people with
depression have reduced volume in brain regions used for emotional pro-
cessing, and fewer neural ‘couplings’ within these regions.36
Yet confusingly, some depressed brains may in contrast use their con-
nections too much, as shown by other studies that indicate that the limbic
and cortical regions of the brain in certain depressed patients, which are
214
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
215
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
216
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
it is quite likely that new molecular and cellular mechanisms that play a
role in these disorders in humans can be uncovered in such studies, and
that is why throughout this book I have made my own references to ani-
mal studies. Nevertheless, it is also important to be cautious about the
interpretation of such studies, because whatever interesting insights we
might make about abnormal brain function and behaviour in a rodent, it
is a great leap to then say we have identified something equivalent to a
human mental disorder.
This caution seems justified when we consider what is being measured
in a study of say ‘depression’ in mice. Assessing such a condition in mice
is hampered by the fact that, as Michael Kaplitt of Cornell Medical College
puts it, ‘a mouse can’t tell you how it’s feeling.’43 Instead, mice are typically
subjected to a ‘forced swim’ test, in which they are placed in a water-filled
cylinder. The longer the mouse swims, the stronger it is judged to have a
will to survive. If it quickly gives up—at which point it is rescued—it is
deemed ‘depressed’.44 In another test, the mouse is held upside down by
its tail. The longer it struggles, the less it is judged to show ‘depressive’
behaviour.45 Such studies have been used to identify the role of genes in
depression, and also to test drugs, for instance new types of SSRIs—the
drug class mentioned earlier of which Prozac is a member.
Now clearly there may be differences in opinion among readers of this
book about the rights and wrongs of carrying out scientific experiments
on animals. My personal opinion is that if such studies take us closer to
understanding mental disorders such as the one that led to my sister’s
death, and therefore the possibility of identifying new types of drugs that
might save the life of someone suffering from such a condition, they are
entirely valid. However, given the emerging picture of the complex genetic
basis of depression, and the various social determinants of this condition
that I will discuss shortly, it seems wise to be critical about overly simplis-
tic assumptions about the possibility of modelling such complexity in a
mouse.
A complicating factor for any attempt to identify a biological basis to
depression, or for that matter any mental disorder, is the important role
217
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
that the social environment plays in the genesis of such disorders. In 1978,
George Brown and Tirril Harris of Bedford College studied the incidence
of depression in London and concluded that the best predictor of this dis
order is being a working class woman with an unstable income and a
child, living in a tower block.46 One conclusion is that such a stressful
environment is far more likely to precipitate depression than a less stress-
ful one. But another factor that might trigger depression, given what I
have said previously about the importance of social interaction in human
society, is some disruption to such interaction. And indeed Brown and
Harris found that 89 per cent of the depressed women they interviewed
had suffered a life-changing event such as family bereavement, a marriage
break-up, or loss of a job after a period of full employment.47 And while
all of these events would presumably be highly stressful, they could also
lead to a big reduction in an affected individual’s social interaction.
Another study that investigated the incidence of depression in New
York in the early 1960s found that this condition was more prevalent in
people of lower socioeconomic status, but the ‘loneliness, the isolation,
the lostness . . . of urban life’ was also an important factor.48 But this does
not mean that biological susceptibilities for depression can be ignored.
Moreover, it would be a mistake to believe that the influence of biology
and environment is easily separable, for as we have seen, it is becoming
clear that the environment can directly affect the genome through epi
genetic mechanisms. Not only can such environmental influence affect
the epigenome of a particular individual during their lifetime, but also
more controversially there is evidence that environmental effects can
affect the epigenomes of their offspring.
A dramatic illustration of how anxiety—a condition often associated
with depression—might be passed down through the generations in an
epigenetic manner came from a recent study led by Tracy Bale of the
University of Pennsylvania.49 This found that anxiety induced in male
baby mice by exposing them to stressful stimuli—such as fox odour,
restraint in a conical tube, and white noise—was passed down to future
generations. Bale and her colleagues showed that the anxiety is transmitted
218
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
219
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
among the survivors. But it has been claimed that Holocaust survivors’
children are also more likely to develop post-traumatic stress disorder,
depression, and anxiety, despite having grown up in a non-stressful envir
onment.
Moreover, a recent study led by Rachel Yehuda at the Icahn School of
Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, claimed to have found evidence that
this apparent higher incidence of mental disorder is associated with epi
genetic differences in the DNA of Holocaust survivors’ children.54 In par-
ticular, Yehuda claimed that the children of male Holocaust survivors
who suffered post-traumatic stress disorder had higher methylation of a
gene involved in the stress response. Recall that such methylation changes
can affect the degree to which a gene is turned on or off. According to
Yehuda, ‘the gene changes in the children could only be attributed to
Holocaust exposure in the parents.’55
Subsequently though, Yehuda’s findings and conclusions have come in
for criticism from various directions.56 One criticism is that only a small
number of individuals—32 survivors and 22 of their offspring—were
studied. Another potential flaw is that the study did not consider the pos-
sibility that survivors might—deliberately or inadvertently—have com-
municated the horror of their experience to their children while the latter
were growing up, thereby exposing them to stress. As Josie Glausiusz, a
participant in Yehuda’s study, and whose father survived the Bergen-Belsen
concentration camp put it, ‘I was troubled by a question: How does one
separate the impact of horrific stories heard in childhood from the influ-
ence of epigenetics?’57 Finally, there has been criticism of the fact that the
study only investigated a small number of genes and only found a small
amount of change in the methylation states of the genes studied. Reflecting
such concerns, John Greally, a geneticist at the Albert Einstein College of
Medicine in New York, has remarked that ‘the story being told by the
Holocaust study is indeed fascinating as a scientific possibility, and will
no doubt prompt others to pursue similar studies. Unfortunately, the
story is typical of many in the field of epigenetics, with conclusions drawn
based on uninterpretable studies.’58
220
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
de pr ession a nd a nx ie t y
221
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 14
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
Communication
Behaviour Social
functioning
223
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
individuals. For instance, it has been proposed that the cognitive skills
that allow us to organize ourselves, be flexible, anticipate, plan, set
objectives and goals, control our impulses, and so on are altered in autis
tic individuals. In addition, such individuals seem more attuned to pro
cessing local information before global information. Because of this,
people on the autistic spectrum tend to first focus on specific details of
images or objectives rather than on their entirety. But their tendency
towards repetitive behaviour may also reflect their need to look for and
create patterns in order to find a safe space within a social world that they
may find threatening.
Autism was first described independently in 1943 by two paediatri
cians of Austrian origin: Leo Kanner, based in Baltimore, USA, and Hans
Asperger, in Vienna.6 For a long time, it was thought that Kanner’s autis
tic children were far more disabled by their condition, since they had
delayed or non-existent speech. In contrast, Asperger described his
charges as ‘little professors’—fluent if pedantic speakers with idiosyn
cratic interests.7 In reality, not all of Asperger’s autistic patients were so
academically gifted and they displayed a wide range of deficits as well as
strengths, but for tactical reasons he chose to focus on the four most tal
ented children, stressing their potential for great achievement, given
appropriate education.
Asperger may have been trying to protect his autistic charges at a time
when Nazi eugenic policy viewed physically and mentally disabled chil
dren as ‘lives unworthy of life’.8 Under the T2 ‘euthanasia’ programme,
more than 5,000 such children were murdered by means of lethal injec
tion or starvation. However, Asperger’s precise historical role is currently
a matter of controversy, with recently uncovered evidence suggesting that
he also helped identify some children with disabilities and sent dozens to
Spiegelgrund, a children’s ward in Vienna where adolescents were euthan
ized or subjected to brutal experimentation.9
Notwithstanding these new revelations, Asperger was definitely posi
tive about ‘autistic intelligence’, seeing traits in both artists and scientists,
and observing that ‘not everything that steps out of line, and is thus
224
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
225
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
226
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
227
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
it plays a role in certain types of cancer, and using such drugs to treat mice
engineered to mimic this form of autism, Brambilla’s team reversed the
symptoms of the disorder. Moreover, female mice pregnant with foetuses
with this genetic defect, but treated with such drugs, gave birth to off
spring without autistic symptoms. Brambilla believes this shows that
while it would not be advisable to treat pregnant women carrying an
affected child, it might be possible ‘to permanently reverse the disorder by
treating a child as early as possible after birth. In the case of adults with
the condition, ongoing medication would probably be required to treat
symptoms.’21
Despite these findings, the majority of less severe autistic conditions
are not clearly defined genetically. There is therefore the possibility that
they have a less obvious biological mechanism underlying them, and are
more linked to environmental factors. In recent years there have been
highly misleading claims about such a link. Notably, a British doctor
called Andrew Wakefield published a paper in The Lancet in 1998—since
retracted by the journal—claiming a link between the vaccine for mea
sles, mumps, and rubella (MMR) and autism.22 This prompted large drops
in vaccination rates. One consequence was that measles cases were shown
to have risen by 300 per cent in Europe in 2017, the result of parents shun
ning vaccines following Wakefield’s claims.23 In fact, such claims have
been totally debunked and Wakefield’s UK medical licence was revoked.
Yet that has not stopped him building a successful career as a leading fig
ure in the ‘anti-vaccine’ movement in the USA; this movement was given
a boost by supportive statements by President Donald Trump.24
One reason why Wakefield’s claims resonated with anxious parents is
that the reported incidence of autism has increased over recent decades.
Parents desperate to find an easy explanation for the disorder that might
lead to some kind of ‘cure’ have become an easy target for people claiming
to have a simple remedy. For instance, some parents have resorted to giv
ing their autistic children a solution of chlorine dioxide, sold under the
name Miracle/Master Mineral Solution (MMS) but basically diluted
bleach.25 The popularity of MMS is attributed to Jim Humble, who heads
228
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
the ‘Genesis II Church of Health and Healing’. Humble claims that autism
is caused by viruses and bacteria, and that MMS kills off those pathogens
and cures the condition.26 Not only has this claim no scientific validity,
but giving bleach to a child is highly dangerous. Jeff Foster, a British clin
ician in Leamington Spa, is highly critical of this treatment: ‘Autism is a
neuro-developmental disease . . . developed in the womb or early stages of
life. You can’t just reverse it and anyone claiming that does not under
stand the condition. When you have very extreme measures like this to
“cure” a condition it’s just a roulette game. Eventually someone will die.’27
In fact, there is a different approach to autism that, far from seeing it as
a disorder to be ‘cured’, instead views at least less severe cases as part of
the natural spectrum of human characteristics. Such an approach high
lights the positive, rather than negative, aspects of autism. But it also sees
the development of the full potential of an autistic individual as a key
issue for society as a whole, rather than simply a problem for that indi
vidual or their parents.
In many ways the recognition that autism can have positive aspects is
not a new one. Ever since Asperger’s identification of high-achieving
autistic children became more widely known about in the 1980s, there
has been increasing recognition that some people with autism can be
highly creative individuals.28 For instance, actor Darryl Hannah has
spoken about her struggles with autism since she was a child. Tito
Mukhopadhyay is virtually mute, but the eloquent poems he writes by
hand or types out have provided a window into how an autistic person
can experience life. The artist Stephen Wiltshire has become well known
for his ability to draw landscapes in exact detail even after seeing them
only once. And many IT experts in California’s ‘Silicon Valley’ have been
noted to have autistic traits.29
However, there is a potential problem with a focus only on high-
achieving autistic individuals, since this can lead to the idea that people
who do not succeed in such conventional terms are somehow less worthy
of attention. Instead it is important that we consider ways in which the
potential of every autistic individual might be boosted. To do so, we
229
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
need to recognize the positive aspects of autism, but also consider how
educational measures might address the more negative features of the
condition. To some extent this means widening our definition of what it
means to be creative.
In general, autistic individuals have been perceived as being more rigid
in their behaviour. This rigidity is linked to the difficulty they often have
in judging the actions and intentions of others, and their tendency towards
restricted, repetitive, or stereotyped behaviour. Because of these charac
teristics, it is often assumed that autistic individuals will also be more
rigid in their imaginative and creative processes. Yet recent studies have
tended to undermine this idea, and instead have suggested that given the
right opportunities people with autistic traits can come up with unusually
creative ideas.
For instance, a study led by Catherine Best at the University of Stirling
assessed creativity in autistic and non-autistic individuals by asking them
to generate ideas about static objects such as paper clips and abstract
images.30 Participants were expected to create their own context; the
objects were not defined by their use in society. Those with autistic traits
offered fewer responses to problems presented, but their solutions were
more original and creative than those without such traits. For instance,
when asked to list possible uses for a paper clip, creative responses of
autistic individuals included using the wire to support flowers, creating a
paper airplane weight, or making a token for a board game.31
The finding that autistic individuals are better able to display their cre
ativity and imagination in situations that are less dependent on social
context makes sense given previous evidence that people with autism
often have trouble interpreting their experiences within such a context.
Most of us habitually identify objects by their relationship to other elem
ents of a social situation. For example, we might distinguish between a
salt and sugar shaker by the presence or absence of a pepper shaker or a
coffee pot. Autistic individuals can find it more difficult to use social cues
in this way. Intriguingly, this association of ‘opposites’ that is a character
istic of much ‘normal’ life—think also of the way we tend to picture a
230
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
231
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
the 1920s and early 1930s.37 He argued that children learn best through
shared social activities and when education is geared towards everyday
experiences. Such an approach is especially important for children with
learning difficulties.38 An apparent lack of motivation and intellect in
some children may conceal an inability to connect the abstract content of
school lessons with their own experiences. Vygotsky also believed that a
vast potential exists in every child and was fond of mentioning the example
of Helen Keller, who, although she became a blind deaf mute at nineteen
months old as a result of scarlet fever, nevertheless grew up to become a
famous philosopher, scholar, and social activist.39
Vygotsky focused on three different areas of special needs education.
First were the children labelled ‘learning disabled’ but whose difficulties
stemmed from a deprived social environment.40 This was particularly
important in the nascent Soviet Union in the 1920s as the First World War,
then civil war and famine, had left as many as seven million homeless,
orphaned, abandoned, and neglected children, many of whom were
severely disturbed in their mental development. In extremely forward
thinking for this period of history, Vygotsky argued that addressing social
problems was central to solving children’s learning difficulties.
Second, there were children whose learning difficulties were an indir
ect consequence of a disorder like deafness or blindness.41 According to
Vygotsky, despite the physical origin of these conditions, it is nevertheless
their effect upon the child’s integration into society which is most import
ant. He campaigned for entry of disabled children into mainstream edu
cation and for their participation in the state youth movement, but also
stressed that such integration should not come at the expense of extra
resources for special education.
One of Vygotsky’s central ideas is that human consciousness has been
transformed throughout history by cultural innovations like reading and
writing. He believed that society has a duty to develop and propagate the
sort of innovations that would allow disabled children to benefit from the
wealth of human culture. In Vygotsky’s time there were only Braille
and various forms of sign language, but computers now surely have
232
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
233
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
234
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
235
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
Bipolar I
Mania
Hypomania
Normal
variation
Minor
depression
Unipolar
(depression)
Major
depression
Bipolar II
22 or 23. Each person was given a score related to how many manic traits
they had previously experienced. The study showed that individuals in
the top 10 per cent of manic characteristics had a childhood IQ 10 points
higher than those who scored in the lowest 10 per cent. This correlation
was strongest for verbal IQ. Smith claims that the study offers a possible
explanation for how bipolar disorder may have been selected for through
generations.55 He hopes the findings will help improve the earlier detec
tion of the disorder, which could then lead to measures ‘to help someone
at high risk of bipolar, such as making certain lifestyle changes, protect
ing sleep patterns, [and] avoiding certain stresses’.56
Bipolar disorder entails dramatic mood swings between extreme hap
piness and severe depression. How might this be linked to creativity?
James Fallon of the University of California, Irvine, notes that people with
bipolar tend to be creative when they are coming out of deep depression.57
When a bipolar patient’s mood improves, their brain activity does too:
activity decreases in the lower part of the frontal lobe brain region and
increases in a higher part of that lobe. Interestingly, the same shift hap
pens when unaffected individuals have creative bouts.58 Interpreting how
the brain patterns translate into conscious thought, Elyn Saks, a mental
health law professor at the University of Southern California, believes
that individuals in the manic phase of bipolar disorder do not filter stimuli
as well as other people. Instead, they can entertain contradictory ideas
simultaneously and become aware of loose associations that most
236
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
237
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
nor m a l i t y a nd di v e r si t y
not only of the positive as well as the negative aspects of mental ‘illnesses’,
but also about the diversity of such conditions, and the fact that such
diversity overlaps with that of supposedly ‘normal individuals’. And,
indeed, one positive aspect of the past few years has been an increased
willingness for people to talk about their experience of having a mental
‘disorder’ as well as increased acceptance of people with such conditions.
There is, however, one exception to such acceptance, and that is when it
comes to the criminally insane, a subject we will now explore.
238
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 15
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
240
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
241
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
242
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
Adverse
environment?
Increased
negative Increased
Hyperactive emotional likelihood and
amygdala and salience and intensity of
underactive decreased aggressive
Biased ventromedial impulse response to
development prefrontal control provocation
of neuronal cortex
systems
MAOA
low
Figure 27 Proposal for how low levels of MAOA are supposed to be linked
to crime.
243
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
he shared more characteristics with the murderers than with the control
group!19 Not only that, but Raine has other indicators that he believes pre-
dict future criminal behaviour, one being a markedly low heart rate—the
idea being that people with a slow resting heartbeat have lower levels of
what psychologists call arousal, or the feeling of being awake and alert,
and therefore seek out stimulating experiences, which could include risky
behaviours and criminal activities, to boost their arousal.
Indeed Raine has admitted that ‘from age nine to 11, I was pretty anti
social, in a gang, smoking, letting car tyres down, setting fire to mail-
boxes . . . [but then at] 11, I changed schools, got more interested in studying
and really became a different sort of kid.’20 But this surely raises questions
about whether biology can ‘determine’ criminal behaviour, if the same
biological and juvenile behaviour profile can occur in criminals, but also
in a neuroscientist studying such individuals. And make what you will of
the fact that I share with Raine not only our academic profession, but also
aspects of his youthful behaviour: as a teenager I too was a juvenile delin-
quent, being a prolific shoplifter for several years. And I also have at least
one of the biological features that Raine has linked to criminal behav-
iour—a pulse rate that is under 57 beats per minute.21
One problem in looking for biological differences as the primary cause
of criminality is that this could mean less acknowledgement of the role of
environment in the development of a criminal personality. Indeed, this is
admitted by Raine, who has said that ‘the sociologist would say if we con-
centrate on these biological things, or even acknowledge them, we are
immediately taking our eyes off other causes of criminal behaviour—
poverty, bad neighbourhoods, poor nutrition, lack of education and so
on. All things that need to change. And that concern is correct. It is why
social scientists have fought this science for so long.’22
But it also means that both Raine’s and my own social circumstances
could have had a major impact on how we turned out in life. While I can-
not speak for Raine, certainly my family’s love and support helped turn
me away from petty crime as a teenager. My sister in particular played an
important role, for when I was caught shoplifting by a store detective, it
244
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
was Kay’s pleas that persuaded her not to hand me over to the police. If I
had been charged with a criminal offence, who knows how different my
life might have turned out. And this raises important questions about
the role of biology versus environment, but also that of chance and cir-
cumstance, as important determinants of criminality and anti-social
behaviour.
Of course, trauma during childhood might itself affect brain function.
This idea has received support from anecdotal evidence that several
notorious US serial killers experienced physically traumatic situations
that may have affected their brains.23 For instance, Ed Gein, a murderer
who fashioned clothing and furniture out of women’s body parts, was
repeatedly beaten around the head by his father. Gary Heidnik, who kid-
napped, tortured, raped, and killed women whom he held prisoner in a
pit in his basement in Philadelphia (an activity recreated in the film The
Silence of the Lambs), fell from a tree at six years old and hit his head hard
enough to deform his skull. Jerr Brudos, who killed four women and had
sex with their dead bodies, was electrocuted as a 21 year old and com-
plained of recurring headaches and blackouts thereafter. And Edmund
Kemper, a murderer and necrophiliac who killed ten people, suffered a
head injury after crashing his motorcycle.
To investigate whether brain trauma might make an individual more
likely to become a criminal, a recent study led by Ryan Darby of
Vanderbilt University in Nashville, Tennessee, imaged the brains of indi-
viduals whose criminal activities ranged from fraud and theft to assault,
rape, and murder and who had also suffered injuries to their brains
because of trauma, stroke, or tumours.24 Darby claimed to have found
evidence that the injuries caused impairments in brain regions involved
in ‘morality, value-based decision-making, and theory of mind’—these
being mental attributes that allow us to understand the mental processes
and perspectives of others.25 Commenting on the findings, Winnifred
Louis, a psychologist at the University of Queensland, Australia, believes
they suggest that ‘the lesions are biasing people towards more utilitarian
decisions’, with the brain impairments leaving intact the ability of the
245
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
brain to make reward calculations for the self, but having a negative
impact on the ability to understand how it would feel to be in the pos
ition of the victims of crime.26
Yet before we get too carried away with the idea of a link between brain
injuries and criminal behaviour, it is worth re-examining the lives of some
of the US serial killers mentioned earlier. For as well as suffering injuries
to their heads, causing possible brain damage, many also experienced
substantial psychological trauma as children. Take Ed Gein. As well as
being regularly beaten by his father, Gein had an unhealthy relationship
with his mother, the nature of which partly inspired Alfred Hitchcock’s
film Psycho.27 Gein’s mother verbally abused her son and taught him that
all women, apart from her, were prostitutes and instruments of the devil.
Yet Gein worshipped his mother, referring to her as a ‘saint’. Edmund
Kemper’s mother belittled, humiliated, and abused her son, often making
him sleep in the basement. She refused to cuddle him for fear she would
‘turn him gay’, yet also told Kemper no woman would ever love him.28
Gary Heidnek was punished by his father for bed-wetting by being dan-
gled from a window by the ankles.29
In my home city of Bradford, Peter Sutcliffe was apparently deeply
affected by growing up in a religious Catholic background at the same
time as seeing his alcoholic father often abuse his wife, Peter’s mother,
including calling her a cheat in public.30 It seems possible that such abu-
sive environments may have contributed to shaping the individuals that
carried out such heinous acts, even if brain injury may also have played a
role in some individuals. And while the examples above are anecdotal, a
systematic study by Heather Mitchell and Michael Aamodt of Radford
University, Virginia, has found that serial killers are six times more likely
to have suffered abuse as children than the general population.31
Besides serial killers, maybe nothing is as likely to generate revulsion in
society as paedophilia. Although girls typically begin puberty at age 10 or
11, and boys at age 11 or 12, paedophiles are generally defined as individuals
attracted to children under 13. A person must be at least 16 years old, and
at least five years older than the prepubescent child, to be diagnosed as a
246
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
247
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
248
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
249
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
adult. That might explain why criminal behaviour often runs in families,
and why individuals abused as children may grow up to be abusers them-
selves. Of course, such patterns of repetition could simply reflect the fact
that crime tends to be concentrated in less affluent layers of society, or
that criminal behaviour can be learned. This latter point is after all what a
simplistic ‘blank slate’ view of human behaviour would predict. Yet a
recent study in rats suggests that exposure to trauma during adolescence
can also lead to epigenetic changes associated with aggressive behaviour
in adulthood. This study, led by Carmen Sandi at the Brain Mind Institute
in Lausanne, Switzerland, briefly exposed 28- to 42-day-old adolescent
rats to a fox odour or a bright light every day for seven days.46 Rats exposed
to such fearful stimuli grew up to become what Sandi termed ‘bullies’
who violently attacked any new rat introduced to their cage. In contrast,
rats not exposed to such stimuli as adolescents were significantly less
aggressive.
Imaging studies showed that rats stressed in adolescence had increased
activity in the amygdala—the brain region associated with fear
responses—and lower activity in the prefrontal cortex, involved in social
behaviour and conduct.47 In addition, stress-exposed rats displayed hor-
monal irregularities (high testosterone and low corticosterone levels)
which are associated with aggression and violence in humans. Studies of
the rats’ DNA showed epigenetic changes in the cells of the prefrontal cor-
tex, while adult rats exposed to the stressful stimuli did not show such
changes. This suggests that trauma incurred early in life—but not in
adulthood—can set the stage for aggression in adulthood. Sandi believes
her findings highlight ‘the importance of developing social programs and
scientific research initiatives to offer valid treatments for individuals that
have been victimized early in life.’48
Of course, this is a study of rat behaviour, and it is important to con-
sider the many differences between aggression in humans and that in rats,
and the danger of using terms such as ‘bullying’ to describe aggression in
an animal. Yet there is some evidence that epigenetic mechanisms may
also be at work in the development of violent and anti-social behaviour in
250
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
251
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
human being could not be inhibited from carrying out such despicable
deeds is made difficult by the revulsion that the overwhelming majority
of us feel when we hear about them. Psychopathic killers characteristic
ally have an apparent absence of remorse or empathy for others, coupled
with a lack of guilt or ability to take responsibility for their actions.54 At
the same time, many serial killers can be superficially charming, which is
one way they lure potential victims into their web of destruction.55 This
dissonance between the violence of their acts and the ability of such indi-
viduals to integrate themselves into wider society as apparently rational,
considerate citizens is one reason why serial killers are rarely considered
to suffer from a mental disorder to such a debilitating extent that they are
considered insane by the criminal justice system.
To be classified as legally insane, an individual should be unable to
comprehend that an action is against the law at the moment the action is
undertaken.56 Because of this, many reports about serial killers present
such individuals as rational—but totally evil—individuals. Yet surely a
truly scientific viewpoint needs to move beyond notions of original sin,
or narrow legal definitions of sanity, and consider instead how an indi-
vidual human can appear rational for much of their existence, yet commit
such horrendous crimes against other people.
One explanation for such cognitive dissonance is that psychopaths are
individuals in whom two minds coexist—one a rational self, able to suc-
cessfully navigate the intricacies of acceptable social behaviour and even
charm and seduce, the other a far more sinister self, capable of the most
unspeakable and violent acts against others. This view of psychopaths has
been a powerful stimulus in fictional portrayals ranging from Dr Jekyll and
Mr Hyde, to Hitchcock’s Psycho, and a more recent film, Split.57 For instance,
in Psycho we learn that the serial killer Norman Bates was so dominated by
his dead mother’s influence that ‘he began to think and speak for her, give
her half his life, so to speak,’ as explained by a ‘psychiatrist’ at the end of
the film.58 Yet there is little evidence that real-life serial killers suffer from
such a form of split personality—now generally referred to as dissociative
identity disorder (DID)—in which an individual has two or more
252
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
253
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
254
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
255
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
city, often linked to widespread use of drugs such as heroin, and the street
gangs that make money selling such drugs to addicts. Yet lest anyone tries
to claim such a state of affairs reflects a natural disposition towards crime
in my fellow Bradfordians, it is worth pointing out the social backdrop for
this situation, namely that a third of adults in Bradford are out of work, 40
per cent of the city’s wards are among the poorest 20 per cent in Britain,
and the city has the highest level of child poverty in the country.68
In this context it is worth mentioning the words of the US writer and
social reformer Studs Terkel, who has said ‘you know [the phrase] “power
corrupts, and absolute power corrupts absolutely?” It’s the same with
powerlessness. Absolute powerlessness corrupts absolutely.’69 Indeed, the
link between crime and poverty was recently highlighted by one of
Britain’s most senior police chiefs—London Metropolitan Police Assistant
Commissioner Patricia Gallan. In an interview in 2018, she argued that
‘children are not born bad’ and called for more effort to deal with inequal-
ities that leave people feeling like ‘they do not have a stake in society’.70
Asked about the link between poverty and alienation and people com-
mitting crime, Gallan replied that ‘I think if you are a young person and
you haven’t got opportunity necessarily—and this isn’t an excuse for it, it
is explanation—what’s your risk? You’ve got a sense of belonging if you
are in a group or a gang . . . and you get the material aspects that you would
like, so that’s part of the challenge.’71
Moving from drug-linked crime in places like Bradford to a different
set of activities, consider a recent article by Charles Ferguson, a former
adviser to the US White House, about individuals who have ‘tolerated,
and in some cases aggressively courted, money laundering by rogue
states, terrorist organizations, corrupt dictators, and major drug cartels’
over recent decades.72 Furthermore, these people also ‘created special
handbooks on how to evade surveillance, created special business units
to handle money laundering, and actively suppressed whistle-blowers
who warned of drug cartel activities.’73 Finally, Ferguson states that ‘we
now possess overwhelming evidence of massive securities fraud,
accounting fraud, perjury, and criminal . . . violations by [the individuals
256
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cr ime a nd pu nishme n t
in question] during the housing bubble that caused the financial crisis [of
2008].’74 At this point you may be wondering who these individuals might
be. Ferguson’s answer is senior executives of some of the world’s main
banking organizations. As he puts it, ‘over the past two decades, the finan-
cial services industry has become a pervasively unethical and highly
criminal industry, with massive fraud tolerated or even encouraged by
senior management.’75 Meanwhile, recent studies conducted by Canadian
psychologist Robert Hare have suggested that while 1 per cent of the gen-
eral population can be categorized as psychopathic, the prevalence in the
finance industry is 10 per cent.76 Yet the individuals mentioned above are
among the richest and most powerful people in the world. So what defines
one set of activities as criminal, yet another as a major route to riches and
the height of high society? And is it really true that so many people who
work in high finance can be classified as having a mental disorder? To
look at these questions further, it is necessary to bring a new factor into
our discussion of human consciousness, namely the question of class,
and the divisions that this gives rise to in current society.
257
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
Part V
CH A P T ER 16
cl ass a nd di v ision
erosion of natural bat habitats and poor safety standards in the wild
animal markets that are now a billion dollar industry in some parts of the
world.4 Meanwhile, in many countries, political parties representing
the neoliberal consensus that has dominated politics over recent decades
are losing ground to organizations that offer more extreme solutions to
the current crisis.5
One factor fuelling unrest has been the continuing stagnation of the
world economy, with wage freezes and cuts in public services proposed
as the only solution to the crisis.6 Yet alongside this, the gap between the
super-rich and the majority of people is widening, with a recent report
predicting that by 2030, the richest 1 per cent will own two-thirds of
global wealth.7 All of this is likely to affect the minds of individual
human beings. At the same time, the current scenario raises questions
about the collective consciousness of humanity—as compared to that of
individuals—given the existence of such major divisions in society. In this
chapter I will look at such issues, but to do so, we need first to consider
how the current organization of society arose, and how it compares to
past human societies.
Homo sapiens is around 200,000 years old, but proto-human species
first became distinguished from apes around 6 million years ago.8 For the
vast majority of that time, our ancestors lived in so-called ‘hunter-
gatherer’ societies. In such societies, most or all food is obtained by for
aging for plants and berries and hunting wild animals, in contrast to
agricultural societies, which rely mainly on cultivated crops and domesti-
cated animals.9 Although only small numbers of people still live in hunter-
gatherer societies, studies of such existing societies, as well as a few past
ones, have identified some key features.
A common assumption about such societies is that they probably live
on the brink of starvation. Yet Richard Lee of the University of Toronto
studied the Ju/’hoansi ‘bush people’ of the Kalahari desert in the 1960s,
and found that they made a good living from only 15 hours’ work per
week.10 One source of this success is skill in exploiting natural resources
in a sustainable manner. The Ju/’hoansi consume over 150 plant species,
262
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
and can hunt and trap effectively any animal they choose, but they only
work to meet immediate needs, do not store surpluses, and also never
harvest more than they can eat in the short term.
Another feature of hunter-gatherers is their egalitarianism. Cambridge-
based anthropologist James Suzman believes that ‘the evidence of our
hunting and gathering ancestors suggests we are hard-wired to respond
viscerally to inequality.’11 A common characteristic of hunter-gatherers is
that they hate individuals showing off; for instance, bush people typically
‘insult’ a hunter’s kill. A Ju/’hoan man explained to Richard Lee why they
do this: ‘When a young man kills much meat, he comes to think of him-
self as a chief or a big man—and thinks of the rest of us as his servants or
inferiors. We can’t accept this. . . so we always speak of his meat as worth-
less. This way, we cool his heart and make him gentle.’12
Such societies also have no formalized leadership institutions.13 Men
and women enjoy equal decision-making powers, children play largely
non-competitive games, and the elderly, while treated with affection, are
not afforded any special privileges. This means that in these societies no
one sees any point in accumulating wealth or influence, or seeks to over-
exploit their marginal environment. Of course, it is important not to
idealize hunter-gatherer societies. Without the benefits of modern medi-
cine such societies are far more vulnerable to disease, whether this be
viral or bacterial infections, or disorders with a more genetic basis.
Yet while many people today live in an infinitely richer society than
hunter-gatherers in material terms—and this includes access to cutting-
edge advances in medical diagnosis and treatment—modern civilization
is deeply divided. Not only wealth divides the world; racism, sexism, and
homophobia can also incite people to hatred, and even murder, of other
human beings because of their skin colour, gender, or sexual choices.14 Yet
although such divisions are often assumed to be fundamental features of
humanity, both the immense power of modern society and its divided
status originated only around 12,000 years ago when certain human
groups first began tending cattle and growing crops—the so-called ‘agri-
cultural revolution’.15 In the ‘fertile crescent’ extending through what is
263
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
now Lebanon, Syria, and Iraq, the domestication of plants led to a more
sedentary life and fixed-field agriculture that, in turn, led to the develop-
ment of cities and the rise of the state.
The development of civilization was once thought to be very rapid;
however, recent studies have indicated a gap of 4,000 years between the
first domestications and the rise of the city as a dominant way of ordering
human affairs.16 Rather than embracing farming with enthusiasm, com-
munities initially adopted subsistence strategies that combined hunting
and gathering with a low level of domestication and cultivation. This
seems to be because, at least in its early days, agriculture was a rather
fraught process.17 Not only did crops sometimes fail, but with less time to
hunt and gather, diets became more restricted—which adversely affected
health—and with more people living in close proximity to their animals,
there were more possibilities for disease to spread. For this reason Jared
Diamond of the University of California, Los Angeles, has declared agri-
culture to be the ‘worst mistake in the history of the human race.’18 Yet
although an important corrective to the idea that the agricultural revolu-
tion was a smooth ladder of progress, this statement neglects the fact that
civilization, and all its later technological marvels, could only have come
about through this revolution.
In particular, agriculture allowed the growth of city states, which led to
the development for the first time of the civilizations that we take for granted
today. However, we should also be aware of the brutality involved in the for-
mation of such city states. James Scott of Yale University has argued that for
a state to exist it needed a crop that was easily taxed, and wheat proved ideal
in this respect.19 Because the fields were fixed and this crop ripened over a
short period of time it was impossible for the farmer to avoid the tax col
lector. Communities elsewhere in the world reliant on tubers or root veget
ables such as yams and manioc as their crop were more able to avoid tax
since these plants can be left in the ground and harvested over a long period,
and indeed such societies seldom developed into city states.
Another advantage of wheat was that it could be stored within the city,
from where it was doled out to slaves and soldiers or used to feed the
264
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
whole population during a siege.20 Through taxation the state became the
quartermaster and the producers subjects. Early states functioned as
‘population machines’ designed to control labour, with slavery a key
element. Maintaining the number of slaves was vital and if this fell a new
batch could be gathered through warfare. Raiding to acquire goods and
manpower became a normal part of life. Far from an aberration, slavery
was built into the first civilizations.21 Above all else, the rise of civilization
led for the first time in history to human beings being divided into ‘classes’.
Other classes besides slaves existed in ancient societies, such as ‘free’
workers—who tended the land or did other jobs in the cities—merchants,
priests, and rulers: either a king or pharaoh, or as in the case of the Roman
Republic, a governing body like a senate. However, the surplus labour
that provided the income and wealth of the ruling class was primarily
extorted from slaves, and there was a continual need to define them as
inferior beings. This was despite ancient societies like those in Greece or
Rome first establishing the principles of democracy: the democracy was
only for a small portion of society. According to Oxford historian
Geoffrey de Sainte Croix, ‘slavery increased the surplus in the hands of
the propertied class to an extent which could not otherwise have been
achieved and was therefore an essential precondition of the magnificent
achievements of classical civilisation.’22
Slaves, but also women of all classes, were excluded from democracy in
the first city states.23 This had an impact on general perceptions of what it
means to be human. For while in hunter-gatherer groups individuals
might have different roles in the group, but no one was seen as superior to
anyone less, in the new city states the idea grew that some human beings
were inferior to others, and that slaves were in many senses actually sub-
human. For instance, the Greek philosopher Aristotle argued that ‘the use
made of slaves and of tame animals is not very different; for both with
their bodies minister to the needs of life.’24
Class society also led to the idea—for the first time in history—that
women were inferior to men. Through their dominance in the warlike
city states, some men began to accumulate wealth and power. And as an
265
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
266
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
267
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
that stem from workers in capitalist society selling their labour in return
for wages. The worker creates something—a plate of food, a piece of
clothing, or for that matter an office report or spreadsheet—and receives
a wage for doing so. The object is then sold for more than it cost to make,
or used to boost a firm’s overall income, which is the source of the
employer’s profit. At the end of the process the object produced no longer
belongs to the worker but to the employer; it now appears to the former
as akin to an alien object. This contradiction was expressed by Marx’s
statement that, ‘the worker feels himself only when he is not working;
when he is working, he does not feel himself . . . His labour is, therefore,
not voluntary but forced, it is forced labour . . . Its alien character is clearly
demonstrated by the fact that as soon as no physical or other compulsion
exists, it is shunned like the plague.’33 Adam Smith, one of the first theorists
of capitalist economics, believed that work required the worker to give up
‘his tranquillity, his freedom, and his happiness.’34 Yet in terms of the
propagation of capitalist society, alienation has the virtue that workers
may often underestimate—or even be totally unaware of—their centrality
to the production process.
One reason for the alienation of workers from the labour process is a
principle Smith identified as a key characteristic of capitalism—a ten-
dency to break down work into simpler sub-tasks, which he famously
illustrated through a detailed study of the manufacture of pins.35 The
industrialist Henry Ford later perfected such a division of labour in his car
plants, which is why this practice is sometimes referred to as ‘Fordist’.36
An important aspect of this approach is a continual drive to increase the
speed of production by putting pressure on each worker to perform their
part of the manufacturing process at the maximum speed.
The consequences of such pressures in a modern context were revealed
in a recent report in Business Insider, which interviewed workers at delivery
warehouses of the Amazon tech-giant.37 Amazon ‘pickers’ move around
such warehouses on a predetermined route to collect items for delivery,
scanning each with a handheld scanner, which monitors the length
between scans. Because of the pressure this puts on workers, one US
268
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
Amazon employee described the ‘awful smell’ that often emanated from
warehouse trash cans, due to workers urinating in these instead of using
the bathroom, because of fears about missing targets.38 Other workers
confirmed a general picture of constant surveillance and a crippling fear
of missing targets. Given what I have said previously in this book about
the link between stress and mental disorders, the likely risks of such work
practices should be obvious.
It might be argued that the conditions in an Amazon warehouse repre-
sent an extreme example, given that this is a non-unionized workplace
whose employees are often relatively unskilled, and on low wages and
temporary contracts. However, a similar increasing pressure to perform,
plus low pay and job insecurity, have been an increasing feature in recent
years even of such skilled occupations as my own area of work—university
teaching. Increasingly UK universities employ junior lecturers on insecure,
non-permanent contracts, which range from ones that typically elapse
within nine months to those in which lecturers are paid by the hour to
give seminars or mark essays and exams, with over 50 per cent of
university teaching now done in this way.39 How it feels to teach students
in such a context was recently summed up by one lecturer who said, ‘we
are seasonal labourers like fruit pickers. You have to e- mail every
September, cap in hand, saying: “Is there any work for me this year?” ’40
Alienation affects more than just workers under capitalism. Previously,
I mentioned claims that people with a ‘psychopathic’ personality are ten
times more likely to be found at the top of the financial industry than in
the general population.41 Yet even if true—and it is important to distin-
guish such a personality from that of a violent psychopath—there is a
danger of seeing such individual behaviour as the cause of the cut-throat
world of high finance, rather than as a consequence of the values of capit
alism at its most intense. It may well be that individuals with ‘superficial
charm, conning, and manipulative behaviour, lack of empathy and
remorse, and a willingness to take risks’—which is how psychopaths
were defined in this case—might thrive in such an environment.42 But it
could also be the case that such behaviour tends to emerge because this
269
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
270
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
it was a man and his wife.’ A common object above the marital bed in
many Russian peasant households was a whip, placed there as a warning
to—and sometimes used on—women who started voicing opinions
deemed inappropriate to the status of their sex.47
In modern society, the status of women has changed in fundamental
ways, although such a change was far from automatic, nearly always
involving bitter struggles. A key step forward was the right to vote,
granted to women in Britain in 1918.48 Women are also increasingly seen
as key members of the workplace in modern capitalist society, at both the
highest and lower levels. However, women are still frequently judged as
inferior for two main reasons. First, they are expected to be the primary
individuals who care for children and look after the home.49 Second,
alienation under capitalism means that everything can be treated as a
commodity.50 Women’s bodies are used in advertising to sell everything
from cars to soft drinks. In the sex industries, a woman’s body is more
directly offered for sale. This affects the perception of women in society in
general, not only of prostitutes and porn stars. The allegations against the
movie producer Harvey Weinstein of the assault and rape of a number of
women in the film industry, for which he was later convicted, unleashed a
flood of others, not just in the entertainment industry, but in a whole
number of other sectors.51
So how might alienation manifest itself in the individual mind under
capitalism? One feature of the human brain that might explain why many
working people can accept and even acquiesce in their state of exploit
ation, and even sometimes see other workers as the primary threat, rather
than as allies in campaigns for better pay and conditions at work, is the
way our brains have evolved to respond to living in a potentially hostile
natural environment. In such an environment, from an evolutionary
point of view, any species, whether this be fish, fowl, bacterium, or even a
virus, has two main imperatives: one is to survive, the other is to repro-
duce. But to do both effectively, two, sometimes opposing, tendencies
need to be at work. One is the tendency to keep doing all the things that
have kept a species alive and reproductive across many generations; the
271
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
other is the ability to respond rapidly to any perceived threat to that status
quo. The first tendency, which encourages sticking with a certain set pat-
tern of behaviour and is rooted in the more primeval parts of our brain,52
may explain why it can take quite a considerable change in socioeco-
nomic forces for the majority of people to think that there can be any
point in challenging the status quo.
As for the second tendency, two brain regions that we have already
mentioned, the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex, are relevant. As a pri-
mary mediator of emotional responses, the amygdala is one of the main
ways that our brains respond to potentially threatening situations.53
Imagine walking down a dark alley and suddenly there is a noise. In such
a situation, the amygdala will immediately become activated. Yet as soon
as we realize that the noise is just a rustle of leaves, or a dog barking in a
neighbouring garden, our rational side, personified by our prefrontal cor-
tex, suppresses such irrational fears and the activity of our amygdala
returns to normal. Yet despite this, there is increasing evidence that many
unconscious fears about a perceived threat of people of a different colour
skin, sexual persuasion, or even male fears about female equality, involve
the amygdala.54 Because of this, although inflammatory speeches by poli-
ticians or media articles may enhance such fears, thereby dividing people
who have much to gain by working together for better pay and condi-
tions, they are helped by being able to trigger deep-seated impulses in the
human brain.
Another way that the structure of current society is relevant to discus-
sions about human brain function is through its impact on mental health.
Earlier in this book, I looked at how differences in individual biology, but
also tensions and stresses in family relationships, can influence whether a
particular person succumbs to a mental disorder. However, it would be a
major omission to ignore the role that modern society plays. People are
taught from an early age that the route to fulfilment in life is to make
themselves acceptable for the job market. Such concerns play upon the
idea that success in future employment is primarily down to individual
merit. Yet a person’s gender, ethnicity, and sexual persuasion still have far
272
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
too much impact on their success in the labour market. And another
factor affecting people’s status is the rapid and dramatic changes that
occur in the economy following economic crisis.
Demonstrating the impact of economic recession on mental health,
suicide rates in the USA jumped from 12.1 to 18.9 per 100,000 people after
the 1929 Wall Street crash. More recently, a study has found that after the
recession of 2008, suicide rates surged in the European Union, Canada,
and the USA. Aaron Reeves, of the University of Oxford, who led the
study, notes that ‘there has been a substantial rise in suicides during the
recession, greater than we would have anticipated based on previous
trends.’55 David Stuckler, a researcher on the study, drew particular atten-
tion to Greece, a country in which austerity policies imposed by EU insti-
tutions and the International Monetary Fund have meant that the country
‘has gone from one extreme to the other. It used to have one of Europe’s
lowest suicide rates; it has seen a more than 60 percent rise.’56 Each sui-
cide corresponds to around 10 suicide attempts and between 100 to 1,000
new cases of depression. In Greece, according to Stuckler, ‘that’s reflected
in surveys that show a doubling in cases of depression; in psychiatry ser-
vices saying they’re overwhelmed; in charity helplines reporting huge
increases in calls.’57
The COVID-19 pandemic and subsequent social distancing measures
as part of a general ‘lockdown’ in countries across the world in 2020 also
had major impacts on mental health.58 This was both due to the lock-
down’s effect on people’s ability to make a living and because lack of
social interaction proved very difficult for some individuals to deal with
and led to mental instability, which is maybe not that surprising given
that, as we have seen, social interactions are key to the development and
maintenance of human consciousness.
The divisions in current society, by ethnicity and gender as well as class,
also exert a heavy toll on mental health. The possible impact of racism on
mental health was suggested by a recent study that found that Afro-
Caribbeans living in Britain are nine times more likely to be diagnosed as
schizophrenic than white people.59 Arguing against this difference having
273
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
274
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
275
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
The treatment of women as sex objects also affects male minds. Since
1999, feminist activist and writer Julie Bindel has interviewed men who
pay prostitutes for sex, as part of a re-education programme for such men
in my home county of West Yorkshire. Many men explicitly compared
their actions to buying goods at a shop, with one stating that ‘I made a list
in my mind. I told myself that I’ll be with different races, e.g. Japanese,
Indian, Chinese . . . Once I have been with them I tick them off the list. It’s
like a shopping list.’69 Another claimed that ‘selecting and purchasing has
something to do with domination and control.’70
Tragically, the vulnerable positions of many prostitutes means that
they are also far more likely to be beaten or even killed in the course of
their work, by men whose idea of ‘domination and control’ can extend to
murder.71 Previously I mentioned Peter Sutcliffe, the ‘Yorkshire Ripper’,
who murdered 13 young women and attempted to kill seven others who
survived, but were scarred, both physically and emotionally, for life. After
his arrest, Sutcliffe was diagnosed as a ‘concealed’ paranoid schizo-
phrenic, mainly based on his claim that a ‘divine voice’ had told him in
1967 that it was his mission to kill prostitutes.72
In fact, some believe that Sutcliffe was merely tapping into the domin
ant image propagated particularly by the media that his primary target
was this group of women. Yet a look at Sutcliffe’s proven and suspected
victims—for this may be a larger group than he has admitted to—under-
mines the idea that he only attacked prostitutes. For instance, one victim,
who escaped although with severe head injuries, was a 14-year-old school-
girl, Tracey Browne.73 What interviews with Sutcliffe have illustrated—
and this echoes a theme that has almost become a cliché in many reports
about male serial killers who prey on the opposite sex—is that in many
such people there seems to be a mental battle going on between seeing
women either as sacred objects, often demonstrated by an adoring but
uneasy relationship with their mothers, or as agents of vice or contamin
ation who must be eradicated like vermin.74 Tellingly, when Sutcliffe was
asked by his brother Carl why he had committed such horrendous crimes,
his response was that ‘I were just cleaning up the streets, our kid.’75
276
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
cl ass a nd di v ision
277
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 17
279
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
280
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
281
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
were debates on the picket line and in ‘teach-ins’ about the gender gap in
pay and lack of childcare facilities, and also the fact that BAME individuals
are underrepresented as both students and senior staff in top British uni
versities like Oxford. Another issue discussed was the sense of an increas-
ing lack of democracy and representation in governance structures.
Notably, the very different social environment of the strike seems to have
helped create a space in which the nature of such institutional structures
could be questioned and challenged.
While clearly a very different dispute, the prominent role that women
played in the miners’ strike also led to transformative experiences.18
Before the strike, miners’ wives were mainly situated in the home. But by
becoming involved in the support networks that played a crucial role in
sustaining the strike, theirs became a collective experience, and also
brought them into contact with much wider groups of people than would
normally have been the case. This in turn created a space in which some
women began to contest assumptions about their place in society and to
raise questions in their minds about issues like gender and sexuality.
While the miners’ strike was ultimately unsuccessful, in other histor
ical eras individual struggles have acted as a catalyst for mass movements
that successfully challenged the very idea of what is possible in society.
One example was the ‘new union’ movement of the late nineteenth
century in Britain, partly because the workers who triggered it—the
‘match girls’ as they were known—were initially seen as unlikely to act as
a catalyst for anything at all.
If anyone had been asked at the start of 1888 which workers were most
likely to go on strike, the employees of Bryant and May’s match factory in
east London would probably have been very low on their list. The age of
the workers, who could be as young as 13 years old, their lower status as
females, and the ‘unskilled’ nature of their work—at a time when trade
unionism was the preserve of skilled, male workers—made the match
girls an unlikely spearhead of a new movement.19
In June 1888, a journalist called Annie Besant wrote an article exposing
the conditions at the factory.20 She drew attention to the fact that employees
282
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
worked 14 hours a day for less than five shillings a week, while exposure
to the white phosphorus used to make the match heads could cause
‘phossy jaw’, a degenerative disorder in which the whole side of the face
turned green and then black, discharging foul-smelling pus, and which
could lead to death. Initially, it seemed as if the article’s publication—
meant to highlight the match girls’ plight—had backfired, for certain girls
who had spoken to Besant were promptly sacked.
However, the company had not reckoned on the response of its young
workforce. Immediately, hundreds of girls stopped work and soon all
1,400 workers were on strike. Particularly interesting was the way the
girls themselves described their action. As one striker noted, ‘one girl
began, and the rest said yes, so we all went. It just went like tinder.’21
Looking to express what she and her fellow workers had done, the striker
had found a metaphor for their actions in the product—the fire-starting
match—produced in the factory.
This metaphor acquired an even greater resonance the following year.
For the success of the strike—after three weeks of intense activity involv-
ing regular mass meetings, a strike committee, and collections from other
workers—led to other unskilled workers also striking for better pay and
conditions. A report in the East London News and Advertiser in September
1889 gave a flavour of the movement: ‘The present week might not inaptly
be called the week of strikes . . . parcels postmen; car men; rag, bone and
paper porters and pickers. The employees in jam, biscuit, rope, iron,
screw, clothing and railway works have found some grievance, real and
imaginary, and have followed the infectious example of coming out on
strike.’22 But it was the initial action of the match girls, the report con-
cluded, that had precipitated this huge strike wave, and picking up on the
girls’ own metaphor, their action was described as the ‘proverbial small
spark’ that had ‘kindled a great fire’.23 Many British trade unions still in
existence today came into being during this period of action.
The speed with which people’s generalized mass actions can spread
means that some of the biggest strike movements in modern history
have often surprised even those sympathetic to the idea of such action.
283
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
In January 1968, the left-wing philosopher Andre Gorz wrote that ‘the
working class will neither unite politically, nor man the barricades, for a
10 per cent rise in wages, or 50,000 more council flats. In the foreseeable
future, there will be no crisis of European capitalism so dramatic as to
drive the mass of workers to revolutionary general strikes.’24 Yet only
months later in May, unrest began in France with a series of student
protests which then spread to factories with strikes involving 11 million
workers—more than 22 per cent of the population at the time. Eventually,
order was restored, but only following major concessions to the strike
movement, and to this day, the events of May 1968 are considered as a
cultural, social, and moral turning point in the history of France.25
Successful revolutions—events by which a society’s whole structure is
transformed—happen rarely, but when they do their consequences are
profound, so a common term used to describe the English revolution of
1642–9 was ‘the world turned upside down’.26 A sense of exhilaration is
also common in revolutions’ initial stages. The English poet William
Wordsworth expressed the hopes of a generation about the French revo-
lution of 1789 when he wrote, ‘Bliss was it in that dawn to be alive. But to
be young was very heaven!’27 After the 1917 Russian revolution, Alexander
Luria, a psychologist who worked closely with Vygotsky, wrote, ‘I began my
career in the first years of the great Russian revolution. This single, momen-
tous event decisively influenced my life and that of everyone I knew.’28
Such views reflect the fast-moving pace of revolutions, the way they
can challenge long-held views about society and human relationships,
but also the unique space they can often offer to younger people to make
their mark on the world. Note that the frontal lobes—the areas of the
brain particularly involved in planning, reasoning, and judgement—are
still maturing in young people, and this may be one reason that individ
uals at this age can make decisions that may seem to be guided more by
emotion than cold rationality—behaviour generally considered a sign
of immaturity.29 However, in a revolution, it might mean younger
people are far less likely to be influenced by what Karl Marx called the
284
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
285
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
286
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
287
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
that a lot of what we take for granted about the state of the world—and
this can include prejudices about others based on their skin colour,
nationality, sexual persuasion, or gender—also involves unconscious
feelings that stem mainly from the amydala.41 But the interaction between
these two brain regions is dynamic, and in a situation of rapid social
change as in a mass movement, I believe that there is the potential for the
prefrontal cortex to play a much more dominant role.
The reason for this is that mass movements force individuals into
unfamiliar situations and lead to new types of social interactions with
other people in which language plays a key role. And this restructuring of
neural impulses in the prefrontal cortex could then start to challenge the
type of ‘unconscious biases’ that are rooted in the amygdala. In fact recent
studies have shown that the prefrontal cortex itself is far from homogen
ous in this respect. Instead, such studies have shown that while the ventro
medial prefrontal cortex is involved in guiding our future actions based
on things that have happened to us in the past, the dorsomedial part of
this brain region in contrast allows us to adjust our behaviour to a rapidly
changing situation.42 This makes evolutionary sense for while past
experiences may be a good guide to action in a relatively stable environment,
they may not remain so in times of great uncertainty. And therefore the
dorsomedial prefrontal cortex is likely to play a more dominant role during
the rapidly unfolding events characteristic of major social upheavals.
Previously in this book we have looked at different types of mental
disorders, and whether they are a product of a defective biology or social
environment. So how is mental disorder regarded in mass social move-
ments? One interesting possibility, given the way revolutions are said to
‘turn the world upside down’, is that this radical disruption of the estab-
lished order may provide a space for individuals with views of the world
deemed unconventional, and perhaps even highly abnormal, to come to
the fore in a way that would be unlikely in a calmer social climate.
Théroigne de Méricourt was an activist in the 1789 French revolution.
Initially Méricourt was welcomed into the ranks of the radical Jacobin
party, so much so that she ‘was not relegated to the gallery with her s isters,
288
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
but [allowed to sit] in the body of the hall with the men . . . wearing a
semi-military costume.’43 However, this relationship soured when Méricourt
tried to organize a radical organization for working women. In doing so,
she asked, ‘Fellow women citizens, why should we not enter into rivalry
with the men? Do they alone lay claim to have rights to glory . . . let us
open a list of French Amazons; and let all who truly love their Fatherland
write their names there.’44
Such feminist views were too radical even for the Jacobins. However,
this souring of relations did not prevent Méricourt being involved in key
further events such as the storming of the Tuileries Palace in 1792.
Ironically, her fall from grace in the revolution was precipitated by other
women, for in 1793, as she crossed the Feuillants Terrace to deliver a
speech, she was attacked by female supporters of the Jacobins, who
stripped her naked and beat her.45 After this, Méricourt never made
another public appearance. Tragically, in 1794, she was certified insane
and sent to the Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital in Paris, where she was said to
crawl on the floor like an animal, eat straw, and strip off her clothes.46
According to novelist and commentator Hilary Mantel, Méricourt’s
‘madness was not without eloquence . . . She denounced her keepers as
royalists, and spoke of decrees and government measures, addressing her
words to the Committee of Public Safety. But the great Committee was
long since disbanded, its members guillotined or in exile. For Théroigne
time had stopped, some time in 1793.’47 Méricourt eventually died in the
Pitié-Salpêtrière in 1817, at the age of 54.
Méricourt’s life raises questions about madness and the passions
aroused during a revolution. Was her psychotic state triggered by the
trauma of her public beating? Or did elements of insanity exist in her
before this time, but in the context of the revolution made Méricourt a
bold and inspiring figure to others, at least for a while? In fact, the very
passions that made Méricourt an influential leader during the revolution
may in a different context have led to her later descent into madness.
Or alternatively, Méricourt’s madness may have been a way of dealing
mentally with the retreat, as Napoleon Bonaparte took power, from the
289
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
290
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
291
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
also marked a break with 1917 in other ways. For instance, the radical view
of human consciousness developed by Lev Vygotsky that has inspired
this book was also suppressed by Stalin after Vygotsky’s death in 1934, as
were the exciting new approaches to education and disability that
accompanied this view.56
In addition, far from developing new ways to help those in mental
distress, the repressive regime that developed under Stalin and his succes-
sors began to use the term ‘mental illness’ to label those who opposed the
regime. As Nikita Khrushchev remarked in 1959, since it should be
impossible for people in a ‘communist’ society to have an anti-communist
consciousness, ‘of those who might start calling for opposition to
Communism on this basis, we can say that clearly their mental state is not
normal.’57 In 1969, Yuri Andropov issued a decree on ‘measures for pre-
venting dangerous behaviour (acts) on the part of mentally ill persons.’58
Consequently, psychiatrists began to diagnose and confine anyone who
fitted the description of a political agitator. According to the archives of
the International Association on the Political Use of Psychiatry, at least
20,000 USSR citizens were sent to mental institutions for political
reasons, but the real figure may be much higher.59
A parallel tragedy had previously taken place in Germany in the 1930s
with the rise to power of Adolf Hitler. In what became a prelude to the
Holocaust that would eventually result in the murder of 17 million people,
a pseudo-scientific view that labelled people with mental health problems
and learning disabilities as ‘degenerate’ led to the extermination by the
Nazis of 200,000 such individuals by the end of World War II.60 In what
British psychiatrist Tom Burns has described as German psychiatry’s ‘most
shameful chapter’, the ‘extermination of the mentally ill is compounded
by several prominent psychiatrists leading it and none vigorously opposing
it . . . The broad mass of the profession probably did not share the extreme
views articulated, but they voiced no effective opposition.’61
Such historical developments raise the question of how all those p eople
who backed the initial revolution in Russia in 1917 with such high hopes
could allow it to end in such a state of terror. Or for that matter, why did
292
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
293
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
Stephen Reicher, Clifford Stott, and John Drury, have found that individuals
in crowds do not abandon their rationality or surrender their identity to a
mob mentality.65 Such individuals do, however, become highly sensitive
to what those around them are doing, and become strongly cooperative
as a result. In line with this, eyewitness accounts of the revolutions men-
tioned above confirm the high degree of creativity of individuals in such
movements as well as their tendency to show tolerance towards, and pro-
mote the rights of, more vulnerable members.66
Indeed, an important feature of any genuine revolution—in this case
meaning a progressive, fundamental change in society—is the extent to
which it tends to challenge divisions between people. Notably the
English revolution of 1642–9 was intertwined with a cry for religious
freedom. At a time when social relations tended to be defined in religious
terms, this was a radical development. The Levellers were particularly
forward-thinking in this respect, for they campaigned for the rights of
Protestants to worship as they saw fit, but also opposed Cromwell’s
subjection of the Catholic citizens of Ireland.67
The French revolution that began in 1789 expanded rights of citizen-
ship to minorities such as Protestants and Jews.68 Similarly, in 1776, the
revolutionaries who wrote the American Declaration of Independence
stated that ‘all men are created equal, that they are endowed by their
Creator with certain unalienable Rights, that among these are Life, Liberty
and the pursuit of Happiness,’ although in practice it would take 87 years
and a bloody civil war for most African Americans to gain their ‘unalien-
able Rights’, and a further 100 years of courageous protests before those
rights could be fully exercised.
In pre-revolutionary Russia, persecution of Jews was state policy, with
pogroms actively encouraged by the authorities.69 In contrast, on gaining
power, the Bolshevik government abolished all legal restrictions on Jews.
The role that Jews played in the Bolshevik party was shown by the fact
that the central committee of the party in October 1917 contained six Jews
out of 21 members in total.70 Lev Vygotsky and Alexander Luria were typical
of the many Jewish intellectuals who initially thrived after the revolution.
294
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
295
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
296
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
the German middle classes, after decades of upward social mobility, now
feel threatened by a ‘downward escalator’.79 Nachtwey believes that ‘for
the lower middle class . . . harsh social competition, the struggle for pros-
perous life, and the disappointed expectations of ascent and security
[has led] to a “brutalisation” of social conflict . . . [where] fear of downward
mobility produces a very specific authoritarianism.’80
Of course it is one thing to hold racist or authoritarian ideas; it is
another to take part in the murderous activities that characterized the
Holocaust. So what might have motivated people to take part in such
activities? One famous scientific study that has strongly influenced our
thinking in this area was carried out by US psychologist Stanley Milgram
from Yale University in the 1960s, who wanted to test the claim by the
German Nazi Adolf Eichmann, during his war crimes trial, that he and his
accomplices in the Holocaust were ‘just following orders’. Milgram
showed that it was possible to persuade volunteers to ‘electrocute’ other
human ‘learners’ by telling them that this was a necessary part of the
study.81 Although the learners appeared to scream in pain, in reality they
were only acting and there was no electric shock. More recently, Tomasz
Grzyb, from the University of Social Sciences and Humanities in Warsaw,
carried out a similar study and confirmed Milgram’s findings. In this
study, 80 volunteers were recruited, including women as well as men, and
90 per cent were shown to be willing to inflict the highest shock level of
450 volts to a complicit ‘learner’.82 Just as in Milgram’s experiment, volun-
teers were spurred on by prompts from the supervising scientist such
as ‘the experiment requires that you continue’, and ‘you have no other
choice, you must go on’. Interestingly, women were far less likely to obey
the orders than men. For Grzyb, the study ‘illustrates the tremendous
power of the situation the subjects are confronted with and how easily
they can agree to things which they find unpleasant.’83
On the face of it, studies such as these seem pretty damning in reinforc-
ing the idea that most people will acquiesce with authoritarianism and
evil. Yet although troubling, there is surely a danger in extrapolating from
a highly artificial laboratory study, without also surveying the actual ways
297
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
298
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
299
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
Part VI
CH A P T ER 18
music a nd r h y t hm
304
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
Supporters of a link between music and sex have also pointed to the
fact that human musical productivity seems to match the course of an
individual’s reproductive life. For instance, Geoffrey Miller, of the
University of New Mexico in Albuquerque, has studied jazz musicians
and found that their output rises rapidly after puberty, reaches its peak
during young-adulthood, and then declines with age and the demands of
parenthood.7
Yet such claims fall apart once submitted to proper scrutiny. In the
animal kingdom, only males use song to court females, but in human
society women are also excellent singers. Tying music too closely to sex
also seems at variance with the complex ways by which this art form
influences us.8 A man does not have to be gay to appreciate an all-male
choir, and a heterosexual woman can enjoy listening to a soprano. But
undoubtedly the biggest problem with the idea that our musical capacity
is analogous to animal courtship sounds is the fixed nature of the latter.
In contrast, a unique feature of music is its immense variety.
Another explanation for the centrality of music in human society is
that it plays a key role in coordinating activity. For instance, psychologist
Annett Schirmer of the University of Singapore has argued that rhythmic
sound ‘not only coordinates the behaviour of people in a group, it also
coordinates their thinking—the mental processes of individuals in the
group become synchronized.’9 This may explain how drums unite tribes
in ceremony, why armies used to march into battle accompanied by bugle
and drum, religious ceremonies are infused by song, and speech is punc-
tuated by rhythmic emphasis on particular syllables and words.
Schirmer’s claims are based on a study her team carried out in which
people were asked to study a series of images and then identify when an
image was shown upside down, while a drum tapped out a simple rhythm
in which the fourth beat was always skipped. Participants identified the
inverted image much more rapidly when it was shown at the same time as
the missed beat, rather than being out of sync with the beat.10 This sug-
gests that the brain’s ability to make decisions can be enhanced by an
external rhythm and heightened at precise points in synchrony with the
305
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
306
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
would spend on a given song. The study found a correlation between how
much people said they would spend and activity in a brain region called
the nucleus accumbens, which is involved in forming expectations.14
Salimpoor’s team also identified a link with another brain region, the
superior temporal gyrus. Studies have shown that the genres of music
that a person listens to over a lifetime have an impact on neural activity in
this brain region. This region alone does not predict whether a person
likes a given piece of music, but it is involved in storing templates from
what they have heard before. For instance, a person who has heard a lot of
jazz is more likely to appreciate a given piece of jazz music than someone
with less experience. Salimpoor believes this shows that ‘the brain kind of
works like a music recommendation system.’15
In another study, led by Daniel Abrams at Stanford University, partici-
pants listened to music by the late Baroque composer William Boyce,
chosen because he was unlikely to be familiar to the participants, while
their brains were imaged by fMRI.16 The study identified similar brain
activity patterns in the different participants. For Abrams, this shows that
‘despite our idiosyncrasies in listening, the brain experiences music in a
very consistent fashion across subjects.’17 Brain regions involved in move-
ment, attention, planning, and memory consistently showed activation
when participants listened to the music. As these regions are not directly
involved in auditory processing, Abrams believes this means that when
we experience music, many other things take place in the brain beyond
merely processing sound.18 These brain regions may help to hold particular
parts of a piece of music—such as the melody—in the mind while the
piece plays on. But it might also be the case that the study has identified a
neural basis for the capacity of music to unite different listeners.
As well as exploring evolutionary reasons for the centrality of music in
human society and the neural mechanisms that mediate this, it would be
a major omission in a book about the unique aspects of the human mind
to neglect the role that music plays in the creation of meaning. Previously,
I pointed to language as important not only for communication, but also
as something that allows us to make sense of the world, and even develop
307
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
radical new ideas about that world. In other words, it allows us to create a
unique sense of the meaning of life. Yet while language is clearly the most
important of the ‘cultural tools’ that Lev Vygotsky proposed as mediators
of human consciousness and which account for the latter’s uniquely
‘extended’ character, music can be viewed as another such tool. Crucially,
music possesses the ability to speak to us in a non-verbal fashion, primar-
ily by manipulating our emotions.
Recent research has identified links between music, emotions, and
social behaviour. Autistic individuals, for instance, tend to perceive less
emotion in music, while those with a condition called Williams Syndrome
show a heightened interest in music.19 People with this syndrome have
learning difficulties, but are very cheerful and associate easily with
strangers. There is evidence that they have enhanced activity in the
amygdala, which, recall, is a brain region particularly associated with the
emotions, and increased brain levels of oxytocin, which as we have seen
mediates social interactions.20 It is interesting then they are also more
likely to look for and play music, and have heightened emotional
responses to music, which tends to reinforce the idea of a link between
music, emotional responses, and sociability.
In addition to expressing emotion, there are other similarities to be
found when we compare the characteristics of music across cultures. For
example, music of any genre nearly always involves some form of com-
plex sound event, such as structured rhythms, or pitch organization,
overlying a regular beat.21 Through allowing people to create something
together via such a regular beat, musical sounds may have provided a
means by which people could share experiences, thereby fostering social
bonds. Similarly, music’s capacity to generate emotions that are felt by
everyone, yet remain specific to an individual because of their own par-
ticular life history and experiences, suggests that it may have arisen as a
way of bringing together individual and group feelings, particularly in
times of social uncertainty.22
Indeed, this particularly seems to be the case in hunter-gatherer soci
eties, the form of society that human beings have existed in for the vast
308
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
309
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
there are dedicated musical instruments, music can also be made with
whatever is available, for instance a hunting bow. In addition music and
dance are central to gatherings of separate hunter-gatherer groups, where
they help dissipate inter-group tensions and cement alliances.
In terms of the effect of music and dance in enhancing social bonds in
hunter-gatherer societies, of central importance is the effect of such activ-
ities in stimulating the secretion of neurochemicals such as endorphins
and oxytocin—which promote well- being and social interaction,
respectively—in the brain. In addition, echoing Vygotsky’s statement
28
mentioned earlier that play allows a child to act as if ‘a head taller than
himself’, the role-playing element in song and dance can be an important
way for individuals in hunter-gatherer societies to explore deeper themes
than their concrete, everyday experience.29 It is not a coincidence that
music and dance in such societies play a major role in spiritual matters.
Spirituality is a topic that I will explore in greater detail later, but music’s
alliance with spiritual experiences seems likely to have added a profound
value to human life in prehistory.
If such is the role of music and dance in the original form of human
society, how have things changed in the class societies that have charac-
terized humanity since the birth of civilization about five to six thousand
years ago? Here two important factors need to be considered: first, the
divisions that characterize class societies and, second, the cumulative
development of technology to an ever accelerating degree in such soci
eties. I would argue that these factors have affected the position of music
in society, and its effects on human conscious awareness, in two notable
ways. First, the development of new technologies has increased the pos-
sible means by which human beings can produce sounds in ways that
have at certain times in history led to profound transformations in both
the type of music available to humanity and the effects of these on the
human mind. Second, changes in the social relations of human society
have often had a major—although often far from direct—influence on
music styles and their social impact, and thereby can be argued to have
310
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
increased the diversity and depth of the ways that music is able to speak
about what it means to be human.
That music may act as a barometer of social aspirations, but also dis-
content, in society has long been recognized. In 380 bc, Plato remarked
that ‘a change to a new type of music is something to beware of . . . [f]or
the modes of music are never disturbed without unsettling of the most
fundamental political and social conventions’, while the rulers of ancient
China set up an office to supervise court music and monitor the musical
tastes of the masses, in case these showed indications of social unrest.30 In
twelfth-century Europe, church authorities placed strict limitations on
music, with artists only allowed to play chords and patterns judged to be
pleasing to the ear. Intervals between notes that went against this idea of
harmony were banned. One banned interval, the flat fifth, or tritone, was
even branded diabolus in musica—the devil in music.31 Yet despite attempts
to stifle it, the dissonance that the authorities tried to prohibit in music
became a growing feature of society in the fourteenth century, a period
characterized by a wave of peasants’ revolts. To try to address the issues
that caused the revolts, concessions were made by the authorities—some
material, others cultural. One such concession was that the church
removed the ban on certain musical intervals.32
The progressiveness of such a move is shown by the diversity of human
emotions and situations that even a single musical interval like the flat
fifth can convey. For on the one hand, as its nickname suggests, this
‘devil’s tritone’ can create a sense of foreboding, and so has been used to
create a tense and sinister atmosphere by artists ranging from Richard
Wagner, who employed it in his opera Götterdämmerung, to the heavy rock
group Black Sabbath, who used it in their song of the same name.33
However, to only associate the tritone with the sinister underestimates its
potential range, since it also features in Jimi Hendrix’s song ‘Purple Haze’,
and in two love songs—‘The Girl from Ipanema’ by João Gilberto and
Stan Getz, and ‘Maria’ from the musical West Side Story.34 For Anthony
Pryer of Goldsmith’s College in London, these varied uses of the same
311
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
interval reflect the fact that it ‘is something that yearns to be resolved.
A very good example would be the opening of . . . Maria. It wants to resolve
into the next note. It is a special kind of tension.’35 Given the ultimate tragedy
of West Side Story, its use in this case may even have been a deliberate—
or unconscious—anticipation of future sadness to come. Such diversity
in the range of what a single musical interval can convey—often in
potentially unconscious ways in a listener—illustrates how music can
sometimes transcend what it is possible to convey through words alone.
Previously I mentioned how the French revolution that began in 1789
had a huge impact on human consciousness, with its slogan liberté, égalité,
fraternité. Yet a study of how this event influenced music demonstrates that
it is not necessarily revolutions per se, but rather the contradictions
within such moments of dramatic historical change that can prove most
productive in stimulating great artistic endeavours. A sign of this is that
far from being generated at the centre of revolutionary change in Paris,
the most profound music at this time was being composed in Vienna, a
relative backwater, by one Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart.
In Austria there was no decisive break with the old order as happened in
France in 1789.36 Although social progress was achieved during Emperor
Joseph II’s rule in the 1780s it came from above in an often half-hearted
manner. Yet the tensions produced by this contradictory situation were
the backdrop for one of the most fertile eras in the history of music, for
Mozart is widely considered by many as the greatest composer of all time.37
Unlike Johann Sebastian Bach, who worked for the church, or Joseph
Haydn, the servant of a local aristocrat, Mozart decided to attempt a
career as an independent musician, something almost unheard of at the
time. What helped Mozart in this pursuit of artistic independence was
that Joseph II initiated reforms aimed at forestalling a revolution like the
one in France.38 These included an attack on church privileges, legal
reforms, and less censorship in the arts. It was this more liberal environment
that allowed Mozart to live independently and create his masterpieces.39
Yet the precarious nature of his existence probably also helped hasten his
death, and burial in a pauper’s grave, at the age of just 35.
312
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
Both the revolutionary spirit of the age and its contradictions influenced
Mozart’s work. His opera The Marriage of Figaro was based on Beaumarchais’
play of a servant outwitting his master, which was banned in Austria
because of its perceived subversive character.40 Interestingly, for Figaro’s
aria of revenge on his master, the Count, Mozart set his words to a min-
uet—a dance form associated with the elegance of the aristocratic ball-
room.41 In this case form and content are clearly opposed, of note given
that Vygotsky believed a central feature of great art is that it always con-
tains an ‘intimate conflict’ between content and form.42 Here this conflict
adds a powerful element of irony to the situation.
Yet perhaps the greatest achievement of Mozart’s operas is the way that
music is used to portray the many psychological contradictions within
human character, behaviour, and intentions. Ever since Don Giovanni was
first performed there has been a debate about whether the central charac-
ter is a villain, a hero, or both of these things at once.43 And while The
Magic Flute is a thinly veiled celebration of the ideals of freemasonry—in
Mozart’s era a progressive movement of which the composer himself was
a prominent member—we are continually wrong-footed in our estima-
tions of the character, motives, and trustworthiness of the two ruling pro-
tagonists, the Queen of the Night and Sarastro. As critic Luke Howard has
noted, The Magic Flute ‘invites the viewer to look past first appearances,
and examine the premises and assumptions on which those appearances
are based.’44 Perhaps, writing in 1791 with the two-year old French revolu-
tion already throwing up complex practical and moral dilemmas, Mozart
was well aware that not everything is reducible to polar opposites.
Moving to a different century, previously I described how scientists are
using brain imaging to study the neural mechanisms underlying the cre
ativity of jazz musicians during an act of improvisation.45 But we should
also consider the social and psychological influences that affected the
development of jazz. The political activist Malcolm X linked together jazz
improvisation and the civil rights movement at a rally in Harlem in 1964
with his claim that ‘when a black musician picks up his horn and starts
blowing, he improvises, he creates, it comes from within. It’s his soul.
313
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
Jazz is the only area in America where the black man is free to create.’46 In
fact, the first truly improvisational jazz, bebop, was heavily influenced
by the radicalizing influence that fighting in World War II had upon a
generation of young black men.47 The war had supposedly been fought
against Nazi racism, yet returning black soldiers found themselves con-
fronted by the same institutionalized racism and public prejudice as
before. This had an important impact on the form and content of jazz in
the post-war years.
In contrast to the big bands that characterized jazz in the pre-war years,
bebop was played by small groups in which the drummer and bassist
were on a musical par with the horn and piano soloists, producing a more
democratic structure in the band setup.48 This democratization was also
aided by the improvisational style, in which the music was guided by an
equal interaction of different musicians, rather than by a sole band leader.
Bebop was also played at a much faster tempo than swing. One reason for
this was a desire to be taken seriously. Many jazz musicians felt that
during the swing era, they were often reduced to providing ambient back-
ground music and forced to take a back seat to their audiences—which
were often predominantly white. In contrast, Billy Eckstine, who led a
bebop band in the 1940s and 1950s, noted that he deliberately rearranged
many popular songs to have fast tempos so that the audience could not
dance to them, forcing them to pay more attention to the musicians. Here
bebop tried to create an equality that wider society still denied to blacks.
Another important development in music since the birth of civiliza-
tion and class society, compared to that in prehistoric hunter-gatherer
societies, is that technological innovations have transformed music in an
increasingly rapid fashion over the centuries. And I would argue that this
has had an important impact on the range of ideas, feelings, and emo-
tions that music is able to convey, and therefore the influence that it can
have on human conscious awareness. One example of this is the way that
changes in musical technology in the late eighteenth and early nineteenth
centuries—particularly the transition from the harpsichord to the
314
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
piano—affected the range and power of first Mozart’s music, and then
Beethoven’s.
These two types of instruments differ in that despite being both key-
board instruments, a harpsichord’s sounds are produced when a series of
quills pluck the instrument’s strings, a mechanical process that allows for
only limited dynamic variations, while on the piano the strings are struck
by hammers, with the varying force exerted on the keyboard by the pian
ist providing superior dynamic contrast and responsiveness, allowing for
unbroken melodic lines and greater emotional expression.49 Because of
this, unlike its predecessor, the piano was able to transmit a far wider
spectrum of dynamics, musical colours, and emotions.
A sign of the rapidly growing importance of pianos in Mozart’s lifetime
is shown by the fact that in 1756, when he was born, pianos were still rare
in Germany, had barely arrived on Britain’s shores, and were basically
unknown in France, but by 1791, when he died, they were being widely
produced in all three countries.50 And this transition was important in
Mozart’s musical development, shown for instance in the piano sonatas
he composed that reflected the new instrument’s potential, leaping
exuberantly from loud to soft, high to low.
Ludwig van Beethoven, coming on to the world stage just after Mozart,
was even more able to realize the true potential of this rapidly developing
instrument. The innovations in piano design that continued after Mozart’s
death provided Beethoven, along of course with his own musical genius,
with the means to express his extraordinary creative impulses and the
capacity to further break from the confining musical conventions of the
eighteenth century.51
Of course while noting the influence of technological developments on
the development of musical styles may be seen as important from a socio
logical point of view, how does this relate to conscious awareness and
specifically the central thesis of this book, that human consciousness has
undergone a qualitative shift compared to that of an animal? We noted
previously that music is another cultural tool besides language that has
315
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
316
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
317
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
318
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
music a nd r h y t hm
In fact, such fears are surely not so different from those voiced by
Eminem—the pseudonym of the rapper Marshall Mathers—in his 2017
song ‘Walk on Water’. In the late 1990s, Eminem became famous for
expressing the hopes, fears, and frustrations of life in the poor white
‘trailer trash’ society in which he grew up in Detroit.66 And in 2017 the
fears and feelings of inadequacy clearly remained, as shown by Eminem’s
mention of his insecurity about whether his latest lyrics might tarnish the
‘legacy, love or respect of his fans.’67 The question is whether such lyrics
can have the same resonance now that Eminem is a multimillionaire.
Richey Edwards, guitarist and chief lyricist with Welsh rock band the
Manic Street Preachers, showed literally how passionately he felt about
the need to ‘keep it real’ when he carved ‘4 REAL’ into his arm with a knife
in response to a journalist who questioned the band’s integrity.68 In fact,
this was just one of several incidents of self-harm by Edwards, who also
suffered from anorexia. Demonstrating the potential of music to provide
insight into mental disorder, the Manics’ song ‘4st 7lb’, which appeared on
their album The Holy Bible, movingly and disturbingly took us inside the
head of an anorexic, although its lyrics were also considered problematic
because of the risk of glamorizing the condition.69
In fact, this double-edged portrayal of mental disorder would become
an epitaph for Richey Edwards himself. Soon after the completion of The
Holy Bible, his car was found abandoned near the Severn Bridge, which
separates Wales and England.70 With his history of neurosis and self-
abuse, an obvious conclusion was that he had committed suicide by
jumping into the river Severn. However, because Richey’s body was never
found and he had withdrawn a significant sum of money from his bank
account before his disappearance, for many years his family, the other
band members, and many fans have clung to the idea that he simply
decided to quit the limelight and start a new identity somewhere else.71
While an obvious way for those grieving to cling to the hope that Richey
is still alive, this belief may also have been influenced by the idea that such
an outcome was more fitting for this unique and talented individual than
a lonely death in the cold, murky waters of the Severn.
319
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 19
a rt a nd design
brought, they were astounded to see revealed in the flickering light, not gold
or silver, but a cultural treasure that would prove far more valuable.
As Marsal described it, the boys had discovered a ‘cavalcade of animals
larger than life painted on the walls and ceiling of the cave; each animal
seemed to be moving.’2 The chamber, now known as ‘The Hall of the
Bulls’, was just the start of a series of passages covered with amazing cave
paintings, which featured not only scenes of hunted animals, but also a
figure of a man with a bird’s head—perhaps a shaman who carried out
rituals in the cave. Later analysis would show that the paintings were
created over 15,000 years ago.
Today the paintings of Lascaux are just one of many examples of pre-
historic art discovered not only in caves, but also above ground, as in the
Côa valley in Portugal where exploration in the 1990s as part of a plan to
create a dam revealed hundreds of engravings in the overhanging rock
walls of the valley.3 Thankfully, the construction of the dam was stopped,
and the area made a UNESCO protected site.4 Most recently, cave paintings
of animals found on the island of Sulawesi, in Indonesia, were estimated
to be at least 35,400 years old, making this the oldest-known example of
figurative art anywhere in the world.5
Quite what motivated our ancestors to descend into the bowels of the
earth or venture into isolated valleys to adorn rock walls with such intri-
cate drawings remains open to interpretation. Some believe the art was
important for its content, while others assert that its primary significance
was in the ritual act of producing it, for since hunting was critical to early
humans’ survival, such art may have been part of a magic ritual whose
aim was to influence the success of the hunt, or increase the fertility of
herds in the wild. Images that seem to have been clawed or gouged with
spears favour the former idea, while a painting of a pregnant-looking
horse in the Lascaux cave supports the latter.6 What is clear is that the
birth of figurative art marked a significant shift in our species’ ability to
represent the world around us, and, presumably, a step forward in our
capacity for conscious awareness.
321
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
322
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
As he puts it, ‘much as philosophers like to believe that our brains contain
a specialized system for the appreciation of artworks, research suggests that
our brain’s responses to a piece of cake and a piece of music are in fact quite
similar.’9 This finding fits with other studies mentioned in this book that
failed to find specific brain regions dedicated to language, mu sic
al
appreciation, or other ‘higher’ human abilities. However, this does not mean
that neuroscience cannot provide important insights about the ways in
which the human brain perceives artworks and their impact in our minds.
One recent study, led by David Freedberg of Columbia University in
New York, tried to understand more about the neural mechanisms that
might underlie an aesthetic response to a work of art. Freedberg’s team
used an imaging method called transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)
to monitor the brains of participants as they viewed a detail from
Michelangelo’s Expulsion from Paradise, a fresco panel on the ceiling of the
Sistine Chapel at the Vatican. The detail was of a fallen-from-grace Adam
warding off a sword-wielding angel with his eyes averted from the
blade and his wrist bent back defensively. The study found that viewing
this detail stimulated regions in the brain’s primary motor cortex that
controlled the observer’s own wrist movements.10 Such a feeling could
form an important part of the aesthetic response that makes viewers of
the fresco feel they are right there with Adam fending off the angel’s
blows. Freedberg believes this may also explain why some viewers of
Edgar Degas’s paintings of ballerinas have reported that they experi-
ence the sensation of dancing. Given what we learned previously about
mirror neurons, which are stimulated when we observe another per-
son carrying out an action and have been proposed to play a role in
allowing us to understand the intentions and emotions behind such an
action, further research should explore whether these neurons play a
role in such aesthetic responses.
Rather than investigating the viewers of artworks, another imaging
study looked for differences in the brains of artists compared to non-
artistic individuals. The study, by Rebecca Chamberlain and colleagues at
the Catholic University in Leuven, Belgium, used an approach called
323
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
324
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
just a stick and some pigment, whoever created what has been referred to
as the ‘Sistine Chapel of prehistoric art’15 nevertheless managed to convey,
in a few strokes, not only the animals’ shape, but also their movements, in
such a way that this made a deeply memorable impression on some
teenage boys 15,000 years after the paintings’ creation.
While this capacity of art to speak to human beings across the gulf of
time is one of its key features, it would be mistaken not to recognize that
art has also undergone change and development over the centuries, and
that what an artwork says to future generations may be radically different
from what its original creator intended. Yet whether there can be any
sense of ‘progression’ in the visual arts remains controversial. At the same
time, we have other issues to consider, such as whether artistic creativity
is primarily a product of the rational mind, or whether it flows more from
unconscious impulses, and even from thoughts that in other circum-
stances might be considered odd or even insane. Finally, I will consider
how visual art is being affected by newer technological developments like
cinema and the Internet.
On the subject of the development of artforms over time, which now
includes moving images, clearly on one level great works of art, whether
from prehistory or any subsequent period following the birth of civiliza-
tion, can be considered equally worthy of our respect. In particular, we
can compare an image such as one of the bovine species depicted in the
Lascaux cave paintings mentioned earlier with the cows rendered in beau-
tiful detail by sixteenth-century Dutch painter Aelbert Cuyp, or Pablo
Picasso’s much more abstract images of bulls.16 Such a comparison shows
that all of these artistic creations have been drawn with tremendous skill,
and as such it would be hard to justify the claim that any of the images are
in any way superior to the others. And indeed a recent study has shown
that the Lascaux paintings of animals are more accurate in their depiction
of how such animals walk, based on photographic evidence of this process,
than similar animal images by later artists.17
Yet on another level I would argue that at certain periods of history,
there have been important steps forward in the capacity of visual art to
325
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
say novel things about the state of the world and the human condition.
This has particularly been the case during periods of major social
upheaval, but may also reflect widening global horizons as humanity has
developed, as shown by the work of the fifteenth-century Dutch artist
Hieronymus Bosch. When I first saw The Garden of Earthly Delights, by
Bosch, in the Museo del Prado in Madrid, my initial feeling was how
modern the painting seemed, despite being half a millennium old, and
that Bosch might have been high on some psychedelic drug when he
painted it. With its lurid colours and hallucinatory images—which
include a bird-headed creature eating a naked man, a pig dressed as a nun,
and a hollowed-out giant with trees for limbs and an inn inside his pale
egg-like torso—this tryptich painting seemed closer to the twentieth-
century surrealist painter Salvador Dali than to other late medieval art.18
Yet Bosch was very much a product of his society, while at the same time
being highly attuned to radical changes in that society.
Bosch lived in the Netherlands during the transition between the late
medieval period and the Renaissance. Trade and urbanization were
increasing, weakening the feudal system, and increasing the demand for
art.19 At the same time the gap between rich and poor was expanding in
the cities. In the late fifteenth century, perceived misbehaviour by reli-
gious leaders led some ordinary people and clergy to set up a religious
reform movement known as Modern Devotion. Their aim was to change
the relationship between church and society, and they created small com-
munities in which participants shared property. There is evidence that
Bosch or his sponsors were influenced by their ideas, which, according to
art historian Jos Koldeweij, emphasized that ‘all human beings have to
think, themselves, about good and evil . . . [and] to read the Bible in our
own language, [and] take the message out of it for our own lives.’20
The influence of this way of thinking is shown particularly clearly in
another of Bosch’s tryptichs, The Haywain. In this, he shows people of all
classes following a hay wagon.21 Some are fighting each other in order to
seize some hay. Tellingly, members of the clergy are also stuffing their bags.
Meanwhile, demons are pulling the wagon towards hell. The message is
326
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
327
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
328
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
329
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
at this time also influenced the way Michelangelo carved David. For unlike
his predecessors Donatello and Verocchio, who sculpted David after the
defeat of Goliath with the giant’s head at his feet, Michelangelo portrayed
him before the battle, thereby making this a work that looks to the future,
not the past. What is more, Michelangelo’s David is not in fact in ‘perfect’
proportion; the head and hands, especially the right hand holding the
stone, are slightly too large, which has the effect of making David a brain
as well as a body, and a doer and maker as well as an object of beauty.29
Therefore, the statue may be seen to celebrate not one specific ‘man’, but
a vision of the ideal that humanity has the potential to become in a new,
republican society.
Today we live in the era of contemporary art, which began with
modernism. In fact the modernist movement—which is often defined as
occurring between the 1890s and the mid-twentieth century—included
not only visual art, but also architecture, poetry, novels, theatre, and
cinema.30 While general definitions are difficult, modernist work tends to
be formally experimental and highly self-conscious—think of Pablo
Picasso’s cubist paintings or the ‘stream of consciousness’ of James Joyce’s
novels. A key characteristic of modernism is an emphasis on dislocation
and fragmentation. This characteristic can be seen to reflect the social
events that took place between 1900 and 1930—a time of great upheaval
in which dreams of peace and prosperity were shattered by war, slump,
revolution, and reaction.31 The accelerating development of technology
and penetration of mass production methods into every sphere of life
also added to a deep sense of uncertainty. In the visual arts, a crucial dis-
tinction between modern art and ‘traditional’ art is that the latter tends
to be more representational and naturalistic, while modern art either
wilfully distorts physical appearances or, in abstract art, abandons them
altogether. And just as with music, this has been accompanied by the fact
that a successful visual artist in modern times is no longer viewed as a
paid servant of relatively lowly stature, but rather an ‘artist’ in the modern
free sense, namely someone that is most interesting when provocative
and pushing at boundaries.
330
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
A contender for the first, truly ‘modern’ work of art is Les Demoiselles
d’Avignon, painted by Picasso between 1906 and 1907.32 In terms of its
impact on future art, Les Demoiselles can be viewed as having opened the
doorway, first to cubism and subsequently to futurism, synthetic cubism,
expressionism, vorticism, abstraction, suprematism, Dadaism, and more
besides.33 Within just ten years artists were producing works, such as
Malevich’s Black Square on White and Duchamp’s ‘found objects’, which
would not previously have been regarded as art at all, but which have
subsequently achieved classic and iconic status. So what was it about Les
Demoiselles that made it such a catalyst? And what might have been going
on inside Picasso’s brain during the production of such a radical vision?
We mentioned earlier that Picasso claimed that his genius as an artist
was based upon an ability to view the world as would a child. And cer-
tainly some of his works have an astonishing childlike simplicity. Take for
instance the ‘bull’s head’ that Picasso created much later in his career in
1942, from a discarded bicycle seat and handlebars.34 And certainly to
someone used to the art of the great masters of the Renaissance, Les
Demoiselles might also appear at first sight childishly crude in its compos
ition and harsh in its treatment of the subject. Yet nothing could be further
from the truth, and it is worth considering some of the complex neuro-
logical and psychological factors that might have been involved in the
creation of this iconic work.
One factor is what might be called Picasso’s ‘intensity of vision’. As
neuropsychologist Christine Temple has pointed out, Picasso had a
fantastic visual memory, but he also spent many hours staring at an object
that he wanted to paint, whether this was a famous artwork of the past that
he was reinterpreting or a living model.35 And according to Temple, con-
sequentially Picasso tended to ‘overlearn’ such objects. As she explained
this term, ‘When you overlearn something, you don’t just look at it,
think about it and try to learn from or remember it. You look at it (or
listen to it) again and again until it becomes completely familiar. Overlearning
enables memory to work much better and in much greater detail.’36 Now
all of this has potential relevance to what we said earlier about the mechan
331
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
332
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
333
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
represented for him: ‘whore and deity, decadent and savage, tempting and
repelling, awesome and obscene, looming and crouching, masked and
naked, threatening and powerless . . . no other work reveals more of the
rock foundation of sexist anti-humanism or goes further and deeper to
justify and celebrate the domination of woman by man.’41
The possibility of multiple interpretations of modern art is enhanced by
its very abstractness. This reached a peak in the work of Kazimir Malevich.
Born in Kiev in 1879, Malevich developed a style—‘suprematism’—which
took abstraction to an ultimate extreme.42 The idea behind this was that
by sticking to simple geometric shapes and a limited range of colours the
artist could focus on the painting itself and not be distracted by represent-
ing a scene, or landscape or a person. Malevich’s Black Square, exhibited in
Kiev in 1915, is exactly that: a painted black square on a white back-
ground.43 The painting might be viewed as the height of formalistic, non-
representational objectivity, yet Malevich himself saw it as liberated
self-expression. In his 1919 essay ‘Non-Objective Art and Suprematism’ he
concluded that ‘I have overcome the lining of the coloured sky, torn it
down and into the bag thus formed, put colour, tying it up with a knot.
Swim in the white free abyss, infinity is before you.’44 So what might be
the basis for such an ambitious claim for this abstract work?
In seeking to answer this question, it is worth looking again at what we
have learned about how visual perception works, and also the importance
of meaning for our species. Recall that Hubel and Wiesel’s experiments
first showed that a key way by which the visual cortex in our brains allows
us to make sense of the world is by breaking it down into lines and edges.
Because of this, the neurobiologist Semir Zeki has argued that Malevich’s
Black Square—and all the other coloured squares and rectangles that came
after it in abstract art—have a particular resonance because they ‘stimu-
late cells in the visual cortex, and the properties of these cells are, to an
extent, the pre-existing “idea” within us.’45 So there is a potential neuro-
logical basis to the power of such artforms to attract our attention. Yet it
is impossible to fully understand the power of a work of art like Black Square
without also recognizing its symbolic content. In particular, Malevich’s
334
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
decision to hang the painting high on the wall against the corner of the
room where it was first exhibited might mean nothing to a non-Russian
viewer today, but this was the same sacred spot that a Russian Orthodox
icon of a saint would sit in a traditional Russian home.46 And not only
would this have symbolic value in its own right, but the fact that it was
happening in 1915, against the backdrop of the slaughter of the First World
War, and with continuing unrest following the failed Russian Revolution
of 1905, all contributed to the painting’s power. This shows that shared
symbolic meaning is vitally important both for ordinary life and for art
appreciation.
More generally, modernist art may be seen as a celebration of the new
technologies made possible by capitalism and at the same time a critique
of the alienation characteristic of this form of society. The challenges of
representing new experiences of speed well beyond customary horse
power, of rendering multiple other novel ways of viewing the world on an
immobile flat canvas or page—absorbing the insights from science—and
the advent of photography and cinema focused young artists’ minds and
provided their motifs.47 Yet despite their appreciation for new technolo-
gies, many modernist artists remained critical of the capitalist system
itself. For cultural critic Perry Anderson, this double-edged character of
modernism reflected the fact that it ‘flowered in the space between a still
usable classical past, a still indeterminate technical present, and a still
unpredictable political future.’48
The unpredictability of such a future, and the ambiguity at the heart of
modernism, can help to explain the very different political currents with
which modernism became associated in the early twentieth century. In
Italy under Mussolini, the modernist art movement celebrated the
inhuman power of technology and the destructiveness of war.49 In con-
trast in Russia, most modernist painters, composers, and poets supported
the 1917 revolution, which seemed to offer untold freedoms, a chance to
use bold new forms—cubism, abstraction, street art, film, jazz, satire,
fantasy—and share in the making of a new type of society.50 Not only
Malevich, but also the mystical Wassily Kandinsky and Marc Chagall and
335
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
336
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
time, and information. For Eisenstein, the power of the edit was that film
shots could be cut together to create brand new meaning that was not
present in the original shots.55 The practical use of this approach was first
demonstrated in the film Battleship Potemkin. This tells the true story of the
mutiny of that vessel’s crew during the 1905 Russian Revolution and the
subsequent brutal response of the Tsarist regime. A highlight of that film
is the montage sequence showing the massacre of civilians on the steps of
Odessa, in which editing created a dynamic, tense, violent, and deeply
allegorical set piece.56 The impact of Eisenstein’s ideas can be seen today
in the rapid cutting of modern action sequences or the rhythmic edits of
music videos. A recent example of the power of this approach is a montage
sequence in Bong Joon-ho’s award-winning 2019 film Parasite, which both
rapidly and skilfully develops a key moment in the storyline, and also
surreptitiously foreshadows coming events and references previous ones
within itself.57
In developing his use of montage, Eisenstein was influenced by Vygotsky’s
belief that human consciousness differs from that of an animal in that
only the former is mediated by cultural tools, a primary one being lan-
guage, and that this is also true of our emotional responses. But while this
was a general influence, a more specific one was the substantial amount
of work Vygotsky devoted to the psychology of different artforms.
Vygotsky argued that ‘art is the social technique of feelings’, and Eisenstein
developed this idea in a practical direction by using film-editing tech-
niques to organize and control viewers’ responses in order to optimally
produce aesthetic effects in his audience.58 He proposed that a piece of
art’s effectiveness works simultaneously at the level of the structural
organization of that artform, and that of its emotional and broader intel-
lectual and psychological effects on the viewer.
Eisenstein believed that a central issue for any work of art is the para-
doxical coexistence of opposing dimensions in the viewer: logical and
sensuous, cognitive and emotional, rational and irrational, conscious
and unconscious. He argued that ‘while the conventional film directs
the emotions, this suggests an opportunity to encourage and direct the
337
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
338
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 22/01/21, SPi
a rt a nd design
339
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 20
did strike me even at that age was the way that a great writer can use
language to manipulate the thoughts and emotions of the reader, in this
case drawing us into the mind of, and even making us empathize with, a
twisted individual.4 I also developed a love for the novels of William
Golding, particularly his Pincher Martin, ostensibly about a shipwrecked
sailor’s struggle for survival on a rock in the Atlantic, but actually a pro-
found discussion about the human condition, what it means to live but
also to die, and how even a highly unlikeable human being may possess
qualities with which we can empathize, and wish for him to achieve some
kind of redemption.5
How much can novels tell us about the various mechanisms under
lying human consciousness? Some might say, very little, and argue that
only scientific study can uncover such mechanisms. I do not agree with
this viewpoint; my reasoning is that because language plays such a key
role in shaping human consciousness, the fictional explorations of the
human condition that we find in novelistic literature can greatly add to
our scientific understanding by concretizing that condition in its diverse
forms. And it seems that I am not alone in this belief, for as influential a
thinker as Noam Chomsky has said that, ‘It is quite possible . . . that we will
always learn more about human life and personality from novels than
from scientific psychology.’6 Personally I believe these two approaches to
be equally valuable, each providing insights that the other cannot, in
ways I will explore in more detail in the rest of this chapter. And I will
also explore a related question: how much do novels draw on new
insights about the nature of consciousness, so increasing their ability to
inform us about the human condition, and its relationship to changing
forms of society?
In pondering such questions, a feature of novels worth noting is how
they differ from a form of artistic expression such as a play which relies
not only on the written text of the playwright, but also on how this is
interpreted by the play’s director—with the possibility of leaving out
or altering parts of the text—the actors’ own interpretations, and the
interactions between actors, and with the audience. Unfortunately a
341
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
consideration of staged drama, or for that matter that other key area of
literature, poetry, is outside the scope of this book. But my point here is
that unlike staged drama, a novel is a fixed piece of text with a readership
that has no necessary contact with the author, or with other readers. And
given what I have said previously about viewing human consciousness as
a kind of dialogue, this might suggest that novels are less dialogic, and
therefore less representative of consciousness, than plays.
Yet such a view neglects two important features of great novels. One is
the fact that as novelist David Lodge has noted, ‘the silence and privacy
of the reading experience afforded by books mimick the silent privacy of
individual consciousness.’7 And given that we have seen that such a con-
sciousness is dialogic, this gives ample capacity for a dialogue to form
between the reader and the words that they are reading on a page or
screen. Interestingly, both the neuroscientist Antonio Damasio and the
literary critic Ian Watt have argued that the very idea of an individual con-
sciousness is a recent concept, dating back only a few hundred years, and
linked this to the rise of the novel as a form of mass entertainment. So
according to Watt, the idea of society being composed of a ‘developing
but unplanned aggregate of particular individuals having particular
experiences at particular times and in particular places’ echoes the fact
that unlike earlier narrative fiction, which tended to recycle familiar stories,
‘novelists were the first storytellers to pretend that their stories had never
been told before.’8
A second important feature of great novels is the capacity for
characters—and interactions between them—to take on a life of their
own that can go way beyond the original intentions of the novelist. This
was expressed by the novelist Somerset Maugham when he wrote that ‘a
character in a writer’s head, unwritten, remains a possession; his thoughts
recur to it constantly, and while his imagination gradually enriches it, he
enjoys the singular pleasure of feeling that there, in his mind, someone is
living a varied and tremulous life, obedient to his fancy and yet in a queer,
wilful way independent of him.’9 Of course, it is impossible for an invented
character to literally have a life and thoughts of their own, independent
342
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
Multivoiced Dialogical
343
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
344
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
of the textual world, to make way for a dynamic in which characters have
more capacity to exert their own forces.14 Given that such characters are
fictional, this raises the question of what drives them.
One possibility is that important social issues of the time—for instance,
as heard in conversation, debate, and argument by the author of the
novel—inform the dialogue that is, after all, still placed in the mouths of
different characters by the author. From this perspective it is surely no
coincidence that Dostoevsky’s novels have been described as teeming
with ‘thwarted characters on the verge of exploding—Raskolnikov in
Crime and Punishment, Ivan Karamazov, the radicals of The Possessed’15—
given that when he was writing, basic democratic rights and freedom
of expression were suppressed by the Tsarist state. And cultural critic
Marshall Berman has argued that Dostoevsky was driven to produce
what are arguably the first truly modernist works of literature because of
the stark contradiction in late-nineteenth-century St Petersburg between
the façade of modernity as expressed in the city’s new architecture and
fashions, which offered the prospect of an exciting freedom from the
shackles of the past, and the actual reality of an autocratic society still
deeply moored in the feudal era.16 However, it would be mistaken to
assume that there is any simple, direct link between social circumstances
and the dynamic of a good novel; indeed, it seems fair to say that if there is
such an obvious link, the novel is likely to be the worse for it, becoming
crude social commentary or thinly veiled propaganda rather than great art.
Instead, I believe that the best novels have a complexity and ambiguity
of meaning—and this might be said to be increasingly a feature of litera-
ture in the modern era—that itself reflects the many contradictions in
society and the individual psyche within that society. Importantly, this
means there can be multiple readings of great novels, with different
readers interpreting them in various ways, some of which may be distinct
from—or even go way beyond—what the original author might have
intended. And in what follows we will examine several novels that have
multiple interpretations and which I also believe illuminate and enhance
our understanding of different aspects of consciousness.
345
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
346
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
347
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
revenge for past ills, but also in the abusive and exploitative manner with
which he treats Isabella Linton and Hareton Earnshaw.24 Heathcliff may
well express the social tensions of his time, but he surely cannot be
reduced to them, and this is surely part of his fascination.
A quite different reading of Wuthering Heights, relevant to what I said
previously about the potential link between creativity and mental dis-
tress, sees evidence of neurosis in the structure and content of the novel.
Here it is worth noting Freud’s view that works of literature represent the
external expression of the author’s unconscious mind and a key motiv
ation for writing a novel is to satisfy repressed wishes that might have
developed during the writer’s infancy and were instantly suppressed and
dumped in the unconscious. Notably, Freud believed that the creative
writer, like a child at play, creates a world of fantasy that is the fulfilment
of unconscious wishes expressed in a disguised form.
Freud surely overstates his case in trying to explain everything according
to his theory of the repressed unconscious, yet it is also true that Emily
Brontë had many reasons to create a fantasy world as a buffer against real-
ity. After his wife died aged only 38, Patrick Brontë sent his girls to the Clergy
Daughters’ School in Cowan Bridge in the Yorkshire Dales, which ‘was run
on spartan lines, designed to encourage resignation and humility’.25 The
terrible conditions at the school—vividly portrayed in Charlotte Brontë’s
Jane Eyre—almost certainly contributed to the deaths of Emily’s sisters
Elizabeth and Maria, from tuberculosis in 1825.26 Later, in 1848, Emily’s only
brother, Branwell, also died from the disease, in his case as a consequence of
his body being weakened by his addiction to alcohol and opiates.27
That Emily Brontë’s fiction may have been driven by neurosis is based
upon the claim that her writing became a compulsive ritual that allowed
her to offset her anxiety about the misfortunes of her family. In support of
this claim, critic Moussa Pourya Asl has pointed to a repetition in
Wuthering Heights involving violent men, a cycle of names, a system of
masters and servants, and reincarnation of character types.28 Orphanhood
and abandonment also characterize the novel and may reflect the losses
Emily experienced in her life. Certainly, many of the characters—including
348
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
349
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
350
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
she was pregnant with Quint’s child. Their phantom visitations with the
children hint at Satanism and possible sexual abuse.
The governess becomes convinced that her primary mission is to save
the souls of the children; yet in trying to shield them from the ghosts, she
ends up traumatizing Flora and possibly contributing to the death of
Miles from a heart attack. The novel has perplexed readers and critics ever
since it was first published, with the key unsolved question being whether
the ghosts are real or merely delusions of the governess’s mind.36 In fact,
literary critic Brad Leithauser has argued that ‘all such attempts to “solve”
the book, however admiringly tendered, unwittingly work toward its
diminution; its profoundest pleasure lies in the beautifully fussed over
way in which James refuses to come down on either side . . . the book
becomes a modest monument to the bold pursuit of ambiguity.’37
However, this does not mean that we cannot learn a lot both about the
novel’s underlying power and about the workings of the human mind, by
exploring different interpretations of the story, for the ambiguity of the
story itself reflects some of the ambiguous nature of thought processes.
From the point of view of seeing the unconscious as a crucial part of
human consciousness, the idea that the ghosts are products of the govern-
ess’s deluded mind is both plausible and potentially highly suggestive about
the workings of that unconscious. For instance, one interpretation of the
novel views the governess as driven by both sexual and economic motives,
being attracted sexually to the children’s aloof, absent guardian uncle on
their initial meeting and entertaining thoughts that if he were to reciprocate
such feelings, she might end up as mistress of a grand estate.38 However,
because such feelings clash with her strict religious upbringing, which has
taught her that both sexual attraction and coveting material wealth are sin-
ful, she represses such feelings, instead projecting them on to the ‘ghosts’
conjured up by her unconscious. This possibility is suggested by scenes
such as one in which the governess accidentally sinks down on a staircase
in the same spot where she had seen Miss Jessel’s ghost earlier, and
then in the schoolroom, where both women seem to have a certain right to
be present; indeed, the governess even feels like the intruder at one point.
351
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
352
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
353
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
clear that the whole battle to survive on the island was merely an illusion
of his dying moments. In fact, clues are scattered throughout the course
of the novel that all was not as it seemed. For instance, we learn that the
‘island’ bears an uncanny resemblance to a tooth that Martin once had
extracted.48 Meanwhile, increasingly odd events begin to occur as the
novel progresses that make us begin to question Martin’s sanity and
therefore the reality that he seems to be experiencing.
Pincher Martin is interesting in several respects in terms of the ideas
about human consciousness that we have been considering in this book.
One is the difference between sanity and madness.49 We expect a sane
person to have a more coherent consciousness than someone who is
mentally disturbed, but the closer that Martin gets to realizing his true
predicament, the more confused become not only his thoughts, but also
the whole ‘reality’ of the island. We realize that the carefully constructed
battle to survive that we have been experiencing with Martin is really just
his conscious mind refusing to accept what is actually happening—that
he is drowning.50 And it is the unconscious that gradually begins to make
itself known, with increasing force, as a way of conveying the reality of
the situation.
Another important aspect of Pincher Martin is that it powerfully illus-
trates how great literature can allow us to identify with the thoughts and
desires of another human being, even one who should be highly unlike
able. In creating Christopher Martin, Golding deliberately went out of his
way to create a deeply objectionable person.51 In flashbacks that occur
with increasing frequency as the novel progresses, we learn that Martin
was selfish, greedy, and a potential rapist and murderer.52 Yet it is very dif-
ficult for the reader not to feel deeply caught up in his struggle to survive,
and to suffer as he suffers, as we share in his plight.53 One reason for this
is that not only does the novel manipulate our sense of empathy for a fel-
low human being, but also Martin’s attempts on the rock to fashion a
shelter for himself, then acquire food and drink, and finally to try and
order the world around him mimic how human beings have distinguished
themselves from animals. In particular, the way that Martin seeks to make
354
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
355
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
named Sophie appears to have once been Ryder’s girlfriend or wife; her
son, Boris, is his child or stepson, and the elderly porter at the hotel turns
out to be Sophie’s father; however, we also learn that he has not spoken to
his daughter in years. At first glance, it seems counterintuitive that Ryder
might be in such a predicament. He is an accomplished artist, renowned
concert pianist, and respected public figure. Yet there is an increasing feel-
ing that his life has come to nothing—at least in terms of personal rela-
tions. This sense of lack of achievement is enhanced by Ryder bizarrely
bumping into a succession of characters from his youth in England—per-
haps echoing the idea developed previously in this book that an individ-
ual consciousness is heavily shaped by the interactions we have with our
peers as well as parents and teachers—who involve him in more distrac-
tions through their demands on his time, and rarely seem in awe of what
he has achieved so far professionally.
In this sense, the novel reads as a catalogue of wasted opportunity, a
failure to seize the moment, an inability to communicate, inconclusive
wondering, and general time-wasting. Crucially, Ryder seems to continually
miss his chance to connect as a human being. This becomes a book, as one
critic has put it, about the ‘destructive power of love without empathy’.60
Ryder always falls short in comprehending the minds of those around
him. He forever misinterprets, misunderstands, and misdirects. And so it
is that Sophie, the mother of his child, says to him in the end, ‘Leave us,
you were always on the outside of our love.’61
In fact, it is not just Ryder himself, but everyone he encounters, who
seems to be suffering from an inability to connect or communicate with
a loved one.62 Brodsky, a local drunk rehabilitated and cast in the role of
resident artistic genius, has been estranged for years from the woman he
loves. And the hotel manager, Hoffman, appears to be seriously out of
touch with the feelings of his wife, and also with their son, Stephan, a tal-
ented young pianist.
Ishiguro himself has claimed that in writing The Unconsoled, he wanted
to ‘present a biography of a person, but instead of using memory or flash-
backs, you have him wandering about in this dream world where he
356
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
bumps into earlier, or later, versions of himself. They’re not literally so;
they are to some extent other people.’63 In line with what I have said
previously about the fluidity of meaning within the inner dialogue that
constitutes an individual person’s thoughts, Ishiguro believes that in
dreams an individual consciousness can ‘bend and twist the whole world
into some big expression of his feelings and emotions.’ This also fits with
another idea put forward in this book: that individual consciousness is a
boundary phenomenon, uniting both our inner thoughts and the influ-
ence of wider society.
By presenting the story in this way, Ishiguro is able to ask profound
questions about our relationships with others, our responsibilities and
obligations, and how we can progress through life without facing up to
our real thoughts and desires. To some extent, for Ryder, any realizations
he makes about the negative features of his life and interactions with
others, and how he might overcome these in future, remain only partial.
For displacement in dreams may be precisely there to shield us from
unmediated confrontation with painful truth.64 Yet in other respects,
there is definitely a sense of resolution and moving forward in this novel.
It is true that the concert never happens, but we do get a firm conclusion
about whether Ryder will make his family relations work. We also find
out what has been happening in hotel-owner Hoffman’s marriage, and a
way into a fulfilling future for his son, while even Brodsky’s troubled life
hastens towards a conclusion. It is impressive, in fact, how many loose
ends are tied up in the concluding pages. Even the series of bizarre
encounters with figures from Ryder’s past lead to a certain sense of clos
ure in finally dealing with problematic issues from his youth. Because of
this, the novel ends on a surprisingly uplifting note: that although life and
relationships may often seem difficult and seemingly intractable, through
perseverance and a willingness to tackle such difficulties and face up to
our thoughts and desires—but also our responsibilities and obligations to
those around us—we all have the possibility to progress in life and per-
haps find real happiness in the process.
357
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 21
P erhaps more than many, I think I can say that I have been fairly
consistent in my chosen career path. At the age of fourteen, I decided
I wanted to be a biochemist—or more specifically a French biochemist, as
I had just been on my first French exchange trip, and fallen in love with the
country and its culture, food, wine, and general ambience. My goal at that
time was therefore to end up working in France, ideally in a renowned
scientific institution such as the Pasteur Institute in Paris. Reality con-
spires against our childhood hopes and desires in various ways, and
I never made that step of moving abroad. But I did achieve my dream of
becoming a biochemist, albeit in Oxford rather than Paris. By curious
coincidence, my wife, who is Portuguese, also wanted at the age of four-
teen to eventually become a biochemist in the Pasteur Institute. As a step
to that goal she did a PhD at the Imperial Cancer Research Fund in London,
where I also did my PhD, and where we met. As a consequence she decided
to remain in Britain and build her career here rather than moving to Paris.
Today, I like to tell my son and daughter that they owe their lives to cancer
research!
My wife and I have had ups and downs in our research careers, but we
have retained our passion for science; today it is what occupies a good
deal of our working hours, and sometimes keeps us awake at night. Above
all though, our research is also just the job that we do, and we both con-
sider ourselves fairly ordinary people. Such a viewpoint is not shared by
the majority of the population. A common stereotypical view of scientists
is that they are very clever, but also rather odd—the typical absent-minded,
socially inept, nerdy individual personified in popular culture through
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
TV shows such as The Big Bang Theory.1 While scientists can be viewed as
doing great good—such as discovering new treatments for cancer—
they may also be seen as responsible for environmental catastrophes
and consequently as irresponsible people, willing to risk all in their
search for knowledge or lacking concern for the consequences of their
actions.2 Whether good or bad though, a common perception is that
scientists as a group think and behave in very different ways from the
rest of humanity.
In this chapter, I want to challenge that perception, and argue that while
scientists may occupy a very specific niche in society in their role in the
construction of new knowledge, they are prone to the same strengths and
weaknesses in terms of their individual minds, and are affected by the
same social prejudices, as the rest of humanity. Moreover, far from being
an alien activity, the pursuit of scientific knowledge is something that is a
key resource for humanity as a whole, and everyone with an interest in
the origins and fundamental nature of human conscious awareness
should have a strong interest in knowing more about the process of scien-
tific discovery, and an input into debates about both the direction and
application of scientific research. To understand why, it is worth looking
at how science first developed.
Ever since our proto-human ancestors first began using and designing
tools to manipulate the world around them, it seems fair to say that
humankind has been practising a form of science and technology. And
unlike other animals ranging from crows to chimps which have been
observed to skilfully use natural objects as tools,3 a distinguishing feature
of our species is that we not only use tools, but also are continually
improving on them and developing new kinds.4 Our ability to develop
different tools over time is closely bound up with our creativity and
imagination, for in order to design a new tool, it is necessary to have some
preconception of something that does not yet exist. Karl Marx recognized
this in relation to creation of animal and human habitats when he noted
that ‘a spider conducts operations that resemble those of a weaver, and a
bee puts to shame many an architect in the construction of her cells. But
359
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
what distinguishes the worst architect from the best of bees is this, that
the architect raises his structure in imagination before he erects it in reality.’5
We humans have another unique quality: our ability to discover new
things about the world and how it functions. The development and use of
novel tools may be described as technology, while finding out new things
about the world can be considered as science. Yet it would be unwise to
draw too narrow a dividing line between these two subjects. We can only
develop new technologies by gaining greater insights about the world
around us and how it works. At the same time, new technologies drive
scientific discovery by making it possible to investigate the world in more
varied and fundamental ways. Indeed, the molecular biologist Sydney
Brenner once stated that scientific progress ‘depends on the interplay of
techniques, discoveries and new ideas, probably in that order of decreas-
ing importance.’6
Scientific discovery generally requires observation but also experi-
mentation. Crucially, the scientific method involves putting forward a
hypothesis and then studying whether reality corresponds to our pro-
posed view.7 Humans and even our proto-human ancestors have probably
been carrying out such observation and experimentation ever since we
first began interacting with nature in novel ways, something made pos
sible by the development of tools. Once we developed language, and an
ability to think in a complex fashion about objects and people, cause and
effect, time and place, and to communicate such ideas to other humans,
we were able to frame questions about the world and test them.
Imagination and creativity are crucial features of humankind, whose
links to the rest of consciousness I discussed previously. Yet when asked
to name creative individuals, most people would tend to think of a musi-
cian, a novelist, or a painter—in other words, artistic individuals—not a
scientist or a technologist.8 Bill Wallace, a science teacher at Georgetown
Day School in Washington, DC, thinks that one reason for this is the way
many people are taught about science. According to him, ‘a lot of kids
think that science is a body of knowledge, a collection of facts they need
to memorize.’9 Wallace believes that allowing students to come up with
360
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
361
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
362
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
stump with pencil and paper she realized through a series of equations
that this ‘splitting of the atom’ was mathematically feasible and, accord-
ing to Einstein’s equation E = mc2, would release huge amounts of energy.
Meitner’s revolutionary insight was a transformative moment in physics
but it also opened the door to the development of the atomic bomb and
the terrible loss of life that resulted when it was used on the Japanese cities
of Hiroshima and Nagasaki. While some physicists helped develop the
bomb because of fears about the Nazis getting there first, Meitner
remained adamant that ‘I will have nothing to do with a bomb!’19
The practical application of the discovery of nuclear fission for pur-
poses of mass destruction demonstrates the double-edged character of
science in modern society, on the one hand leading to great good through
new medicines or novel ways of feeding humanity, and on the other, fuel-
ling the development of new forms of warfare or environmental pollu-
tion. In fact, this double-edged character of science has been true ever
since the birth of class society. It is important not to forget, however, that
we owe the very development of science to this type of society. Previously,
I pointed to some oppressive features of class society and that it brought
with it not just class divisions, but also sexism and racism, war and other
forms of destruction. However, it is also true that this form of society is
responsible for the amazing knowledge that we now possess, not only
about our own planet, but also about the whole Universe. At the same
time, we have gained important insights into the factors that govern not
just our bodies but also our minds, and finally about human society itself.
One way that class society led to the development of science and tech-
nology was through the birth of the city, and subsequent new types of
production and trade between cities. Both writing and the first forms of
mathematics were initially developed to allow cities to track food supplies
and other goods that for the first time in history formed a surplus.20
Meanwhile, the development of agriculture led to increased interest in
predicting the weather and the seasons. This was particularly important
in a society like ancient Egypt whose agriculture relied heavily on the sea-
sonal flood of the river Nile to fertilize the land.21
363
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
364
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
365
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
progress involves testing hypotheses against reality, this does not mean
that society’s values do not have an influence on scientific ideas them-
selves. Darwin’s discovery that all species are a product of evolution by
natural selection was a revolutionary step forward in our understanding
of life, and indeed the origins of our own species. However, the emphasis
on competition—the survival of the fittest—detracts from the fact that
cooperation is as much a feature of the natural world and of the evolu-
tionary process.25 In this case, Darwin’s objective view about the dynam-
ics in nature was overly influenced by the values of free-market economics
that tended to dominate all discussions at that time in Victorian England.
I mentioned previously that a key idea at the heart of the revolutions
that took place in the Netherlands and England in the sixteenth and seven
teenth centuries, and in France and America at the end of the eighteenth
century, was the importance of the primacy of the individual. The view of
society as being just a collection of individuals had an influence on per-
ceptions about the physical world. For instance, it inspired Newton’s view
of the Universe as being akin to a piece of clockwork, that is, something
reducible to its separate parts. Such an approach to understanding the
natural world is known as reductionism. It was the guiding principle for
the British renaissance in science that occurred in the seventeenth cen-
tury following the revolution in 1649 that brought the capitalist class to
power. As well as leading to Newton’s great discoveries in physics, this
period saw major steps forward in biology and medicine. In fact, this is
not the only example where a social revolution has acted as a major cata-
lyst for dramatic changes in the sciences. The period around the French
revolution of 1789 was associated with major scientific discoveries and
technologies that included the isolation of chemical elements, a better
understanding of human metabolism, the physics of electricity, and the
first theory of evolution.26 Remarkable technical innovations at this time
included the metric system, ballooning, the invention of canned food,
and the semaphore telegraph.
While reductionism has been an immensely powerful approach for
understanding the natural world, it also suffers from important limita-
366
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
367
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
368
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
shows how such a unit is not necessarily a fixed entity, but one that can
expand in meaning.
This is important because one of the revelations of recent years—out-
lined previously in this book and in more detail in my book The Deeper
Genome—is that there is far more to genes than simply stretches of DNA
coding for proteins.35 In particular, there is now increasing recognition
that genomes can only be understood as 3D entities, and that chemical
modifications to the DNA and the proteins associated with it also play key
roles in gene expression, as do the newly discovered class of regulatory
RNAs. Finally, tying all these discoveries together is the new field of epi
genetics, which has revealed that both the cellular and bodily environ-
ment, working via the control mechanisms just mentioned, can influence
not only genome function in an individual organism but also in its off-
spring. Importantly, this suggests that the twentieth-century ‘modern
synthesis’ of Darwinism and traditional genetics needs modernizing if it
is truly to explain the multiple levels of biology.36
Meanwhile, in physics, we currently face a curious schism in explanatory
power. On the one hand, Einstein’s theory of relativity explains the struc-
ture and dynamics of the Universe at a macroscopic level, accounting for
gravity and all of the things it dominates: orbiting planets, colliding galax-
ies, the dynamics of the expanding Universe as a whole.37 On the other
hand, quantum theory, which handles the other three forces—electromag-
netism and the two nuclear forces—is extremely adept at describing what
happens when a uranium atom decays, or when individual particles of light
hit a solar cell.38 However, the problem is that relativity and quantum
mechanics are fundamentally different theories with different formula-
tions. It is not just a matter of scientific terminology; it is a clash of genu-
inely incompatible descriptions of reality.39 Relativity gives nonsensical
answers when scaled down to quantum size, eventually descending to
infinite values in its description of gravity. And quantum mechanics runs
into serious trouble when blown up to cosmic dimensions.
Here the problem is that two plausible units of analysis have been iden-
tified that each explain nature across a wide sphere of influence, but then
369
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
370
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
371
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
Part VII
CH A P T ER 22
mind a nd me a ning
might have originated and why it still maintains such a strong hold on
the human mind, even amidst the science and technology of the twenty-
first century.
Of course, a religious person could answer that their feelings stem from
the existence of God. But since this book is concerned primarily with
aspects of consciousness that can be examined scientifically, my approach
here will be solely to ask what material reasons we can find to explain
belief in the supernatural and link this to this book’s theme, which is how
culture has shaped the human mind. One suggestion is that such belief
arose as a by-product of response mechanisms that evolved to protect our
species in its first phase of existence. For instance, consider a scenario in
which one of our prehistoric ancestors walking in the African savannah
heard a rustling in the grass. While this might be just the wind, it could
equally signify the presence of a predator, such as a lion. Kelly James
Clark, of the Kaufman Interfaith Institute at Grand Valley State
University in Michigan, has argued that those of our ancestors who
recognized that other organisms have ‘agency’—an ability to act of their
own accord—could have been more likely to survive than those without
this recognition.1
Another feature of human beings is what has been called our ‘theory of
mind’—the capacity to understand that other humans have thoughts and
desires and to have some idea what these might be.2 In particular, only
people, through the way that language has uniquely transformed our
brains, have a true sense of ourselves as individual rational beings but also
possess the ability to decipher the thoughts of other people, either by
observing their actions and words or through direct interrogation of
others through a series of questions.
Yet while helping humans to survive, such attributes may have led to
supernatural beliefs. For as well as attributing agency to predators, and
rationality to each other, our ancestors may have started extending them
to things with no agency or rationality at all. As Clark notes, ‘you might
think that raindrops aren’t agents. They can’t act of their own accord.
They just fall. And clouds just form; they’re not things that can act. But
376
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
what human beings have done [in the past] is to think that clouds are
agents.’3 Similarly, as our ancestors could see that other human beings
had rational minds, they might have started to wonder whether such
rationality was more widespread in the world than just within our species.
And such a misplaced perception of agency and rationality in nature
could have given rise to the idea of conscious, powerful, and unruly invis-
ible spirits that need placating by ritual ceremony.
Indeed, a characteristic feature of hunter-gatherer society—the type of
society in which our species lived for the vast majority of its existence—is
‘animism’, the belief that objects, places, and animals all have a distinct
spiritual essence.4 To some extent, this belief may have helped to develop
the sustainable manner in which hunter-gatherer societies interact with
nature, by engendering feelings of respect for other organisms. And in a
world in which floods, lightning, disease, and other natural disasters
could strike out of the blue, it may also have helped our ancestors make
sense of an unpredictable, often unkind, world in which loved ones might
meet their end in the prime of life, or even in early childhood.
The associated idea that human beings might somehow survive in
some form after death—what might be called the ultimate comfort—
seems to have been a feature of our species from early in its existence.
Indeed, recent finds have indicated that belief in an afterlife may even have
been shared by Neanderthals. For instance, Enrique Baquedano and
colleagues of the Regional Archaeological Museum of Madrid discovered
that in a cave 93 kilometres north of Spain’s capital city, the jaw and six
teeth of a Neanderthal child had been buried, and surrounding the burial
site were several blackened hearths, within which the researchers found
the horn or antler of a herbivore, apparently carefully placed there.5
Baquedano has argued that this arrangement ‘may therefore have been of
ritual or symbolic significance.’6 He believes that the fires were lit as a sort
of funeral ceremony.
Today, a belief in life after death is a central feature of most religions,
although in some Eastern faiths, such as Hinduism or Buddhism, there is
instead a belief in reincarnation on Earth until perfection of the soul
377
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
results in release from the cycle of death and rebirth into some sort of
Nirvana.7 The fear of one’s own death, and the pain of losing someone
dear, can fuel hope that something of oneself and one’s loved ones per-
sists after death. Following my father’s sudden death, my mother and
sister, both formerly atheist or agnostic, became deeply religious. In my
mother’s case, this followed a visit to a spiritualist church where she
claimed she had been presented with evidence that my father still existed
in an afterlife.
This is not the place to consider the evidence that some spiritualists
have deliberately sought to trap and deceive bereaved individuals desper-
ately searching for some sign that a loved one still exists in some spiritual
plane. Of course, not all mediums are necessarily frauds; some may actu-
ally believe they are channelling messages from the dead. What is clear is
that during periods of great tragedy, such as World War I in which millions
of young men lost their lives in battle, there has been an increase in the
number of people seeking to communicate with the dead. So a British
commentator wrote in 1919 about ‘mothers and friends of fallen soldiers
resorting to table-rapping, creakings, automatic writing through the medium
of the planchette, Ouija, heliograph, etc., in the hope of once more com-
municating with their loved ones.’8 A desire to believe in the supernatural
can rely upon a human tendency to seize upon an unlikely chance occur-
rence as meaningful, rather than just coincidence. If a medium calls out to
their audience, ‘I have a message for someone called John’, if there is
someone in the audience of that name, they may believe that the message
is for them, and the medium may subsequently respond to a bereaved
person’s questions in such an ambiguous fashion that they may really
think they are communing with a dead loved one.
Of course, there will be people who will swear that the messages or
signals they believe they have received from a dead loved one only make
sense if there is an afterlife. I once had a highly unlikely encounter that
certainly tested my sense of scientific rationality. As I related earlier, a
week prior to my father’s death from a fall down the pothole Gaping
Ghyll, I had set off for a walking holiday in the Scottish Highlands with
378
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
379
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
380
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
were deprived of their sense organs, and forced to walk on their heads and
eat their own excrement.12
Today, we could laugh at such beliefs as a product of their time were it
not that the notion of hell remains an important aspect of many modern
religions, as shown by the outcry in the Catholic Church when a news
paper article in 2018 quoted Pope Francis as apparently saying that ‘bad
souls’ are not punished. Instead the Pope was supposed to have said that
‘those who repent obtain God’s forgiveness and take their place among
the ranks of those who contemplate him, but those who do not repent
and cannot be forgiven disappear.’13 Vatican officials quickly rebuked the
newspaper, saying that it had misquoted the Pope, and affirmed the view
that hell ‘really exists and is eternal’.14 Clearly, the prospect of sinning
souls quietly ‘disappearing’ does not have the same impact as eternal suf-
fering. Yet to only focus on religion as a ruling class form of control
beyond the grave—with the reward of a blissful heaven balanced against
the threat of eternal damnation—or for that matter, to solely concentrate
on the irrational and superstitious aspects of religion does not do justice
to the fact that religion has at times also been associated with much
more progressive aims.
The idea that religion simply acts to keep ordinary people in their
place is sometimes supported by a famous quote from Karl Marx, in
which he spoke of religion being ‘the opium of the people’. Yet focusing
on this phrase in isolation is misleading, since it was part of a longer
statement: ‘Religion is . . . the expression of real suffering and a protest
against real suffering. Religion is the sigh of the oppressed creature, the
heart of a heartless world, and the soul of soulless conditions. It is the
opium of the people.’15 This double-edged character of religion means
that while it may sometimes be used to stifle ideas of struggle against
oppressive conditions in society, and even be used in highly reactionary
ways, at other times it can act as a very powerful stimulus to revolts
against earthly injustices.
One factor that has allowed religion to play such a dual role throughout
history is the ambiguity at the heart of all major religions regarding their
381
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
382
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
and Eve span, who then was the gentleman?’ He added that ‘from the
beginning all men by nature were created alike, and our bondage or servi-
tude came in by the unjust oppression of naughty men.’17 This reinter
pretation of the Book of Genesis expressed the peasants’ dreams of liberty
and equality. During the armed peasants’ march on London, Ball preached
that ‘things cannot go right in England and never will, until goods are held
in common and there are no more villeins and gentlefolk, but we are all
one and the same.’18 What is interesting for the current discussion is how
the rebellious peasants in this case developed an ideology that subverted
the traditional reading of the Bible so that instead of upholding the exist-
ing rigid feudal order, the stories in this religious text were used as a plea
for a much more progressive society.
The years following the English Revolution of the mid-seventeenth
century saw a flourishing of new scientific theories and discoveries ran
ging from Newton’s laws of motion to the discovery of the cell as the
biological unit of life by Robert Hooke.19 Yet it would be a mistake to
separate the English Revolution from the religious fervour that inspired
its activists. For although groups like the Levellers can appear remark
ably modern, with their demands for a form of universal suffrage, their
use of newspapers, petitions, mass demonstrations, and particularly the
involvement of women in their movement, in other ways many of their
followers were a clear product of their time, believing in magic, proph
ecies, and miracles.20
We can get a sense of the fervour of such activists from the work of a
particularly gifted individual of this era—John Bunyan. A soldier in the
Parliamentary army during the English Civil War, but then imprisoned
for his beliefs during the religious clampdown that accompanied the
monarchy’s return in 1660, in jail Bunyan began writing The Pilgrim’s
Progress.21 Portrayed as a dream, Bunyan’s book became an instant clas-
sic with its story of Christian’s pilgrimage to the Celestial City that
takes him through many adventures, dangers, and false turns.22 One
particularly powerful aspect of Bunyan’s allegory is the way he renders
abstract moral dilemmas concrete through encounters with characters
383
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
384
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
also a future in which ‘one day on the red hills of Georgia, the sons of
former slaves and the sons of former slave owners will be able to sit down
together at the table of brotherhood.’26 Perhaps most importantly, King
looked forward to a world when religious divisions themselves would be
a thing of the past, with his concluding sentence that when ‘we let free-
dom ring, when we let it ring from every village and every hamlet, from
every state and every city, we will be able to speed up that day when all of
God's children, black men and white men, Jews and Gentiles, Protestants
and Catholics, will be able to join hands and sing in the words of the old
Negro spiritual: Free at last! Free at last! Thank God Almighty, we are free
at last!’27 In this respect it is interesting that King employed the language
of religious fervour, but used it to make a highly rational plea for a unified
form of humanity that was not divided by skin colour or particular faith.
From a consistently scientific point of view religion can only be seen
ultimately as a form of illusion, being based on a belief in supernatural
forces, yet it provides both meaning and sometimes a justification for
action in ways that cannot simply be dismissed as irrational. But what
about people who do not believe in the supernatural? Is there a way for
non-believers to find deeper meaning in life while at the same time main-
taining that everything in the Universe, including human consciousness,
can be explained in purely material terms? For surely, finding such deeper
meaning is a highly important aspect of human conscious awareness, if
that awareness is to rise beyond the mundane reality of daily existence.
To some extent, merely recognizing the connection of human beings,
with our conscious awareness, to the rest of nature can provide a sense of
deeper meaning. Addressing critics who believed that his theory of evo-
lution by natural selection diminished our sense of wonder about the
world around us, Charles Darwin replied that ‘there is grandeur in this
view of life’.28 Knowing that the intensity of what we feel when we fall in
love or experience the birth of our first child is partially based on a surge
of brain chemicals like dopamine or oxytocin should not undermine the
experience, but rather can help us connect it to the rest of nature.
Similarly, recognizing that great human cultural achievements—whether
385
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
386
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
actions
bodily
sleep
Figure 29 Cartesian dualism and the idea of a homunculus controlling the brain.
387
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
388
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
389
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
390
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
391
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
392
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
of the symbol by Hitler and the German Nazis reflected their rather mystical
ideas about nationality and race, and their desire to propagate a ‘thousand-
year Reich’.43
To the person who embroidered my sister’s jacket, the swastika symbol
clearly held a very different cultural and religious relevance, so that they
were not concerned by its more sinister connotations, assuming they
were even aware of these. In contrast, the reason that I was shocked to see
swastikas on my sister’s jacket was because it is difficult today as a person
in Europe not to associate this symbol with the Nazi movement. And it is
testament to the influence of a changing society on the mind that a sym-
bol can also powerfully change in meaning. For imagine, how percep-
tions of this particular symbol must have rapidly changed in Germany in
the 1920s and 1930s, perhaps being viewed initially as an object of ridicule
when the Nazis were only a fringe organization, giving way to unease as
the organization began to dominate the political scene, first through vio-
lence on the streets then through increased influence in the German
Reichstag or parliament, and finally fear, following the Nazis’ rise to
power in 1933, after which they rapidly moved to suppress opposition to
their dictatorship.44 For Nazi supporters, however, the swastika was prob-
ably an important unifying factor in all sorts of ways, combining as it did
the colours of the previous German regime defeated in World War I and
an ancient mystical symbol. Given that Hitler himself described mass
demonstrations like the Nuremberg rallies as events that ‘burn into the
little man’s soul the conviction that, though a little worm, he is part of a
great dragon’,45 it would be a mistake to underestimate the potency of
such a combination of different symbolic elements.
On a more personal note, the significance of one piece of music has
changed dramatically for me over the past two decades because of the dif-
fering contexts in which it was heard. Like many people, I first heard the
piano piece ‘Comptine d’Un Autre Été’ by Yann Tiersen while watching
the film Amélie. This film, by French director Jean-Pierre Jeunet, is about a
quirky young Parisian waitress Amélie, who decides to surreptitiously
393
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
work to change the lives of those around her for the better, while struggling
with her own isolation.46
As Amélie’s theme music, ‘Comptine d’Un Autre Été’ captured per-
fectly the bittersweet nature of Amélie’s experiences portrayed in the
film. But the piece acquired a more personal relevance for me when my
sister’s son—my nephew Jack—learned to play it and often did so when
we visited my sister’s home, and it became a typical accompaniment to
our family gatherings there. However, the music acquired a much deeper
significance after my sister’s suicide when Jack played the piece—one of
my sister’s favourites—at her funeral. Now, it is impossible for me to hear
this piece of music without associating it not only with my sister’s funeral,
but also many past times—both happy and sad—spent together.
The subjective nature of experience is what makes being an individual
unique. Humans have evolved to be social beings. This extreme sociability
defines our species, yet possessing conscious awareness, we are ironically
also the only species aware of our individuality. According to the view of
the mind proposed in this book, this sense of individuality is to some
extent an illusion, ‘a tenant lodging in the social edifice of ideological
signs’,47 as Voloshinov described the human mind. Yet that tenant is us,
and however tenuous, our individuality is what defines us. Each of us finds
meaning through our experiences and actions in a social world. My
research, teaching, writing, political activities, hobbies, and leisure inter-
ests acquire meaning because of my interactions with other human beings,
whether friends, family, or total strangers. But ultimately only we can
know how it feels to be us, that ‘I’ who seems to operate from the skull sit-
ting at the top of our bodies. We cling to this sense of being a unique indi-
vidual through life, and it is the fear of ceasing to be that ‘I’ that can haunt
us as we contemplate the time when we will be no more through death.
One of the key drivers of religious ideas is the hope—against all scien-
tific evidence—that we can continue to exist after death. Meanwhile for
those of us who do not believe in the supernatural, one of the biggest
challenges we face is how we deal with our own transient existence,
and those of our loved ones, while still retaining a deeper sense of the
394
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 26/01/21, SPi
mind a nd me a ning
395
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
CH A P T ER 23
M aking predictions about the future is seldom easy. Such is the pace
of modern life that predictions can greatly overestimate progress
in the future, yet fail to anticipate major social transformations that we
now take for granted. I was five years old in July 1969 when I watched
awestruck as the flickering black and white TV screen in my home showed
men walking on the Moon, and I assumed while growing up in the 1970s
that by the time I was an adult our species would have colonized the
Moon, Mars, and perhaps even the rest of the Solar System. My youth may
have been a factor in such optimism, but I was far from being alone in
such expectations at this time; the British 1970s TV programme Space:
1999 was typical in predicting the existence of a human colony on the
Moon before the turn of the millennium.1
In fact, it would be easy to produce a long list of technological predic-
tions made by enthusiasts in the twentieth century that have still not been
realized. Technologies ranging from teleportation to scanners that can
non-invasively diagnose and cure disease, which were presented as com-
monplace in the futuristic series Star Trek, are little closer now to practical
realization than they were in the 1960s when first conceived.2 Yet other
developments that we now take for granted, like the Internet, personal
computers, smart phones, and precision gene editing, may have featured
in science fiction, but also seemed likely to remain as fiction not so many
years ago. So how might future technologies impact on human con-
sciousness? In this chapter we will consider whether artificial intelligence
might come to rival that of human beings and possible interfaces between
human and machine intelligence.
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
397
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
in China.5 Potential ethical issues include the fact that creating models of
conditions like severe forms of autism in a primate like a macaque may
have more detrimental effects on the well-being of an individual of this
species—precisely because its forms of social interaction are more simi-
lar to those of a human—than is the case for mice. Such sensitivity to
possible consequences will be even more important if scientists use
gene-edited primates to study the biological basis of human conscious
awareness.
Showing the pace of research in this area, Bing Su, a geneticist at the
Kunming Institute of Zoology in China, has already introduced a human
version of a gene called SRGAP2, thought to endow the human brain with
processing power by allowing the growth of connections between
neurons, and of MCPH1, a gene related to brain size, into macaques and
reported the consequences.6 As mentioned previously, a distinguishing
feature of human brain development is that it is delayed compared to the
situation in other primates. Although as a consequence, human babies
are far more helpless at birth, such a delay also means that our brains have
a far greater learning capacity, for far longer, than those of other primates.
As a baby’s brain develops after birth, MCPH1 is expressed in abundance,
but much less so in non-human primates, and it has been suggested to
play a role in the human brain’s developmental delay. And Su and his
team’s recent study showed that the brains of monkeys with the human
version of MCPH1 took longer to develop, and the animals performed
better in tests of short-term memory as well as reaction time compared to
normal monkeys.7
Su claims that this is the ‘first attempt to understand the evolution of
human cognition using a transgenic monkey model’.8 However, Jacqueline
Glover, a University of Colorado bioethicist, believes the study is likely to
raise concerns about ‘Planet of the Apes’-type scenarios ‘in the popular
imagination’ and thinks that ‘to humanise [the monkeys] is to cause
harm. Where would they live and what would they do? Do not create a
being that can’t have a meaningful life in any context.’9 But Larry Baum, a
researcher at Hong Kong University’s Centre for Genomic Sciences, has
398
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
399
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
400
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
with the real world—through our senses, but also our relationships with
other human beings—that we find evidence that we exist in such a real
world. But how can we be sure then that such interactions are real?
Such a conundrum was explored in the 1999 science-fiction film The
Matrix, in which a computer hacker called Neo comes to realize that he,
and the rest of humanity, are actually bodies in suspended animation,
enslaved by sentient machines who feed on their life essence, and ‘reality’
is just an illusion fed to the brain.15 Only a few humans have recognized
their plight, and the rest of the film concerns their battle to liberate
humanity. The main premise of the film is that virtual reality (VR) tech-
nology has become so advanced that the human race—bar these few
enlightened individuals—is not aware it is merely part of a simulation.16
Yet although there have been significant advances in VR technology since
the film’s release, we are still not at the point where anyone could mistake
such a simulated world for the real thing—unless somehow the film’s
premise is true, and all human experience really is an illusion!
Returning to our scenario of an artificial human brain in a vat, given
that such a brain would never have experienced being in an actual human
body, it might be less of an issue if the simulated world were not an accur
ate reconstruction of human life. But we need to address how feasible it
would be to recreate the senses for such an artificial brain. For the more
we learn about the biology of both vision and hearing, but also smell,
taste, and touch, the clearer it is becoming that the way we process inputs
from the outside world through such senses is complex not only in terms
of the brain’s role, but also with regard to the biology of the eye, the nose,
the tongue, the skin, and so on. Because of this, the artificial senses that
we might create for a brain in a vat would surely be far inferior to our own
biological means for experiencing the world. However, for an artificial
brain that had never experienced anything else, perhaps such an experience
would not be seen as impoverished.
An important difference between the evil intelligent machines in The
Matrix and a conscientious and ethically minded human scientist who
had created an artificial human brain would surely be that such a scientist
401
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
402
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
The only organ of human origin in a cyborg like the one described
would be its brain. Yet such is the importance of the brain for our concep-
tion of what it means to be human that many people might be able to
accept the idea that such a cyborg could be considered part of humanity.
What, then, of a form of conscious awareness that was purely machine?
How would this be considered, and how likely is it that such an entity
could ever exist?
Certainly, there has been no shortage of predictions in recent years that
an ‘artificial intelligence’ (AI) based on a computer will soon have not only
reached the same mental capabilities as a human being, but also sur-
passed them. Upon reaching such a point, often referred to as a techno-
logical ‘singularity’, AI is then predicted to undergo an exponential
technological growth that will transform life as we know it. For instance,
Vernor Vinge, a mathematician at San Diego State University, who first
coined this term in 1993, believes that ‘we will soon create intelligences
greater than our own. When this happens, human history will have
reached a kind of singularity, an intellectual transition as impenetrable as
the knotted space-time at the centre of a black hole, and the world will
pass far beyond our understanding.’18 Such predictions have helped to
trigger a debate in recent years about whether such an all-powerful AI
will save humanity or instead hasten our extinction.
Representing the first point of view, Frank Lansink, European CEO at
IPsoft, believes that ‘AI . . . has an integral role to play in the workplaces of
the future. It will help unleash creativity, create new job activities and
new occupations which combine ground-breaking technology with our
most human skills.’19 However, others are less convinced about such a
rosy scenario. For instance, in 2017 the biotech entrepreneur Elon Musk
stated that ‘I have exposure to the very cutting-edge AI, and I think people
should be really concerned about it . . . I keep sounding the alarm bell. But
until people see robots going down the street killing people, they don't
know how to react, because it seems so ethereal.’20 And the physicist
Stephen Hawking told a BBC interviewer in 2014 that ‘the development of
403
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
full artificial intelligence could spell the end of the human race.’21
Expressing how such a scenario could arise, Kilian Weinberger, a computer
scientist at Cornell University, has recently said that ‘if super-intelligent
AI—more intelligent than us—becomes conscious, it could treat us like
lower beings, like we treat monkeys. That would certainly be undesirable.’22
Not surprisingly, such statements can leave many people feeling highly
uneasy about the development of ever-more sophisticated computer and
robotic systems. But is the rise of an all-powerful AI really such a likely
scenario? One reason for caution is that such predictions are not new.
Notably, Alan Turing, the mathematician who was one of the first pion
eers of computer science, predicted in 1951 that machines would ‘outstrip
our feeble powers’ and ‘take control’ within the next human generation.23
In 1965, Turing’s colleague Irving Good argued that ‘ultra-intelligent’ AI
systems would soon design even more intelligent ones, ad infinitum:
‘Thus the first ultra-intelligent machine is the last invention that man need
ever make, provided that the machine is docile enough to tell us how to
keep it under control.’24 Clearly, such predictions were hopelessly mis-
guided in their timescale, but how about their basic substance? Given that
one commentator has described AI as ‘all the things that computers still
can’t do’, could the singularity be creeping up on us, and may computers
already be close to reaching that point of all-powerfulness?
Certainly, computing power has made possible some amazing advances
in recent years. Computers are already proficient at picking financial
stocks, translating speech, and identifying cancer cells in a biopsy. And
although the board game Go was thought to be so guided by intuition
that it was unsusceptible to mastery by a computer, in 2016, AlphaGo, a
‘deep learning’ programme designed by a team at Google’s DeepMind AI
subsidiary in London, was able to dethrone Lee Sedol, the world’s leading
player of the game.25
Despite such successes, is it true that computers are approaching the
point of matching, or even surpassing, human consciousness? To address
this question, we need to take a closer look at the mode of action of pro-
grammes like AlphaGo. Such programs are sometimes referred to as
404
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
405
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
dinner or the argument they had earlier with their spouse (although there
are limits—and risks—to the number of mental tasks on which a person
can safely be engaged, especially if in charge of a fast-moving vehicle).
Not only can human beings multitask in this way, but we also have a
memory system that allows us to acquire skills in diverse other tasks, all
of which can be called into operation as required as we go through life,
even if we have not used such skills in years. In contrast, for a program
like AlphaGo that has become expert at Go to learn a new task—such as
modelling 3D structures of proteins—it has to forget everything it has
learned about Go.
Second, human brains are far more than just computational devices. In
particular, our brains are first of all organic structures, which has import
ant implications in comparing their action to that of a computer. Indeed,
there are huge differences between even an animal brain—say that of a
monkey—and a computer program like AlphaGo. While the name
‘neural network’ is deliberately meant to suggest similarities between
such a network and an organic brain—with even the individual units of
the electronic circuit named ‘neurons’—in reality there is a huge differ-
ence between these two entities.31 For as we have seen, real neurons are
far from being simple digital on/off switches like the units of a computer
circuit; instead, they have a complex structure, a genome, but also a pat-
tern of gene expression distributed across the different parts of the cell—
and neurons also come in all shapes and sizes. In addition, as well as
individual neurons being connected in local circuits via synapses, there is
increasing evidence that different brain regions are connected via brain
waves into a larger whole.
Moreover, both the local circuitry and these longer-range connections
may be quite different in a human compared to a chimp brain, because of
the role of language and other cultural factors mediating consciousness
in our species. As well as electrical communication, chemical signals—
including complex informational molecules like regulatory RNAs—are,
as we have seen, now recognized as playing an important role in facilitat-
ing communication between neurons, but also between neurons and glial
406
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
cells. Such signals also allow the brain to communicate with the body and
vice versa. And all of this structure and electrical and chemical communi-
cation is the product of four billion years of evolution.
The third difference between human consciousness and a computer
program like AlphaGo is that only the former sees meaning in what it
does.32 While AlphaGo is superb at number crunching as a way to get a
successful result, there is no evidence that it has any awareness at all of
what it is doing. For a grandmaster Go player like Lee Sedol to beat another
human grandmaster at this game means more than just the result itself,
because he has invested a huge amount of his life in getting to such a level,
with all of the fame and recognition that this can bring him as an indi-
vidual expert. It must be somewhat soul-destroying then to be beaten so
easily by a machine, and yet for AlphaGo’s part, all this program has done
is unconsciously carry out an algorithm. At least in their current form, it
is far from clear that any AI will acquire conscious awareness, despite
being able to carry out an increasing variety of tasks that include not just
playing games like Go, but also activities ranging from medical diagnosis
to self-driving cars.
The different levels of structure and communication in the human
brain, which range from synapses between individual neurons to waves
that coordinate the different brain regions, also make unlikely another
prediction—that it might soon be possible to ‘upload’ an individual
human consciousness into a computer. In 2016 physicist Stephen
Hawking stated that it should be possible to ‘copy the brain onto a com-
puter and so provide a form of life after death.’33 Not only that, but a bio-
tech company, Netcome, has been set up by MIT graduate Robert McIntyre
to begin work in this very area. McIntyre believes that if we can decipher
the ‘connectome’—all the synaptic connections—of an individual human
brain, a project which I mentioned earlier, it should then be possible to
upload this information into a computer.34 However, there are two major
flaws in such proposals.
First, while it may be possible to map all the synapses in a human brain,
there is no obvious way at the moment to reproduce the informational
407
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
408
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
mind a nd m achine
many issues. A key one, assuming that everything went to plan, and our
proto-human colony did not end up dying because of unforeseen adverse
events, would be the fact that at least the first generation of this space col-
ony would be brought up and educated solely by robots that, while highly
skilled, had no conscious awareness of their own.
What type of human beings would emerge from such an experience?
How might human minds and consciousness develop under such condi-
tions? Would they be a joyful new extension of humanity, or would our
ambitious plan leave them emotionally scarred for life? We would prob-
ably only find out as a species back on Earth once our extra-planetary
offspring started sending back messages to their home planet. Now obvi-
ously such a scenario is still science fiction but the fact that it is not that
far-fetched given recent technological progress is surely testament to the
power of the human mind and the society it has produced.
409
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 20/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
EPILOGUE
A Twenty-First-Century Mind
A ll books must end and it is time to draw this one to a close. But can
any account of so vast a topic as human consciousness ever have a
conclusive ending? For even if this book has managed to provide some
important new ideas about this subject, it will still only have scratched the
surface of what is really going on inside our heads. Nevertheless, drawing
things to a conclusion is what I must do, and I will do so by returning to
the various alternative views about human consciousness first mentioned
at the start of this book and assess how my account compares to, and
hopefully builds on, these other viewpoints.
René Descartes surely makes a suitable starting point. He revolution-
ized our understanding of how the human body works by proposing that
it can be understood as a machine, yet fell short in this project by refusing
to extend his mechanistic view to the mind. Instead, Descartes argued
that ‘higher’ mental functions can only be explained by reference to an
immortal soul, opaque to scientific investigation. This introduced a dual-
ism that persists to this day in discussions about human consciousness.1
So we need to consider how much the view of the human mind developed
here both recognizes and overcomes this dualism.
The difficulties in developing a view of human consciousness that does
not end up as Cartesian dualism arise from two main directions. The first
comes from the difficulty in accepting that something as personal as the
individual ‘I’ is merely based on the unconscious actions of cells in the
brain, particularly given the apparent complexity of human thought that
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
412
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
413
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
414
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
structured way that can only occur in our species because of the word
meaning and grammatical structure that language alone can provide.
It is increasingly recognized that not only electrical communication
but also that mediated by chemicals such as neurotransmitters and by
newly discovered ‘signalling’ molecules like regulatory RNAs play import
ant roles in the brain. It will therefore be important in future to study how
this other form of communication might be involved in reconfiguring the
human brain in response to language and other cultural mediators.
In fact the examples mentioned above still only provide tentative indi-
cations that the human brain is radically different from those of other
species, including the great apes. As the basic units of brain structure—
neurons and glial cells—and the molecular mechanisms underlying brain
function are very similar between humans and these other species, it will
rather be the organization of brain structure, interconnections between
brain regions, and brain dynamics that are likely to subtly differ in human
beings. And this is where the practical and ethical obstacles to studying
the dynamics of human brains have limited us.
To some extent, studies of molecular and cellular mechanisms in
human brain slices from recently deceased individuals who have donated
their bodies to science might be of value, although given how much I have
stressed the importance of treating the brain as a whole entity, it is not
clear how much would be gained from studying physiological responses
in such human brain slices, as opposed to those taken from another pri-
mate, or even a rodent. However, given that I have also argued that the
mediation of brain function by cultural factors—language being primary
in this respect—is key to understanding human consciousness, it would
be interesting to see what insights might be gained from molecular ana
lyses of different brain regions taken from aborted foetuses at different
stages of development and from corpses of human individuals of differ-
ent ages and suffering from various mental disorders, all samples of
course donated through ethical consent agreements.
In addition, as advances are made in our ability to generate increasingly
complex brain organoids from stem cells derived from specific human
415
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
416
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
claims of this book, namely that language and other cultural mediators
transform even the most basic brain functions in humans and that there
has been a qualitative shift during human evolution in how different brain
regions interact that has opened the human brain to such cultural medi
ation.
Of particular interest would be to carry out studies of the developing
brains of children and adolescents. Imaging analysis of the brains of
babies learning language is already happening, and therefore it would be
very interesting to revisit some of the studies carried out by Lev Vygotsky
and his colleagues in the 1920s and 1930s of children employing inner
speech while performing different tasks or learning to group objects in a
conceptual fashion, with a view to replicating these, but with imaging
analysis of participants’ brains.
A key reason for studying brain function, beyond intellectual curiosity,
is to better understand psychiatric disorders like depression, schizophre-
nia, and bipolar disorder, as well as conditions like autism spectrum dis
order. So what impact, if any, might the approach to understanding
human consciousness that I have been developing in this book have upon
diagnosis and treatment of such disorders? We should first acknowledge
how complex a phenomenon mental disorder is, in both biological and
social terms. There are unlikely to be easy answers for those seeking to
reduce human mental disorders to either simple genetics or purely soci-
etal pressures. However, this does not mean that the view of human con-
sciousness advanced here has nothing to offer in terms of better diagnosis
and treatment.
For a start, the view advanced here that human brain function is regu-
lated by top-down as well as bottom-up mechanisms, with brain waves of
different frequencies and other global signalling mechanisms potentially
playing an important role in coordinating the activities of different brain
regions, suggests that mental disorders may occur due to a breakdown of
such coordination, but in a way that distinguishes one disorder from
another. This could explain why multiple regions of the genome have
been linked to a condition like schizophrenia, because although mutations
417
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
in very different genomic regions may affect different parts of the brain,
the end result could be a breakdown in overall brain function that mani-
fests itself in the symptoms by which we tend to define a schizophrenic.
Recall Lev Vygotsky’s idea that if schizophrenia is due to a higher mental
function being ‘switched off’, the mind may try to compensate by resorting
to a developmentally more archaic function, but finding itself without the
higher levels of control, this produces abnormal forms of behaviour and
thought.9 If schizophrenia is due to a breakdown of coordination between
brain regions, then this could result in each region continuing to function,
but in isolation. And if the abstract symbolism of language is one of the key
coordinating elements in a human brain that provides it with the concep-
tual framework central to the development of conscious awareness, this
could explain both the breakdown in capacity for conceptual thinking
that Vygotsky noted in schizophrenics, as well as the loss of conscious
awareness that can be one of the features of the disorder.10
Such an interpretation also allows us to explain a well-known symp-
tom of schizophrenia—the tendency to hear ‘voices’. For a major claim of
the socially mediated view of human consciousness is that our innermost
thoughts find expression through a form of ‘inner speech’. Individual
thought is seen as a kind of internal dialogue, with the different voices
within that dialogue being drawn from our social interactions, and indi-
viduality itself as a boundary phenomenon that is a product of both such
social interactions and the biology of the individual’s brain. To some
extent this leads to a conundrum, namely that individuality itself is partly
an illusion, expressed by Charles Fernyhough by the statement that ‘we
are all fragmented. There is no unitary self. We are all in pieces, struggling
to create the illusion of a coherent “me” from moment to moment.’11 And
yet, it is clear that most people exist from day to day with the feeling that
they are a unified self. This is despite the fact that every time we go to
sleep at night, we lose our waking conscious state, but still regain our
sense of a unified consciousness when we wake up in the morning.
However, for some schizophrenics, a breakdown of conscious awareness
may express itself through an inability to recognize that the voices that
418
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
make up the inner speech within our heads are merely expressions of our
individuality, and not another person somehow existing inside our heads
but external to us.12
How might such an explanation of the schizophrenic state lead to
improvements in the diagnosis and treatment of this disorder? In prac
tical terms, brain scans of schizophrenic individuals engaged upon some
problem-solving activity that involves conceptualization—for instance,
the block-sorting exercises pioneered by Lev Vygotsky and his colleagues—
might make it possible to see whether coordination of different brain
regions during such exercises is different compared to the situation in
unaffected individuals. Such studies might identify distinctive dynamic
features of the brains of schizophrenic individuals and also highlight dif-
ferences between such individuals. After all, while a particular range of
symptoms define a schizophrenic—symptoms which can include hearing
internal voices, experiencing hallucinations, assigning unusual signifi-
cance or meaning to normal events, and experiencing delusions—two
people can be diagnosed as suffering from this condition while having a
completely different set of symptoms in this range.13 In other words, what
we define as schizophrenia may be a spectrum of loosely related condi-
tions, and brain imaging might help define how this relates to different
brain dynamics of individuals in such a spectrum.
In addition to being used to study schizophrenics, such an approach
might be used to investigate the brain dynamics of those with other men-
tal disorders, such as clinical depression. Some types of depression can
also be associated with delusions—as, for instance, was the case with my
sister’s condition—but recent studies have indicated that depression is
not associated with the breakdown of conscious awareness found with
severe schizophrenia.14 Therefore, it may be that delusions in a depressive
individual have a different root, possibly linked to the feelings of worth-
lessness characteristic of depression, and such differences might therefore
be identified by imaging. In addition, such analyses might help define dif-
ferent types of depression, since the condition is increasingly recognized
as being as heterogeneous as schizophrenia in its symptoms and perhaps
419
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
420
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
421
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
the mind is actually quite far removed from the view of human conscious-
ness developed by Lev Vygotsky in the years following the Russian
Revolution that saw conscious awareness as a sophisticated interplay of
both biological and social factors and that has been an inspiration for this
book. Instead, the blank slate view owes far more to the ideology of the
Stalinist regime that swept away the influence of thinkers like Vygotsky
and Voloshinov. But such has been the powerful influence of Stalinism
among left-wing circles for much of the twentieth century that it con
tinues to play a far more pervasive role in progressive thought—even that
nominally opposed to Stalinism—than is often realized.20
Such are the potential ideological differences between these two sets of
professionals. But there are also social factors at work. Psychiatry is often
viewed as medicine’s ‘poor cousin’, and there are good reasons for this.21
During the past century, our understanding of bodily mechanisms has
advanced through scientific understanding on a whole number of differ-
ent fronts and led to important new drug treatments in areas as diverse as
cardiology, cancer therapy, and treatment of bacterial and viral infections.
Yet as we have seen throughout this book, our understanding of how the
human brain works as a whole still remains far from clear, and as a conse-
quence, drug treatments for mental disorders have often been far from
inspiring, with even the mechanisms of action of those drugs that do seem
to have a positive impact on mental health remaining obscure. This lack of
clarity is due to the brain being so much more complex than any other
organ in the body, but it also reflects a central theme of this book, namely
that human conscious awareness is as much a social as a biological entity,
and therefore human mental disorders have a major social input.
Faced with such a lack of clarity about both the mechanisms of human
consciousness and how to intervene when it becomes disordered, one
strategy for health professionals under pressure as cuts bite into the health
service—coupled with rising levels of reported mental disorder—is to
retreat into a ‘two camps’ mentality. So psychiatrists can end up arguing
that the limited available funds should go into the development and pro-
vision of drug-based treatments for mental disorders, while psychologists
422
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
423
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
424
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
425
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
people to quit the habit since this link was established in the 1950s by the
British epidemiologist Richard Doll.27 These disorders, and I could name
many more, clearly have highly significant environmental causes, and
treating them is as much a social issue as a medical one. Nevertheless, the
human mind seems particularly susceptible to social pressures.
Some believe that the human mind is particularly vulnerable to mental
disorder as a by-product of our evolution. For instance, Ole Andreassen
and colleagues at the University of Oslo claim to have found evidence that
evolved genetic differences that distinguish our species from Neanderthals
may have enhanced our powers of creativity and imagination, but also
made us more vulnerable to schizophrenia.28 So as well as showing that
genetic differences linked to this disorder were only found in the genomes
of modern humans and not in those of Neanderthals, their findings indi-
cate that the brain regions in which the genes are expressed are linked to
our human ability to think and understand in complex ways.29
But while vulnerability to mental disorder may be the unfortunate
price we pay for our conscious awareness, it would be a major oversight
not to also seek explanations for the current epidemic of mental disorder
in existing society. This society has given us immense power, as our cities
and technologies increasingly reach into every corner of the globe, and
indeed even outer space no longer seems quite the ‘final frontier’ that it
would have appeared to be to past generations. Proof of our species’ abil-
ity to sustain itself is demonstrated by the fact that the human population
is now almost eight billion and has grown more since the eighteenth cen-
tury than in the entire history of humanity prior to that moment.30
Human individuals are also living to a far older age, with global average
life expectancy at birth in 2020 being estimated to be 73.2 years.31 Yet
while current society has brought us civilization and provided great
material prosperity and wealth for humankind as a whole, it is also char-
acterized by an increasing feeling of instability that can be destabilizing
to individual human minds and which may ultimately threaten the future
of human civilization itself, with all the potential implications this may
have for the mental health of our species in the future.
426
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
427
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
Such signs of mental disorder in young people may be merely the latest
manifestation—perhaps exacerbated by the latest technologies—of a
general problem of alienation within modern society, despite all its amazing
material achievements. This means not only that most people on the
planet are estranged from the fruits of their labours and typically see
work not as an act of creativity but something burdensome and oppres-
sive to them as individuals, but also that life itself becomes seen as a com-
petition—at school, in the job market, for friends and sexual partners—
with all the mental stresses that this can produce. But perhaps the most
worrying aspect of society today is that alongside all these problems we
are hurtling towards environmental catastrophe.
For all the great concentration of wealth and power in current society,
it is not clear that any of the people supposedly in control of the different
countries of the world are doing anything tangible to halt, let alone
reverse, the continuing flow of greenhouse gases into the atmosphere
which, if unchecked, will surely spell disaster. One of the reasons for this
paralysis is that no one really seems in control of our society; instead it
seems to be the market and the drive for profit that ultimately makes this
type of society tick. As such, there are substantial vested interests who do
not want to risk any threat to their possibility of short-term gain, even if
it risks the long-term viability of current society and possibly that of civ
ilization itself.37 And the problem for those influential individuals who
can see beyond the short-term—for instance Bill Gates, one of the world’s
richest men, who has recently pointed to the limitations of a free-market
economic system for solving a problem as big as global warming38—is
how to persuade companies and governments to move away from putt
ing the drive for profit first, in favour of sustainable technologies that may
not yield such an immediate profit but are ultimately better for the planet
and humanity, given the tendency of capitalism to punish those not com-
petitive enough to survive in the free market.
Given such major anxieties and societal pressures, it is not surprising
that many individuals today feel a sense of despair for the future—not just
in terms of their own immediate futures and those of their families and
428
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
friends, but also about the long-term prospects for humanity.39 This feel-
ing was only exacerbated by the global COVID-19 pandemic caused by
the coronavirus SARS-CoV-2 that swept the world in 2020 and led to not
only many deaths but also a lockdown that brought the global economy
to a juddering halt and confined individuals to their homes, cut off from
physical contact with many family members and friends. And politically
it has been accompanied by a trend described as the ‘collapse of the polit
ical centre’—that is, a rise in populist movements and increasing political
polarization.40
All of which sounds like grounds for a great deal of pessimism—at
least for people who see themselves as progressive in their political
views—and could feed into further rises in mental disorders such as
depression. Perhaps given the tragic history of my family I might be seen
as someone particularly likely to succumb to such a feeling. After all, for
all the insights I hope this book may have provided about the potential
mechanisms underlying mental disorders, on a personal level it is still
hard for me to comprehend just how successive members in my family
that I held so dear, and who seemed vibrant, sociable, and happy individ
uals, could end up dying in such horrific ways. And my progressive polit
ical ideals might seem to be thwarted by current circumstances. However,
I personally do not feel pessimistic, and to end this book, let me explain
why I have a sense of optimism about what lies in store in the years ahead.
To some extent my sense of optimism may be an aspect of my personal-
ity. Given that biology plays a role in personality, it seems not implausible
to me that certain people are born with a tendency to be optimistic. But as
we are shaped as much by our environment and experiences as our biol-
ogy, even the sunniest personalities will surely have that sense of sunshine
knocked out of them by a series of adverse circumstances, and here the
influence of others matters. In my own case, I owe thanks to a variety of
people—friends, colleagues, but most of all my family—for helping me
keep an optimistic state of mind.
It might seem strange that I have derived any optimism from my fam-
ily, given that two of my grandparents, my father, and my sister all died in
429
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
430
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
is in a gene that boosts the action of FAAH, and the combination of the
two mutations dramatically boosts levels of anandamide in the body.
Somewhat appropriately, given that Cameron seems to exist in a state of a
permanent natural ‘high’, this second gene is named FAAH-OUT.44
So should we all aspire to have our genomes engineered to make us like
Jo Cameron? In fact our pain response plays a vital role in protecting us
from physical injury and death. And indeed over the seven decades of
Cameron’s life she has accumulated a variety of injuries that include a
degenerated hip and thumbs deformed by osteoarthritis, which only
came to the attention of medical authorities when Cameron became
unable to walk and handle items, not because she reported any pain from
these injuries.45
While we might not want to emulate Cameron’s imperviousness to
pain, how about finding a way to provide the rest of us with her sunny
disposition on life? In fact here too there are some negative features. One
contributing factor to Cameron’s positive outlook is that she quickly for-
gets negative incidents in her life. However, she is also more generally
highly forgetful.46 This provides another reason why it would not be a
good idea to genetically engineer everyone to be more like Cameron,
unless we want a planet of people who feel no pain and are perpetually
happy, yet who are prone to serious injury because they have no painful
warning signals, and cannot remember what you told them last week.
Still, by studying how Cameron’s two gene mutations result in her
resistance to both physical and emotional pain, it might be possible to
develop new types of painkillers and drugs and other therapies for the
treatment of depression. More generally, while Cameron is clearly a highly
unusual individual, the existence of such an individual suggests that there
may be many other people who have a less extreme tendency to be resist-
ant to pain and have a more positive outlook on life, but who nevertheless
have a genetic basis for this tendency.
At the same time, it would seem mistaken given the emphasis in this
book on the ways that culture mediates our minds to only focus on bio
logical reasons for someone adopting a positive outlook on life.
431
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
Importantly, there is evidence that changes that we make in our lives can
also help us cope with, and see a way beyond, even the greatest misfortunes
that the world throws at us. One story that inspired me following the
deaths of my sister and mother was that of a 100-year-old black woman
called Ida Keeling from the Bronx in New York, who is a record-breaking
sprinter, having decided to take up running at the age of 60.47 This was not
just the whim of someone approaching old age but a person trying to find
some meaning in life after undergoing the deepest personal tragedy. In
her earlier years Ida—a single mother after her husband died of a heart
attack aged only 42—was active in the black civil rights movements, shut-
tling her children to Malcolm X speeches and boarding a predawn bus for
the 1963 March on Washington in which Martin Luther King made his
famous ‘I have a dream’ speech. Ida’s daughter Shelley Keeling has said, ‘I
always understood from mother that you die on your feet rather than live
on your knees.’48
However in the late 1970s, Ida’s resilience was tested to the limit by the
descent of her two sons into serious drug addiction and their subsequent
deaths from drug-related violence—one son being hanged and the other
beaten to death with a baseball bat.49 But as Ida fell into a deep depression,
Shelley Keeling—a track-and-field athlete—suggested that her mother
take up running as one way of coming to terms with her grief. And after
some coaxing, Ida registered for a five-kilometre race through Brooklyn.
It was a seminal moment for her. ‘Good Lord, I thought that race was
never going to end, but afterwards I felt free,’ she noted. ‘I just threw off all
of the bad memories, the aggravation, the stress.’50 My own running rou-
tine—a mere one-mile jog around my local park every morning—pales
into insignificance besides Ida Keeling’s efforts, but it was directly inspired
by reading her story and realizing that daily running could be a way for
me to deal positively with my grief.
Ida Keeling’s story raises the question of why running has proved so
beneficial as a way of maintaining a positive outlook on life, and how this
differs from the way that genetic mutations have had a similar impact on
Jo Cameron. In fact these two individuals may have more in common
432
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
than might first appear, since there is increasing evidence that exercise
can stimulate the production of the bodily chemical anandamide—the
same cannabis-like substance that exists at such high levels in Cameron’s
body because of a defect in the proteins that control its breakdown. This
explains why even moderate exercise—like a 15-minute jog in the
park—can leave us feeling more satisfied with ourselves and at peace
with the world.
Maintaining an optimistic outlook on life is important if we are to
maintain a feeling of happiness, and sanity, despite the many adverse inci-
dents that can affect us as individuals. But what if humanity itself seems to
have lost its sanity? In the face of coming environmental catastrophe, a
viral pandemic, and a political climate that seems increasingly reaction-
ary, it is not surprising that some people believe that optimism about the
future is misplaced.
However, I disagree with this view, firstly because it seems like an overly
simplistic view of the current situation, and secondly because a survey of
some key historical moments shows that ultimately ordinary people act-
ing as a collective have made possible the great leaps forward that occa-
sionally occur in human history. And those great leaps often occurred at
moments in history that must have seemed, prior to such leaps, as the
most depressing circumstances possible to those individuals who lived
through them.
Take, for instance, the English Peasants’ Revolt of 1381 that we have
already mentioned. Today, with hindsight, we can view this revolt as a
seminal moment in British history, which weakened the feudal system in
England and may have been an important factor that facilitated the later
overthrow of this system during the English Revolution of 1642–9.51 Yet
only a few decades before the Peasants’ Revolt, a deadly bubonic plague
strain known as the Black Death had swept across Europe, killing around
50 million people, equivalent to 60 per cent of its population.52 But the
resulting shortage of labour gave the peasants in southern England
greater bargaining power, and this was one factor that led them to start
having ideas ‘above their station’.53 Similar things could be said about the
433
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
434
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e pilogu e
role to play in social movements, yet as we have also seen in this book,
creativity and imagination are clearly key aspects of every person’s con-
sciousness, not just the preserve of a few gifted individuals. Finally, a char-
acteristic that is unique to our species is our power to shape the world
around us in a controlled, planned way.
Currently, we seem to have relinquished that control as human activ
ities threaten to destroy the environment and our civilization with it. Yet
the potential remains, and although there are daunting obstacles ahead,
collectively human beings have the powers of creativity and imagination
that could allow us to build a very different sort of society, one that is sus-
tainable and works not against nature, but in harmony with it, and in
which technology enriches our lives, rather than oppressing us. If we can
develop such a type of civilization, then who knows what marvels we
might expect to achieve in the future, not only on Earth, but also on more
distant planetary destinations. If so, all such possible futures will have
come from the human brain, which for an object weighing only a kilo-
gram and a half, and with the appearance and consistency of cold por-
ridge, is surely the most remarkable thing of all.
435
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
GLOS SA RY
action potential Occurs when there is a cerebellum Brain structure located at the
significant increase in the electrical activity top of the brain stem that coordinates the
along the membrane of a neuron. brain’s instructions for skilled, repetitive
Associated with neurons passing electro- movements and helps maintain balance
chemical messages down the axon, and posture. Recent research suggests it
releasing neurotransmitters to neigh- may also be involved in some emotional
bouring cells in the synapse. and cognitive processes.
amygdala Part of the brain’s limbic system. cerebrum Largest brain structure in
This primitive brain structure lies deep in humans and positioned over and around
the centre of the brain and is involved in most other brain structures. The cere-
emotional reactions, such as anger or fear, brum is divided into left and right hemi-
as well as emotionally charged memories. spheres, as well as specific areas called
astrocyte Star- shaped glial cell that sup- lobes that are associated with specialized
ports neurons, by helping to both feed and functions.
remove waste from the cell, and otherwise cortex Outer layer of the cerebrum.
modulates the activity of the neuron. Sometimes referred to as the cerebral
Astrocytes also play critical roles in brain cortex.
development and the creation of synapses. dendrites Short nerve fibres that project
axon Long, single nerve fibre that transmits from a neuron, which generally receive
messages, via electrochemical impulses, messages from the axons of other neurons
from the body of the neuron to dendrites and relay them to the cell’s nucleus.
of other neurons, or directly to body tis- frontal lobe Front of the brain’s cerebrum,
sues such as muscles. beneath the forehead. It is the area of the
brain imaging Various techniques, such as brain associated with higher cognitive
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and processes such as decision- making,
positron emission tomography (PET), that reasoning, social cognition, and planning,
make it possible to capture images of as well as motor control.
brain tissue and structure and reveal glial cells Supporting cells of the central
which brain regions are associated with nervous system. They may contribute to
behaviours or activities. the transmission of nerve impulses and
brain waves Rhythmic patterns of neural play a critical role in protecting and nour-
activity in the central nervous system. ishing neurons.
These waves are also sometimes referred hippocampus Primitive brain structure
to as neural oscillations. located deep in the brain that is particu-
cell body Central part of the nerve cell larly important for memory and learning.
that contains the nucleus of the neuron. hypothalamus Small structure located at
The axon and dendrites connect to this the base of the brain where signals from
part of the cell. It is also known as the the brain and the body’s hormonal system
soma. interact.
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
glossa ry
limbic system Group of evolutionarily older released into the synaptic cleft when a
brain structures that encircle the top of the nerve impulse reaches the end of an axon.
brain stem and play complex roles in emo- optogenetics Innovative neuroscientific
tions, instincts, and appetitive behaviours. techn
ique that uses light to turn genetic
long- term potentiation (LTP) Persistent ally modified neurons on and off at will in
strengthening of a synapse with increased living animals.
use, thought to underlie learning and plasticity Brain’s capacity to change and
memory. adapt in response to developmental forces,
microglia Small, specialized glial cell that learning processes, injury, or ageing.
operates as the first line of immune prefrontal cortex Area of the cerebrum
defence in the central nervous system. located in the forward part of the frontal
midbrain Small part of the brain stem that lobe. It mediates many of the higher cog-
plays an important role in movement as nitive processes such as planning, reason
well as auditory and visual processing. It is ing, and the ability to assess social
also referred to as the mesencephalon, situations in light of previous experience
myelin Fatty substance that encases most and personal knowledge.
nerve cell axons. It helps to insulate and pro- striatum Small group of subcortical struc-
tect the nerve fibre and effectively speeds tures, including the caudate nucleus, puta-
up the transmission of nerve impulses. men, and nucleus accumbens, located in
neurogenesis Production of new neurons the midbrain. These regions are impli-
by neural stem and progenitor cells. Rapid cated in both movement and reward-
and widespread neurogenesis occurs in related behaviours.
the foetal brain in humans and other ani- synapse Junction where an axon approa
mals, but this process also occurs in adult ches another neuron’s dendrite. It is the
humans in the hippocampus and possibly point at which nerve-to-nerve communi-
other brain regions. cation occurs. Nerve impulses traveling
neuron Basic unit of the central nervous down the axon reach the synapse and
system. It is responsible for the transmis- release neurotransmitters into the synap-
sion of nerve impulses. Unlike any other tic cleft, the gap between neurons.
cell in the body, a neuron consists of a thalamus Brain structure located at the top
central cell body as well as several of the brain stem. It acts as a two-way
threadlike branches called axons and relay station, sorting, processing, and
dendrites. directing signals from the spinal cord and
neurotransmitter Chemical that acts as a midbrain structures to the cerebrum, and
messenger between neurons and is from the cerebrum downwards.
438
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
E N DNOT E S
Introduction
1. C. Baraniuk, ‘It Took Centuries, But We Now Know the Size of the Universe’, BBC Earth
(13 June 2016), http://www.bbc.co.uk/earth/story/20160610-it-took-centuries-but-we-now-
know-the-size-of-the-universe.
2. Editorial, ‘The Human Brain Is the Most Complex Structure in the Universe. Let’s Do All We
Can to Unravel Its Mysteries’, The Independent (2 April 2014), https://www.independent.co.
uk/voices/editorials/the-human-brain-is-the-most-comple-structure-in-the-universe-let-
s-do-all-we-can-to-unravel-its-9233125.html.
3. J. Amos, ‘Voyager Probe “Leaves Solar System” ’, BBC (12 September 2013), https://www.bbc.
co.uk/news/science-environment-24026153.
4. P. Bazalgette, ‘We Have to Recognise the Huge Value of Arts and Culture to Society’, The
Guardian (27 April 2014), https://www.theguardian.com/culture/2014/apr/27/value-of-arts-
and-culture-to-society-peter-bazalgette.
5. A. Kirk, P. Scott, and J. Wilson, ‘World Mental Health Day: The Charts that Show that the
UK is in the Midst of a Mental Health Awakening’, The Telegraph (10 October 2017), https://
www.telegraph.co.uk/health-fitness/mind/world-mental-health-day-charts-show-uk-
midst-mental-health-awakening/.
6. A. MacDonald, ‘New Insights into Treatment-Resistant Depression’, Harvard Medical School
(9 December 2010), https://www.health.harvard.edu/blog/new-insights-into-treatment-
resistant-depression-20101209891.
7. A. Abbott, ‘Colours Light up Brain Structure’, Nature (31 October 2007), https://www.
nature.com/news/2007/071031/full/news.2007.209.html.
8. M. Z. Donahue, ‘New Clues to How Neanderthal Genes Affect Your Health’, National
Geographic (5 October 2017), https://news.nationalgeographic.com/2017/10/how-neanderthal-
genes-affect-human-health-dna-science/.
9. C. Bergland, ‘What Is the Human Connectome Project? Why Should You Care?’, Psychology
Today (27 November 2013), https://www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/the-athletes-way/
201311/what-is-the-human-connectome-project-why-should-you-care.
10. K. Deisseroth, ‘Optogenetics: Controlling the Brain with Light’, Scientific American (20
October 2010), https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/optogenetics-controlling/.
11. J. Parrington, Redesigning Life: How Genome Editing Will Transform the World (Oxford University
Press, 2016).
12. S. Jäkell and L. Dimou, ‘Glial Cells and Their Function in the Adult Brain: A Journey through
the History of Their Ablation’, Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 11:24 (2017).
13. J. Parrington, The Deeper Genome: Why There is More to the Human Genome than Meets the Eye
(Oxford University Press, 2015).
14. R. R. Kanherkar, N. Bhatia-Dey, and A. B. Csoka, ‘Epigenetics across the Human Lifespan’,
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology, 2:49 (2014).
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
440
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
36. D. Campbell, ‘Spike in Mental Health Patient Deaths Shows NHS “Struggling to Cope” ’, The
Guardian (26 January 2016), https://www.theguardian.com/society/2016/jan/26/rise-mental-
health-patient-deaths-nhs-struggling-to-cope.
37. Ibid.
38. Ibid.
39. J. Beattie, ‘Cuts Blamed for “Appalling” Rise in Mental Health Deaths’, The Mirror
(26 January 2016), https://www.mirror.co.uk/news/uk-news/cuts-blamed-appalling-rise-
mental-7248054.
40. K Griffiths, ‘Depressed Bradford Woman Took Own Life in France’, The Telegraph and Argus
(10 August 2016), http://www.thetelegraphandargus.co.uk/news/14673774.Depressed_
Bradford_woman_took_own_life_in_France/.
41. R. Spencer, ‘America’s Mental Health Care Crisis: Families Left to Fill the Void of a Broken
System’, The Guardian (27 May 2014), https://www.theguardian.com/world/2014/may/27/
sp-americas-mental-health-care-system-crisis.
42. M. Maruthappu, R. Ologunde, and A. Gunarajasingam, ‘Is Health Care a Right? Health
Reforms in the USA and their Impact Upon the Concept of Care’, Annals of Medicine Surgery
2:15–17 (2013).
43. Spencer, ‘America’s Mental Health Care Crisis’.
44. L. Szabo, ‘Cost of Not Caring: Nowhere to Go’, USA Today (12 January 2015), https://
eu.usatoday.com/story/news/nation/2014/05/12/mental-health-system-crisis/7746535/.
45. L. Szabo, ‘Psychiatric Beds Disappear Despite Growing Demand’, USA Today (12 May 2014),
https://eu.usatoday.com/story/news/nation/2014/05/12/disappearing-hospital-beds/
9003677/.
46. D. Brown, ‘Government Survey Finds that 5 Percent of Americans Suffer from a “Serious
Mental Illness” ’, Washingon Post (19 January 2012), https://www.washingtonpost.com/
national/health-science/government-survey-finds-that-5-percent-of-americans-suffer-
from-a-serious-mental-illness/2012/01/18/gIQAjp5h9P_story.html?utm_term=.
d3e60a4255d3.
47. J. Belluz, ‘Anthony Bourdain’s Death is One in a Growing Public Health Tragedy’, Vox (8 June
2018), https://www.vox.com/science-and-health/2018/6/8/17441330/anthony-bourdain-suicide-
rates-us-cdc.
48. K. Weir, ‘Worrying Trends in U.S. Suicide Rates’, American Psychological Association (March
2019), https://www.apa.org/monitor/2019/03/trends-suicide.
49. C. Koons, ‘Latest Suicide Data Show the Depth of U.S. Mental Health Crisis’, Bloomberg
(20 June 2019), https://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2019-06-20/latest-suicide-data-
show-the-depth-of-u-s-mental-health-crisis?fbclid=IwAR3wbuY3QJXOBm_gCS5fmGP_
LdvHSk4Sps9pqTUviiWNUcdgqu2Xn24QeF8.
50. Ibid.
51. Royal College of Psychiatrists, Mental Health Network, NHS Confederation, and London
School of Economics and Political Science, Mental Health and the Economic Downturn (2009),
https://www.base-uk.org/sites/default/files/%5Buser-raw%5D/11-06/op70.pdf.
52. J. Wertsch, Vygotsky and the Social Formation of Mind (Harvard University Press, 1985), p. 186.
53. A. Kozulin, Vygotsky’s Psychology: A Biography of Ideas (Harvester Wheatsheaf, 1990), p. 88.
54. Ibid.
55. Ibid.
56. P. Broks, ‘The Mystery of Consciousness’, Prospect (29 April 2007), https://www.
prospectmagazine.co.uk/magazine/themysteryofconsciousness.
441
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
442
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
24. I. Miko, ‘Gregor Mendel and the Principles of Inheritance’, Nature Education, 1:134 (2008).
25. R. A. Lewis, ‘Genetic Checkup: Lessons from Huntington Disease and Cystic Fibrosis’,
The Scientist (19 October 2003), https://www.the-scientist.com/research/a-genetic-checkup-
lessons-from-huntington-disease-and-cystic-fibrosis-50868.
26. M. Pagel, Wired for Culture (Penguin, 2012), p. 81.
27. H. Lacey, ‘What Men and Women Do Best’, The Independent (21 June 1998), https://www.
independent.co.uk/life-style/what-men-and-women-do-best-1166324.html.
28. L. Hoopes, ‘Introduction to the Gene Expression and Regulation Topic Room’, Nature
Education, 1:160 (2008).
29. F. Emmert-Streib, M. Dehmer, and B. Haibe-Kains, ‘Gene Regulatory Networks and Their
Applications: Understanding Biological and Medical Problems in Terms of Networks’,
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology, 2:38 (2014).
30. B. Dennis, ‘How Do You Tell a Kid He’s Not Going to Grow Up?’, Washington Post (6 May
2015), https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/to-your-health/wp/2015/05/06/how-do-
you-tell-a-kid-hes-not-going-to-grow-up/?utm_term=.95ec512cf236.
31. P. Forbes, ‘How Close Are We to a Cure for Huntington’s?’, The Independent (19 March 2018),
https://www.independent.co.uk/news/long_reads/how-close-are-we-to-a-cure-for-
huntington-s-a8235921.html.
32. I. Sample, ‘Scientists Identify 40 Genes That Shed New Light on Biology of Intelligence’, The
Guardian (22 May 2017), https://www.theguardian.com/science/2017/may/22/scientists-
uncover-40-genes-iq-einstein-genius.
33. N. Angier, ‘Report Suggests Homosexuality Is Linked to Genes’, The New York Times (16 July
1993), https://www.nytimes.com/1993/07/16/us/report-suggests-homosexuality-is-linked-
to-genes.html.
34. K. O’Riordan, ‘The Life of the Gay Gene: From Hypothetical Genetic Marker to Social
Reality’, Journal of Sex Research, 49:362–68 (2012).
35. S. Pinker, How the Mind Works (Norton, 2009).
36. S. Rose, ‘How the Mind Works by Steven Pinker’, New Scientist (24 January 1998), https://www.
newscientist.com/article/mg15721185-500-how-the-mind-works-by-steven-pinker/.
443
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
6. ‘Homo neanderthalensis’, Smithsonian National Museum of Natural History (10 January 2020), http://
humanorigins.si.edu/evidence/human-fossils/species/homo-neanderthalensis.
7. P. Madison, ‘Neanderthals and Giant’s Bones’, Fossil History (30 August 2016), https://
fossilhistory.wordpress.com/2016/08/30/neanderthals-and-giants/.
8. B. Trigger, ‘Comment on Tobias, Piltdown, the Case against Keith’, Current Anthropology,
33:274–5 (1992).
9. Ibid.
10. F. Engels, ‘The Part Played by Labour in the Transition from Ape to Man’, Die Neue Zeit
(1895), https://www.marxists.org/archive/marx/works/1876/part-played-labour/.
11. Ibid.
12. R. Gray, ‘The Mystery of Piltdown Man Is Solved: Charles Dawson, Who Found the Fake
Human “Fossil”, Was Probably behind the Hoax’, Daily Mail (12 August 2016), http://www.
dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-3731455/The-mystery-Piltdown-Man-solved-Charles-
Dawson-fake-human-fossil-probably-hoax.html.
13. Ibid.
14. C. K. Brain, ‘The Day the Missing Link Turned up in the Post’, The Guardian (18 December
2003), https://www.theguardian.com/science/2003/dec/18/science.research2.
15. K. Wong, ‘Human Origins’, Scientific American (24 November 2014), https://blogs.scientifi-
camerican.com/observations/40-years-after-lucy-the-fossil-that-revolutionized-the-
search-for-human-origins/.
16. J. Hawks, ‘Human Evolution Is More a Muddy Delta Than a Branching Tree’, Aeon (8
February 2016), https://aeon.co/ideas/human-evolution-is-more-a-muddy-delta-than-a-
branching-tree.
17. C. Stringer, ‘Human Evolution and Biological Adaptation in the Pleistocene’, in R.A. Foley
(ed.), Hominid Evolution and Community Ecology (Academic Press, 1984), p. 53.
18. M. Holloway, ‘Mary Leakey: Unearthing History’, Scientific American (16 December 1996),
https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/mary-leakey-unearthing-hi/.
19. Z. Zorich, ‘Which Came First, Humans or Tools?’, The New Yorker (20 May 2015), https://
www.newyorker.com/tech/elements/which-came-first-humans-or-tools.
20. C. Q. Choi, ‘Human Evolution: The Origin of Tool Use’, Live Science (11 November 2009),
https://www.livescience.com/7968-human-evolution-origin-tool.html.
21. Ibid.
22. Ibid.
23. Ibid.
24. Ibid.
25. C. Harman, ‘Engels and the Origins of Human Society’, International Socialism, 2:65 (Winter
1994) http://pubs.socialistreviewindex.org.uk/isj65/harman.htm.
26. Ibid.
27. Y. Anwar, ‘World’s Oldest Butchery Tools Gave Evolutionary Edge to Human Communication’,
Berkeley News (13 January 2015), http://news.berkeley.edu/2015/01/13/stone-age-tools/.
28. Ibid.
29. M. Balter, ‘Striking Patterns: Study Suggests Tool Use and Language Evolved Together’,
Wired (3 September 2013), https://www.wired.com/2013/09/tools-and-language/.
30. Ibid.
31. D. Guess, W. Sailor, G. Rutherford, and D. M. Baer, ‘An Experimental Analysis of Linguistic
Development: The Productive Use of the Plural Morpheme’, Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 1:297–306 (1968).
444
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
32. B. Carey, ‘Washoe, a Chimp of Many Words, Dies at 42’, New York Times (1 November 2007),
https://www.nytimes.com/2007/11/01/science/01chimp.html.
33. Ibid.
34. E. Wayman, ‘Six Talking Apes’, Smithsonian Magazine (11 August 2011) https://www.
smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/six-talking-apes-48085302/.
35. S. Keegan, ‘Gorilla Befriended by Robin Williams “Cried in Mourning” When She Learnt of
Star’s Death’, The Mirror (13 August 2014), https://www.mirror.co.uk/3am/celebrity-news/
gorilla-befriended-robin-williams-cried-4043840.
36. D. C. Palmer, ‘On Chomsky’s Appraisal of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior: A Half Century of
Misunderstanding’, The Behavior Analyst, 29:253–67 (2006).
37. Ibid.
38. H. Terrace, ‘Can Chimps Converse?: An Exchange’, New York Review of Books (24 November
2011), https://www.nybooks.com/articles/2011/11/24/can-chimps-converse-exchange/.
39. J. C. Hu, ‘The Troubling World of Koko the Gorilla and the Decline of Ape Language
Research’, National Post (21 August 2014), https://nationalpost.com/news/the-troubling-
world-of-koko-the-gorilla-and-the-decline-of-ape-language-research.
40. Ibid.
41. Ibid.
42. Ibid.
43. V. Gill, ‘Chimpanzee Language: Communication Gestures Translated’, BBC News (4 July
2014), https://www.bbc.com/news/science-environment-28023630.
44. Ibid.
45. Ibid.
46. F. Y. Lin, ‘A Refutation of Universal Grammar’, Lingua, 193: 1–22 (2017).
445
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
9. S. Wolpert, ‘’Tis better to give than to receive?’, UCLA Newsroom (9 November 2011), https://
www.uclahealth.org/tis-better-to-give-than-to-receive.
10. Ibid.
11. E. Esfahani Smith, ‘Social Connection Makes a Better Brain’, The Atlantic (29 October 2013),
https://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2013/10/social-connection-makes-a-better-
brain/280934/.
12. A. Woodward, ‘With a Little Help from My Friends’, Scientific American (1 May 2017), https://
www.scientificamerican.com/article/with-a-little-help-from-my-friends/.
13. S. Toulmin, ‘The Mozart of Psychology’, New York Review of Books (28 September 1978),
https://www.nybooks.com/articles/1978/09/28/the-mozart-of-psychology/.
14. J. Reader, ‘What Made the Animal Human’, New Scientist, 29 July:314 (1982).
15. L. S. Vygotsky, Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes (Harvard
University Press, 1976), p. 127.
16. S. McLeod, ‘Jean Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development’, Simply Psychology (2018),
https://www.simplypsychology.org/piaget.html.
17. Ibid.
18. I. Sample, ‘Comparing Piaget and Vygotsky’, University of Iowa (28 October 2004), https://
www2.education.uiowa.edu/html/eportfolio/tep/07p075folder/Piaget_Vygotsky.htm.
19. S. McLeod, ‘Lev Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory’, Simply Psychology (2020), https://www.
simplypsychology.org/vygotsky.html.
20. Sample, ‘Comparing Piaget and Vygotsky’.
21. Ibid.
22. R. van der Veer and J. Valsiner, Understanding Vygotsky: A Quest for Synthesis (Blackwell, 1991),
pp. 262–6.
23. J. Parrington, ‘In Perspective: Valentin Voloshinov’, International Socialism, 2:75 (July 1997),
https://www.marxists.org/history/etol/newspape/isj2/1997/isj2-075/parrington.htm.
24. A. Kozulin, Vygotsky’s Psychology: A Biography of Ideas (Harvester Wheatsheaf, 1990), pp. 158–9.
25. Ibid.
26. Van der Veer and Valsiner, Understanding Vygotsky.
27. A. Blunden, Concepts: A Critical Approach (Brill, 2012), p. 242.
28. Ibid.
29. Wertsch, J. V., Vygotsky and the Social Formation of Mind (Harvard University Press, 1985), p. 103.
30. Ibid.
31. F. Newman and L. Holzman, Lev Vygotsky: Revolutionary Scientist (Routledge, 1993), pp. 80–1.
32. Ibid.
33. Ibid.
34. M. Holborow, The Politics of English: A Marxist View of Language (Sage Publications, 1999), p. 24.
35. C. Brandist, in C. Brandist, D. Sheperherd, and G. Tihanov (eds), The Bakhtin Circle
(Manchester University Press, 2004), p. 103.
36. V. N. Voloshinov, Freudianism: A Critical Sketch (Indiana University Press, 1976), p. 21.
37. V. N. Voloshinov, Marxism and the Philosophy of Language (Harvard University Press, 1986), p. 19.
38. Ibid., 13.
39. Ibid., 19.
40. Ibid., 81.
41. Ibid., 102.
42. J. V. Wertsch, Voices of the Mind: A Sociocultural Approach to Mediated Action (Harvester
Wheatsheaf, 1991), pp. 53–6.
446
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
43. M. Epstein, ‘Hyperauthorship in Mikhail Bakhtin: The Primary Author and Conceptual
Personae’, Russian Journal of Communication 1:280–90 (2008).
44. M. M. Bakhtin, The Dialogic Imagination: Four Essays by M.M. Bakhtin (University of Texas
Press, 1981), p. 262.
45. M. M. Bakhtin, Speech Genres and Other Late Essays (Texas University Press, 1986), p. 78.
46. Ibid., 87.
47. C. L. Briggs and R. Bauman, ‘Genre, Intertextuality, and Social Power’, Journal of Linguistic
Anthropology, 2:131–72 (1992).
48. Bakhtin, The Dialogic Imagination, pp. 293–4.
49. A. Larrain and A. Haye, ‘The Discursive Nature of Inner Speech’, Theory and Psychology,
22:3–22 (2012).
50. Ibid.
51. J. F. Ehrich, ‘Vygotskian Inner Speech and the Reading Process’, Australian Journal of
Educational and Developmental Psychology, 6:12–25 (2006).
52. B. Alderson-Day and C. Fernyhough, ‘Inner Speech: Development, Cognitive Functions,
Phenomenology, and Neurobiology’, Psychological Bulletin, 141: 931–65 (2015).
53. L. S. Vygotsky, Thought and Language (Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 1986), pp. 235–49.
54. J. Beck, ‘The Running Conversation in Your Head’, The Atlantic (23 November 2016), https://
www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2016/11/figuring-out-how-and-why-we-talk-to-
ourselves/508487/.
55. Alderson-Day and Fernyhough, ‘Inner Speech: Development’.
56. Beck, ‘The Running Conversation in Your Head’.
57. A. Dana, R. R. Shirazi, and F. Z. Jalili, ‘The Effect of Instruction and Motivational Self Talk
on Performance and Retention of Discrete and Continuous Motor Tasks’, Australian Journal
of Basic and Applied Sciences, 5, 312–15 (2011).
58. S. Vickers, ‘The Voices Within: The History and Science of How We Talk to Ourselves by Charles
Fernyhough—review’, The Guardian (24 April 2016), https://www.theguardian.com/
books/2016/apr/24/voices-within-history-science-how-we-talk-to-ourselves-charles-
fernyhough-review.
59. Beck, ‘The Running Conversation in Your Head’.
60. C. Fernyhough, ‘Do Deaf People Hear an Inner Voice?’, Psychology Today (24 January 2014),
https://www.psychologytoday.com/intl/blog/the-voices-within/201401/do-deaf-people-
hear-inner-voice?amp.
61. Ibid.
447
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
448
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
33. L. Sanders, ‘Brain Waves May Focus Attention and Keep Information Flowing’, Science News
(13 March 2018), https://www.sciencenews.org/article/brain-waves-may-focus-attention-
and-keep-information-flowing.
34. Ibid.
35. Ibid.
36. Ibid.
37. M. A. Hofman, ‘Evolution of the Human Brain: When Bigger is Better’, Frontiers in
Neuroanatomy, 8:15 (2014).
38. A. Hawks, ‘How Has the Human Brain Evolved?’, Scientific American (1 July 2013), https://
www.scientificamerican.com/article/how-has-human-brain-evolved/.
39. C. C. Sherwood, F. Subiaul, and T. W. Zawidzki, ‘A Natural History of the Human Mind: Tracing
Evolutionary Changes in Brain and Cognition’, Journal of Anatomy, 212:426–54 (2008).
40. Ibid.
41. Ibid.
42. Hawks, ‘How Has the Human Brain Evolved?’
43. Sherwood, Subiaul, and Zawidzki, ‘A Natural History of the Human Mind’.
44. Ibid.
45. A. Wnuk, ‘Brain Evolution: Searching for What Makes Us Human’, Brain Facts (9 April 2015),
https://www.brainfacts.org/Brain-Anatomy-and-Function/Evolution/2015/Brain-
Evolution-Searching-for-What-Makes-Us-Human.
46. J. Stromberg, ‘Science Shows Why You’re Smarter Than a Neanderthal’, Smithsonian
Magazine (12 March 2013), https://www.smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/science-
shows-why-youre-smarter-than-a-neanderthal-1885827/.
47. Ibid.
48. C. Q. Choi, ‘Brain Scans Reveal Difference between Neanderthals and Us’, Live Science
(8 November 2010), https://www.livescience.com/11078-brain-scans-reveal-difference-
neanderthals.html.
49. S. Jacob, ‘The Brains of Neanderthals and Modern Humans Developed Differently’, Max
Planck Institute (8 November 2010), https://www.mpg.de/623578/pressRelease201011021.
50. Stromberg, ‘Science Shows Why You’re Smarter Than a Neanderthal’.
51. J. L. Smit, ‘Top 10 Remarkable Traits Neanderthals Have In Common with Modern Humans’,
List Verse (20 April 2017), https://listverse.com/2017/04/20/top-10-remarkable-traits-
neanderthals-have-in-common-with-humans/.
52. Wnuk, ‘Brain Evolution: Searching for What Makes Us Human’.
53. Ibid.
54. H. Glowacka, ‘Babies, Birth and Brains’, Ask An Anthropologist (2018), https://
askananthropologist.asu.edu/stories/babies-birth-and-brains.
55. Wnuk, ‘Brain Evolution: Searching for What Makes Us Human’.
56. Ibid.
57. T. Newman, ‘White Matter: The Brain’s Flexible But Underrated Superhighway’, Medical
News Today (16 August 2017), https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/318966.php.
58. Ibid.
59. R. A. Hoglund and A. A. Maghazachi, ‘Multiple Sclerosis and the Role of Immune Cells’,
World Journal of Experimental Medicine, 4:27–37 (2014).
60. N. Muhlert, V. Sethi, L. Cipolotti, H. Haroon, G. J. Parker, T. Yousry, C. Wheeler-Kingshott,
D. Miller, M. Ron, and D. Chard, ‘The Grey Matter Correlates of Impaired Decision-Making
in Multiple Sclerosis’, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 86:530–6 (2015).
449
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
450
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
21. T. Ghose, ‘ “Big Brain” Gene Found in Humans, Not Chimps’, Live Science (26 February 2015),
https://www.livescience.com/49960-human-big-brain-gene-found.html.
22. Ibid.
23. Ibid.
24. K. Boes, ‘Researchers Discover Human-Specific Gene for Building a Bigger Brain’, Sci Tech
Daily (17 January 2019), https://scitechdaily.com/researchers-discover-human-specific-
gene-for-building-a-bigger-brain/.
25. R. Feltman, ‘Scientists Pinpoint a Gene Regulator That Makes Human Brains Bigger’,
Washington Post (19 February 2015), https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/speaking-of-
science/wp/2015/02/19/scientists-pinpoint-a-gene-regulator-that-makes-human-brains-
bigger/?utm_term=.e8c44bbc577b.
26. Ibid.
27. K. Miller, ‘How Our Ancient Brains Are Coping in the Age of Digital Distraction’, Discover
Magazine (20 April 2020), https://www.discovermagazine.com/mind/how-our-ancient-
brains-are-coping-in-the-age-of-digital-distraction?fbclid=IwAR2lzqxOphb8UOwOr14N
dZHAY_JdWEBzNBrTe70zpTKiYUMoznCHqVbu5M0.
28. D. Shultz, ‘Humans Can Outlearn Chimps Thanks to More Flexible Brain Genetics’,
Science (16 November 2015), http://www.sciencemag.org/news/2015/11/humans-can-outlearn-
chimps-thanks-more-flexible-brain-genetics.
29. Ibid.
30. Ibid.
31. ‘Lifelong Learning is Made Possible by Recycling of Histones, Study Says’, The Rockefeller
University (1 July 2015), https://www.rockefeller.edu/news/9722-lifelong-learning-is-made-
possible-by-recycling-of-histones-study-says/.
32. Ibid.
33. ‘Transposons Stir in Germline, but Small RNAs Still Them’, GEN News (3 November 2017),
https://www.genengnews.com/gen-news-highlights/transposons-stir-in-germline-but-
small-rnas-still-them/81255130.
34. K. Clancy, ‘The Strangers in Your Brain’, The New Yorker (17 October 2015), https://www.
newyorker.com/tech/elements/the-strangers-in-your-brain.
35. Ibid.
36. Salk Institute, ‘Scientists Discover Protein Factories Hidden in Human Jumping
Genes’, Science Daily (22 October 2015), https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/10/
151022124518.htm.
37. Ibid.
38. S. Makin, ‘Scientists Surprised to Find No Two Neurons Are Genetically Alike’, Scientific
American (3 May 2017), https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/scientists-surprised-to-
find-no-two-neurons-are-genetically-alike/.
39. Cell Press, ‘ “Brain-Only” Mutation Causes Epileptic Brain Size Disorder’, Science Daily
(11 April 2012), https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/04/120411132055.htm.
40. Makin, ‘No Two Neurons Are Genetically Alike’.
41. ‘Exosomes: The FedEx of the Nervous System?’, Alzforum (12 December 2014), https://www.
alzforum.org/news/conference-coverage/exosomes-fedex-nervous-system.
42. Ibid.
43. Ibid.
44. Ibid.
451
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
452
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
25. Ibid.
26. M. Costandi, ‘An Activity Map of the Whole Zebrafish Brain’, The Guardian (18 March 2013),
https://www.theguardian.com/science/neurophilosophy/2013/mar/18/an-activity-map-of-
the-whole-zebrafish-brain.
27. Y. Tsuruwaka, E. Shimada, K. Tsutsui, and T. Ogawa, ‘Ca(2+) Dynamics in Zebrafish
Morphogenesis’, Peer Journal, 5:e2894 (2017).
28. Ibid.
29. J. Liu, Y. Zhou, X. Qi, J. Chen, W. Chen, G. Qiu, Z. Wu, and N. Wu, ‘CRISPR/Cas9 in
Zebrafish: An Efficient Combination for Human Genetic Diseases Modeling’, Human
Genetics, 136:1–12 (2017).
30. A. Doyle, M. P. McGarry, N. A. Lee, and J. J. Lee, ‘The Construction of Transgenic and Gene
Knockout/Knockin Mouse Models of Human Disease’, Transgenic Research, 21:327–49 (2012).
31. A. C. Komor, A. H. Badran, and D. R. Liu, ‘CRISPR-Based Technologies for the Manipulation
of Eukaryotic Genomes’, Cell, 168:20–36 (2017).
32. J. Wright, ‘Monkey Model Reveals New Role for Top Autism Gene’, Spectrum (11 September
2017), https://www.spectrumnews.org/news/monkey-model-reveals-new-role-top-autism-
gene/.
33. Ibid.
34. I. Johnstone, ‘Artificial “Embryo” Created for First Time in Historic Breakthrough’,
The Independent (2 March 2017), https://www.independent.co.uk/news/science/embryo-lab-
creation-scientists-life-cells-lab-breakthrough-a7608446.html.
35. Ibid.
36. I. Hyun, A. Wilkerson, and J. Johnston, ‘Embryology Policy: Revisit the 14-Day Rule’, Nature,
533:169–71 (2016).
37. Ibid.
38. S. Gregory, ‘Human “Mini-brains” in Lab Mimic Development and Disease’, Bio News
(2 May 2017), https://www.bionews.org.uk/page_95978.
39. S. Fan, ‘Bizarre Mini Brains Offer a Fascinating New Look at the Brain’, Singularity Hub (16
May 2017), https://singularityhub.com/2017/05/16/bizarre-mini-brains-offer-a-fascinating-
new-look-at-the-brain/.
40. B. Bae and C. A. Walsh, ‘What Are Mini-Brains?’, Science, 342:200–1 (2013).
41. J. Rennie, ‘Mini-Brains Go Modular’, Quanta Magazine (9 August 2017), https://www.
quantamagazine.org/mini-brains-go-modular-20170809/.
42. Ibid.
43. Ibid.
44. J. Cepelewicz, ‘The Oldest Mini-Brains Have Lifelike Young Cells’, Quanta Magazine (29
August 2017), https://www.quantamagazine.org/the-oldest-mini-brains-have-lifelike-young-
cells-20170829/.
45. Ibid.
46. S. Kane, ‘Giant Heads Nearly Killed our Ancestors but Human Immaturity Saved us’, Business
Insider (11 April 2016), https://www.businessinsider.com/human-head-hips-evolution-2016-
4?IR=T.
47. C. Barras, ‘The Real Reasons Why Childbirth is So Painful and Dangerous’, BBC (22
December 2016), http://www.bbc.com/earth/story/20161221-the-real-reasons-why-childbirth-
is-so-painful-and-dangerous (2016).
48. B. Hrvoj-Mihic, T. Bienvenu, L. Stefanacci, A. R. Muotri, and K. Semendeferi, ‘Evolution,
Development, and Plasticity of the Human Brain: From Molecules to Bones’, Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience, 7:707 (2013).
453
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
49. L. S. Vygotsky, in The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky, Vol. 4, ed R. W. Rieber (Plenum Press,
1997), p. 223.
50. Ibid., 106.
51. L. S. Vygotsky, Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes (Harvard
University Press, 1978), p. 39.
52. Ibid.
53. A. Kozulin, Vygotsky’s Psychology: A Biography of Ideas (Harvester Wheatsheaf, 1990), pp. 138–9.
54. Ibid.
454
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
20. N. R. Smalheiser, ‘The RNA-Centred View of the Synapse: Non-coding RNAs and
Synaptic Plasticity’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences,
369:20130504 (2014).
21. E. G. Jones, ‘Microcolumns in the Cerebral Cortex’, Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences USA, 97:5019–21 (2000).
22. Smalheiser, ‘The RNA-Centred View of the Synapse’.
23. J. H. Ross, ‘The Seahorse In Your Brain: Where Body Parts Got Their Names’, WBUR News
(16 December 2016), http://www.wbur.org/npr/505754756/the-seahorse-in-your-brain-
where-body-parts-got-their-names.
24. S. Shapin, ‘The Man Who Forgot Everything’, The New Yorker (14 October 2013), https://
www.newyorker.com/books/page-turner/the-man-who-forgot-everything.
25. Ibid.
26. Ibid.
27. Ibid.
28. D. Dieguez, ‘Remembering Henry Molaison’, Brain Blogger (3 March 2014), http://
brainblogger.com/2014/03/03/remembering-henry-molaison/.
29. D. Zalewski, ‘Life Lines’, The New Yorker (23 March 2015), https://www.newyorker.com/
magazine/2015/03/30/an-artist-with-amnesia.
30. M. D. Lemonick, ‘Living in the now’, Aeon (13 February 2017), https://aeon.co/essays/what-
amnesiac-patients-can-tell-us-about-how-memories-are-made.
31. Ibid.
32. Ibid.
33. A. Abbott and E. Callaway, ‘Nobel Prize for Decoding Brain’s Sense of Place’, Nature
(6 October 2014), https://www.nature.com/news/nobel-prize-for-decoding-brain-s-sense-
of-place-1.16093.
34. Ibid.
35. J. Goldhill, ‘The “Jennifer Aniston Neuron” is the Foundation of Compelling New Memory
Research’, Quartz (23 July 2016), https://qz.com/740481/the-jennifer-aniston-neuron-is-the-
foundation-of-compelling-new-memory-research/.
36. Ibid.
37. Ibid.
38. Ibid.
39. A. Trafton, ‘Neuroscientists Identify Brain Circuit Necessary for Memory Formation’,
MIT News (6 April 2017), http://news.mit.edu/2017/neuroscientists-identify-brain-circuit-
necessary-memory-formation-0406.
40. Ibid.
41. N. Ng, ‘ “Heart of Brain” Breakthrough May Aid Treatment of Disorders, Hong Kong
Scientists Say’, South China Morning Post (18 September 2017), https://www.scmp.com/news/
hong-kong/health-environment/article/2111678/heart-brain-breakthrough-may-aid-
treatment.
42. Goldhill, ‘The “Jennifer Aniston Neuron” ’.
43. Ng, ‘ “Heart of Brain” ’.
44. B. Rasch and J. Born, ‘About Sleep’s Role in Memory’, Physiology Reviews, 93:681–766 (2013).
45. Institute for Basic Science, ‘Controlling Memory by Triggering Specific Brain Waves
during Sleep’, Science Daily (6 July 2017), https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2017/07/
170706121204.htm.
46. Ibid.
455
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
47. D. Orenstein, ‘Rhythmic Interactions between Cortical Layers Underlie Working Memory’,
MIT News (15 January 2018), http://news.mit.edu/2018/rhythmic-interactions-cortical-lay-
ers-control-working-memory-0115.
48. Ibid.
49. Ibid.
50. Y. Bhattacharjee, ‘The First Year’, National Geographic (January 2015), https://www.
nationalgeographic.com/magazine/2015/01/baby-brains-development-first-year/.
51. Ibid.
52. Ibid.
53. Ibid.
456
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
19. D. A. McLennan, ‘The Concept of Co-option: Why Evolution Often Looks Miraculous’,
Evolution: Education & Outreach, 1:247–58 (2008).
20. M. C. Corballis, ‘Mirror Neurons and the Evolution of Language’, Brain & Language, 112:25–35
(2010).
21. Ibid.
22. G. F. Marcus and S. E. Fisher, ‘FOXP2 in Focus: What Can Genes Tell Us about Speech and
Language?’, Trends in Cognitive Science, 7:257–62 (2003).
23. I. Sample, ‘Neuroscientists Create “Atlas” Showing How Words Are Organised in the Brain’,
The Guardian (27 April 2016), https://www.theguardian.com/science/2016/apr/27/brain-
atlas-showing-how-words-are-organised-neuroscience.
24. Ibid.
25. Ibid.
26. P. Klass, ‘Language Lessons Start in the Womb’, New York Times (21 February 2017), https://
www.nytimes.com/2017/02/21/well/family/language-lessons-start-in-the-womb.html.
27. Ibid.
28. R. Nuzzo, ‘Babies’ Brains May Be Tuned to Language before Birth’, Nature (25 February 2013),
https://www.nature.com/news/babies-brains-may-be-tuned-to-language-before-
birth-1.12489.
29. Ibid.
30. G. Miller, ‘Pioneering Study Images Activity in Fetal Brains’, Science (13 January 2017), http://
science.sciencemag.org/content/355/6321/117.2.
31. Ibid.
32. J. Ducharme, ‘Why It’s So Hard to Learn Another Language after Childhood’, Time (2 May
2018), http://time.com/5261446/language-critical-period-age/.
33. Ibid.
34. N. J. Ramirez, ‘Why the Baby Brain Can Learn Two Languages at the Same Time’, The
Conversation (15 April 2016), https://theconversation.com/why-the-baby-brain-can-learn-
two-languages-at-the-same-time-57470.
35. J. Palermo, ‘Infant Language Learning Linked to Social Interaction’, Space Coast Daily
(16 July 2016), http://spacecoastdaily.com/2016/07/infant-language-learning-linked-to-social-
interaction/.
36. T. Ghose, ‘Baby Talk: Infants May Practice Words in Their Minds’, Live Science (14 July 2014),
https://www.livescience.com/46789-baby-brains-practice-language.html.
37. Y. Bhattacharjee, ‘The First Year’, National Geographic (January 2015), https://www.
nationalgeographic.com/magazine/2015/01/baby-brains-development-first-year/.
38. Ibid.
39. Ibid.
40. Ibid.
41. Ibid.
42. Ibid.
43. O. M. Lourenço, ‘Piaget and Vygotsky: Many Resemblances, and a Crucial Difference’,
New Ideas in Psychology 30:281–95 (2012).
44. C. Emerson, ‘The Outer Word and Inner Speech: Bakhtin, Vygotsky, and the Internalization
of Language’, Critical Inquiry, 10:245–64 (1983).
45. J. V. Wertsch, Vygotsky and the Social Formation of Mind (Harvard University Press, 1985), p. 116.
46. L. Kohlberg, J. Yaeger, and E. Hjertholm, ‘Private Speech: Four Studies and a Review of
Theories’, Child Development, 39:691–736 (1968).
457
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
47. J. Beck, ‘The Running Conversation in Your Head’, The Atlantic (23 November 2016), https://
www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2016/11/figuring-out-how-and-why-we-talk-to-
ourselves/508487/.
48. C. Fernyhough, The Voices Within: The History and Science of How We Talk to Ourselves (Profile
Books, 2016), p. 63.
49. V. N. Voloshinov, Marxism and the Philosophy of Language (Harvard University Press, 1973),
p. 38.
50. F. Jabr, ‘Catching Ourselves in the Act of Thinking’, Scientific American (18 November 2013),
https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/brainwaves/catching-ourselves-in-the-act-of-
thinking/.
51. J. V. Wertsch and P. Tulviste, ‘L. S. Vygotsky and Contemporary Developmental Psychology’,
Developmental Psychology, 28:548–57 (1992).
52. Voloshinov, Marxism and the Philosophy of Language, p. 39.
53. Ibid., 38.
54. Fernyhough, The Voices Within, p. 98.
55. Beck, ‘The Running Conversation in Your Head’.
458
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
15. P. Thagard, ‘Empathy in Literature and Film’, Psychology Today (18 May 2017), https://www.
psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/hot-thought/201705/empathy-in-literature-and-film.
16. K. G. Klinge, ‘Mapping Creativity in the Brain’, The Atlantic (21 March 2016), https://www.
theatlantic.com/science/archive/2016/03/the-driving-principles-behind-creativity/474621/.
17. Ibid.
18. Ibid.
19. C. Mackay, ‘Researchers Identify Specific Neurons That Distinguish between Reality and
Imagination’, Western University (1 June 2017), http://mediarelations.uwo.ca/2017/06/01/
researchers-identify-specific-neurons-distinguish-reality-imagination/.
20. Ibid.
21. A. García-Molina, ‘Phineas Gage and the Enigma of the Prefrontal Cortex’, Neurología,
27:370–5 (2012).
22. Ibid.
23. Ibid.
24. Ibid.
25. Z. Kotowicz, The Strange Case of Phineas Gage’, History of the Human Sciences, 20:115–31 (2007).
26. C. Gregoire, ‘Research Uncovers How and Where Imagination Occurs in the Brain’,
Huffington Post (18 September 2013), https://www.huffingtonpost.com/2013/09/17/imagination-
brain_n_3922136.html.
27. Ibid.
28. A. Piore, ‘Attention, Please: Earl Miller Wants to Make Us All Smarter’, Discover Magazine
(1 September 2016), http://discovermagazine.com/2016/oct/your-attention-please.
29. Ibid.
30. Gregoire, ‘Research Uncovers How and Where Imagination Occurs in the Brain’.
31. T. Lewis, ‘Human Brain: Facts, Functions & Anatomy’, Live Science (28 September 2018),
https://www.livescience.com/29365-human-brain.html.
32. L. Vandervert, ‘The Prominent Role of the Cerebellum in the Learning, Origin and
Advancement of Culture’, Cerebellum Ataxias 3:10 (2016).
33. Ibid.
34. Stanford University Medical Center, ‘Unexpected Brain Structures Tied to Creativity, and
to Stifling It’, Science Daily (28 May 2015), https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/05/
150528084158.htm.
35. Ibid.
36. Ibid.
37. Ibid.
38. A. Lipsett, ‘My Message: “Anybody Can Learn” ’, The Guardian (2 September 2008), https://
www.theguardian.com/education/2008/sep/02/languages.schools.
39. Vandervert, ‘The Prominent Role of the Cerebellum’.
40. C. Kalb, ‘What Makes a Genius?’, National Geographic (May 2017), https://www.
nationalgeographic.com/magazine/2017/05/genius-genetics-intelligence-neuroscience-
creativity-einstein/.
41. A. Cho, ‘Gravitational Waves, Einstein’s Ripples in Spacetime, Spotted for First Time’,
Science (11 February 2016), http://www.sciencemag.org/news/2016/02/gravitational-waves-
einstein-s-ripples-spacetime-spotted-first-time.
42. H. Cotter, ‘Michelangelo Is the Divine Star of the Must-See Show of the Season’, New York
Times (9 November 2017), https://www.nytimes.com/2017/11/09/arts/design/michelangelo-
review-metropolitan-museum-of-art-carmen-bambach.html.
459
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
43. D. Goldsmith and M. Bartusiak, E = Einstein: His Life, His Thought, and His Influence on Our
Culture (Barnes and Noble, 2008), p. 159.
44. W. Kremer, ‘The Strange Afterlife of Einstein’s Brain’, BBC News (17 April 2015), https://www.
bbc.com/news/magazine-32354300.
45. Ibid.
46. Ibid.
47. Ibid.
48. Ibid.
49. Ibid.
50. Ibid.
51. M. Neihart, ‘Creativity, the Arts, and Madness’, Roeper Review, 21:47–50 (1998).
52. G. Wilson, ‘Delusions and Grandeur’, Times Higher Education (1 November 2012), https://
www.timeshighereducation.com/features/delusions-and-grandeur/421624.article.
53. A. Fink, M. Benedek, H. F. Unterrainer, I. Papousek, and E. M. Weiss, ‘Creativity and
Psychopathology: Are There Similar Mental Processes Involved in Creativity and in
Psychosis-Proneness?’, Frontiers in Psychology, 5:1211 (2014).
54. E. Jaafe, ‘What Neuroscience Says about the Link between Creativity and Madness’, Fast
Company (14 November 2013), https://www.fastcompany.com/3021561/the-neuroscience-
linking-creativity-and-mental-illness.
55. Ibid.
460
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
14. L. Konkel, ‘What Is Dopamine?’, Everyday Health (10 August 2018), https://www.everydayhealth.
com/dopamine/.
15. T. R. Mhyre, J. T. Boyd, R. W. Hamill, and K. A., Maguire-Zeiss, ‘Parkinson’s Disease’,
Subcellular Biochemistry, 65:389–455 (2012).
16. K. Kelland, ‘Work on Brain’s Reward System Wins Scientists a Million Euro Reward’, Reuters
(6 March 2017), https://www.reuters.com/article/us-science-brain-idUSKBN16D1TF.
17. Ibid.
18. Ibid.
19. C. Bergland, ‘What Makes Us Human? Dopamine and the Cerebellum Hold Clues’,
Psychology Today (24 November 2017), https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/
the-athletes-way/201711/what-makes-us-human-dopamine-and-the-cerebellum-hold-clues.
20. Ibid.
21. C. Fernyhough, ‘Getting Vygotskian about Theory of Mind: Mediation, Dialogue, and the
Development of Social Understanding’, Developmental Review, 28:225–62 (2008).
22. L. S. Vygotsky, The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky (Plenum Press, 1987), p. 347.
23. G. R. Mesquita, ‘Vygotsky and the Theories of Emotions: In Search of a Possible Dialogue’,
Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25:809–16 (2012).
24. Ibid.
25. S. McLeod, ‘The Zone of Proximal Development and Scaffolding’, Psychology Today (2019),
https://www.simplypsychology.org/Zone-of-Proximal-Development.html.
26. Ibid.
27. H. Grimmett, The Practice of Teachers Professional Development: A Cultural-Historical Approach
(Springer, 2014), p. 13.
28. H. Mahn and V. John-Steiner, ‘The Gift of Confidence: A Vygotskian View of Emotions’, in
G. Wells and G. Claxton (eds), Learning for Life in the 21st Century: Sociocultural Perspectives on the
Future of Education (Wiley-Blackwell, 2002), pp. 46–58.
29. Ibid.
30. M. Nudelman and E. Brodwin, ‘The 5 Most Addictive Substances on the Planet, Ranked’,
Business Insider (2 December 2018), http://uk.businessinsider.com/most-addictive-drugs-
ranked-2016-10.
31. R. Gray, ‘No Wonder They Called It the Stone Age! Ancient Humans Were Taking Drugs—
Including Magic Mushrooms and Opium—up to 10,600 Years Ago’, Daily Mail (4 February
2015), http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-2939830/No-wonder-called-stone-
age-Ancient-humans-taking-drugs-including-magic-mushrooms-opium-10-600-years-
ago.html.
32. Ibid.
33. Ibid.
34. Ibid.
35. Ibid.
36. F. Smith, ‘How Science Is Unlocking the Secrets of Addiction’, National Geographic
(September 2017), https://www.nationalgeographic.com/magazine/2017/09/the-addicted-
brain/.
37. B. A. Mason, ‘Slaves to Dopamine and the Hijacking of Our Brains’, Huffington Post
(29 December 2017), https://www.huffingtonpost.com/entry/slaves-to-the-rhythm-how-
dopamine-hijacking-is-taking_us_5a453bd4e4b0d86c803c7583.
38. ‘Biology of Addiction’, National Institutes of Health News in Health (October 2015), https://
newsinhealth.nih.gov/2015/10/biology-addiction.
461
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
39. Ibid.
40. A. Matthews-King, ‘Alcohol and Tobacco by Far the Worst Drugs for Human Health, Global
Review Finds’, The Independent (11 May 2018), https://www.independent.co.uk/news/health/
alcohol-drinking-smoking-drugs-addictive-health-worst-bad-cannabis-cocaine-
amphetamines-opioids-a8345741.html.
41. Ibid.
42. D. F. Maron, ‘How Opioids Kill’, Scientific American (8 January 2018), https://www.
scientificamerican.com/article/how-opioids-kill/.
43. Ibid.
44. N. Vega, ‘Sean Parker on Facebook: We Created a Monster’, New York Post (9 November 2017),
https://nypost.com/2017/11/09/sean-parker-on-facebook-we-created-a-monster/.
45. D. Brooks, ‘How Evil Is Tech?’, The New York Times (20 November 2017), https://www.
nytimes.com/2017/11/20/opinion/how-evil-is-tech.html.
46. Ibid.
47. L. Kim, ‘Multitasking Is Killing Your Brain’, Huffington Post (6 December 2017), https://www.
huffingtonpost.com/larry-kim/multitasking-is-killing-your_b_9821244.html.
48. Ibid.
49. Ibid.
50. Ibid.
51. M. Gummer, ‘The Positive Impact of Social Media and Technology on Society’, Pi Media (6
February 2018), https://uclpimedia.com/online/the-positive-impact-of-social-media-and-
technology-on-society.
52. D. Spector, ‘How Beer Created Civilization’, Business Insider (26 December 2013), https://
www.businessinsider.com/how-beer-led-to-the-domestication-of-grain-2013-12.
53. Ibid.
54. Ibid.
55. J. P. Kahn, ‘How Beer Gave Us Civilization’, New York Times (15 March 2013), https://www.
nytimes.com/2013/03/17/opinion/sunday/how-beer-gave-us-civilization.html.
56. S. Sloat, ‘The “Stoned Ape” Theory Might Explain Our Extraordinary Evolution’, Inverse (14
July 2017), https://www.inverse.com/article/34186-stoned-ape-hypothesis.
57. A. Simon-Lewis, ‘Brain Scans Reveal How Psychedelic Drugs Create a “Higher State of
Consciousness” ’, Wired (20 April 2017), https://www.wired.co.uk/article/psychedelic-
drugs-found-to-cause-a-higher-state-of-consciousness.
58. E. Ekman and G. Agin.-Liebes, ‘Can a Psychedelic Experience Improve Your Life?’, Greater
Good Magazine (23 October 2019), https://greatergood.berkeley.edu/article/item/can_a_
psychedelic_experience_improve_your_life.
59. I. Sample, ‘Psychedelic Drugs Induce “Heightened State Of Consciousness”, Brain Scans
Show’, The Guardian (19 April 2017), https://www.theguardian.com/science/2017/apr/19/
brain-scans-reveal-mind-opening-response-to-psychedelic-drug-trip-lsd-ketamine-
psilocybin.
60. Ibid.
462
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
2. B. K. Keim, ‘Brain Scanners Can See Your Decisions Before You Make Them’, Wired (13 April
2008), https://www.wired.com/2008/04/mind-decision/.
3. B. Hughes, ‘Genius of the Modern World’, BBC (11 November 2019), https://www.bbc.co.uk/
programmes/b07ht3cd.
4. J. LeFanu, ‘Wrong Image of Freud Has Entered the Subconscious: Dr James LeFanu’, The
Telegraph (14 May 2006), https://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/uknews/1518310/Wrong-
image-of-Freud-has-entered-the-subconscious.html.
5. S. Pinker, ‘The Blank Slate Controversy’, New York Times (13 October 2002), https://www.
nytimes.com/2002/10/13/books/chapters/the-blank-slate.html.
6. R. J. Richards, ‘The Impact of German Romanticism on Biology in the Nineteenth Century’,
University of Chicago (2011), http://home.uchicago.edu/rjr6/articles/Idealism & biology.pdf.
7. M. Iseli, ‘Thomas De Quincey and the Cognitive Prospects of the Unconscious’, European
Romantic Review, 24:325–33 (2013).
8. T. C. Gannon, Immortal Sea, Eternal Mind: Romanticism and the Unconscious Psyche,
M.A. thesis, University of South Dakota (1979).
9. Ibid.
10. J. S. Hendrix, Unconscious Thought in Philosophy and Psychoanalysis (Palgrave Macmillan, 2015),
p. 19.
11. Ibid.
12. S. Austin, ‘Freud, Sigmund (1856–1939)’, Oxford Dictionary of National Biography (2011), http://
www.oxforddnb.com/view/10.1093/ref:odnb/9780198614128.001.0001/odnb -
9780198614128-e-55514.
13. Ibid.
14. S. McLeod, ‘Id, Ego and Superego’, Simply Psychology (2019), https://www.simplypsychology.
org/psyche.html.
15. Ibid.
16. K. Cherry, ‘The Interpretation of Dreams by Sigmund Freud: History and Significance’, Very Well
Mind (7 April 2020), https://www.verywellmind.com/the-interpretation-of-dreams-by-
sigmund-freud-2795855.
17. S. McLeod, ‘Psychoanalysis’, Simply Psychology (2019), https://www.simplypsychology.org/
psychoanalysis.html.
18. K. Cherry, ‘The Oedipus Complex in Children’, Very Well Mind (14 May 2020), https://www.
verywellmind.com/what-is-an-oedipal-complex-2795403.
19. Ibid.
20. B. Borrell, ‘Oedipus Wrecked: Study Supporting the Mother of All Psychological Complexes
Withdrawn’, Scientific American (24 February 2009), https://www.scientificamerican.com/
article/oedipus-complex-study-withdrawn/.
21. S. McLeod, ‘Psychosexual Stages’, Simply Psychology (2019), https://www.simplypsychology.
org/psychosexual.html.
22. C. Sieczkowski, ‘Unearthed Letter From Freud Reveals His Thoughts On Gay People’,
Huffington Post (7 December 2017), https://www.huffingtonpost.com/2015/02/18/sigmund-
freud-gay-cure-letter_n_6706006.html.
23. M. Billig, ‘The Dialogic Unconscious: Psycho-analysis, Discursive Psychology and the
Nature of Repression’, Massey University (2018), http://www.massey.ac.nz/~alock/
virtual/p-a4.htm.
24. R. Young, ‘Back to Bakhtin’, Cultural Critique, 2:71–92 (1985–6).
25. M. Meares, The Metaphor of Play: Origin and Breakdown of Personal Being (Routledge, 2002), p. 38.
463
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
464
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
independent.co.uk/voices/mental-health-help-how-change-awareness-causes-treat-
a8357741.html.
3. R. Bentall, ‘Mental Illness Is A Result of Misery, Yet Still We Stigmatise It’, The Guardian (26
February 2016), https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2016/feb/26/mental-illness-
misery-childhood-traumas.
4. D. Spence, ‘The Psychiatric Oligarchs Who Medicalise Normality’, British Medical Journal,
344:e3135 (2012).
5. B. J. Deacon, ‘The Biomedical Model of Mental Disorder: A Critical Analysis of Its Validity,
Utility, and Effects on Psychotherapy Research’, Clinical Psychology Review, 33:846–61 (2013).
6. R. Prasad, ‘Why US Suicide Rate Is On The Rise’, BBC News (11 June 2018), https://www.bbc.
co.uk/news/world-us-canada-44416727.
7. J. Christensen, ‘Why the US Has the Most Mass Shootings’, CNN (5 October 2017), https://
edition.cnn.com/2015/08/27/health/u-s-most-mass-shootings/index.html.
8. P. M. Visscher, N. R. Wray, Q. Zhang, P. Sklar, M. I. McCarthy, M. A Brown, and J. Yang, ‘10
Years of GWAS Discovery: Biology, Function, and Translation’, American Journal of Human
Genetics, 101:5–22 (2017).
9. M. Balter, ‘Schizophrenia’s Unyielding Mysteries’, Scientific American, 316:54–61 (2017).
10. O. D. Howes, R. McCutcheon, M. J. Owen, and R. M. Murray, ‘The Role of Genes, Stress, and
Dopamine in the Development of Schizophrenia’, Biological Psychiatry, 81:9–20 (2017).
11. Ibid.
12. P. J. Harrison, ‘Recent Genetic Findings in Schizophrenia and Their Therapeutic Relevance’,
Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29:85–96 (2015).
13. Ibid.
14. A. G. Diehl and A. P. Boyle, ‘Deciphering ENCODE’, Trends in Genetics, 32:238–49 (2016).
15. A. Park, ‘Junk DNA—Not So Useless After All’, Time (6 September 2012), http://healthland.
time.com/2012/09/06/junk-dna-not-so-useless-after-all/.
16. Harrison, ‘Recent Genetic Findings in Schizophrenia’.
17. E. Callaway, ‘New concerns raised over value of genome-wide disease studies’, Nature
News (15 June 2017), https://www.nature.com/news/new-concerns-raised-over-value-of-
genome-wide-disease-studies-1.22152.
18. Ibid.
19. Ibid.
20. P. M. Visscher, M. A. Brown, M. I. McCarthy, and J. Yang, ‘Five Years of GWAS Discovery’,
American Journal of Human Genetics, 90:7–24 (2012).
21. M. Wolfe, ‘What Is Crossing Over in Genetics?’, Sciencing (18 September 2018), https://
sciencing.com/crossing-over-genetics-6628252.html.
22. Ibid.
23. I. Lobo and K. Shaw, ‘Discovery and Types of Genetic Linkage’, Nature Education, 1:139 (2008).
24. J. M. Heather and B. Chain, ‘The Sequence of Sequencers: The History of Sequencing DNA’,
Genomics, 107:1–8 (2016).
25. C. Zhuo, W. Hou, C. Lin, L. Hu, and J. Li, ‘Potential Value of Genomic Copy Number
Variations in Schizophrenia’, Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 10:204 (2017).
26. T. P. Rutkowski, J. P. Schroeder, G. M. Gafford, S. T. Warren, D. Weinshenker, T. Caspary,
and J. G. Mulle, ‘Unraveling the Genetic Architecture of Copy Number Variants Associated
with Schizophrenia and Other Neuropsychiatric Disorders’, Journal of Neuroscience Research,
95:1144–60 (2017).
465
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
27. H. Devlin, ‘Radical New Approach to Schizophrenia Treatment Begins Trial’, The Guardian
(3 November 2017), https://www.theguardian.com/society/2017/nov/03/radical-new-
approach-to-schizophrenia-treatment-begins-trial.
28. Ibid.
29. Ibid.
30. D. Rudacille, ‘Attention Deficit, Autism Share Genetic Risk Factors’, Spectrum News (22
August 2011), https://www.spectrumnews.org/news/attention-deficit-autism-share-genetic-
risk-factors/.
31. M. G. Thompson, ‘R.D. Laing and Anti-Psychopathology: The Myth of Mental Illness
Redux’, Mad in America (26 October 2013), https://www.madinamerica.com/2013/10/
r-d-laing-anti-psychopathology-myth-mental-illness-redux/.
32. P. Gibney, ‘The Double Bind Theory: Still Crazy-Making after All These Years’, Psychotherapy
in Australia, 12:48–55 (2006).
33. Ibid.
34. J. Diski, ‘Rhythm Method’, London Review of Books, 16:20–1 (1994).
35. Ibid.
36. Ibid.
37. T. Friedman and N. N. Tin, ‘Childhood Sexual Abuse and the Development of Schizophrenia’,
Postgraduate Medical Journal, 83:507–8 (2007).
38. S. Bressert, ‘Schizophrenia Symptoms’, Psych Central (9 May 2020), https://psychcentral.
com/disorders/schizophrenia/schizophrenia-symptoms/.
39. L. S. Vygotsky, ‘Thought in Schizophrenia’, Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 31:1067 (1934).
40. A. Kozulin, Vygotsky’s Psychology: A Biography of Ideas (Harvester Wheatsheaf, 1990), pp. 226–8.
41. Ibid.
42. Ibid.
43. Ibid.
44. H. Werner and B. Kaplan, Symbol Formation (Wiley, 1963), p. 257.
45. Vygotsky, ‘Thought in Schizophrenia’.
46. A. Kozulin, Vygotsky’s Psychology: A Biography of Ideas (Harvester Wheatsheaf, 1990), p. 231.
47. Ibid., 229.
48. B. Rund, ‘Attention, Communication, and Schizophrenia’, Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine,
58:265–73 (1985).
49. Ibid.
50. Ibid.
51. L. S. Vygotsky, ‘The Development of Higher Forms of Attention in Childhood’, in
J.V. Wertsch, (ed.), The Concept of Activity in Soviet Psychology (M. E. Sharpe, 1981), pp. 193–4.
52. Rund, ‘Attention, Communication, and Schizophrenia’.
53. Ibid.
54. Ibid.
55. D. G. Smith, ‘The Placenta Is Now a Suspect In Heightening Schizophrenia Risk’, Scientific
American (28 May 2018), https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/the-placenta-is-now-
a-suspect-in-heightening-schizophrenia-risk/.
56. Ibid.
57. S. Peters, ‘Researcher Acknowledges His Mistakes in Understanding Schizophrenia’, Mad
in America (26 January 2017), https://www.madinamerica.com/2017/01/researcher-
acknowledges-mistakes-understanding-schizophrenia/.
58. S. McCarthy-Jones, ‘The Concept of Schizophrenia Is Coming to an End—Here’s Why’,
The Independent (4 September 2017), https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-
466
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
families/healthy-living/concept-schizophrenia-coming-to-end-psychology-genetics-
psychiatry-schizophrenia-a7925576.html.
59. Ibid.
60. C. Dodd, ‘Open Dialogue: The Radical New Treatment Having Life-Changing Effects on
People’s Mental Health’, The Independent (6 December 2015), https://www.independent.co.
uk/life-style/health-and-families/health-news/open-dialogue-the-radical-new-treatment-
having-life-changing-effects-on-peoples-mental-health-a6762391.html.
61. Ibid.
62. Ibid.
63. Ibid.
64. S. Malhotra and S. Sahoo, ‘Rebuilding the Brain with Psychotherapy’, Indian Journal of
Psychiatry, 59:411–19 (2017).
467
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
13. Ibid.
14. K. Griffiths, ‘Depressed Bradford Woman Took Own Life in France’, The Telegraph and Argus
(10 August 2016), http://www.thetelegraphandargus.co.uk/news/14673774.Depressed_
Bradford_woman_took_own_life_in_France/.
15. M. Smith, ‘Balancing Your Humors’, Psychology Today (2 November 2013), https://www.
psychologytoday.com/us/blog/short-history-mental-health/201311/balancing-your-humors.
16. L. Appignanesi, Mad, Bad And Sad: A History of Women and the Mind Doctors from 1800 to the
Present (Virago Press, 2009), pp. 136–7.
17. L. Fitzpatrick, ‘A Brief History of Antidepressants’, Time (7 January 2010), http://content.
time.com/time/health/article/0,8599,1952143,00.html.
18. Ibid.
19. Ibid.
20. A. Edemariam, ‘ “I Don’t Know Who I Am without It”: The Truth about Long-Term
Antidepressant Use’, The Guardian (6 May 2017), https://www.theguardian.com/society/
2017/may/06/dont-know-who-am-antidepressant-long-term-use.
21. Fitzpatrick, ‘A Brief History of Antidepressants’.
22. S. Mutalik, ‘A Short History of the SSRI’, Psychiatric Times (8 December 2014), http://www.
psychiatrictimes.com/psychopharmacology/short-history-ssri.
23. A. Khan and W. A. Brown, ‘Antidepressants versus Placebo in Major Depression: An
Overview’, World Psychiatry, 14:294–300 (2015).
24. S. Boseley, ‘Antidepressant Withdrawal Symptoms Severe, Says New Report’, The Guardian
(2 October 2018), https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/oct/02/antidepressant-
withdrawal-symptoms-severe-says-new-report.
25. S. Boseley, ‘Antidepressants: Is There a Better Way to Quit Them?’, The Guardian (22 April
2019), https://www.theguardian.com/lifeandstyle/2019/apr/22/antidepressants-is-there-a-
better-way-to-quit-them.
26. H. Arkowitz and S. O. Lilienfeld, ‘Is Depression Just Bad Chemistry?’, Scientific American (1
March 2014), https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/is-depression-just-bad-chemistry/.
27. J. Dryden, ‘Study Reverses Thinking on Genetic Links to Stress, Depression’, Washington
University School of Medicine in St Louis (4 April 2017), https://medicine.wustl.edu/news/study-
reverses-thinking-genetic-links-stress-depression/.
28. Ibid.
29. K. Oved, A. Morag, M. Pasmanik-Chor, M. Rehavi, N. Shomron, and D. Gurwitz, ‘Genome-
Wide Expression Profiling of Human Lymphoblastoid Cell Lines Implicates Integrin Beta-3
in the Mode of Action of Antidepressants’, Translational Psychiatry, 3:e313 (2013).
30. J. Chen, ‘How New Ketamine Drug Helps with Depression’, Yale Medicine (21 March 2019),
https://www.yalemedicine.org/stories/ketamine-depression/.
31. Ibid.
32. S. Knapton, ‘Depression Is a Physical Illness Which Could Be Treated with Anti-inflammatory
Drugs, Scientists Suggest’, The Telegraph (8 September 2017), https://www.telegraph.co.uk/
science/2017/09/08/depression-physical-illness-could-treated-anti-inflammatory/.
33. Ibid.
34. Ibid.
35. J. R. Thorpe, ‘What Causes Depression?’, Bustle (31 May 2017), https://www.bustle.com/
p/what-causes-depression-new-research-says-it-might-be-malfunctioning-brain-
circuitry-54289.
36. Ibid.
468
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
37. Ibid.
38. M. MacGill, ‘What Is Depression and What Can I do about It?’, Medical News Today (22
November 2019), https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/kc/depression-causes-symptoms-
treatments-8933.
39. H. Ledford, ‘First Robust Genetic Links to Depression Emerge’, Nature (15 July 2015), https://
www.nature.com/news/first-robust-genetic-links-to-depression-emerge-1.17979.
40. Ibid.
41. G. Lu, J. Li, H. Zhang, X. Zhao, L. J. Yan, and X. Yang, ‘Role and Possible Mechanisms of Sirt1
in Depression’, Oxidative Medicine and Cellular Longevity, 2018:8596903 (2018).
42. E. J. Nestler and S. E. Hyman, ‘Animal Models of Neuropsychiatric Disorders’, Nature
Neuroscience, 13:1161–9 (2010).
43. R. M. Henig, ‘How Depressed Is That Mouse?’, Scientific American (7 March 2012), https://
www.scientificamerican.com/article/depression-how-depressed-is-mouse/.
44. Ibid.
45. Ibid.
46. G. W. Brown and T. O. Harris, Social Origins of Depression: A Study of Psychiatric Disorder in
Women (Free Press, 1978) .
47. K. Oatley, ‘Depression: Crisis without Alternatives’, New Scientist, 29–31 (1984).
48. M. Smith, ‘Social Psychiatry Could Stem the Rising Tide of Mental Illness’, The Conversation
(3 June 2020), https://theconversation.com/social-psychiatry-could-stem-the-rising-tide-
of-mental-illness-138152.
49. R. Zamzow, ‘Male Mice Pass Stress Signatures down to Their Pups’, Spectrum (19 October
2015), https://www.spectrumnews.org/news/male-mice-pass-stress-signatures-down-to-
their-pups/.
50. Ibid.
51. N. Rahhal, ‘Stressed Men Have “Softer” Children: Study Finds Stress Affects Sperm Quality
and Makes Offspring Less Resilient to Pressure’, Daily Mail (13 November 2017), http://www.
dailymail.co.uk/health/article-5078233/Father-s-stress-affect-sperm-children-study-
shows.html.
52. Ibid.
53. S. Paulsson, ‘A View of the Holocaust’, BBC History (17 February 2011), http://www.bbc.
co.uk/history/worldwars/genocide/holocaust_overview_01.shtml.
54. D. Millward, ‘Holocaust Survivors Pass on Trauma to Their Children’s Genes’, The Telegraph
(22 August 2015), https://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/worldnews/northamerica/usa/11817892/
Holocaust-survivors-pass-on-trauma-to-their-childrens-genes.html.
55. Ibid.
56. S. Yasmin, ‘No, Trauma Is Not Inherited’, Dallas News (30 May 2017), https://www.dallasnews.
com/news/debunked/2017/05/30/trauma-inherited.
57. J. Glausiusz, ‘Doubts Arising about Claimed Epigenetics of Holocaust Trauma’, Haaretz
(30 April 2017), https://www.haaretz.com/science-and-health/.premium-doubts-arising-
about-claimed-epigenetics-of-holocaust-trauma-1.5466710.
58. Yasmin, ‘No, Trauma Is Not Inherited’.
469
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
2. P. Beresford, ‘Towards a Social Model of Madness and Distress? Exploring What Service
Users Say’, Joseph Rowntree Foundation (22 November 2010), https://www.jrf.org.uk/report/
towards-social-model-madness-and-distress-exploring-what-service-users-say.
3. B. Taylor, H. Jick, and D. MacLaughlin, ‘Prevalence and Incidence Rates of Autism in the UK:
Time Trend from 2004–2010 in Children Aged 8 Years’, BMJ Open, 3:e003219 (2013).
4. U. Frith, ‘Autism—Are We Any Closer to Explaining the Enigma?’, The Psychologist, 27:744–5
(2014).
5. Ibid.
6. S. Baron-Cohen, ‘Leo Kanner, Hans Asperger, and the Discovery of Autism’, The Lancet,
386:1329–30 (2015).
7. S. Baron, ‘Neurotribes Review—the Evolution of Our Understanding of Autism’, The Observer
(23 August 2015), https://www.theguardian.com/books/2015/aug/23/neurotribes-legacy-
autism-steve-silberman-book-review-saskia-baron.
8. Ibid.
9. K. Connolly, ‘Hans Asperger Aided and Supported Nazi Programme, Study Says’, The
Guardian (19 April 2018), https://www.theguardian.com/world/2018/apr/19/hans-asperger-
aided-and-supported-nazi-programme-study-says.
10. Baron, ‘Neurotribes Review’.
11. Ibid.
12. Ibid.
13. H. Schofield, ‘France’s Autism Treatment “Shame” ’, BBC News (2 April 2012), https://www.
bbc.co.uk/news/magazine-17583123.
14. Ibid.
15. E. Anthes, ‘Lab-Grown Neurons Showcase Effects of Autism Mutations’, Spectrum (15
January 2018), https://www.spectrumnews.org/news/lab-grown-neurons-showcase-
effects-autism-mutations/.
16. Ibid.
17. Ibid.
18. Ibid.
19. A. Sandoiu, ‘Reversing Autism with a Cancer Drug’, Medical News Today (27 June 2018),
https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/322274.php.
20. Ibid.
21. Ibid.
22. S. Boseley, ‘Andrew Wakefield Case Highlights the Importance of Ethics in Science’, The
Guardian (24 May 2010), https://www.theguardian.com/society/2010/may/24/andrew-
wakefield-analysis-ethics-science.
23. S. Boseley, ‘WHO Warns over Measles Immunisation Rates as Cases Rise 300% across
Europe’, The Guardian (19 February 2018), https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/
feb/19/who-warns-over-measles-immunisation-rates-as-cases-rise-400-across-europe.
24. A. Buncombe, ‘Andrew Wakefield: How a Disgraced UK Doctor Has Remade Himself in
Anti-vaxxer Trump’s America’, The Independent (4 May 2018), https://www.independent.
co.uk/news/world/americas/andrew-wakefield-anti-vaxxer-trump-us-mmr-autism-link-
lancet-fake-a8331826.html.
25. Z. Drewett, ‘Desperate Parents Trying to “Cure” Autism by Giving Children Bleach’, Metro
(28 January 2018), https://metro.co.uk/2018/01/28/desperate-parents-trying-cure-autism-
giving-children-bleach-7266567/.
26. Ibid.
470
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
27. Ibid.
28. A. E. Cha, ‘Study: Autism, Creativity and Divergent Thinking May Go Hand in Hand’,
Washington Post (25 August 2015), https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/to-your-health/
wp/2015/08/25/study-autism-creativity-and-divergent-thinking-may-go-hand-in-hand/?utm_
term=.e0136faa9746.
29. Ibid.
30. Ibid.
31. Ibid.
32. T. McVeigh, ‘People with Autism and Learning Disabilities Excel in Creative Thinking,
Study Shows’, The Guardian (22 August 2015), https://www.theguardian.com/society/2015/
aug/22/autism-creative-thinking-study.
33. N. Hinde, ‘Just 16% of People with Autism Are in Full-Time Paid Work and It Needs to
Change, Says Charity’, Huffington Post (27 October 2016), https://www.huffingtonpost.
co.uk/entry/16-percent-of-people-with-autism-are-in-full-time-paid-work_uk_
5811e71be4b0ccfc9561da6f.
34. N. L. Pesce, ‘Most College Grads with Autism Can’t Find Jobs. This Group Is Fixing That’,
Market Watch (2 April 2019), https://www.marketwatch.com/story/most-college-grads-
with-autism-cant-find-jobs-this-group-is-fixing-that-2017-04-10-5881421.
35. Hinde, ‘Just 16% of People with Autism’.
36. J. Parrington, ‘Early Learning’, Socialist Review (October 1994), https://www.marxists.org/
history/etol/newspape/socrev/1994/sr179/parrington.html.
37. J. E. Knox, ‘The Changing Face of Soviet Defectology: A Study in Rehabilitating the
Handicapped’, Studies in Soviet Thought, 37:217–36 (1989).
38. Parrington, ‘Early Learning’.
39. Ibid.
40. Ibid.
41. Ibid.
42. Ibid.
43. Ibid.
44. P. Wolfberg, K. Bottema-Beutel, and M. DeWitt, ‘Including Children with Autism in Social
and Imaginary Play with Typical Peers’, American Journal of Play, 5:55–80 (2012).
45. Ibid.
46. Ibid.
47. Ibid.
48. Parrington, ‘Early Learning’.
49. Wolfberg, Bottema-Beutel, and DeWitt, ‘Including Children with Autism’.
50. Ibid.
51. Ibid.
52. Ibid.
53. K. Gander, ‘World Bipolar Day: What Is Bipolar Disorder? What Are Its Symptoms?’, The
Independent (29 March 2016), https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-families/
features/world-bipolar-day-what-is-bipolar-disorder-and-what-are-its-symptoms-
a6958426.html.
54. H. Thomson, ‘Intelligence, Creativity and Bipolar Disorder May Share Underlying Genetics’,
The Guardian (19 August 2015), https://www.theguardian.com/science/2015/aug/19/intelligence-
creativity-and-bipolar-disorder-may-share-underlying-genetics.
55. Ibid.
471
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
56. Ibid.
57. N. Wolchover, ‘Why Are Genius and Madness Connected?’, Live Science (2 June 2012), https://
www.livescience.com/20713-genius-madness-connected.html.
58. Ibid.
59. Ibid.
60. Ibid.
61. Ibid.
62. H. J. Parkinson, ‘Having a Mental Illness Doesn’t Make You a Genius’, The Guardian (10 June
2015), https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2015/jun/10/mental-illness-study-
bipolar-disorder-creativity.
472
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
16. Ibid.
17. T. Adams, ‘How to Spot a Murderer’s Brain’, The Guardian (12 May 2013), https://www.
theguardian.com/science/2013/may/12/how-to-spot-a-murderers-brain.
18. Ibid.
19. Ibid.
20. Ibid.
21. J. Wells, ‘Low Resting Heart Rate Linked to “Future Violent or Anti-social Behaviour” ’,
The Telegraph (17 September 2015), https://www.telegraph.co.uk/men/the-filter/11863460/
Low-resting-heart-rate-linked-to-future-violent-or-anti-social-behaviour.html.
22. Adams, ‘How to Spot a Murderer’s Brain’.
23. T. Wallace, ‘Brain Lesions Contribute to Criminal Behaviour, Study Finds’, Cosmos (20
December 2017), https://cosmosmagazine.com/social-sciences/brain-lesions-contribute-
to-criminal-behaviour-study-finds.
24. Ibid.
25. Ibid.
26. Ibid.
27. R. Keller, ‘Macabre World of the Warped Killer Who Inspired Silence of the Lambs—Featuring
Soup Bowls Made from Human Skulls and Even Grislier “Trophies” ’, Daily Mirror (30 August
2017), https://www.mirror.co.uk/news/real-life-stories/grisly-world-warped-killer-who-
11081022.
28. G. Paoletti, ‘The Story of Serial Killer Edmund Kemper, Whose Story Is Almost Too Gross
to Be Real’, All That’s Interesting (7 November 2018), https://allthatsinteresting.com/edmund-
kemper.
29. ‘Gary Heidnik’, Criminal Minds (2018), http://criminalminds.wikia.com/wiki/Gary_
Heidnik.
30. C. Carter, ‘From Lonely Weakling to the Yorkshire Ripper: How Serial Killer Peter Sutcliffe
Developed His Murderous Hatred’, Daily Mirror (13 November 2020), https://www.mirror.
co.uk/news/uk-news/lonely-weakling-yorkshire-ripper-how-11213299.
31. H. Mitchell and M. G. Aamodt, ‘The Incidence of Child Abuse in Serial Killers’, Journal of
Police and Criminal Psychology, 20:40–7 (2005).
32. G. Tenbergen, M. Wittfoth, H. Frieling, J. Ponseti, M. Walter, H. Walter, K. M. Beier,
B. Schiffer, and T. H. Kruger, ‘The Neurobiology and Psychology of Pedophilia: Recent
Advances and Challenges’, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9:344 (2015).
33. Ibid.
34. R. Sanders, ‘Are Paedophiles’ Brains Wired Differently?’, BBC News (24 November 2015),
https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/magazine-34858350.
35. Ibid.
36. J. Grayson, ‘He Is a Paedophile, But That Does Not Make Him a Child Molester’, Huffington Post
(17 September 2017), https://www.huffingtonpost.co.uk/juliet-grayson/he-is-a-paedophile-
but-th_b_12046562.html.
37. Ibid.
38. K. Gander, ‘The Man Whose Brain Tumour “Turned Him into a Paedophile” ’, The Independent
(24 February 2016), https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-families/features/a-
40-year-old-developed-an-obsession-with-child-pornography-then-doctors-discovered-
why-a6893756.html.
39. Ibid.
40. Ibid.
473
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
41. C. Weaver, ‘Are You Raising a Paedophile?’, Now to Love (7 June 2016), https://www.
nowtolove.com.au/news/real-life/nature-vs-nurture-are-you-raising-a-paedophile-10454.
42. Ibid.
43. Ibid.
44. L. Buchen, ‘Neuroscience: In Their Nurture’, Nature 467:146–8 (2010).
45. Ibid.
46. B. Weidmann, ‘The Making of a Bully’, The Scientist (25 January 2013), https://www.the-scientist.
com/daily-news/the-making-of-a-bully-39884.
47. Ibid.
48. Ibid.
49. S. C. Johnson, ‘The New Theory That Could Explain Crime and Violence in America’,
Medium (18 February 2014), https://medium.com/matter/the-new-theory-that-could-
explain-crime-and-violence-in-america-945462826399.
50. Ibid.
51. Ibid.
52. Ibid.
53. Ibid.
54. W. Hirstein, ‘What Is a Psychopath?’, Psychology Today (30 January 2013), https://www.
psychologytoday.com/us/blog/mindmelding/201301/what-is-psychopath-0.
55. Ibid.
56. S. A. Bonn, ‘Serial Killer Myth #1: They’re Mentally Ill or Evil Geniuses’, Psychology Today (16
June 2014), https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/wicked-deeds/201406/serial-killer-
myth-1-theyre-mentally-ill-or-evil-geniuses.
57. S. Rose, ‘From Split to Psycho: Why Cinema Fails Dissociative Identity Disorder’,
The Guardian (12 January 2017), https://www.theguardian.com/film/2017/jan/12/cinema-
dissociative-personality-disorder-split-james-mcavoy.
58. Ibid.
59. Ibid.
60. J. Jitchotvisut, ‘How Ted Bundy Got Away with So Many Murders, According to a Forensic
Psychologist’, Insider (28 January 2019), https://www.insider.com/ted-bundy-case-
explained-forensic-psychologist-2019-1.
61. J. C. Motzkin, J. P. Newman, K. A. Kiehl, and M. Koenigs, ‘Reduced Prefrontal Connectivity
in Psychopathy’, Journal of Neuroscience, 31:17348–57 (2011).
62. S. Vaknin, ‘Serial and Mass Killers’, Mental Health Matters (14 April 2009), https://mental-
health-matters.com/serial-and-mass-killers/.
63. S. A. Bonn, ‘Understanding What Drives Serial Killers’, Psychology Today (15 September 2019),
https://www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/wicked-deeds/201909/understanding-what-
drives-serial-killers.
64. R. Zachary, ‘Night Stalker: The Life and Death of Richard Ramirez’, Serial Killer (14 October
2020), https://serialkillershop.com/blogs/true-crime/richard-ramirez-night-stalker.
65. V. Allan, ‘Why We Must Understand Savile Psyche’, The Herald (29 June 2014), https://www.
heraldscotland.com/opinion/13167665.why-we-must-understand-savile-psyche/.
66. A. Kalia, ‘ “We’re All Competing for the Same Jobs”: Life in Britain’s Youngest City’, The
Guardian (5 February 2018), https://www.theguardian.com/cities/2018/feb/05/life-britain-
youngest-city-bradford-uk-unemployment.
67. Ibid.
68. Ibid.
474
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
475
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
13. Ibid.
14. A. Chua, ‘How America’s Identity Politics Went from Inclusion to Division’, The Guardian (1
March 2018), https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/mar/01/how-americas-identity-
politics-went-from-inclusion-to-division.
15. J. Suzman, ‘How Neolithic Farming Sowed the Seeds of Modern Inequality 10,000 Years
Ago’, The Guardian (5 December 2017), https://www.theguardian.com/inequality/2017/
dec/05/how-neolithic-farming-sowed-the-seeds-of-modern-inequality-10000-years-ago.
16. B. Cunliffe, ‘Against the Grain by James C Scott Review—the Beginning of Elites, Tax, Slavery’,
The Guardian (25 November 2017), https://www.theguardian.com/books/2017/nov/25/
against-the-grain-by-james-c-scott-review.
17. Ibid.
18. J. Diamond, ‘The Worst Mistake in the History of the Human Race’, Discover Magazine (1 May
1999), http://discovermagazine.com/1987/may/02-the-worst-mistake-in-the-history-of-
the-human-race.
19. Cunliffe, ‘Against the Grain by James C Scott Review’.
20. Ibid.
21. Ibid.
22. G. E. M. De Sainte Croix, The Class Struggle in the Ancient World (Duckworth, 2001), p. 40.
23. M. Beard, ‘A Radical, Short-Lived and Violent Experiment: the Origins of Democracy’,
The Guardian (29 April 2006), https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2006/apr/29/
comment.politics1.
24. Aristotle, ‘Politics’, MIT Internet Classics Archive (2009), http://classics.mit.edu/Aristotle/
politics.1.one.html.
25. M. E. Price, ‘Why We Think Monogamy Is Normal’, Psychology Today (9 September 2011),
https://www.psychologytoday.com/intl/blog/darwin-eternity/201109/why-we-think-
monogamy-is-normal.
26. D. Ross, ‘Feudalism and Medieval life’, Britain Express (2020), https://www.britainexpress.
com/History/Feudalism_and_Medieval_life.htm.
27. L. G. Bowman, ‘The Paradox of the Declaration of Independence’, Aspen Institute (1 July 2016),
https://www.aspeninstitute.org/blog-posts/every-american-know-paradox-declaration-
independence/.
28. J. M. C. Lustiger, ‘Liberty, Equality, Fraternity’, First Things (October 1997), https://www.
firstthings.com/article/1997/10/002-liberty-equality-fraternity.
29. R. Cox, ‘Inequality Gap: Growing Gulf between Rich and Poor Leaves 42 People with Same
Wealth as World’s 3.7bn Worst off’, The Independent (22 January 2018), https://www.inde-
pendent.co.uk/news/business/news/oxfam-report-worlds-richest-poorest-inequality-
gap-wealth-2017-davos-wef-a8167676.html.
30. H. O’Shaughnessy, ‘Chilean Coup: 40 Years Ago I Watched Pinochet Crush a Democratic
Dream’, The Guardian (7 September 2013), https://www.theguardian.com/world/2013/sep/
07/chile-coup-pinochet-allende.
31. E. O’Carroll, ‘Karl Marx: 10 Great Quotes on His Birthday’, Christian Science Monitor (4 May
2012), https://www.csmonitor.com/Books/2012/0504/Karl-Marx-10-great-quotes-on-his-
birthday/On-the-mainstream-media.
32. S. A. Diamond, ‘Let’s Talk about Loneliness: Alienation in a Linked Up Age’, Psychology Today
(21 February 2014), https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/evil-deeds/201402/lets-
talk-about-loneliness-alienation-in-linked-age.
33. J. Elster, Karl Marx: A Reader (Cambridge University Press, 2008), p. 39.
476
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
34. K. Moos, ‘Working Out the Meaning of “Meaningful” Work’, Chronicle Vitae (30 October
2014), https://chroniclevitae.com/news/781-working-out-the-meaning-of-meaningful-
work.
35. G. Kennedy, ‘Of Pins and Things’, Adam Smith Institute (28 May 2012), https://www.
adamsmith.org/blog/economics/of-pins-and-things.
36. T. Hindle, ‘Mass Production’, The Economist (20 October 2009), https://www.economist.
com/news/2009/10/20/mass-production.
37. S. Ghosh, ‘Peeing in Trash Cans, Constant Surveillance, and Asthma Attacks on the Job:
Amazon Workers Tell Us Their Warehouse Horror Stories’, Business Insider (5 May 2018),
https://www.businessinsider.com/amazon-warehouse-workers-share-their-horror-
stories-2018-4?r=US&IR=T.
38. Ibid.
39. J. Grove, ‘Fixed-Term Now the Norm for Early Career Academics, Says UCU’, Times Higher
Education (14 April 2016), https://www.timeshighereducation.com/news/fixed-term-now-
the-norm-for-early-career-academics-says-university-and-college-union-ucu.
40. S. Weale, ‘Part-Time Lecturers on Precarious Work: “I Don’t Make Enough for Rent” ’, The
Guardian (16 November 2016), https://www.theguardian.com/uk-news/2016/nov/16/part-
time-lecturers-on-precarious-work-i-dont-make-enough-for-rent.
41. L. Dodgson, ‘Here’s Why CEOs Often Have the Traits of a Psychopath’, Business Insider (7 July
2017), http://uk.businessinsider.com/ceos-often-have-psychopathic-traits-2017-7.
42. J. Silver, Is Wall Street Full of Psychopaths?, The Atlantic (2012), https://www.theatlantic.
com/health/archive/2012/03/is-wall-street-full-of-psychopaths/254944/
43. A. Rzepniknowska, ‘Racism and Xenophobia Experienced by Polish Migrants in the UK
before and after Brexit Vote’, Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies, 45:61–77, (2018).
44. M. Berry, I. Garcia-Blanco, and K. Moore, ‘UK Press Is the Most Aggressive in Reporting on
Europe’s “Migrant” Crisis’, The Conversation (14 March 2016), https://theconversation.com/
uk-press-is-the-most-aggressive-in-reporting-on-europes-migrant-crisis-56083.
45. A. Bovey, ‘Women in Medieval Society’, British Library (30 April 2015), https://www.bl.uk/
the-middle-ages/articles/women-in-medieval-society.
46. H. J. Sharman, E. K. Hunt, R. F. Nesiba, and B. Wiens-Tuers, Economics: An Introduction to
Traditional and Progressive Values (M.E. Sharpe, 2008), p. 37.
47. R. Stites, The Women’s Liberation Movement in Russia: Feminism, Nihilsm, and Bolshevism,
1860–1930 (Princeton University Press, 1978), p. 160.
48. E. Goddard and J. Cox, ‘Women’s Suffrage: After 100 Years since Millions of Women
Got the Vote around the World, How Do Their Rights Compare Now?’, The Independent
(6 February 2018), https://www.independent.co.uk/news/long_reads/women-suffrage-
100-years-get-vote-right-uk-britain-ireland-us-world-countries-compare-switzerland-
a8191506.html.
49. K. Parker, ‘Despite Progress, Women Still Bear Heavier Load Than Men in Balancing Work
and Family’, Pew Research Center (10 March 2015), http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-
tank/2015/03/10/women-still-bear-heavier-load-than-men-balancing-work-family/.
50. R. Solnit, ‘A Broken Idea of Sex is Flourishing. Blame Capitalism’, The Guardian (12 May 2018),
https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2018/may/12/sex-capitalism-incel-movement-
misogyny-feminism.
51. A. Langone, ‘#MeToo and Time’s Up Founders Explain the Difference between the 2
Movements—And How They’re Alike’, Time (22 March 2018), http://time.com/5189945/
whats-the-difference-between-the-metoo-and-times-up-movements/.
477
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
52. R. Lewis, ‘What Actually is a Belief? And Why Is It So Hard to Change?’, Psychology Today (7
October 2018), https://www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/finding-purpose/201810/what-
actually-is-belief-and-why-is-it-so-hard-change.
53. P. Agarwal, ‘What Neuroimaging Can Tell Us about Our Unconscious Biases’, Scientific
American (12 April 2020), https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/observations/what-
neuroimaging-can-tell-us-about-our-unconscious-biases/.
54. Ibid.
55. M. Haiken, ‘More Than 10,000 Suicides Tied to Economic Crisis, Study Says’, Forbes (12 June
2014), https://www.forbes.com/sites/melaniehaiken/2014/06/12/more-than-10000-suicides-
tied-to-economic-crisis-study-says/.
56. J. Henley, ‘ “Recessions Can Hurt, But Austerity Kills” ’, The Guardian (15 May 2013), https://
www.theguardian.com/society/2013/may/15/recessions-hurt-but-austerity-kills.
57. Ibid.
58. J. Kanter and K. Manbeck, ‘COVID-19 Could Lead to an Epidemic of Clinical Depression,
and the Health Care System Isn’t Ready for That, Either’, The Conversation (1 April 2020),
http://theconversation.com/covid-19-could-lead-to-an-epidemic-of-clinical-depression-
and-the-health-care-system-isnt-ready-for-that-either-134528.
59. R. Pinto, M. Ashworth, and R. Jones, ‘Schizophrenia in black Caribbeans Living in the UK:
An Exploration of Underlying Causes of the High Incidence Rate’, British Journal of General
Practice, 58:429–434 (2008).
60. Ibid.
61. Ibid.
62. ‘UK Life Blamed for Ethnic Schizophrenia’, BBC News (14 July 2000), http://news.bbc.
co.uk/1/hi/health/807945.stm.
63. T. Evans, ‘Understanding the Gender Pay Gap in the UK’, Office for National Statistics
(17 January 2018), https://www.ons.gov.uk/employmentandlabourmarket/peopleinwork/
earningsandworkinghours/articles/understandingthegenderpaygapintheuk/2018-01-17.
64. R. Neate, ‘Global Pay Gap Will Take 202 Years to Close, Says World Economic Forum’, The
Guardian (18 December 2018), https://www.theguardian.com/world/2018/dec/18/global-
gender-pay-gap-will-take-202-years-to-close-says-world-economic-forum.
65. S. Zacharek, E. Dockterman, and H. S. Edwards, ‘TIME Person of the Year: The Silence
Breakers’, Time (5 March 2017), https://time.com/time-person-of-the-year-2017-silence-
breakers/.
66. J. Ducharme, ‘Any Type of Sexual Harassment Can Cause Psychological Harm, Study Says’,
Time (9 November 2017), http://time.com/5017072/sexual-harassment-psychological-
damage/.
67. Ibid.
68. P. R. Albert, ‘Why Is Depression More Prevalent in Women?’, Journal of Psychiatry and
Neuroscience, 40:219–21 (2015).
69. J. Bindel, ‘Tell Me If You Still Think Prostitution Is Empowering after Hearing What The
Buying Punters Have to Say’, The Independent (19 September 2017), https://www.independent.
co.uk/voices/sex-work-punters-what-do-they-think-prostitution-exploitation-rape-danger-
a7889511.html.
70. Ibid.
71. M. Farley, ‘Risks of Prostitution: When the Person Is the Product’, Journal of the Association for
Consumer Research, 3:97–108 (2018).
478
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
72. A. Travis, ‘Yorkshire Ripper Mental Health Review Revives Central Issue of Trial’, The
Guardian (1 December 2015), https://www.theguardian.com/uk-news/2015/dec/01/yorkshire-
ripper-peter-sutcliffe-mental-health-1981-trial.
73. L. Thornton, ‘Yorkshire Ripper Killed EIGHT More Women, Claims the Ex-cop Who
Interviewed Him More Than 30 Times’, Daily Mirror (6 April 2017), https://www.mirror.co.
uk/news/uk-news/yorkshire-ripper-linked-eight-more-10174613.
74. L. Wattis, ‘The Social Nature of Serial Murder: The Intersection of Gender and Modernity’,
European Journal of Women’s Studies, 243:381–93 (2016).
75. J. Bindel, 'Peter Sutcliffe Should Never Be Freed’, The Guardian (2 March 2010), https://www.
theguardian.com/commentisfree/2010/mar/02/peter-sutcliffe-hate-crimes-women.
479
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
18. J. Spence and C. Stephenson, ‘ “Side by Side with Our Men?” Women’s Activism, Community,
and Gender in the 1984–1985 British Miners’ Strike’, International Labor and Working-Class
History, 75:68–84 (2009).
19. J. Rees and L. German, A People’s History of London (Verso Books, 2012), p. 41.
20. Ibid.
21. Ibid.
22. M. Lavalette and G. Mooney, Class Struggle and Social Welfare (Routledge, 2013), p. 61.
23. Ibid.
24. A. Gorz, ‘Reform and Revolution’, Socialist Register (March 1968), https://socialistregister.
com/index.php/srv/article/view/5272/0.
25. A. J. Rubin, ‘May 1968: A Month of Revolution Pushed France Into the Modern World’, New
York Times (5 May 2018), https://www.nytimes.com/2018/05/05/world/europe/france-may-
1968-revolution.html.
26. C. Hill, The World Turned Upside Down: Radical Ideas during the English Revolution (Penguin, 1991).
27. M. O. Grenby, ‘Writing Revolution: British Literature and the French Revolution Crisis, a
Review of Recent Scholarship’, Literature Compass, 3:1351–85 (2006).
28. M. Cole, K. Levitin, and A.R. Luria, The Autobiography of Alexander Luria: A Dialogue with the
Making of Mind (Psychology Press, 2010), p. 17.
29. C. A. Hartley and L. H. Somerville, ‘The Neuroscience of Adolescent Decision-Making’,
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 5:108–15 (2015).
30. K. Marx, The Eighteenth Brumaire of Louis Bonaparte (1852), https://www.marxists.org/archive/
marx/works/1852/18th-brumaire/ch01.htm.
31. G. Tiruneh, ‘Social Revolutions: Their Causes, Patterns, and Phases’, SAGE Open (18
September 2014), http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/2158244014548845.
32. D. Saldanha, ‘Antonio Gramsci and the Analysis of Class Consciousness: Some
Methodological Considerations’, Economic and Political Weekly, 23:11–18 (1988).
33. Ibid.
34. C. Hill, The Century of Revolution: 1603–1714 (Routledge, 1980).
35. D. Sandbrook, ‘The Man Who Wouldn’t Be King’, New Statesman (29 December 2010),
https://www.newstatesman.com/society/2010/12/cromwell-god-essay-history.
36. T. Benn, ‘The Levellers and the Tradition of Dissent’, BBC History (17 February 2011), http://
www.bbc.co.uk/history/british/civil_war_revolution/benn_levellers_01.shtml.
37. J. Rees, ‘In Defence of October’, International Socialism, 52:3–79 (Autumn 1991), https://www.
marxists.org/history/etol/writers/rees-j/1991/xx/october.html.
38. C. Harris, ‘Russia’s February Revolution Was Led by Women on the March’, Smithsonian
Magazine (17 February 2017), https://www.smithsonianmag.com/history/russias-february-
revolution-was-led-women-march-180962218/.
39. T. Vallance, ‘Fresh Perspectives on the Levellers’, History Today (6 June 2017), https://www.
historytoday.com/reviews/fresh-perspectives-levellers.
40. S. Weissman, ‘The Golden Era’, Jacobin Magazine (18 December 2017), https://www.
jacobinmag.com/2017/12/victor-serge-russian-revolution-bolsheviks.
41. B. J. Luskin, ‘MRIs Reveal Unconscious Bias in the Brain’, Psychology Today (7 April 2016),
https://www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/the-media-psychology-effect/201604/mris-
reveal-unconscious-bias-in-the-brain.
42. M. Rouault, J. Drugowitsch, and E. Koechlin, ‘Prefrontal Mechanisms Combining Rewards
and Beliefs in Human Decision-Making’, Nature Communications, 10:301 (2019).
480
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
43. G. Walton, ‘Théroigne de Méricourt, Heroine of the French Revolution’, Amazing Women in
History (2016), http://www.amazingwomeninhistory.com/theroigne-de-mericourt/.
44. Ibid.
45. Ibid.
46. H. Mantel, ‘Rescued by Marat’, London Review of Books, 14:15–16 (1992).
47. Ibid.
48. Walton, ‘Théroigne de Méricourt, Heroine of the French Revolution’.
49. I. Ferguson, Politics of the Mind: Marxism and Mental Distress (Bookmarks, 2017), p. 30.
50. E. Fee and T. M. Brown, ‘Freeing the Insane’, American Journal of Public Health, 96:1743 (2006).
51. A. Rogers and D. Pilgrim, Mental Health Policy in Britain: A Critical Introduction (Palgrave
Macmillan, 1996), p. 50.
52. B. Manning, The English People and the English Revolution (Bookmarks, 1995).
53. S. Roberts, ‘Col. Thomas Rainborowe: “The Poorest He That Is in England Hath a Right to Live
as the Greatest He” ’, The History of Parliament (18 April 2013), https://thehistoryofparliament.
wordpress.com/2013/04/18/col-thomas-rainborowe-the-poorest-he-that-is-in-england-hath-
a-right-to-live-as-the-greatest-he/.
54. R. Coalson, ‘Gulags Were a Horrific Cornerstone of Stalinist Russia’, The Atlantic (5 March
2013), https://www.theatlantic.com/international/archive/2013/03/gulags-were-a-horrific-
cornerstone-of-stalinist-russia/273703/.
55. D. Sherry, Russia 1917: Workers’ Revolution and the Festival of the Oppressed (Bookmarks, 2017),
p. 219.
56. A. Kozulin, Vygotsky’s Psychology: A Biography of Ideas (Harvester Wheatsheaf, 1990),
pp. 239–40.
57. G. Dvorsky, ‘How the Soviets Used Their Own Twisted Version of Psychiatry to Suppress
Political Dissent’, Gizmodo (4 September 2012), https://io9.gizmodo.com/how-the-soviets-
used-their-own-twisted-version-of-psych-5940212.
58. Ibid.
59. Ibid.
60. R. D. Strous, ‘Psychiatry during the Nazi Era: Ethical Lessons for the Modern Professional’,
Annals of General Psychiatry, 6:8 (2007).
61. T. Burns, Our Necessary Shadow: The Nature and Meaning of Psychiatry (Pegasus Books, 2014), p. 50.
62. G. Le Bon, The Psychology of Revolutionary Crowds (London: T Fisher Unwin, 1913).
63. Ibid.
64. Ibid.
65. J. Drury, ‘Impact: From Riots to Crowd Safety’, The Psychologist (February 2016), https://
thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-29/february/riots-crowd-safety.
66. C. T. Miéville, ‘The Day That Shook the World’, Jacobin Magazine (7 November 2017), https://
jacobinmag.com/2017/11/october-revolution-china-mieville-bolsheviks.
67. N. Carlin, ‘The Levellers and the Conquest of Ireland in 1649’, The Historical Journal, 30:269–88
(1987).
68. J. Israel, Revolutionary Ideas: An Intellectual History of the French Revolution from the Rights of Man
to Robespierre (Princeton University Press, 2014), pp. 182–3.
69. S. Sagall, ‘Antisemitism and the Russian Revolution’, Socialist Review (April 2018), http://
socialistreview.org.uk/434/antisemitism-and-russian-revolution.
70. Ibid.
71. J. Rees, The Leveller Revolution (Verso, 2016), pp. 290–2.
481
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
72. J. Orr, Marxism and Women’s Liberation (Bookmarks, 2015), pp. 94–5.
73. D. Sherry, Russia 1917: Workers’ Revolution and the Festival of the Oppressed (Bookmarks, 2017),
p. 185.
74. M. Bond, ‘Mob Mentality’, Slate (17 March 2015), https://slate.com/technology/2015/03/
crowd-psychology-people-are-friendly-altruistic-happy-in-large-gatherings.html.
75. B. Trew, ‘How Distaste of LGBT People in Egypt Has Turned into State-Sponsored
Persecution’, The Independent (17 May 2015), https://www.independent.co.uk/news/world/
middle-east/how-distaste-of-lgbt-people-in-egypt-has-turned-into-state-sponsored-
persecution-10256869.html.
76. M. L. Thomas, ‘Fascism in Europe Today’, International Socialism, 162:27–64 (2019).
77. P. Brown, in P. Brown (ed.), Radical Psychology (Tavistock Publications, 1973), pp. 244–56.
78. Ibid.
79. O. Nachtwey, Germany’s Hidden Crisis: Social Decline in the Heart of Europe (Verso, 2018), p. 133.
80. Ibid.
81. S. Knapton, ‘Nine in 10 People Would Electrocute Others If Ordered, Rerun of infamous
Milgram Experiment Shows’, The Telegraph (14 March 2017), https://www.telegraph.co.uk/
science/2017/03/14/nine-10-people-would-electrocute-others-ordered-re-run-milgram/.
82. Ibid.
83. Ibid.
84. M. Stuchbery, ‘What We Can Learn from the White Rose Siblings’, The Local De (22 February
2019), https://www.thelocal.de/20190222/76-years-later-what-the-white-rose-siblings-
teach-us.
85. O. B. Waxman, ‘ “He Was Sent by God to Take Care of Us”: Inside the Real Story behind
Schindler’s List’, Time (7 December 2018), https://time.com/5470613/schindlers-list-true-
story/.
86. S. B. Carroll, ‘Deep Secrets and the Thrill of Discovery’, Quanta (25 February 2016) https://
www.quantamagazine.org/deep-secrets-and-the-thrill-of-discovery-20160225/.
482
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
13. S. Szeles, ‘7 Theories on Why We Evolved to Love Music’, Nova (21 May 2014), http://www.
pbs.org/wgbh/nova/blogs/secretlife/blogposts/the-evolution-of-music/.
14. E. Landau, ‘This Is Your Brain on Music’, CNN (23 January 2018), https://edition.cnn.
com/2013/04/15/health/brain-music-research/index.html.
15. Ibid.
16. Ibid.
17. Ibid.
18. Ibid.
19. J. Schulkin and G. B. Raglan, ‘The Evolution of Music and Human Social Capability’, Frontiers
in Neuroscience, 8:1–13 (2014).
20. Ibid.
21. L. Marquand-Brown, ‘Music and Human Evolution’, OUP Blog (25 July 2017), https://blog.
oup.com/2017/07/music-human-evolution/.
22. Ibid.
23. I. Morley, The Prehistory of Music (Oxford University Press, 2013), Chapter 2.
24. J. Richter and R. Ostovar, ‘ “It Don’t Mean a Thing if It Ain’t Got That Swing”—An
Alternative Concept for Understanding the Evolution of Dance and Music in Human
Beings’, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10:485 (2016).
25. P. Gray, ‘Play as a Foundation for Hunter-Gatherer Social Existence’, American Journal of Play,
1:476–522 (2009).
26. P. Wiessner, ‘Embers of Society: Firelight Talk among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen’, Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences USA, 111:14027–35 (2014).
27. Morley, The Prehistory of Music.
28. Ibid.
29. Gray, ‘Play as a Foundation for Hunter-Gatherer Social Existence’.
30. M. Del Nevo, Art Music: Love, Listening and Soulfulness (Transaction Publishers, 2013), p. 100.
31. F. Rohrer, ‘The Devil’s Music’, BBC News (28 April 2006), http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/
magazine/4952646.stm.
32. D. Randall, Sound System (Pluto Press, 2017), pp. 98–9.
33. Rohrer, ‘The Devil’s Music’.
34. Ibid.
35. Ibid.
36. S. Behrman, ‘From Revolution to Irrelevance: How Classical Music Lost Its Audience’,
International Socialism, 121 (2 January 2009), http://isj.org.uk/from-revolution-to-irrelevance-
how-classical-music-lost-its-audience/.
37. Ibid.
38. P. McGarr, Mozart: Overture to Revolution (Redwords, 2001), pp. 20–2.
39. Ibid., 20–2.
40. Ibid., 69.
41. Behrman, ‘From Revolution to Irrelevance’.
42. L. S. Vygotsky, ‘Art and Life’, in The Psychology of Art (1925), https://www.marxists.org/
archive/vygotsky/works/1925/art11.htm.
43. S. Jeffries, ‘Don Giovanni—Hero or Villain?’, The Guardian (11 April 2012), https://www.
theguardian.com/music/2012/apr/11/don-giovanni-hero-or-villain.
44. L. Howard, ‘The Story and Backstory of Writing The Magic Flute’, Utah Opera (19 February
2019), https://utahopera.org/explore/2019/02/the-story-and-backstory-of-writing-the-
magic-flute/.
483
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
45. M. Lopez-Gonzalez and C. J. Limb, ‘Musical Creativity and the Brain’, Cerebrum, 2012:2
(2012).
46. N. Njoroge, ‘Dedicated to the Struggle: Black Music, Transculturation, and the Aural
Making and Unmaking of the Third World’, Black Music Research Journal, 28:85–104 (2008).
47. M. Smith, John Coltrane: Jazz, Racism and Resistance (Redwords, 2003), pp. 30–1.
48. Ibid., 34–5.
49. A. Midgette, ‘Knowing Mozart Better in the Evolution of the Piano’, New York Times (30
August 2006), https://www.nytimes.com/2006/08/30/arts/music/knowing-mozart-better-
in-the-evolution-of-the-piano.html.
50. Ibid.
51. A. Bateman, ‘Beethoven and the Piano’, Music Unwrapped (2020), http://www.musicun-
wrapped.co.uk/filemanager/Beethoven_Spread.pdf.
52. Ibid.
53. A. Lefkovitz, Jimi Hendrix and the Cultural Politics of Popular Music (Amazon Media, 2018), p. 9.
54. M. Clague, ‘50 Years Ago, Jimi Hendrix’s Woodstock Anthem Expressed the Hopes and
Fears of a Nation’, The Conversation (14 August 2019), http://theconversation.com/50-years-
ago-jimi-hendrixs-woodstock-anthem-expressed-the-hopes-and-fears-of-a-nation-
120717.
55. ‘Fifty Years of Jimi Hendrix’, The Economist (14 December 2016), https://www.economist.
com/prospero/2016/12/14/fifty-years-of-jimi-hendrix.
56. J. Storey, Cultural Theory and Popular Culture: A Reader (Pearson Education, 2006), p. 80.
57. J. Thompson, ‘Top 20 Political Songs: “Imagine”, John Lennon, 1971’, New Statesman (25
March 2010), https://www.newstatesman.com/music/2010/03/lennon-imagine-political.
58. Ibid.
59. K. Macdonald, ‘The Harmonic Magic behind the Timeless Beauty of John Lennon’s
“Imagine” ’, Classic FM (3 July 2020), https://www.classicfm.com/discover-music/harmonic-
analysis-imagine-john-lennon/.
60. D. Gray, ‘Drift Away’, Genius (2020), https://genius.com/Dobie-gray-drift-away-lyrics.
61. D. Randall, Sound System (Pluto Press, 2017), pp. 66–7.
62. Ibid., 69.
63. Ibid., 69.
64. T. Ali and R. Blackburn, ‘The Lost John Lennon Interview’, Counterpunch (8 December 2005),
https://www.counterpunch.org/2005/12/08/the-lost-john-lennon-interview/.
65. G. Campbell, ‘Country Boy’, Genius (2020), https://genius.com/Glen-campbell-country-
boy-you-got-your-feet-in-la-lyrics.
66. G. Adams, ‘Eminem: The Fall and Rise of a Superstar’, The Independent (4 February 2009),
https://www.independent.co.uk/arts-entertainment/music/features/eminem-the-fall-and-
rise-of-a-superstar-1544787.html.
67. Eminem, ‘Walk on Water’, Genius (2020), https://genius.com/Eminem-walk-on-water-
lyrics.
68. G. Cartwright, ‘Richey Edwards’, The Guardian (26 November 2008), https://www.theguardian.
com/music/2008/nov/26/richey-edwards-manic-street-preachers.
69. Manic Street Preachers, ‘4st 7lb’, Genius (2020), https://genius.com/Manic-street-preachers-
4st-7lb-lyrics.
70. Cartwright, ‘Richey Edwards’.
71. Ibid.
484
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
485
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
20. Editorial, ‘Hieronymus Bosch Died 500 Years Ago, But His Art Will Still Creep You Out’,
NPR Illinois (26 June 2016), http://www.nprillinois.org/post/hieronymus-bosch-died-
500-years-ago-his-art-will-still-creep-you-out.
21. Baan, ‘Hieronymus Bosch: Visionary of Change’.
22. A. Denney, ‘Bosch’s Monsters Explained’, Another Man Magazine (27 June 2018), http://www.
anothermanmag.com/life-culture/10393/hieronymus-bosch-s-monsters-explained.
23. Jones, ‘Bosch’s Garden of Earthly Delights’.
24. J. Burleigh, ‘Was Michelangelo’s Artistic Genius a Symptom of Autism?’, The Independent
(1 June 2004), https://www.independent.co.uk/news/uk/this-britain/was-michelangelos-
artistic-genius-a-symptom-of-autism-756718.html.
25. Ibid.
26. S. Shaikh and J. Leonard-Amodeo, ‘The Deviating Eyes of Michelangelo’s David’, Journal of
the Royal Society of Medicine, 98:75–6 (2005).
27. G. Vasari, The Lives of the Artists (Oxford University Press, 1991), pp. 277–8.
28. J. Molyneux, ‘Michelangelo and Human Emancipation’, International Socialism, 128 (14
October 2010), http://isj.org.uk/michelangelo-and-human-emancipation/.
29. Shaikh and Leonard-Amodeo, ‘The Deviating Eyes of Michelangelo’s David’.
30. ‘What is Modernism?’, Saylor Organisation (2011), https://www.saylor.org/site/wp-content/
uploads/2011/05/Modernism.pdf.
31. C. Nineham, ‘The Two Faces of Modernism’, International Socialism, 64 (Autumn 1994),
http://pubs.socialistreviewindex.org.uk/isj64/nineham.htm.
32. J. Jones, ‘Pablo’s Punks’, The Guardian (9 January 2007), https://www.theguardian.com/
culture/2007/jan/09/2.
33. J. Molyneux, ‘A Revolution in Paint: 100 Years of Picasso’s Demoiselles’, International
Socialism, 115 (2 July 2007), http://isj.org.uk/a-revolution-in-paint-100-years-of-picassos-
demoiselles/.
34. E. Gibson, ‘A Magical Metamorphosis of the Ordinary’, Wall Street Journal (16 April 2011),
https://www.wsj.com/articles/SB10001424052748703551304576261042931202326.
35. C. Temple, ‘Visual Memory and What Picasso Was Really Seeing’, The Guardian (9 October
2016), https://www.theguardian.com/lifeandstyle/2016/oct/09/visual-memory-and-what-
picasso-was-really-seeing.
36. Ibid.
37. Jones, ‘Pablo’s Punks’.
38. Molyneux, ‘A Revolution in Paint’.
39. Ibid.
40. Ibid.
41. C. Duncan, The Aesthetics of Power (Cambridge University Press, 1993), pp. 96–7.
42. ‘Five Ways to Look at Malevich’s Black Square’, Tate (2020), https://www.tate.org.uk/art/
artists/kazimir-malevich-1561/five-ways-look-malevichs-black-square.
43. Ibid.
44. C. Harrison and P. Wood, Art in Theory 1900–2000: An Anthology of Changing Ideas (Blackwell,
2003).
45. S. Zeki, ‘Art and the Brain’, Daedalus, 127:71–103 (1999).
46. ‘Five Ways to Look at Malevich’s Black Square’.
47. ‘What is Modernism?’
48. P. Anderson, A Zone of Engagement (Verso, 1992), p. 36.
486
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
49. W. L. Adamson, ‘Avant-Garde Modernism and Italian Fascism: Cultural Politics in the Era of
Mussolini’, Journal of Modern Italian Studies, 6:230–48 (2001).
50. J. Uglow, ‘When Art Meets Power’, New York Review of Books (8 March 2017), https://www.
nybooks.com/daily/2017/03/08/when-art-meets-power-russia-revolution/.
51. Ibid.
52. C. Brandist, Carnival Culture and the Soviet Modernist Novel (Palgrave Macmillan, 1996), p. 52.
53. A. S. Shatskikh, Vitebsk: The Life of Art (Yale University Press, 2007), p. 118.
54. B. Nicholson, ‘Where to begin with Sergei Eisenstein’, British Film Institute (23 January 2018),
https://www.bfi.org.uk/news-opinion/news-bfi/features/where-begin-sergei-eisenstein.
55. Ibid.
56. Ibid.
57. J. Hellerman, ‘How “Parasite” Made One of the Greatest Montages Ever’, No Film School (31
December 2019), https://nofilmschool.com/parasite-montage.
58. J. Vassilieva, ‘Eisenstein/Vygotsky/Luria’s Project: Cinematic Thinking and the Integrative
Science of Mind and Brain’, Screening the Past (December 2013), http://www.screeningthepast.
com/2013/12/eisenstein-vygotsky-luria%E2%80%99s-project-cinematic-thinking-and-the-
integrative-science-of-mind-and-brain/.
59. Ibid.
60. Ibid.
61. Ibid.
62. M. Wilmington, ‘ “Nevsky’s” Bloody Ballet for the Ages’, Los Angeles Times (1 November
1987), https://www.latimes.com/archives/la-xpm-1987-11-01-ca-18046-story.html.
63. Vassilieva, ‘Eisenstein/Vygotsky/Luria’s Project’.
487
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
10. K. Clark and M. Holquist, Mikhail Bakhtin (Harvard University Press, 1986), p. 240.
11. I. R. Makaryk, Encyclopedia of Contemporary Literary Theory: Approaches, Scholars, Terms
(University of Toronto Press, 1993), p. 609.
12. A. Ananthaswamy, ‘Ecstatic Epilepsy: How Seizures Can Be Bliss’, New Scientist (22 January
2014), https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg22129531-000-ecstatic-epilepsy-how-
seizures-can-be-bliss/.
13. Ibid.
14. Clark and Holquist, Mikhail Bakhtin, p. 245.
15. P. J. Leithart, ‘The Monologue beyond the Dialogue’, First Things (24 March 2017), https://
www.firstthings.com/web-exclusives/2017/03/the-monologue-beyond-the-dialogue.
16. M. Berman, All That Is Solid Melts into Air (Verso, 1982).
17. N. Collins, ‘How Wuthering Heights Caused a Critical Stir When First Published in 1847’,
The Telegraph (22 March 2011), https://www.telegraph.co.uk/culture/tvandradio/8396278/
How-Wuthering-Heights-caused-a-critical-stir-when-first-published-in-1847.html.
18. A. Green, ‘Lockwood’s Cruelty in Wuthering Heights’, Owlcation (8 February 2018), https://
owlcation.com/humanities/Lockwoods-Cruelty-in-Wuthering-Heights.
19. N. Rogers, ‘Review: Chartism and Class Struggle’, Labour/Le Travail, 19:143–51 (1987).
20. T. Chaffin, ‘Frederick Douglass’s Irish Liberty’, The New York Times (25 February 2011), https://
opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2011/02/25/frederick-douglasss-irish-liberty/.
21. C. Fowler, ‘Was Emily Brontё’s Heathcliff black?’, The Conversation (25 October 2017), https://
theconversation.com/was-emily-bront-s-heathcliff-black-85341.
22. D. Bryfonski, Class Conflict in Emily Bronte’s Wuthering Heights (Greenhaven Press, 2011), p. 104.
23. K. Hughes, ‘The Strange Cult of Emily Brontë and the “Hot Mess” of Wuthering Heights’,
The Guardian (21 July 2018), https://www.theguardian.com/books/2018/jul/21/emily-bronte-
strange-cult-wuthering-heights-romantic-novel.
24. T. Eagleton, ‘Emily Brontë and the Great Hunger’, The Irish Review 12:108–19 (1992).
25. M. P. Asl, ‘Recurring Patterns: Emily Brontë’s Neurosis in Wuthering Heights’, International
Journal of Education and Literacy Studies, 2:46–50 (2014).
26. R. Onion, ‘A School Progress Report for the Brontë Sisters’, Slate (22 July 2014), http://www.
slate.com/blogs/the_vault/2014/07/22/history_of_the_bront_family_school_report_
assessing_their_progress_at_cowan.html.
27. B. Barnett, ‘Branwell Bronte: The Mad, Bad and Dangerous Brother of Charlotte, Emily and
Anne’, The Independent (17 September 2017), https://www.independent.co.uk/news/long_
reads/branwell-bronte-emily-charlotte-anne-family-haworth-yorkshire-a7940396.
html.
28. Asl, ‘Recurring Patterns’.
29. Ibid.
30. Ibid.
31. E. Bronte, Wuthering Heights (Penguin, 1965), p. 11.
32. Ibid.
33. Ibid., 12.
34. Ibid., 11.
35. C. Tóibín, ‘Pure Evil—Colm Tóibín on The Turn of the Screw’, The Guardian (3 June 2006),
https://www.theguardian.com/books/2006/jun/03/fiction.colmtoibin.
36. M. Norris, ‘Investigating Ambiguity: Sources of Insanity in “The Turn of the Screw” (P8)’,
Medium (12 May 2016), https://medium.com/@mrnorris/investigating-ambiguity-sources-
of-insanity-in-the-turn-of-the-screw-p8-551975c0c08c.
488
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
37. B. Leithauser, ‘Ever Scarier: On “The Turn of the Screw” ’, The New Yorker (29 October 2012),
https://www.newyorker.com/books/page-turner/ever-scarier-on-the-turn-of-the-screw.
38. J. Sexton, ‘A Non-Apparitionist Reading of The Turn of the Screw’, BC Open Textbooks (2014),
https://opentextbc.ca/englishliterature/wp-content/uploads/sites/27/2014/05/Non-
apparitionist-reading-of-Turn-of-Screw.pdf.
39. Norris, ‘Investigating Ambiguity’.
40. M. A. Nichols, ‘The Victims of Jack the Ripper’, The History Press (1 September 2011), https://
www.thehistorypress.co.uk/articles/the-victims-of-jack-the-ripper/.
41. Norris, ‘Investigating Ambiguity’.
42. Leithauser, ‘Ever Scarier’.
43. W. K. Penny, ‘Shattered Eden: Subjectivity and the Fall out of Language in Henry James’s
The Turn of the Screw’, Literary Imagination, 18:255–73 (2016).
44. Ibid.
45. Leithauser, ‘Ever Scarier’.
46. Ibid.
47. J. Carey, William Golding: The Man Who Wrote Lord of the Flies (Faber and Faber, 2009), pp. 190–205.
48. L. Surette, ‘A Matter of Belief: Pincher Martin’s Afterlife’, Twentieth Century Literature,
40:205–25 (1994).
49. D. Peter, ‘William Golding’s Pincher Martin: A Study of Self and Its Terror of Negation’,
McMaster University (December 1982), https://macsphere.mcmaster.ca/bitstream/11375/
10954/1/fulltext.pdf.
50. Surette, ‘A Matter of Belief’.
51. R. McCrum, ‘William Golding’s Crisis’, The Observer (11 March 2012), https://www.
theguardian.com/books/2012/mar/11/william-golding-crisis.
52. M. N. Singh, ‘Golding’s Pincher Martin: Monomania Caused Moral Degradation in Modern
Man’, International Journal on Studies in English Language and Literature, 4:22–6 (2016).
53. H. Goyal, ‘Predicament of the Protagonist in William Golding’s Pincher Martin’, Research
Journal of English Language and Literature, 5:285–91 (2017).
54. Ibid.
55. Y. Nir and G. Tononi, ‘Dreaming and the Brain: From Phenomenology to Neurophysiology’,
Trends in Cognitive Science, 14:88–100 (2010).
56. P. Iyer, ‘The Butler Didn’t Do It Again’, Times Literary Supplement (28 April 1995), https://www.
the-tls.co.uk/articles/public/the-unconsoled/.
57. M. Kakutani, ‘From Kazuo Ishiguro, A New Annoying Hero’, New York Times (17 October 1995),
https://www.nytimes.com/1995/10/17/books/books-of-the-times-from-kazuo-ishiguro-a-
new-annoying-hero.html.
58. Ibid.
59. S. Jordison, ‘The Unconsoled Deals in Destruction and Disappointment’, The Guardian (27
January 2015), https://www.theguardian.com/books/booksblog/2015/jan/27/kazuo-ishiguro-
reading-group.
60. Ibid.
61. C. Quarrie, ‘Impossible Inheritance: Filiation and Patrimony in Kazuo Ishiguro’s The
Unconsoled’, Studies in Contemporary Fiction, 55:138–51 (2014).
62. Jordison, ‘The Unconsoled Deals’.
63. A. H. Fairbanks, ‘Ontology and Narrative Technique in Kazuo Ishiguro’s “The Unconsoled”,
Studies in the Novel, 45:603–19 (2013).
64. Ibid.
489
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
490
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
25. R. Ford Denison and K. Muller, ‘The Evolution of Cooperation’, The Scientist (31 December
2016), https://www.the-scientist.com/features/the-evolution-of-cooperation-34284.
26. J. Carey, ‘No Need for Geniuses: Revolutionary Science in the Age of the Guillotine by Steve
Jones’, Sunday Times (27 March 2016), https://www.thetimes.co.uk/article/no-need-for-
geniuses-revolutionary-science-in-the-age-of-the-guillotine-by-steve-jones-w29mmpd39.
27. J. Parrington, ‘You Can Lead a Sperm to Ovum’, The Guardian (25 July 2002), https://www.
theguardian.com/science/2002/jul/25/medicalresearch.medicalscience.
28. L. W. Swanson and J. W. Lichtman, ‘From Cajal to Connectome and Beyond’, Annual Review
of Neuroscience, 39:197–216 (2016).
29. M. I. Kaiser, ‘The Limits of Reductionism in the Life Sciences’, History and Philosophy of the Life
Sciences, 33:453–76 (2011).
30. A. Blunden, ‘The Germ Cell of Vygotsky’s Science’, in Vygotsky and Marx: Towards a Marxist
Psychology (Routledge, 2017), pp. 132–45.
31. E. R. Scerri, ‘The Evolution of the Periodic System’, Scientific American (21 January 2011),
https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/the-evolution-of-the-periodic-system/.
32. C. Orzel, ‘What Has Quantum Mechanics Ever Done for Us?’, Forbes (13 August 2015),
https://www.forbes.com/sites/chadorzel/2015/08/13/what-has-quantum-mechanics-ever-
done-for-us/.
33. D. Overbye, ‘A Century Ago, Einstein’s Theory of Relativity Changed Everything’, New York
Times (24 November 2015), https://www.nytimes.com/2015/11/24/science/a-century-ago-
einsteins-theory-of-relativity-changed-everything.html.
34. D. Greenbaum and M. Gerstein, ‘Illuminating the Genome’s Dark Matter’, Cell, 163:1047–8
(2015).
35. Ibid.
36. Ibid.
37. C. S. Powell, ‘Relativity versus Quantum Mechanics: The Battle for the Universe’, The
Guardian (4 November 2015), https://www.theguardian.com/news/2015/nov/04/relativity-
quantum-mechanics-universe-physicists.
38. Ibid.
39. Ibid.
40. J. V. Wertsch, Vygotsky and the Social Formation of Mind (Harvard University Press, 1985), p. 158.
41. L. S. Vygotsky, Thought and Language (MIT Press, 1962), p. 318.
491
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
6. Ibid.
7. ‘Buddhism’, History (22 July 2020), https://www.history.com/topics/religion/buddhism.
8. S. Grogan, ‘ “A Solace to a Tortured World…” – The Growing Interest in Spiritualism during
and after WW1’, World War I Centenary (2014), http://ww1centenary.oucs.ox.ac.uk/author/
suzannegrogan.
9. Peoples et al., ‘Hunter-Gatherers and the Origins of Religion’.
10. Ibid.
11. L. Wade, ‘To Foster Complex Societies, Tell People a God Is Watching’, Science (4 March
2015), http://www.sciencemag.org/news/2015/03/foster-complex-societies-tell-people-god-
watching.
12. C. Seawright, ‘Shesmu, Demon-God of the Wine Press, Oils and Slaughterer of the
Damned’, Tour Egypt (2013), http://www.touregypt.net/featurestories/shesmu.htm.
13. J. Sharman, ‘Vatican Rushes to Deny Reports Pope Thinks Hell Doesn’t Exist and Sinning
Souls Just “Disappear” ’, The Independent (30 March 2018), https://www.independent.co.uk/
news/world/europe/pope-francis-hell-does-not-exist-catholic-vatican-full-quote-
a8281041.html.
14. Ibid.
15. K. Marx, ‘A Contribution to the Critique of Hegel’s Philosophy of Right: Introduction’,
Deutsch-Französische Jahrbücher (1844), https://www.marxists.org/archive/marx/works/
1843/critique-hpr/intro.htm.
16. F. Engels, ‘On the History of Early Christianity’, Die Neue Zeit (1894), https://www.marxists.
org/archive/marx/works/1894/early-christianity/index.htm.
17. S. McIntire and W. E. Burns, Speeches in World History (Facts on File, 2009), p. 104.
18. M. Empson, Kill All The Gentlemen: Class Struggle and Change in the English Countryside
(Bookmarks, 2018), p. 31.
19. A. Saville, ‘The 17th Century English Scientific Revolution’, The Queen’s College, Oxford (2020),
https://www.queens.ox.ac.uk/scientific%20revolution.
20. K. Thomas, Religion and the Decline of Magic: Studies in Popular Beliefs in Sixteenth and Seventeenth
Century England (Penguin, 1991).
21. J. Walker, ‘An Introduction To John Bunyan’s The Pilgrim’s Progress’, Banner of Truth (15
September 2005), https://banneroftruth.org/uk/resources/articles/2005/an-introduction-
to-john-bunyans-the-pilgrims-progress/.
22. R. McCrum, ‘The 100 Best Novels: No 1—The Pilgrim’s Progress by John Bunyan (1678)’, The
Guardian (23 September 2013), https://www.theguardian.com/books/2013/sep/23/100-best-
novels-pilgrims-progress.
23. Ibid.
24. Ibid.
25. R. Hampson, ‘What You Didn’t Know about King’s “Dream” Speech’, USA TODAY (12
August 2013), https://eu.usatoday.com/story/news/nation/2013/08/12/march-on-washington-
king-speech/2641841/.
26. Ibid.
27. N. Morgan, ‘The Story behind Martin Luther King’s “I Have a Dream” ’, Forbes (16 January
2012), https://www.forbes.com/sites/nickmorgan/2012/01/16/the-story-behind-martin-
luther-kings-i-have-a-dream/.
28. A. Rutherford, ‘ “There Is Grandeur in This View of Life” ’, The Guardian (15 February 2008),
https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2008/feb/15/thereisgrandeurinthisviewoflife.
492
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
29. B. Reese, ‘Interview with Christof Koch’, GigaOm (25 May 2018), https://gigaom.com/
2018/05/25/interview-with-christof-koch/.
30. S. McLeod, ‘Behaviorist Approach’, Simply Psychology (2020), https://www.simplypsychology.
org/behaviorism.html.
31. G. Johnson, ‘What Really Goes On in There’, New York Times (10 November 1991), https://
archive.nytimes.com/www.nytimes.com/books/98/12/06/specials/dennett-consciousness.
html?mcubz=1.
32. S. Ritchie, ‘Do Daniel C. Dennett’s Memes Deserve to Survive?’, The Spectator (4 March 2017),
https://www.spectator.co.uk/2017/03/do-daniel-c-dennetts-memes-deserve-to-survive/.
33. R. Dawkins, The Selfish Gene: 30th Anniversary Edition (Oxford University Press, 2006), p. 192.
34. Ibid.
35. J. Hughes, ‘Meme Theory: Do We Come up with Ideas or Do They, in Fact, Control Us?’,
The Independent (13 July 2012), https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/gadgets-and-tech/
features/meme-theory-do -we-come-up-with-ideas-or-do -they-in-fact-control-
us-7939077.html.
36. A. Ripstein, ‘Commodity Fetishism’, Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 17:733–48 (1987).
37. C. Koch, ‘Is Consciousness Universal?’, Scientific American (1 January 2014), https://www.
scientificamerican.com/article/is-consciousness-universal/.
38. J. White, ‘Is the Universe Self-aware?’, Episcopal Café (21 June 2017), https://www.
episcopalcafe.com/44054-2/.
39. J. Piper, ‘In the Beginning Was the Word’, Desiring God (21 September 2008), https://www.
desiringgod.org/messages/in-the-beginning-was-the-word.
40. T. Metzinger, Being No One: The Self-Model Theory of Subjectivity (MIT Press, 2004), p. 66.
41. H. James, ‘The Art of Fiction’, Longman’s Magazine (1884), https://public.wsu.edu/
~campbelld/amlit/artfiction.html.
42. M. J. Campion, ‘How the World Loved the Swastika—until Hitler Stole It’, BBC News (23
October 2014), https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/magazine-29644591.
43. Ibid.
44. L. Boissoneault, ‘The True Story of the Reichstag Fire and the Nazi Rise to Power’,
Smithsonian Magazine (21 February 2017), https://www.smithsonianmag.com/history/true-
story-reichstag-fire-and-nazis-rise-power-180962240/.
45. W. Bennett, ‘F is for Fascism’, Socialist Review (November 2007), http://socialistreview.org.
uk/319/f-fascism.
46. P. Bradshaw, ‘Amelie’, The Guardian (5 October 2001), https://www.theguardian.com/
film/2001/oct/05/1.
47. V. N. Voloshinov, Marxism and the Philosophy of Language (Harvard University Press, 1986),
p. 13.
493
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
3. Y. Chen, Y. Niu, and W. Ji, ‘Genome Editing in Nonhuman Primates: Approach to Generating
Human Disease Models’, Journal of Internal Medicine, 280:246–51 (2016).
4. S. Zhang, ‘China Is Genetically Engineering Monkeys with Brain Disorders’, The Atlantic
(8 June 2018), https://www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2018/06/china-is-genetically-
engineering-monkeys-with-brain-disorders/561866/.
5. Ibid.
6. D. Cyranoski, ‘China Is Positioning Itself as a World Leader in Primate Research’, Nature,
532:300–2 (2016).
7. Z. Rahim, ‘Scientist Injects Human Genes into Monkeys’ Brains and Makes Them More
Intelligent, Study Says’, The Independent (12 April 2019), https://www.independent.co.uk/
news/science/monkey-human-genes-brains-china-dna-intelligence-study-a8866616.html.
8. H. Brewis, ‘Chinese Scientists Create Super Monkeys by Injecting Brains with Human
DNA’, Evening Standard (12 April 2019), https://www.standard.co.uk/news/world/chinese-
scientists-create-super-monkeys-by-injecting-brains-with-human-dna-a4116916.html.
9. Ibid.
10. J. Hamill, ‘Chinese Scientists Create Super-intelligent Monkeys by Injecting Human DNA
into Their Brains’, Metro (12 April 2019), https://metro.co.uk/2019/04/12/chinese-scientists-
create-super-intelligent-monkeys-injecting-human-dna-brains-9183475/.
11. M. E. Coors, J. J. Glover, E. T. Juengst, and J. M. Sikela, ‘The Ethics of Using Transgenic Non-
human Primates to Study What Makes Us Human’, Nature Reviews in Genetics, 11:658–62
(2010).
12. I. Kelava and M. A. Lancaster, ‘Dishing out Mini-brains: Current Progress and Future
Prospects in Brain Organoid Research’, Developmental Biology, 420:199–209 (2016).
13. M. Molteni, ‘Mini Brains Just Got Creepier—They’re Growing Their Own Blood Vessels’,
Wired (3 April 2018), https://www.wired.com/story/mini-brains-just-got-creepiertheyre-
growing-their-own-veins/.
14. L. P. Hickey, ‘ “The Brain in a Vat” Argument’, Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy (2020), https://
www.iep.utm.edu/brainvat/.
15. I. Nathan, ‘Empire Essay: The Matrix Review’, Empire (1 January 2000), https://www.
empireonline.com/movies/empire-essay-matrix/review/.
16. Ibid.
17. T. Robey, ‘Ghost In the Shell Review: Scarlett Johansson’s Soulful Action Spectacle Proves
the Purists Wrong’, The Telegraph (31 March 2017), https://www.telegraph.co.uk/films/0/
ghost-review-scarlett-johanssons-soulful-action-spectacle-proves/.
18. J. Falconer, ‘What is the Technological Singularity?’, The Next Web (19 June 2011), https://
thenextweb.com/insider/2011/06/19/what-is-the-technological-singularity/.
19. N. Ismail, ‘AI: The Greatest Threat in Human History?’, Information Age (20 October 2016),
https://www.information-age.com/ai-greatest-threat-human-history-123462789/.
20. A. Sulleyman, ‘AI Is Highly Likely to Destroy Humans, Elon Musk Warns’, The Independent
(24 November 2017), https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/gadgets-and-tech/news/
elon-musk-artificial-intelligence-openai-neuralink-ai-warning-a8074821.html.
21. R. Cellan-Jones, ‘Stephen Hawking Warns Artificial Intelligence Could End Mankind’, BBC
News (2 December 2014), https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/technology-30290540.
22. M. Weisberger, ‘Why Does Artificial Intelligence Scare Us So Much?’, Live Science (8 June
2018), https://www.livescience.com/62775-humans-why-scared-of-ai.html.
23. T. Friend, ‘How Frightened Should We Be of A.I.?’, The New Yorker (7 May 2018), https://www.
newyorker.com/magazine/2018/05/14/how-frightened-should-we-be-of-ai.
494
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
24. Ibid.
25. M. Zastrow, ‘ “I’m in Shock!” How an AI Beat the World’s Best Human at Go’, New Scientist
(9 March 2016), https://www.newscientist.com/article/2079871-im-in-shock-how-an-ai-
beat-the-worlds-best-human-at-go/.
26. C. Woodford, ‘Neural Networks’, Explain That Stuff (17 June 2020), https://www.explainthatstuff.
com/introduction-to-neural-networks.html.
27. I. Sample, ‘ “It’s Able to Create Knowledge Itself”: Google Unveils AI That Learns on Its
Own’, The Guardian (18 October 2017), https://www.theguardian.com/science/2017/oct/18/
its-able-to-create-knowledge-itself-google-unveils-ai-learns-all-on-its-own.
28. Ibid.
29. Ibid.
30. J. Sokol, ‘Why Artificial Intelligence Like AlphaZero Has Trouble with the Real World’,
Quanta Magazine (21 February 2018), https://www.quantamagazine.org/why-alphazeros-
artificial-intelligence-has-trouble-with-the-real-world-20180221/.
31. Ibid.
32. J. C. Baillie, ‘Why AlphaGo Is Not AI’, Spectrum (17 March 2016), https://spectrum.ieee.org/
automaton/robotics/artificial-intelligence/why-alphago-is-not-ai.
33. M. Bennett-Smith, ‘Stephen Hawking: Brains Could Be Copied to Computers to Allow Life
after Death’, Huffington Post (25 September 2013), https://www.huffingtonpost.co.uk/entry/
stephen-hawking-brains-copied-life-after-death_n_3977682.
34. A. Regalado, ‘Nectome Will Preserve Your Brain, But You Have to Be Euthanized First’,
MIT Technology Review (13 March 2018), https://www.technologyreview.com/s/610456/a-
startup-is-pitching-a-mind-uploading-service-that-is-100-percent-fatal/.
35. D. B. Kirtley, ‘Futurist Says We’ll Use Lasers to Beam Our Minds Into Space Someday Soon’,
Wired (1 March 2014), https://www.wired.com/2014/03/geeks-guide-michio-kaku/.
495
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
11. R. Ostrow, ‘Crowd Control for the Mind: Coping with Inner Voices’, The Australian (15
December 2016), https://www.theaustralian.com.au/life/health-wellbeing/crowd-control-
for-the-mind-coping-with-inner-voices/news-story/103ea4e51b8bb3c5ee13a83ff2927e54.
12. P. de Sousa, W. Sellwood, A. Spray, C. Fernyhough, and R. P. Bentall, ‘Inner Speech and
Clarity of Self-Concept in Thought Disorder and Auditory-Verbal Hallucinations’, Journal of
Nervous Mental Disorders, 204:885–93 (2016).
13. C. Nordqvist, ‘Understanding the Symptoms of Schizophrenia’, Medical News Today (23 April
2020), https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/36942.php.
14. L. A. Sass and J. Parnas, ‘Schizophrenia, Consciousness, and the Self’, Schizophrenia Bulletin,
29:427–44 (2003).
15. S. Matthews, ‘Forget Anti-depressants, Doctors Should Be Able to Prescribe Music, Arts
and Writing Courses to Help Patients Suffering with the Blues, Claims GP’, Daily Mail (11 July
2018), http://www.dailymail.co.uk/health/article-5918013/Doctors-able-prescribe-music-
depressed-patients-claims-GP.html.
16. Ibid.
17. J. Doward, ‘Psychiatrists under Fire in Mental Health Battle’, The Observer (12 May 2013),
https://www.theguardian.com/society/2013/may/12/psychiatrists-under-fire-mental-
health.
18. H. Fraad, ‘Profiting from Mental Ill-Health’, The Guardian (15 March 2011), https://www.
theguardian.com/commentisfree/cifamerica/2011/mar/15/psychology-healthcare.
19. Ibid.
20. D. Joravsky, ‘The Mechanical Spirit: The Stalinist Marriage of Pavlov to Marx’, Theory and
Society, 4:457–77 (1977).
21. H. Katschnig, ‘Are Psychiatrists an Endangered Species? Observations on Internal and
External Challenges to the Profession’, World Psychiatry, 9:21–8 (2010).
22. ‘What is the Difference between Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry?’, Psychology School
Guide (2020), https://www.psychologyschoolguide.net/guides/difference-between-clinical-
psychology-and-psychiatry/.
23. W. Self, ‘Psychiatrists: The Drug Pushers’, The Guardian (3 August 2013), https://www.
theguardian.com/society/2013/aug/03/will-self-psychiatrist-drug-medication.
24. J. Nicolas, ‘Why Pretend Social Work Is about Social Justice? It’s Not’, The Guardian (20
October 2015), https://www.theguardian.com/social-care-network/2015/oct/20/why-pretend-
social-work-is-about-social-justice-its-not.
25. J. Larson, ‘Blaming Parents: What I’ve Learned and Unlearned as a Child Psychiatrist’,
Scientific American (15 April 2011), https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/guest-blog/blaming-
parents-what-ive-learned-and-unlearned-as-a-child-psychiatrist/.
26. K. Pickles, ‘Diabetes is the Fastest Modern Health Crisis after the Number of Cases Doubles
in the Last 20 Years with 3.7 Million People Being Diagnosed’, Daily Mail (27 February 2018),
http://www.dailymail.co.uk/health/article-5438433/Diabetes-fastest-modern-health-
crisis-cases-double.html.
27. S. Boseley, ‘Expert Revered for Painstaking Work That Proved Link between Smoking and
Cancer’, The Guardian (8 December 2006), https://www.theguardian.com/science/2006/
dec/08/cancer.uk.
28. L. Surugue, ‘Schizophrenia Is a “Modern” Disease, Developing after Humans Diverged
from Neanderthals’, International Business Times (16 August 2016), https://www.ibtimes.co.
uk/schizophrenia-modern-disease-developing-after-humans-diverged-neanderthals-
1576327.
496
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
29. Ibid.
30. D. E. Bloom, ‘7 Billion and Counting’, Science, 333:562–9 (2011).
31. ‘Life Expectancy of the World Population’, Worldometer (2020), worldometers.info/
demographics/life-expectancy/.
32. V. Cable, ‘Vince Cable Speech: Capitalism in Crisis’, Liberal Democrats (7 June 2018), https://
www.libdems.org.uk/capitalism_in_crisis.
33. M. El-Erian, ‘Is Stagnation the “New Normal” for the World Economy?’, The Guardian
(3 February 2016), https://www.theguardian.com/business/2016/feb/03/is-stagnation-the-
new-normal-for-the-world-economy.
34. D. Campbell, ‘Stress and Social Media Fuel Mental Health Crisis among Girls’, The Guardian
(23 September 2017), https://www.theguardian.com/society/2017/sep/23/stress-anxiety-
fuel-mental-health-crisis-girls-young-women.
35. Ibid.
36. Ibid.
37. J. Stiglitz, ‘Joseph Stiglitz: Why We Have to Change Capitalism’, The Telegraph (23 January
2010), https://www.telegraph.co.uk/finance/newsbysector/banksandfinance/7061058/
Joseph-Stiglitz-Why-we-have-to-change-capitalism.html.
38. J. Bennet, ‘ “We Need an Energy Miracle” ’, The Atlantic (November 2015), https://www.
theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2015/11/we-need-an-energy-miracle/407881/.
39. E. Green, ‘The Existential Dread of Climate Change: How Despair about Our Changing
Climate May Get in the Way of Fixing It’, Psychology Today (13 October 2017), https://www.
psychologytoday.com/us/blog/there-is-always-another-part/201710/the-existential-dread-
climate-change.
40. A. Marr, ‘Andrew Marr: British Politics Is Broken—the Centre Cannot Hold’, New Statesman
(23 March 2015), https://www.newstatesman.com/politics/2015/03/andrew-marr-british-
politics-broken-centre-cannot-hold.
41. I. Sample, ‘Scientists Find Genetic Mutation That Makes Woman Feel No Pain’, The Guardian
(28 March 2019), https://www.theguardian.com/science/2019/mar/28/scientists-find-
genetic-mutation-that-makes-woman-feel-no-pain.
42. Ibid.
43. Ibid.
44. Ibid.
45. Ibid.
46. Ibid.
47. N. Remnick, ‘At 100, Still Running for Her Life’, New York Times (22 April 2016), https://well.
blogs.nytimes.com/2016/04/22/at-100-still-running-for-her-life/.
48. Ibid.
49. Ibid.
50. Ibid.
51. N. Jayapalan, History of English Literature (Atlantic, 2001), p. 9.
52. O. J. Benedictow, ‘The Black Death: The Greatest Catastrophe Ever’, History Today (3 March
2005), https://www.historytoday.com/archive/black-death-greatest-catastrophe-ever.
53. M. Ibeji, ‘Black Death: Political and Social Changes’, BBC History (17 February 2011), http://
www.bbc.co.uk/history/british/middle_ages/blacksocial_01.shtml.
54. J. Neumann and J. Dettwiller, ‘Great Historical Events That Were Significantly Affected by
the Weather: Part 9, the Year Leading to the Revolution of 1789 in France (II)’, Bulletin
American Meteorological Society, 71:33–41 (1990).
497
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
e ndnot es
55. P. Ames, ‘Portugal Celebrates the World’s Coolest Coup’, Global Post (24 April 2014), https://
www.pri.org/stories/2014-04-24/portugal-celebrates-worlds-coolest-coup.
56. I. Birrell, ‘Greta Thunberg Teaches Us about Autism as Much as Climate Change’, The
Guardian (23 April 2019), https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2019/apr/23/greta-
thunberg-autism.
498
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
I N DE X OF NAME S
For the benefit of digital users, indexed terms that span two pages (e.g., 52–53) may, on
occasion, appear on only one of those pages
inde x of na mes
500
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of na mes
501
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of na mes
502
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of na mes
503
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of na mes
504
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of na mes
505
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of na mes
506
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
I N DE X OF SU BJ EC T S
For the benefit of digital users, indexed terms that span two pages (e.g., 52–53) may, on
occasion, appear on only one of those pages
inde x of su bjec ts
508
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
cannabis 163, 200, 430, 433 climate change 261, 428, 433–4
capitalism 267–71, 284, 333, 335, 365, 428 clinical depression 2, 7–11, 29, 170, 187, 189,
carriers of genetic disorders 26, 28, 242 203, 206–12, 214–20, 237, 273, 275, 417,
Cartesian dualism 22, 411 419–20, 424, 427–32
catatonic behaviour 195 clinical psychologists 6–7, 16, 421, 423
Catholic Church 19, 246, 294, 323, 381, 385 clockwork view of Universe 366
cats 94, 200, 349 cocaine 123
Cave Rescue Organization 379 cognition 76, 152, 168, 398
celebrities 55, 117–18, 416 cognitive behavioural therapy
cell body 63–4, 66, 97, 113 (CBT) 10–11, 207
cell signalling 28, 110–11, 129, 415, 417 cognitive behaviourism 10, 23
cellular differentiation 93, 197, 338 cognitive disinhibition 153
cerebellum 73, 135, 148–51, 324, 332, 413 collagen 82
cerebrum 72–3 collective consciousness 262, 317
charity 12, 47, 231, 266, 273 commodities 270–1, 333, 364–5, 389
Chartism 347 communication 44, 55–6, 69, 74, 91, 112,
chemicals 8, 69, 159, 162–3, 179, 188, 210–11, 114, 120, 125, 128, 137, 162, 194, 196, 198–9,
385, 392, 400, 414–15, 425 204, 223, 233–4, 307, 328, 384, 406–7, 415
chemistry 160, 203, 368, 414 compensation 116, 195, 418
child development 50–51, 123 competition 5, 297, 329, 366, 428
children 23, 26, 29–30, 42, 49–54, 57–8, 90, Comptine d’Une Aute Été
104–5, 123–5, 131–7, 140–2, 157–8, 161, 176, (instrumental) 393–4
193, 195, 198, 219–20, 224–5, 227–9, 231–5, computer games 165, 167
242, 246–50, 256, 263, 266, 271, 295, 303, computerized tomography 74
320–2, 350–2, 385, 400, 408, 417, 427, 432 concentration camps 219–20
chimpanzees 3, 24, 32–4, 39, 41–4, 60, 71, conceptual thought 52, 124, 196, 198,
74, 76–7, 79, 85–8, 126, 132, 159, 359, 370, 231–2, 322, 338
406, 413 connectivity 77, 119–20, 134, 183, 213, 253
Christianity 379–80, 382 conscious awareness 21, 24, 32, 49, 63, 87,
CHRNA7 gene 227 153, 201, 203, 278–9, 299, 304, 309–10,
chromosome deletion 226–7 314–6, 321–2, 352, 359, 371, 384–6, 389–91,
chromosome duplication 226–7 394, 398, 400, 402–3, 407, 409, 412,
chromosomes 28, 30, 82–3, 190–2, 418–20, 422, 426
226–7, 242 contradictions 15, 103, 178, 194, 231, 254,
cinematography 325, 330, 335-9 267–8, 286, 308, 312–3, 317, 345, 382
circulatory system 20 contradictory consciousness 285–6
cities 33, 123, 135, 261, 264–5, 267, 280, 298, cooperation 152, 233, 366, 382
326, 363–4, 426 cooperative learning strategy 233
city states 264–6 Copernican revolution 32, 344
civil war 21, 232, 294, 336, 383 copy number variants (CNVs) 191–3
civilization 1, 33, 46, 109, 168, 261, 263–5, corpus callosum 152, 180–1
267, 278, 310, 314, 325, 380, 389, 426–8, 435 corticosterone 250
classroom teaching 53, 149, 161, 234, 361 cortisol 166, 219
509
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
510
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
511
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
frontal lobes 78, 146, 184, 236, 284 gorillas 42–4, 74, 85, 159, 413
fruit flies 96, 98, 100, 361 Götterdämmerung (opera) 311
functional magnetic resonance imaging grammar 42, 44, 54, 126–7, 182
(fMRI) 96, 133, 143, 147, 149, 153, 306–7 gravitational waves 151
funerals 375, 377, 394 Great Depression (1930s) 215, 296
Futurism 331 group identity 78
fuzzy nature of inner thought 138, 392 growth factors 93
guilt 160, 225, 252
GABA neurotransmitter 179–80 gypsies 219
Gaboxadol drug 179
galaxies 1, 369 haemoglobin 82
gambling 165 haemophilia 28
Gaping Ghyll pothole 209, 378 hallucinations 11, 169, 195, 212, 419
gastrulation 92 harassment 275
gender 263, 272–3, 282, 288 HARE5 gene regulatory region 86
gene editing 3, 99, 129, 216, 396–7, 399 harpsichord 314–5
gene expression 83–6, 91, 98, 112–4, 159, Healing Expressive and Recovery Arts
189, 249, 369, 371, 386, 406 (HERA) programme 420
gene regulatory networks 28 health insurance 13
gene regulatory proteins 110, 112–3 health professionals 6, 201, 422, 424
gene variants 8, 190, 215, 242 healthcare 11–12, 203, 421, 423
genetic code 5, 25 hearing 59, 73, 96, 132, 135, 192, 306, 338,
genetic disorders 26 401, 419
genetics 5–6, 25, 31, 193, 212, 369, 417 heart disease 7, 29, 189, 201
genius 69, 140, 150–3, 172, 237, 315, 328–9, heart rate 73, 244
331, 346, 356 heaven 327, 381
genome-wide association studies hell 326–7, 380–1
(GWAS) 7, 188–91 hemimegalencephaly 90
genomic regions 30, 188, 191, 200, 418 heresy 382
germline mutations 90 heroin 164–7, 206
gestures 40, 44, 131, 194, 233 higher mental functions 21, 73, 104, 138,
ghost in the machine 387 146–50, 158, 160, 195–7, 199, 243, 370
Ghost in the Shell (film) 402 hindbrain 72–3, 92
giant squids 65 Hinduism 377
gibbons 304 hippocampus 70, 73, 110, 113–121, 124,
glamorization 319, 333 179–80
glial cells 4, 68–71, 80, 86, 91, 93, 101–2, histones 83–5, 88, 249
112, 114, 151, 192, 213, 216, 386, 390, Holocaust 219–20, 292, 297
406–8, 415 homeless 6, 13, 232, 295
global positioning systems (GPS) 117, 368 hominids 36, 39
glutamate neurotransmitter 179–80, 188 Homo erectus 39, 75, 168
Go board game 405 Homo habilis 38, 75
gonads 156 Homo sapiens 32–3, 75, 168, 261–2, 304
512
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
513
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
514
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
measles, mumps, and rubella (MMR) Mind (British mental health charity) 12
vaccine 228 mind shift 16
medial ganglionic eminence (MGE) 101–2 miners’ strike (UK, 1984–5) 280–2
medial prefrontal cortex 183 mini-brains 101
Medici family 329 minicolumns 114
medieval era 19, 326, 382 Miracle/Master Mineral Solution
meditation 143, 169, 183 (MMS) 228
medulla 73 mirror neurons 67–8, 131–2, 323
melody 307 miscarriage 101
memes 387–8, 390, 412 Modern Devotion religious
memories 71, 73, 109–10, 112, 114–5, 117–8, movement 326
157, 173–4, 178–80, 183, 402, 408, 416, 432 modern synthesis in genetics 369
memorization 105, 121 modernism in art 330, 335
mental development 50, 52, 54, 232 money laundering 256
mental disorder 2–4, 6–14, 23, 101, 139, monkeys 67, 99–100, 129, 131, 144, 158–9,
153–5, 175, 184, 187–8, 191, 193, 198, 203, 398–9, 404, 406
205–6, 208, 214–5, 217–22, 225, 235, 237, monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) 242
247, 252, 257, 269, 272, 274, 288, 290, 319, monogamy 266
328, 399, 415, 417, 419–30 monophonic fiction 343
mental distress 14, 187, 203, 292, 348, montage in film 336–7
421, 427 Moon 1, 396
mental health 12–14, 206, 211, 236–7, 247, morality 174–5, 245, 379
251, 272–5, 292, 420–2, 426–7 motivation 23, 58, 177, 232–3, 235, 329, 348,
mental health nurses 12 353, 386
mental illness 8, 10, 13, 153, 237, 292 motor cortex 77, 323
mental processes 73, 140, 171, 245, 279, 305 motor neurons 66
mental state 156, 179, 208, 221, 223, 292 movies 142
mescaline 163 MRI scanners 149, 368
mesoderm 92 multi-voicedness 56
messenger RNA 81–3, 90–1, 112–3 multiple personality disorder 59
meta-analysis 11, 212, 322 multiple sclerosis 78
metabolism 366 multipolar neurons 66
metric system 366 multitasking 166
mice 3, 71, 86, 94–5, 99–100, 118, 121, murder 206, 219, 224, 239–45, 247, 251, 253,
126–8, 179, 217–9, 228, 397–8 255, 263, 267, 276–7, 290–2, 297, 352, 354
micro RNAs 113, 179, 219 muscles 66
microcephaly 227 music 2, 16, 96, 137, 143, 149, 152, 299,
microelectrodes 67, 117 303–19, 322–3, 330, 337–8, 355, 360–1, 371,
microglia 70–1 386, 393–4, 412, 416, 420
midbrain 72–3, 92 musical instruments 303, 310
middling sort in English revolution 291 mutations 8, 83, 89–90, 97–9, 131–2,
military tensions 261 226–7, 368, 417, 430–2
Milky Way 1 myelin 64–5, 68, 77–9, 413
515
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
516
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
517
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
518
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
519
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
society 2, 5–6, 13, 15, 21–2, 27, 39, 45–7, 49, St Paul’s Cathedral 209, 375
54–5, 104, 138–9, 161, 163–4, 167, 177–8, staged drama 342
187, 193–4, 199, 218, 221–2, 229–30, 232, Stalinism 54, 291, 353, 422
237, 246, 250, 252, 255–7, 261–3, 265–8, Star Trek (TV and film series) 396
270–5, 277–9, 282, 284–7, 291–2, 294–6, stars 1, 5, 150, 389
298–9, 305, 307–11, 314, 316–9, 326–8, stem cells 85–8, 93, 100–1, 226, 399–400,
330, 332–3, 335, 341–2, 345, 357, 359, 402, 415
363–6, 370, 377, 380–4, 388, 390, 393, stomach 156
402, 409, 423, 426–8, 434–5 stoned ape hypothesis 168
socioeconomic status 218 strangers 43, 47, 308, 391, 394
sodium ions 65 stream of consciousness 330
Solar System 1, 261, 344, 396 street art 335
somatic mutations 89–90 stress 4, 84, 166, 179–80, 187, 198, 201, 211–2,
songbirds 128, 158 218–20, 236, 250, 269, 272, 427–8, 432
songs 36, 123, 128, 158, 304–7, 309–11, 314, striatum 100, 159, 413
317–9 strikes 278–84, 352, 352
soul 19–22, 23–4, 63, 174, 281, 313, 317–8, 346, stuttering 71
351, 375, 377, 381, 386, 393, 407, 411, 414 Suez Crisis (1956) 353
Soviet Union 49, 231–2, 291, 295–6, 338 suicide 2, 12–14, 187, 202, 205–6, 209, 273,
soviets (workers’ and peasants’ councils) in 319, 350, 394
Russian revolution 287 Sun 32, 344, 392
space travel 1, 140, 396, 408–9, 426 superego 174–6
Space: 1999 (TV series) 396 superior temporal gyrus 135, 307
space-time 150, 403 supernatural 21–2, 32, 346, 349–50, 375–6,
special needs 231–2 378–9, 384–6, 394
Spectrum: Flight of Forms (painting) 336 support network 12, 282
speech 34, 39, 41, 51, 53–9, 72, 126–7, suprematism 331, 334
129–30, 133, 135–9, 158, 178, 195–7, 224, swastika 392–3
234, 272, 289, 305, 338, 384, 404, 432 symbolism 324, 329, 418
speech genres 56–7, 138 symbols 42, 59, 96, 142, 147, 304, 322, 412
spelling 54, 126–7 synaesthesia 338
sperm 4, 84, 88, 90, 92, 190–1, 219, 367, 388 synapses 1, 65–6, 69–71, 88, 97, 99, 102,
spinal cord 64, 66, 92 110–3, 127, 210, 213, 226, 406–7
spiritualism 378 synaptic pruning 70–1, 192
spirituality 310, 371, 377 syncretic thought 52
Split (film) 252 synthetic cubism 331
split brains 180–2
split personality 252–3 T2 Nazi euthanasia programme 224
spontaneity 43, 53, 183, 287, 293 talking cure 12, 178, 184
sport 58, 171, 349 taxation 265, 382
SRGAP2 gene 398 teachers 43, 103, 135, 139, 161–2, 204, 234,
SRPX2 gene 127–8 254, 356, 360, 430
520
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
technology 1–2, 39–40, 100, 111, 128, 151, The Waste Land (poem) 239
168, 299, 310, 314, 327, 330, 335, 339, theft 245
359–60, 363–5, 376, 389, 401, 403, 408, 435 theory of mind 223, 245, 376
teenagers 78, 114, 165, 198, 208, 244, 324 thermodynamics 365
teeth 36, 163, 377 thirst 73, 278, 340
telepathy 194 Thirty Years War 380
tempo 314 thoughts 10, 14–15, 48, 57, 121, 138, 153,
terrorist organizations 205, 256, 402 171–2, 174–5, 178, 180, 183, 194, 202, 208,
testicles 219 285, 322, 325, 338, 341–4, 351, 354–5, 362,
testosterone 250 364, 370, 375–6, 390–2, 397, 400, 404,
textile industry 239, 365 418–9, 422, 434
thalamic reticular nucleus (TRN) 120–1 thryoid gland 156
thalamus 73, 120–1, 156 tissues 63, 74–5, 86, 92, 156, 189, 367, 392
The Big Bang Theory (TV series) 359 tobacco 163–4
The Brothers Karamazov (book) 343 tone deafness 71
The Castle (book) 355 tongue 58, 66, 126, 401
The Crowd: A Study of the Popular Mind tool-making 38–41, 49, 76
(book) 293 tools 33–40, 49, 104, 133, 141, 150, 157,
The Deeper Genome (book) 82, 189, 369 159, 278, 304, 306, 308–9, 322, 337,
The Descent of Man (book) 34 359–60, 370
The Garden of Earthly Delights top-down model of mind 122, 147, 417
(painting) 326–7 torture 245, 327, 349
The Girl from Ipanema (song) 311 tortured-genius 237
The Hall of the Bulls, Lascaux Toxoplasma gondii 200
(paintings) 321 trade 240, 282–3, 326, 347, 363
The Haywain (painting) 326 trade union 56
The Holy Bible (album) 319 traditional art 330, 332
The Magic Flute (opera) 313 transcranial Doppler ultrasonography
The Marriage of Figaro (opera) 313 (fTCD) 40
The Matrix (film) 401 transcranial magnetic stimulation
The Moth Radio Hour (radio series) 132 (TMS) 323
The Part Played by Labour in the transcription 81, 113
Transition from Ape to Man transcription factors 82–3
(essay) 35 transitions 35, 51, 73, 104, 131, 141–2, 195,
The Pilgrim’s Progress (book) 383–4 314–5, 326, 382, 403
The Possessed (painting) 345 translation 113, 225
The Promenade (painting) 336 transposons 84, 88–9
The Selfish Gene (book) 243, 388 trauma 6, 11, 174, 179–80, 187, 192, 201, 214,
The Star Spangled Banner 219–20, 222, 226, 245–7, 250, 289, 349,
(instrumental) 316 351, 430
The Turn of the Screw (book) 350, 352 tricyclics drugs 210–11
The Unconsoled (book) 355–6 tritone 311
521
OUP CORRECTED AUTOPAGE PROOFS – FINAL, 25/01/21, SPi
inde x of su bjec ts
522