Fernandez 2015
Fernandez 2015
Fernandez 2015
Rocío Fernández-Ballesteros
“As they say, when the age is in, the wit is out.”
Much Ado About Nothing III, 5
William Shakespeare
“It should be noted that one writes not with grey hairs but with the
understanding that generally improves with the years.”
Prologue, Don Quixote, Part II
Miguel de Cervantes
The stereotype threat perspective (Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995;
see Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002, for a review) explains underperfor-
mance in members of minority groups (in domains relevant for the group
stereotype) as being due in part to the influence of negative stereotypes.
When such negative stereotypes exist, and the member of the group has
to perform a task in the specific domain negatively associated with the
group stereotype, he or she worries about confirming the stereotype. This
self-evaluative threat will hinder performance.
A number of studies have focused on the effects of stereotype threat in
older people, finding reductions in different aspects of their cognitive func-
tioning, such as memory performance (Chasteen, Bhattacharyya, Horhota,
Tam, & Hasher, 2005; Hess, Auman, Colcombe, & Rahhal, 2003; Hess,
Hinson, & Hodges, 2009; Rahal, Hasher, & Colcombe, 2001), general
cognitive performance, including different intellectual abilities (Abrams,
Eller, & Bryant, 2006), and mathematics test performance (Abrams et al.,
2008).
In the case of memory, Rahal et al. (2001) compared performance in
a memory task by younger and older participants by varying the task
instructions: in one condition the task was presented as a test of mem-
ory, whereas in the other the memory component was de-emphasized,
by presenting the task as a test of the ability to learn facts (neutral
condition). These researchers found that although older participants’ per-
formance was poorer than that of the younger ones in the memory task,
there was no difference in performance between them in the neutral con-
dition. Further support for the stereotype threat effect was obtained by
Hess and colleagues (2003), who used a stronger manipulation that con-
sisted in presenting information, allegedly from research reports about
aging and memory, to older and younger adults and comparing their per-
formance. Depending on the condition, the information either backed up
(negative condition) or contradicted (positive condition) negative views on
the effects of aging on mental abilities. A third, neutral control condition,
with no prior information, was also included. Older participants showed
lower recall in the negative condition compared with either the positive
or the neutral condition. In another study, Chasteen et al. (2005) studied
the effects of memory and impression formation instructions in a memory
Compensating for Stereotype Threat? 413
task with older and younger participants and expected a smaller differ-
ence between them in the nonthreatening impression formation task. They
found poorer performance of the older group in both conditions—that is, a
main effect of age and no interaction with task instructions. However, they
found that feelings of stereotype threat mediated the relation between age
and performance, indicating that age increased the feelings of threat and
these were negatively related to performance. In general, studies in this
area of cognitive performance in older people have shown that minimizing
the diagnosticity of the task appears to counteract stereotype threat effects
on performance.
In line with works in the more general area of stereotype threat (Maass &
Cadinu, 2003), attention has been focused on individual difference vari-
ables that may moderate the effects of stereotype threat on cognitive task
performance by older adults. Hess and coworkers (2003) found that the
value participants placed on their memory ability increased the effects of
stereotype threat on performance. This result is in line with the view that
people who value a given domain are more vulnerable to stereotype threat
(Steele & Aronson, 1995). Desrichard and Köpetz (2005) showed the mod-
erator role of memory self-efficacy on stereotype threat effects in older
adults when they performed a memory task and the memory component
was emphasized. Lower memory self-efficacy correlated with lower lev-
els of performance in that condition, but not in the condition where the
memory component was not highlighted.
Hess et al. (2009) considered the moderating role of age, education,
and concern about being stigmatized in the effects of stereotype threat on
memory performance. They were able to show that this threat had greater
effects in the young seniors group (60–70) than in the older group (71–80),
whereas stigma consciousness affected performance more in the older
group. Moreover, these effects were stronger in the more educated partic-
ipants. Kang and Chasteen (2009) examined stereotype threat effects in a
sample of older women (62–85). These authors included not only memory
performance but also positive and negative emotions experienced by par-
ticipants. Identification with the age group and perceived stereotype threat
were considered as potential moderators. They used a stereotype threat
manipulation that approaches what older people may encounter in their
daily lives: a memory task (memory for a prose passage) in the presence
of younger adults. Different recall (free and cued recall) and recognition
414 R. Fernández-Ballesteros et al.
measures were employed. The main results of this study show the effect of
stereotype threat on decrements in free recall. Also, recall performance fell
as age group identification increased. Perceived stereotype threat moder-
ated the effects of threat so that those in the threat condition who perceived
greater threat showed greater memory decrements and a reduction of pos-
itive emotions. No moderation effects of age group identification were
found.
In other domains of cognitive performance by older people, prior posi-
tive intergenerational contact moderated the impact of stereotype threat,
thus appearing to reduce their vulnerability to it (Abrams et al., 2006,
2008).
The aim of this research is to study the effects of stereotype threat and
an intervention strategy that includes counterstereotypical information for
preventing the effects of stereotype threat on performance in older par-
ticipants. These effects are expected to be moderated by the participant’s
self-perception of aging. As pointed out earlier, according to Levy and
coworkers’ view of self-stereotyping (Levy et al., 2002a,b; Levy, 2003,
2009), positive self-perception of aging is related to the internalization
of positive stereotypes of older adults. That is, the better one’s self-
perception of aging, the more positive one’s personal beliefs about the
aged. It has also been shown from the same perspective that positive
self-perception of aging is associated with longevity and other positive
health outcomes in older adult people (Levy et al., 2002a,b). It could be
hypothesized that older people who internalize positive age stereotypes
and reject negative ones will be less vulnerable to the activation of a neg-
ative older adult stereotype in stereotype threat situations. The relation
between positive views about the elderly and healthy and successful aging
has also been clearly pointed out and tested through a quasi-experimental
design by Fernández-Ballesteros and her group (Fernández-Ballesteros,
Caprara, Iñíguez, & García, 2005). These authors showed that a pro-
gram for active aging (training healthy and active lifestyles) significantly
improved self-perception of aging, as well as certain positive behaviors
regarding aging (for a review, see Caprara et al., 2013). Also, and again
using a quasi-experimental design, they reported that 1-year university
courses for older adults (in both humanities and sciences) taught at five
universities in four countries (Cuba, Chile, Mexico, and Spain) produced
an improvement in self-perception of aging in older participants, and also
better group stereotypes, whereas controls did not change from baseline
(Fernández-Ballesteros et al., 2012).
Our study explores the role of self-perception of aging in moderating
the effects on memory performance, under conditions of stereotype threat.
In order to test whether or not we obtain effects consistent with aging
stereotypes, two types of information were employed in two different con-
ditions: the first type referred to the fact that older adults could maintain
good memory skills, whereas the second type referred to underperfor-
mance of the older adults in memory tasks as compared with younger
416 R. Fernández-Ballesteros et al.
METHODS
The sample was composed of 112 participants (61% women) taking part
in the first year of the University Programme for Older Adults (PUMA),
with an age range of 55 to 78 years (M = 61.72, SD = 5.26). PUMAs are
3-year courses on humanities, social and natural sciences. Admission is by
means of a standard examination, and educational level, reported health,
and cognitive abilities are higher in these students than in people from the
same-age Spanish population.
Participants were randomly assigned to the experimental conditions, at
the beginning of a course on the psychology of the life span. Experimental
conditions involved positive information about the memory performance
of older adults or negative information about their memory performance
(the positive information and negative information conditions, respec-
tively). A control group (n = 34; 61% women, ages from 55 to 78,
Compensating for Stereotype Threat? 417
Measures
Memory Task
The first trial of the Auditory Verbal Learning Test developed by
Fernández-Ballesteros, Zamarrón, and Tárraga (2005) was administered.
Participants were instructed as follows: You are going to read a list of
15 words. When you have finished you must write down as many words as
you can remember from the list on the second page. It does not matter in
which order your write the words. Your task is to remember as many words
as you can, after reading it just once. Please, read the word list just once,
and on the second page, write as many words as you can. Do not go back
to the first page.
Self-perceptions of Aging
Following Levy et al. (2002a,b), five items from the PGCMS (Lawton,
1975) were administered: Things keep getting worse as I get older; I have
as much pep as I did last year; As you get older, you are less useful; I am as
happy now as I was when I was younger; and As I get older, things are (bet-
ter than, worse than, or the same as) I thought they would be. Following
the procedure proposed by Levy et al. (p. 263), negative responses to the
first four items were coded as 0, whereas positive answers were coded as 1.
Responses to the fifth item were changed to a dichotomous format com-
bining the responses the same as and better than as 1 and worse than as 0.
418 R. Fernández-Ballesteros et al.
Hypothesis
In a first step, and following the findings from the stereotype threat lit-
erature (see Steele & Aronson, 1995), poorer memory performance is
expected in the stereotype threat, negative information condition, as com-
pared with the positive information condition and the control condition
(H1). Additionally, an interaction effect of self-perception of aging on
the memory performance task is expected. Participants with better self-
perception of aging will be less influenced by the negative information,
whereas participants with lower scores in self-perception of aging will
display the poorest free recall in the negative feedback condition (H2).
RESULTS
Main effects
β0 8.58 .39 21.79 .00
βself-perception 1.58 .82 1.91 .06
βDummy1 −.67 .54 1.23 .22
βDummy2 .25 .53 .47 .64
Interaction effects
βDummy1 × Self-perception −2.27 2.18 1.04 .30
βDummy2 × Self-perception −5.77 2.20 2.73 .01
DISCUSSION
factor in a different data set. Levy et al. (2002a, p. 412) also noted that these five items load high on a
single factor (above .80), using an Ohio Longitudinal Study of Aging and Retirement sample (OLSAR;
Levy et al., 2002a).
422 R. Fernández-Ballesteros et al.
similar result was obtained by Kang and Chasteen (2009), who found that,
regardless of the stereotype threat condition, a decrease in free recall as
a function of age-group identification occurred. Abrams et al. (2006) did
not find that high identifiers among the older adults were more affected by
stereotype threat than low identifiers in cognitive task performance.
The major limitation of the present work is that our design is not
a true experiment with random assignment to experimental and control
conditions, and this shortcoming will be addressed in future studies that
would allow us to see whether we replicate the present findings of the
intermediate effects of the allegedly neutral condition.
We should also broaden the scope of the research by looking at per-
formance in types of cognitive task other than memory tasks, in line with
recent work in this area focusing on mathematics performance (Abrams
et al., 2008) or general cognitive tasks such as those used in aptitude tests
(Abrams et al., 2006).
In accordance with the present emphasis in stereotype threat research
on the processes involved in it (Schmader, Johns, & Forbes, 2008), we
should consider what kind of processes might be activated when cogni-
tive tasks are presented to older people who differ in their self-perception
of aging. It may be that different regulatory focus is involved (promotion
or prevention), the former in the case of positive self-perception of aging
and the latter in the case of negative self-perception. Furthermore, stereo-
type threat situations might involve an alternative perception—challenge
versus threat—for older people who differ in their views about their own
aging.
FUNDING
This project was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Older
Adults and Social Services (IMSERSO-I+D+i-34-2006) and by PSI2010-
17700
REFERENCES
Abrams, D., Crisp, R. J., Marques, S., Fagg, E., Bedford, L., & Provias, D. (2008).
Threat inoculation: Experienced and imagined intergenerational contact prevents
stereotype threat effects on older people’s math performance. Psychology and
Aging, 23, 934–939. doi:10.1037/a0014293
Abrams, D., Eller, A., & Bryant, J. (2006). An age apart: The effects of inter-
generational contact and stereotype threat on performance and intergroup bias.
Psychology and Aging, 21, 691–702. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.691
Compensating for Stereotype Threat? 423
Blanchard-Fields, F., & Horhota, M. (2006). How can the study of aging inform
research on social cognition? Social Cognition, 24, 207–217. doi:10.1521/
soco.2006.24.3.207
Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M., & Harvey, R. (1999). Perceiving pervasive
discrimination among African-Americans: Implications for group identifica-
tion and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 135–149.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.1.135
Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD. Neurone,
22, 196–208.
Caprara, M. G., Molina, M. A., Schettini, R., Santacreu, M., Orosa, T., Mendoza,
V., . . . Fernández-Ballesteros, R. (2013). Active aging promotion. Results from
vital aging program. Current Gerontology and Geriatric Research, Special Issue on
Active Aging. A Global Goal. Art ID: 817813,14 pages. Retrieved from. http://dx.
doi.org/10.1155/2013/817813.
Chasteen, A. L., Bhattacharyya, S., Horhota, M., Tam, R., & Hasher, L. (2005).
How feelings of stereotype threat influence older adults’ memory performance.
Experimental Aging Research, 31, 235–260. doi:10.1080/03610730590948177
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple
regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Cuddy, A. J. C., & Fiske, S. T. (2002). Doddering but dear: Process, content and func-
tion in stereotyping of older persons. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping
and prejudice against older persons (pp. 3–26). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Norton, M. I., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). This old stereotype: The per-
vasiveness and persistence of the elderly stereotype. Journal of Social Issues, 61,
267–285. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00405.x
Desrichard, O., & Köpetz, C. (2005). A threat in the elder: The impact of
task instructions, self-efficacy and performance expectations on memory per-
formance in the elderly. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 537–552.
doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992
Fernández-Ballesteros, R., Caprara, M. G., Iñiguez, J. y García, L .F. (2004).
Promoción del envejecimiento activo: Efectos del programa “Vivir con
Vitalidadâ”. Revista de Geriatría y Gerontología, 40, 150–161.
Fernández-Ballesteros, R., Caprara, M. G., Schettini, R., Bustillos, A., Mendoza-
Núñez, V., Orosa, R., . . . Zamarrón, M. D. (2012). Effects of University programs
for older adults: Changes in cultural and group stereotypes, self-perception of
aging, and emotional balance. Educational Gerontology, 39, 119–131.
Fernández-Ballesteros, R. Zamarrón, M. D. & Tarraga, L. (2005). Learning
potential: A new method for assessing cognitive impairment. International
Psychogeriatrics, 17, 119–128.
Garska, T. A., Schmitt, M. T. Branscomb, N. R. E. & Hummert, M. L. (2004). How
young and older adults differ in their responses to perceived age discrimination.
Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 326–335. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.326
Geraci, L., & Miller, T. M. (2013). Improving older adults’ memory per-
formance using prior task success. Psychology and Aging, 28, 340–345.
doi:10.1037/a0030332
424 R. Fernández-Ballesteros et al.
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical mediation analysis with a mul-
ticategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical
Psychology, 67, 451–470.
Hess, T. M., Auman, C., Colcombe, S. J., & Rahhal, T. A. (2003). The impact
of stereotype threat on age differences in memory performance. The Journals
of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, P3–P11.
doi:10.1093/geronb/58.1.P3
Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., & Hodges, E. A. (2009). Moderators of and mecha-
nisms underlying stereotype threat effects on older adults’ memory performance.
Experimental Aging Research, 35, 153–177. doi:10.1080/03610730802716413
Kang, S. K., & Chasteen, A. L. (2009). The moderating role of age-group
identification and perceived threat on stereotype threat among older adults.
The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 69, 201–220.
doi:10.2190/AG.69.3.c
Kite, M. E., Stockdale, G. D., Whitley, B. E., & Johnson, B. T. (2005). Attitudes
toward younger and older adults: An up-dated meta-analytic review. Journal of
Social Issues, 61, 241–266. doi:10.1111/josi.2005.61.issue-2
Lawton, M. P. (1975). The Philadelphia Geriatric Center Morale Scale: A Revisión.
Journal of Gerontology, 30(1), 85–89. doi:10.1093/geronj/30.1.85
Levy, B. R. (1996). Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 71, 1092–1107. doi:10.1037/
0022-3514.71.6.1092
Levy, B. R. (2003). Mind matters: Cognitive and physical effects of aging self-
stereotypes. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social
Sciences, 58, P203–P211. doi:10.1093/geronb/58.4.P203
Levy, B. R. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: A psychosocial approach to
aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 332–336. doi:10.1111/
(ISSN)1467-8721
Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., & Kasl, S. V. (2002a). Longitudinal benefit
of positive self-perceptions of aging on functional health. The Journals of
Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, P409–P417.
doi:10.1093/geronb/57.5.P409
Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., Kunkel, S. R., & Kasl, S. V. (2002b). Longevity increased
by positive self-perceptions of aging. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 83,
261–270. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.261
Levy, B. R., Zonderman, A., Slade, M. D., & Ferrucci, L. (2009). Negative age stereo-
types held earlier in life predict cardiovascular events in later life. Psychological
Science, 20, 296–298. doi:10.1111/psci.2009.20.issue-3
Liang, J., & Bollen, K. A. (1985). Sex differences in the structure of the
Philadelphia Geriatric Center Morale Scale. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 468–477.
doi:10.1093/geronj/40.4.468
Löckenhoff, C. E., De Fruyt, F., Terracciano, A., McCrae, Robert, R., De Bolle, M.,
Costa Jr., P. T., . . . & Yik, M. (2009). Perceptions of aging across 26 cultures and
their culture-level associates. Psychology and Aging, 24(4): 941–954. doi:http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016901
Compensating for Stereotype Threat? 425
Maass, A., & Cadinu, M. (2003). Stereotype threat: When minority mem-
bers underperform. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 243–275.
doi:10.1080/10463280340000072
Nelson, T. D. (2005). Ageism: Prejudice against our feared future self. Journal of
Social Issues, 61, 207–221. doi:10.1111/josi.2005.61.issue-2
O’Brien, L. T., & Hummert, M. L. (2006). Memory performance of late
middle-aged adults: Contrasting self-stereotyping and stereotype threat
accounts of assimilation to age stereotypes. Social Cognition, 24, 338–358.
doi:10.1521/soco.2006.24.3.338
Pinquart, M. (2002). Good news about the effects of bad old-age stereotypes.
Experimental Aging Research, 28, 317–336. doi:10.1080/03610730290080353
Rahhal, T. A., Hasher, L., & Colcombe, S. J. (2001). Instructional manipulations
and age differences in memory: Now you see them, now you don’t. Psychology and
Aging, 16, 697–706.
Salthouse, T. A. (2001). Structural models of the relations between age and measures
of cognitive functioning. Intelligence, 29, 93–115.
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of
stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336–356.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336
Steele, C. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellec-
tual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613
Steele, C. L., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image:
The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology, 34, 379–440.
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test per-
formance of African Americans. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 69,
797–811. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity of intergroup behavior. In
S. Worchel & W. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–48).
Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall.
Weiss, D., & Lang, F. R. (2012). The two faces of age identity. GeroPsychology, 25,
4–14.
Copyright of Experimental Aging Research is the property of Routledge and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.