The document discusses various ways that language use differs between men and women. It notes that gender is a key part of identity and that differences in language use may be influenced by societal expectations. It also provides several examples of gender differences in vocabulary choices, speech patterns, and social expectations around language in different cultures.
The document discusses various ways that language use differs between men and women. It notes that gender is a key part of identity and that differences in language use may be influenced by societal expectations. It also provides several examples of gender differences in vocabulary choices, speech patterns, and social expectations around language in different cultures.
The document discusses various ways that language use differs between men and women. It notes that gender is a key part of identity and that differences in language use may be influenced by societal expectations. It also provides several examples of gender differences in vocabulary choices, speech patterns, and social expectations around language in different cultures.
The document discusses various ways that language use differs between men and women. It notes that gender is a key part of identity and that differences in language use may be influenced by societal expectations. It also provides several examples of gender differences in vocabulary choices, speech patterns, and social expectations around language in different cultures.
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online from Scribd
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 10
Gender is also something we cannot avoid; it is part of
the way in which societies are ordered around us, with
each society doing that ordering differently. As Eckert and McConnell-Ginet (2003, p. 50) say: ‘The force of gender categories in society makes it impossible for us to move through our lives in a nongendered way and impossible not to behave in a way that brings out gendered behavior in others.’ Gender is a key component of identity. Differences in voice quality may be accentuated by beliefs about what men and women should sound like when they talk, and any differences in verbal skills may be explained in great part through differences in upbringing. (It has often been noted that there is far more reading failure in schools among boys than girls, but it does not follow from this fact that boys are inherently less well equipped to learn to read, for their poor performance in comparison to girls may be sociocultural in origin rather than genetic.) The next person you meet on the street may be male or female, tall or short, long-lived or short-lived, high-voiced or low-voiced, and so on, with not one of these characteristics being predictable from any other. Numerous observers have described women’s speech as being different from that of men. I should also observe that there is a bias here: men’s speech usually provides the norm against which women’s speech is judged. For example, in discussing language change in Philadelphia, Labov (2001, pp. 281–2) deliberately recasts his statement that ‘Women conform more closely than men to sociolinguistic norms that are overtly prescribed, but conform less than men when they are not’ to read that men ‘are less conforming than women with stable linguistic variables, and more conforming when change is in progress within a linguistic system.’ He does this so as to avoid appearing to bias his findings. Men indulge in a kind of phatic small talk that involves insults, challenges, and various kinds of negative behavior to do exactly what women do by their use of nurturing, polite, feedback-laden, cooperative talk. In doing this, they achieve the kind of solidarity they prize. It is the norms of behavior that are different. 1a. Oh dear, you’ve put the peanut butter in the refrigerator again. 1b. Shit, you’ve put the peanut butter in the refrigerator again. In the linguistic literature perhaps the most famous example of gender differentiation is found in the Lesser Antilles of the West Indies among the Carib Indians. Male and female Caribs have been reported to speak different languages, the result of a long-ago conquest in which a group of invading Carib-speaking men killed the local Arawak-speaking men and mated with the Arawak women. The descendants of these Carib-speaking men and Arawak-speaking women have sometimes been described as having different languages for men and women because boys learn Carib from their fathers and girls learn Arawak from their mothers. There is also a very interesting example from English of a woman being advised to speak more like a man in order to fill a position previously filled only by men. Margaret Thatcher was told that her voice did not match her position as British Prime Minister: she sounded too ‘shrill.’ She was advised to lower the pitch of her voice, diminish its range, and speak more slowly, and thereby adopt an authoritative, almost monotonous delivery to make herself heard. She was successful to the extent that her new speaking style became a kind of trademark, one either well-liked by her admirers or detested by her opponents. One of the consequences of such work is that there is now a greater awareness in some parts of the community that subtle, and sometimes not so subtle, distinctions are made in the vocabulary choice used to describe men and women. This kind of work does two things: it draws our attention to existing inequities, and it encourages us to make the necessary changes by establishing new categorizations (e.g., Ms), and suggesting modifications for old terms (e.g., changing policeman to police officer and chairman to chairperson). There certainly are gender differences in word choice in various languages. Japanese women show they are women when they speak, for example, by the use of a sentence-final particle ne or another particle wa. In Japanese, too, a male speaker refers to himself as boku or ore whereas a female uses watasi or atasi. Whereas a man says boku kaeru ‘I will go back’ in plain or informal speech, a woman says watasi kaeru wa (Takahara, 1991). Children learn to make these distinctions very early in life. According to Bradley (1998), men use one dialect among themselves and women use the other. Men also use men’s dialect to speak to women and women use women’s dialect to speak to men. Children are brought up in women’s dialect with boys required to shift – not always done easily – to men’s dialect as they are initiated into manhood. Gal’s study in the Oberwart of Austria (pp. 205–6) showed how it is not only what women say but who they are willing to say it to that is important. We have also noted that there are often different politeness requirements made of men and women. Still other gender-linked differences are said to exist. Women and men may have different paralinguistic systems and move and gesture differently. The suggestion has been made that these often require women to appear to be submissive to men. Women are also often named, titled, and addressed differently from men. Women are more likely than men to be addressed by their first names when everything else is equal, or, if not by first names, by such terms as lady, miss, or dear, and even baby or babe. Women are said to be subject to a wider range of address terms than men, and men are more familiar with them than with other men. Women are also said not to employ the profanities and obscenities men use, or, if they do, use them in different circumstances or are judged differently for using them. (However, the successful American television series ‘Sex and the City’ might seriously challenge that idea!) Women are also sometimes required to be silent in situations in which men may speak. Among the Araucanian Indians of Chile, men are encouraged to talk on all occasions, but the ideal wife is silent in the presence of her husband, and at gatherings where men are present she should talk only in a whisper, if she talks at all. In setting out a list of what she calls ‘sociolinguistic universal tendencies,’ Holmes (1998) does offer some testable claims. There are five of these: 1. Women and men develop different patterns of language use. 2. Women tend to focus on the affective functions of an interaction more often than men do. 3. Women tend to use linguistic devices that stress solidarity more often than men do. 4. Women tend to interact in ways which will maintain and increase solidarity, while (especially in formal contexts) men tend to interact in ways which will maintain and increase their power and status. 5. Women are stylistically more flexible than men. Stereotype: Women spoke less forcefully than men, and men swore much more than women. Men were also more blunt and to the point in their speaking. Mills (2003) contests the view that women are more polite than men. She says that ‘politeness’ is not a property of utterances; it is rather ‘a set of practices or strategies which communities of practice develop, affirm, and contest’ (p. 9). When the two genders interacted, men tended to take the initiative in conversation, but there seemed to be a desire to achieve some kind of accommodation so far as topics were concerned: the men spoke less aggressively and competitively and the women reduced their amount of talk about home and family. Another interesting claim is that in cross-gender conversations men frequently interrupt women but women much less frequently interrupt men (Zimmerman and West, 1975). James and Clarke (1993) looked at fifty- four studies that addressed the claim that men are much more likely than women ‘to use interruption as a means of dominating and controlling interactions’ (p. 268). One explanation is that languages can be sexist. 1. The first claim is that men and women are biologically different and that this difference has serious consequences for gender. Women are somehow predisposed psychologically to be involved with one another and to be mutually supportive and non- competitive. 2. The second claim is that social organization is best perceived as some kind of hierarchical set of power relationships. 3. The third claim, which does not actually deny the second claim, is that men and women are social beings who have learned to act in certain ways. They conclude that women and men observe different rules in conversing and that in cross-gender talk the rules often conflict. Language behavior reflects male dominance. Men use what power they have to dominate each other and, of course, women, and, if women are to succeed in such a system, they must learn to dominate others too, women included. Men constantly try to take control, to specify topics, to interrupt, and so on. They do it with each other and they do it with women, who, feeling powerless, let them get away with it, preferring instead to seek support from other women. Consequently, since women are relatively powerless they opt for more prestigious language forms to protect themselves in dealing with the more powerful. At the same time the use of such forms serves to mark them off from equally powerless males of the same social class. Women may also have weaker social networks than men but they show a greater sensitivity to language forms, especially standard ones. The genders have different views of what questioning is all about, women viewing questions as part of conversational maintenance and men primarily as requests for information; different conventions for linking; different views of what is or is not ‘aggressive’ linguistic behavior, with women regarding any sign of aggression as personally directed, negative, and disruptive, and men as just one way of organizing a conversation; different views of topic flow and topic shift; and different attitudes toward problem-sharing and advice-giving, with women tending to discuss, share, and seek reassurance, and men tending to look for solutions, give advice, and even lecture to their audiences. We may expect that the more distinct the roles, the greater the differences, and there seems to be some evidence to support such a claim, for the greatest differences appear to exist in societies in which the roles of men and women are most clearly differentiated. Most of those differences can be explained by the different positions men and women fill in society. Men have more power and may be more assertive; women tend to be kept ‘in their place’ but aspire quite often to a different and ‘better’ place. Women therefore appear to be more conscious of uses of language which they associate with their ‘betters’ in society, that is, those they regard as being socially superior Language and gender studies have seen an interesting development in recent years, known by such terms as queer linguistics and lavender linguistics. These studies deal with the language of non-mainstream groups such as gays, lesbians, bisexuals, the transgendered, etc., and focus on ‘sexuality’ rather than sex or gender. In fact, a major claim is that the focus on sex or gender may have been misdirected.