Family Dynamics Sibling Bullying
Family Dynamics Sibling Bullying
Family Dynamics Sibling Bullying
research-article2018
JIVXXX10.1177/0886260518800313Journal of Interpersonal ViolencePlamondon et al.
Original Research
Journal of Interpersonal Violence
1–23
Family Dynamics © The Author(s) 2018
Article reuse guidelines:
and Young Adults’ sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0886260518800313
https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260518800313
Well-Being: The journals.sagepub.com/home/jiv
Mediating Role of
Sibling Bullying
Abstract
Although a common form of family violence, sibling bullying is often viewed
as harmless by families and society. Consequently, it has not received as
much attention in research compared with peer bullying or other types
of family violence, such as interpartner violence, child abuse, and elder
abuse. Considering that sibling relationships have lasting effects on children’s
development, this retrospective study focused on the antecedents and
outcomes of sibling bullying. Grounded in family system theory and social
learning theory, it explored whether sibling bullying during childhood and
adolescence mediated the relationship between negative family dynamics
(i.e., sibling rivalry, interparental hostility, and parental hostility) and
young adults’ well-being (i.e., sense of competence, internalized problems,
self-esteem, and satisfaction with life). Data from 216 respondents were
analyzed using structural equation modeling. Results revealed that young
adults who reported higher levels of sibling rivalry and interparental hostility
were more likely to be victims of sibling bullying. In turn, sibling bullying was
associated with lower sense of competence, self-esteem, and life satisfaction,
Corresponding Author:
Andréanne Plamondon, École de Psychologie, Université de Moncton, 18 Antonine-Maillet
Avenue, Moncton, New Brunswick, Canada E1A 3E9.
Email: eap4361@umoncton.ca
2 Journal of Interpersonal Violence 00(0)
Keywords
family dynamics, sibling bullying, well-being, young adults
and how sibling bullying during childhood and adolescence mediates the rela-
tionship between family dynamics and young adults’ well-being. By docu-
menting the complexity of sibling bullying, our hope is that the current study
will serve as an impetus for greater efforts to address the problem.
Sibling Bullying
Sibling bullying has been defined as aggressive behaviors between brothers
and sisters (e.g., hitting, throwing objects, name-calling, hostile arguing) that
are repeated over time to cause harm and to dominate (Bowes et al., 2014;
Menesini, Camodeca, & Nocentini, 2010; Monks et al., 2009; Skinner &
Kowalski, 2013). Researchers have used different terms to describe these
aggressive sibling interactions, such as bullying, aggression, violence, or
abuse (Tippett & Wolke, 2015). Thus, studies on aggressive sibling interac-
tions often included behaviors that fit the definition of bullying, but were not
labeled this way by the authors (Hoetger et al., 2015). For a matter of simplic-
ity, we used the term sibling bullying throughout the article when the concept
measured by the other researchers corresponded to the definition of sibling
bullying presented above (Hoetger et al., 2015; Monks et al., 2009).
Findings converge to indicate that sibling bullying is far more common
than peer bullying (Hoetger et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2015; Skinner &
Kowalski, 2013; Tanrikulu & Campbell, 2015). The prevalence of sibling bul-
lying during childhood varies between studies ranging from 30% (Duncan,
1999) to 78% (Skinner & Kowalski, 2013). In contrast, 10% to 33% of stu-
dents reported having experienced bullying at school (Hymel & Swearer,
2015). Despite the prevalence of sibling bullying, Kiselica and Morrill-
Richards (2007) indicated that it is less likely to be reported outside the family
than peer bullying. Researchers argued that the lack of reporting outside the
home is part of the reason why sibling bullying has received far less attention
from researchers and practitioners than peer bullying (Hoetger et al., 2015).
Sibling bullying seems to take place for different reasons than peer bully-
ing, and the relation between the victim and the perpetrator varies remarkably
from one type of bullying to the other (Skinner & Kowalski, 2013). For
instance, it is easier for siblings, compared with peers, to provoke or upset
one another by exploiting the other’s weaknesses due to the amount of time
siblings spend together and how well they know each other (Ensor et al.,
2010; Tippett & Wolke, 2015). Thus, by comparison with peer bullying, per-
petration and victimization through sibling bullying are more strongly related,
indicating an almost reciprocal dimension to sibling bullying (Mathis &
Mueller, 2015; Skinner & Kowalski, 2013; Tippett & Wolke, 2015). This
reciprocal dimension makes the imbalance of power, an important criterion to
4 Journal of Interpersonal Violence 00(0)
2003; Minuchin, 1988) and the social learning theory (Bandura, 1978). The
family system theory postulates that family members are interdependent and
continuously exert a mutual influence on one another, whereas the social
learning theory posits that children can learn aggressive behavior patterns
either directly through experience or indirectly by observing people around
them. Empirical findings support both theoretical approaches (Tippett &
Wolke, 2015; Tucker et al., 2014). More precisely, results have suggested that
high levels of conflict between family members increase the risk of sibling
bullying (Bowes et al., 2014; Menesini et al., 2010; Tippett & Wolke, 2015;
Tucker et al., 2014). Both parents’ interactions with their children and with
one another influence the incidence of sibling bullying. Specifically, negative
parenting behaviors, such as shouting, quarreling, and hitting, and greater
exposure to interparental conflict were strongly associated with sibling bully-
ing (Bowes et al., 2014; Hoffman, Kiecolt, & Edwards, 2005; Tippett &
Wolke, 2015; Tucker et al., 2014; Wolke et al., 2015). Sibling rivalry was also
linked to higher levels of sibling aggression (Tucker, Cox et al., 2013). In this
regard, many theorists “point to sibling rivalry as a root of sibling conflict”
(Whiteman, McHale, & Soli, 2011, p. 129).
These results highlight the crucial role of family dynamics in sibling rela-
tionships and the need to further explore these issues in the context of sibling
bullying. Although Tucker and colleagues (2014) have examined the influence
of family dynamics during childhood for sibling bullying, family dynamics,
sibling bullying, and well-being have yet to be studied simultaneously. Testing
the influence of family dynamics during childhood (or adolescence) on sibling
bullying and young adults’ psychological well-being is an important next step
in the literature of sibling bullying, as it will add to the existing knowledge
about the role of family dynamics on well-being later in life.
Method
Participants
The participants in this study were 244 French–Canadian young adult univer-
sity students. Twenty individuals were excluded from all analyses because
they had not reported their gender, their number of siblings, or their birth
order. Furthermore, respondents who did not complete all the questionnaires
or who failed to answer more than 80% of the items in a questionnaire were
also excluded (n = 8) from all analyses, leaving a final sample of 216 indi-
viduals. The remaining participants (44 males and 172 females) were living
in New Brunswick and their mean age was 19.01 years (minimum = 17, max-
imum = 33, SD = 2.01). Participants were predominantly Caucasian and 65%
were undergraduate first-year university students (M = 1.70 completed uni-
versity years, SD = 1.26). On average, they had 1.81 siblings (SD = 1.53) and
were mostly the first-born (45% of the sample) or second-born (39% of the
sample) child. Fifty-five percent of the participants were still living in the
parental home. The vast majority of participants (69% of the sample) lived in
a family of origin with two parents in a first marriage, 7% had parents who
cohabited together without being married, 3% reported the death of one par-
ent (mostly during their adolescence or adulthood), and 20% had parents who
were divorced/separated (mostly during childhood). Almost half (47%) of the
respondents in non-intact families revealed that they lived in a blended fam-
ily following their parents’ divorce or separation. The highest level of educa-
tion most commonly completed by mothers and fathers was an associate’s
degree (33% and 40%, respectively).
Procedure
Prior to conducting the research, a certification of compliance with ethical
principles was obtained from the institutional research ethics board of the
university at which the research was done. The research was conducted in
Plamondon et al. 7
accordance with the approved protocol. The following criteria were used to
select participants for the current study to form a convenience sample: having
at least one sibling and being able to report on one’s experience with both
parents in the family of origin. Participants were recruited via announce-
ments in lectures in many departments of a Canadian university. Participants
were informed that they would be participating in a study on family dynamics
during their childhood and adolescent years and on their current psychologi-
cal functioning. This study was not about the current situation at home, as
sibling bullying tends to diminish with age (Finkelhor, Turner, & Ormrod,
2006; Tanrikulu & Campbell, 2015; Tucker, Finkelhor, Shattuck, & Turner,
2013). Participants first consented and then completed the questionnaires (in
French) measuring demographic variables, sibling rivalry, interparental hos-
tility, parental hostility, sibling bullying, sense of competence, internalized
problems, self-esteem, and life satisfaction.
Measures
Demographic variables and characteristics of the family of origin. Participants
provided relevant background data, such as their age, number of siblings, and
birth order. Participants were also asked to report their parents’ current mari-
tal status and educational levels.
Family dynamics
Sibling rivalry. We used the rivalry scale of the Sibling Relationship
Inventory developed by Stocker and McHale (1992; Perlmutter, Touliatos,
& Holden, 2001) to measure sibling rivalry. The four-item scale assessed
respondents’ belief that their parents treated them unfairly and inequitably
in comparison with the treatment of their siblings (e.g., “Many kids com-
plain that their mothers aren’t fair about how they treat them compared to
how they treat their brothers and sisters. How often did you feel that your
mother treated your siblings better than she treated you?”; “How about with
your father? How often did you think that he treated your siblings better
than he treated you?”). Items were answered using a 5-point Likert-type scale
where 1 = hardly ever, 2 = occasionally, 3 = sometimes, 4 = pretty often,
and 5 = always. Overall scores were calculated by taking the average of the
four items, with higher scores indicating greater sibling rivalry. In the current
study, the alpha coefficient was .77.
their childhood and adolescent years their parents engaged in various hostile
behaviors (e.g., “Shouted or yelled at each other because they were mad”)
when interacting with each other. Responses were 0 = almost never or never,
1 = once in a while, 2 = quite often, 3 = often, and 4 = almost always. The sum
of the six items produced an overall score. A higher score indicated a higher
level of hostile interparental behaviors. An earlier version of this scale has
been shown to correlate with observational assessments of marital interaction
(Simons et al., 2012). The scale’s internal reliability for the current sample
was very good (α = .90).
Well-being
Sense of competence. Respondents reported their sense of competence,
which included feelings of global mastery and efficacy to navigate challenges.
This construct was measured by the Mastery Scale, a seven-item scale con-
structed by Pearlin, Menaghan, Lieberman, and Mullan (1981). Respondents
were asked to evaluate how strongly they agree or disagree with a series of
statements about themselves. The responses ranged from 1 = strongly disagree
to 5 = strongly agree and consisted of items like: “There is really no way I can
solve some of the problems I have” (reverse scored) and “What happens to me
in the future mostly depends on me.” Overall scores were calculated by taking
the average of the seven items, with higher scores indicating greater sense of
competence. Results have shown that a sense of competence, as measured by
this scale, can buffer the emotional impact of persistent problems (Seeman,
1991). The alpha reliability was .75 for the current sample.
Satisfaction with life. The Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener, Emmons,
Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) comprises five items (e.g., “In most ways my life
10 Journal of Interpersonal Violence 00(0)
Results
For all questionnaires, missing values were replaced with the participant’s
mean score when 80% of the items on the scale or subscale in question were
answered (n = 15).
being made fun of) was reported more frequently than physical bullying (i.e.,
being hit, kicked, pushed or shoved, and having things damaged or taken) and
social bullying (i.e., being ignored or left out and being lied to or having
rumors spread about them), with being made fun of by their sibling as the
most commonly reported act of aggression (see Table 1 for percentages).
Descriptive Statistics
Means, standard deviations, and Pearson bivariate correlations are presented
in Table 2. On average, young adults reported having been occasionally
treated unfairly by their parents in comparison with their siblings, as mea-
sured by sibling rivalry, and having witnessed interparental hostility once in
a while. The mean frequency of parental hostility fell between “almost never”
and “once in a while.” Inspection of Table 2 also reveals a low average report
of acts of sibling aggression during childhood and adolescence, and moder-
ately high scores on the well-being measures.
As shown in Table 2, all correlations between key variables were signifi-
cant. More precisely, negative family dynamics during childhood and adoles-
cence were positively correlated with sibling bullying. The more participants
perceived issues of sibling rivalry and were exposed to parental and interpa-
rental hostility, the more they reported being victims of sibling bullying.
Negative family dynamics and sibling bullying were also positively correlated
with internalized problems, and negatively correlated with sense of compe-
tence, self-esteem, and life satisfaction. Correlations between predicted vari-
ables in the model and some demographic data (e.g., age, gender, number of
siblings, birth order in the family, and nationality) were also analyzed (not
12 Journal of Interpersonal Violence 00(0)
Internalized -.13
Gender
problems
Parental
hostility .05
.21
Sense of
-.2
9 competence
Interparental .23 Sibling
hostility bullying -.26
Life
.19 satisfaction
-.3
0
Sibling rivalry .14
.16 Number of
Self-esteem
siblings
Birth order
Note. S-B χ2 = Satorra–Bentler χ2; GFI = goodness of fit index; CFI = comparative fit index;
SRMR = standardized root mean square residual; RMSEA = root mean square error of
approximation; CI = confidence interval.
Internalized -.15
Gender
problems
.28
.14
Interparental Sense of
.25 -.27
hostility -.2
2 competence
Sibling Parental
bullying -.19 hostility
.20 -.31
Life
Sibling rivalry
satisfaction
5
-.2
-.2
5
.14
.17 Number of
Self-esteem
siblings
Birth order
Figure 2. The final path model with parental hostility entered as a covariate.
Note. All values represent standardized parameter estimates. All parameters are significant
(p < .05). For gender, 0 = female and 1 = male.
Discussion
The aim of the current study was to examine a theoretical model of risk fac-
tors and outcomes of sibling bullying victimization. The hypothesized model,
in which sibling bullying served as a mediator, postulated indirect effects
between negative family dynamics during childhood and adolescence, and
young adults’ well-being. More precisely, we expected that negative family
dynamics would be associated with more experiences of sibling bullying,
which in turn would be negatively associated with young adults’ well-being.
Statistical analyses revealed that the original model could be improved by
dropping the path between parental hostility and sibling bullying. Yet the fit
did not reach acceptability by removing parental hostility from the model. In
the final model, parental hostility was treated as a covariate given its associa-
tion with adolescents’ psychological outcomes (Polcari et al., 2014). The
final path model provided an acceptable fit for the data. Partially confirming
our hypothesis, our results suggest that the relationship between family
dynamics and young adults’ well-being is, in part, mediated by experiences
Plamondon et al. 15
Implications
From a clinical point of view, our study supports the notion that therapeu-
tic interventions and prevention techniques for parents should include
education about the interdependence of family relationships and the link
between family dynamics, sibling bullying, and young adults’ well-being.
Knowledge about the salience of parental relations in promoting healthy
relationships between siblings is also critical to the development of effec-
tive interventions in the sibling-bullying field, as parents are often
unaware of their implication in the process of sibling bullying among
their children (Pickering & Sanders, 2017). Parent management training
programs can also serve as an opportunity to help parents support their
children in developing effective conflict management strategies (Shadik,
Perkins, & Kovacs, 2013).
Conclusion
Our work significantly adds to the literature by investigating the effect of the
interplay of family dynamics and sibling bullying on young adults’ well-
being. This study shows that disharmonious interactions between some fam-
ily members can influence young adults’ well-being indirectly through sibling
bullying. Clinicians and human service workers aiming to reduce bullying
between siblings should advocate for a family-level approach by incorporat-
ing the underlying principles of the family system theory and the social learn-
ing theory into their clinical interventions. Further research in this area should
be conducted to learn more about the risk factors and psychological outcomes
of sibling bullying and to develop effective programs aimed at preventing
and reducing this form of bullying.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Jolène Doucet, Karine Perron, Karine Roy, and
Jannie Thibodeau for their assistance in running participants and in data entry.
Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publi-
cation of this article.
Plamondon et al. 19
References
Amato, P. R. (2001). Children of divorce in the 1990s: An update of the Amato
and Keith (1991) meta-analysis. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 355-370.
doi:10.1037//0893-3200.15.3.355
Bandura, A. (1978). Social learning theory of aggression. Journal of Communication,
28, 12-29. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.tb01621.x
Bazińska, R. (2015). Validation of the polish version of the collective self-esteem
scale. Current Issues in Personality Psychology, 3, 125-137. doi:10.5114/
cipp.2015.52891
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1991). Measures of self-esteem. In J. P. Robinson, P. R.
Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psycho-
logical attitudes (pp. 115-160). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bowes, L., Wolke, D., Joinson, C., Lereya, S. T., & Lewis, G. (2014). Sibling bully-
ing and risk of depression, anxiety, and self-harm: A prospective cohort study.
Pediatrics, 134, e1032-1039. doi:10.1542/peds.2014-0832
Buist, K. L., Deković, M., & Prinzie, P. (2013). Sibling relationship quality and psy-
chopathology of children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology
Review, 33, 97-106. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.10.007
Buist, K. L., & Vermande, M. (2014). Sibling relationship patterns and their asso-
ciations with child competence and problem behavior. Journal of Family
Psychology, 28, 529-537. doi:10.1037/a0036990
Button, D. M., & Gealt, R. (2010). High risk behaviors among victims of sibling
violence. Journal of Family Violence, 25, 131-140. doi:10.1007/s10896-009-
9276-x
Cox, M. J., & Paley, B. (1997). Families as systems. Annual Review of Psychology,
48, 243-267. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.243
Cox, M. J., & Paley, B. (2003). Understanding families as systems. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 12, 193-196. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01259
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction
with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71-75. doi:10.1207/
s15327752jpa4901_13
Duncan, R. D. (1999). Peer and sibling aggression: An investigation of intra- and
extra-familial bullying. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 871-886. doi:10
.1177/088626099014008005
Dunn, J. (1983). Sibling relationships in early childhood. Child Development, 54,
787-811. doi:10.2307/1129886
Ensor, R., Marks, A., Jacobs, L., & Hughes, C. (2010). Trajectories of antisocial
behaviour towards siblings predict antisocial behaviour towards peers. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1208-1216. doi:10.1111/j.1469-
7610.2010.02276.x
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., & Ormrod, R. (2006). Kid’s stuff: The nature and impact of
peer and sibling violence on younger and older children. Child Abuse & Neglect,
30, 1401-1421. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.06.006
20 Journal of Interpersonal Violence 00(0)
Garcia, M. M., Shaw, D. S., Winslow, E. B., & Yaggi, K. E. (2000). Destructive sibling
conflict and the development of conduct problems in young boys. Developmental
Psychology, 36, 44-53. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.1.44
Gardner, F. (2000). Methodological issues in the direct observation of parent–
child interaction: Do observational findings reflect the natural behavior of par-
ticipants? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 3, 185-198. doi:10
.1023/A:1009503409699
Graham-Bermann, S. A., Cutler, S. E., Litzenberger, B. W., & Schwartz, W. E. (1994).
Perceived conflict and violence in childhood sibling relationships and later emo-
tional adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 8, 85-97. doi:10.1037/0893-
3200.8.1.85
Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data
analyses (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Hoetger, L. A., Hazen, K. P., & Brank, E. M. (2015). All in the family: A retro-
spective study comparing sibling bullying and peer bullying. Journal of Family
Violence, 30, 103-111. doi:10.1007/s10896-014-9651-0
Hoffman, K. L., Kiecolt, K. J., & Edwards, J. N. (2005). Physical violence between
siblings: A theoretical and empirical analysis. Journal of Family Issues, 26,
1103-1130. doi:10.1177/0192513X05277809
Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. (2008). Structural equation modeling:
Guidelines for determining model fit. The Electronic Journal of Business
Research Methods, 6, 53-60.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance struc-
ture analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation
Modeling, 6, 1-55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
Hymel, S., & Swearer, S. M. (2015). Four decades of research on school bullying: An
introduction. American Psychologist, 70, 293-299. doi:10.1037/a0038928
Ilfeld, F. W. (1976). Further validation of a psychiatric symptom index in a normal pop-
ulation. Psychological Reports, 39, 1215-1228. doi:10.2466/pr0.1976.39.3f.1215
Johnson, R. M., Duncan, D. T., Rothman, E. F., Gilreath, T. D., Hemenway, D.,
Molnar, B. E., & Azrael, D. (2015). Fighting with siblings and with peers among
urban high school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30, 2221-2237.
doi:10.1177/0886260514552440
Kazarian, S. S. (2009). Arabic contingencies of self-worth: Arabic translation and
validation of the contingencies of Self-Worth Scale in Lebanese youth. The Arab
Journal of Psychiatry, 20, 123-134.
Kiselica, M. S., & Morrill-Richards, M. (2007). Sibling maltreatment: The forgot-
ten abuse. Journal of Counseling & Development, 85, 148-160. doi:10.1002
/j.1556-6678.2007.tb00457.x
Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (4th ed.).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Mathis, G., & Mueller, C. (2015). Childhood sibling aggression and emotional dif-
ficulties and aggressive behavior in adulthood. Journal of Family Violence, 30,
315-327. doi:10.1007/s10896-015-9670-5
Plamondon et al. 21
McDougall, P., & Vaillancourt, T. (2015). Long-term adult outcomes of peer vic-
timization in childhood and adolescence. American Psychologist, 70, 300-310.
doi:10.1037/a0039174
McHale, S. M., Updegraff, K. A., Jackson-Newsom, J., Tucker, C. J., & Crouter, A.
C. (2000). When does parents’ differential treatment have negative implications
for siblings? Social Development, 9, 149-172. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00117
McHale, S. M., Updegraff, K. A., & Whiteman, S. D. (2012). Sibling relationships
and influences in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Marriage and Family,
74, 913-930. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01011.x
Menesini, E., Camodeca, M., & Nocentini, A. (2010). Bullying among siblings: The
role of personality and relational variables. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 28, 921-939. doi:10.1348/026151009X479402
Milevsky, A. (2011). Sibling relationships in childhood and adolescence: Predictors
and outcomes. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.
Minuchin, P. (1988). Relationships within the family: A systems perspective on
development. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within
families: Mutual influences (pp. 7-26). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
Monks, C. P., Smith, P. K., Naylor, P., Barter, C., Ireland, J. L., & Coyne, I. (2009).
Bullying in different contexts: Commonalities, differences and the role of theory.
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14, 146-156. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2009.01.004
Olweus, D. (2007). The Olweus Bullying Questionnaire. Center City, MN: Hazelden.
Pearlin, L. I., Menaghan, E. G., Lieberman, M. A., & Mullan, J. T. (1981). The stress pro-
cess. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22, 337-356. doi:10.2307/2136676
Perlmutter, B. F., Touliatos, J., & Holden, G. W. (2001). Handbook of family mea-
surement techniques: Instruments & index (Vol. 3). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Pickering, J. A., & Sanders, M. R. (2017). Integrating parents’ views on sibling rela-
tionships to tailor an evidence-based parenting intervention for sibling conflict.
Family Process, 56, 105-125. doi:10.1111/famp.12173
Pike, A., Coldwell, J., & Dunn, J. F. (2005). Sibling relationships in early/middle
childhood: Links with individual adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 19,
523-532. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.523
Polcari, A., Rabi, K., Bolger, E., & Teicher, M. H. (2014). Parental verbal affection
and verbal aggression in childhood differentially influence psychiatric symp-
toms and wellbeing in young adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 38, 91-102.
doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.10.003
Richmond, M. K., Stocker, C. M., & Rienks, S. L. (2005). Longitudinal associations
between sibling relationship quality, parental differential treatment, and children’s
adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 550-559. doi:10.1037/0893-
3200.19.4.550
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2010). Ensuring positiveness of the scaled difference
chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika, 75, 243-248. doi:10.1007/S11336-009-
9135-Y
22 Journal of Interpersonal Violence 00(0)
Author Biographies
Andréanne Plamondon, MA, is a doctoral student at the Université de Moncton. Her
research interest focuses on family and romantic relationships.
Geneviève Bouchard, PhD, is a full professor of psychology at the Université de
Moncton. Her research focuses mainly on family psychology, couple psychology,
psychopathology, and well-being.
Mylène Lachance-Grzela, PhD, is an assistant professor in the School of Psychology
at the Université de Moncton. Her interests involve interpersonal relationships, par-
enting, and psychological well-being.