IJIR ProblemsinCross CulturalContact
IJIR ProblemsinCross CulturalContact
IJIR ProblemsinCross CulturalContact
net/publication/256404410
CITATIONS READS
110 8,118
1 author:
Bruce W. Stening
Peking University
67 PUBLICATIONS 882 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Bruce W. Stening on 10 October 2017.
BRUCE W. STENING
The author would hke to thank Professor Dexter Dunphy and Dr. Tohr Yamaguchi of
the University of New South Wales and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on
earlier versions of this manuscript. Requests for reprints should be sent to Bruce W.
Stening, Department of Management, University of Western Australia, Nedlands, 6009,
Australia.
269
270 Bruce W. Stening
The degree of differentiation among classes may differ for the sender and the
receiver. If the sender differentiates more than the receiver, he activates only
one or a few classes for a given message, while in the receiver’s cognition more
neighbouring classes will be activated as well. The differentiation between
love and hatred, for example, is stronger in the American culture. Conse-
quently, criticism of performance does not imply personal dislike for an
Problems in Cross-Cultural Contact 273
American as it does for an Arab by whom the message “you can do a better
job” is decoded as “I dislike you and your work.” (p. 275)
members of faith X may look askance at members of faith Y, not because the
doctrines differ in content, but because religion in culture X is more salient
and less differentiated from other societal structures than it is in culture Y.
(P. 16)
Intercultural Communication
Normally persons are aided in understanding another’s social
274 Bruce W. Stening
The greater the real differences between groups on any particular custom,
detail of physical appearance, or item of material culture, the more likely it is
that that feature will appear in the stereotyped imagery each group has of the
other. (p. 821)
the Japanese and Koreans, when asked to judge “myself” and “my people”,
produced profiles that were quite close to each other; that is, the Japanese
idea of the Japanese and the Korean idea of the Koreans were not much
different. But when the two groups were asked to rate each other, the profiles
were far apart! (p. 233)
Time Factor
There is a considerable weight of evidence in the literature to
suggest that the problems of adjustment in intercultural relation-
ships, particularly of an expatriate to his host environment, are
greatest in the early stages of familiarity. (Though, as discussed
below, at the very beginning of the sojourn experience there may
be a “honeymoon” phase of good adjustment.) It is correspond-
ingly during this initial period of adjustment that we would expect
the relations between individuals interculturally to be prone to the
greatest number of misunderstandings.
For the expatriate’s part, he often discovers a wide disparity
between what he has been used to and what he finds in the host
environment; so many of the aspects affecting both his profes-
sional and his personal life may be different. Though varying from
location to location and between expatriates depending upon a
variety of factors to be discussed subsequently, a not uncommon
reaction of an expatriate to such a situation is culture shock, a
term coined by Oberg (1960). Lundstedt (1963) has described
culture shock as:
Taft (1977) has gone further and identified six different types
of culture shock. Though it may be so that culture shock has
affected all kinds of travellers since human society began (Bock,
1970) it must be acknowledged that the ability to cope with a
foreign environment varies widely from individual to individual,
with certain persons being able to adapt with very few problems
284 Bruce W. Stening
managers of foreign subsidiaries have often failed because they did not
understand the foreign nation in which they worked, but others have also
failed because they understood the foreign country so well that their head
office decided they had “gone native.” (Wilkins, 1966, p. 90)
Cultural Distance
Pm0 nalit?l
To some extent the impact of personality factors on inter-
cultural adjustment and intercultural understanding has already
been dealt with; for example, in the discussion of xenophiles and
ethnocentrism. In this section certain additional personality-
related material will be discussed.
One concept which has received some attention in the sojourn
research literature is authoritarianism. Implicitly, one might
expect that authoritariallisln would be inversely related to
empathy and that individuals high on authoritarianism would
experience adjustment problems in intercultural relationships. (In
this sense, authoritarianism is closely related to ethnocentrism.)
The empirical evidence is, however, not clear. While Basu and
Ames (1970) research showed that sojourners with authoritarian
personalities were more likely to experience unpleasant aspects
during their sojourn than individuals who were not authoritarian
in nature. limited evidence exists (Smith, 1966) to suggest no
relationship between authoritarianism, on the one hand, and either
general competence or the administrative evaluations of others in
the sojourn environment, on the other.
Problems in Cross-Cultural Contact 289
Data from the involvement phase of the W-curve support Heider’s generaliza-
tion that both proximity and interaction frequently increase the effect of
perceived similarity or dissimilarity of attitudes and values. Given similar
attitudes, proximity and frequent interaction tend to increase the degree of
positive sentiment. With slight dissimilarity of attitudes an initial assimilation
292 Bruce W. Stening
While heterogeneous groups had less pleasant group atmosphere and higher
anxiety scores, these teams performed as well as homogeneous groups in all
but the letter-writing task which demanded a high degree of verbal facility,
These results suggest the need for reevaluating the importance of the
communication variable in group interaction. (Fiedler, 1966, p. 260)
nal Australians. For each encounter one “correct” and several “in-
correct” explanations of the behavior of the host party are supplied.
The trainee is required to select the alternative he considers the
correct explanation. Having made his choice he is supplied with an
analysis of why his choice was or was not correct. In this way it is
anticipated that the trainee will build up a basic knowledge of the
other culture which will assist him in accurately anticipating the
attitudes and behavior of his intercultural partners (Fiedler et al.,
1971). The assimilator, then, seeks to increase the isomorphic
attributions of persons engaged in cross-cultural relations
(Triandis, 197 5).
A number of both laboratory and field experiments have been
conducted to assess the degree to which the assimilator does, in
fact, assist in aiding intercultural relations (e.g., Chemers, 1966;
Chemers, Lekhyananda, Fiedler, & Stolorow, 1966; Mitchell &
Foa, 1969; O’Brien, Fiedler, & Hewett, 1971; O’Brien & Plooij,
1977; Worchel & Mitchell, 1972). In an article summarising the
results of experiments conducted up until 1972 (Mitchell et al.,
1972) it was concluded that:
REFERENCES
ADLER, P.S. The transitional experience: An alternative view of culture
shock. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 1975, 15 (4), 13-23.
AJIFERUKE, M., & BODDEWYN, J. “Culture” and other explanatory
variables in comparative management studies. Academy of Management
Journal, 1970, 12 (2), 153-163.
ALLPORT, G.W. The nature of prejudice. New York: Doubleday Anchor,
1958.
ALPANDER, G.G. Drift to authoritarianism: The changing managerial styles
of the U.S. executives overseas. Journal of International Business Studies,
1973,4, 1-14.
AMIR, Y. Contact hypothesis in ethnic relations. Psychological Bulletin,
1969, 71 (5), 319-342.
ARAM, J.D., & STONER, J.A.F. Development of an organizational change
role. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 1972, 8 (4), 438-449.
304 Bruce W. Stening
FIEDLER, F.E., MITCHELL, T., & TRIANDIS, H.C. The culture assimilator:
An approach to cross-cultural training. Journal of Applied Psychology,
1971, 55 (2), 95-102.
FOA, U.G., & CHEMERS, M.M. The significance of role behavior differentia-
tion for cross-cultural interaction training. International Journal of
Psychology, 1967, 2, 45-57.
FOA, U.G., & FOA, E.B. Societal structures of the mznd. Springfield, Ill.:
Thomas, 1974.
FOA, U.G., MITCHELL, T.R., & FIEDLER, F.E. Differentiation matching.
BehavioraE Science, 1971, 16, 130-142.
GARDNER, G.H. Cross cultural communication. Journal of Social Psy-
chology, 1962, 58, 241-256.
GOODMAN, P.S., & MOORE, B.E. Critical issues of cross-cultural manage-
ment research. Human Organization, 1972, 31 (l), 39-45.
GROUP FOR THE ADVANCEMENT OF PSYCHIATRY. Working abroad: A
discussion of psychological attitudes and adaptation in new situations.
New York, 1958.
GUDYKUNST, W.B., WISEMAN, R.L., & HAMMER, M. Determinants of the
soJourner’s attitudinal satisfaction: A path model. In Brent D. Ruben
(Ed.), Communication yearbook I. Austin, Tex.: International Communi-
cation Association, 1977.
GULLAHORN, J.T., & GULLAHORN, J.E. An extension of the U-curve
hypothesis. Journal of Social Issues, 1963, 14 (3), 33-47.
GUTHRIE, G.M. A behavioral analysis of culture learning. In Richard W.
Brislin, Stephen Bochner, & Walter J. Lonner (Eds.), Cross-cultural
perspectives on learning. New York: Sage and Wiley/Halstead, 1975.
HALL, E.T. The silent Zanguage. New York: Doubleday, 1959.
HALL, E.T. The silent language in overseas business. Harvard Business
Review, 1960, 38 (3), 87-96.
HALL, E.T. The hidden dimension: Man’s use of space in public and private.
London: Bodley Head, 1966.
HAMMER, M.R., GUDYKUNST, W.B., & WISEMAN, R.L. Dimensions of
intercultural effectiveness: An exploratory study. International Journal of
Intercultural Relations, 1978, 2 (4), 382-393.
HARARI, E., & ZEIRA, Y. Morale problems in non-American multinational
corporations in the United States. Management International Review,
1974, 14 (6), 43-53.
HARDING, J., PROSHANSKY, H., KUTNER, B., & CHEIN, I. PreJudice and
ethnic relations. In Gardner Lindzey & Elliot Aronson (Eds.), The
handbook of social psychology, (2nd ed.). Vol. 5: Applied social psy-
chology. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1969.
HARRISON, R., & HOPKINS, R.L. The design of cross-cultural training: An
alternative to the university model. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science,
1967, 3 (4), 431-460.
HARRISON, R.P., COHEN, A.A., CROUCH, W.W., GENOVA, B.K.L., &
STEINBERG, M. The nonverbal communication literature. Journal of
Communication, 1972, 22, 460-476.
HAYS, R.D. Ascribed behavioral determinants of success-failure among U.S.
Problems in Cross-Cultural Contact 307