Principles of Behavior Modification Albert Bandura

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 2012

Principles of

Behavior
Modification
Albert Bandura
to Ginny, Mary, and Carol
Copyright © 1969

e-Book 2019 International Psychotherapy


Institute

All Rights Reserved

This e-book contains material protected under


International and Federal Copyright Laws and
Treaties. This e-book is intended for personal
use only. Any unauthorized reprint or use of this
material is prohibited. No part of this book may
be used in any commercial manner without
express permission of the author. Scholarly use
of quotations must have proper attribution to
the published work. This work may not be
deconstructed, reverse engineered or
reproduced in any other format.

Created in the United States of America

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 4
Table of Contents
Preface

1. Causal Processes
Interpretation of Causal Processes

Social Learning as a Reciprocal Influence Process

Symptom Substitution

Efficacy of Conventional Methods of Behavioral


Change

Summary

2. Value Issues and Objectives


Behavioral Specification of Objectives

Factors Impeding Specification of Objectives

Decision Processes in the Selection of Objectives

Summary

3. Modeling and Vicarious Processes


Theoretical Conceptions of Observational
Learning

Establishment of New Response Patterns


through Modeling

Vicarious Conditioning of Emotional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 5
Responsiveness

Vicarious Extinction

Inhibitory and Disinhibitory Effects of Vicarious


Experiences

Response Facilitating Effects of Modeling


Influences

Utilization of Modeling Principles in Planned


Sociocultural Change

Summary

4. Positive Control
Theoretical Interpretations of Reinforcement
Processes

Essential Components of Reinforcement


Practices

Ethical Implications of Reinforcement Practices

Applications of Contingency Systems

Social Organizational Applications of


Reinforcement Contingencies

Summary

5. Aversive Control
Presentation of Negative Reinforcers

Applications of Aversive Contingency Systems

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 6
Removal of Positive Reinforcers

Summary

6. Extinction
Interpretations of the Extinction Process

Extinction of Positively Reinforced Behavior

Extinction of Defensive Behavior

Summary

7. Desensitization through Counterconditioning


Controlling Variables in Desensitization

Identification of the Stimulus Determinants of


Emotional Behavior

Neutralization of Threats in Symbolic or Realistic


Forms

Antagonistic Activities in Counterconditioning

Physiological Accompaniments of Emotional


Behavior

Summary

8. Aversive Counterconditioning
Development of Conditioned Aversion and
Avoidance

Sexual Deviance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 7
Modification of Symbolic Activities

Alcoholism

Ethical Considerations in Aversion Therapy

Summary

9. Symbolic Control of Behavioral Changes


Role of Awareness of Contingencies in
Behavioral Change

Verbal Conditioning as a Function of Awareness

Interactive Effects of Cognitive and Incentive


Variables

Symbolic Control of Classical Conditioning


Phenomena

Implications of Symbolic Control for Behavioral


Modification

Discrepancy between Response Systems and the


Unconscious

Attitudinal Consequences of Behavioral and


Affective Changes

Strategies of Attitude Change

“Internalization” and Persistence of Behavioral


Changes

Stabilization of Behavioral Changes through

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 8
Development of Self-Regulatory Functions

Summary

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 9
Preface
This book presents basic psychological

principles governing human behavior within the


conceptual framework of social learning. Over the

years an impressive body of knowledge about the

mechanisms through which behavior is acquired

and modified has been accumulated. But despite

this vigorous growth of research on human


behavior, a number of psychological processes

that are highly influential in human functioning

have been overlooked or only partially

investigated. This volume reviews the recent

theoretical and experimental advances in the field


of social learning. It gives special emphasis to the

important roles played by vicarious, symbolic, and


self-regulatory processes, which receive relatively

little notice even in most contemporary theories of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 10
behavior.

The worth of a psychological theory must be

judged not only by how well it explains laboratory


findings but also by the efficacy of the behavioral

modification procedures that it produces. Recent

years have witnessed widespread applications of


methods derived from principles of social learning

to the modification of important social phenomena

in familial, educational, clinical, and other social

settings. By requiring clear specification of


treatment conditions and objective assessment of

outcomes, the social-learning approach presented

in this book contains a self-corrective feature that

distinguishes it from change enterprises in which

interventions remain ill-defined and their


psychological effects are seldom objectively
evaluated.

New social change procedures are by tradition

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 11
enthusiastically promoted, and it is not until after
the methods have been applied for some time by a

coterie of enthusiasts that systematic tests of


efficacy are conducted. Usually the methods are

then unceremoniously retired by subsequent

controlled studies. Professional workers in this

field have, therefore, come to view any new


approach as a passing fad. However, when

laboratory tests of efficacy precede social

applications, new methods are subjected to close

scrutiny at each stage of development, and those

that evolve are likely to produce outcomes


sufficiently favorable to weather rigorous

evaluation. The successful results obtained by


social-learning procedures in carefully controlled

studies justify optimistic expectations for future


developments of this approach. The numerous

investigations reported in this book also illustrate


how understanding of major change processes can

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 12
be advanced by inventive research on socially

significant problems. Contrary to much of the

current criticism, basic research need not settle


for inconsequential dependent measures.

This book is concerned not only with the


validity of the principles set forth but also with the

conditions under which they can serve as an

instrument for human advancement. The value


issues that arise in the applications of social-
learning procedures to achieve various

psychological changes are, therefore, closely

examined, and special attention is given to the

effects of social practices on man’s self-evaluation


and self-enhancement.

While this book was being written the author

contributed chapters on modeling processes to

Volume II of Advances in Experimental Social

Psychology (Bandura, 1965) and to the Ciba

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 13
Foundation Symposium: The Role of Learning and
Psychotherapy (Bandura, 1968). Chapter 3

contains a revised and updated version of some of


the material that originally appeared in the latter

publications.

Many people contributed in one way or


another to this venture. To Ted Rosenthal and
Rogers Elliott, who read preliminary versions of

the manuscript and made many valuable


suggestions, I offer my sincere thanks. I am also

indebted to countless students and colleagues who

have helped through collaborative research and

sharing of ideas to enhance the value of what I

have written. I owe a special personal debt to my


former student and colleague, Richard Walters,

who died tragically at the height of his productive


career. Although he never read what I have

written here, our lively discussions during


collaborative projects did much to clarify some of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 14
the theoretical issues discussed in this book.

The preparation of this volume involved

considerable work, and I wish to express my


gratitude to the people who helped lighten my

labors. I am especially grateful to Jane Crane for

deciphering illegible draft versions and for many

hours of painstaking effort in preparing the

manuscript for publication. Thanks are due to


Robert O’Connor for his assistance with drafting
and photographic matters. I should also like to pay

tribute to Darlene Lapham for her remarkably

efficient typing of the manuscript.

Finally, the dedication of this volume signifies


my profound indebtedness to my family, who

sacrificed many weekend activities and vacations

while I was absorbed in the task of writing this

book.

Albert Bandura

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 15
Stanford, California April 1969

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 16
1

Causal Processes
The development of principles and procedures
of behavioral change is largely determined by the

model of causality to which one subscribes. The

methods used to modify psychological phenomena

therefore cannot be fully understood

independently of the personality theory upon

which they are based. The major differences


between rival theoretical orientations are most
strikingly revealed in their interpretations of

grossly deviant behavior. Consequently the


systems that have been advanced to explain these

perplexing conditions will be considered in some

detail here, although this book is only partially


concerned with issues relating to deviant

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 17
behavior.

The earliest conceptions of psychopathology

viewed behavioral anomalies as external


manifestations of evil spirits that entered the

victim’s body and adversely affected his behavior.

Treatment accordingly was directed toward


exorcising demons by various methods, such as

cutting a hole in the victim’s skull, performing

various magical and religious rituals, or brutally

assaulting-—physically and socially—the bearer


of the pernicious spirits. Hippocrates was

influential in supplanting the demonological

conceptions of deviant behavior by relabeling it

disease rather than demonic manifestations.

Wholesome diets, hydrotherapy, bloodletting, and


other forms of physical intervention, some benign,
others less humane, were increasingly employed

as corrective treatments.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 18
Although psychological methods gradually

replaced physical procedures in modifying deviant

response patterns, the analogy of physical health


and disease nevertheless continued to dominate

theories of psychopathology. In this


conceptualization, behavioral patterns that depart
widely from accepted social and ethical norms are

considered to be derivatives or symptoms of an

underlying disease. Modification of social deviance


thus became a medical specialty, with the result

that persons exhibiting atypical behavior are

labeled “patients” suffering from a “mental


illness,” and they generally are treated in

medically oriented facilities. The disease concepts


are likewise indiscriminately applied even to

social phenomena, as evidenced by the frequent

designation of cultural response patterns as


“healthy” or “sick.” Had Hippocrates represented

behavioral anomalies as products of idiosyncratic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 19
social-learning experiences rather than as

expressions of a somatic illness, the


conceptualization and treatment of divergent

response patterns might have taken an entirely

different course.

A quasi-disease model is still widely employed

in explanations of grossly deviant behavior, but

the underlying pathology is generally considered


to be psychic rather than neurophysiological in
nature. This conceptual scheme became further

confused when the appropriateness of the disease

analogy to social behavior was increasingly

challenged (Szasz, 1961). Most personality


theorists eventually discarded the notion that
deviant behavior is a manifestation of an

underlying mental disease, but they nevertheless

unhesitatingly label anomalous behaviors as

symptoms and caution against the dangers of

symptom substitution. In these theories, the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 20
conditions supposedly controlling behavior

continue to function analogously to toxic

substances in producing deviant responses;


however, the disturbing agents comprise a host of

inimical psychodynamic forces (for example,

repressed impulses, energized traits, psychic

complexes, latent tendencies, self-dynamisms, and

other types of energy systems) somewhat akin to


the pernicious spirits of ancient times. Many
contemporary theories of psychopathology thus

employ a quasi-medical model fashioned from an

amalgam of the disease and demonology

conceptions, which have in common the belief that


deviant behavior is a function of inimical inner

forces. Consequently, attention is generally


focused, not on the problem behavior itself, but on

the presumably influential internal agents that

must be exorcised by “catharsis,” “abreaction,” and


acquisition of insight through an extended

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 21
interpretive process. Indeed, direct modification of

so-called symptomatic behavior is considered not

only ineffective but actually dangerous, because, it


is held, removal of the symptom has no effect upon

the underlying disorder, which will manifest itself


again in a new, possibly more debilitating
symptom.

SOCIAL LABELING OF DEVIANT BEHAVIOR

Although most psychotherapists agree that

direct “symptom” removal is inadvisable and few

of them would acknowledge engaging in such

forms of treatment, remarkably little attention has


been devoted to the definition of what constitutes

a “symptom.” Categorizing a pattern of behavior as

symptomatic of an underlying disorder actually

involves a complex set of criteria, most of which

are quite arbitrary and subjective. Whether


specific actions are called normal or symptomatic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 22
expressions will depend upon whether certain
social judges or the person himself disapproves of

the behavior being exhibited. Since symptom


labeling primarily reflects the evaluative

responses that a given behavior evokes from

others, rather than distinguishable qualities of the

behavior itself, an identical response pattern may


be viewed as a pathological derivative or as

wholesome behavior by persons whose

judgmental orientations differ. Aggressiveness in

children, for example, may be positively reinforced

and regarded as a sign of masculinity and healthy


social development by some parents, while the

same behavior is generally viewed by educational,


legal, and other societal agents as a symptom of a

personality disorder (Bandura, 1960; Bandura &


Walters, 1959).

The designation of behavior as pathological


thus involves social judgments that are influenced

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 23
by, among other factors, the normative standards

of persons making the judgments, the social

context in which the behavior is exhibited, certain


attributes of the behavior, and numerous

characteristics of the deviator himself. An

adequate theory of deviant behavior must

therefore be concerned with the factors

determining evaluative judgments. Unfortunately,


in spite of widespread use of diagnostic
classifications and the potentially serious

consequences of labeling persons as mentally

disturbed, there has been surprisingly little

systematic study of the factors governing such


judgmental behavior.

Psychopathology is characteristically inferred


from the degree of deviance from the social norms

that define how persons are expected to behave at

different times and places. Consequently, the

appropriateness of symbolic, affective, or social

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 24
responses to given situations constitutes one

major criterion in labeling “symptomatic”

behavior. Departures from normative standards


that do not inconvenience or interfere with the

wellbeing of others are usually tolerated;

deviations that produce rewarding consequences

for the members of a society, as in the case of

technological inventions and intellectual and


artistic innovations, may be actively promoted and
generously rewarded. On the other hand, deviance

that generates aversive consequences for others

elicits strong societal disapproval, is promptly

labeled abnormal, and generally is met by coercive


pressures to eliminate it.

The appropriateness criterion poses serious


problems in societies, such as our own, that are

differentiated into many subcultures whose

members subscribe to divergent behavioral norms

and therefore do not agree on what is suitable

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 25
social behavior. Members of social groups who

want rewards that are highly valued in the culture

but lack the means of obtaining them in legitimate


ways (Cloward & Ohlin, 1960; Merton, 1957) are

often forced to resort to socially unacceptable

activities. In these instances, antisocial patterns

are not only normatively sanctioned, but the social

environment provides these persons ample


opportunities, through appropriate reinforcement
contingencies and role models, to develop and to

perfect deviant modes of behavior. According to

the prevailing normative structure of these

subcultures, skillfully executed antisocial behavior


represents emulative rather than sick behavior

and is governed by the same types of variables


that control the prosocial response patterns

displayed by members of the larger society.

Other subgroups are classified as social

deviants, and therefore “sick” or “crazy,” not

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 26
because they adhere to culturally disapproved

means of gaining highly rated objectives but

because they withdraw from the dominant social


system and reject the basic cultural goals

themselves. The conforming majority within a

society may label nonconformist groups, such as

“Bohemians,” “beatniks,” and “hippies,” that refuse

to strive for the goals highly valued in the culture


as exhibiting maladaptive behavior. From the
perspective of the deviants, the life style of

conforming members is a symptomatic

manifestation of an overcommercialized, “sick”

society. Thus the same pattern of behavior may be


deemed a symptom by one social group but judged

healthy and positively reinforced by persons who


adhere to a different code of behavior. Similarly,

when a society radically alters its social and legal

norms, either the presence or absence of the same


responses may be judged inappropriate, and,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 27
consequently, labeled symptoms of an underlying

pathology. Thus, a citizen socialized in other

respects who commits a brutal homicide will be


diagnosed as suffering from a serious mental

disorder, but a military recruit’s inability to


behave homicidally on the battlefield will likewise
be viewed as symptomatic of a “war neurosis.” The

latter example further illustrates how behavior

can come to be thought of as symptomatic because


of changes in societal norms rather than because

of a psychopathology reflected in the behavior

itself.

The discussion thus far has been concerned


with the deviant behavior of members of groups,
who mutually support and reinforce each other’s

ideologies and actions. Some individuals display

gross behavioral eccentricities that appear totally

inexplicable; persons from different subgroups

who do not share the same normative systems are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 28
apt to view these eccentricities as pathological

manifestations. Even in these instances, when the

idiosyncratic social-learning history for the


behavior is known there is no need to assume an

underlying disease process. Lidz, Cornelison,

Terry, & Fleck (1958) report a case, for example,

in which sibling schizophrenics believed, among

other strange things, that “disagreement” meant


constipation. This clearly inappropriate
conceptual behavior was the result of exposure to

bizarre social-learning contingencies and not an

expression of a mental illness. Whenever the sons

disagreed with their mother, she informed them


that they were constipated and required an

enema. The boys were then disrobed and given


anal enemas, a procedure that dramatically

conditioned an unusual meaning to the word

“disagreement.” The cases cited by Lidz and his


associates (Lidz, Fleck, & Cornelison, 1965)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 29
provide compelling evidence of development of

delusions, suspiciousness, grandiosity, extreme

denial of reality, and other forms of


“schizophrenic” behavior through direct

reinforcement, and of their social transmission by


parental modeling of incredibly deviant behavior
patterns.

In addition to the influence of normative


commitments in determining judgmental
responses, certain properties of behavior readily

invite one to label an emotional disorder

symptomatic. Responses of high magnitude, for

instance, often produce unpleasant experiences


for others; they are therefore more likely to be
considered pathological manifestations than are

responses of low or moderate intensities. A

youngster who is continually wrestling other

children will generally be viewed as exhibiting

youthful exuberance; in contrast, a child whose

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 30
physically aggressive behavior is more forceful

and hurtful will in all likelihood be regarded as

emotionally disturbed. Although pervasive and


intense emotional responses may be reliably

categorized, disagreements are apt to arise in the

labeling of behavior that falls at less extreme

points on the response-intensity continuum. The

line separating normality and abnormality may be


variously located depending upon the tolerance
limits for aversiveness of different judges. Even if a

high degree of consensus could be achieved in

designating the acceptable limits of amplitude for

various behaviors, no evidence exists that


emotional responses of high intensity are

mediated by psychopathological internal


processes, whereas similar responses of lesser

strength are governed by nonpathological internal

processes.

Behavioral deficits are also frequently

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 31
interpreted as symptoms of emotional disorder,

particularly when the deficits produce hardships

and aversiveness for others. Adequately endowed


children, for example, who are incontinent and

who exhibit marked deficiencies in interpersonal,

verbal, and academic skills, and adults who are

unable to meet social, marital, and vocational task

requirements tend to be labeled as emotionally


disturbed. It is generally assumed, moreover, that
the greater the deficits, the more extensive the

underlying psychopathology. The arbitrary and

relativistic nature of the deficit or competence

criterion would become readily apparent if one


were to vary the minimum standards of

competence required in any given situation. If the


standards were set at a comparatively low level,

practically all members of a society would be

judged competent and healthy, whereas the vast


majority would suddenly acquire a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 32
psychopathology if exceedingly high standards

were adopted. In the latter case, therapists and

diagnosticians might devote much time to locating


the source of pathology within the individuals.

The intention attributed to an action will affect


its categorization by others as a symptomatic

expression. When the variables governing physical

and biological phenomena remained unknown, a


host of internal forces and deities were invoked as
causal agents. As scientific knowledge increased,

these fanciful driving forces were replaced by

explanatory concepts involving manipulable

variables. Similarly, interpretations of


psychological phenomena often assume
pathological inner agents in cases where deviance

appears unintelligible. If a person engages in

disapproved behavior to attain generally valued

material objects, his activities—being readily

understandable—are less likely to be regarded as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 33
manifestations of emotional disease than if his

deviant behavior has no apparent utilitarian value.

Delinquents who strike victims on the head to


extract their wallets expediently are generally

labeled semiprofessional thieves exhibiting

income-producing instrumental aggression. By

contrast, delinquents who simply beat up

strangers but show no interest in their victims’


material possessions are supposedly displaying
emotional aggression of a peculiarly disturbed

sort. It is evident that in many cases of so-called

nonutilitarian aggression, the behavior is highly

instrumental in gaining the approval and


admiration of peers and in enhancing status in the

social hierarchy of the reference group. Peer-


group approval is often more powerful than

tangible rewards as an incentive for, and

reinforcer of, aggressively deviant behavior


(Buehler, Patterson, and Furniss, 1966).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 34
The influential role of social reinforcement in

regulating dangerous, senseless behavior is clearly

revealed in a field study by Yablonsky (1962), who


found that the dominant reinforcement

contingencies in many delinquent gangs have


shifted from utilitarian antisocial activities to
destructive assaults executed in a “cool” and

apparently indifferent manner on persons and

property. The way in which aggression has taken


on status-conferring value and in which threat of

loss of “rep” may compel a person to engage in a

homicidal assault is graphically illustrated in the


following excerpt from an interview with one of

the boys studied by Yablonsky.

“Momentarily I started to thinking about it


inside; I have my mind made up I’m not
going to be in no gang. Then I go on inside.
Something comes up, then here comes all my
friends coming to me. Like I said before, I’m
intelligent and so forth. They be coming to
me—then they talk to me about what they

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 35
gonna do. Like, ‘Man, we’ll go out there and
kill this cat.’ I say, ‘Yeah.’ They kept on
talkin’. I said, ‘Man, I just gotta go with you.’
Myself, I don’t want to go, but when they
start talkin’ about what they gonna do, I say,
‘So, he isn’t gonna take over my rep. I ain’t
gonna let him be known more than me.’ And
I go ahead [p. vii].”

External contingencies of reinforcement rather

than internal emotional disease also appear to be


the major determinants of the behavior of another

youth involved in a gang killing: “If I would of got


the knife, I would have stabbed him. That would
have gave me more of a build-up. People would

have respected me for what I’ve done and things


like that. They would say, ‘There goes a cold killer’

[p. 8].” Similar reinforcement contingencies


operated in the practice of a gang apprehended

that used attacks upon people without


provocation as its main admissions requirement.

Each physical assault, which had to be observed by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 36
a club member to be valid, was valued at 10

points; and a total of 100 points was required for

full-fledged membership (San Francisco Chronicle,


1964).

It should be noted in passing that prosocial

approval-seeking behavior like athletic

achievements or musical accomplishments, which

may likewise have no apparent utilitarian value, is


seldom labeled as emotionally disturbed behavior.
Certain subgroups simply value and reward

skillful “stomping” more highly than musical

virtuosity.

The instrumental versus emotional dichotomy,


therefore, appears primarily to reflect differences

in the types of rewards sought, and not basic

differences in the purposiveness of the behavior

itself, or in the nature of the mediating internal

events. Since some members of a society are likely

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 37
to be brought up under atypical contingencies of
social reinforcement, events which are ordinarily

neutral or aversive for others may acquire a


strong positive valence; consequently, the puzzling

behavior exhibited by these individuals may

appear to have little or no instrumental value, and

thus tend to be explained by reference to internal


psycho- pathological processes.

Certain behavioral requirements are


prescribed according to a person’s age, sex, social

position, occupation, race, ethnic origin, or

religion. Therefore personal attributes also enter

into social judgment of behavior that deviates

from role demands. For example, behavior


considered to be normal at an early age may be

regarded as a symptom of personality disturbance


later, as in the case of enuresis. It is very

appropriate, in this connection, to repeat


Mowrer’s (1950) query: “And when does

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 38
persisting behavior of this kind suddenly cease to

be normal and become a symptom [p. 474]?” Or

consider the attribute of sex. The differential


cultural tolerance for cross-sex behavior displayed

by males and females illustrates the role of sex

characteristics in the assignment of symptomatic

status to deviant behavioral patterns. The wearing

of female apparel by males is considered to be


indicative of a serious psychological disorder,
requiring prompt legal and psychiatric attention.

On the other hand, females may adopt masculine

garb, hair styles, and a wide range of

characteristically masculine response patterns


without being labeled as mentally disturbed. Since

masculine role behavior occupies a position of


relatively high prestige and power in our society

and often is more generously rewarded than

feminine role behavior, the emulation of


masculine tendencies by females is more

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 39
understandable and, therefore, less likely to be

interpreted by reference to disease processes.

There is another side to the influence of

personal attributes on judgmental responses. The

social-learning background and characteristics of


the person making the judgments may

significantly affect his designation of particular

behaviors as indicative of mental health or psychic


pathology. Spohn (1960) found that therapists’
social values were related to their mental health

judgments of patients’ behavior that reflected

similar value dimensions: that is, therapists

thought the patients more like themselves were


the healthier ones.

Although the presence of psychic illness is

frequently judged in terms of deviance from

particular social norms, in many cases it is

primarily based on self-definition. As Terwilliger &

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 40
Fiedler (1958) have shown, persons often label
themselves as emotionally disturbed, whereas

others may judge them to be functioning


satisfactorily within the prevailing social norms.

Evaluative discrepancies of this type typically

arise when persons impose excessive demands

upon themselves and suffer subjective distress as


a result of failure to meet self-imposed standards.

A comprehensive theory of deviance must take

into consideration self-reactions as well as societal

reactions to one’s behavior.

It is apparent from the foregoing discussion

that the categorization of behavior as symptomatic

of an underlying pathology depends upon a host of


subjective criteria, and as a consequence, the same

behavior may be characterized as “healthy” or


“sick” by different judges, in different social

contexts, and on the basis of performers’ social


characteristics. It is true, of course, that questions

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 41
of value and social judgment arise also in the

diagnosis of physical disorders. In such cases the

symptom-disease model is quite appropriate since


internal organic pathologies do in fact exist and

can be verified independently of their peripheral

manifestations. Brain tumors and dysfunctions

involving respiratory, circulatory, or digestive

organs are observable events. Where deviant


behavior is concerned, analogy with the symptom-
disease model is misleading because there are no

infected organs or psychic disease entities that can

be identified as causal agents. The psychic

conditions that are assumed to underlie


behavioral malfunctioning are merely abstractions

from the behavior. In the disease analogy these


abstractions are not only given substance and

existence independent of the behavior from which

they were inferred, but they are then invoked as


the causes of the same behavioral referents. For

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 42
these reasons, so-called symptomatic behavior can

be more adequately explained in terms of social

learning and value theory than through


inappropriate medical analogizing. An extended

account of a social-learning taxonomy of


behavioral phenomena generally subsumed under
the term “psychopathology” is presented

elsewhere (Bandura, 1968). The preceding

discussion reviewed some of the principal factors


determining the attribution of sickness to deviant

behavior. Similar social judgment processes are, of

course, involved in the attachment of descriptive


labels such as aggression, altruism, dependency, or

achievement to particular response patterns.

HYPOTHETICAL INTERNAL DETERMINANTS OF


BEHAVIOR

The questions raised concerning the utility and


validity of the concept of “symptom” apply equally

to the psychopathology presumed to underlie the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 43
troublesome behavior. From the focusing of
attention on inner agents and forces, many fanciful

theories of deviant behavior have emerged. The


developmental history of social behavior is rarely

known, and its reconstruction from interview

material elicited by therapists or diagnosticians is

of doubtful validity. In fact, the content of


reconstruction is highly influenced by the

interviewer’s suggestive probing and selective

reinforcement of content that is in accord with his

theoretical orientation. Heine (1953), for example,

found that clients who were treated by client-


centered, Adlerian, and psychoanalytic therapists

tended to account for changes in their behavior in


terms of the explanations favored by their

respective interviewers. Even a casual survey of


interview protocols would reveal that

psychotherapists of different theoretical


affiliations tend to find evidence for their own

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 44
preferred psychodynamic agents rather than those

cited by other schools. Thus, Freudians are likely

to unearth Oedipus complexes and castration


anxieties, Adlerians discover inferiority feelings

and compensatory power strivings, Rogerians find


compelling evidence for inappropriate self-
concepts, and existentialists are likely to diagnose

existential crises and anxieties. It is equally true

that Skinnerians, predictably, will discern


defective conditions of reinforcement as

important determinants of deviant behavior. In

the latter explanatory scheme, however, the


suspected controlling conditions are amenable to

systematic variation; consequently the functional


relationships between reinforcement

contingencies and behavior are readily verifiable.

Theoretical models of dubious validity persist

largely because they are not stated in refutable

form. The lack of accurate knowledge of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 45
genesis of behavioral deviations further precludes

any serious evaluation of suggested determinants

that are so involved that they could never be


produced under laboratory conditions. When the

actual social-learning history of maladaptive

behavior is known, principles of learning appear

to provide a completely adequate interpretation of

psychopathological phenomena, and


psychodynamic explanations in terms of
symptom-underlying disorder become

superfluous. The spuriousness of the supposition

that psychodynamic forces produce symptomatic

behavior can be best illustrated by cases in which


the antecedents of aberrant response patterns are

known. Such examples are hard to obtain since


they require the production of deviant behavior

under controlled conditions. Ayllon, Haughton, &

Hughes (1965) furnish a graphic illustration of


how a bizarre pattern of behavior—which was

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 46
developed, maintained, and subsequently

eliminated in a schizophrenic woman simply by

altering its reinforcing consequences—was


interpreted erroneously as a symptomatic

manifestation of complex psychodynamic events


by diagnosticians who were unaware of the
specific conditions of reinforcement regulating the

patient’s behavior.

Unfortunately, the exact antecedents of deviant


behavior are rarely known, and in the absence of

powerful techniques that permit adequate control

over behavioral phenomena, clinical endeavors

have until recently lacked the self-corrective


features necessary for eliminating weak or invalid
theories of psychopathology. As a consequence,

rival interpretations of social behavior have for

decades retained a secure status with little risk

that any one type of theory might prove more

cogent than another.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 47
In recent years there has been a fundamental

departure from conventional views regarding the

nature, causes, and treatment of behavioral


dysfunctions. According to this orientation,

behavior that is harmful to the individual or


departs widely from accepted social and ethical
norms is viewed not as symptomatic of some kind

of disease but as a way that the individual has

learned to cope with environmental and self-


imposed demands. Treatment then becomes

mainly a problem in social learning rather than

one in the medical domain. In this conceptual


scheme the remaining vestiges of the disease-

demonic model have been discarded. Response


patterns are not viewed as symptoms and their

occurrence is not attributed to internal, pernicious

forces.

Social learning and psychodynamic theories

differ not only in whether they view deviant

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 48
behavior as a quasi disease or as a by-product of

learning, but also in what they regard to be the

significant controlling factors, and in the status


assigned to internal events. As will be shown later,

social-learning approaches treat internal

processes as covert events that are manipulable

and measurable. These mediating processes are

extensively controlled by external stimulus events


and in turn regulate overt responsiveness. By
contrast, psychodynamic theories tend to regard

internal events as relatively autonomous. These

hypothetical causal agents generally bear only a

tenuous relationship to external stimuli, or even to


the “symptoms” that they supposedly produce.

Freud’s famous case of Little Hans, which has been


reinterpreted by Wolpe & Rachman (1960),

illustrates some of the major differences in

explanatory models.

Little Hans exhibited, among other things, a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 49
phobia for horses. Freud (1955) interpreted the

phobic behavior in the following manner:

He was not only afraid of horses biting him…


but also of carts, of furniture-vans, and of
buses (their common quality being, as
presently became clear, that they were all
heavily loaded), of horses that started
moving, of horses that looked big and heavy,
and of horses that drove quickly. The
meaning of these specifications was
explained by Hans himself: he was afraid of
horses falling down, and consequently
incorporated in his phobia everything that
seemed likely to facilitate their falling
down…[p. 265],

He (father) elicited from Hans the


recollection of an event at Gmunden, the
impression of which lay concealed behind
that of the falling bus-horse. While they were
playing at horses, Fritzl, the playmate of
whom he was so fond, but at the same time,
perhaps, his rival with his many girl friends,
had hit his foot against a stone and had fallen
down, and his foot had bled. Seeing the bus-
horse fall had reminded him of this accident.
…The first person who had served Hans as a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 50
horse must have been his father; and it was
this that had enabled him to regard Fritzl as
a substitute for his father when the accident
happened at Gmunden.…In the end his father
went into the lumf symbolism, and
recognized that there was an analogy
between a heavily loaded cart and a body
loaded with faeces, between the way in
which a cart drives out through a gateway
and the way in which faeces leave the body,
and so on…[p. 126-127],

We can now recognize that all furniture-vans


and drays and buses were only stork-box
carts, and were only of interest to Hans as
being symbolic representations of
pregnancy; and that when a heavy or heavily
loaded horse fell down he can have seen in it
only one thing—a childbirth, a delivery. Thus
the falling horse was not only his dying
father but also his mother in childbirth [p.
128].

Freud’s paper reports at least four incidents in

which horses, actual or symbolic, were associated

with fear-provoking experiences capable of

producing a conditioned phobic reaction. Hans

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 51
had been frightened at seeing horses being beaten
at a merry-go-round; he was warned to avoid

horses for they might injure him; he became


frightened when a friend was accidentally hurt

while playing horses; and, in the episode that

immediately preceded the onset of the phobic

behavior, he was terrified by a bus accident in


which he believed a horse was killed.

In the psychoanalytic schema the internal


psychic disturbance is the basic cause or instigator

of the phobic responses, while external stimuli

(horses) supposedly exert little or no controlling

influence over the deviant behavior except as a

convenient focal point for Hans’s projected


Oedipal and castration feelings.

It (the phobia) extends to horses and on to


carts, on to the fact that horses fall down and
that they bite, on to horses of a particular
character, on to carts that are heavily loaded.
I will reveal at once that all these

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 52
characteristics were derived from the
circumstance that the anxiety originally had
no reference at all to horses but was
transposed on to them secondarily [italics
added] and had now become fixed upon
those elements of the horse complex which
showed themselves well adapted for certain
transferences [p. 51].

This exposition fails to account for the

variation in both the pattern and the intensity of

Hans’s anxiety reactions under different


circumstances. In fact, the case data provide

considerable evidence that external cues served as


the primary eliciting and controlling stimuli for

Hans’s phobic responses rather than simply as


incidental targets for projected feelings.

Let us consider the major traumatic episode

which was related to the onset of Hans’s phobia.


While out walking with his mother Hans saw a

large bus-horse fall and kick with its feet. He was


terrified and thought the horse was killed in the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 53
accident. There were three important elements in
this stimulus complex—large horse, heavily

loaded transport vehicle, and horse and vehicle


traveling at high speed. The occurrence and

intensity of Hans’s subsequent phobic reactions

varied predictably as a function of the specific

patterning of these three critical stimulus


elements. Hans was more frightened of large dray

horses than of small horses, more frightened of a

rapidly moving vehicle than of a slowly moving

one, more frightened of heavily loaded vehicles

than of empty ones, and frightened when a horse-


drawn cart made a turn:

Hans: And I’m most afraid of furniture-vans too.

Father: Why?

Hans: I think when furniture-horses are dragging a


heavy van they’ll fall down.

Father: So you’re not afraid with a small cart?

Hans: No. I’m not afraid with a small cart or with a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 54
post-office van. I’m most afraid too when a
bus comes along.

Father: Why? Because it’s so big?

Hans: No. Because once a horse in a bus fell down.

Father: What did you think when the horse fell


down?

Hans: Now it’ll always be like this. All horses in


buses’ll fall down…[p. 49],

Father: When the horse fell down, did you think of


your daddy?

Hans: Perhaps. Yes. It’s possible…[p. 51].

Father: What carts are you still afraid of?

Hans: All of them.

Father: You know that’s not true.

Hans: I’m not afraid of carriages and pair or cabs


with one horse. I’m afraid of buses and
luggage-carts, but only when they’re loaded
up, not when they’re empty. When there’s one
horse and the cart’s loaded full up, then I’m
afraid; but when there are two horses and it’s
loaded full up, then I’m not afraid.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 55
Father: Are you afraid of buses because there are so
many people inside?

Hans: Because there’s so much luggage on the top.

Father: When Mummy was having Hanna, was she


loaded full up too? [pp. 90-91],

The Oedipal interpretation fails not only to

account for the discriminative pattern of Hans’s

phobic behavior but also to explain satisfactorily


why he was afraid of railways and locomotives as
well, a phobia which probably generalized from

the transport vehicle stimulus complex. The

psychoanalytic interpretation would demand that


the locomotive and the railway tracks were

likewise symbolic representations of the

castrating father and the impregnated mother.

The conceptual structure of causal sequences

in psychodynamic theories of behavior is

beleaguered by serious problems. An amorphous


internal determinant cannot possibly account for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 56
the remarkable variety of heterogeneous

behaviors as well as changes both in their

incidence and magnitude under different stimulus


conditions, toward different persons, and at

different times. How can a horse phobia be

attributed to an underlying Oedipus complex and


projected castration fears if a person responds

phobically to one horse pulling a heavy loaded

vehicle, but is relatively unafraid of two horses


drawing a loaded vehicle? When diverse stimulus
inputs produce correspondingly diverse

behavioral expressions then any internal


mediators implicated in the causal sequence must

be at least equally specific and their activation


must be closely regulated by discriminative

environmental stimuli.

The conceptual difficulties associated with

psychodynamic formulations apply equally to trait


theories of personality. These approaches assume

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 57
that people possess generalized and stable

response dispositions that determine behavior in

a variety of situations. Consequently it is


considered sufficient to sample some limited

classes of response that are regarded as

dependable indicators of how persons are likely to

behave under particular conditions. The types of

behaviors selected for measurement vary. A few of


the assessment procedures that have been
advocated at one time or another are brief

samplings of overt behavior that bear some

resemblance to the trait description,

endorsements of statements that describe


affective states, interests, or response patterns,

and farfetched responses elicited by relatively


ambiguous stimuli such as inkblots, ill-defined

pictures, doll families, and incomplete sentences.

The basic assumption of trait theories—that

persons display generalized modes of behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 58
that can be predicted from a restricted sampling of

responses—finds little empirical support. For

purposes of illustration, let us consider the “trait”


of aggressiveness. Several investigations

(Bandura, 1960; Bandura & Walters, 1959) of

social-learning determinants of aggressive

behavior have shown that both adolescent and

preadolescent boys display highly discriminative


patterns of aggressive responses that vary
considerably as a function of the persons with

whom they are interacting (for example, parents,

teachers, siblings, or peers). Furthermore, the

incidence of aggression even toward the same


objects differs widely depending upon whether

physical, verbal, or more attenuated forms of


responses are measured. The boys’ discriminative

aggressive responsiveness closely reflected the

considerable amount of discrimination training


that they had undergone. The parents consistently

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 59
punished aggression directed toward themselves,

but simultaneously encouraged and positively

reinforced their sons’ aggressive behavior toward


persons outside the home.

It is evident from informal observation of


differential contingencies characteristically

applied to social response systems that,

fortunately for survival purposes, cultural


practices are much too variable to produce
generalized traits. The likelihood that a given

pattern of behavior will be rewarded, ignored, or

punished is dependent upon, among other factors,

the characteristics of the performer, the specific


form and intensity of the behavior, the objects
toward whom the actions are directed, the social

situations in which they occur, and various

temporal factors. Thus a high degree of behavioral

flexibility is required if a person is to meet the

complexities of ever changing environmental

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 60
demands. In the case of social response systems,

most of which are characterized by high

behavioral specificity, trait measurement is a


disappointing pursuit. Indeed, a comprehensive

review of the relevant empirical literature by

Mischel (1968) reveals low intercorrelations

among different measures purporting to assess

the same trait, weak relationships between


component parts of gross trait dimensions, and
little consistency of behavior patterns in different

stimulus situations. On the other hand, intellectual

performances, which are more or less uniformly

rewarded by different agents at different times


and in different settings, show substantial

consistency.

In the assessment process, behavioral data,

however obtained, are typically converted into

trait or psychodynamic constructs that are far

removed from the actual feelings and actions of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 61
the person being evaluated. This practice rests on

the assumption that the abstractions represent

more generic systems and, therefore, possess


greater predictive power. As Mischel (1968) has

noted in a review of evidence bearing on this

issue, the transformation shifts the focus of

attention from what a person does to speculations

about what he has; from concern about the client’s


behavior to engrossment in the diagnosticians
categories of behavior. The evidence indicates that

these hypothetical constructions are better

predictors of diagnosticians’ semantic and

conceptual stereotypes than of clients’ actual


attributes and psychological reality. It therefore

comes as no surprise that assessment strategies


deriving from the dynamic trait point of view have

generally failed to match the predictive efficacy of

actuarial methods (Meehl, 1954).

The tenacious belief in generalized response

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 62
dispositions is attributed by Mischel (1968) to the

tendency to construe behavioral consistencies

even from variable performances. Hence,


generality may emerge in the inferential construct

domain, whereas a high degree of specificity may

obtain at the behavioral level. Among the factors

listed as reinforcing the impression of consistency

are included physical constancies in appearance,


linguistic characteristics, and stylistic features;
regularities in the stimulus situations in which a

person is repeatedly observed; reliance upon

broad and ambiguous trait categories that

encompass heterogeneous behaviors; utilization of


test items that require a person to rate his

behavior in “typical” social contexts rather than in


a variety of specific situations; and strong

psychological pressures to maintain a consistent,

stable view of events. Inconsistencies, therefore,


tend to be resolved by glossing over, ignoring, or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 63
reinterpreting discrepant evidence.

The preoccupation with internal psychic


agents and energized traits has been largely

responsible for the limited progress in

development of empirically sound principles of


human behavior. The gap between stimulus inputs

and overt response events tends to be filled

readily with diverse all-powerful, animistic


constructs capable of generating and explaining
almost any psychological phenomenon. These

constructs, of course, lend themselves easily to

pseudo explanations (Skinner, 1961) in which

renaming of a behavioral phenomenon is offered


as an explanation. For example, persons who
exhibit withdrawal, delusional and hallucinatory

behavior, inappropriate emotional responses, and

behavioral deficits, will be labeled schizophrenic.

The presence of these deviant behaviors is then

attributed to an underlying schizophrenia, an

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 64
explanation that is completely circular and

contains no information whatsoever about causal

determinants. An adequate causal explanation


must specify clearly the independent variables

that produce and maintain the observed

schizophrenic behavior. In a similar manner,

traits, complexes, and dynamics, which represent

the descriptive constructs of the assessor, often


are made active entities within the client that
supposedly cause his behavior.

The major deficiencies of theories that explain

behavior primarily in terms of conjectural inner


causes would have been readily demonstrated had
they been judged, not in terms of their facility in

interpreting behavioral phenomena that have


already occurred, but rather on the basis of their

efficacy in predicting or modifying them. Because

the internal determinants propounded by these

theories (such as mental structures, Oedipal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 65
complexes, collective unconscious) could not be

experimentally induced, and rarely possessed

unequivocal consequences, psychodynamic


formulations enjoyed an immunity to genuine

empirical verification. If progress in the

understanding of human behavior is to be

accelerated, psychological theories must be judged

by their predictive power, and by the efficacy of


the behavioral modification procedures that they
produce.

ADVERSE CONSEQUENCES OF DISEASE


INTERPRETATIONS OF DEVIANT BEHAVIOR

The conceptualization of deviant behavior as

manifestations of disease has, in several ways,

impeded development of efficacious methods of

behavioral change. In the first place, it led to heavy


reliance upon physical and chemical interventions,

unremitting search for drugs as quick remedies for


interpersonal problems, and long-term neglect of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 66
social variables as influential determinants of
deviant response patterns. Secondly, the

mislabeling, partly by historical accident, of social


deviations as symptoms of mental illness

established medical training as the optimal

preparation for psychotherapeutic work. In fact,

such training, because of its primary concern with


somatic processes and pathologies, leaves one ill-

prepared for devising and implementing methods

that are successful in promoting favorable social

change. Had educational processes, which also

depend upon neurophysiological functioning, been


historically misconstrued as principally medical

phenomena, our society would undoubtedly be


faced with the same critical shortage of

educational facilities and well-trained


instructional personnel that characterize our

current “mental health” enterprises.

Although the designation of behavioral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 67
eccentricities as manifestations of disease initially

resulted in more humane treatment, as Szasz

cogently points out, continued adherence to this


analogy has become a serious hindrance. Many

people who would benefit greatly from

psychological treatment avoid seeking help

because they fear being stigmatized as mentally

deranged, which often carries deleterious social


consequences. Those who are compelled by
chronic distress to seek a solution to their

interpersonal problems are typically ascribed a

sick role and are regarded as relatively helpless,

dependent, and incompetent in managing their


daily lives. By having their behavioral deviations

treated as expressions of internal psychic


pathologies they are thereby relieved of the

natural consequences of their actions. In this

connection, it is important to distinguish judicious


management of reinforcement contingencies

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 68
aimed at altering the course of future behavior

from moral judgments of personal responsibility

for past actions. There is little to be gained from


condemning delinquents for their history of

antisocial behavior, but there is much to be gained


from having them experience new response
consequences that will help them develop a more

effective way of life. When individuals are labeled

mentally ill, this often results not only in


suspension of customary response consequences

essential for change, but in substitution of

contingencies that foster maladaptive tendencies


(Ayllon & Michael, 1959). Moreover, as will be

shown later, for people who undergo long-term


institutionalization, the attendant stigmatization,

the patient-role requirements of the mental

hospital culture, the limited opportunities to


perform behaviors that are necessary in

community life, and the development of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 69
institutional dependency produce further

impediments to successful readjustment to typical

environmental demands.

The medical orientation toward deviant

behavior has resulted also in a disinterest in, and


lack of facilities for, the modification of lesser, but

nevertheless troublesome, forms of psychological

problems. People with circumscribed behavioral


difficulties are justifiably unwilling to label
themselves mentally deranged and to enter into a

protracted expensive treatment that offers no

guarantee of success. Thus, for example, people

who suffer from snake phobias may be unable to


perform their work under certain conditions, to
participate in camping and other outdoor

activities, or to reside in locales inhabited by

harmless snakes. Treatments derived from social-

learning principles are now available that can

effectively eliminate such phobias in any person in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 70
a few sessions (Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter,

1969). Psychological centers that offer brief and

highly efficacious treatments for specific


behavioral dysfunctions would provide valuable

therapeutic services to many persons who would

otherwise endure unnecessary restrictions in

certain areas of their psychological functioning.

The designation of divergent beliefs and


actions as “sick” may also have an important
impact on the more general process of social

change. Improvements in the conditions of life

within a society require the continuous


modification of its institutionalized patterns of
behavior and the replacement of old standards of

conduct with new ones that are more fitting to the


altered circumstances. Proposed social reforms,

however, typically meet with strong resistance,

particularly if they represent marked departure

from established traditions and threaten vested

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 71
interests. Consequently persons often find it

necessary to violate institutionalized codes of

behavior in order to force a change in the social


system. In such instances, deviance serves a

positive function in promoting constructive

modifications. The conforming populace, despite

its protestations, eventually profits from the

nonconformists’ deviance.

Resistance to advocated social changes


sometimes takes the form of publicly labeling

those who advocate divergent practices as

emotionally disturbed. This diagnostic devaluation


is most easily applied when social deviants
attempt, as they usually do, to differentiate

themselves from the general populace by adopting


unconventional attire and hair styles or peculiar

symbols and rituals. In some totalitarian societies

it is not uncommon to silence authors who

propose certain social and political reforms by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 72
diagnosing them as mentally deranged and

committing them to psychiatric hospitals

(Crankshaw, 1963). Although our own society


rarely imposes such legal sanctions, active

nonconformists are often discredited by

characterizing them as “perverts” and members of

the “lunatic fringe.” A society would better

preserve its potential for change by defining social


deviance as innovative rather than “sick” behavior.
Such a practice would favor evaluation of

proposed changes on the basis of their merits and

probable long-term consequences, as should be

the case.

Since social control through stigmatizing

deviance as psychic malfunction has gained


currency in our society, it would be surprising if

such mislabeling were confined to matters of

cultural norms and objects. Even the

diagnosticians themselves may yield to the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 73
temptation to brand any dissidence as

psychopathological. In one such illustration

(Gitelson, 1962), departure from orthodoxy in


psychoanalytic theory is explained not by factual

and theoretical disagreements, but in terms of

“pathological narcissism,” “transference

neuroses,” and other psychodynamic malfunctions

in dissenting members.

Szasz (1965), who has been especially


concerned about the promotion of moral

prescriptions in the guise of psychiatric diagnoses,

has written widely on the contemporary misuse of


the notion of mental illness. He argues that, in an
effort to ensure more benevolent treatment of

persons in difficulty, they are certified as afflicted


with a mental disease. This advantage, however, is

gained at the expense of stigmatization,

degradation, and restriction of personal freedom.

Rather than the “bootlegging of humanism” on

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 74
psychiatric grounds he advocates frank

confrontation of the socio-ethical issues involved

in societal practices and active efforts to bring


about needed reforms. To take legalized abortion

as an example, Szasz (1962) contends that it

would be more honest to grant people the right to

determine for themselves whether they wish to

bring a child into the world than to invoke


psychiatric illness as a subterfuge for performing
abortions. As an analogy, if divorces were granted

only on the basis of psychiatric certification of

mental illness, the incidence of mental

derangement would suddenly rise astronomically.

Interpretation of Causal Processes

Preoccupation with internal response-

producing agents has resulted in a disregard of

external variables that have nevertheless been

shown to exercise control over behavior. An

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 75
organism that is impelled from within but is

relatively insensitive to environmental stimuli or

to the immediate consequences of its actions


would not survive for long. Human functioning, in

fact, involves interrelated control systems in

which behavior is determined by external

stimulus events, by internal information-

processing systems and regulatory codes, and by


reinforcing response-feedback processes.

Stimulus Control of Behavior

During initial phases of human development,


stimuli, except those which are inherently

aversive, exert little or no influence upon

individuals. Eventually, however, as a result of

undergoing either direct or vicarious experiences,


individuals’ behavior comes to be regulated by
antecedent stimulus events that convey

information about probable consequences of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 76
certain actions in given situations. The

development of appropriate anticipatory reactions

to recurrent environmental cues has considerable


functional and survival value. Indeed, an

individual who did not learn to avoid physical

hazards, who did not respond appropriately to

traffic signals and other guiding cues, for example,

and who remained indifferent to important social


and symbolic stimuli, would suffer a painfully
rapid extinction.

STIMULUS CONTROL OF AUTONOMIC


RESPONSIVENESS

Many problems for which people seek relief

involve distressing autonomic overactivity

reflected in a variety of somatic complaints of a

functional nature, including chronic “tension” and


anxiety reactions, gastrointestinal disorders, and

respiratory and cardiovascular disturbances.


Conditioned emotionality is also generally

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 77
implicated, particularly during the acquisition
phase, in obsessive-compulsive reactions,

behavioral inhibitions, and phobic and other


avoidance behaviors. Depressant drugs may

provide temporary relief from intense autonomic

responses, but in cases where they are under

stimulus control, social-learning procedures that


are capable of neutralizing the emotion-arousing

properties of stimulus events offer the most direct

and effective treatment.

Autonomic responses can be most readily

brought under the control of environmental

stimuli through classical conditioning operations.

If a formerly ineffective or conditioned stimulus is


closely associated with an unconditioned stimulus

capable of eliciting a given physiological response,


the former stimulus alone gradually acquires the

power to evoke the physiological response or its


equivalent. Although some types of autonomic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 78
responses are more difficult to condition than

others, almost every form of somatic reaction that

an organism is capable of making, including


respiratory and heart-rate changes, increases in

muscular tension, gastrointestinal secretions,

vasomotor reactions, and other indices of

emotional responsiveness (Bykov, 1957; Kimble,

1961), has been classically conditioned to


innocuous stimuli. Environmental events can
likewise acquire the capacity to control

electroencephalographic arousal through

association with either external evocative stimuli

or direct central stimulation (John, 1967).

Laboratory studies concerned with the

production of asthmatic attacks illustrate how


psychosomatic reactions can be brought under

stimulus control. Noelpp & Noelpp-Eschenhagen

(1951, 1952), for example, demonstrated that

following repeated pairing of induced asthmatic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 79
attacks with an auditory stimulus, many of the

guinea pigs in the study exhibited respiratory

dysfunctions characteristic of bronchial asthma in


response to the conditioned auditory stimulus

alone. Stimulus control of human asthmatic

attacks is similarly demonstrated in an experiment

by Dekker, Pelser, & Groen (1957). Two patients

suffering from severe bronchial asthma inhaled


nebulized allergens to which they were
hypersensitive. After repeated inhalations of the

allergen extract that served as the unconditioned

stimulus for asthmatic attacks, inhalation of a

neutral solvent of the allergen alone, which


initially produced no respiratory changes, elicited

attacks of asthma as demonstrated by clinical


signs and vital capacity measures. In later phases

of the experiment inhalations of pure oxygen and

even the presentation of the mouthpiece, both


formerly neutral stimuli, had acquired the power

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 80
to provoke asthmatic attacks which were

indistinguishable from those induced by the

allergen itself.

In the experiment described, asthmatic

responses were conditioned to elements of the


inhalation situation and apparatus through

contiguous association. It is not surprising,

therefore, that analyses of asthmatic behavior by


Dekker & Groen (1956) produced an extremely
varied array of highly specific eliciting stimuli in

the group of patients studied; these included the

sight of dust, radio speeches by influential

politicians, children’s choirs, the national anthem,


elevators, goldfish, caged birds, the smell of
perfume, waterfalls, bicycle races, police vans, and

horses. Once the critical eliciting stimuli had been

identified in a particular case, Dekker and Groen

were able to induce attacks of asthma by

presenting the conditioned stimuli in actual or in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 81
pictorial form. In some cases, of course, more

complex interpersonal events may serve as major

evocative stimuli.

Of particular interest is the investigators’

observation that intense emotional arousal itself

failed to produce asthmatic reactions, whereas

exposure to specific asthmatic conditioned stimuli

typically provoked marked respiratory


dysfunction. The latter observation is
corroborated by Ottenberg, Stein, Lewis, &

Hamilton (1958) in a study of the classical

conditioning and extinction of asthmatic


responses in guinea pigs. Asthma-like attacks,
which readily occurred in the presence of

conditioned stimuli, could not be induced by


means of emotion-provoking procedures involving

loud noises, painful stimulation, and electric

shock. In view of these findings, one would expect

that direct neutralization of specific eliciting

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 82
stimuli (Moore, 1965; Walton, 1960) would be

effective in modifying asthmatic responses under

the control of environmental stimuli, but that


reduction of general emotional disturbances may

have little impact on the respiratory disorder.

Both the processes and outcomes

accompanying classical conditioning operations

are considerably more complex than the general


principle might imply. Persons often display
differential susceptibility to autonomic

conditioning, which suggests that other variables

—possibly genetic, physiological, or psychological


—are contributory factors. It will also be shown
later that cognitive representation of the

contingent relationship between conditioned and


unconditioned stimuli markedly facilitates

classical conditioning. These findings call into

question peripheral theories of conditioning.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 83
Higher-Order Conditioning. Many of the

emotional responses that persons exhibit toward

specific objects are not products of direct


associations of affective experiences with the

objects themselves. Some people, for example,


may respond anxiously toward snakes without
having had any direct aversive encounters with

them. Similarly, persons often display strong

emotional arousal at the sight or mention of


unpopular minority groups or nationalities on the

basis of little or no personal contact. These types

of reactions are frequently established on the


basis of higher-order sequences depending on the

presence or absence of particular stimuli. This


process is most clearly illustrated in simple

laboratory studies in which certain responses are

reinforced only in the presence of one stimulus


(e.g., green light), but never in a different stimulus

context (e.g., red light). After the discrimination

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 84
has been formed, a person responds only in the

presence of the green light. Thus by introducing


into the environment a discriminative stimulus

that signifies whether a particular performance is

likely to be reinforced, a considerable degree of

control over behavior can be achieved.

The following quotation presents a more

telling example of stimulus control of behavior


occurring under naturalistic conditions. In this
illustration an elaborate pattern of aggressive

behavior by an autistic boy was rarely exhibited in

the father’s presence but freely expressed in his

absence.

Whenever her husband was home, Billy was


a model youngster. He knew that his father
would punish him quickly and
dispassionately for misbehaving. But when
his father left the house, Billy would go to the
window and watch until the car pulled out.
As soon as it did, he was suddenly
transformed. . . . ‘He’d go into my closet and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 85
tear up my evening dresses and urinate on
my clothes. He’d smash furniture and run
around biting the walls until the house was
destruction from one end to the other. He
knew that I liked to dress him in nice clothes,
so he used to rip the buttons off his shirts,
and used to go in his pants’ [Moser, 1965, p.
96].

Laboratory investigations of stimulus control

processes often involve simple situations in which

stimuli differ either on a single attribute or on a


few easily identifiable dimensions. In most real life

circumstances the cues which designate probable


consequences usually appear as part of a

bewildering variety of irrelevant events. One must,


therefore, abstract the critical feature common to

a variety of situations. Behavior can be brought

under the control of abstract stimulus properties if

responses to situations containing the critical


element are reinforced, whereas responses to all
other stimulus patterns lacking the essential

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 86
element go unreinforced. It should be noted here

that the controlling function of various social and

environmental stimuli is usually established


simply by informing people about the conditions

of reinforcement that are operative in different

situations, rather than by leaving them to discover

it for themselves through a tedious process of

selective reinforcement. However, the existence of


differential consequences is essential to maintain
stimulus control produced through instructional

means.

In discussions of stimulus control processes it


has been customary to distinguish between the
eliciting and the discriminative or response-

directing functions of stimulus events (Skinner,


1961). As noted earlier, autonomic responses are

elicited by their controlling stimuli, independently

of their subsequent consequences. An asthmatic

conditioned stimulus, for example, will induce

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 87
respiratory changes apart from the social effects

resulting from somatic reactions. On the other

hand, in the case of instrumental responses, the


discriminative stimuli simply modify the

probability that a given response will occur, but

they do not elicit it. Moreover, the stimulus control

of operant or instrumental behaviors is

established and maintained by differential


response consequences rather than through
temporal association of sets of stimulus events.

Under naturalistic conditions behavior is

generally regulated by the characteristics of


persons toward whom responses are directed, the
social setting, temporal factors, and a host of

verbal and symbolic cues that signify predictable


response consequences. Social situations,

particularly those involving a large number of

multidimensional cues, seldom recur with exactly

the same constituent elements. Because of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 88
constant variation in the nature and patterning of

stimuli, social learning would be an interminable

and exceedingly laborious process if responses


were entirely specific to the situation in which

they had been originally reinforced. However,

performances that have been reinforced in the

presence of certain cues are also controlled by

other stimuli which are related to them either


physically or semantically. After generalized
stimulus control has been established it can be

narrowed, if necessary, by differential

reinforcement of responses to stimuli whose

differences are progressively reduced (Terrace,


1966).

Outcome Control of Behavior

An organism that responded anticipatorily to


informative environmental cues but remained

unresponsive to the outcomes produced by its

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 89
behavior would enjoy a tragically brief life-span.

Fortunately, instrumental responses are

extensively controlled by their immediate


consequences. Responses that result in nonreward

or punishing effects are generally eliminated,

whereas those that are successful in securing

positively reinforcing outcomes are retained and

strengthened. There is some evidence (Kimmel,


1967; Miller, 1969) that autonomic responses,
which formerly were believed to be subject only to

classical conditioning, can also be modified

instrumentally to some degree by differential

consequences. Indeed, DiCara & Miller (1968)


were able to establish remarkably precise control

over vasomotor activities through differential


reinforcement.

External Reinforcement. Research conducted by

Harris, Wolf & Baer (1964), designed to modify

gross behavior disorders in nursery school

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 90
children by altering their teachers’ attentional

responses, provides impressive demonstrations of

how deviant behavior can be controlled by its


social consequences. Each case involved an

intrasubject replication design in which behavior

was successively eliminated and reinstated by

systematic variation of reinforcement

contingencies. This is a most powerful method for


isolating the controlling conditions of behavioral
phenomena. The procedure in any given case

contains four steps.

First, the child is observed for a period of time


to measure the incidence of the deviant behavior,
the contexts in which it typically occurs, and the

reactions it elicits from teachers. In one case an


extremely withdrawn boy spent approximately 80

percent of his time in solitary activities in isolated

areas of the nursery school. Observation revealed

that the teachers unwittingly reinforced his

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 91
solitariness by paying a great deal of attention to

him, reflecting his feelings of loneliness, consoling

him and encouraging him to play with other


children. When he did happen to join other

children, the teachers took no particular notice.

In the second phase of the program a new set

of reinforcement practices is substituted.

Continuing with the above example, the teachers


stopped rewarding solitary play with attention
and support. Instead, whenever the boy sought out

other children, the teacher immediately joined the

group and gave it her full attention. In a short


time, the boy’s isolation declined markedly and he
was spending about 60 percent of his time playing

with other children (Figure 1-1).

After the desired changes in behavior have

been produced, the original reinforcement

practices are reinstated to determine if the initial

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 92
Figure 1-1. Percentage of time a withdrawn boy spent in
social interaction before treatment began, during
periods when social behavior toward peers was
positively reinforced, and during periods when
teachers gave attention for solitary play. Harris, Wolf, &
Baer, 1964.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 93
behavior was in fact maintained by its social
consequences. In this third stage, for example, the

teachers again paid no attention to the child’s


sociability but instead responded with comforting

ministrations whenever he was alone. The effect of

this traditional “mental hygiene” treatment was to

increase the child’s withdrawal to the original high


level (Figure 1-1).

In the final phase of the program the


therapeutic contingencies are reintroduced, the

deviant behavior is eliminated and the desired

behavior patterns are generously reinforced. In

the above case, after social responsivity was well

established the frequency of positive attention


from adults was gradually diminished as the boy

derived increasing enjoyment from play activities


with his peers. Follow-up observations disclosed

that the boy maintained his sociable pattern of


behavior, which contrasted markedly with his

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 94
previous isolation.

Children with a wide variety of behavior

disorders have participated in such programs, and


in each case their maladaptive behavior was

eliminated, reinstated, and removed a second time

simply by altering teachers’ social responsiveness

(Harris, Wolf, & Baer, 1964). Additional

demonstrations of reinforcement control of


grossly deviant behavior in both children and
adults are provided by Ayllon and his associates

(Ayllon & Azrin, 1965; Ayllon & Michael, 1959)

and by Wolf, Risley, & Mees (1964).

Reinforcement control of behavior is further


demonstrated by evidence that different

frequency and patterning of outcomes produce

different types of performance (Ferster & Skinner,

1957). When subjects are rewarded each time

they exhibit the desired behavior (continuous

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 95
schedule), and later the reinforcement is
completely withdrawn, they are likely to increase

responsiveness for a brief period of time and then


to display a rapid decrease in performance, often

accompanied by emotional reactions.

Sometimes behavior is reinforced only after a


specified period of time has elapsed (fixed-interval
schedule). Pay periods, eating schedules,

recreational times, and other regularly scheduled


rewarding activities illustrate the temporal cycles

of reinforcement regulating some aspects of

human behavior. When rewards are dispensed on

a fixed temporal basis the payoff is the same

regardless of the amount of behavior produced


during the intervening period. Under these

conditions, once a person develops a temporal


discrimination, the response output following

reinforcement is very low but accelerates rapidly


as the time for the next reinforcement approaches.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 96
In naturalistic situations where temporal

reinforcement cycles may range over several

hours, days, weeks, or even months, social


approval or coercive forms of pressure are usually

brought to bear in order to maintain a steady rate

of performance. Nevertheless, even with these

added inducements, the fixed-interval schedule is

likely to generate only the minimum output


expected in a given situation, particularly if the
activity itself is somewhat unpleasant. On the

other hand, where given performances have

become intrinsically rewarding, satisfactions

derived from the activity itself may greatly


outweigh the influence of temporally occurring

rewards.

Much human behavior is sustained by ratio

schedules in which reinforcement is made

contingent upon the amount of behavior rather

than on the passage of time. In a fixed-ratio

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 97
schedule a person must complete a specified

amount of work for each reinforcement. Since

under these circumstances reinforcement depends


upon the person’s own behavior, these schedules

usually generate high and stable responsiveness.

By starting with a low ratio and gradually raising

the number of performances required per

reinforcement, very high performance rates can be


developed and maintained for a long period with
minimum reinforcement. Although ratio schedules

are exceedingly effective in generating a high

behavioral output, persons in extra-laboratory

situations, where they have considerably more


freedom of action, are likely to withdraw from

situations with schedules requiring substantial


performances for minimal returns, and to select

more beneficent reinforcing agents.

In everyday life most reinforcements are

available not only on an intermittent basis, but

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 98
also on variable schedules. The effects of variable-

interval and variable-ratio schedules on

performance have been extensively studied under


controlled laboratory conditions. In the former

case, the length of time between successive

reinforcements is varied randomly around some

mean temporal value; in the variable-ratio

schedules, the number of responses per


reinforcement is varied around a selected aver age
ratio. Since the reinforcers are dispensed

unpredictably, the usual temporal or rate

discriminations that result in cyclic responsivity

cannot develop; consequently, variable schedules


generate higher rates of response and more stable

and consistent performances than those in which


outcomes occur on a regular or fixed basis.

However, even under irregular reinforcement,

ratio schedules are more effective than interval


schedules. Research evidence in fact reveals that,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 99
of all the variations in scheduling procedures

available, the variable-ratio schedule is most

powerful in sustaining behavior. A casual


observation of the patrons of the gambling devices

at Las Vegas attests to the generality and validity


of laboratory findings.

Evidence of schedule control of behavior has

important implications for the understanding of


behavior and for its modification. Those who have
been reared under more or less continuous

reinforcement conditions are likely to become

easily discouraged and to cease responding when

faced with frustrating nonreward or failure. By


contrast, persons whose response patterns have
been reinforced only intermittently will persist in

their behavior for a considerable time despite

setbacks and infrequent reinforcement. This, of

course, is the reinforcement history that is most

characteristic of all stable response patterns

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 100
including deviant ones. Moreover, when efforts

are made to extinguish such behavior, it is not

unusual for a parent or other persons to give in


temporarily by rewarding the behavior,

particularly if it goes on unabated or increases in

rate or intensity. Any reinforcements occurring

during the extinction process, however, will

reinstate the behavior, often at a higher level than


if extinction had not been attempted.

There are other subtle variations in the

patterning of reinforcement that significantly

influence the characteristics of behavior. As will be


shown later, differential reinforcement of behavior
that is persistent, or of high magnitude, is another

form of intermittence that establishes deviant and


obstreperous behavior of unusual resiliency.

Reinforcements can also be applied in such a way

as to produce delayed behavior. This outcome is

achieved in laboratory studies by making rewards

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 101
available after a given period of time has elapsed,

but only if the subject has refrained from

responding during the interval. Each time the


subject responds prematurely the enforced

waiting period is begun all over again. By

gradually lengthening the time interval, self-

control in the subject can be increased.

In everyday life different classes of social


behavior are controlled by multiple schedules of
reinforcement operating either concurrently or

alternately. This process is most dramatically

illustrated in an experiment conducted by Ferster


(Ferster & Skinner, 1957), in which the right-hand
responses of a subject were reinforced on a fixed-

ratio schedule, whereas responses with the left


hand were reinforced simultaneously on a

variable-ratio schedule. The subject produced two

remarkably different sets of performances, each

corresponding to the typical response-rate curves

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 102
of these types of schedules. Finally, it should be

noted that different types of positive and negative

consequential events possess differential


controlling power. The theoretical issues and

empirical findings relevant to this reinforcement

variable will be considered in subsequent chapters

of this book.

Vicarious Reinforcement. The discussion thus


far has been concerned with the extent to which
responsiveness is regulated by external outcomes

impinging directly upon a performer. There is

considerable evidence (Bandura, 1965) that the


behavior of observers can be substantially
modified as a function of witnessing other people’s

behavior and its consequences for them.


Observation of rewarding consequences generally

enhances similar performances, whereas

witnessing punishing outcomes has an inhibiting

effect on behavior. Systematic investigations of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 103
relative efficacy of vicarious and direct

reinforcement reveal that the changes exhibited

by observers are of the same magnitude (Kanfer,


1965) or, under certain conditions, may even

exceed those achieved by reinforced performers

(Berger, 1961; Marlatt, 1968). Moreover, vicarious

reinforcement processes are governed by

variables such as the percentage (Bisese, 1966;


Kanfer, 1965), intermittence (Rosenbaum &
Bruning, 1966), and magnitude (Bruning, 1965) of

reinforcement in essentially the same manner as

when they are applied directly to a performing

subject. Although the efficacy of vicarious


reinforcement practices is well established, the

behavioral changes displayed by observers may be


interpreted in several ways.

One possible explanation is in terms of the

discriminative or informative function of

reinforcing stimuli presented to the model.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 104
Response consequences experienced by another

person undoubtedly convey information to the

observer about the probable reinforcement


contingencies associated with analogous

performances in similar situations. Knowledge

concerning the types of responses that are likely

to meet with approval or disapproval can later

serve a self-instructional function in facilitating or


inhibiting emulative behavior. The information
gained from witnessing outcomes experienced by

others would be particularly influential in

regulating behavior under conditions where

considerable ambiguity exists as to what actions


are permissible or punishable, and where the

observer believes that the models’ contingencies


apply to himself as well. It is highly unlikely, for

example, that witnessing social approval for

physical aggression exhibited by a person


occupying a unique role, such as a policeman,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 105
would enhance imitative aggressiveness in

observant citizens to any great extent.

Experiments are therefore needed that test the


magnitude of vicarious reinforcement effects as a

function of comparability of social sanctions


customarily applied to models and to observers.

Typically, models’ responses are differentially

reinforced depending upon the persons toward


whom the behavior is directed and the social
settings in which it is expressed. When differential

consequences are correlated with different

stimulus conditions, observation of the

reinforcement pattern associated with the models’


responses helps the observer to identify the social
or environmental stimuli to which the modeled

behavior is most appropriate. These relevant cues

may be difficult to distinguish without the

observed informative feedback. Hence, through

repeated exposure to the outcomes of others, an

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 106
observer not only acquires knowledge of

predictable reinforcement contingencies, but he

may also discern the situations in which it is most


appropriate to exhibit a given pattern of behavior.

The resultant discrimination learning can later

facilitate the performance of matching responses

in the presence of the cues to which the model

previously had been responding with favorable


consequences (Church, 1957; McDavid, 1962;
Paschke, Simon, & Bell, 1967).

Observation of reinforcing outcomes and the

models’ concomitant reactions may also have


important activating or motivational effects on an
observer. The mere sight of highly valenced

reinforcers can produce anticipatory arousal


which, in turn, will affect the level of imitative

performance. Thus, for example, witnessing a

performer rewarded with a culinary treat for

executing a given sequence of responses will

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 107
convey the same amount of information about the

probable reinforcement contingencies to a

famished and to a satiated observer, but their


subsequent imitative performances will, in all

likelihood, differ radically because of the

differential effects of deprivation state on the

activating power of the anticipated incentive.

Similarly, variations in the magnitude of observed


reinforcers, while providing equivalent
information about the permissibility of matching

responses, have different motivational effects on

observers (Bruning, 1965). As in the case of direct

reinforcement, incentive-produced motivation in


observers is most likely to affect the speed,

intensity, and persistence with which matching


responses are executed.

A vicarious reinforcement event not only

provides information concerning probable

reinforcement contingencies, knowledge about the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 108
types of situations in which the behavior is

appropriate, and displays of incentives possessing

activating properties, but it also includes affective


expressions of models undergoing rewarding and

punishing experiences. As was mentioned earlier,

the pleasure and pain cues emitted by a model

generally elicit corresponding affective responses

in the viewer. These vicariously aroused


emotional responses can readily become
conditioned either to the modeled responses

themselves, or to environmental stimuli that are

regularly correlated with the performer’s affective

reactions. As a consequence the subsequent


initiation of matching responses by the observer

or the presence of the correlated environmental


stimuli is likely to generate some degree of

emotional arousal. In a similar manner, witnessing

the nonoccurrence of anticipated aversive


consequences to a model can extinguish in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 109
observers previously established emotional

responses that are vicariously aroused by modeled

displays. It is therefore possible that the


facilitative or suppressive effects of observing the

affective consequences for the model may be


partly mediated by the vicarious conditioning or
extinction of emotional responses.

Finally, reinforcements administered to


another person may have important consequences
in social evaluation. Punishment is apt to devalue

the model and his behavior, whereas models who

receive praise and admiration tend to be

attributed prestige and competence (Bandura,


Ross, & Ross, 1963; Hastorf, 1965). Changes in
model status, in turn, can significantly affect

observers’ subsequent performance of matching

responses. A particular vicarious reinforcement

event, depending upon its nature and context, may

thus produce behavioral changes in observers

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 110
through any one or more of the five processes

outlined.

The effects of observed consequences upon


performance are also likely to be influenced by the

social conditions under which the vicarious events

occur. Almost without exception, the studies

discussed above employ a paradigm in which

observers’ behavior is measured after they have


witnessed another person either rewarded or
punished by an agent with whom the observers

never have any contact and in social settings that

differ from their own. Observed consequences


may have different behavioral effects under
conditions where the reinforced performers and

the observers are members of the same group who


are present in the same setting and interacting

with the same social agents. Observers who

witness other members rewarded for a certain

pattern of behavior may temporarily increase

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 111
similar responding, but if their behavior is

consistently ignored they are apt to discontinue

the modeled behavior or even respond negatively


to .the agent’s preferential treatment.

Self-Reinforcement. Although the controlling

power of externally occurring consequences

cannot be minimized, self-reinforcement may

frequently outweigh the influence of external


outcomes in governing social behavior,
particularly in the case of older children and

adults. Until recently, self-reinforcement

phenomena have been virtually ignored in


psychological theorizing and experimentation,
perhaps as a result of preoccupation with

infrahuman learning. Unlike humans, who


continually engage in self-evaluative and self-

reinforcing behavior, rats or chimpanzees are

disinclined to pat themselves on the back for

commendable performances, or to berate

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 112
themselves for getting lost in culs-de-sac. By

contrast, people typically set themselves certain

standards of behavior and self-administer


rewarding or punishing consequences depending

on whether their performances fall short of,

match, or exceed their self-prescribed demands.

Self-reinforcing responses are to some extent

directly established through selective


reinforcements administered initially by
socialization agents. In this learning process an

agent adopts a criterion of what constitutes a

worthy performance and consistently rewards


persons for matching or exceeding the adopted
standard, while nonrewarding or punishing

performances that fall short of it. When


subsequently persons are given control over the

administration of reinforcers they are likely to

reinforce themselves in a similarly selective

manner. In a study investigating the effects of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 113
miserly and indulgent training on rate of self-

reinforcement, Kanfer & Marston (1963)

rewarded the performances of some adults


generously with token reinforcers accompanied by

an approving attitude toward self-reward,

whereas with others the experimenter parted

grudgingly with a few tokens and cautioned

subjects against requesting rewards for


undeserving performances. Those who received
lenient training subsequently rewarded

themselves far more frequently on a different task

than subjects who were stringently trained even

though the achievements for both groups were


comparable.

There exists a substantial body of evidence


that modeling processes play a highly influential

role in the transmission of self-reinforcement

patterns. In the prototypic experiment (Bandura &

Kupers, 1964) subjects observe a model

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 114
performing a task in which he adopts either a high

performance standard or a relatively low criterion

for self-reinforcement. On trials in which the


model attains or exceeds the self-imposed demand

he rewards himself materially and expresses

positive self-evaluations, but when his attainments

fall short of the adopted behavioral requirements

he denies himself available rewards and reacts in a


self-derogatory manner. Later observers perform
the task, during which they receive a

predetermined range of scores and the

performances for which they reward themselves

are recorded. Within this general paradigm the


independent and interactive effects of a variety of

theoretically relevant variables have been studied


including, among others, prior reinforcement

history for achievement behavior and degree of

difference in ability from comparison models


(Bandura & Whalen, 1966); presence of conflicting

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 115
modeling cues (Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1967;

McMains & Liebert, 1968), rewarding qualities of

the model and social reinforcement of the model’s


standard-setting behavior (Bandura, Grusec, &

Menlove, 1967); whether material self-reward is


accompanied by verbal self-evaluation (Liebert &
Allen, 1967); and the generosity with which

symbolic rewards are self- administered (Marston,

1965a).

The results of these studies show that people

generally adopt the standards for self-

reinforcement exhibited by exemplary models,

they evaluate their own performances relative to


that standard, and then they serve as their own
reinforcing agents. For instance, those who have

been exposed to models setting low standards

tend to be highly self-rewarding and self-

approving for comparatively mediocre

performances. By contrast, persons who have

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 116
observed models adhere to stringent performance

demands display considerable self-denial and self-

dissatisfaction for objectively identical


accomplishments. These findings illustrate how

self-esteem, self-concept and related self-

evaluative processes can be conceptualized within

a social-learning framework. From this

perspective, a negative self-concept is defined in


terms of a high frequency of negative self-
reinforcement and conversely, a favorable self-

concept is reflected in a relatively high incidence

of positive self-reinforcement (Marston, 1965b).

Although specific patterns of self-reinforcing


responses can be acquired observationally

without the mediation of direct external


reinforcement, undoubtedly the valuation of

performances that fall short of, match, or exceed a

reference norm results partly from past

differential reinforcements. Thus, for example,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 117
parents who expect their children to exceed the

average performance of their group in whatever

tasks they undertake will selectively reward


superior achievements and punish or nonreward

average and lower level attainments. Differential

achievement levels thus assume positive and

negative valence and the performance standard

common to the various activities is eventually


abstracted and applied to new endeavors. That is,
a person for whom average performances have

been repeatedly devalued will come to regard

modal achievements on new tasks as inadequate

and attainments that surpass modal levels as


commendable. Once the evaluative properties of

differential accomplishments are well established,


adequate or inadequate matches are likely to elicit

similar self-reinforcing responses irrespective of

the specific performances being compared. At this


stage the whole process becomes relatively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 118
independent of external reinforcement and the

specific contingencies of the original training

situations, but it remains dependent upon


cognitive evaluations based on the match between

self-prescribed standards, performance, and the


attainments of reference models. Social
comparison processes become involved because in

the case of most performances objective criteria of

adequacy are lacking; hence the attainments of


other persons must be utilized as the norm against

which meaningful self-evaluation can be made.

Under naturally occurring conditions modeling

and reinforcement practices often operate


concurrently in ways that either supplement or
counteract each other. Findings of research in

which both of these sources of influence are varied

simultaneously (McMains & Liebert, 1968; Mischel

& Liebert, 1966; Rosenhan, Frederick, & Burrowes,

1968) show that self-rewards are most sparingly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 119
administered when stringent performance

standards have been consistently modeled and

imposed, whereas social- learning conditions in


which persons both model and reinforce lenient

behavioral demands produce generous self-

reward patterns of behavior. In everyday life

people frequently model the very behavior they

decry in others. Discrepant practices in which


models prescribe stringent standards for others
but impose lenient ones upon themselves, or

impose austere demands on themselves and

lenient ones on others, reduce the likelihood that

high norms will be internalized.

Of particular relevance to self-regulatory

processes is evidence that self-monitored


reinforcement can, in fact, maintain behavior. To

test the relative efficacy of self-monitored and

externally imposed systems of reinforcement,

Bandura & Perloff (1967) conducted an

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 120
experiment in the following manner: Children

worked at a task in which they could achieve

progressively higher scores by performing


increasingly more effortful responses. Children in

the self-reinforcement condition selected their

own achievement standards and rewarded

themselves whenever they attained their self-

prescribed norms. Children assigned to an


externally imposed reinforcement condition were
matched with the self-reward group so that the

same performance standard was set for them and

the reinforcers were automatically delivered

whenever they reached the predetermined level.


To ascertain whether subjects’ behavioral

productivity was due to the operation of


contingent reinforcement or to gratitude for the

rewards that were made available, children in an

incentive-control group performed the task after


they had received the supply of rewards on a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 121
noncontingent basis. A fourth group worked

without any incentives to estimate the amount of

behavior produced by the properties of the task


itself. Because the capacity to maintain effortful

behavior over time is the most important attribute


of a reinforcement operation, the dependent
measure was the number of responses the

children performed until they no longer wished to

continue the activity.

As shown graphically in Figure 1-2, both self-

monitored and externally imposed reinforcement

systems sustained substantially more behavior

than did either the noncontingent reward or the


nonreward condition, which did not differ from
each other. Of even greater interest is the

prevalence with which children in the self-

monitored condition imposed upon themselves

highly unfavorable schedules of reinforcement.

Not a single child chose the lowest score which

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 122
Figure 1-2. Behavioral productivity of children under
conditions in which their responses were self-
reinforced or externally reinforced, or in which they
were rewarded noncontingently or not at all. Bandura
& Perloff, 1967.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 123
required the least effort, while approximately half

of them selected the highest achievement level as

the performance meriting self-reward. Moreover,


a third of the children subsequently altered their

initial standard to a higher level, without a

commensurate increase in amount of self-reward,

thereby imposing upon themselves a more

unfavorable ratio of work to reinforcement. This


behavior is all the more striking because the self-
imposition of stringent performance demands

occurred in the absence of any social surveillance

and under high permissiveness for self-reward.

It can be reasonably assumed that most older


children have acquired standards of achievement

through modeling and differential reinforcement


and have undergone experiences in which

rewarding oneself for performances judged to be

unworthy has been socially disapproved. Hence,

under conditions where persons are provided

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 124
with ample opportunities to optimize their

material outcomes by engaging in behavior which

has low self-regard value, strongly conflicting


tendencies are likely to be aroused. On the one

hand, individuals are tempted to maximize

rewards at minimum costs of effort to themselves,

but on the other hand, low quality performances

produce negative self-evaluative consequences


which, if sufficiently strong, may inhibit
undeserving self-compensation. Indeed, many of

the children in the experiment set themselves

performance requirements that incurred high

effort costs at minimum material recompense.


These findings are at variance with what one

might expect on the basis of reward-cost theories,


unless these formulations include the self-esteem

costs of rewarding devalued behavior.

After a self-monitored reinforcement system

has been well established, a given performance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 125
produces two sets of consequences—a self-

evaluative reaction as well as some external

outcome. In many instances self-generated and


externally occurring consequences may conflict, as

when certain courses of action are approved and

encouraged by others, but if carried out would

give rise to self-critical and negative self-

evaluative reactions. Under these circumstances,


the effects of self-reinforcement may prevail over
external influences. Conversely, response patterns

may be effectively maintained by self-

reinforcement operations under conditions of

minimal external support. It is perhaps because of


the stabilizing effects of self-reinforcement that

persons do not ordinarily behave like


weathervanes in the face of conflicting

contingencies of reinforcement which they

repeatedly encounter in their social environment.


The fact that self-reinforcement may substitute

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 126
for, supplement, or override the effects of

externally occurring outcomes (Kanfer, 1968)

complicates interpretation of behavioral changes


supposedly due to external reinforcement.

Discussions of psychopathology generally


emphasize deficit conditions, response inhibitions,

and avoidance mechanisms. However, personal

problems frequently result from dysfunctions in


self-reinforcement systems. Many of the people
who seek treatment are neither incompetent nor

anxiously inhibited, but they experience a great

deal of personal distress stemming from

excessively high standards for self-evaluation,


often supported by unfavorable comparisons with
models noted for their extraordinary

achievements. This process typically gives rise to

depressive reactions, to feelings of worthlessness

and lack of purposefulness, and to lessened

disposition to perform because of negative self-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 127
generated consequences. In its more extreme

forms this problem is reflected in behaviors

designed to escape self-generated anguish through


alcoholism, grandiose ideation, unwillingness to

engage in activities that may have important self-

evaluative implications, and other forms of

avoidance behavior. The modification of self-

reinforcement patterns constitutes a principal


psychotherapeutic objective in conditions
involving burdensome self-demands.

Social behavior is usually regulated to some

extent by covert self-reinforcing operations which


rely upon symbolically generated consequences in
the form of self-commendation, esteem-enhancing

reactions, or self- deprecation. Persons who have


failed to develop self-monitoring reinforcement

systems, or those who make self-reward

contingent upon skillful performance of antisocial

behavior, require considerable social surveillance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 128
to ensure that they do not transgress. Similarly,

individuals who set lax behavioral standards for

themselves are inclined to display low


achievement behavior and a liberal self-

gratification pattern of life.

Symbolic Regulation of Behavior

Some psychological theories, while

acknowledging that stimulus-response


covariations are mediated by covert events,
nevertheless adhere rigorously to causal

explanations of behavior couched almost


exclusively in terms of external manipulable

variables. The pursuit of external causes rests on

the basic assumption that covert processes are

lawfully determined by externally occurring


events and, therefore, they can be bypassed in the
prediction and control of behavior. This view has

been advocated most forcefully by Skinner (1953):

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 129
“The objection to inner states is not that they do

not exist, but that they are not relevant in a

functional analysis. We cannot account for the


behavior of any system while staying wholly

inside it; eventually we must turn to forces

operating upon the organism from without [p.

35].”

The common practice of invoking spurious


inner states or agents as determiners of behavior
has also produced justifiable wariness of

inferential variables. After a given response

pattern has been attributed to the action of a


psychic homunculus, the search for controlling
conditions promptly ceases. Although the use of

the more colorful animistic entities in explanatory


schemes is declining, the tendency to offer new

descriptive labels for behavioral phenomena in the

guise of explanations remains a flourishing

practice.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 130
The relative neglect of experiential phenomena

results primarily from their limited accessibility.

Thought processes are directly accessible only to


the person within whom they occur and therefore

their presence, absence, and exact nature cannot


be independently verified. As a consequence, one
is forced to rely upon verbal self-reports and other

indirect indices of events occurring at a private

level. In discussions of the methodological


problems and theoretical issues regarding

symbolic processes it is customary to belabor the

limitations and inaccuracies of self-reports. It is


emphasized that, due to defective self-descriptive

facility and various distorting influences, public


and private events may be imperfectly correlated.

Not only are private events difficult to identify, but

since they cannot be directly manipulated they


have limited value in the causal analysis or

practical control of behavior. These dissuading

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 131
arguments, however, never cite the innumerable

studies demonstrating that, under many

conditions, self-described covert events have


much greater predictive power and regulatory

influence over behavior than the externally


manipulated variables typically assigned the
central explanatory role in change processes.

There exists ample evidence that one cannot


account satisfactorily for human behavior while
remaining entirely outside the organism, because

overt behavior is often governed by self-generated

stimulation that is relatively independent of

environmental stimulus events. For purposes of


illustration, let us consider an experiment
conducted by Miller (1951) to demonstrate how

emotional responding can be brought under

thought control. Students were asked to

pronounce aloud the symbols of T and 4 as they

were presented in a random sequence. The

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 132
utterance T was consistently followed by shock

stimulation, whereas the 4 was never shocked.

After the discrimination had been established,


subjects were presented with a series of dots and

instructed to think T to the first dot, 4 to the

second one, and so on in an alternating sequence.

Subjects displayed a highly discriminative pattern

of autonomic responses with thoughts of T


eliciting large autonomic responses and thoughts
of 4 producing virtually no reaction. This

discriminative responding cannot be accounted

for in terms of the properties of the external dot

stimuli, which are identical and merely signal the


occasions for self-generated cognitive activities

that produce emotional responsiveness. In fact,


the trivial function of external stimuli could be

entirely eliminated simply by instructing subjects

to generate the aversive and neutral thoughts in


an unpredictable sequence and to signal by a key

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 133
press whichever cognitive event they were about

to produce. Knowledge of the subject’s pattern of

self-generated thoughts would permit accurate


prediction of his autonomic responses. In

naturalistic situations a brief external stimulus


often initiates a long chain of cognitive activities
that is largely determined by mediational

associative linkages rather than by the temporally

remote environmental input.

Under conditions where thought processes

essentially serve as the first link in causal

sequences, one can predict behavior most

accurately on the basis of subject-defined internal


stimulation. Until instruments that can
discriminate subtle differences between symbolic

events are developed, a comprehensive approach

to the understanding of human behavior will have

to rely upon an individual both as the agent and

the object of study. Most current experimentation

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 134
simply avoids the issues of internal stimulus

control by confining research to behavioral

phenomena that can be brought under the


influence of physical properties of external stimuli.

In a paper devoted to the control of implicit

events Homme (1965) indicates that the problems

of covert response definition and detection have

been needlessly exaggerated. He rightfully


contends that under most conditions the presence
or absence of covert activities can be easily

detected by the person in whom they are

occurring. As will be shown in the concluding


chapter, persons can not only reliably discriminate
internal events, but they can manipulate them by

making self-reinforcement contingent upon their


occurrence. Furthermore, thought-induced

affective reactions may be successfully employed

for purposes of controlling one’s own overt

behavior. In the above instances implicit activities

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 135
constitute either important phenomena in their

own right or causal antecedents rather than mere

internal accompaniments of behavioral and


environmental events.

There are innumerable psychological

processes in which internal mediating events must

occur before external stimuli will exercise control

over overt performances. Verbal mediators, in the


form of self-instructions, implicit categorizing
responses, or linkages through common word

associates, are perhaps the most prevalent

symbolic regulators of behavior. Persons must


often rely on verbal self-control when external
stimuli for correct responses are absent (Bern,

1967; Luria, 1961). Also, in many forms of


conceptual behavior or in semantic generalization

persons display a common response to highly

dissimilar stimuli (e.g., artichokes, strawberries,

lobsters, onion soup, leg of lamb, rye bread, wine,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 136
and chocolate soufflé). Performance under these

conditions is governed by a mediating rule or a

common verbally labeled attribute (healthful


edibles) rather than by the physical characteristics

of the external stimuli alone (Bourne, 1966). In a

nonmediational account of conceptual behavior,

Ferster (1968) equates conceptualization with

abstract stimulus control whereby, through


selective reinforcement, a common property of
diverse complex stimuli comes to control the

response. The view is advanced that “the term

abstract stimulus control is somewhat preferable

to concept formation because it emphasizes the


controlling properties of the stimulus rather than

an inner and unreachable process [p. 404].” The


limitations of this type of approach become readily

apparent in cases, such as the one cited above,

where different stimuli have no physical property


in common but must be categorized on the basis of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 137
a symbolically labeled attribute.

In most higher-level functioning, the implicit


rules regulating behavior cannot be defined solely

in terms of stimulus properties or combinations of

stimulus elements. In an experiment conducted by


Sassenrath (1962), for example, students were

presented with a series of words of various

lengths, to which they were required to respond


with correct numbers that could be consistently
produced only by recourse to a complicated but

unspecified code. The principle upon which

reinforcement was administered consisted of 11

minus the number of letters in the stimulus word,


so that correct responding had to be determined
by symbolic transformations of external stimuli.

Subjects eventually made accurate symbolic

transformations, which then became inner stimuli

for accurate responsiveness. The process of self-

reinforcement, in which persons self-administer

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 138
rewarding or punishing consequences on the basis

of implicit standards of conduct, is another

phenomenon involving internal rule-regulated


behavior.

Behavior may also be governed to some extent

by imagined mediators which represent previously

observed behavioral events and environmental

situations. It is exceedingly difficult to think about


the actions of people in given settings or features
of one’s physical environment without

experiencing corresponding visual imagery. The

highly influential role of symbolic processes in


behavioral change is most evident in vicarious or
observational learning (Bandura, 1965). The

paradigm utilized to study this phenomenon


involves a nonresponse acquisition procedure in

which a person merely observes a model’s

behavior but otherwise exhibits no overt

instrumental responses; nor is any reinforcing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 139
stimuli administered during the acquisition

period. Exposure to modeling influences is an

exceedingly effective means of transmitting and


modifying conceptual and social behavior. Since in

this mode of response acquisition observers can

acquire only perceptual and other implicit

responses resembling the modeled patterns while

they are occurring, imaginal and verbal mediators


that govern subsequent response retrieval and
reproduction clearly play a prominent role in

observational learning.

There is a growing body of evidence (Bower,


1969; Paivio, 1969) that imaginal processes serve
a mediating function in facilitating verbal

associative learning. In these studies, imaginal


mediators are manipulated experimentally by

instructing subjects to link the members of each

pair of stimulus and response terms with a

distinctive image, and by using stimulus items that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 140
vary in their capacity to evoke vivid imagery. The

findings demonstrate that during paired

presentations subjects code stimulus and response


events into mental images for memory

representation; later, the stimuli serve as cues that

reinstate the compound image from which the

response component is decoded to its original

verbal form. Imaginally mediated associative


learning is far superior to that in which this type of
representational process is minimally operative.

Some evidence exists to suggest that arousal

mediators may also exercise a regulatory function


over emotional behavior. According to the dual
process theory of avoidance behavior, stimuli

acquire, through their temporal conjunction with


aversive experiences, the capacity to produce

arousal reactions which have both central and

autonomic components. It is further assumed that

instrumental avoidance responses become partly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 141
conditioned to arousal-correlated stimuli. The

most direct evidence that arousal mediators

operating primarily at the central level exercise


discriminative control over avoidance behavior is

provided by Solomon & Turner (1962). Animals

first learned to make an avoidance response to a

light stimulus. They were then skeletally

immobilized by curare to prevent avoidance


responses from being conditioned directly to
external stimuli; shock was paired with one tone,

while a contrasting tone was never associated

with aversive stimulation. In subsequent tests the

animals displayed essentially the same degree of


avoidance in response to the negatively valenced

tone and the light, both of which evoked common


arousal reactions, whereas avoidance responses

rarely occurred to the neutral tone. Considering

that the light and the tones were never associated,


and assuming that the curare blocked all skeletal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 142
activity (Black, 1967), thus precluding any

differential conditioning of avoidance responses to

the tones, the controlling power of the negatively


valenced auditory stimulus must be mediated

through either events in the central nervous


system or autonomic feedback mechanisms.

Further demonstrations of internal regulation

of behavior are furnished by studies (Bailey, 1955;


Bailey & Porter, 1955; Levine, 1953), in which
infrahuman subjects must learn to respond

differentially on the basis of internal stimulation

associated with different drive states like thirst or

hunger because the environment contains no


distinguishable guiding cues. Under these
conditions the differential cues provided by

internal drive states, or even different intensities

of the same drive, give rise to dissimilar patterns

of behavior. These findings are consistent with

those cited earlier in which internal stimuli are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 143
endowed with controlling properties through

interoceptive conditioning.

The powerful internal control of behavior is


most vividly illustrated in grossly deviant

behavior for which the controlling contingencies

are almost entirely symbolically generated. The

passage quoted below (Bateson, 1961), was taken

from a patient’s account of his psychosis long


before it was fashionable to write about one’s
psychiatric experiences. The narrator had received

a scrupulously moralistic upbringing according to

which even most socially approved patterns of


behavior were considered deviant, sinful, and
likely to provoke the wrath of God; consequently

many innocuous acts, such as accepting


medication, elicited dreadful apprehensions,

which, in turn, motivated and maintained

exceedingly painful atonement rituals designed to

forestall the imagined disastrous consequences.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 144
In the night I awoke under the most dreadful
impressions, I heard a voice addressing me,
and I was made to imagine that my
disobedience to the faith, in taking the
medicine overnight, had not only offended
the Lord, but had rendered the work of my
salvation extremely difficult, by its effect
upon my spirits and humours. I heard that I
could only be saved now by being changed
into a spiritual body…A spirit came upon me
and prepared to guide me in my actions. I
was lying on my back, and the spirit seemed
to light on my pillow by my right ear, and to
command my body. I was placed in a
fatiguing attitude, resting on my feet, my
knees drawn up and on my head, and made
to swing my body from side to side without
ceasing. In the meantime, I heard voices
without and within me, and sounds as of the
clanking of iron, and the breathing of great
forge bellows, and the force of flames. I
understood that I was only saved by the
mercy of Jesus, from seeing, as well as
hearing, hell around me, and that if I were
not obedient to His spirit, I should inevitably
awake in hell before the morning. After some
time I had a little rest, and then, actuated by
the same spirit, I took a like position on the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 145
floor, where I remained, until I understood
that the work of the Lord was perfected, and
that now my salvation was secured; at the
same time the guidance of the spirit left me,
and I became in doubt what next I was to do.
I understood that this provoked the Lord, as
if I was affecting ignorance when I knew
what I was to do, and, after some hesitation, I
heard the command, to “Take your position
on the floor again then,” but I had no
guidance or no perfect guidance to do so, and
could not resume it. I was told, however, that
my salvation depended upon my maintaining
that position as well as I could until the
morning; and oh! great was my joy when I
perceived the first brightness of the dawn,
which I could scarcely believe had arrived so
early [pp. 28-29],

The above quotation provides a clear example

of how behavior can come under the complete


control of fictional contingencies and fantasied

reinforcements powerful enough to override the

influence of the reinforcement contingencies

existing within the social environment. Thus the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 146
acceptance of medicine, an act that was later

considered a rebellion against, and the mistrust of,

the Almighty, generated extremely aversive


hallucinations of hellish torture, the cessation of

which was contingent upon the performance of

arduous bizarre behavior. The nonoccurrence of

subjectively experienced, but objectively

nonexistent threats, undoubtedly serves as an


important mechanism maintaining many other
types of psychotic behavior. Given the conjunction

of fictional contingencies and a powerful internal

reinforcing system, a person’s behavior is likely to

remain under very poor environmental control


even in the face of severe externally administered

punishments and blatant disconfirming


experiences.

When I opened the door, I found a stout man


servant on the landing, who told me that he
was placed there to forbid my going out, by
the orders of Dr. P. and my friend; on my

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 147
remonstrating, he followed me into my room
and stood before the door. I insisted on going
out; he, on preventing me. I warned him of
the danger he incurred in opposing the will
of the Holy Spirit, I prayed him to let me
pass, or otherwise an evil would befall him,
for that I was a prophet of the Lord. He was
not a whit shaken by my address, so, after
again and again adjuring him, by the desire
of the Spirit whose word I heard, I seized one
of his arms, desiring to wither it; my words
were idle, no effect followed, and I was
ashamed and astonished.

Then, thought I, I have been made a fool of!


But I did not on that account mistrust the
doctrines by which I had been exposed to
this error. The doctrines, thought I, are true;
but I am mocked at by the Almighty for my
disobedience to them, and at the same time, I
have the guilt and the grief, of bringing
discredit upon the truth, by my obedience to
a spirit of mockery, or, by my disobedience
to the Holy Spirit; for there were not wanting
voices to suggest to me, that the reason why
the miracle had failed, was, that I had not
waited for the Spirit to guide my action when
the word was spoken and that I had seized

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 148
the man’s arm with the wrong hand…[p. 33],

The voices informed me, that my conduct


was owing to a spirit of mockery and
blasphemy having possession of me…that I
must, in the power of the Holy Spirit, redeem
myself, and rid myself of the spirits of
blasphemy and mockery that had taken
possession of me.

The way in which I was tempted to do this


was by throwing myself on the top of my
head backwards, and so resting on the top of
my head and on my feet alone, to turn from
one side to the other until I had broken my
neck. I suppose by this time I was already in
a state of feverish delirium, but my good
sense and prudence still refused to
undertake this strange action. I was then
accused of faithlessness and cowardice, of
fearing man more than God.

I attempted the command, the servant


prevented me. I lay down contented to have
proved myself willing to obey in spite of his
presence, but now I was accused of not
daring to wrestle with him unto blows. I
again attempted what I was enjoined. The
man seized me, I tore myself from him,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 149
telling him it was necessary for my salvation;
he left me and went down stairs. I then tried
to perform what I had begun; but now I
found, either that I could not so jerk myself
round on my head, or that my fear of
breaking my neck was really too strong for
my faith. In that case I then certainly
mocked, for my efforts were not sincere.

Failing in my attempts, I was directed to


expectorate violently, in order to get rid of
my two formidable enemies; and then again I
was told to drink water, and the Almighty
was satisfied; but that I was not satisfied
(neither could I be sincerely, for I knew that I
had not fulfilled his commands), I was to
take up my position again; I did so, my
attendant came up with an assistant and
they forced me into a straight waistcoat.
Even then I again tried to resume the
position to which I was again challenged.
They then tied my legs to the bed-posts, and
so secured me [pp. 34-35].

The process of behavioral change will be

conceptualized quite differently depending upon


whether one assumes that responses are regulated

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 150
predominantly by external stimulus events or
partly by mediating symbolic events. In

nonmediational interpretations, learning is


depicted as a more or less automatic process

wherein stimuli become associated with overt

responses through differential reinforcement. By

contrast, in mediational formulations the learner


plays a far more active role and his responsiveness

is subject to extensive cognitive determination. On

the basis of salience of environmental events and

past learning experiences persons select the

stimuli to which they will respond; environmental


events are coded and organized for representation

in memory; provisional hypotheses regarding the


principles governing the occurrence of

reinforcement are derived from differential


consequences accompanying overt behavior; and

after a given implicit hypothesis has been


adequately confirmed by successful corresponding

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 151
actions, the mediating rules or principles serve to

guide the performance of appropriate responses

on future occasions. Relevant empirical evidence


bearing on these two theoretical approaches will

be reviewed in the concluding chapter of this


book.

It has been customary in psychological

theorizing to construct entire explanatory


schemes around a single form of behavioral
control, to the relative neglect of other obviously

influential variables and processes. Thus, for

example, some psychologists have tended to

concentrate upon stimulus control effected


principally through classical-conditioning
operations; Skinnerians have primarily focused

upon external reinforcement control of behavior;

and researchers favoring cognitive interpretations

have been most preoccupied with mediational

processes. These resolute allegiances to partial

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 152
processes are typically accompanied by some

disdain for variables patronized by out-group

theorists. A comprehensive theory of human


behavior must encompass all three sources of

behavioral regulation, i.e., stimulus control,

internal symbolic control, and outcome control. In

many situations, of course, two or more of these

processes may operate simultaneously in


governing responsiveness.

Social Learning as a Reciprocal Influence


Process

Psychodynamic theories of personality

typically depict the deviant actions of individuals


as being impelled by powerful internal forces that
they not only are unable to control, but whose

existence they do not even recognize. On the other


hand, behavioral formulations often characterize

response patterns as depending on environmental

contingencies. The environment is presented as a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 153
more or less fixed property that impinges upon

individuals and to which their behavior eventually

adapts. Neither view of man is particularly


heartening nor entirely accurate.

Psychological functioning, in fact, involves a

continuous reciprocal interaction between

behavior and its controlling conditions. Although

actions are regulated by their consequences, the


controlling environment is, in turn, often
significantly altered by the behavior. Examples of

the way in which behavior shapes the

environment can be found even in simple


experiments with infrahuman subjects. As a means
of studying the acquisition of avoidance responses,

Sidman (1960, 1966) devised a paradigm in which


animals could postpone the occurrence of aversive

shocks by depressing a lever. Under these

conditions some animals created for themselves

an essentially punishment-free environment,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 154
whereas others who, for one reason or another,

were slow in acquiring the requisite coping

response produced a highly aversive milieu. When


response changes are selected as the data for

analysis, as is almost invariably the case, then the

environmental contingencies appear to be fixed,

controlling conditions; if, instead, one analyzed the

data for the amount of aversive stimulation


created by each subject, then the environment
becomes the change worthy event that may vary

considerably for different subjects and at different

times for the same subject. Within the framework

of environmental analysis, one might, for instance,


administer alcohol to one group of subjects in the

Sidman paradigm and water to another, and then


compare the types of aversive environments

produced under intoxicated and sober conditions.

Interpersonal situations, of course, provide

much greater latitude for determining the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 155
contingencies that maintain one’s behavior. In

social interchanges the behavior of one person

exerts some degree of control over the actions of


others. To take an example, counterreactions

drawn by hostile responses are likely to be quite

different from those elicited by friendly ones.

Experimental analysis by Rausch (1965) of

sequential interchanges between children, in fact,


reveals that the immediately preceding stimulus
act on the part of one person was the major

determinant of the other person’s response. In

approximately 75 percent of the instances, hostile

behavior elicited unfriendly responses, whereas


cordial antecedent acts seldom did. Aggressive

children thus created through their actions a


hostile environment, whereas children who

displayed friendly interpersonal modes of

response generated an amicable social milieu.


These findings demonstrate that persons, far from

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 156
being ruled by an imposing environment, play an

active role in constructing their own

reinforcement contingencies through their


characteristic modes of response. The theory of

social interaction advanced by Thibaut & Kelley


(1959) relies heavily upon mutual reinforcement
contingencies. Research stimulated by this

conceptualization provides numerous

demonstrations of how outcomes in dyadic


interchanges are jointly determined by the

behavior of both participants.

It might be argued that if each person partly

creates his own environment then there is no one


remaining to be influenced. This apparent paradox
overlooks the fact that reciprocity is rarely perfect,

since one’s behavior is not the sole determinant of

subsequent events. Furthermore, controlling and

controllable events usually occur in an alternating

pattern rather than concurrently until the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 157
interaction sequence is terminated. The reciprocal

reinforcement process involved in the unwitting

production and strengthening of tantrum behavior


in children will serve to illustrate the latter point.

On most occasions children’s mild requests go

unheeded because the parent is preoccupied with

other activities. If subsequent bids also go

unrewarded the child will generally display


progressively more intense forms of behavior
which become increasingly aversive to the parent.

At this point in the interaction sequence the child

is exercising aversive control over the parent.

Eventually the parent is forced to terminate the


troublesome behavior by attending to the child,

thereby reinforcing obstreperous responsiveness.


Such differential reinforcement practices are

highly effective in producing aversive forms of

behavior of unusual resiliency. Some of the most


vivid examples of pernicious reciprocal control are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 158
provided in Levy’s (1943) classic study of

childhood overdependency:

Patient (4 years, 9 months) rules the


household by his screaming and imperative
voice. Mother will always comply with his
demands rather than hear him scream.…The
patient is disobedient, hyperactive,
impudent to the parents; calls them names,
kicks and scratches when not given his own
way… [pp. 361-363].

In complete command, dominating mother


and sister, who yielded in every instance
rather than endure his scenes, a fourteen-
year-old refused to go to school. He lay in
bed, ordered his sister to get his breakfast,
bring his clothes, and struck her when she
disobeyed [p. 163],…

Mother states that he (10-year-old) was


spoiled by herself and maternal
grandmother, and later she gave in to his
demands for the sake of peace…Whenever
refused, he always commanded obedience by
screaming [pp. 383-384]…After screaming
no longer availed, he used the method of
nagging, monotonously repeating his

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 159
demands [p. 163],

The above case material illustrates how certain

reinforcement practices generate particular

behavior, which, due to its aversive properties, in


turn creates the very conditions likely to

perpetuate it. Thus while nature’s programming

ensured that people’s distress would not go


unheeded for long, it also provided the basis for

the establishment of socially disturbing response

patterns. Interpersonal difficulties are most likely


to arise under conditions where a person has
developed a narrow range of social responses

which periodically force reinforcing actions from


others through aversive control (e.g., nagging

complaints, aggressive behavior, helplessness,

sick-role behavior, and emotional expressions of


rejection, suffering, and distress, and other modes
of responding that command attention). The

treatment strategies are quite different depending

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 160
upon whether one views such behavior in terms of

its functional value in controlling the

responsiveness of others or as by-products of


intrapsychic disturbances. Deleterious reciprocal

processes can be best eliminated by withdrawing

the reinforcement supporting the deviant

behavior and by hastening the development of

more constructive means of securing desired


reactions from others.

It is only because there is some degree of self-

determination of outcomes that treatment of an

individual is justifiable. To the extent that newly


established patterns of behavior create favorable
reciprocally reinforcing processes, they will be

effectively sustained over time. However, in


instances where one person’s behavior exerts little

or no control over the actions of others, perhaps

from disparities in status or power, it may become

necessary to effect changes in other people

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 161
important to him, or in the social system itself.

Symptom Substitution

It is generally assumed by therapists who


subscribe to psychodynamic models that direct

modification of deviant behavior is likely to result

in “symptom substitution.” This issue, like others


pertaining to the development and treatment of

behavioral dysfunctions, has become hopelessly

muddled by the use of an inappropriate

conceptual scheme which thoroughly obscures the


very phenomena it is designed to elucidate It is

further obfuscated by partisan claims that no such


phenomena exist (Yates, 1958), and counterclaims
that symptom substitution not only occurs, but

that the commuted forms may endanger the very

life of ill-fated clients (Bookbinder, 1962).

Relevant outcome data cited later lead one to

suspect that prognostications of dire

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 162
consequences are intended more to dissuade

therapeutic innovation than to protect clients

welfare. Indeed, as Grossberg (1964) has noted,


much more serious from a humanitarian

standpoint is the failure of “depth”

psychotherapies to effect significant changes in

behavioral conditions that produce chronic

suffering and disheartening social and vocational


incapacitation.

The dispute about symptom substitution does

involve an important psychological phenomenon,

but little headway will be made in resolving this


issue as long as it is misconstrued as one of
symptomatic versus non- symptomatic treatment,

or modification of causal versus behavioral events.


Even if the concepts of symptom and mental

disease were pertinent to behavioral dysfunctions,

which they are not, the symptom substitution

hypothesis could never be satisfactorily tested

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 163
because it fails to specify precisely what

constitutes a “symptom,” when the substitution

should occur, the social conditions under which it


is most likely to arise, and the form that the

substitute symptom will take. If consensus could

ever be attained in devising an exhaustive list of

possible symptomatic behaviors, one would be

forced, in order to prove definitively that symptom


substitution does not occur, to conduct thorough
and repeated assessments of clients’ behavior for

an indefinite period. This exhaustive toil would

still be all for naught, since there exist no reliable

criteria for determining whether the occurrence of


so-called symptomatic behaviors after completion

of treatment represents emergent substitute by-


products of a psychic pathology, the development

of new modes of maladaptive response to

environmental pressures, or the persistence of old


modes of maladaptive behavior which had gone

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 164
unnoticed until even worse behavior was

eliminated.

The symptom substitution issue would never

have been cast in its present misleading form had

it been recognized that one cannot eliminate


behavior as such, except perhaps through direct

removal of requisite neurophysiological systems.

Response patterns can be modified only by


altering the stimulus conditions that regulate their
occurrence. Hence, all forms of psychotherapy,

regardless of their self-conferred honorific titles

and virtuous aims, effect behavioral changes

through either deliberate or unwitting


manipulation of controlling variables.

Psychodynamic and social-learning

approaches to psychotherapy are, therefore,

equally concerned with modifying the

“underlying” determinants of deviant response

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 165
patterns; however, these theories differ, often
radically, in what they regard these “causes” to be,

a crucial difference which in turn influences the


types of stimulus conditions favored in the

respective treatments. To take a simple but telling

example, in an effort to gain a better

understanding of some of the factors governing


deviant behavior likely to be labeled

“symptomatic,” Ayllon, Haughton, & Hughes

(1965) induced and sustained for a time a bizarre

broom-carrying response in an adult

schizophrenic by periodic positive reinforcement


of the behavior. A psychotherapist, who was

unaware of the conditions which had established


and maintained this response pattern, invoked the

following underlying causes:

Her constant and compulsive pacing, holding


a broom in the manner she does, could be
seen as a ritualistic procedure, a magical
action.…Her broom would be then: (1) a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 166
child that gives her love and she gives him in
return her devotion, (2) a phallic symbol, (3)
the sceptre of an omnipotent queen…this is a
magical procedure in which the patient
carries out her wishes, expressed in a way
that is far beyond our solid, rational and
conventional way of thinking and acting [p.
3],

In treating the persistent display of bizarre and

apparently purposeless behavior this therapist, on

the basis of his causal explanation, would subject


the woman to extended interpretive probing of
her sexual conflicts and delusions of omnipotence.

On the other hand, the behavioral therapist,

viewing the rewarding outcomes as the major

determinant of the so-called psychotic symptom,

would alter the reinforcement contingency


governing the behavior. Indeed, when the

occasional rewards for carrying a broom were


completely withdrawn the “symptom” promptly

vanished and, according to a two-year follow-up

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 167
study, never reappeared.

In light of the above considerations, it would

be both more accurate and advantageous to


redefine the causal versus symptomatic treatment

controversy as being primarily concerned with the

question of whether a particular form of therapy


chooses to modify conditions that, in actuality,
exercise strong or weak or no significant control

over the behavior in question.

According to the social-learning point of view,


in the course of social development a person

acquires different modes of coping with


environmental stresses and demands. These

various response strategies form a hierarchy


ordered by their probability of effecting favorable

outcomes in certain situations. A particular mode


of responding may occupy a dominant position in

various hierarchies; subordinate strategies may

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 168
differ from one situation to another and may vary
widely in their frequency of occurrence relative

both to the dominant response tendencies and


among themselves. Consequently the effects of

removing a dominant response pattern will

depend upon the number of different areas of

functioning in which it is characteristically


activated, and the nature and relative strength of

the initially weaker response dispositions.

One can distinguish several different types of

treatment approaches that are likely to produce

small, unpredictable, or unenduring changes in

deviant behavior suggestive of “symptom

substitution.” A treatment that fails to alter the


major controlling conditions of the deviant

behavior will most certainly prove ineffective.


Similarly, a poorly designed program of therapy

aimed solely at eliminating maladaptive behavior


patterns does not in itself guarantee that desired

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 169
modes of behavior will ensue. This is particularly

true when removal of deviant behavior is brought

about through withdrawal of its usual positive


consequences or by punishment or the imposition

of external restraints.

In extinction treatment, as dominant response

tendencies are eliminated through nonreward, the

person will revert to alternative courses of action,


which have proved of some value in the past. If
these initially weaker forms are nondeviant and

are adequately reinforced, then deviant patterns

are likely to be abandoned in favor of the


competing alternatives without the emergence of
any negative characteristics. If, on the other hand,

the subordinate set of responses in the client’s


repertoire is for the most part unsatisfactory, the

therapist will be faced with the task of eliminating

a long succession of ineffective patterns of

response.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 170
Response substitution is also likely to occur

when deviant behavior is eliminated not by

removal of its maintaining conditions but by


superimposing a competing set of controlling

variables (Bandura, 1962). Thus, for example,


antisocial behavior that serves as an effective
means of securing positive reinforcement may be

temporarily suppressed through severe

punishment. However, if the offender has learned


relatively few prosocial modes of behavior,

elimination of one deviant pattern will probably

be followed by another set of deviant responses


that are more successful in avoiding detection and

subsequent punishments. Moreover, the


suppressed behavior is likely to reappear in

situations where the probability of detection is

low, or the threat of punishment is weaker.

Successive substitution of deviant behavior

likewise readily arises under conditions where

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 171
defensive responses are either punished or

physically restrained without neutralizing the

aversive properties of subjectively threatening


situations. This process is well illustrated in

Miller’s (1948) classic study of avoidance

behavior. Animals were administered shocks in a

white compartment of a shuttle box and learned to

escape the painful stimulation by running through


an open door into a black compartment. The
formerly neutral white cues rapidly acquired

aversive properties, and the animals continued to

perform the avoidant running responses even

though the shock stimulation had been completely


discontinued. The animals were then placed in the

white compartment with the door closed to block


the running behavior. However, the door could be

released by rotating a wheel. Wheel-turning was

rapidly learned and maintained by fear reduction.


When conditions were further changed so that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 172
wheel-turning no longer released the door, but the

animal could escape from the threatening

compartment by pressing a bar, the former


response was quickly discarded while the latter

became strongly established. Thus interventions


that eliminated avoidance responses without
reducing the arousal potential of conditioned

aversive stimuli merely produced new forms of

defensive behavior.

The preceding discussion has focused on

approaches which, if used as the sole method of

treatment, may eliminate one form of deviant

behavior but lead to a different one. The problem


of deviant response substitution, however, can be
easily forestalled by including in the original

treatment program procedures that effectively

remove the reinforcing conditions which sustain

deviant behavior and concurrently foster

desirable alternative modes of behavior. Such

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 173
treatment strategies, which will be fully reviewed

in succeeding chapters, not only produce enduring

changes in the selected direction, but may also set


in motion beneficial changes in related areas of

psychological functioning.

Efficacy of Conventional Methods of


Behavioral Change

A casual survey of contemporary methods of

behavioral change would disclose a multiplicity of


“schools” of approaches, each claiming respectable

improvement rates for their particular clientele. A

closer examination of these treatment approaches,

however, reveals that the apparently multifarious


systems represent essentially a single procedure:
they all utilize a social relationship and place

heavy reliance upon verbal interpretive methods


for inducing changes in social behavior. Moreover,

only a small range of persons exhibiting

behavioral deviations are actually treated, with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 174
varying degrees of success, by interpretive

methods.

In the first place, most antisocial personalities,


who constitute a sizable proportion of the deviant

population, simply “serve time” in penal

institutions or remain under legal surveillance.

Since such persons generally prove unresponsive

to traditional techniques, many psychotherapists


have become pessimistic about the value of
psychotherapy for modifying “psychopathic” or

antisocially deviant behavior. In the case of

younger delinquents, correctional institutions,


though often providing a more structured and
nonpunitive environment than the children have

formerly experienced, rarely offer systematic


programs that are efficacious in producing

enduring behavioral and attitudinal changes.

Similarly, most persons exhibiting gross

behavioral dysfunctions, who also derive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 175
relatively little benefit from conventional

interview approaches, are provided mainly with

medication, “occupational therapy” in the form of


carrying out institutional routines, recreational

activities and custodial care in “mental”

institutions, where they become intermittent or

permanent residents. Indeed, the least socially

responsive psychotics are customarily assigned to


essentially custodial wards where they receive
only medication and where they mutually

extinguish one another’s limited social behaviors.

Nor have conventional methods of behavioral

change had much beneficial impact upon the


widespread problems of alcoholism, drug

addiction, and a host of other major social


problems which, in some instances, require

modification of social systems rather than the

behavior of isolated individuals.

Even in the restricted sample of persons who

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 176
consult psychotherapists and are accepted for

treatment, the dropout rates and the estimates of

behavioral change for those who remain in


treatment give little cause for complacency.

Between 30 and 60 percent of this highly selected

group (diagnosed predominantly as neurotic and

excluding grossly psychotic, alcoholic, antisocial,

and neurologically involved cases), terminate


treatment against the advice of their therapists
after several initial interviews (Frank, Gliedman,

Imber, Nash, & Stone, 1957; Garfield & Kurz, 1952;

Imber, Nash, & Stone, 1955; Kirtner & Cartwright,

1958; Knight, 1941; Kurland, 1956; Mensh &


Golden, 1951; Rickles, Klein, & Bassan, 1950). Of

those clients who continue in the therapy


programs, irrespective of the type of treatment

administered, approximately two-thirds are

usually rated as exhibiting some degree of


improvement (Appel, Lhamon, Myers, & Harvey,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 177
1951; Eysenck, 1952; Frank et al., 1957; Kirtner &

Cartwright, 1958; Zubin, 1953). Although the

above figures are based on studies of adults, there


is little reason to believe that the picture is very

different in the case of children (Levitt, 1963).

CRITERIA OF CHANGE

The two-thirds improved figure, which has


been widely and uncritically accepted as the
typical base rate of change accompanying

interview therapies, overestimates the amount of

benefit that people actually derive from such

treatment. The criteria upon which judgments of


therapeutic efficacy are usually based leave much

to be desired. In many instances psychotherapists’

global impressions of their results serve as the

major indicants of outcome. Considering that such

ratings reflect upon therapists’ professional


competence, it is reasonable to assume that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 178
therapists do not underrate the therapeutic value
of their methods.

Projective tests and personality questionnaires


have also been extensively employed as the

principal measures for evaluating psychotherapy.

Their widespread popularity is probably more


attributable to their availability and ease of
administration and scoring than to their direct

relevance to types of psychological changes that


clients hope to achieve by undergoing

psychotherapy. If the proverbial Martian were to

review the therapy outcome literature he would

undoubtedly conclude that earth men embark

upon expensive and time-consuming programs of


treatment to effect modifications in their

Rorschach, TAT, or MMPI responses, rather than


to overcome behavioral inhibitions, to resolve

chronic interpersonal problems, to gain control


over alcoholism, or otherwise to enhance their

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 179
level of social functioning. Since the behavioral

correlates of these personality test measures are

considerably in doubt (Mischel, 1968), evidence


that test responses have changed is of limited

value in judging the relative success of given

approaches to treatment. This is particularly true

in view of the fact that responses to personality

tests are readily amenable to response-set biases,


to implicit expectations inherent in the setting,
and to other extraneous influences.

A third course for the evaluation of

psychotherapeutic efficacy, in vogue for a long


time, focuses on changes in clients’ verbal
behavior in interview situations. Dedicated

researchers have devoted literally thousands of


arduous hours to counting the frequency of

clients’ self-reference statements, affective

verbalizations, resistive comments, self-

exploratory remarks, type-token ratios, and a host

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 180
of other verbal contents. Although this approach

yields readily quantifiable data that possess some

face validity, there is little evidence that changes


observed in clients’ verbal behavior influence

appreciably their daily interpersonal

responsiveness. These verbal indices are,

therefore, more pertinent to evaluating verbal

conditioning than fundamental behavioral change


processes.

Inasmuch as persons typically seek the help of

psychotherapists in order to modify faulty

interpersonal modes of responding and the


adverse consequences these engender, it is
remarkable that until recently behavioral changes

as a measure of success had not only been


seriously neglected, but often derogated as

superficial. Indeed, there exists no other avowedly

humanitarian enterprise in which clients’ major

concerns are so cavalierly disregarded. Whatever

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 181
personality changes a psychotherapist may choose

to promote, they should be considered of dubious

value if they are not reflected in the client’s social


behavior. To take an analogous example, medical

treatments that, on the basis of physicians’

impressions and other ambiguous indicants,

supposedly effected profound physiological

changes but, in actuality, produced no evident


changes in clients’ suffering and physical
dysfunctions, would be summarily dismissed as

both ineffectual and misleading. Clearly, objective

measures of changes in behavior constitute the

most stringent and the most important criteria of


the power of a given treatment method. Since the

areas of functioning that require modification may


differ extensively from person to person, global,

all-purpose measures of change must be replaced

by behavioral criteria that are specific and


individually tailored to the treatment objectives

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 182
selected by the client (Pascal & Zax, 1956).

Findings of comparative studies utilizing indices of

improvement based on behavioral change


(Fairweather, 1964; Lazarus, 1961; Paul, 1966)

yield success rates that are substantially below the


legendary two-thirds improved figure customarily
quoted for interview therapies.

Moreover, improvement figures usually


present a misleading picture of the effectiveness of
interview methods because dropouts have been

invariably excluded from statistical analyses.

When a particular procedure yields a relatively

high attrition rate, discarding terminators in


assessing psychotherapy becomes especially
critical. Let us assume, for instance, that of 100

persons who entered treatment, 80 withdrew

after several initial interviews, while all of the

remaining 20 cases exhibited significant

improvement. If terminators are ignored the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 183
treatment proves to be 100 percent effective

when, in fact, only 20 percent of the cases have

been benefited. It will be recalled that a sizable


percentage of clients who enter into interview

treatments terminate after a few visits.

IMPROVEMENT RATES FOR NONTREATED CASES

In order to demonstrate that psychotherapy is

a condition that contributes to observed outcomes,


it is necessary to compare changes exhibited by

clients who have undergone treatment with those

of a comparable group of nontreated cases. Such a


comparison group is essential in order to provide
an estimate of the influence of concomitant

extratherapeutic experiences that may contribute

importantly to demonstrable changes in clients’

behavior. Assuming that the two groups are

reasonably well matched on relevant variables,

any differential change between treated and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 184
nontreated cases can thus be regarded as
therapeutically induced. There are relatively few

studies of psychotherapeutic outcomes that meet


the minimum requirements of an adequate control

group and clear specification and objective

measurement of outcomes.

Bergin (1966) has reviewed findings of seven


studies (that met the minimal requirements of a

two-group design and some measures of change)


in which outcomes from a treated group and a

comparable nontreated group of clients were

compared. All seven studies, involving diverse

forms of therapy and diverse criteria, show that

persons who have undergone psychotherapy do


not differ significantly in average amount of

change from nontreated controls, but treatment


generally produces more variable effects. Whereas

controls do not change or improve to some extent,


those who have received treatment either remain

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 185
unchanged, benefit somewhat, attain considerable

improvement, or become worse. Lest these

variance differences temporarily revive interest in


weak behavioral change methods, it should be

noted that treatment-induced effects are less

favorable and, hence, less variable when more

stringent and socially meaningful measures are

employed. This is well illustrated by results of an


investigation conducted by Rogers (1967) and his
collaborators on the efficacy of client-centered

therapy.

Schizophrenics were administered a battery of


tests including the Rorschach, MMPI, Thematic
Apperception Test, Wechsler Intelligence Scale,

Anxiety Reaction Scales, Stroop Tests, F


Authoritarian Scale, Q-Sort, and Wittenborn

Psychiatric Rating Scales. One group of

schizophrenics participated in intensive client-

centered treatment with highly qualified

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 186
therapists, whereas matched controls received no

therapy. After completion of the treatment phase

the test battery was readministered and two


clinical psychologists made global judgments,

principally from the Rorschach and the MMPI, of

the degree of change in patients’ levels of

psychological functioning. Treated and nontreated

groups did not differ in mean improvement,


although some of the patients who received
treatment, unlike the controls, showed somewhat

larger gains while others displayed a change for

the worse. In an effort to account for this

variability, therapists’ behavior was rated from


tape-recorded samples of their interviews for

positive regard, empathy, and genuineness. Except


for scores on one scale of the MMPI test, patients

receiving high levels of the supposedly therapeutic

conditions did not differ significantly from


patients whose therapists displayed low positive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 187
responsiveness or from nontreated controls in

self-concepts, intellectual functioning, ratings of

their behavior on the hospital ward, and global


assessments based on various personality tests. It

would seem from the overall pattern of results


that a hospitalized patient has little to gain from
undergoing client- centered treatment and may, in

fact, suffer some slight losses if his therapist

happens to be lacking in amiability.

Faced with growing evidence that interview

therapies have limited efficacy, some researchers

concluded that outcome studies should be held in

abeyance while intensified efforts are made to


elucidate the process underlying these
procedures. Outcome studies were therefore

promptly downgraded, investigators became

absorbed in minute analyses of verbal

interchanges between therapists and their clients

and, in the absence of any promising alternatives,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 188
the traditional practices not only survived

essentially unaltered but were professionally

sanctified. The possibility that a conversational


approach to the modification of deviant behavior

is inherently too weak to justify exhaustive

process studies was rarely entertained. Under

conditions where a given treatment procedure

exercises weak behavioral control many other


extraneous variables (e.g., personality
characteristics of therapists, social attributes of

clients, minor technical variations in procedures)

singly or in combination will emerge as

determinants of change. Rather than pursue these


limiting factors, it would be far more profitable to

devise new methods that are sufficiently powerful


to override their influences. If similar errors in

research strategy are to be avoided in the

development of new treatment approaches it is


essential to establish the relative superiority of a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 189
particular approach before undertaking intricate

process studies that might elucidate underlying

mechanisms or suggest further procedural


refinements. It is also necessary to select stringent

and unambiguous criteria of change so as to


establish precisely what a given treatment method
can or cannot accomplish.

Multiprocesses Governing Behavioral Changes.


Evaluation of psychological procedures is often
unnecessarily obscured by the use of concepts

such as “cure,” “spontaneous remission,” and

“relapse,” which may be appropriate in describing

the course of physical disease processes but are


misleading when applied to behavioral changes
that are governed by social variables. In the latter

case, the pertinent issues of concern are whether a

given set of conditions can successfully induce a

change in behavior, whether the established

changes generalize to extratherapeutic situations,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 190
and whether the changes are maintained over

time. Since these phenomena are fundamentally

different from disease processes they require a


separate and more fitting conceptual scheme.

Thus if a primary malignant tumor has been

surgically removed, it is reasonable to speak of

cures and of possible relapses, since cancerous

cells may not have been completely extirpated. By


contrast, deviant behavior cannot be eradicated by
the removal of a global internal determinant;

rather, the occurrence of deviant behavior is

extensively controlled by its likely consequences,

and may therefore vary considerably in different


environmental settings, toward different persons,

and at different times. This would be analogous to


having malignancies appear in a given person

under one set of social circumstances and

disappear under others.

Unlike physical therapies, in the appraisal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 191
psychological methods it is important to

distinguish among the induction, generalization

and maintenance of behavior, because these


processes are governed by somewhat different

variables. The fact that established changes may

no longer be evident some time after treatment

has been discontinued does not necessarily mean

that the method is inadequate. On the contrary, it


may be exceedingly powerful for inducing
changes, but the gains may prove short-lived

because the proper maintaining conditions have

not been arranged. Similarly, in some cases

enduring behavioral changes are achieved, but


they do not transfer to extratherapeutic situations,

thus requiring supplementary procedures to


ensure optimal transfer effects. Outcome studies

should therefore be designed to provide

unconfounded data regarding the magnitude,


generality, and durability of outcomes associated

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 192
with given treatment approaches.

PSYCHOTHERAPY AND FRIENDSHIP EXPERIENCES

It would appear from the absence of

differential improvement rates for treated and


nontreated groups that favorable behavioral

changes, when they do occur, must be produced by

factors that are unrelated to the special methods


that are rigorously applied by psychotherapists. It
is therefore not surprising to find that intensive

specialized training and experience in traditional

psychotherapeutic procedures may not only fail to

increase the incidence of favorable outcomes but


may in some instances interfere with the

establishment of social-learning conditions likely

to foster beneficial changes. Poser (1966), in a

bold research project, compared modifications in

the psychological functioning of psychotic patients


who received either five months of group

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 193
psychotherapy by psychiatrists and psychiatric
social workers, group discussions with

undergraduate students, or no special treatment.


The undergraduates, who responded to an

advertisement for summer employment, were

selected without any additional requisites, they

had no training or experience in psychotherapy,


and they were given no suggestion as to how they

should conduct their sessions. Patients seen by the

undergraduates displayed greater gains than both

the controls or cases treated by professional

psychotherapists; the latter two groups did not


differ much from each other. Rioch and her

associates (Rioch, Elkes, Flint, Usdansky, Newman,


& Silber, 1963) likewise found that selected

married women who received part-time practical


training over a two-year period in the application

of psychotherapeutic methods performed as well


as their professional counterparts. However, in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 194
view of Poser’s findings, it would be essential to

study the comparative efficacy of a group of

untrained therapists in order to determine


whether the protracted instruction was irrelevant

to the outcomes achieved by the trained


housewives.

The question nevertheless remains why some

persons undergo changes and others do not,


whether or not they are involved in formal
therapy. Comparative investigations of the

attributes of clients who terminate treatment

prematurely with those of clients who remain and

improve are particularly relevant in this respect.


Relative to persons who continue in treatment,
terminators typically come from lower

socioeconomic levels, are nonconforming toward

authority figures, are impulsive, relatively non-

anxious, report a history of antisocial behavior,

present deficits in verbal and emotional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 195
responsiveness, exhibit a relative inability to

establish and to maintain social relationships, and

acknowledge little contingency between their own


behavior and the actions of others toward them.

By contrast, remainers generally come from

higher socioeconomic levels, are better educated,

are willing to explore their personal problems, are

responsive to social reinforcement, are


suggestible, introspective, relatively anxious, self-
dissatisfied, and self-condemning (Auld & Myers,

1954; Frank et al. 1957; Hiler, 1954; Imber et al.,

1955; Katz, Lorr, & Rubinstein, 1958; Lorr, Katz, &

Rubinstein, 1958; McNair, Lorr, & Callahan, 1963;


Rubinstein & Lorr, 1956). Except for

socioeconomic and educational indices—which


generally correlate significantly with continuation

in treatment but tend to be unrelated to outcome

—most of the latter personality variables are


likewise predictive of rated subsequent

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 196
improvement in psychotherapy. Thus the type of

people who continue to participate and improve in

psychotherapy have attributes similar to those of


persons who, in laboratory studies of conformity,

attitude change, and conditionability, show more


responsiveness to almost any form of social
influence procedure (Berg & Bass, 1961; Biderman

& Zimmer, 1961; Janis & Hovland, 1959).

The above findings indicate that the social


characteristics of clients, rather than the chosen

psychotherapeutic method, are the main

determinants of the successes of traditional

psychotherapy. This may explain why, in spite of


wide conceptual divergences, all “schools” of
psychotherapy achieve very similar rates of

improvement (Appel et ah, 1951; Miles, Barrabee,

& Finesinger, 1951) and, although differences may

occasionally slightly favor the treated groups

(Frank, Gliedman, Imber, Stone, & Nash, 1959;

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 197
Leary & Harvey, 1956), the magnitude of

behavioral change exhibited by nontreated cases

is not substantially less than change in clients who


have undergone some traditional form of

psychotherapy (Bergin, 1966). The types of clients

who derive some benefit from participation in

conventional forms of psychotherapy are likely to

exhibit varying degrees of favorable improvement


with little or no formal treatment (Frank et al.,
1959; Saslow & Peters, 1956; Taylor, 1955). These

demonstrable changes are probably a function of

social-learning experiences resulting from casual

or more structured interpersonal interactions


with physicians, attorneys, clergymen, teachers,

close and respected friends, and other societal


agents who possess some degree of social power,

prestige, and good judgment. All these different

sources of social influence apparently rely


primarily upon common—though not the most

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 198
reliable or potent—therapeutic elements for the

modification of social behavior.

The overall outcome data accompanying

conversational treatment approaches indicate the

necessity for distinguishing between


psychotherapy on the one hand, and friendship

experiences on the other. In a thoughtful book

entitled Psychotherapy: The Purchase of Friendship,


Schofield (1964) contends that psychotherapists
are essentially offering their clients a supportive

substitute friendship which does not require

technical professional training. He further argues

that a wide range of persons within a society, by


virtue of their superordinated social roles, their
wisdom and devotion to service, are equally

capable of providing friendships and satisfying

discussions of personal concerns. Therefore,

individuals who are in need of an understanding

and trustworthy friend with whom they can

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 199
periodically share their problems, and those who

are searching for a faith or a commitment that

would add more purpose to their lives, might do


better to seek the counsel and emotional support

of respected colleagues and enlightened societal

agents rather than to flock to psychotherapists

whose training does not ensure special expertise

in the value domain.

It should be recognized that, although


thoughtful discussions in the context of a

supportive friendship can be highly meaningful

and satisfying, they generally have little impact on


persons’ specific behavioral difficulties. Few
chronic stutterers, for example, have been cured

through amity, introspective conversation, and


wise counsel. In modifying persistent deviant

behavior and in overcoming behavioral deficits,

friendship alone is not enough. Special learning

conditions must also be arranged and skillfully

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 200
implemented over a long period if desired

psychological changes are to be consistently

achieved and adequately maintained. The latter


activities, for which the label “psychotherapy” is

appropriate, require unique skills and specialized

procedures for effecting predictable behavioral

changes.

Recent years have witnessed a marked


proliferation of psychological ventures designed to
cure all types of social maladies. These endeavors

include, among other things, meditation, massage,

sensitivity training, and marathon social


encounters in which participants from all walks of
life are provided with opportunities to analyze

each other’s interpersonal reactions. As long as


such programs are not misrepresented and people

find them personally rewarding they require no

further validation. If, on the other hand, they are

marketed as forms of psychotherapy, then

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 201
advocates of such procedures must be concerned

about the consequences of their practices and they

must assume responsibility for empirical


verification of their claims. Moreover, ethical

considerations require that clients specify the

ways in which they wish to be changed, that the

intended outcomes of the therapeutic process be

made known, and that clients be informed of the


likelihood that the treatment interventions will
enable them to deal more effectively with the life

problems for which they seek help.

While psychotherapists are promoting their


favored insights in interview approaches they may
often simultaneously (if inadvertently) reward

their clients with approval for exhibiting desired


response patterns and show disapproval of

maladaptive forms; they may reduce anxieties

through their permissive and supportive reactions

toward clients’ disturbing self-revelations; and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 202
they inevitably model various attitudes, values,

and interpersonal modes of behavior which clients

are inclined to emulate. Many of the therapeutic


changes that occur in conventional psychotherapy

may therefore derive primarily from the unwitting

application of social-learning principles. The point

is that these beneficial outcomes are more readily

attainable when principles are applied in a more


considered and systematic manner.

Even if the traditional forms of psychotherapy

had proved highly effective, they would still have

limited social value. A method that requires


extended and highly expensive training, that can
be performed only by professional personnel, that

must be continuously applied on a one-to-one


basis over a prolonged period of time, and is most

beneficial to self-selected highly suggestible

persons cannot possibly have much impact on the

countless social problems that demand

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 203
psychological attention. Major progress will be

made in resolving these problems by

concentrating on the development of highly


efficacious principles of behavioral change and by

utilizing the large pool of nonprofessional persons

who can be trained to implement programs under

competent guidance and direction. This approach

would provide more people with more help than


they receive under current professional practices.

APPROACHES BASED ON SOCIAL-LEARNING


PRINCIPLES

In subsequent chapters of this book various

social-learning approaches to the modification of

diverse psychological phenomena will be

considered in detail. The principles underlying

each method will be reviewed along with


experimental tests of their efficacy. In addition, the

types of behavioral changes for which each


procedure is best suited will be discussed.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 204
Although major emphasis will be given to

psychological variables that have been shown to

exercise strong control over behavior, some


attention will be devoted to pharmacological

procedures, particularly when they are employed


as adjuncts to social-learning procedures. The
psychological emphasis, however, is not meant to

minimize the genetic, biochemical, and

neurophysiological determinants of behavior. A


social-learning model does not, of course, assume

that behavior is determined exclusively by

psychological variables. Genetic endowment and


constitutional factors may set certain limits on

both the types of behavioral repertoires that can


be developed in a given person, and the rate of

response acquisition. In certain cases,

neurophysiological conditions may contribute to


the observed behavioral malfunctioning.

Moreover, biological and psychological factors

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 205
typically interact in subtle and complex ways in

producing certain patterns of social behavior.

It should also be noted in passing that

physiological variables, to the extent that they

serve as contributory factors, are most likely to be


associated with nonspecific effects as reflected in

the general tempo of responsiveness and the rate

and level of response acquisition. Such variables


do not, however, determine specific behavioral
patterns, which are due to particular social-

learning experiences. Genetic endowment cannot

account for the difference between one

schizophrenic who firmly believes that he is Jesus


Christ, and another one who entertains no
grandiose delusions. The idiosyncratic behavioral

content is obviously learned rather than

physiologically produced. Nor do capacity

variables account for gross deficits in motor,

conceptual, or affective responses that are clearly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 206
within a person’s capabilities. Unfortunately,

deviant behavior is often prematurely attributed

to physiological determinants, an attribution


which results not only in therapeutic pessimism,

but also effectively retards further psychological

investigation of behavioral phenomena.

Summary

This chapter has presented a social-learning

interpretation of the mechanisms regulating

behavior and contrasts this approach with


theories that tend to assign causal properties to

hypothetical internal forces. The differences in


conceptual models are especially striking in
explanations of deviant behavior that have

traditionally been depicted as symptomatic by-

products of a quasi-mental disease. From a social-

learning perspective, behaviors that may be

detrimental to the individual or that depart widely

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 207
from accepted social and ethical norms are

considered not as manifestations of an underlying

pathology but as ways, which the person has


learned, of coping with environmental and self-

imposed demands.

Psychopathology is not solely a property

inherent in behavior but reflects the evaluative

responses of societal agents to actions that violate


prescribed codes of conduct. The social labeling of
a given response pattern as a pathological

expression is, in fact, influenced by numerous

subjective criteria including the aversiveness of


the behavior, the social attributes of the deviator,
the normative standards of persons making the

judgments, the social context in which the


behavior is performed, and a host of other factors.

Consequently, the same response pattern may be

diagnosed as “sick” or may be normatively

sanctioned and considered emulative by different

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 208
groups, at different times, or in different

environmental settings. Considering the arbitrary

and relativistic nature of the social judgment and


definition of deviance, the main value of the

normal versus abnormal dichotomy lies in guiding

the social and legal actions of societal agents

concerned with the maintenance of an efficiently

functioning society. This dichotomy, however, has


little theoretical significance, because no evidence
exists that the behaviors so dichotomized are

either qualitatively different or are under the

control of fundamentally different variables.

Personality theories generally assume that


energized traits and concealed motivational states

impel behavior in a variety of directions. These


hypothetical internal conditions tend to be

regarded as relatively autonomous of external

stimulation and their relationship to behavior

remains somewhat loose. In social-learning theory

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 209
both deviant and prosocial behaviors are acquired

and maintained on the basis of three distinct

regulatory systems.

Some response patterns are primarily under

external stimulus control. Autonomic

responsiveness, such as changes in cardiovascular

and gastrointestinal reactions, and emotional

behavior, can be effectively brought under the


control of environmental events through their
contiguous association with either direct or

vicarious affective experiences. Instrumental

behavior is likewise precisely regulated by


environmental stimuli that, by virtue of their
association with different contingencies of

reinforcement, signify the consequences that are


likely to accompany certain courses of action.

Some forms of deviant behavior primarily reflect

defective or inappropriate stimulus control.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 210
A second behavioral control system involves

response feedback processes, mainly in the form

of reinforcing consequences. Both prosocial and


grossly deviant behaviors can be successively

eliminated and reinstated by varying their


immediate consequences. These influential
aftereffects may include sensory experiences that

are intrinsically produced by the activity itself,

externally arranged tangible or symbolic


outcomes, or self-evaluative reactions. The

susceptibility of behavior to reinforcement control

is further shown by the fact that even subtle


variations in the frequency and patterning of

outcomes result in distinct performance


characteristics.

The third, and in many respects the most

influential, regulatory mechanism operates

through central mediational processes. At this

higher level stimulus inputs are coded and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 211
organized; tentative hypotheses about the

principles governing the occurrence of rewards

and punishments are developed and tested on the


basis of differential consequences accompanying

the corresponding actions; and, once established,

implicit rules and strategies serve to guide

appropriate performances in specified situations.

Symbolically generated affective arousal and


covert self-reinforcing operations may also figure
prominently in the regulation of overt

responsiveness.

In this conceptual scheme man is neither an


internally impelled system nor a passive reactor to
external stimulation. Rather, psychological

functioning involves a reciprocal interaction


between behavior and its controlling environment.

The type of behavior that a person exhibits partly

determines his environmental contingencies

which, in turn, influence his behavior. In

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 212
succeeding chapters of this book the social-

learning principles necessary to account

adequately for the development of prosocial and


deviant behavior will be further elaborated. We

shall also demonstrate how these principles may

be successfully applied to ameliorate

developmental and clinical problems, and to effect

broader social and cultural change.

REFERENCES

Appel, K. E., Lhamon, T. W., Myers, J. M., & Harvey, W.


A. Long term psychotherapy. In Psychiatric
treatment. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, 1951.
pp. 21-34.

Auld, F., Jr., & Myers, J. K. Contributions to a theory for


selecting psychotherapy patients. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 1954, 10, 56-60.

Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. The measurement and


reinforcement of behavior of psychotics. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1965, 8,
357-383.

Ayllon, T., Haughton, E., & Hughes, H. B. Interpretation

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 213
of symptoms: Fact or fiction. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1965, 3, 1-7.

Ayllon, T., & Michael, J. The psychiatric nurse as a


behavioral engineer. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1959, 2, 323-334.

Bailey, C. J. The effectiveness of drives as cues. Journal


of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
1955, 48, 183-187.

Bailey, C. J., & Porter, L. W. Relevant cues in drive


discrimination in cats. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1955, 48, 180-181.

Bandura, A. Relationship of family patterns to child


behavior disorders. Progress Report, 1960,
Stanford University, Project No. M-1734, United
States Public Health Service.

Bandura, A. Punishment revisited. Journal of


Consulting Psychology, 1962, 26, 298-301.

Bandura, A. Vicarious processes: A case of no-trial


learning. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology, Vol. II. New York:
Academic Press, 1965. pp. 1-55.

Bandura, A. A social learning interpretation of


psychological dysfunctions. In P. London & D.
Rosenhan (Eds.), Foundations of abnormal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 214
psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston,
1968. pp. 293-344.

Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. The relative


efficacy of desensitization and modeling
approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and
attitudinal changes, Stanford University, 1969,
13, 173-199.

Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. Some social


determinants of self-monitoring reinforcement
systems. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1967, 5, 449-455.

Bandura, A., & Kupers, C. J. Transmission of patterns


of self-reinforcement through modeling. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964, 69, 1-9.

Bandura, A., & Perloff, B. Relative efficacy of self-


monitored and externally imposed reinforcement
systems. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1967, 7, 111-116.

Bandura, A., & Rosenthal, T. L. Vicarious classical


conditioning as a function of arousal level. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 54-
62.

Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. Vicarious


reinforcement and imitative learning. Journal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 215
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 601-
607.

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression.


New York: Ronald Press, 1959.

Bandura, A., & Whalen, C. K. The influence of


antecedent reinforcement and divergent
modeling cues on patterns of self-reward. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 373-
382.

Bateson, G. (Ed.) Perceval’s narrative: A patient’s


account of his psychosis. 1830—1832. Stanford,
Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1961.

Bern, S. L. Verbal self-control: The establishment of


effective self-instruction. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1967, 74, 485-491.

Berg, J. A., & Bass, B. M. (Eds.) Conformity and


deviation. New York: Harper, 1961.

Berger, S. M. Incidental learning through vicarious


reinforcement. Psychological Reports, 1961, 9,
477-491.

Berger, S. M. Conditioning through vicarious


instigation. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 450-
466.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 216
Bergin, A. E. Some implications of psychotherapy
research for therapeutic practice. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 1966, 71, 235-246.

Biderman, A. D., & Zimmer, H. The manipulation of


human behavior. New York: Wiley, 1961.

Bisese, V. S. Imitation behavior as a function of direct


and vicarious reinforcement. Dissertation
Abstracts, 1966, 26, 6155.

Black, A. H. Transfer following operant conditioning in


the curarized dog. Science, 1967,155, 201-203.

Bookbinder, L. J. Simple conditioning vs. the dynamic


approach to symptoms and symptoms
substitution: A reply to Yates. Psychological
Reports, 1962, 10, 71-77.

Bourne, L. E., Jr. Human conceptual behavior. Boston:


Allyn & Bacon, 1966.

Bower, G. H. Mental imagery and associative learning.


In L. Gregg (Ed.), Cognition in learning and
memory. New York: Wiley, 1969.

Bruning, J. L. Direct and vicarious effects of a shift in


magnitude of reward on performance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 278-
282.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 217
Buehler, R. E., Patterson, G. R., & Furniss, J. M. The
reinforcement of behavior in institutional
settings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966,
4, 157-167.

Bykov, K. M. The cerebral cortex and the internal


organs. Translated and edited by W. H. Gantt.
New York: Chemical Publishing Co., 1957.

Church, R. M. Transmission of learned behavior


between rats. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1957, 54, 163-165.

Cloward, R. A., & Ohlin, L. E. Delinquency and


opportunity: A theory of delinquent gangs. New
York: Free Press, 1960.

Crankshaw, E. Another Russian “rebel” author put in


asylum. The Observer, February 17, 1963.

Davenport, J. W. Higher-order conditioning of fear.


Psychonomic Science, 1966, 4, 27-28.

Dekker, E., & Groen, J. Reproducible psychogenic


attacks of asthma: A laboratory study. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 1956, 1, 58—67.

Dekker, E., Pelser, H. E., & Groen, J. Conditioning as a


cause of asthmatic attacks. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 1957, 2, 97-108.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 218
DiCara, L. V. & Miller, N. E. Instrumental learning of
vasomotor responses by rats: Learning to
respond differentially in the two ears. Science,
1968, 159, 1485-1486.

Eysenck, H. J. The effects of psychotherapy: An


evaluation. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
1952, 16, 319-325.

Fairweather, G. W. Social psychology in treating


mental illness. New York: Wiley, 1964.

Ferster, C. B. Behavior principles. New York: Appleton-


Century-Crofts, 1968.

Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. Schedules of


reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1957.

Frank, J. D., Gliedman, L. H., Imber, S. D., Nash, E. H., &


Stone, A. R. Why patients leave psychotherapy.
American Medical Association Archives of
Neurological Psychiatry, 1957, 77, 283-299.

Frank, J. D., Gliedman, L. H., Imber, S. D., Stone, A. R., &


Nash, E. H., Jr., Patients’ expectancies and
relearning as factors determining improvement
in psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychiatry,
1959, 115, 961-968.

Freud, S. The complete psychological works of ...

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 219
Translated by James Strachey. Vol. 10. London:
Hogarth, 1955.

Garfield, S. L., & Kurz, M. Evaluation of treatment and


related procedures in 1216 cases referred to a
mental hygiene clinic. Psychiatric Quarterly, 1952,
26, 414-24.

Gitelson, M. Communication from the president about


the neoanalytic movement. Journal of the
International Psycho-Analytic Association, 1962,
43, 373-375.

Grossberg, J. M. Behavior therapy: A review.


Psychological Bulletin, 1964, 62, 73-88.

Harris, F. R., Wolf, M. N., & Baer, D. M. Effects of adult


social reinforcement on child behavior. Young
Children, 1964, 20, 8-17.

Hastorf, A. H. The reinforcement of individual actions


in a group situation. In L. Krasner & L. P. Ullmann
(Eds.), Research in behavior modification. New,
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. pp. 268-
284.

Heine, R. W. A comparison of patients’ reports on


psychotherapeutic experience with
psychoanalytic, nondirective and Adlerian
therapists. American Journal of Psychotherapy,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 220
1953, 7, 16-23.

Hiler, E. W. Psychological factors associated with


premature termination in psychotherapy.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Michigan, 1954.

Homme, L. E. Perspectives in psychology: XXIV.


Control of coverants, the operants of the mind.
Psychological Record, 1965, 15, 501-511.

Imber, S. D., Nash, E. H., Jr., & Stone, A. R. Social class


and duration of psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 1955, 11, 281-284.

Janis, I. L., & Hovland, C. I. (Eds.) Personality and


persuasibility. New Haven: Yale University Press,
1959.

John, E. R. Mechanisms of memory. New York:


Academic Press, 1967.

Kanfer, F. H. Vicarious human reinforcement: A


glimpse into the black box. In L. Krasner & L. P.
Ullmann (Eds.), Research in behavior modification.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. pp.
244-267.

Kanfer, F. H. Verbal conditioning: A review of its


current status. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton
(Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 221
theory. New York: Prentice-Hall, 1968. pp. 254-
290.

Kanfer, F. H., & Marston, A. R. Human reinforcement:


vicarious and direct. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1963, 65, 292-296.

Kanfer, F. H., & Marston, A. R. Conditioning of self-


reinforcing responses: An analogue to self-
confidence training. Psychological Reports, 1963,
13, 63-70. (b)

Katz, M. M., Lorr, M., & Rubinstein, E. A. Remainer


patient attributes and their relation to
subsequent improvement in psychotherapy.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1958, 22, 411-
413.

Kimble, G. A. Hilgard and Marquis’ conditioning and


learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,
1961.

Kimmel, H. D. Instrumental conditioning of


autonomically mediated behavior. Psychological
Bulletin, 1967, 67, 337—345.

Kirtner, W. L., & Cartwright, D. S. Success and failure


in client-centered therapy as a function of client
personality variables. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 1958, 22, 259-264.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 222
Knight, R. O. Evaluation of the results of
psychoanalytic therapy. American Journal of
Psychiatry. 1941, 98, 434-446.

Kurland, S. H. Length of treatment in a mental hygiene


clinic. Psychiatric Quarterly Supplement, 1956, 30,
83-90.

Lazarus, A. A. Group therapy of phobic disorders by


systematic desensitization. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1961, 63, 504-510.

Leary, T., & Harvey, J. S. A methodology for measuring


personality changes in psychotherapy. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 1956, 12, 123-132.

Levine, S. The role of irrelevant drive stimuli in


learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1953, 45, 410-416.

Levitt, E. E. Psychotherapy with children: A further


evaluation. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
1963, 1, 45-51.

Levy, D. M. Maternal overprotection. New York:


Columbia University Press, 1943.

Lidz, T., Cornelison, A. R., Terry, D., & Fleck, S.


Intrafamilial environment of the schizophrenic
patient: VI. The transmission of irrationality.
American Medical Association Archives of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 223
Neurological Psychiatry, 1958, 79, 305-316.

Lidz, T., Fleck, S., & Cornelison, A. R. Schizophrenia and


the family. New York: International Universities
Press, 1965.

Liebert, R. M., & Allen, M. K. Effects of rule structure


and reward magnitude on the acquisition and
adoption of self-reward criteria. Psychological
Reports, 1967, 21, 445-452.

Lorr, M., Katz, M. M., & Rubinstein, E. A. The prediction


of length of stay in psychotherapy. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 1958, 22, 321-327.

Luria, A. R. The role of speech in the regulation of


normal and abnormal behaviour. New York:
Pergamon Press, 1961.

McDavid, J. W. Effects of ambiguity of environmental


cues upon learning to imitate. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962, 65, 381-
386.

McMains, M. J., & Liebert, R. M. Influence of


discrepancies between successively modeled self-
reward criteria on the adoption of a self-imposed
standard. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1968, 8, 166-171.

McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Callahan, D. M. Patient and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 224
therapist influences on quitting psychotherapy.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1963, 27, 10-17.

Marlatt, G. A. Vicarious and direct reinforcement


control of verbal behavior in an interview setting.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana
University, 1968.

Marston, A. R. Imitation, self-reinforcement, and


reinforcement of another person. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 255-
261. (a)

Marston, A. R. Self-reinforcement: The relevance of a


concept in analogue research in psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice,
1965, 2, 1-5. (b)

Meehl, F. E. Clinical vs. statistical prediction.


Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press,
1954.

Mensh, I. N., & Golden, J. M. Factors in


psychotherapeutic success. Journal of the
Missouri Medical Society. 1951, 48, 180-184.

Merton, R. K. Social theory and social structure. (Rev.


ed.) New York: Free Press, 1957.

Miles, H. W. H., Barrabee, E. L., & Finesinger, J. E.


Evaluation of psychotherapy. Psychosomatic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 225
Medicine, 1951, 12, 83-105.

Miller, N. E. Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I.


Fear as motivation and fear-reduction as
reinforcement in the learning of new responses.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1948, 38, 89-
101.

Miller, N. E. Learnable drives and rewards. In S. S.


Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental
psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951. pp. 435-472.

Miller, N. E. Learning of visceral and glandular


responses. Science, 1969, 163, 434-445.

Mischel, W. Personality and assessment. New York:


Wiley, 1968.

Mischel, W., & Liebert, R. M. Effects of discrepancies


between observed and imposed reward criteria
on their acquisition and transmission. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 45-53.

Moore, N. Behaviour therapy in bronchial asthma: A


controlled study. Journal of Psychosomatic
Research, 1965, 9, 257-276.

Moser, D. Screams, slaps and love. Life, May 7, 1965,


90A-101.

Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and personality

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 226
dynamics. New York: Ronald Press, 1950.

Noelpp, B., & Noelpp-Eschenhagen, I. Das


experimentelle Asthma bronchiale des
Meerschweinschens: II. Mitterlung die Rolle
bedingter Reflexes in des Pathogenese des
Asthma bronchiale. International Archives of
Allergy, 1951, 2, 321-329.

Noelpp, B., & Noelpp-Eschenhagen, I. Das


experimentelle Asthma bronchiale des
Meerschweinschens: III. Studien zur Bedeutung
bedingter Reflexe, Bahnungsbereitschaft und
Heftfahigkeit unter Stress. International Archives
of Allergy, 1952, 3, 108-135.

Ottenberg, P., Stein, M., Lewis, J., & Hamilton, C.


Learned asthma in the guinea pig. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 1958, 10, 395-400.

Paivio, A. Mental imagery in associative learning and


memory. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 241-
263.

Pascal, G. R., & Zax, M. Psychotherapeutics: Success or


failure. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1956, 20,
325-331.

Paschke, R. E., Simon, S., & Bell, R. W. Vicarious


discriminative learning in retardates. Journal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 227
Abnormal Psychology, 1967, 72, 536-542.

Paul, G. L. Insight vs. desensitization in psychotherapy.


Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1966.

Poser, E. G. The effect of therapists’ training on group


therapeutic outcome. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 1966, 30, 28-289.

Rausch, H. L. Interaction sequences. Journal of


Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 487-
499.

Razran, G. The observable unconscious and the


inferable conscious in current soviet
psychophysiology. Psychological Review, 1961,
68, 81-147.

Rickles, N. K., Klein, J. J., & Bassan, M. E. Who goes to a


psychiatrist? American Journal of Psychiatry,
1950, 106, 845-850.

Rioch, M. J., Elkes, E., Flint, A. A., Usdansky, B. C.,


Newman, R. G., & Silber, E. National Institute of
Mental Health pilot study in training mental
health counselors. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 1963, 33, 678-689.

Rogers, C. R. (Ed.) The therapeutic relationship and its


impact: a study of psychotherapy with
schizophrenics. Madison, Wis.: University of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 228
Wisconsin Press, 1967.

Rosenbaum, M. E., & Bruning, J. L. Direct and vicarious


effects of variations in percentage of
reinforcement on performance. Child
Development, 1966, 37, 959-966.

Rosenhan, D., Frederick, F., & Burrowes, A. Preaching


and practicing: Effects of channel discrepancy on
norm internalization. Child Development, 1968,
39, 291-301.

Rubinstein, E. A., & Lorr, M. A comparison of


terminators and remainers in outpatient
psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
1956, 12, 345-349.

San Francisco Chronicle, November 26, 1964, p. 3.

Saslow, G., & Peters, A. D. A follow-up study of


“untreated” patients with various behavior
disorders. Psychiatric Quarterly, 1956, 30, 283-
302.

Sassenrath, J. M. Transfer of learning without


awareness. Psychological Reports, 1962, 10,411-
420.

Schofield, W. Psychotherapy: The purchase of


friendship. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall,
1964.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 229
Sidman, M. Tactics of scientific research. New York:
Basic Books, 1960.

Sidman, M. Avoidance behavior. In W. K. Honig (Ed.),


Operant behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1966, pp. 448-498.

Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York:


Macmillan, 1953.

Skinner, B. F. Cumulative record. New York: Appleton-


Century-Crofts, 1961.

Slucki, H., Adam, G., & Porter, R. W. Operant


discrimination of an interoceptive stimulus in
rhesus monkeys. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1965, 8, 405-414.

Solomon, R. L. & Turner, L. H. Discriminative classical


conditioning in dogs paralyzed by curare can
later control discriminative avoidance responses
in the normal state. Psychological Review, 1962,
69, 202-219.

Spohn, H. E. The influence of social values upon the


clinical judgments of psychotherapists. In J. G.
Peatman & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Festschrift for
Gardner Murphy. New York: Harper, 1960. pp.
274-290.

Szasz, T. S. The myth of mental illness. New York:

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 230
Hoeber-Harper, 1961.

Szasz, T. S. Bootlegging humanistic values through


psychiatry. The Antioch Review, 1962, 22, 341-
349.

Szasz, T. S. Psychiatric justice. New York: Macmillan,


1965.

Taylor, D. M. Changes in self concept without


psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
1955, 19, 205-209.

Terrace, H. S. Stimulus control. In W. K. Honig (Ed.),


Operant behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1966. pp. 271-344.

Terwilliger, J. S., & Fiedler, F. E. An investigation of


determinants inducing individuals to seek
personal counseling. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 1958, 22, 288.

Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. The social psychology of


groups. New York: Wiley, 1959.

Walton, D. The application of learning theory to the


treatment of a case of bronchial asthma. In H. J.
Eysenck (Ed.), Behaviour therapy and the
neuroses. New York: Pergamon, 1960. pp. 188-
189.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 231
Wolf, M., Risley, T., & Mees, H. Application of operant
conditioning procedures to the behaviour
problems of an autistic child. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1964, 1, 305-312.

Wolpe, J., & Rachman, S. Psychoanalytic “evidence”: A


critique based on Freud’s case of Little Hans.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 1960,
131,135-148.

Yablonsky, L. The violent gang. New York: Macmillan,


1962.

Yates, A. J. Symptoms and symptom substitution.


Psychological Review, 1958, 65, 371-374.

Zubin, J. Evaluation of therapeutic outcome in mental


disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases,
1953, 117, 95-111.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 232
2

Value Issues and Objectives


In developing and implementing programs for
modifying behavior, the specification of goals is of

central importance. If the objectives are poorly

defined, an agent of behavioral change has no

rational basis for selecting the appropriate

treatment procedures or for evaluating the

effectiveness of his efforts. Illustrations of how the


choice of outcomes determines the selection of
procedures are provided in diverse social

practices. A physician, for example, does not


prescribe medication or surgical intervention for

his patient without first deciding what physical

changes he wants to induce; a researcher does not


choose independent variables for study in advance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 233
of specifying the phenomena he wishes to modify;
a travel agent does not select a route for a client

before ascertaining his destination; and a teacher

does not make assignments to his students in the


absence of some type of educational objective.

Similarly, the first major task in any successful


program of behavior modification is to delineate
the changes it aims to achieve.

Often the principal aims of social change

enterprises are never clearly articulated, with the


result that programs remain directionless or offer

learning experiences that are selected fortuitously

by personal preferences of the change agents

rather than specifically for the needs of the

recipients. Even more often, however, broad


objectives are specified only in terms of ill-defined
hypothetical states (rather than behavioral

outcomes), which furnish little direction for the

selection of appropriate methods and learning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 234
experiences. Indeed, conceptualizing psychological

abstractions as internal properties of clients

rather than as hypothetical constructs of


therapists has resulted in considerable confusion

about the types of changes effected by different

approaches to the modification of behavior.

It is widely assumed that behavioral and

psychodynamic approaches are concerned with


fundamentally different subject matters. The latter
methods supposedly treat complexes, repressed

impulses, ego strengths and mental apparatuses,

the underlying causes of behavior, whereas


behavioral approaches are believed to modify only
superficial behavior. This apparent difference in

subject matter, however, exists primarily in the


therapists’ conceptualizations, not in actual

practice.

Ego strength, to take an example, is a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 235
hypothetical construct and not an entity within the
client. One can neither observe nor modify

hypothetical constructs. The person’s behavior—


broadly defined to include cognitive, emotional,

and motor expressions—is the only class of events

that can be altered through psychological

procedures, and therefore it is the only meaningful


subject matter of psychotherapy. Similarly,

stimulus variables are the only events that the

therapist can modify to effect behavioral change.

Psychotherapy, like any other social influence

enterprise, is thus a process in which the therapist


arranges stimulus conditions that produce desired

behavioral changes in the client. If, for instance, a


psychotherapist creates conditions that increase

the frequency of the behaviors from which ego


strength is inferred, the client will be said to have

acquired increased ego strength as a function of


treatment. On the other hand, if the frequency of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 236
ego-strength behaviors has been reduced in the

course of psychotherapy, the client has suffered a

loss in ego strength. Clearly, ego strength is simply


a hypothetical abstraction whose presumed

behavioral referents are the only reality the


psychotherapist can modify.

In the final analysis, social-learning approaches

and all other existing forms of treatment modify


the same subject matter, namely, behavioral
phenomena. Most discussions of change-inducing

processes, however, focus on treating the

inferences made from behavioral events as though

these abstractions existed independently and


caused their behavioral referents. Philosophers of
science have cautioned against the attribution of

causal potency to described properties of

behavior. Their warnings have had little impact on

personality theorizing.

Neither traits nor types, as concepts, have

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 237
any real existence. They are merely words,
and words do not exist in the eye of the
observer nor in the people observed. A man
can not be said to have either a type or a
trait, but he can be said to fit either a type or
a trait. At present the fit will be inexact, for
dimensions of personality have not yet been
quantified well enough to permit of accurate
measurement. In the case of height, the
measurement can be precise, and little
confusion results from saying that a man has
a certain height. Observation and concept
are so closely related that the phrase is not
ordinarily understood to mean more than it
says, namely, that the extent of a given
datum of observation in one direction fits a
certain section of an ideal dimension of
distance. But if an attempt is made to fit
some mode of human conduct to the trait of
courage, the looseness of correspondence
between behavior and concepts leads to
mischievous reification. The concept parts
company with behavior, picks up undefined
notions in its flight from reality, and finally
acquires an independent real existence in its
own right, so that when it is said that a man
has courage, he will be thought of as the
fortunate owner of something considerably

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 238
more significant than a certain pattern of
behavior [Pratt, 1939, p. 115].

Similarly, a person who is plagued with “weak ego

strength” will be viewed as suffering from

something vastly more significant than the


behavioral referents from which the construct is

inferred.

For purposes of further illustration, let us


designate behaviors in which persons violate

social and legal codes of behavior and frequently

engage in assaultive activities as the external


expressions of an inferred zoognick. Based on

prevailing clinical practices, the zoognick would

come to represent an intrapsychically functioning


agent. An honorific causative power would be

conferred upon this hypothetical zoognick,


whereas the observed behavior from which its

existence is inferred would be depreciated as


superficial behavioral manifestations. Before long,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 239
psychological tests would be constructed to
measure zoognick strength on the basis of which

diagnosticians would tautologically attribute

clients’ behavior to the action of the underlying


zoognick. Proceeding on the assumption that

“patient variables are not conceived to be


behaviors, but constructs concerning internal
constellations” (Wallerstein, 1963),

psychotherapeutic goals would be stated in terms

of removing the pernicious zoognick. On the other


hand, direct modification of the deviant behavior

would be considered not only superficial but

potentially dangerous, since elimination of the

symptomatic expressions might force the zoognick

to emerge in equally pernicious substitute forms.


A sufficiently charismatic exponent of zoognick
theory could undoubtedly develop a sizable

following with the same extraordinary conviction

in the vital importance and causative potency of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 240
zoognicks as that shown by adherents of libidinal

forces. Oedipal complexes, collective

unconsciouses, and self-dynamisms. Finally,


humanists would embrace zoognick theory as

more befitting the complexities of human beings


than those simplistic mechanistic doctrines that
stubbornly insist that the zoognick is the deviant

behavior rechristened.

Most treatment approaches devote remarkably


little attention to the selection of objectives; when

they are specified (Mahrer, 1967), the intended

outcomes generally include a variety of abstract

virtues described in socially desirable terms, such


as reorganization of the self, restoration of
functional effectiveness, development of

individuation and self-actualization, establishment

of homeostatic equilibrium, where there is id there

shall ego be and where superego was there shall

conscious ego be, achievement of identity,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 241
acceptance of self-consciousness, enhancement of

ego strength, or the attainment of self-awareness,

emotional maturity, and positive mental health.


While some of these objectives allude to vaguely

defined behavioral characteristics, most refer to

nebulous hypothetical states. These abstractions

convey little information unless they are further

defined in terms of specifically observable


behavior.

Behavioral Specification of Objectives

A meaningfully stated objective has at least

two basic characteristics (Mager, 1961). First, it


should identify and describe the behaviors
considered appropriate to the desired outcomes.

The term “behavior” is used in the broad sense to

include a complex of observable and potentially

measurable activities including motor, cognitive,

and physiological classes of responses.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 242
After the intended goals have been specified in

performance, and preferably in measurable terms,

decisions can be made about the experiences that


are most likely to produce the desired outcomes.

For example, the statement, “Increase the person’s


self-confidence and self-esteem,” designates a
therapeutic intent; but it furnishes little guidance,

since it does not reveal the kinds of behaviors the

person will exhibit after he has achieved increased


self-esteem. Once self-esteem and the behaviors

that will be esteem producing for a particular

client have been delineated, one can arrange


conditions that will create the requisite behaviors

and thereby produce the condition of positive self-


evaluation. In some instances learning vocational

skills may be most relevant to acquiring self-

esteem; in some cases developing interpersonal


competencies that will secure positive responses

from others may be most appropriate; in other

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 243
cases eliminating alienating social behaviors may
be required if self-evaluation is to be altered; and

finally, in cases where a person is relatively

competent socially and vocationally, an increase in

self-esteem behavior may require the modification


of stringent, self-imposed standards of behavior

upon which self-approving and self-deprecatory


responses are contingent. Similarly, unless the
goals specify the behavior that persons will exhibit

when successfully self-actualized, internally

integrated, self-accepted, personally

reconstructed, homeostatically equilibrated, or


emotionally matured, such goals provide little

guidance.

In addition to describing the behaviors which

reflect the chosen goals, objectives must often be

further delineated by specifying the conditions

under which one may expect the behavior to

occur. Let us assume that increased assertiveness

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 244
is the goal for the treatment of an excessively

passive individual. After assertive behavior is

defined in sufficient detail that there is little


ambiguity about the interpersonal skills to be

learned, appropriate conditions can be arranged to

produce the desired changes. To demonstrate,

however, that the person has achieved the

objective, one would not require him to exhibit


assertive behavior in all social situations. Because
interpersonal demands are complex, effective

social functioning requires a well-discriminated

repertoire of behavior. Therefore a complete

statement of objectives should specify to what


degree the modified behavior is expected to be

linked to social conditions.

The emphasis on behavioral specification of

goals is not intended to encourage the selection of

inconsequential outcomes. Instead, it places

greater demands on change agents for careful

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 245
analysis of complicated objectives that cannot be

successfully attained by any method as long as

they remain couched in ill-defined, general terms.


Complex behavior is an aggregate of simpler

components which must be individually learned

and appropriately integrated. After complex

performances have been adequately analyzed,

conditions that will permit learning of the


component behaviors can be designated. Without
this type of behavioral analysis, change agents

remain at a loss how to proceed and simply fall

back on favorite routines.

Behaviorally defined objectives not only


provide guidance in selecting appropriate

procedures, but they serve an important


evaluative function as well. When desired

outcomes are designated in observable and

measurable terms, it becomes readily apparent

when the methods have succeeded, when they

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 246
have failed, and when they need further

development to increase their potency. This self-

corrective feature is a safeguard against


perpetuation of ineffective approaches, which are

difficult to retire if the changes they are supposed

to produce remain ambiguous.

SEQUENCING OF INTERMEDIATE OBJECTIVES

Establishing complex social behavior and


modifying existing response patterns can be

achieved most consistently through a gradual

process in which the person participates in an


orderly learning sequence that guides him
stepwise toward more intricate or demanding

performances. Although the specification of

ultimate objectives provides some direction and

continuity to a program of change, day-to-day

progress is most influenced by defining

intermediate objectives and the learning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 247
experiences necessary for their attainment. A
comprehensive statement of objectives should,

therefore, contain a sequence of intermediate


goals that lead gradually to more complex modes

of behavior.

This principle of gradation is applied


extensively in the social-learning procedures
discussed in succeeding chapters. In each case,

complex behavior outcomes are analyzed into


smaller subtasks and sequenced so as to ensure

optimal progress. For example, fearful

responsiveness and defensively avoidant behavior

can be successfully eliminated by either direct

exposure to aversive events (Grossberg, 1965;


Herzberg, 1945); by exposure to models boldly

exhibiting approach behavior toward fear-


provoking situations (Bandura, Blanchard, &

Ritter, 1968; Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1967);


or by symbolic reinstatement of threatening

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 248
events in the context of strong competing positive

responses (Wolpe, 1958). The therapist first

devises a ranked set of threatening situations to


which the client responds with increasing degrees

of anxiety. Initially the client is presented with the

least threatening event under favorable conditions

until his emotional responses have been

thoroughly extinguished. As treatment progresses


the fear-arousing properties of the aversive
situations are gradually increased until emotional

responsiveness to events that originally he found

most threatening is extinguished. While stimulus

gradation is not a necessary condition for


extinguishing fearful behavior, it permits greater

control over the direction and progress of


behavior changes.

Hierarchical organization of learning

experiences is even more useful in programs

designed to develop new patterns of behavior,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 249
because the response elements that compose

complex performances may themselves be

relatively intricate compounds. Therefore,


complicated response patterns cannot be taught

without first establishing the necessary

components. In social practice, intricate modes of

behavior are best attained stepwise by modeling

progressively more complex responses Bandura,


1969; Lovaas, 1967) and reinforcing gradual
response elaborations.

Skillful sequencing of intermediate objectives

can help achieve desired goals in several ways. By


approaching a complicated learning outcome
through successive subtasks, experiences of

failure can be reduced to a minimum, because no


subtask requires constituent skills that

participants do not already possess. The degree of

positive reinforcement can therefore be

maintained at a high level by continuous progress.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 250
If, on the other hand, people are required to

attempt complex behavior prematurely, they

experience a great many unnecessary failures.


These .experiences may jeopardize the treatment

program by decreasing positive motivation, by

inviting obstructive and avoidant responses, and

even by augmenting deviant behaviors that the

treatment was designed to modify. Graded


objectives both permit greater control over
learning outcomes and guide and focus the

behavior of participants throughout all stages of

treatment. Change programs that are poorly

organized as evidenced by isolated, haphazard,


and inadequately sequenced learning experiences

will produce discouraging results, however valid


the principles supposedly guiding the social

practices.

Factors Impeding Specification of


Objectives

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 251
In view of the importance of defining the

necessary learning conditions in terms of clearly

specified goals, it is surprising that objectives are


accorded little consideration in the theorizing and

practice of psychotherapy. Almost without


exception, treatises on psychotherapy contain
detailed prescriptions of the conditions essential

for effecting changes and admonitions about the

hazards of deviating from prescribed methods.


The outcomes that these procedures are designed

to produce and the value judgments implied by

these goals are inadequately explicated. Several


possible reasons may account for this traditional

inattention to issues of goal selection.

ADVOCACY OF NONCONTINGENT SOCIAL


REINFORCEMENT

It is widely believed that noncontingent


“relationship” experiences are the primary

determinants of behavioral change, and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 252
consequently that the specific methods employed
are of secondary importance. In a “therapeutic”

atmosphere in which the therapist exhibits


permissive, non- judgmental and unconditionally

positive attitudes, it is contended, a variety of

methods, within certain broad limits, will produce

essentially similar changes in behavior.

This view—which is somewhat analogous to

relying on “bedside manner” rather than on


specific therapeutic interventions in the

alleviation of physical disorders—can be seriously

questioned by an example in which objectives are

clearly identified. Let us assume that two children

have been referred for treatment, one passive and


nonaggressive, the second exhibiting a

hyperaggressive pattern of behavior. Since the


goal is to increase assertiveness in the passive

child and to decrease the domineering tendencies


of the hyperaggressive child, should the therapist

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 253
employ the same methods? Clearly the answer is

in the negative. Based on established principles of

behavior change, procedures aimed at reducing


inhibitions (Wolpe, 1958), the provision of

assertive models of behavior (Bandura, 1965) and

the reinforcement of assertive response patterns

(Jack, 1934; Page, 1936; Walters & Brown, 1963)

are most appropriate and effective for promoting


increased assertiveness. These methods, however,
would be clearly inappropriate in the treatment of

the hyperaggressive child, since they would simply

strengthen the already persistent deviant

behavior. Withdrawal of rewards for aggression


(Brown & Elliott, 1965) combined with modeling

and positive reinforcement of nonaggressive


frustration responses (Chittenden, 1942) is highly

effective for decreasing aggressiveness. Although

in both of these hypothetical cases warmth,


interest, understanding, and other relationship

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 254
factors would apply equally, it is unrealistic to

expect these general factors to increase

aggressiveness in one child and to reduce it in the


other. Nevertheless therapists often adhere to a

single set of therapeutic conditions, disregarding


the nature of the client’s deviant behavior.
Maladaptive behavior may thus be strengthened

rather than weakened in cases for whom the

learning conditions are inappropriate.

The relationship view of behavioral

modification also implies that no significant

permanent changes in social behavior can be

achieved unless a social relationship is firmly


established. Until recently it has likewise been
confidently believed that a beneficent teacher-

student relationship is a necessary precondition in

the educational process. Comparative studies,

however, reveal that self-instructional programs

can equal or even surpass the efficacy of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 255
instructors in promoting learning. The assumption

that relationship factors are requisite for the

acquisition and modification of social behavior is


refuted by countless studies of social learning. One

can, for example, acquire complex patterns of

social behavior by observing either symbolic or

real life models with whom no prior relationship

has been developed (Bandura, 1965). Moreover,


many responses that are utilized interpersonally
were originally acquired under noninterpersonal

conditions. This transfer process is demonstrated

experimentally by Walters & Brown (1963), who

found that children who were intermittently


reinforced for hitting an automated Bobo doll

subsequently displayed an increase in physically


aggressive behavior toward other children in

thwarting situations.

Relationship experiences are often designated

nonspecific influences and contrasted with various

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 256
learning procedures which are referred to as

specific influences. It is difficult to conceive of

nonspecific influences in social interchanges. Each


expression by one person elicits some type of

response from the other participant, which

inevitably creates a specific reinforcement

contingency that has a specific effect on the

immediately preceding behavior. Numerous


studies of change processes stimulated by social
reinforcement theory disclose that interpersonal

responses have specific and predictable effects on

behavior. It is possible, of course, for a change

agent to display uniformly positive or negative


responses without regard to the behavior of

another person. In such instances, however, it


might be more accurate to characterize the social

interaction as involving indiscriminate, rather

than noncontingent, reinforcement. It has been


shown by Hart, Reynolds, Baer, Brawley, & Harris

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 257
(1968) and others that abundant social

responsiveness provided on such a

“nonconditional” basis can neither create nor


maintain beneficial personality characteristics.

Guideless interest is clearly not enough.

Lest readers conclude that social-learning

approaches neglect relationship variables it

should be emphasized here that, quite the


contrary, social reinforcement processes assume a
role of major importance in the modification and

maintenance of personality patterns. Indeed, it is

research conducted within the social-learning

framework that has shown most conclusively that


relationship experiences can exert powerful
control over behavior. The central issues are,

therefore, whether a social relationship is

regarded as a facilitative or a necessary condition

for learning, and whether it is utilized

ritualistically or considerately to benefit the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 258
recipients. Chapter 4 includes a large body of

empirical evidence demonstrating that grossly

deviant behavior in both children and adults —


including infantile behavior, self-destructive

tendencies, hypochondriacal and delusional

behavior, extreme withdrawal, chronic anorexia,

psychogenic seizures, psychotic tendencies and

other deleterious behaviors— can be eliminated,


reinstated, and substantially increased depending
upon the amount of interest, attention, and

solicitous concern such behaviors elicit from

others. A positive relationship thus has the

potentiality both to help and to harm. The well-


intentioned, benign attitudes frequently advocated

by many theories of personality may actually


foster social reinforcement contingencies that

have injurious consequences; this consideration

suggests that child-rearing, educational, and


therapeutic practices must be evaluated by their

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 259
effects upon recipients rather than by the

humanitarian intent of change agents. Many well-

meaning people who subscribe to these mental


hygiene practices, which have been widely

promulgated over the years, may at times


inadvertently support or even increase the very
problems their earnest efforts are designed to

ameliorate (Harris, Wolf, & Baer, 1964; Gelfand,

Gelfand, & Dobson, 1967; Lovaas, Freitag, Gold, &


Kassorla, 1965).

A principal assumption of most conventional

approaches to treatment is that clients will reenact

in their relationship with the psychotherapist the


maladaptive interpersonal patterns that
characterize their everyday interactions with

significant persons. Once evoked in various

strengths and guises, the inappropriate nature of

these transferred reactions can be demonstrated

and presumably modified within the therapeutic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 260
setting. Alexander (1956), among others,

questioned these assumptions regarding

transference phenomena. He argued that the


marked dissimilarity of the therapy situation and

the social characteristics of the therapist may not

constitute a suitable stimulus for eliciting strong

generalized responses. Hence, many of the clients’

behavioral problems could not be effectively


modified solely in relation to the therapist.
Moreover, those who lead emotionally

impoverished lives often become more interested

in securing positive reinforcement from their

therapists than in solving their interpersonal


problems. Personality changes are further

obstructed if therapists, due to limited satisfaction


in their own nonprofessional relationships, use

their clients as a substitute source of gratification.

For these and other reasons, Alexander


recommended greater utilization and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 261
extratherapeutic relationships for effecting

changes in social behavior.

It is evident from outcome studies reviewed in

Chapter 1 that, whatever clients may reenact with

their psychotherapists, relatively few beneficial


effects of these reenactments trickle down to daily

interpersonal living. Most likely the artificial

relationship provides substitute gratifications for


those lacking in the clients’ natural relationships
instead of serving as a major vehicle for

personality change. Persons would be helped

more fundamentally if their behavior patterns

were modified to enable them to derive greater


satisfactions from their everyday relationships,
thereby making the purchased relationship

unnecessary.

Many psychotherapists who do not subscribe

to the transference theory nevertheless assume

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 262
that a benign, noncontingent attitude toward
clients will produce beneficial personality

changes. Strict adherence to the position that


therapists should be unconditionally accepting is

virtually impossible, as shown in numerous

content analyses (Bandura, Lipsher, & Miller,

1960; Dittes, 1957; Goldman, 1961; Winder,


Ahmad, Bandura, & Rau, 1962). Therapists,

including those who advocate unconditional

positive regard (Murray, 1956; Truax, 1966),

display consistent patterns of approving and

disapproving responses to their clients’ behavior.


Even if unconditional social approval and

acceptance were possible, it would be no more


meaningful as a precondition for change than

noncontingent reinforcement in modifying any


form of behavior. If this principle were, in fact,

applied in child-rearing, parents would respond


approvingly and affectionately when their children

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 263
appeared with stolen goods, behaved

unmanageably in school, physically injured their

siblings and peers, refused to follow any


household routines, and behaved maliciously.

“Unconditional love” would make children


directionless, irresponsible, and completely
unpredictable. Similarly, if researchers practiced

indiscriminate positive reinforcement in

experiments in the process of social learning, they


would undoubtedly obtain meager results.

Perhaps this circumstance is relevant to the

psychotherapy outcome data discussed in the


introductory chapter.

Another corollary of the relationship view is


that psychotherapists should select the methods of

treatment that they feel most comfortable in

employing. If such reasoning guided the practice of

medicine—suppose a patient with a brain tumor

consults a surgeon who feels most comfortable in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 264
performing appendectomies and therefore

extracts the patient’s appendix—a sizeable

portion of the patient population would have long


since departed, while an even larger number

would find themselves in short supply of

convenient anatomical structures. Successful

modification of behavior requires certain learning

conditions. Therefore, in planning a change


program, the primary focus should be on desired
objectives and their requisite conditions rather

than on the comforts of the change agents. This

does not minimize the individual differences in

psychotherapists’ capabilities for creating


different types of learning conditions. Rather it

highlights the necessity of selecting change agents


on the basis of the desired learning outcomes.

The common deemphasis of methods and

objectives also derives from the fact that most

psychotherapists are trained essentially in a single

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 265
treatment approach, which they apply with minor

variations to a wide range of deviant behavior

patterns. Rogerians offer their clients a particular


all-purpose brand of psychotherapy,

psychoanalysts provide a somewhat different

standard brand; similarly, Adlerians, Jungians,

Sullivanians, Gestaltists, existentialists, and

Rankians present still different forms of omnibus


psychotherapy. Since the client must conform to
the method offered rather than having procedures

selected for him in terms of specified objectives,

the treatment he will receive is fortuitously

determined by the school affiliation of his


psychotherapist.

School affiliations not only determine the


range of procedures that a therapist will employ in

his practice; they also define the client’s central

problems, which the techniques of the school are

designed to resolve. Psychoanalysts will uncover

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 266
and resolve Oedipal conflicts; Adlerians will

discover inadequacy problems and alter the

resultant compensatory power striving; Rogerians


will unearth and reduce self-ideal discrepancies;

Rankians will resolve separation anxieties;

existentialists will actively promote awareness of

self-consciousness. Thus in traditional approaches

therapeutic procedures and objectives tend to be


preselected with little reference to the diverse
forms of deviance exhibited by different persons.

Considering the accidental way in which

behavioral deviations are matched with learning

conditions, it is not at all surprising that clients


often terminate therapy after only a few

interviews, and that one cannot determine the


probability of improvement for those who remain.

A social-learning approach does not rely upon a

single set of conditions for effecting personality


changes, but rather it provides, within a unified

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 267
framework, diverse methods for modifying

multiform psychological phenomena.

Psychotherapists who are less strongly

committed to a particular theoretical orientation

generally attempt to vary techniques adopted


from different systems to particular problems.

However, because the literature does not provide

explicit criteria for the choice of different methods,


the range of procedures therapists do possess is
utilized more according to their intuition. These

attempts are therefore less definite, less

comprehensive, and usually less effective than a

program in which particular interventions are


used because of their demonstrated effects on
social behavior.

SELECTION OF OBJECTIVES AND ETHICAL ISSUES OF


BEHAVIORAL CONTROL

Behavioral objectives are frequently

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 268
unspecified in order to avoid acknowledging the
value judgments and social influences involved in

the modification of behavior. Psychotherapists

who subscribe to conversational methods


customarily portray their form of treatment as a

noncontingent social influence process in which


the therapist serves as an unconditionally loving,
permissive, understanding, empathizing catalyst

in the client’s efforts toward self-discovery and

self-actualization. In contrast, behaviorally


oriented psychotherapists are typically depicted

as antihumanistic, Machiavellian manipulators of

human behavior (Jourard, 1961; Patterson, 1963;

Rogers, 1956; Shoben, 1963). In truth, to the

extent that the psychotherapist—regardless of his


theoretical allegiances—has been successful in
modifying his clients’ behavior, he has either

deliberately or unwittingly manipulated the

factors that control it. It is interesting to note in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 269
this connection that conditions that are

undesignedly imposed upon others are generally

regarded with favor, whereas identical conditions


created after thoughtful consideration of their

effects on others are often considered culpable.


There exists no other enterprise which values
incognizance so highly, often at the expense of the

client’s welfare. One suspects that this therapist-

centered value system would change rapidly if


therapeutic contracts required financial

remuneration to be made at least partially

contingent upon the amount of demonstrable


change achieved by clients in the interpersonal

problems for which they seek help.

In view of the substantial research evidence

that psychotherapists serve as models for, and

selective reinforcers of, their clients’ behavior

(Bandura, Lipsher, & Miller, 1960; Goldman, 1961;

Murray, 1956; Rosenthal, 1955; Truax, 1966;

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 270
Winder et al., 1962), it is surprising that many

therapists continue to view the psychotherapeutic

process as one that does not involve behavioral


influence and control.

In later writings, Rogers (1956), a leading

proponent of the anticontrol position, has

acknowledged that psychotherapists do in fact

manipulate and control their clients’ behavior


within the treatment setting. He contends,
however, that this benevolent external control

yields “self-actualized,” “flexible,” and “creatively

adaptive” persons whose post-therapy behavior is


under internal control and no longer subject to the
psychotherapist’s influences. The actual outcomes,

however, are considerably at variance with these


idealized pretensions. A brief comparison of

interview protocols of cases treated by Rogerian

therapists with those of clients seen by therapists

representing differing theoretical orientations

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 271
clearly reveals that, far from being individuated

and self-actualized, the clients have been

thoroughly conditioned and converted to the


belief system, vernacular, and interpretations of

reality favored by their respective

psychotherapists. Such conformity in verbal

behavior is partly achieved through selective

reinforcement. Sequential analyses of verbal


interchanges in cases treated by Rogers revealed
that the therapist consistently approved certain

behaviors and disapproved others (Murray, 1956;

Truax, 1966). As treatment progressed, approved

responses increased in frequency while


disapproved verbalizations diminished.

In the often quoted debate between Rogers


and Skinner (1956) concerning the moral

implications of behavioral control, Rogers

distinguishes among three types of control; this

provides an excellent illustration of the use of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 272
propitious relabeling to minimize the ethical

decisions that confront therapists and other

agents of change. In the first category, designated


as external control, person A creates conditions

that alter person B’s behavior without his

concurrence. The second and presumably more

humanitarian form, labeled influence, involves

processes in which A arranges conditions that


modify B’s behavior, to which he gives some
degree of consent. The distinction between

external control and influence, however, is more

apparent than real. In many instances certain

conditions are imposed upon individuals without


their agreement, knowledge, or understanding,

from which they can later free themselves by


willingly changing their behavior in a direction

subtly prescribed by controlling agents. Thus, for

example, persons who have been legally


committed to mental hospitals or penal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 273
institutions may voluntarily enter into treatment

programs to acquire the types of behavior that will

improve their living circumstances in the


institution and ensure a speedy discharge. A more

fundamental ethical distinction can be made in


terms of whether the power to influence others is
utilized for the advantage of the controller or for

the benefit of the controllee, rather than in terms

of the illusory criterion of willing consent.

Internal control, Rogers’ third category,

involves a process in which a person arranges

conditions so as to manage his own

responsiveness. Although self-monitoring systems


play an influential role in the regulation of human
behavior, they are not entirely independent of

external influences. Self-monitoring systems are

transmitted through modeling and reinforcement

processes. After a person has adopted a set of

behavioral standards for self-evaluation he tends

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 274
to select associates who share similar value

systems and behavioral norms (Bandura &

Walters, 1959; Elkin & Westley, 1955). The


members of his reference group, in turn, serve to

reinforce and to uphold his self-prescribed

standards of conduct. A person who chooses a

small select reference group that does not share

the values of the general public may appear highly


individualistic and “inner- directed,” whereas in
fact he is very much dependent on the actual and

fantasied approval and disapproval of a few

individuals whose judgments he values highly.

During the course of psychotherapy, clients


likewise adopt, through modeling, their therapists’

values, attitudes, and standards of conduct for self-


evaluation (Pentony, 1966; Rosenthal, 1955).

Responsiveness to modeling influences is apt to be

particularly enhanced in a relationship in which a

person has developed a strong positive tie to a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 275
prestigious model (Bandura & Huston, 1961;

Henker, 1964; Mussen & Parker, 1965), a

condition which is emphasized considerably in


most forms of psychotherapy. Studies of modeling

effects further disclose that persons tend to

perform the model’s behavior in his absence

(Bandura & Kupers, 1964; Bandura, Ross, & Ross,

1963), and they respond to new situations in a


manner consistent with the model’s dispositions
even though they have never observed the model’s

behavior in response to the same stimuli (Bandura

& Harris, 1966; Bandura & McDonald, 1963;

Bandura & Mischel, 1965). These findings indicate


that after the model’s attitudes and behavioral

attributes have been adopted, he continues to


influence and indirectly to control the subject’s

actions, though he is no longer physically present.

In fact, in Rogers’ (1951) conceptualization of


maladjustment, introjected parental values are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 276
construed as continuing pathological influences

that maintain disturbing incongruities in the

clients’ self-structure. However, after internalized


parental values are supplanted by adoption of the

therapist’s attitudes and standards, the client is


flatteringly portrayed—by the psychotherapist—
as self-actualized, flexibly creative, and self-

directed!

Much of the controversy between Rogers and


Skinner centers around their own value

preferences for others. Skinner advocates that

people be made “truly happy, secure, productive,

creative, and forward-looking”; Rogers argues in


favor of self-direction and self-actualization of
potentialities as the prescribed objective of social

influence. It might be noted parenthetically that in

the context of proclaiming the self-actualization

objective, Rogers argues vigorously against self-

actualization in Skinnerian directions. The

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 277
leitmotif in this discourse appears to be one of

belief conformity rather than self-realization. As

usually happens in disputes over therapeutic


outcomes, “happiness” and “conformity to societal

norms” are selected as examples of unwholesome

outcomes equated with slothfulness; self-

actualization, on the other hand, is proffered as an

ennobling aim. To balance the evaluative scales, it


should be noted that the self-centered ethic of self-
actualization might be equally questioned on

moral grounds, particularly by innocent victims of

self-actualized despots or less notorious but

selfish, self-directed persons. Universally accepted


goals are difficult to come by because all the

various patterns of behavior enthusiastically


promoted by therapists of different persuasions

can be used to produce inimical human effects.

The most remarkable feature of the foregoing,

seemingly humanistic, rhetoric is that neither

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 278
participant acknowledges that the choice of

behavioral objectives is rightfully the client’s. A

person may seek from therapy neither Skinner’s


security nor a Rogerian conversion in the guise of

self-realization. We shall return shortly to this

issue of value standardization and the inclination

of therapists to impose their own cherished

objectives upon their clients.

Contrary to the beliefs of Rogers, Shoben, and


other critics, behaviorally oriented approaches

usually involve considerably less unnecessary

control and manipulation of attitudes and values


than do the procedures based upon the
psychodynamic model. In the latter treatments,

any behavior, no matter how trivial or apparently


irrelevant, tends to be viewed as a derivative of

concealed psychodynamic forces and is therefore

subject to analysis and reinterpretation in terms of

the therapist’s theoretical predilections. Thus

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 279
virtually no aspect of the client’s life—his social,

marital, and sexual behavior, his political and

religious beliefs, his vocational choice, his child-


training practices—escapes the therapist’s

repeated scrutiny and influence over a period of

several years. Since this approach tends to regard

behavioral difficulties as superficial

manifestations of more fundamental and often


unconscious internal events, influence attempts
are primarily directed toward subject matters of

questionable relevance. It is not uncommon,

therefore, to find clients whose belief systems

have been thoroughly modified despite little


amelioration of the behavioral difficulties for

which they originally sought help.

In contrast, behaviorally oriented therapists

generally confine their therapeutic efforts to the

behavioral problems presented by the client.

These are labeled as learned styles of behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 280
rather than as expressions of esoteric unconscious

processes or as manifestations of mental disease.

Moreover, the procedures and objectives are


undisguised, the treatment is typically of shorter

duration, and clearly goal-directed. To be sure,

within this highly structured interaction, the

therapist must exercise responsible control over

conditions affecting relevant segments of the


client’s behavior if he is to fulfill his therapeutic
obligations. In this type of approach, however, the

psychotherapist is less inclined to condition and to

shape his client’s belief systems in accordance

with his own views. Paradoxical as it may seem,


the psychotherapists who pride themselves on

being nonmanipulative and noncontrolling are,


albeit unwillingly, often engaged in a more

disguised and manipulative enterprise than is true

of most behaviorally oriented practitioners. It


should be made clear, however, that behavioral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 281
principles do not dictate the manner in which they

are applied. Undoubtedly some behavioral

therapists encroach on people’s rights to decide


the direction in which their behavior will be

modified, and act as therapeutic agents devoid of


consideration and regard for values.

ESTABLISHMENT OF FREEDOM OF CHOICE


THROUGH BEHAVIORAL APPROACHES

Discussions of the moral implications of

behavioral control almost always emphasize the

Machiavellian role of change agents and the self-


protective maneuvers of controllees. The fact that

most persons enter treatment only as a last resort,

hoping to modify patterns of behavior that are

seriously distressful to themselves or to others, is

frequently overlooked. To the extent that


therapists engage in moral agonizing, they should
fret more about their own limited effectiveness in

helping persons who are willing to undergo

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 282
financial hardships to achieve desired changes,
than in fantasizing about their potential powers.

The tendency to exaggerate the powers of


behavioral control by psychological methods

alone, irrespective of willing cooperation by the

client, and the failure to recognize the reciprocal

nature of interpersonal control obscure both the


ethical issues and the nature of social influence

processes.

In discussing moral and practical issues of

behavioral control it is essential to recognize that

social influence is not a question of imposing

controls where none existed before. All behavior is

inevitably controlled, and the operation of


psychological laws cannot be suspended by

romantic conceptions of human behavior, any


more than indignant rejection of the law of gravity

as antihumanistic can stop people from falling. As


Homme and Tosti (1965) point out, “either one

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 283
manages the contingencies or they get managed by

accident. Either way there will be contingencies,

and they will have their effect [p. 16].” The process
of behavior change, therefore, involves

substituting new controlling conditions for those

that have regulated a person’s behavior. The basic

moral question is not whether man’s behavior will

be controlled, but rather by whom, by what


means, and for what ends.

The primary criterion that one might apply in

judging the ethical implications of social influence

approaches (Kelman, 1965) is the degree to which


they promote freedom of choice. It should be
added, however, that if individualism is to be

guaranteed, it must be tempered by a sense of


social obligation. Custodial institutions created by

societies are highly populated with socially

injurious individualists. A person’s freedom of

self-expression can be restricted in several ways,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 284
each of which presents somewhat different ethical

problems in the reestablishment of self-

determination.

Self-restraints in the form of conditioned

inhibitions and self-censuring responses often

severely curtail a person’s effective range of

behaviors and the types of options that they are

likely to consider for themselves. In many


instances, for example, persons are unable to
participate freely in potentially rewarding social

interactions because of severe phobias; they are

unable to engage in achievement, aggressive and


heterosexual activities; and they deny themselves
socially permissible gratification because of

austere, self-imposed standards of conduct.


Treatment programs designed to reduce rigid self-

restraints are rarely viewed as ethically

objectionable, since they tend to restore

spontaneity and freedom of choice among various

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 285
options of action. Ethical issues arise only if a

change agent uses his influence selfishly or to

make his clients socially irresponsible.

Behavioral deficits also greatly restrict freedom

of choice and otherwise curtail opportunities for

self-direction. Persons’ positions in various status

and power hierarchies are to a large extent

determined by their social, educational, and


vocational competencies. The degree of control
that one can exercise over one’s own activities, the

power to form and to modify one’s environment,

and the accessibility to, and control over, desired


resources increase with higher status positions.
Persons who have developed superior intellectual

and vocational capabilities enjoy a wide latitude of


occupational choices; they are granted

considerable freedom to regulate both their own

activities and the behavior of others; and they

have the financial means of obtaining additional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 286
privileges that further increase their autonomy. By

contrast, high school dropouts who lack

sociovocational proficiencies are relegated to a


subordinate status, in which not only is their

welfare subject to arbitrary external controls, but

they are irreversibly channeled into an economic

and social life that further restricts their

opportunities to use their potentialities and to


affect their own life circumstances. Eliminating
such behavioral deficits can substantially increase

the level of self-determination in diverse areas of

social functioning.

Societally imposed restrictions on freedom of


self-expression occur as responses to deviant

behavior that violates legal codes. Chronic


alcoholics, drug addicts, sexual deviates,

delinquents, psychotics, and social nonconformists

and activists may have their liberties revoked for

fixed or indefinite periods when their public

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 287
actions are judged to be socially detrimental and

therefore to be subject to social control. Special

ethical problems are most likely to arise wherever


restoration of his freedom is made contingent

upon the individual’s relinquishing socially

prohibited patterns of behavior. If an agent of

change acts in opposition to the society which

supports him institutionally, then he evades his


broader social responsibilities with which he has
been entrusted. If, on the other hand, he imposes

conditions upon his captive clients designed to

force conformity to social norms, he is subverting

the client’s right to choose how he shall live his


life. These moral dilemmas are less difficult to

resolve in cases where the person’s behavior


injures or infringes the freedom of others. Such

persons have the choice of regaining their

autonomy by undergoing changes within a broad


range of socially tolerated alternatives, or setting

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 288
no limits on their own behavior and having society

restrict them to institutions. The ethical dilemma

is more serious when conventional norms are


questioned by many members of society and new

standards of behavior are advocated. Today there


are open controversies over the morality of
homosexuality, premarital sexual intercourse, use

of nonaddictive drugs, civil disobedience to unjust

rules, and many forms of social behavior that are


publicly defined as illegal. In such cases as these,

therapeutic agents may support changes in

socially prescribed directions or give legitimacy to


deviant patterns, depending upon the social and

personal consequences of the behavior, the client’s


preferences, and the therapist’s own value

orientation.

Most people whose freedom is curtailed by

societally imposed restrictions and who

voluntarily seek psychotherapeutic help are not

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 289
that strongly wedded to deviant behavior; but

because it is powerfully reinforcing, or because

they lack more satisfying alternatives, they have


difficulty relinquishing it. The establishment of

self-control and the reduction of positive valences

associated with deviant activities may sometimes

require the use of aversive procedures as part of

the treatment program. The use of aversive


methods is apt to be criticized as being, if not anti-
therapeutic, then certainly antihumanistic. But is it

not far more humanitarian to offer the client a

choice of undergoing a brief, painful experience to

eliminate self-injurious behavior, or of enduring


over many years the noxious, and often

irreversible, consequences that will inevitably


result if his behavior remains unaltered?

Restrictions of behavioral freedom arise also

from socially sanctioned discrimination. In such

cases a person’s freedom is curtailed because of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 290
his skin color, his religion, his ethnic background,

his social class, or other secondary characteristics.

When a person’s warranted self-determination is


externally restricted by prejudicial social

practices, the required changes must be made at

the social systems level.

It is often mistakenly assumed (London, 1964)

that traditional psychotherapies fervently


embrace humanism whereas behavioral
approaches, for reasons never explicated, are

supposedly uninterested in the moral implications

of their practices or are antagonistic toward


humanistic values. In fact, behavioral therapy is a
system of principles and procedures and not a

system of ethics. Its methods, and any other


effective procedures for that matter, can be

employed to threaten human freedom and dignity

or to enhance them.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 291
When freedom is discussed in the abstract it is

generally equated with nondeterminism;

conversely, automatonism is associated with a


deterministic position. Whether freedom and

determinism are compatible or irreconcilable


depends upon the manner in which causal
processes are conceptualized. According to

prevailing theories of personality, human actions

are either impelled from within by concealed


forces or externally predetermined. If individuals

were merely passive reactors to external

influences, then their behavior would be


inevitable; it would be absurd to commend them

for their achievements or to penalize them for


their transgressions. It would be more sensible,

from this point of view, to praise and to chastise

the external determinants. But since these events


are also unavoidably determined by prior

conditions, the analysis results in an infinite

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 292
regression of causes. Some degree of freedom is

possible within a deterministic view if it is


recognized that a person’s behavior is a

contributing factor to subsequent causal events. It

will be recalled from the previous discussion of


reciprocal influence processes that individuals

play an active role in creating their own

controlling environment.

From a social-learning point of view freedom is


not incompatible with determinism. Rather a

person is considered free insofar as he can partly

influence future events by managing his own

behavior. One could readily demonstrate that a


person can, within the limits of his behavioral
capabilities and environmental options, exercise

substantial control over his social life by having

him plan and systematically carry out radically

different courses of action on alternate days.

Granted that the selection of a particular course of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 293
behavior from available alternatives is itself the

result of determining factors, a person can

nevertheless exert some control over the variables


that govern his own choices. Indeed, increasing

use is being made of self-control systems (Ferster,

Nurnberger, & Levitt, 1962; Harris, 1969; Stuart,

1967) in which individuals regulate their activities

to fulfill their own wishes by deliberate self-


management of reinforcement contingencies. The
self-control process begins by informing

individuals of the types of behaviors they will have

to practice to produce desired outcomes, of ways

in which they can institute stimuli to increase the


occurrence of requisite performances, and of how

they can arrange self-reinforcing consequences to


sustain them. Behavioral change procedures that

involve role enactment also depend upon the self-

determination of outcomes through clients’


regulation of their own behavior and the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 294
environmental contingencies that reciprocally

influence it. Contrary to common belief, behavioral

approaches not only can support a humanistic


morality, but because of their relative

effectiveness in establishing self-determination


these methods hold much greater promise than
traditional procedures for enhancement of

behavioral freedom and fulfillment of human

capabilities.

BEHAVIORAL OBJECTIVES IN THE MODIFICATION


OF INTERNAL STATES AND COMPLEX
DYSFUNCTIONS

Thus far the failure to orient treatment to


desired behavioral outcomes has been attributed
to the prevalence of all-purpose single-method

therapies, to reliance on benign relationship

factors to produce diverse changes, and to

reluctance to acknowledge the issues of values and

behavioral control involved in the modification of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 295
social behavior. The failure to specify objectives in
behavioral terms also stems in part from the view

that, in many cases, internal psychic states may


constitute the major problems requiring

modifications. These conditions are usually

defined in such broad terms as unhappiness,

absence of meaning and purpose in life, and


feelings of worthlessness. Before speculating on

how phenomenological events can be most

effectively altered, it should be noted that it is

highly fashionable to construe one’s concrete

behavioral problems in abstract, cosmic terms. It


is understandably less distressing to present one’s

plight as a manifestation of social maladies of


alienation, exploitation, or dehumanization than it

is to acknowledge despairing personal


shortcomings, evident heterosexual inadequacies,

intellectual failures, lack of vocational ingenuity


and productivity, and inability to form satisfying

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 296
interpersonal relationships.

Abstract problems such as “unhappiness,” and


“purposelessness” cannot be successfully modified

by any form of treatment as long as they remain

disconnected from their concrete experiential


determinants. A person does not feel abstractly

unhappy; he is most likely distressed about

specific problems arising from his mode of


functioning in social, vocational, sexual, or familial
areas. After the contributing conditions have been

identified, an appropriate treatment program can

be devised. The principal difficulty in modifying

complex conditions is not that behavioral


approaches are inapplicable, but that the
psychological phenomenon is generally described

in global abstract terms and the constituent

determinants are never clearly specified.

Greatest progress will be made in the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 297
successful treatment of so-called complex
disorders when they are conceptualized, not as

nebulous general states, but as psychological


conditions involving multiple problems with

varying degrees of interdependence. From this

perspective, altering complex behavioral

dysfunctions does not require radically different


methods from those applied to the modification of

single disorders. This issue can perhaps be

illustrated by considering learning deficits. A child

may have developed satisfactory academic skills in

all areas except mathematics. Another child is


grossly deficient in mathematics and in other

academic skills, lacks social behavior skills that


would enable him to maintain satisfying

interpersonal relationships, and has not developed


motor competencies required for play activities.

There exists no single nonspecific treatment that


can simultaneously create competencies in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 298
intellectual, linguistic, social, and motoric areas of

functioning. Separate programs would have to be

devised for each type of problem. But the


procedures used to develop arithmetic

competencies would be essentially the same in the


single-problem and the multiple-problem case.
This is precisely the approach employed by Lovaas

(1967) in establishing language functions,

interpersonal capabilities, and intellectual skills,


and in eliminating grossly bizarre behavior in

autistic children who present, in extreme forms,

one of the most generalized and complex


psychological disorders that therapists are called

upon to treat. Additional examples of successful


modification of multiform problems through

specific diverse treatments is provided by

Patterson & Brodsky (1967), and by Risley & Wolf


(1966). The developments in behavioral therapy

in some respects parallel those in medicine, where

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 299
global all-purpose treatments of limited efficacy

were eventually replaced by powerful specific

procedures for treating particular physical


disorders.

The behavioral change process is not as


piecemeal as the preceding remarks might imply.

Most psychological functions are at least partially

interdependent. Therefore, desirable changes in


one area of behavior may produce beneficial
modifications in other areas not directly involved

in the treatment program. Often, as will be shown

later, a relatively circumscribed problem has

widespread social consequences; and a change in a


specific deviant behavior can have pervasive
psychological effects.

If the major aim of therapy is the modification

of phenomenological events, the empirical

question remains how such changes can be made

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 300
most successfully. Some theorists hold that
behavior is essentially a byproduct of

phenomenological experiences; therefore they


select the latter events as the major subject matter

of therapeutic conversations. According to social-

learning theory, self-descriptions and

phenomenological experiences are partly by-


products of behaviorally produced outcomes.

People, for example, who lack the social and

vocational competencies required for meeting

environmental demands, and who resort to

defective coping strategies will undoubtedly


engender numerous adverse consequences, which

will give rise to despondency, negative self-


evaluations, and other subjective distresses.

Similarly, those who derive inadequate positive


reinforcement from their vocational and

interpersonal activities will experience feelings of


purposelessness and alienation. From a social-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 301
learning perspective, phenomenological and other

internal events can be more effectively modified

through behavioral changes and the feedback from


resulting consequences than through conventional

interview procedures.

A laboratory study conducted by Keister

(1938) illustrates bow phenomenological events

can be altered by feedback from a series of


carefully guided mastery experiences. The author
selected a group of children who exhibited

extreme maladaptive tendencies, including

withdrawal, destructiveness, sulking and crying,

and expressions of feelings of helplessness when


faced with problem-solving tasks. Keister did not
obtain ratings of the children’s self-concepts, but it

is highly probable that, as a result of repeated

failure experiences, these children would

eventually evaluate themselves in negative terms.

In the treatment program the children solved a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 302
series of graded problems that grew progressively

more difficult, thus ensuring a gradual build-up of

skill in coping with increasingly difficult tasks. In


addition, the experimenter consistently rewarded

the children’s successful solutions and persistent

task-oriented behavior. A pre- and post-test

comparison of the children’s responses to

exceedingly difficult tasks showed that the success


experiences were highly effective in replacing the
formerly maladaptive tendencies with

constructive, confidence-producing behavior.

Because cognitive and attitudinal changes have


rarely been systematically assessed in
behaviorally oriented programs, it is generally

assumed that these types of treatment approaches


alter only specific behavioral functioning. Several

experiments have recently been designed

especially to provide empirical evidence of the

affective and cognitive consequences of behavioral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 303
changes. Bandura, Blanchard, and Ritter (1969)

found that elimination of phobic behavior was

accompanied by marked attitudinal changes


toward previously feared situations. In addition,

disturbing emotional responsiveness not only

toward the phobic stimulus but toward situations

beyond the specifically treated condition was

substantially reduced. In a preliminary study,


Wahler and Pollio (1968) similarly demonstrated
that behavioral changes produced in a boy

through selective social reinforcement altered

favorably his evaluations of himself and others. As

might be expected, his evaluation of events closely


related to the treatment objectives changed most

markedly.

Not only are self-attitudes and feeling states

fundamentally affected by behaviorally produced

experiences, but a favorable change also gains the

person acceptance and increased social status

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 304
(Hastorf, 1965). The positive social feedback

engendered by behavioral competence can thus

have important phenomenological consequences.


In subsequent chapters research evidence will be

presented showing that cognitive and affective

modifications can be achieved more successfully

through planned behavioral change than through

attempts to alter internal events directly. The


relative superiority of a behaviorally oriented
approach probably stems from the fact that a basic

change in behavior provides an objective and

genuine basis by which one feels self-respect, self-

confidence, and dignity.

INSIGHT AS A THERAPEUTIC OBJECTIVE

Most traditional approaches to psychotherapy

consider the achievement of insight or self-

awareness to be a prerequisite for the production

of widely generalized and enduring behavioral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 305
changes. Therefore, development of insight
constitutes one of the primary objectives of

interview strategies. For this reason, among the


numerous technical issues discussed in

expositions of psychotherapeutic procedures,

those pertaining to timing and depth of

interpretations, methods for channeling


verbalizations into areas assumed to be conflict-

laden, strategies for handling clients’ resistances,

and explanations of the possible symbolic

significance of verbal and nonvocal responses

have all received considerable attention.

In therapeutic practice, the development of

insight is largely accomplished by therapists


repeatedly interpreting the verbal, affective, and

social responses that their clients report or exhibit


within the treatment setting. A number of

authorities have proposed rules for the optimal


level of interpretive responses for promoting

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 306
insights. According to Rogers (1951), for example,

clients will engage in progressively deeper self-

exploration provided that therapists label only the


feelings that are expressed more or less explicitly.

On the other hand, Fenichel (1941) and other

advocates of psychoanalytical procedures

recommend that therapists proceed slightly

beyond what the client is able to accept and


experience emotionally at any given time. By
contrast, Klein (1960), Berg (1947), and Rosen

(1953), among others, contend that rapid and

fundamental personality changes can be achieved

only by deep interpretations of internal processes


of which the client is completely unaware.

Research bearing on this issue (Collier, 1953;


Dittmann, 1952; Harway, Dittmann, Raush, Bordin,

& Rigler, 1955) has been mainly concerned with

attempts to scale the depth of therapists’


interpretive responses, which are typically rated

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 307
on a continuum ranging from superficial

restatements of clients’ remarks to suggestions of

causal relationships and psychological events that


are entirely foreign to clients’ views of themselves.

In addition, client- therapist verbal interchanges


have been occasionally analyzed in an effort to
establish relationships between variations in

therapists’ interpretive responses and different

verbal indices of therapeutic progress (Colby,


1961; Dittmann, 1952; Frank & Sweetland, 1962;

Speisman, 1959).

Despite the lack of consensus regarding

optimal interpretive procedures, it is generally


assumed that through skillful labeling of repressed
strivings, which manifest themselves in various

derivative forms, the unconscious determinants of

the client’s behavior are gradually made

conscious. After these unconscious events are

brought into awareness they presumably cease to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 308
function as powerful instigators of behavior, or

they become more susceptible to cognitively

mediated control. Hence it is believed that with


the achievement of insight, flexible, voluntarily

guided behavior replaces automatic,

indiscriminate responding.

Although the acquisition of insight is

considered an essential goal of treatment and


supposedly results in a wide variety of beneficial
effects, insight has never been adequately defined

(Zilboorg, 1952), nor has the manner in which it

supposedly mediates behavioral change ever been


clearly specified or demonstrated. Apart from the
difficulties of defining insight, the history of a

client’s behavior is rarely known, and the


reconstructed content of both historical and

contemporary events is highly influenced by the

therapist’s suggestive probing and selective

reinforcement of the client’s verbalizations. Thus,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 309
as Marmor (1962) has pointed out, schools of

psychotherapy have emerged with their own

favored set of hypothetical internal agents, and


their own preferred brand of insight; these can be

readily confirmed by self-validating interview

procedures. For these reasons, psychotherapists of

differing theoretical orientations repeatedly

discover their preferred psychodynamic agents,


but are unlikely to find evidence for the
underlying causes emphasized by their theoretical

rivals:

But what is insight? To a Freudian, it means


one thing, to a Jungian another, to a Rankian,
a Horneyite, an Adlerian or a Sullivanian, still
another. Each school gives its own particular
brand of insight. Whose are the correct
insights? The fact is that patients treated by
analysts of all these schools may not only
respond favorably, but also believe strongly
in the insights which they have been given.
Even admittedly ‘inexact’ interpretations
have been noted to be of therapeutic value!

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 310
Moreover, the problem is even more
complicated than this; for, depending upon
the point of view of the analyst, the patients
of each school seem to bring up precisely the
kind of phenomenological data which
confirm the theories and interpretations of
their analysts! Thus each theory tends to be
self-validating. Freudians elicit material
about the Oedipus Complex and castration
anxiety, Adlerians about masculine strivings
and feelings of inferiority, Horneyites about
idealized images, Sullivanians about
disturbed interpersonal relationships, etc.
The fact is that in so complex a transaction as
the psychoanalytic therapeutic process, the
impact of the patient and the therapist upon
each other, and particularly that of the latter
upon the former, is an unusually profound
one. What the analyst shows interest in, the
kinds of questions he asks, the kind of data
he chooses to react to or to ignore, and the
interpretations he makes, all exert a subtle
but significant suggestive impact upon the
patient to bring forth certain kinds of data in
preference to others [Marmor, 1962, p. 289].

The above assessment of the arbitrariness of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 311
psychotherapeutically derived insights finds some

support in the findings of an experiment

conducted by Heine (1953), in which clients who


had been treated by psychoanalytic, Rogerian, and

Adlerian therapists were asked to specify the

factors responsible for their personality changes.

Although clients treated by therapists of these

different theoretical affiliations reported a similar


degree of improvement, they tended to account for
their behavior in terms of the explanation favored

by their respective therapists. These results, and

other findings that will be cited later, strongly

indicate that the content of a particular client’s


insights and emergent “unconscious” could be

predicted more accurately from knowledge of his


therapist’s theoretical belief system than from the

client’s actual social-learning history.

INSIGHT: A SOCIAL-CONVERSION OR A SELF-


DISCOVERY PROCESS?

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 312
In the preceding section it was suggested that

interpretive activities might be more accurately

represented as a direct social influence rather than


as a process involving delicate levitation of

repressed forces from the region of the client’s


unconscious mind. Psychotherapists’ reports that
their clients have achieved self-awareness

generally mean, in behavioral terms, that clients

have learned to label social stimulus events, past


and present causal sequences, and their own

responses in terms of the theoretical predilections

and language of their psychotherapists. In


traditional practice insight primarily represents a

form of self-evaluative behavior that is


conditionable and extinguishable, as are

nonverbal performances. By subsuming the

development of insight under the broad


framework of social persuasion, much of the

knowledge discovered by experimental social

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 313
psychology can be applied to the understanding of
how therapists induce, alter, and control their

clients’ self-insights—even though, in some cases,

therapists subscribe to such idiosyncratic beliefs

about the conditions governing human behavior as

to strain the broad limits of rationality.

Several factors of the treatment situation

contribute to the process of persuasion,


particularly as it applies to changes in the manner
in which clients construe their own actions and

what determines them. As noted in the preceding

chapter, because of initial selectivity and later

attrition of cases during the course of treatment,


the types of people who seek out and remain in
psychotherapy display personal attributes similar

to those of persons who, in laboratory studies of

conformity, attitude change, and conditionability,

are highly amenable to social influence. In addition

to the selection of persuasible clients, therapists,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 314
by virtue of their advanced training and expertise,

are usually accorded high prestige and credibility.

Views expressed by sources of high credibility


generally exert a stronger influence on recipients’

opinions than those of low credibility sources

(Berg & Bass, 1961; Bergin, 1962; Hovland, Janis,

& Kelley, 1953). Interpretations made by

prestigious psychotherapists are, therefore, more


likely to alter clients’ opinions of themselves than
to produce disbelief or to destroy their confidence

in the therapist.

A closely related factor that seems both to


augment attitudinal conformity and to reduce
discrediting of the psychotherapist is the

ambiguity of the psychotherapeutic situation.


Usually the goals of treatment, if discussed in any

detail, are stated only in general terms; clients are

given only general instructions about the nature of

the therapeutic task and the manner in which the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 315
objectives are to be realized. The therapist often

deliberately strives to remain ambiguous in order

to facilitate inappropriate generalization of


maladaptive patterns of behavior toward the

therapist. Most important, the subject matter of

interpretations is primarily concerned with

inferences about unobservable internal processes

rather than with more objective behavioral events.


Clients would, of course, have no difficulty in
ascertaining the validity of therapists’ judgments

of factual matters; however, clients have little

objective basis for evaluating whether they

possess Oedipus complexes, repressed hostilities,


latent homosexual urges, oral-sadistic drives, and

other esoteric motivational forces whose


identification is further complicated by the fact

that they are often inferred from both the high

incidence and the absence of the same behavior.


Studies of social compliance (Asch, 1952; Berg &

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 316
Bass, 1961) have abundantly documented that

persons can be more easily induced to accept the

opinions of others on subjective and unfamiliar


matters than on interpretations of events for

which objective cues are available. Having altered


their judgments, subjects typically underestimate
the extent of their compliance and the role of

social influence in modifying their opinions

(Rosenthal, 1963).

The fact that psychological treatment promises

relief from the distress occasioned by the client’s

behavioral difficulties also works against his quick

dismissal of insights proffered by the


psychotherapist, who is often sought out as a last
resort. Distress generally facilitates persuasion,

especially if solutions allegedly effective at stress

reduction are also made available (Chu, 1966;

Dabbs & Leventhal, 1966).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 317
In attitude change research the opinions

selected for modification have generally involved

social rather than highly personal matters. A study


by Bergin (1962) of interpretations as persuasive

communications demonstrates that the variables


shown to control social attitudes play a similarly
influential role in altering the self-attitudes that

often concern psychotherapists.

In making interpretations, a therapist


communicates information about the client which

is somewhat discrepant with the client’s view of

himself. The controversy regarding the optimal

depth of interpretation might therefore be recast


in the following form: Can a person’s self-attitudes
be altered more rapidly by presenting him with a

progressive series of mildly discrepant

communications slightly beyond what the client is

willing to accept, or by confronting him with

extremely divergent communications as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 318
recommended by Rosen (1953) and Klein (1960)?

The search for an optimal level of

interpretation may be a fruitless pursuit since,


according to persuasion theory, the effectiveness

of varying degrees of discrepant communications

is highly dependent upon the attributes,

credibility, social prestige, and power of the

communicator. Therapists to whom are attributed


low credibility and prestige, for example, may be
relatively ineffectual in producing attitudinal

changes even though they faithfully adhere to

interpretations that are only moderately at


variance with their clients’ beliefs about
themselves. On the other hand, when

psychotherapists are considered to be a source of


high credibility, and possess power to reward and

punish the client’s behavior, then “deep”

interpretations may be highly influential in

shaping clients’ self-insights. Perhaps this is the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 319
reason why Rosen, who exercises considerable

rewarding and coercive power over his psychotic

patients, finds that deep interpretations produce


rapid attitudinal changes, whereas similar

interpretive strategies by therapists who lack

control over their patients’ environment generally

prove ineffectual. The interactive effects of these

different social variables upon conforming self-


evaluations are most clearly illustrated in Bergin’s
study (1962), which manipulated independently

both credibility of the communicator and degree

of incongruity of interpretations.

In the high credibility condition, college


students were seen individually in the Psychiatry

Department of a medical center by the


experimenter, who was ostensibly director of a

depth personality assessment project. To enhance

further the verisimilitude of the situation, students

were escorted by the clinic receptionist to the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 320
experiment room, which was furnished with,

among other things, psychophysiological

recording equipment, an impressive array of


medical and psychiatric tomes, and a large portrait

of Sigmund Freud.

After the students had rated their

interpersonal characteristics on several rating

scales, they were administered an extensive


battery of psychological tests which were
presented as valid measures of underlying

personality dynamics. In a session conducted

several days later, the experimenter informed the


students that, according to results of the depth
assessment, their level of self-understanding was

quite accurate on all the traits rated except for the


area of masculinity-femininity. They then received,

according to random assignments, interpretations

that depicted them as either moderately, highly, or

extremely more feminine (masculine for girls)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 321
than they judged themselves to be. Later the

students rated themselves again so that changes in

their self-evaluations could be assessed. Students


in the low credibility condition likewise completed

the initial self-ratings, received one of the three

levels of discrepant interpretations concerning

their masculine status, and then repeated the self-

evaluation. In these cases, however, the judgments


were made in a decrepit basement office by a
scrawny youngster on the basis of casual

observation.

The results, presented graphically in Figure 2-


1, show that under high credibility conditions the
more divergent the interpretation the greater the

change in self-attitudes; on the other hand, when


interpretations issued from a source of low

credibility, the amount of attitude change

decreased with increasing discrepancy between

the judgments of the participants.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 322
Figure 2-1. Mean change in self-appraisal considered most
acceptable by subjects as a function of credibility of the
communicator and degree of discrepancy of the
interpretation from subjects’ view of themselves.
Bergin, 1962.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 323
Although the generality of the self-evaluative

conforming behavior cannot be determined from

the findings of the foregoing study, it nevertheless


suggests strongly that people are willing to adopt

erroneous underlying attributes suggested to


them by prestigious specialists. It might be
supposed that the persuasive efforts of

psychotherapists would be especially effective

because the same interpretations are made


repeatedly during prolonged treatment and are

directed not only toward assumed unconscious

determinants but also toward clients’ resistances


against the prompted insights.

Suggestive communications offered by


prestigious agents under conditions of ambiguity

and high personal distress may be well suited for

imparting insights to clients, but after the self-

beliefs have been socially induced their

maintenance is strongly governed by existing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 324
conditions of reinforcement. Results of

innumerable verbal conditioning experiments and

analyses of client-therapist interactions, which


have been cited earlier, furnish ample evidence

that psychotherapists selectively reinforce

conformity to their own opinions about the causes

of behavior, and that clients can readily secure

their therapists’ appreciation and approval by


reiterating the appropriate insights.

It would seem from the findings presented

above that interpretive psychotherapies may

primarily represent a conversion of the client to


the therapist’s point of view rather than a process
of self-discovery. It is not surprising, therefore,

that insight can be achieved without helping the


client with the difficulties for which the client

originally sought help. There is no reason to

expect, for example, that a stutterer converted to

Freudianism, Jungianism, Existentialism,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 325
Behaviorism—or to any other theoretical system

—will begin to speak fluently. His stuttering is

more likely to be eliminated by necessary


relearning experiences than by the gradual

discovery of predetermined insights. To account

for the lack of relationship between insight and

social behavior, different varieties of insights have

been distinguished. On the one hand, there is


“intellectual insight,” which is believed to exist
when cognitive responses are present but the

accompanying social or emotional behavior is

absent. Then there is “emotional insight” which is

typically defined in terms of the effects which it


presumably causes: If the client exhibits

behavioral changes, he has achieved emotional


insight; if he fails to modify his social behavior

then he has simply acquired intellectual insight.

While the view that insight is a prerequisite of

behavioral change is widely accepted, some

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 326
theorists (Alexander, 1963) have considered

insight to be a consequence of change, rather than

its determinant. That is, as anxieties are


progressively reduced through the permissive

conditions of the treatment situation, formerly

inhibited thoughts are gradually restored to

awareness. In recent years, however, many

therapists have become increasingly skeptical


about the value of insights regarding hypothetical
psychodynamic events. The ethical and empirical

questions that have been raised with respect to

interpretive modes of therapy would apply equally

to behavioral approaches if these used interview


procedures similarly to teach clients to construe

their psychological functioning in behavioral


terms and did not effect any significant changes in

the personality problems for which the clients

sought aid.

Although insight into presumed psychic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 327
determinants of interpersonal responses is of

questionable validity and has little effect on

behavior, considerable experimental evidence,


which will be reviewed in the concluding chapter,

suggests that awareness of response-

reinforcement contingencies can markedly

influence overt performances. Unlike the arbitrary

and enigmatic nature of psychodynamic events,


the controlling function of environmental
contingencies is readily demonstrable and

amenable to testing and verification.

BEHAVIORAL OBJECTIVES AND “POSITIVE MENTAL


HEALTH”

Discussions of psychotherapeutic and

socialization practices customarily decry the lack

of consensus among social scientists as to what


constitutes “positive mental health.” Underlying

this concern for agreement is the belief that


behavioral change principles cannot be judiciously

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 328
applied until an adequate conception of mental
health and the nature of the “good life” is

developed. The fact that a universal conception of


mental health would require value

standardization is usually obscured by the

abstract nature of the discourse. On the other

hand, when the issues are cast in a more specific


form, it becomes apparent that the search for

uniform criteria of “good” functioning is not only a

fruitless pursuit; it is also one that would raise

serious ethical concerns if standards were ever

officially adopted and imposed on the populace.


Who is to prescribe what is the “healthiest”

occupational activity, the “healthiest” political or


religious belief, the “healthiest” style of living, the

“healthiest” form of marital or social relationships,


or the “healthiest” artistic preferences?

People differ widely across social groups and


over time in their views of the ideal pattern of life.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 329
Indeed, as noted in the introductory chapter,

modes of behavior that are judged abnormal and a

source of distress in one social group may be


regarded as commendable and emulative in

another subculture. In a society that values

individualism the “good life” may assume a wide

variety of acceptable patterns. Although some

common elements might be abstracted from the


heterogeneity, the distillation would most likely
yield a set of general bland attributes. Social

scientists can make their greatest contributions in

the ethical domain by assessing the consequences

of different styles of life. Such information would


provide others with useful bases for making value

choices.

Decision Processes in the Selection of


Objectives

A frequent objection to behavioral approaches

is that the people are often unaware that their

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 330
behavior is being modified, and verbal

conditioning studies are typically cited as

evidence. This portrayal of controlling power may


be flattering, but in fact it is exceedingly difficult to

influence the behavior of another person without

his awareness and concurrence. Indeed, as has

been pointed out elsewhere (Bandura, 1962),

verbal conditioning experiments actually


demonstrate the relative weakness of subtle
influence attempts. In the typical verbal

conditioning study, the response class to be

modified is not identified for the subject and the

experimenter purposely employs subtle verbal


and nonverbal reinforcers (e.g., “good,” “right,”

nods, smiles, and other gestures) so that the


subject will have difficulty in recognizing the

response-reinforcement contingency. Under these

circumstances subjects who discern the basis


upon which reinforcement is administered

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 331
produce incremental changes in the critical

responses, whereas those who remain unaware

generally show no conditioning effects at all. If, on


the other hand, the experimenter were to select

attractive incentives and specified what behavior


would be rewarded, it is safe to predict that
subjects would produce the desired responses at

asymptotic level almost instantaneously.

The psychological fascination with subtle and


disguised social influence processes, and the

comparative ineffectiveness of these procedures,

are also demonstrated by the short-lived interest

in experimentation on subliminal perception. The


initial studies generated considerable public alarm
that behavioral scientists had paved a freeway to

the “unconscious mind,” thus providing hidden

persuaders of Madison Avenue a means of

trafficking in subliminal messages that could

shape and control the interests, attitudes, and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 332
social actions of persons without their awareness.

This picture is further reinforced by popular

descriptions of the potentialities of psychological


control conjuring up macabre associations of 1984

and Brave New World, in which people are

dominated by occult technocrats who possess

awesome methods of behavioral control. Some

state legislatures even enacted laws designed to


control the potential controllers. Research
evidence, as usual, introduced a sobering note into

extravagant fantasies. Investigations of subliminal

stimulation clearly showed that stimuli at

supraliminal levels have more pronounced effects


.upon subjects’ behavior than stimuli that are

below the threshold of awareness (McConnell,


Cutler, & McNeil, 1958). Subliminal stimulation

either produces no behavioral changes or, at most,

weak and fragmentary ones.

Nevertheless, the conduct of change programs

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 333
in shadowy ambiguity is sometimes recommended

on the assumption that persons’ awareness of

influence attempts will not only arouse interfering


counter control behavior, but will also reduce the

potency of reinforcing stimuli. Although these

assumptions may have some validity in situations

where the influence attempts are designed

primarily to induce persons to perform actions


contrary to their interests and value systems (e.g.,
advertising, political persuasion), they are less

appropriate for situations in which the learner

selects his own objectives. In fact, awareness of

and commitment to specified outcomes that are


shared by agents of change tend to enhance

positive evaluation of change agents’ efforts and to


facilitate the acceptance of their influence.

DECISIONAL RESPONSIBILITIES OF CHANGE


AGENTS AND CLIENTS

The ethical implications of behavioral control

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 334
cannot be discussed meaningfully without
specifying the scope of decision-making behavior

of both the client and the change agent. In any type


of social influence enterprise there exist two basic

decision systems. One set of decisions pertains to

the selection of goals; these decisions require

value judgments. The second set of decisions,


which involve empirical issues, relates to the

selection of specific procedures for achieving

selected goals. In the latter domain the agent of

change must be the decision-maker, since the

client is in no position to prescribe the learning


contingencies necessary for the modification of his

behavior. But though the change agent determines


the means by which specified outcomes can be

achieved, the client should play a major role in


determining the directions in which his behavior

is to be modified. To the extent that the client


serves as the primary decision-maker in the value

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 335
domain, the ethical questions that are frequently

raised concerning behavioral control become

pseudo issues.

When the client wishes to change a limited

range of deviant behavior, the objectives are self-


evident and the change agent can proceed with

treatment as soon as the learning experiences

appropriate to the desired outcomes have been


specified. More often, however, because clients are
uncertain about the benefits they hope to derive

from treatment, or because their goals are stated

too broadly, the identification of relevant

outcomes must constitute the initial objective of


the program. In such instances it is necessary to
conduct a thorough behavioral analysis in order to

identify the social conditions governing the client’s

response patterns and the range of behavioral and

situational modifications likely to promote the

desired psychological changes. After possible

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 336
alternative courses of action and their probable

consequences are specified, the client can

participate in the selection of his treatment


outcomes. This decisional process is not unlike

medical diagnosis in which a patient desires relief

from pain but cannot specify the cause of pain or a

remedy for it. Rather, the therapist must detect the

factors producing pain and indicate the chances


for immediate and long-term benefits from
alternative remedial interventions. Once the

patient has selected one of the alternatives, he not

only expects but demands that the therapist

manipulate and control events to accomplish the


desired relief. A physician who fails to assume full

control over the progress of treatment may be


charged with malpractice. On the other hand,

serious ethical problems would arise if a patient

consulting a medical specialist were promptly


subjected to radical surgical or medical

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 337
procedures without his concurrence based on a

clear understanding of the manner in which his

physical status was to be modified. Although the


preceding example has focused on the ethical

implications of therapeutic work, analogous


decision processes and value issues are involved
when a person consults lawyers, architects,

bankers, and other societal agents who possess

the power to influence by reason of their


expertise. Until recently the major obstacle to

serious use of a decisionmaking approach such as

this in behavioral change endeavors is that the


treatment alternatives were limited and the

outcomes uncertain.

It would be naive to assume that agents of

change play no role whatsoever in the

determination of goals. In psychotherapy, for

example, in order not to influence the client’s

choice of behavior, a therapist would be forced to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 338
conduct with aloof objectivity an exhaustive

survey of all possible alternative outcomes from

which the client could make his choice. In practice,


however, only a few feasible objectives are likely

to be examined and compared. The

psychotherapist’s value orientation may partly

determine not only the range and types of

outcomes selected for consideration but also the


relative emphasis given to the probable
consequences associated with the various

alternatives. Thus some encroachment on the

client’s decision-making primacy in the value

domain is inevitable. If the change agent’s value


preferences are explicitly identified as his

personal biases and not represented to the client


as scientific truths, this problem is much less

serious. If values were stated more explicitly,

clients would be more inclined to select therapists


on the basis of similar moral commitments and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 339
might well be more receptive to the therapist’s

influence.

Occasionally a person may select goals that the

change agent has no desire to promote because

the intended outcomes conflict with his basic


values or he lacks skill in the methods necessary

for attaining the chosen objectives. In such cases

he may refuse to participate in the treatment or, if


the desired changes seem appropriate, he may
refer the client elsewhere.

Special problems in goal selection also arise


when persons are confused over their own values
and purposes, or when they exhibit severe deficits

in reality-oriented behavior and low capacity for

communication. It might be questioned whether

such persons are capable of selecting meaningful

objectives for themselves. Fairweather, Sanders,

Maynard, and Cressler (1969) have shown in their

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 340
work with chronic schizophrenics that such
individuals can successfully participate in the

selection of personal goals provided the


alternatives are defined in comprehensible terms

of performance and the clients are given

responsibility for decision-making that affects

their daily lives. Some grossly deviant persons, of


course, may refuse to seek modifications of any

sort. Often they constitute threats to themselves or

to the welfare of others. If such persons are

unwilling to assist in selecting treatment goals, it

does not mean that one should abandon treatment


attempts. Sometimes it is necessary to assume that

the person cannot exercise sufficient control over


his behavior and to hope that, with appropriate

interventions, the person will reach a state of


aware self-interest in which he will desire further

modifications within a broad range of societally


tolerated alternatives.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 341
REDEFINITION OF CLIENT’S OBJECTIVES

The preceding discussion has been mainly


concerned with problems created by uncertainties

about what people wish to gain from treatment. A

far more prevalent, but largely ignored ethical

issue, is raised by therapists’ unilateral


redefinition of the goals presented by the clients.

This revision of the therapeutic contract occurs


most frequently in approaches that focus major
attention not on the behavior of the client but on

inferential inner states. The therapist usually takes


the position that the client does not know what his

real problems are and that they can be revealed

only through a protracted series of interpretive

interviews; the client’s behavioral problems are

normally underrated as superficial derivatives of


underlying conditions that are believed to be most

effectively modified through the achievement of


insight. After restructuring the central problem

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 342
the therapist pursues objectives that are often

quite different from those originally sought by the

client. If the client has been sufficiently convinced


that he is resolving more generic problems his

behavioral difficulties assume secondary

importance in the course of therapy, so that even if

they are not modified, he supposes the contract to

be fulfilled. Insight has been attained.

A therapeutic contract involves an obligation


on the part of the therapist to modify the problems

presented by his clients. A therapist may market a

particular brand of insight without raising ethical


objections provided he adds two important
qualifiers: First, he informs his clients that the

insights they are likely to attain reflect his own


belief system and second, that attaining them is

apt to have little impact on the behavioral

difficulties that brought the client to treatment. It

is evident from the results of interpretive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 343
approaches that a therapist who leads his clients

to believe that insight will alleviate their

behavioral malfunctioning is unlikely to


accomplish the changes he implies.

SEQUENTIAL DECISION-MAKING

Decisions about objectives are not irrevocable.


Consequences resulting from behavioral changes

representing the initially selected outcomes may


lead to revision of subsequent aims. The initial

objectives should be assigned a provisional status

in order to provide the client opportunities to


experiment with new behaviors and to experience
their consequences; then he can decide whether

he wishes to pursue further the chosen course of

action. Moreover, during the course of treatment,

previously ignored areas of behavioral functioning

may become more important than original goals.

Whenever this situation arises the treatment

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 344
program can be easily reoriented toward new
objectives and appropriate learning experiences.

By retaining flexibility in the selection, sequencing,


and timing of objectives, the treatment program

remains highly sensitive to feedback from

resultant changes and the therapist is less inclined

to invoke an extended moratorium on behavioral


modification while he searches for the

fundamental objective. Preoccupation with the

accurate identification of the core problem reflects

a remnant of the revivalist view of

psychopathology, according to which diverse


interpersonal problems are presumed to stem

from a central pathogenic experience. It is further


believed that interpretive revival and abreaction

of the core trauma will result in rapid and widely


generalized personality changes.

Contrary to the latter view, investigations of


the social-learning process (Bandura & Walters,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 345
1963) provide considerable evidence that deviant

behavior is typically controlled by diverse

variables and is not generated by a single


pathogenic agent. Successful treatment therefore

requires the selection and attainment of a variety

of specific objectives rather than a single omnibus

outcome. The extent to which changes in one

system of behavior affect other areas of


functioning will be partly determined by the
similarity of the two systems and by the degree to

which the altered behavior brings the client into

contact with new role models and with new

patterns of reinforcement.

SELECTION OF CHANGE AGENTS AND THE LOCUS


OF TREATMENT

After the goals and requisite learning


experiences have been established, another set of

decisions arises in the selection of change agents


who, by virtue of their specialized training or close

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 346
relationship with the client, are best suited to
implement treatment procedures. In traditional

clinical practice, changes in behavior are


characteristically effected by professional

psychotherapists in office settings, mainly through

the modification of verbal-symbolic contents.

Although the decided preference for artificial


environments and symbolic substitutes for

naturally occurring events has been theoretically

justified, these treatment conditions were

probably adopted more for the therapists’

convenience than for any proven therapeutic


superiority. In fact, results of controlled studies

demonstrate that deviant behavior can be


modified more thoroughly and more expeditiously

by treating actual events rather than their


symbolic equivalents (Bandura, Blanchard, &

Ritter, 1969), and that change programs


conducted in natural settings are far superior to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 347
similar ones administered in psychiatric

institutions (Fairweather, et al., 1969).

It follows from principles of generalization that

the optimal conditions for effecting behavioral

changes, from the standpoint of maximizing


transfer effects, would require people to perform

the desired patterns of behavior successfully in

the diverse social situations in which the behavior


is most appropriate. On the other hand, when
treatment is primarily centered around verbal

responses expressed in an invariant, atypical

context one cannot assume that induced changes

will necessarily generalize to real-life


performances to any great extent.

Issues regarding the locus and content of

treatment are closely linked with the choice of

change agents. From a social-learning perspective

those who have the most intensive contact with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 348
the client, if given appropriate training, can serve
as the most powerful agents of change. Their

potential efficacy derives from the fact that in such


positions they exercise considerable control over

the very conditions that regulate the behavior.

Successful applications of this general principle

are provided in new approaches to child therapy


in which parents are utilized in the treatment of

their own children’s behavior (Hawkins, Peterson,

Schweid, & Bijou, 1966; O’Leary, O’Leary, &

Becker, 1967; Patterson, Ray, & Shaw, 1968; Risley

& Wolf, 1966; Russo, 1964; Wahler, Winkel,


Peterson, & Morrison, 1965; Williams, 1959).

In a well-designed program a thorough


behavioral analysis is first conducted to identify

the social conditions that maintain the various


behavior disorders. Next the deviant response

patterns to be eliminated and the desirable


behaviors to be strengthened are clearly specified.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 349
The parents are then given a detailed description

of how they must alter their characteristic ways of

reacting to their child’s behavior to achieve


therapeutic changes. This typically involves a

reversal of parents’ differential reinforcement

practices. Whereas the child’s deviant behavior

previously commanded attention and his desirable

behavior received little special notice, the parents


are advised now to ignore or to reinforce
negatively his aberrant behavior and to respond

positively to the forms of behavior they wish to

promote. In the case of deficit problems (Lovaas,

1966), a program of graduated modeling is also


devised, while in fear-motivated disorders

(Bentler, 1962) a graduated reexposure to


threatening situations is implemented by the

parents.

It should be noted in this context that attempts

to modify behavior through giving advice, have an

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 350
extended history, mainly negative. Its paltry

outcomes probably result from the nature of the

advice given and from the fact that instructions


alone are of limited effectiveness unless they are

combined with other procedures that help to alter

and to support parental behavior. Parents may

understand principles of change but may have

difficulty translating them into appropriate


actions. To alleviate this problem, not only
treatment strategies are sketched out in

considerable detail, but initially the recommended

practices are modeled by the person planning the

program while parents observe the interactions.


After the procedures have been adequately

demonstrated and some control of the child’s


deviant behavior has been achieved, the parents

gradually take over the therapeutic function. The

parents are directly supervised until they attain


proficiency in handling their child’s behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 351
without external direction.

Detailed instructions, combined with


demonstrations and supervised practices, are

effective means of instituting changes in parental

behavior, but favorable outcomes are necessary to


ensure adherence to the recommended practices.

The problem of parental reinforcement is

particularly critical in initial stages of treatment


when withdrawal of the positive consequences
that had been periodically evoked by the child’s

deviant behavior often produces a temporary

increase in such behavior. During this period it

may be necessary to provide extensive social


support to maintain the desired parental behavior.
In later phases beneficial changes in the child

serve as a natural and powerful source of reward

for the parents’ efforts so that the new familial

patterns become reciprocally reinforcing and

thereby self-sustaining. At times it may be difficult

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 352
for parents to carry out the necessary programs

because of social conditions, independent of the

child, that affect their behavior. Such hindrances


can be most successfully overcome by modifying

the conflicting influences impinging upon the

parents.

When a child’s deviant behavior is sufficiently

prevalent to occur frequently within a clinical


setting, parents may gain facility through
supervised consultation sessions on treatment

strategies that they can apply at home. On the

other hand, in instances where the major


behavioral problems are not readily reenacted at a
clinic, the change process can be most effectively

initiated in the home with the parents functioning


as therapists. The feasibility of the home

treatment approach has been demonstrated by

Hawkins et al. (1966).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 353
Their illustrative case involved a four-year-old

boy who aggressively demanded constant

attention, often behaved in a physically abusive


and belligerent manner, and generally was

extremely difficult to manage. After a baseline


measurement of the incidence of hyperaggressive
behavior was made, the treatment program was

initiated. The mother was instructed to go about

her usual household activities and whenever her


son displayed behavior that required handling, the

observer would signal one of three modes of

response. Each time the boy behaved


reprehensibly the mother was advised either to

tell him to stop or to place him in his room for a


brief time. In contrast, when he behaved

commendably the mother was encouraged to

express interest and approval. As shown in Figure


2-2, the new reinforcement practices produced a

marked decrease in undesirable behavior. In the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 354
Figure 2-2. Number of 10-second intervals in which the boy
displayed objectionable behavior during each one-hour
session. Hawkins et al., 1966.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 355
next phase the mother was asked to resume her

original practices of chastising undesirable


behaviors while ignoring desirable ones, but she

found it difficult to recapture her former style. The

therapeutic contingencies were again reinstated,


and a follow-up study was conducted

approximately a month later in which the mother-


child interaction was observed for several sessions
without airy further guidance. The overall results

show not only that the mother maintained the

favorable changes in her son’s behavior long after

the supervising therapist had dropped out of the


picture, but that the boy generally behaved in a

more considerate and affectionate manner, which


contrasted markedly with his former

indiscriminate belligerence. As Hawkins points

out, a major benefit of enlisting parents as change

agents is that, having gained facility in effective


treatment methods, they can successfully apply

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 356
them to future developmental problems in a

variety of circumstances.

Although the discussion thus far has

concentrated on the implementation of change


programs by parents, the same general principles
apply when other change agents perform similar

functions. The direction of change must be defined

in terms of observable behavior; the methods for


achieving these outcomes must be clearly

specified and preferably modeled; enough


guidance must be provided to ensure success; and,

if necessary, special favorable consequences of

carrying out the recommended practices must be


arranged. Behavioral approaches, as will be shown
later, use teachers, nurses, peers, and students

extensively as agents of change under the

guidance of persons who possess professional


knowledge and competencies in principles of

behavioral change. To some extent, also,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 357
individuals are called upon to function as their

own change agents by learning how to manage

contingencies and self-reinforcing consequences


in order to modify their own behavior in desired

directions. Nonprofessionals are frequently

selected to implement change programs, not just

as an economical way of alleviating serious

manpower shortages, but because they are in a


more advantageous position to effect better
outcomes than professionals, who may have only

brief contact with the client in an artificial setting

in which the deviant behavior is infrequently

displayed. When behavior is modified in the


natural social environment by persons who

normally exercise some control over the behavior,


the problems of induced changes failing to

generalize or to be sustained over time are much

less likely to arise.

In many behavioral change programs, the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 358
supervisory staff instructs change agents on how

to implement selected procedures but fails to

demonstrate the desired practices or to arrange


for favorable consequences for their endeavors.

Since new behavioral practices often require

change agents to devote increased attention to the

persons whose behavior is being modified and to

discard old routines that had some functional


value, some resistance is to be expected. In the
initial phase of a project by Ayllon and Azrin

(1964) designed to restore self-care in chronic

schizophrenics it was noted, for example, that

hospital attendants often failed to put the


designated procedures into effect even though

they had repeatedly been instructed to do so. Only


after the attendants were provided feedback about

their own performances and social consequences

for their own behavior did they faithfully carry out


the prescribed program.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 359
The efforts of change agents are reinforced and

maintained to some extent by positive experiences

resulting from favorable changes in the behavior


of their clients. In fact, some investigations

(Hawkins et al., 1966; Wahler & Pollio, 1968) have


encountered difficulties in employing the
intrasubject replication design to dramatize the

functional relationships between behavior and its

consequences because, after experiencing the


benefits from the behavioral changes they

produced in their children, the parents were

exceedingly reluctant to revert to their former


reinforcement practices. However, when the

required treatment conditions are difficult to


create and to sustain, when the rate of

improvement is relatively slow or evidence of

progress has weak reinforcing value, it is desirable


to provide adequate rewards for change agents as

well. For example, to enhance the performance of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 360
remedial instructors, Wolf, Giles, and Hall (1968)

created a bonus monetary contingency that was

linked to their students’ productivity. The

provision of appropriate supports for the agent’s


behavior, which is a critical aspect of behavioral

change programs, is usually given little attention,

with the consequence that essential procedures


are halfheartedly or only sporadically applied. Any
temporary suspension of contingencies,

particularly in initial phases of a program, usually

results in intermittent reinforcement of the


undesired behavior. Therefore, treatment

programs should not be attempted unless the

appropriate contingencies will be systematically


applied.

ETHICAL ISSUES IN SOCIAL AND CULTURAL


CHANGE

Most of the preceding discussion of goal

selection was primarily concerned with the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 361
achievement of behavioral changes on an
individual basis. It is generally acknowledged that

many of the problems confronting a society cannot


be solved at an individual level but necessitate

changes in entire social systems.

A variety of situations in which new


contingencies are introduced on a society-wide
basis raise important questions about the morality

and decision processes guiding instituted changes.


In cases involving widespread deviant behavior,

such as delinquency or prevalent deficit conditions

resulting from impoverished environments, major

social changes are required for rehabilitation. For

example, attempts to reduce the incidence of


antisocial behavior by treating individual

members who happen to be apprehended is a


futile endeavor. Group problems demand group

solutions. New social environments involving


appropriate contingencies, role models, and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 362
incentives, must be created if constructive modes

of behavior are to be established and normatively

sanctioned.

As knowledge accumulates about the causes

and consequences of different social patterns and

efficacious principles of behavioral change are

further developed, a society gains the means not

only of preventing the development of major


social problems but also of realizing its avowed
aims. Preventive programs and improved systems

of social life entail new social practices, some of

which may clash with the ideologies and traditions


of various interest groups. Ethical controversies,
therefore, inevitably arise over the types of social

changes advocated as well as the methods by


which they are to be achieved.

The value conflicts resulting from intrasocietal

pressures for change occur on a much broader

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 363
scale in cross-cultural ventures in which one
society strives to introduce new patterns of

behavior into other societies occupying


subordinate positions. In many cases advocated

changes involving preventive medical practices,

reorganization of economic and agricultural

systems, creation of educational programs, and


introduction of technologies that release people

from demeaning labor have the potentiality for

enriching social life and enhancing human

freedom. Although the changes may have

beneficial outcomes, they often require radical


modifications of established beliefs and ways of

living and are therefore understandably opposed.


Moreover, intersocietal attempts at influence

typically involve the export not only of better


means of achieving cultural aims, but also of new

ideologies and ultimate ends themselves. It is


primarily the imposition of new moral standards,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 364
some of which may be dysfunctional in the foreign

setting, and the external prescription of how

people within another culture should live their


lives, that give rise to ethical concerns.

The decision processes and value issues


involved in the selection of group goals are, in

many respects, similar to those that operate at the

individual level. First, it is necessary to decide


what social objectives from among a variety of
alternatives shall be pursued. The major question

here is whether the authority for goal selection

resides in a political or technological elite or is

determined through informed collaborative


participation of those whose lives will be affected
by whatever policies are adopted. If one seriously

subscribes to the value of group determination of

social objectives, then more attention must be

given to developing optimal methods for clarifying

the consequences associated with different value

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 365
choices, for ascertaining collective preferences,

and for resolving conflicts among different

interest groups. In addition, adequate safeguards


and social supports must be provided for

warranted attempts at personal influence of social

policies. Looking into the not-too-distant future,

Hofstadter (1967), for example, envisions the use

of computer technology, in which individual voting


devices are connected to computers which
assemble data almost instantaneously, to permit

greater individual participation in society’s

decision-making whenever feasible.

Under extensive bureaucratization, which


effectively obscures decision-making

responsibilities, most people come to feel that they


can exert little positive control over their

environment. Consequently they are inclined to

respond with grudging acquiescence to major

social changes that are often guided by economic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 366
considerations, slide rule decrees, and political

expedience. Persons who are more actively

inclined are often thwarted by the lack of readily


accessible means of affecting decisions about the

cultural priorities that should be promoted.

However, the recent years have witnessed

vigorous demands, particularly among the

younger members of society, for a greater role in


making decisions that affect the course and quality
of their life.

Value conflicts arise not only in formulating

common goals, but also in selecting methods for


inducing preferred changes. In one way or another
decisions are made about how much social

objectives are advanced through coercive


methods, through positive reinforcement of

appropriate behaviors, or through provision of

models for emulation who exemplify the desired

behavioral patterns.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 367
The notion of planned social change is likely to

arouse in people’s minds negative associations of

regimentation, invasion of privacy, and


curtailment of self-determination. In fact, as Benne

(1949) and Mannheim (1941) have cogently


argued, collectively planned social change, rather
than being anti-individualistic, generally

safeguards and extends human freedom. The need

for social planning stems from the fact that, in


many areas of behavioral functioning, people’s

outcome experiences are jointly determined by

each other’s actions. Thus if motorists did not have


the benefit of traffic codes they would repeatedly

obstruct and injure one another, whereas agreeing


to a few sensible regulations greatly enhances

their personal welfare and freedom of movement.

Without some social controls over human


behavior, personal freedoms would be

continuously in jeopardy. Paradoxically, zealous

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 368
individualists often attack the very social

institutions that are established to protect

freedom of self-expression.

Problems of dysfunctional restraints often

occur when social control is unwisely extended to


areas of functioning that do not involve

interdependent consequences to any significant

degree. Unconventional beliefs, styles of living,


and personal habits may be negatively sanctioned
even though these activities, apart from their

minor irritant value, rarely affect the welfare of

others. Such pressures toward the standardization

of life do constitute threats to personal freedom.

Summary

One of the major obstacles to the development

of effective change programs arises from the

failure to specify precisely what is to be

accomplished, or the more common practice of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 369
defining the intended goals in terms of
hypothetical internal states. When the aims

remain ambiguous, learning experiences are


haphazard, and whatever procedures are

consistently applied tend to be determined more

by personal preferences of change agents than by

clients’ needs.

The appropriate methods and learning

conditions for any given program of behavioral


change cannot be meaningfully selected until the

desired goals have been clearly defined in terms of

observable behavior. Rapid progress is further

assured by designating intermediate objectives,

which delineate optimal learning sequences for


establishing the component behaviors of more

complicated social performances. The necessity


for behavioral specification of objectives is most

clearly illustrated in the case of complex patterns


of behavior which cannot be achieved with any

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 370
degree of success until they are analyzed into

essential constituent functions.

The selection of goals involves value choices.


To the extent that people assume major

responsibility for deciding the direction in which

their behavior ought to be modified, the frequently

voiced concerns about human manipulation

become essentially pseudo issues. The change


agent’s role in the decision process should be
primarily to explore alternative courses of action

available, and their probable consequences, on the

basis of which clients can make informed choices.


However, a change agent’s value commitments
will inevitably intrude to some degree on the goal

selection process. These biases are not necessarily


detrimental, provided clients and change agents

subscribe to similar values and the change agent

identifies his judgments as personal preferences

rather than purported scientific prescriptions.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 371
Much more serious from an ethical standpoint is

the unilateral redefinition of goals by which

psychotherapists often impose insight objectives


(which mainly involve subtle belief conversions)

upon persons desiring changes in their behavioral

functioning.

Behavioral problems of vast proportions can

never be adequately eliminated on an individual


basis but require treatment and prevention at the
social systems level. As behavioral science makes

further progress toward the development of

efficacious principles of change, man’s capacity to


create the type of social environments he wants
will be substantially increased. The decision

processes by which cultural priorities are


established must, therefore, be made more explicit

to ensure that “social engineering” is utilized to

produce living conditions that enrich life and

behavioral freedom rather than aversive human

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 372
effects. Control over value choices at the societal

level can be increased by devising new systems of

collective decision-making which enable members


to participate more directly in the formulation of

group objectives.

In discussions of the ethical implications of

different modes of achieving personality changes,

commentators often mistakenly ascribe a negative


morality to behavioral approaches, as though this
were inherent in the procedures. Social-learning

theory is not a system of ethics; it is a system of

scientific principles that can be successfully


applied to the attainment of any moral outcome. In
actuality, because of their relative efficacy,

behavioral approaches hold much greater promise


than traditional methods for the advancement of

self-determination and the fulfillment of human

capabilities. If applied toward the proper ends,

social learning methods can quite effectively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 373
support a humanistic morality.

REFERENCES

Alexander, F. Psychoanalysis and psychotherapy;


developments in theory, techniques, and training.
New York: Norton, 1956.

Alexander, F. The dynamics of psychotherapy in the


light of learning theory. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 1963, 120, 440-448.

Asch, S. E. Social psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall,


1952.

Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. Reinforcement and


instructions with mental patients. Journal of
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 327-
331.

Bandura, A. Social learning through imitation. In M. R.


Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation:
1962. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press,
1962. pp. 211-269.

Bandura, A. Vicarious processes: A case of no-trial


learning. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology. Vol. II. New York:
Academic Press, 1965. pp. 1-55.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 374
Bandura, A. Social-learning theory of identificatory
processes. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of
socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand
McNally, 1968.

Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. The relative


efficacy of desensitization and modeling
approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and
attitudinal changes, Stanford University, 1969,
13, 173-199.

Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. Vicarious


extinction of avoidance behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 5, 16-23.

Bandura, A., & Harris, M. B. Modification of syntactic


style. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
1966, 4, 341-352.

Bandura, A., & Huston, A. C. Identification as a process


of incidental learning. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1961, 63, 311-318.

Bandura, A., & Kupers, C. J. Transmission of patterns


of self-reinforcement through modeling. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964, 69, 1-9.

Bandura, A., Lipsher, D. H., & Miller, P. E.


Psychotherapists’ approach- avoidance reactions
to patients’ expressions of hostility. Journal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 375
Consulting Psychology, 1960, 24, 1-8.

Bandura, A., & McDonald, F. J. The influence of social


reinforcement and the behavior of models in
shaping children’s moral judgments. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 274-
281.

Bandura, A., & Mischel, W. The influence of models in


modifying delay of gratification patterns. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 698-
705.

Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. Imitation of film-


mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1963, 66, 3-11.

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression.


New York: Ronald, 1959.

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Social learning and


personality development. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston, 1963.

Benne, K. D. Democratic ethics in social engineering.


Progressive Education, 1949, 26,201-207.

Bentler, P. M. An infant’s phobia treated with


reciprocal inhibition therapy. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 1962, 3, 185-189.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 376
Berg, C. Deep analysis. New York: Norton, 1947.

Berg, J. A., & Bass, B. M. (Eds.) Conformity and


deviation. New York: Harper, 1961.

Bergin, A. E. The effect of dissonant persuasive


communications upon changes in a self-referring
attitude. Journal of Personality, 1962, 30, 423-38.

Brown, P., & Elliott, R. Control of aggression in a


nursery school class. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 1965, 2, 103-107.

Chittenden, G. E. An experimental study in measuring


and modifying assertive behavior in young
children. Monographs of the Society for Research
in Child Development, 1942, 7 (1, Serial No. 31).

Chu, C. G. Fear arousal, efficacy and imminency.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966,
4, 517-524.

Colby, K. M. On the greater amplifying power of


causal-correlative over interrogative inputs on
free association in an experimental
psychoanalytic situation. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Diseases, 1961, 133, 233-239.

Collier, R. M. A scale for rating the responses of the


psychotherapist. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
1953, 17, 321-326.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 377
Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Leventhal, H. Effects of varying the
recommendations in a fear-arousing
communication. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1966, 4, 525-531.

Dittes, J. E. Galvanic skin responses as a measure of


patient’s reaction to therapist’s permissiveness.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1957,
55, 295-303.

Dittmann, A. T. The interpersonal process in


psychotherapy: development of a research
method. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1952, 47,236-244.

Elkin, F., & Westley, W. A. The myth of adolescent


culture. American Sociological Review, 1955, 20,
680-684.

Fairweather, G. W., Sanders, D. H., Maynard, H., &


Cressler, D. L. Community life for the mentally ill:
an alternative to institutional care. Chicago:
Aldine, 1969.

Fenichel, O. Problems of psychoanalytic technique. New


York: Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 1941.

Ferster, C. B., Nurnberger, J. I., & Levitt, E. B. The


control of eating. Journal of Mathetics, 1962, 1,
87-109.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 378
Frank, G. H., & Sweetland, A. A study of the process of
psychotherapy: The verbal interaction. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 1962, 26, 135-138.

Gelfand, D. M., Gelfand, S., & Dobson, W. R.


Unprogrammed reinforcement of patients’
behavior in a mental hospital. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1967, 5, 201-207.

Goldman, J. R. The relation of certain therapist


variables to the handling of psychotherapeutic
events. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Stanford University, 1961.

Grossberg, J. M. Successful behavior therapy in a case


of speech phobia (“stage fright”). Journal of
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1965, 30, 285-288.

Harris, F. R., Wolf, M. M., & Baer, D. M. Effects of adult


social reinforcement on child behavior. Young
Children, 1964, 20, 8-17.

Harris, M. B. A self-directed program for weight


control: A pilot study. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 1969, 74, 263-270.

Hart, B. M., Reynolds, N. J., Baer, D. M., Brawley, E. R., &


Harris, F. R. Effect of contingent and non-
contingent social reinforcement on the
cooperative play of a preschool child. Journal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 379
Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 73-76.

Harway, N. I., Dittmann, A. T., Raush, H. L., Bordin, E.


S., & Rigler, D. The measurement of depth of
interpretation. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
1955,19, 247-253.

Hastorf, A. H. The “reinforcement” of individual


actions in a group situation. In L. Krasner & L. P.
Ullmann (Eds.), Research in behavior modification.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. pp.
268-284.

Hawkins, R. P., Peterson, R. F., Schweid, E., & Bijou, S.


W. Behavior therapy in the home: Amelioration
of a problem in parent-child relations with the
parent in a therapeutic role. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 1966, 4, 99-107.

Heine, R. W. A comparison of patients’ reports on


psychotherapeutic experience with
psychoanalytic, nondirective, and Adlerian
therapists. American Journal of Psychotherapy,
1953, 7, 16-23.

Henker, B. A. The effect of adult model relationships


on children’s play and task imitation. Dissertation
Abstracts, 1964, 24, 4797.

Herzberg, A. Active psychotherapy. New York: Grune &

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 380
Stratton, 1945.

Hofstadter, R. The instant electorate. Commencement


address presented at Carlton University, Ottawa,
Canada, May 1967.

Homme, L. E., & Tosti, D. T. Contingency management


and motivation. Journal of the National Society for
Programmed Instruction, 1965, 4, 14-16.

Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H.


Communication and persuasion: Psychological
studies of opinion change. New Haven: Yale
University Press, 1953.

Jack, L. M. An experimental study of ascendant


behavior in preschool children. University of Iowa
Studies in Child Welfare, 1934, 9, 3-65.

Jourard, S. M. On the problem of reinforcement by the


psychotherapist of healthy behavior in the
patient. In F. J. Shaw (Ed.), Behavioristic
approaches to counseling and psychotherapy: A
Southeastern Psychological Association
Symposium. University, Ala.: University of
Alabama Press, 1961.

Keister, M. E. The behavior of young children in


failure: An experimental attempt to discover and
to modify undesirable responses of preschool

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 381
children to failure. University of Iowa Studies in
Child Welfare, 1938, 14, 27-82.

Kelman, H. C. Manipulation of human behavior: An


ethical dilemma for the social scientist. Journal of
Social Issues, 1965, 21, 31-46.

Klein, M. The psychoanalysis of children. Translated by


Alix Strachey. New York: Grove, 1960.

London, P. The modes and morals of psychotherapy.


New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1964.

Lovaas, O. I. A program for the establishment of


speech in psychotic children. In J. K. Wing (Ed.),
Early childhood autism. New York: Pergamon,
1966.

Lovaas, O. I. A behavior therapy approach to the


treatment of childhood schizophrenia. In J. P. Hill
(Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology,
Vol. 1. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota
Press, 1967. pp. 108-159.

Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., & Kassorla, I. C.


Experimental studies in childhood schizophrenia:
Analysis of self-destructive behavior. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 1965, 2, 67-84.

McConnell, J. V., Cutler, R. L., & McNeil, E. B. Subliminal


stimulation: An overview. American Psychologist,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 382
1958, 13, 229-242.

Mager, R. F. Preparing objectives for programmed


instruction. San Francisco: Fearon, 1961.

Mahrer, A. R. The goals of psychotherapy. New York:


Appleton-Century- Crofts, 1967.

Mannheim, K. Man and society in an age of


reconstruction. New York: Harcourt, Brace &
World, 1941.

Marmor, J. Psychoanalytic therapy as an educational


process: Common denominators in the
therapeutic approaches of different
psychoanalytic schools. In J. H. Masserman (Ed.),
Science and psychoanalysis. Vol. 5. Psychoanalytic
education. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1962. pp.
286- 299.

Murray, E. J. A content-analysis method for studying


psychotherapy. Psychological Monographs, 1956,
70 (13, Whole No. 420).

Mussen, P. H., & Parker, A. L. Mother-nurturance and


girls’ incidental imitative learning. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 94-97.

O’Leary, K. D., O’Leary, S., & Becker, W. C. Modification


of a deviant sibling interaction pattern in the
home. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1967, 5,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 383
113-120.

Page, M. L. The modification of ascendant behavior in


preschool children. University of Iowa Studies in
Child Welfare, 1936, 12, 7-69.

Patterson, C. H. Control, conditioning, and counseling.


Personnel and Guidance, 1963, 41, 680-686.

Patterson, G. R., & Brodsky, G. A behavior modification


programme for a child with multiple problem
behaviors. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 1967, 7, 277-295.

Patterson, G. R., Ray, R., & Shaw, D. Direct intervention


in families of deviant children. Unpublished
manuscript, University of Oregon, 1968.

Pentony, P. Value change in psychotherapy. Human


Relations, 1966, 19, 39-46.

Pratt, C. C. The logic of modern psychology. New York:


Macmillan, 1939.

Risley, T., & Wolf, M. M. Experimental manipulation of


autistic behavior and generalization into the
home. In R. Ulrich, R. Stachnik, & J. Mabry (Eds.),
Control of human behavior. Glenview, Ill.: Scott,
Foresman, 1966. pp. 193-198.

Rogers, C. R. Client-centered therapy. Boston:

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 384
Houghton Mifflin, 1951.

Rogers, C. R., & Skinner, B. F. Some issues concerning


the control of human behavior: A symposium.
Science, 1956, 124, 1057-1066.

Rosen, J. N. Direct analysis: Selected papers. New York:


Grune and Stratton, 1953.

Rosenthal, D. Changes in some moral values following


psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
1955, 19, 431-436.

Rosenthal, T. L. Anxiety-proneness and susceptibility


to social influence. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Stanford University, 1963.

Russo, S. Adaptations in behavioural therapy with


children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1964,
2, 43-47.

Shoben, E. J., Jr. The therapeutic object: Men or


machines? Journal of Counseling Psychology,
196.3, 10, 264-268.

Speisman, J. C. Depth of interpretation and verbal


resistance in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 1959, 23, 93-99.

Stuart, R. B. Behavioral control of overeating.


Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 357-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 385
365.

Truax, C. B. Reinforcement and nonreinforcement in


Rogerian psychotherapy. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 1966, 71, 1-9.

Wahler, R. G., & Pollio, H. R. Behavior and insight: A


case study in behavior therapy. Journal of
Experimental Research in Personality, 1968, 3, 45-
56.

Wahler, R. G., Winkel, G. H., Peterson, R. F., &


Morrison, D. C. Mothers as behavior therapists for
their own children. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1965, 3, 113-124.

Wallerstein, R. S. The problem of the assessment of


change in psychotherapy. The International
Journal of Psycho-analysis, 1963, 44, 31-41.

Walters, R. H., & Brown, M. Studies of reinforcement


of aggression: III. Transfer of responses to an
interpersonal situation. Child Development, 1963,
34, 563-571.

Williams, C. D. The elimination of tantrum behavior by


extinction procedures. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1959, 59, 269.

Winder, C. L., Ahmad, F. Z., Bandura, A., & Rau, L. C.


Dependency of patients, psychotherapists’

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 386
responses, and aspects of psychotherapy. Journal
of Consulting Psychology, 1962, 26, 129-134.

Wolf, M. M., Giles, D. K., & Hall, R. V. Experiments with


token reinforcement in a remedial classroom.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1968, 6, 51-64.

Wolpe, J. Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition.


Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1958.

Zilboorg, G. The emotional problem and the


therapeutic role of insight. Psychoanalytic
Quarterly, 1952, 21, 1-24.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 387
3

Modeling and Vicarious


Processes
One of the fundamental means by which new

modes of behavior are acquired and existing

patterns are modified entails modeling and

vicarious processes. Indeed, research conducted

within the framework of social-learning theory

(Bandura, 1965a; Bandura & Walters, 1963)


demonstrates that virtually all learning
phenomena resulting from direct experiences can

occur on a vicarious basis through observation of


other persons’ behavior and its consequences for

them. Thus, for example, one can acquire intricate

response patterns merely by observing the


performances of appropriate models; emotional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 388
responses can be conditioned observationally by
witnessing the affective reactions of others

undergoing painful or pleasurable experiences;

fearful and avoidant behavior can be extinguished


vicariously through observation of modeled

approach behavior toward feared objects without


any adverse consequences accruing to the
performer; inhibitions can be induced by

witnessing the behavior of others punished; and,

finally, the expression of well-learned responses


can be enhanced and socially regulated through

the actions of influential models. Modeling

procedures are, therefore, ideally suited for

effecting diverse outcomes including elimination

of behavioral deficits, reduction of excessive fears


and inhibitions, transmission of self-regulating
systems, and social facilitation of behavioral

patterns on a group-wide scale.

Vicarious phenomena are generally subsumed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 389
under a variety of terms. Among those in common

usage are “modeling,” “imitation,” “observational

learning,” identification,” “copying,” “vicarious


learning,” “social facilitation,” “contagion,” and

“role-playing.” In personality theory identification

has been most frequently differentiated from

imitation on the assumed basis that imitation

involves the reproduction of discrete responses,


whereas identification involves the adoption of
either diverse patterns of behavior (Kohlberg,

1963; Parsons, 1955; Stoke, 1950), symbolic

representations of the model (Emmerich, 1959),

or similar meaning systems (Lazowick, 1955).


Sometimes the distinction is made in terms of

differential antecedent or maintaining conditions


as illustrated by Parsons’ (1951) view that “a

generalized cathectic attachment” is a prerequisite

for identification but is unessential or absent in


the case of imitation. Kohlberg (1963), on the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 390
other hand, reserves the term “identification” for

matching behavior that is presumed to be

maintained by the intrinsic reinforcement of


perceived similarity, and employs the construct

“imitation” for instrumental responses supported


by extrinsic rewards. Others define imitation as
matching behavior occurring in the presence of

the model, reserving identification for

performance of the model’s behavior in the latter’s


absence (Kohlberg, 1963; Mowrer, 1950). Not only

is there little consensus with respect to

differentiating criteria, but some theorists assume


that imitation produces identification, whereas

others contend, with equally strong conviction,


that identification results in imitation.

Unless it can be shown that vicarious learning

of different classes of matching behavior is

governed by separate variables, distinctions

proposed in terms of the types of emulated

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 391
responses not only are gratuitous but also cause

unnecessary confusion. Limited progress would be

made in elucidating behavioral change processes


if, for example, fundamentally different learning

mechanisms were invoked, without adequate

empirical basis, to account for the acquisition of

one social response versus ten interrelated social

responses that are arbitrarily designated as


various aspects of a given role. Results of
numerous studies to be reviewed later

demonstrate that the acquisition of isolated

matching responses and of entire behavioral

repertoires is, in fact, determined by the same


types of antecedent conditions. Further, retention

and delayed reproduction of even discrete


matching responses require representational

mediation of modeling stimuli. There is also little

reason to suppose, either on empirical or


theoretical grounds, that the principles and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 392
processes involved in the acquisition of matching

responses that are performed in the presence of

models are different from those later performed in


their absence. Indeed, if the diverse criteria

enumerated above were seriously applied, either


singly or in various combinations, in categorizing
modeling outcomes, most instances of matching

behavior that have been traditionally labeled

imitation would qualify as identification, and much


of the naturalistic data cited as evidence of

identificatory learning would be reclassified as

imitation.

It is possible, of course, to draw distinctions


among numerous descriptive terms based on
antecedent, mediating, or behavioral variables.

One might question, however, whether it is

advantageous to do so, since there is every

indication that essentially the same learning

process is involved regardless of the generality of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 393
what is learned, of the models from whom the

response patterns are acquired, and of the

stimulus conditions under which matching


behavior is subsequently performed.

THREE EFFECTS OF MODELING INFLUENCES

To elucidate vicarious influences it is essential


to distinguish among different types of behavioral

modifications resulting from exposure to modeling


stimuli, but the differentiation must be made in

terms of more fundamental criteria than those

discussed above. There is abundant evidence


(Bandura, 1965a; Bandura & Walters, 1963) that
exposure to modeling influences has three clearly

different effects, each of which is determined by a

separate set of variables. First, an observer may

acquire new response patterns that did not

previously exist in his behavioral repertoire. In

demonstrating this observational learning or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 394
modeling effect experimentally, it is necessary for a
model to exhibit novel responses which the

observer has not yet learned to make and which


he must later reproduce in a substantially

identical form. Any behavior that has a very low or

zero probability of occurrence in the presence of

appropriate stimuli qualifies as a novel response.

Second, observation of modeled actions and

their consequences to the performer may


strengthen or weaken inhibitory responses in

observers. These inhibitory and disinhibitory

effects are evident when the incidence of imitative

and nonmatching behavior is increased, generally

as a function of having witnessed a model


experience positive outcomes, and decreased by

having observed a model undergo punishing


consequences.

Third, the behavior of others often serves

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 395
merely as discriminative stimuli for the observer
in facilitating the occurrence of previously learned
responses in the same general class. This response
facilitation effect can be distinguished from
disinhibition and modeling by the fact that no new
responses are acquired; disinhibitory processes
are not involved because the behavior in question
is socially sanctioned and, therefore, has rarely, if
ever, incurred punishment. A simple example of
social facilitation is provided in situations where a
person gazes intently into a display window and
passersby respond in a similar manner. In the
following sections the variables and mediating
processes governing these diverse modeling
phenomena will be discussed at length. The ways
in which modeling influences can be successfully
used to effect individual and broader social
changes will also be reviewed.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 396
Theoretical Conceptions of
Observational Learning

The earliest formulations, dating back to


Morgan (1896), Tarde (1903), and McDougall
(1908), regarded modeling as an innate
propensity. These instinctual interpretations
dissuaded empirical investigations of the
conditions under which modeling occurs; and
because of the vehement reactions against the
instinct doctrine, until recently even the
phenomena subsumed under the concept tended
to be either repudiated or widely ignored in
theoretical explanations of learning processes.
ASSOCIATIVE AND CLASSICAL CONDITIONING
THEORIES

As the instinct doctrine fell into disrepute, a

number of psychologists, notably Humphrey

(1921), Allport (1924), and Holt (1931),

accounted for modeling behavior in terms of

associative principles. Temporal contiguity

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 397
between modeling stimuli and the imitator’s
matching response was considered to be a

sufficient condition for the occurrence of

imitation. According to Holt’s conceptualization,


for example, when an adult copies the response of

a child, the latter tends to repeat the reiterated


behavior, and as this circular associative sequence
continues, the adult’s behavior becomes an

increasingly effective stimulus for the child’s

responses. If, during this spontaneous mutual


imitation, the adult performs a response that is

novel for the child, the latter will copy it. Piaget

(1952) likewise depicted the modeling process as

one in which the imitator’s spontaneous behaviors

serve initially as stimuli for matching responses by


the model in alternating imitative sequences.
Allport also viewed modeling phenomena as

instances of classical conditioning of

verbalizations, motor responses, or emotions to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 398
matching social stimuli with which they have been

contiguously associated.

The various associative theories isolated one of

the conditions under which modeling cues may

acquire eliciting functions for matching responses


that already exist in the imitator’s behavioral

repertoire. These theories failed to explain,

however, the psychological mechanisms governing


the acquisition of novel responses during the
model-observer interaction sequence. Moreover,

demonstrations of observational learning in

humans and animals do not ordinarily commence

by having a model reproduce semi-irrelevant


responses of the learner. In using modeling
procedures to teach a mynah bird to talk, for

example, the trainer does not engage initially in

circular crowing behavior; rather, he begins by

saying what he wishes to teach, which expressions

clearly do not exist in integrated form in the bird’s

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 399
vocal repertoire.

REINFORCEMENT THEORIES

With the advent of reinforcement principles,

theoretical explanations of learning shifted the

emphasis from classical conditioning to

instrumental response acquisition based on


reinforcing outcomes. Theories of modeling

phenomena similarly assumed that the occurrence


of observational learning is contingent upon

reinforcement of imitative behavior. This point of

view was most clearly expounded by Miller &


Dollard (1941) in the classic publication, Social
Learning and Imitation. According to this

formulation, the necessary conditions for learning

through modeling include a motivated subject who

is positively reinforced for matching the correct

responses of a model during a series of initially

random, trial-and-error responses.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 400
The experiments conducted by Miller and

Dollard involved a series of two-choice

discrimination problems, in which a trained leader


responded to environmental stimuli that were

concealed from the subject so that he was


dependent solely upon the cues provided by the
model’s behavior. The leader’s choices were

consistently rewarded and the observing subject

was similarly reinforced whenever he matched


these choice responses. This form of imitation was

labeled by the authors “matched-dependent”

behavior, because the subjects relied on the leader


for relevant cues, and matched his responses.

Based on this paradigm, it was shown that


subjects readily learn to follow their respective

models, and generalize copying responses to new

situations, to new models, and to different


motivational states.

While these experiments have been widely

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 401
accepted as demonstrations of imitative learning,

they in fact represent only the special case of

discrimination place-learning, in which the


behavior of others provides discriminative stimuli

for responses that already exist in the subject’s

behavioral repertoire. Indeed, had the relevant

environmental cues been made more distinctive,

the behavior of the models would have been quite


irrelevant, and perhaps even a hindrance, to the
acquisition process. By contrast, most forms of

imitation involve responses rather than place-

learning, in which subjects combine behavioral

elements into new compound responses solely by


observing the performance of social models,

without any opportunity to perform the model’s


behavior at the time of exposure and without any

reinforcers administered either to the models or

to the observers (Bandura, 1965a). In the latter


instance, modeling cues constitute an

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 402
indispensable aspect of the learning process.

Moreover, since the reinforcement paradigm for

observational learning requires the subject to


perform the imitative response before he can

learn it, the theory advanced by Miller and Dollard


evidently accounts more adequately for the
performance of previously learned matching

responses than for their acquisition. Continuing

with the example of language learning, in order for


a mynah bird to learn the word “reinforcement”

imitatively, it would first have to utter the word

“reinforcement” in the course of random


vocalization, match it accidentally with the

trainer’s verbal responses, and secure a positive


reinforcement. The conditions that Miller and

Dollard assumed to be necessary for imitative

learning severely limit the types of behavioral


changes that can be attributed to the influence of

social models.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 403
The Skinnerian analysis of modeling

phenomena (Baer & Sherman, 1964; Skinner,

1953), which is similar in many respects to the


one originally advanced by Miller and Dollard, also

specifies reinforcement as a necessary condition


for observational learning. In this approach
modeling is treated as a form of stimulus matching

in which a person matches the stimulus pattern

generated by his own responses to the


appropriate modeling cues. The stimulus

duplication is presumably achieved through a

process of differential reinforcement. When


matching behavior has been positively reinforced

and divergent responses either nonrewarded or


punished, the behavior of others comes to function

as discriminative stimuli for reinforcement in

controlling social responsiveness.

More recently, Gewirtz & Stingle (1968) have

conceptualized modeling as analogous to the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 404
matching-to-sample paradigm used to study

discrimination learning. In this procedure a

subject chooses from among a number of


comparison stimuli one that shares a common

property with the sample stimulus. Although

modeling and matching-to-sample performances

have some likeness in that both involve a matching

process, they can hardly be equated. A person can


achieve errorless choices on matching comparison
operatic arias with a sample Wagnerian recital,

but remain totally unable to perform the vocal

behavior exhibited in the sample. Accurate

stimulus discrimination is a precondition for, but


not equivalent with, observational response

learning. The major controversy among theories of


modeling centers around the question of what are

the necessary and sufficient conditions for the

acquisition of new responses on an observational


basis.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 405
Under naturalistic conditions the behavior

exhibited by models is typically reproduced in the

absence of direct reinforcement. Consequently,


theories that assume that some form of

reinforcement is necessary for learning tend to


invoke an intrinsic source of reinforcement. It is
assumed that if accurate reproduction of modeling

stimuli is consistently rewarded, behavioral

similarity per se acquires secondary reinforcing


properties. Thereafter a person will tend to

display a high incidence of precisely imitative

actions, which, because of their acquired reward


value, will be strengthened and sustained even

though they may never be externally reinforced.

Baer and his colleagues have conducted

several experiments designed to demonstrate

intrinsic reinforcement control of generalized

imitation. In one study (Baer & Sherman, 1964)

three imitative responses (head-nodding,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 406
mouthing, and novel verbalizations) were

established in young children by social

reinforcement from a puppet who had explicitly


instructed the subjects to match his modeled

behavior. For a subgroup of children who showed

an increase in imitative responding the puppet

displayed nonreinforced bar-pressing

interspersed among the other three rewarded


matching responses. Under these conditions some
of the children imitated bar-pressing in varying

amounts even though this particular response was

never positively reinforced. In order to further

demonstrate the dependence of generalized


imitation on direct reinforcement of other

matching responses, social approval for imitative


head-nodding, mouthing and novel verbalizations

was discontinued with two subjects. This

extinction procedure resulted in decreased


imitative bar-pressing in one of the two children;

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 407
when reinforcement of the other three modeling

responses was reinstated, imitative bar-pressing

also reappeared.

The frequent references to the above study as

providing evidence for the self-reinforcing


function of response similarity overlook the fact

that, even under explicit demands, the imitative

behavior of one-third of the children was


completely unaffected by the reinforcement
operations, and that approximately half of the

remaining children whose data are reported

showed increments in reinforced imitative

behavior but failed to perform the nonreinforced


modeled response to any significant degree. Since
reinforcement exerted no clearly predictable

effects on the occurrence of generalized imitation

it must have been largely determined by other

unmeasured and uncontrolled variables.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 408
Using similar reinforcement procedures with

social models and more powerful incentives, Baer,

Peterson, & Sherman (1967) were able to


establish generalized imitativeness in three

severely retarded children who initially displayed


a very low level of matching behavior (see Figure
3-1). After an extensive period of imitation-

contingent reinforcement had markedly increased

imitative behavior in these children (sessions 1-


14), some matching responses could be effectively

maintained without reinforcement by randomly

interspersing them among positively reinforced


imitations (sessions 15-26). However, both types

of imitative responses rapidly declined when


social approval and food were given to the

children on a temporal basis rather than

contingent upon imitative behavior (sessions 27-


31). It was further shown that both types of

matching responses could be quickly restored to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 409
Figure 3-1. Percentage of reinforced and nonreinforced
modeled responses reproduced by a child during
periods when rewards were made contingent upon the
occurrence of matching responses or upon the passage
of a given period of time (DRO). Baer, Peterson, &
Sherman, 1967.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 410
their previously high level by reintroduction of

response-contingent reinforcement (sessions 32-

38).

It has been similarly demonstrated that

schizophrenic children could acquire and maintain


Norwegian words imitatively without any

reinforcement (Lovaas, Berberich, Perloff, &

Schaeffer, 1966), and preschool children imitated


nonreinforced Russian words (Brigham &
Sherman, 1968) as long as the children were

rewarded for English words when correctly

reproduced.

Although a generalized disposition to imitate

the behavior of others can be developed by having

different persons reinforce diverse types of

responses in a variety of situations, this fact does

not necessarily demonstrate that reinforcing

properties inhere in behavioral similarity. If this

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 411
were in fact the governing mechanism, matching
responses would not undergo abrupt and marked

extinction (see Figure 3-1) the moment that


reinforcement for the larger subclass of imitative

responses is withdrawn, since one would not

expect similarity cues to lose their rewarding

value that suddenly. Rather, the intrinsic rewards


arising from precise response duplication should

sustain imitative behavior for some time even in

the absence of externally administered

reinforcers. Studies including more extensive

variations in incentive conditions, indeed, show


that generalized imitation is largely under

incentive control rather than its inherently


rewarding value. Berkowitz (1968) found that

retarded children who were rewarded for


imitative responses only at the end of the

experimental session displayed a high rate of


matching behavior as long as the food rewards

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 412
were present in the room. During sessions when

food was not displayed, imitation dropped

significantly; it was promptly re-established by


introducing the sight of food.

It should be noted that the laboratory


phenomenon that has been labeled “generalized

imitation” involves only imitation across

responses under conditions where subjects are


instructed to repeat the experimenter’s behavior.
A more stringent test of generalized imitation

would include different models performing

different responses in different social situations.

An alternative explanation for this limited form of


generalized modeling can be offered in terms of
discrimination rather than secondary

reinforcement processes. When a few

nonrewarded, modeled responses are randomly

distributed in a large number that are consistently

reinforced, the two sets of responses cannot easily

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 413
be distinguished and are therefore likely to be

performed with similar frequency. If, on the other

hand, the discriminative complexity of the


modeling task were reduced by having the model

portray a series of reinforced responses, followed

by the set of readily discriminable responses that

are never reinforced, the observer would

eventually recognize that the latter responses


never produce positive outcomes and he would, in
all likelihood, stop reproducing them. A

discrimination hypothesis thus leads to a

prediction which is opposite to that derived from

the principle of secondary reinforcement.


According to the acquired-reward interpretation,

the longer imitative responses are positively


reinforced, the more strongly behavioral similarity

is endowed with reinforcing properties and,

consequently, the greater should be the resistance


to extinction of unreinforced matching responses.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 414
In contrast, a discrimination hypothesis would

predict that the longer the differential

reinforcement practices are continued, the more


likely the observer is to distinguish between

rewarded and unrewarded imitative behaviors,


with resulting rapid decline of unrewarded
imitative responses.

The occurrence of generalized modeling is also


probably determined in part by the invariant
conditions under which laboratory tests are

conducted. Reinforced and unreinforced

responses are typically exhibited by the same

model, in the same social setting, during the same


period of time, and after subjects have been
explicitly instructed to behave imitatively. On the

other hand, under natural conditions, which are

highly variable and more easily distinguishable,

there appears to be considerable specificity to

modeling behavior. If matching responses do, in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 415
fact, automatically produce self-reinforcing effects,

then people should display widespread

reproduction of all types of behavior modeled by


children, barbers, policemen, delinquents,

professors and others. Actually, people tend to be

selective in what they reproduce, suggesting that

imitative performance is primarily governed by its

utilitarian value rather than by inherent


reinforcement derived from response similarity
per se. In other words, the theory of generalized

imitation explains more than has ever been

observed. The issue would appear to be one of

regulated performance rather than learning, since


people do know how to match the behavior of

others. Performance is primarily a function of


anticipated outcome which, in turn, are partly

determined by the degree of similarity between

new situations and past situations in which


particular responses have been reinforced.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 416
Important treatment implications follow from

the interpretations of generalized modeling, since

in both cases the goal is to establish modeling


tendencies that will not be restricted to the

treatment setting but will generalize to other,


more natural settings. On the basis of a secondary
reinforcement hypothesis, the treatment program

should include considerable imitation training

under a generous schedule of reinforcement. The


assumption made is that the more reinforcement a

person experiences for behavioral matching, the

more reinforcing it will become for him to imitate


in any situation. On the basis of a discrimination

hypothesis, on the other hand, the program would


involve only as much reinforcement as is

necessary to establish matching behavior, which

would then be rewarded by different people in a


variety of situations. Generalization is not

assumed to occur automatically; it must be built

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 417
into the treatment program.

The Skinnerian analysis of modeling


phenomena relies entirely upon the standard

three-component paradigm Sd → R → Sr, where Sd

denotes the discriminative modeled stimulus, R


represents an overt matching response, and Sr

designates the reinforcing stimulus. It is difficult to

see how this scheme is applicable to observational


learning in which an observer does not overtly
perform the model’s responses during the

acquisition phase, reinforcers are not

administered either to the model or to the

observer, and the first appearance of the acquired


response may be delayed for days, weeks, or even
months. In the latter case, which represents one of

the most prevalent forms of social learning, two of

the events (R → Sr) in the three-term paradigm are

absent during acquisition, and the third element

(Sd or modeling stimulus) is typically absent from

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 418
the situation in which the observationally learned

response is performed. Like the Miller and Dollard

theory, the Skinnerian interpretation of modeling


phenomena accounts satisfactorily for the control

of previously learned matching responses by their

stimulus antecedents and their immediate

consequences. However, it fails to explain how a

new matching response is acquired


observationally in the first place. This occurs
through covert, symbolic processes during the

period of exposure to modeling stimuli, prior to

overt responding or to appearance of any

reinforcing events. Indeed, had the children in the


experiment by Baer & Sherman been tested for

vicarious learning immediately after the model


had demonstrated the four critical responses, they

could probably have reproduced the modeled

repertoire without undergoing any imitation-


contingent reinforcement. As will be shown later,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 419
observational learning entails symbolic coding and

central organization of modeling stimuli, their

representation in memory, in verbal and imaginal


codes, and their subsequent transformation from

symbolic forms to motor equivalents. Because of


the inferential nature of these basic processes,
functional behaviorists are inclined to consider

them of limited scientific interest. However,

modeling phenomena must be analyzed in terms


not only of response-selection variables but also of

their mediational determinants before the

necessary and sufficient conditions for modeling


can be specified accurately.

In evaluating the role of reinforcement in


modeling processes, it is essential to distinguish

between response acquisition and performance

because these events are determined by different

variables. Numerous investigations, differing

considerably in the choice of incentives, types of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 420
matching responses, and age of the subjects, have

shown that performance of matching responses is

substantially increased by rewarding such


behavior in either the model (Bandura, 1965a;

Kanfer, 1965; Parke & Walters, 1967) or the

subjects (Kanareff & Lanzetta, 1960; Lanzetta &

Kanareff, 1959; Metz, 1965; Schein, 1954; Wilson

& Walters, 1966); whereas imitative


responsiveness is reduced by direct or vicarious
punishment. However, results of an experiment

bearing on the learning-performance distinction

lend support to the theory that the acquisition of

matching responses results primarily from


stimulus contiguity and associated symbolic

processes, whereas the performance of


observationally learned responses will depend to

a great extent upon the nature of reinforcing

consequences to the model or to the observer.

In this study (Bandura, 1965b), children

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 421
observed a filmed model who exhibited a

sequence of novel physical and verbal aggressive

responses. In one treatment condition the model


was severely punished following the display of

aggressive behavior; in the second, the model was

generously rewarded with delectable treats and

lavish praise; the third condition presented no

response consequences to the model. A post-


exposure performance test of imitation revealed
that the reinforcement contingencies applied to

the model’s responses resulted in differential

degrees of matching behavior. Compared to

subjects in the model-punished condition, children


in the model-rewarded and the no-consequence

groups spontaneously performed a significantly


greater variety of imitative responses. Moreover,

boys reproduced substantially more of the model’s

behavioral repertoire than girls, the differences


being particularly marked in the model-punished

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 422
treatment (Figure 3-2).

Following the performance test, children in all


three groups were offered highly attractive

incentives contingent upon their reproducing the

model’s responses in order to promote


performance of what they had acquired through

observation. As shown in Figure 3-2, the

introduction of positive incentives completely


eliminated the previously observed performance
differences, revealing an equivalent amount of

learning among children in the model-rewarded,

model-punished, and no-consequence; conditions.

Similarly, the initially large sex differential, which


in similar studies has been typically interpreted as
reflecting a deficit in masculine-role identification

by girls, was virtually eliminated.

Findings of the foregoing experiment, and

others reviewed later, suggest that the behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 423
Figure 3-2. Mean number of different matching responses
reproduced by children as a function of response
consequences to the model and positive incentives.
Bandura, 1965b.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 424
analysis advocated by proponents of the
Skinnerian approach might further advance

understanding of modeling processes if it were


separated into a learning analysis and a

performance analysis. The learning analysis is

concerned with the manner in which variables

operating at the time of exposure to modeling


stimuli determine the degree to which the

modeled behavior is learned. The performance

analysis, on the other hand, is concerned with

factors governing persons’ willingness to perform

what they have learned.

Although there is ample evidence that

reinforcing consequences can significantly alter


the probability of future occurrence of preceding

matching responses, consequent events can hardly


serve as a precondition for the acquisition of

responses that have already been performed. The


major issue of whether reinforcement is a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 425
prerequisite for observational learning can be

most definitively resolved by the use of

infrahuman subjects whose reinforcement history


can be controlled. In a preliminary study, Foss

(1964) found that birds will imitate unusual sound

patterns played on a tape recorder in the absence

of any prior reinforcement of matching responses.

In human learning, under conditions where


incentives are repeatedly given to a model as he
displays an ongoing series of responses,

observation of reinforcing outcomes occurring

early in the sequence might be expected to

increase the observer’s vigilance in respect to


subsequently modeled behavior. The anticipation

of positive reinforcement for matching responses


by the observer may, therefore, indirectly

influence the course of observational learning by

enhancing and focusing observing responses.

AFFECTIVE FEEDBACK THEORY

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 426
Mowrer’s (1960) sensory feedback theory of

imitation similarly highlights the role of

reinforcement but, unlike the preceding


approaches which reduce imitation to a special

case of instrumental learning, Mowrer emphasizes


the classical conditioning of positive and negative
emotions accompanying reinforcement to stimuli

arising from matching responses. Mowrer

distinguishes two forms of imitative learning in


terms of whether the observer is reinforced

directly or vicariously. In the former case, the

model performs a response and at the same time


rewards the observer. Through repeated

contiguous association of the model’s behavior


with rewarding experiences, these responses

gradually take on positive value for the observer.

On the basis of stimulus generalization, the


observer can later produce self-rewarding

feedback experiences simply by reproducing as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 427
closely as possible the model’s positively valenced

behavior.

In the second or “empathetic” form of imitative

learning, the model not only exhibits the response

but also experiences the reinforcing consequences.


It is assumed that the observer, in turn,

experiences empathetically the sensory

concomitants of the model’s behavior, and also


intuits his satisfactions or discomforts. As a result
of this higher-order vicarious conditioning, the

observer will be predisposed to reproduce the

matching responses for the attendant positive

sensory feedback.

There is substantial evidence (Bandura &

Huston, 1961; Grusec, 1966; Henker, 1964;

Mischel & Grusec, 1966; Mussen & Parker, 1965)

that modeling can be augmented by increasing the

rewarding qualities of a model or by having the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 428
observer witness the model experience rewarding
outcomes. These same studies, however, contain

some contradictory findings with regard to the


affective feedback theory. Even though a model’s

rewarding qualities are equally associated with

the different types of behaviors he performs,

modeling effects nevertheless tend to be specific


rather than general. That is, model nurturance

enhances the reproduction of some responses, has

no effect upon others, and may actually diminish

the adoption of still others (Bandura, Grusec &

Menlove, 1967a). A limited study by Foss (1964),


in which mynahs were taught unusual whistles

played on a tape recorder, also failed to confirm


the proposition that modeling is enhanced

through positive conditioning. His mynahs


imitated different sounds to the same extent

whether they were played in the absence of any


reinforcement or only when the birds were being

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 429
fed. It should be noted, however, that neither the

Foss study nor the experiments cited earlier

employed the type of temporal relationship


between modeling cues and the administration of

rewards that would be considered optimal for


endowing the modeled events with affective
valence.

In an elaboration of the affective feedback


theory of imitation, Aronfreed (1968) has
advanced the view that pleasurable and aversive

affective states become conditioned to cognitive

templates of a model’s behavior. Imitative

performances are presumed to be controlled by


affective feedback from intentions and from
proprioceptive cues generated during the

performance of an overt act. This

conceptualization of imitation is difficult to verify

empirically because it does not specify in sufficient

detail the characteristics of templates, the process

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 430
through which cognitive templates are acquired,

the manner in which affective valences become

conditioned to templates, or how the emotion-


arousing properties of templates are transferred

to intentions and to proprioceptive cues intrinsic

to overt responses. There is some experimental

evidence, however, that has important

implications for the basic assumptions of feedback


notions.

Feedback theories, particularly those that

partly rely on controlling functions of

proprioceptive cues, are seriously challenged by


findings of curare-conditioning experiments in
which animals are skeletally immobilized during

aversive conditioning or extinction. These studies


(Black, 1958; Black, Carlson, & Solomon, 1962;

Solomon & Turner, 1962) demonstrate the

occurrence of learning phenomena in the absence

of skeletal responding and its correlated

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 431
proprioceptive feedback. Results of

deafferentation studies (Taub, Bacon, & Berman,

1965; Taub, Teodoru, Ellman, Bloom, & Berman,


1966) also show that responses can be acquired,

performed discriminatively, and extinguished

even though sensory somatic feedback is

surgically abolished by limb deafferentation. It

would seem from these findings that the


acquisition, integration, facilitation and inhibition
of responses can be achieved through central

mechanisms independently of peripheral sensory

feedback.

It is also evident that rapid selection of


responses from among a varied array of

alternatives cannot be governed by proprioceptive


feedback since relatively few responses could be

activated, even incipiently, during

characteristically brief pre-decision periods

(Miller, 1964). In recognizing this problem,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 432
Mowrer (1960) has conjectured that the initial

scanning and selection of responses may occur

primarily at the symbolic rather than at the action


level.

Human functioning would be inflexible and

unadaptive if responsiveness were controlled by

affectivity inherent in the behavior itself. Because

social responsiveness is highly discriminative, it is


extremely doubtful that behavioral patterns are
regulated by affective qualities implanted in

behavior. To take aggression as an example,

hitting responses directed toward parents, peers,


and inanimate objects differ little, if at all;
nevertheless, physically aggressive responses

toward parents are generally strongly inhibited,


whereas physical aggression toward peers is

freely expressed (Bandura, 1960; Bandura &

Walters, 1959). Moreover, in certain well-defined

contexts, particularly in competitive, physical

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 433
contact sports such as boxing, people will easily

initiate and maintain unattenuated, physically

aggressive behavior. One would, therefore, predict


more accurately the expression or inhibition of

identical aggressive responses from knowledge of

the stimulus context (e.g., church, athletic

gymnasium), the object (e.g., parent, priest,

policeman, or peer), and other cues that signify


predictable consequences, than from assessment
of the affective value of aggressive behavior. It has

been amply demonstrated (Bandura, 1968) that

the selection and performance of matching

responses is mainly governed by anticipated


outcomes based on previous consequences that

were directly encountered, vicariously


experienced, or self-administered.

Although feedback conceptions of modeling do

not require a response to be performed before it

can be learned, they nevertheless fail to explain

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 434
the acquisition of matching behavior when

reinforcers are not dispensed either to the model

or to observers. Moreover, a vast majority of the


responses that are acquired observationally are

not affectively valenced. This is exemplified by

studies of observational learning of perceptual-

motor tasks from filmed demonstrations (Sheffield

& Maccoby, 1961) that do not contain positive or


aversive stimuli essential for the classical
conditioning of emotional responses. Mowrer has,

of course, pointed out that sensory experiences

not only classically condition positive or negative

emotions, hut also produce conditioned sensations


or images. In most cases of observational learning

images or other forms of symbolic representations


of modeling stimuli may be the only important

mediators. Sensory-feedback theories of imitation

may therefore be primarily relevant to instances


in which the modeled responses incur relatively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 435
potent reinforcing consequences capable of

endowing response-correlated stimuli with

motivational properties. Affective conditioning


should therefore be regarded as a facilitative

rather than a necessary condition for modeling.

CONTIGUITY-MEDIATIONAL THEORIES

When a person observes a model’s behavior,


but otherwise performs no overt responses, he can
acquire the modeled responses while they are

occurring only in cognitive, representational

forms. Any learning under these conditions occurs

purely on an observational or covert basis. This


mode of response acquisition has accordingly

been designated as no-trial learning (Bandura,

1965a), because the observer does not engage in

any overt responding trials, although he may

require multiple observational trials in order to


reproduce modeled stimuli accurately. Several

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 436
theoretical analyses of observational learning
(Bandura, 1962, 1965a; Sheffield, 1961) assign a

prominent role to representational mediators that


are assumed to be acquired on the basis of a

contiguity learning process. According to the

author’s formulation, observational learning

involves two representational systems—an


imaginal and a verbal one. After modeling stimuli

have been coded into images or words for memory

representation they function as mediators for

subsequent response retrieval and reproduction.

Imagery formation is assumed to occur

through a process of sensory conditioning. That is,

during the period of exposure, modeling stimuli


elicit in observers perceptual responses that

become sequentially associated and centrally


integrated on the basis of temporal contiguity of

stimulation. If perceptual sequences are


repeatedly elicited, a constituent stimulus

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 437
acquires the capacity to evoke images (i.e.,

centrally aroused perceptions) of the associated

stimulus events even though they are no longer


physically present (Conant, 1964; Ellson, 1941;

Leuba, 1940). Thus, for example, if a bell is

sounded in association with a picture of an

automobile the bell alone tends to elicit imagery of

the car. Under conditions where stimulus events


are highly correlated, as when a name is
consistently associated with a given person, it is

virtually impossible to hear the name without

experiencing imagery of the person’s physical

characteristics. The findings of studies cited above


indicate that, in the course of observation,

transitory perceptual phenomena produce


relatively enduring, retrievable images of modeled

sequences of behavior. Later reinstatement of

imaginal mediators serves as a guide for


reproduction of matching responses.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 438
The second representational system, which

probably accounts for the notable speed of

observational learning and long-term retention of


modeled contents by humans, involves verbal

coding of observed events. Most of the cognitive


processes that regulate behavior are primarily
verbal rather than visual. To take a simple

example, the route traversed by a model can be

acquired, retained, and later reproduced more


accurately by verbal coding of the visual

information into a sequence of right-left turns

(e.g., RRLRR) than by reliance upon visual imagery


of the itinerary. Observational learning and

retention are facilitated by such codes because


they can carry a great deal of information in an

easily stored form. After modeled sequences of

responses have been transformed into readily


utilizable verbal symbols, later performances of

matching behavior can be effectively controlled by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 439
covert verbal self- directions.

The influential role of symbolic representation


in observational learning is disclosed by a study

(Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1966) in which

children were exposed to several complex


sequences of modeling behavior on film, during

which they either watched attentively, verbalized

the novel responses as they were performed by


the model, or counted rapidly while watching the
film to prevent implicit verbal coding of modeling

cues. A subsequent test of observational learning

disclosed that children who verbally labeled the

modeled patterns reproduced significantly more


matching responses than those in the viewing-
alone condition who, in turn, showed a higher

level of acquisition than children who engaged in

competing symbolization.

Further supporting evidence for the influence

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 440
of symbolic coding operations in the acquisition
and retention of modeled responses is furnished

by Gerst (1969). Subjects observed a filmed model


perform complex motor responses varying in the

ease with which they could be verbally coded.

They were instructed to code the items into either

vivid images, concrete verbal descriptions of the


response elements, or convenient summary labels

that incorporated the essential ingredients of the

responses. Compared to the performance of

control subjects who had no opportunity to

generate symbolic mediators, all three coding


operations enhanced observational learning

(Figure 3-3). Concise labeling and imaginal codes


were equally effective in aiding immediate

reproduction of modeled responses and both


systems proved superior in this respect to the

concrete verbal form. However, a subsequent test


for retention of matching responses showed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 441
Figure 3-3. Percentage of modeled responses reproduced by
control subjects and those who coded the modeled
behavior as either images, concrete verbal descriptions,
or summary labels for memory representation. Gerst,
1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 442
concise labeling to be the best coding system for

memory representation. Subjects in the latter

conditions retained a significant amount of what


they learned, whereas those who relied upon

imagery and concrete verbalizations displayed a


substantial loss of matching responses.

Results of a program of research using a

nonresponse acquisition procedure (Bandura,


1965a) indicate that the organization of behaviors
elements into novel patterns resembling modeled

responses can occur at a central level without

overt responding. The present theory assumes,

however, that stimulus contiguity is a necessary,


but not a sufficient, condition for observational
learning. Modeling phenomena, in fact, involve

several complexly interrelated subprocesses, each

with its own set of controlling variables. A

comprehensive theory of observational learning

must therefore encompass the diverse subsystems

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 443
governing the broader phenomena. The main

component functions that markedly influence the

nature and degree of observational learning are


discussed next.

ATTENTIONAL PROCESSES

Since repeated contiguous stimulation alone


does not always result in response acquisition, it is

evident that additional conditions are required for


the occurrence of observational learning. Simply

exposing persons to distinctive sequences of

modeled stimuli does not in itself guarantee that


they will attend closely to the cues, that they will
necessarily select from the total stimulus complex

the most relevant events, or that they will even

perceive accurately the cues to which their

attention has been directed. An observer will fail

to acquire matching behavior, at the sensory

registration level, if he does not attend to,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 444
recognize, or differentiate the distinctive features
of the model’s responses. To produce learning,

therefore, stimulus contiguity must be


accompanied by discriminative observation.

A number of attention-controlling variables,

some related to incentive conditions, others to


observer characteristics, and still others to the
properties of the modeling cues themselves, will

be influential in determining which modeling


stimuli will be observed and which will be ignored.

Selectivity of modeling stimuli may be partly a

function of their inherent physical properties

based on intensity, size, vividness and novelty. Of

much greater importance for social learning,


however, is the acquired distinctiveness of model

attributes (Miller & Dollard, 1941). By being


repeatedly rewarded for imitating certain types of

models and not rewarded for matching the


behavior of models possessing different

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 445
characteristics, persons eventually learn to

discriminate between modeling cues that signify

differential probabilities of reinforcement. Thus,


models who have demonstrated high competence

(Gelfand, 1962; Mausner, 1954a, b; Mausner &

Bloch, 1957; Rosenbaum & Tucker, 1962), who are

purported experts (Mausner, 1953) or celebrities

(Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953), and who possess


status-conferring symbols (Lefkowitz, Blake, &
Mouton, 1955) are likely to command more

attention and to serve as more influential sources

of social behavior than models who lack these

qualities. Other distinctive characteristics, such as


age (Bandura & Kupers, 1964; Hicks, 1965;

Jakubczak & Walters, 1959), sex (Bandura, Ross, &


Ross, 1963a; Maccoby & Wilson, 1957; Ofstad,

1967; Rosenblith, 1959, 1961), social power

(Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963b; Mischel & Grusec,


1966), and ethnic status (Epstein, 1966), which

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 446
are correlated with differential probabilities of

reinforcement, likewise influence the degree to

which models who possess these attributes will be


selected for emulation.

The affective valence of models, as mediated


through their attractiveness and other rewarding

qualities (Bandura & Huston, 1961; Grusec &

Mischel, 1966), may augment observational


learning by eliciting and maintaining strong
attending behavior. At the social level, one’s

organizational affiliations and living

circumstances, which affect associational

networks and preferences, will also determine to a


large degree the types of models to whom one is
repeatedly exposed, and consequently, the modes

of behavior that will be most thoroughly learned.

An adequate theory of vicarious learning must

also explain why, under essentially identical

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 447
conditions of modeling stimulation, some persons
display higher levels of response acquisition than

others. There is suggestive evidence that


characteristics of observers, deriving from their

previous social-learning experiences, may be

associated with different observational patterns.

The extent to which modeled patterns are


reproduced is significantly influenced by observer

characteristics such as dependency (Jakubczak &

Walters, 1959; Kagan & Mussen, 1956; Ross,

1966), self-esteem (de Charms & Rosenbaum,

1960; Gelfand, 1962; Lesser & Abelson, 1959),


level of competence (Kanareff & Lanzetta, 1960),

and socioeconomic and racial status (Beyer & May,


1968); and countless studies have shown that the

effects of modeling stimuli are partly determined


by the sex of observers. Persons who have been

frequently rewarded for displaying matching


behavior (Miller & Dollard, 1941; Schein, 1954)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 448
are also apt to be most attentive to modeling cues.

Finally, motivational variables and transitory

emotional arousal significantly alter perceptual


thresholds and in other ways facilitate, impede,

and channel observing responses (Bandura &


Rosenthal, 1966; Easterbrook, 1959; Kausler &
Trapp, 1960).

It is difficult to evaluate from performance


measures alone whether the effects of observer
characteristics reflect differences in degree of

observational learning or in willingness to

perform what has been learned. Results of several

studies employing a learning analysis of modeling


(Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1966; Grusec &
Brinker, 1969; Maccoby & Wilson, 1957) disclose

that observer characteristics can serve as

determinants of observational learning.

Viewers’ observing behavior can be effectively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 449
enhanced and focused through arrangement of
appropriate incentive conditions. Persons who are

informed in advance that they will later be asked


to reproduce a given model’s responses and

rewarded in terms of the number of elements

performed correctly would be expected to pay

much closer attention to relevant modeling stimuli


than persons who are exposed to the same

modeled events without any predisposition to

observe and to learn them. The facilitative

influence of incentive set on observational

learning will be most operative under exposure to


multiple models requiring selective attentiveness

to conflicting cues. Indeed, incentive control of


observing behavior can, in most instances,

override the effects of variations in observer


characteristics and model attributes. It should be

noted, however, that in the present theory


reinforcement variables, to the extent that they

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 450
influence the acquisition process, do so principally

by augmenting and sustaining attentiveness to

modeling cues.

In addition to attention-directing variables,

stimulus input conditions (i e., rate, number,


distribution, and complexity of modeling stimuli

presented to observers) will regulate the

acquisition of modeled responses to some extent.


The observer’s capacity to process information
sets definite limits on the number of modeling

cues that can be acquired during a single exposure.

Therefore, if modeling stimuli are presented at a

rate or level of complexity that exceeds the


observer’s receptive capabilities, observational
learning will necessarily be limited and

fragmentary. Under such conditions repeated

presentations of the modeling stimuli would be

required in order to produce complete and precise

response matching.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 451
Finally, the rate and level of observational

learning will be affected by the discriminability of

modeling stimuli. Modeled characteristics that are


highly discernible can be more readily acquired

than subtle attributes which must be abstracted


from heterogeneous responses differing on
numerous stimulus dimensions. In highly intricate

response systems, such as language behavior, for

example, children typically encounter


considerable difficulty in acquiring linguistic

structures because the identifying characteristics

of different grammatical constructions cannot be


readily distinguished within extremely diverse

and complex utterances. However, when verbal


modeling cues are combined with procedures

designed to increase syntactic discriminability

(Bandura & Harris, 1966; Lovaas, 1966a; Odom,


Liebert, & Hill, 1968) relatively complicated

linguistic patterns of behavior can be acquired and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 452
modified observationally.

In therapeutic applications of modeling


procedures observational learning is often

retarded by discrimination failures arising from

deficiencies in cognitive skills, sensory-motor


handicaps, or faulty prior learning. In such cases a

program of discrimination pretraining may greatly

accelerate modeling processes. Winitz and Preisler


(1965) have shown, for example, that children
who learned to discriminate erroneous sounds

from correct sounds that they had misarticulated

subsequently displayed better imitative word

learning than children who did not receive


relevant discrimination pretraining.

RETENTION PROCESSES

The discussion thus far has been concerned

with sensory registration and symbolic coding of


modeling stimuli. Another basic component

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 453
function involved in observational learning, but
one that has been virtually ignored in theories of

imitation, concerns the retention of modeled

events. In order to reproduce social behavior


without the continued presence of external

modeling cues a person must retain the original


observational inputs in some symbolic form. This
is a particularly interesting problem in instances

where persons acquire social patterns of behavior

observationally and retain them over extended


periods of time, even though the response

tendencies are rarely, if ever, activated into overt

performance until attainment of the age or social

status at which the activity is appropriate and

permissible.

There are a number of theoretical


controversies regarding memory processes which

will not be reviewed here since they fall beyond

the scope of this book. The major questions are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 454
whether memory traces are established in an

incremental or an all-or-none fashion; whether

there exists a dual or a single memory mechanism;


and speculations about the biochemical and

neurophysiological processes whereby transient

neural after-effects of stimulation result in

relatively permanent structural alterations in the

central nervous system. Although memory


mechanisms have not as yet been adequately
explained, laboratory investigations have

identified a number of conditions that facilitate

retention, some of which have been shown to

augment modeling performances.

Among the many variables governing retention

processes, rehearsal operations effectively


stabilize and strengthen acquired responses. The

level of observational learning can, therefore, be

considerably enhanced through practice or overt

rehearsal of modeled response sequences,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 455
particularly if the rehearsal is interposed after

natural segments of a larger modeled pattern

(Margolius & Sheffield, 1961). Of greater import is


evidence that covert rehearsal, which can be

readily engaged in when overt participation is

either impeded or impracticable, may likewise

enhance retention of acquired matching responses

(Michael & Maccoby, 1961). Data are meager,


however, on the types of responses that are most
susceptible to strengthening through covert

rehearsal. Several experiments involving a variety

of tasks (Morrisett, 1956; Perry, 1939; Twining,

1949; Vandell, Davis, & Clugston, 1943), have


shown that symbolic rehearsal of activities

significantly improves their later performance.


Such practice appears to be most effective in tasks

that rely heavily upon symbolic functions.

The influential role of covert practice of

modeled behaviors has received greatest

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 456
emphasis in Maccoby’s (1959) account of the

identification process. According to this view,

controlling, nurturing, and caretaking activities


require explicit reciprocal behaviors on the part of

parents and children. Consequently, in the course

of frequent mutually dependent interactions both

participants learn, anticipate, and covertly

rehearse each other’s customary responses. In


addition to the frequency md intimacy of social
interactions, the degree of power exercised by the

model over desired resources is considered to be

an important determinant of the frequency of

fantasy role-playing. In this theory, vicarious role-


rehearsal primarily serves a defensive function;

that is, in an effort to guide his behavior toward


models who possess controlling power, a person

will imagine different courses of action for

receiving help or avoiding censure, and he will try


to anticipate as accurately as possible the model’s

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 457
probable responses to these approaches. On the

other hand, there would be little incentive to

prepare oneself for, or to practice covertly, the


behavior of models who command no rewarding

or punishing power.

Anticipatory implicit rehearsal of modeled

responses may be supported to some extent by

role reciprocity and threat from resource


controllers, but it should be noted that persons
will also be inclined to practice modeled responses

that are effective in producing rewarding

outcomes. Moreover, according to social-learning

theory, the behavior of powerful models will be


attended to, rehearsed, and reproduced even
though observers have had no direct interaction

with them, because their behavior is likely to have

high utilitarian value. This is particularly true in

the case of models who possess expert power in

particular specialties. It would be unnecessary, for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 458
example, for a novice to establish a

complementary role relationship with a qualified

automobile mechanic in order to master his skills


through observation during apprenticeship

training. Rehearsal behavior is undoubtedly

governed by different types of incentive

conditions, some of which may be entirely

independent of the model whose behavior is being


emulated.

It is generally assumed that the facilitative

effects of rehearsal result not from sheer

repetition, but rather from more active processes.


The interpolation of rehearsal in intricate modeled
sequences distributes the learning; this reduces

loss through intraserial interference from other


displayed elements (Margolius & Sheffield, 1961).

Reproduction of matching responses, either on an

overt or covert level, also provides the observer

with opportunities to identify the response

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 459
elements that he has failed to learn and thus to

direct his attention to the overlooked modeling

cues during subsequent exposure (McGuire,


1961). Finally, periodic reproduction of modeled

segments is likely to elicit and to sustain greater

attentiveness to modeling stimuli than passive

observation of lengthy, uninterrupted sequences

of behavior (Hovland, Lumsdaine & Sheffield,


1949; Maccoby, Michael, & Levine, 1961).

Symbolic coding operations, to which reference

was made earlier, an even more efficacious than

rehearsal processes in facilitating long-tern


retention of modeled events. During exposure to
stimulus sequence observers are inclined to code,

classify, and reorganize elements into familiar and


more easily remembered schemes (Bower, 1969;

Mandler 1968; Paivio, 1969; Tulving, 1968). These

coding devices may take various forms, such as

representing stimulus elements in vivid imagery

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 460
translating action sequences into abbreviated

verbal systems, and grouping constituent patterns

of behavior into larger integrated units. The


benefits accruing from rehearsal may, in fact, be

largely attributable not to associative

strengthening effects of repetition, but rather to

coding and organizational processes operating

during repeated enactments.

Decrements in retention often result primarily


from interference or unlearning arising from

either previously acquired contents or succeeding

observational inputs. These interference processes


are most influenced by the rate, temporal
distribution, and serial organization of stimulus

inputs. Under massed exposure conditions where


modeling stimuli are presented in lengthy,

uninterrupted sequences, substantial interference

effects are created which not only impair

retention, but may result in the development of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 461
highly erroneous modeling responses. In one

study (Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1966), for

example, children who had observed five


relatively complex sequences of modeled

responses during a single exposure sometimes

erroneously combined elements from the different

segments in subsequent behavioral reproductions.

The amount of forgetting and interpattern


intrusion will vary with the degree of similarity of
behavioral elements in the various modeled

sequences. On the other hand, modeling cues that

are presented in smaller units and at spaced

intervals are much less susceptible to loss through


associative interference.

MOTOR REPRODUCTION PROCESSES

The third major component of modeling

phenomena involves the utilization of symbolic

representations of modeled patterns in the form of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 462
imaginal and verbal contents to guide overt
performances. It is assumed that reinstatement of

representational schemes provides a basis for self-


instruction regarding the manner in which

component responses must be combined and

sequenced to produce new patterns of behavior.

The process of representational guidance is


essentially the same as response learning under

conditions where a person behaviorally follows an

externally depicted pattern, or is directed through

a series of instructions to enact novel response

sequences. The only difference is that, in the latter


cases; performance is directed by external cues,

whereas, in delayed modeling, behavioral


reproduction is monitored by symbolic

counterparts of absent stimuli.

The rate and level of observational learning

will be partly governed, at the motor level, by the


availability of necessary component responses.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 463
Behavior patterns of high-order complexity are

produced by combinations of previously learned

components which may, in themselves, represent


relatively intricate compounds. Modeled response

patterns are most readily achieved when they

require primarily the synthesis of previously

acquired components into new patterns exhibited

by models. On the other hand, observers who lack


some of the necessary components will, in all
probability, display only partial reproduction of a

model’s behavior. In such cases, the constituent

elements first must be established through

modeling and then, in a stepwise fashion,


increasingly complex compounds can be acquired

imitatively. Thus, for example, when a mute


autistic child failed to imitate the word baby, the

therapist modeled the component sounds, and

after these elements were established through


imitation, the child readily reproduced the word

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 464
baby (Lovaas, 1966b). As will be illustrated later,

graduated modeling procedures have proved

highly effective for modifying gross behavioral


deficits.

In many instances modeled response patterns


have been acquired and retained in

representational forms but they cannot be

reproduced behaviorally because of physical


limitations. Few basketball enthusiasts could ever
successfully match the remarkable performances

of a towering professional player regardless of

their vigilance and dutiful rehearsal.

Accurate behavioral enactment of modeling

cues is also difficult to achieve under conditions

where the model’s performance is governed by

subtle adjustment of internal responses that are

unobservable and not easily communicable. An

aspiring operatic singer may benefit considerably

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 465
from observing an accomplished voice instructor;
nevertheless, skilled vocal reproduction is

hampered by the fact that the model’s laryngeal


and respiratory muscle responses are neither

readily observable nor easily described verbally.

The problem of behavioral reproduction is further

complicated in the case of highly coordinated


motor skills, such as golf, in which a person cannot

observe many of the responses he is making and

must therefore primarily rely upon proprioceptive

feedback cues. For these reasons, performances

that contain many motor factors usually require,


in addition to the guidance of a proficient model,

some overt practice.

INCENTIVE AND MOTIVATIONAL PROCESSES

A person may acquire and retain modeled

events and possess the capabilities for skillful


execution of modeled behavior, but the learning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 466
may rarely be activated into overt performance if
negative sanctions or unfavorable incentive

conditions obtain. Under such circumstances,


when positive incentives are introduced

observational learning is promptly translated into

action (Bandura, 1965b). Incentive variables not

only regulate the overt expression of matching


behavior, but they also affect observational

learning by exerting selective control over the

modeling cue to which a person is most likely to

be attentive. Further, they facilitate selective

retention by activating deliberate coding and


rehearsal of modeled responses that have high

utilitarian value.

It is evident from the foregoing discussion that

observers do not function as passive video-tape


recorders which indiscriminately register and

store all modeling stimuli encountered in


everyday life. From a social-learning perspective,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 467
observational learning constitutes a complex

multiprocess phenomenon in which absence of

appropriate matching responses following


exposure to modeling stimuli may result from

failures in sensory registration, inadequate

transformation of modeled events to symbolic

modes of representation, retention decrements,

motor deficiencies, or unfavorable conditions of


reinforcement.

Establishment of New Response Patterns


through Modeling

Research and theoretical interpretations of

learning processes have focused almost


exclusively on a single mode of response
acquisition which is exemplified by the operant or

instrumental conditioning paradigm. In this


procedure an organism is instigated, in one way or

another, to perform responses, and

approximations progressively closer to the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 468
desired final behavior are selectively reinforced. It

is generally assumed that complex human

behavior is likewise developed under naturalistic


conditions through this type of gradual shaping

process.

Fortunately, for reasons of survival and

efficiency, most social learning does not proceed

in the manner described above. In laboratory


investigations of learning processes
experimenters usually arrange comparatively

benign environments in which errors will not

produce fatal consequences for the organism. In


contrast, natural settings are loaded with
potentially lethal consequences that unmercifully

befall anyone who makes hazardous errors. For


this reason, it would be exceedingly injudicious to

rely primarily upon trial-and-error and successive

approximation methods in teaching children to

swim, adolescents to drive automobiles, or adults

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 469
to master complex occupational and social tasks. If

rodents, pigeons, or primates toiling in contrived

situations could likewise get electrocuted,


dismembered, or bruised for errors that inevitably

occur during early phases of learning, few of these

venturesome subjects would ever survive the

shaping process.

Apart from the question of survival, it is


doubtful if many classes of responses would ever
be acquired if social training proceeded solely by

the method of successive approximations through

differential reinforcement of emitted responses.


The technique of reinforced shaping requires a
subject to perform some approximation of the

terminal response before he can learn it. In


instances where a behavioral pattern contains a

highly unusual combination of elements selected

from an almost infinite number of alternatives the

probability of occurrence of the desired response,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 470
or even one that has some remote resemblance to

it, will be zero. Nor is the shaping procedure likely

to be of much aid in evoking the necessary


constituent responses from spontaneously

emitted behavior. It is highly doubtful, for

example, that an experimenter could teach a

mynah bird the phrase “successive

approximations” by selective reinforcement of the


bird’s random squeaks and squawks. On the other
hand, housewives establish extensive verbal

repertoires in their feathered friends by verbally

modeling desired phrases either in person or by

means of recordings. Similarly, if children had no


exposure to verbalizing models it would probably

be impossible to teach them the kinds of verbal


responses that constitute a language. In cases

involving intricate patterns of behavior, modeling

is an indispensable aspect of learning.

Differential reinforcement alone can be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 471
employed to evoke new patterns of behavior

under conditions where responses are composed

of readily available elements, stimuli exist that are


capable of arousing actions that resemble the

desired pattern, erroneous responsiveness does

not produce injurious consequences, and the

learning agent possesses sufficient endurance.

Even in these cases the response acquisition


process can be considerably shortened and
accelerated by the provision of appropriate social

models. This is particularly true if a pattern of

behavior contains some elements that are rarely

performed. For example, Luchins and Luchins


(1966) found that college students made over a

thousand errors and never did fully acquire a


complicated sequence of behavior when the only

response guidance they received was in the form

of differential feedback of correctly performed


elements. By contrast, subjects provided with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 472
reinforced models learned the entire role behavior

rapidly and were spared the exasperation and

frustration evidenced by the trial- and-error


group.

A similar problem arises if the presence of


dominant pre-established behaviors precludes

emission of the desired subordinate responses

which seldom occur and, therefore, cannot be


influenced by reinforcement (Bandura & Harris,
1966; Bandura & McDonald, 1963). An experiment

designed to test whether moral judgments reflect

a fixed developmental sequence, as suggested by

Piaget’s theory (1948), or are modifiable by social-


learning variables illustrates the latter point. In
one condition of the study (Bandura & McDonald,

1963), children who exhibited a predominant,

subjective moral orientation either observed adult

models who expressed objective moral judgments,

or had no exposure to the models but were

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 473
positively reinforced whenever they expressed

objective moral judgments that ran counter to

their dominant evaluative tendencies. The


provision of models was found to be highly

effective in altering the children’s judgmental

responses (Figure 3-4). On the other hand, the

reinforcement procedure alone effected little

change in the children’s judgmental orientation


because of the relative absence of the desired
behavior.

It is evident from informal observation that

vicarious learning experiences and response


guidance procedures involving both symbolic and
live models are utilized extensively in social

learning to short-circuit the acquisition process.


Indeed, it would be difficult to imagine a culture in

which the language, mores, vocational and

avocational patterns, familial customs, and

educational, social, and political practices were

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 474
Figure 3-4. Mean percentage of objective moral judgment
responses produced by subjective children who were
either reinforced for objective judgments or exposed to
reinforced models who exemplified an objective
evaluative orientation. Plotted from the data of
Bandura & McDonald, 1967.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 475
shaped in each new member through a gradual

process of differential reinforcement without the

response guidance of models who exemplify the


accumulated cultural repertoires in their own

behavior. In social learning under naturalistic

conditions responses are typically acquired

through modeling in large segments or in toto

rather than in a piecemeal, trial-and-error fashion.

Much social learning is fostered through


exposure to behavioral modeling cues in actual or

pictorial forms. However, after adequate language

development is achieved, people rely extensively


upon verbal modeling cues for guiding their
behavior. Thus, for example, one can usually

assemble relatively complicated mechanical


equipment, acquire rudimentary social and

vocational skills, and learn appropriate ways of

behaving in almost any situation simply by

matching the responses described in instructional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 476
manuals. If the relevant responses are specified

clearly and in sufficient detail, verbally symbolized

models may have effects similar to those induced


by analogous behavioral displays (Bandura &

Mischel, 1965). The use of verbal forms of

modeling makes it possible to transmit an almost

infinite variety of values and response patterns

that would be exceedingly difficult and time


consuming to portray behaviorally.

The foregoing discussion is relevant to the

issue of instructional control of behavior. In

investigating the process of verbal control it is


essential to distinguish between the instigational
and the modeling functions of instructions.

Instructions are most likely to result in correct


performance when they both activate a person to

respond and describe the appropriate responses

and the order in which they should be performed.

Little would be gained, for example, by simply

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 477
instructing a person who has had no prior contact

with cars to drive an automobile. In studies

comparing the relative efficacy of instructions and


verbal modeling (Masters & Branch, 1969), both

types of influences produce their effects through

verbal modeling and they differ only in the

explicitness with which the required responses

are defined. As might be expected, greater


performance gains are achieved when the desired
behavior is clearly specified than when it must be

inferred from a few examples.

The basic components in the development of


complex integrated units of behavior are usually
present in subjects’ behavioral repertoires as

products either of maturation or of prior


observational learning and instrumental

conditioning. For example, persons can produce a

variety of elementary sounds as part of their

natural endowment. By combining existing sounds

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 478
one can create a novel and exceedingly complex

verbal response such as

supercalifragilisticexpialidocious. Similarly, people


are endowed with the capacity to move their

fingers, but intricate sequential arrangements of

movements are required to perform a piano

concerto. While most of the elements in activities

that are typically modeled in studies of


observational learning are undoubtedly present,
the particular combination of components in each

response may be unique.

There have been numerous experiments of


observational learning ii infrahuman species
dating back to the early studies of Thorndike

(1898) and Watson (1908). These initial


investigations, which were conducted at a time

when interpretations of imitation as instinct were

in vogue summarily dismissed the existence of

observational learning on the basis of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 479
disappointing results from a few animals tested

under weak incentives and conditions that failed

to ensure adequate observation of the


demonstrator’s performance. Subsequent studies

conducted under more favorable experimental

conditions have generally shown that primates

can learn to solve manipulative problems (Hayes

& Hayes, 1952) and animals of lower order can


acquire discriminations (Bayroff & Lard, 1944;
Church, 1957; Miller & Dollard, 1941; Solomon &

Coles, 1954), lever-pressing responses (Corson,

1967), and escape behavior (Angermeier, Schaul,

& James, 1959) and can master relatively complex


tasks (Herbert & Harsh, 1944) more rapidly

through observation than the original models


achieved by trial-and-error or response-shaping

techniques. For example, Warden and his

associates (Warden, Fjeld, & Koch, 1940; Warden


& Jackson, 1935) spent a considerable amount of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 480
time in training rhesus monkeys by trial-and-error

methods to master four problem-solving tasks in

which the animals opened doors to obtain raisins


by pulling chains, turning knobs, or manipulating

latches in certain prescribed ways. Following


training, the primate models manipulated the
puzzle devices while naive monkeys, presented

with a duplicate set of problems, observed the

skilled demonstrators. The naive observers


achieved instantaneous imitative solutions in 76

percent of the test trials! Adler and Adler (1968)

found that puppies solve problems through


observational learning soon after their eyes

become functional. Results of several experiments


(Darby & Riopelle, 1959; Herbert & Harsh, 1944)

show that the increments in performance

resulting from observation are not attributable to


the fact that the model’s demonstration may have

simply drawn attention to relevant nonsocial

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 481
stimuli in the situation.

The animal studies, with few exceptions, have


involved relatively simple responses that were

reproduced either simultaneously or immediately

after demonstration. Although relevant


comparative data are lacking, it is highly probable

that, unlike humans who are capable of acquiring

observationally and retaining large integrated


units of behavior, lower species would display a
limited capacity for delayed reproduction of

modeling stimuli due to sensory-motor

deficiencies. Delayed imitation also requires some

capacity for symbolization since the absent


modeling stimuli must be retained in symbolic
memory codes. As might be expected, the most

striking evidence of observational response

learning in animals comes from naturalistic

studies of both immediate and delayed imitation

of human responses by primates reared in human

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 482
families (Hayes & Hayes, 1952; Kellogg & Kellogg,

1933). Field studies of primate social behavior

(Imanishi, 1957; Kawamura, 1963) likewise


provide dramatic illustrations of the manner in

which idiosyncratic patterns of behavior are

acquired and transmitted to other members of the

subculture through modeling. The propagation

process is greatly influenced by pre-existing


associational networks and the social status of the
innovator.

The available cross-species data thus suggest

that the rate and level of observational learning


will be governed by the extent to which subjects
possess the requisite sensory capacities for

accurate receptivity of modeling stimuli, the motor


capacities necessary for precise behavioral

reproduction, and the capacity for representational

mediation and covert rehearsal, which is crucial for

successful acquisition and long-term retention of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 483
extended complex sequences of behavior.

In the case of humans a wide variety of

response patterns differing considerably in


content, novelty, and complexity have been

transmitted through modeling procedures under

laboratory conditions. Among the diverse classes

of behavior that have been developed are included

stylistic response patterns (Bandura, Grusec, &


Menlove, 1966; Bandura, Ross, & Ross 1963b),
distinctive modes of aggressive behavior

(Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963a; Hicks, 1965; Kuhn,

Madsen, & Becker, 1967), dramatic play patterns


(Marshall & Hahn, 1967), prosocial frustration
reactions (Chittenden, 1942), and teaching styles

(Feshbach, 1967; McDonald & Allen, 1967). At an


even higher level of complexity, it has been shown

that through exposure to the behavior of models

persons can acquire standards for self-

reinforcement and self-evaluative responses

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 484
(Bandura & Kupers, 1964; Bandura & Whalen,

1966; Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1967b),

conceptual behavior (Flanders & Thistlethwaite,


1969; Reed, 1966), moral judgmental orientations

(Bandura & McDonald, 1963), self-imposed delay-

of-gratification patterns (Bandura & Mischel,

1965), linguistic structures (Lovaas, 1966a), and

distinctive phonetic variations in verbal behavior


(Alyokrinskii, 1963; Hanlon, 1964).

GENERALITY OF MODELING INFLUENCES

It is widely assumed, on the basis of evidence


that people often produce new responses which
they have never formed or seen before, that

learning principles cannot account for innovative

behavior. Theories employing modeling principles

have often been similarly questioned on the

mistaken assumption that exposure to the

behavior of others can produce at most mimicry of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 485
specific modeled responses.

In most experimental investigations of

modeling processes a single model exhibits a


limited set of responses, and observers are

subsequently tested for precise response

duplication under similar stimulus conditions


These restricted experimental paradigms cannot
yield outcomes that extend beyond the particular

responses demonstrated. On the other hand


studies employing more complex procedures

indicate that innovative behavior, generalized

behavioral orientations, and principles for

generating novel combinations of responses can

be transmitted to observers through exposure to


modeling cues. Under conditions in which

opportunities are provided to observe the


behavior of heterogeneous models (Bandura, Ross,

& Ross, 1963b), observers typically display novel


patterns of behavior representing diverse

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 486
combinations of elements from the different

models. Illustrations of the efficacy of modeling

procedures for developing generalized conceptual


and behavioral propensities are provided in

studies designed to modify moral judgmental

orientations (Bandura & McDonald, 1963) and

delay-of-gratification patterns of behavior

(Bandura & Mischel, 1965). In these experiments


the models and observers respond to entirely
different sets of stimuli in the social-influence

setting. Tests for generalized modeling effects are

conducted by different experimenters, in different

settings, with the models absent, and with


different stimulus items. The results disclose that

observers respond to new stimulus situations in a


manner consistent with the models’ dispositions

even though the subjects have never witnessed the

models’ behavior in response to the same stimuli.

In the higher-order form of modeling

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 487
described above the modeling stimuli convey

information to observers about the characteristics

of appropriate responses. Observers must abstract


common attributes exemplified in diverse

modeled responses and formulate a principle for

generating similar patterns of behavior. Responses

performed by subjects that embody the

observationally derived rule are likely to resemble


the behavior that the model would be inclined to
exhibit under similar circumstances, even though

subjects had never witnessed the model’s

behavior in these particular situations. The

abstraction of rules from modeling cues is


achieved through vicarious discrimination

learning (Bandura & Harris, 1966), in which the


model’s responses containing the relevant

attributes are reinforced, whereas those that lack

the critical features are consistently nonrewarded.

Although modeling variables play an important

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 488
role in the development of most social behaviors,

their position with respect to language learning is

unique. Since individuals cannot acquire words


and syntactic structures without exposure to

verbalizing models, it is obvious that some amount

of modeling is indispensable for language

acquisition. However, because of the highly

generative character of linguistic behavior, it is


usually assumed that imitation cannot play much
part in language development and production. The

main argument, which is based on the mimicry

view of modeling, is as follows: Children can

construct an almost infinite variety of sentences


that they have never heard. Consequently, instead

of imitating and memorizing specific utterances


that they have heard, children learn sets of rules,

on the basis of which they can generate an

unlimited number of grammatical sentences.

It is obvious that rules about grammatical

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 489
relations between words cannot be learned unless

they are exemplified in the verbal behavior of

models. An important question therefore concerns


the conditions that facilitate abstraction of rules

from verbal modeling cues. The principle

underlying a model’s varied responses can be

most readily discerned if its identifying

characteristics are repeated in responses


involving a variety of different stimuli. If, for
example, one were to place a series of objects on

tables, on chairs, on boxes and on other objects,

and simultaneously verbalized the common

prepositional relationship between these objects,


a child would eventually discern the grammatical

principle. He could then easily generate a novel


grammatical sentence if a toy hippopotamus were

placed on a xylophone and the child were asked to

describe the stimulus event enacted.

Unlike social responses which are often readily

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 490
acquired, language learning is considerably more

difficult, because sentences represent complex

stimulus patterns in which the identifying features


of syntactic structures cannot be easily

discriminated. The influential role of both

modeling and discrimination processes in

language development is shown by findings of an

experiment (Bandura & Harris, 1966) designed to


alter the syntactic style of young children who had
no formal grammatical knowledge of the linguistic

features that were manipulated. The grammatical

constructions chosen to be modified were the

prepositional phrase, which has a high base rate of


occurrence, and the passive voice, which is

grammatically more complex and rarely displayed


by young children.

As might be expected, social reinforcement,

even when combined with a strong attentional set

to identify the characteristics of “correct”

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 491
sentences, was ineffective in increasing the use of

passives in sentences generated by the children in

response to a set of simple nouns. The majority of


subjects did not produce a single passive sentence,

and consequently, no responses occurred that

could be reinforced. Nor were the children able,

within the relatively brief exposure period, to

discern the critical syntactic category simply from


observing a model construct a series of passive
sentences. In contrast, children generated

significantly more passives when verbal modeling

cues were combined with procedures designed to

increase syntactic discriminability. The most


powerful treatment condition was one in which

the attentional set was induced, modeled passive


constructions were interspersed with some

sentences in the active voice so as to enhance

differentiation of relevant grammatical properties,


and both the model and the children were

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 492
rewarded for passive constructions. In the case of

a syntactic category as common as prepositional

phrases, reinforcement together with an active


attentional set were effective in altering children’s

usage of prepositions, but modeling cues were not


a significant contributory factor.

Further evidence for the influential role of

modeling processes in language acquisition is


provided by naturalistic studies involving
sequential analyses of children’s verbalizations

and the immediately following parental responses.

Such studies disclose that young children’s speech

is at best semi-grammatical; in approximately 30


percent of instances adults repeat children’s
verbalization in a grammatically more complex

form accenting the elements that may have been

omitted or inaccurately employed (Brown &

Bellugi, 1964); and children often reproduce the

more complicated grammatical reconstructions

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 493
modeled by adults (Slobin, 1968).

The promising findings based on laboratory

studies of modeling processes indicate that an


efficacious program of behavioral modification is

one in which change agents model the behaviors

they wish their clients to acquire. During recent

years, a number of modeling procedures have

been devised and systematically applied to effect


psychotherapeutic changes. These treatment
approaches are reviewed next.

ELIMINATION OF DEFICIT CONDITIONS THROUGH


MODELING

Many of the generalized behavior disorders

that are most intractable are characterized by

gross deficits not only in behavior but also in the

basic psychological functions essential for

learning. The more severe cases, such as autistic


children and adult schizophrenics, generally

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 494
manifest little or no functional speech; they lack
social skills that are conducive to reciprocally

rewarding relationships; and interpersonal

stimuli, which ordinarily serve as the principal


medium of social influence, often have relatively

little impact on them. Since human behavior is


largely acquired through modeling and regulated
by verbal cues and symbolic reinforcers, profound

deficiencies in functions of this nature create

major obstacles to treatment. These issues are


best exemplified by the treatment of autism.

The elimination of autistic behavior is further

complicated by the fact that such children are

characteristically engrossed in repetitive motor

activities and other forms of self-stimulatory


behavior. Consequently, they remain oblivious
much of the time to relevant environmental

influences. The marked self-isolation is also

generally coupled with strong resistance to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 495
situational demands, as evidenced by their

unwillingness to perform appropriate responses

that they are obviously capable of making (Cowan,


Hoddinoth, & Wright, 1965). When behavioral

demands within their capabilities are firmly

applied, the children are inclined to avoid

responding by evading the therapist or by

resorting to tantrums and bizarre motor activities


(Lovaas, 1966a; Colby, 1967). After such aversive
behaviors lose their functional value for avoiding

social demands through consistent

nonreinforcement, autistic children typically

respond with appropriate behavior (Risley & Wolf,


1967). However, the aversive countercontrol and

lack of positive responsiveness eventually


extinguish the concerted efforts of less durable

therapists. Disappointing treatment outcomes,

therefore, are frequently attributed to


neurophysiological malfunction.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 496
Although physiological variables are probably

contributing factors in autism, it should be noted

that even biologically deficient organisms are


capable of learning provided that appropriate

conditions are arranged. It is evident, however,


from the adverse behavior characteristics of
autism that extraordinary interventions must be

employed, particularly in initial phases, if any

fundamental changes are to be effected in the


psychological functioning of autistic children.

One of the most provocative behavioral

approaches to the treatment of autism, in which

modeling procedures figure prominently, has been


developed by Lovaas and his colleagues (Lovaas,
1967). The therapeutic program is based on the

view that the total rehabilitation of autistic and

schizophrenic children can be best achieved

through the establishment of stimulus functions

which make one amenable to social influence. This

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 497
process primarily involves developing children’s

responsiveness to modeling cues, increasing the

discriminative value of stimulus events so that


children attend and respond appropriately to

aspects of their environment that they have

previously ignored, and endowing social approval

and other symbolic stimuli with reinforcing

properties. After a strong modeling set has been


created, and children have become adequately
responsive to environmental influences, the major

task of broadening children’s social and

intellectual competencies can be effectively

carried out by parents, teachers, and other agents.


Since interpersonal communication and social

learning are extensively mediated through


language, the development of linguistic skills is

also selected as a central objective of treatment.

As noted previously, modeling outcomes

depend upon accurate perceptual input. Autistic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 498
children generally show defective reception of

external stimuli, a deficit which has been

attributed by some researchers to


neurophysiological impairment (Hutt, Hutt, &

Ounsted, 1965; Rimland, 1962). It cannot be

determined from the available data whether the

weak registration of external stimuli results from

the interfering effects of high central arousal, from


insufficient activation, from children’s intense
preoccupation with their own self-produced

stimulation, or from some other factors. Whatever

the reasons may be, it is evident that little

headway can be made toward effecting behavioral


change unless adequate control is gained over

children’s attending behavior. Lovaas’ method for


developing language functions in profoundly

autistic children, who display marked withdrawal

and bizarre self-stimulatory behaviors most of the


time, achieves attentional control through several

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 499
means. First, the therapist establishes close

physical contact by sitting directly in front of the

child so he cannot easily ignore the responses that


are being modeled. Second, during the session the

child is not permitted to avoid the therapeutic task


by withdrawal or by resorting to bizarre activities.
If necessary, the therapist physically restrains the

child from turning away, he establishes eye

contact by asking the child to look at him, and he


may withhold positive attention, address the child

sharply, or even slap him on the thigh to terminate

stereotyped bizarre behavior. Firm intervention of


this type, if thoughtfully employed, may serve a

therapeutic function when failure to respond


appropriately to situational demands reflects

unwillingness rather than inability. This is

dramatically illustrated in a telling sequence from


a film depicting the language learning program

(Lovaas, 1966b). A therapist repeatedly asks a girl

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 500
to name the color of a yellow crayon, to which she

responds with increasingly bizarre arm-flapping

and peculiar grimacing. Finally, the girl is slapped


on the thigh, and instructed to name the color,

whereupon she abruptly ceases the bizarre


behavior and calmly answers, “Yellow.” As a
further means of augmenting and sustaining the

child’s attentiveness to modeling cues, food

rewards, expressions of affection and social


approval are made contingent upon imitation.

If children’s behavioral repertoires are

impoverished, their behavioral reproductions may

be deficient even though they pay close attention


to modeling cues, because the requisite
components for the modeled responses are

lacking. In such cases complex patterns of

behavior must be reduced to small subunits of

behavior, each of which is established through

modeling. Poorly designed learning sequences,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 501
which result in stressful failure experiences,

jeopardize attentional control by reducing the

child’s motivation to observe the modeled


responses and by arousing disruptive escape

behaviors. To obviate this problem modeled

responses are carefully graduated in complexity to

assure the child a high degree of success in

behavioral reproduction.

In teaching autistic children communicative


speech a modeling-reinforcement procedure is

employed in which the therapist displays

progressively more complex forms of verbal


behavior and rewards increasingly closer
reproductions of the modeled responses. In

teaching a mute child to talk, for example, the


therapist first rewards any visual attentiveness

and random sounds made by the child. When

vocalization has been increased, the therapist

utters a sound and the child is rewarded only if he

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 502
produces a vocal response within a certain time

limit. After the therapist’s speech is established as

an effective stimulus for the child’s vocalizations,


he is reinforced only for precise verbal

reproduction of specific sounds, words, and

phrases modeled by the therapist. By this method

children are first taught elementary sounds that

have pronounced visual components and can be


manually prompted, and then, in a stepwise
fashion, more complicated utterances and

combinations of words are added. Essentially

similar methods for establishing verbal

imitativeness are described in considerable detail


by Risley & Wolf (1967) in the treatment of

autistic children, and by Sloane, Johnston, & Harris


(1968) in remedial programs for speech-deficient

young children.

As exemplified by a case illustrated in Figure 3-

5, it may require several days for an autistic child

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 503
Figure 3-5. Rate of verbal imitation by a previously mute
autistic child during the first 26 days of training. The
words and sounds are printed in lower case letters on
the days they were introduced and trained, and in
capital letters on the days they were mastered. Lovaas,
Berberich, Perloff, & Schaeffer, 1966.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 504
to master the first word, but subsequent imitative

word learning generally proceeds at a

comparatively rapid rate. The fact that the


establishment of two sounds and one verbal

response is accompanied by immediate

production of many new words composed of

elements that were never directly trained

indicates that autistic children possess greater


linguistic competencies and comprehension of
grammatical features than is commonly believed.

One would expect some language acquisition to

occur through observational learning as a function

of extensive exposure to grammatical speech. The


absence of verbal behavior in autistic children

may, therefore, partly represent a motivational


rather than a behavioral deficit. The question

remains as to whether the abrupt rise in

productivity results from children’s acquisition of


a modeling set, from realization that oppositional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 505
tactics have become nonfunctional, or some other

factors.

Lovaas also provides some evidence to indicate

that, during the initial phase of imitation training,

extrinsic incentives may be essential for accurate


observation and reproduction of the therapist’s

performances. Children displayed a high level of

accurate imitative responsiveness when rewards


were made contingent upon matching the adult’s
speech perfectly; by contrast, when children were

equally generously rewarded after a certain time

had elapsed without regard to the quality of their

verbalizations, their imitative behavior


progressively deteriorated until it bore little
resemblance to the model’s responses (Figure 3-

6). However, in later stages of treatment, similar

shifts from response- to time-contingent

reinforcement did not adversely affect modeling

outcomes.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 506
507
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 3-6. Percentage of modeled responses correctly and incorrectly reproduced by an autistic child during periods
when rewards were made contingent upon matching perfectly the adult’s speech (response
contingent) or the elapsing of a certain amount of time (time contingent). Lovaas, 1967.
When children are able to imitate new words
they are taught a labeling vocabulary so that they

understand what the words mean. This is achieved

through a form of paired-associate learning in

which the therapist presents an object (e.g., glass

of milk) or models an activity (e.g., claps hands)


and simultaneously provides the correct verbal

label. On succeeding trials the adult’s verbal

prompt is gradually withdrawn until eventually

the child gives correct verbal responses to the

nonverbal events alone. In this way a wide variety


of object-word associations are learned and

discriminated. Reading skills are established in a


similar manner except that letter-picture and

letter-word associations are presented to the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 508
children until they learn to make the appropriate

verbal responses to printed words in the absence

of pictorial or vocal prompts.

After children have been taught to speak and

to correctly label common objects and activities,

training in abstract linguistic functions begins.

This program consists essentially of rewarding the

child’s discriminative responsiveness to verbally


or behaviorally modeled events. Whenever the
child fails to respond or responds incorrectly he is

aided by verbal and manual prompts which are

gradually faded out on succeeding trials.


Prepositional training will illustrate the basic
discriminations that are developed. Behavioral

matching of a verbal stimulus can be more easily


achieved by autistic children than verbally

labeling nonverbal events. Therefore, initially the

adult gives a verbal instruction involving a

preposition (e.g., “Put the ball inside the box”) and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 509
the child is rewarded for performing the motor

response appropriate to the verbal stimulus. If the

child fails to execute the response correctly, the


therapist moves the child’s hand with the ball to

the box while verbalizing the action. In the second

discrimination, objects are arranged in a particular

way and the child is asked to describe verbally the

relationships between the objects, using the


proper preposition. In the third step, which calls
for grammatical conversation, the child responds

verbally to a verbal stimulus (e.g., “Where did I put

the bicycle?”) without concomitant behavioral

enactment of the events to which reference is


made. As in other forms of rule learning, children

are taught to generalize the linguistic rule by


modeling a variety of objects in a variety of

prepositional relationships. Essentially the same

procedures have been successfully employed to


establish increasingly complex forms of linguistic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 510
and conceptual behaviors (Lovaas, Berberich,

Kassorla, Klynn, & Meisel, 1966; Lovaas, Dumont,

Klynn, & Meisel, 1966). In the case of echolalic


children, inappropriate matching responses are

extinguished through reinforcement withdrawal,


but otherwise the training program is similar to
that employed with mute cases. However, since

echolalic children have already developed

imitative speech, they start at a more advanced


level and proceed at a much faster rate.

Formal language training is well suited for

establishing verbal skills but it may result in

speech that is lacking in spontaneity and overly


dependent upon specific external cuing. To
remove this problem, after the requisite skills for

generative grammatical speech have been

established, children are taught to use their

language to initiate and maintain social

interactions, to express their feelings and desires,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 511
and to seek and exchange information about their

environment. Self-generated spontaneous speech

is initially fostered in several ways. First, by


withholding desired objects and activities until

children verbalize their wants, they are taught to

influence and control their environment verbally;

second, they are encouraged to develop comments

and stories about activities depicted pictorially in


magazines and books and are rewarded for
increasingly elaborate and novel verbalizations;

third, they are asked to recount, in detail, past

experiences; and finally, the concepts that they

have learned in the formal tasks are extended into


informal daily interactions. Indeed, as treatment

progresses the formal training procedures are


incorporated into more natural interpersonal

interactions, where verbal approval, affectional

expressions, play activities and a sense of


accomplishment replace primary rewards as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 512
major reinforcing events.

Self-care skills, play patterns, appropriate sex-


role behaviors, intellectual skills, and

interpersonal modes of behavior can be

established in autistic children more rapidly than


linguistic patterns by modeling the appropriate

activities and rewarding the children’s emulations

(Lovaas, Freitag, Nelson, & Whalen, 1967). The


training program in nonverbal behavior relies
upon the same basic methods employed in

language learning. The therapist first establishes

control over children’s attending behavior;

complex response patterns are gradually


elaborated by modeling activities in small steps of
increasing difficulty; manual prompts are utilized

if children fail to respond. The prompts are

gradually withdrawn and reinforcement for

prompted behavior is later withheld to counteract

passive responsiveness. After imitative behavior is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 513
strongly developed, stimulus control of children’s

behavior is shifted from modeling cues to verbal

prompts and appropriate environmental stimuli.


Children may, for example, initially engage in

painting activities only when they are modeled by

an adult, but by reinforcing painting in response to

verbal suggestions and art materials they

eventually learn to pursue such activities without


requiring a performing model.

The encouraging results of the project

described above would suggest that a modeling-

reinforcement approach merits serious


consideration in the treatment of schizophrenic
disorders. Since the beneficial outcomes are

achieved with nurses, parents, and college


students serving in the role of therapists, this

treatment approach gains further social

significance. However, evidence that children vary

tremendously in their rates of learning,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 514
particularly in early stages of training, indicates

the need for comparative studies to evolve

procedures that would permit even greater


control over the change process. For example,

discrimination of modeling stimulus inputs is an

important prerequisite to their acquisition. In the

case of language learning, a brief program of

discrimination pretraining may greatly accelerate


modeling outcomes and reduce variability
resulting from deficiencies in speech perception.

For children who do not know the meanings of

modeled utterances, word reproduction is apt to


be a dull and tiresome exercise. A preliminary
program aimed at producing word comprehension

would make the situation more meaningful and


perhaps facilitate productive word learning. A

sequence similar to this type has been employed

by Humphery (1966) in developing language

functions in autistic children. As a way of ensuring

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 515
necessary attentiveness, children are seated in a

semi-darkened room and equipped with

earphones. In the initial language comprehension


phase of the program children see pictures of

objects projected on a screen and hear the

corresponding verbal labels without having to

reproduce them. After the word-object association

has been repeated sufficiently to establish the


meanings of the utterances, children are
reinforced for correct production of modeled

verbalizations. Generalization and discrimination

are not left to chance: Thus, children may first see

a dog as the focal object of a slide, but later it is


presented as part of increasingly complex arrays

of animals that will have to be accurately


discriminated. By including pictures or

demonstrations representing actions, qualifying

attributes, and object interrelationships the same


procedure can be extended to develop

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 516
increasingly complex linguistic skills. Humphery

has also found it advantageous to include samples

of the children themselves and their peers


pursuing activities in their natural surroundings,

because the immediacy of these stimuli make


them especially vivid and compelling inputs. This
approach is similar in many respects to language

learning under naturalistic conditions where

children observe a considerable amount of verbal


behavior before they are taught to produce words

and grammatical sentences. However, the optimal

sequences for word and meaning training remain


to be demonstrated.

Except for a few minor applications (Sherman,


1965; Wilson & Walters, 1966), there has been no

systematic use of modeling procedures in the

treatment of adult psychotics. This is all the more

surprising considering that a majority of the

chronic cases suffer from debilitating behavioral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 517
deficits which must be overcome if they are to

function effectively in community life. The relative

neglect of this powerful approach probably results


in large part from therapists’ strong allegiances

solely to operant conditioning methods or to

interview procedures in which a great deal of time

is devoted to analyzing patients’ ineffectual

behaviors.

MODIFICATION OF PREPOTENT RESPONSE


PATTERNS THROUGH SYMBOLIC MODELING

The discussion thus far has been concerned


with the use of modeling procedures to overcome

behavioral deficits. In many instances, a change

agent is faced with the opposite problem—that of

eliminating strongly established patterns of

deviant or maladaptive behavior. One might


attempt to accomplish this objective by a program

of differential reinforcement, in which socially


desirable behavior is positively reinforced and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 518
deviant response patterns are either nonrewarded
or punished. Selective reinforcement is often a

slow and inefficient process when a person


displays a strong dominant response tendency and

desired alternative modes of behavior are only

weakly established or nonexistent in his

behavioral repertoire. Under these circumstances,


one may have to wait an unnecessarily long or

indefinite time for the appearance of alternative

responses. In such cases, the change process may

be greatly facilitated by the use of modeling

procedures designed to transmit, elicit, and


support modes of response that are incompatible

with the deviant behavior that a therapist is


attempting to eliminate. This, in effect, was the

strategy employed by Chittenden (1942) in


modifying children’s hyperaggressive and

domineering responses to frustration.

It has been widely assumed on the basis of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 519
psychodynamic theories and energy models of

personality that either vicarious participation in,

or the direct expression of, aggressive behavior


serves to discharge “pent-up energies and affects”

and thereby to reduce, at least temporarily, the

incidence of aggressive behavior. Guided by this

catharsis theory, many parents, educators,

rehabilitation workers, and child psychotherapists


subtly or openly encourage hyperaggressive
children to express aggression in one form or

another. The overall evidence from laboratory

studies (Bandura, 1965a; Berkowitz, 1969)

strongly indicates that psychotherapies employing


these conventional cathartic or abreactive

procedures may be unwittingly maintaining


deviant behavior at its original strength or, more

likely still, increasing it rather than producing the

expected reductions in aggressive tendencies. In


contrast, therapy based upon social-learning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 520
principles would concentrate, from the outset,

upon developing and strengthening constructive

alternative patterns of behavior. Proceeding on


this basis, Chittenden employed symbolic

modeling procedures for altering children’s


aggressive reactions to frustration.

Children who were excessively domineering

and hyperaggressive observed and discussed a


series of eleven 15-minute plays in each of which
dolls, representing preschool children, exhibited

an aggressive solution and a cooperative,

alternative solution to interpersonal conflicts

under circumstances that the children were likely


to encounter in everyday interactions. In addition
to modeling these two competing response

patterns, the consequences of aggression were

shown to be unpleasant and those of

cooperativeness to be rewarding. In one of the

modeled situations, for example, two boys engage

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 521
in a fight over the possession of a wagon; during

the struggle the wagon is broken, and both boys

end up unhappy. By contrast, the cooperative


alternative presents the boys enjoying themselves

as they take turns playing with the wagon.

Children for whom the different reactions and

consequences were modeled showed a decrease in

dominative aggressiveness (as measured by


situational tests in which two children were
placed in a room with a single attractive toy)

compared with a group of similarly

hyperaggressive children who received no


treatment. Of even greater interest is the finding
that children who had observed the discriminative

modeling displayed a significant decrease in


domination and an increase in cooperativeness as

assessed from behavior observations in the

nursery school made prior to treatment,

immediately after treatment, and a month later

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 522
(Figure 3-7). One cannot determine from these

data the relative contribution of vicarious

reinforcement and modeling to the obtained


outcomes. The children’s spontaneous comments

and enactments during test trials, in which they

were required to provide their own solutions to

social conflicts involving the dolls, indicated that

they had learned the cooperative strategies. Some,


however, gave evidence of also being strongly
affected by the consequences depicted: “Well, let’s

don’t have them fight; I don’t like to have them

bump their faces together, that hurts.…Let’s have

them take turns; then they won’t fight. Let them


ask Darrell (subject’s name) what to do. ‘Ask me,

Sandy and Mandy (dolls’ names). I’ll tell you to


take turns; then you won’t have a fight’

(Chittenden, 1942, pp. 53-54).”

In a preliminary report Gittelman (1965)

illustrates how behavioral enactment methods can

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 523
Figure 3-7. Amount of cooperative and domineering
behavior exhibited by hyperaggressive children before
and after receiving symbolic modeling treatment.
Drawn from the data of Chittenden, 1942.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 524
be adapted for modifying aggressive behavior in

older children. They are first asked to describe

situations that typically provoke them to


aggression and belligerence. A hierarchy of

irritating situations is then constructed, ranging

from those causing only mild annoyance to

extremely instigatory ones. The child and other

group members enact these progressively


aggravating situations and practice effective
nonviolent means of coping with them.

The treatment program devised by Chittenden

primarily relied upon modeling techniques. After


desired patterns of behavior have been
established through some form of modeling, their

maintenance will be largely controlled by the


reinforcement practices existing within the

naturalistic setting. Hence, it may be necessary to

arrange favorable consequences to support newly

acquired response patterns. This would apply

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 525
particularly to behavior that is ordinarily

associated with less optimal reinforcement

conditions, as in the case of cooperativeness which


was more difficult to establish and to maintain.

The combined use of modeling and reinforcement

procedures is probably the most efficacious

method of transmitting, eliciting, and maintaining

social response patterns.

There is additional evidence that symbolic


modeling approaches, in which desired response

patterns are demonstrated concretely through

play activities, may be especially well suited for


modifying the behavior of young children,
Marshall & Hahn (1967) showed that preschool

children who participated in several sessions of


doll play with an adult who enacted topics

commonly used in children’s play subsequently

increased their dramatic play with peers in daily

interactions. The absence of any significant

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 526
changes in the play behavior of control groups of

children who either received the same amount of

adult warmth and attention during the assembly


of blocks and puzzles or had no contact with the

adult indicates that modeling and support of social

play behavior was the major determinant.

The foregoing studies illustrate the way in

which the same method, doll play, is utilized in


radically different ways depending upon whether
one views behavior from a psychodynamic or a

social-learning perspective. In the former case,

children are typically prompted to enact in doll


play assaultive and other negative response
tendencies toward parents, teachers, siblings and

peers which, if transferred to real life situations,


would further exacerbate their problems. In

contrast, the latter approach provides more

satisfactory solutions to interpersonal conflicts

and models beneficial modes of behavior that are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 527
likely to foster positive social experiences.

Results of a study by O’Connor (1969)

involving positive symbolic modeling lend further


empirical support to the above view. Preschool

children were selected who showed extreme

social withdrawal, a behavior problem that often

reflects both deficits in social skills and fear of

close interpersonal contact. Half of these children


were shown a control film, while a matched group
of isolates observed a sound film depicting a

variety of social interactions at a progressively

more spirited activity level. Each filmed sequence


portrayed a child initially watching the ongoing
activities at a distance but eventually joining and

interacting with the children, with evident positive


consequences. In a behavioral assessment

conducted immediately after the treatment

session the controls remained markedly

withdrawn, whereas children who received the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 528
symbolic modeling showed a substantial increase

in social interaction to the baseline level displayed

by nonwithdrawn children (Figure 3-8). With the


provision of adequate practice and reinforcement

of newly established social skills, such behavior

would undoubtedly assume greater functional

value and endure.

OTHER THERAPEUTIC AND INSTRUCTIONAL


APPLICATIONS OF MODELING

Applications of modeling procedures are by no

means confined to children or to grossly deviant


conditions. Behavioral enactment methods are

frequently utilized for a wide variety of purposes

in which people who want to develop new

competencies are provided with actual or

symbolic models of desired behavior. They are


given opportunities to perform these patterns

initially under nonthreatening conditions before


they are encouraged to apply them in their

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 529
Figure 3-8. Amount of social interaction shown by
withdrawn children in the symbolic modeling and
control conditions before and after the experimental
sessions. The dotted line represents the amount of
social interaction displayed by a group of nonisolate
children whose behavior was observed at the pretest
phase of the study. O’Connor, 1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 530
everyday lives. Since, in modeling approaches, a
person observes and practices alternative ways of

behaving under lifelike conditions, transfer of


learning to naturalistic situations is greatly

facilitated.

Some treatment approaches, such as Kelly’s


(1955) fixed-role therapy, rely almost exclusively
upon modeling procedures. In the initial phase the

therapist writes a personality sketch suitable for


enactment by the client. He is then asked to

perform the role behaviors continuously as if he

were, in fact, the person portrayed in the sketch.

For example, a passive nonassertive person may

be assigned an active assertive role. The new


behavioral patterns, which are usually in marked

contrast to the client’s customary modes of


responses, are consistently enacted for several

weeks or some other preselected period. This


phase of the program is structured to the client as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 531
representing brief experimentation with, rather

than permanent adoption of, new characteristics.

Moreover, the client is never told that he should be


the new character, only that he should act like him

on a trial basis. The emphasis on brief

experimentation and simulation is considered

essential for minimizing the initial threat of

making sweeping changes in one’s mode of life.

Prescribing a role by itself will be of limited


value unless a person knows how to translate it

into concrete actions under a variety of

circumstances. In Kelly’s approach the treatment


sessions, usually scheduled on alternate days, are
mainly devoted to rehearsing the prescribed role

as it might apply to everyday events involving


vocational and social relationships, heterosexual

interactions, parental relations, and life

orientations. Therapist and client usually alternate

in the role enactment. Through such role-reversal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 532
the client not only benefits from the therapist’s

demonstration of skillful ways of relating to

others, but he also experiences how people are


likely to be affected by the behaviors being

modeled.

After new forms of responsiveness to different

types of interpersonal situations have been

adequately rehearsed, and the client’s actual


experiences in implementing the role have been
thoroughly discussed, the client decides whether

or not he wishes to adopt the new role behaviors

on a more lasting basis. If he has found the new


role effective and wishes to go on with the
program the behavioral rehearsals are continued

as long as necessary. With further experience the


client becomes increasingly skillful and

comfortable in the new role behaviors until

eventually they are spontaneously performed.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 533
Although there is every reason to expect from

evidence of modeling studies that the type of

approach advocated by Kelly should be highly


efficacious, there have been no systematic

attempts made to measure the degree of success


associated with this particular method. Research
is also needed to determine whether the

recommended practices—the selection of

markedly contrasting behavior that is


continuously enacted under a simulated set in all

areas of social functioning—are, in fact, the

optimal conditions for establishing new role


behaviors. Desired outcomes might be more

consistently attained by gradual role adoption in


progressively more difficult social situations than

by complete role enactment from the outset.

Under a graduated procedure the behavioral


requirements would be adjusted to the client’s

capabilities at any given time and would hence

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 534
reduce the possibility that his initial attempts at

new ways of behaving would be poorly received


by others. By careful selection of both the real-life

situations in which the client enacts new modes of

behavior and the manner in which they are


expressed, the likely consequences of modeled

behavior can be controlled to a considerable

extent rather than left to fortuitous circumstances.

There are many other treatment approaches in


which modeling techniques, variously labeled

psychodramatic enactment (Moreno, 1958; Sturm,

1965), behavior rehearsal (Lazarus, 1966; Wolpe

& Lazarus, 1966), and role playing (Corsini &


Putzey, 1957) are employed to overcome specific
response deficits or to transmit more extensive

repertoires of social behavior. Modeling in the

form of role practice has also been extensively

adapted for training of industrial and managerial

skills (Corsini, Shaw, & Blake, 1961). Strategies to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 535
be followed in implementing modeling principles

are presented in strong prescriptive terms and the

methods are credited with much success, but as is


generally true of the psychotherapy literature,

rigorously controlled studies of outcomes are

virtually nonexistent.

The efficacy of modeling approaches will be

largely determined by what is being enacted. If


change agents mainly encourage clients to
perform their customary ineffectual forms of

behavior, to reconstruct past relationship

experiences, and to revivify the emotional


reactions engendered by their inadequacies, then
these methods are unlikely to fare any better than

interpretive interview approaches that similarly


accentuate the negatives. On the other hand,

treatment approaches that employ modeling

procedures to establish effective modes of

behavior often lack an adequate transfer training

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 536
program in which clients are provided with

opportunities to test their newly acquired skills

under conditions likely to produce rewarding


consequences. If change agents themselves

portray requisite interpersonal competencies, and

arrange optimal conditions for their clients to

learn and to practice more effective means of

coping with potential problems, then this type of


approach is almost certain to prove successful.

Before turning to other issues we should like to

comment briefly on the nature of the effects

produced through modeling processes. When


people are deliberately instructed to observe and
to reproduce either the behavior exemplified by

others or an imaginatively reconstructed role,


there may be a tendency to view the resultant

changes as feigned and superficial. In fact, as will

be shown in the concluding chapter, role

enactment techniques have proved to be one of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 537
the most effective means of inducing stable

affective and attitudinal changes. These findings

provide support for the view that self-evaluative


and cognitive events may be partly epiphenomena

arising from one’s competencies and the

consequences of one’s behavior. Modeling, even

under simulated conditions, can have far-reaching

effects.

MODELING PROCESSES IN INTERVIEW


PSYCHOTHERAPIES

It is generally assumed that personality


modifications in conventional verbal treatments

are achieved in part by clients’ identification with

their psychotherapists. However, as Mowrer

(1966) has noted, therapists characteristically

model a very limited range of social behavior, and


what they do exemplify most prominently may

have little utilitarian value for clients. The paucity


of helpful modeling cues applies particularly to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 538
treatment approaches that advocate a behavioral
incognito in which therapists’ feelings, personal

opinions, and social responses are exhibited as


little as possible in order to facilitate the

occurrence of infantile transference reactions. To

the extent that therapists’ taciturnity and

interpretive behaviors are mimicked by clients in


their social relationships, as is not infrequent, they

are apt to be considered as bores or pests. In

contrast to conventional practices that invoke

some degree of therapist ambiguity and

concealment, Mowrer advocates that therapeutic


agents actively model what their clients are

supposed to learn and arrange conditions that will


foster identificatory outcomes. Hence, in integrity

therapy (Drakeford, 1967; Mowrer, 1964), which


is designed to get clients to recognize that they are

partly accountable for their life situations because


of their objectionable and duplicitous behavior,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 539
the therapist himself consistently models self-

disclosure and personal accountability.

During the course of conversational treatment

some of the therapists’ attitudes and personal

preferences are inevitably revealed through their


selective responsiveness and interpretive

comments (Parloff, Iflund, & Goldstein, 1960).

These inferred attitudes are likely to be emulated


by clients even though therapists may strive to
maintain neutrality in the value domain. Some

suggestive evidence of this effect is reported by

Rosenthal (1955) who found that clients who

were judged as showing greatest clinical


improvement changed their values in the areas of
sex, aggression and authority in the direction of

their therapists’ values whereas clients who were

rated unimproved became less like their

therapists. The occurrence of value congruences

during the course of therapy is also shown by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 540
Pentony (1966). It cannot be determined from

these data, however, whether the value

similarities are attributable to modeling or to


differential reinforcement of clients’

verbalizations; undoubtedly both kinds of

influence processes are operative.

There have been several recent

demonstrations that the classes of responses that


traditional psychotherapists are interested in
modifying can be significantly influenced by

modeling procedures. Schwartz & Hawkins (1965)

found that adult schizophrenics whose emotional


statements were positively reinforced in group
therapy increased affective expressions when

their group was provided with two patient models


who frequently verbalized their feelings; under

the same reinforcement conditions affective

responsiveness was decreased when the added

models displayed predominantly nonaffective

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 541
verbalizations. Marlatt, Jacobsen, Johnson, &

Morrice (1966) found that interviewees were

more inclined to reveal personal problems after


witnessing a brief waiting-room conversation in

which a model’s self-disclosure was either

accepted or socially rewarded by the interviewer

than if the model’s behavior was discouraged or

subjects had no exposure to a problem-admitting


model.

One of the obstacles to efficient conduct of

interview therapy arises from the fact that clients

are usually confused about what they are


supposed to do in order to achieve beneficial
effects, and verbal explanations inadequately

convey the requisite role behaviors. This


ambiguity can be easily overcome by providing

clients with concrete examples of appropriate

therapeutic responsiveness (Marlatt, 1968a,

1968b). In several studies Truax and his

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 542
colleagues (Truax & Carkhuff, 1967) demonstrated

that clients who listened to tape-recorded

excerpts exemplifying self-exploration


(considered to be “good” therapy behavior) prior

to undergoing treatment subsequently achieved

greater positive changes on a variety of

personality tests than did clients who received the

same type of treatment without the initial


modeling experience.

The foregoing studies indicate that modeling

procedures can be successfully employed to

induce changes in verbal behavior. However,


considering the weak relationships that exist
between alterations at the verbal level—whether

in the form of value preferences, verbal


statements, or endorsements of personality test

items—and nonverbal modes of response, it

would seem that models could be used far more

advantageously to promote effective interpersonal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 543
behaviors directly.

Vicarious Conditioning of Emotional


Responsiveness

It is generally assumed that persons develop

emotional responses on the basis of direct painful


or pleasurable stimulation experienced in

association with certain places, people, or events.


Although many emotional responses are

undoubtedly acquired by means of direct classical


conditioning, affective learning in humans

frequently occurs through vicariously aroused

emotions. Many phobic behaviors, for example,

arise not from actual injurious experiences with


the phobic objects, but rather from witnessing
others either respond fearfully toward, or be hurt

by, certain things (Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter,


1969; Bandura & Menlove, 1968). Similarly,

persons often acquire, on the basis of exposure to

modeled stimulus correlations, intense emotional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 544
attitudes toward members of unpopular minority

groups or nationalities with whom they have had

little or no personal contact.

As suggested above, vicarious emotional

conditioning results from observing others

experience positive or negative emotional effects

in conjunction with particular stimulus events.

Both direct and vicarious conditioning processes


are governed by the same basic principle of
associative learning, but they differ in the source

of the emotional arousal. In the direct prototype,

the learner himself is the recipient of pain- or


pleasure-producing stimulation, whereas in
vicarious forms somebody else experiences the

reinforcing stimulation and his affective


expressions, in turn, serve as the arousal stimuli

for the observer. This socially mediated

conditioning process thus requires both the

vicarious activation of emotional responses and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 545
close temporal pairing of these affective states

with environmental stimuli.

VICARIOUS EMOTIONAL AROUSAL

Experimental investigations of this

phenomenon have been concerned with

determining the factors that govern the degree to


which people become emotionally aroused by the

experiences of others. Some of the studies have


attempted to identify the social cues that are most

influential in producing vicarious arousal, while

still others have been designed to elucidate the


social-learning conditions whereby social cues
become endowed with emotion-eliciting potency.

One of the earliest studies of vicarious affective

arousal was reported by Dysinger & Ruckmick


(1933), who measured the autonomic responses

of children and adults to movie scenes depicting

dangerous situations and romantic-erotic displays.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 546
The findings showed that scenes of danger,
conflict, or tragedy elicited the greatest emotional

reactions among young children, but

responsiveness decreased progressively with


increasing age. The inverse relationship obtained

was attributed to the greater ability of older


persons both to discriminate between fantasied
and realistic situations and to attenuate the

aversiveness of danger cues by forecasting

eventual favorable outcomes. As would be


expected, emotional reactions to erotic scenes

were stronger among subjects in older age groups.

More recent demonstrations of vicarious

emotional instigation through filmed stimulation

is provided in a series of experiments by Lazarus


and his associates (Lazarus, Speisman, Mordkoff, &
Davison, 1962). Continuous recordings of subjects’

autonomic responses were obtained during

presentation of a film portraying a primitive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 547
puberty ritual of an Australian tribe in which a

native boy underwent a crude genital operation.

College students displayed heightened autonomic


responsiveness while viewing the genital

subincision scenes, the reactions being

particularly marked when the operation was

accompanied by sobs and other pain cues on the

part of the young initiate. Both the deletion of the


vocal pain cues and the provision of sound-tract
commentaries that minimized the aversiveness of

the depicted operation significantly reduced the

subjects’ level of emotional arousal; conversely,

commentaries highlighting the suffering and


hazards of such operations enhanced observers’

physiological arousal (Speisman, Lazarus,


Mordkoff, & Davison, 1964).

In an erudite analysis of vicarious processes,

Berger (1962) restricts the phenomenon of

vicarious instigation to situations in which an

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 548
observer responds emotionally to a performer’s

presumed affective experiences. Since the

emotional state of another person is not directly


observable, its presence, quality, and intensity is

typically inferred both from stimuli impinging

upon the performer, and behavioral cues

indicative of emotional arousal. As Berger points

out, a person may be vicariously instigated on the


basis of erroneous inferences from stimulus
events, as in the case of a mother who responds

fearfully at seeing her child fall, even though the

child is, in fact, unhurt and undisturbed. Similarly,

a bystander may react apprehensively to hearing a


sudden loud scream although, unknown to him,

the distressing vocalizations are simulated as part


of a game.

Berger has reasoned that a loud scream that

elicits a fear response from the observer may

represent a case of pseudovicarious instigation,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 549
because the vocal cue may serve merely as a

conditioned fear stimulus independent of the

performer’s unconditioned emotional response or


the stimulus situation. The basis for this

distinction is debatable, since expressive cues are

the observable indicants of a performer’s assumed

emotional state and, as will be shown later, it is

precisely because such social cues have acquired


emotion-provoking properties that an observer
can be at all vicariously aroused by the

experiences of another person. There are,

however, instances in which covariations in the

emotional responses of observers and performers


do not necessarily involve vicarious instigation

processes. After a given environmental stimulus


has acquired strong eliciting potency for an

observer, his emotional responses are likely to be

evoked directly by the conditioned stimulus,


regardless of the behavior of others. Thus, for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 550
example, when individuals become fearful upon

hearing the sound of a fire alarm in the building in

which they are working, they may be responding


similarly, because of like conditioning histories,

but independently to the same nonsocial cue.


Under these circumstances it is exceedingly
difficult to establish precisely the stimulus sources

of the observer’s emotional state since the

behavior of others, depending on its character,


undoubtedly augments or reduces the effects of

environmental eliciting stimuli. The most

convincing demonstration of vicarious instigation


is therefore provided under conditions where the

observer’s emotional responses are elicited


entirely by the performer’s affective expressions.

Such conditions are established by ensuring that

the stimuli which elicit emotional responses in the


performer either are unobservable by, or of

neutral valence for, the observing subject.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 551
Miller and his colleagues (Miller, Banks, &

Ogawa, 1962, 1963; Miller, Murphy, & Mirsky,

1959) have identified, through the use of an


ingenious cooperative avoidance-conditioning

procedure, some of the social cues that serve as


conditioned stimuli for affective arousal in
observers. Rhesus monkeys were first trained to

avoid an electric shock by pressing a bar

whenever a stimulus light appeared. After the


avoidance training, the animals were seated in

different rooms, and the bar was removed from

the chair of one monkey and the stimulus light


from the other. Thus, the animal having access to

the light stimulus had to communicate by means


of affective cues to his partner, equipped with the

response bar, who could then perform the

appropriate instrumental response that would


enable both animals to avoid painful stimulation.

Distress cues exhibited by the stimulus monkeys

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 552
in anticipation of shock were highly effective in

eliciting fear in their observing companions as

reflected in increased heart rate and rapid

performance of discriminated avoidance


responses (Miller, 1967). The finding that color

slides showing the stimulus animal in fear or pain

elicited more avoidance responses than pictures of


the same animal in nonfearful poses indicates that
simple facial and postural expressions alone are

sufficient cues for eliciting emotional responses.

The investigators further showed that emotional


responses in monkeys could be vicariously

aroused not only by the sight of their experimental

counterparts, but also, through stimulus

generalization, by another monkey who was never

involved in the original aversive contingencies.


Moreover, mere exposure to a monkey reacting in

an apprehensive or fearful manner could reinstate


avoidance responses in the observer after they

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 553
had been extinguished to a zero level.

The above studies demonstrate that affective


expressions by others can serve as conditioned

aversive stimuli, but they do not explain how such

cues acquire their potency. That sensitivity to


expressive cues results from social-learning

experiences receives support from Miller, Caul, &

Mirsky (1967), who found that monkeys reared in


total social isolation during their infancy were
unresponsive, either behaviorally or

autonomically, to facial expressions of emotions of

other monkeys. There is evidence that social cues

signifying affective arousal acquire emotion-


provoking properties through essentially the same
process of classical conditioning that is involved in

the establishment of positive or negative valence

for nonsocial environmental stimuli. That is, if

affective expressions of others have been

repeatedly followed by emotional consequences

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 554
for observers, affective social cues alone gradually

attain the power to instigate emotional reactions

in observers. In naturalistic situations such


emotional covariations occur frequently. Persons

who are experiencing positive emotions are likely

to treat others in amiable ways which arouse in

them pleasurable affects; conversely, when

persons are dejected, ailing, distressed, or angry,


others are also likely to suffer negative
consequences. The clearest demonstrations of

how vicarious responsiveness is established are

furnished by laboratory studies with infrahuman

subjects in which the requisite social and temporal


contingencies are instituted.

Church (1959) subjected groups of rats either


to paired aversive consequences or unpaired

consequences, or assigned them to a control

condition in which no aversive stimuli were

presented. In the paired-consequences condition

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 555
animals were administered brief shocks after

another rat had been shocked for 30 seconds, with

the aversive stimulation to both animals


terminating simultaneously. Animals in the

unpaired-consequences condition received the

same number of brief shocks, but these were not

temporally associated with painful stimulation to

another rat. Following the emotional conditioning


phase of the experiment vicarious emotional
arousal was measured in response to the pain

reactions of another rat that was continuously

shocked in an adjacent cage. Animals that had

previously experienced paired consequences were


markedly affected by the pain responses of

another rat; the control group showed little


empathetic responsiveness; and animals whose

past distressing experiences were unassociated

with the pain responses of another member of


their species showed an effect intermediate

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 556
between the two groups.

Conditioning in humans is frequently mediated


through self-generated symbolic stimulation,

which also plays an influential role in vicarious

responding (Bandura & Rosenthal, 1966; Stotland,


Shaver, & Crawford, 1966). In personality theory

vicarious emotional arousal is typically discussed

under the concept of empathy. Within the


personality framework it is generally assumed
that an observer becomes empathetically aroused

as a result of intuiting the experiences and

affective states of another person. The research

reported by Stotland indicates, however, that a


somewhat different process may be involved.
Observers reacted more emotionally to the sight of

a person undergoing painful stimulation when

they were previously asked to imagine how they

themselves would feel if they were being hurt than

when they were told to imagine how the other

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 557
person felt during the treatment. These findings

suggest that modeled affective cues produce

vicarious arousal largely through an intervening


self-stimulation process involving imaginal

representation of aversive or pleasurable

consequences occurring to oneself in similar

situations.

Of the various interpersonal determinants of


empathetic responsiveness the perceived
similarity between model and observer has

received greatest attention. It has been generally

found that perceived similarity enhances vicarious


arousal (Stotland, 1969), but why this should be so
has not been adequately established. A likely

explanation could be put in terms of outcome


similarities. One would expect people who possess

similar interests and characteristics to share many

experiences and outcomes in common. It is much

easier for a person to imagine that the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 558
consequences to individuals similar to himself

would apply to him than to imagine the same thing

about the experiences of people with whom he has


little in common. Thus, for example, a person who

often travels the airways is apt to be more

empathetically aroused upon hearing of fatalities

resulting from a commercial airplane accident

than someone who never flies. This explanation


assumes that vicarious responsiveness is based
upon active self-arousal rather than automatic

identification through similarity. Indeed, if people

who possess similar characteristics rarely

experienced concordant outcomes, they would


most likely exhibit weak empathy. The relative

influence of personal similarity and outcome


similarity on vicarious arousal could be best

evaluated by an experiment in which similar

people experience opposite consequences prior to


the empathy test, whereas dissimilar people

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 559
encounter identical outcomes. It would be

predicted from social-learning theory that

discrepant outcomes would override the effects of


personal similarity. The strongest empathetic

responsiveness would, of course, be expected to


occur under conditions of high observer- model
similarity and analogous consequences.

VICARIOUS CLASSICAL CONDITIONING

In the preceding section we reviewed some of

the conditions under which emotional responses

of a model, as conveyed through auditory, facial,

and postural manifestations, acquire the capacity


to arouse emotional responses in observers. In the

case of vicarious classical conditioning, the

observers’ vicariously elicited emotions become

conditioned, through contiguous association, to

formerly neutral stimuli. One of the earliest


laboratory investigations of this process was

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 560
reported by Kriazhev (1934), who conditioned
one animal in each of seven pairs of dogs to stimuli

presented in conjunction with food or electric


shock, while the other member of the pair merely

witnessed the procedure. The observing dogs

rapidly developed anticipatory salivary responses

to the signal for food, and conditioned agitation


and respiratory changes to the signal for shock.

However, this brief report does not contain

sufficient information on the details of the

experimental procedure to determine whether the

observers’ reactions to the conditioned stimulus


were tested in the absence of the models.

Laboratory investigations of vicarious classical


conditioning in humans (Barnett & Benedetti,

1960; Berger, 1962) typically involve the


conditioning of autonomic responses to neutral

environmental stimuli through observational


experiences. In Berger’s (1962) studies, for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 561
example, one group of observers was informed

that the performing model would receive a shock

whenever a light dimmed, the dimming of the light


being in each trial preceded by a buzzer. A second

group of observers was instructed that the

performer would make a voluntary arm

movement whenever the light dimmed but that he

was receiving no aversive stimulation. In two


other conditions the model was supposedly
shocked but refrained from making arm

movements, or the model was neither shocked nor

withdrew his arm. The measure of vicarious

conditioning was the frequency of observers’


galvanic skin responses to the buzzer, which

served as the conditioned stimulus. Observers


who were informed that the model was receiving

aversive stimulation and who witnessed the model

simulate pain responses by jerking his arm


displayed a greater degree of vicarious

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 562
conditioning than observers in the other three

groups. In a further extension of socially mediated

conditioning, Craig & Weinstein (1965) found that


observation of a performer experiencing repeated

failure produces vicarious emotional arousal that


becomes conditioned to previously neutral
environmental cues.

Although the phenomenon of vicarious


conditioning has been clearly demonstrated,
people differ widely in the rate with which they

develop conditioned emotional responses

observationally and in the stability of the acquired

responses. Since this process requires the


observer to experience painful consequences
vicariously, thereby producing affective arousal,

variables that influence an observer’s general level

of emotionality are likely to enhance or retard

vicarious learning. There is some evidence

(Bandura & Rosenthal, 1966) that emotional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 563
arousal is, indeed, a significant determinant of

vicarious conditioning, but the latter variables are

not related in a simple linear fashion. In this


experiment groups of adults observed another

person undergoing aversive conditioning

experiences in which a buzzer sounded at periodic

intervals and shortly thereafter the model feigned

pain, supposedly in response to having received


painful electric shocks. Prior to the vicarious
conditioning phase of the study, the groups of

observers were subjected to differential degrees of

emotional arousal manipulated both

psychologically and physiologically through the


administration of varying doses of epinephrine, a

sympathetic stimulant. The frequency with which


observers manifested conditioned galvanic skin

responses to the buzzer alone was found to be a

positive function of the degree of psychological


stress (Figure 3-9). However, a monotonic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 564
Figure 3-9. Mean percentage of GSRs exhibited by subjects
during the acquisition phase, in which the tone and
model’s pain cues occurred in close temporal
association, and during tests in which the formerly
neutral tone was presented alone to assess its
conditioned aversive properties. The five treatment
conditions represent increasing degrees of affective
arousal. Bandura & Rosenthal, 1966.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 565
decreasing function is obtained when, in addition

to situational stress, subjects experience

increasing physiologically induced arousal. If it


can be assumed that the five treatment conditions

represent incremental levels of emotional arousal


on a single dimension, then the combined results
suggest an inverted-U relationship between

magnitude of arousal and vicarious conditioning.

While the above study establishes a


relationship between arousal level and vicarious

conditioning, the manner in which high arousal

produces disruptive effects remains to be

demonstrated. Subjects’ reports suggested that


disruptive effects may, in part, be mediated by
self-generated competing responses designed to

reduce the aversiveness of the vicarious

instigation situation. In some cases, this took the

form of an intensive focus on irrelevant external

stimuli, to the exclusion of the disturbing pain

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 566
cues: “When I noticed how painful the shock was

to him I concentrated my vision on a spot which

did not allow me to focus directly on either his


face or hands.” Most observers attempted to

decrease the aversive stimulation arising from the

model’s pain reaction by conjuring up competing

cognitive activities: “I tried to be cool. I thought

about Latin verbs and about Latin composition.” A


few subjects, however, marshaled considerably
more potent contravening cognitive responses: “I

finally just tried to think about the girl I slept with

last night. It kept my mind off those damn shocks.”

To the extent that an observer who is faced with


distressing events succeeds either in attenuating

unpleasant arousal by producing competing


thoughts or in diverting his attention from

disturbing stimuli, associated stimulus events are

likely to become endowed with relatively weak


aversive properties. In the above experiment

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 567
deliberate use of avoidant and stimulus

neutralization stratagems was reported most

frequently by persons in the highest arousal


conditions.

The research discussed thus far has been


entirely concerned with vicarious conditioning

based on autonomic indices. Conditioned

emotionality is also often measured in terms of


behavioral suppression. If unpleasant experiences
are repeatedly paired with a neutral stimulus, it

acquires the power to evoke emotional reactions

that tend to inhibit instrumental behavior in its

presence. Crooks (1967) has shown that strong


behavioral suppression can be established solely
on the basis of observational experiences. After

being tested for the extent to which they handled

play objects, monkeys participated in a vicarious

fear conditioning experiment in which they

observed distress vocalizations sounded (through

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 568
a tape recorder) whenever a model monkey

touched a particular object. Later the observers

also received a control conditioning procedure


wherein they witnessed the model’s contacts with

a different object paired with the distress

vocalizations played backwards, thus obliterating

the distressing value of the sounds. In a

subsequent test the observing animals played


freely with the control items, but actively avoided
objects that accompanied supposedly painful

experiences for another animal.

Although emotional behavior is probably often


developed in everyday situations through
vicarious means, there are few occasions when

aversive forms of classical conditioning might be


intentionally employed for therapeutic purposes.

There are clinical reports (Miller, Dvorak, &

Turner, 1960), however, in which aversive

counterconditioning has been applied in a group

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 569
setting for creating aversion to alcohol in chronic

alcoholics. Aversion reactions are rapidly

established under such conditions, and most of the


clients display strong vicarious conditioning

effects. Positive vicarious conditioning, on the

other hand, has rarely been employed

systematically to develop empathy, pleasurable

reactions, and favorable social attitudes.

Vicarious Extinction

Emotional response patterns can be


extinguished as well as acquired on a vicarious

basis. Vicarious extinction of fears and behavioral


inhibitions is achieved by having persons observe
models performing fear-provoking behavior

without experiencing adverse consequences. How

avoidance responses can be extinguished without

having been elicited can be best explained in terms

of a dual-process theory of avoidance behavior. As

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 570
noted in the previous discussion of causal

processes, conditioned aversive stimuli evoke

emotional arousal that exerts some degree of


control over instrumental responding. It would

follow from this theory that if the arousal capacity

of a threatening stimulus is extinguished, then

both the motivation and one set of controlling

stimuli for avoidance behavior are removed. Black


(1958) has shown that neutralization of an
aversive stimulus through classical extinction

procedures alone markedly facilitates subsequent

elimination of avoidance behavior.

Some early suggestive evidence for the


occurrence of vicarious extinction is provided by

Masserman (1943) and Jones (1924) in


exploratory studies of the relative therapeutic

efficacy of modeling procedures. Masserman

produced strong feeding inhibitions in cats by

pairing food approach responses to a conditioned

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 571
stimulus with aversive stimulation. In the

remedial phase of the experiment, the inhibited

animals observed a cagemate, who had never been


negatively conditioned, exhibit prompt approach

and feeding responses. The observers initially

cowered at the presentation of the conditioned

stimulus, but with continued exposure to their

fearless companion, they advanced, at first


hesitantly and then more boldly, to the goal box
and consumed the food. Some of the animals,

however, showed little reduction in avoidance

behavior despite prolonged hunger and repeated

modeling trials. Moreover, avoidance responses


reappeared in a few of the animals after the

fearless cat was removed, indicating that in the


latter cases the modeling stimuli served merely as

temporary external inhibitors of avoidance

responses. Jones (1924) similarly obtained


variable results in extinguishing children’s phobic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 572
responses by having them observe their peers

behave in a nonanxious manner in the presence of

the avoided objects.

Since nonoccurrence of anticipated aversive

consequences is a requisite condition for fear


extinction, the modeling displays most likely to

have strong effects on fearful observers are ones

in which performances that they regard as


hazardous are repeatedly shown to be safe under
a variety of threatening circumstances. However, if

people are to be influenced by modeled behavior

and its accompanying consequences, then the

necessary observing responses must be elicited


and maintained. Presentation of modeled
approach responses toward the most threatening

situation at the outset, as in the studies cited

above, is likely to generate high levels of fear

arousal in observers. To the extent that such

conditions activate avoidance responses (such as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 573
withdrawing or looking away) designed to reduce

vicariously instigated distress, they will impede

vicarious extinction. Therefore, the efficacy of


vicarious extinction procedures may partly

depend on the manner in which modeled

performances are presented.

Avoidance responses can be consistently

extinguished with minimal distress if persons are


exposed to a graduated sequence of aversive
stimuli that progressively approximates the most

feared event. In the application of this stimulus

generalization principle to vicarious extinction,


persons initially observe a model responding in a
positive manner to situations that have low

arousal value. After emotional responses to


attenuated threats have been extinguished,

progressively more aversive modeling cues, which

are weakened by generalization of anxiety

extinction from preceding displays, are gradually

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 574
introduced and neutralized. Stimulus graduation is

not a necessary condition for vicarious extinction,

but it permits greater control over the change


process and it entails less anxiety elicitation than

approaches involving repeated exposure to

modeled events having high threat value.

In addition to stimulus exposure variables,

qualitative aspects of the modeled behavior are


likely to influence vicarious extinction outcomes.
The studies of vicarious emotional arousal

reviewed earlier demonstrate that negative

affective impressions by others can serve as


powerful cues for arousing fear and avoidance in
observers. One would therefore expect modeled

approach responses accompanied by positive


affective expressions to produce greater extinction

effects than those accompanied by anxiety. For

example, parental modeling efforts intended to

overcome children’s fears are frequently nullified

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 575
because the parents themselves suffer

apprehensions and force themselves into tense

contact with feared objects.

As part of a program of research designed to

elucidate the phenomenon of vicarious extinction,

several efficacious modeling procedures have

been developed for modifying anxiety disorders.

The first study in the series (Bandura, Grusec, &


Menlove, 1967b) involved a stringent test of the
degree to which strong avoidance behavior of long

standing can be extinguished vicariously. It also

explored the possibility that induction of positive


affective responses in observers during exposure
to potentially threatening modeling cues may

expedite the vicarious extinction process.

Young children, who exhibited fear of dogs as

revealed by parental ratings and an actual test of

dog avoidance behavior, were assigned to one of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 576
four treatment conditions. One group participated
in eight brief sessions during which they observed

a fearless peer model exhibit progressively more


fear-provoking interactions with a dog. For these

children, the modeled approach behavior was

presented within a highly positive party context

designed to counteract anxiety reactions. The fear-


arousing properties of the modeled performances

were gradually increased from session to session

by varying simultaneously the physical restraints

on the dog, the directness and intimacy of the

modeled approach responses, and the duration of


interaction between the model and his canine

companion. A second group of children observed


the same graduated modeled performances, but in

a neutral context. In the two treatment conditions


described the stimulus complex contained both

modeling cues and repeated observation of the


feared animal. Therefore, in order to measure the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 577
effects of exposure to the threatening object itself,

a third group of children observed the dog in the

positive context but with the model absent. A


fourth group participated in the positive activities

but was never exposed to either the dog or the


modeled displays.

Following completion of the treatment series,

children were readministered the avoidance test


consisting of the graded sequence of dog
interaction tasks. They were asked, for example, to

approach and to pet the dog, to release her from a

playpen, to remove her leash, to feed her dog

biscuits, and to spend a fixed period of time alone


in the room with the animal. The final and most
difficult set of tasks required the children to climb

into the playpen with the dog and, after having

locked the gate, to pet her and to remain alone

with the animal under the confining, fear-arousing

conditions.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 578
Evidence that deviant behavior can be

modified by a particular method is of limited

therapeutic significance unless it can be


demonstrated that established response patterns

generalize to stimuli beyond those encountered in


treatment, and that induced changes endure after
the therapeutic conditions have been

discontinued. Therefore, the children were

readministered tests for avoidance behavior


toward different dogs after completion of the

treatment program, and again a month later.

The modeling procedure produced highly

stable and generalized vicarious extinction of


avoidance responses (Figure 3-10). The two
groups of children who had observed the peer

model interact fearlessly with the dog displayed

significantly greater approach behavior toward

both the experimental and an unfamiliar animal

than did children in both the dog exposure and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 579
Figure 3-10. Mean dog-approach scores achieved by children
in each of the treatment conditions on the three
different periods of assessment. Bandura, Grusec, &
Menlove, 1967.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 580
control conditions, who did not differ from each

other. The positive context, however, did not

contribute much to the favorable outcomes


obtained. Further evidence for the effectiveness of

this method is that 67 percent of the children

receiving the modeling treatment were able to

remain alone with the dog in the playpen. In

contrast, this ultimate test was met by relatively


few children in the two control conditions.

One would expect, from knowledge of

generalization processes, that vicarious extinction

effects would be partly determined by the variety


of stimulus elements that are neutralized.
Exposure to diverse models who display fearless

behavior toward variant forms of the feared object


without adverse consequences should produce

thorough extinction of fear arousal, and

consequently extensive reduction in avoidance

behavior. On the other hand, observers whose

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 581
emotional responsiveness to a restricted set of

modeled aversive elements is extinguished are apt

to display weaker extinction effects. Moreover,


under conditions where a series of aversive

stimuli is presented only once, certain observer

characteristics might also influence the extent to

which emotional responses are extinguished.

Observers who are highly susceptible to emotional


arousal would be inclined to respond to
threatening modeling displays with pronounced

fear and might, therefore, show relatively strong

resistance to vicarious extinction. Thus,

emotionality might serve as an additional


determinant of the rate at which avoidance

behavior is reduced through modeling procedures.

The above propositions were tested in a

second experiment (Bandura & Menlove, 1968)

employing the same assessment methodology

with children who displayed severe dog-avoidance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 582
behavior. In this project, however, the

performances of models were presented in a

series of brief movies in order to test the efficacy


of symbolic modeling techniques that might lend

themselves conveniently to psychotherapeutic

applications. One group of children, who

participated in a single-model treatment, observed

a fearless male model display the same


progressively fear-provoking interactions with a
dog as in the preceding experiment. The second

group of children, receiving a multiple-model

treatment, observed several different girls and

boys of varying ages interacting positively with


many dogs. The size and fearsomeness of the dogs

increased progressively from that of small,


nonthreatening dogs to more massive varieties.

Children assigned to a control group were shown

movies that had no canine characters.

The dog-approach scores obtained by children

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 583
in each of the three conditions in the pre-test,

post-test, and follow-up phases of the experiment

are shown graphically in Figure 3-11. Children


who observed approach behavior modeled

without adverse consequences to the performer

displayed enduring and generalized reductions in

avoidance behavior, whereas the controls showed

no changes in this regard. Comparison of the


incidence of terminal performances (remaining
alone with the dog in the playpen) by children

presented with the single-modeling display and

those who witnessed the multiple modeling

showed the latter form of treatment to be superior


for completely eliminating dog-avoidance

behavior. Although modeling was equally effective


regardless of the severity of children’s phobic

behavior, those who manifested a wide variety of

fears benefited somewhat less from the multiple-


modeling treatment than children who had fewer

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 584
Figure 3-11. Median dog-approach scores achieved by
children who received either single-model or multiple-
model treatments, or who participated in a control
condition. Bandura & Menlove, 1968.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 585
fears.

As a further test of the therapeutic value of


symbolic modeling, control children were

administered the multiple-model treatment after

the main experiment was completed. The control


children, whose avoidance behavior remained

unchanged in several tests conducted during the

control period, displayed a sharp increase in dog-


approach behavior following treatment. The
increased boldness of one of the control children

who had been subsequently treated is portrayed

in Figure 3-12. The top frames show the model’s

dauntless behavior; the lower frames depict the


child’s fearless interaction with the animals, both
of which she boldly corralled in the playpen, after

the formal test.

Comparison of results of the two experiments

suggests that symbolic modeling is less powerful

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 586
Figure 3-12. A girl who was apprehensive about dogs
engaging in fearless interactions with dogs after
exposure to the series of films in which a peer model
displays progressively threatening interactions with
dogs. Bandura & Menlove 1968.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 587
than live demonstrations of essentially the same
behavior. Although the single-model treatment

effected significant reductions in children’s


avoidance responses, it did not sufficiently weaken

their fears to enable them to carry out the

threatening terminal approach behavior. However,

the diminished efficacy of symbolic modeling can


be offset by a broader sampling of models and

aversive stimulus objects. Children who received

the diverse modeling treatment not only showed

continuous improvement in approach behavior

between the post-test and follow-up periods, but


also achieved terminal performances at rates

comparable to equally avoidant children who, in


the previous experiment observed fearless

behavior performed by a single real-life model.


Hill, Liebert, & Mott (1968) and Spiegler, Liebert,

McMains, & Fernandez (1968) have also


successfully eliminated persistent avoidance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 588
behavior in children and adults through brief

symbolic modeling, but in the latter studies the

modeled performances are accompanied by a


persuasive narrative and other fear-mitigating

variables.

The potency of modeling influences in the

transmission of anxiety is widely acknowledged,

but their therapeutic value has sometimes been


questioned (Jersild & Holmes, 1935) on the
grounds that fears persist even though modeling

frequently occurs under ordinary conditions of

life. The effectiveness of any principle of learning

depends not only on its validity but also on the


manner in which it is implemented. Inconsistent,
haphazard, and inadequately sequenced learning

experiences will produce disappointing outcomes

regardless of the cogency of the principle

supposedly guiding the treatment.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 589
In many instances weak fears are undoubtedly

extinguished, or substantially reduced, through

fortuitous naturalistic modeling. However,


carefully planned modeling experiences are

essential for the modification of more tenacious


avoidance tendencies. There is some evidence
(Bandura & Menlove, 1968) that parents of

children who exhibit severe fearfulness make no

attempts to overcome their children’s fears


because they suffer from similar apprehension.

Consequently, they seldom model fearlessness

and, on the infrequent occasions when they do, the


modeling endeavors do not involve carefully

graded presentation of threatening stimuli,


without which this method is not only likely to be

ineffective but may actually exacerbate anxiety

reactions. A not uncommon domestic modeling


scene, for example, is one in which a parent is

busily petting a dog that is jumping about and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 590
simultaneously bidding the child, who is clinging

fearfully, to touch the bounding animal. By


contrast, the modeling treatments, in addition to

utilizing the principle of graduation to reduce fear

arousal, involved concentrated exposures to


modeling displays under protected observation

conditions, and extensive variation of model


characteristics, intimacy of approach behavior,
and aversive properties of the feared object. Had

the modeling sequences been presented in a

widely dispersed and haphazard fashion and

restricted to the more reserved petting responses


by adults (whom children are likely to

discriminate as better able to protect themselves),


the vicarious extinction outcomes might have been

relatively weak and unpredictable.

The third project (Bandura, Blanchard, &

Bitter, 1969) employed an elaborate experimental

design to assess the comparative efficacy of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 591
modeling and desensitization treatment

approaches for producing behavioral, affective,

and attitudinal changes. The participants were


adolescents and adults who suffered from snake

phobias that, in most cases, unnecessarily

restricted their activities and adversely affected

their psychological functioning in various ways.

Some of the people were unable to perform their


jobs in situations in which there was any remote
possibility that they might come into contact with

snakes; others could not take part in recreational

activities such as hunting, gardening, camping, or

hiking, because of their dread of snakes; and still


others avoided purchasing homes in rural areas,

or experienced marked distress whenever they


would be unexpectedly confronted with pet

snakes in the course of their social or occupational

activities.

In the initial phase of the experiment the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 592
participants were administered a behavioral test

that measured the strength of their avoidance of

snakes. In addition, they completed a


comprehensive fear inventory to determine

whether elimination of fear of snakes is associated

with concomitant changes in other areas of

anxiety. Attitudinal ratings on several scales

describing various encounters with snakes and on


the evaluative dimensions of the semantic
differential technique were also obtained. The

latter measures were included to furnish data

regarding the interesting but inadequately

explored attitudinal effects of behavioral changes


induced through social-learning methods.

The cases were individually matched on the


basis of their avoidance behavior and assigned to

one of four conditions. One group participated in a

self-administered symbolic modeling treatment in

which they observed a film depicting young

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 593
children, adolescents, and adults engaging in

progressively threatening interactions with a large

king snake (Figure 3-13). To increase even further


the power of this method two other features were

added: subjects were taught to induce and to

maintain anxiety- inhibiting relaxation throughout

the period of exposure, and they were permitted

to regulate the rate of presentation of stimuli by


means of remote control starting and reversing
devices. The rationale for the second feature was

that a self-regulated modeling treatment should

permit greater control over extinction than one in

which persons are exposed to a sequence of


aversive cues without regard to their anxiety

reactions. Subjects were instructed to stop the film


whenever a particular modeled performance

provoked anxiety, to reverse the film to the

beginning of the aversive sequence, and to


reinduce deep relaxation. They then reviewed the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 594
---
,....-!, -i M ' " " ; . A
• . ;._ 'J ' ' • '

Figure 3.13 Children and adults modeling


progressively fear-arousing
interaction with a king snake.
Bandura, Blanchard, & Rtiter, 1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 595
threatening scene repeatedly in this manner until

it was completely neutralized before proceeding to

the next item in the graduated sequence. After


subjects became skillful in handling the projector

controls and the self-induction of relaxation, the


experimenter absented himself from the situation,
and the subjects conducted their own treatment

until their anxieties to the depicted scenes were

thoroughly extinguished.

The second group of subjects received a form

of treatment combining graduated modeling with

guided participation. The principal elements of

this method were developed by Ritter (1968,


1969a) as contact desensitization. In the
procedure employed in the present study, the

model initially demonstrates the desired behavior

under secure observational conditions, after which

subjects are aided through further demonstration

and joint performance to execute progressively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 596
more difficult responses. Whenever subjects are

unable to perform a given behavior after

demonstration alone, they enact the feared


activities concurrently with the model. The

physical guidance is then gradually reduced until

they are able to perform the behavior alone.

In the application of this method to the

elimination of snake phobia, at each step the


experimenter himself performed fearless behavior
and gradually led subjects into touching, stroking,

and then holding the snake’s body with first

gloved and then bare hands while he held the


snake securely by the head and tail. If a subject
was unable to touch the snake after ample

demonstration, she was asked to place her hand


on the experimenter’s and to move her hand down

gradually until it touched the snake’s body. After

subjects no longer felt any apprehension about

touching the snake under these secure conditions,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 597
anxieties about contact with the snake’s head area

and entwining tail were extinguished. The

experimenter again performed the tasks


fearlessly, and then he and the subject performed

the responses jointly; as subjects became less

fearful the experimenter gradually reduced his

participation and control over the snake until

subjects were able to hold the snake in their laps


without assistance, to let the snake loose in the
room and retrieve it, and to let it crawl freely over

their bodies. Progress through the graded

approach tasks was paced according to the

subjects’ apprehensiveness. When they reported


being able to perform one activity with little or no

fear, they were eased into a more difficult


interaction.

Subjects assigned to the third group received

the standard form of desensitization treatment

devised by Wolpe (1958). In this procedure deep

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 598
relaxation was successively paired with imaginal

representations of snakes arranged in order of

increasing aversiveness. As in the other


conditions, the treatment was continued until

subjects’ anxiety reactions were totally

extinguished or the maximum time allotment was

completed.

Subjects assigned to the control conditions


participated in the behavioral and attitudinal
assessments without intervening treatment. This

group primarily furnished a control for changes

resulting from repeated measurements. A


relationship pseudotherapy was not employed
because several previous investigations have

shown that snake-avoidance behavior is


unaffected by such experiences. In addition, the

controls were later administered the symbolic

modeling treatment without relaxation to evaluate

its contribution to the changes produced by this

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 599
method.

Following completion of the treatment series

the assessment procedures were readministered


to all subjects. In order to determine the generality

of extinction effects, half the subjects in each of the

conditions were tested initially with the familiar

brown-striped king snake and then with an

unfamiliar crimson-splotched corn snake that was


strikingly different in coloration; the remaining
subjects were tested with the two snakes in the

reverse order. The behavioral test consisted of a

series of tasks requiring the subjects to approach,


look at, touch, and hold a snake with bare and
gloved hands; to remove the snake from its cage,

let it loose in the room, and then replace it in the


cage; to hold it within five inches of their faces;

and finally to tolerate the snake in their laps while

they held their hands passively at their sides.

Immediately before and during the performance of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 600
each task subjects rated the intensity of their fear

arousal on a 10-interval scale to measure

extinction of affective arousal accompanying


specific approach responses.

As shown in Figure 3-14, control subjects

remained unchanged in avoidance behavior,

symbolic modeling and desensitization produced

substantial reductions, and live modeling


combined with guided participation eliminated
snake phobias in virtually all subjects (92

percent). The modeling procedures not only

extinguished avoidance responses of long


standing, but they also neutralized the anxiety-
arousing properties of the phobic stimuli. Both of

the modeling treatments achieved marked


decrements in anticipatory and performance

anxiety. Although subjects who had received

desensitization treatment also experienced less

emotional arousal while performing snake-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 601
Figure 3-14. Mean number of snake-approach responses
performed by subjects before and after receiving
different treatments. Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter,
1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 602
approach responses, their magnitude of fear

reduction was less than that of their counterparts

in tire modeling conditions.

Findings of this experiment also reveal that

applications of social-learning procedures have

important attitudinal consequences. Both

symbolic modeling and desensitization, which

primarily involve extinction of negative affect


aroused by aversive stimuli, produced favorable
changes in attitudes toward snakes. Consistent

with theoretical expectation, the treatment

condition that reduced the anxiety-arousing


properties of snakes and enabled subjects to
engage in intimate interactions with snakes

effected the greatest attitudinal changes. These


findings will be given more detailed consideration

in a later chapter specifically concerned with

processes governing the modification of attitudes.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 603
Analysis of the fear inventory scores disclosed

some degree of fear reductions beyond the

specifically treated phobia, the decrements being


roughly proportional to the potency of the

treatments employed. Nontreated controls


showed no changes in either number or intensity
of fears. Desensitization produced a decrease only

in severity of fears toward other animals, whereas

symbolic modeling was accompanied by a


reduction in the number of animal fears and a

general diminution in the intensity of anxiety in

several other areas of functioning. Participant


modeling, on the other hand, effected widespread

fear reductions in relation to a variety of threats


involving both interpersonal and nonsocial events.

The transfer obtained reflects the operation of at

least two somewhat different processes. The first


involves generalization of extinction effects from

treated stimuli to related anxiety sources. The

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 604
second entails positive reinforcement of a sense of

capability through success, which mitigates


emotional responses to potentially threatening

situations. Having successfully overcome a phobia

that had plagued them for most of their lives,


subjects reported increased confidence that they

could cope effectively with other fear-provoking

events

After the posttreatment assessment, subjects


in the control group received the symbolic

modeling treatment without the relaxation

component. Symbolic modeling alone achieved

substantial decrements in fear arousal and


avoidance behavior: 45 percent of the subjects
exhibited terminal performances toward both

snakes. No significant differences were found in

approach behavior between subjects who were

administered symbolic modeling alone and those

who received symbolic modeling with relaxation.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 605
However, subjects who paired modeling with

relaxation required fewer reexposures to

neutralize the aversive scenes, subsequently


experienced less fear arousal while performing

snake- approach responses, and showed greater

positive changes in their attitudes toward snakes.

In order to determine, in cases involving only

partial improvement, whether the deficiencies


reside in the treatment method or in the subject,
all persons who failed to achieve terminal

performances were subsequently administered

the participant modeling treatment. Snake-phobic


behavior was thoroughly extinguished in all these
subjects within a few brief sessions regardless of

their age, sex, proneness to anxiety, or severity of


avoidance behavior (Figure 3-15). Moreover, this

supplementary treatment produced further

reductions in fearfulness toward other types of

threats, and also additional attitudinal changes.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 606
Figure 3-15. Mean number of snake-approach responses
performed by subjects before and after (post-test)
receiving different treatments. Control subjects were
subsequently administered symbolic modeling
treatment without relaxation. All subjects in the
desensitization, symbolic modeling, and treated control
conditions who failed to perform the terminal
approach behavior were then given the live modeling
and guided participation treatment (post- live). The
snake-approach behavior of subjects in all four groups
was measured again in a follow-up study conducted
one month later. Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter, 1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 607
A one-month follow-up assessment revealed

that the beneficial changes produced in behavior,

attitudes, and emotional responsiveness were


effectively maintained. The clients also displayed

evidence that the behavioral improvements had


generalized from therapeutic to real-life
situations. They were able to participate in

recreational activities, such as hunting, camping,

hiking, and gardening, that they formerly avoided


because of their dread of snakes; they no longer

experienced marked distress when unexpectedly

confronted with snakes in the course of their


social or occupational activities; they were able to

handle harmless snakes; and a few even served as


model therapists for their own children and

fainthearted friends.

Ritter (1968) has obtained similarly uniform

success with group modeling procedures

administered to children who displayed fear of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 608
snakes. Groups of children participated in two 35-

minute sessions in which they either merely

observed several fearless children exhibit intimate


interactions with a snake, or they received the

participant modeling form of treatment during

which the therapist displayed positive responses

toward the snake and then gradually eased the

children into performing the feared behavior.


Snake phobias were completely extinguished in 53
percent of the children by modeling alone and in

80 percent of the children who received modeling

combined with guided participation. The potency

of this approach receives further confirmation by


Rimm & Mahoney (1969), who successfully

extinguished snake-avoidance behavior with


participant modeling in adults who were unable to

achieve behavioral improvement when offered

increasing monetary rewards for performing a


graduated series of approach responses.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 609
Within the participant modeling treatment

three processes are operative that might

contribute in varying degrees to such striking


psychological changes. These include observation

of fearless behavior being repeatedly modeled


without any unfavorable consequences, incidental
information received about the feared objects, and

direct personal contact with threatening objects

that engender no adverse effects. In an experiment


aimed at isolating the relative influence of these

component variables, Blanchard (1969) matched

subjects in terms of their snake-avoidance


behavior and assigned them to one of four

conditions. One subject in each quartet received


the standard procedure, which includes the

benefits of modeling, information, and guided

interaction with a snake. A second subject


simultaneously observed the modeling sessions

and listened to the verbal interchanges, thus being

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 610
exposed to both modeling and informational

influences. The third subject received only the

modeling component, while the fourth, who

merely participated in the testing procedures,


experienced none of the constituent influences.

Figure 3-16 summarizes the behavioral, affective,

and attitudinal changes associated with these


various treatment conditions. Modeling accounted
for approximately 60 percent of the behavior

change and 80 percent of the changes in attitudes

and fear arousal; guided participation contributed


the remaining increment. Informational

influences, on the other hand, had no effect on any


of the three response classes.

The guided participation component of the

modeling approach under discussion contains two

important aspects. Participant observers enact

progressively more difficult responses and, if

necessary, the model physically assists them in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 611
Figure 3-16. Percentage of change in approach behavior,
fearfulness, and attitudes displayed by subjects who
received different components of the modeling-guided
participation treatment. Blanchard, 1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 612
performing the behavior required at each step in

the graded series of tasks. In order to evaluate the

influence of these elements, Ritter (1969b)


administered one of three treatments to

acrophobic subjects during a single 35-minute

session. For one group of subjects, the

experimenter exhibited increasingly threatening

climbing responses and physically assisted


subjects in performing the same activities; in the
second condition the experimenter demonstrated

the behavior but only verbally guided subjects in

enacting matching performances; a third group

simply observed the demonstrated activities. At


the completion of the session all subjects were

readministered a behavioral test requiring them to


climb to a series of heights atop a seven-story

building. Modeling accompanied by physically

guided performance produced greater changes


than modeling with verbally guided enactment,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 613
which in turn was superior to brief demonstration

alone.

Further research is needed to clarify the

mechanisms through which modeling procedures

achieve extinction effects. Results of the


experiment by Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter

(1969) provide tentative support for the

proposition that avoidance behavior is reduced


through vicarious extinction of fear arousal.
During the symbolic modeling treatment subjects

rated the intensity of their fear arousal to each

modeled scene and to subsequent reexposures to

the same stimuli. As shown in Figure 3-17,


subjects showed a progressive decline in fear
arousal with each successive exposure to modeled

approach behavior. Subjects who combined

symbolic modeling with relaxation experienced a

greater reduction in fear on the second exposure

to the aversive scenes than their counterparts who

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 614
Figure 3-17. Mean level of fear arousal evoked by the
modeling stimuli initially and by each subsequent
exposure to the same filmed scenes in subjects
receiving symbolic modeling with relaxation and
symbolic modeling alone. The data are averaged across
scenes at each exposure and plotted for the first six
exposures only since subjects rarely required more
than six presentations to neutralize any given scene.
Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter, 1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 615
received symbolic modeling alone, but on

subsequent reexposures the rate of fear extinction

was essentially the same. The major theoretical


assumption receives additional support from the

study by Blanchard (1969), who also recorded

decrements in fear arousal with successive

reexposures to modeling stimuli. He found that the

more thoroughly fear arousal was vicariously


extinguished the greater was the reduction in
avoidance behavior and the more generalized

were the behavioral changes.

The process of change associated with the


powerful procedure involving modeling combined
with guided participation may be conceptualized

as follows: Repeated modeling of approach


responses decreases the arousal potential of

aversive stimuli below the threshold for activating

avoidance responses, thus enabling persons to

engage, albeit somewhat anxiously, in approach

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 616
behavior. Direct contact with threats that are no

longer objectively justified provides a variety of

new experiences that, if favorable, further


extinguish residual anxiety and avoidance

tendencies. Without the benefit of prior vicarious

extinction, the reinstatement of severely inhibited

behavior generally requires a tedious and

protracted program. After approach behavior


toward formerly avoided objects has been fully
restored the resultant new experiences give rise to

substantial reorganization of attitudes.

The findings of studies reviewed above


indicate that a powerful form of treatment is one
in which therapeutic agents themselves model the

desired behavior and arrange optimal conditions


for clients to engage in similar activities until they

can perform the behavior skillfully and fearlessly.

The therapeutic outcomes associated with this

approach are sufficiently promising to warrant its

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 617
further extension to other types of anxiety

conditions. It is undoubtedly best suited for

behavioral dysfunctions in which the feared


consequences are inspectional.

Inhibitory and Disinhibitory Effects of


Vicarious Experiences

In addition to the acquisition of instrumental


and emotional behaviors through observational

experiences, exposure to modeled events may


strengthen or weaken observers’ inhibitions of

well-learned response patterns. The occurrence of

inhibitory effects is indicated when, as a function of

observing negative response consequences to a


model, observers show either decrements in the
same class of behavior, or a general reduction of

responsiveness. It should be noted that when the


subject witnesses behavior that is subsequently

punished, the response-facilitative effects of

modeling cues are counteracted by the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 618
suppressive effects of adverse outcomes. When

these opposing influences are of comparable

strength, persons who have observed modeled


behavior punished and those who have had no

exposure to the model may display an equally low

incidence of response. Therefore, inhibitory effects

can be best evaluated either by measuring

response decrements from baseline levels or by


comparison with performances of subjects who
have observed the same modeled behavior

without any consequences. In the experiment cited

earlier (Bandura, 1965b), for example, children

who had observed a model’s aggressive behavior


result in severe punishment performed

significantly fewer matching responses than


subjects who observed the same actions result

either in reward or in no evident consequences.

Indeed, the vicarious punishment produced


virtually complete suppression of imitative

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 619
aggression in girls, whose inhibitions regarding

physical forms of aggression are initially relatively

strong. Further evidence for the suppressive


effects of vicarious punishment is furnished by

studies comparing consistent vicarious reward


with successive reward and punishment of the
model’s behavior (Rosekrans & Hartup, 1967).

Subsequent punishment tends to cancel the

behavioral enhancement effects of rewarding


consequences to the model.

The above studies demonstrate the inhibitory

influence of observed negative outcomes to a

model on the aggressive behavior of viewers.


Walters and his associates (Parke & Walters,
1967; Walters, Leat, & Mezei, 1963; Walters,

Parke, & Cane, 1965) have likewise shown that

witnessing peer models punished for engaging in

forbidden play activities increased observers’

resistance to deviation when they were similarly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 620
tempted with the prohibited objects. In a

comparative study, Benton (1967) found that

observers who witnessed others reprimanded for


handling prohibited toys later showed the same

amount of response inhibition as did the punished

performers. The possible mechanisms through

which vicarious punishment produces inhibitory

effects are discussed in some detail in the


introductory chapter of this book.

In many instances persons respond with self-

punitive and self-devaluative reactions to behavior

of their own that may be considered permissible,


or even rewardable, by others. Results of
investigations concerned with the social

transmission of self-monitoring reinforcement


systems (Bandura & Kupers, 1964; Bandura,

Grusec, & Menlove, 1967b) provide evidence that

witnessing punishments self-administered by a

model inhibits observers from performing the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 621
devalued behavior. Observation of self-

administered reinforcements by a model have

been shown by Porro (1968) to have similar


effects on transgressive behavior. For children

who viewed a filmed model exhibit self-approving

responses to her transgressions, 80 percent

subsequently handled toys they were forbidden to

touch, whereas the transgression rate was only 20


percent for children who had observed the same
model express self-critical reactions concerning

her transgressions.

Behavioral restraints, established through


previous modeling or direct aversive conditioning,
can be reduced on the basis of observational

experiences. Such disinhibitory effects are evident


when observers display increases in socially

disapproved behavior as a function of viewing

models either rewarded or experiencing no

adverse consequences for performing prohibited

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 622
responses. The reduction of inhibitions through

modeling has been demonstrated most clearly in

studies of intense physical forms of aggression


that tend to be inhibited in viewers as a result of

past social training (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963a;

Epstein, 1966; Walters & Llewellyn Thomas, 1963;

Wheeler, 1966). It has also been shown (Grosser,

Polansky, & Lippitt, 1951; Ross, 1962) that the


incidence of other types of deviation by observers
is significantly increased as a result of witnessing

models’ unpunished transgressions. On the other

hand, conforming models tend to strengthen the

observer’s self-controlling responses and thereby


to reduce conflictive behavior in tempting

situations (Ross, 1962).

Blake and his associates (Blake, 1958)

conducted investigations of some of the conditions

determining the influence of noncompliant and

conforming models on observers’ inhibitions in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 623
prohibition situations. In one study, Freed,

Chandler, Mouton, & Blake (1955) found that,

although exposure to a noncompliant model


lowered students’ resistance to deviation,

transgressions occurred most frequently if the

restriction was relatively weak and the model

violated the prohibitory signs, whereas the

combination of a strong restriction and a


conforming model produced the lowest incidence
of deviation. A second experiment (Kimbrell &

Blake, 1958) demonstrated that the efficacy of

modeling cues for modifying inhibitions varies

with the observer’s level of instigation to


transgression. Under extreme provocation,

subjects disregarded both the imposed restriction


and the conforming model. However, under

conditions where the instigation was not so strong

as to force deviation, subjects who observed a


conforming model displayed more compliant

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 624
behavior than others who witnessed a model

violate the prohibition.

In naturalistic situations observers often

actually see a model’s transgressions rewarded or

punished. At other times, however, they can only


infer probable consequences from discriminative

symbols and attributes of the model that tend to

be correlated with differential reinforcements. The


manner in which distinctive model characteristics
signifying probable outcomes may increase a

model’s effectiveness in reducing inhibitions is

illustrated in an experiment conducted by

Lefkowitz, Blake, & Mouton (1955). Traffic-signal


violations by a presumably high-status person
attired in a freshly pressed suit, shined shoes,

white shirt, and tie produced a higher pedestrian

violation rate than the same transgression

performed by the same model dressed in soiled,

patched trousers, scuffed shoes, and a blue denim

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 625
shirt. The differential reduction in restraints noted

in the latter experiment is probably attributable to

the fact that transgressions by persons who


occupy a high position in a prestige hierarchy are

likely to be punished less frequently and less

severely than those performed by low-status

transgressors. The differential leniency is apt to be

temporarily extended to the imitator as well, when


the transgressive behavior is performed at the
same time along with the deviating model.

Other discriminative properties of the model,

such as age, sex, socioeconomic status, social


power, ethnic background, and intellectual and
vocational status, which are associated with

predictable contingencies of reinforcement, may


likewise influence the extent to which prohibited

acts will be imitated. Vicarious reinforcement

effects are, of course, considerably weakened or

nullified under conditions where the model’s

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 626
transgressive behavior is so markedly

inappropriate to the sex (Dubanoski, 1967), status,

or social role occupied by the observer that any


imitative tendencies are personally suppressed.

Under most circumstances people readily

adopt modeled responses that seem appropriate

or have utilitarian value. However, in some

problem-solving and achievement-like situations


they display counter-matching tendencies for fear
that imitative behavior will be considered

cheating, copying, or subservience, and, therefore,

socially disapproved (Luchins & Luchins, 1961;


Patterson, Littman, & Brown, 1968; Schein, 1954).
The inhibiting effect of anticipated negative

sanctions for imitation can be overcome in


observers through positive reinforcement of the

model’s responses (Clark, 1965).

It is interesting to note that, when a model

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 627
displays punishable behavior, absence of
anticipated adverse consequences increases

transgressive behavior in observers to the same


degree as witnessing the model experience

rewarding outcomes (Bandura, 1965b; Walters,

Parke, & Cane, 1965). These findings suggest that

nonreaction to formerly prohibited activities may


take on, through contrast, positive significance.

Similar contrast-of-reinforcement effects have

been demonstrated in studies of direct

reinforcement (Buchwald, 1959a, 1959b) in which

nonreward following punishment had functioned


analogously to a positive incentive, whereas

nonreward following a series of rewards had


operated as a negative reinforcer. In fact, even a

weak positive incentive, when contrasted with


more rewarding prior events, may acquire

negative reinforcing value (Buchwald, 1960). The


effects of witnessed outcomes on matching

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 628
behavior may therefore be determined to a large

extent by the context in which the events occur

and the customary sanctions associated with


particular modeled response patterns.

Because previous studies have utilized deviant


modes of behavior, which may be readily

disinhibited through omission of negative

consequences, the results provide no clear


evidence for the occurrence of positive vicarious
reinforcement. However, findings of an

experiment (Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1967b)

involving modeled behavior that is positively

sanctioned reveal that social rewards dispensed to


a model augment matching responses compared
to a condition in which the exemplified actions

produce no evident consequences.

It is generally easier to disinhibit than to

inhibit response patterns through vicarious

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 629
means. The main reason for this difference is that
behavior which is customarily subject to negative

sanctions is often positively reinforcing for the


user, but it is socially suppressed for the

convenience and well-being of others. Thus, for

example, by violating prohibitions and restrictions

people can gratify their immediate needs more


directly and effectively than by following irksome

institutionalized requirements; similarly, by

adopting transgressive behavior they can gain

access to attractive resources that might

otherwise be denied them. Therefore, it does not


require much successful deviant modeling to

reduce vicariously the suppressive effects over


personally rewarding behavior. On the other hand,

inhibitory effects are far more difficult to establish


and to sustain through either direct or vicarious

punishment, when it involves relinquishing


behaviors that lead to immediate and direct

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 630
reinforcement. Considering that exposure to

deviant filmed models tends to weaken behavioral

restraints, one would expect televised portrayals


of successfully executed transgressions to have

disinhibitory effects on viewers.

Response Facilitating Effects of Modeling


Influences

The behavior of models often serves merely as

discriminative cues for observers in facilitating the

expression of previously learned responses that


ordinarily are not subject to negative sanctions.

Laboratory and field studies have shown that the


probability of occurrence of a wide variety of

neutral and socially approved behavior can be


substantially increased as a function of witnessing
the action of real-life or symbolic models. Some of

the behaviors that have been thus facilitated

include volunteering one’s services (Rosenbaum,

1956; Rosenbaum & Blake, 1955; Schachter & Hall,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 631
1952), performing altruistic acts (Blake,

Rosenbaum, & Duryea, 1955; Bryan & Test, 1967;

Harris, 1968; Rosenhan & White, 1967), pledging


oneself to a course of social action (Blake, Mouton,

& Hain, 1956; Helson, Blake, Mouton, & Olmstead,

1956), assisting persons in distress (Bryan & Test,

1967), seeking relevant information (Krumboltz &

Thoresen, 1964; Krumboltz, Varenhorst, &


Thoresen, 1967), and selecting certain types of
foods (Duncker, 1938; Barnwell, 1966), activities

(Madsen, 1968), or articles (Bandura, Ross, &

Ross, 1963b; Gelfand, 1962). Some of the most

influential theoretical formulations of imitative


processes (Miller & Dollard, 1941; Skinner, 1953)

have, in fact, been almost exclusively concerned


with the discriminative function of social cues. In

the prototypic experiment the model’s responses

serve as the occasion upon which another


organism is likely to be reinforced for performing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 632
similar responses. After a period of exposure to

differential reinforcement, imitative tendencies

become strongly established; conversely, by


reversing the contingencies so that matching

responses are never reinforced but nonmatching


behavior is consistently rewarded, imitativeness is
reduced to a very low or zero level (Miller &

Dollard, 1941).

Ethologists provide extensive documentation


of the response-facilitating function of social cues

in birds, fish, and mammals (Hall, 1963; Thorpe,

1956). Typically, the sight of certain responses

performed by an animal elicits a similar or


identical pattern of behavior in other members of
the same species. This process is generally

referred to as “social facilitation” or “behavioral

contagion” when it is presumably determined by

prior discriminative reinforcement, and “mimesis”

when corresponding unconditioned response

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 633
patterns are supposedly instinctively aroused.

As Hinde (1953) points out, the occurrence of

matching behavior in animals is often erroneously


attributed to mimetic processes. In the first place,

what appears to be mimetic behavior may involve

response patterns that have, in fact, been

established through prior social learning. Even in

cases where matching behavior is clearly


instinctive, it is frequently difficult to determine
whether social cues constitute the critical eliciting

stimuli. Readily discriminable “sign stimuli”

(Tinbergen, 1951) or “releasers” (Thorpe, 1956)


in the form of color displays, preparatory
movement sequences, postural cues, and specific

vocalizations frequently serve as unconditioned


stimuli for complete patterns of instinctive

behavior in other members of the species.

Therefore, when appropriate releasing stimuli are

displayed by a model during the performance of a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 634
given activity, the corresponding responses on the

part of observing animals may be primarily

controlled by releasing stimuli, rather than the


model’s behavioral cues. Thus, for example, the

white tail feathers of a bird flying upward can

function as flight-eliciting stimuli for other

members of a flock (Armstrong, 1942). A suitably

feathered but nonflying artificial model might


likewise succeed in getting a flock of birds
airborne.

Pseudo-mimesis is also evident in instances

where a model’s behavior directs the observer’s


attention to environmental stimuli which, in turn,
elicit similar innate response patterns. It has been

shown, for example, that animals will consume


considerably more food when they are fed in pairs

than when they are fed alone, and satiated

chickens will begin to eat at the sight of other

birds feeding. It is entirely possible that in such

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 635
cases modeling cues primarily serve an orienting

function, whereas the consummately responses of

the sociable chicks are reinstated and maintained


by the grain to which their attention has been

redirected. The fact that the stimulus complex to

which observing animals are responding

frequently contains, in addition to social cues,

releasing stimuli and other response-controlling


environmental events complicates the
identification and analysis of genuine mimetic

phenomena.

The behavior of models may not only function


as discriminative cues for similar responses, but
also serve to direct the observers’ attention to the

particular stimulus objects manipulated by the


performer (Crawford & Spence, 1939). As a

consequence, observers may subsequently utilize

the same objects to a greater extent, though not

necessarily in an imitative way. In one modeling

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 636
experiment (Bandura, 1962), for example, the

model pummeled a plastic doll with a mallet.

Children who had observed this aggressive act


later displayed significantly more behavior in

which they pounded a peg board with the mallet

than did both the control subjects and those who

had viewed a nonaggressive model. Stimulus

enhancement effects are distinguished from social


facilitation in that the observer’s behavior in the
former case may bear little or no resemblance to

the model’s activities.

It is evident that observers are not equally


affected by the actions of others with whom they
may come into contact, nor are performers equally

influential in evoking the types of behaviors in


which they themselves are engaged. Susceptibility

to social facilitation is largely governed by three

sets of variables that have been discussed at

length in preceding sections and elsewhere. These

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 637
include observer characteristics, the

reinforcement contingencies associated with

matching behavior in the particular setting, and


the attributes of the model (Bandura, 1968;

Campbell, 1961; Flanders, 1968).

In learning analyses of response facilitation as

a function of model attributes (Miller & Dollard,

1941), stimulus generalization and differential


reinforcement are utilized as the main explanatory
principles. According to this interpretation, social

models differ in the extent to which their behavior

is likely to be successful in producing favorable


outcomes. Hence, persons are most frequently
rewarded for matching the behavior of models

who are intelligent, who possess certain social and


technical competencies, who command social

power, and who, by virtue of their adroitness,

occupy high positions in various status

hierarchies. On the other hand, the behaviors of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 638
models who are ineffectual, uninformed, and who

have attained low vocational, intellectual, and

social status, are apt to have considerably less


utilitarian value. As a result of differential

reinforcement for matching models who possess

diverse attributes, the identifying characteristics

gradually come to serve as discriminative stimuli

that signify the probable consequences associated


with behavior modeled by different social agents.
Moreover, through the process of stimulus

generalization, the effect of a model’s prestige

carries over from one area of behavior to another,

and imitative responses tend to generalize to


unfamiliar persons to the extent that they share

similar characteristics with past reward-


producing models.

The fact that social behavior is extensively

under modeling stimulus control suggests that

social phenomena can be partly regulated through

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 639
alteration of focal modeling influences. Lippitt and

his colleagues (Lippitt, Polansky, & Rosen, 1952;

Polansky, Lippitt, & Redl, 1950) have shown in


several field studies that persons to whom is

attributed high social power are the major sources

of imitative behavior for other group members.

These findings indicate that the attitudes and

actions of entire groups can be modified most


rapidly and pervasively by changing the conduct
norms modeled by key sources of behavioral

contagion, whereas attempts to alter the behavior

of each member individually would prove

exceedingly laborious and ineffectual.

Utilization of Modeling Principles in


Planned Sociocultural Change

Societies are continuously faced with the


problem of introducing and gaining widespread

acceptance of new practices designed to improve

the quality of social life. This often involves

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 640
effecting changes in relatively circumscribed

groups, as in the case of specific community

development projects. At other times, however,


far-reaching modifications are sought in economic,

political, educational, and social practices that

implicate the entire culture.

Most socially significant changes involve some

negative consequences that are likely to serve


initially as barriers to change. In the first place,
people are required to expend a certain amount of

their time, energy, and resources, that might

otherwise be used for personal gratification, to


learn new personal habits and modes of living.
Second, the beneficial outcomes that may accrue

from new response patterns usually cannot be


clearly demonstrated until they have been tried

over a period of time. Since innovations often

prove unsuccessful and promoters generally

overvalue their potentialities, people are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 641
understandably apprehensive about forsaking

existing behaviors of established utility for new

ones involving possible superior but uncertain


consequences. Most people, therefore, are

reluctant to change their customary practices until

they have observed new behaviors to be

rewarding for more venturesome adopters. Third,

conventional patterns are usually fortified by


belief systems and moral codes that portend
hazardous consequences for departures from

socially sanctioned practices. Thus, for example,

ineffective psychotherapeutic methods and folk

medical systems are much more difficult to


supplant when people are frightened by beliefs

that innovative procedures will adversely affect


them in ill-defined times and ways than when such

foreboding beliefs have not been used to reinforce

adherence to existing customs.

A fourth obstacle to the successful introduction

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 642
and diffusion of new modes of behavior is created

by individuals in positions of authority who have a

vested interest in preserving traditional prestige


and power structures. They are apt to oppose

actively any changes that may threaten their social

and economic status, particularly if new programs

are associated with outside agencies. Elite

countercontrol is generally maintained through


coercive pressures on less advantaged members
who have the most to gain from changes and are

therefore more receptive to new ways.

It is evident from the preceding discussion that


if programs designed to alter sociocultural
patterns are to meet with success, they must

employ powerful change procedures to overcome


the unfavorable conditions of reinforcement

initially associated with unaccustomed practices.

Attitude-change approaches have been employed

extensively on the assumption that a modification

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 643
in the belief system is a vital prerequisite to

acceptance of new behaviors. This strategy has

proved only partially successful. A different


approach, which concentrates on new alternatives

rather than on hindrances, arranges optimal

conditions for producing the desired behavioral

changes. New practices that are beneficial to the

user eventually become strongly established, and


incongruent attitudes either will be modified to
coincide with adopted behavior or they will be

construed in a manner that is consistent with pre-

existing beliefs.

Among the variety of methods available for


accelerating social changes, modeling plays a

highly influential role. If new response patterns


are to be learned, potential adopters must be

provided with models competent to display the

desired behavior and who are especially likely to

be emulated. Since vicarious reinforcement can

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 644
facilitate modeling, those exemplifying advocated

patterns should be appropriately rewarded to

demonstrate to others the benefits of new


practices. In addition to modeling influences, new

reinforcement contingencies must be introduced

into the social system to favor adoption and

continued performance of new behavior patterns

(Holmberg, 1960). The beneficial effects of new


skills and practices usually do not become
apparent until they have been applied over an

extended period. A change agent may, for example,

be faced with the problem of getting skeptical

people to institute and continue an irksome water


purification project over a long period before they

can obtain any clear evidence that it reduces


infectious diseases. As Erasmus (1961) has noted,

new cultural practices are most readily accepted

when they produce immediate inspectional


benefits and the causal relationship between new

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 645
behavior and advantageous outcomes can be

easily verified. The issue of spectacularity and

immediacy of results probably accounts for the


preference of aggressive over less obstreperous

means of forcing social changes.

In cases where the advantages to be gained

from new behavior patterns are considerably

delayed, it is necessary to provide subsidiary


immediate incentives to sustain them until the
long-term benefits occur and assume the

reinforcing function. These temporary substitute

rewards may involve financial compensations,

social recognition, positions in new leadership


hierarchies, and appropriate forms of status-
conferring symbolic rewards. It is quite possible

that many of the failures of cultural change

programs that are attributed to resistance arising

from conflicting beliefs in fact result from failure

to provide emulative models and adequate

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 646
reinforcement supports for unaccustomed

practices.

Another important factor that militates against


social change is that persons who adopt the new

patterns of behavior are often subjected to

negative sanctions from envious peers and

powerful officials whose vested interests may be

jeopardized. This creates especially difficult


problems when those in positions of power
undermine and block social reforms that do not

promote their own self-interests but are beneficial

for, and desired by, less advantaged persons.


Under these circumstances, little change will
result unless persons who adopt new patterns are

protected from maltreatment, and conditions are


arranged so that the new practices provide some

benefits for all concerned. This can be partly

achieved through the use of socially

interdependent contingencies in which a given

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 647
person’s rewarding outcomes are determined by

both the degree to which he performs the desired

behavior and a composite of individual


performances of the entire group. It will be shown

in Chapter 4 how the addition of group

reinforcement can favorably affect the

performance of its members. However, if an

organized minority continues to force compliance


with traditional practices, then aversive controls
must be applied. Desired objectives must be

enforced through social legislation, and defiance

must produce costly consequences. This

presupposes that change agencies exercise some


degree of control over the rewarding resources

available to communities and their leadership,


that they have the power to impose negative

sanctions, and that they have sufficient social

support to withstand the political repercussions of


enforced changes. In an effort to avoid offending

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 648
the existing leadership, social agencies generally

rely for the implementation of desired changes

upon the traditional elite, who, unfortunately,


often utilize such opportunities to further promote

their self-interests.

Under conditions where advocates of

innovations have no rewarding nor controlling

power, they must first establish their value by


demonstrating, in areas that engender little or no
resistance, that the practices they advocate yield

highly favorable outcomes. After they have thus

enhanced their credibility and modeling potency

they are in a more favorable position to attempt


modifications that conflict with existing traditions
and vested interests.

Summary

This chapter is principally concerned with

modeling processes whereby new modes of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 649
behavior are acquired and existing response
patterns are extensively modified through

observation of other people’s behavior and its


consequences for them.

A multiprocess theory of observational

learning was advanced, according to which


modeled stimulus events are transformed and
retained in imaginal and verbal memory codes.

Later, reinstatement of these representational


mediators, in conjunction with appropriate

environmental cues, guide behavioral

reproduction of matching responses. Performance

of observationally learned responses is largely

regulated by reinforcing outcomes that may be


externally applied, self-administered, or

vicariously experienced. Since modeling


phenomena are controlled by several interrelated

subprocesses, the absence of modeling effects in


any given case may result from either failures in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 650
sensory registration due to inadequate attention

to relevant social cues, deficient symbolic coding

of modeled events into functional mediators of


overt behavior, retention decrements, motor

deficiencies, or unfavorable conditions of

reinforcement.

Modeling procedures have been extensively

employed, with considerable success, for many


purposes, especially for developing conceptual
and interpersonal modes of behavior. In this

approach agents of change model requisite

behaviors and arrange optimal conditions for


recipients to learn and to practice the activities
until they are performed skillfully and

spontaneously. In addition to the utilization of


modeling principles for establishing social and

cognitive competencies, emotional responsiveness

can be conditioned and extinguished on a

vicarious basis. In the case of vicarious affective

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 651
conditioning, exposure to a model’s emotional

reactions arouses in observers emotional

responses which become conditioned, through


contiguous association, to distinctive cues present

in the situation. However, the degree of vicarious

responsiveness is partly dependent upon an

intermediary self-stimulation process involving

symbolic representation of similar consequences


occurring to oneself in the same situation.
Affective expressions of a model are most likely to

elicit high self-arousal in observers under

conditions where the participants have

experienced similar pleasurable or painful


experiences.

Vicarious extinction of emotional behavior is


achieved by exposing an observer to modeled

events in which a performer’s approach responses

toward feared objects do not produce adverse

effects or may engender positive consequences.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 652
Studies of vicarious extinction reveal that this

procedure, particularly when combined with

guided participation, not only produces enduring


and generalized reductions in tenacious avoidance

behavior, but it also induces long-lasting

attitudinal changes and decrements in fearfulness

toward objects that were never specifically

included in the treatment program. A major factor


in modeling procedures that expedites behavioral
changes is assumed to involve vicarious extinction

of arousal reactions below the level for activating

avoidance responses, thus enabling persons to

perform approach behaviors. The fact that


elimination of the arousal potential of threatening

stimuli through a nonresponse extinction


procedure subsequently reduces avoidance

behavior provides further support for a dual-

process learning theory in which classically


conditioned effects partly govern instrumentally

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 653
learned responses.

Exposure to modeled events may also


strengthen or weaken observers’ inhibitions of

existing patterns of behavior. The occurrence of

these inhibitory and disinhibitory effects is mainly


determined by actual or inferred response

consequences to the model. Positive

reinforcement of models’ actions generally


facilitates similar behavior in observers if it is
appropriate to their social role and status,

whereas observation of punishing consequences

to models tends to inhibit similar responsiveness

in others. These vicarious reinforcement effects


may result from the information conveyed by the
model’s outcomes as to what constitutes

permissible or punishable actions in particular

situations, from motivational increases created by

witnessing others receive desired incentives, from

changes in model status produced by disparaging

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 654
or laudatory social reactions, and from vicarious

acquisition or extinction of emotional responses

through exposure to the affective expressions of


models undergoing rewarding or punishing

experiences.

The behavior of models often functions merely

as discriminative stimuli in facilitating the

expression by others of similar behaviors that


ordinarily are not subject to negative sanctions
and therefore do not involve inhibitory

mechanisms. Social models differ considerably in

the extent to which their behavior is likely to be


successful in producing favorable outcomes. As a
result of repeated differential reinforcement for

matching models who differ in intelligence, age,


socioeconomic status, social and vocational

competencies, prestige and power, model

attributes that signify probable consequences for

exemplified behavior determine in large part

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 655
which models will have greatest response-

facilitating effects. Because social behavior is

extensively under modeling stimulus control, the


attitudes and actions of groups can be modified by

altering the conduct norms modeled by major

sources of behavioral contagion.

REFERENCES

Adler, L. L., & Adler, H. E. Age as a factor in


observational learning in puppies. American
Dachshund, 1968, March, 13-14.

Allport, F. H. Social psychology. Cambridge, Mass.:


Riverside Press, 1924.

Alyokrinskii, V. V. Imitatsiya det’mi (4-7 let) russkikh i


nekotoroykh angluikikh glacnykh [Imitation by 4-
7 year-old children of Russian and several
English vowels], Voprosy Psikhologii, 1963, 9, 80-
88.

Angermeier, W. F., Schaul, L. T., & James, W. T. Social


conditioning in rats. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1959, 52, 370-372.

Armstrong, E. A. Bird display and behaviour.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 656
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1942.

Aronfreed, J. Conduct and conscience. New York:


Academic Press, 1968.

Baer, D. M., Peterson, R. F., & Sherman, J. A. The


development of imitation by reinforcing
behavioral similarity to a model. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1967, 10, 405-
416.

Baer, D. M., & Sherman, J. A. Reinforcement control of


generalized imitation in young children. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 1964, 1, 37-49.

Bandura, A. Relationship of family patterns to child


behavior disorders. Progress Report, 1960,
Stanford University, Project No. M-1734, United
States Public Health Service.

Bandura, A. Social learning through imitation. In M. R.


Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation:
1962. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press,
1962. pp. 211-269.

Bandura, A. Vicarious processes: A case of no-trial


learning. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology. Vol. II. New York:
Academic Press, 1965, pp. 1-55. (a)

Bandura, A. Influence of models’ reinforcement

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 657
contingencies on the acquisition of imitative
responses. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1965, 1, 589-595. (b)

Bandura, A. Social-learning theory of identificatory


processes. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of
socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand
McNally, 1968.

Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. The relative


efficacy of desensitization and modeling
approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and
attitudinal changes, Stanford University, 1969,
13, 173-199.

Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L.


Observational learning as a function of
symbolization and incentive set. Child
Development, 1966, 37, 499-506.

Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. Some social


determinants of self-monitoring reinforcement
systems. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1967, 5, 449-455. (a)

Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. Vicarious


extinction of avoidance behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 5, 16-23.
(b)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 658
Bandura, A., & Harris, M. B. Modification of syntactic
style. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
1966, 4, 341-352.

Bandura, A., & Huston, A. C. Identification as a process


of incidental learning. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1961, 63, 311-318.

Bandura, A., & Kupers, C. J. Transmission of patterns


of self-reinforcement through modeling. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964, 69, 1-9.

Bandura, A., & McDonald, F. J. The influence of social


reinforcement and the behavior of models in
shaping children’s moral judgments. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 274-
281.

Bandura, A., & Menlove, F. L. Factors determining


vicarious extinction of avoidance behavior
through symbolic modeling. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1968, 8, 99-108.

Bandura, A., & Mischel, W. The influence of models in


modifying delay of gratification patterns. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 698-
705.

Bandura, A., & Rosenthal, T. L. Vicarious classical


conditioning as a function of arousal level. Journal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 659
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 54-
62.

Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. Imitation of film-


mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1963, 66, 3-11. (a)

Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. A comparative test


of the status envy, social power, and secondary
reinforcement theories of identificatory learning.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963,
67, 527-534. (b)

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression.


New York: Ronald Press, 1959.

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Social learning and


personality development. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston, 1963.

Bandura, A., & Whalen, C. K. The influence of


antecedent reinforcement and divergent
modeling cues on patterns of self-reward. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 373-
382.

Barnett, P. E., & Benedetti, D. T. Vicarious conditioning


of the GSR to sound. Paper read at the meeting of
the Rocky Mountain Psychological Association,
Glenwood Springs, 1960.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 660
Barnwell, A. K. Potency of modeling cues in imitation
and vicarious reinforcement situations.
Dissertation Abstracts, 1966, 26, 7444.

Bayroff, A. G., & Lard, K. E. Experimental social


behavior of animals: III. Imitational learning of
white rats. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
1944, 37, 165-171.

Benton, A. A. Effects of the timing of negative


response consequences on the observational
learning of resistance to temptation in children.
Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, 27, 2153-2154.

Berger, S. M. Conditioning through vicarious


instigation. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 450-
466.

Berkowitz, L. Control of aggression. In B. M. Caldwell


& H. Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of child development
research. Vol. 3. New York: Russell Sage
Foundation, 1969.

Berkowitz, S. Acquisition and maintenance of


generalized imitative repertoires of profoundly
retarded children with retarded peers
functioning as models and reinforcing agents.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Maryland, 1968.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 661
Beyer, N. L., & May, J. G., Jr. The effects of race and
socioeconomic status on imitative behavior in
children using white male and female models.
Unpublished manuscript, Florida State
University, 1968.

Black, A. H. The extinction of avoidance responses


under curare. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 519-524.

Black, A. H., Carlson, N. J., & Solomon, R. L. Exploratory


studies of die conditioning of autonomic
responses in curarized dogs. Psychological
Monographs, 1962, 76 (29, Whole No. 548).

Blake, R. R. The other person in the situation. In R.


Tagiuri & L. Petrullo (Eds.), Person perception and
interpersonal behavior. Stanford: Stanford
University Press, 1958. pp. 229-242.

Blake, R. R., Mouton, J. S., & Hain, J. D. Social forces in


petition signing. Southwestern Social Science
Quarterly, 1956, 36, 385-390.

Blake, R. R., Rosenbaum, M., & Duryea, R. Gift-giving as


a function of group standards. Human Relations,
1955, 8, 61-73.

Blanchard, E. B. The relative contributions of


modeling, informational influences, and physical

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 662
contact in the extinction of phobic behavior.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford
University, 1969.

Bower, G. M. Mental imagery and associative learning.


In L. Gregg (Ed.), Cognition in learning and
memory. New York: Wiley, 1969.

Brigham, T. A., & Sherman, J. A. An experimental


analysis of verbal imitation in preschool children.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 151-
158.

Brown, R., & Bellugi, U. Three processes in the child’s


acquisition of syntax. Harvard Educational
Review, 1964, 34, 133-151.

Bryan, J. H., & Test, M. A. Models and helping:


Naturalistic studies in aiding behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 6, 400-
407.

Buchwald, A. M. Extinction after acquisition under


different verbal reinforcement combinations.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1959, 57, 43-
48. (a)

Buchwald, A. M. Experimental alterations in the


effectiveness of verbal reinforcement
combinations. Journal of Experimental Psychology,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 663
1959, 57, 351-361. (b)

Buchwald, A. M. Supplementary report: Alteration of


the reinforcement value of a positive reinforcer.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1960, 60,
416-418.

Campbell, D. T. Conformity in psychology’s theories of


acquired behavioral dispositions. In I. A. Berg &
B. M. Bass (Eds.), Conformity and deviation. New
York: Harper, 1961. pp. 101-142.

Chittenden, G. E. An experimental study in measuring


and modifying assertive behavior in young
children. Monographs of the Society for Research
in Child Development, 1942, 7 (1, Serial No. 31).

Church, R. M. Transmission of learned behavior


between rats. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1957, 54, 163-165.

Church, R. M. Emotional reactions of rats to the pain


of others. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1959, 52, 132-134.

Clark, B. S. The acquisition and extinction of peer


imitation in children. Psychonomic Science, 1965,
2, 147-148.

Colby, K. M. Computer-aided language development in


nonspeaking mental! disturbed children.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 664
Technical Report No. CS85. Stanford University,
1967

Conant, M. B. Conditioned visual hallucinations.


Unpublished manuscript Stanford University,
1964.

Corsini, R. J., & Putzey, L. J. Bibliography of group


psychotherapy. Beacon, N. Y.: Beacon House,
1957.

Corsini, R. J., Shaw, M. E., & Blake, R. R. Roleplaying in


business and industry. New York: Free Press,
1961.

Corson, J. A. Observational learning of a lever pressing


response. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 7, 197-198.

Cowan, P. A., Hoddinoth, B. A., & Wright, B. A.


Compliance and resistance in the conditioning of
autistic children: An exploratory study. Child
Development, 1965, 36, 91.3-923.

Craig, K. D., & Weinstein, M. S. Conditioning vicarious


affective arousal. Psychological Reports, 1965, 17,
955-963.

Crawford, M. P., & Spence, K. W. Observational


learning of discrimination problems by
chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
1939, 27, 133-147.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 665
Crooks, J. L. Observational learning of fear in
monkeys. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Pennsylvania, 1967.

Darby, C. L., & Riopelle, A. J. Observational learning in


the Rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1959, 52, 94-98.

deCharms, R., & Rosenbaum, M. E. Status variables


and matching behavior. Journal of Personality,
1960, 28, 492-502.

Drakeford, J. W. Integrity therapy. Nashville, Tenn.:


Broadman, 1967.

Dubanoski, R. A. Imitation as a function of role


appropriateness of behavior and response
consequences to the model. Unpublished
manuscript, University of Iowa, 1967.

Duncker, K. Experimental modification of children’s


food preferences through social suggestion.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1938,
33, 489-507.

Dysinger, W. S., & Ruckmick, C. A. The emotional


responses of children to the motion-picture
situation. New York: Macmillan, 1933.

Easterbrook, J. A. The effect of emotion on cue


utilization and the organization of behavior.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 666
Psychological Review, 1959, 66, 183-201.

Ellson, D. G. Hallucinations produced by sensory


conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1941, 28, 1-20.

Emmerich, W. Parental identification in young


children. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 1959,
60, 257-308.

Epstein, R. Aggression toward outgroups as a function


of authoritarianism and imitation of aggressive
models. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1966, 3, 574-579.

Erasmus, C. J. Man takes control. Minneapolis:


University of Minnesota Press, 1961.

Feshbach, N. D. Variations in teachers’ reinforcement


style and imitative behavior of children differing
in personality characteristics and social
background. Unpublished manuscript, University
of California, Los Angeles, 1967.

Flanders, J. P. A review of research on imitative


behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 1968, 69, 316-
337.

Flanders, J. P., & Thistlethwaite, D. L. Effects of


informative and justificatory variables upon
imitation. Unpublished manuscript, Vanderbilt

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 667
University, 1969.

Foss, B. M. Mimicry in mynas (Gracula religiosa): A


test of Mowrer’s theory. British Journal of
Psychology, 1964, 55, 85-88.

Freed, A., Chandler, P. J., Mouton, J. S., & Blake, R. R.


Stimulus and background factors in sign
violation, journal of Personality, 1955, 23, 499
(abstract) .

Gelfand, D. M. The influence of self-esteem on rate of


verbal conditioning and social matching
behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1962, 65, 259-265.

Gerst, M. S. Symbolic coding operations in


observational learning. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Stanford University, 1969.

Gewirtz, J. L., & Stingle, K. C. The learning of


generalized imitation as the basis for
identification. Psychological Review, 1968, 75,
374-397.

Gittelman, M. Behavior rehearsal as a technique in


child treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 1965, 6, 251-255.

Grosser, D., Polansky, N., & Lippitt, R. A laboratory


study of behavioral contagion. Human Relations,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 668
1951, 4,115-142.

Grusec, J. E. Some antecedents of self-criticism.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966,
4, 244-252.

Grusec, J. E., & Brinker, D. B., Jr. Reinforcement for


imitation as a social learning determinant with
implications for sex role development.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Toronto,
1969.

Grusec, J. E., & Mischel, W. The model’s characteristics


as determinants of social learning. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 211-
215.

Hall, K. R. Observational learning in monkeys and


apes. British Journal of Psychology, 1963, 54, 201-
226.

Hanlon, C. The effects of social isolation and


characteristics of the model on accent imitation
in fourth-grade children. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Stanford University, 1964.

Harris, M. B. Some determinants of sharing in


children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Stanford University, 1968.

Hayes, K. J., & Hayes, C. Imitation in a home-raised

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 669
chimpanzee. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1952, 45, 450-459.

Helson, H., Blake, R. R., Mouton, J. S., & Olmstead, J. A.


Attitudes as adjustments to stimulus,
background, and residual factors. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1956, 52, 314-
322.

Henker, B. A. The effect of adult model relationships


on children’s play and task imitation. Dissertation
Abstracts, 1964, 24, 4797.

Herbert, J. J., & Harsh, C. M. Observational learning by


cats. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1944, 37,
81-95.

Hicks, D. J. Imitation and retention of film-mediated


aggressive peer and adult models. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 97-
100.

Hill, J. H., Liebert, R. M., & Mott, D. E. W. Vicarious


extinction of avoidance behavior through films:
An initial test. Psychological Reports, 1968, 12,
192.

Hinde, R. A. The term “mimesis.” British Journal of


Animal Behavior, 1953, 1,7-11.

Holmberg, A. R. Changing community attitudes and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 670
values in Peru: a case study. In R. N. Adams (Ed.),
Social change in Latin America today. New York:
Harper, 1960. pp. 63-107.

Holt, E. B. Animal drive and the learning process. Vol. 1.


New York: Holt, 1931.

Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H.


Communication and persuasion. New Haven: Yale
University Press, 1953.

Hovland, C. I., Lumsdaine, A. A., & Sheffield, F. D.


Experiments in mass communication. Princeton:
Princeton University Press, 1949.

Humphery, J. Personal communication, 1966.

Humphrey, G. Imitation and the conditioned reflex.


Pedagogical Seminary, 1921, 28, 1-21.

Hutt, S. J., Hutt, C., Lee, D., & Ounsted, C. A behavioural


and electroencephalographic study of autistic
children. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 1965, 3,
181-197.

Imanishi, K. Social behavior in Japanese monkeys,


Macaca fuscata. Psychologia, 1957, 1, 47-54.

Jakubczak, L. F., & Walters, R. H. Suggestibility as


dependency behavior. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1959, 59, 102-107.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 671
Jersild, A. T., & Holmes, F. B. Methods of overcoming
children’s fears. Journal of Psychology, 1935, 1,75-
104.

Jones, M. C. The elimination of children’s fears. Journal


of Experimental Psychology, 1924, 7, 383-390.

Kagan, J., & Mussen, P. H. Dependency themes on the


TAT and group conformity. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 1956, 20, 29-32.

Kanareff, V. T., & Lanzetta, J. T. Effects of task


definition and probability of reinforcement upon
the acquisition and extinction of imitative
responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1960, 60, 340-348.

Kanfer, F. H. Vicarious human reinforcement: A


glimpse into the black box. In L. Krasner & L. P.
Ullmann (Eds.), Research in behavior modification.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. pp.
244-267.

Kausler, D. H., & Trapp, E. P. Motivation and cue


utilization in intentional and incidental learning.
Psychological Review, 1960, 67, 373-379.

Kawamura, S. The process of sub-culture propagation


among Japanese Macaques. In C. II. Southwick
(Ed.), Primate social behavior. Princeton: Van

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 672
Nostrand, 1963. pp. 82-90.

Kellogg, W. N., & Kellogg, L. A. The ape and the child: A


study of environmental influence upon early
behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1933.

Kelly, G. A. The psychology of personal constructs. Vol.


11. Clinical diagnosis and psychotherapy. New
York: Norton, 1955.

Kimbrell, D. L., & Blake, R. R. Motivational factors in


the violation of a prohibition. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1958, 56, 132-133.

Kohlberg, L. Moral development and identification. In


H. W. Stevenson (Ed.), Child Psychology: The sixty-
second yearbook of the National Society for the
Study of Education. Part I. Chicago: The National
Society for the Study of Education, 1963. pp. 277-
332.

Kriazhev, V. I. The objective investigation of the


higher nervous activity in a collective
experiment. Psychological Abstracts, 1934, 8,
2532.

Krumboltz, J. D., & Thoresen, C. E. The effect of


behavioral counseling in group and individual
settings on information-seeking behavior. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 1964, 11, 324-333.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 673
Krumboltz, J. D., Varenhorst, B. B., & Thoresen, C. E.
Nonverbal factors in the effectiveness of models
in counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
1967, 14,412-418.

Kuhn, D. Z., Madsen, C. H., Jr., & Becker, W. C. Effects of


exposure to an aggressive model and
“frustration” on children’s aggressive behavior.
Child Development, 1967, 38, 739-745.

Lanzetta, J. T., & Kanareff, V. T. The effects of a


monetary reward on the acquisition of an
imitative response. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1959, 59,120-127.

Lazarus, A. A. Behaviour rehearsal vs. nondirective


therapy vs. advice in effecting behaviour change.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 209-
212.

Lazarus, R. S., Speisman, J. C., Mordkoff, A. M., &


Davison, L. A. A laboratory study of psychological
stress produced by a motion picture film.
Psychological Monographs, 1962, 76 (Whole No.
553).

Lazowick, L. On the nature of identification. Journal of


Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, 51, 175-
183.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 674
Lefkowitz, M. M., Blake, R. R., & Mouton, J. S. Status
factors in pedestrian violation of traffic signals,
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955,
51, 704-706.

Lesser, G. S., & Abelson, R. P. Personality correlates of


persuasibility in children. In C. I. Hovland & I. L.
Janis (Eds.), Personality and persuasibility. New
Haven: Yale University Press, 1959. pp. 187-206.

Leuba, C. Images as conditioned sensations. Journal of


Experimental Psychology, 1940, 26, 345-351.

Lippitt, R., Polansky, N., & Rosen, S. The dynamics of


power. Human Relations, 1952, 5, 37-64.

Lovaas, O. I. A program for the establishment of


speech in psychotic children. In J. K. Wing (Ed.),
Early childhood autism. Oxford: Pergamon, 1966.
pp. 115-144. (a)

Lovaas, O. I. Reinforcement therapy. (16 mm. sound


film). Philadelphia: Smith, Kline and French
Laboratories, 1966. (b)

Lovaas, O. I. A behavior therapy approach to the


treatment of childhood schizophrenia. In J. P. Hill
(Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology,
Vol. 1. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota
Press, 1967. pp. 108-159.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 675
Lovaas, O. I., Berberich, J. P., Kassorla, I. C., Klynn, G. A.,
& Meisel, J. Establishment of a texting and
labelling vocabulary in schizophrenic children.
Unpublished manuscript, University of California,
Los Angeles, 1966.

Lovaas, O. I., Berberich, J. P., Perloff, B. F., & Schaeffer,


B. Acquisition of imitative speech by
schizophrenic children. Science, 1966, 151, 705-
707.

Lovaas, O. I., Dumont, D. A., Klynn, G. A., & Meisel, j.


Establishment of appropriate response to, and
use of, certain prepositions and pronouns in
schizophrenic children. Unpublished manuscript,
University of California, Los Angeles, 1966.

Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, L., Nelson, K., & Whalen, C. The


establishment of imitation and its use for the
development of complex behavior in
schizophrenic children. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1967, 5, 171-181.

Luchins, A. S., & Luchins, E. H. Imitation by rote and by


understanding. Journal of Social Psychology, 1961,
54, 175-197.

Luchins, A. S., & Luchins, E. H. Learning a complex


ritualized social role The Psychological Record,
1966, 16, 177-187.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 676
Maccoby, E. E. Role-taking in childhood and its
consequences for social learning. Child
Development, 1959, 30, 239-252.

Maccoby, E. E., & Wilson, W. C. Identification and


observational learning from films. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1957, 55, 76—
87

Maccoby, N., Michael, D. N., & Levine, S. Further


studies of student participation procedures in
film instruction: Review and preview, covert
practice, and motivational interactions. In A. A.
Lumsdaine (Ed.), Student response in
programmed instruction. Washington, D. C.:
National Academy of Sciences—-National
Research Council, 1961. pp. 295-325.

McDonald, F., & Allen, D. W. Training effects of


feedback and modeling procedures on teaching
performance. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford
University, 1967.

McDougall, W. An introduction to social psychology.


London: Methuen, 1908.

McGuire, W. J. Interpolated motivational statements


within a programmed series of instructions as a
distribution-of-practice factor. In A. A. Lumsdaine
(Ed.), Student response in programmed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 677
instruction. Washington, D. C.: National Academy
of Sciences—National Research Council, 1961.
pp. 411-415.

Madsen, C., Jr. Nurturance and modeling in


preschoolers. Child Development, 1968, 39, 221-
236.

Mandler, G. Association and organization: Facts,


fancies, and theories. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton
(Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior
theory. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall,
1968. pp. 109- 119.

Margolius, G. J., & Sheffield, F. D. Optimum methods of


combining practice with filmed demonstration in
teaching complex response sequences: Serial
learning of a mechanical-assembly task. In A. A.
Lumsdaine (Ed.), Student response in
programmed instruction. Washington, D. C.:
National Academy of Sciences—National
Research Council, 1961. pp. 33—53.

Marlatt, G. A. Vicarious and direct reinforcement


control of verbal behavior in an interview setting.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana
University, 1968. (a)

Marlatt, G. A. Exposure to a model and task ambiguity


as determinants of verbal behavior in an

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 678
interview. Paper read at the Western
Psychological Association meeting, San Diego,
April 1968. (b)

Marlatt, G. A., Jacobsen, E. A., Johnson, D. L., & Morrice,


D. J. Effect of exposure to a model receiving
varied informational feedback upon consequent
behavior in an interview. Paper presented at the
Midwestern Psychological Association meeting,
Chicago, 1966.

Marshall, H. R., & Hahn, S. C. Experimental


modification of dramatic play. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 5, 119-
122.

Masserman, J. H. Behavior and neurosis. Chicago:


University of Chicago Press, 1943.

Masters, J. C., & Branch, M. N. A comparison of the


relative effectiveness of instructions, modeling,
and reinforcement procedures for inducing
behavior change. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1969 (in press).

Mausner, R. Studies in social interaction: III. Effect of


variation in one partner’s prestige on the
interaction of observer pairs. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 1953, 37, 391-393.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 679
Mausner, B. The effect of prior reinforcement on the
interaction of observer pairs. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1954, 49, 65-68. (a)

Mausner, B. The effect of one partner’s success in a


relevant task on the interaction of observer pairs.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1954,
49,557-560. (b)

Mausner, B., & Bloch, B. L. A study of the additivity of


variables affecting social interaction. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1957, 54, 250-
256.

Metz, J. R. Conditioning generalized imitation in


autistic children, journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 1965, 2, 389-399.

Michael, D. N., & Maccoby, N. Factors influencing the


effects of student participation on verbal learning
from films: Motivating versus practice effects,
“feedback,” and overt versus covert responding.
In A. A. Lumsdaine (Ed.), Student response in
programmed instruction, Washington, D. C.:
National Academy of Sciences—National
Research Council, 1961. pp. 271-293.

Miller, E. C., Dvorak, A., & Turner, D. W. A method of


creating aversion to alcohol by reflex
conditioning in a group setting. Quarterly Journal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 680
of Studies on Alcohol, 1960, 21, 424-431.

Miller, N. E. Some implications of modern behavior


theory for personality change and psychotherapy.
In P. Worchel & D. Byrne (Eds.), Personality
change. New York: Wiley, 1964. pp. 149-175.

Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. Social learning and imitation.


New Haven: Yale University Press, 1941.

Miller, R. E. Experimental approaches to the


physiological and behavioral concomitants of
affective communication in rhesus monkeys. In S.
A. Altmann (Ed.), Social communication among
primates. Chicago: University of Chicago Press,
1967. pp. 125-134.

Miller, R. E., Banks, J. H., & Ogawa, N. Communication


of affect in “cooperative conditioning” of rhesus
monkeys. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1962, 64, 343-348.

Miller, R. E., Banks, J. H., Jr., & Ogawa, N. Role of facial


expression in “cooperative avoidance
conditioning” in monkeys. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 24-30.

Miller, R. E., Caul, W. F., & Mirsky, I. A. Communication


of affect between feral and socially isolated
monkeys. Journal of Personality and Social

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 681
Psychology, 1967, 7, 2.31-239.

Miller, R. E., Murphy, J. V., & Mirsky, I. A. Nonverbal


communication of affect. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 1959, 15, 155-158.

Mischel, W., & Grusec, J. Determinants of the rehearsal


and transmission of neutral and aversive
behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1966, 3, 197-205.

Moreno, J. L. Fundamental rules and techniques of


psychodrama. In J. H. Masserman & J. L. Moreno
(Eds.), Progress in psychotherapy. Vol. 3.
Techniques of psychotherapy. New York: Grune &
Stratton, 1958. pp. 86-131.

Morgan, C. L. Habit and instinct. London: E. Arnold,


1896.

Morrisett, L. N., Jr. The role of implicit practice in


learning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale
University, 1956.

Mowrer, O. H. Identification: A link between learning


theory and psychotherapy. In Learning theory
and personality dynamics. New York: Ronald
Press, 1950. pp. 573-615.

Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and the symbolic


processes. New York: Wiley, 1960.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 682
Mowrer, O. H. The new group therapy. Princeton, N. J.:
Van Nostrand, 1964.

Mowrer, O. H. The behavior therapies, with special


reference to modeling and imitation. American
Journal of Psychotherapy, 1966, 20, 439—461.

Mussen, P. H., & Parker, A. L. Mother-nurturance and


girls’ incidental imitative learning. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 94-97.

O’Connor, R. D. Modification of social withdrawal


through symbolic modeling. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 1969, 2, 15-22.

Odom, R. D., Liebert, R. M., & Hill, J. H. The effects of


modeling cues, reward, and attentional set on the
production of grammatical and ungrammatical
syntactic constructions. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 1968, 6, 131-140.

Ofstad, N. S. The transmission of self-reinforcement


patterns through imitation of sex-role
appropriate behavior. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Utah, 1967.

Paivio, A. Mental imagery in associative learning and


memory. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 241-
263.

Parke, R. D., & Walters, R. H. Some factors influencing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 683
the efficacy of punishment training for inducing
response inhibition. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 1967, 32 (1, Serial
No. 109).

Parloff, M. B., Iflund, B., & Goldstein, N.


Communication of “therapy values” between
therapist and schizophrenic patients. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Diseases, 1960, 130, 193-199.

Parsons, T. The social system. Glencoe, 111.: Free


Press, 1951.

Parsons, T. Family structure and the socialization of


the child. In T. Parsons & R. F. Bales, Family,
socialization, and interaction process. Glencoe,
111.: Free Press, 1955. pp. 35-131.

Patterson, G. R., Littman, I., & Brown, T. R. Negative set


and social learning. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1968, 8, 109-116.

Pentony, P. Value change in psychotherapy. Human


Relations, 1966, 19, 39-46.

Perry, H. M. The relative efficiency of actual and


“imaginary” practice in five selected tasks.
Archives of Psychology, 1939, 34, No. 243.

Piaget, J. The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe,


111.: Free Press, 1948.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 684
Piaget, J. Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New
York: Norton, 1952.

Polansky, N., Lippitt, R., & Redl, F. An investigation of


behavioral contagion in groups. Human Relations,
1950, 3, 318-348.

Porro, C. R. Effects of the observation of a model’s


affective responses to her own transgression on
resistance to temptation in children. Dissertation
Abstracts, 1968, 28, 3064.

Reed, J. L. Social modeling and object interaction as a


means of inducing change in children’s scientific
concepts. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard
University, 1966.

Rimland, B. Infantile autism. New York: Appleton-


Century-Crofts, 1962.

Rimm, D. C., & Mahoney, M. J. The application of


reinforcement and modeling- guidance
procedures in the treatment of snake phobic
behavior. Unpublished manuscript, Arizona State
University, 1969.

Risley, T., & Wolf, M. Establishing functional speech in


echolalic children. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1967, 5, 73-88.

Ritter, B. The group treatment of children’s snake

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 685
phobias using vicarious and contact
desensitization procedures. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 6, 1-6.

Ritter, B. Eliminating excessive fears of the


environment through contact desensitization. In
J. B. Krumboltz & C. E. Thoresen (Eds.),
Behavioral counseling: Cases and techniques. New
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1969. (a)

Ritter, B. Treatment of acrophobia with contact


desensitization. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
1969, 7, 41-45.

Rosekrans, M. A., & Hartup, W. W. Imitative influences


of consistent and inconsistent response
consequences to a model on aggressive behavior
in children. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1967, 7, 429-434.

Rosenbaum, M. E. The effect of stimulus and


background factors on the volunteering response.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1956,
53,118-121.

Rosenbaum, M. E., & Blake, R. R. Volunteering as a


function of field structure. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1955, 50, 193-196.

Rosenbaum, M. E., & Tucker, I. F. The competence of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 686
the model and the learning of imitation and
nonimitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1962, 63, 183-190.

Rosenblith, J. F. Learning by imitation in kindergarten


children. Child Development, 1959, 30, 69-80.

Rosenblith, J. F. Imitative color choices in


kindergarten children. Child Development, 1961,
32, 211-223.

Rosenhan, D., & White, G. M. Observation and


rehearsal as determinants of prosocial behavior.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967,
5, 424-131.

Rosenthal, D. Changes in some moral values following


psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
1955, 19, 431-436.

Ross, D. Relationship between dependency,


intentional learning, and incidental learning in
preschool children. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 374-381.

Ross, S. A. The effect of deviant and nondeviant


models on the behavior of preschool children in a
temptation situation. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Stanford University, 1962.

Schachter, S., & Hall, R. Group-derived restraints and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 687
audience persuasion. Human Relations, 1952, 5,
397-406.

Schein, E. H. The effect of reward on adult imitative


behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1954, 49, 389-395.

Schwartz, A. N., & Hawkins, H. L. Patient models and


affect statements in group therapy. Proceedings of
the 73rd annual convention of the A.P.A.
Washington, D.C.: American Psychological
Association, 1965. pp. 265-266.

Sheffield, F. D. Theoretical considerations in the


learning of complex sequential tasks from
demonstration and practice. In A. A. Lumsdaine
(Ed.), Student response in programmed
instruction. Washington, D. C.: National Academy
of Sciences—National Research Council, 1961.
pp. 13-32.

Sheffield, F. D., & Maccoby, N. Summary and


interpretation of research on organizational
principles in constructing filmed demonstrations.
In A. A. Lumsdaine (Ed.), Student response in
programmed instruction. Washington, D. C.:
National Academy of Sciences—National
Research Council, 1961. pp. 117-131.

Sherman, J. A. Use of reinforcement and imitation to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 688
reinstate verbal behavior in mute psychotics.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 70, 155-
164.

Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York:


Macmillan, 1953.

Sloane, H. N., Jr., Johnston, M. K., & Harris, F. R.


Remedial procedures for teaching verbal
behavior to speech deficient or defective young
children. In H. N. Sloane, Jr. & B. A. MacAulay
(Eds.), Operant procedures in remedial speech and
language training. Boston: Houghton Mifflin,
1968. pp. 77-101.

Slobin, D. I. Imitation and grammatical development


in children. In N. S. Endler, L. R. Boulter, & H.
Osser (Eds.), Contemporary issues in
developmental psychology. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston, 1968. pp. 437^43.

Solomon, R. L., & Coles, M. R. A case of failure of


generalization of imitation across drives and
across situations. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1954, 49, 7-13.

Solomon, R. L., & Turner, L. H. Discriminative classical


conditioning in dogs paralyzed by curare can
later control discriminative avoidance responses
in the normal state. Psychological Review, 1962,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 689
69, 202-219.

Speisman, J. C., Lazarus, R. S., Mordkoff, A., & Davison,


L. Experimental reduction of stress based on ego-
defense theory. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1964, 68, 367-380.

Spiegler, M. D., Liebert, R. M., McMains, M. J., &


Fernandez, L. E. Experimental development of a
modeling treatment to extinguish persistent
avoidance behavior. Unpublished manuscript,
Vanderbilt University, 1968.

Stoke, S. M. An inquiry into the concept of


identification. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1950,
76, 163-189.

Stotland, E. Exploratory investigations of empathy. In


L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology, Vol. 4. New York: Academic
Press, 1969.

Stotland, E., Shaver, K., & Crawford, R. Empathizing


with pain and with pleasure, perceived similarity,
and birth order. Unpublished manuscript,
University of Washington, 1966.

Sturm, I. E. The behavioristic aspect of psychodrama.


Group Psychotherapy, 1965, 18, 50-64.

Tarde, G. The laws of imitation. New York: Holt, 1903.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 690
Taub, E., Bacon, R. C., & Berman, A. J. Acquisition of a
trace-conditioned avoidance response after
deafferentation of the responding limb. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1965,
59, 275-279.

Taub, E., Teodoru, D., Ellman, S. J., Bloom, R. F., &


Berman, A. J. Deafferentation in monkeys:
Extinction of avoidance responses, discrimination
and discrimination reversal. Psychonomic Science,
1966, 4, 323-324.

Thorndike, E. L. Animal intelligence: An experimental


study of the associative processes in animals.
Psychological Review Monograph Supplement,
1898, 2 (4, Whole No. 8).

Thorpe, W. H. Learning and instinct in animals.


London: Methuen, 1956.

Tinbergen, N. The study of instinct. Oxford: Clarendon,


1951.

Truax, C. B., & Carkhuff, R. R. Toward effective


counseling and psychotherapy. Chicago: Aldine,
1967.

Tulving, E. Theoretical issues in free recall. In T. R.


Dixon, & D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and
general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.:

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 691
Prentice-Hall, 1968. pp. 2-36.

Twining, W. E. Mental practice and physical practice


in learning a motor skill. Research Quarterly,
1949, 20, 432-435.

Vandell, R. A., Davis, R. A., & Clugston, H. A. The


function of mental practice in the acquisition of
motor skills. Journal of General Psychology, 1943,
29, 243-250.

Walters, R. H., Leat, M. & Mezei, L. Inhibition and


disinhibition of responses through empathetic
learning. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1963,
17, 235-243.

Walters, R. H., & Llewellyn Thomas, E. Enhancement


of punitiveness by visual and audiovisual
displays. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1963,
17, 244-255.

Walters, R. H., Parke, R. D., & Cane, V. A. Timing of


punishment and the observation of consequences
to others as determinants of response inhibition.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1965, 2,
10-30.

Warden, C. J., Fjeld, H. A., & Koch, A. M. Imitative


behavior in Cebus and Rhesus monkeys.
Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 692
Psychology, 1940, 56, 311-322.

Warden, C. J., & Jackson, T. A. Imitative behavior in the


Rhesus monkey. Pedagogical Seminary and
Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1935, 46, 103-125.

Watson, J. B. Imitation in monkeys. Psychological


Bulletin, 1908, 5, 169-178.

Wheeler, L. Toward a theory of behavioral contagion.


Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 179-192.

Wilson, F. S., & Walters, R. H. Modification of speech


output of near-mute schizophrenics through
social-learning procedures. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1966, 4, 59-67.

Winitz, H., & Preisler, L. Discrimination pretraining


and sound learning. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
1965, 20, 905-916.

Wolpe, J. Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition.


Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1958.

Wolpe, J., & Lazarus, A. A. Behavior therapy techniques.


Oxford: Pergamon, 1966.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 693
4

Positive Control
In the modification of psychological conditions
that reflect primarily behavioral deficits, the

development of complex repertoires of behavior

and the strengthening of existing responses

constitute important objectives. Moreover, after

behavior has been well established, appropriate

conditions must be created to maintain it at a


satisfactory level. Reinforcement procedures are
best suited for these purposes.

It has been amply demonstrated that behavior


is controlled to a large extent by its consequences.

Any attempts, therefore, to produce enduring

changes in responsiveness must alter the

incidence, and often the nature, of reinforcing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 694
outcomes that are customarily produced by given

modes of response. There are two broad classes of

consequences—rewarding and punishing events


—that serve as important determinants of

behavior. The present chapter is mainly concerned

with the establishment of response patterns and


their maintenance through systematic application

of positive reinforcement.

Theoretical Interpretations of
Reinforcement Processes

When a given response is followed by a


positively reinforcing consequence, it increases

the likelihood that the response will be repeated

on subsequent occasions. Although there is little

dispute about the validity of the empirical


principle of reinforcement, numerous alternative

explanations have been proposed for the manner


in which reinforcement produces its effects

(Hilgard & Bower, 1966; Kimble, 1961). The

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 695
various theoretical approaches differ in the extent

to which they consider drive states, stimulus

events, or response properties to be the critical


factors governing reinforcement processes.

DRIVE-REDUCTION HYPOTHESIS

One influential theory of reinforcement (Hull,


1943) assumes that the effects of reinforcing

consequences are produced by need-reduction. In


interpreting the process of primary reinforcement,

it is assumed that deprivation or painful

stimulation produces a physiological need giving


rise to a drive that activates behavior. A
reinforcing event is one that reduces a drive by

satisfying or removing the need. A more

comprehensive form of this drive-reduction view

was advanced by Miller & Dollard (1941), who

emphasize the activating properties of strong

stimuli rather than needs. According to their

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 696
stimulus-reduction theory, any stimulation,
regardless of whether it is based upon a need, can

become a drive if it is made sufficiently intense;


reduction of aversive stimulation has reinforcing

effects.

There exists an extensive body of evidence that


drive-inducing operations greatly enhance the
potency of reinforcing stimuli, and that

attenuation or termination of aversive stimulation


can have strong reinforcing effects on behavior.

The homeostatic conception of reinforcement

receives some further support from studies

demonstrating that procedures designed to alter

physiological states directly, while excluding


secondary reinforcement deriving from sensory

stimulation and consummatory responses, can


function as effective reinforcers for overt

behavior. Food-deprived animals, for example,


learn to make responses that result in nutrients

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 697
being delivered directly into their stomachs

(Miller & Kessen, 1952) or glucose being injected

intravenously (Chambers, 1956; Coppock &


Chambers, 1954) contingent upon correct

performances. Similarly, intravenous insulin

injections, which produce rapid decreases in blood

sugar level, have a punishing effect upon behavior

(Coppock, Headlee, & Hood, 1953).

The assumption that reinforcement requires


drive-reduction was initially questioned by

experiments demonstrating that nonnutritive

saccharin reinforced behavior (Sheffield & Roby,


1950). Similarly, copulation without ejaculation,
which produces no reduction in tension (Sheffield,

Wulff, & Backer, 1951; Whalen, 1961), could serve


as an effective reward. However, the conclusions

drawn from these findings regarding the validity

of the drive-reduction theory of reinforcement

have been challenged by Miller (1963), on the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 698
basis of evidence that prefeeding hungry animals

with saccharin does in fact reduce their

subsequent consumption of food, and on the


assumption that sex may not involve a unitary

drive that can be reduced only by ejaculation. In

replying to criticisms of drive-reduction theory on

the grounds that people often engage in behavior

that produces heightened stimulation, Brown


(1955) pointed out that drive cannot be defined
solely in terms of intensity of stimulation. The

reason for this is that strong stimuli can lose their

activating function if presented in gradually

increasing values, if they have been associated


with rewarding experiences, or if they become

discriminative for less active responding.


Therefore, Brown cogently argues that definition

of a drive stimulus must include, in addition to its

intensity, other criteria such as the amount and


type of previous experience with the stimulus, and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 699
the manner and context in which it is presented.

It should be noted in passing that, although


results from experiments involving fistula feeding

and nutritive injections demonstrate that

reduction of a physiological need can be sufficient


to reinforce an instrumental response, such

studies do not necessarily establish the

physiological bases of reinforcement. Any such


physiological explanation may ultimately be
carried to the point where reinforcing effects are

interpreted in terms of intracellular changes.

While relationships established at the molecular

level have considerable theoretical significance


regarding the basic mechanisms of reinforcement,
knowledge of this type is of limited usefulness in

devising incentive programs, since it is extremely

unlikely that in social practice one would alter

neurophysiological events directly in order to

influence responsiveness.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 700
SENSORY-STIMULATION HYPOTHESIS

Although some reinforcement effects may be


governed by visceral drive states, there are many

reinforcing conditions that do not appear to

involve reduction of physiological needs or

removal of aversive stimuli, unless one were to


invoke a host of sensory and activity drives.

Animals will learn to perform responses that


produce visual and auditory stimulation or
opportunities to engage in manipulative and

exploratory activities (Barnes & Baron, 1961;


Butler, 1958a; Kish, 1966; Miles, 1958). A number

of studies, conducted principally with infants

(Rheingold, Stanley, & Doyle, 1964) and children

(Odom, 1964; Stevenson & Odom, 1964), have

likewise shown that visual and auditory feedback


can be effective in modifying and sustaining

behavior over time. These findings would seem to


indicate that much human behavior—particularly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 701
approach, attending, and manipulative responses

—is reinforced by the sensory feedback that is

automatically produced.

Investigations of factors that might contribute

to the reinforcing properties of auditory and visual

events have revealed that novel and complex

stimuli function as more effective reinforcers than

simple and familiar stimulus events. The data


furthermore indicate that, as is the case with
biologically related incentives, the potency of

sensory reinforcers is increased by deprivation of

sensory experiences and diminished by stimulus-


satiation operations.

The existence of sensory reinforcement has

been convincingly demonstrated, but the nature of

the mechanisms underlying the phenomenon is by

no means clear. Berlyne (1960) and Harlow

(1953) have posited curiosity, manipulative, and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 702
exploratory drives that are presumed to be
elicited by external, novel stimuli and reduced by

continuous exposure to such stimulation. In the


prototypic experiment, animals placed in a

lightproof and sound-attenuated box learn to

perform discriminative responses that open a

door allowing them either to view the outside


environment for a few minutes, or to press levers

for auditory stimulation. The major difficulty in

accounting for the animals’ behavior in terms of an

externally aroused curiosity drive is that the

animals are not exposed to the novel stimuli until


after the responses, of which the stimuli are the

presumed cause, have been successfully executed.


As Brown (1953) points out, “If visual exploration

provided the only significant motive, then the


monkeys must have been unmotivated until the

window was opened following a correct response.


But the monkeys did appear to be motivated. One

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 703
might conclude, therefore, that the effective

motivation was aroused before the monkeys were

allowed to see, not as a consequence of seeing [p.


54].” The more distally occurring visual and

auditory experiences can serve as reinforcing


events, but it is evident that the instrumental
responses must be activated by antecedent

stimuli.

Both Miller (Myers & Miller, 1954) and


Mowrer (1960) have reinterpreted sensory

reinforcement effects in terms of reduction of an

aversive drive. They assume that monotony

produces boredom, which has aversive properties,


and that close confinement and drastic reduction
of sensory contact with one’s environment can

generate considerable apprehension. Miller and

Mowrer therefore contend that, if changes in

sensory stimulation are boredom-relieving or

anxiety-reducing, then behavior is reinforced by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 704
its sensory feedback in a manner consistent with

drive-reduction theory. One would encounter no

problems in testing the efficacy of sensory


reinforcers under conditions that do not arouse

anxiety, but it is exceedingly difficult to maintain a

low or unvaried level of sensory input, which in

large part determines the incentive value of

specific auditory and visual stimuli, without


producing concomitant boredom. The problem of
determining whether sensory reinforcement

effects are attributable to elimination of boredom

or to inherent rewarding properties of novel

stimuli is further complicated by the fact that most


exploratory activities undergo rapid satiation.

Some investigators (Fox, 1962; Isaac, 1962;


Leuba, 1955) have offered a neurophysiological

explanation of sensory reinforcement that is

similar in some respects to the operation of

homeostatic drives. Based on evidence that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 705
prolonged deprivation of sensory input results in

psychological dysfunctioning, it is assumed that

for normal physiological functioning the organism


must maintain an optimal level of sensory

stimulation. Therefore, subjects will perform

instrumental responses to increase sensory input

if there is a deficit, and conversely, they will work

to reduce sensory stimulation if it exceeds the


optimal level.

It is difficult to explain, on the basis of a drive

for optimal quanta of sensory stimulation, why

animals confined in a drab, light-proof, sound-


attenuated box will work unflaggingly for certain
sights and sounds but will refrain from performing

responses that are instrumental in producing


negatively valenced stimuli in the same sensory

modalities. Thus, monkeys perform responses for

the opportunity to view other monkeys, movies,

electric trains, and to hear sounds of a monkey

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 706
colony in the outside environment, whereas the

sight or sound of a dog, and distress vocalizations,

readily suppress their exploratory tendencies


(Butler, 1954; 1958b). The findings clearly

indicate that the content rather than the amount of

sensory input may be the critical factor

determining the incidence of exploratory

responses. Studies in which both the amount of


sensory stimulation and the conditioned valence
of auditory or visual stimuli are systematically

manipulated would provide the basis for

determining whether sensory reinforcement

effects are best interpreted in terms of principles


of optimal stimulation, secondary reinforcement,

or their interactive effect.

The existence of sensory, manipulative, and

exploratory drives is usually inferred from

response patterns rather than defined in terms of

antecedent conditions for producing the specific

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 707
drive. Unless drives and responses are

operationally differentiated, there are no limits to

the proliferation of drive states which can be more


economically explained in terms of response

dispositions. If independent criteria are not

employed, new drives or motives may be invoked

for each reinforcing event or prevalent behavior.

PREPOTENT-RESPONSE HYPOTHESIS

In describing the essential properties of

reinforcing events, emphasis has usually been

placed on the nature of the reinforcing stimuli


(e.g., food, money, novel sensations, social
attention and approval, intracranial stimulation,

etc.), and their efficacy under varying conditions of

deprivation. On the basis of results from an

ingenious series of experiments, Premack (1965)

has presented an explanation of reinforcement

that emphasizes the reinforcing response rather

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 708
than the reinforcing stimulus. In these
investigations the reinforcement values of

different activities are estimated from the


duration for which subjects spontaneously engage

in particular behaviors when no time or response

restrictions exist. If the opportunity to engage in

the more rewarding activity is made conditional


upon the prior performance of low probability

behaviors, then the latter responses increase in

frequency. Based on these findings, Premack has

proposed the following principle of reinforcement:

For any pair of activities, the more probable one


will reinforce the less probable one.

Data observed by Premack indicate that, under


appropriate conditions, almost any activity can

function as an effective reinforcer. Thus, animals


that prefer running to eating will perform

consummatory responses in order to release an


activity wheel that permits them to sprint,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 709
whereas subjects that would rather eat than run

will engage in running behavior in order to gain

access to food. Moreover, by manipulating


deprivational conditions, the reinforcement

relation between activities can be easily reversed,

with the result that reinforcing events are

converted into reinforceable events. To continue

with the previous example, eating will reinforce


running behavior in food-deprived animals, but
after they have been fed and their mobility

restricted, eating can serve as the instrumental

activity that is reinforced by opportunities to run.

Reversibility is apparently a general phenomenon


extending even to intracranial self-stimulation,

which can serve as an extremely powerful


reinforcer of instrumental responses. When the

probability of drinking, for example, is greater

than brain stimulation, drinking reinforces


intracranial self-stimulation (ICS), and conversely,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 710
in animals for which ICS is more rewarding than

drinking, making ICS conditional upon drinking

produces an increase in drinking behavior


(Holstein & Hundt, 1965). If further research

should demonstrate that electrical stimulation of


the limbic system, which is assumed to be
inherently reinforcing, is itself reinforce- able by

contingent response events, then one might

question the existence of an integrated brain


center that governs all reinforcement. As Premack

points out, one would have to locate another

region of central reinforcement in cases where


intracranial stimulation is increased by its

instrumental value in producing more highly


preferred response events. It is also unclear how

explanations in terms of central reinforcement

focuses can readily account for the reversal of


instrumental and rewarding functions of any given

activity. Indeed, the experimental evidence

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 711
convincingly demonstrates that reinforcement is a

relational rather than an absolute property of the

activity. A particular response event will have no


reinforcing potency in relation to a more highly

preferred activity, but it will function as an


effective positive reinforcer when paired with
responses of lesser value.

When reinforcing events are defined in terms


of their effects—as stimuli that increase the
probability of preceding responses—the empirical

principle of reinforcement is open to the criticism

of circularity. In rebuttal, Meehl (1950) contended

that this criticism ignores the fact that reinforcers


are transsituational, that is, a stimulus that has
been found to be effective in reinforcing one

response can be used predictively to reinforce

other types of responses. The findings discussed

above, however, indicate that the assumption of

transsituationality is valid only under certain

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 712
limiting conditions because most stimuli do not

possess generalized reinforcing potency. Premack

solves the circularity problem by defining the


reinforcing power of different consequences

independently of response changes.

It appears highly doubtful that the reinforcing

effects of prepotent responses are interpretable as

drive-reduction processes or sensory drive


mechanisms. There has been virtually no
speculation or research concerning the specific

aspects of prepotency that contribute to its

reinforcing potential, and, therefore, the


associated processes remain obscure. Although
the prepotent-response principle can encompass a

wide range of conditions which function as


effective reinforcers, it cannot account for the

efficacy of consequences that do not involve

performance of responses. Thus in cases where

behavior is strengthened either by nutrients

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 713
delivered directly into the stomach or blood

stream, or by more conventional consequences

such as praise, positive attention, monetary


rewards, or various types of sensory feedback, it

appears difficult to apply the prepotent- response

principle. Even when pairs of responses are

arranged in a contingent relationship, correct

specification of the reinforcing event is


complicated by the fact that changes in sensory
stimulation accompanying the behavior rather

than the activity per se may be primarily

responsible for the reinforcement effects. The

relative contribution of sensory consequences of


behavior to the total reinforcing effect can be

assessed by vicarious paradigms in which the


responses of a yoked observer are maintained by

witnessing the changes in visual and auditory

stimulation produced by a performer’s actions.

It is evident from this brief review that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 714
exceedingly diverse events, which have no

apparent common properties, can all serve a

reinforcing function. A theory of reinforcement


that adequately integrates these heterogeneous

consequences into a unified system has yet to be

formulated. Considering that the reinforcing

potency of a given event is relationally

determined, a comprehensive theory of


reinforcement cannot be based on properties
inherent in the event itself. The experimental

findings nevertheless indicate that better use can

be made of a wider range of reinforcers than is

generally employed in programs of behavioral


change.

INCENTIVE FUNCTION OF REINFORCERS

The discussion thus far has been primarily

concerned with the performance-enhancing

effects of various contingent events, whether they

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 715
be drive-reducing, sensory, or in the form of
prepotent activities. Two different explanations

have been proposed as to how reinforcing


consequences affect behavior. Some reinforcement

theories assume that positive response outcomes

have a direct strengthening effect on stimulus-

response associations and that therefore learning


occurs only as a consequence of reinforcement.

Contiguity theory, on the other hand, distinguishes

between acquisition and performance. Learning,

according to this view, can occur through

contiguous association of stimulus events and


accompanying cognitive processes in the absence

of immediate rewards and punishments. To test


for contiguity learning a variety of experimental

paradigms have been employed in each of which


either overt responding or reinforcement, both

necessary for associative strengthening, are


eliminated. The overall results of these

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 716
investigations provide substantial support for the

contiguity principle. In sensory preconditioning

studies, for example, if one of two neutral stimuli


that have been repeatedly paired is then

conditioned to a response, the second stimulus


also becomes capable of evoking the response
without any direct reinforced association (Seidel,

1959). Many experiments, utilizing surgical and

curare procedures to prevent motor responding


during acquisition or extinction, have consistently

obtained learning in the absence of overt

responding. Similarly, innumerable modeling


studies have shown that new response patterns

can be acquired observationally without observers


themselves either engaging in any overt activity or

receiving any reinforcing stimulation.

Although response acquisition is largely

dependent upon stimulus contiguity,

reinforcement variables are considered to be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 717
highly influential in regulating performance.

However, in this more cognitive interpretation of

behavioral change processes, reinforcers are


assumed to affect performance primarily through

their informative and incentive functions.

Reinforcing consequences convey information

about the type of behavior required in a given

situation. Anticipation of desired rewards for


performing the requisite behaviors can increase
and maintain appropriate responsiveness even

though presentation of the earned reinforcers may

be delayed for a considerable time. Indeed, in most

instances persons are motivated by, and work for,


anticipated rewards rather than immediate

reinforcing outcomes.

Contiguous occurrence of stimulus events is no

assurance that they will necessarily be observed.

Anticipated rewards can influence to some degree

what people will pay attention to. Thus by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 718
arousing, focusing, and sustaining attentiveness to

relevant stimulus events, which is necessary for

learning, reinforcers may serve as indirect


determinants of response acquisition. The major

controversy between learning theories is

therefore concerned with the manner in which

reinforcement affects learning rather than with

whether reinforcement plays a role in the


acquisition process.

The basic assumption that reinforcement is a

prerequisite for learning is difficult to refute

empirically. Demonstrations of learning through


contiguity alone are often discounted by invoking
obscure or undetected sources of reinforcement

that are presumed to be operative in the situation.


A purely cognitive interpretation of reinforcement

effects is, however, challenged by results of

experiments with infrahuman subjects in which

reinforcing nutrients are introduced either

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 719
directly into the stomach or intravenously. In

these instances the reinforcers are not observable

and consequently, their response-enhancing


effects cannot be attributed to informative or

incentive factors. The overall evidence would

seem to indicate that reinforcers can have both

associative strengthening and performance-

enhancing effects.

Essential Components of Reinforcement


Practices

There are three essential features in the

successful application of reinforcement

procedures. First, one must select reinforcers that


are sufficiently powerful and durable to maintain
responsiveness over long periods while complex

patterns of behavior are being established and


strengthened. Second, the reinforcing events must

be made contingent upon the desired behavior if

they are to be optimally effective. And third, a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 720
reliable procedure for eliciting or inducing the

desired response patterns is essential; otherwise,

if they rarely or never occur there will be few


opportunities to influence them through

contingent reinforcement.

INCENTIVE SYSTEM

It is generally acknowledged that motivation is

crucial for behavioral change. In most personality


theories motivation is conceptualized as enduring

energy systems within the organism, variously

labeled as needs, drives, or motives, which impel


and sustain responsiveness. When motivation is
treated as though it were a persisting internal

entity, this type of orientation not only impedes

development of efficacious change programs, but

it creates pessimism about the possibility of

treating persons who presumably lack the

requisite motivation. It also provides a convenient

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 721
rationale for failures that primarily result from
reliance upon weak methods of behavioral control.

Incentive theories of motivation assume that


behavior is largely activated by anticipation of

reinforcing consequences. From this point of view,

motivation can be regulated through arrangement


of incentive conditions and by means of satiation,
deprivation, and conditioning operations that

affect the relative efficacy of various reinforcers at


any given time. Thus, for example, in producing

intellectual strivings in children who display little

interest in academic pursuits, one would arrange

favorable conditions of reinforcement with

respect to achievement behavior rather than


attempt to create in some ill-defined way an

achievement motive, the presence of which is


typically inferred from the behaviors it

presumably actuates.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 722
Given that performance is extensively

determined by reinforcement conditions, the

development and selection of an effective


incentive system is of central importance. The

influential role of reinforcement variables in


behavioral change is illustrated by results of
experiments comparing responsiveness with and

without contingent reinforcement. As part of a

program of research on reading, for example,


Staats and his colleagues (Staats, Staats, Schutz, &

Wolf, 1962) presented to preschool children

programmed material designed to teach them to


read words individually and then combined into

short sentences. When the children were praised


for correct responses but were offered no extrinsic

rewards, they worked at the reading tasks for 15

to 20 minutes and then became bored and restless


and asked to leave. After they no longer wished to

remain in the situation, tangible rewards,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 723
consisting of candy treats, trinkets, and tokens

that could be exchanged for attractive toys, were


introduced. Under the influence of the positive

reinforcers, made conditional upon reading

achievements, the children’s limited “attention


span” suddenly expanded, and they not only

worked enthusiastically at the reading task for 45


minutes, but participated actively in additional

sessions.

A second group of four-year-olds originally

performed the reading task under reinforcement

conditions for two sessions, then the rewards

were discontinued until the children ceased to


participate, following which extrinsic incentives
were again reinstated. During the initial reinforced

sessions the children attended closely to the

reading material and worked actively at acquiring

new reading responses. When reinforcers were

withdrawn, however, the children’s attention,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 724
participation, and reading achievements rapidly

deteriorated. Staats (1965) has further

demonstrated that, given an appropriate incentive


system, even very young children will engage in

complex learning activities with sustained interest

over an extended series of sessions.

The marked changes in positive

responsiveness noted in the above studies


illustrate how low persistence on academic tasks
resulting from inadequate incentives is often

erroneously attributed to basic deficits in the child

in the form of short attention span or low


frustration threshold. Levin & Simmons (1962)
similarly found that low persistence in hyper-

aggressive boys, which is generally interpreted in


clinical theory (Redl & Wineman, 1951) as

reflecting high impulsivity, weak ego control, and

a generalized inability to tolerate frustration, may

in fact be due to inadequate positive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 725
reinforcement. When boys were merely praised

for appropriate responses, they rapidly ceased

responding, sometimes in a highly disruptive


fashion, by tossing the material out of the window

or by climbing on filing cabinets. On the other

hand, when food was used as a reinforcer, the boys

continued to work at the task even though

reinforcement was progressively reduced and


eventually discontinued altogether. The
supposedly short attention span of brain-damaged

and retarded children has also been markedly

increased by creating favorable incentive

conditions (Martin & Powers, 1967). The


foregoing studies, and results obtained by other

investigators (Slack, 1960; Whitlock & Bushell,


1967; Wolf, Giles, & Hall, 1968) indicate that

extrinsic incentives are often essential,

particularly during early phases of behavioral


change programs.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 726
The incentive question poses greatest

problems in the treatment of people who present

severe and profound retardation in social


development. As previously noted, such

individuals are generally unresponsive to verbal


stimuli, customary social reinforcers are
ineffective in modifying their behavior, and the

selected activities often lack acquired reward

value for them. In such cases, change agents are


forced to rely initially upon primary reinforcers,

usually in the form of food. In order to enhance

treatment effects, conditioning sessions are


typically conducted prior to, or during, mealtimes

when food rewards are most effective in


sustaining a high level of responding (Lovaas,

Berberich, Perloff, & Schaeffer, 1966).

Although food rewards may be effectively

employed for short periods, they cannot be relied

upon exclusively in change programs. Food

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 727
preferences often vary considerably among

individuals and even within the same person from

time to time. More important, however, the


incentive value of food is highly dependent upon

the level of food deprivation at any given moment;

consequently, food rapidly loses its reinforcing

power through satiation. Since change programs

require frequent, and sometimes lengthy, sessions,


it is necessary to make use of reinforcing events
possessing more enduring incentive value.

For individuals who present gross deficiencies

in conditioned reinforcers, and who are therefore


responsive only to primitive physical
consequences, an important initial objective of

treatment is to endow social and symbolic stimuli


with reinforcing properties. The development of

social reinforcers is particularly critical, since

human behavior is frequently strengthened,

sustained, and modified by praise, approval,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 728
encouragement, positive attention, and affection.

Ordinarily a neutral stimulus acquires

reinforcing properties through repeated


association with primary reinforcement (Kelleher

& Gollub, 1962; Zimmerman, 1957). In their work

with autistic children, Lovaas and his associates

(Lovaas, Freitag, Kinder, Rubenstein, Schaeffer, &

Simmons, 1966) found that negatively reinforcing


properties could readily be conditioned to the
verbal stimulus “no” through association with

aversive stimulation. On the other hand, numerous

sessions in which the word “good” was


contiguously paired with food failed to endow the
social stimulus with any reward value. The

contrasting results were attributed to differential


attentiveness on the part of the children. They

were highly attentive to external cues during

negative stimulation, whereas in sessions

employing rewards they engaged in considerable

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 729
self-stimulatory behavior and appeared oblivious

to the relevant social stimuli. It was therefore

decided to employ an instrumental conditioning


paradigm in which the children received food

rewards only if they approached the therapist

whenever he said the word “good.” The children

were thus required to attend closely to the

appropriate verbal cue and to discriminate it from


other stimuli occurring at the same time. After the
social stimulus had been established as

discriminative for primary reinforcement, the

children’s approach responses were intermittently

rewarded on a gradually increasing ratio in order


to further enhance the rewarding capacity of the

verbal cue. This procedure proved highly effective.


In later phases of the experiment, new responses

could be established and maintained in autistic

children through contingent presentation of


verbal approval alone. Moreover, the social

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 730
stimulus retained its reinforcing potency over an

extended period on the basis of periodic

association with food rewards.

With less severely autistic children social

reinforcers were established more readily. In


these cases verbal approval and affection in the

form of demonstrative pats and hugs sustained the

children’s positive responsiveness during


numerous sessions devoted to language learning
and the acquisition of social skills. Occasionally,

however, food rewards accompanied the social

reinforcers as a means of preserving their efficacy.

Many of the change programs discussed later rely


heavily upon interpersonal reinforcers in which
desired behavior is responded to with attention,

interest, and approval while undesired activities

are either consistently ignored or socially

disapproved.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 731
A stimulus that has been associated on

numerous occasions with many types of primary

as well as secondary reinforcements acquires the


capacity to function as a generalized reinforcer. In

the treatment of young children or adults for


whom positive social and verbal stimuli have weak
incentive value, tangible generalized reinforcers

are frequently employed. Appropriate

performances are rewarded with monetary


credits, tokens, or points that can later be used to

obtain a variety of rewarding objects and special

privileges. A token incentive system has several


advantages over other forms of material rewards:

The reinforcing value of tokens is relatively


independent of momentary deprivational states;

tokens are not subject to satiation effects and

therefore retain their incentive properties over


long periods; they can be easily presented, if

necessary, immediately upon appropriate

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 732
performance; and finally, since individuals can

exchange their token savings for a variety of


attractive items of their own choosing, motivation

and responsiveness are likely to remain at a

consistently high level.

In recent years extensive use has been made of

Premack’s (1965) differential probability principle

in selecting reinforcing events. Because certain


preferred activities can reinforce activities of
lesser value, countless events may be effectively

employed to initiate and maintain desired

behavior. In practical applications of this principle

(Homme, 1966), a person essentially agrees to


perform a certain amount of low probability
behavior to engage in a more preferred activity for

a specified time. Apart from its flexibility and

simplicity, this type of reinforcement system

permits one to utilize naturally occurring activities

as reinforcers simply by arranging them in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 733
appropriate temporal contingencies. Rewarding

activities are frequently used in combination with

generalized reinforcers. In such applications


individuals earn tokens for performing desired

behaviors which can later be used to obtain,

among other things, access to preferred activities.

The preceding discussion has emphasized the

influential role of extrinsic reinforcement with


persons who, for one reason or another, display
intrinsic motivation insufficient to develop the

behavioral repertoires necessary for coping

effectively with customary environmental


demands. It should be emphasized at this point,
however, that in a thoughtfully planned treatment

program, as newly established patterns of


behavior acquire secondary reinforcing properties

extrinsic incentives should be gradually

withdrawn and replaced with more symbolic and

self-monitored reinforcement systems. This issue,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 734
which bears on questions of the durability of

induced changes and the humanistic implications

of different forms of behavioral influence, will be


discussed at length in a subsequent section of this

chapter.

ARRANGEMENT OF CONTINGENCIES

After appropriate reinforcers that have

sufficient incentive value to maintain stable


responsiveness have been chosen, the

contingencies between specific performances and

reinforcing stimuli must be arranged. Parents,


teachers, and psychotherapists intuitively employ
rewards in their attempts to influence and modify

behavior, but their efforts often produce limited

results because the methods are used improperly,

inconsistently, or inefficiently. In many instances

considerable rewards are bestowed, but they are

not made conditional upon the behavior that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 735
change agents wish to promote; long delays often
intervene between the occurrence of the desired

behavior and its intended consequences; special


privileges, activities, and rewards are generally

furnished according to fixed time schedules rather

than performance requirements; and, in many

cases, positive reinforcers are inadvertently made


contingent upon the wrong types of behavior.

Most residential treatment programs, for


example, are conducted on a contingent-

punishment, noncontingent-reward basis. That is,

the participants obtain the maximum rewarding

benefits with few strings attached, but these

rewards and privileges are promptly withdrawn


whenever the residents are uncooperative,

defiant, or disruptive. In one residential treatment


center for delinquent boys that the writer had

occasion to visit, children are given 20 points upon


their arrival, which initially ensures them access

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 736
to all of the rewarding resources that the

institution has to offer. However, the boys are

penalized by loss of points and accompanying


privileges for deviant behavior and infractions of

house rules. Within an institutional setting in

which noncontingent rewards are provided at a

high level, the staff members are cast in the

unenviable role of punitive agents, and the boys


can move only in a downward direction. Thus, the
threat of punishment is ever present, but the

positive incentives for behavioral change, though

abundantly available, are poorly managed. Under

these circumstances, the majority of the


participants comply halfheartedly with the

minimum demands of the institution in order to


avoid penalties for any breach of the rules.

Similarly, in most psychiatric facilities, patients

can best maximize their rewards by merely


adopting a passive patient role.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 737
The necessity for arranging appropriate

reinforcement contingencies is dramatically

illustrated by studies in which rewards are shifted


from a response-contingent to a time-contingent

basis (Lovaas, Berberich, Perloff, & Schaeffer,


1966; Baer, Peterson, & Sherman, 1967). During
sessions in which rewards are made conditional

upon occurrence of the desired behavior, the

appropriate response patterns are exhibited at a


consistently high level; by contrast, under

conditions where the same rewards are given but

after a certain time has elapsed, independent of


the client’s behavior, there is a marked drop in the

desired behavior. Reinstatement of response-


contingent reinforcement promptly restores the

high level of responsiveness. These behavioral

changes are particularly striking considering that


interpersonal relationship factors and the amount

of reward remain the same in all phases of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 738
treatment except for the arrangement of the

contingencies. Essentially similar reductions in

responsiveness are obtained when individuals are

provided rewards in advance without any


performance requirements (Ayllon & Azrin, 1965;
Bandura & Perloff, 1967).

In an effective program of change

reinforcement contingencies should be arranged


to provide positive guidance and support for new
modes of behavior, rather than to extract minimal

compliance with situational demands. Social

change programs would become considerably

more efficacious, especially in modifying


pervasively aberrant disorders, if initially the
environment were devised to provide

noncontingent rewards at an adequate but

relatively low level, and preferred reinforcers

were readily available contingent upon the

occurrence of desired response patterns. Under

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 739
these conditions, a rehabilitative program can be

managed primarily on a positive reinforcement

basis without resort to those punitive measures


that are commonly employed in residential

treatments.

Behavior is influenced not only by the

contingencies operative in the situation, but also

by temporal aspects of reinforcement. Findings


from experimental studies (Renner, 1964)
demonstrate that behavioral changes proceed

most effectively when reinforcement is made

immediately contingent upon the behavior one


wishes to foster; generally, the degree of control
exercised by reinforcement decreases with

increasing delay. Whenever a delay occurs


between a particular response and its intended

consequences, other behaviors appear during the

intervening period and that response occurring

most closely to the delayed outcome is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 740
immediately reinforced. Since one typically has

little control over the responses that may arise

during a specific temporal interval, delayed


reinforcement may actually strengthen forms of

behavior that change agents have no intention

whatsoever of promoting.

It is widely assumed, on the basis of results

from laboratory studies of delayed reinforcement,


that the effects of rewarding consequences will be
diminished, or even obviated, unless they are

made instantly contingent upon desired

performances. This conclusion needs qualification


because it is based on evidence from
experimentation either with infrahuman subjects

or under conditions where the basis for


reinforcement is not explained. When the

contingencies imposed upon an organism are not

clearly specified in advance, interposing a delay

between the occurrence of a response and its

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 741
consequences increases the difficulty of

identifying the arbitrary relationship, particularly

if a series of responses is performed during the


intervening period. As a result, inappropriate

responses tend to be adventitiously reinforced.

Although relevant experimental evidence is

lacking, there is every reason to expect from

informal observation that, in the case of humans,


symbolic activities can effectively mediate a
delayed reinforcement contingency without any

appreciable loss of behavioral control. Therefore,

if contingencies are explicitly defined for an

individual he is able to link eventual consequences


with particular performances. Verbal mediation

will, in all probability, eliminate irrelevant


responses even though a considerable time may

elapse between performance of the requisite

behavior and its consequences. A person who is


paid on a piecework basis, for example, is likely to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 742
maintain a high performance level, although he

receives his total payment at the end of the month

rather than in small amounts immediately after


each unit of work has been completed.

With young children, grossly deviant adults


whose behavior is under weak stimulus control,

and individuals whose efforts extinguish rapidly

under delayed reinforcement contingencies, it may


be necessary initially to employ immediate
concrete rewards; otherwise, such persons are

likely to display rapid decrements in

responsiveness if reinforcing consequences are

postponed. On the other hand, persons who are


responsive to instructional control are usually able
to function adequately under delayed

reinforcement provided the contingencies are

explicitly defined and the incentives are

sufficiently attractive. Moreover, immediate

satisfactions derived from the activity itself and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 743
signs of progress often supplement, and may

eventually replace, ultimate extrinsic

reinforcements in maintaining behavior.

RESPONSE INDUCTION AND EVOCATION

Selection of powerful incentives and skillful

contingency management will, in itself, be of little


consequence unless methods are available for

producing the responses to be reinforced. If the


behavior that a change agent wishes to strengthen

is already present and occurs with some

frequency, then contingent application of


incentives can, from the outset, increase and
maintain the desired response patterns at a high

level. Most cases referred for treatment, however,

present behavioral deficits, and therefore complex

modes of behavior must be organized in

incremental steps, each of which can be easily

acquired. When the initial level of the desired

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 744
behavior is extremely low, if the criterion for
reinforcement is initially set too high, most, if not

all, of the person’s responses go unrewarded, so


that his efforts are gradually extinguished and his

motivation diminished. Consequently, in the

beginning stages a low criterion for reinforcement

is generally adopted so that responses that are


within the individual’s capabilities, but may have

only slight resemblance to the desired behavior,

are reinforced. After gross approximations to the

complex pattern of behavior become more

frequent, reinforcement is made contingent upon a


closer response variant. The criterion for

reinforcement is thus raised in small successive


steps in the direction of more complicated forms

of behavior until eventually only the desired


behavior is reinforced.

The effective utilization of successive


approximation procedures is illustrated in a study

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 745
by King, Armitage, & Tilton (1960) designed to

increase interpersonal responsiveness in severely

withdrawn schizophrenics. Working on the


assumption that motor responses could be more

easily elicited from these patients than verbal or

social behavior, the therapists first set them the

task of performing a simple motor response which

brought social and material rewards. In successive


phases, the complexity of the task was increased,
and verbal and interpersonal responses were

elicited and rewarded. Also, in later phases,

rewards were presented only when the patients

communicated verbally and cooperated with the


therapist and other patients in order to solve

problems of some complexity. Three other groups


of patients, matched with the reinforcement group

for severity of disorder and length of

hospitalization, concurrently participated in either


traditional interview therapy, recreational

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 746
therapy, or received no treatment. The

reinforcement approach proved more efficacious

than all other three techniques in producing


favorable changes in social behavior assessed in

terms of ward observation and standardized


interviews. After 15 weeks of therapy, patients
treated by the reinforcement method displayed

more verbal behavior, less resistance to therapy,

more interest in occupational activities, and were


better prepared for transfer to a more advanced

ward, than patients in the other three groups.

Bensberg and his colleagues (Bensberg, 1965;


Bensberg, Colwell, & Cassel, 1965) provide

additional illustrations of how substantial


behavioral changes can be achieved even with

profoundly retarded children by rewarding small

increments in performance until the more


complex skills are established.

It is widely assumed among proponents of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 747
operant conditioning that the above procedure,

which is variously labeled successive

approximation, shaping, or response differentiation,


is ideally suited for developing new, organized

modes of response previously absent from the

behavioral repertoire of the organism.

Consequently, many therapists spend countless

hours patiently shaping behavior bit by bit when


much of this tedious process can be drastically
reduced. As demonstrated in the preceding

chapter, complex patterns of behavior can be

developed in humans most rapidly through

graduated modeling combined with positive


reinforcement for matching responses. Operant

conditioning through successive approximation


may, however, be exclusively employed with

considerable success to reinstate previously

acquired responses that have been extinguished


and to strengthen performances weakly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 748
established as a result of inadequate incentive

conditions. Thus, by selecting powerful reinforcers

and arranging the requisite contingencies, a


therapist can induce a mute catatonic who

possesses a language repertoire to resume verbal


communication (Isaac, Thomas, & Goldiamond,
1960); schizophrenics who have developed

adequate work repertoires can be led to

participate again in vocational activities (Ayllon &


Azrin, 1965); delinquents who refuse to attend to

school assignments can be motivated to improve

their academic performances (Cohen, 1968); and,


in verbal conditioning experiments, college

students who command an abundant supply of


personal pronouns can be subtly prompted to emit

these verbal responses at a relatively high rate

(Krasner, 1958).

In addition to utilizing the method of

successive approximation and behavioral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 749
modeling for producing complex responses, one

can rely upon verbal prompts that specifically

instruct individuals how and when to perform the


reinforceable behavior (Baer & Wolf, 1967).

However, in cases that are unresponsive to social

forms of response guidance it may be necessary to

employ nonsocial stimuli that exercise strong

control over the behavior in question, even though


the eventual aim is to have the behavior occur in
response to quite different stimulus conditions. In

using nonsocial cueing procedures one initially

introduces discriminative stimuli that exert strong

control over the desired behavior. After the


responses have been evoked and firmly

established, the arbitrary stimulus supports are


“faded” or gradually withdrawn as control is

transferred to stimuli likely to function as the

major elicitors under naturalistic conditions. Thus,


for example, in augmenting attending behavior in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 750
severely retarded children who were totally

unresponsive, Bensberg (1965) initially flashed

lights on a wall while he simultaneously instructed


them to look, and rewarded them for gazing at the

light. In this way attending responses, which are


prerequisite for social learning, were increased
and eventually brought under verbal stimulus

control. The use of tasks graduated in difficulty

also includes instances in which stimulus


conditions are arranged so that rewardable

behavior can be readily elicited at each successive

step.

A final method for evoking desired behavior,


which is sometimes employed with persons who
prove unresponsive to extensive stimulus

prompts, involves physical response guidance,

wherein individuals are assisted physically in

making the correct responses. In teaching autistic

children grammatical relationships between

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 751
objects (Lovaas, 1966), for example, if a child fails

to execute the response corresponding to the

verbal instruction, “Put the block inside the box,”


the therapist moves the child’s hand with the

block to the box and rewards the passively

performed action. On subsequent trials the

amount of manual guidance is gradually reduced

until the behavior is performed without


assistance.

Ethical Implications of Reinforcement


Practices

The deliberate use of positive reinforcement,

particularly in the form of tangible rewards, often


gives rise to ethical objections and concerns about
harmful effects that may result from such

practices. The attitude most commonly expressed


is that desirable behavior should be intrinsically

satisfying. It is feared that, if persons are

frequently rewarded, they will be disinclined to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 752
behave appropriately unless continually paid to do

so, and when the customary rewards are

discontinued they will cease responding


altogether. It is further assumed that rewarding

practices not only establish weak and unenduring

behavior, but that contingent reinforcement is

likely to interfere with the development of

spontaneity, creativity, intrinsic motivational


systems, and other highly valued self-determining
personality characteristics. Some of the more

intemperate criticism considers the deliberate use

of reinforcement to be deceptive, manipulative,

and an insult to the personal integrity of human


beings.

For reasons presented above most persons


whose own behavior is strongly influenced by

social recognition, praise, approval, special

privileges, and monetary incentives are quick to

disclaim the use of rewarding practices (Bandura

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 753
& Walters, 1959; Sears, Maccoby, & Levin, 1957)

and to deny that their behavior has been

externally regulated (Rogers, 1960). It should also


be noted that, paradoxically, one is apt to

encounter less concern over the use of aversive

methods of control by threat, coercion, and

deprivation of privileges, methods which often do

produce the negative behavioral outcomes


inappropriately attributed to procedures relying
upon positive incentives.

The fact that behavior is strongly influenced by

its consequences is not a phenomenon created by


behavioral scientists, any more than physicists are
responsible for the laws of gravity. The process of

natural selection has favored organisms with


adaptive feedback control systems in which

reinforcing consequences serve as a major

regulator of behavior. Indeed, if behavior did not

change as a function of its outcomes, one’s life

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 754
span would be drastically curtailed. Selection of

the types of incentives by which the behavior of

others is to be established, guided, and maintained


is, of course, an ethical issue. However, the

behavioral effects resulting from applications of

different psychological procedures are entirely an

empirical matter. Available evidence from

laboratory and psychotherapeutic studies


suggests that reinforcement procedures, if
thoughtfully and skillfully implemented, can

produce enduring changes in social behavior and

facilitate the acquisition of self-monitoring

reinforcement systems. If, on the other hand,


operant techniques are crudely applied, and the

incentives are inappropriate to individuals’


developmental levels, then the change program

may be insulting as well as ineffective.

In discussing the systematic utilization of

positive incentives it is important to recognize

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 755
that a change program represents a continuum of

psychological experiences in which the type,

amount, and source of reinforcement regulating


behavior are gradually modified. Hence, the

incentives employed initially to establish new

patterns of social behavior and to develop

symbolic reinforcers may differ considerably from

the stimulus events that ultimately assume


controlling and reinforcing functions.

Critics of reinforcement methods generally

create the impression that change agents work

with mature and intrinsically motivated persons,


but, rather than appealing to higher symbolic
motivations, insist on imposing crass materialistic

incentives upon them. There are undoubtedly


some practitioners who apply incentive

procedures thoughtlessly and ineffectively.

Ordinarily, however, primary rewards are

employed in initial stages with persons who are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 756
not reinforceable with other types of events and

who would otherwise remain inaccessible to

treatment. In the latter cases it would be no more


appropriate to rely upon developmentally

advanced incentives than to teach young children

how to count by commencing with the principles

of advanced mathematics. After reinforcing

functions have been imparted to social and


symbolic stimulus events, then more subtle and
naturally occurring reinforcers are increasingly

employed. Without the initial concrete training,

psychologically incapacitated persons are

relegated to a subhuman existence in custodial


institutions.

REINFORCEMENT SYSTEMS AND DURABILITY OF


BEHAVIORAL CHANGES

Demonstrations that behavior can be

maintained at a satisfactory level through


reinforcements mediated by change agents are of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 757
limited significance unless the response patterns
endure long after the specially created

contingencies have been discontinued. There are


several ways in which reinforcement systems can

be devised and altered during the course of

treatment to ensure that existing behavior does

not readily extinguish.

Change in Frequency or Magnitude of

Reinforcement. After response patterns have been


firmly established through continuous

reinforcement, the schedule is gradually thinned

out, by providing rewards on increasingly variable

ratios and intervals so that the rewarding

consequences occur only periodically. As shown in


the introductory chapter, intermittently reinforced

behavior is extremely resistant to extinction. The


durability of behavior under less favorable

reinforcement conditions can also be increased by


gradually reducing the amount of reward after the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 758
behavior has been sufficiently strengthened, or by

increasing the amount of work per reinforcement

(Staats & Butterfield, 1965).

Change in the Locus of Reinforcement. In most

instances many different interpersonal and

material rewards are potentially available, but

they remain inaccessible to individuals who lack

the social and vocational skills to attain them. In a


similar manner, because of behavioral deficits or
inhibitory tendencies persons may refrain from

participating in activities that would provide rich

sources of enjoyment. If proficiency in the


necessary skills and social responses were
established, they could be adequately supported

by reinforcements regularly available within the


environment. The main purpose of specially

arranged reinforcement is to develop and to

sustain behavioral repertoires to the point where

the individual makes successful contact with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 759
existing sources of positive reinforcement. Once

this is achieved, the arbitrary contingencies can be

completely withdrawn without weakening or


reducing social behavior.

An excellent example of the successful shift of

the locus of reinforcement from adult change

agents to peer group members is provided in the

treatment of an extremely withdrawn girl,


referred to in Chapter 6 (Allen, Hart, Buell, Harris,
& Wolf, 1964). After a brief period in which

teachers’ interest and attention were made

contingent upon interaction with peers, the girl


entered into a great deal of social play with other
children. However, when adult reinforcement for

peer interactions was temporarily removed during


the early phase of treatment, she reverted to her

isolate pattern of behavior; reinstatement of the

therapeutic contingency restored social play to its

previous high level. As the girl derived increasing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 760
enjoyment from play activities with peers, adult

rewards for interaction with children were

progressively diminished to a normal amount of


attention, and the schedule for nonreinforcement

of adult contacts gradually relaxed. Eventually the

treatment program was discontinued altogether

and no special contingencies were arranged

thereafter. The increase in social interaction with


other children nevertheless endured, as revealed
in observations of behavior conducted at various

times following the termination of the program.

Other case studies, specifically designed to

investigate the durability of behavioral change


(Baer & Wolf, 1967), have shown that if adults

maintain their reinforcement support of social


behavior in children until they achieve

reciprocally rewarding interactions with peers,

the children’s behavior comes increasingly under


peer control and is little affected by withdrawal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 761
adult social reinforcement.

Results from the above studies, and others


conducted in the same manner, show that

established patterns of behavior maintain their

strength after specially arranged consequences


are discontinued provided the behavior is brought

under the influence of favorable contingencies

within the individual’s social milieu. In cases,


however, where the reinforcement practices in
naturalistic situations are either deficient or

grossly deviant it is doubtful that lasting

behavioral changes can be achieved, unless the

program is extended to encompass significant


members of the individual’s social environment.

Change in the Form of Reinforcement. In

previous discussions of the treatment of autistic

children it was shown how their behavior could

initially be modified only through the use of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 762
immediate primary reinforcers which were
gradually reduced and eventually discontinued as

social stimuli acquired reinforcing functions.


Another illustration of the transformation of

reinforcing supports for behavior during the

course of treatment is provided by Wahler (1968)

who successfully modified extreme oppositional


behavior in children by altering their parents’

reinforcement practices. An initial program in

which parents ignored their children’s resistance

to requests and rewarded cooperative behavior

with approval proved relatively ineffective. A


subsequent reinforcement system combining

parental approval with tokens exchangeable for


prized toys produced dramatic and enduring

increases in cooperativeness. Thereafter, the


tokens were gradually eliminated and cooperative

behavior was stably maintained by social approval


alone.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 763
In order to acquire proficiency in complex

behaviors people must engage in long hours of

arduous work, give up attractive competing


activities, and delay a host of immediate

gratifications that may be readily available.


Because the learning process involves some
degree of self-denial and other negative aspects,

many people fail to develop minimal competencies

even though threats and coercive pressures are


continuously brought to bear. This prevalent

problem can be rectified more satisfactorily and

humanely by applying an arbitrary reward


contingency until the behavior is developed to the

stage at which it produces natural reinforcing


consequences. Thus, for example, extrinsic

rewards may be employed temporarily to teach

children how to read, but after written subject


matter becomes sufficiently reinforcing in itself to

sustain further development of reading skills, the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 764
artificial contingency may be withdrawn. Many

forms of behavior, such as communicative facility

and manipulatory skills, which permit an

individual to regulate his environment more


effectively, persist with little external support

because they are functional in producing

rewarding outcomes. New performances are also


partially sustained by the sensory feedback that
they naturally produce.

If rewards are recurrently and explicitly

associated with cues that signify competency or

correctness, then symbolic stimulus events that

have informative value, and qualitative differences


in performance, acquire secondary reinforcing
properties. At this higher developmental level,

cues designating the adequacy of one’s

performance may be as effective a reinforcer as

monetary incentives (Lewis, Wall, & Aronfreed,

1963; Miller & Estes, 1961). Once informative

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 765
response feedback becomes a source of personal

satisfaction, maintenance of behavior is less

dependent upon extrinsic social or material


incentives. It should be noted, however, that any

reinforcement ensuing from confirmation of the

correctness of one’s responses is probably

mediated through a self-reinforcement process

rather than automatically generated. It is


exceedingly unlikely, for example, that correctness
feedback on tasks that are personally devalued or

are regarded as elementary will have much, if any,

reinforcing value. On the other hand, confirmation

of attainments that exceed personal standards of


what constitutes a worthy performance will tend

to activate positive self-evaluations.

The highest level of autonomy is achieved

when behavior generates self-evaluative and other

self-reinforcing consequences. In such instances, a

person sets himself explicit standards of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 766
achievement and creates either self-rewarding or

self-punishing consequences depending upon the

quality of his behavior relative to his self-imposed


standards. Self-evaluative reactions can not only

maintain behavior under conditions of minimal

external support, but they may override the

influence of social rewards for behavior that

conflicts with the person’s own norms of


acceptable conduct. The manner in which
modeling and reinforcement procedures can be

used to establish intrinsic performance standards

by which a person monitors his own behavior has

been previously discussed.

Established patterns of behavior are most

likely to endure across a bewildering variety of


reinforcement contingencies if the significant

reinforcing events are either intrinsically related

to the behavior, or self-administered. The

conditions necessary for developing complex

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 767
behavioral repertoires through reinforcement

methods have been explicitly defined and are

usually faithfully implemented. Procedures for


developing reinforcement functions to symbolic

events need to be further refined and applied

systematically in programs of behavioral change.

Judging by evidence from laboratory studies, the

types of self-regulatory mechanisms that


humanistically oriented commentators consider to
be antithetical to behavioral approaches are, in

fact, most readily developed by methods derived

from social-learning theory.

It is also often erroneously assumed that


change programs based upon principles of

reinforcement involve a simple mechanistic


technology that can be applied almost reflexively

by anyone who possesses sufficient perseverance.

On the contrary, successful implementation of

social-learning methods calls for considerable

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 768
ingenuity and sensitive responsiveness to

psychological changes in individuals throughout

the period of treatment. Among other


requirements, one must devise efficacious

incentive systems, select appropriate

reinforcement schedules, arrange essential

contingencies and gradually modify them as

treatment progresses. One must also create


methods for evoking desired responses with
sufficient frequency for them to be strongly

established. And finally, it is necessary to select

and to train appropriate persons to implement the

procedures in naturalistic contexts. Whereas in


conventional treatments people are frequently left

to their own devices in transferring whatever they


may have learned to their everyday life, social-

learning approaches devote considerable attention

to arranging conditions necessary to ensure


optimal generalization and maintenance of newly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 769
established modes of behavior. In view of the

amount of inventiveness required, it is not

surprising that, although principles of


reinforcement have been in existence for many

decades, the derivation of efficacious procedures


has been disappointingly slow. For similar
reasons, the implementation of contingency

systems by amateur or less artful operant

practitioners is often appallingly crude.

SPECIFICATION OF REINFORCEMENT
CONTINGENCIES

In most experimental investigations of

reinforcement processes, instructions that specify

the desired behavior and its programmed

consequences are deliberately minimized or

ignored altogether. Indeed, Skinner (1963) has


strongly cautioned against the use of instructional
control procedures in learning experiments on the

grounds that they circumvent and obscure the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 770
functional analysis of behavior. Laboratory
experimentation designed to explore the extent to

which behavioral phenomena can be brought


under the control of different types of

reinforcement conditions should, of course, avoid

combining variables in ways such that their

individual contributions cannot be distinguished.


However, rigid adherence to isolated procedures

is ill-advised in change programs which must

frequently combine a variety of methods in order

to achieve optimal results. Some devoted partisans

of the operant approach, nevertheless, often rely


exclusively upon reinforcement practices to

develop response patterns that can be readily


produced by the use of simple instructions,

behavioral demonstrations, or appropriate verbal


modeling cues.

Considerable experimental evidence, which


will be reviewed in a later chapter, demonstrates

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 771
that awareness of response-reinforcement

contingencies can markedly accelerate behavioral

change. It is also abundantly evident, as any parent


will attest, that advice, instructions, requests, and

other verbal forms of guidance often have no

enduring effects or go unheeded. Systematic

studies conducted with children (O’Leary, 1968)

and adolescents (Phillips, 1968) indeed show that


prescribing behavioral rules alone is relatively
ineffective in changing their behavior. The power

of verbal influence is largely determined by the

anticipated or accompanying response

consequences. This is revealed in a study by


Ayllon and Azrin (1964) designed to evaluate the

relative efficacy of instructions and reinforcement,


used singly and in combination, for reinstating

acceptable dining behavior in adult

schizophrenics.

The treatment program attempted to get

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 772
patients to pick up cutlery at the serving center,

which they rarely did; instead they ate their food

with their hands. Following the baseline period of


observation during which no special consequences

were arranged, a reinforcement procedure was

introduced in which patients who picked up the

necessary utensils were immediately given,

without any explanation, their choice of extra food


or cigarettes. In a subsequent phase, instructions
were added to the reinforcement procedures, by

having the attendants explain, “Please pick up

your knife, fork, and spoon, and you have a choice

of extra milk, coffee, cigarettes, or candy.”

As shown in Figure 4-1, reinforcement alone

produced no change whatsoever in the patients’


behavior. Here the reinforcement procedure

proved totally ineffective because the vast

majority of patients never exhibited any responses

that could be reinforced, and the few who

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 773
Figure 4-1. Percentage of patients who picked up cutlery
during the baseline period, during the reinforcement
phase in which appropriate responses were promptly
rewarded, and during a period when instructions were
combined with reinforcement. Ayllon & Azrin, 1964.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 774
occasionally picked up cutlery never figured out

the basis upon which they were given extra treats.

By contrast, when instructions were combined


with reinforcing consequences the patients

showed a marked and sudden increase in

appropriate behavior, and a number of them

maintained this change after the contingent

rewards were discontinued.

In order to assess the efficacy of instructions


alone, a second group of patients was simply

asked to pick up the necessary eating utensils at

each mealtime. Instructions were found to be


initially effective for about half of the patients, but
in the absence of any consequences for following

or ignoring the requests, verbal directives


eventually lost most of their controlling power

(Figure 4-2). During the next phase, the

instructions were continued but, in addition,

patients gained immediate access to the food

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 775
Figure 4-2. Percentage of patients who displayed the
appropriate responses during baseline, instruction, and
combined reinforcement-instruction phases of the
study. Ayllon & Azrin, 1964.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 776
counter whenever they picked up the cutlery and

were sent to the end of the serving line if they did

not. Under the combined influence of instructions


and response consequences virtually every patient

regularly exhibited the appropriate dining

behavior. Comparison of the two sets of data

reveals that delayed access to rewards produced

by nonresponsiveness was considerably more


powerful in modifying the patients’ behavior than
extra treats for performance of appropriate

responses.

Applications of Contingency Systems

The extensive growth of programs utilizing


positive incentives in one form or another

precludes a complete review of the countless

clinical, remedial, and developmental applications

of reinforcement principles. Rather, some

representative contributions that illustrate the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 777
procedures and their efficacy in modifying a wide

range of behaviors will be discussed. Since

investigations of reinforcement variables alone


are most often conducted by researchers working

within the Skinnerian conceptual framework, the

degree of success of these methods is rarely

evaluated through the use of control groups to

measure the contribution of uncontrolled


variables, or comparisons between groups
involving different experimental operations.

Instead, the intrasubject replication design is

generally employed for isolating the variables

governing change. In this method of research a


given pattern of behavior is repeatedly induced

and eliminated in the same subject through


successive reversal of treatment conditions

(Sidman, 1960). Intrasubject replication is the

most convincing means of demonstrating the


functional relationship between behavioral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 778
phenomena and their controlling conditions.

However, there are certain limitations and

evaluative problems in the use of this


methodology.

Intrasubject replication is well-suited for


investigating performance control processes but it

cannot be employed in studying learning

phenomena in which certain experiences produce


a more or less irreversible change in the behavior
of an organism. For example, after persons have

acquired communicative speech, reading skills,

and various social and psychomotor competencies,

one cannot erase these response capabilities and


thus restore the original behavioral deficits
through nonreinforcement operations or any

other psychological procedure.

A number of interpretive complications arise

in the use of this methodology even in the study of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 779
performance changes. There are no difficulties in
evaluating findings when large successive changes

in behavior occur rapidly and consistently across


different subjects. In many instances, however, not

only are the accompanying behavioral changes

less dramatic, but some individuals remain

essentially unaffected by repeated exposure to the


same treatment conditions. Replicative failures

are usually attributed to inadequacies in the

reinforcers employed, whereas successes are

assumed to result from the manipulated

reinforcement variables. It is entirely possible,


however, that in successful cases the behavioral

changes are largely due to the influence of


unobserved variables that happen to co-vary with

the reversal of treatment conditions. The


evaluative problem is further complicated by the

fact that, in cases where successful behavioral


control is achieved, no statistical criteria have

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 780
been developed to evaluate whether the

magnitude of change produced by a given

treatment exceeds the variability resulting from


uncontrolled factors operating while the

treatment condition is not in effect. Many readers


have undoubtedly experienced frustration in
attempting to evaluate conclusions drawn by

investigators on the basis of visual inspection of

frequency curves that not only involve


considerable variability during baseline

conditions, but differ widely between subjects and

are somewhat irregular across successive


replications.

The intrasubject replication design also


precludes accurate assessment of the relative

efficacy of different treatment variables. Changes

that are sequentially produced in a given

individual by the application of diverse methods

cannot be directly compared for several reasons.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 781
The degree of influence required to create an

initial change may differ from that needed to effect

additional improvements in the same behavior. In


developing linguistic functions in autistic children,

for example, Lovaas (1966) found that children

were slow to acquire the first few words but

subsequent word learning proceeded at a

relatively rapid rate. A similar increase in the rate


of response acquisition as treatment progressed
was noted by Staats, Minke, Goodwin, & Landeen

(1967).

In many instances the original baseline is not


recoverable; consequently, the effects that
different variables have on behavior must be

compared against dissimilar performance levels.


Even if the original response baseline can be

recovered, it may be much easier to reinstate a

given behavior than to create it initially.

Zeilberger, Sampen, and Sloane (1968) have, in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 782
fact, shown that behavior can be modified more

quickly the second time, a finding that is evident in

most graphs based on experiments in which the


same controlling variables are successively

applied and withdrawn. Not only does prior

performance of responses increase the speed with

which they can be reinstated after being

extinguished, but during the initial experiences


learning sets are acquired which can result in
marked improvement in performance of quite

different responses (Kimble, 1961; Harlow, 1949).

Finally, the reinforcement value of a given


event can be markedly altered through contrast
with previous or contemporary conditions of

reinforcement (Buchwald, 1960; Dunham, 1968).


Therefore, sequential changes associated with

different types of reinforcement operations reflect

relational influences as well as the specific

properties of reinforcement procedures. Hence,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 783
the implicit assumptions that repetitive control

does not alter the modifiability of the behavior in

question, that behavior at different levels is


equally modifiable, and that reinforcement

operations are unaffected by contrast in incentive

conditions are all probably untenable. The relative

potency of different controlling variables can,

therefore, be best assessed through experimental


designs involving matched groups.

Some of the published reports involving

reinforcement techniques are based upon

individual cases in which, for practical or ethical


reasons, successive reversals of contingencies
have not been attempted. Although these types of

studies provide less convincing evidence


regarding the variables responsible for observed

changes, the results nevertheless have important

suggestive value, particularly when changes are

effected in persons who have proved totally

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 784
unresponsive to other methods of change.

MODIFICATION OF DEVIANT SOCIAL BEHAVIOR

Differential reinforcement has been widely

employed for the modification of deviant behavior

in both adults and children. In these treatment

programs rewarding consequences for desired


behavior are typically combined with extinction,

modeling, and in some cases with punishment


procedures. However, the studies reported in the

present section are primarily concerned with the

control of behavior through its positive


consequences.

Ayllon and his associates have conducted an


extensive program of research in the development

of reinforcement procedures for the modification


of gross behavior disorders in adult psychotics. In

the early studies (Ayllon & Michael, 1959) nurses

and hospital attendants were trained to record the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 785
frequency with which patients exhibited specific
patterns of behavior, and to arrange in naturalistic

situations the reinforcement contingencies—

usually in the form of social attention and food


rewards— necessary to bring about desired

changes. By withholding attention for bizarre


forms of behavior while selectively reinforcing
rational response patterns, nurses succeeded in

markedly reducing or completely eliminating

psychotic verbalizations (Ayllon & Haughton,


1964), chronic anorexia (Ayllon, Haughton, &

Osmond, 1964), and a host of other deviant

behaviors of long standing considered indicative

of psychotic pathology (Ayllon, 1963; Ayllon &

Michael, 1959). In later studies (Ayllon & Azrin,


1965, 1968) the scope of operant conditioning
methods was broadened by the use of a wider

range of positive incentives, which were applied

on a group basis for establishing social and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 786
vocational competencies in chronic psychiatric

patients. A detailed account of the latter program

will be presented later.

Numerous projects have been reported in

which reinforcement principles are systematically


employed to alter deviant behavior in children.

These studies provide impressive testimony that

children’s behavior can be powerfully controlled


by the social consequences provided by adults.
Each case involves intrasubject replication in

which the incidence of particular response

patterns is objectively recorded under naturally

occurring contingencies and during subsequent


periods when therapeutic contingencies are
alternately applied and withdrawn. The findings

demonstrate that persistent problem behaviors

can be successfully eliminated, reinstated, and

extinguished a second time by altering the amount

of adult interest and attention produced by the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 787
deviant behavior. Among the disorders

successfully treated through such selective

reinforcement are extreme withdrawal (Allen, et


ah, 1964; Brawley, Harris, Allen, Fleming, &

Peterson, 1969; Johnston, Kelley, Buell, Harris &

Wolf, 1963), regressive crawling (Harris, Johnston,

Kelley, & Wolf, 1964), extreme passivity (Johnston,

Kelley, Harris, & Wolf, 1966), hyperactivity and


aggressive behavior (Allen, Henke, Harris, Baer, &
Reynolds, 1967; Hall, Lund & Jackson 1968), and

depressive feelings and marked overdependency

(Wahler & Pollio, 1968).

A noteworthy feature common to the


procedures discussed above, apart from their

demonstrated efficacy, is the fact that the change


programs are conducted within natural settings by

teachers and parents utilizing reinforcing events

that form a natural part of spontaneous

interpersonal relationships. It is possible, of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 788
course, to modify behavior under contrived

conditions with incentives that are highly

attractive but are rarely employed, for practical or


other reasons, in everyday situations. Although

results of such studies may have some value in

demonstrating that a particular form of behavior

can be controlled by artificially arranged

consequences, such treatment procedures


eventually have to be supplanted for several
reasons: First, behavioral changes established

under artificial conditions must be supplemented

with generalization training, with respect to both

the types of tasks and incentives employed, in


order to ensure adequate transfer effects. Even if

contrived procedures were equally effective, they


frequently necessitate specialized equipment and

personnel, which limit their applicability.

Furthermore, although relevant data are rarely


obtained, one must also take into account possible

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 789
self-evaluative consequences that arbitrary

interventions have upon their recipients, as well

as social effects upon the attitudes and behavior of


others who have opportunities to observe the

treatment.

Countless studies employing reinforcement

techniques have been published in recent years,

but they lack the replicative control feature.


Nevertheless, they report favorable outcomes with
such diverse clinical problems as autistic behavior

(Lovaas, 1968) severe anorexia (Bachrach, Erwin,

& Mohr, 1965; Leitenberg, Agras, & Thomson,

1968), school phobias (Patterson, 1965), socially


disruptive behavior (Zimmerman & Zimmerman,
1962), mutism (Sherman, 1965; Straughan, 1968),

psychogenic seizures (Gardner, 1967), self-

mutilative activities (Allen & Harris, 1966),

antisocial behavior (Colman & Baker, 1968), and

innumerable other types of deviant behavior,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 790
some of which are discussed in sections that

follow. Although one finds a paucity of

experiments in which the relative efficacy of


diverse methods of therapy is systematically

evaluated, results of several controlled

investigations with schizophrenic patients (King,

Armitage, & Tilton, 1960; Peters & Jenkins, 1954;

Schaefer & Martin, 1966) and antisocial character


disorders (Colman & Baker, 1968), disclose that
treatment based upon reinforcement principles

produces greater change in interpersonal behavior

than do programs following conventional lines.

In a significant extension of reinforcement


procedures, Patterson and his colleagues

(Patterson, Ray, & Shaw, 1968) have achieved


some success in modifying deviant behavior by

altering the reinforcement patterns of familial

systems and peer groups. According to the

authors’ etiological formulation, deviant behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 791
typically arises under conditions of low levels of

positive reinforcement and nonreciprocal social

interactions among family members. The children


are, therefore, forced to resort to extreme forms of

behavior to elicit reinforcing reactions from

others. As a further consequence of

nonreciprocity, children are likely to become

increasingly controlled by the peer group and less


responsive to adults. This change, in turn, leads
adults to resort to aversive forms of control, which

further reduces their influence as reinforcing

agents.

The treatment approach, which involves a


four-step program, takes place in the home. After

two weeks of baseline observation of familial


interactions, parents are provided with a specially

prepared booklet designed to familiarize them

with general principles of reinforcement,

extinction, aversive control, inadvertent

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 792
reinforcement of deviant behavior, and with

procedures for recording interpersonal behavior.

In the second phase, parents are asked to list the


child behaviors they wish to modify. They are then

assigned a special hour during each day to record

the incidence of these behaviors, the various

consequences they engender, and the family

members who provide the consequences. After


they have learned to observe interpersonal
contingencies accurately, parents are helped,

through ample demonstration and supervised

practice, to alter the reinforcement contingencies

that they provide for both deviant and desired


response patterns. The family problems are thus

modified one at a time.

When necessary, new reinforcement practices

are also introduced in the classroom setting and in

the peer group. Control over deviant behavior in

extra-familial situations is typically achieved by a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 793
peer contingency procedure in which both the

child and his peers initially earn desired rewards

for his good behavior. The material reinforcers are


then gradually withdrawn until eventually the

child’s behavior is entirely maintained by social

reinforcement from teachers and peers. Following

termination of the formal treatment program,

telephone contact is maintained on a diminishing


schedule, and home observations are conducted
periodically over a six-month follow-up period.

Results based on six families that have

participated in the above program show that


parents reduced the frequency with which they
positively reinforced deviant behavior from an

average rate of 35 percent during the baseline


period to 10 percent at the end of the intervention

program. Modification of familial contingencies

not only decreased the family’s output of deviant

behavior, but it increased the amount of positive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 794
social reinforcement in the entire social system,

and it produced a more reciprocal quality to the

interactions between the various family members.


Moreover, these favorable changes tend to be

effectively maintained over time. Although this

approach appears to hold promise, the marked

variability in baseline rates of deviant behavior

before the new reinforcement practices were


inaugurated, and the differential responsiveness of
families to the program, indicate that further

refinements and assessments are needed.

DEVELOPMENT OF SOCIAL AND SELF-


MANAGEMENT SKILLS IN SEVERELY RETARDED
CHILDREN

Reinforcement techniques have also proved to


be of value for establishing basic social and self-

care skills in profoundly retarded children who,

because of their primitive level of behavior, are

usually considered uneducable and are hence

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 795
relegated to institutional back wards. Such
children have been toilet-trained (Giles & Wolf,

1966; Hundziak, Mowrer, & Watson, 1965), taught


to dress and undress themselves, to feed

themselves with utensils, to manage their personal

grooming, and to respond to verbal directions,

which is of considerable aid in the social training


process (Bensberg, Colwell, & Cassel, 1965;

Girardeau & Spradlin, 1964; Minge & Ball, 1967;

Roos, 1965). In addition, the efficacy of these

methods for developing communication skills,

interpersonal response patterns, and other


complex forms of behavior in severe retardates is

being explored. Bensberg notes parenthetically


that such programs have not only benefited

retarded children, but the functions of attendants,


who implement the training procedures, have

changed from dismal custodial care to active


participation in assisting children to gain

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 796
competencies within the limits of their ability.

SYMBOLIC LEARNING

In recent years, reinforcement procedures

have been used effectively in conjunction with


programmed instructional materials to establish

complex symbolic forms of behavior. Staats’

(1965) program of research on the acquisition of


reading behavior furnishes one such example.

Reading involves complicated processes in

which children must learn both to discriminate

among intricate verbal symbols and to associate

appropriate verbal responses to them. Complexity


arises primarily because the same elements in a

compound word stimulus must elicit different


responses depending upon the context in which

they occur. Since words contain many common

stimulus properties (e.g., counsel, council) and in


most cases, word differentiation relies upon subtle

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 797
cues, the development of reading responses
constitutes a demanding associative form-

discrimination task. In addition to difficulties

created by high stimulus similarity, the


instructional material itself typically serves as a

weak source of positive reinforcement,


particularly for young children. An effective
reading program therefore requires extensive

training utilizing material that is carefully

sequenced, repeated pairing of words with their


pictorial or verbal associates, immediate and

continuous feedback of the correctness of the

responses, and an incentive system capable of

sustaining children’s attention and active

responsiveness over long periods. These essential


conditions are incorporated in the semi-
automated method devised by Staats to study

reading acquisition.

In the initial pre-training phase, children are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 798
reinforced for imitating single vowels or words

spoken by the experimenter. After verbal

modeling has been well established, a word is


shown with several drawings of simple objects,

one of which corresponds to the printed stimulus,

and the experimenter pronounces the word. When

children have learned through discriminative

consequences to match words with their pictorial


representations without verbal prompts, the
actual reading training is begun. The reading task

is presented to children primarily in the form of a

matching-to-sample discrimination procedure. In

each learning sequence a sample word is shown in


the top aperture of a panel, and is matched by one

of three words presented simultaneously in a


bottom row of windows. The experimenter names

the stimulus word and asks the children to repeat

the word and to select the comparable item from


among the alternatives. If the child reads the word

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 799
correctly, he is immediately reinforced with

tokens which may be exchanged for preselected

toys or other desired items. Whenever the child’s


response is incorrect, the entire sequence is

repeated. After children have learned to read


words individually, they are presented in
sentences and in short paragraphs composed of

previously learned material.

In a discrimination learning task the error rate


can be effectively controlled by the use of cueing

procedures and stimulus alternatives that are

easily discriminable from the correct choice. By

gradually fading out the stimulus supports for the


appropriate behavior and employing
progressively finer contrasts between the stimulus

alternatives (Rocha e Silva & Ferster, 1966),

children can eventually learn to respond to subtle

features of words. Most other symbolic activities,

such as abstraction and concept formation,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 800
similarly depend upon the establishment of subtle

discriminations. This can be best achieved by

working from relatively gross contrasts to


successively smaller differences between stimuli.

Although comparisons with other instructional

methods have not been made because of the

exploratory nature of this research, nevertheless

Staats has accumulated considerable data (Staats,


Finley, Minke, & Wolf, 1964; Staats, Minke, Finley,
Wolf, & Brooks, 1964) regarding the potential

value of this approach for establishing reading

behavior in preschool children. Further, the


influence of schedules of reinforcement on rate of
reading acquisition has been studied

systematically in several cases with intrasubject


replications. The results, though somewhat

variable, demonstrate that under conditions of

reinforcement children maintain strong interest in

the reading task, and continue to acquire new

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 801
reading responses over an extended series of

sessions even though, for experimental reasons,

the social interaction between tutor and child is


severely curtailed. In educational or remedial

applications one would, of course, question the

wisdom of rigid adherence to such highly

impersonal practices and exclusive reliance upon

material reinforcers. As might be expected from


previous research, intermittent reinforcement
generally produced higher reading response rates

than continuous reinforcement. Moreover, during

periods when reinforcement was temporarily

discontinued reading behavior rapidly


deteriorated.

These procedures were initially extended by


Staats to the study of reading acquisition in

retarded children and remedial reading in a

delinquent adolescent (Staats & Butterfield, 1965).

The latter case involved a 14-year-old boy who, in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 802
addition to accumulating a long and varied history

of aggressive destructive behavior, had never

received a single passing grade in any school


subject; despite eight and a half years of classroom

instruction, his reading achievement was only at

the second grade level. Because of his uncharitable

attitudes and behavior toward school personnel,

and occasional dismantling of school property, he


was considered to be uneducable, incorrigible, and
mentally retarded.

Paralleling the methods employed in the

laboratory studies, the boy learned first to read


words presented singly, then combined in
sentences, and finally organized into short stories.

The vocabulary items, which were selected from


standard reading material arranged according to

difficulty level, were presented individually on

index cards and the boy was asked to pronounce

them. Following each correct reading response the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 803
boy received token rewards which he saved for

various articles and money allotments. After the

boy had mastered the vocabulary items he earned


additional tokens for accurate oral reading of

paragraphs, silent reading of entire stories, and for

correct answers to questions assessing

comprehension of story content.

During the training program the boy covered a


sizable range of reading material with
undiminished interest. He not only acquired new

reading responses at a relatively high rate, but he

showed increasing ability to read new words on


first presentation, and retained much of what he
had learned. This notable progress was also

reflected in reading achievement test scores


obtained prior to, during, and after completion of

approximately 40 hours of reading training

distributed over a four and a half month period

(Figure 4-3). That the brief treatment program

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 804
Figure 4-3. Reading test scores achieved after 8/2 years of
regular classroom instructions and after 4K months
during which reading responses were positively
reinforced. Staats & Butterfield, 1965.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 805
had produced generalized educational and

behavioral effects is indicated by the fact that the

boy received undistinguished but passing grades


in all subjects for the first time in his turbulent

academic career, and he markedly decreased and

eventually ceased his aggressively defiant

behavior. The entire program, which was

administered by a probation officer, involved a


total expenditure of $20.31 for token exchange
items.

Essentially the same procedures were applied

with some degree of success by adult volunteers


and high school seniors in teaching reading skills
to retarded, emotionally disturbed, and culturally

deprived children (Staats et al., 1967). The


children acquired reading responses at an

accelerating rate even though the instructional

material was increasing in difficulty and the

amount of extrinsic reinforcement was

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 806
progressively reduced. However, several

methodological problems associated with the

criterion tasks and the fact that control subjects


received special training with similar materials

precluded a stringent test of whether a program

involving tight contingencies and material

reinforcers yields better outcomes than the

standard educational practices.

Self-instructional Systems. The acquisition of


basic cognitive skills and knowledge, prerequisite

for higher symbolic activities, requires repeated

presentation of substantial amounts of abstract


content and principles, as well as intricate
discrimination training. Since many of these

functions can be performed more efficiently by


programmed self-instructional techniques than by

conventional training methods, increasing use is

made of semi-automated instructional systems for

symbolic learning. In evaluating the role of these

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 807
approaches in the educational process, it should

be emphasized that the critical issue is not

reliance upon mechanical versus social


presentation of stimulus material, but rather

which tutorial systems, applied either singly or in

combination, best approximate optimal conditions

for learning. Although social commentators often

attribute legions of virtues to conventional modes


of instruction and hosts of pernicious effects to
programmed methods, many instructors do not, in

fact, provide the type of content organization that

would ensure rapid learning and effective

retention; many present material in ways that


extinguish students’ intellectual interests; and

often many inadvertently establish strong


avoidance tendencies toward the subject matter

being taught. As a consequence, many students,

particularly those who are weakly motivated or


less well endowed intellectually, display marked

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 808
intellectual deficits despite numerous years

expended fruitlessly in school attendance.

Provided that they are skillfully designed and

adaptive to individual requirements, self-

instructional systems possess several features that


can facilitate the learning process. First, they

present material to the student in a well-organized

graduated order. The utilization of logically


ordered sequences prevents students from
becoming contused or lost through omission of

essential intermediate steps in exposition; this

removes one major aversive aspect of

conventional instruction. Second, they provide the


student with immediate feedback about the
accuracy of his responses, helping him to

continuously monitor his comprehension of the

subject matter. Third, since a student can proceed

to new information only by making correct

responses to preceding items, the required active

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 809
participation of the student forces careful

observation of stimulus material. Thus, if a student

should lapse into classroom reverie, the


instructional content, like Old Man River, keeps

rolling along, whereas in programmed instruction

the patient tutor remains idle as long as the

student is disengaged. Fourth, the self-pacing

feature of programmed teaching methods makes


individualized instruction possible for persons
who differ in ability and mastery of the material.

In computerized systems, in which new

instructional content is selected at each step on

the basis of the learner’s past performances,


students can generate their own optimal learning

sequences. Finally, because errors are drastically


reduced by gradual progression in content

difficulty, learning from self-instructional

programs is minimally threatening. The self-


pacing and nonthreatening characteristics are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 810
particularly important in clinical and remedial

applications to persons who have undergone

extensive failure experiences and who differ


widely in the content areas in which they show

deficits.

In an effort to assuage fears that machines may

displace warm-blooded pedagogues, it has been

customary to relegate the teaching of hackneyed,


factual material to programmed instructional
devices, reserving creative, problem-solving, and

conceptual skills to teachers who would be

liberated from mundane functions. As Resnick

(1963) cogently points out, the difficulty in


teaching complex intellectual skills results
primarily not from inherent limitations in

programmed instruction procedures but from the

fact that cognitive activities are usually described

in very general terms or remain essentially

undefined. For this reason, even talented teachers

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 811
are often at a loss in deciding what types of

learning experiences might be most appropriate to

the development of abstract abilities. After


component behaviors of more complex skills have

been adequately specified there is no reason why

they cannot be taught by carefully prepared

learning sequences. Indeed, there is some

evidence to suggest that self-instructional


methods can be utilized effectively for teaching
relatively complex intellectual skills such as

language functions, mathematical reasoning,

decision making, abstract thinking, problem-

solving strategies, a wide range of vocational and


avocational skills, and the basic concepts and

principles of diverse fields of study. Moreover,


numerous comparative investigations (Silberman,

1962; Stolurow, 1963) have generally shown

programmed instruction to be at least as effective


as, and sometimes better than, conventional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 812
teaching methods while also substantially

reducing student time, cost, and professional

personnel. Since the development of favorable


attitudes toward subject matters is as important a

part of the learning process as the acquisition of


specific intellectual skills (Mager, 1968), the
assessment of tutorial systems should measure

both attitudes and achievements. Unfortunately,

the attitudinal effects of programmed instruction,


or conventional teaching for that matter, are

rarely taken into account.

With further advances in auto-instruction, both

technologically (in the areas of picture projecting


systems, vocal programming, and the use of
computers to permit students more complete

control over learning sequences), and in

knowledge of acquisition processes, it should be

possible to arrange optimal learning conditions

more readily and to extend programmed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 813
instruction to even more complex forms of

symbolic behavior. Indeed, in an elaborate study

of computer-assisted instruction conducted by


Atkinson and Suppes (Atkinson, 1968) first grade

students receive their entire reading and

mathematics instruction through television

learning units controlled by a central process

computer. The computer presents the material,


monitors the students’ performances, and
continuously adjusts the learning sequence to the

capabilities and individual rate of achievement of

each pupil. Instruction provided by this system

produces greater proficiency than does the regular


classroom method of teaching. If costs can be

reduced, computer-based systems of education


may eventually replace conventional techniques of

instruction in many areas of study. Such

instructional systems could, in fact, furnish varied


forms of educational material at different levels

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 814
under optimal learning conditions to homes,

schools, commercial facilities, and any other

settings equipped with television learning units.

Progress in this area has been hampered to

some extent by the identification of programmed


learning with audio-visual aids. As a consequence,

research activities have been unduly preoccupied

with comparisons of conventional instructions and


minor variations of program characteristics,
rather than undertaking systematic investigations

of acquisition processes associated with self-

instructional systems. The latter type of research

would not only increase the effectiveness of


programmed instruction, but would also elucidate
fundamental learning processes involved in

complex skills. For example, the teaching of

reading through an automated matching-to-

sample method, applied to young children over an

extended period of time, should provide basic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 815
information about discriminative processes to

supplement knowledge derived from brief

laboratory studies employing identical procedures


with less meaningful content.

Although ample attention is currently being

devoted to hardware and program variables, the

incentive requirements for learning have been

essentially ignored. This oversight results in large


part from widespread assumptions that the
programmed format is effective in holding

attention and that informative feedback about the

accuracy of one’s responses functions as an


automatic positive reinforcer in sustaining
responsiveness. This may be true for persons who

have learned to value intellectual achievements, or


who expect to derive some immediate benefit

from increasing their competency in specific areas

of functioning. However, for highly motivated

bright students, small step, linear programming

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 816
involving strongly prompted responses, which

ensures successful learning in less talented

individuals, may provide such trivial increments in


performance that the learning task becomes

neither challenging nor personally rewarding.

While these negative effects can be avoided to

some extent by adjusting the size of informational

units to ability level, the incentive problem grows


far more serious in the case of persons for whom
signs of intellectual achievement must be

established as effective rewards. In these

instances an extrinsic incentive system must be

added to self- instructional procedures if students’


interest and responsiveness are to be durably

maintained.

SELF-MANAGED BEHAVIORAL CHANGE

Most of the programs that have been discussed

so far achieve behavioral changes primarily

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 817
through external management of reinforcement
contingencies. Recent years have witnessed a

growing interest in self- control processes in


which individuals regulate their own behavior by

arranging appropriate contingencies for

themselves. These self-directed endeavors

comprise a variety of strategies, some of which


were originally proposed by Ferster, Nurnberger

& Levitt (1962).

Efforts at self-influence usually prove

unsuccessful because they involve vague self-

instructions that have no immediate behavioral

implications. Moreover, unless self-directives are

supported by reinforcement operations, either


external or self-administered, they are unlikely to

exert much control over behavior. The selection of


well-defined objectives, both intermediate and

ultimate, is an essential aspect of any self-directed


program of change. The goals that individuals

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 818
choose for themselves must be specified in

sufficiently detailed behavioral terms to provide

adequate guidance for the actions that must be


taken daily to attain desired outcomes.

To further increase goal commitment

participants are asked to make contractual

agreements to practice self-controlling behaviors

in their daily activities. Thus, for example, in


modifying smoking behavior (Tooley & Pratt,
1967) and obesity (Ferster, Nurnberger, & Levitt,

1962), clients agree to restrict increasingly, in

graduated steps, the times and places in which


they will engage in the undesired behavior. Under
conditions where individuals voluntarily commit

themselves to given courses of action, subsequent


tendencies to deviate are likely to be counteracted

by negative self-evaluations. Through this

mechanism, and anticipated social reactions of

others, contractual commitments reinforce

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 819
adherence to corrective practices.

Satisfactions derived from evident changes

help to sustain successful endeavors. Individuals


can, therefore, utilize objective records of

behavioral changes as an additional source of

reinforcement for their self-controlling behavior.

In studies of self-directive processes by Kolb,

Winter, & Berlew (1968) students used miniature


counters to keep an accurate record of the
frequency with which they displayed desired and

undesired behavior throughout each day. These

data were plotted graphically to provide a clear


picture of the behavioral improvements students
were accomplishing by their own efforts. Daily

feedback of this type not only serves a reinforcing


function but it also safeguards against irregular

and halfhearted implementation of self-prescribed

procedures. In a study designed to improve self-

instruction behavior, Fox (1966) found that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 820
students who recorded their daily productivity

continued to work on assignments until they

exceeded their preceding performances, thus


ensuring continued improvement.

Since behavior is extensively under external

stimulus control, persons can regulate the

frequency with which they engage in certain

activities by altering stimulus conditions under


which the behavior customarily occurs.
Overeating, for example, will arise more often

when appetizing foods are prominently displayed

in frequented places in the household than if they


are stored out of sight and made less accessible.
Indeed, it has been shown that, compared to

individuals of normal weight, obese persons are


less responsive to internal hunger states

(Stunkard & Koch, 1964), whereas their eating

behavior is excessively dependent upon external

food-related stimuli (Schachter, 1967). Some

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 821
degree of self-control can thus be achieved by

judicious environmental arrangements that

reduce the external determinants of the behavior.


Conversely, the incidence of desired activities can

be increased by introducing appropriate stimuli

for them.

Behavior that provides immediate positive

reinforcement, such as eating, smoking, and


drinking, tends to be performed in diverse
situations and at varied times. Therefore, another

important aspect of self-managed change involves

progressive narrowing of stimulus control over


behavior. Continuing with the obesity illustration,
individuals are encouraged gradually to delimit

the circumstances under which they eat until


eventually their eating behavior is brought under

the control of a specific set of stimulus conditions.

This outcome is achieved by having clients commit

themselves to a graduated program in which they

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 822
refrain from eating in non-dining settings,

between regular mealtimes, and while engaging in

other activities such as watching television,


reading, or listening to the radio. An essentially

similar procedure is employed to increase effortful

behavior that is under weak situational control.

Thus in getting students to study productively a

specific desk and time are designated for study


and all potentially distracting stimuli are removed.
To preserve the desk’s stimulus value for study

behavior, whenever students find their thoughts

wandering or their interests lagging they are

instructed to leave the situation and turn to other


activities. In this way progressively longer periods

of concentrated study are achieved (Fox, 1966;


Goldiamond, 1965).

The foregoing procedures are primarily aimed

at instituting self-controlling behavior, but unless

positive consequences are also arranged the well-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 823
intentioned practices are likely to be short-lived.

Self-controlling behavior is difficult to sustain

because it is associated, at least initially, with


relatively unfavorable conditions of

reinforcement. Prepotent activities typically

provide immediate positive reinforcement for the

individual, whereas their aversive consequences

are not experienced for some time. Conversely,


self-control measures usually produce immediate
unpleasant effects while the personal benefits are

considerably delayed. Self-reinforcing operations

are, therefore, employed to provide immediate

support for self-controlling behavior until the


benefits that eventually accrue take over the

reinforcing function.

The contingencies that individuals arrange for

themselves may involve different types of

reinforcing events. They are asked to select a

variety of activities that they find rewarding and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 824
to make them contingent on the performance of

desired behavior. Viewing television, drinking

coffee, reading magazines, recreational breaks,


monetary rewards, or food treats may, for

example, be made conditional upon a certain

amount of study behavior. As was noted in the

introductory chapter, self-managed reinforcement

can serve an important behavior maintenance


function (Bandura & Perloff, 1967). In the case of
powerful appetitive behaviors, positive competing

activities may be engaged in, to aid self-control, at

times when the instigation to perform undesired

behavior is high. The disposition to perform


prepotent behavior can also be reduced by

generating immediate aversive consequences


either symbolically (Cautela, 1966; Homme, 1965),

or through the use of portable stimulation devices

(McGuire & Vallance, 1964). The manner in which


self-generated aversive effects have been utilized

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 825
to gain control over sexual perversions, chronic

alcoholism, and other types of addictive behavior

receives detailed consideration in Chapter 8.

As a final feature of self-directed change

programs, increases in desired behavior and


reductions in undesired behavior are attempted

gradually. In this way the incidence of experienced

discomforts is kept low, and steady progress


toward the eventual goal can be achieved.

The efficacy of self-directed approaches to

behavioral change is best illustrated in the


modification of obesity, which has proved
refractory to a variety of medical and

psychological procedures. Stunkard (1958)

succinctly describes the usual outcomes

associated with traditional treatments as follows:

“Most obese persons will not stay in treatment for

obesity. Of those who stay in treatment, most will

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 826
not lose weight and of those who lose weight, most
will regain it [p. 79].” Unlike approaches that focus

on caloric intake or inferential inner causes of


overeating, self- control programs attempt to

accomplish an enduring change in the pattern of

eating behavior by regulating stimulus conditions

and self-generated consequences for the behavior.


Stuart (1967) reports uniformly marked and

lasting weight reductions in eight obese women

who followed a program combining the various

elements discussed above (Figure 4-4). In a

controlled study Harris (1969) found that men


and women who were trained to use similar self-

control procedures lost weight and maintained the


loss, whereas a matched control group given

calorie charts and urged to reduce remained


obese. Both studies further reveal that this

approach is accompanied by low dropout rates


and no unpleasant emotional effects. The

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 827
Figure 4-4. Weight losses achieved by eight women using
self-control procedures. Stuart, 1967.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 828
encouraging preliminary findings indicate that

self- control methods merit further systematic

investigations both as treatments in their own


right and as adjuncts to other procedures.

VERBAL CONDITIONING

The method of positive reinforcement has also

been widely applied in studies of verbal


conditioning. These investigations typically utilize
either a free-interview or discrimination-learning

situation in which an experimenter selectively

reinforces certain classes of verbal responses but

ignores all other verbalizations. The reinforcement


usually consists of nodding, smiling, repeating or

paraphrasing the interviewee’s remarks, or simple

verbal utterances with positive connotations. The

simplicity of the procedure and its similarity to

clinical interviews led to ready adoption of verbal-


conditioning paradigms for testing hypotheses

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 829
concerning psychotherapeutic interaction
processes. The overall results of innumerable

studies (Kanfer, 1968; Krasner, 1962; Salzinger,


1959) reveal that interviewers can exercise

substantial control over the content of subjects’

verbal behavior through selective responding. It

has been demonstrated, for example, that affective


expressions, positive or negative self-reference

statements, confiding, hostile, and affiliative

verbalizations, expressions of opinion or beliefs,

“hallucinatory” and “neurotic” responses, maternal

references, early childhood memories, and


common responses to word association stimuli

can be increased by minimal social reinforcement,


and decreased by withholding reactions conveying

interest or approval. Experiments designed to


isolate variables governing the extent of verbal

conditioning have shown that subjects’


responsiveness to social reinforcers is affected by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 830
such factors as the characteristics of the

experimenter, the types of reinforcing events

employed, the content of responses selected for


modification, subjects’ interpretations of the

reinforcing stimuli, the personality characteristics


and emotional state of interviewees, and the
quality of the relationship between the

experimenter and his subjects.

The theoretical and practical significance of


experiments demonstrating that verbal content is

modifiable through reinforcement largely depends

upon the psychological events that one wishes to

explain or to modify. If one is interested in


elucidating the communication processes
associated with conversational forms of treatment,

then verbal conditioning procedures furnish a

laboratory analogue to the clinical interview,

provided that the situation bears some

resemblance to psychotherapy, the interviewer’s

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 831
reinforcing interventions are analogous to those

regularly employed by therapists, and the classes

of responses chosen for study exemplify


psychotherapists’ treatment concerns. In this

connection, verbal-conditioning studies strongly

indicate that the specific content of clients’

verbalizations, which is often assumed to reflect

intrapsychic processes, may be largely determined


by interviewers’ selective interest and attention.
This is corroborated by analyses of response-

reinforcement contingencies as they occur

naturally in psychotherapeutic interactions

(Bandura, Lipsher, & Miller, 1960; Goldman, 1961;


Murray, 1956; Truax, 1966; Winder, Ahmad,

Bandura, & Rau, 1962). Therapists’ positive


reinforcement of certain types of verbal responses

increases their occurrence, whereas clients avoid

discussing matters that produce less favorable


reactions. Considering that people often remain in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 832
treatment for extended periods, and that the

therapist’s potency as a reinforcing agent is

enhanced by his high status and his client’s


emotional dependence upon him, it is not

surprising to find that clients frequently undergo


marked changes in verbal behavior.

People rarely enter psychotherapy and

willingly expend considerable time, money, and


effort merely to learn to talk differently. A
question of the utility of verbal conditioning as a

method of treatment therefore arises. Little

therapeutic importance can be attached to verbal-

conditioning procedures unless it is demonstrated


that verbal behaviors established in treatment
settings generalize to other persons in the natural

environment and, even more important, that

verbal changes influence nonverbal behaviors to a

significant extent. Several investigators have

found that changes in verbal responses display

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 833
some transfer to different situations (Ullmann,

Krasner, & Collins, 1961) and can affect nonverbal

responding (Lovaas, 1961, 1964); however, most


studies have failed to obtain generalization as

measured by tasks varying in similarity (Rogers,

1960; Tobias, 1960; Ullmann, Krasner, & Edinger,

1964; Williams, 1959).

The contradictory and weak generalization


effects noted above are not at all surprising when
one considers that experimental manipulations in

most conditioning studies barely suffice to

produce a conditioning effect, let alone stimulus or


response generalization. It is possible that greater
changes could be achieved through verbal

conditioning if interviewers continued the


treatment for longer periods and instituted

systematic programs of generalization training.

Generality is usually ensured by varying stimulus

configurations. This requires changing reinforcing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 834
agents and modifying treatment conditions so that

both the social settings and the responses being

reinforced are increasingly similar to those


encountered in the natural environment. As noted

previously, therapists often choose to modify

verbal behavior in hospital or office settings rather

than to alter social behavior directly under natural

conditions, more for reasons of convenience than


therapeutic efficacy. It would be far more
meaningful and advantageous to effect desired

behavioral changes from the outset and to provide

clients with graduated performance tasks to carry

out in their social milieux. Such an approach


avoids the unnecessary problems associated with

circuitous treatment strategies that commence


with verbal conditioning, which must later be

supplemented by a series of procedures designed

to establish and to transfer social response


patterns to extra- therapeutic situations. There are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 835
occasions, of course, when change agents are faced

with the problem of reinstating verbal

communication in mute persons (Isaac, Thomas, &


Goldiamond, 1960; Salzinger, Feldman, Cowan, &

Salzinger, 1965; Sherman, 1965) or of modifying


delusional and other types of deviant
verbalizations (Ayllon & Haughton, 1964; Ayllon &

Michael, 1959; Richard & Dinoff, 1962). Verbal

conditioning may be an appropriate, though not


necessarily the most efficient, procedure for these

purposes.

In addition to the use of verbal conditioning as

a method for gaining understanding of interview


processes and as a treatment technique in its own
right, this approach has been employed to study

the influence of certain variables on learning

processes. Initial findings from verbal

conditioning experiments were accepted as

striking demonstrations of automatic, unconscious

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 836
learning. However, more detailed analyses of

conditioning performances as a function of

subjects’ awareness of reinforcement


contingencies raised fundamental questions about

what, in fact, is being learned in such experiments

(i.e., verbal responses or hypotheses about

contingencies). With conceptualization of verbal

conditioning as a hypothesis-testing rather than


an automatic response-strengthening process, the
focus of research interest shifted from traditional

learning variables to the role of awareness in the

learning process. Results of this line of research

and their implications for theories of behavioral


change will be reviewed later.

Social Organizational Applications of


Reinforcement Contingencies

The discussion thus far has been primarily

concerned with the alteration of circumscribed

responses of single individuals through positive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 837
reinforcement. In many cases change agents are

faced with the far more complex task of effecting

extensive modification in the attitudes and


behavior of large groups of individuals in

educational, rehabilitative, and other social

establishments. In order to accomplish

widespread behavioral changes it is necessary to

alter institutionally organized practices of the


larger social system in ways that will
simultaneously affect the behavior of each

member in beneficial ways. Some of the issues and

problems associated with group-oriented

contingency systems are best illustrated in


treatment applications of reinforcement

procedures to institutionalized populations.

Over the years there have appeared numerous

sociological studies of the psychiatric hospital as a

social system (Dunham & Weinberg, 1960;

Goffman, 1961; Stanton & Schwartz, 1954;

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 838
Wessen, 1964), each of which documents the

debilitating effects that prevailing institutional

practices have upon inmate populations. Because


of the large numbers of patients who must be

cared for with limited staff resources and facilities,

most institutions, regardless of their avowed

objectives, are primarily concerned with the

management and social control of patients, rather


than with, their rehabilitation. In order to
maintain efficiency and economy, the institution

must effect certain changes in patients’ behavior

that are incompatible with achievement of

effective social functioning and often prove more


deleterious than the behavioral problems that

originally led to the patients’ hospitalization.

The initial socialization of inmates usually

involves some degree of suppression of

individualized modes of behavior. On admission to

the institution, people are characteristically

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 839
deprived of most of their personal possessions,

their civil rights, their social status, their

accustomed satisfactions, their privacy, and their


individuality, so that they can be handled

expeditiously in large groups. Throughout the

period of institutionalization, the patients’

behavior is closely regulated and accommodated

to fixed hospital routines. Under these types of


organizational contingencies, initiative, self-
reliance, and self-determination, which are

necessary for attaining satisfactory independent

adjustment outside the hospital, are generally

extinguished, whereas the more docile patient-


role behaviors bring about the greatest rewards

and promotion in graded ward systems. Moreover,


whatever chores the patients may be assigned

contribute primarily to hospital maintenance

rather than to the further development of


occupational skills. With prolonged withdrawal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 840
the customary incentives for sustaining complex

behavioral repertoires, patients generally display

a progressive loss of social and vocational


competence, which renders them even less

capable of managing their lives on the outside.

In addition to reinforcement of institutional

dependence and behavioral losses through

deficient incentives, the gradual abandonment of


patients by their relatives, their stigmatization as
mentally diseased, and their loss of contact with

persons and contemporary events outside the

hospital further contribute to chronicity. Most

patients who are subjected to traditional


contingency patterns in psychiatric facilities over
a period of several years become permanently

resigned either to a simple regimented

institutional life or to a pattern of “intermittent

patienthood” (Friedman, von Mering, & Hinko,

1966). Although, in exchange for self-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 841
determination, hospital residency provides more

physical comforts and fewer taxing demands than

patients with limited personal resources can attain


within their own social environments, it is evident

that they are rarely overjoyed or contented with

their sheltered existence. Major changes in

reinforcement contingency patterns at the social

system level are therefore required if institutional


establishments are to serve an important
rehabilitative function.

The recent years have witnessed increasing

use of contingent reinforcement on a group basis.


These procedures have been systematically
applied, for example, to hospital wards for

severely debilitated psychotics (Atthowe &


Krasner, 1968; Ayllon & Azrin, 1965) and

alcoholics (Narrol, 1967), in social-educational

programs for retarded children (Bijou, 1965;

Girardeau & Spradlin, 1964), in remedial academic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 842
programs for school dropouts and low achievers

(Clark, Lackowicz, & Wolf, 1968; Wolf, Giles, &

Hall, 1968), in educational systems for managing


behavior disorders (O’Leary & Becker, 1967) and

fostering productive classroom behavior (Hall,

Panyan, Rabon & Broden, 1968), and in

rehabilitative institutions for delinquent

adolescents (Cohen, 1968). Unlike traditional


treatment systems, these programs contain work-
payment incentive systems and contingency

structures that are highly compatible with those in

the larger society.

There are three main characteristics of group-


oriented reinforcement practices as they are being

applied to populations that require, at least


initially, the use of extrinsic incentives. First,

behaviors essential for effective day-to-day

functioning (e.g., self-management, educational

accomplishments, appropriate social behavior,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 843
satisfactory work performance, etc.) are

designated by the staff as reinforceable responses.

Second, a simulated currency, which is


exchangeable for a variety of desired objects,

activities, and special privileges, can be earned by

performance of the appropriate behaviors. Third,

an exchange system is devised in which a specified

number of points or tokens is required for the


purchase of various objects and privileges, similar
to monetary transactions in the outside

community.

The powerful control exercised by group


reinforcement procedures over the behavior of an
entire psychiatric ward population is best

exemplified by a series of experiments (Ayllon &


Azrin, 1965) in which organizational

reinforcement contingencies were systematically

varied. In each of the studies, tokens earned could

be used to secure, among other things, privacy

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 844
(e.g., choice of bedroom, choice of eating group,

selection of personal furniture and a room

divider), freedom to leave the ward and hospital


grounds, private audience with members of the

hospital staff, recreational opportunities (e.g.,

movies, band concerts, social functions, exclusive

rental of a radio or a television set), and a varied

array of commissary articles that could be


obtained by special request.

One experiment, conducted with a small group

of chronic female patients, studied the influence of

reinforcement contingencies on patients’


performance of off-ward jobs that were staffed by
paid hospital personnel. In the initial phase each

patient selected her preferred work assignment


and received 70 tokens for each full 6-hour day. In

order to ascertain whether job selection was

indeed determined by extrinsic incentives or by

social and intrinsic rewards deriving from the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 845
vocational activity itself, in the second phase of the

study patients were no longer paid for

participating in their preferred jobs, but tokens


could be earned by work in non-preferred

assignments. During the third phase, the original

reinforcement for the preferred job was

reinstated.

In marked contrast with the usually erratic and


lethargic work performance of patients prior to
the incentive program, under the simulated

economy system all patients reported promptly

and regularly to work without complaint, even


though they were free to take time off from their
jobs whenever they wished. That the contingency

structure was highly influential in regulating the


behavior of the group is further shown by the fact

that all but one of the patients changed their work

assignments immediately when reinforcement

was shifted from preferred to non-preferred jobs

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 846
(Figure 4-5). When informed that the people with

whom they had been working were very pleased

with their performance and would like them to


continue, but in order to give other patients a turn

at the job, no tokens would be available, several

patients explained to ward attendants: “No, honey,

I cannot work at the laundry for nothing. I’ll work

at the lab. I just couldn’t make it to pay my rent, if I


didn’t get paid.”…“You mean if I work at the lab I
won’t get paid? I need tokens to buy cigarettes for

my boy friend and to buy new clothes so I’ll look

nice like the other girls [pp. 363-365].”

In a subsequent experiment, when patients


were paid the tokens on a noncontingent basis

each morning rather than at completion of the


day’s assignment, they all ceased working within a

week. On the other hand, when reinforcement was

again made conditional upon work performance,

the patients immediately resumed their job

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 847
Figure 4-5. Mean number of hours patients worked per day
when positive reinforcement was varied between
preferred and nonpreferred jobs. Ayllon & Azrin, 1965.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 848
assignments. These marked changes in

performance are particularly interesting in view of

patients’ reports that they worked to keep active,


because of enjoyable social relationships, personal

satisfactions deriving from the work itself, and

other intrinsic gratifications. Obviously in this case

self-reports were unreliable indicants of the actual

conditions maintaining their behavior.

The manner in which patients spent their


currency is also highly informative. It was used

mainly to secure privacy (45%), commissary items

(34%), and ward leaves (21%), whereas virtually


no tokens were expended for private audiences
with hospital staff (.001%), for religious services

(.0002%), or recreational activities (.0008%).

The contingency system was found to be

equally effective in maintaining active patient

participation in varied on-ward activities

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 849
including complex duties as dietitians, secretarial
assistants, waitresses, and sales clerks in the

commissary, in janitorial, ward cleaning, and


laundry work, assisting in recreational programs

and personal care of other patients, and special

services. As shown in Figure 4-6, when rewards

were dependent upon successful completion of


performances the patients worked industriously;

when they were simply given the tokens

noncontingently they gradually stopped working;

and when contingent reinforcement procedures

were reinstated their participation was restored


immediately and maintained at the previously

high level.

It is of particular interest that when the

incentives were completely withdrawn and the


institutional rewards and privileges were made

freely available in a manner similar to usual


hospital practices, a marked loss in behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 850
Figure 4-6. Total number of hours a group of 44
schizophrenic patients participated in rehabilitative
activities when rewards were conditional upon
successful completion of assignments and when the
same rewards were provided regardless of whether or
not the patients took part in the activities. Ayllon &
Azrin, 1965.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 851
resulted (Figure 4-7).

The rapid behavioral changes produced by

alternating incentive conditions does not


necessarily demonstrate that reinforcement is the

sole controlling factor. In social situations

behavior always remains partly under modeling

stimulus control. Persons who occupy a

prestigeful position in a social group usually serve


as major sources of social behavior for other
members. Consequently, to the extent that the

incentive conditions modified the behavior of

prestigious models, other patients may have


emulated their actions independently of the token
rewards. As Schwartz & Hawkins (1965) have

shown, substantially different group behavior can


occur under the same reinforcement conditions

depending upon the behavior of influential

models. The ubiquitous influence of modeling cues

complicates interpretation of intragroup

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 852
Figure 4-7. Total number of hours spent each day by the
group of 44 patients performing “on-ward” activities
during periods when rewards were given upon
completion of work assignments, when positive
incentives were not used and the various activities and
privileges were freely available, and when the
reinforcement contingencies were reintroduced into
the social system. Ayllon & Azrin, 1965.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 853
replicative control through manipulation of

reinforcement variables. An additional

complication was unfortunately introduced in


several of the studies by designating the

noncontingent period as a “vacation with pay,”

which carries a strong suggestion that work might

be temporarily discontinued. The changes

observed were, therefore, probably a compound


function of incentive conditions, modeling
influences, and instructional sets.

The successive reversals of work performances

consistently achieved through systematic


variation of contingency structures provide
convincing experimental demonstrations that

organizational reinforcement practices can


determine the degree to which persons will

exhibit initiative, involvement, and active

participation in available activities. These

principles and procedures have been applied on

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 854
an extended basis with deliberate therapeutic

intent in various institutional settings.

Atthowe & Krasner (1968) established an


incentive program in an entire psychiatric ward

population of 86 chronic schizophrenic patients,

whose length of hospitalization varied from 4 to

49 years with a median institutional residency of

24 years. As a group, these patients had


maintained an apathetic, isolated, and almost
vegetative existence on the ward. They

participated minimally in the hospital routines,

and they often neglected to care for themselves or


to change their clothes unless aided by attendants.
Most had lost complete contact with the outside

community and were essentially resigned to a


permanent institutional residency.

In an effort to reinstate interpersonal and self-

directing behavior so that the patients could

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 855
function more effectively, a token system involving
most phases of ward and hospital life was

adopted. The patients were informed that, in the


future, certain privileges, recreational activities,

passes, money allowances, and numerous other

rewarding events could be purchased with tokens.

The patients were provided with many


opportunities for earning tokens through

appropriate social behavior. In addition to self-

care, responsibleness, and housekeeping

behaviors, the patients could earn tokens for

participating in various social and vocational


activities. Each patient was rewarded immediately

after successful completion of some desired


activity. A system of negative sanctions and token

fines was established to control seriously


disruptive behaviors and theft of tokens. The latter

problem attested to the fact that the range of


behaviors created by the simulated economy was,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 856
indeed, analogous to that occurring in the larger

outside community.

Those who functioned more independently

and could assume full responsibility for their work

assignments were compensated more generously.


However, they were paid the tokens only once a

week in order to teach them to budget their

expenditures. By utilizing banking and budget


management systems patients were further
prepared for successful community life. Special

token bonuses were also offered if patients

received satisfactory ratings in their industrial

training and made worthy contributions to their


social group.

The ward incentive program mirrored societal

practices in still another respect. Patients who

were able to function productively on their jobs,

who had a realistic discharge plan, and who had

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 857
sufficient token savings for a substantial
membership fee could, if vacancies existed, join a

privileged group on the ward which freed them


from most of the institutional constraints.

Members received a credit card that permitted

them considerable freedom to regulate their own

behavior. It entitled them, not only to all the


privileges within the token system, but additional

advantages as well. They were provided, for

example, choice dining and sleeping

arrangements, extra passes on weekdays and

unlimited pass privileges on weekends, and they


could bank their money in the community without

any restrictions on withdrawal.

The efficacy of this treatment program was

evaluated in terms of several indices including


both changes in the specific behaviors that were

involved in the reinforcement contingencies and


more generalized outcomes. Consistent with the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 858
results of Ayllon & Azrin (1965), it was found that

the target behaviors were restored quickly and

dramatically after introduction of incentives. This


is shown most clearly by the marked decrease in

the frequency with which attendants had to

awaken patients, assist them with personal

grooming, make their beds, and prepare them for

daily activities. The number of morning infractions


dropped from a baseline rate of approximately 75
per week prior to reinforcement to about 9 after

several months of treatment. The patients likewise

displayed increased participation in group

activities, which were also specifically reinforced


performances. Some additional evidence that the

incentive system was, in fact, a significant


contributor to the observed changes is shown in

patients’ increased social participation when they

were rewarded more generously, whereas


participation in group activities declined

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 859
somewhat when the token rate was later reduced

to its original level.

Among generalized changes observed were the

patients’ display of substantially more social

communication as measured by behavioral


ratings, and greater interest in the outside

community. This was reflected in increased use of

passes, from an average of 9 per week before


treatment to 37 per week after the incentive
program had been instituted. Indeed, 26 percent

of the patients left the hospital on daytime or

overnight passes for the first time in many years,

and one patient, for whom the hospital had


become a permanent abode, ventured out for the
first time in 43 years!

Discharge rates also verified the beneficial

effects of the new reinforcement practices.

Twenty-one patients left the hospital via the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 860
privileged group, almost double the discharge rate
for the same ward the previous year. The overall

findings of this project indicate that alteration of


contingency structures in a hospital social system

can not only counteract the stupefying effects of

prolonged institutionalization, but also produce

generalized increases in self-directive and


interpersonal modes of behavior.

A comprehensive treatment program does not


simply aim to produce effective functioning in

circumscribed areas within the institution, but

rather to establish the requisite social and

vocational competencies for successful self-

management in the larger society.


Institutionalized patients who possess marketable

skills and who have a reasonably adequate


environment to which to return can undoubtedly

benefit greatly from programs that provide


adequate guidance and reinforcing support for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 861
adaptive patterns of behavior. Favorable outcomes

can be further ensured by the use of transitional

facilities embodying contingency systems that


foster gainful employment and progressively more

active participation in community life. However,

chronically hospitalized patients, who are grossly

lacking in personal and social resources, present

the most challenging rehabilitation problem.


Results based on follow-up studies disclose that
approximately 70 percent of chronic patients who

are discharged from mental hospitals return

within 18 months regardless of the type of

treatment received during the period of


hospitalization (Fairweather, Simon, Gebhard,

Weingarten, Holland, Sanders, Stone, & Reahl,


1960; Fairweather & Simon, 1963). As a

consequence of this high readmission rate,

hospitals are faced with a continuously growing


population of chronic patients. Moreover, for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 862
reasons given earlier, most of these patients are

destined for a permanent institutional residency.

It is generally acknowledged that unless the

social roles and competencies acquired by patients

in institutional settings approximate the behaviors


essential for meeting the more rigorous demands

of community living, it is highly unlikely that they

will be able to achieve a satisfactory independent


adjustment. With this in mind, Fairweather and his
coworkers (Fairweather, 1964) conducted an

elaborate field experiment to determine whether

chronically hospitalized patients can be restored

to the community by providing them with


opportunities to develop, within the hospital,
some of the problem-solving and self-management

behaviors required in the outside community.

The participants, predominantly schizophrenic

patients, were matched on the basis of age,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 863
diagnosis, and length of hospitalization and
assigned randomly either to a conventional

hospital program or to patient-led problem-


solving groups. The treatment programs were

similar in all respects, with the single exception

that patients in the two groups took part in

different activities for two hourly sessions each


day. Those in the traditional treatment program

participated in individual work assignments and a

recreational hour during the time that their

counterparts engaged in group work assignment

and decision-making sessions.

In the task group condition an incentive

system was applied in which the participants


received increasing monetary and pass privilege

rewards, contingent on the development of four


levels of progressively complex social and self-

directive behavior. The responsibility for


evaluating and modifying the behavior of each

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 864
member, and for implementing the incentive

system, was delegated to the group. It met daily to

discuss the progress of individual members, their


problems, and constructive ways in which these

might be managed or modified. Any staff member

could be invited to furnish factual information

needed by the group to reach a reasonable

decision, but the staff refrained from


recommending what courses of action should be
taken.

Each week the group also met with the staff to

present their recommendations concerning each


individual member’s step-level and concomitant
money and passes for the following week, the

actions taken with respect to problem behavior,


and their evaluations of the progress, morale, and

functioning of their group. The staff then either

approved all recommendations made by the

group, approved some and disapproved others, or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 865
rejected their recommendations. If warranted, the

entire group could be rewarded or penalized by

being raised or lowered one step- level, depending


upon the appropriateness of the group’s decision-

making behavior.

Thus, the social-role behaviors required of

both the patients and the staff differed markedly

in the two programs. In the conventional


treatment the hospital staff was primarily
responsible for regulating the patients’ daily

activities, for making decisions concerning money

allotments, passes and other types of privileges,


and for implementing disciplinary and remedial
courses of action. The patients, in turn, occupied

the usual subordinate, patient role. By contrast,


although the staff in the group decision-making

program could overrule or amend action-oriented

recommendations proposed by patients, staff

members functioned primarily as consultants and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 866
resource persons. This social structure provided

patients considerable freedom and responsibility

to manage their own activities and to make


decisions that significantly affected each other’s

behavior.

In an effort to control for possible effects of

different staff characteristics on patients’ social

behavior, the two sets of staff members changed


wards halfway through the experiment. The
relative efficacy of the treatment approaches was

objectively assessed in terms of diverse criteria

including a multitude of behavior ratings,


sociometrically derived preferences, self-
evaluations, administrative indices, and attitude

questionnaires. Most of the behavioral


assessments were conducted throughout the 27

weeks that the experiment was in progress, while

others were obtained at the completion of the

study, and six months later.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 867
The voluminous data from this ambitious, well-

executed field study demonstrate that the

program specifically designed to reinstate


interpersonal responsiveness and self-directive

behavior in patients yielded consistently superior


outcomes. Patients in the latter treatment
condition rapidly formed cohesive groups, in

which the members exhibited increasing mutual

interest, help, and responsibility. They organized


their own employment group, interviewed and

counseled patients, and assumed full

responsibility in locating employment for eligible


members. They also established informal

educational programs taught by group members


who possessed specialized skills or knowledge.

The contrasting ward climates are even more

strikingly revealed in patients’ specific attitudes

and social behaviors. Relative to the patients

receiving the traditional care and treatment, those

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 868
in the graded reward program showed

significantly greater interpersonal responsiveness,

more verbal communication, and less bizarre


behavior. Some of these comparative data are

summarized graphically in Figure 4-8. In their

sociometric and questionnaire responses, patients

in the incentive-group program viewed their

fellow members more positively; they regarded


their treatment as more difficult and demanding
but, at the same time, more beneficial; and they

displayed greater optimism about their eventual

discharge, future employment, and development

of close interpersonal relationships upon their


return to the community. These positive

expectations were largely confirmed by the follow-


up study. Compared to patients who received the

traditional treatment, those who participated in

the incentive-group program spent a shorter time


in the hospital, were more frequently gainfully

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 869
Figure 4-8. Behavioral changes displayed by patients
receiving either conventional hospital treatment or a
program designed to establish problem-solving and
self-management competencies, (a), percentage of
observations during which patients manifested
pathological behavior; (b), percentage of observations
during which patients engaged in social interactions
involving three or more persons; (c), total time the two
groups of patients remained silent during weekly ward
meetings; (d), mean number of patients participating in
weekly ward meeting discussions. Fairweather, 1964.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 870
employed, met with friends more regularly, and

engaged in a higher level of verbal interaction with

others.

It is interesting to note that in the present

treatment program favorable generalized changes


were effected in patients’ behavior even though

the staff explicitly provided only a general set of

contingencies and relatively few concrete


incentives. The investigators report that the
monetary and pass privilege rewards were

essential in the early phase of treatment, but after

the patients established mutually rewarding

relationships with each other, pride in their


accomplishments, competitiveness with other
groups, and mutual social approval and

disapproval became the major reinforcing events

regulating their day-to-day behavior. These

findings suggest that patients can successfully

modify and sustain each other’s behavior through

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 871
mutual social reinforcement provided that

behavioral guidelines and incentives are

furnished, conditions are arranged that lead to the


formation of a cohesive group, and the

responsibility for change is primarily delegated to

the group. Since the behavioral demands that a

group places upon its members through common

agreement are less likely to be resisted than staff-


imposed contingencies, a group-mediated
treatment program may achieve a more natural

and higher degree of change in social behavior. In

a project evaluating the efficacy of a token

reinforcement system for treating predelinquent


boys, Phillips (1968) found that large token

penalties administered by the staff to the entire


group failed to reduce their undesired behavior,

whereas it was promptly and enduringly

eliminated when lesser fines were individually


levied by a group member who assumed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 872
managerial responsibility for the troublesome

activities (Figure 4-9).

The success of group-managed contingency

systems still relies upon reinforcement processes,

except that interpersonal reinforcers are favored


over material rewards, and group members rather

than the staff serve as the main reinforcing agents.

Although Fairweather emphasizes the


autonomous functioning of the groups, and the
gradual emergence of group norms, it should be

noted that the staff members continuously

imposed specific contingencies on the groups,

both in their written communications describing


problem behaviors of individual members that the
group was expected to control, and in their

evaluative responses to the groups’ decisions. It is

therefore not surprising that each group evolved

patterns of reinforcement that fostered desired

behavioral changes. In the absence of adequate

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 873
Figure 4-9. Number of infractions occurring under two types
of contingency systems: (1) In the manager condition
one of the boys purchased a managership which
assigned to him the responsibility for selecting
individuals to perform the tasks and for paying or
fining them according to the quality of their work. The
manager received or lost monetary points according to
the number of tasks completed. (2) In the fines
condition the staff imposed penalties on the entire
group for failure to perform the required tasks. The
numbers under the arrows indicate the number of
monetary points the group was fined. Phillips, 1968.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 874
staff monitoring of a group’s activities, particularly

in early stages of treatment, the types of

reinforcement contingencies that a given patient


group might adopt would be left to fortuitous

factors.

In view of the generalized changes in

interpersonal responsiveness achieved by

Fairweather, it would be of considerable interest


to compare systematically the relative efficacy of
staff-administered reinforcement systems

involving an elaborate set of precise contingencies

of the type employed by Ayllon, and Atthowe &


Krasner, with one in which some incentives are
used to foster strong group cohesiveness but

controlling functions are in large part delegated to


group members. The merits of these two

approaches, which involve many common

principles, could be easily combined to form a

program in which a specified set of reinforcement

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 875
contingencies is developed and implemented by

the patients themselves under staff guidance.

Another important contingency variable that


requires systematic investigation is concerned

with whether rewards are tied to individual

performances or to entire sets of behavior. In the

latter system, successive phases are devised which

require increasingly higher levels of functioning in


several different areas. As individuals progress
through these sequential steps by adopting the

requisite patterns of behavior they receive

increased rewards and privileges. In treating a


group of delinquent adolescents, Martin,
Burkholder, Rosenthal, Tharp, & Thorne (1968)

found that a phase-contingent system of


reinforcement produced more rapid and

uniformly positive changes in behavior than a

previous system in which specific responses were

individually reinforced. Indeed, the latter

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 876
contingency structure produced much wrangling

and accusations of unfairness because, in an effort

to ensure adequate reinforcement of progress


made by youngsters functioning at different levels,

they were required to meet different behavioral

standards and achievements for similar rewards.

The authors attribute the greater efficacy of the

reinforcement system linked to role behaviors to


the fact that clear specification of sequential goals
and the behaviors required for promotion from

one phase to the next serve as prompts and

positive guides for changes in desirable directions.

In most applications of reinforcement


principles to severely incapacitated persons,

behavioral improvements are initially achieved by


immediate reinforcement of specific

performances. However, as their competencies are

increased, individuals are promoted to a phase

system analogous to hierarchical reinforcement

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 877
structures existing in community life.

Despite the favorable changes in behavior

produced by the incentive group program


developed by Fairweather, readmission rates for

patients who had received the different forms of

treatment were essentially the same. Eighty

percent of the short-term psychotics maintained

an adequate community adjustment, whereas only


45 percent of those who had been institutionalized
for two or more years remained outside the

hospital by the end of six months. These outcome

data provide further justification for questions


that are increasingly raised about the wisdom of
conducting behavioral change programs within

hospital settings. The behavioral requirements for


effective hospital and community adjustment

differ in so many fundamental respects that, even

though institutional reinforcement practices may

establish and strengthen some of the response

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 878
patterns consistent with those in the larger

society, most of the social and vocational

behaviors that can be developed in hospitals have


limited transfer value. More radical departures

from conventional institutional approaches are

clearly required if chronically hospitalized

psychotics are to be restored to society as socially

productive members.

A second experimental project conducted by


Fairweather and his associates (Fairweather,

Sanders, Maynard, & Cressler, 1969) represents

one example of an innovative program that holds


considerable promise for the rehabilitation of
chronic psychotics. Based on evidence that the

type of social environment to which patients


return is the major determinant of successful

community adjustment, Fairweather decided to

institute the social subsystem that proved highly

successful in the hospital in a community

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 879
residence where behavioral requirements are

essentially identical to those of any member

residing in the community. One group of chronic


patients was formed in the hospital and, after they

achieved a sufficient degree of organization and

cohesiveness within the hospital and adequate

training in decision-making behavior to function

effectively, the group was transferred to a lodge


located in the community. There the members
were responsible, as a unit, for regulating each

other’s behavior, managing the affairs of the lodge,

including the purchase and preparation of food,

keeping records of expenditures and personal


loans from their own savings bank, and, when

necessary, obtaining information about drug


dosages from a local physician as well as

administering the necessary medication. In

addition, the patients operated an income-


producing business (a janitorial service, both

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 880
commercial and residential, supplemented by

yardwork, general hauling, and painting) in the

community. They assumed major responsibility


for receiving and processing work orders,

arranging transportation to and from work,


assignment of patients to specific jobs appropriate
to their level of functioning, and management of

the monetary incentive system. Initially a research

staff member coordinated the daily operations of


the lodge, but later this function was performed

even more successfully by a lay person with

periodic consultation provided by a member of the


research staff. The primary function of the

consultation was to supply needed information, to


assess the group’s functioning, to review any

personal or organizational problems that arose,

and to evaluate the group’s recommended


solutions. The income derived from the business,

which amounted to a total of $52,000 over a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 881
period of two and three-fourths years, was

distributed weekly among the participants with

each member’s share being determined by his


productivity and the amount of responsibility that

he assumed in the organization.

In order to evaluate the relative efficacy of this

community-based program, an individually

matched group of 75 patients received the group


decision-making treatment in the hospital along
with the traditional types of assistance and out-

patient therapy following discharge from the

institution. Repeated measurements were made of

changes in patients’ self-evaluations, attitudes,


interpersonal behavior, and ability to maintain a
satisfactory independent adjustment in the

community.

Results of this project show that, within a

supportive subcommunity, chronic marginal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 882
individuals can manage their daily affairs and lead
a worthwhile and constructive life. A forty-month

follow-up study revealed that the experimentally


created subsystem sustained patients in the

community, whereas those treated in the hospital

were unable to adjust to life on the outside (Figure

4-10). The differences between these groups are


even more striking in their vocational functioning:

the lodge system enabled patients to maintain

gainful employment, while none of the patients

who received treatment within the hospital setting

were employed full time (Figure 4-11). These


beneficial results were obtained at an individual

cost of $6 per day, as compared to $14 at the


hospital from which the participants were drawn,

$12 at a local state hospital, $45 at a local private


hospital, and $56 at a local county hospital.

The main purpose of the type of residential


program discussed above is to create a semi-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 883
Figure 4-10. Percentage of time that patients in the lodge
and hospital programs spent in the community for 40
months of follow-up. The lodge program was
discontinued after 33 months. Fairweather et al., 1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 884
Figure 4-11. Percentage of time that patients in the lodge
and hospital programs were employed full time for 40
months of follow-up. Fairweather et al., 1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 885
autonomous subcommunity in which marginal

individuals can function in a socially productive

manner, rather than to serve as a transitional


facility. Some of the participants may eventually

leave after they have developed the social and

vocational competencies required to live

independently. However, those who have no

immediate families to return to or little or no


financial resources, and who are unlikely to gain
employment individually in the open job market

because of their advanced age, limited vocational

skills, and stigmatization, may achieve the most

satisfactory adjustment in their own supportive


social system. Individually they may be unable to

meet the demands of community life, but as a


group they can function as self-supporting and

productive individuals who would otherwise be

institutionalized for the rest of their lives.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 886
GROUP REINFORCEMENT SYSTEMS IN THE
MODIFICATION OF DELINQUENCY

In a program devised by Cohen (1968; Cohen,


Filipczak, Bis, Cohen, Goldiamond, & Larkin, 1968)
for overcoming educational deficits in adolescent

delinquents, the institutional reinforcement

practices are, in many respects, also analogous to


the contingency structures that obtain in the

larger community.

The adoption of, and adherence to, prosocial


patterns of behavior is highly dependent upon
adequate proficiency in educational and

vocational skills necessary for legitimate


acquisition of resources that are highly valued by

the culture. Most delinquents are handicapped by

gross deficits in the educational abilities requisite


for satisfactory vocational adjustment. As a

consequence, those who value costly possessions


and the social rewards attendant upon symbols of

high status are forced to resort to deviant means

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 887
for obtaining them. Regardless of whatever other
objectives may be selected in the rehabilitation of

delinquents, little progress can be made in altering


antisocial behavior unless habitual offenders are

provided with vocational skills that will permit

legitimate attainment of desired social and

material rewards. Therefore, the experimental


program devised by Cohen focused primarily upon

the creation of educational competencies and

favorable attitudes toward academic pursuits.

The adolescent boys, who were randomly

selected for the experimental program from the

institutional population, presented exceedingly

poor academic histories: All had dropped out of


school, they showed little interest in academic

matters, and none derived much intrinsic


satisfaction from intellectual activities. A system of

extrinsic rewards based on a point economy was


therefore employed to establish academic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 888
behaviors. Points earned for high achievement

scores in programmed instructional courses and

other academic activities could purchase favorable


accommodations and a wide range of services and

commodities. Starting at a base pay of $10 to $15 a

week, payable on high scholastic achievement and

a minimum number of hours of academic work,

the boys could earn additional money points by


studying.

Most coercive sanctions imposed on

delinquents by correctional instructions were

absent from this program. Rather, heavy reliance


was placed on positive incentives, individual
initiative, and self-determination. Consistent with

the basic contingency system in daily life on the


outside, boys paid for their private rooms ($6 a

week, or 600 points) and selected meal plans that

varied in choice of foods. They used point currency

to rent recreational items and private offices, and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 889
to pay tuition for requested courses. They could

also purchase admission to entertainment or

leisure-time activities provided at a project


lounge, and merchandise available in either the

project store or through mail order catalogues.

Whenever applicable, the prices of items

corresponded to the rates in the outside

community. Within this environment the boys


were provided with considerable freedom: They
determined their own study and bedtime routines,

they selected their own leisure-time activities and

planned their own outside programs, they aided in

planning menus, and they had open mailing and


visiting privileges.

A boy was free to choose not to participate in


any of the scholastic or rehabilitative activities,

but if his points fell below 1200 he was placed “on

relief.” Although this problem rarely arose, while

on relief status the boy lost his private room, was

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 890
served the regular institutional food, and enjoyed

no extra luxuries. This system of treatment

corresponds closely to principles suggested earlier


for altering behavior on a positive reinforcement

basis by creating an environment where

noncontingent gratifications are provided at an

adequate but low level, and in which the

performance of desired modes of behavior


produces further rewards. If such a system is well
managed, hostile-resistive actions by participants

should be markedly reduced because their own

behavior determines their welfare and not

management fiat.

Under environmental contingencies

specifically designed to support learning,


delinquent boys who had received few prior

accolades for scholarship and were school

dropouts worked productively at self-managed

educational activities. They studied

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 891
conscientiously in their spare time, and gained

more than two grade levels on standard

achievement tests within an eight-month period.


These favorable results suggest that reinforcing

environments for learning can be successfully

employed in the rehabilitation not only of

delinquents, but of other educationally

disadvantaged children as well.

The contingency system discussed above


aimed almost exclusively at educational behaviors

and, although beneficial changes in attitudes and

social behavior were noted, no concerted effort


was made to alter interpersonal response
patterns. The contingencies could, of course, be

easily extended to include cooperative,


responsible, and self-controlling behavior which,

supplemented with proficiency in a selected

occupation, would remove the major personal

barriers to a successful prosocial adjustment.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 892
INTERDEPENDENT CONTINGENCY SYSTEMS

The preceding social systems primarily involve


group-wide contingencies, but reinforcements are

still administered on an individual basis. Work

payment arrangements, for example, are usually

the same for all members of the group, although


the actual compensation depends upon the type

and amount of work performed by each person.


Under such conditions, a member’s outcomes are
not affected by the behavior of others. One can, of

course, influence the degree of social interaction


between persons even on an individualistic

reinforcement basis simply by rewarding

cooperative responses (Cohen & Lindsley, 1964),

or other forms of social behavior.

On some occasions a change agent may be


called upon to increase the level of mutual

responsibility, cohesiveness, commitment, and


contribution to a common goal among all

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 893
members of a group. This objective can be
accomplished most effectively by instituting

reinforcement contingencies on a group basis.


Under these circumstances individual rewarding

outcomes depend upon the level of group

performance and, conversely, censurable behavior

by any given member may produce negative


consequences for the entire group. By having

people share in the consequences of their

decisions and actions, the degree of social

responsibility and involvement is thereby

increased.

There have been some laboratory

investigations of group productivity as a function


of different types of group contingency structures.

Glaser & Klaus (1966) found that group behavior


was influenced by reinforcement contingencies in

the same way as individual behavior when all


members were required to perform correctly to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 894
produce rewarding outcomes for themselves.

Group output increased when correct team

performances were positively reinforced, whereas


the rate of correct responding declined when

group performance no longer resulted in

reinforcing consequences. On the other hand, a

contingency structure in which a correct response

by any member produced reinforcement for the


entire group often resulted in a deterioration of
performance for the group or individual members.

The latter condition is analogous to naturalistic

situations where individuals benefit from

someone else’s efforts. The decline of


responsiveness can be easily offset by the use of a

double contingency so that a given individual’s


outcomes are determined by both the extent of his

own contribution and the group’s overall

performance. It will be recalled that Fairweather’s


residential treatment program relies upon this

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 895
type of contingency system in the allocation of

monetary rewards.

Some suggestive evidence concerning the

relative efficacy of individual and group

contingency systems is provided by Wolf & Risley


(1967). They studied the amount of disruptive

classroom behavior displayed by a child in the

absence of any special reinforcement and during


subsequent periods when either she alone earned
five points, or she and her immediate peers each

earned one point for her commendable behavior.

It is interesting to note that the child’s activities

were more effectively controlled under the peer


contingency even though it produced only one-
fifth of the amount of reinforcement provided on

the individual basis. Apparently, through the

group reward, change agents were able to enlist

the peers’ aid in modifying the behavior of their

companion. The findings of the present case study,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 896
and those cited earlier, are sufficiently interesting

to warrant further systematic exploration of the

effects of different types of group contingency


structures on social performance.

Group-oriented reinforcement practices have

been adopted on a society-wide basis in the Soviet

Union for the explicit purpose of developing

strong collectivistic morality in its citizenry


(Bronfenbrenner, 1962). This aim is implemented
by use of school collectives where children’s

behavior is regulated by rewards and

punishments administered on a group basis, so


that all members of a given social unit are affected
by the actions of each individual. Socialization at

the school is commenced in the primary grades by


assigning children to row units. Daily records are

kept of each group’s performance on a variety of

social and academic activities. The grades that a

person receives are based on the overall

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 897
performance of his unit rather than his individual

accomplishment, and, from time to time, the most

successful rows are publicly acclaimed, and given


special privileges. Collective achievement is

further reinforced by introducing competition not

only between rows, but also between classrooms,

between schools, between districts, and so on. In

this manner cooperative contingencies are


maintained within groups of expanding
membership, while simultaneously, strong

competitive contingencies are instituted between

progressively larger social units.

In addition to inculcating collective


orientations through group incentives, the control

and modification of behavior is delegated to the


peer group. Initially teachers set the standards of

behavior and evaluate the group’s performance.

As soon as possible, however, the responsibility

for evaluating the behavior of individuals and for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 898
administering rewards and sanctions is turned

over to members of the social unit. This transfer is

accomplished by designating row monitors for


each activity to judge work assignments, to

criticize shortcomings of their comrades, and to

devise effective means for helping other members

of one’s collective. Whenever individuals are

praised for their mutual aid, the contribution to


group achievement is carefully noted.

Pervasive and unrelenting application of

group-oriented systems of reinforcement which

stifle autonomy and self-determination clearly are


antithetical to goals that are highly valued in most
societies. Therefore, where interdependent

contingencies are instituted to increase group


unity and responsibility, each member should also

be given opportunities for independent

accomplishment.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 899
Summary

The present chapter reviewed theories of

reinforcement in terms of their relative emphasis

upon associative-strengthening or incentive


functions of reinforcers. Evidence bearing on

alternative explanations of reinforcement effects


in terms of drive reduction, sensory stimulation,
and opportunities to engage in prepotent

responses was also reviewed. The fact that

markedly diverse events can assume reinforcing


functions and their value is determined by

relational rather than fixed properties present

difficulties for the construction of an inclusive

theory of reinforcement.

Since behavior is largely governed by its


consequences, differential reinforcement has been
extensively applied, both singly and in

combination with other methods, to overcome


behavioral deficits, to maintain existing response

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 900
patterns, and to alter deviant behavior which is
supported by its rewarding effects.

Three sets of variables are importantly


involved in effective implementation of

reinforcement principles. First, it is essential to

devise an incentive system that is capable of


maintaining a high level of responsiveness over
long periods. Without adequate incentives

behavioral control is likely to remain weak and


unstable. Second, the reinforcing events must be

made conditional upon occurrence of the desired

behavior. Although reinforcement practices are

commonly used to modify behavior in natural

settings, these efforts are often minimally effective


because the intended consequences are poorly

timed, the wrong responses may be inadvertently


reinforced, and, even when appropriate

contingencies are arranged, they are often only


sporadically applied.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 901
The third requirement concerns methods

powerful enough to induce or to elicit the desired

modes of response with sufficient frequency for


them to be strongly established through positive

reinforcement. Several different strategies can be


used for this purpose. The most popular approach,
though not necessarily the most efficient one,

relies upon a process of gradually shaping emitted

responses into desired patterns by selective


reinforcement of successively closer

approximations. However, in most cases complex

responses can be more rapidly created by the


provision of performance guides in the form of

appropriate verbal or behavioral modeling cues.


Another method, applicable when responses are

already available but rarely exhibited, depends

upon prompting and fading techniques in which


any stimuli known to exercise strong control over

the desired behavior are enlisted until its

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 902
incidence is sufficiently increased, after which the

ancillary stimulus supports are gradually

withdrawn.

The behavioral change process is further

complicated because additional learning


conditions must be arranged if established

response patterns are to generalize beyond the

specific treatment situation, and to persist long


after the specially created contingencies have been
discontinued. Enduring changes in behavior can

be achieved by gradually reducing the frequency

or magnitude of reinforcement; by shifting the

locus of reinforcement from transitory change


agents to favorable contingencies existing within
the person’s social milieu; and by altering the form

of the events that assume reinforcing functions.

Thus arbitrary extrinsic incentives, which may be

necessary during early stages of treatment, can be

gradually withdrawn and replaced by symbolic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 903
cues signifying performance achievements

coupled with self-evaluative and other self-

reinforcing consequences. As such stimulus events


which are more intrinsically related to

performances acquire reinforcing capacity,

personally valued patterns of behavior can be self-

maintained with minimal external support.

Changes effected in social response patterns


can be widely generalized by conducting
treatment within natural social contexts, by

directly modifying the reinforcement practices of

persons who normally exercise some control over


the crucial behavior, and by varying systematically
the stimulus conditions under which the behavior

is established.

Results of myriad projects in which requisite

conditions are adequately arranged demonstrate

that contingent reinforcement can be a highly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 904
effective means of establishing and modifying
diverse classes of response. This is most

impressively revealed by studies in which


tenacious deviant behavior is successively

eliminated and reinstated by varying its social

consequences. Such powerful replicative control

over behavior not only has considerable


therapeutic significance, but accents the influential

role played by environmental contingencies in the

regulation of behavior disorders: Conditions

which are usually attributed to complex internal

determinants presumed to be operating largely at


an unconscious level respond in orderly fashion to

external control.

In recent years there has been increased

recognition that most social problems must be


treated primarily at the social-systems rather than

at the individual level. Therefore, considerable


importance is attached to extensions of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 905
reinforcement procedures demonstrating that

widespread changes in attitudes and behavior can

be achieved by applying organizational


contingencies to members of a given group.

Interdependent contingency systems, in which the

outcomes for individual members are dependent

upon the composite group performance rather

than their own attainments, have also been


successfully employed to increase the productivity
and level of mutual support, responsibility and

cohesiveness in social groups. Double

reinforcement contingencies in which individuals’

outcomes are jointly determined by the nature of


their own contributions and by the group’s overall

performance are likely to produce the most


socially productive functioning.

Although behavior can be effectively controlled

by varying its positive consequences, negative

sanctions are commonly employed, particularly in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 906
efforts to reduce deleterious response patterns.

The processes involved in aversive control are

reviewed next.

REFERENCES

Allen, K. E., & Harris, F. R. Elimination of a child’s


excessive scratching by training the mother in
reinforcement procedures. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1966, 4, 79-84.

Allen, K. E, Hart, B, Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & Wolf, M.


M. Effects of social reinforcement on isolate
behavior of a nursery school child. Child
Development, 1964, 35, 511—518.

Allen, K. E., Henke, L. B., Harris, F. R., Baer, D. M., &


Reynolds, N. J. Control of hyperactivity by social
reinforcement of attending behavior. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 1967, 58, 231-237.

Atkinson, R. C. Computerized instruction and the


learning process. American Psychologist, 1968,
23, 225-239.

Atthowe, J. M., Jr., & Krasner, L. Preliminary report on


the application of contingent reinforcement
procedures (token economy) on a “chronic”
psychiatric ward. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 907
1968, 73, 37-4.3.

Ayllon, T. Intensive treatment of psychotic behavior


by stimulus satiation and food reinforcement.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1963, 1, 53-62.

Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. Reinforcement and


instructions with mental patients. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 327-
331.

Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. The measurement and


reinforcement of behavior of psychotics. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1965, 8,
357-383.

Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. A motivating environment for


therapy and rehabilitation. New York: Appleton-
Century-Crofts, 1968.

Ayllon, T., & Haughton, E. Modification of


symptomatic verbal behaviour of mental
patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1964,
2, 87-97.

Ayllon, T., Haughton, E., & Osmond, H. O. Chronic


anorexia: A behaviour problem. Canadian
Psychiatric Association Journal, 1964, 9, 147-154.

Ayllon, T., & Michael, J. The psychiatric nurse as a


behavioral engineer. Journal of the Experimental

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 908
Analysis of Behavior, 1959, 2, 323-334.

Bachrach, A. J., Erwin, W. J. & Mohr, J. P. The control of


eating behavior in an anorexic by operant
conditioning techniques. In L. P. Ullmann & L.
Krasner (Eds.), Case studies in behavior
modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston, 1965. pp. 153-163.

Baer, D. M., Peterson, R. F., & Sherman, J. A. The


development of imitation by reinforcing
behavioral similarity to a model. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1967, 10, 405-
416.

Baer, D. M., & Wolf, M. M. The entry into natural


communities of reinforcement. Paper read at the
American Psychological Association meeting,
Washington, September, 1967.

Bandura, A., Lipsher, D. H., & Miller, P. E.


Psychotherapists’ approach- avoidance reactions
to patients’ expressions of hostility. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 1960, 24, 1-8.

Bandura, A., & Perloff, B. The efficacy of self-


monitoring reinforcement systems. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 7, 111—
116.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 909
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression.
New York: Ronald Press, 1959.

Barnes, G. W., & Baron, A. Stimulus complexity and


sensory reinforcement. Journal of Comparative
and Physiological Psychology, 1961, 54, 466-469.

Bensberg, G. J. (Ed.) Teaching the mentally retarded: A


handbook for ward personnel. Atlanta: Southern
Regional Education Board, 1965.

Bensberg, G. J., Colwell, C. N., & Cassel, R. H. Teaching


the profoundly retarded self-help activities by
behavior shaping techniques. American Journal of
Mental Deficiency, 1965, 69, 674-679.

Berlyne, D. E. Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New


York: McGraw-Hill, 1960.

Bijou, S. W. Experimental studies of child behavior,


normal and deviant. In L. Krasner, & L. P. Ullmann
(Eds.), Research in behavior modification. New
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. pp. 56-81.

Brawley, E. R., Harris, F. R., Allen, K. E., Fleming, R. S.,


& Peterson, R. F. Behavior modification of an
autistic child. Behavioral Science, 1969, 14, 87-97.

Bronfenbrenner, U. Soviet methods of character


education: Some implications for research.
American Psychologist, 1962, 17, 550-564.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 910
Brown, J. S. Comments on Professor Harlow’s paper.
In Current theory and research on motivation: A
symposium. Lincoln: University of Nebraska
Press, 1953. pp. 49-55.

Brown, J. S. Pleasure-seeking behavior and the drive-


reduction hypothesis. Psychological Review, 1955,
62, 169-179.

Buchwald, A. M. Supplementary report: Alteration in


the reinforcement value of a positive reinforcer.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1960, 60,
416-418.

Butler, R. A. Incentive conditions which influence


visual exploration. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1954, 48, 19-2.3.

Butler, R. A. Exploratory and related behavior: A new


trend in animal research. Journal of Individual
Psychology, 1958, 14, 111-120. (a)

Butler, R. A. The differential effect of visual and


auditory incentives on the performance of
monkeys. American Journal of Psychology, 1958,
71, 591-593. (b)

Cautela, J. R. Treatment of compulsive behavior by


covert sensitization. Psychological Record, 1966,
16, 33-41.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 911
Chambers, R. M. Effects of intravenous glucose
injections on learning, general activity, and
hunger drive. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1956, 49, 558-564.

Clark, M., Lackowicz, I., & Wolf, M. A pilot basic


education program for school dropouts
incorporating a token reinforcement system.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1968, 6, 183-
188.

Cohen, D. J., & Lindsley, O. R. Catalysis of controlled


leadership in cooperation by human stimulation.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1964,
5, 119-137.

Cohen, H. L. Educational therapy: The design of


learning environments. In J. M. Shlien (Ed.),
Research in psychotherapy. Washington, D. C.:
American Psychological Association, 1968. pp.
21-53.

Cohen, H. L., Filipczak, J., Bis, J., Cohen, J., Goldiamond,


I., & Larkin, P. Case II—Model: A contingency-
oriented 24-hour learning environment in a
juvenile correctional institution. Silver Spring,
Md.: Educational Facility Press, 1968.

Colman, A. D., & Baker, S. L., Jr. Utilization of an


operant conditioning model for the treatment of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 912
character and behavior disorders in a military
setting. Unpublished manuscript, Walter Reed
Army Institute of Research, Washington, D. C.,
1968.

Coppock, H. W., & Chambers, R. M. Reinforcement of


position preference by automatic intravenous
injections of glucose. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1954, 47, 355-357.

Coppock, H. W., Headlee, C. P., & Hood, W. R. Negative


reinforcing effects of insulin injections. American
Psychologist, 1953, 8, 337 (Abstract).

Dunham, H. W., & Weinberg, S. K. The culture of the


state mental hospital. Detroit: Wayne State
University Press, 1960.

Dunham, P. J. Contrasted conditions of reinforcement:


A selective critique. Psychological Bulletin, 1968,
69,295-315.

Fairweather, G. W. Social psychology in treating


mental illness: An experimental approach. New
York: Wiley, 1964.

Fairweather, G. W., Sanders, D. H., Maynard, H., &


Cressler, D. L. Community life for the mentally ill:
An alternative to institutional care. Chicago:
Aldine, 1969.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 913
Fairweather, G. W., & Simon, R. A further follow-up
comparison of psychotherapeutic programs.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1963, 27, 186.

Fairweather, G. W., Simon, R., Gebhard, M. E.,


Weingarten, E., Holland, J. I., Sanders, R., Stone, G.
B., & Reahl, J. E. Relative effectiveness of
psychotherapeutic programs: A multicriteria
comparison of four programs for three different
patient groups. Psychological Monographs, 1960,
74 (5, Whole No. 492).

Ferster, C. B., Nurnberger, J. I., & Levitt, E. B. The


control of eating. Journal of Mathetics, 1962, 1,
87-109.

Fox, L. Effecting the use of efficient study habits. In R.


Ulrich, T. Stachnik & J. Mabry (Eds.), Control of
human behavior. Glenview, Ill.: Scott, Foresman,
1966. pp. 85-93.

Fox, S. S. Self-maintained sensory input and sensory


deprivation in monkeys: A behavioral and
neuropharmacological study. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1962,
55, 438-444.

Friedman, I., von Mering, O., & Hinko, E. Intermittent


patienthood—the hospital career of today’s
mental patient. Archives of General Psychiatry,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 914
1966, 14, 386-392.

Gardner, J. E. Behavior therapy treatment approach to


a psychogenic seizure case. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 1967, 31, 209-212.

Giles, D. K., & Wolf, M. M. Toilet training


institutionalized, severe retardates: An applicant
of operant behavior modification techniques.
American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1966, 70,
766-780.

Girardeau, F. L., & Spradlin, J. E. Token rewards in a


cottage program. Mental Retardation, 1964, 2,
345-351.

Glaser, R., & Klaus, D. J. A reinforcement analysis of


group performance. Psychological Monographs:
General & Applied, 1966, 80 (13).

Goffman, E. Asylums. Chicago: Aldine, 1961.

Goldiamond, I. Self-control procedures in personal


behavior problems. Psychological Reports, 1965,
17, 851-868.

Goldman, J. R. The relation of certain therapist


variables to the handling of psychotherapeutic
events. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Stanford University, 1961.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 915
Hall, R. V., Lund, D., & Jackson, D. Effects of teacher
attention on study behavior. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 1-12.

Hall, R. V., Panyan, M., Rabon, D., & Broden, M. Teacher


applied contingencies and appropriate classroom
behavior. Paper presented at the meeting of the
American Psychological Association, San
Francisco, September, 1968.

Harlow, H. F. The formation of learning sets.


Psychological Review, 1949, 56, 51-65.

Harlow, H. F. Motivation as a factor in the acquisition


of new responses. In Current theory and research
in motivation: A symposium. Lincoln: University of
Nebraska Press, 1953. pp. 24-49.

Harris, F. R., Johnston, M. K., Kelley, C. S., & Wolf, M. M.


Effects of positive social reinforcement on
regressed crawling of a nursery school child.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 1964, 55, 35-
41.

Harris, F. R., Wolf, M. M., & Baer, D. M. Effects of adult


social reinforcement on child behavior. Young
Children, 1964, 20, 8-17.

Harris, M. B. A self-directed program for weight


control: A pilot study. Journal of Abnormal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 916
Psychology, 1969, 74, 263-270.

Hilgard, E. R., & Bower, G. H. Theories of learning. (3d


ed.) New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.

Holstein, S., & Hundt, A. G. Reinforcement of


intracranial self-stimulation by licking.
Psychonomic Science, 1965, 3, 17-18.

Homme, L. E. Perspectives in psychology: XXIV.


Control of coverants, the operants of the mind.
Psychological Record, 1965, 15, 501-511.

Homme, L. E. Contiguity theory and contingency


management. Psychological Record, 1966, 16,233-
241.

Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-


Century-Crofts, 1943.

Hundziak, M., Mowrer, R. A., & Watson, L. S., Jr.


Operant conditioning in toilet training of severely
mentally retarded boys. American Journal of
Mental Deficiency, 1965, 70, 120-124.

Isaac, W. Evidence for a sensory drive in monkeys.


Psychological Reports, 1962, 11, 175-181.

Isaac, W., Thomas, J., & Goldiamond, I. Application of


operant conditioning to reinstate verbal behavior
in psychotics. Journal of Speech and Hearing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 917
Disorders, 1960, 25, 8-12.

Johnston, M. S., Kelley, C. S., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. F., &


Wolf, M. M. Effects of positive social
reinforcement on isolate behavior of a nursery
school child. Unpublished manuscript, University
of Washington, 1963.

Johnston, M. S., Kelley, C. S., Harris, F. R., & Wolf, M. M.


An application of reinforcement principles to
development of motor skills of a young child.
Child Development, 1966, 37, 379-387.

Kanfer, F. H. Verbal conditioning: A review of its


current status. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton
(Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior
theory. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall,
1968. pp. 254-290.

Kelleher, R. T,, & Gollub, L. R., A review of positive


conditioned reinforcement. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5, 543-
597.

Kimble, G. A. Hilgard and Marquis’ conditioning and


learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,
1961.

King, G. F., Armitage, S. G., & Tilton, J. R. A therapeutic


approach to schizophrenics of extreme

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 918
pathology. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1960, 61, 276-286.

Kish, G. B. Studies of sensory reinforcement. In W. K.


Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966. pp. 109-159.

Kolb, D. A., Winter, S. K., & Berlew, D. E. Self-directed


change: Two studies. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 1968, 4, 453-471.

Krasner, L. Studies of the conditioning of verbal


behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 1958, 55, 148-
170.

Krasner, L. The psychotherapist as a social


reinforcement machine. In H. H. Strupp & L.
Luborsky (Eds.), Research in psychotherapy. Vol.
II. Washington, D. C.: American Psychological
Association, 1962. pp. 61-94.

Leitenberg, H., Agras, W. S., & Thomson, L. E. A


sequential analysis of the effect of selective
positive reinforcement in modifying anorexia
nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1968,
6, 211-218.

Leuba, C. Toward some integration of learning


theories: The concept of optimal stimulation.
Psychological Reports, 1955, 1, 27-33.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 919
Levin, G. R., & Simmons, J. J. Response to food and
praise by emotionally disturbed boys.
Psychological Reports, 1962, 11, 539-546.

Lewis, M., Wall, A. M., & Aronfreed, J. Developmental


change in the relative values of social and
nonsocial reinforcement. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1963, 66, 133-137.

Lovaas, O. I. Interaction between verbal and


nonverbal behavior. Child Development, 1961, 32,
329-3.36.

Lovaas, O. I. Control of food intake in children by


reinforcement of relevant verbal behavior.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964,
68, 672-678.

Lovaas, O. I. A program for the establishment of


speech in psychotic children. In J. K. Wing (Ed.),
Early childhood autism. Oxford: Pergamon, 1966.
pp. 115-144.

Lovaas, O. I. Some studies on the treatment of


childhood schizophrenia. In J. M. Shlien (Ed.),
Research in psychotherapy. Washington, D. C.:
American Psychological Association, 1968. pp.
103-129.

Lovaas, O. I., Berberich, J. P., Perloff, B. F., & Schaeffer,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 920
B. Acquisition of imitative speech by
schizophrenic children. Science, 1966, 151, 705—
707.

Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Kinder, M. I., Rubenstein, B. D.,


Schaeffer, B., & Simmons, J. Q. Experimental
studies in childhood schizophrenia. III.
Developing social behaviors using electric shock.
Journal of Experimental Research in Personality,
1965, 1, 99-109.

Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Kinder, M. I., Rubenstein, B. D.,


Schaeffer, B., & Simmons, J. Q. Establishment of
social reinforcers in two schizophrenic children
on the basis of food. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 1966, 4, 109-125.

McGuire, R. J., & Vallance, M. Aversion therapy by


electric shock: A simple technique. British
Medical Journal, 1964, 1, 151-153.

Mager, R.F. Developing attitudes toward learning. Palo


Alto, Calif.: Fearon, 1968.

Martin, G. L., & Powers, R. B. Attention span: An


operant conditioning analysis. Exceptional
Children, 1967, 33, 565-570.

Martin, M., Burkholder, R., Rosenthal, T. L., Tharp, R.


G., & Thorne, G. L. Programming behavior change

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 921
and reintegration into school milieux of extreme
adolescent deviates. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1968, 6, 371-383.

Meehl, P. E. On the circularity of the law of effect.


Psychological Bulletin, 1950, 47, 52-75.

Miles, R. C. Learning in kittens with manipulatory,


exploratory, and food incentives. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1958,
51, 39-42.

Miller, L. B., & Estes, B. W. Monetary reward and


motivation in discrimination learning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1961, 61, 501-504.

Miller, N. E. Some reflections on the law of effect


produce a new alternative to drive reduction. In
M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on
motivation: 1963. Lincoln: University of Nebraska
Press, 1963. pp. 65-112.

Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. Social learning and imitation.


New Haven: Yale University Press, 1941.

Miller, N. E., & Kessen, M. L. Reward effects of food via


stomach fistula compared with those of food via
mouth. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 1952, 45, 555-564.

Minge, M. R., & Ball, T. S. Teaching of self-help skills to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 922
profoundly retarded patients. American Journal of
Mental Deficiency, 1967, 71, 864-868.

Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and behavior. New


York: Wiley, 1960.

Murray, E. J. A content-analysis method for studying


psychotherapy. Psychological Monographs, 1956,
70 (13, Whole No. 420).

Myers, A. K., & Miller, N. E. Failure to find a learned


drive based on hunger: Evidence for learning
motivated by exploration. Journal of Comparative
and Physiological Psychology, 1954, 47, 428-436.

Narrol, H. G. Experimental application of


reinforcement principles to the analysis and
treatment of hospitalized alcoholics. Quarterly
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1967,28, 105-115.

Odom, R. D. Effects of auditory and visual stimulus


deprivation and satiation on children’s
performance in an operant task. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 1964, 1, 16-25.

O’Leary, K. D. A token reinforcement system in the


public schools. Paper presented at the meeting of
the American Psychological Association, San
Francisco, September, 1968.

O’Leary, K. D., & Becker, W. Behavior modification of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 923
an adjustment class: A token reinforcement
program. Exceptional Children, 1967, 33, 637-
642.

Patterson, G. R. A learning theory approach to the


treatment of the school phobic child. In L. P.
Ullmann & L. Krasner (Eds.), Case studies in
behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston, 1965. pp. 279- 285.

Patterson, G. R., Ray, R., & Shaw, D. Direct intervention


in families of deviant children. Unpublished
manuscript, University of Oregon, 1968.

Peters, H. N., & Jenkins, R. L. Improvement of chronic


schizophrenic patients with guided problem-
solving motivated by hunger. Psychiatric
Quarterly Supplement, 1954, 28, 84-101.

Phillips, E. L. Achievement place: Token


reinforcement procedures in a home- style
rehabilitation setting for “pre-delinquent” boys.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 213-
223.

Premack, D. Reinforcement theory. In D. Levine (Ed.),


Nebraska symposium on motivation: 1965.
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1965. pp.
123-180.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 924
Redl, F., & Wineman, D. Children who hate: The
disorganization and breakdown of behavior
controls. Glencoe, Ill.: Free Press, 1951.

Renner, K. E. Delay of reinforcement: A historical


review. Psychological Bulletin, 1964, 61, 341-361.

Resnick, L. B. Programmed instruction and the


teaching of complex intellectual skills: Problems
and prospects. Harvard Educational Review,
1963, 33, 439-471.

Rheingold, H. L., Stanley, W. C., & Doyle, G. A. Visual


and auditory reinforcement of a manipulatory
response in the young child. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 1964, 1,316-326.

Richard, H. C., & Dinoff, M. A follow-up note on “verbal


manipulation in a psychotherapeutic
relationship.” Psychological Reports, 1962, 11,
506.

Rocha e Silva, M. I., & Ferster, C. B. An experiment in


teaching a second language. Sonderdruck aus
IRAL, 1966, 4, 85-113.

Rogers, J. M. Operant conditioning in a quasi-therapy


setting. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1960, 60, 247-252.

Roos, P. Development of an intensive habit-training

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 925
unit at Austin State School. Mental Retardation,
1965, 3, 12-15.

Salzinger, K. Experimental manipulation of verbal


behavior: A review. Journal of General Psychology,
1959, 61, 65-94.

Salzinger, K., Feldman, R. S., Cowan, J. E., & Salzinger,


S. Operant conditioning of verbal behavior of two
young speech-deficient boys. In L. Krasner & L. P.
Ullmann (Eds.), Research in behavior modification.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. pp.
82-105.

Schachter, S. Cognitive effects on bodily functioning:


Studies of obesity and eating. In D. C. Glass (Ed.),
Neurophysiology and emotion. New York:
Rockefeller University Press and Russell Sage
Foundation, 1967, pp. 117-144.

Schaefer, H. H., & Martin, P. L. Behavioral therapy for


“apathy” of hospitalized schizophrenics.
Psychological Reports, 1966, 19, 1147-1158.

Schwartz, A. N., & Hawkins, H. L. Patient models and


affect statements in group therapy. Proceedings of
the 73rd Annual Convention of the A.P.A.
Washington, D. C.: American Psychological
Association, 1965. pp. 265- 266.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 926
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. Patterns of child
rearing. Evanston, Ill.: Row, Peterson, 1957.

Seidel, R. J. A review of sensory preconditioning.


Psychological Bulletin, 1959, 56, 58-73.

Sheffield, F. D., & Roby, T. B. Reward value of a non-


nutritive sweet taste. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1950, 43, 471-81.

Sheffield, F. D., Wulff, J. J., & Backer, R. Reward value of


copulation without sex drive reduction. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
1951,44, 3-8.

Sherman, J. A. Use of reinforcement and imitation to


reinstate verbal behavior in mute psychotics.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 70, 155-
164.

Sidman, M. Tactics of scientific research. New York:


Basic Books, 1960.

Silberman, H. F. Characteristics of some recent studies


of instructional methods. In J. E. Coulsen (Ed.),
Programmed learning and computer-based
instruction. New York: Wiley, 1962. pp. 13-24.

Skinner, B. F. Operant behavior. American


Psychologist, 1963, 18, 503-515.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 927
Slack, C. W. Experimenter-subject psychotherapy: A
new method of introducing intensive office
treatment for unreachable cases. Mental Hygiene,
1960, 44, 238-256.

Staats, A. W. A case in and a strategy for the extension


of learning principles to problems of human
behavior. In L. Krasner & L. P. Ullmann (Eds.),
Research in behavior modification. New York:
Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. pp. 27-55.

Staats, A. W., & Butterfield, W. H. Treatment of


nonreading in a culturally deprived juvenile
delinquent: An application of reinforcement
principles. Child Development, 1965, 36, 925-942.

Staats, A. W., Finley, J. R., Minke, K. A., & Wolf, M.


Reinforcement variables in the control of unit
reading responses. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 1.39-149.

Staats, A. W., Minke, K. A., Finley, J. R., Wolf, M., &


Brooks, L. O. A reinforcer system and
experimental procedure for the laboratory study
of reading acquisition. Child Development, 1964,
35, 209-231.

Staats, A. W., Minke, K. A., Goodwin, W., & Landeen, J.


Cognitive behavior modification: ‘motivated
learning’ reading treatment with subprofessional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 928
therapy-technicians. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1967, 5, 283-299.

Staats, A. W., Staats, C. K., Schutz, R. E., & Wolf, M. M.


The conditioning of textual responses using
“extrinsic” reinforcers. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5, 33-40.

Stanton, A. H., & Schwartz, M. S. The mental hospital.


New York: Basic Books, 1954.

Stevenson, H. W., & Odom, R. D. Visual reinforcement


with children. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 1964, 1, 248-255.

Stolurow, L. M. Programed instruction for the


mentally retarded. Review of Educational
Research, 1963, 33, 126-136.

Straughan, J. The application of operant conditioning


to the treatment of elective mutism. In H. N.
Sloane, Jr., & B. A. MacAulay (Eds.), Operant
procedures in remedial speech and language
training. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1968. pp. 242-
255.

Stuart, R. B. Behavioral control of overeating.


Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 357-
365.

Stunkard, A. J. The management of obesity. New York

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 929
State Journal of Medicine, 1958, 58, 79-87.

Stunkard, A., & Koch, C. The interpretation of gastric


motility. I. Apparent bias in the reports of hunger
by obese persons. Archives of General Psychiatry,
1964, 11,74-82.

Tobias, S. Effects of verbal reinforcement on response


changes in a nonreinforced situation. American
Psychologist, 1960, 15, 390 (Abstract).

Tooley, J. T., & Pratt, S. An experimental procedure for


the extinction of smoking behavior. Psychological
Record, 1967, 17, 209-218.

Truax, C. B. Reinforcement and nonreinforcement in


Rogerian psychotherapy. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 1966, 71, 1-9.

Ullmann, L. P., Krasner, L., & Collins, B. J. Modification


of behavior through verbal conditioning. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961, 62, 128-
132.

Ullmann, L. P., Krasner, L., & Edinger, R. L. Verbal


conditioning of common associations in long-
term schizophrenic patients. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1964, 2, 15-18.

Wahler, R. G. Behavior therapy for oppositional


children: Love is not enough. Paper read at

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 930
Eastern Psychological Association meeting,
Washington, April, 1968.

Wahler, R. G., & Pollio, H. R. Behavior and insight: A


case study in behavior therapy. Journal of
Experimental Research in Personality, 1968, 3, 45-
56.

Wessen, A. F. (Ed.). The psychiatric hospital as a social


system. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas, 1964.

Whalen, R. E. Effects of mounting without


intromission and intromission without
ejaculation on sexual behavior and maze
learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 1961, 54, 409-415.

Whitlock, C., & Bushell, D., Jr. Some effects of “back-


up” reinforcers on reading behavior. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 1967, 5, 50-57.

Williams, R. I. Verbal conditioning in psychotherapy.


American Psychologist, 1959, 14, 388 (Abstract).

Winder, C. L., Ahmad, F. Z., Bandura, A., & Rau, L. C.


Dependency of patients, psychotherapists’
responses, and aspects of psychotherapy. Journal
of Consulting Psychology, 1962, 26, 129-134.

Wolf, M. M., Giles, D. K., & Hall, R. V. Experiments with


token reinforcement in a remedial classroom.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 931
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1968, 6, 51-64.

Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. Analysis and modification of


deviant child behavior. Paper read at the
American Psychological Association meeting,
Washington, September, 1967.

Zeilberger, J., Sampen, S. E., & Sloane, H. N., Jr.


Modification of a child’s problem behaviors in the
home with the mother as therapist. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 47-53.

Zimmerman, D. W. Durable secondary reinforcement.


Psychological Review, 1957, 64, 373-383.

Zimmerman, E. H., & Zimmerman, J. The alteration of


behavior in a special classroom situation. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5,
59-60.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 932
5

Aversive Control
Negative consequences are widely used to
modify behavior, but such practices are generally

disavowed. There are several reasons, some

rationally grounded and others unwarranted, why

punishment is regarded with disfavor. One of the

principal objections to aversive control stems

from the widespread belief that internal, and often


unconscious, forces are the major determinants of
behavior. From this perspective, punishment may

temporarily suppress certain expressions but the


underlying impulses retain their strength and

press continuously for discharge through

alternative actions. Moreover, when punishment is


described in behavior theory as having inhibitory

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 933
or suppressive effects, it also carries the
implication that no expressed response tendencies

remain active at a covert level and, therefore,

require continual counteraction.

Both the immediate and long-term effects of

negative sanctions are viewed with a less


jaundiced eye when behavior is assumed to be

largely determined by its consequences. As long as

a given response pattern creates aversive

outcomes of sufficient strength to override the


effects of other maintaining conditions, it will not

be utilized. If more effective means of securing

desired outcomes are developed during this

period, the behavioral changes initiated through

punishment endure after the aversive


contingencies have been discontinued. Thus, for
example, if competitive behavior is rendered

nonfunctional by arranging adverse consequences

while cooperativeness proves more rewarding,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 934
competitiveness will eventually be abandoned

without any internal energy strains. When the

person is behaving cooperatively it does not mean


that he is actively suppressing a simultaneously

aroused competitive response, any more than a

person who is sitting is continually subduing a

standing response that is simultaneously pressing

for expression.

The use of aversive control is also frequently


questioned on the grounds that it produces a

variety of undesirable by-products. This concern is

warranted, as we shall see later. Many of the


unfavorable effects, however, that are sometimes
associated with punishment are not necessarily

inherent in the methods themselves but result


from the faulty manner in which they are applied.

A great deal of human behavior is, in fact, modified

and closely regulated by natural aversive

contingencies without any ill effects. On the basis

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 935
of negative consequences people learn to avoid or

to protect themselves against hazardous falls,

flaming or scalding objects, deafening sounds, and


other hurtful stimuli; they change their clothing to

remain comfortable in sweltering or frigid

temperatures; and they engage in a considerable

amount of behavior that is supported almost

entirely by removal of irritants. In instances where


certain activities can have injurious effects
aversive contingencies must be socially arranged

to ensure survival. Punishment is rarely indicted

for ineffectiveness or deleterious side effects when

used, for example, to teach young children not to


insert metal objects into electrical outlets, not to

cross busy thoroughfares against red signal lights,


nor to perform behavior that would result in

mutilation. Certain types of negative sanctions, if

applied considerately, can likewise aid in


eliminating self-defeating and socially detrimental

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 936
behavior without creating any special problems.

Punishment is rarely employed as a sole


method for modifying behavior; but if it is used

judiciously in conjunction with other techniques

designed to promote more effective response


options, such combined procedures can hasten the

change process. In addition, aversive

consequences are frequently used to modify


deviant behavior that is automatically self-
reinforcing upon occurrence and in cases where

certain response patterns must be brought rapidly

under control because of their injurious effects

upon the performer or other persons.

The negative consequences may involve either

the removal of positive reinforcers or the

presentation of aversive stimuli. The events in the

latter category may take the form of physical

punishment, shock stimulation, unpleasant

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 937
auditory feedback, or verbal reprimands. In the
former case response costs may be instituted or

persons may be deprived of privileges,


possessions, social contacts, and other positively

reinforcing events to which they are accustomed.

Although both operations represent forms of

punishment, they not only have somewhat


different effects on behavior, but they may

produce different side effects and contrasting

reactions toward prohibiting agents.

Consequently, the research findings and

applications of these two forms of punishment will


be discussed separately.

Presentation of Negative Reinforcers

Theories and research regarding punishment

have been primarily concerned with the extent to


which response patterns can be removed through

direct administration of aversive stimuli. For

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 938
obvious ethical reasons studies of the behavioral

changes produced by punishment of high intensity

or of long duration have been confined to


infrahuman subjects. Laboratory investigations of

the effects of punishment on humans typically

employ either relatively weak physical stimuli or

negative symbolic consequences. In these studies

punishment is applied either to responses that are


concurrently being maintained on an intermittent
schedule of positive reinforcement or to response

patterns that are undergoing extinction.

THEORETICAL CONCEPTIONS OF PUNISHMENT


EFFECTS

When aversive consequences follow upon any

behavior, they generally produce a reduction or

cessation of responsiveness. After the aversive


contingencies have been discontinued the

behavior sometimes reappears, suggesting that


punishment suppresses response tendencies but

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 939
does not eliminate them. Under some conditions,
however, punishment may produce enduring

changes in behavior. As will be shown later, the


degree to which behavior is positively reinforced

is one determinant of both the reductive power of

punishment and the extent to which punished

responses are subsequently reinstated. In addition


to the reward contingencies maintaining the

punished behavior, the effects of punishment may

vary considerably as a function of many other

variables (Azrin & Holz, 1966; Church, 1963;

Solomon, 1964) including the intensity, duration,


frequency, and distribution of aversive

consequences; their temporal relation to the


behavior to be modified; the strength of punished

responses; the availability of alternative


behavioral patterns that are positively

reinforceable; the presence of discriminative


stimuli that signify the probability that a given

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 940
performance will result in adverse consequences;

the level of instigation to perform the negatively

sanctioned behavior; and the characteristics of


punishing social agents. The degree of control

exercised by aversive outcomes over behavior in


any particular case is, therefore, highly dependent
upon a host of other operative variables in

addition to the punishment contingencies.

Several theoretical formulations have been


advanced to account for the varied behavioral

effects produced by different types of punishment.

Conditioned emotionality theories (Estes, 1944),

for example, primarily attribute the effects of


punishment to emotional reactions that are
classically conditioned to environmental stimuli

during the course of aversive treatment. Later,

exposure to the threatening situation generates

emotional arousal that may disrupt or inhibit

responding. Major support for this view comes

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 941
from countless studies demonstrating that

behavior can be suppressed by the presentation of

environmental stimuli that had been previously


paired with aversive experiences. The theory of

punishment advanced by Mowrer (1960) assumes

that negative emotions are conditioned not only to

environmental stimuli but also to proprioceptive

cues generated by the punished behavior itself.

According to competing-response
interpretations (Guthrie, 1935), punishment

produces behavioral changes by eliciting

incompatible responses in the presence of cues


that formerly controlled the punished behavior.
Competing responses presumedly are directly

conditioned by contiguity to stimuli present at the


time of punishment. The generality of this

nonmediational explanation is most seriously

challenged by results of experiments in which

previously neutral stimuli are endowed with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 942
response-inhibiting properties under conditions

where animals are skeletally immobilized by

curare to prevent motor responding (Rescorla &


Solomon, 1967). Nevertheless, in ordinary

circumstances punishment activates instrumental

responses as well as fear; consequently,

punishment can produce markedly different

behaviors under similar levels of emotional


arousal depending upon the types of responses
that it originally evoked (Bolles, 1967). The same

punishing stimulus may thus accelerate or retard

performance of the same behavior depending

upon whether it is applied in such a way as to


evoke responses that are compatible or in conflict

with the ongoing activities (Fowler & Miller,


1963).

Whereas the preceding theory construes

punishment effects in terms of competing

associative learning, avoidance conditioning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 943
explanations (Dinsmoor, 1954) emphasize the

reinforcing consequences that are produced by,

and serve to maintain, incompatible behavior. To


the extent that withdrawal, avoidance, and

response inhibition provide relief from disturbing

stimulation the behavior is thereby reinforced.

The above-mentioned theories emphasize the

motivating, reinforcing, and response-eliciting


capacity of punishment. On the other hand,
discrimination hypotheses (Holz & Azrin, 1961,

1962) highlight the informative function of

punishing events. The inhibitory effects produced


through vicarious punishment, for example, result
in large part from the information conveyed to

observers that certain types of performances are


negatively sanctioned in particular situations

(Bandura, 1965). The discriminative properties of

punishment are also clearly revealed by

experiments in which the temporal order of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 944
punishment, reward, and extinction are

systematically varied. Severe punishments that

regularly precede reward come to signal positive


reinforcement and accelerate responding;

conversely, otherwise weak and ineffective

punishments that precede the removal of rewards

decrease responding. Similarly, punishments that

initiate a period of relief from further painful


stimulation become safety signals that lead to
increased responsiveness (Hendry & Van Toller,

1964).

Further evidence of the way in which


punishing stimuli can acquire markedly different
properties through their relation with other

reinforcing events is strikingly demonstrated by


Sandler and Quagliano (1964). After monkeys

learned to press a lever to avoid being shocked, a

second contingency involving self-administered

painful stimulation was introduced. A lever press

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 945
prevented the occurrence of the original shock,

but it also produced an electric shock of lesser

magnitude. As the experiment progressed the self-


administered shock was gradually increased until

it equalled the aversive stimulus being avoided.

However, the animals showed no reduction in the

frequency of self-punishing lever-pressing

responses though this behavior no longer served


as a “lesser of two evils.” Even more surprising,
after the avoided shock was completely

discontinued but lever-pressing responses (which

had now become objectively functionless) still

produced painful consequences, the animals


continued to punish themselves unnecessarily

with shock intensities that they had previously


worked hard to avoid. Anyone observing the

needless self-injurious behavior of these animals

without knowledge of their prior learning history


would undoubtedly be baffled by their tenacious

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 946
“masochism.”

Findings of a study by Ayllon & Azrin (1966)


identify conditions under which a formerly

punishing stimulus can not only maintain

responding through its informative value, but also


serve as a positive reinforcer to strengthen new

performances. After white noise completely

suppressed a rewarded response in chronic


schizophrenics, the white noise was then
intermittently paired with tokens having reward

value. Later, contingent noise alone maintained

several thousand responses on a new task. The

preceding experiments illustrate how punishing


events can achieve enduring reinforcing functions
that become dissociated from the original

conditions of reinforcement. The resultant self-

punitive behavior, whether maintained through

fear of threats that no longer exist or by

anticipation of occasional rewards that are no

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 947
longer forthcoming, is clearly inappropriate if

viewed in terms of objective reality.

In the material presented above we reviewed


separately the various effects that punishing

stimuli can have on behavior. Most theorists

subscribe to a multi-process conceptualization of

the manner in which punishment achieves

reductive effects and the factors that govern any


subsequent recovery of punished responses.
According to this view, painful stimulation

produces both generalized emotional arousal and

escape- withdrawal responses, which are usually


incompatible with and, therefore, capable of
replacing the ongoing behavior. Any

environmental stimuli and responses that


regularly precede or accompany the aversive

experiences acquire, through their contiguous

association, the capacity to arouse emotional

reactions for some time after punishment has

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 948
been discontinued. In addition to emotional

conditioning, any responses that successfully

terminate or avoid aversive stimulation are


instrumentally reinforced. The punished

responses remain suppressed as long as the

threatening events maintain their capacity to

generate prepotent fear reactions and

incompatible avoidance behavior. This period


provides the opportunity for other modes of
behavior to be established which, if sufficiently

strong, will permanently supplant punished

response tendencies. However, in the absence of

adequately rewarded alternatives, after


punishment has been discontinued, the aversive

properties of the maintaining stimuli may be


extinguished through either repeated exposure to

fear-provoking situations or elicitations of

partially suppressed responses without adverse


consequences, by observation of nonpunished

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 949
performances, or by other informative means.

Under these types of conditions inhibited behavior

will eventually reappear.

LOCUS OF AVERSIVE CONTROL

As noted in the preceding discussion several

alternative interpretations have been proposed

concerning the source of the controlling stimuli for


inhibitory processes created through punishment.
According to one explanation, control primarily

resides in environmental stimuli. That is, if a

person regularly undergoes punishing experiences

in a particular situation, the environmental cues,


through their endowed emotion-provoking

properties, produce either generalized

suppression of behavior, avoidance of the

threatening situational events, or activities

designed to prevent their recurrence.

A second interpretation assumes that aversive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 950
control resides in the punished behavior itself.
When punishing consequences are made

contingent upon the occurrence of certain types of

responses, it is assumed that proprioceptive cues


accompanying the responses acquire the capacity

to arouse conditioned emotional reactions.


Aversive stimulation generated by initiation of
previously punished behavior prevents

completion of the action sequence. Response

inhibition is believed to be reinforced by the


termination of response-produced emotional

arousal. Thus, in the latter case, individuals learn

to avoid self-generated distress by suppressing the

negatively valenced behavior.

A number of investigators have compared the


suppressive power of punishment when it is
administered either on a response-contingent

basis or in conjunction with specific

environmental stimuli independently of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 951
ongoing behavior. These studies reveal that both

types of aversive contingencies decrease

responding, but there is disagreement as to which


method produces greater behavioral suppression.

In the original experiment, Estes (1944) found

that animals which had been shocked only at times

when they were not engaging in lever-pressing

behavior displayed essentially the same degree of


suppression and subsequent recovery of lever-
pressing responses as subjects whose punishment

was strictly contingent upon the occurrence of the

response. In a further test of whether suppressive

effects are governed by environmental stimuli or


response- produced cues, animals whose lever-

pressing responses had been punished were left in


the situation for an adaptation period with the

lever removed. This arrangement prevented

elicitation of the punished response but permitted


the neutralization of threatening situational cues.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 952
A subsequent test for extinction revealed that

mere re-exposure to the fear-provoking situation

with no further unpleasant experiences resulted in


almost complete extinction of conditioned

suppression. The findings of this study thus


suggested that internal cues accompanying the
punished response exercised relatively little

influence upon the inhibitory process.

Hunt & Brady (1955) extended the above


research in a comparative study of the influence of

response-contingent and stimulus-correlated

punishment upon the acquisition, generalization,

and extinction of conditioned suppression of


responses that were intermittently rewarded. For
subjects in the stimulus group, shocks were

contiguously associated with a tone, but lever-

pressing responses were never punished; on the

other hand, in the response condition, the tone

was presented and animals were shocked only

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 953
when they pressed the lever in the presence of the

auditory stimulus. Both procedures resulted in

almost complete response suppression whenever


the tone was presented. The method designed

specifically to endow the environmental cue with

aversive properties, however, produced greater

emotional disturbance, and greater generalized

inhibition that was more resistant to extinction.


Essentially identical results were obtained in an
earlier study (Hunt & Brady, 1951) even though

subjects in the response-contingent treatment

received more shocks. In a well-designed

experiment that equated for the number and


temporal distribution of shocks, Hoffman &

Fleshier (1965) found that animals that were


punished only if they responded in the presence of

certain cues displayed less behavioral suppression

and extinguished more rapidly than their


counterparts that were punished during

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 954
presentations of the same cues without regard to

their behavior. The foregoing results thus provide

evidence that, under certain conditions,


inhibitions are primarily situation- rather than

response-bound.

At variance with the above conclusion, Azrin

(1956) found that response-produced punishment

was considerably more effective than


noncontingent aversive stimulation in suppressing
rewarded behavior. Azrin attributes the

conflicting results to the fact that subjects in his

experiment received severe shocks over an

extended time, whereas previous studies had


employed relatively brief periods of punishment.
This interpretation does not fully explain the

diverse results, because Boe & Church (1967)

report that response-contingent punishment is

more suppressive than noncontingent shock even

when administered during a brief period.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 955
Moreover, Camp (1965), employing a wide range

of shock intensities, found that the suppressive

effects of response-contingent punishment were


greater than the inhibitory effects of

noncontingent punishment, but the differences

between the two procedures were relatively small.

Response inhibition is most likely to be highly

situation-bound when aversive experiences occur


repeatedly in the same setting. If, on the other
hand, selected responses are punished in the

presence of specific stimuli but incur no negative

consequences within the same environment when


other factors are absent, then omnipresent stimuli
are less likely to become endowed with strong

suppressive power. Under natural conditions, of


course, punishment is seldom contingent upon

behavior alone; nor are people invariably

punished in certain situations. Rather, the same

behavior may be permissible or punishable in the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 956
same settings depending upon the person toward

whom the behavior is directed, the role occupied

by the performer, the time and specific


circumstances under which the behavior is

exhibited, the instigating conditions, and many

other factors.

Peripheral theories of response inhibition that

emphasize skeletal responding and its attendant


proprioceptive feedback cannot adequately
account for the highly discriminative manner in

which the same responses are freely performed or

suppressed under slightly differing circumstances.


Regulation of human behavior on the basis of
punishing experiences is undoubtedly mediated to

a considerable degree through central


mechanisms. The major theoretical issues and

supporting evidence concerning symbolic control

of responsiveness, which constitutes a third locus

of aversive control, is presented in the final

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 957
chapter of this book. This view assumes that, on

the basis of information conveyed through prior

response consequences experienced in


conjunction with different situational, temporal,

and social cues, individuals infer the probability

that a given course of action will be ignored,

rewarded, or punished. Behavior is then partly

guided by anticipatory consequences that are


symbolically produced. That is, anticipatory
aversive consequences will have response-

inhibiting effects, whereas anticipation of

rewarding outcomes will facilitate performance of

the same behavior. In addition to the influence of


expectant outcomes, self-evaluative consequences

also figure prominently in the self-regulation of


behavior.

A number of studies employing punishing

operations have been conducted with children in

an effort to elucidate internalization processes. In

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 958
a comprehensive monograph, Aronfreed (1968)

conceptualizes internalization as essentially a

process in which aversive or pleasurable affective


states become attached to both response-

produced cues and their cognitive

representations. This outcome is presumably

achieved on the basis of classical conditioning of

anxiety to the proprioceptive stimuli inherent in


the action itself and, through the temporal
gradient of reinforcement, to its behavioral and

cognitive precursors. Thereafter, stimuli

accompanying preparatory movements, or even

intentions, arouse anxiety which in turn prevents


further responding. Behavioral suppression is self-

reinforced by the resultant reduction in anxiety.


Thus, according to this view, behavior becomes

internalized or self-maintained when changes in

affective states, which constitute intrinsic


reinforcing consequences, are mediated by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 959
response-produced cues.

Empirical tests of the affective feedback theory


of internalization have primarily investigated

behavioral suppression as a function of timing of

punishment. In the typical experimental paradigm,


on each of a series of trials children are asked to

choose between a highly attractive toy and one

which is relatively uninviting. In one condition the


experimenter verbally reprimands children as
soon as they make a motion toward the attractive

toy; in other treatments children are verbally

rebuked only after they have picked up the toy and

handled it for varying periods of time. In the test


for internalized behavioral suppression children
are presented with a highly attractive and an

uninteresting toy and it is noted whether or not

they touch the attractive object during the

experimenter’s absence. The rationale for

selecting the temporal variable is that punishment

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 960
at the initiation of a transgression is expected to

attach maximal anxiety to cues that accompany

preparatory responses; as a result, behavior is


suppressed in its early stages. On the other hand, if

punishment is administered following

transgression, anxiety and its attendant inhibitory

responses are not evoked until after the

disapproved act has been completed.

Before evaluating the main findings of these


studies it should be noted that tests of internalized

behavioral control typically involve so many

external stimulus supports that response


suppression cannot be attributed solely to
intrinsically mediated consequences. Post-training

measures of self-control are characteristically


obtained by the same experimenter, during the

same experimental session, in the same

experimental room, in which children are

presented either identical or similar stimulus

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 961
objects possessing the same relational properties

as the stimuli utilized during training. Although

the punishing agent absents himself from the


room, nevertheless many controlling external cues

remain. It would, in fact, be of interest to study the

progressive decrease in self-controlling responses

as progressively more cues common to the

punishment situation are removed.

Results of several experiments based on this


procedure (Aronfreed, 1968; Aronfreed & Reber,

1965; Benton, 1967; Walters, Parke, & Cane, 1965)

show that children who have been punished early


in the response sequence deviate less frequently
than children punished only after completion of

the transgression. Moreover, the longer that


punishment is withheld while children are

engaging in the deviant behavior, the weaker the

subsequent behavioral suppression. These

findings have been interpreted as providing strong

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 962
evidence that behavior is regulated by affective

consequences conditioned to sequential intrinsic

stimulus correlates of a punished act. Results of


animal experiments investigating the effects of

delay of punishment on resistance to temptation

(Solomon, Turner, & Lessac, 1968) are also

frequently cited as corroborating the intrinsic

mediation of behavioral suppression.

Findings yielded by the timing-of-punishment


experiments cannot be meaningfully interpreted

with respect to the affective feedback hypotheses

because, among other methodological problems,


the timing manipulations involve a confounding of
variables. In the early punishment condition

transgressive behavior produces only punishment,


whereas in delayed conditions the behavior

receives both immediate reward and subsequent

punishment. The suppressive effect of instant

reward followed some time later by punishment is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 963
much weaker than punishment alone. In the study

conducted by Solomon, for example, food-

deprived puppies that were swatted by the


experimenter as they touched forbidden food

exhibited stronger resistance to deviation than

puppies that were punished shortly after they had

begun to consume the palatable horsemeat. The

difference in response inhibition, which has been


often attributed by other authors to the
attachment of anxiety at different points in the

sequence of response-produced cues, simply

demonstrates that a response is more effectively

inhibited by punishment alone than if it is initially


rewarded and then punished. Confounding of

reward and punishment effects, as Solomon points


out, is advantageous for understanding natural

socialization practices, but the data are of limited

value for elucidating intrinsic conditioning


processes.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 964
It is likewise difficult to ascertain whether

similar differences obtained in investigations cited

earlier derive from sequential aversive


conditioning of response-produced cues, or from

the fact that in late-punishment conditions


children are rewarded by being allowed to play
briefly with a highly attractive toy before the

reprimand is administered. As might be expected

from the response-enhancing effects of positive


reinforcement, the longer children are allowed to

engage in the rewarding transgressive behavior

the weaker is the subsequent punishment. On the


other hand, when children are punished almost

immediately upon committing the deviant act


(Parke & Walters, 1967; Walters & Demkow,

1963), differences in response suppression

between early-punished and late-punished


subjects become quite small and, for the most part,

statistically insignificant. The latter negative

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 965
results have been attributed to insufficient

separation of sequential stimulus components in

early- and late-punishment conditions, but an

alternative interpretation may be cast in terms of


curtailment of confounding rewarding effects
which occur under delayed punishment.

It also seems highly probable that a person

who reprimands a child even before a deviant act


actually occurs would be viewed as considerably
more forbidding and punitive than one who shows

no disapproval until after the child has engaged in

the transgressive behavior for some time. Indeed,

in the latter case, the disciplinarian must appear to


be a more lenient, or at least an inconsistent,
person. Early-punished children may therefore be

reluctant to transgress not because of attachment

of affective states to incipient response-produced

cues, but rather due to stronger anticipatory fears

evoked cognitively by the stricter disciplinarian.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 966
In addition to the methodological problems

considered above, the type of discrimination task

employed in most timing-of-punishment


experiments is not the most suitable one for

determining the extent to which response


suppression is mediated by its proprioceptive
cues. This is because selection of both attractive

and unattractive objects involves virtually

identical reaching responses, and therefore


arousal of the same pattern of interoceptive

stimulation. Consequently, to the extent that

anxiety is conditioned to cues inherent in the


response itself, the act of reaching for the

unattractive object should be equally suppressed.


Demonstration of proprioceptive stimulus control

of behavior would require subjects to make

distinctive responses, each producing dissimilar


patterns of internal stimulation which would have

been associated with differential consequences.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 967
Moreover, in order to assess what contribution, if

any, is made by proprioceptively aroused affects

to response suppression, it would be necessary to


measure the independent effects of cognitive

discriminations and symbolically generated


affective consequences. By analogy with the
timing-of-punishment experiments, a person

reaching for a repulsive snake and for a savory

dessert would in all likelihood experience aversive


and pleasurable emotional states, respectively.

Since, however, the reaching responses in both

instances produce essentially identical patterns of


interoceptive stimulation, the resultant affective

states must be centrally, rather than peripherally,


mediated.

For reasons given above it is doubtful that

timing-of-reinforcement paradigms can provide

decisive evidence concerning sensory feedback

theories of internalization. However, curare

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 968
conditioning experiments—in which animals are

skeletally immobilized during aversive

conditioning or extinction—conducted by
Solomon and his associates (Black, 1958; Black,

Carlson, & Solomon, 1962; Solomon & Turner,

1962), shed considerable light on the issue of

central or peripheral mediation of inhibitory

response tendencies. The findings demonstrate


that conditioned emotional responses can be
readily acquired and extinguished independently

of skeletal responding and its correlated

proprioceptive feedback. It would appear from the

overall findings of the series of curare


experiments that when approach responses

toward certain discriminable stimulus objects are


undergoing punishment, the external stimuli also

acquire the capacity to elicit conditioned

emotional responses which can, in turn, control


instrumental responses quite independently of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 969
response-correlated feedback mechanisms.

The role of central processes in behavioral


inhibition could be established through traditional

procedures by including training conditions in

which subjects merely verbalize their object


choices and are punished whenever they elect

disapproved items without performing any

reaching responses. If children, in these conditions


displayed a degree of response suppression equal
to that of subjects whose motor choice responses

were punished, then the findings would indicate

that proprioceptive feedback does not enter into

the regulatory process.

This issue can also be easily investigated by

comparing the inhibitory effects of punishment

when administered to the responding as opposed

to the nonresponding part of the body. In an

experiment reported by Kaufman (1964), adults

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 970
participated in a series of sessions that consisted
of alternating periods of punished and unpunished

responding. In half the punishment periods shock


was delivered to the performing hand, while in the

other half the nonresponding hand was shocked.

At low and high intensities, variations in the locus

of punishment had no differential effects, but at


moderate intensities shock applied to the

responding hand produced slightly more

suppression. Some further evidence that response

inhibition is primarily a cognitively controlled

phenomenon is that under severe punishment


subjects completely suppressed responding but

the same responses were performed at a stable


high rate during nonshock periods in the same

session.

Another effective means of assessing whether

response-produced cues assume controlling


functions through affective conditioning is to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 971
include observing partners in punishment

paradigms. This procedure was, in fact, employed

by Benton (1967). While groups of children were


verbally reprimanded for either approaching or

handling tabooed toys in a two-choice

discrimination task, their matched partners simply

observed the punished performances. In a

subsequent test for transgression, the observers


showed the same amount of response inhibition as
the performers whose motor responding was

repeatedly punished.

In Aronfreed’s (1968) theory of internalization,


affective states become affixed not only to
proprioceptive cues but also, through backward

generalization, to intentions and cognitive


representations of the punished act. No empirical

investigations have as yet been conducted in

which verbalized intentions alone are punished.

However, there is some evidence, which is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 972
reviewed in Chapter 8, demonstrating that

punishment of imaginal representations of deviant

sexual activities is associated with a reduction in


the corresponding behavior. Aronfreed has shown

that if during punishment children are informed

that selection of the forbidden attractive toys is

disapproved because their functions are difficult

to describe and they are therefore only


appropriate for older children, subjects are later
less inclined to violate the prohibition. It is

difficult to evaluate without additional

assessments whether such instructions facilitate

compliant behavior because they “inject a


cognitive structure” or for other reasons. One

might expect, for example, punishments that are


arbitrarily administered to generate more

resentment and oppositional behavior than when

the basis for the negative sanctions is


considerately explained (Pastore, 1952). Under

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 973
conditions where the basis for punishment is

prominently conveyed by the physical attributes

of the forbidden objects, the role of cognitive


functions is considerably reduced. On the other

hand, when the factors determining whether given


performances will incur punishment are complex
and not easily distinguishable, statement of the

contingency rules governing punishment would be

expected to facilitate self-regulation of


responsiveness.

In evaluating the role of cognitive processes in

behavioral control it is essential to distinguish

between the effects of cognitive representation of


responses themselves and symbolically generated
anticipatory consequences. In theories assuming

that emotion-arousing properties are directly

conditioned to responses, the initiation of

negatively valenced behavior or its cognitive

equivalents is presumed automatically to evoke

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 974
negative affects that actuate response inhibition.

An alternative conceptualization of the control

mechanisms is that performances are internally


regulated not by emotions directly affixed to the

behavior but rather by anticipated aversive

consequences. Given different expected outcomes,

the same behavior may be inhibited or freely

expressed, which could not occur if emotional


arousal were directly response-cued.

Several studies that have been conducted

within the framework of dissonance theory also

raise an issue that is relevant to aversive control.


It is assumed in this formulation that if a person is
provided with insufficient justification for his

behavior, the resultant inconsistent cognitions


generate an aversive state that can be reduced by,

among other means, devaluating the activity.

When an individual refrains from transgressing

because of a severe threat, he presumably has

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 975
adequate excuse for his compliant behavior and,

therefore, continues to value the tabooed activities

highly. On the other hand, if he displays response


inhibition following a mild threat, he may, to

eliminate disturbing dissonance created by the

incongruous behavior, convince himself that the

desired objects are less worthwhile.

Aronson & Carlsmith (1963) tested the above


notion by having children rate their second-
ranked toy after each of three interventions. In the

phase involving mild threat the experimenter

informed children that he would be annoyed if


they played with the forbidden toy; in the strong
threat condition children were told that if they

played with the toy the experimenter would be


very angry and that he would take all his toys,

never to return again; while in the control phase,

the experimenter simply removed the crucial toy

to determine whether its value may be enhanced

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 976
by being singled out for attention. Both threats

produced complete behavioral compliance in all

children, suggesting that the presence of other


attractive toys, including the unprohibited most

preferred item, greatly weakened instigation to

transgression. With respect to toy preferences,

following mild threat 36 percent of the children

decreased their liking for the forbidden toy,


whereas none of the children decreased their
rating of the tabooed object following severe

threat or physical removal of the items. Although

threats varying in severity affected preference

ratings, it remains to be demonstrated whether


one could produce negative valuations of desired

activities by threats alone, regardless of their


severity.

Punishment is usually applied with the intent

of creating behavioral controls that will endure in

appropriate situations even when punishing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 977
agents may no longer be present. Preference

changes alone are therefore of limited significance

unless it is also demonstrated that such changes


influence subsequent self-controlling behavior. In

a well-designed experiment by Freedman (1965)

groups of children were threatened with either

mild or severe punishment for touching an

attractive but forbidden toy. Half the subjects in


each treatment were provided with a brief period
free of surveillance during which they could

transgress without risk of punishment, whereas

the prohibiting experimenter remained in the

room with the other half of the subjects. Virtually


none of the children in any of the groups violated

the prohibition. Immediately after the session they


re-rated their liking for the different toys and,

approximately a month later, they were given an

opportunity under permissive conditions to play


with the forbidden object. Transgressive behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 978
was lowest among children who received the mild

threat and refrained from deviating in the absence

of social surveillance. Children who were


threatened with severe punishment and complied

in the absence of the experimenter were more


inclined to handle the previously forbidden toy
and did not differ in this respect from subjects in

the condition combining low threat and social

surveillance. The results are somewhat difficult to


explain in terms of dissonance reduction because,

contrary to expectation and the findings of

Aronson & Carlsmith, the forbidden toy was not


devalued any more under mild sanctions than

under threats of severe punishment. The author


interprets the discrepant findings as indicating

that subjects did not select devaluation of the

forbidden object as the principal means of


dissonance reduction in this particular

experiment. This explanation may have some

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 979
validity, but it raises questions as to whether the

hypothesized dissonance processes could ever be

refuted empirically.

Results of the preceding study differ from a

large body of evidence based on experiments with


animals (Azrin & Holz, 1966; Church, 1963),

children (Parke & Walters, 1967), and adults

(Powell & Azrin, 1968; Rotenberg, 1959)


demonstrating that the degree of response
reduction is an increasing function of the intensity

of punishment. Mild punishment generally

produces little change in positively reinforced

performances; at intermediate levels aversive


consequences have partially suppressive effects;
while intense punishment typically results in large

and stable reductions in behavior.

The conflicting evidence may be interpreted in

several ways. In the above experiments, punishing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 980
consequences were actually administered
contingent upon occurrence of transgressive

behavior, whereas the dissonance studies involved


a single verbal threat of punishment. A second and

more critical difference concerns the type of

behavior that is being controlled. In the

dissonance paradigm approach responses are


inhibited toward one of several positive

alternatives. Under these advantageous conditions

the instigation to transgression is apparently so

weak that a mild verbal threat is sufficient to

produce compliance in all subjects regardless of


whether the prohibiting agent is present or

absent. Given a response tendency of any strength,


transgressive behavior is ordinarily performed

more frequently in situations free of social


surveillance than when the disapproving agent is

physically present (Hicks, 1968). By contrast, in


studies of aversive control investigators either

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 981
select responses that are highly resistant to

change, or the behavior to be eliminated is

increased in strength and concurrently maintained


on a favorable schedule of positive reinforcement.

It is precisely under these types of circumstances


that the punishment is employed in everyday life.
In applications of aversive procedures one would

employ the minimum negative sanctions needed

to achieve adequate behavioral control.


Thereafter, control is usually maintained by

occasional negative consequences in weaker and

largely symbolic forms.

POTENTIAL PROBLEMS ARISING FROM AVERSIVE


CONTROL

Because of the varied and complex effects of

punishment, aversive control, particularly when


socially mediated, must be employed with care
and skill in programs of behavioral change. Many

of the undesirable consequences that may

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 982
accompany punishment are to some extent
preventable. Some of these common by-products,

and ways of minimizing them, are discussed next.

Generalization of Conditioned Inhibition.

Punishment is most often employed to reduce the

incidence of recurrent disturbing patterns of


behavior. The effects of aversive consequences,
however, are not specifically confined to the

responses that are negatively sanctioned. Severe


punishments, particularly if applied over a long

time, can result in broad generalization of

suppressive effects to socially desirable patterns

of behavior. Thus, for example, repeated harsh

punishment of aggression may not only eventually


eliminate the troublesome behavior, but stifle

assertiveness as well. The extent of generalized


inhibition arising from contingent punishment

varies inversely with the degree of similarity of


the new situations to those of the original aversive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 983
training (Desiderato, 1964; Hoffman & Fleshier,

1961; Honig & Slivka, 1964). Large segments of

behavior are therefore most likely to be adversely


affected under conditions where punishment

contingencies are ambiguous, or where the

negative sanctions are applied to a wide range of

social responses in diverse settings.

Although at first conditioned inhibition


transfers broadly, if further punishment is
discontinued the generalization gradient gradually

narrows until eventually the subject suppresses

responding only to the stimulus context in which


his behavior was punished. Nevertheless, a study
by Hoffman, Fleshier & Jensen (1963) suggests

that some aftereffects may persist. When animals


were subjected to unrelated emotional stress

three years after behavioral suppression was

almost completely extinguished, the animals again

exhibited substantial inhibition in the presence of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 984
the formerly aversive stimuli. These cues not only

partly regained, under general emotional arousal,

their previous capacity to suppress behavior, but


the original conditioned stimuli retained increased

potency for some time after termination of the

stress condition. Evidently, punishments

administered at an early period may sensitize the

organism to formerly aversive stimuli so that their


suppressive power is easily reinstated, at least
temporarily, by stressful experiences arising from

other sources.

Inappropriate or excessive generalization of


inhibitions and sensitivities can be easily
prevented by the use of discrimination

procedures, as demonstrated by Hoffman &


Fleshier (1964). During the development of

conditioned suppression animals were punished

for responding in the presence of a 1000 cycles

per second (cps) tone, but response to the adjacent

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 985
900 cps tone was never accompanied by shock.

Subsequent tests for generalization revealed that,

whereas generalization tones at or above 1000 cps


had strong suppressive effects, the discrimination

training produced very little behavioral inhibition

to stimuli on the lower side of the gradient (Figure

5-1). Moreover, when the animals were placed

under emotional stress after inhibitions were


largely extinguished, response suppression was
markedly enhanced in the presence of 1000 cps

and louder tones, but rate of responding to

auditory stimuli of 900 cps or lower remained

essentially unaffected. As depicted graphically in


Figure 5-2, discrimination procedures both

sharply curtailed the spread of inhibitory effects


and prevented certain generalized stimuli from

acquiring and retaining latent suppressive power

that could subsequently be reinstated by


unrelated stressful experiences.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 986
Figure 5-1. Generalization of response suppression exhibited
by subjects after discrimination training during which a
1000 cps tone was paired with shock but a 900 cps tone
was never accompanied by shock. A ratio of 1.0
indicates complete suppression. Plotted from data of
Hoffman & Fleshier, 1964.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 987
Figure 5-2. Effects of emotional stress on the generalization
of response suppression produced by prior
discrimination training. A ratio of 1.0 represents
complete suppression. Hoffman & Fleshier, 1964.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 988
It would follow from the above findings that a

change agent who wishes to restrict the range and

direction of behavioral suppression should not


merely apply negative sanctions to undesirable

response patterns but should also arrange


different consequences for related forms of
behavior in different social contexts. For instance,

physically assaultive behavior may be punished

but desirable assertiveness rewarded. In addition


to selective reinforcement, discrimination is

greatly facilitated by the use of verbal aids. By

clearly labeling the modes of behavior that are


permissible and those that are punishable, and by

specifying the times and places at which certain


courses of action are appropriate or unsuitable,

greater specificity of punishment effects can be

ensured.

Emotional Conditioning. Another possible

accompaniment of aversive control, which has

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 989
been discussed at length in earlier sections, is the

capacity of punishment to endow formerly neutral

stimulus events with emotion-provoking


properties. Any behavior that serves to avoid,

remove, or postpone threatening stimuli reduces

emotional distress and is thereby automatically

reinforced even though the punishment

contingencies may no longer be in effect. These


inadvertently established fear elicitors often give
rise to avoidant patterns of behavior capable of

creating their own maintaining conditions. The

resulting avoidant responses may be more socially

undesirable than the behavior that punishment


was originally intended to reduce and, once

established, they may be considerably more


difficult to eliminate.

As we shall see later, not all forms of

punishment create conditioned emotional arousal.

Fear learning is most likely to accompany

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 990
procedures based on the social presentation of

painful stimuli. Negative sanctions involving

chiefly the removal of positive reinforcers


generally do not have fear-arousing effects. By

careful choice of punishment procedures one can

prevent or minimize the occurrence of undesirable

emotional conditioning.

Behavioral Inflexibility. In many instances


change agents are faced with the task of
discouraging patterns of behavior that are not

only permitted but expected at some later period

of life. Such problems are most likely to occur


when marked discontinuities exist in cultural
demands, as in the case of sexual behavior. Thus, a

child who has been severely punished for all


expressions of sexual curiosity may be rendered

anxious about sex and remain sexually inhibited in

later life when such behavior is socially approved

and expected of him. When marked temporal or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 991
situational discontinuities exist, the use of social

training procedures that result in rigid and

inflexible behavior are contraindicated.

It is apparent from the results of an

experiment by Whiting & Mowrer (1943) that,

depending upon the manner in which it is

employed, punishment may be most efficient in

inhibiting behavior at the expense of later


adaptability to changing circumstances. Using a
socialization paradigm, Whiting & Mowrer

employed three methods to train animals to select

an effortful, circuitous route to food reward


instead of a considerably easier and direct path.
Whenever the easy route was chosen during

training one group of animals encountered a


physical barrier, the second group was denied

reward, while the third group was administered

an electric shock. The punished animals

abandoned the short passage most rapidly but

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 992
they also persisted longest on the effortful

tortuous route after the original negative

sanctions were removed. Lest the reader conclude


that behavioral rigidity is an unavoidable

concomitant of aversive control, it should be

emphasized that punishment combined with

discrimination training would likewise have

resulted in appropriately flexible responsiveness.


If, for example, short cuts were shocked in the
presence of a red light, but rewarded whenever

the light was absent, the animals would

undoubtedly have acquired discriminative

inhibitions and reverted rapidly to the easy route


after discontinuance of the socialization

contingency was signalled by permanent removal


of the environmental cue signifying punishment.

The transitory nature of behavioral

suppression induced through punishment,

frequently noted in laboratory studies, is not

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 993
surprising in view of the short periods during

which punishment is typically applied. However,

one finds limited but consistent evidence


(Solomon, 1964) that under certain conditions

even exceedingly brief punishment can result in

profound autonomic disturbances and lasting

inhibitions. A few shocks administered to an

animal during a consummatory response may


produce extremely powerful feeding inhibitions
(Lichtenstein, 1950), often leading to self-

starvation in the midst of plentiful food supplies

(Appel, 1961; Masserman, 1943). The factors

responsible for such extremely rapid and enduring


inhibitory learning have not as yet been identified,

but the timing of punishment appears to be an


influential variable. It is not possible, from the

limited data available, to determine whether the

extraordinary suppressive power of well-timed


punishment is primarily confined to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 994
consummatory responses in subhuman species.

One would need to exercise considerable caution

in the use of aversive procedures if the latter


phenomena were replicated with instrumental

behavior performed by humans.

Avoidance of Punishing Agents and Situations.

With few exceptions, in laboratory investigations

of punishment effects, animals are confined to the


apparatus or their freedom of movement is
otherwise physically limited so that no escape

from the experimental situation is possible. Nor

are the subjects at liberty to choose whether they

will return to situations in which their behavior is


punished, or to cut short their stay whenever they
are displeased with inhospitable treatment

inflicted by their hosts. In naturalistic situations,

however, persons can generally avoid or restrict,

to some extent, contact with punishing agents and

social settings in which negative sanctions are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 995
frequently applied (Azrin, 1958; Powell & Azrin,

1968). For this reason, even if punishment proved

to be highly effective in controlling behavior, it


might be of limited value under circumstances

where unpleasant events could be easily avoided.

The few studies permitting escape from

situations containing some aversive features

reveal that subjects will repeatedly withdraw to


nonpunitive situations even though the places of
refuge are less rewarding (Azrin, Halse, Holz, &

Hutchinson, 1965), require greater expenditure of

effort for a given reward (Dardano & Sauerbrunn,


1964), or offer no positive reinforcement at all
(Hearst & Sidman, 1961). Apparently withdrawal

from partly aversive situations is sufficiently


reinforcing to outweigh the effects of relatively

unfavorable rewarding conditions brought about

by escape behavior. Punishments at relatively low

intensities, although ineffective in suppressing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 996
behavior, will nevertheless drive the subject out of

the situation (Azrin et al., 1965). This illustrates

the limitations of aversive controls when applied


in the absence of constraints or attractions to

check withdrawal.

To the extent that negative sanctions foster

fear and active avoidance of change agents, their

opportunities to influence the behavior of others is


reduced. This is a particularly serious hindrance
when widespread changes in attitudes and social

behavior are brought about most effectively

through unforced modeling. With restricted social


contact, there can be little identificatory learning.
It should not be assumed from these comments

that punishment invariably reduces spontaneous


modeling. Risley (1968) has shown that in the case

of children who are so preoccupied with bizarre,

self-injurious activities that they remain oblivious

to social stimuli, elimination of the hindering

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 997
behavior through punishment increases their

attentiveness and responsiveness to modeling

cues.

Escape tendencies can, of course, be

counteracted to some degree by increasing the

reward value of environments in which

punishments are periodically administered, and

by enhancing the positive valence of change


agents. A high level of positive reinforcement may
create psychological barriers against withdrawal

so strong that persons will remain in situations

that have punishing aspects as well. It should also


be noted that punishment based upon removal of
positive reinforcers, though temporarily

frustrating, nevertheless tends to maintain, and


may even increase, approach tendencies toward

change agents.

Negative Modeling. In social applications of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 998
aversive control one must also consider the
modeling function of punishing behavior. In many

instances modeling cues furnished by disciplinary


actions are inconsistent with, and therefore

contravene, the effects of direct training. If, for

example, a parent punishes his child physically for

having struck a playmate, the intended outcome of


the punishment is that the child should refrain

from hitting others. Concomitantly with the

intentional training, however, the parent is

unwittingly providing vivid examples of the very

behavior that he is attempting to reduce in the


child. From fear of retaliation, the child may not

counter-aggress in his parent’s presence, but he


may nevertheless model his behavior after that of

the parent when the child must himself cope with


or control the behavior of others.

Consistent with modeling theory, Hoffman


(1960) found that mothers who forced compliance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 999
with their demands through the use of power-

assertive techniques had children who exhibited

aggressive power-assertiveness in controlling the


behavior of their peers. Although the direction of

the causal relationship cannot be unequivocally

established from these data, results based on

laboratory studies (Bandura, 1965) clearly

demonstrate that aggressive patterns of behavior


can be readily transmitted through adult
modeling. In an experiment referred to earlier,

Mischel & Liebert (1966) found that negative

sanctions imposed on children were less effective

when the agent’s modeling behavior was


inconsistent than when the imposed and modeled

standards of behavior were congruent. These


findings indicate that anyone attempting to

control specific troublesome responses should

avoid modeling punitive forms of behavior that


may not only counteract the effects of direct

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1000
training but also increase the probability that on

future occasions the individual may respond to

interpersonal thwarting in an imitative manner.

EFFECTS OF PUNISHMENT ON CONCURRENTLY


REINFORCED BEHAVIOR

Punishment is frequently resorted to as a

method of social control when the positive


reinforcements maintaining troublesome behavior
cannot be identified, or, if known, cannot be

readily modified. It is considerably easier, though

less effective, for example, to punish the antisocial


behavior of delinquents than to remove the

subcultural contingencies that mold and control

their actions.

In most of the research reviewed earlier,

punishment was applied to responses after the


rewards maintaining them were removed, in order

to determine whether the addition of aversive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1001
consequences accelerated the extinction process.
Of considerably greater significance are

investigations of the effects of punishment on

behavior that is concurrently maintained by


positive reinforcement, since the response

patterns that people frequently attempt to modify


result in some rewarding outcomes for the
performer. The available evidence generally

indicates that punishment does not have enduring

reductive effects on behavior that is


simultaneously being maintained by a favorable

schedule of positive reinforcement. Mild and

moderately punishing stimuli typically reduce the

occurrence of intermittently reinforced behavior,

but as punishment is continued subjects adapt to


the aversive consequences and exhibit some
recovery of responsiveness even while the

punishment contingency is still in effect (Azrin,

1959, 1960; Holz, Azrin, & Ulrich, 1963). Parents

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1002
who make frequent use of aversive controls would

undoubtedly provide corroborative testimonials

that, after a time, their punishments cease to have


much effect on their children’s behavior.

Severe punishment produces marked


reduction of intermittently reinforced behavior

with little or no recovery as long as responses

continue to incur aversive consequences. High


intensities of punishment are required, however,
to maintain behavioral suppression if punishment

is administered only intermittently, rewarded

alternatives do not exist, the behavior is strongly

established, and is concurrently supported by


highly favorable conditions of reinforcement
(Azrin & Holz, 1966; Boe, 1964).

PUNISHMENT AND AVAILABILITY OF ALTERNATIVE


MODES OF RESPONSE

Severe punishing consequences would have to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1003
be applied over a long period to reduce behavior
effectively in persons who, because of their

restricted behavioral repertoires, possess few

alternative means for securing positive


reinforcement. The effects of response availability

on the suppressive power of punishment are


clearly illustrated in a laboratory experiment
conducted by Mowrer (1940), in which animals

were taught two modes of adjustment to an

aversive situation. One group first learned to sit on


the hind legs in order to reduce the intensity of

shock stimulation. After this response was

acquired, conditions were altered so that the

subjects could turn the shock off completely by

striking a pedal. Since the latter competing


response was considerably more effective it soon
became strongly established. A second group of

subjects learned only the pedal-pressing response.

When pedal-pressing was subsequently punished

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1004
in both groups, the animals that had learned a

single response continued to perform the

punished behavior for the entire duration of the


experiment, whereas all but one of the animals

that had available two different modes of


adjustment quickly reverted to the earlier pattern
of behavior.

Azrin and his associates (Herman & Azrin,


1964; Holz, Azrin, & Ayllon, 1963) similarly
demonstrated with adult psychotics that

punishment was generally ineffective when the

punished response constituted the sole means of

securing positive reinforcement. On the other


hand, when patients were provided an alternative
means of obtaining rewards, punishment

produced an immediate and complete reduction of

the undesired behavior.

The above findings suggest that persons who

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1005
have few response options will be slow to
abandon behavior that results in negative

outcomes. This is one reason that punishment is


ineffective in modifying the antisocial patterns of

delinquents and adult offenders who lack

alternative prosocial modes of response for

acquiring possessions that they value highly.


Under these conditions, punishment of antisocial

behavior is likely to lead offenders either to adopt

safer forms of illegitimate activities, or to alter

their techniques in order to avoid detection and

punishment on future occasions. An excellent


example of the way in which punishment results

in refinement rather than elimination of antisocial


behavior is provided in the autobiography of a

talented habitual offender.

My prison surroundings have been


completely a life apart, something so far
away that at times it was my real
circumstances that seemed so fantastic. In

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1006
between I went over jobs which I had pulled
off and mentally surveyed them to see how
they could be improved upon. Then I went
over my mistakes again, and learned how
they had occurred and let me down. So you
see there was always plenty for me to do
when I lay on that board with no occupation
but thinking. And plan future jobs. Oh yes, if
a survey could be taken it would be proved
that most of the big criminal jobs, and
thousands of small ones, are planned in gaol.
Planned to the last detail because there is not
sufficient alternative interest to occupy
prisoners’ minds [Hill, 1955, p. 39],

To the extent that refinements in deviant behavior

increase an individual’s confidence that he can

avoid detection and punishment on subsequent


occasions the behavior will most likely be

repeated.

BEHAVIORAL REDUCTION THROUGH POSITIVE


REINFORCEMENT OF COMPETING RESPONSES

The laboratory findings reviewed above

indicate that pre-existing modes of behavior are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1007
likely to emerge when dominant response

patterns are reduced through punishment. Stimuli

that exercise some degree of control over different


types of responses can also be utilized in

conjunction with aversive procedures to elicit

weaker response tendencies. The temporary


suppression of disapproved behavior thus

provides an opportunity to strengthen either

emergent or actively elicited responsiveness.


When the desired alternatives do not exist in the
individual’s behavioral repertoire, positive

modeling and reinforcement procedures can be


effectively employed to establish and to

strengthen response patterns incompatible with


the maladaptive behavior.

The results of several experiments (Boe, 1964;


Holz, Azrin, & Ayllon, 1963; Whiting & Mowrer,

1943) have consistently shown that responses can


be rapidly and durably eliminated when the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1008
behavior in question is punished and competing

responses are simultaneously rewarded. A

treatment program relying upon a combination of


aversive control of deviant responses and positive

reinforcement of desired responses may be most

effective for eliminating highly disturbing patterns

of behavior. When differential reinforcement is

applied to competing modes of behavior, the


punished responses may be counteracted by
either interfering responses elicited by the

aversive stimuli or competing behavior

maintained on the basis of positive reinforcement.

Under these circumstances, long-lasting


suppression is probably achieved less through

inhibitory responses established by punishment


than by the prepotency of rewarded alternatives.

This is suggested by findings from an experiment

by Timmons (1962), who compared the relative


efficacy of extinction, verbal punishment, and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1009
counterconditioning of a competing response in

eliminating formerly appropriate verbal

responses. Counterconditioning proved most


powerful, but the addition of punishment to

counterconditioning contributed little to the


change process. However, this conclusion may not
hold true when undesired behavior is so strongly

established that few opportunities arise to reward

competing tendencies. It has also been shown that


even mild punishment, which has more

informative than inhibitory value, may facilitate

behavioral change to the same extent as severe


levels of punishment, provided that alternative

responses are concomitantly rewarded (Boe,


1964).

In a comprehensive analysis of punishment

effects Solomon (1964) has termed the

widespread belief that punishment is only a

temporarily effective controller of behavior a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1010
legend springing from tenderheartedness and

sentimentalism. The findings reviewed in the

preceding sections, though not entirely consistent,


nevertheless provide considerable empirical

documentation that under a wide variety of

conditions the reductive effects of punishment

tend, in fact, to be impermanent. The conclusions

drawn from this large body of research cannot be


readily dismissed as legendary or spurious. It is
equally true that punishments administered in

conjunction with other procedures may produce

enduring changes in behavior (Beach, Conovitz,

Steinberg, & Goldstein, 1956; Boe & Church, 1967;


Storms, Boroczi, & Broen, 1963). Moreover,

innumerable studies have shown that tenacious


inhibitions and avoidance behaviors are created

when emotional arousal is conditioned to

environmental and self-generated stimuli through


aversive means.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1011
It would appear from these diverse outcomes

that any sweeping judgment regarding

punishment effects results in self-contradiction.


One cannot contend that punishment is ineffective

because it has only temporary suppressing effects


and at the same time argue against its use on the
grounds that it produces behavior that is

unusually resistant to change. Similarly, self-

contradictions arise when studies in which


punishment results in self-starvation

(Lichtenstein, 1950) or needless inhibition of

effective means of securing reinforcement


(Whiting & Mowrer, 1943) are cited as evidence of

powerful aversive control of behavior, but


behavioral rigidity and other undesirable by-

products of punishment are treated as

unsubstantiated concerns.

Applications of Aversive Contingency


Systems

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1012
When aversive procedures are essential for

alleviating a detrimental condition, they are

generally viewed by adults as an unpleasant


though necessary part of the treatment,

comparable to the painful routines of physical


medicine, rather than as an interpersonal assault.
For this reason, patients rarely develop widely

generalized fears and hostile behavior toward

dentists and surgeons whose ministrations


initially produce highly distressing experiences.

Consequently, if presented in a treatment context,

aversive contingencies may have fewer adverse


side effects than when they are used dictatorially

to eliminate behavior that has high functional


value for the performer.

When aversive control is employed to modify

harmful social behavior the same punishing

consequences may be strongly resented or

willingly accepted depending upon the perceived

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1013
intent of the agent, and whether the sanctions are

applied mainly for his own convenience or for the

benefit of the recipient. Undesirable emotional


effects can be substantially reduced by arranging

in advance explicit contractual contingencies

which clearly define the broad limits of

permissible and punishable behaviors. Whenever

the undesirable behavior is performed the


aversive consequences should be applied
immediately, consistently, and in a matter-of- fact

way. If prearranged contingencies are

implemented in a nonpunitive fashion, the

negative sanctions will tend to be regarded by the


recipient as legitimate, predictable consequences

of his behavior, rather than as arbitrary and


vindictive reactions.

SPEECH DISORDERS

Aversive contingencies have been extensively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1014
employed by Goldiamond (1965a) in both
experimental production of stuttering behavior

and its elimination. Before discussing the details of


this approach and its therapeutic efficacy, some of

the conditions that have been presumed to control

disfluencies will be reviewed. Learning

interpretations of functional speech disorders


generally conceptualize stuttering as avoidance

responses that are evoked by verbal stimuli in the

presence of potentially threatening situational

cues. The various explanatory schemes (Brutten &

Shoemaker, 1967; Shames & Sherrick, 1963;


Sheehan, 1958; Wischner, 1950) primarily differ

in the roles they assign to conditioned


emotionality and to positive and negative

reinforcement processes in the regulation of dis


fluencies.

Innumerable assessments have been made of


the personality characteristics of stutterers and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1015
their parents, but observational studies of the

naturally occurring contingencies associated with

disfluencies are lacking. Based on retrospective


data from families of stutterers and nonstutterers,

Johnson (1942), who advocated a semantic-

learning approach, considered the following

conditions to be critical in the initial development

of speech disorders. All young children display


some repetitions of words, phrases, and syllables
without any accompanying emotional arousal or

self-awareness that their speech is defective

(Davis, 1939, 1940). However, parents of children

who later exhibit serious speech problems label


normal disfluencies as stuttering, to which they

then respond with increased vigilance, verbal


reprimands, and anxious remedial efforts. It is

assumed that, as a consequence of negative

evaluations and mislabeling of natural


disfluencies, anxiety reactions become

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1016
conditioned to the act of verbal communication.

Formerly effortless repetitions are now

accompanied by blockages, prolongations,


muscular tension, respiratory changes, and

compensatory facial and body movement. Once


disfluencies take on aversive properties they are
transformed into stuttering responses, and, like

other forms of avoidance behavior, become

capable of creating their own maintaining


conditions.

Results of laboratory studies that will be

reviewed later are not entirely consistent with the

above etiological formulation. Stimuli that have


been regularly associated with punishment can
have disruptive effects on speech, but negative

consequences made specifically contingent on

disfluencies generally reduce their occurrence

(Brookshire & Martin, 1967; Siegel & Martin,

1966; Quist & Martin, 1967). It would follow from

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1017
these findings that punishment might increase the

frequency of disfluencies only if the disruptive

effects of classically conditioned arousal outweigh


the reductive effects of punishing consequences.

Another important determinant of disfluencies,

which is usually either ignored or assigned a

secondary role in anxiety theories of stuttering, is

that solicitous parental concern made contingent


upon disfluencies can function as a powerful
positive reinforcer for such behavior. Parents may

thus inadvertently increase their children’s

disfluencies through their selective attention.

Familial modeling may also play an influential


role, either directly or indirectly, in the

development of disfluent speech patterns. Nelson


(1939) compared the incidence of stutterers in

filial, parental, and grandparental generations of

families of 204 stutterers and of a matched group

that manifested no speech disorders. Stuttering

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1018
appeared in more than one generation in only 2

percent of the nonstutterers, whereas the

incidence was 51 percent in the families of


stutterers. More direct evidence for the force of

example is provided by Van Riper (1937), who

found that stutterers not only displayed a higher

rate of disfluency following exposure to a

stuttering model as compared to a nonstuttering


model, but they even adopted some of the
idiosyncratic features of the stutterer’s verbal

behavior. Nelson argues for a genetic transmission

on the grounds that, in some cases, the parents no

longer stuttered or contact with disfluent


grandparents was limited. The genetic

interpretation may very well be valid, but an


explanation in terms of social learning is equally

tenable. Adults who themselves suffered from

speech disorders at an earlier period of their life,


or whose parents stuttered, would be prone to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1019
respond with excessive concern to their children’s

natural disfluencies, thus increasing such behavior

even in the absence of modeled stuttering.

Sheehan (1958) and Wischner (1950) provide

the most detailed accounts of the negative


reinforcement mechanism presumed to maintain

stuttering responses. Their interpretations differ

mainly on the point in the speech sequence at


which reinforcement supposedly occurs.
According to Wischner, anxiety elicited by specific

words and situational cues results in momentary

blocking of a later portion of a verbal response in

an attempt to postpone or avoid anticipated social


disapproval, embarrassment or other negative
experiences. Stuttering behavior is believed to be

reinforced by virtue of its close temporal

juxtaposition with anxiety- tension reduction

accompanying successful completion of the word

on which difficulty was experienced. Although

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1020
stuttering generally produces negative

consequences as well, Wischner assumes that the

rewarding effects of immediate tension reduction


outweigh the inhibitory effects of temporarily

more remote punishment.

In his approach-avoidance conflict theory of

stuttering, Sheehan (1958) similarly regards

stuttering as an anxiety reducer, but he posits a


two-stage reinforcement process. Stuttering,
according to Sheehan, is a resultant of competing

urges to communicate and to avoid speaking.

Whenever the conflicting approach and avoidance


tendencies reach a point of equilibrium, the flow of
speech is interrupted or blocked. Momentary

inhibition of speech reduces the fear generated by


verbal communication which both reinforces

blocking and, by lowering the fear-motivated

avoidance gradient, releases the blocked word. In

addition, tension reduction following completion

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1021
of the word reinforces the preceding stuttering

responses, as well as any accompanying facial and

body movements designed to help restore fluency.

Brutten & Shoemaker (1967) consider

stuttering to be a phenomenon involving both

emotional and instrumental conditioning

processes, with the former assuming the more

influential role. According to this view,


disfluencies reflect the disruptive effects of
emotional arousal that have become classically

conditioned to certain situational and word cues

through unpleasant experiences. Several studies


have been published that provide some
supporting evidence for the influence of classically

conditioned arousal on disfluencies. Hill (1954)


found that students displayed disorganization of

speech in the presence of a light that had

previously been paired with shock stimulation.

Similar increases in disfluencies have also been

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1022
obtained during periods of emotional stress

produced by arbitrary punishment (Stassi, 1961),

or by exposure to stimuli that signaled the


occurrence of punishing events (Savoye, 1959).

While stuttering is conceptualized primarily as a

form of behavioral disorganization rather than an

avoidance response, it is assumed to include a

secondary instrumental component. That is,


stutterers adopt idiosyncratic phonatory,
articulatory, and resonatory modes of expression

designed to escape or forestall emotional

disturbances occasioned by disfluencies. These

adjustive responses, most of which take nonverbal


forms, are instrumentally reinforced by

subsequent word completion and attendant


reduction of distress. It would follow from this

explanatory scheme that stuttering can be

effectively eliminated only by extinguishing the


emotion-provoking properties of threatening

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1023
stimuli.

In view of the widespread belief that anxiety-


reduction is the primary maintaining mechanism

in functional speech disorders, it is somewhat

surprising that relatively few laboratory studies


have been conducted for the specific purpose of

evaluating this central hypothesis. The

reinforcement mechanisms adopted in the


theories discussed earlier are well suited to
account for unusual persistence of speech

disorders. If stuttering responses produce almost

instantaneous reinforcement through anxiety

reduction upon their occurrence, as assumed, one


would expect stuttering never to undergo
extinction unless special consequences were

somehow promptly interposed between the onset

of blocking and completion of the word. However,

a retrospective study by Sheehan & Martyn (1966)

showing that approximately 80 percent of college

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1024
students who had been stutterers eventually

achieved fluent speech suggests that recovery

from stuttering without special intervention


requires explanation just as much as does the

persistence of stuttering.

In addition to the meager experimental data on

the acquisition and maintenance of stuttering

responses, the anxiety-reduction theories


furnished no distinctive modes of treatment. An
early experiment by Sheehan (1951), however,

had therapeutic implications that were never

pursued. In this study it was assumed that the


point of reinforcement of stuttering responses is
the anxiety reduction following completion of the

word. Therefore, in an effort to prevent


reinforcement of stuttering responses, conditions

were so arranged that only fluent speech could be

instrumental in terminating each word in spoken

sentences. Adult stutterers read passages aloud on

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1025
different days under two conditions in

counterbalanced order. Under the control

conditions subjects merely read the material six


times in their usual way without arrangement of

any special consequences for disfluencies. In the

nonreinforcement treatment, however, subjects

read the passages aloud five times, except that

they were required to repeat each stuttered word


until they had said it fluently before proceeding to
the next word. Thus, stuttering prolonged rather

than terminated the attendant stress and tension.

On the sixth experimental trial and on the seventh

trial of both conditions subjects read the passages


as they normally would. Comparison of the

frequency of disfluencies between conditions and


successive readings (Figure 5-3) shows that

stuttering was substantially reduced and

remained significantly lower when it produced


negative consequences; whereas, under ordinary

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1026
Figure 5-3. Frequency of stuttering through successive
readings in experimental and control conditions.
Sheehan, 1951.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1027
conditions, stuttering rate decreased slightly,

probably as a function of adaptation, but was

subsequently restored to its original level. Despite


the encouraging findings, the experimental

procedure was never extended to test its


therapeutic efficacy. Instead the author (Sheehan,
1954) advocated the same time-consuming

interview approaches that have proved of limited

value in altering other forms of deviant behavior.

In the preceding experiment any possible

decremental reinforcement of stuttering

responses occurring at the terminal point of the

verbal sequence was removed. The effectiveness


of reinforcement can be reduced if an interval of
time and other activities intervenes between a

response and its intended consequences. In

Sheehan’s study the temporal arrangement of

events was not the most favorable one for

eliminating disfluency; i.e., stuttering responses

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1028
eventually ended in word completions which, in

turn, were followed by negative consequences of

having to repeat the word on which the subject


encountered difficulty. Hence, successful word

completion was punished more strongly than

temporally remote repetitions and blockages.

Results of a study by Daly & Cooper (1967),

however, raise doubt that stutter-contingent


punishment would have achieved a greater
reductive effect. These investigators compared the

frequency of disfluencies under conditions where

shock was administered either during the act of

stuttering or immediately following completion of


each stuttered word. The stutter-contingent

system reduced disfluencies most, but the


difference was not of statistically significant

magnitude. In the method devised by Goldiamond

(1965a) for modifying chronic stuttering, which


will be reviewed next, each moment of disfluency

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1029
produces an immediate unpleasant consequence.

It is not entirely clear from Goldiamond’s


otherwise informative article what he considers to

be the critical conditions for the acquisition and

maintenance of stuttering responses. While


acknowledging that stuttering may involve an

emotional component, the anxiety-reduction

theory is summarily dismissed. Instead, numerous


anecdotal reports are presented to illustrate that
stutterers are generally subjected to lower

achievement demands, they are called on less

often to perform onerous tasks, their errors are

more likely to elicit sympathetic reactions from


listeners, they are provided more time to
formulate answers to questions, and they can

effectively command polite attention through

disfluency. These reports highlight both the

avoidance function of stuttering responses and

their value for drawing positive responses from

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1030
others.

Because of the nature of the contingencies

employed, findings from an experiment designed


to produce stuttering responses in a normally

fluent subject (Flanagan, Goldiamond, & Azrin,

1959) are of limited value in elucidating the

conditions under which stuttering is most likely

acquired. In this experiment, a subject received


continual shock; a verbal blockage, however,
turned off the shock for 10 seconds, and each

additional disfluency occurring during the interval

further delayed the aversive stimulation for a fixed


time. As might be expected, blockage rate
increased markedly to the point where the subject

almost completely averted the unpleasant


stimulation by continuous disfluent speech. While

this study demonstrates that it is possible to

induce deviant verbal behavior, it is exceedingly

improbable that parents of stutterers continuously

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1031
punish their children’s fluent verbal patterns, but

respond nonpunitively whenever their children

block and stutter. Indeed, the findings discussed


earlier indicate that the pattern of naturalistic

contingencies is probably the exact opposite of the

one imposed in the above study. We may grant

that experiments are not designed to reproduce in

every detail the stimulus events that occur in real


life, but we should require experimental
contingencies to bear some resemblance to social

reality if their findings are to have explanatory

value.

The studies previously reported suggest that


disfluent speech can be influenced to varying

degrees by at least three sets of controlling


conditions. First, threatening stimulus events can

produce speech disorganization through arousal

of anticipatory emotional responses. Second,

anxiety and tension reduction associated with the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1032
completion of stuttering responses can serve to

reinforce them. And third, disfluency may have

some functional value in commanding attention


and lessening performance demands as suggested

by Goldiamond. A situational analysis of

disfluencies in children by Davis (1940), in fact,

disclosed that disfluent speech occurred most

frequently when children either were emotionally


aroused or wished to gain the attention of others.

Practically every form of psychotherapeutic

approach has been applied at one time or another

to speech disorders, with limited degrees of


success. Goldiamond (1965a) reports uniformly
favorable outcomes with stutterers by disfluency-

contingent punishment which, if confirmed by


more extensive assessments conducted over a

longer period, would represent a notable

therapeutic achievement.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1033
In the first step of the procedure, stutterers are

instructed to read aloud from printed pages for

approximately 50-minute periods to provide a


baseline measure of stuttering. In this initial

assessment reading speed and frequency of


disfluencies are recorded. During the treatment
phase of the program reading rate is deliberately

slowed down and negative consequences are

made contingent upon the occurrence of stuttering


responses. Delayed auditory feedback of the

person’s own voice is utilized as the stutter-

produced aversive stimulus.

Pauses frequently occur as natural parts of


fluent speech and, consequently, an independent
observer would have to delay judgment as to

whether a particular hesitancy represented a

natural pause or a speech block. In order to ensure

that aversive consequences are immediately

contingent upon disfluencies, the client self-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1034
administers the negative feedback on the basis of

his own response definitions. During oral reading

of material presented at a low rate the client


presses a microswitch for each word blocked,

which immediately shunts speech to a delayed

feedback device for a fixed time. This procedure

eventually establishes a slow stutterless pattern of

verbal behavior.

After the substitute fluent pattern is stabilized,


the client’s verbal behavior is progressively

modified in the direction of normal speech. This is

achieved by accelerating the reading rate in


graduated steps through mechanical control of the
material to normal or beyond baseline levels. At

the same time, delayed feedback is also gradually


faded out. For example, the delay period may be

reduced from an initial 250 millisecond duration

to 200, 150, 100, 50 milliseconds, and finally

eliminated completely.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1035
A summary of the procedures and concomitant

changes in verbal fluency achieved with the first

subject to receive this form of treatment is


presented in Figure 5-4. During the baseline

period, which extended over the first 21 days, the


subject read approximately 110 words per minute
and stuttered on about 15 words per minute. In

sessions 22 through 33, he engaged in self-

definition of stuttering without any response-


contingent consequences. Beginning with the 34th

session, reading rate was lowered and stutter-

produced delayed feedback was introduced. At


session 47, time control of reading rate was

instituted, and several sessions later delayed


feedback was gradually faded out on successive

days. As shown in the figure, during the terminal

phase of the experiment the subject was reading


approximately 140 words per minute, well above

his previous baseline, while stuttering responses,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1036
1037
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 5-4. Reduction in stuttering rate and establishment of fluent reading
in a chronic stutterer. Goldiamond, 1965a.
which ranged between 0.2 and 0.6 words per

minute, were almost completely eliminated.

Goldiamond reports even more dramatic

changes in verbal fluency (Figure 5-5) in a second

stutterer who, because of time limitations,


participated in a highly condensed version of the

standard procedure. Stutter-produced delayed

feedback combined with slow presentation of


reading material was introduced after three
sessions; shortly thereafter reading rate was

raised in successive stages, and the aversive

contingency was gradually removed. By the eighth

and final day the client was reading 256 words per
minute without manifesting a single disfluency.

Quantitative data are presented for eight

chronic stutterers. In each case fluent speech was

achieved and maintained in the laboratory

situation even when rate of verbalization was

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1038
1039
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 5-5. Elimination of disfluencies through stutter-produced delayed
feedback and development of rapid fluent reading. Goldiamond,
1965a.
increased and negative consequences withdrawn.
Goldiamond also reports concomitant

improvements in clients’ speech in naturalistic


settings, but apparently no objective assessments

were conducted. According to a later research

report (Goldiamond, 1965b), fluent patterns of

rapid reading, which endure under laboratory


conditions, have been established in 30 stutterers

within a remarkably short time. These preliminary

findings indicate that the procedures devised by

Goldiamond may have considerable promise.

However, evaluation of their therapeutic efficacy


must be deferred until more stringent tests are

carried out, and long-term follow-up results are


furnished.

Reports of changes in stuttering behavior must


be accepted with caution in view of evidence that

disfluency varies considerably as a function of the


social characteristics and the communicative

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1040
demands of different situations. A review of the

relevant literature by Bloodstein (1949) reveals

that stuttering responses are greatly diminished


under conditions of reduced requirements for

interpersonal communication. For example,

stutterers can generally sing and count fluently,

they can speak smoothly when acting, when

imitating another person’s verbal style, or when


alone. For this reason attainment of rapid
stutterless reading by a person alone in a booth

does not constitute a powerful test. Disfluency is

likewise decreased in situations where negative

social reactions from listeners are minimized.


Thus stutterers may experience little verbal

difficulty when speaking to persons much younger


than themselves, to audiences regarded as socially

or intellectually inferior, or to persons over whom

they exercise authority. These findings suggest


that stringent tests of fluency would require social

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1041
communication rather than oral reading in

evaluative situations involving intimidating

audiences. Tape recordings of speech under these


types of conditions can be obtained without

difficulty.

When behavior is modified in clinic offices or

laboratories the problem of insufficient transfer of

changes to everyday situations frequently arises.


Goldiamond has attempted to overcome this
difficulty in the elimination of stuttering by

utilizing additional procedures specifically

designed to aid generalization of fluent speech to

the natural social environment. Speech exercises


are prescribed for the client to perform at home.
Essentially these involve reading for brief periods

in the slow stutterless manner developed in the

laboratory, interspersed with rapid oral reading.

The method devised by Goldiamond follows an

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1042
exclusively operant approach. If all stutterers who
receive this form of treatment are able to converse

fluently, even under stressful social conditions,


then obviously no additional procedures are

required. On the other hand, if their speech

performance is substantially improved but they

continue to exhibit some disfluencies in certain


emotion-arousing situations they could benefit

from a program of desensitization. Some case data

have been published to suggest that disfluencies

controlled by conditioned aversive stimuli can be

successfully eliminated by extinguishing


emotional responsiveness to such threats.

An illustrative case is provided by Walton &


Mather (1963) in the treatment of a 40-year-old

architect who suffered from an articulatory


disorder characterized by speech blocks

associated with teeth grinding. The controlling


stimuli for the client’s disfluencies included

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1043
situations in which he was required to convey

specific information, particularly in the presence

of strangers, supervisors, and persons whom he


wished to impress. Initially a “speech shadowing”

technique was employed in which the therapist

read passages from a book and the client matched

his verbalizations without seeing the text.

Although this method proved highly effective in


producing fluent speech in the treatment situation,
the stammering nevertheless persisted in natural

contexts. The second treatment strategy was

aimed directly at neutralizing the stimulus

determinants of the speech blocks. At the


completion of a desensitization treatment, in

which stammer-generating situations were


repeatedly paired with relaxation, the client was

able to converse fluently, even when

communicating specific information to persons in


authority. Because of the multiple determination

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1044
of disfluencies, a treatment strategy combining

reinforcement procedures that restore fluent

speech with methods that can extinguish the


arousal potential of stutter-eliciting stimuli would

prove most effective in eliminating disfluencies


under stressful social circumstances.

CONTROL OF SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR

One of the most perplexing and dangerous


disorders, which is especially prevalent among

schizophrenic children, involves self-injurious

behavior. In its more extreme forms, children

pummel their faces repeatedly, they bang their


heads forcibly against hard or sharp objects, they

tear and bite off pieces of flesh from their bodies,

or they exhibit some other type of self-mutilating

behavior. Because of the serious risk of permanent

physical injury, such children are usually kept


continuously in physical restraints.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1045
The maintaining conditions of self-injurious

behavior are not fully understood, but several

experiments have shown that it is amenable to


control through variation of reinforcing

consequences. Lovaas, Freitag, Gold, & Kassorla


(1965) found that the self-injurious responses are
readily cued off by stimuli signifying withdrawal

of social reinforcement for other behaviors and

that they tend to increase in frequency and


intensity when social reactions are made

contingent upon their occurrence. Demonstrations

that self-injurious behavior can be reduced


through reinforcement of physically incompatible

responses and increased by extinction of


competing activities cast little light on the

variables that control this behavior. Of much

greater interest are the changes produced by


variation of the contingencies applied directly to

self-injurious behavior.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1046
It has been demonstrated (Bucher & Lovaas,

1968; Lovaas, et al., 1965) that physical affection,

and sympathetic and reassuring comments made


to children whenever they engage in self-injurious

behavior, leads them to respond even more self-


destructively, whereas such behavior is promptly
reduced to its original baseline level when positive

reactions are discontinued. These findings suggest

that self-injurious behavior may be partly


maintained by its social consequences, though the

evidence is conflicting on this point. Subtle

changes in social reinforcement, such as an


experimenter remaining with a child without

attending to him, does not seem to affect the rate


of self-injurious responses (Lovaas et al., 1965;

Risley, 1968). More complete withdrawal of social

interaction for a brief period whenever children


behave self-injuriously reduces such activities, but

occasionally they are unaffected even by these

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1047
more powerful consequences (Risley, 1968).

Whatever the controlling variables might be in any

given case, preliminary findings indicate that

chronic and intractable self-injurious behavior can


be successfully eliminated with beneficial effects
by brief application of an aversive contingency.

Punishment is generally used as an adjunct to

either extinction procedures or differential


reinforcement of competing response patterns.
Brief social withdrawal made contingent on self-

destructive behavior is usually sufficient for its

permanent removal (Hamilton, Stephens, & Allen,

1967; Lovaas et al., 1965; Wolf, Risley, & Mees,


1964). In some cases, however, when self-
mutilating behavior is first placed on extinction it

may temporarily increase in intensity, which could

be potentially dangerous. These responses can be

promptly and completely eliminated by contingent

application of a few painful shocks. In one of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1048
several cases reported by Bucher & Lovaas (1968)

a seven-year-old schizophrenic boy who had been

self-injurious since the age of two performed


approximately 3,000 self-pummelling responses

during a period of 90 minutes when his physical

restraints were removed. This behavior was

almost totally eliminated in four sessions by the

use of 12 contingent shocks. Self-destructive


behavior that had persisted over a period of six
years was also rapidly and durably removed in a

schizophrenic girl after she received a total of 15

shocks for beating her head (Figure 5-6). In each

case reported, contingent aversive consequences


not only removed self-mutilating behavior, but the

children whined less and they were much more


inclined to attend to the therapists.

Tate & Baroff (1966) similarly achieved quick

control over chronic self-injurious behavior in a

partially blind psychotic boy through punishment

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1049
Figure 5-6. Frequency of self-injurious behavior and amount
of avoidance and crying displayed by an autistic child
during pre-treatment sessions (1 through 15), and
when such behavior was punished by shock (P) or a
verbal reprimand “no” (N) during sessions 16, 17, 19,
and 21. The numerals below the session numbers
identify the therapist present during the session.
Bucher & Lovaas, 1968.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1050
procedures. The behavior, which he had exhibited

continuously for five years, included vigorous

head-banging, face-slapping, and self- kicking.


Because physical contact with people was highly

reinforcing to the boy it was employed as the

consequent event in the first phase of the

treatment program. A series of daily walks was

arranged in each of which the therapist removed


his hand from the child’s grasp and ceased talking
whenever a self-injurious response occurred;

physical contact was reinstated if the boy did not

hit himself for a period of three seconds. As can be

seen in Figure 5-7, the time-out contingency


produced a dramatic reduction in self-injurious

behavior.

In the second phase of the program, response-

contingent shock was used to eliminate the

remaining head-banging that threatened further

damage to his eyes. It was explained to him that if

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1051
Figure 5-7. Daily average frequency of self-injurious
responses performed per minute on control days when
such responses were followed by no special
consequences, and on experimental days when self-
striking behavior produced brief withdrawal of
physical contact. Tate & Baroff, 1966.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1052
he continued to hit himself he would receive

painful shocks. The aversive contingency,

combined with verbal praise and affectionate


reactions for desirable behavior, permanently

eliminated the self-injurious responding. Whereas

previously the boy had been physically restrained

in bed, after the treatment program was

completed he participated freely in daily activities


with increased enjoyment and spontaneity.

Risley (1968) provides a detailed report of a

case in which social consequences were totally

ineffective in decreasing self-injurious behavior. It


involved a markedly deviant 6-year-old girl who
suffered many serious body and facial injuries

from hazardous climbing activities which she


engaged in continuously. A program of differential

reinforcement, wherein climbing behavior was

ignored and incompatible responses were

rewarded, failed to produce any significant

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1053
changes. Unlike the cases cited above, brief

physical isolation contingent upon dangerous

climbing behavior also proved ineffectual. After


these other methods failed, shock punishment was

applied in conjunction with a verbal reprimand.

Administration of several shocks, and later verbal

reprimands alone, completely eliminated

inappropriate climbing when the experimenter


was present, but there was no noticeable decrease
in this behavior at home. When the mother

subsequently applied contingent shock at home,

perilous clambering promptly declined from an

average rate of 29 to 2 per day within a few days.


Thereafter, the behavior was successfully

controlled by having the child sit in a chair for a


brief period after each instance of climbing.

It is noteworthy that surprisingly brief

programs of contingent shock and reinforcement

withdrawal are not only effective in removing self-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1054
injurious behavior of long standing, but they

generally improve social functioning as well.

These related changes are most clearly revealed in


the findings reported by Risley (1968). After

disruptive self-injurious behavior is removed,

children become more attentive, socially

responsive, and display increased imitativeness,

thus enabling them to acquire new patterns of


behavior. If further studies support this
conclusion, there is no justification for having

children mutilating themselves or spending their

early years uselessly in physical restraints.

MOTOR DYSFUNCTIONS

In one of the early applications of aversive

contingencies, Liversedge & Sylvester (1955;

Sylvester & Liversedge, 1960) treated 39 cases of

writer’s cramp with a procedure employing

response-contingent shock. In the majority of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1055
cases, tremors and spasms of hand muscles were
elicited only by highly specific writing stimuli, but

the same muscle groups were unaffected when


involved in nonwriting situations. As has been

found in other types of deviant behavior, the

individuals exhibiting this occupational

impairment shared no common psychological


characteristics, suggesting that specific

reinforcement contingencies rather than

psychodynamic factors were the critical

determinants. It is therefore not surprising that a

number of these clients who had undergone


various conventional forms of psychotherapy

experienced little or no amelioration of their “craft


neurosis.” Consequently, Liversedge & Sylvester

explored the efficacy of aversive consequences for


altering each component of the physical disorder.

In order to remove tremors, one element of the


motor disability, the clients were required to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1056
insert a stylus into a series of progressively

smaller holes; each time the stylus made contact

with the side of the hole it resulted in a shock. The


removal of the spasm component of the disorder

was obtained in two ways: First, the clients traced

various line patterns (similar to the movements

required in writing) on a metal plate with a stylus,

and any deviation from the path produced a shock.


Following training on the apparatus, they then
wrote with an electrified pen which delivered a

shock whenever excessive thumb pressure was

applied. In treating typists’ cramp a small

electrified pad was attached to the palm of the


hand so that whenever the fingers contracted into

the palm a shock was delivered.

The authors report that after 3 to 6 weeks’

training, writing of satisfactory quality was

restored in 24 of the cases; the clients were able to

resume work, which often involved writing for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1057
extended periods, and follow-up studies

conducted up to four and a half years later

disclosed that the improvement was being


maintained. Five clients responded favorably to

the treatment but subsequently experienced a

recurrence of muscular dysfunction, while 10

cases showed no improvement. The failures had

exhibited the motor disability over a longer period


(6 to 21 years), which may partly explain why they
were less responsive to the treatment.

The procedures devised by Liversedge &

Sylvester are sufficiently effective to merit


controlled studies to isolate factors responsible for
the success of this treatment approach. Since the

technique involves both response guidance and


punishment of spasmodic and tremorous

responses, it is conceivable that guided retraining

with nonaversive feedback may in itself effect

changes. In addition, information is needed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1058
regarding the types of individuals who achieve

lasting benefit from application of aversive

contingencies alone, those who require


supplementary or alternative forms of treatment,

and the cases for whom aversive procedures are

contraindicated.

It would appear, from the findings of

Liversedge & Sylvester, that aversive


contingencies are most successful in modifying
motor dysfunctions controlled directly by stimuli

in writing situations that do not have strong

emotion-provoking value. On the other hand, in


instances where muscular tremors and tension
reflect the disruptive effects of high emotional

arousal, punishment may further augment the


negative valence of writing situations and thereby

exacerbate the condition. In this connection, Beech

(1960) has furnished some preliminary

experimental evidence that extinction and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1059
desensitization methods may prove successful

with highly anxious individuals whose writing

disturbances are unimproved by contingent


negative consequences.

In one case, for example, the person had

received a full year of psychoanalysis, a year of

hypnotherapy, and 10 sessions of aversive

conditioning without any amelioration of a


writer’s cramp of five years’ duration. Whenever
he grasped a pen the index finger would contract

and the wrist would bend sharply, causing severe

pain, fatigue, and immobilization of the hand. The


client participated in seven extinction sessions,
each of which consisted of massed evocation of

effortful finger contraction, to the point at which


he could no longer retract the finger. Writing tests

administered following each extinction period

disclosed not only a rapid and progressive decline

in the incidence of finger contraction, but also a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1060
similar decremental change in the untreated arm

spasm. Moreover, on the day after the final

experimental session the client was able to write


24 lines with only one instance of hand spasm.

Similar, though less stable, results were

obtained by extinction involving repeated

response evocation with a second client, both of

whose hands were severely immobilized.


Attempts at writing produced intense painful
muscular contractions and, at best, illegible

scribbling; his left hand, which he had previously

used in typing, was clenched tightly into a fist


which he could open for only a few seconds at a
time. In an attempt to extinguish fist-clenching the

client repeatedly performed effortful hand-closure


responses on a dynamometer until he could no

longer squeeze the handle of the apparatus. After

33 extinction sessions he was able to open his

hand for an indefinite period; however, thoughts

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1061
about his work, and stressful interpersonal

experiences, typically reinstated the motor

disorder. Since writing situations elicited


considerable anxiety in the client, the tremors and

spasms associated with the act of writing were

treated by the standard desensitization procedure,

in which imagined writing activities were

progressively performed in the context of


relaxation. Tremorless writing of satisfactory
quality was temporarily restored in this manner,

only to be followed by repeated relapses.

In the above case the motor dysfunction was


apparently controlled, in large part, by distressing
vocational and interpersonal events. Had

emotional arousal to the latter situations also been


neutralized, it is possible that more enduring

changes would have been achieved. This type of

approach was successfully utilized with a foreign

student who was unable to take lecture notes due

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1062
to marked hand tremors and attendant fatigue.

After participating in a brief desensitization

program in which imagined and actual writing


situations were presented in conjunction with

relaxation, the student was able to write in a rapid

and relaxed manner without manifesting any

muscular disturbances. Announcement of a final

examination, however, re-established the


tremorous responses, though in less marked form.
A second series of desensitization sessions,

directed toward examination situations,

effectively eliminated the student’s writing

disability.

The case studies reported by Beech were

primarily designed to demonstrate that writing


disorders which apparently are under aversive

stimulus control and may therefore be

exacerbated by punishment can be modified under

laboratory conditions by other methods. While

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1063
these findings have suggestive value, more

extended applications and assessments of these

procedures are necessary to establish their utility


for eliminating occupational motor disorders.

A study by Barrett (1962) provides a

laboratory demonstration of reduction in tics by

the use of automatically programmed response

consequences. The client, a 38-year-old


accountant, suffered from extensive multiple tics
that proved refractory to psychotherapeutic and

pharmacological treatments. According to the

client’s report, the tics developed after a


frightening experience in the army when he awoke
one night with a choking sensation accompanied

by a momentary inability to breathe or swallow. At


the time of the study, his motor pattern included

contractions of neck, shoulder, chest, and

abdominal muscles, head-nodding, eye-blinking,

mouth-opening, other facial movements, and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1064
swallowing difficulties.

During the experimental sessions, the tiqueur

was seated comfortably in an armchair designed


so that spasmodic movements produced by the tic

were automatically recorded, and activated the

contingency controller. The contingency

arrangement was programmed so that each tic

produced either a brief interruption of music, or


white noise. Music was chosen as the positive
stimulus since the client was a part-time musician

and the interruption of recorded jazz concerts that

he had personally selected could serve as an


adequate negative event. The effects of the
withdrawal of music and the presentation of noise

were compared with the client’s deliberate efforts


to control his tics. Results based on eight sessions

showed that, while the rate of tics could be

reduced somewhat by self- control, tic-produced

white noise, and contingent music, the most

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1065
dramatic and stable decrements resulted from tic-

produced interruption of music. The latter

procedure reduced the rate of tics from 55 to 85


percent below the baseline level during the

various sessions, but the question remains

whether continuance of the mild punishment

contingency for a longer period could eventually

eliminate or markedly reduce the occurrence of


tics in naturalistic settings when negative
feedback is absent. The author suggests that in

therapeutic applications of this method, the client

might be able to modify motor and other

disturbances under his own direction if he were


furnished with a portable contingency controller

to plug into a home radio, television, or record


player. The intricate apparatus could be dispensed

with, however, if comparative studies disclosed

that a simple extinction procedure involving


repeated nonreinforced evocation of tics was

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1066
equally or more effective than methods utilizing

response-contingent punishment.

SEXUAL DEVIATIONS

Chapter 8 describes classical conditioning


procedures designed to eliminate sexual

aberrations by endowing stimuli that elicit the

behavior with aversive properties. Some attempts


have been made to bring sexually deviant behavior
under control through response-contingent

aversive stimulation. Feldman & MacCulloch

(1964, 1965) provide a detailed account of a

treatment method, primarily based on an


avoidance conditioning paradigm, that they have

developed for the modification of homosexuality.

Clients are asked initially to rate the

attractiveness of an extensive series of slides

depicting both clothed and completely nude males.


A similar hierarchy of slides of females varying in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1067
attractiveness is prepared. In order to facilitate
transfer effects, whenever possible photographs of

males and females with whom the client is closely

acquainted are used.

In the avoidance conditioning procedure, a

picture of a male is presented on a screen in a


darkened room and the client is instructed to

leave the picture on the screen as long as he finds

it sexually attractive. The client is informed that he

might be administered shocks during the viewing


periods, but that he can depress a switch that

simultaneously terminates the picture and the

aversive stimulation. If he turns off the slide

within 8 seconds, shock is avoided; if, on the other

hand, he continues to view the picture beyond the


designated period he is administered an
unpleasant shock through electrodes attached to

his leg.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1068
On the assumption that variable training

procedures will produce avoidance responses that

are highly resistant to extinction, the occurrence


of negative consequences is varied randomly with

respect to their frequency and timing.


Unpredictable aversive contingencies can produce
stable rates of avoidance responses in the

treatment situation, but it is questionable whether

they will have much effect on extinction in


everyday situations. The reason for this, which

will be explained more fully later, is that clients

can easily recognize that the arbitrary punishment


contingency employed in treatment is completely

absent in extratherapeutic situations. Under


conditions where the schedules of reinforcement

in different situations are highly distinguishable,

the partial reinforcement effect would not be


expected to carry over to the new context. In any

event, during the treatment series, one-third of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1069
client’s avoidance responses to pictures of males

are reinforced by shock termination; on one-third


of the trials aversive stimuli are administered even

though the client performs appropriate avoidance

responses within the allotted time; and on the


remaining trials termination of the picture is

delayed for varying periods of time after the

avoidance response has been performed.

In addition to establishing avoidance


responses toward males, an effort is made to

condition anxiety relief properties to females by

introducing slides of females contiguously with

the removal of pictures of males. To further


increase approach tendencies toward females, the
client can request return of a slide of a female after

it has been removed and thus postpone aversive

experiences periodically associated with male

pictorial stimuli. The presentation and removal of

the slides of females is controlled by the therapist

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1070
in order to prevent any inadvertent reinforcement

of avoidant behavior toward females.

Initially, the slide of the least attractive male is


paired with that of the most fetching female. A

particular male slide is displayed repeatedly until

the client reports indifference or a dislike for it,

and, in addition, turns off the slide within a second

or two; the next feminine picture is introduced


when the client consistently requests the return of
the preceding feminine item. This same process is

repeated with succeeding pairs of stimuli in the

pictorial hierarchy. A typical session involves


completion of about 30 trials and requires some
20 minutes to conduct. The treatment series,

which averages about 15 sessions, is continued


until a client exhibits a clear change of sexual

orientation. In addition, the client returns for

about 8 or 10 “booster” sessions during the year

following completion of the program.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1071
Feldman & MacCulloch (1965) present

preliminary outcome data for 19 unselected

chronic homosexuals treated by this method.


Three cases discontinued therapy; 10 out of 12

clients under 40 years of age, and 1 out of 4 cases


over 40, achieved marked changes in their sexual
orientations. According to the authors, the clients’

interests in men have greatly diminished and

homosexual practices have been virtually


eliminated, whereas heterosexual interests,

fantasies, and behavior have been substantially

increased. Moreover, in all but one case the


increased heterosexual behavior noted

immediately after treatment has been either


maintained or augmented during follow-up

periods ranging from 2 to 14 months.

MacCulloch, Feldman, & Pinshoff (1965) also

recorded response latencies and physiological

concomitants of avoidance conditioning for a small

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1072
subsample of cases to determine if response

measures obtained during treatment have value in

predicting post-therapy sexual behavior. Clients


who achieved and maintained a heterosexual

orientation displayed progressively shorter

avoidance response latencies to homoerotic

stimuli; they showed strong approach responses

to feminine photographs as therapy progressed,


and they exhibited conditioned autonomic
responses to pictures of males. In contrast, those

who reverted to homosexual practices exhibited

considerable irregularity in response latencies,

weak approach tendencies toward feminine


stimuli, and little or no autonomic conditioning.

These results, while most interesting, and


consistent with the correlates that one would

expect for successful avoidance learning, must be

confirmed on a larger sample before their


predictive value can be adequately evaluated.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1073
Feldman & MacCulloch attribute the

therapeutic outcomes primarily to the avoidant

component of the treatment procedure. The


behavioral changes also reflect the effects of

classically conditioned aversion to male stimuli


and stress-reducing value to feminine cues. The
highly favorable outcomes reported by Feldman

and MacCulloch are particularly striking when one

considers that there was no biased selection of


cases, and that supplemental programs designed

to develop adequate heterosexual repertoires of

behavior were rarely employed.

Bond & Evans (1967) successfully eliminated


underwear fetishism by contingent application of
aversive consequences in two boys who

repeatedly raided clotheslines for women’s

undergarments. The boys were presented in

random order 20 fetishistic and 20 neutral objects

and intermittently shocked while removing the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1074
fetishistic items. The authors report that after

several sessions the boys lost their interest in

female underwear and permanently discontinued


their clothesline forays. The methods employed in

the above studies appear to hold some promise for

modifying deviant sexual behavior, but full

evaluation must await controlled studies.

Removal of Positive Reinforcers

Aversive consequences in the form of physical

punishment are seldom employed as methods for


controlling behavior in naturalistic situations.

Removal of positive reinforcers, on the other hand,


is a very common mode of aversive control
(Bandura & Walters, 1959; Sears, Maccoby, &

Levin, 1957). This method is exemplified by

negative sanctions in which persons are deprived

for a time of rewards and privileges that are

ordinarily available, such as use of television,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1075
automobiles, or certain facilities; they are

temporarily removed from rewarding situations;

they are restricted from going out or participating


in enjoyable activities; or they are temporarily

forced to relinquish other objects and pastimes

they value. Monetary penalties are also frequently

levied as punishments, particularly with adults.

The removal of positive reinforcers as a


punishment technique should be distinguished
operationally from extinction procedures,

although both methods may reduce responding

through some common processes. In extinction,


consequences that ordinarily follow the behavior
are simply discontinued; in punishment, behavior

results in the application of aversive consequences


through forfeiture of positive reinforcers. Thus, in

extinguishing aggression sustained by peer

attention, the behavior is consistently ignored;

under the punishment contingency, however, the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1076
rewards of peer attention are pitted against the

negative effects of confinement to one’s room, loss

of television privileges, or some other type of


negative outcome. As is true of other forms of

aversive control, the amount of behavioral

reduction produced by punishment through

removal of positive reinforcers will depend,

among other factors, upon the relative magnitude


of the opposing consequences. The most effective
way of permanently eliminating undesirable

responses, of course, would be to remove the

positive conditions maintaining the behavior. In

many situations, however, the rewards dispensed


by others cannot be easily controlled. Under these

circumstances negative sanctions may be


effectively employed to reduce deviant response

patterns and to hold them in check while

alternative modes of behavior are being


established and strengthened.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1077
EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES OF PUNISHMENT BY
REINFORCEMENT WITHDRAWAL

Laboratory studies of the efficacy of


punishment by reinforcement withdrawal have

been largely confined to procedures in which the

occurrence of selected responses produces a loss

or temporary time-out from positive

reinforcement that was previously available. A


number of these experiments were primarily
designed to determine whether time-out

punishment functions as an aversive stimulus

analogous to shock stimulation or other physically

hurtful events (Azrin & Holz, 1966; Leitenberg,


1965). The findings generally show that

reinforcement time-out consequences can produce


durable reductions in responses if an alternative

mode of behavior is available to the subject (Holz,


Azrin, & Ayllon, 1963), or the contingencies

maintaining the behavior have been removed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1078
(Baer, 1961; Nigro, 1966).

Results of an illustrative experiment by Baer

(1962), however, disclose that reductive effects


are transitory in the case of behavior that

continues to produce powerful self-reinforcing

consequences. Young boys who displayed

persistent thumbsucking were shown cartoons.

Both boys watched the films together, but


interruption of the cartoon was made contingent
upon thumbsucking for one of the subjects, whose

response brought noncontingent film stoppages

for the other boy. Halfway through the experiment


their roles were reversed. Contingent interruption
of enjoyable cartoons produced a marked

decrement in thumbsucking, but no decrease


occurred when punishment was randomly related

to the behavior. However, during periods when

the punishment contingency was suspended the

boys promptly reverted to sucking their thumbs.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1079
The preceding studies demonstrate that brief

reinforcement withdrawal can function

analogously to an aversive stimulus in reducing


behavior. Similar reductive effects are achieved by

punishment through response-cost contingencies


in which monetary points are lost each time
negatively sanctioned behavior is performed

(Elliott & Tighe, 1968; Weiner, 1962). Laboratory

investigations of other behavioral effects of


reinforcement withdrawal provide further

evidence that it possesses some of the functional

properties of an aversive event. It has been shown


(Ferster, 1958; Morse & Herrnstein, 1956;

Zimmerman, 1963) that behavior which prevents


or terminates reinforcement withdrawal is

effectively maintained in much the same way as

avoidance responses are sustained by their


success in preventing the occurrence of physically

painful consequences. Moreover, neutral stimuli

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1080
that are regularly associated with reinforcement

withdrawal tend to acquire negative properties

(Ferster, 1960; Ferster, Appel, & Hiss, 1962).


Unlike the effects of physical punishment,

however, stimulus events that signal the advent of


reinforcement withdrawal do not seem to
generate disruptive emotional arousal

(Leitenberg, 1965).

Several attempts have been made to compare


the relative power of reinforcement withdrawal

and presentation of physically aversive stimuli in

reducing selected response patterns. Tolman &

Mueller (1964) employed different types of


punishers with a young rhesus monkey who
developed a marked affinity for one toe,

commonly sleeping with it in his mouth and

sucking it while climbing about on two hands and

one foot. Since the monkey had mainly interacted

with humans (monkey-rearing practices would

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1081
probably not have produced a chronic toe-sucker),

visual contact with people (viewed through a

small window) served as the positive reinforcer.


Punishment periods, in which the window was

closed whenever the monkey placed its toe in its

mouth and was opened upon removal of the toe,

alternated with nonpunished periods during

which the window remained continually open. The


second phase of the experiment followed a similar
procedure except that window-closing was

replaced by unpleasant sounds at the onset of toe-

sucking; the sounds terminated as soon as the toe

was removed. During noncontingent punishment


the aversive sounds were simply presented at

periodic intervals without regard to the animal’s


behavior. Punishment through aversive sound

stimulation produced a marked reduction in toe-

sucking; recovery was delayed, but once the


response recurred it was emitted at a relatively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1082
high rate. On the other hand, punishment by the

removal of social stimuli resulted in less abrupt

decrement; recovery was more gradual and less


complete.

Comparative data are difficult to evaluate


when based on an experiment with a single

subject, in which potency of the punishing stimuli

was not equated, and possible order effects were


not controlled. However, the findings of Tolman &
Mueller are essentially corroborated by McMillan

(1967), who assessed the relative efficacy of

contingent shock and temporary withdrawal of

rewards in eliminating a concurrently reinforced


response. Both types of punishers reduced
responding to about the same degree, but the

time-out procedure was associated with less

behavioral recovery.

The adjunctive use of punishment by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1083
reinforcement withdrawal has certain advantages
over physically aversive procedures. As previously

shown, aversive interventions may arouse fear


and avoidance of punishing agents, and thus

weaken their potential influence. In contrast,

methods that chiefly involve the removal of

positive reinforcers not only generate much


weaker emotional effects, but they tend to foster

and maintain orientation toward the agents who

control the desired positive resources. If

restoration of the positive reinforcers is made

contingent upon performance of alternative


modes of behavior, rapid behavioral changes may

in fact result.

BEHAVIORAL CONTROL BY WITHDRAWAL OF


POSITIVE REINFORCERS

Reinforcement withdrawal has proved to be an

effective means of managing deleterious behavior


that often impedes the person’s own development

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1084
and seriously infringes on the well-being of others.
If combined with methods that foster constructive

alternatives this form of behavioral control can aid


achievement of enduring changes in social

behavior.

In applications of time-out procedures,


behaviors that are considered unacceptable and
the consequences that they will produce are

clearly explained in advance. When social


exclusion is employed as the negative outcome, as

is usually the case, each transgression results in

brief social withdrawal that is carried out

immediately, naturally, and in a firm but

nonhostile manner. If, during the time-out


interval, the person continues to display

obstreperous behavior, the period of exclusion is


extended until cessation of the behavior. Under

this type of contingency self-control is quickly


established. Since social attention accompanying a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1085
disciplinary intervention may reinforce the

preceding deviant behavior, the change agent

minimizes social and verbal interaction as much as


possible while the negative sanction is being

applied.

The way in which time-out contingencies may

be employed as part of a broad program is

illustrated in the treatment of an autistic three-


year- old boy by Wolf, Risley, & Mees (1964). In
addition to grossly retarded social and verbal

development, the boy exhibited violent tantrums

that included head-banging, face-slapping, hair-


pulling, and face-scratching. After a tantrum he
was badly bruised and bleeding, and refused to

sleep at night, forcing one or both parents to


remain by his bed. Sedatives, tranquilizers, and

physical restraints were applied without success.

When it became clear that refusal to wear

eyeglasses (necessitated by the removal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1086
cataractal lenses) might result in ultimate

blindness, psychologists were invited by the

hospital staff to devise a treatment program for


him. Ward attendants, and later his parents,

carried out the prescribed program under the

guidance and direction of the consultants.

Most of the boy’s recurrent problem behaviors,

which obstructed any treatment efforts, were


eliminated by a procedure combining extinction
and punishment through reinforcement

withdrawal. In modifying the tantrum behavior,

for example, whenever the boy slapped himself


and whined he was placed in his room where he
remained until the tantrum ceased. During the

initial phase of treatment the attendants offered


elaborate apologetic explanations while escorting

him to his room, and showered him with attention

when he returned. These accompanying reactions

converted the exclusion into a rewarding

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1087
experience, with the result that the boy displayed

frequent tantrums followed by brief perfunctory

trips to the room. A minimum time of 10 minutes


in the room was therefore instituted and the

attendants were instructed to minimize the

inadvertent social reinforcement. Under this

contingency the frequency of violent tantrums

gradually declined and eventually disappeared.


Eating problems, in which the boy grabbed food
from other children’s plates, threw it about the

room, or ate with his fingers, were rapidly

eliminated in a similar manner. The attendants

simply removed him from the dining room for the


remainder of his meal for snatching or tossing

food after a warning, and withdrew his plate for a


few minutes whenever he ate with his fingers.

Use of social exclusion sometimes leads to new

problems which must be dealt with. During the

period of isolation the child may, for example,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1088
proceed to wreck the furnishings in the room, he

may inflict injury upon himself, or exhibit other

harmful behaviors. This problem arose in mild


form during the treatment of the boy’s sleeping

patterns, which were extremely irregular and

required the extended presence of the parents or

attendants at bedtime. After completion of

pleasant bedtime routines, the boy was put to bed,


and left with the door open. If he refused to
remain in bed, the door was closed, which initially

gave rise to violent temper tantrums. These

tantrums were controlled by extending the time

that the door remained closed until after the


tantrum subsided. Under the cumulative

punishment contingency the tantrums rapidly


disappeared and normal sleeping patterns were

established by the sixth night. Similarly,

destructive discarding of eyeglasses ceased within


five days when the boy was placed in his room for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1089
10 minutes whenever he threw his glasses, or if

tantrums developed, until they abated.

The foregoing account has emphasized

aversive controls. It should be noted that, in the

total program, positive reinforcement was also


extensively employed to get the boy to wear

eyeglasses, and he was positively guided and

rewarded for more appropriate behavior patterns.


Prior to treatment, the boy was totally lacking in
communicative skills, which were gradually

established through reinforced modeling. The

development of more rewarding competencies

undoubtedly contributed to the effectiveness of


mild punishment.

As the boy’s condition improved, contacts with

his family and home were progressively increased.

At first the parents visited the hospital for one

hour and observed the way in which tantrums and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1090
bedtime problems were handled by the
attendants. Subsequently the parents made

several visits a week, during which an attendant


observed and instructed them in their handling of

their son. Then he began short home visits

accompanied by an attendant, followed by

progressively longer visits. After discharge, he no


longer manifested severe behavior problems, he

had become increasingly verbal, and the family

interactions were considerably more enjoyable.

The reinforcement procedures were extensively

applied for several years in a nursery school


setting where the boy made sufficient progress to

enroll in public school (Risley & Wolf, 1966; Wolf,


Risley, Johnston, Harris, & Allen, 1967). Time-out

procedures were used occasionally in early phases


to eliminate tantrum behavior, exhibited

whenever he was asked to perform an


instructional task, and to control hurtful behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1091
toward other children.

Reference has already been made to several


studies in which self-injurious behavior in autistic

children was completely eliminated or markedly

reduced by reinforcement withdrawal. Similar


methods have been shown by Hamilton, Stephens,

& Allen (1967) to be uniformly successful in

eliminating injurious aggressive and self-


destructive behavior in severely retarded
adolescents. In each instance, the individual was

physically confined to a chair in a time-out area for

a fixed period following the occurrence of

injurious behavior. In one case, for example, a girl


beat her head and back against the wall a total of
35,906 times during four 6-hour observations—

about once every three seconds! When the time-

out contingency was later put in effect, head-

banging precipitously dropped to a negligible level

of 7, 2, 0, 1, 0 for five successive weeks and never

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1092
reappeared during nine months of follow-up

study. Of considerable import, both from a clinical

and ethical standpoint, is that after the continual


self-mutilating behavior was eliminated the

retardates participated with evident enjoyment in

daily social and recreational activities. The authors

make the interesting observation that, following

removal of pervasive deviant behavior, individuals


begin to make contact with potentially rewarding
aspects of the environment that were always

available, which automatically reinforce beneficial

modes of behavior. Once a self-regulating

interaction between behavior and environmental


contingencies is initiated, widespread changes

may result even though alternative behaviors


were never deliberately established.

Several additional case reports have been

published which furnish quantitative data

indicating the efficacy of time-out procedures in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1093
modifying diverse behavioral disorders. Sloane,

Johnston, & Bijou (1968) rapidly eliminated

extreme aggressiveness in a preschool boy, and


Burchard & Tyler (1965) reduced the antisocial

behavior of a delinquent adolescent by contingent

social exclusion. This procedure has also been

employed by Tyler & Brown (1967) on a group

basis with institutionalized delinquents. The staff


of a rehabilitation center was unable to check
aggressive, disruptive behavior of delinquent boys

that was highly troublesome during recreational

periods but not serious enough to warrant severe

sanctions. A program involving short periods of


mild punishment for misbehavior was instituted

to control the group. Every time a boy displayed


offensive behavior he was immediately placed in a

room in the cottage for 15 minutes without any

threats, invective, sermonizing, or negotiations for


second chances. Consistent with previous findings,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1094
brief social withdrawal produced a marked

decline in the incidence of disruptive behavior. In

order to determine whether punishment had


lasting effects, the aversive consequences were

discontinued; instead, the staff reprimanded the


boys verbally and occasionally closed the
recreational facility temporarily when their

behavior got completely out of hand. During the

period when negative sanctions were removed


there was a rapid increase in the rate of offenses.

However, when the punishment contingency was

subsequently reinstated disruptive behavior


subsided with equal rapidity and remained at a

low level. Apparently the boys were quick to


discriminate the changes in conditions of

reinforcement and regulated their behavior

accordingly. The aversive control thus proved to


be an exceedingly effective management technique

but, unlike previous findings, it produced no

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1095
enduring changes in behavior.

The conflicting results are most likely due to


the types of reinforcement systems created by

peers in institutions for delinquents. An

observational study by Buehler, Patterson, &


Furniss (1966) revealed that delinquent peers

provide extensive positive reinforcement for

antisocial behavior, whereas they typically punish


attitudes and behavior that conform to
institutional norms. If negative sanctions applied

by staff members are strong enough to outweigh

peer influences, socially conforming behavior may

be achieved and sustained as long as the


institutional sanctions remain in effect. However,
when aversive controls are removed, peer

reinforcement practices will quickly reinstate

deviant patterns of behavior. To achieve stable

changes in behavior would require modifications

of the contingency systems practiced by peers like

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1096
those, for example, that Cohen (1968) successfully

accomplished by placing delinquents’ livelihood

within the institution on a self-determining basis.


When contingencies are arranged so that

constructive behavior is adequately reinforced

and antisocial behavior has limited pay-off,

antisocial personalities are less inclined to seek

their rewards from outwitting staff members or


from perturbed reactions of others to crisis-
producing transgressions (Colman & Baker, 1968).

“Systematic exclusion” is increasingly

employed in school settings (Chapman, 1962;


Kiersey, 1958) as a means of controlling seriously
disturbing behavior in children after other

available methods have failed. In this program the


child, his parents, his teacher, the school

psychologist, and the principal meet as a group to

arrange explicit contingencies between the child’s

disruptive conduct and its social consequences. At

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1097
this meeting, each participant’s role is specifically

outlined. It is explained that the school cannot

permit a child continually to disrupt the


educational activities of an entire class. The child’s

help is enlisted to control, as best he can, behavior

that has disturbing effects on all concerned.

Whenever he displays behavior that exceeds

certain clearly defined limits, the teacher must ask


him to leave school for the remainder of the day.
In order to remove any inadvertent positive

reinforcement of the deviant behavior, the teacher

is instructed not to threaten, coax, urge, or scold

the child, nor is she to engage in persuasive


attempts to alter his behavior. Rather, the

previously agreed-upon sanctions are applied


immediately, straightforwardly, and in a matter-

of-fact way. By having the teacher apply

prearranged contingencies in a consistent and


objective fashion, the child is more likely to regard

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1098
the disciplinary interventions as natural,

inevitable consequences of his behavior, than as

arbitrary or malevolent treatment by his teacher.


If the child should refuse to leave when he is

requested, the principal removes him and the


period of exclusion is automatically extended.

When the child arrives home, the parents are

instructed to keep him on the premises during the


remaining school hours, but to refrain from
punishing, scolding, or applying other disciplinary

measures. The psychologist’s function is to

structure and to supervise the program, to offer

the participants positive assistance when needed,


and to decide when the contingencies are to be
discontinued.

The authors report that systematic application

of exclusion consequences produces rapid and

lasting reduction in chronically disruptive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1099
behavior, but no quantitative data are presented,
nor is there any specification of the conditions

under which this method is most efficacious. It is


apparent that an exclusion procedure will produce

no behavioral changes, or may even augment

deviant responsiveness, if the situation from

which a person has been removed is unrewarding


or highly unpleasant. Under these circumstances,

withdrawal has positively reinforcing rather than

punishing effects. It has been shown in laboratory

studies (Herrnstein, 1955), for example, that

behavior which produces a temporary time-out


from reinforcement increases in frequency if the

conditions of reinforcement that the behavior


avoids are relatively unfavorable.

The effectiveness of exclusion procedures can


probably be greatly enhanced if, in addition to the

punishment contingency, the child is provided


with certain privileges and rewards for each class

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1100
period during which he does not engage in

disruptive behavior. It would also be of interest to

explore the rate at which deviant behavior


decreases as a function of varying durations of

exclusion, the types of situations to which the

child is removed, and the attractiveness of the

setting from which he is withdrawn. Findings of

studies employing time-out contingencies to


control grossly deviant behavior indicate that
much briefer periods of exclusion might work as

well, or even better, than full-day suspensions

from classroom activities.

Summary

The present chapter has discussed the

processes whereby response patterns are

eliminated through the use of punishing stimuli.

Punishing consequences may involve either

removal of positive reinforcers or presentation of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1101
aversive events. Punishment is believed to achieve

its reductive effects by producing conditioned fear

that elicits inhibitory behavior’s, or by facilitating


the appearance of responses that are incompatible

with and, therefore, capable of supplanting the

punished behavior. The degree of control exerted

by punishment is largely a function of the

intensity, duration and distribution of aversive


consequences, their temporal relation to the
behavior to be modified, the strength with which

punished responses are concurrently reinforced,

the availability of alternative modes of behavior

for securing rewards, the level of instigation to


perform the negatively sanctioned behavior, and

the psychological characteristics of punishing


agents.

Several different theories have been proposed

concerning the locus of aversive control.

Environmental stimuli that are regularly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1102
associated with punishing experiences may

become conditioned elicitors of fear and have

suppressive effects on behavior. A second


interpretation is that proprioceptive stimuli

arising from the punished behavior itself acquire

negatively reinforcing properties through

association with punishing experiences on

previous occasions. Although response-contingent


punishment produces reductive effects, evidence
that behavioral inhibitions can be readily acquired

and extinguished under curare or through

vicarious experiences without any motoric

responding seriously challenge peripheral views


that aversive control resides mainly in response-

produced cues. Rather, these findings lend weight


to the theory that the effects of punishment are

mediated through central controlling mechanisms.

On the basis of previous response consequences,


experienced either directly or vicariously under

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1103
differing circumstances, individuals infer the

likelihood that a given course of action will

produce punishing outcomes. Symbolic


representation of these anticipated consequences

can exercise some degree of control over overt


responsiveness. The same behavior may,
therefore, be freely expressed or inhibited in

similar environmental situations as a result of

intricate discriminations of differences in


reinforcement contingencies.

Aversive forms of control have been primarily

employed to eliminate persistent responses that

are automatically self-reinforcing upon


occurrence, to reduce the incidence of seriously
disturbing patterns of behavior for which the

maintaining positive reinforcements cannot be

identified or readily eliminated, and to bring

rapidly under control responses that have

injurious consequences for the performer or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1104
others. While findings of controlled studies are

still somewhat limited, preliminary results of

applications of aversive procedures to the


modification of persistent deviant behavior are

considerably more encouraging than laboratory

findings would lead one to expect. Chronic self-

mutilating behavior, debilitating occupational

cramps of long standing, stuttering, antisocial


aggression, and deviant patterns of sexual
behavior have been substantially reduced or

eliminated by methods relying upon contingent

application of negative consequences.

The relative ineffectiveness of punishment in


producing durable reductive effects in laboratory

situations has probably resulted from the fact that,


with few exceptions, the punished response

constitutes the sole means of securing rewards.

Hence, it comes as no surprise that in single-

response situations punished behavior is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1105
performed for some time even though it incurs

aversive consequences, and it often reappears

when punishment is discontinued. In contrast,


people generally have numerous options available

in everyday life. Even though punishment may

only temporarily inhibit dominant responses,

during the period of suppression alternative

modes of behavior may be strengthened


sufficiently to supplant the original response
tendencies. Moreover, brief cessation of behavior

that is highly disturbing to others eventually

draws positive reactions from appreciative

associates. The new conditions of reinforcement


created by cessation of deviant responses may

foster and maintain their relinquishment. By the


same token, rodents or pigeons that were

suddenly showered with food pellets and

increased positive attention from relieved


comrades, after inhibiting a socially distressing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1106
bar-press response, would undoubtedly abandon

the cherished bar more rapidly than they would if

no alternative activities were available and


response inhibition produced no outcomes other

than the removal of aversive stimulation and loss


of food rewards. For these and other reasons, the
social implications of laboratory findings

regarding aversive control must be accepted with

reservations.

Some of the cited negative by-products of

aversive control can be avoided or reduced to a

large extent by the use of discrimination

procedures in conjunction with punishing


consequences. Moreover, punishment based upon
the removal of positive reinforcers ordinarily

reduces undesired behavior without producing

fear learning or avoidant behavior. This procedure

also tends to maintain strong approach tendencies

toward change agents, and, when reinstatement of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1107
approval, possessions, or privileges is made

conditional upon more appropriate behavior, it

provides more positive support and guidance than


mere administration of negative outcomes.

Lasting elimination of detrimental behavior

can be most effectively facilitated by punishment if

competing response patterns are simultaneously

rewarded. Negative sanctions may therefore be


successfully employed to hold undesired
responses in check while alternative modes of

behavior are being established and strengthened.

Another means of weakening undesired behavior,


relying upon extinction operations, is discussed in
the next chapter.

REFERENCES

Appel, J. B. Punishment in the squirrel monkey Saimiri


sciurea. Science, 1961, 133, 36.

Aronfreed, J. Conduct and conscience. New York:


Academic Press, 1968.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1108
Aronfreed, J., & Reber, A. Internalized behavioral
suppression and the timing of social punishment.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965,
1, 3-16.

Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. Effect of the severity of


threat on the devaluation of forbidden behavior.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963,
66, 584-588.

Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. Punishment as a


discriminative stimulus and conditioned
reinforcer with humans. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, 411-
419.

Azrin, N. H. Some effects of two intermittent


schedules of immediate and non- immediate
punishment, Journal of Psychology, 1956, 42, 3-
21.

Azrin, N. H. Some effects of noise on human behavior.


Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
1958, 1, 183-200.

Azrin, N. II. Punishment and recovery during fixed-


ratio performance. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1959, 2, 301-305.

Azrin, N. H. Effects of punishment intensity during

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1109
variable-interval reinforcement. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1960, 3, 123-
142.

Azrin, N. H., Halse, D. F., Holz, W. C., & Hutchinson, R.


R. Motivational aspects of escape and
punishment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
of Behavior, 1965, 8, 31-44.

Azrin, N. H., & Holz, W. C. Punishment. In W. K. Honig


(Ed.), Operant behavior. New York: Appleton-
Century-Crofts, 1966. pp. 380-447.

Baer, D. M. Effect of withdrawal of positive


reinforcement on an extinguishing response in
young children. Child Development, 1961, 32, 67-
74.

Baer, D. M. Laboratory control of thumbsucking by


withdrawal and reinstatement of reinforcement.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
1962, 5, 525-528.

Bandura, A. Vicarious processes: A case of no-trial


learning. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology. Vol. II. New York:
Academic Press, 1965. pp. 1-55.

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression.


New York: Ronald, 1959.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1110
Barrett, B. Reduction in rate of multiple tics by free
operant conditioning methods. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Diseases, 1962, 135, 187-195.

Beach, F. A., Conovitz, M. W., Steinberg, F., & Goldstein,


A. C. Experimental inhibition and restoration of
mating behavior in male rats. Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 1956, 89, 165-181.

Beech, H. R. The symptomatic treatment of writer’s


cramp. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), Behavior therapy
and the neuroses. New York: Pergamon, 1960. pp.
349-372.

Benton, A. A. Effects of the timing of negative


response consequences on the observational
learning of resistance to temptation in children.
Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, 27, 2153-2154.

Black, A. H. The extinction of avoidance responses


under curare. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 519-524.

Black, A. H., Carlson, N. J., & Solomon, R. L. Exploratory


studies of the conditioning of autonomic
responses in curarized dogs. Psychological
Monographs, 1962, 76 (29, Whole No. 548).

Bloodstein, O. Conditions under which stuttering is


reduced or absent: a review of literature. Journal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1111
of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1949, 14, 295-
302.

Boe, E. E. Extinction as a function of intensity of


punishment, amount of training, and
reinforcement of a competing response. Canadian
Journal of Psychology, 1964, 18, 328-342.

Boe, E. E., & Church, R. M. Permanent effects of


punishment during extinction. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1967,
63, 486-492.

Bolles, R. C. Theory of motivation. New York: Harper &


Row, 1967.

Bond, I. K., & Evans, D. R. Avoidance therapy: Its use in


two cases of underwear fetishism. Canadian
Medical Association Journal, 1967, 96, 1160-
1162.

Brookshire, R. H., & Martin, R. R. The differential


effects of three verbal punishers on the
disfluencies of normal speakers. Journal of Speech
and Hearing Research, 1967, 10, 496-505.

Brutten, E. J., & Shoemaker, D. J. The modification of


stuttering. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall,
1967.

Bucher, B., & Lovaas, O. I. Use of aversive stimulation

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1112
in behavior modification. In M. R. Jones (Ed.),
Miami symposium on the prediction of behavior,
1967: Aversive stimulation. Coral Gables, Fla.:
University of Miami Press, 1968. pp. 77-145.

Buehler, R. E., Patterson, G. R., & Furniss, J. M. The


reinforcement of behavior in institutional
settings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966,
4, 157- 167.

Burchard, J., & Tyler, V., Jr. The modification of


delinquent behaviour through operant
conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
1965, 2, 245- 250.

Camp, D. S. Response suppression as a function of


punishment intensity and of response-
punishment contiguity. Dissertation Abstracts,
1965, 26, 3494.

Chapman, R. W. School suspension as therapy.


Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1962, 40, 731-
732.

Church, R. M. The varied effects of punishment on


behavior. Psychological Review, 1963, 70, 369-
402.

Cohen, H. L. Educational therapy: The design of


learning environments. In J. M. Shlien (Ed.),

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1113
Research in psychotherapy. Washington, D. C.:
American Psychological Association, 1968. pp.
21-53.

Colman, A. D., & Baker, S. L., Jr. Utilization of an


operant conditioning model for the treatment of
character and behavior disorders in a military
setting. Unpublished manuscript, Walter Reed
Army Institute of Research, Washington, D. C.,
1968.

Daly, D. A., & Cooper, E. B. Rate of stuttering


adaptation under two electroshock conditions.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 49-54.

Dardano, J. F., & Sauerbrunn, D. Selective


reinforcement of concurrent progressive ratio
behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior, 1964, 7, 51-65.

Davis, D. M. The relation of repetitions in the speech


of young children to certain measures of
language maturity and situational factors: Part I.
Journal of Speech Disorders, 1939, 4, 303-318.

Davis, D. M. The relation of repetitions in the speech


of young children to certain measures of
language maturity and situational factors: Part II.
Journal of Speech Disorders, 1940, 5, 235-246.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1114
Desiderato, O. Generalization of conditioned
suppression. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1964, 59, 434-437.

Dinsmoor, J. A. Punishment: I. The avoidance


hypothesis. Psychological Review, 1954, 61, 34-
46.

Elliott, R., & Tighe, T. J. Breaking the cigarette habit: A


technique involving threatened loss of money.
Psychological Record, 1968, 18, 503-513.

Estes, W. K. An experimental study of punishment.


Psychological Monographs, 1944, 57 (3, Whole
No. 26.3).

Feldman, M. P., & MacCulloch, M. J. A systematic


approach to the treatment of homosexuality by
conditioned aversion: Preliminary report.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 1964, 121, 167-
171.

Feldman, M. P., & MacCulloch, M. J. The application of


anticipatory avoidance learning to the treatment
of homosexuality. 1. Theory, technique and
preliminary results. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1965, 2, 165- 183.

Ferster, C. B. Control of behavior in chimpanzees and


pigeons by time out from positive reinforcement.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1115
Psychological Monographs, 1958, 72 (8, Whole
No. 461).

Ferster, C. B. Suppression of a performance under


differential reinforcement of low rates by a pre-
time-out stimulus. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1960, 3, 143-153.

Ferster, C. B., Appel, J. B., & Hiss, R. A. The effect of


drugs on a fixed-ratio performance suppressed
by a pre-time-out stimulus. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5, 73-88.

Flanagan, B., Goldiamond, I., & Azrin, N. H.


Instatement of stuttering in normally fluent
individuals through operant procedures. Science,
1959, 130, 979-981.

Fowler, H., & Miller, N. E. Facilitation and inhibition of


runway performance by hind- and forepaw shock
of various intensities. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 196.3, 56, 801-805.

Freedman, J. L. Long-term behavioral effects of


cognitive dissonance. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 1965, 1, 145-155.

Goldiamond, I. Fluent and nonfluent speech


(stuttering): Analysis and operant techniques for
control. In L. Krasner & L. P. Ullmann (Eds.),

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1116
Research in behavior modification. New York:
Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. pp. 106-156. (a)

Goldiamond, I. Operant properties of verbal fluency


and nonfluency. Progress Report, U.S.P.H.
Research Grant M-8876. Institute for Behavioral
Research, 1965. (b)

Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of learning. New York:


Harper, 1935.

Hamilton, J., Stephens, L., & Allen, P. Controlling


aggressive and destructive behavior in severely
retarded institutionalized residents. American
Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1967, 71, 852-856.

Hearst, E., & Sidman, M. Some behavioral effects of a


concurrently positive and negative stimulus.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
1961, 4, 251-256.

Hendry, D. P., & Van Toller, C. Fixed-ratio punishment


with continuous reinforcement. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 293-
300.

Herman, R. L., & Azrin, N. H. Punishment by noise in


an alternative response situation. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 185-
188.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1117
Herrnstein, R. J. Behavioral consequences of removal
of a discriminative stimulus associated with
variable-interval reinforcement. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, 1955.

Hicks, D. J. Effects of co-observer’s sanctions and adult


presence on imitative aggression. Child
Development, 1968, 39, 303-309.

Hill, B. Boss of Britain’s underworld. London: Naldreth,


1955.

Hill, H. E. An experimental study of disorganization of


speech and manual responses in normal subjects.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1954, 19,
295-305.

Hoffman, M. L. Power assertion by the parent and its


impact on the child. Child Development, 1960, 31,
129-143.

Hoffman, H. S., & Fleshier, M. Stimulus factors in


aversive controls: The generalization of
conditioned suppression. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, 371-
378.

Hoffman, H. S., & Fleshier, M. Stimulus aspects of


aversive controls: Stimulus generalization of
conditioned suppression following

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1118
discrimination training. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 233-
239.

Hoffman, H. S., & Fleshier, M. Stimulus aspects of


aversive controls: the effects of response
contingent shock. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1965, 8, 89-96.

Hoffman, H. S., Fleshier, M., & Jensen, P. K. Stimulus


aspects of aversive controls: The retention of
conditioned suppression. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 575-
583.

Hoffman, M. L. Power assertion by the parent and its


impact on the child. Child Development, 1960, 31,
129-143.

Holz, W. C., & Azrin, N. H. Interactions between the


discriminative and aversive properties of
punishment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
of Behavior, 1962, 5, 229-234.

Holz, W. C., Azrin, N. H., & Ayllon, T. Elimination of


behavior of mental patients by response-
produced extinction. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 407-12.

Holz, W. C., Azrin, N. H., & Ulrich, R. E. Punishment of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1119
temporally spaced responding. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 115-
122.

Honig, W. K., & Slivka, R. M. Stimulus generalization of


the effects of punishment. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 21-25.

Hunt, H. F., & Brady, J. V. Some quantitative and


qualitative differences between “anxiety” and
“punishment” conditioning. American
Psychologist, 1951,6,276-277 (abstract).

Hunt, H. F., & Brady, J. V. Some effects of punishment


and intercurrent anxiety on a simple operant.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 1955, 48, 305-310.

Johnson, W., et al. A study of the onset and


development of stuttering. Journal of Speech
Disorders, 1942, 7, 251-257.

Kaufman, A. Increased suppression during


punishment applied to the responding member.
Psychonomic Science, 1964, 1, 311-312.

Kiersey, D. W. Systematic exclusion, an experimental


regimen for chronic misbehavior. Paper read at
the Annual Conference of California Association
of School Psychologists and Psychometrists,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1120
Asilomar, Calif., 1957.

Leitenberg, H. Is time-out from positive reinforcement


an aversive event? A review of the experimental
evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 1965, 64, 428-
441.

Lichtenstein, F. E. Studies of anxiety: I. The production


of a feeding inhibition in dogs. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1950,
43, 16-29.

Liversedge, L. A., & Sylvester, J. D. Conditioning


techniques in the treatment of writer’s cramp.
Lancet, 1955, 2,1147-1149.

Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., & Kassorla, I. C.


Experimental studies in childhood schizophrenia:
Analysis of self-destructive behavior. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 1965, 2, 67-84.

MacCulloch, M. J., Feldman, M. P., & Pinshoff, J. M. The


application of anticipatory avoidance learning to
the treatment of homosexuality. II. Avoidance
response latencies and pulse rate changes.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1965, 3, 21-43.

McMillan, D. E. A comparison of the punishing effects


of response-produced shock and response-
produced time out. Journal of the Experimental

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1121
Analysis of Behavior, 1967, 10, 439-449.

Masserman, J. M. Behavior and neurosis. Chicago:


University of Chicago Press, 1943.

Mischel, W., & Liebert, R. M. Effects of discrepancies


between observed and imposed reward criteria
on their acquisition and transmission. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 45-53.

Morse, W. H., & Herrnstein, R. J. The maintenance of


avoidance behavior using the removal of a
conditioned positive reinforcer as the aversive
stimulus. American Psychologist, 1956, 11, 430
(Abstract).

Mowrer, O. H. An experimental analogue of


“regression,” with incidental observations on
“reaction formation.” Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1940, 35, 56-87.

Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and behavior. New


York: Wiley, 1960.

Nelson, S. E. Personal contact as a factor in the


transfer of stuttering. Human Biology, 1939, 11,
393-401.

Nigro, M. R. Punishment of an extinguishing shock-


avoidance response by time-out from positive
reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1122
Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, 53-62.

Parke, R. D., & Walters, R. H. Some factors influencing


the efficacy of punishment training for inducing
response inhibition. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 1967, 32 (1, Serial
No. 109).

Pastore, N. The role of arbitrariness in the frustration-


aggression hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1952, 47, 728-731.

Powell, J., & Azrin, N. The effects of shock as a


punisher for cigarette smoking. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 63-71.

Quist, R. W., & Martin, R. R. The effect of response


contingent verbal punishment on stuttering.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1967, 10,
795-800.

Rescorla, R. H., & Solomon, R. L. Two-process learning


theory: Relationships between Pavlovian
conditioning and instrumental learning.
Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 151-182.

Risley, T. R. The effects and side effects of punishing


the autistic behaviors of a deviant child. Journal
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 21-34.

Risley, T., & Wolf, M. M. Experimental manipulation of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1123
autistic behaviors and generalization into the
home. In R. Ulrich, T. Stachnik, & J. Mabry (Eds.),
Control of human behavior. Glenview, 111.: Scott,
Foresman, 1966. pp. 193-198.

Rotenberg, I. C. Effect of schedule and severity of


punishment on verbal behavior. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1959, 57, 193-200.

Sandler, J., & Quagliano, J. Punishment in a signal


avoidance situation. Paper read at the
Southeastern Psychological Association Meeting,
Gatlinburg, Tenn., 1964.

Savoye, A. L. The effect of the Skinner-Estes operant


conditioning punishment paradigm upon the
production of non-fluencies in normal speakers.
Unpublished master’s thesis, University of
Pittsburgh, 1959.

Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. Patterns of child


rearing. Evanston, 111.: Row, Peterson, 1957.

Shames, G. H., & Sherrick, C. C., Jr. A discussion of


nonfluency and stuttering as operant behavior.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1963, 28,
3-18.

Sheehan, J. G. The modification of stuttering through


non-reinforcement. Journal of Abnormal and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1124
Social Psychology, 1951, 46, 51-63.

Sheehan, J. G. An integration of psychotherapy and


speech therapy through a conflict theory of
stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Disorders, 1954, 19, 474-482.

Sheehan, J. G. Conflict theory of stuttering. In J.


Eisenson (Ed.), Stuttering: A symposium. New
York: Harper, 1958. pp. 121-166.

Sheehan, J. G., & Martyn, M. M. Spontaneous recovery


from stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Research, 1966, 9, 121-135.

Siegel, G. M., & Martin, R. R. Punishment of


disfluencies in normal speakers. Journal of Speech
and Hearing Research, 1966, 9, 208-218.

Sloane, H. N., Jr., Johnston, M. K., & Bijou, S. W.


Successive modification of aggressive behavior
and aggressive fantasy play by management of
contingencies. Unpublished manuscript,
University of Illinois, 1968.

Solomon, R. L. Punishment. American Psychologist,


1964, 19, 239-253.

Solomon, R. L., & Turner, L. H. Discriminative classical


conditioning in dogs paralyzed by curare can
later control discriminative avoidance responses

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1125
in the normal state. Psychological Review, 1962,
69, 202-219.

Solomon, R. L., Turner, L. H., & Lessac, M. S. Some


effects of delay of punishment on resistance to
temptation in dogs. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1968, 8, 233-238.

Stassi, E. J. Disfluency of normal speakers and


reinforcement. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Research, 1961, 4, 358-361.

Storms, L. M., Boroczi, C., & Broen, W. E., Jr. Effects of


punishment as a function of strain of rat and
duration of shock. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1963, 56, 1022-1026.

Sylvester, J. D., & Liversedge, L. A. Conditioning and


the occupational cramps. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.),
Behaviour therapy and the neuroses. New York:
Pergamon, 1960. pp. 334-348.

Tate, B. G., & Baroff, G. S. Aversive control of self-


injurious behavior in a psychotic boy. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 281-287.

Timmons, E. O. Weakening verbal behavior: A


comparison of four methods. Journal of General
Psychology, 1962, 67, 155-158.

Tolman, C. W., & Mueller, M. R. Laboratory control of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1126
toe-sucking in a young rhesus monkey by two
kinds of punishment. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 323-325.

Tyler, V. O., Jr., & Brown, G. D. The use of swift, brief


isolation as a group control device for
institutionalized delinquents. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1967, 5, 1-9.

Van Riper, C. The influence of empathic response on


frequency of stuttering. Psychological
Monographs, 1937, 49 (1, Whole No. 217).

Walters, R. H., & Demkow, L. Timing of punishment as


a determinant of resistance to temptation. Child
Development, 1963, 34, 207-214.

Walters, R. H., Parke, R. D., & Cane, V. A. Timing of


punishment and the observation of consequences
to others as determinants of response inhibition.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1965, 2,
10-30.

Walton, D., & Mather, M. D. The relevance of


generalization techniques to the treatment of
stammering and phobic symptoms. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 196.3, 1, 121-125.

Weiner, H. Some effects of response cost upon human


operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1127
Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5, 201-208.

Whiting, J. W. M., & Mowrer, O. H. Habit progression


and regression—a laboratory study of some
factors relevant to human socialization. Journal of
Comparative Psychology, 1943, 36, 229-253.

Wischner, G. J. Stuttering behavior and learning: A


preliminary theoretical formulation. Journal of
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1950, 15, 324-335.

Wolf, M., Risley, T., Johnston, M., Harris, F., & Allen, E.
Application of operant conditioning procedures
to the behavior problems of an autistic child: A
follow-up and extension. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1967, 5, 103-111.

Wolf, M., Risley, T., & Mees, H. Application of operant


conditioning procedures to the behavior
problems of an autistic child. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1964, 1, 305-312.

Zimmerman, J. A. A multiple schedule of time out


avoidance with monkeys. American Psychologist,
1963, 18,422 (Abstract).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1128
6

Extinction
When reinforcement for a learned response is
withheld, individuals will continue to exhibit that

behavior for a limited time. Under repeated

nonreinforcement, however, the behavior

decreases and eventually disappears. This decline

in responsiveness as a function of nonrewarded

repetition of a response is called extinction. In


naturally occurring situations response patterns
sustained by positive reinforcement are frequently

eliminated simply by discontinuing the rewards


that ordinarily produce the behavior. Avoidance

behavior, which is strongly maintained by its

success in forestalling anticipated aversive


experiences, can similarly be extinguished if, when

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1129
it is prevented from occurring in the presence of
threatening stimuli no adverse consequences are

engendered. With repeated nonreinforced

exposure to subjective threats, protective behavior


that is inappropriate to the altered circumstances

is eventually abandoned. In both instances the


extinction process is essentially the same,
although the procedures vary according to the

nature of the maintaining consequences.

Interpretations of the Extinction Process

The rate of extinction is governed by a number

of factors, among them the irregularity with which

the behavior was reinforced in the past, the


amount of effort required to perform it, the level

of deprivation present during extinction, the ease


with which changes in conditions of reinforcement
can be discerned, and the availability of alternative

modes of response. Because of the diversity of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1130
controlling variables, a number of different

theoretical conceptualizations of extinction have

been proposed (Kimble, 1961), each emphasizing


a separate aspect of the phenomenon. These

formulations and their supporting evidence are

reviewed next.

CONDITIONED INHIBITION THEORY

According to Hull’s (1943) theory of extinction,


repeated nonreinforced elicitation of an effortful

response generates an inhibitory state analogous

to fatigue, which tends to counteract the


recurrence of the response. Since this reactive
inhibition dissipates with time, it presumably

exerts only a temporary suppressing influence on

behavior. Some support for the fatigue hypothesis

is provided by the well-known phenomenon of

spontaneous recovery. When a response has been

initially extinguished, it tends to reappear, though

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1131
typically at reduced strength, with the passage of
time, suggesting the dissipation of a transitory

form of inhibitory control. The fact, however, that


the amount of recovery following successive

extinctions becomes progressively smaller until it

eventually reaches zero clearly indicates that

additional processes are involved. Therefore, Hull


postulated that in addition to response-produced

inhibition, extinction also involves the production

of conditioned inhibition. The enduring decrement

in behavior was explained as follows: When

reactive inhibition reaches a high level, cessation


of activity alleviates the aversive motivational

state, and, consequently, any stimuli associated


with stoppage of the response become

conditioned inhibitors. In this formulation, fatigue


reduction resulting from the termination of

behavior that produces aversive stimulation


constitutes the primary reinforcing event.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1132
It would follow from inhibition theory that any

conditions which increase the amount of relative

inhibition should facilitate extinction. The two


factors that have been investigated most

intensively in this regard are the rate of response


evocation and the amount of effort required to
perform the behavior. In general, research

findings (Kimble, 1961) indicate that there is little

diminution in response when extinction trials are


widely distributed over time, whereas extinction

occurs rapidly with massed response evocation.

The evidence concerning the effect of effortfulness


on extinction is somewhat equivocal, but the

majority of investigations has shown that


performances involving a large expenditure of

effort extinguish more rapidly than those

requiring little exertion.

Although a fatigue theory accounts for certain

extinction phenomena, there are many facts that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1133
cannot be adequately explained in terms of

response-generated inhibition (Gleitman,

Nachmias, & Neisser, 1954; Kimble, 1961; Mowrer,


1960). First, both reactive and conditioned

inhibition depend upon repeated response

evocation. There is ample evidence, however, that

nonreinforced performance of behavior is a

facilitative, but not a necessary, condition for


extinction. These findings are based on different
paradigms, in each of which responses are either

partially or totally extinguished without ever

being performed. In “latent extinction” studies, for

example, animals that are merely placed directly


in the empty goal box a number of times

subsequently extinguish instrumental responses


more rapidly than control groups placed in other

situations lacking rewards (Deese, 1951; Seward

& Levy, 1949; Moltz, 1955). Extinction of


avoidance responses likewise can be greatly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1134
facilitated independently of skeletal responding by

repeatedly presenting a conditioned aversive

stimulus to subjects who are muscularly


immobilized by curare (Black, 1958). Moreover,

autonomic responses, which are not known to


generate fatigue states, also undergo extinction
with repeated nonreinforced elicitation.

Nonresponse extinction is perhaps most clearly

revealed by experiments of vicarious extinction


(Bandura, 1968) in which intractable avoidance

responses of long standing are completely

eliminated on the basis of observational


experiences alone.

The limitation of explanations of extinction in


terms of response-produced inhibition is also

apparent in investigations of resistance to

extinction as a function of variations in the

percentage, magnitude, and serial patterning of

reinforcement, and of changeable acquisition

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1135
conditions. Though the terminal levels of learning

achieved under constant and variable

circumstances are essentially identical, increasing


the irregularities of reinforcement and training

conditions increases subsequent resistance to

extinction. Finally, in some instances

nonreinforced elicitation may result in extremely

rapid or even single-trial extinction before much


reactive inhibition could possibly develop. The
overall research evidence thus indicates that,

while response-produced inhibition may be one

determinant of extinction, additional processes

are undoubtedly involved.

COMPETING-RESPONSE THEORY

In the interference interpretation of extinction

(Guthrie, 1935; Estes, 1959), the decrement of a

response during nonreinforced evocation results

from the appearance of incompatible responses

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1136
strong enough to supersede the ongoing behavior.
These competing responses may be linked either

to the same stimuli or to different stimulus events.


In the latter case, response diminution primarily

reflects external inhibition brought about by

simultaneous occurrence of new prepotent stimuli

that evoke antagonistic tendencies, or by


attentional shifts to other distinctive features of

the environment. In the former instance, which

essentially involves a counterconditioning

process, extinction results from the development

of new incompatible responses to the same


stimuli, or the reappearance of interfering

responses that have been previously learned.

Any conditions, apart from the omission of

reinforcement, that reduce the probability of


occurrence of the original behavior will facilitate

the appearance of competing response patterns.


Some of these conditions, originally proposed by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1137
Guthrie (1935), include introducing the

controlling stimuli at such reduced intensity that

they fail to evoke the undesired behavior;


repeating the controlling stimuli after the original

responses have been inhibited through fatigue so

that new behavioral tendencies can be learned;

and presenting the stimuli in the context of more

powerful stimulus events that evoke incompatible


responses of sufficient strength to override the
undesired behavior. Response-prevention

techniques relying upon physical barriers (Carlson

& Black, 1959; Solomon, Kamin, & Wynne, 1953)

also provide a means of ensuring the occurrence of


alternative responses in the presence of

conditioned aversive stimuli. In a theory of


extinction emphasizing conditioned relaxation

processes, Denny and his associates (Denny &

Weisman, 1964; Weisman, Denny, Platt, &


Zerbolio, 1966) contend that termination or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1138
omission of aversive stimulation automatically

produces relaxation responses that generalize

backward to the fear-provoking situation and


compete with avoidance behavior.

Given the emergence of competing responses,


whatever their source, the nature of the

reinforcement that maintains new behavioral

tendencies within the extinction situation remains


to be explained. It will be recalled that reduction in
fatigue associated with the cessation of effortful

behavior was assumed in Hullian theory to

reinforce inhibitory responses. Some research

evidence suggests that the termination of aversive


emotional effects generated either by fear-
producing stimuli or by repeated nonreward may

also provide supporting reinforcement.

Page reports a series of experiments that

illustrate the extinction of avoidance behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1139
through the development of incompatible
responses. In the initial study (Page & Hall, 1953),

animals learned to avoid shocks administered in


one compartment of a shuttle box by escaping into

a neutral chamber. The avoidance responses were

then extinguished in two different ways: Control

animals were given regular extinction trials in


which they performed avoidance responses until

they stopped running from the threatening

compartment; the experimental subjects were

detained in the fear-provoking box for the first five

trials and then given traditional extinction trials.


The barrier group extinguished approximately

three times as fast as the controls.

In order to determine if elimination of

avoidance responses in the first study was due to


the acquisition of competing protective responses

or to neutralization of the fear-evoking stimuli,


Page (1955) conducted a second experiment that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1140
proceeded in the following manner: The initial

phase of the study, which duplicated the

procedure of the earlier experiment, similarly


demonstrated that animals first detained in the

threatening compartment subsequently

extinguished much more rapidly than controls

given regular extinction trials. In the second phase

of the study, designed to measure the aversive


properties of the conditioned stimuli, the animals
were placed in the neutral chamber after being

deprived of food and the speed with which they

entered the shock compartment for food was

measured. In addition, a control group of animals


never exposed to shock stimulation was tested.

The approach response latencies averaged


approximately 25, 60, and 110 seconds for the

control, regular extinction, and response-

prevention groups, respectively. It seems clear


from the two sets of data that under forced

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1141
exposure to fear-arousing stimuli a dominant

mode of avoidance behavior was eliminated, but

the animals nevertheless retained some fear of the


negative compartment. These findings indicate

that the threatening stimuli continued to generate


aversive stimulation and whatever protective
responses the animals adopted in the situation

were reinforced by the omission of painful shocks.

In the extinction of avoidance behavior,


absence of expected adverse consequences

provides a powerful source of reinforcement for

competing responses. In the elimination of

behavior previously maintained by positive


reinforcement, reduction of aversive emotional
arousal produced by omission of anticipated

rewards may likewise constitute the main

reinforcement for antagonistic responses.

According to the frustration interpretation of

extinction (Amsel, 1962; Wagner, 1966),

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1142
nonrewarded repetition of responses generates

aversive arousal capable of evoking conflicting

response tendencies that interfere with the


ongoing behavior. By supplanting the

nonrewarded behavior the competing responses

reduce disturbing emotional arousal and are

thereby negatively reinforced. Consistent with

these theoretical speculations, it has been shown


that nonreward produces aversive effects
analogous to punishment operations. Stimuli

previously associated with nonreward acquire

arousal properties (Wagner, 1963), their presence

attenuates responding (Amsel & Surridge, 1964),


and escape from cues signifying nonreward can

reinforce new performances (Wagner, 1963).

The appearance of new behavior that is

antagonistic to nonreinforced responses will

undoubtedly accelerate the extinction process. In

many instances, however, rapid elimination of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1143
nonrewarded behavior results from the

development of expectations about the future

probability of reinforcement rather than from the


gradual conditioning of incompatible responses to

the same controlling stimuli. The discrimination

theory of extinction, which is reviewed next, treats

extinction as a centrally, rather than a

peripherally, mediated phenomenon.

DISCRIMINATION THEORY

Interpretations of extinction in terms of

discrimination emphasize the role of


observational and cognitive processes. According
to this formulation, behavior is performed for

some time after reinforcement has been

discontinued because the subject has failed to

recognize that previous reinforcement

contingencies are no longer in effect. It would

follow from this hypothesis that variables which

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1144
reduce the discriminability between prior
conditions of reinforcement and those of

extinction should prolong nonrewarded


responding.

Numerous experiments, in which the

difference between acquisition and extinction has


either been systematically varied or is easily
inferable, provide supporting evidence for the

discrimination hypothesis. Behavior established


under intermittent reinforcement, for example, is

more resistant to extinction than responses

following continuous reinforcement. When

rewards are suddenly discontinued, it is

reasonable to suppose that persons who have


been reinforced each time they respond will

recognize the change more readily than those who


have always been reinforced irregularly. The

lower the frequency of reinforcement, the less


discernible are the changes. It is perhaps for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1145
similar reasons that, even under the same degree

of partial reinforcement, behavior that has been

rewarded irregularly extinguishes less rapidly


than responses established by a regular,

predictable schedule of intermittent

reinforcement (Ferster & Skinner, 1957; Kimble,

Mann, & Dufort, 1955; Longenecker, Krauskopf, &

Bitterman, 1952). Moreover, behavior is highly


resistant to extinction following training that
includes long series of consecutively

nonreinforced trials (Slamecka, 1960). The latter

conditions would make it particularly difficult to

determine when extinction has begun.

Discriminations can be formed not only on the

basis of the frequency and patterning of


reinforcing stimuli, but also in terms of other

distinguishable features of the environment that

signify a change in reinforcement practices. The

presence during extinction of stimuli that have

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1146
previously signified that appropriate performance

will be reinforced results in faster extinction than

if the positive discriminative cues are absent


(Elam, Tyler, & Bitterman, 1954; McNamara &

Paige, 1962; Slamecka, 1960). These findings,

which are contrary to secondary reinforcement

theory, are in accord with the discrimination

hypothesis. If responses in the presence of stimuli


that formerly signaled a high likelihood of reward
are no longer reinforced, it is made apparent that

the original reinforcement contingencies have

been discontinued. However, Longstreth (1966)

interpreted similar findings with children in terms


of frustration theory, which holds that when a

stimulus previously paired with reward is


subsequently presented alone it generates

aversive emotional responses that interfere with

ongoing behavior.

Irregular conditions of learning would also be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1147
expected to increase the complexity of

discrimination and hence to prolong the

persistence of nonrewarded performances. In


several experiments, response patterns that are

acquired under variable stimulus conditions (e.g.,

gross changes in drive states, environmental

stimuli, required performances, and in the

frequency, magnitude, and delay of reinforcement)


are extinguished under unchanging circumstances.
Results of these studies disclose that resistance to

extinction increases with increased variability in

conditions of learning (McClelland & McGown,

1953; McNamara & Wike, 1958; Mackintosh,


1955).

While the above findings are consistent with


the discrimination hypothesis, they can be

adequately explained without the necessity for

invoking symbolic processes. Under variable

circumstances different types of responses are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1148
learned to a variety of stimuli. It would therefore

require a longer time to extinguish a varied

assortment of responses than a single response


made to a limited number of stimuli presented

under invariant conditions. The results of a study

by Brown & Bass (1958), however, bring into

question both the discrimination and the multiple-

learning interpretations. In this experiment


subjects were both trained and extinguished
under constant or variable stimulus conditions.

Persistence of non-rewarded behavior was

primarily affected by variation in stimulation

during extinction rather than by the degree of


contrast between acquisition and extinction that

should facilitate discrimination, or by the amount


of change in stimulus conditions during

acquisition that should promote more generalized

learning.

The potentially influential role of observational

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1149
experiences in extinction is disclosed by studies

alluded to earlier, employing nonresponse

extinction procedures. Mere observation that


formerly rewarding situations no longer contain

rewards facilitates the elimination of instrumental

approach responses. Moreover, the larger the

number of positively discriminative stimuli

retained in the situation in which rewards were


previously dispensed, the faster the response
extinction (Denny & Ratner, 1959; Moltz, 1955).

While the efficacy of observational extinction

procedures has been well established under a


variety of conditions (Deese, 1951; Dyal, 1963;
Koppman & Grice, 1963; Seward & Levy, 1949;

Wilson & Dyal, 1963), the response decrements


may be interpreted in several ways. One possible

explanation is in terms of cognitive processes.

Repeated observation that formerly available

rewards are now absent undoubtedly conveys

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1150
information to the observer about the altered

conditions of reinforcement. However, when

environmental stimuli that signify the


reinforcement contingencies associated with given

behavior are also removed, the observed situation

bears little resemblance to, and hence provides

little information about, the original one. There is

no reason to suppose that under such


circumstances expectations regarding the
customary reinforcement contingencies should be

modified to any significant extent. Repeated

exposure to formerly rewarding stimulus contexts

may also extinguish the secondary reinforcing


properties of environmental stimuli that had been

regularly associated with primary reinforcement


(Moltz & Maddi, 1956). The latter outcome would

likewise serve to hasten the extinction process.

The research cited above has been primarily

confined to infrahuman subjects; perhaps for this

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1151
reason fractional anticipatory response

mechanisms and associated proprioceptive cues

have frequently been invoked as explanatory


factors. In the case of humans, who possess

superior discriminative and symbolic capacities,

the informative value of observational experiences

regarding reinforcement contingencies would

assume considerably greater importance in


eliminating nonreinforced behavior.

According to this more cognitive view,

extinction primarily reflects the operation of

inhibitory sets rather than the loss of behavior or


its disconnection from previous controlling
stimuli. For this reason behavior can be discarded

even without having been performed on the basis


of observation that such behavior is no longer

reinforced, it can be promptly replaced by more

utilitarian modes of response, and readily

reinstated whenever the original reinforcement

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1152
contingencies are restored.

The theory of cognitive control of extinction is

supported by several lines of evidence, some of


which are discussed fully in the concluding

chapter. Extinction is greatly facilitated by

awareness that the usual consequences have been

discontinued; and, conversely, it is retarded under

diverting instructions that reduce discriminability


of the change in reinforcement (Spence, 1966). In
fact, when presentation of reinforcement is

embedded in a diverting task that ensures

exposure to stimulus events but prevents


recognition of their contingent relationship, the
rate of extinction is the same for responses

originally acquired under either partial or


continuous reinforcement.

The common finding that irregular

reinforcement produces behavior that is more

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1153
resistant to extinction than continuous
reinforcement is also disconfirmed when

discriminability between acquisition and


extinction is equalized by informing subjects at the

onset of extinction that painful stimuli will not be

presented on subsequent trials (Bridger & Mandel,

1965). As can be seen from Figure 6-1, the partial


reinforcement effect was obtained for

noninformed subjects but induced awareness

essentially abolished conditioned autonomic

responses, regardless of whether they were

acquired on a 100 percent or 25 percent schedule


of reinforcement. Numerous other investigations

of symbolic control of extinction reveal that


persons who are simply informed that

reinforcement has been discontinued display a


precipitous decrement in both conditioned

autonomic responses (Cook & Harris, 1937; Grings


& Lockhart, 1963; Notterman, Schoenfeld, & Bersh,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1154
Figure 6-1. Rate of extinction of GSRs as a function of
awareness and the schedule of reinforcement
employed during the acquisition phase. Bridger &
Mandel, 1965.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1155
1952; Wickens, Allen, & Hill, 1963) and

instrumental avoidance behavior (Lindley &

Moyer, 1961; Moyer & Lindley, 1962), whereas


uninformed subjects show a more gradual decline

in responsiveness.

The influence of verbalized contingencies in

facilitating extinction presupposes a history of

differential reinforcement on the basis of which


verbal cues become reliable indicants of probable
response consequences. In cases where social

agents or verbal communications are considered

untrustworthy, where the real or imagined

consequences of certain actions are highly


injurious, and where environmental events are not
entirely predictable, verbal control of extinction is

apt to be relatively weak. It is extremely unlikely,

for example, that informing snake-phobic persons

that a particular reptile is harmless will result in

any appreciable decrease in snake avoidance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1156
behavior.

The powerful symbolic control over emotional

responses developed under laboratory conditions


contrasts sharply with the refractory quality of

fears acquired through natural experiences. The

difference may arise partly from the degree of

control exercised by change agents over the feared

events. By turning off the shock apparatus or


removing shock electrodes, experimenters can
completely remove any potential threats from the

situation. In contrast, naturally feared objects that

are ordinarily innocuous can nevertheless


occasionally produce hurtful effects despite
assurances to the contrary. Even harmless snakes

or dogs do bite. However, this explanation does


not fully reconcile the divergent findings because

snake phobics experience considerable emotional

disturbance to pictures of reptiles (Bandura,

Blanchard, & Ritter, 1969) while acknowledging

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1157
that the agitation is groundless because pictorial

snakes cannot possibly inflict any injury. The

overall evidence would seem to indicate that


emotional behavior may be controlled by two

different stimulus sources. One is the emotional

arousal self-generated by symbolic activities in the

form of emotion-provoking thoughts about

frightening or pleasurable events. The second is


the response evoked directly by conditioned
aversive stimuli. The former component would be

readily susceptible to extinction through cognitive

restructuring of probable response consequences,

whereas elimination of the latter component may


require repeated nonreinforced exposure to

threatening events either directly or vicariously


(Bridger & Mandel, 1964). The differential rate of

extinction of emotional responses arising from

symbolic self-stimulation and from direct external


evocation are discussed more fully in later

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1158
considerations of symbolic regulation of behavior.

Laboratory investigations in which verbal reports

of experimenters are accorded high credulity, the


aversive stimuli are of comparatively weak

intensities, and experimenters have full control


over the occurrence of aversive events, may
therefore provide an insufficient explanation of

the process of extinction, particularly as it applies

to refractory avoidance behavior.

COGNITIVE DISSONANCE THEORY

Lawrence & Festinger (1962) have proposed

an explanation, in terms of cognitive dissonance


processes, of why behavior that has been

originally acquired under relatively unfavorable

conditions of reinforcement may be especially

resistant to extinction. According to this

interpretation, when a subject is induced to


engage in behavior that is insufficiently rewarded,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1159
an aversive state of cognitive dissonance is created
by the conflicting information of having expended

effort for inadequate recompense. Under


circumstances where the subject cannot easily

cease responding, the resultant dissonance is

reduced by enhancing the attraction or value of

the activity to justify continuation of the behavior.


After a subject has persuaded himself that he

really likes engaging in the behavior it become

more resistant to extinction when extrinsic

rewards are later removed.

Several different types of reinforcement

conditions are identified as especially prone to

induce dissonance during the period of


acquisition. Behavior that frequently goes

unrewarded, that requires high expenditure of


effort, and for which rewards are delayed, would

be most resistant to extinction. Lawrence &


Festinger have demonstrated in a series of well-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1160
designed experiments with infrahuman subjects

that responses established under these less

advantageous conditions are indeed more


persistent than those that are rewarded

continuously, immediately, and at little

expenditure of effort.

Other investigators have, of course, attributed

the influence on extinction of these reinforcement


variables to the operation of other mechanisms
involving discrimination processes, frustration

effects, and counterconditioning of competing

responses. These alternative theories, therefore,


need to be tested under conditions where they
make opposing predictions. For example,

resistance to extinction following both a highly


variable and an entirely regular schedule of the

same total partial reinforcement has been studied.

The absolute number of unrewarded trials is

identical in both conditions; consequently,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1161
subjects are provided with the same number of

occasions on which dissonance could be aroused

and presumably reduced. Dissonance theory


would predict the same rate of extinction under

both conditions, whereas discrimination theory

would lead one to expect the unpredictable

schedule to produce the more durable behavior.

Bitterman and his associates have conducted


several such experiments in which subjects are
reinforced on 50 percent of the training trials; for

one group the rewards are administered

haphazardly, while subjects in the other group are

regularly reinforced on odd-numbered trials.


Behavior is much more resistant to extinction

(Tyler, Wortz, & Bitterman, 1953) after random 50


percent reinforcement than after regularly

alternated 50 percent reinforcement. Analogous

results are obtained in extinction of autonomic


reactions which do not involve performance of any

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1162
effortful responses (Longenecker, Krauskopf, &

Bitterman, 1952). Moreover, increased training,

which provides more dissonance-reducing


opportunities, facilities extinction following

alternating reinforcement, but it has no effect on


behavior rewarded according to an unpredictable
pattern (Capaldi, 1958).

Whether or not findings of the type reported


above contradict dissonance theory cannot be
resolved as long as there exists some ambiguity as

to the conditions most likely to produce high

dissonance. Do subjects who repeatedly perform

effortful behavior knowing that it is unlikely to be


rewarded experience more or less dissonance
than if they had expected rewarding outcomes?

The authors assume that the latter condition is

more dissonance producing. It would seem,

however, that subjects in the former condition are

exhibiting the more irrational behavior and would,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1163
therefore, experience greater pressure to justify

their actions by persuading themselves that they

really enjoy the activity. On the other hand,


subjects who performed because they expected to

be rewarded have adequate justification without

needing to endow the activity with additional

attractions. If reluctant performance of an action is

accepted as behavioral evidence for the existence


of dissonance (Lawrence & Festinger, 1962) then
subjects trained under alternating reinforcement

in the above experiments experienced more

severe dissonance. As training progressed they

continued to perform the behavior on unrewarded


trials albeit with evident hesitancy.

The literature contains other findings that


cannot be adequately accounted for by either

dissonance theory or the alternative formulations.

Young (1966) measured resistance to extinction

as a function of variations in the effortfulness of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1164
the response, the frequency of reward, and the

number of unrewarded trials. The results showed

that, under conditions where animals experienced


many unrewarded trials during acquisition, the

more effort required to perform the response, the

more rapidly it was extinguished, whereas the

opposite relationship was obtained when the

training period included fewer nonreinforced


trials. To further complicate the picture, no
relationship was found between effortfulness of

response and resistance to extinction for subjects

trained under continuous reinforcement.

It is apparent from the experimental findings


reviewed in the preceding sections that no single

theoretical conceptualization can encompass


adequately all of the diverse variables governing

decrements in behavior when reinforcement is

withdrawn. A person may cease responding for

many different reasons and, therefore, a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1165
comprehensive explanation of extinction

phenomena requires a multi-process theory.

Extinction of Positively Reinforced Behavior

It was previously shown how persistent

deviant behavior is often maintained by

intermittent positive reinforcement. Such


contingencies are most likely to arise under

conditions where desirable responses and even

mild forms of deviant behavior are typically

disregarded. On the other hand, the more


persistent or intense responsiveness produces

aversive consequences for others, who unwittingly


reinforce the troublesome behavior in their efforts
to terminate it. Because of its aversive quality,

deviant behavior readily creates conditions that

are likely to perpetuate it. It is true, of course, that

troublesome activities are also frequently

punished, but a reinforcement schedule combining

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1166
intermittent reward with occasional punishment

generally results in behavior that is resistant to

change. Moreover, many interventions intended as


punishments actually serve as positive reinforcers

that maintain undesirable behavior. Self- defeating

contingencies usually go unnoticed because

people tend to see only the immediate results,

whereas they rarely systematically evaluate the


changes produced by their practices, or the long-
term effects that their behavior may have on

others. Therefore, detrimental social systems are

often unknowingly created and mutually

sustained, because deviant behavior is rewarded


by the attention it commands and ineffective

control techniques are reinforced by their success


in temporarily checking disturbing performances.

The positively reinforcing effects of verbal

reprimands are well illustrated in a field study by

Madsen et al. (1968) of disruptive behavior in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1167
classroom settings. After the frequency with which

children left their desks was recorded, teachers

consistently reprimanded them for standing and


told them to sit down. The admonishments

promptly increased the number of children

standing (Figure 6-2). In the subsequent baseline

phase teachers reduced reprimands to their

original moderate level, which produced a


corresponding reduction in the number of upright
students. However, they were springing up again

at high rates when procedures were altered so

that transgressive behavior evoked frequent “sit-

down” commands. During the final phase of the


study, teachers ignored standing and praised the

children for working at their desks, a practice that


reduced the incidence of disruptive behavior to its

lowest level.

In eliminating behavior maintained by positive

reinforcement, extinction can be accomplished

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1168
Figure 6-2. Number of children standing in class during
baseline periods and when such behavior produced
verbal admonishments or incompatible responses were
positively reinforced. Madsen et al., 1968.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1169
simply by discontinuing the reinforcing

consequences. In social change programs

extinction procedures are often combined with


positive reinforcement of incompatible behavior.

When extinction is a component in a multiform

procedure its separate contribution to behavioral

changes is difficult to evaluate, and categorization

of the treatment method is somewhat arbitrary.

Several issues relating to extinction processes


are revealed in Williams’ (1959) successful

elimination of aggressively demanding behavior in

a young boy. This child had been ill for the first 18
months of his life and had required considerable
attention and special care. During this period,

strong dependency behavior was undoubtedly


established. When the child recovered, his parents

attempted to withdraw some of the attention that

they had previously given him. The child

responded with intense protests, forcing the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1170
parents to attend to him and thus unwittingly to

reinforce crying spells, tantrums, and demands for

their extended presence and undivided attention,


especially at bedtime. The parents were instructed

to put the child to bed in a leisurely and

nonpunitive fashion and, after the completion of

bedtime routines, to ignore the child’s screaming

and raging. Under the extinction contingencies an


immediate marked drop in the duration of
tantrums occurred, followed by almost complete

extinction of tantrums within a few days (Figure 6-

3). The child no longer cried when left in his room,

but instead played happily until he dropped off to


sleep.

In everyday situations it is sometimes difficult


to achieve generalized extinction of deviant

behavior because different social agents are

inconsistent in their reinforcement practices.

Thus, if parents no longer reward temper

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1171
tantrums, but other significant adults continue to

do so, a child will, in all likelihood, exhibit a

discriminative pattern of negative behavior


toward others in accord with their customary

reinforcement practices. The rate of extinction can

be further impeded if the same agent, through

inconsistency in his own actions, places the

behavior on a schedule of partial reinforcement. In


the case discussed above, for example, the parents
and an aunt alternated in the painful bedtime

routines. The tantrums were briefly reinstated and

reinforced, after having been extinguished, by

attention from the aunt on an occasion when the


child fussed after having been put to bed. A second

extinction series was therefore carried out and


resulted in complete and stable elimination of the

tantrums (Figure 6-3). Food-throwing and other

disruptive behaviors were similarly extinguished


by promptly removing the child from the table

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1172
Figure 6-3. Duration of crying in two extinction series in
which tantrum behavior was no longer socially
reinforced. Williams, 1959.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1173
whenever he deliberately spilled or threw food

from his plate. When this practice was first

instituted the boy was removed from his high


chair 12 times, but thereafter food-throwing

declined rapidly and ceased by the seventh meal


(Williams, 1962).

A child’s aversive control of his parents, if

intermittently reinforced, is likely to generalize to


other areas of behavior and to other people. As
shown by Williams, after inappropriate coercive

behaviors are extinguished the familial

atmosphere changes from one of recurrent drawn-

out battles to reciprocally rewarding interactions.

The importance of establishing uniform

contingencies in implementing a change program

based on extinction is further shown in a case

reported by Ayllon & Michael (1959). A female

patient, who exhibited extremely persistent

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1174
psychotic talk, had been subjected to considerable
verbal abuse and beaten on several occasions by

other patients in an effort to keep her quiet.


Patients responded negatively to this woman’s

delusional verbalizations, but the nurses from

time to time attended to her bizarre talk in order

to “get at the roots of her problems” or responded


with perfunctory statements of sympathy and

understanding. The patient was thus provided

intermittent social reinforcement for behavior that

was at other times punished or ignored. The

nurses were instructed not to attend to psychotic


talk and to reinforce sensible verbalizations.

Although the patient’s psychotic responses had


persisted over the previous three years, during a

relatively brief period of treatment the percentage


of these responses dropped from 91 to less than

25. However, an increase in psychotic talk


occurred during the ninth week of treatment,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1175
when, unknown to the ward personnel, a social

worker had been conducting interviews with the

patient and inadvertently reinforcing her


psychotic verbalizations; the effects of these

interviews generalized to the patient’s


interactions with the nurses and other patients as
well. Reinforcements provided by hospital

employees and other visitors to the ward

produced other temporary increases. However,


the psychotic talk still remained less frequent than

it had been at the commencement of the extinction

program and therefore no longer provoked


punitive behavior from the other patients.

A report by Groot (1966) provides some


information on the collateral changes that may

result from extinction of a related deviant

behavior. It also illustrates how, under conditions

where treatment practices are supported

independently of the consequences they have on

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1176
recipients, it is frequently more difficult to modify

the practices of the treatment staff than to change

the behavior of clients. A chronic schizophrenic,


who had received insulin and shock treatment,

individual and group therapy, and a lobotomy,

engaged continually in psychotic talk and wrote

numerous bizarre letters. An extinction program

was applied to psychotic talk, and its concomitant


effects on bizarre letter writing and incidence of
disturbed behavior were measured. When

extinction was first instituted, the number of

bizarre letters increased from a baseline rate of

approximately 13 letters a week to 43 letters in


two days, after which it declined and stabilized at

about 5 letters a week. The relative frequency of


disturbed behavior also decreased from 71

percent during the baseline period to only 16

percent when the extinction program was in


effect. Both disturbed behaviors and the number

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1177
of psychotic letters increased after the staff

reverted to their own preferred practices, whereas

deviant performances diminished when the nurses


were again persuaded to withhold attention from

psychotic verbalizations.

As part of a program of research in the

development of procedures for the modification of

psychotic behavior, Ayllon and his associates


(Ayllon & Haughton, 1962; Ayllon & Michael,
1959) provide numerous examples in which

deviant behavior of hospitalized psychotics is

extinguished by withdrawal of its positively

reinforcing consequences. In one study (Ayllon &


Haughton, 1962), a group of schizophrenics, who
exhibited severe eating problems of long standing,

remained totally unresponsive to announcements

that meals were being served and to other

persuasive appeals. Consequently, the patients

had been individually escorted to the dining room

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1178
by ward personnel, spoon-fed, tube-fed, and

subjected to electroshock “therapy” and other

forms of infantilizing and punitive treatments.

It was assumed by the research staff that the

nurses’ inadvertent social reinforcement in the

form of coaxing, persuading, and feeding the

patients maintained their eating problems, a

contingency that also served to reduce the


controlling function of verbal stimuli. All social
reinforcement for ignoring the announcement of

mealtime and refusals to eat was therefore

withdrawn; following meal call, the dining room


remained open for 30 minutes and any patient
who failed to appear unassisted during that time

simply missed his meal. Under this new


reinforcement contingency, patients responded

promptly to meal call and the chronic feeding

problems were completely eliminated.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1179
It is interesting to note that delusional

statements to the effect that the food was

poisoned, or that God had instructed the patients


to refuse to eat, dropped out soon after they began

to feed themselves (Ayllon & Michael, 1959).


These findings suggest that in some cases
delusional responses may be a product, rather

than a source, of deviant behavior. By adopting a

sick role, supported by delusional justifications,


patients can be more successful in forcing

attending and caretaking responses from busy

ward personnel, who would otherwise reject


inappropriate demands for personalized attention.

Indeed, the nurses frequently encouraged and


positively reinforced infantile response patterns

on the assumption that the patients were

incapable of more mature reality-oriented


behavior because they were “mentally ill.” The

changes noted in delusional beliefs following the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1180
reinstatement of self-feeding are in accord with

considerable research evidence from social


psychological studies which demonstrates that

after a response pattern has been durably

modified, cognition appears to accommodate over

time to support or to justify the new behavior.

Similar extinction procedures were applied by

Ayllon & Haughton (1964) in modifying aberrant


verbal repertoires in three females diagnosed as
chronic schizophrenics. In each case, following a

baseline period of observation, the deviant verbal

behavior was extinguished by withholding social

attention and tangible rewards whenever the


patients engaged in either psychotic talk or
psychosomatic complaints. At such times the

nurses and ward attendants appeared distracted,

bored, or would simply shift their attention to

some other event taking place in the ward. In

order to demonstrate that the changes observed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1181
during the extinction period were not due to some

other variables, the deviant verbalizations were

positively reinforced in the second stage of the


experiment, following which extinction

contingencies were again reinstated.

Figure 6-4 shows the modification of

delusional talk in a patient whose conversational

content over the preceding 14 years had been


dominated by regal self-references (e.g., “I’m the
Queen. The Queen wants a smoke. . . . How’s King

George, have you seen him?”). The findings

provide striking evidence that the staff’s social


attention exercised considerable control over the
client’s aberrant verbal behavior. Psychotic verbal

responses were progressively increased as a


function of positive reinforcement, but they

decreased rapidly when social attention was

withdrawn. The frequency of appropriate verbal

responses was similarly decreased or increased by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1182
Figure 6-4. Reversals in the incidence of psychotic and
neutral verbal behavior as a result of variations in
social reinforcement of these two classes of
verbalizations. Ayllon & Haughton, 1964.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1183
altering the reinforcement contingencies.

Figures 6-5 and 6-6 illustrate the modification

of somatic complaint behavior in two women who


continually alleged innumerable physical

symptoms in the absence of any organic

dysfunction. Their somatic preoccupation and

accompanying emotional responses such as crying

and sobbing were drastically reduced when they


were no longer consoled, or given sympathy or
attention for complaints of various aches and

pains. The extinction process was undoubtedly

hastened in the latter case by the concurrent


positive reinforcement of more appropriate verbal
responsiveness.

As shown in the studies of Ayllon and his

colleagues, institutional environments that are

lacking in positive social reinforcement are likely

to produce a high incidence of attention-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1184
1185
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 6-5. Frequency of somatic complaints during the baseline period and
while somatic verbalizations were successively rewarded with attention
and ignored. The temporary increase in somatic complaints shown by
the arrow in the fourth phase of the treatment coincides with a
visit by a relative. Ayllon & Haughton, 1964.
1186
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 6-6. Frequency of somatic complaints during the baseline period and while somatic
verbalizations were successively rewarded with attention and ignored. Ayllon &
Haughton, 1964.
compelling deviant behavior. In those familial
environments that likewise provide minimal social

reinforcement, interest and attention may be


supplied primarily for somatic disturbances or

psychological disorders that cannot be easily

ignored. Walton (1960a), for example, reports a

case of a 20-year-old woman who suffered from


long-standing neurodermatitis on the back of her

neck, which was continuously aggravated by

persistent scratching. The client had undergone

numerous medical treatments, including

ointments, pills, lotions, and x-ray therapy, but her


skin condition remained essentially unchanged.

Assessment of the family interrelationships


revealed that the son, who had always enjoyed a

preferred status, was currently receiving most of


the parental attention and their limited financial

resources, whereas the daughter had been


relegated to an inferior, ignored position. With the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1187
advent of the dermatitis, however, the daughter

received more solicitous attention than ever

before, and her fiancé, who similarly expressed


concern over the dermatitis, frequently assisted

with the application of the prescribed ointments.

On the assumption that the scratching

behavior, which perpetuated the skin condition,

was being inadvertently reinforced by the high


level of attention, the family members were
instructed to ignore the dermatitis and the fiancé

to discontinue the ointment routine. Following

discontinuance of the solicitous ministrations, the

scratching decreased and at the end of three


months the dermatitis had completely
disappeared. A four- year follow-up study

disclosed no recurrence of the neurodermatitis;

the client was happily married and successfully

employed. Walton attributes the elimination of

compulsive scratching to the rapid accumulation

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1188
of inhibitory potential with nonreinforced

performance and the eventual selection of less

effortful means of securing attention. A more likely


explanation would be in terms of clients’

discriminating the altered reinforcement

contingencies rather than the build-up of

response-produced inhibitory potentials.

In the above case a somatic disorder was


indirectly perpetuated through reinforced motor
behavior that exacerbated the condition. An

ingenious program of research by Miller (1969)

provides impressive evidence that physiological


responses involved in psychosomatic conditions
can be directly modified by contingent

reinforcement. In these studies animals are


curarized to eliminate skeletally mediated

influences, and spontaneous physiological

responding is altered by administering reinforcing

consequences whenever visceral responses of a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1189
selected rate or magnitude occur. The animals are

typically reinforced by rewarding brain

stimulation or by the cessation of electric shock.


Results of numerous experiments consistently

show that when increases or decreases in

physiological responding are reinforced, visceral

changes are produced in the rewarded direction. A

wide variety of visceral responses, including


changes in heart rate, blood pressure, vasomotor
activity, intestinal contractions, and rate of urine

formation, have been substantially modified by

this procedure. The preciseness of reinforcement

control of visceral responses is most strikingly


illustrated by an experiment (Di Cara & Miller,

1968) in which animals were rewarded for


relatively greater vasomotor responses in one ear

than in the other. The animals learned to respond

with differential vasomotor activity in the two


ears, indicating much greater specificity to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1190
autonomic functioning than has been previously

assumed.

Based on the evidence that visceral responses

are subject to reinforcement control, Miller (1969)

speculates that psychosomatic conditions may be


partly developed through contingent attention and

other reinforcing consequences. If this is the case,

it should be possible to modify the visceral


responses involved in psychosomatic disorders by
the use of extinction and differential

reinforcement procedures.

TEMPORAL COURSE OF EXTINCTION

When reinforcement for previously rewarded

behavior is discontinued, the subject is likely to


exhibit, during initial phases of extinction, a

temporary acceleration or intensification of the

behavior in an effort to produce the customary


reinforcement. This is particularly true of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1191
performances that have been maintained on a
continuous schedule of reinforcement. A temper

tantrum that continues to go unheeded will

typically soar to deafening intensities; initially


mild dependency demands, if disregarded, may

culminate in a sharp kick in the shins; and


negative attention-getting behavior that is
consistently ignored typically assumes

increasingly ludicrous forms. But if the more

vigorous responding also proves unsuccessful, it


gradually declines and alternative patterns of

behavior emerge.

The particular sequences and patterns of

responses that appear during later phases of

extinction are primarily determined by the


response options available to the individual. As
dominant modes of behavior are extinguished, a

person will try alternative courses of action that

have been successful on previous occasions in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1192
similar situations. No special problems are created

by the use of extinction alone, provided the

available alternatives are desirable. If, however,


the responses in the individual’s repertoire are

largely inadequate, a change agent may be faced

with the arduous task of extinguishing a long

succession of inappropriate patterns of behavior.

This problem can be easily avoided by combining


extinction procedures with methods that foster
more effective modes of response.

The simultaneous utilization of extinction and

reinforcement procedures is illustrated by one of


several studies (Allen, Hart, Buell, Harris, & Wolf,
1964) designed to modify behavior disorders in

young children on the basis of reinforcement


principles. The case involved a preschool girl who

exhibited marked passivity and withdrawal from

peer interactions, and high dependence on adults,

which generally took the form of hypochondriacal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1193
complaints, drawing attention to collections of

incidental objects, and to simple stereotyped

activities that might gain and hold adult interest.


Most of the time, however, she spent either

standing alone, or sitting in packing boxes, despite

her teachers’ supportive efforts to structure play

activities for her with a group of accepting

children. These attempts were partly unsuccessful


because the teacher would leave soon after the girl
became involved in the group, thus making the

loss of the teacher contingent upon social

participation. Before long she would be standing

on the periphery, seeking out a teacher, or


reverting to some form of solitary activity. In

addition to the autistic behavior, the girl displayed


speech defects and a variety of tic-like behaviors.

In order to increase her social responsiveness,

isolate behavior was consistently nonrewarded

and she was given a minimum of attention for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1194
adult contacts that competed with peer-group

play. Attention was, however, given freely and

warmly whenever she approached or interacted


with peers. Initially, because of her strong

avoidance behavior, approximations to social

interactions, such as standing near other children

or parallel play, were positively reinforced. A

teacher would go to her whenever she was in the


proximity of peers, converse with her, comment
on the nearby play activity, and suggest ways of

joining in the game. Later, adult attention,

approval, and closeness were made contingent

upon direct play with other children. The


introduction of these new contingencies produced

a rapid and marked change in the girl’s social


behavior: peer interactions increased from

approximately 10 to about 60 percent, while adult

contacts dropped from 40 to less than 20 percent


(Figure 6-7).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1195
Figure 6-7. Amount of social behavior displayed by a child as
a result of variations in social reinforcement of adult
and peer interactions. Allen et al., 1964.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1196
In order to determine whether the differential

reinforcement was the determinant of the

behavioral changes, the original contingencies


were reinstated. During this period the teachers

gave full attention to the girl when she contacted


them, played with her when she was alone,
watched her when she engaged in solitary

activities, and conversed with her as long as she

remained in close proximity. No attempts were


made, however, to initiate or to reward social

interaction with peers. Under these conditions, the

girl’s contacts with adults increased, the


hypochondriacal complaints and articulation

problems reappeared, and she promptly reverted


to the isolate pattern of behavior (Figure 6-7).

Several days after the therapeutic contingencies

were again instituted, the girl’s contacts with


adults stabilized at about 25 percent, and social

interactions with peers increased to the previous

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1197
level of about 60 percent.

As play activities and peers became


increasingly enjoyable, adult rewards for

interaction with children were progressively

diminished, and the schedule of nonreinforcement


of adult contacts gradually relaxed. At the end of

the 10-day period, the program was discontinued

and no special contingencies were arranged


thereafter. Observational studies conducted
during several periods following treatment

revealed that the girl continued to display a high

level of social interaction with other children

(Figure 6-7). Not only was the isolated pattern of


behavior successfully modified, but her speech,
which had been slow, drawn-out, and frequently

inaudible, improved considerably. She no longer

demanded unnecessary medication, and she

displayed increased capabilities to assert and

defend herself when necessary.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1198
Similar results were obtained with a combined

extinction-reinforcement procedure in the

treatment of two preschool boys who exhibited


frequent crying episodes whenever mildly

frustrated or thwarted by other children (Hart,


Allen, Buell, Harris, & Wolf, 1964). Observation
revealed that their vociferous crying generally

elicited comforting ministrations and solicitous

concern from teachers. Consequently, teachers


were instructed to pay no attention to the crying

episodes, unless the child was actually hurt. If he

was in close proximity to the teacher when he


began to cry, she walked away or occupied herself

with other duties. On the other hand, whenever


that child handled stressful situations more

constructively he received prompt approving

attention. Within five days after introduction of


the new contingencies the rate of crying declined

from 5-10 times per morning to practically a zero

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1199
level and remained negligible thereafter. Etzel &

Gewirtz (1967) achieved equally favorable results

with persistent infant criers by combining


extinction of crying with reinforcement of more

cheerful behavior.

As revealed by the above studies, modification

of behavior through extinction can be

accomplished quickly and predictably by eliciting


and reinforcing desirable modes of response in
addition to eliminating the rewarding

consequences for dysfunctional behavior. Some of

the emotional effects that accompany extinction

can also be avoided in this manner. If, however,


constructive alternatives are only weakly
established or nonexistent in an individual’s

behavioral repertoire, a change agent may have to

wait unnecessarily long or indefinitely for their

appearance. Under these conditions, rather than

relying on the fortuitous occurrence of favorable

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1200
events, a change agent can both facilitate and

more effectively control the course of treatment

by providing rewarded models who display


alternative means for securing desired outcomes.

Individuals undergoing extinction alone may learn

what is no longer effective but remain uncertain

about possible constructive courses of action,

whereas the combined use of extinction and


modeling provides considerable positive guidance.

MODIFICATION OF AGGRESSIVE RESPONSE


PATTERNS

Theorizing about the conditions governing the

occurrence and modification of aggressive

behavior has, until recent years, been considerably

influenced by the frustration-aggression

hypothesis (Berkowitz, 1962; Dollard, Miller,


Doob, Mowrer, & Sears, 1939). According to this

view, aggression is the naturally dominant


response to frustration, and nonaggressive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1201
reactions are likely to occur only if aggression has
been nonrewarded or punished. Since aggression

was considered an unlearned response to


frustration, the research stimulated by this theory

was mainly concerned with the effects of

frustration on aggression, with its inhibition and

displacement, and with the occurrence of


catharsis. On the other hand, the crucial issues of

how relatively complex patterns of aggressive

behavior are originally learned, and the influence

of a variety of controlling variables other than

“frustration” were largely ignored.

Man is endowed with neurophysiological

mechanisms that enable him to behave


aggressively, but the activation of these

mechanisms depends on stimulation and is subject


to cortical control. Therefore, the frequency with

which aggressive behavior is displayed, the


specific forms that it takes, the situations in which

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1202
it is expressed, and the targets that are selected for

attack are strongly influenced by social

experience. A social-learning theory of aggression


distinguishes the acquisition of instrumental

responses that have destructive or pain-producing

potential from the conditions governing their

subsequent performance. Aggressive response

patterns are characteristically acquired under


nonfrustrating conditions in the absence of
injurious intent and often toward inanimate

objects. Thus, for example, military recruits

acquire and perfect combat skills through many

hours of target practice and simulated skirmishes;


boxers develop hurtful pummeling abilities by

using punching bags and sparring partners whom


they do not necessarily intend to hurt; and

huntsmen acquire the basic rudiments of hunting

by shooting at inanimate targets before they go


out in search of game. Indeed, if aggressive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1203
repertoires were taught only while individuals

were hostilely aroused and entertained injurious

designs, many of the tutors and learners would


probably be maimed during the acquisition phase.

Most theories of aggression single out a limited set


of variables that may influence performance of
aggressive responses, but for the most part they

overlook the essential skill component.

The process of acquisition and subsequent


utilization of aggressive behavior is best

exemplified in laboratory studies employing

markedly different training and test situations.

Walters & Brown (1963) showed that boys who


had been intermittently reinforced with marbles
for punching an automated Bobo doll later

exhibited more physically aggressive behavior

toward other children in a competitive situation

than did boys who had received no prior training

in punching responses. Conversely, in the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1204
previously reviewed study by Chittenden (1942),

children whose aggressive responses were

reduced in a doll-play situation through


differential modeling and vicarious reinforcement

were much less prone to respond aggressively to

interpersonal thwarting, both in situational tests

and in their everyday interactions.

There is a substantial body of evidence


(Bandura & Walters, 1963) that novel modes of
aggressive behavior are readily acquired through

observation of aggressive models. Findings of

these controlled investigations lend support to


field studies demonstrating the crucial role of
modeling in the genesis of antisocial aggressive

behavior (McCord & McCord, 1958), and in the


cultural transmission of aggressive response

patterns (Bateson, 1936; Whiting, 1941). Modeling

influences continue to regulate aggressive

responsiveness to some extent even after the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1205
behavior has been acquired. The behavior of

models continually exerts selective control over

the types of responses exhibited by others in any


given situation. Moreover, seeing individuals

behaving aggressively without adverse

consequences reduces restraints in observers,

thereby increasing both the frequency with which

they engage in aggressive activities (Wheeler,


1966) and the harshness with which they treat
others (Epstein, 1966; Hartmann, 1969).

After aggressive patterns of behavior have

been learned, they can be maintained by a variety


of reinforcing events. Theories that invoke
aggressive drives (Dollard et al., 1939; Feshbach,

1964) assume that pain cues and other injurious


consequences experienced by the victim constitute

the major reinforcers of aggressive behavior. The

process whereby signs of injury and distress

acquire positively reinforcing properties has never

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1206
been established. Sears, Maccoby, & Levin (1957)

suggest that expressions of pain and discomfort

produced in others by aggressive behavior are


frequently followed by removal of frustrations or

rewarding outcomes for the aggressor. Through

such paired association pain cues acquire

conditioned reward value. One might also expect

expressions of suffering to gain satisfying


properties under conditions of interdependent
competing consequences in which success for one

member produces punishing outcomes for the

other. Feshbach (1964) offers a somewhat

different interpretation of the phenomenon.


Through example and precept children learn a

retaliation norm: infliction of injury requires that


the initial aggressor must be hurt. It is further

assumed that perception of pain in one’s

tormentors is experienced as satisfying because


successful retaliation restores self-esteem.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1207
To assess the reinforcing function of infliction

of pain, Feshbach, Stiles, & Bitter (1967)

conducted an experiment in which female


students participated in a verbal-conditioning task

after being treated in either a hostile or a friendly


manner by a confederate of the experimenter. For
half the subjects in each condition the correct

verbal response was reinforced by witnessing the

confederate being shocked, while for the


remaining students the contingent event was a

light flash. Angered subjects who observed the

provocateur experiencing pain showed an


increase in conditioned responses, but the

noninsulted subjects did not. These findings would


seem to indicate that under anger arousal pain

cues can serve as positive reinforcers, although

additional information is needed before the


conditioning differences can be unequivocally

attributed to the influence of anger arousal.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1208
Witnessing another person experience pain

evokes emotional responses (Berger, 1962), and

observers who are moderately aroused are more

easily conditioned by such displays than


nonaroused subjects (Bandura & Rosenthal,

1966). Since emotional arousal facilitates

conditioning, an experimental design comparing


the effects of pain cues on fear-aroused and anger-
aroused subjects would be best suited for

evaluating whether conditioning outcomes are

due specifically to anger or to the general effects of


emotional arousal.

A comprehensive theory of aggression must


account not only for aggressive actions that are
primarily reinforced by injurious consequences,

but also for much broader classes of aggressive

behavior in which the infliction of injury is

essentially irrelevant or, at best, a secondary

controlling condition. If there is any validity to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1209
naturalistic studies demonstrating that social

recognition is often made contingent upon

performance of assaultive behavior, and if


aggression is defined as behavior intended to

produce injurious consequences, then some of the

most violent interpersonal activities would be

excluded from consideration. It is evident that

people frequently resort to aggression not for pain


cues but because it has high utilitarian value. By
aggressive behavior, or dominance through

physical and verbal force, individuals can obtain

material resources, change rules to fit their own

wishes, gain control over and extract subservience


from others, terminate provocation, and remove

physical barriers which block or delay attainment


of desired outcomes. It is, therefore, not surprising

that aggressive-domineering patterns of behavior

are so prevalent.

Systematic analysis of reinforcement

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1210
contingencies in naturally occurring interactions,

indeed, reveals that aggressive behavior is

strengthened and maintained to some extent by its


positive social consequences. In deviant

subcultures, where physical aggression is

regarded as emulative behavior (Buehler,

Patterson, & Furniss, 1966; Wolfgang & Ferracuti,

1967; Yablonsky, 1962) aggressiveness is often


deliberately rewarded and collectively sanctioned.
Studies of aggression as it occurs in familial and

other social situations disclose that individuals are

often inadvertently trained to behave aggressively

by persons who normally eschew such behavior.


Because of its aversive properties aggressive

behavior not only commands attention, but is


often successful in removing unpleasant

performance demands and in controlling the

behavior of others. Both parents (Hawkins,


Peterson, Schweid, & Bijou, 1966) and peers

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1211
(Patterson, Littman, & Bricker, 1967) thus

intermittently reinforce aggressive responding.

A similar reinforcement process is typically

operative at broader social levels. When legitimate

demands and constructive efforts to produce


needed changes are repeatedly thwarted by

persons who benefit from the prevailing system

this evokes more intense and disruptive actions


that cannot be ignored. In many instances, existing
practices lack sufficient justification to withstand

any concerted and aggressive efforts to force

changes. As a consequence, aggressive behavior

eventually succeeds in securing desired goals and,


like any other efficacious, modeled behavior, it is
widely emulated as a method of achieving social

change.

Drive theories of aggression assume that

frustration arouses an aggressive impulse or drive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1212
that can be reduced only through some form of
aggressive behavior. From a social-learning

perspective, frustration is regarded as a facilitative


rather than a necessary condition for aggression.

That is, frustration produces a general state of

emotional arousal that may lead to a variety of

responses depending upon the types of frustration


reactions that have been previously learned, and

the reinforcing consequences typically associated

with different courses of action. This

conceptualization is supported by several lines of

evidence.

Psychophysiological studies, some of which are

discussed in Chapter 8, demonstrate that fear-


arousing and anger-provoking situations produce

highly similar physiological arousal (Ax, 1953;


Schachter, 1957). Moreover, the same state of

physiological arousal induced directly by a


sympathetic stimulant may be experienced as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1213
anger or euphoria depending upon the type of

emotional behavior exhibited by others in the

same situation (Schachter & Singer, 1962). These


data question the assumption that frustration

creates a distinct form of emotional arousal that

can be reduced only through aggressive behavior.

Indeed, contrary to aggressive drive theory,

findings of numerous controlled experiments


reveal that far from producing a cathartic
reduction of aggression, performance of

aggressive behavior within a permissive setting

maintains it at its original level (Feshbach, 1956;

Kenny, 1952; Mallick & McCandless, 1966). It has


also been shown repeatedly that vicarious

participation in aggressive activities increases the


probability that observers will behave in an

aggressive fashion (Bandura, 1965; Berkowitz,

1969).

The persistence of emotional frustration

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1214
effects may be more adequately interpreted in

terms of a self-arousal mechanism rather than as a

lingering aggressive drive requiring discharge


through assaultive or destructive behavior. After a

person has been insulted, unjustly treated, or

otherwise thwarted, the resultant emotional

arousal is repeatedly revivified and often

enhanced on later occasions through symbolic


reinstatement of the anger-provoking incidents.
Thus thinking about the ill treatment and the

possible adverse consequences of disturbing

episodes can reinstate intense feelings long after

the initial reactions to the situation have subsided.


The persistence of elevated arousal, according to

the social-learning view, is attributed to self-


generated stimulation rather than to the existence

of an undischarged reservoir of aggressive drive. If

the person should become immersed in new


activities that supersede the arousing ruminations,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1215
or if he should reinterpret the original provoking

experience in a more favorable light, then

“tension” is likely to be reduced noticeably. For


example, a person who is angered over a

presumed snub will probably experience a rapid


and thorough reduction in anger arousal, without
having to assault someone, upon learning that he

has been invited to the social function after all.

Similarly, on the supposition that diminution of


emotional arousal is a consequence of changes in

symbolic activities rather than a cathartic effect of

having experienced aggression vicariously, one


would expect aroused individuals to experience

equally salutary effects from getting involved in an


absorbing book, a movie, a stage play, or a

television program lacking aggressive displays.

Results of several experiments, differing

considerably in subject characteristics, in the form

in which aggression is expressed, and in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1216
dependent measures, are in general agreement

with the foregoing theoretical considerations.

Kahn (1966) subjected college students to anger-


arousing experiences, after which they either

expressed their feelings of anger to an

encouraging and sympathetic “physician,” or they

merely sat for an equivalent period. Students who

participated in the cathartic interview disliked the


provocateur significantly more than the controls,
and during the recovery period, the catharted

subjects were generally more aroused

physiologically than students in the control

condition. In an experiment conducted with


children, Mallick & McCandless (1966) found that

reinterpretation of the anger-eliciting events


substantially reduced aggressive behavior toward

the antagonist, whereas free expression of

physical aggression did not lessen their punitive


behavior. Kaufmann & Feshbach (1963a, b)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1217
provide further suggestive evidence for the anger-

mitigating effects of cognitive processes.

The term frustration has been applied to

exceedingly diverse conditions including the

obstruction, omission, or delay of reinforcement,


the withdrawal of rewards, and the administration

of punishing stimuli. It has been demonstrated in

research involving both human and animal


subjects that these operations produce quite
different outcomes, and even the same condition

does not have an invariant behavioral effect. The

manner in which individuals respond to

conditions regarded as frustrative is primarily


determined by the patterns of behavior that they
have previously learned for coping with such

situations.

The importance of direct training in the

development of frustration responses is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1218
demonstrated in an experiment by Davitz (1952).
After first being observed in free interaction,

several groups of children participated in training


sessions in which competitive and aggressive

behaviors were praised and encouraged. In

contrast, other groups of children were rewarded

for constructive and cooperative behavior. All


children were then severely frustrated and

immediately following this experience they were

observed for a second time in free interaction.

Children who had been trained to behave

aggressively showed an increase in aggression,


whereas children who had received training in

cooperativeness behaved more constructively in


response to frustration. This study illustrates how

frustration serves as an arousal stimulus that


enhances whatever responses are dominant in

subjects’ behavioral repertoires.

The influential role played by social-learning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1219
factors in aggression is further shown by studies

in which aggressive behavior is induced in

primates through electrical stimulation of the


hypothalamus (Delgado, 1967). Thalamic

stimulation of a monkey who assumes a dominant

role in the colony instigates him to attack

subordinate male members. By contrast, thalamic

stimulation elicits cowering and submissive


behavior in a monkey of low social rank. Even
more impressive is evidence that electrical

stimulation of the same cerebral mechanism can

evoke differential amounts of aggression in the

same animal as his social rank is modified by


changing the membership of the colony. Thus,

thalamic stimulation elicits submissiveness in the


animal when he occupies a low hierarchical

position and marked aggressiveness when he is

the dominant member in the group.

In human learning, responses to frustration

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1220
frequently originate from observation of parental

and other models who provide repeated examples

of how to deal with thwarting events.


Consequently, when observers encounter stressful

situations they are more likely to respond

imitatively than to engage in initial trial-and-error

behavior. Only when a person has learned

aggression as a dominant response to emotional


arousal will there be a high probability of his
reacting aggressively to frustration. For example,

Bandura (1962) found that children who had

observed a model behaving in an aggressive

manner responded to frustration with kicking,


striking with mallets, and other imitative

aggressive responses, while equally frustrated


children who had watched a nonaggressive model

displayed less aggression than a control group that

matched the inhibited behavior of their model.


The influential role of modeling in shaping

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1221
frustration or stress reactions is well documented

in countless studies showing that deviant behavior

patterns are often transmitted through familial


modeling.

It is evident that, as a result of differential


modeling and reinforcement patterns, frustration

may elicit a wide variety of responses. When

thwarted, some people become dependent and


seek help and support, some display withdrawal
and resignation, some experience psychosomatic

dysfunctions, some seek refuge in drug-induced

experiences and anaesthetic doses of alcohol,

some respond aggressively, and most simply


intensify constructive efforts to overcome the
obstacles they face. It therefore comes as no

surprise that in laboratory studies in which

frustration is systematically varied it sometimes

increases aggression (Berkowitz, 1964; Geen,

1968; Hartmann, 1969), has no effect on

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1222
aggressive behavior (Buss, 1966; Jegard &

Walters, 1960; Walters & Brown, 1963), or

reduces aggressive responding (Kuhn, Madsen, &


Becker, 1967). In experiments reporting positive

results frustration usually exerts an influence only

in conjunction with prior training in aggression or

exposure to aggressive modeling cues. The fact

that the negative findings occurred in studies in


which other variables were highly influential lends
support to the view that frustration is only one,

but not necessarily the most important, variable

determining aggressive behavior. Indeed,

according to social-learning theory, one could


readily produce highly aggressive individuals by

providing them with successful aggressive models


and intermittently rewarding aggressive behavior,

while keeping frustration at a low level. It would

follow from the findings reviewed in the preceding


sections that lasting changes in aggressive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1223
behavior can be most successfully achieved by

reducing the utilitarian value of aggression

through the development of more effective


alternative modes of response.

A variety of social-learning procedures has


been employed with success in modifying extreme

aggressive behavior. Chittenden (1942) achieved

reductions in aggression by modeling more


constructive means of coping with interpersonal
conflicts. Several investigators (Hawkins et al.,

1966; Sloane, Johnston, & Bijou, 1968; Zeilberger,

Sampen, & Sloane, 1968) have eliminated violent

temper tantrums and physically assaultive


behavior by reducing the amount of social
reinforcement that parents and teachers provide

for such behavior. In the foregoing programs

aversive consequences, usually consisting of social

withdrawal, are administered for physical assault

and destructive behavior, and desirable social

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1224
behavior is actively promoted.

Aggression has also been effectively modified

by differential social reinforcement in which


aggressive behavior is placed on an extinction

schedule. Brown & Elliott (1965) instructed

teachers to ignore aggression and reward

cooperative behavior in an effort to reduce the

amount of aggression exhibited by 27 boys in a


nursery school class. Under these reinforcement
contingencies the incidence of both physical and

verbal aggression declined. After the program was

discontinued physically aggressive behavior


showed some recovery during a follow-up period,
which was attributed to the fact that teachers

found it difficult not to attend to and interact with


the boys when they engaged in such activities. The

social reinforcement procedures were again

consistently applied and produced additional

reductions in physical and verbal assaults. Scott,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1225
Burton, & Yarrow (1967) report similar results in

a controlled study of a nursery school boy who

displayed frequent unprovoked aggression. When


the usual conditions of adult reinforcement were

in effect the boy exhibited a high rate of negative

behavior toward peers. In contrast, during periods

when significant adults consistently ignored

aggressive actions and concurrently attended to


desired behavior the boy showed a substantial
increase in positive forms of interaction with other

children.

Extinction of Defensive Behavior

As Mowrer (1950) has previously noted,


human behavior is frequently activated not by

immediate physical discomforts but by anticipated

aversive effects. That is, housewives do not

depend upon hunger pangs to prompt them to

purchase groceries; homeowners do not wait until

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1226
they experience the discomfort of a burning house

to buy fire insurance; students ordinarily do not

rely upon distress created by examination failures


to begin to study; and usually motorists do not

wait until inconvenienced by a stalled automobile

to replenish gasoline. Through representational

mechanisms future events can be converted into

current stimuli that are functionally similar to


physical stimuli in their capacity to evoke adaptive
courses of action.

Similarly, avoidance behavior can be strongly

reinforced by its success in preventing the


occurrence of anticipated painful experiences. This
process is illustrated by the apocryphal case

report of a severe compulsive who, when asked by


a psychiatrist why he incessantly snapped his

fingers, replied that it kept ferocious lions away.

When informed that there were no lions in the

vicinity, the compulsive client responded, “See, it

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1227
works.” The reality of the phenomenon is most

strikingly demonstrated by laboratory studies of

unsignaled avoidance in which animals can


forestall shock for a fixed period each time they

perform a defensive response (Sidman, 1966).

Under these conditions animals display a stable

rate of avoidance behavior and, as a result, they

rarely encounter the actual punishing events.


Moreover, the avoidance behavior persists for a
long time after aversive stimuli have been

withdrawn, and it is easily reinstated on later

occasions by a few negative experiences.

After successful avoidance behavior has been


developed it may be controlled cognitively and by

discriminative stimuli without requiring


emotional arousal. In a comparison of different

extinction procedures, Notterman, Schoenfeld, &

Bersh (1952) conditioned heart-rate responses to

a tone through shock stimulation. When the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1228
extinction trials began one group of subjects was

presented the tone without comment, the second

group was informed that the shocks had been


permanently discontinued, while the third group

was told that they could successfully avoid being

shocked by tapping a telegraph key whenever the

tone was presented. Awareness of the altered

contingencies facilitated extinction, but autonomic


responding was almost completely eliminated
when subjects had a suitable means of avoiding

aversive stimulation (Figure 6-8).

Under naturally occurring conditions


individuals periodically encounter punishing
experiences and frequently find themselves in

fear-provoking situations. Defensive activities are,


therefore, reinforced not only by forestallment of

potential threats but also by fear reduction

accompanying escape from aversive situations

that produce disturbing arousal. In experiments

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1229
Figure 6-8. Extinction of heart-rate responses to a
conditioned stimulus in subjects who either were
uninformed that shock was discontinued, were told
there would be no further shocks, or were provided
with a motor response for avoiding shock. B-l
represents the baseline heart-rate response to the tone
before aversive conditioning. Redrawn from
Notterman, Schoenfeld, & Bersh, 1952.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1230
designed to evaluate separately the various factors

that might reinforce avoidance behavior, Kamin

(1956, 1957) found that either termination of the


fear-provoking signal or avoidance of physically

painful stimulation increased the frequency of

avoidance responses and the speed with which

they were performed. Avoidance behavior was

most pronounced when it both terminated fear-


arousing cues and prevented shock stimulation.

Extinction of avoidance behavior is achieved

by repeated exposure to threatening events

without the occurrence of any adverse


consequences. The major obstacle to obtaining
rapid extinction is the self-reinforcing character of

avoidant behavior deriving from its capacity to


remove or postpone anticipated threat. Moreover,

inhibition of responses that have been punished in

the past and the successful avoidance of fear-

provoking situations effectively prevent the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1231
individual from reappraising the currently

prevailing conditions of reinforcement.

Anticipatory arousal and defensive behaviors that


are no longer objectively justified are thus

protected from extinction. Continued reexposure

to fear-producing stimuli without unfavorable

consequences eventually eliminates both

emotional and instrumental behavior through


mechanisms previously discussed. Several variant
extinction procedures have been devised in an

effort to accelerate and to gain better control over

extinction processes.

FORCED EXPOSURE THROUGH RESPONSE


PREVENTION

Avoidance behavior can be rapidly eliminated

by blocking its occurrence in the presence of fear-


arousing stimuli. However, there is some evidence

to indicate that forced exposure may produce only


temporary changes without altering the arousal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1232
potential of subjectively threatening situations. In
some instances, for example, cessation of

avoidance responding simply reflects the


formation of erroneous discriminations that

protect the fear-eliciting capacity of threatening

stimuli from extinction. Solomon, Kamin, & Wynne

(1953) trained dogs to jump over a barrier in a


shuttle box whenever a buzzer was sounded

preceding an intense electric shock. After the

avoidance behavior was well learned, extinction

was instituted. Under the regular extinction

procedure, the animals continued to perform the


effortful avoidance response to the buzzer with

undiminished speed for several hundred trials


without receiving a further shock. At this stage of

the investigation, various modification procedures


were introduced. For some animals, a glass

barrier, which prevented the jumping response,


was placed between compartments of the shuttle

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1233
box, but this method also proved ineffective in

facilitating extinction for most animals. In this

experiment, the physical obstruction was utilized


only on the fourth and seventh of the ten trials

which the dogs received during each daily session.


Under these predictable stimulus changes the
animals apparently discriminated between trials

on which the barrier was present as being safe and

those on which it was absent as dangerous.


Consequently, they continued to jump rapidly on

the latter trials, but remained unperturbed by the

aversive tone whenever the glass barricade was


introduced.

Evidence for the discrimination interpretation


of the marked resistance to extinction is provided

by Carlson & Black (1959), who replicated the

above experiment, with the exception that the

glass barrier was employed throughout the initial

series of extinction trials, after which it was

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1234
permanently removed. Under this procedure,

avoidance behavior was rapidly eliminated. Using

a similar method, Page & Hall (1953) likewise


demonstrated that the response-prevention

technique can accelerate extinction, provided it is

employed on every trial in a lengthy series during

the initial phase of extinction. Weinberger (1965)

has further shown that the rate of extinction of


avoidance behavior is increased with longer
durations of forced exposure to fear-provoking

events.

Response blocking in the presence of aversive


stimuli can produce behavioral changes through
several different means. It may extinguish the

aversive properties of threatening stimuli so that


they lose their capacity to evoke fear and

avoidance. Alternatively, it may eliminate the

obstructed avoidance responses without altering

the arousal potential of feared stimuli by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1235
producing new forms of defensive behavior that

are inevitably successful in forestalling

nonexistent threats. This process of response


substitution is best exemplified by Miller’s (1948)

experiment, in which animals confined in a

threatening situation acquired a long succession of

avoidance responses as each preceding one was

obstructed. The importance of distinguishing


between changes reflecting stimulus
neutralization and response substitution is further

illustrated by evidence that subjects that have

extinguished avoidance responding to a given CS

may nevertheless be somewhat fearful of that


stimulus, as measured by suppression of rewarded

behavior whenever the stimulus is presented


(Kamin, Brimer, & Black, 1963).

Assessments of the varied effects

accompanying response prevention suggest that

this method may produce rapid behavioral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1236
changes without achieving fear extinction. This is

shown by evidence that, compared to regular

extinction, behavior that has been eliminated by


response blocking reappears with greater

frequency to later presentations of the

conditioned stimuli (Benline & Simmel, 1967);

subjects are more reluctant to approach the fear-

provoking cues, indicating that they have retained


some of their aversive properties (Page, 1955);
and not only are subjects whose defensive

behavior has been removed in this manner more

susceptible to subsequent aversive conditioning,

but the reestablished avoidance behavior is more


resistant to extinction (Polin, 1959).

Response-prevention methods have rarely


been employed clinically. However, Meyer (1966)

presents interesting results with severe

obsessional disorders, which suggest that this

approach may have value in modifying certain

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1237
behavioral conditions. One case involved a 33-

year-old woman who was almost totally

incapacitated by washing rituals. She phobically


avoided touching common objects and

discontinued sexual relations because of fear of

contamination. Most of her time was spent

compulsively washing and scrubbing the house.

The second woman suffered for 36 years from


intrusive “blasphemous thoughts” that centered
around having sexual intercourse with the Holy

Spirit. The resultant guilt and anxiety were

alleviated by performing various ritualistic

behaviors a certain number of times. These


obsessive thoughts and rituals remained unaltered

by electric shock therapy and a leucotomy but,


according to the client, the condition was

aggravated by 11 years of psychoanalysis where

most stimuli were interpreted as sexual symbols.


Consequently she stopped eating oblong objects,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1238
and any activity that could conceivably have

sexual connotations (e.g., shutting drawers,

putting in plugs, wiping tall receptacles, entering


underground trains, etc.) evoked ritualistic

behavior.

The clients were requested to perform

threatening activities (e.g., touching door knobs,

handling dust bins, imagining sexual relations with


the Holy Spirit, eating sausages, etc.) and the
nursing staff prevented them from engaging in the

ritualistic behavior designed to forestall

foreboding consequences. The women displayed

intense distress when performance of the


ritualistic behavior was first blocked. However,
their emotional reactions gradually diminished,

and both avoidance behavior and the compulsive

rituals were substantially reduced after the

restrictions had been removed. According to

follow-up studies, the first client continued her

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1239
washing routines, but she was much less disturbed

by dirt, her family relationships improved greatly,

she resumed sexual relations, and she was able to


participate in a number of social activities which

she previously had avoided for fear of

contamination. The second client decreased the

ritualistic behaviors from approximately 80 to 4

per day and the occurrence of intrusive thoughts


was similarly reduced. These encouraging
preliminary results suggest that avoidance

behavior that is powerfully maintained by staving

off fantasied direful consequences may initially

require a forced extinction procedure for their


elimination.

EXPOSURE TO THREATS GRADUATED IN


AVERSIVENESS

Inappropriate defensive behavior is frequently

extinguished by introducing aversive stimuli at


weak intensities that do not evoke avoidance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1240
responses, and then gradually increasing their
threat value until the most fearsome situations

have been neutralized. By skillful application of


stimulus change procedures the arousal capacity

of aversive stimulus events can be eliminated

without eliciting fear or alternative forms of

defensive behavior.

Several studies have compared the relative

efficacy of regular extinction and a stimulus


change procedure used either alone or as a

component of a multiple method for eliminating

emotional responding. In an experiment by Kimble

& Kendall (1953) animals performed avoidance

responses to a light (CS) that was previously


associated with shock. The avoidance responses of

half the animals were extinguished by the


conventional method of repeatedly presenting the

fear-provoking CS at training intensity without the


shock. For subjects in the second condition the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1241
intensity of the CS was gradually raised in small

steps from zero to the level used during training,

and thereafter the light was presented at the


training intensity as in the first group. Subjects

initially exposed to the conditioned aversive

stimulus at graduated intensities abandoned the

avoidance behavior twice as fast as the group

confronted from the outset by the CS at full


intensity. In fact, 80 percent of the animals in the
stimulus change condition rapidly extinguished

emotional responses to weaker versions of the CS

and consequently, they displayed no avoidance

behavior at all when confronted with the formerly


aversive stimulus at high intensity.

A graduated procedure can produce


comparatively rapid extinction because

superseding competing responses are more likely

to occur to weak aversive stimuli than to more

intense forms which activate strong avoidance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1242
behavior. Once nonavoidant responses occur to

situations containing few fear-provoking

elements, the competing responses generalize also


to similar stimuli containing more fearsome

elements. In this way it is possible to extinguish

emotional behavior without eliciting any

avoidance behavior, provided the conditioned

aversive stimuli are increased in sufficiently small


increments.

The process of extinction can be further

hastened by combining aversive stimulus change

with positive stimulus conditions designed to


evoke behavior capable of supplanting avoidance
tendencies. By employing this type of multiple

procedure, which is treated at length in the next


chapter, the occurrence of nonfearful behavior to

subjective threats can be better controlled. This is

corroborated by Poppen (1968) in a laboratory

study comparing five different methods for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1243
eliminating behavioral inhibitions. After animals

learned to press a lever for food, they were

shocked immediately following a tone until it


acquired the capacity to suppress responding

thoroughly. The shock stimulation was then

discontinued and the animals were assigned to

one of five treatment conditions. For one group of

subjects receiving regular extinction, the fear-


arousing tone was presented periodically at the
intensity used in training; for a

counterconditioning group the training tone was

accompanied by food rewards; a third group was

administered graduated extinction in which the


aversive tone was introduced at low intensity and

progressively increased as weaker variants were


neutralized; for subjects receiving the graduated-

counterconditioning treatment aversive stimulus

change was combined with food rewards; and


finally, a fifth group of subjects participated in a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1244
flooding procedure wherein the training tone was

presented continuously for 10-minute periods.

The degree of response suppression was


measured by differences in rate of responding

prior to, and in the presence of, the threatening


tone.

The reductions in response suppression

achieved by the various treatment procedures are


summarized in Figure 6-9. Regular extinction was
found to be least effective for eliminating

conditioned fear, but the potency of this method

was considerably enhanced by presenting the

threatening stimuli in a gradual fashion.


Behavioral inhibitions were most rapidly and
thoroughly removed by reducing the aversiveness

of fear- provoking stimuli through graduated

presentation, and simultaneously eliciting

incompatible responses. An essentially similar

pattern of results was obtained for the number of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1245
1246
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 6-9. Reductions in response suppression achieved by subjects in each of the
five treatment conditions during ten extinction sessions. A value of
zero indicates complete suppression of the intermittently rewarded
lever-pressing response, whereas a ratio of 0.50 represents no
response inhibition. Poppen, 1968.
trials required to eliminate completely the

behavioral inhibitions. Presenting fearsome

stimuli in a graded series and eliciting competing


responses both accelerated extinction, but the

procedure combining these two factors reduced

extinction time by half.

A number of case studies have been published

in which a graduated extinction procedure was


employed to modify severe emotional behavior.
An illustration of the use of this principle is

provided by Grossberg (1965), in the treatment of

a woman who suffered from a public speaking


phobia so incapacitating that she was unable to
complete a speech course required for college

graduation, even with the aid of tranquilizers,


group therapy with other speech phobics, and 30

hours of individual psychotherapy.

The extinction program consisted of 17

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1247
sessions in which the student delivered
increasingly longer speeches to progressively

larger audiences in situations that gradually


approximated speech-class conditions. Initially the

student read familiar and unfamiliar passages

from a book and then made brief speeches to the

therapist alone in his office and in a small, empty


classroom. Auditors were later introduced one by

one until she delivered a variety of speeches to an

audience of nine listeners. To further ensure

adequate generalization of extinction effects, the

stimulus conditions were continuously varied by


utilizing several different classrooms, by having

the student visualize her new classmates during


demonstration speeches, and by having the

therapist himself absent during some of the


sessions. At the completion of the extinction

series, the student delivered six speeches and


attained a grade of “B” in the speech course from

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1248
which she had previously fled to a physician after

a frightful struggle to complete a one-minute

oration.

According to psychoanalytic theory,

interpretations designed to reduce the strength of


defenses should precede the labeling of impulse

expressions. On the basis of deductions consistent

with the latter theory made from Miller’s (1948)


conflict paradigm, Dollard & Miller (1950)
advanced the view that anxiety that motivates

avoidance responses in an approach-avoidance

conflict should be reduced before attempting to

actuate approach behavior.

The person with a severe neurosis who does


reach the psychotherapist is a specially
selected case with extremely strong
avoidance tendencies. Therefore, trying to
increase his motivation to approach goals
will only increase his fear and conflict. This
increase in misery will tend to drive him out
of therapy. This is indeed what seems to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1249
happen. Therapists have found that the first
thing to do is to concentrate on reducing the
fears motivating avoidance (i.e., to analyze
resistances) rather than to try to increase the
motivation to approach the feared goal [p.
359].

This theory implies that avoidance behavior


can be modified most effectively by interpretive

interview procedures and that no attempt should

be made, during initial stages of treatment, to have

clients perform the feared behavior. It is highly

probable that if therapists were to force their


clients to approach the most fear-provoking

situations at the outset, they would indeed

experience intense anxiety, and might even

terminate psychotherapy. However, the

experimental evidence previously reviewed

demonstrates that avoidance responses can be


readily extinguished if subjects are exposed to

initially weak, but gradually increasing, aversive

stimuli. An extinction procedure of this type was,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1250
in fact, also successfully employed by Herzberg

(1941) in the treatment of an agoraphobic

housewife. This woman displayed severe anxiety


and psychosomatic reactions whenever she went

out alone; she consequently refused to leave the

household unless accompanied by another person,

or transported in a taxi. She was first assigned the

task of walking by herself in a park, which


constituted a considerably weaker threat than
walking in the street. The anxiety reactions to the

park situation were readily extinguished, and she

was then instructed to walk alone on a quiet street

in her neighborhood. In this way, the client was


gradually reexposed to progressively more fear-

provoking cues, until eventually she would walk


almost anywhere alone without experiencing

anxiety or psychosomatic reactions. Herzberg

(1945) has employed similar graded performance


tasks in conjunction with interview methods for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1251
eliminating varied forms of avoidance behavior

and for promoting new response patterns. In some

of these cases, however, clients are presented


relatively demanding tasks with insufficient

preparatory experiences that would assure more


effective progress.

It should be noted in passing that treatment

approaches utilizing graded performance tasks are


just as consistent with Miller’s conflict paradigm
as theories that advise focusing on avoidance

tendencies. That is, unpunished evocation of

weakly inhibited responses produces extinction

effects that will generalize to the more strongly


inhibited forms of behavior, thus reducing the
entire avoidance gradient. In this manner anxiety

associated with successively closer variants of the

desired behavior can be progressively

extinguished until clients are able to execute the

goal responses without experiencing undue

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1252
emotional arousal. This strategy has, in fact, been

successfully applied to the modification of

agoraphobias (Jones, 1956; Meyer, 1957; White,


1962), claustrophobias (Meyer, 1957; Walton &

Mather, 1963a), compulsive response patterns

(Walton, 1960b), school phobias (Garvey &

Hegrenes, 1966; Kennedy, 1965), severe sexual

inhibitions (Haslam, 1965; Walton, 1960c), and


more circumscribed avoidance responses
(Freeman & Kendrick, 1960). In a series of

interesting individual studies Foster (1967; Foster

& Campos, 1964) was able to ameliorate clinical

seizures and EEG dysrhythmia evoked by


stroboscopic stimulation or certain musical

selections through repeated presentation of the


eliciting sensory stimuli initially at innocuous

levels and gradually approaching the evocative

forms.

Walton & Mather (1963b) report that similar

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1253
extinction procedures yielded variable results

with obsessive-compulsive behavior that

presumably was originally conditioned to


response-produced stimuli rather than to

environmental cues. In attempting to account for

these diverse outcomes, Walton & Mather

distinguish between treatment strategies aimed at

extinguishing “the more basic conditioned


autonomic drive (CAD)” from those directed
toward the elimination of avoidance behavior to

generalized environmental stimuli. According to

their reasoning, in acute anxiety disorders

instrumental avoidance responses are elicited by


the underlying conditioned autonomic drive;

consequently, in the latter condition, treatment


should concentrate on the extinction of autonomic

responsiveness since its removal will eliminate

associated avoidance responses without any direct


intervention.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1254
To support this supposition, Walton & Mather

cite two successfully treated males who displayed

severe obsessive-compulsive disorders of recent


origin. In the one case, a handwashing ritual,

which was believed to be evoked by anxiety and


guilt over violently aggressive fantasies,
disappeared after the passive client received

training in self-assertive behavior. Similar

development of self-assertiveness in the second


client resulted in reduction of obsessional

thoughts about homosexuality and

destructiveness, which were assumed to arise


from anticipatory concern over negative social

reactions to his obsequious behavior.

In behavioral disorders of long standing,

Walton & Mather maintain that cues other than

those originally involved in the aversive

conditioning may, through the process of stimulus

generalization, acquire eliciting potency so that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1255
avoidance responses become “functionally

autonomous” or partially independent of the

chronologically earlier CAD. Hence, modification of


chronic disorders would require the extinction of

both the initial conditioned autonomic responses

and the avoidance behavior. Results from several

chronic cases, which displayed only partial

improvement when their treatment was restricted


either to the original conditioned stimuli or to the
avoidance responses themselves, are presented as

tentative support for the latter hypothesis.

The assumptions made by Walton & Mather


about the conditions regulating avoidance
behavior are disputed by a substantial body of

evidence that avoidance behavior is not under


autonomic control. These findings, which are

discussed at length in Chapter 7, support the view

that autonomic and instrumental avoidance

responses are coeffects of aversive conditioning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1256
rather than causally linked events. When

emotional responses are conditioned to a

particular stimulus, other cues falling on the same


physical or semantic stimulus dimension also

acquire eliciting potency through the process of

generalization. A systematic behavioral

assessment would most likely reveal that both

primary and secondary stimuli evoke autonomic


and avoidant responsiveness. Nor is the range of
stimulus generalization necessarily determined by

temporal factors. The autonomic-motor focus in

treatment approaches proposed by Walton &

Mather, in fact, reduces to the question of whether


emotional reactions should be extinguished to

primary or to generalized stimuli rather than to a


drive-behavior distinction. The outcomes reported

by the authors are completely in accord with

predictions from the principle of generalization,


that reduction in emotional behavior will be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1257
greatest toward the stimuli that have been

neutralized, regardless of where they happen to

fall on the generalization gradient. The


decrements in conditioned emotionality would

become progressively smaller the farther the


nontreated evocative stimuli are removed from
those selected for extinction treatment.

In one of the reports (Walton & Mather,


1963b) that lends support to the above
formulation, a 24-year-old single female who had

had an extremely moralistic upbringing suffered

from severe sexual anxieties. Any form of physical

or social contacts with men, and even sexual


intercourse in wedlock, was considered sinful.
Following adolescence, when exposure to sexual

information and a masturbatory episode

generated intense guilt feelings, the child’s marked

sexual anxieties generalized to urogenital

functions. During this time she developed an

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1258
obsessive concern about urination and defecation,

and instituted elaborate toilet rituals designed to

ensure complete cleanliness. The anxiety


responses further transferred to animal feces and

urine so that she also carefully avoided park seats,

lamp posts, and chairs in private homes containing

pets. Because of her inability to use public

lavatories and benches, and her marked


curtailment of social interactions with men, the
woman was eventually forced to give up her job.

The treatment was designed specifically to

decrease sexual anxieties by pairing drug-induced


relaxation with scenes of progressively more
intimate interactions with men. As a result, her

anxiety responses to heterosexual stimuli were


markedly reduced:

She could pass men in the street, sit next to


them on public vehicles, wait in shop or bus
queues with them and speak to them. She
related two such incidents with satisfaction.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1259
She had waited with a young man, a stranger,
for half an hour at a bus stop and had
become engaged in a lengthy conversation.
This almost resulted in a date. On a second
occasion she renewed a childhood
acquaintanceship with a young man of her
age [p. 167].

In accord with generalization principles, the

client’s generalized fear of urination, defecation,


and excreta, stimuli far removed from the primary

sexual stimuli, was only partially reduced.

In a second set of outcome data presented by

Walton & Mather a single woman developed


obsessive concerns about contamination by dirt,

and compulsive handwashing rituals, subsequent

to a guilt-producing love affair with a married

man. In this particular case, however, sexual

anxieties were untreated but compulsive


responses to generalized eliciting stimuli were
extinguished. The woman was required to perform

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1260
a series of tasks graded according to their
contamination value and potency in evoking

handwashing, such as use of public washbowls,


toilets, and seats, touching door knobs, picking up

objects from floors, and walking along dusty

thoroughfares. The client’s compulsive behavior

was substantially reduced by this program but her


sexual anxieties were undiminished.

It would appear from the data discussed above,


together with laboratory findings on the

generalization of extinction effects (Bass & Hull,

1934; Hoffeld, 1962; Hovland, 1937), that

decisions about whether to orient an extinction

program toward primary or generalized stimuli,


or both types of events, should be determined by

the nature and range of changes that one wishes to


produce.

An interesting group application of graduated

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1261
extinction is described by Saul and his associates
(Saul, Rome, & Leuser, 1946), in the treatment of

severe and widely generalized anxiety arising


from traumatic military experiences. The soldiers

were shown, in a secure and relaxing context, a

graded series of movies of battle scenes beginning

with exposures that they could tolerate. Initially


the films depicted preparatory battle scenes,

followed by motion pictures of surface and aerial

bombardment from which displays of injury and

destruction were deleted. In later sessions the

soldiers were gradually presented more


frightening combat scenes. In addition to

regulating the aversiveness of pictorial stimuli, the


presentation of anxiety-arousing combat noises

was likewise controlled. At first the battle scenes


were shown silently, and only gradually was

sound introduced. Day by day the sounds of


gunfire, explosions, and aerial bombardment were

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1262
increased until full intensity was reached. As a

further safeguard against excessive emotional

arousal, each soldier was provided his own


volume control with which he could regulate the

amount of aversive stimulation.

An average of 12 showings of approximately

15 minutes each effectively extinguished the

soldiers’ intense emotional responses, as shown


by reactions of calm, and even boredom, to scenes
that had previously terrified them. Additional

evidence that the soldiers had been successfully

desensitized is provided by their relatively

undisturbed responses to a test film of a Marine


invasion depicting intense combat and severe
casualties. Moreover, they were able to attend

commercial movies, which most of them had

previously avoided because of the newsreels, and

they displayed a generalized diminution of

emotional responsiveness to a variety of sounds,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1263
noises, and even music to which they had been

formerly hypersensitive.

Results of the above study cannot be fully


evaluated in the absence of an untreated control

group and more systematic assessment of changes

in emotional responsiveness. However, the

favorable outcomes yielded by modeling studies

utilizing films graduated in aversiveness (Bandura


& Menlove, 1968) suggest that group extinction
procedures involving pictorially presented threats

could be employed effectively to extinguish

common fears that are no longer appropriate.

ROLE OF POSITIVE INCENTIVES IN EXTINCTION

The selection of appropriate performance

tasks and their sequential arrangement is usually


given detailed consideration, but the important

role played by incentive factors in extinction

programs is often overlooked. Even though a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1264
change agent has planned an optimal sequence of
activities, his efforts will be of little avail unless

individuals carry out the necessary procedures

that have been prescribed for them. Considering


that the program, if it is to be successful, requires

people to enter into threatening situations that


were previously avoided and to perform fear-
provoking responses, some degree of reluctance

or even opposition is not unexpected. Fortunately,

there are several factors that support approach


efforts despite anxiety. The distress and impaired

functioning created by inappropriate fears and

inhibitions, coupled with expectations of eventual

benefits, undoubtedly serve as strong

inducements to engage in formerly inhibited


activities. In addition, social rewards in the form of
interest and approval from change agents and

other significant individuals function as positive

incentives for performing essential behaviors.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1265
Finally, the skill with which extinction experiences

are organized is an influential factor facilitating or

hampering behavioral change. If people are


initially encouraged to carry out inhibited

behaviors under highly favorable conditions, the


possibility of adverse outcomes, which might
jeopardize positive motivation, is minimized. By

making the progression in each successive

assignment so gradual that failures seldom occur


the rewards associated with continual tangible

progress will help to strengthen willingness to

attempt more difficult tasks. In some cases,


however, the change agent may have to introduce

more powerful positive incentives to keep


individuals in subjectively threatening situations.

Moreover, when the extinction program is self-

managed in everyday situations, as is frequently


the case, performance tasks must be specified in

considerable detail if they are to be implemented

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1266
by clients with high probability of success.

The influential role of feedback in sustaining


and accelerating extinction of phobic behavior is

shown by studies in which precise feedback of

performance is sequentially added and removed.


In one experiment (Leitenberg et ah, 1968)

claustrophobic and knife phobic women were

instructed to engage in feared activities for


progressively longer durations, under conditions
where for each trial, they recorded the exact time

spent in a small room or looking at knives or did

not receive any time scores. Explicit feedback

facilitated behavioral change, omission of time


scores produced a decline in performance, and
reinstatement of feedback led to renewed

improvement. However, adding praise to

informative feedback did not further enhance the

rate of progress. Using a similar sequential design,

Agras, Leitenberg, & Barlow (1968) demonstrated

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1267
that performance feedback was also a powerful

factor in eliminating severely agoraphobic

behavior (Figure 6-10). These findings indicate


that failure to recognize progressive improvement

in performance can seriously hinder progress and

create unnecessary feelings of discouragement.

It is important to bear in mind that not all

avoidance behavior necessarily represents an


anxiety problem. In some instances, the original
aversive conditions have ceased to operate and

the avoidance behavior is, in fact, primarily

maintained by its positive consequences. A school-


phobic child, for example, may continue to avoid
scholastic situations after they have lost their

threatening value because of increased attention


and other rewards associated with remaining at

home. Under these conditions, a fear-extinction

program would be inappropriate and fruitless. If

any significant behavioral change is to be achieved

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1268
1269
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 6-10. Effect of social reinforcement and
nonreinforcement of performance improvements upon
the rate of progress of two agoraphobic clients. Agras,
Leitenberg, & Barlow, 1968.
the rewards associated with avoidance behavior

must be withdrawn and made contingent upon

more beneficial modes of response.

In many instances avoidance behavior is

supported by both positive and negative

reinforcers. Hence, partial attainment of the

treatment objectives may produce some

disappointment because of the loss o benefits


formerly derived from the behavior disorder. In
such case; adequate substitute rewards must be

provided. It may also be advisable to delay

temporarily the removal of deviant behavior that


has high functional value until alternative sources
of reward are established. One must, therefore,

identify the factors maintaining deviant behavior


before embarking on change programs, and utilize

this information in preparing individuals for

changes in accustomed reinforcement that their

recovery will most likely produce.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1270
EXTINCTION THROUGH PROLONGED OR MASSIVE
EXPOSURE TO AVERSIVE STIMULI

In the preceding extinction approach, aversive


stimuli are initially presented at low intensities

that are easily tolerable, and more stressful

situations are gradually introduced as emotional

responses to weaker threats are progressively

eliminated. Considering that in laboratory


investigations extinction is typically carried out in
relation to aversive stimuli at training intensity, it

is evident that fear extinction can be achieved

without stimulus graduation. Indeed, even

prolonged or massive exposure to aversive stimuli


at high intensities may produce rapid and stable

extinction of avoidance responses.

Polin (1959) trained animals to jump a hurdle

at the sound of a buzzer in order to avoid electric

shock. The animals were then given four days of

differential extinction training: One group

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1271
received 20 trials daily of five-second exposures to
the buzzer with a physical barrier erected to

prevent the avoidance response; the “flooding”


group each day received 100 seconds of

continuous auditory stimulation in a free-

responding situation; the control group was

merely given a four-day rest. In subsequent phases


of the experiment, all animals received an identical

series of regular extinction trials, followed by two

days of avoidance reconditioning in which the

buzzer was again associated with electric shock,

and a final series of regular re-extinction trials.

As summarized graphically in Figure 6-11,

animals that experienced long durations of


continuous exposure to the fear-arousing stimulus

extinguished avoidant responding considerably


faster than either the control or barrier group in

both extinction phases. The results further


revealed that extinction based on forced exposure

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1272
Figure 6-11. Rate of extinction and reconditioning of
avoidance behavior eliminated by different extinction
procedures. Polin, 1959.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1273
through physical restraints had no lasting value

and may, in fact, have prevented the elimination of

avoidance behavior. If responses are physically


restrained, they cannot occur and therefore they

cannot be eliminated through nonreward. Thus,

initially, the physically restrained subjects showed

more rapid extinction than the controls, but in

both the later phases and in the re-extinction, the


barrier group displayed a much higher incidence
of avoidant responding even though the groups

started at the same reconditioned level.

Stampfl (Stampfl & Levis, 1967) has developed


a method of treatment, entitled implosive therapy,
which is based on massive exposure of clients to

highly aversive stimuli in imaginal form.


Evaluation of this particular approach is

somewhat complicated by the fact that the

conceptualization of psychological disorders

appears to have limited relationship to the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1274
extinction procedure actually employed. It is

assumed that stimuli most closely associated with

traumatic experiences are invested with intense


anxiety and are, therefore, repressed and

inaccessible. Other stimuli more remotely

connected with the trauma also acquire anxiety-

arousing properties, but to a lesser degree. These

weaker aversive stimuli, that are experienced as


frightening, elicit avoidance behavior even though
the focal threat is absent. The avoidance responses

activated at the early stage of the stimulus

sequence successfully protect the remaining, more

threatening elements from extinction.

In laboratory studies cited in support of the

above formulation, Levis (1966, 1967) employed a


paradigm in which animals underwent regular

aversive conditioning except that several distinct

stimuli preceded the onset of shock stimulation. In

one experiment, first a door of the shock

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1275
compartment was raised, six seconds later lights

flashed, then a buzzer was sounded which was, in

turn, followed by shock. During extinction trials


animals quickly exited from the threatening

compartment at the appearance of the first signal

of danger, thus preventing reexposure to the

remaining aversive stimuli. Eventually avoidance

responses to the initial cue were extinguished but


the resultant contact with the second feared cue,
which retained aversiveness transmitted by the

primary experiences, temporarily reinstated the

arousal potential of the first stimulus so that it

regained its capacity to maintain avoidance


behavior for some time before it was permanently

neutralized. A similar, though progressively


shorter, reacquisition process occurred with each

stimulus in the sequence, resulting in a

phenomenal amount of avoidance responding. One


animal, for example, performed 921 avoidance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1276
responses to the most remote stimulus, 75 to the

second fearsome cue, but only 4 to the stimulus

directly associated with the painful experiences.

The above paradigm is well suited for

demonstrating sequential conditioning and


extinction of aversive stimuli, but it is unclear

what relevance, if any, the concept of repression

has to the phenomenon. The term repression is


usually employed to denote thought inhibition.
The arousal potential of an aversive stimulus can

be preserved from extinction by avoidance of

preceding cues regardless of whether or not the

protected events are symbolically represented.


Furthermore, the fact that one stops thinking
about fear-provoking situations does not prevent

him from being repeatedly exposed to them.

Implosive therapy is based on the premise that

extinction of anxiety can be most effectively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1277
achieved by repeated elicitation of intense
emotional responses without the occurrence of

physically injurious consequences. Mainly for


reasons of ease, the emotional responses are

activated symbolically. The therapist vividly

describes the most revolting and terrifying

experiences conceivable, and clients are urged to


imagine themselves actively engaged in these

shocking activities. A compulsive hand washer

who is obsessed about dirt, for example, is asked

to visualize himself reaching into a wastebasket

and then withdrawing his hand, which is depicted


as dripping with a sickening mixture of mucus,

saliva, vomit, and feces. If the dirt phobia is


believed to arise from anxiety over anal functions,

the client is further instructed to imagine himself


residing in a septic tank where he eats his meals,

entertains his friends, and mushes around in this


soggy abode. Stampfl reasons “that he who has

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1278
lived in a septic tank need not fear the dirt found

in a wastebasket.” This is obviously not a method

for fastidious therapists. The distressing scenes


are presented again and again with appropriate

embellishments until they cease to evoke


emotional reactions. This procedure is repeated
with other variations on major sources of

disturbances. In order to accelerate the process of

extinction, clients are also instructed to recreate


disturbing scenes imaginally on their own

between treatment sessions.

Relatively little time is spent in ferreting out

the crucial sources of anxiety in any given case.


This is due, in part, to the assumption that
extinction of emotional responsiveness to

extremely fearsome situation will generalize

broadly to less frightening ones. A second reason

is that the anxiety elicitors assumed to be

repressed are routinely selected from a limited set

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1279
of categories relating to aggression, sex, rejection,

oral an anal functions, bodily injury, punishment,

loss of impulse control, and guilt.

Extinction is initially carried out with

environmental stimuli that are evident elicitors of

avoidance behavior. After these “symptom-

contingent” cues, which are believed to be the

least anxiety provoking, have been neutralized,


clients are repeatedly presented with the
hypothesized repressed events in harrowing

forms. It seems exceedingly improbable from the

case material cited that the heterogeneous cues


selected for extinction could have occurred
sequentially in traumatic conditioning. There is

also some ambiguity in the implementation of


implosive procedures because no explicit criteria

are presented for determining when treatment

should be confined to the evident determinants of

avoidance behavior or extended to hypothetical

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1280
sources of anxiety. Clients may therefore be

needlessly subjected to aversive stimulation while

therapists are neutralizing hypothesized


determinants of questionable relevance. The loose

relationship between conceptual rationale and

practice is further shown in experimental

evaluations of implosive therapy where

supposedly dynamically significant contents are


never pursued.

Results of animal experimentation (Polin,

1959; Poppen, 1968) and a few clinical

applications (Malleson, 1959) indicate that


avoidance behavior can be extinguished by
prolonged or massive exposure to subjectively

threatening stimuli. Preliminary studies (Hogan,


1966; Levis & Carrera, 1967) demonstrating that

implosive therapy produced greater reduction in

deviant responses on the MMPI test than

conventional treatment were somewhat

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1281
unconvincing because of the weak criterion

measure employed. Subsequent laboratory

investigations present evidence, based on


objective measures of behavioral change, that this

method can achieve extinction of avoidance

behavior. In one experiment (Kirchner & Hogan,

1966) coeds who feared rats were either assigned

to a control condition in which they were


instructed to imagine pleasant scenes while
listening to music, or they received group

implosive therapy. To minimize possible social

influences, subjects in the latter condition listened

through earphones in a language laboratory to a


one-hour tape-recording that described, among

other frightening scenes, rats biting, ripping flesh


and attacking a person en masse. A test for

avoidance behavior disclosed that 62 percent of

the subjects in the implosive condition were able


to pick up a white rat, while 26 percent of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1282
controls performed the same behavior. Essentially

similar outcomes were obtained in a second

experiment (Hogan & Kirchner, 1967) on the basis


of a single session of individual implosive

treatment. Sixty-seven percent of the treated


subjects, and 9 percent of the controls, could pick
up a rat in a subsequent behavioral test.

The efficacy of this method was further


evaluated in a comparative study (Hogan &
Kirchner, 1968) with coeds who feared snakes.

One group participated in a 45-minute implosive

session in which they were asked to imagine slimy

snakes crawling over them, biting them


relentlessly, and finally wrapping tightly around
their necks, slowly strangling them. A second

group of subjects, assigned to a verbal therapy

condition, discussed their interpersonal

relationships, their prior experiences with snakes,

and were reassured that snakes are harmless. The

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1283
third group of subjects read material concerning

the myths and habits of snakes. Whereas the

percentage of subjects able to pick up a snake after


treatment did not differ significantly in the

implosive (70 percent) and the verbal therapy (40

percent) groups, these subjects were considerably

braver than those receiving bibliotherapy (10

percent). Moreover, the implosive procedure was


successful with 67 percent of the coeds who had
previously failed to pick up the snake after

completing verbal therapy or bibliotherapy. The

fact that a brief verbal discussion produced

criterion performances in 40 percent of the cases


suggests that either the avoidance behavior of

many of the subjects was relatively weak to begin


with or the test was not sufficiently difficult. In

future assessments of implosive therapy, it would

therefore be of value to require more fear-


provoking performances toward the phobic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1284
objects, and to test for the generality and stability

of behavioral changes brought about by this

method.

Contrary to results of the foregoing studies,

Mealiea (1967) reports findings based on a well-


designed experiment that cast doubt on the

efficacy of the implosive method. Snake-phobic

subjects were administered either taped


desensitization, implosive therapy that evoked
extremely anxiety-arousing imagery, a modified

desensitization procedure in which relaxation was

paired with scenes taken from the implosive

treatment, a pseudotherapy combining relaxation


with pleasant imagery, or no treatment at all. The
snake-approach behavior performed by the

different groups of subjects prior to treatment,

immediately after treatment, and a month later is

summarized graphically in Figure 6-12. Graduated

desensitization proved superior to the other

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1285
Figure 6-12. Mean number of snake-approach responses
performed by subjects in each of five conditions before
treatment, immediately after treatment, and a month
later. Plotted from data of Mealiea, 1967.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1286
conditions in reducing avoidance behavior toward

a snake and a second feared animal that served as

a measure of generalization. However, subjects


who received implosive therapy did not differ

from any of the control groups. In view of these

negative results, the implosive method should be

utilized with caution until it has been subjected to

further laboratory tests.

A distinction should be drawn between


flooding procedures in which conditioned aversive

stimuli are simply presented in intense forms from

implosive procedures that provide vivid accounts


of hazardous consequences that the feared objects
can produce. There is considerable difference

between exposing people repeatedly to a fearsome


collection of rodents without any adverse effects

and depicting them eating human flesh. Some of

the portrayed consequences may never have

occurred to phobic subjects and could establish, at

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1287
least temporarily, a new basis of fearful self-

arousal. It is interesting to note in this regard that

the types of shocking and nauseating


consequences employed in implosive therapy to

extinguish avoidance behavior are also being used

to create strong aversions toward attractive or

addictive objects. It is entirely conceivable that

aversive stimuli may have markedly different


effects depending upon subjects’ fear level and the
valence of the objects with which they are paired.

There is also reason to expect that conditioned

aversive stimuli might initially increase negative

responsiveness but with repeated presentations


would eventually lose their emotion-arousing

capacity. To gain a better understanding of both


fear-extinction and aversion-conditioning

processes would require detailed analysis of

changes in the magnitude and quality of emotional


arousal over a series of trials in which aversive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1288
experiences are elicited in conjunction with

attractive, neutral, and fear-provoking objects.

EXTINCTION BASED ON MASSED EVOCATION OF


RESPONSES

Repeated nonreinforced evocation of effortful


behavior creates aversive consequences in the

form of pain and fatigue, which inhibit responses


that will produce the discomfort. Successive
extinction operations of this type typically result

in progressive decline, and eventually complete

elimination, of the behavior. The method of


massed performance has been applied on a limited

basis to incapacitating tics and other spasmodic

movements that have proved refractory to a host

of medicative and psychological ministrations.

These patterns of muscular contraction are


usually conceptualized as conditioned avoidance

responses that were originally evoked in highly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1289
traumatic situations (Yates, 1958). It is assumed
that tics probably occurred by chance in close

temporal proximity to the termination or

reduction of intense aversive stimulation and,


through the accidental correlation, they acquired

emotion-reducing qualities. Muscular contraction


has some inherent pain- and tension-reducing
value which would further enhance the self-

reinforcing character of such responses. The fact

that the rate of tics increases under conditions of


stress and excitement is considered as suggestive

evidence for the “tension-reduction”

interpretation.

It is impossible to unravel from retrospective

accounts the actual contingencies under which tics


are established; there is nevertheless ample
observational evidence that animals in aversive

conditioning experiments frequently acquire tic-

like responses that are highly resistant to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1290
extinction long after noxious stimulation has been

discontinued. Considering, however, that

persistent stereotyped movements have also been


established through adventitious reinforcement in

experiments employing reward contingencies

(Skinner, 1948), it is probable that tics, in fact,

originate under a variety of learning conditions.

Avoidance responses that are automatically


self-reinforcing upon occurrence can be
eliminated in several different ways. In a stimulus-

oriented approach one would neutralize the

conditioned aversive stimuli controlling the


occurrence of avoidance behavior. On the other
hand, in a response-oriented approach, efforts are

made to nullify the rewarding value of avoidance


responses either by externally administered

negative consequences (Barrett, 1962;

Goldiamond, 1965), or by massed evocation that

results in response-produced aversive effects.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1291
Desensitization procedures have not been utilized

to eliminate tics, but reactive extinction methods

have.

One factor that influences the rate of extinction

based on reactive procedures is the frequency

with which the responses are performed. In

general, there tends to be little diminution in the

strength of avoidance behavior when extinction


trials are widely distributed, whereas under
conditions of massed evocation aversive effects

presumably build up rapidly, and, consequently,

extinction is accelerated (Calvin et al., 1956;


Edmonson & Amsel, 1954). In accord with
laboratory findings, Yates (1958) reports that

massed evocation of responses in which a woman


voluntarily performed multiple tics for as long as

one hour, followed by prolonged rest, was the

most effective procedure for extinguishing the

motor responses. After the experimental program,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1292
the client carried out the exercises at home (Jones,

1960) and recorded the number of tics that she

was able to produce intentionally during each one-


minute period. The results show a progressive

decline in the rate of voluntarily emitted

responses with successive extinction periods, as

well as a significant reduction in her involuntary

tics in everyday situations.

Clark (1966) treated three adults, all of whom


manifested explosive repetition of obscenities or

other expletives along with various motor tics.

Because of this peculiar behavior one of the males


was unable to appear in public or retain any
friends, while the second was in danger of losing

his job because of his incessant loud barking. A


massed-practice regimen was employed in which

the clients repeated the verbal tics as often as

possible until they could no longer emit them. One

of the three cases, a female in whom the motor tic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1293
was more prominent, discontinued treatment

after a reluctant start; in the other two, the

muscular spasms disappeared spontaneously as


the verbal tics were successfully extinguished. The

clients remained free of tics as corroborated by

recorded follow-up interviews.

Similar positive results are reported by

Costello (1963) in the treatment of a 12-year-old


boy who displayed a persistent eye-blink tic. The
boy was initially instructed to produce the tics

deliberately in front of a mirror for five-minute

periods several times a day. The duration of the


massed practice was then gradually increased to
one-hour sessions. Although no quantitative data

are presented, it is reported that the frequency of


tics declined markedly and remained at a low level

when evaluated again one year later.

On the assumption that extinction occurs faster

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1294
under conditions of low rather than high drive
states, Walton (1961; 1964) utilized massed

performance combined with medication to reduce


emotional arousal in eliminating severe tics in two

boys. One of the tiqueurs exhibited violent arm

and leg movements that made it exceedingly

difficult for him to eat his meals, and unsettled


anyone in his immediate vicinity; the second

suffered for eleven years from vigorous head-

shaking and explosive nasal expiration. In both

cases the tics were durably eliminated by reactive

extinction. The contribution of the medication to


these changes cannot be assessed, however, in the

absence of cases treated without the


pharmacological supplement. Even though

responses may be extinguished more rapidly in a


tranquilized than in a nondrug state, the clinical

use of drugs may be contraindicated. The reason


for this, as will be shown later, is that extinction

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1295
effects achieved in the drug condition often fail to

transfer to the nondrug state.

Contrary to the preceding favorable outcomes,

Feldman & Werry (1966) were unable to achieve

any decline in head-jerking and eye-blinking tics


in an adolescent boy through massed practice. The

authors ascribe the failure to the presence of high

anxiety. It is entirely possible that the conflicting


findings are partly due to considerable differences
in the way in which extinction was conducted.

Previous investigators have utilized prolonged

periods of massed performances lasting several

hours, whereas in the study by Feldman & Werry


the boy performed the head tic for only five-
minute sessions because of dizziness, while the

eye-blink served as an unpracticed control.

However, this interpretation may not fully account

for the discrepancies. Data published by Abi Rafi

(1962) show that even the same procedure of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1296
extended response evocation may produce

differential results. In one case, a man who lost

considerable sleep because of interference from


pronounced facial grimacings benefited greatly

from this method. The second case was an elderly

woman who was forced to relinquish many

activities she enjoyed because of a foot-tapping tic

that was highly disturbing to others. Prolonged


massed evocation failed to produce any
appreciable response decrement. Her obstinate tic

was subsequently modified successfully by self-

control training in which incipient foot

movements activated a buzzer to prevent further


responding. One might wonder, because of the

client’s favorable response to a simple alternative


treatment, whether greater progress would be

achieved in behavioral modification if failures

were less frequently attributed to inferred anxiety


states.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1297
The studies reported above indicate that a

program of prolonged massed practice can

extinguish extremely persistent tics, but the


specific factors responsible for the observed

changes and the most efficacious procedures


cannot be determined from these data. The
interpretation of results is especially complicated

when medicinals or other methods are utilized in

conjunction with repeated performance. Even the


recommended optimum conditions of massed

practice must be accepted with reservation, since

the supporting experimental data (Yates, 1958)


are based on a single case in which both duration

and intensity of responding were continuously


varied; consequently, it is by no means clear

whether changes in extinction rate represent the

cumulative effects of prior nonreinforced


performance or variations in duration of evocation

sessions. If response-produced aversive effects

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1298
play an influential role in the efficacy of massed-

practice methods, it would be of considerable

interest to investigate extinction rates as a

function of both length of repeated performance


and effortfulness of response. According to the

inhibition hypothesis, increased effortfulness,

which could be varied in terms of the vigor with


which responses are performed, should result in
faster extinction.

The elimination of persistent behavior under

conditions of massed nonreinforced evocation is

generally attributed to the development of

conditioned inhibitory responses arising from


reactive fatigue states. As noted earlier, however,
interfering responses can originate from a variety

of sources; therefore, extinction outcomes may

reflect several different processes. Moreover,

some reduction in tics is probably attributable to

increased efforts at self-control (Barrett, 1962).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1299
EXTINCTION IN INTERVIEW APPROACHES

Interview treatment approaches generally


consider permissiveness to be an important

condition for therapeutic change. It is expected

that when a client repeatedly expresses thoughts

and feelings which, as a result of a previous


history of punishment, elicit anxiety or guilt, but

which the therapist does not disapprove or


criticize, the client’s inappropriate emotional
responses will be gradually extinguished through

nonreinforcement. It is further assumed that


extinction effects will generalize to thoughts

concerning related topics which also may be

inhibited, and to the corresponding verbal and

physical behavior as well (Dollard & Miller, 1950).

Some suggestive evidence for the relationship


between permissiveness and extinction of

conditioned emotionality associated with verbal


behavior is provided in two studies reported by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1300
Dittes (1957a, b). In one investigation (1957b),
involving analyses of specific client-therapist

interaction sequences, Dittes found that


permissive responses on the part of the therapist

toward sexual statements were followed by

decreases in the client’s autonomic responses,

resistive or avoidant remarks, and interruptions in


speech. A sequential analysis of 30

psychotherapeutic interviews with the same client

revealed that initially sexual statements were

accompanied by strong emotional arousal, but

with repeated evocation anxiety responses to


verbal sexual expressions were gradually

extinguished.

There is every reason to expect that if

psychotherapists respond favorably toward their


clients’ verbal expressions of thoughts and feelings

that were previously punished, the attendant


emotional responses will eventually extinguish.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1301
The critical issues, therefore, that remain to be

answered are concerned with the degree of

generalization of extinction effects to nonverbal


behavior and to other persons. These questions

are particularly critical since it is not uncommon

for clients to express thoughts and feelings freely

within the safety of the interview setting, but to

remain inhibited and fearful in their everyday


interactions. If a satisfactory degree of transfer can
be demonstrated, which is doubtful in view of the

generally discouraging outcome data, controlled

studies would be needed to assess the relative

efficacy of verbal extinction procedures and


approaches employing graded performance tasks

in eliminating inappropriate affective and


avoidant behavior.

“Abreaction” and Extinction. Changes effected

by abreactive procedures, in which clients are

induced by hypnosis, intravenous barbiturates, or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1302
inhalational anesthetics to revivify past traumatic

events, may likewise be explained in terms of an

extinction process. During symbolic reinstatement


of traumatic episodes, individuals typically

express the intense emotional responses

experienced at the time of the fear-provoking

incidents. The emotional expression is also

frequently accompanied by a reduction in


avoidance behavior that was originally elicited in
the traumatic situation and subsequently

generalized to other similar stressful situations.

The process of traumatic aversive


conditioning, generalization, and extinction is
illustrated in a successful ether abreaction of an

anxiety disorder apparently originating in a


gruesome battle experience 18 years earlier (Little

& James, 1964). The client had shot two young

soldiers in the back with a concealed weapon

while being taken captive near enemy lines. After

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1303
disarming two other soldiers, he burst through a

doorway in a small farmhouse to find 12 enemy

troops in the process of wakening. He stood guard


over them for 10 strained hours, finally shot their

sergeant, who kept urging the soldiers to rush

their captor, and brought in the prisoners when

night fell. The next day he developed a temporary

paraplegia when a rifle grenade exploded nearby.


Following his army discharge, the client continued
to experience chronic anxiety and guilt, avoided all

military functions and, for 18 years, was unable to

open and walk through a door if he could hear

voices on the other side.

The client received five abreaction sessions in

which he recreated, in action and with violent


emotion, the traumatic military episode. The

authors report progressive reduction in anxiety

and guilt with each session. Moreover, the door

phobia was eliminated, and according to the 12-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1304
month follow-up report, the client extended his

range of social interactions and continued to

experience no difficulties in walking through


doors anywhere.

The hypnotic and pharmacological abreactive

techniques currently in use are derived

historically from the early work of Freud & Breuer

(1940), who employed hypnotic abreaction in the


modification of functional sensory-motor
disorders such as anesthesias, neuralgias,

paralyses, visual disorders, epileptoid convulsions,

and other forms of defensive reactions. This


method, however was abandoned by Freud in
favor of free association and interpretive

procedures because affective expression appeared


to produce only a temporary elimination of the

associated behavioral disorders.

As shown earlier, in order to achieve

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1305
permanent or complete extinction of emotional
responses it is necessary to present the fear-

provoking stimuli repeatedly without


reinforcement. During the course of extinction,

emotional responses are likely to reappear at

some strength, although the amount of recovery

diminishes with successive extinctions. It is,


therefore, not surprising that a few extinction

sessions, in which a client was symbolically

reexposed to highly traumatic stimulus events,

failed to reduce emotional responding to a stable

zero level. Had Freud extended the extinction


series it is probable that his original “cathartic”

procedure might have proved more efficacious


than the protracted interpretive form of treatment

that he subsequently adopted.

An interesting laboratory demonstration of the

progressive decline of emotional behavior with


repeated hypnotic abreactions of a traumatic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1306
episode is furnished by Lifshitz & Blair (1960).

The subject, a 23-year-old female, revivified under

hypnotic age regression a near drowning that she


had experienced at 10 years of age. “She was at the

beach with her father and waded too far out into

the water, was knocked down by a succession of

waves, inhaled and swallowed water, and was in

fear of drowning when rescued by her father [p.


248].”

Under hypnosis the subject spontaneously

recalled this specific episode seven times, during

each of which the following autonomic reactions


were continuously recorded: duration of
abreaction as revealed principally by facial

expressions; basal heart rate immediately prior to


the description of the unpleasant experience;

maximum respiratory rate during abreaction; GSR

reactivity; frontalis muscle activity; cheek

temperature; and total body movement. As shown

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1307
in Table 6-1, repeated nonreinforced evocation of

emotional responses to the past traumatic event

produced a diminution of emotional


responsiveness. The fact that the subject

continued to exhibit marked physiological

responses to other unrelated traumatic incidents

indicates that the former changes reflect a genuine

extinction effect, rather than a general adaptation


process.

Table 6-1 Analysis of Poly graphic Recordings (Lifshitz &


Blair, 1960)
Duration of 2' 2' 1'10" 1' 1'
abreaction
Maximum 36/min 39/min 18/min 24/min 24/min
respiratory
rate during
abreaction
No. of GSR 16 16 6 9 1
waves
during
abreaction
Basal heart 81/min 72/min 90/min 87/min 87/min
rate
(20 sec.
average)
Maximum 100/min 120/min 95/min 100/min 100/min

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1308
heart rate
during
abreaction
Frontalis Moderate Moderate Slight Very Very
muscle slight slight
activity
during
abreaction
Cheek Fall Fall (toe No No No
temperature temp) change change change
during
abreaction
Bed Moderate Marked None None None
movement
during
abreaction

Abreactive procedures are probably best


suited for producing rapid and stable extinction of

emotional responses developed in traumatic


conditioning situations, provided that the threats

are no longer present. Clinical case data (Shrovon


& Sargant, 1947) seem to bear this out, although in

instances where abreaction is used in conjunction


with drastic environmental changes as well as

other treatment procedures, it is impossible to


isolate the factors responsible for the reported

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1309
behavioral modifications.

Traditional accounts of the abreactive process

generally ascribe beneficial outcomes to the


“discharge of pent-up emotions” and the “working

through” of spontaneously recalled material. From

a learning point of view, the critical therapeutic


factor is the repeated elicitation of emotional
responses without reinforcement rather than the

energy discharges or the historical insights. For


this reason, it is not unexpected that persons who

express strong hostile, dependent, or depressive

feelings while under the effects of barbiturates or

anesthetics fail to attain enduring benefits

(Hordern, 1952) when the reinforcement


contingencies generating and maintaining these

unpleasant emotional states remain unaltered.


Discussions of the efficacy of abreactive

procedures are usually confined to the influence of


clients’ personality characteristics and emotion

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1310
induction procedures, hypnotic, barbiturate, or

anesthetic. However, both in theorizing and in

clinical practice, virtually no attention has been


devoted to the variables that determine the rate of

extinction.

Summary

In the process of extinction, when the

reinforcing consequences for a particular response

pattern are consistently discontinued, the

recurrence of the behavior is diminished and


eventually ceases. Since the decremental effects of

nonreinforcement are controlled by many


variables, several different theoretical
interpretations of extinction have been proposed.

Contrary to the connotation of the term,

extinguished behavior is displaced rather than

permanently lost. In fact, nonreinforced behavior

is often abandoned without being performed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1311
solely as a result of observing changes in
conditions of reinforcement, and it is easily

recovered by reinstating the original


reinforcement contingencies. Such rapid changes

in behavior suggest that extinction phenomena

primarily reflect the operation of cognitively

mediated inhibitory sets rather than the alteration


of specific stimulus-response associations. That is,

when an organism discerns that the usual

response consequences have been discontinued,

the behavior is discarded and supplanted by

alternative response patterns. However, in the


case of severe avoidance behavior, cognitive

control may be relatively weak and the absence of


aversive consequences must be repeatedly

experienced, either directly or vicariously, before


the behavior is abandoned.

Under conditions where no reinforcement is


externally administered during the extinction

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1312
phase, it is assumed that continued performance

of nonrewarded behavior generates aversive

effects and its cessation provides negative


reinforcement for competing response patterns.

This may take the form of fatigue reduction,

elimination of aversive emotional effects resulting

from the omission of expected rewards, or fear

reduction resulting from the absence of aversive


consequences.

Behavior that is maintained by positive

reinforcement is extinguished by discontinuing its

rewarding consequences. Omission of expected


rewards for given performances can generate
aversive emotional effects that function

analogously to punishment, as shown by evidence


that stimuli previously associated with nonreward

acquire arousal capacity, their presence reduces

responsiveness, and escape from cues signifying

nonreward can reinforce new performances. As

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1313
behavior is reduced through nonreward,

alternative modes of response eventually emerge.

The degree of behavioral variability and the


characteristics of the new actions occurring during

the course of extinction depend upon the options

that individuals have previously learned for

securing reinforcement. Reliance on extinction

alone, therefore, does not guarantee that desired


response patterns will appear unless they happen
to be strongly developed. Behavioral changes can

be hastened and effectively controlled, however,

by combining extinction of undesired responses

with stimulus control procedures and with


modeling and positive reinforcement of competing

response tendencies.

Extinction of avoidance behavior is achieved

by repeated exposure to subjectively threatening

stimuli under conditions designed to ensure that

neither the avoidance responses nor the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1314
anticipated adverse consequences occur. The

major obstacle to eliminating defensive behavior

arises because successful avoidance of events that


are no longer dangerous preserves their

aversiveness and forestallment of anticipated

consequences reinforces the defensive activities.

Attempts have been made to facilitate extinction of

avoidance behavior by blocking its occurrence in


the presence of fear-arousing stimuli. Such forced
exposure may simply produce other types of

avoidance responses without altering the arousal

potential of the feared situations.

Inappropriate defensive behavior is most


frequently eliminated by an extinction procedure

involving gradual stimulus change. This is


achieved by reexposing individuals initially to

aversive stimuli at weak intensities that do not

evoke avoidance responses, and then to gradually

increasing threats until the most fearsome

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1315
situations have been completely neutralized. If the

aversive stimuli are increased in sufficiently small

increments emotional behavior can be successfully


extinguished with minimal fear elicitation and

avoidance responding.

The third and most recently developed

extinction procedure involves prolonged and

massive exposure to intensely disturbing events


that are symbolically created. Preliminary findings
show that avoidance behavior can be eliminated in

this manner, but the full effects of this method

have not as yet been adequately evaluated.

REFERENCES

Abi Rafi, A. Learning theory and the treatment of tics.


Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 1962, 6, 71-
76.

Agras, S., Leitenberg, H., & Barlow, D. H. Social


reinforcement in the modification of
agoraphobia. Archives of General Psychiatry,
1968, 19, 423-427.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1316
Alexander, F. Psychoanalysis and psychotherapy. New
York: Norton, 1956.

Allen, K. E., Hart, B., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & Wolf, M.
M. Effects of social reinforcement on isolate
behavior of a nursery school child. Child
Development, 1964, 35, 511-518.

Amsel, A. Frustrative nonreward in partial


reinforcement and discrimination learning: Some
recent history and a theoretical extension.
Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 306-328.

Amsel, A., & Surridge, C. T. The influence of magnitude


of reward on the aversive properties of
anticipatory frustration. Canadian Journal of
Psychology, 1964, 18, 321-327.

Ayllon, T., & Haughton, E. Control of the behavior of


schizophrenic patients by food. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5, 343-
352.

Ayllon, T., & Haughton, E. Modification of


symptomatic verbal behaviour of mental
patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1964,
2, 87-97.

Ayllon, T., & Michael, J. The psychiatric nurse as a


behavioral engineer. Journal of the Experimental

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1317
Analysis of Behavior, 1959, 2, 323-334.

Ax, A. F. The physiological differentiation between


fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 1953, 15, 433-442.

Bandura, A. Social learning through imitation. In M. R.


Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation:
1962. Lincoln: Univer. of Nebraska Press, 1962.
pp. 211-269.

Bandura, A. Vicarious processes: A case of no-trial


learning. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology. Vol. II. New York:
Academic Press, 1965. pp. 1-55.

Bandura, A. Modelling approaches to the modification


of phobic disorders. In Ciba Foundation
Symposium. The role of learning in psychotherapy.
London: Churchill, 1968. pp. 201-217.

Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. The relative


efficacy of desensitization and modeling
approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and
attitudinal changes, Stanford Univer., 1969, 13,
173-199.

Bandura, A., & Menlove, F. L. Factors determining


vicarious extinction of avoidance behavior
through symbolic modeling. Journal of Personality

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1318
and Social Psychology, 1968, 8, 99-108.

Bandura, A., & Rosenthal, T. L. Vicarious classical


conditioning as a function of arousal level. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 54-
62.

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Social learning and


personality development. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston, 1963.

Barrett, B. H. Reduction in the rate of multiple tics by


free operant conditioning methods. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 1962, 135, 187-195.

Bass, M. J., & Hull, C. L. The irradiation of a tactile


conditioned reflex in man. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 1934, 17, 47-65.

Bateson, G. The Naven. Stanford: Stanford Univer.


Press, 1936.

Benline, T. A., & Simmel, E. C. Effects of blocking of the


avoidance response on the elimination of the
conditioned fear response. Psychonomic Science,
1967, 8, 357-358.

Berger, S. M. Conditioning through vicarious


instigation. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 450-
466.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1319
Berkowitz, L. Aggression: A social psychological
analysis. New York: McGraw Hill, 1962.

Berkowitz, L. Aggressive cues in aggressive behavior


and hostility catharsis. Psychological Bulletin,
1964, 71, 104-122.

Berkowitz, L. Control of aggression. In B. M. Caldwell


& H. Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of child development
research, Vol. .3. 1969.

Black, A. H. The extinction of avoidance responses


under curare. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 519-524.

Bridger, W. H., & Mandel, I. J. Abolition of the PRE by


instructions in GSR conditioning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1965, 69, 476-482.

Bridger, W. H., & Mandel, I. J. A comparison of GSR


fear responses produced by threat and electric
shock. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 1964, 2,
31-40.

Brown, J. S., & Bass, B. The acquisition and extinction


of an instrumental response under constant and
variable stimulus conditions. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1958,
51, 499-504.

Brown, P., & Elliott, R. Control of aggression in a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1320
nursery school class. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 1965, 2, 103-107.

Buehler, R. E., Patterson, G. R., & Furniss, J. M. The


reinforcement of behavior in institutional
settings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966,
4, 157-167.

Buss, A. H. Instrumentality of aggression, feedback,


and frustration as determinants of physical
aggression. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1966, 3, 153-162.

Calvin, A. D., Clifford, L. T., Clifford, B., Bolden, L., &


Harvery, J. Experimental validation of
conditioned inhibition. Psychological Reports,
1956, 2, 21-56.

Capaldi, E. J. The effect of different amounts of


training on the resistance to extinction of
different patterns of partially reinforced
responses. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 367-371.

Carlson, N. J., & Black, A. H. Traumatic avoidance


learning: Note on the effect of response
prevention during extinction. Psychological
Reports, 1959, 5, 409-412.

Chittenden, G. E. An experimental study in measuring

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1321
and modifying assertive behavior in young
children. Monographs of the Society for Research
in Child Development, 1942 (1, Serial No. 31).

Clark, D. F. Behaviour therapy of Gilles de la


Tourette’s syndrome. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1966, 112, 771-778.

Cook, S. W., & Harris, R. E. The verbal conditioning of


the galvanic skin reflex. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1937, 21, 202-210.

Costello, C. G. The essentials of behaviour therapy.


Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 1963, 8,
162-166.

Davitz, J. R. The effects of previous training on


postfrustration behavior. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1952, 47, 309-315.

Deese, J. The extinction of a discrimination without


performance of the choice response. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1951,
44, 362-366.

Delgado, J. M. Social rank and radio-stimulated


aggression in monkeys. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 1967, 144, 383-390.

Denny, M. R., & Ratner, S. C. Distal cues and latent


extinction. Psychological Record, 1959, 9, 33-35.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1322
Denny, M. R., & Weisman, R. G. Avoidance behavior as
a function of length of nonshock confinement.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 1964, 58, 252-257.

Di Cara, L. V., & Miller, N. E. Instrumental learning of


vasomotor responses by rats: Learning to
respond differentially in the two ears. Science,
1968, 159, 1485-1486.

Dittes, J. E. Extinction during psychotherapy of GSR


accompanying “embarrassing” statements.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1957,
54, 187-191. (a)

Dittes, J. E. Galvanic skin response as a measure of


patient’s reaction to therapist’s permissiveness.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1957,
55,295-303. (b)

Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. Personality and


psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950.

Dollard, J., Miller, N. E., Doob, L. W., Mowrer, O. H., &


Sears, R. R. Frustration and aggression. New
Haven: Yale University Press, 1939.

Dyal, J. A. Latent extinction as a function of number of


training trials. Psychological Reports, 1963, 13,
407^14.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1323
Edmonson, R. W., & Amsel, A. The effects of massing
and distribution of extinction trials on the
persistence of a fear-motivated instrumental
response. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1954, 47, 117-123.

Elam, C. R., Tyler, D. W., & Bitterman, M. E. A further


study of secondary reinforcement and the
discrimination hypothesis. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1954,
47, 381-384.

Epstein, R. Aggression toward outgroups as a function


of authoritarianism and imitation of aggressive
models. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1966, 3, 574-579.

Estes, W. K. The statistical approach to learning


theory. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a
science. Vol. 2. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959. pp.
380-491.

Etzel, B. C., & Gewirtz, J. L. Experimental modification


of caretaker-maintained high-rate operant crying
in a 6- and a 20-week-old infant (infans
tyrannotearus): Extinction of crying with
reinforcement of eye contact and smiling. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 1967, 5, 303-
317.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1324
Feldman, R. B., & Werry, J. S. An unsuccessful attempt
to treat a tiqueur by massed practice. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 111-117.

Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. Schedules of


reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1957.

Feshbach, S. The catharsis hypothesis and some


consequences of interaction with aggressive and
neutral play objects. Journal of Personality, 1956,
24, 449-462.

Feshbach, S. The function of aggression and the


regulation of aggressive drive. Psychological
Review, 1964, 71, 257-272.

Feshbach, S., Stiles, W. B., & Bitter, E. The reinforcing


effect of witnessing aggression. Journal of
Experimental Research in Personality, 1967, 2,
133-139.

Foster, F. M. Conditioning of cerebral dysrhythmia


induced by pattern presentation and eye closure.
Conditional Reflex, 1967, 2, 236-244.

Foster, F. M., & Campos, G. B. Conditioning factors in


stroboscopic-induced seizures, Epilepsia, 1964, 5,
156-165.

Freeman, H. L., & Kendrick, D. C. A case of cat phobia:

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1325
Treatment by a method derived from
experimental psychology. British Medical Journal,
1960, 2, 497-502.

Freud, S., & Breuer, J. On the psychical mechanisms of


hysterical phenomena. In E. Jones (Ed.), Collected
papers. Vol. 1. London: Hogarth, 1940. pp. 24-41.

Garvey, W. P., & Hegrenes, J. R. Desensitization


techniques in the treatment of school phobia.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1966, 36,
147-152.

Geen, R. G. Effects of frustration, attack, and prior


training in aggressiveness upon aggressive
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1968, 9, 316-321.

Gleitman, H., Nachmias, J., & Neisser, U. The S-R


reinforcement theory of extinction. Psychological
Review, 1954, 61, 23-33.

Goldiamond, I. Fluent and nonfluent speech


(stuttering): Analysis and operant techniques for
control. In L. Krasner & L. P. Ullmann (Eds.),
Research in behavior modification. New York:
Holt, Rinehart & Winston, pp. 106-156.

Grings, W. W., & Lockhart, R. A. Effects of “anxiety-


lessening” instructions and differential set

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1326
development on the extinction of GSR. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 292-299.

Groot, H. Extinction of psychotic letter writing. Paper


read at the California State Psychological
Association, San Francisco, 1966.

Grossberg, J. M. Successful behavior therapy in a case


of speech phobia (“stage fright”). Journal of
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1965, 30, 285-288.

Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of learning. New York:


Harper, 1935.

Hart, B. M„ Allen, K. E., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & Wolf,


M. M. Effects of social reinforcement on operant
crying. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
1964, 1, 145-153.

Hartmann, D. Influence of symbolically modeled


instrumental aggression and pain cues on
aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1969,11, 280-288.

Haslam, M. T. The treatment of psychogenic


dyspareunia by reciprocal inhibition. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 1965, 111, 280-282.

Hawkins, R. P., Peterson, R. F., Schweid, E., & Bijou, S.


W. Behavior therapy in the home: Amelioration
of problem parent-child relations with the parent

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1327
in a therapeutic role. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 4, 99-107.

Herzberg, A. Short treatment of neuroses by


graduated tasks. British Journal of Medical
Psychology, 1941, 19, 36-51.

Herzberg, A. Active psychotherapy. New York: Grune


and Stratton, 1945. Hoffeld, D. R. Primary
stimulus generalization and secondary extinction
as a function of strength of conditioning. Journal
of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
1962, 55, 27-31.

Hogan, R. A. Implosive therapy in the short-term


treatment of psychotics. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research and Practice, 1966, 3, 25-32.

Hogan, R. A., & Kirchner, J. H. Preliminary report of


the extinction of learned fears via shortterm
implosive therapy. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 72, 106-109.

Hogan, R. A., & Kirchner, J. H. Implosive, eclectic


verbal and bibliotherapy in the treatment of fears
of snakes. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
6,167-171.

Hordern, A. The response of the neurotic personality


to abreaction. Journal of Mental Science, 1952, 98,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1328
630-639.

Hovland, C. I. The generalization of conditioned


responses. I. The sensory generalization of
conditioned responses with varying frequencies
of tone. Journal of General Psychology, 1937, 17,
125-148.

Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-


Century-Crofts, 1943. Jegard, S., & Waiters, R. H. A
study of some determinants of aggression in
young children. Child Development, 1960, 31,
739-747.

Johnston, M. S., Kelley, C. S., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., &


Wolf, M. M. Effects of positive social
reinforcement on isolate behavior of a nursery
school child. Unpublished manuscript, University
of Washington, 1963.

Jones, H. G. The application of conditioning and


learning techniques to the treatment of a
psychiatric patient. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1956, 52,414-419.

Jones, H. G. Continuation of Yates’ treatment of a


tiqueur. In H. J. Eysenck, (Ed.), Behaviour therapy
and the neuroses. New York: Pergamon, 1960. pp.
250-258.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1329
Kahn, M. The physiology of catharsis. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 278-
286.

Kamin, L. J. The effects of termination of the CS and


avoidance of the US on avoidance learning.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 1956, 49, 420-424.

Kamin, L. J. The effects of termination of the CS and


avoidance of the US on avoidance learning: An
extension. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1957,
11,48-56.

Kamin, L. J., Brimer, C. J., & Black, A. H. Conditioned


suppression as a monitor of fear of the CS in the
course of avoidance training. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1963,
56, 497-501.

Kaufmann, H., & Feshbach, S. Displaced aggression


and its modification through exposure to
antiaggressive communications. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 79-83.
(a)

Kaufmann, H., & Feshbach, S. The influence of


antiaggressive communications upon the
response to provocation. Journal of Personality,
1963, 31, 428-444. (b)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1330
Kennedy, W. A. School phobia: Rapid treatment of fifty
cases. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 70,
285-289.

Kenny, D. T. An experimental test of the catharsis


theory of aggression. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Washington, 1952.

Kimble, G. A. Hilgard and Marquis’ conditioning and


learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,
1961.

Kimble, G. A., & Kendall, J. W., Jr. A comparison of two


methods of producing experimental extinction.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1953, 45, 87-
90.

Kimble, G. A., Mann, L. I., & Dufort, R. H. Classical and


instrumental eyelid conditioning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1955, 49, 407-417.

Kirchner, J. H., & Hogan, R. A. The therapist variable in


the implosion of phobias. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research and Practice, 1966, 3, 102-104.

Koppman, J. W., & Grice, R. G. Goal-box and alley


similarity in latent extinction. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 611-612.

Kuhn, D. Z., Madsen, C. H., Jr., & Becker, W. C. Effects of


exposure to an aggressive model and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1331
“frustration” on children’s aggressive behavior.
Child Development, 1967, 38, 739-745.

Lawrence, D. H., & Festinger, L. Deterrents and


reinforcement: The psychology of insufficient
reward. Stanford: Stanford University Press,
1962.

Leitenberg, H., Agras, W. S., Thomson, L. E., & Wright,


D. E. Feedback in behavior modification: An
experimental analysis in two phobic cases.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 131-
137.

Levis, D. J. Effects of serial CS presentation and other


characteristics of the CS on the conditioned
avoidance response. Psychological Reports, 1966,
18, 755-766.

Levis, D. J. Implosive therapy: Part II. The subhuman


analogue, the strategy, and the technique. In S. G.
Armitage (Ed.), Behavior modification techniques
in the treatment of emotional disorders. Battle
Creek, Mich.: Veterans Administration, 1967. pp.
22-37.

Levis, D. J., & Carrera, R. Effects of ten hours of


implosive therapy in the treatment of
outpatients: A preliminary report. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 1967, 72, 504-508.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1332
Lifshitz, K., & Blair, J. H., The polygraphic recording of
a repeated hypnotic abreaction with comments
on abreaction psychotherapy. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease, 1960, 130, 246-252.

Lindley, R. H., & Moyer, K. E. Effects of instructions on


the extinction of a conditioned finger-withdrawal
response. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1961, 61, 82-88.

Little, J. C., & James, B. Abreaction of conditioned fear


reaction after eighteen years. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1964, 2, 59-63.

Longenecker, E. G., Krauskopf, J., & Bitterman, M. E.


Extinction following alternating and random
reinforcement. American Journal of Psychology,
1952, 65, 580-587.

Longstreth, L. E. Frustration and secondary


reinforcement concepts as applied to human
instrumental conditioning and extinction.
Psychological Monographs, 1966, 80 (11, Whole
No. 619).

Mackintosh, I. The resistance to extinction of


responses acquired under irregular conditions of
learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 1955, 48, 363-370.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1333
Madsen, C. H., Jr., Becker, W. C., Thomas, D. R., Koser,
L., & Plager, E. An analysis of the reinforcing
function of “sit down” commands. In R. K. Parker
(Ed.), Readings in educational psychology. Boston:
Allyn and Bacon, 1968. pp. 265-278.

Malleson, N. Panic and phobia. Lancet, 1959, 1, 225-


227.

Mallick, S. K., & McCandless, B. R. A study of catharsis


of aggression. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1966, 4, 591-596.

McClelland, D. C., & McGown, D. R. The effect of


variable food reinforcement on the strength of a
secondary reward. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1953, 46, 80-86.

McCord, J., & McCord, W. The effects of parental role


model on criminality. Journal of Social Issues,
1958, 14, 66-75.

McNamara, H. J., & Paige, A. B. Resistance to extinction


as a function of presenting a discriminative
stimulus before and after the runway response.
Psychological Reports, 1962, 11, 675-680.

McNamara, H. J., & Wike, E. L. The effects of irregular


learning conditions upon the rate and
permanence of learning. Journal of Comparative

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1334
and Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 363-366.

Mealiea, W. L., Jr. The comparative effectiveness of


systematic desensitization and implosive therapy
in the elimination of snake phobia. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri,
1967.

Meyer, V. The treatment of two phobic patients on the


basis of learning principles. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1957, 55, 261-266.

Meyer, V. Modification of expectations in cases with


obsessional rituals. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1966, 4, 273-280.

Miller, N. E. Theory and experiment relating


psychoanalytic displacement to stimulus-
response generalization. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1948, 43, 155-178.

Miller, N. E. Learning of visceral and glandular


responses. Science, 1969, 163, 434-445.

Moltz, H. Latent extinction and the reduction of


secondary reward value. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1955, 49, 395-400.

Moltz, H., & Maddi, S. R. Reduction of secondary


reward value as a function of drive strength and
latent extinction. Journal of Experimental

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1335
Psychology, 1956, 52, 71-76.

Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and personality


dynamics. New York: Ronald Press, 1950.

Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and behavior.. New


York: Wiley, 1960.

Moyer, K. E., & Lindley, R. H. Supplementary report:


Effects of instructions on extinction and recovery
of a conditioned avoidance response. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1962, 64, 95-96.

Notterman, J. M., Schoenfeld, W. N., & Bersh, P. J. A


comparison of three extinction procedures
following heart rate conditioning. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1952, 47, 674-
677.

Page, H. A. The facilitation of experimental extinction


by response prevention as a function of the
acquisition of a new response. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1955,
48, 14-16.

Page, H. A., & Hall, J. F. Experimental extinction as a


function of the prevention of a response. Journal
of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
1953, 46, 33-34.

Patterson, G. R., Littman, R. A., & Bricker, W. Assertive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1336
behavior in children: A step toward a theory of
aggression. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 1967, 32 (5, Serial
No. 113).

Polin, A. T. The effects of flooding and physical


suppression as extinction techniques on an
anxiety motivated avoidance locomotor
response. Journal of Psychology, 1959, 47, 235-
245.

Poppen, R. L. Counterconditioning of conditioned


suppression. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Stanford University, 1968.

Saul, L. J., Rome, H., & Leuser, E. Desensitization of


combat fatigue patients. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 1946, 102,476-478.

Schachter, J. Pain, fear, and anger in hypertensives


and normotensives: A psychophysiological study.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 1957, 19, 17-29.

Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. Cognitive, social, and


physiological determinants of emotional state.
Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 379-399.

Scott, P. M., Burton, R. V., & Yarrow, M. R. Social


reinforcement under natural conditions. Child
Development, 1967, 38, 53-63.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1337
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. Patterns of child
rearing. Evanston, Ill.: Row, Peterson, 1957.

Seward, J. P., & Levy, N. Sign learning as a factor in


extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1949, 39, 660-668.

Shrovon, H. J., & Sargant, W. Excitatory abreaction:


With special reference to its mechanism and the
use of ether. Journal of Mental Science, 1947, 93,
709-732.

Sidman, M. Avoidance behavior. In W. K. Honig (Ed.),


Operant behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1966. pp. 448-498.

Skinner, B. F. “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of


Experimental Psychology, 1948, 38, 168-172.

Slamecka, N. J. Tests of the discrimination hypothesis.


Journal of General Psychology, 1960, 63, 63-68.

Sloane, H. N., Jr., Johnston, M. K., & Bijou, S. W.


Successive modification of aggressive behavior
and aggressive fantasy play by management of
contingencies. Unpublished manuscript,
University of Illinois, 1968.

Solomon, R. L., Kamin, L. J., & Wynne, L. C. Traumatic


avoidance learning: The outcomes of several
extinction procedures with dogs. Journal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1338
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1953, 48, 291-
302.

Spence, K. W. Cognitive and drive factors in the


extinction of the conditioned eye blink in human
subjects. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 445-458.

Stampfl, T. G., & Levis, D. J. Essentials of implosive


therapy: A learning- theory-based
psychodynamic behavioral therapy. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 1967, 72, 496-503.

Tyler, D. W., Wortz, E. C., & Bitterman, M. E. The effect


of random and alternating partial reinforcement
on resistance to extinction in the rat. American
Journal of Psychology, 1953, 66, 57-65.

Wagner, A. R. Conditioned frustration as a learned


drive. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963,
66, 142-148.

Wagner, A. R. Frustration and punishment. In R. N.


Haber (Ed.), Current research in motivation. New
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1966. pp. 229-
239.

Walters, R. H., & Brown, M. Studies of reinforcement


of aggression: III. Transfer of responses to an
interpersonal situation. Child Development,
1963,34, 563-571.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1339
Walton, D. The application of learning theory to the
treatment of a case of neurodermatitis. In H. J.
Eysenck (Ed.), Behaviour therapy and the
neuroses. New York: Pergamon, 1960. pp. 272-
274. (a)

Walton, D. The relevance of learning theory to the


treatment of an obsessive- compulsive state. In H.
J. Eysenck (Ed.), Behaviour therapy and the
neuroses. New York: Pergamon, 1960. pp. 153-
164. (b)

Walton, D. Strengthening of incompatible reactions


and the treatment of a phobic state in a
schizophrenic patient. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.),
Behaviour therapy and the neuroses. New York:
Pergamon, 1960. pp. 170-180. (c)

Walton, D. Experimental psychology and the


treatment of a tiqueur. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 1961, 2, 148-155.

Walton, D. Massed practice and simultaneous


reduction in drive level: Further evidence of the
efficacy of this approach to the treatment of tics.
In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), Experiments in behaviour
therapy. New York: Pergamon, 1964. pp. 398-400.

Walton, D., & Mather, M. D. The relevance of


generalization techniques to the treatment of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1340
stammering and phobic symptoms. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1963, 1, 121-125. (a)

Walton, D., & Mather, M. D. The application of learning


principles to the treatment of obsessive-
compulsive states in the acute and chronic
phases of illness. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1963, 1, 163-174. (b)

Weinberger, N. M. Effect of detainment on extinction


of avoidance responses. Journal of Comparative
and Physiological Psychology, 1965, 60, 135-138.

Weisman, R. G., Denny, M. R., Platt, S. A., & Zerbolio, D.


J., Jr. Facilitation of extinction by a stimulus
associated with long nonshock confinement
periods. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 1966, 62, 26-30.

Wheeler, L. Toward a theory of behavioral contagion.


Psychological Review, 1966, 73,179-192.

White, J. G. Neurotic habit formations and the


experimental analysis of learning (conditioning).
Paper read to the Ulster Neuropsychiatric
Society, Belfast, April, 1962.

Whiting, J. W. M. Becoming a Kwoma. New Haven: Yale


University Press, 1941.

Wickens, D. D., Allen, C. K., & Hill, F. A. Effects of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1341
instruction on extinction of the conditioned GSR.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66,
235-240.

Williams, C. The elimination of tantrum behavior by


extinction procedures. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1959, 59, 269.

Williams, C. D. Extinction and other principles of


learning in the treatment and prevention of
children’s disorders. Paper read at the meeting of
the American Psychological Association, St. Louis,
September, 1962.

Wilson, W. J., & Dyal, J. A. Effects of nonresponse


acquisition on latent extinction in a differential
reinforcement setting. Psychological Reports,
1963, 13, 879-884.

Wolfgang, M. E., & Ferracuti, F. The subculture of


violence. London: Tavistock Publications, 1967.

Yablonsky, L. The violent gang. New York: Macmillan,


1962.

Yates, A. J. The application of learning theory to the


treatment of tics. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1958, 56, 175-182.

Young, A. G. Resistance to extinction as a function of


number of nonreinforced trials and effortfulness

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1342
of response. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1966, 72,610-613.

Zeilberger, J., Sampen, S. E., & Sloane, H. N., Jr.


Modification of a child’s problem behaviors in the
home with the mother as therapist. Journal of
Applied Behaviour Analysis, 1968, 1, 47-53.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1343
7

Desensitization through
Counterconditioning
Of the various methods of behavioral

modification derived from learning theory, those

based on the principle of counterconditioning are

most widely applied to behavior in which

conditioned emotionality plays a prominent role.

These psychological conditions, which are most


frequently seen in conventional interview
treatments, include anxiety states, chronic

tensions, and other forms of autonomic


overactivity reflected in a variety of somatic

disturbances of a functional nature. Conditioned

emotionality is also involved in most behavioral


inhibitions and avoidant response patterns.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1344
The introductory chapter discussed the

process whereby formerly neutral events, through

their conjunction with aversive experiences,


acquire emotion-arousing properties. If negatively

valenced events are repeatedly associated with


positive experiences, the stimuli gradually lose
their aversive quality. This outcome is achieved by

eliciting activities that are incompatible with

emotional responses in the presence of fear- or


anxiety-arousing stimuli.

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EMOTIONAL


CONDITIONING AND INSTRUMENTAL RESPONDING

Most of the desensitization procedures that

will be discussed in this chapter are predicated on

the assumption that elimination of conditioned

emotional arousal will decrease or eliminate


instrumental avoidance behavior. In the present
discussion emotional arousal encompasses both

autonomic and central arousal processes. This

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1345
outcome presupposes that classically conditioned
effects exert mediating control over instrumental!)

learned behavior. It has been demonstrated in


numerous experiments with infrahuman subjects

that avoidance responses to a given stimulus can

be established through prior classical pairings of

that stimulus with aversive experiences (Rescorla


& Solomon, 1967). These studies typically employ

a three-stage paradigm in which animals initially

learn to make instrumental avoidance responses

to aversive stimulation in the absence of the

critical cues. In the second phase, the animals


undergo a classical conditioning procedure in

which one tone (S–) is repeatedly paired with


shock to endow it with arousal potential, whereas

a different tone (S°) is never associated with shock


stimulation so as to preserve its neutral

properties; during the phase of classical


conditioning the animals are skeletally

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1346
immobilized by curare to prevent instrumental

responding. After the differential emotional

conditioning has been completed, S– and S° are


presented at random intervals under conditions

where subjects are free to make motor responses,


and the frequency with which these two stimuli
elicit avoidance responses is measured. The

stimulus that has been endowed with arousal

capacity characteristically evokes high rates of


avoidance behavior, which rarely occurs in the

presence of the neutral stimulus. Moreover, other

variables in classical conditioning that affect the


activating properties of stimuli generally produce

corresponding differences in instrumental


avoidance behavior. It has also been shown that

pairing of a stimulus with rewarding experiences

in advance later facilitates learning and retards


extinction of instrumental responding to the same

or similar cues (Bower & Grusec, 1964; Trapold &

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1347
Winokur, 1967).

Although the influence of classically


conditioned processes on instrumental

responding has been well established, the nature

of the mediators and the mechanisms through


which behavioral control is achieved have not

been determined. Several alternative explanations

have been proposed and tested primarily with


aversive conditioning paradigms (Rescorla &
Solomon, 1967). Painful stimulation elicits not

only internal emotional reactions but also

previously acquired and unlearned escape

responses. It is possible that, under conditions


where subjects are free to respond motorically
while undergoing classical conditioning,

instrumental responses are also being learned and

operantly reinforced. Some evidence suggesting

that skeletal mediators alone cannot account for

transfer is provided by Solomon & Turner (1962).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1348
They observed such transfer effects even when

animals were classically conditioned under curare,

which prevents skeletal responding. However,


curare procedures do not conclusively rule out

motor mediators because, under lower levels of

curarization, electromyographic responses can be

increased through contingent reinforcement and

later can facilitate the occurrence of avoidance


responses in the normal state (Black, 1967). In
addition, as Rescorla & Solomon (1967) have

noted, even in completely curarized subjects

efferent neural events, which regulate responding

centrally, may be elicited and modified during


classical conditioning.

Most currently popular theories of


psychopathology assume that the effects of

aversive conditioning control avoidance behavior

through autonomic mediators. According to this

interpretation, negatively valenced cues elicit

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1349
autonomic arousal (usually designated as anxiety)

that produces afferent feedback having both

stimulus and drive properties. Avoidance behavior


eventually becomes conditioned to this

autonomically produced stimulation, so that it

both instigates and directs the performance of

defensive response patterns.

Such an anxiety theory receives little empirical


support if anxiety is equated with peripheral
autonomic reactivity, as is generally the case.

Studies in which autonomic and avoidance

responses are measured concurrently reveal that


these two sets of response events may be partially
correlated but not causally related. Black (1959)

found that avoidance responses during extinction


persisted long after autonomic responses had

been extinguished. Notterman, Schoenfeld, &

Bersh (1952) likewise demonstrated that after

subjects were provided with an effective means of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1350
coping with a potentially threatening situation,

they continued to perform appropriate avoidance

behavior although their autonomic responsiveness


was completely extinguished. This finding is

further corroborated by Grings & Lockhart (1966),

who report that subjects exhibit a sudden drop in

autonomic arousal after learning that they can

successfully avert painful stimulation by


performing an appropriate avoidance response.
The generality of the preceding results is limited,

however, by the fact that only a single autonomic

response was measured. In view of evidence

(Lacey, 1950) that individuals display


considerable variation in their characteristic

modes of physiological reactivity to stress, and


that different responses are not highly

intercorrelated, no single measure of autonomic

reactivity can be considered an adequate index of


autonomic arousal.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1351
Laboratory investigations of the acquisition

and maintenance of avoidance responses in

sympathectomized animals (Wynne & Solomon,


1955) provide a more critical test of the

hypothesis that autonomic responses serve a


mediating function in avoidance behavior. In the
latter experiment, autonomic function was

eliminated in a group of dogs by surgical section of

the sympathetic segment of the autonomic


nervous system, by vagus-drug parasympathetic

blocking procedures, or by combined surgical and

pharmacological treatments. The animals were


then trained to avoid an intense shock by jumping

over a barrier at a light signal. Following


avoidance learning, the shock was discontinued to

test for extinction of jumping responses to the

light alone. Unoperated animals, which served as


the comparison control group, participated in the

same experimental situation. In addition, two dogs

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1352
underwent the surgical-drug procedures and the

test for extinction after the avoidance response


had been well established.

The results disclose that removal of peripheral

autonomic responses has only a partial effect on


the acquisition of avoidance behavior, with the

differences occurring mainly in the initial phase of

learning. Sympathectomized animals were more


delayed than the controls in escaping shock,
required significantly more trials to learn their

first avoidance response, and tended to extinguish

more rapidly, although differences were slight in

this respect. However, speed of extinction in


animals deprived of normal autonomic functioning
after avoidance responses had been firmly

established did not differ from that of the normal

controls. Moreover, no consistent relationship was

obtained between the avoidance learning pattern

and the portion of the autonomic nervous system

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1353
that was blocked or resected.

All the sympathectomized animals eventually

acquired stable avoidance responses. This


suggests that autonomic arousal may play a

facilitative role, but is not required for the

establishment of avoidance behavior; maintenance

of previously learned avoidance responses is

apparently even less dependent upon autonomic


feedback stimulation. The overall evidence thus
indicates that mechanisms other than autonomic

arousal govern avoidance responding. Indeed, the

latencies of autonomic reactions and their


associated feedback are much longer than those of
skeletal responses; consequently, avoidance

behavior is typically executed before autonomic


reactions could possibly be elicited. This factor

alone precludes autonomic control of avoidance

behavior.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1354
In a comprehensive review of the pertinent

literature, Rescorla & Solomon (1967) propose the

tenable view, principally on the basis of exclusion


rather than direct corroborative evidence, that

instrumental responsiveness is mainly regulated


by central mediators which can be established and
eliminated through classical conditioning

operations. Since central processes exert control

over both autonomic and instrumental


responding, these two response systems are, in

general, partially correlated. Major obstacles to

clarification of the role of central mediators in


avoidance behavior are created by the failure to

specify the locus and nature of the mediating


systems and the most valid indices of their

activities. The problem is further complicated by

suggestive evidence (Lacey, 1967) that the


different arousal systems—

electroencephalographic, autonomic, and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1355
behavioral—are functionally separable. Although

they generally appear concomitantly, physiological


and behavioral arousal can be markedly

dissociated pharmacologically. Thus organisms

may be centrally aroused but behaviorally


unresponsive, or conversely, they may be

behaviorally excited in the absence of central


activation as measured by standard electrocortical
signs. These findings indicate that, under certain

conditions, external stimuli may control avoidance

responses independently of physiological arousal.

Nevertheless, it is clear from studies in which


stimuli are endowed with physiological arousal

properties under curare that the stimuli are not


directly conditioned to avoidance responses since

these never occur. Rather, in the early stages the

responses appear to be controlled by mediating

events that are common to other stimuli to which


avoidance responses have been previously

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1356
learned. After avoidance responses habitually
occur with reinforcing consequences in the

presence of cues, such conditioned aversive


stimuli eventually acquire discriminative value

and can exert control over avoidance behavior

without emotional arousal. This type of shift in the

locus of stimulus control accords with common


observation that mediating functions diminish as

response patterns become routine.

Whatever the specific regulatory mechanisms


might be, the fact that overt behavior is modifiable

by classical conditioning procedures has

important treatment implications. Of particular


relevance are studies demonstrating that
neutralization of an aversive stimulus alone

markedly facilitates subsequent extinction of

avoidance behavior. In an experiment conducted


by Black (1958), after animals learned to make

shock avoidance responses to a tone, they were

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1357
skeletally immobilized by curare to prevent

confounding results from any performance

extinction during the treatment period. Animals in


the control group received 50 performance

extinction trials and were then given curare

without special treatment. The experimental

group simply experienced 50 presentations of the

fear-provoking tone while in a curarized state. In a


subsequent test, subjects who had received the
classical extinction treatment required

approximately 40 regular extinction trials to

completely eliminate their avoidance behavior,

whereas the group that was given performance


extinction needed an additional 450 trials before

they discontinued making avoidance responses.

In the foregoing study, classical extinction was

achieved by repeated exposure to anxiety-

arousing stimuli without any adverse experiences.

The extinction process can be hastened by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1358
presenting threatening stimulus events along with

positive stimuli that elicit incompatible responses

strong enough to supersede anxiety reactions. For


example, Gale, Sturmfels, & Gale (1966) found that

conditioned emotionality was eliminated more

rapidly by repeated paired presentations of

aversive stimuli with food than when the same

aversive stimuli were presented alone.

The facilitative effects of eliciting antagonistic


responses on extinction of emotional behavior are

even more clearly illustrated by Poppen (1968) As

part of a larger experiment, he compared the


speed with which behavioral inhibitions were
eliminated in animals when graduated aversive

stimuli were presented either alone or in


conjunction with food rewards. Figure 7-1 shows

the mean number of exposures required to

extinguish fear at each of the stimulus values of

the aversive hierarchy for subjects receiving

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1359
Figure 7-1. Mean number of nonreinforced trials required to
eliminate response suppression at each of the stimulus
values of the aversive hierarchy through extinction and
counterconditioning procedures. Poppen, 1968.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1360
graduated extinction and graduated

counterconditioning. Emotional responsiveness in

both groups was eliminated at an equally rapid


rate toward stimuli of low and of moderate threat

value. When confronted with severe threats,

however, subjects administered the

counterconditioning treatment required

substantially fewer exposures to overcome their


fearfulness.

In the behavior therapy literature operations

in which aversive stimuli are presented alone are

typically designated “extinction,” whereas multiple


procedures combining fear-arousing and positive
stimuli are labeled “counterconditioning.” These

methods are often discussed as though they


involved fundamentally different processes.

Actually, counterconditioning is a major factor in

extinction. That is, during nonreinforced

repetitions of a stimulus, temporary inhibitory

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1361
states and inevitable changes in the stimulus

complex eventually elicit competing responses of

sufficient strength to replace original reactions.


According to this interpretation of the process,

conventional extinction procedures often involve a

form of unguided counterconditioning. A major

advantage of methods that include counter-

response elicitation is that the occurrence and


strength of competing activities are managed
rather than left to fortuitous factors; this permits

greater control over desired outcomes.

CONCEPTUALIZATION OF THE
COUNTERCONDITIONING PROCESS

Although applications of the principle of

counterconditioning were reported by Jones as

early as 1924, the approach received little


attention until Wolpe (1958) devised an ingenious

procedure that greatly increased the range of


disorders subject to treatment by this method.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1362
Based on careful analysis of the stimulus
determinants of emotional responsiveness, the

therapist constructs a ranked list of situations to


which the client reacts with increasing degrees of

anxiety or avoidance. When counterconditioning is

based on relaxation procedures, the therapist

induces in the client a state of deep relaxation,


which presumably counteracts anxiety, and asks

the client to visualize the weakest item in the

hierarchy of emotion-arousing stimuli. If the client

experiences any emotional disturbance the

aversive scene is promptly withdrawn, relaxation


is reinstated and then the item is repeatedly

presented until it ceases to evoke anxiety; if the


relaxation remains unimpaired in the imagined

presence of the threat, the client’s emotional


responses to the next item in the hierarchy are

extinguished and so on throughout the graduated


series. In this manner the intensity of aversive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1363
stimuli is increased from session to session until

the most threatening events have been completely

neutralized. Further details of this particular


method and its variant forms have been published

by Wolpe (1961), Wolpe & Lazarus (1966), and


Lazarus (1964).

Wolpe considers three sets of variables

essential for achieving consistent


counterconditioning outcomes. First, it is
necessary to select an anxiety-neutralizing

stimulus capable of inducing a competing

condition of sufficient strength to overcome the

reactions ordinarily evoked by the emotion-


arousing cues. Second, the aversive events are
presented initially in attenuated forms so that

emotional responses to be counteracted are

relatively weak and hence can be readily

extinguished. The arousal potential of more

aversive situations supposedly is progressively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1364
reduced by generalization of anxiety extinction

from preceding weaker items. Through successive

advances of extinction and generalization, stimuli


of increasing aversiveness can be gradually

neutralized without evoking anxiety of an

intensity difficult to countercondition. The third

variable pertains to necessary temporal

prerequisites, that is, both the anxiety reducing


and the aversive stimuli must be contiguously
associated.

As will be shown later, Wolpe’s desensitization

method has generally proved successful in


modifying emotional behavior, but the theoretical
speculations about the manner in which anxiety is

acquired and the mechanisms governing the


counterconditioning process (Wolpe, 1958) are

largely disputed by empirical findings. In accord

with Hullian formulations, Wolpe favors a drive-

reduction theory of classical conditioning, and a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1365
fatigue theory of extinction. In contrast to this

view, experimental results (Mowrer, 1960;

Solomon & Brush, 1956) clearly support a


contiguity theory of conditioning, in that

emotional responsiveness is best acquired and

strengthened through association of a stimulus

with shock onset rather than shock reduction.

Although fatigue resulting from nonreinforced


evocation of effortful behavior may foster the
appearance of inhibitory responses in

performance extinction, it is doubtful that

sufficient fatigue, if any, can be generated by

symbolic and autonomic responses to account for


anxiety decrements achieved through distributed

trials in symbolic desensitization treatments. A


more plausible interpretation of extinction under

the latter conditions is that emotional responses

are gradually eliminated by deliberate elicitation


of incompatible responses and by superimposing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1366
aversive stimuli onto positive events that mitigate

self-generated arousal.

In accord with most traditional theories of

psychopathology, Wolpe (1958) adopts the

position that anxiety is a major causal determinant


of inappropriate avoidance behavior. Anxiety is

defined principally in terms of over-reactivity of

the sympathetic division of the automatic nervous


system. Counterconditioning effects are explained
by Wolpe in terms of reciprocally inhibitory

processes occurring at the level of the autonomic

nervous system. This conceptual scheme is mainly

based on the assumption that sympathetic and


parasympathetic responsiveness are generally
physiologically antagonistic. It is further assumed

that muscular relaxation, sexual behavior,

assertive responses, and other pleasurable

stimulation elicit parasympathetic responsiveness

which, if sufficiently strong, inhibits the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1367
predominantly sympathetic responses of anxiety.

It is important to bear in mind that the

psychological principle of counterconditioning and


the efficacy of procedures based on this principle

are independent of the validity of Wolpe’s

neurophysiological speculations. Indeed, contrary

to Wolpe’s peripheral theory of anxiety, research

findings cited earlier clearly indicate that


autonomic and avoidance responses are coeffects
rather than causally linked events. To the extent

that extinction is governed by mutually inhibitory

mechanisms, they are more likely to operate


subcortically rather than in the autonomic system.
It is interesting to note in this connection that

some evidence exists (John, 1961) for two


reciprocally inhibitory arousal systems in the

reticular formation which mediate defensive and

approach behavior.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1368
Controlling Variables in Desensitization

A number of laboratory experiments have been

conducted to determine whether the component

variables in desensitization procedures are


necessary, facilitative, or irrelevant to extinction

outcomes associated with this method.


Unfortunately, results of many of these studies are
uninterpretable because their sample sizes are

much too small to prove anything, outcome

measures are inadequate, treatment conditions


are applied for exceedingly brief periods, and

other methodological deficiencies exist that are

typically excused on the grounds that the

experiments are merely exploratory. Although

such studies are usually acknowledged by the


authors to be technically insufficient, the resultant
findings are rarely dismissed as having little

evidential value. The findings of some experiments


that are otherwise well designed may be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1369
misleading because the behavioral test used

requires, at the most, brief contact with the feared

object (e.g., touching or holding a snake).


Treatment conditions that appear equally

efficacious on the basis of a weak criterion test

might yield differential outcomes if more

threatening and demanding performance tasks

were utilized.

The research discussed in subsequent sections


is mainly confined to experiments that are

sufficiently well designed to permit meaningful

interpretation of the data. Many of these studies


employ the snake phobia paradigm originally
devised by Lazovik & Lang (1960). This type of

phobic disorder is especially well suited for


clarifying the role of variables considered to be

influential contributors to the counterconditioning

process. The reason for this is that the incidence of

snake phobias is relatively high, the strength of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1370
avoidance behavior can be objectively measured,

and extra-experimental encounters with snakes

that might confound treatment effects rarely occur


or can be easily controlled.

ROLE OF ANXIETY-COMPETING RESPONSES AND


EXTRANEOUS PROCESSES

If, in fact, desensitization methods involve a

counterconditioning process, then contiguous


association of threatening stimuli with anxiety-

inhibiting responses would constitute a necessary

condition for rapid elimination of avoidance


behavior. In a relatively complex treatment

containing numerous elements it is possible that

any number of variables, operating either singly or

in combination, may be responsible for observed

outcomes. Thus, for example, avoidance behavior


may be reduced to some degree by relaxation

training alone, by gradual exposure to


progressively more threatening situations, or by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1371
expectations that participation in a treatment
program will result in favorable changes. Another

possible source of influence is the social


relationship that develops between change agents

and their clients. In order to test whether

desensitization methods achieve their effects

through counterconditioning or extraneous


processes, Davison (1968) conducted an

experiment that proceeded in the following

manner.

Snake-phobic students were individually

matched on the basis of the strength of their

avoidance behavior toward a snake and assigned

to one of four conditions. For students who


received the treatment that fulfilled

counterconditioning requirements, imaginal


representations of progressively more threatening

interactions with snakes were contiguously paired


with muscular relaxation, as in the standard

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1372
practice. A second group participated in a pseudo-

counterconditioning treatment that was identical

to the procedure employed in the first condition


except that the symbolic contents paired with

relaxation were childhood experiences entirely

unrelated to snakes. Because of the widespread

belief that anxiety disorders represent derivative

manifestations of reactivated infantile conflicts, it


was possible to use snake-irrelevant items without
jeopardizing the verisimilitude of the treatment

approach. This group provided a control for the

effects of extraneous variables associated with

relationship experiences, expectations of


beneficial changes, relaxation training, or other

possibly unrecognized factors. The third group


was administered the same set of graded fear-

provoking scenes involving snakes but in the

absence of relaxation. This exposure condition


primarily served as a control for the influence of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1373
repeated exposure to threatening stimuli. Finally,

a small control group of students participated in

the assessment procedures without receiving any


form of intervening treatment.

In order to equate the groups for durations and


specific patterns of experiences, students in the

pseudo-counterconditioning and the exposure

groups were yoked to their matched partners in


the counterconditioning treatment, whose
progress determined the total number of

treatment sessions, the length of each session, and

the number and duration of each stimulus

exposure. After the treatments were completed


students were readministered the behavioral test
involving 13 progressively more intimate

interactions with a snake. Only the subjects for

whom fear-arousing events were paired with

relaxation displayed substantial increases in

snake-approach behavior, whereas students in the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1374
pseudo-counterconditioning, exposure, and

control groups failed to achieve any significant

reductions in avoidance responses.

The question of whether extinction of

avoidance behavior through desensitization is

attributable to relationship influences or other

unsuspected variables has also been investigated

by Lang and his associates. In the initial project


(Lang & Lazovik, 1963; Lang, Lazovik, & Reynolds,
1965), which involved snake-phobic adults, one

group received the standard form of

desensitization treatment; a second group


participated in a form of relationship therapy in
which, after receiving a plausible explanation for

their placebo treatment, they discussed


experiences unrelated to their phobia in the

context of deep relaxation. A no-treatment control

group was also included.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1375
Control subjects and those who received the

pseudotherapy showed no significant changes

either in their snake-avoidance behavior or on any


of the self-report indices of anxiety. By contrast,

students who received the counterconditioning


treatment exhibited greater snake approach
behavior relative to the combined results of the

latter two groups, and they experienced less

anxiety about snakes. However, results of this


study must be accepted with reservations for

several reasons. During the measurement of

phobic behavior the tester modeled each approach


response before requesting the subject to perform

the same task. Although the amount of modeling


may not be sufficient to reduce inhibitory

responses in control subjects, it may facilitate

approach behavior in subjects whose avoidance


tendencies had been weakened to some extent

through prior counterconditioning. Results of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1376
other studies (Wolpin & Raines, 1966) are

similarly confounded by extensive modeling of


intimate interactions with feared objects during

the administration of avoidance tests. It also

appears from the magnitude of change scores that


some of the group differences at a borderline level

of significance would most likely prove


nonsignificant had desensitization been compared
to pseudotherapy alone rather than combined

with the nontreated control group. There is no

question, however, that students who had been

successfully desensitized to most of the items in


the anxiety hierarchy achieved substantially

greater reductions in avoidance behavior than


subjects in either the relationship or control

conditions.

Lang (1968) has devised a self-directed

desensitization procedure that makes it possible

to manage counterconditioning variables more

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1377
reliably and permits greater experimental control

over extraneous processes. Graduated sequences

of threatening situations and relaxation


instructions are prerecorded on magnetic tapes

that are controlled by the person undergoing the

treatment. After relaxation instructions have been

played an anxiety-arousing item is automatically

presented. Whenever subjects signal distress they


are instructed to stop visualizing the scene,
relaxation is reinduced, and then the item is

repeated. If subjects signify an increase in anxiety

during reexposure to the same aversive scene they

are returned to the preceding item in the


hierarchy. As long as subjects signal decreasing

arousal to successive exposures to a given scene, it


is repeatedly presented until it ceases to elicit

emotional responses. In this way, subjects manage

their own desensitization treatment throughout


the graduated series.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1378
The above procedure was primarily designed

for research purposes, but a case report by Migler

& Wolpe (1967) suggests that it may have clinical


applications as well. A male client who was unable

to participate in staff meetings because of severe


public speaking anxieties successfully desensitized
himself at home through the use of a modified tape

recorder that contained prerecorded relaxation

instructions and scenes of increasingly


threatening public speaking situations. These

preliminary clinical data are corroborated by

results from comparative studies by Melamed &


Lang (1967), Donner (1967), and Krapfl (1967),

who found that self-administered desensitization


produced the same amount of reduction in

avoidance behavior as the standard, socially

administered form. Lang has also successfully


employed the semiautomated procedure to

investigate changes in autonomic indicants of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1379
emotional arousal through the course of the

desensitization process.

Results of a study designed by Moore (1965) to

assess some of the factors operative in the

desensitization method are of interest because


they essentially replicate the findings cited above

with a radically different type of emotional

dysfunction. Asthmatics who had proved


unresponsive to medical treatment were given
either relaxation alone, relaxation combined with

suggestions that they would show both

progressive improvement in respiratory function

and reduced sensitivity to situations that evoke


asthmatic attacks, or they received the
counterconditioning treatment. In the latter

procedure deep relaxation was paired with graded

situations based on respiratory difficulties,

infective and allergic factors, and stress-provoking

events. Each patient was administered two of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1380
treatments over a period of two months according

to an experimental design that presented the

methods in every combination and in every order.


The effects of these various procedures were

assessed in terms of subjective reports of

asthmatic attacks and two objective measures of

respiratory function. These included the Maximum

Peak Flow and the percentage this performance


represented of optimal flow after an inhaled dose
of isoprenalin.

The changes accompanying the different

treatment conditions are summarized graphically


in Figure 7-2. All three treatments produced
reductions in attacks of wheezing according to

clients’ self-reports, but only the


counterconditioning method significantly

improved respiratory function based on physical

measurements.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1381
Figure 7-2. Changes in reported asthmatic attacks, and two
physiological measures of respiratory function
associated with each of three treatment conditions.
Moore, 1965.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1382
Although the findings reported by Davison

(1968) and Rachman (1965) would seem to

indicate that relaxation plays a highly influential


role in symbolic desensitization, this conclusion

requires qualification in the light of results of a


study conducted by Schubot (1966). It will be
recalled that in Davison’s experiment, subjects in

the exposure condition were often required to

continue visualizing disturbing scenes after they


had signaled anxiety, in order to equalize exposure

durations in the different treatments. Miller

(1967) has shown that the desensitization


procedure achieves equally beneficial results

regardless of whether the subject or the


experimenter controls the termination of aversive

stimuli. It is conceivable, however, that if subjects

who were administered only the aversive stimuli


had been allowed to terminate threatening scenes

before they generated excessive anxiety, which

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1383
was the procedure followed for the

counterconditioning group, repeated exposure


alone eventually might have produced some

extinction of avoidance behavior. To test this

notion, Schubot (1966) compared the elimination


of phobic behavior in groups of adults who were

administered either exposure to anxiety-


provoking scenes paired with relaxation or
exposure alone under conditions where aversive

stimuli were promptly terminated for all subjects

whenever they signified distress.

Interpretation of results of this otherwise well-

designed experiment is somewhat complicated by


lack of a yoked exposure condition in which
visualization durations are externally controlled

independently of subjects’ emotional responses.

Nevertheless, the available data (Figure 7-3)

demonstrate that relaxation was essential for

modifying extreme phobic behavior, but it did not

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1384
Figure 7-3. Differential responsiveness of moderate and
severe phobic subjects to counterconditioning and
extinction procedures. Schubot, 1966.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1385
facilitate extinction of avoidance responses of

moderate strength.

It is of considerable interest to find that covert


extinction operations, in which conditioned

emotionality is eliminated by repeated symbolic

reinstatement of threatening events below the

stress threshold level, significantly reduce

avoidance behavior. If people could extinguish


inappropriate emotional reactions by thought
alone, one would expect such disorders to be

much less prevalent. To be sure, under naturalistic

conditions people rarely construct hierarchies of


emotionally disturbing situations and
systematically engage in covert extinction trials.

Moreover, on the occasions when they do think


about threatening events they are apt to revivify

the aversive consequences that accompanied their

behavior, thereby reinforcing rather than

weakening their fears. Because of the relevance of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1386
covert extinction for the general issue of symbolic

control of overt behavior, it would be of

importance to determine whether this


phenomenon is easily reproducible and its limiting

conditions.

Several investigators have found that the

components of desensitization separately achieve

reductions in avoidance behavior, but they do not


gain additional efficacy in combination with each
other. In some of these experiments, however, the

behavioral test is exceedingly brief, experimenters

lack experience in the use of the method (Cooke,


1968), or subjects receive limited training in
relaxation (Proctor, 1968). Other studies, such as

the one reported by Folkins, Lawson, Opton, &


Lazarus (1968), suffer from methodological

deficiencies and a tendency to read more into the

data than they actually yield. Folkins and his

associates measured self-reports and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1387
physiological responses of students to a film

depicting industrial accidents after they had

received one of four types of treatments. For


subjects in the simulated desensitization

condition, brief scenes from the film were

presented verbally in the context of muscular

relaxation and pleasant imagery during three

sessions. Unlike the standard procedure, however,


the stressful stimuli were presented by tape
recording without regard to the students’

emotional reactions. The second condition

included both the positive imagery and exposure

to the aversive scenes. A third group received only


relaxation training, while students in the fourth

condition served as nontreated controls.

A treatment effect was obtained on one of

three self-report indicators of stress reactions, and

on the skin conductance measure. No significant

differences were found, however, in heart rate

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1388
responses. The authors conclude, on the basis of

visual inspection of the data rather than statistical

evaluation of inter-group differences, that


complete desensitization is a less effective stress

reducer than either of its components, relaxation

or cognitive rehearsal, and that of the two

elements, cognitive rehearsal is the more

powerful. They further suggest that insight-


oriented approaches may be superior to
desensitization techniques. These conclusions are

supported by neither the data nor the treatment

operations. All treatments probably reduced

emotional responding compared to the control


group, but it appears from the summary data that

the treatments do not differ significantly among


themselves. With regard to the procedures, since

the “cognitive rehearsal” involved both stimulus

exposure and positive imagery that has


counterconditioning potential, this method

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1389
actually represents a variant form of

desensitization. It might also be noted in passing

that if visualization of aversive stimuli is


conceptualized as an insight operation, then the

term has little meaning.

The various findings, taken as a whole, indicate

that relaxation is a facilitative rather than a

necessary condition for elimination of avoidance


behavior. Evidence that relaxation often hastens
the extinction process does not verify that the

benefits derive from the explicit manipulation of

muscular activities. Indeed, Rachman (1968) has

argued that feelings of calmness induced by the


procedure rather than muscular relaxation per se
is the decisive factor at work. In this alternative

explanation, relaxation instructions and

presentation of pleasant scenes to the imagination

reduce affective arousal which attenuates

responsiveness to aversive stimuli. This

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1390
interpretation is consistent with the view

advanced in this book that behavioral changes are

largely governed by central mechanisms rather


than by peripheral processes.

If the competing activities that function to

reduce the arousal capacity of threatening stimuli

are, in fact, symbolically mediated, then certain

changes in the standard desensitization practice


may be advantageous. As Rachman suggests,
greater emphasis would be placed on the

development of tranquil and pleasant imagery

than on motor relaxation exercises. This issue can


be best resolved by laboratory studies of the
anxiety- mitigating effects of positive imagery and

muscular relaxation when used alone and in


combination with each other.

GRADUATED STIMULUS PRESENTATION

Research discussed thus far discloses that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1391
deconditioning effects occur even when social and
expectancy influences are controlled, and that

relaxation functions as a facilitative rather than as

a necessary condition for change. The question of


whether graduated presentation of aversive

stimuli is a fundamental requirement of


desensitization has not been systematically
investigated. If the process of anxiety elimination

through performance extinction and symbolic

desensitization involves analogous processes, then


reductions in avoidance behavior can be achieved,

both by reexposure to progressively more

threatening events and by repeated confrontation

with the most feared situation at the outset.

However, these two treatment strategies would be


associated with markedly different amounts of
anxiety elicitation. The more stressful

confrontation method is apt to generate high

levels of emotional arousal that are gradually

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1392
reduced with successive nonreinforced occurrence

of fear- provoking events. On the other hand, when

aversive stimuli are introduced in attenuated form


and gradually increased to their full threat value,

extinction effects can be attained with minimal


anxiety arousal. Indeed, by initially presenting an
aversive stimulus in a weak form so that it will not

elicit any anxiety, and by increasing its duration

and intensity in small, progressive steps, it should


be possible to extinguish emotional

responsiveness without the occurrence of

emotional responses.

Anxiety-free extinction in aversive situations


has received little study. Experiments with
infrahuman subjects (Kimble & Kendall, 1953;

Poppen, 1968) have shown that exposure to

stimuli graduated in aversiveness produces more

rapid extinction of emotional behavior than when

they are repeatedly presented at their full value.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1393
Terrace (1966) has provided considerable

evidence that discriminative behavior can be

established with virtually no responses to negative


stimuli, through the use of progressive stimulus

change procedures. Thus to alter the responses


made to a negative stimulus (S–) it is gradually
introduced into a positive stimulus complex (S+)

that evokes a desirable form of behavior. The


elements in the latter stimulus are progressively
reduced until eventually S– alone produces the

responses that were originally controlled by S+.

Evidence that stimulus control can be transferred

by this method without negative responses brings


into question the widely shared belief that the

occurrence of anxiety responses is a necessary


condition for their elimination. It is essential,

however, that the stimuli that are being

neutralized should have anxiety-arousing potency


in their original form. One would expect little

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1394
therapeutic gain from programs that fostered

nonemotional reactions to stimuli that ordinarily

do not exercise control over emotional responses.


In other words, the requirements pertain to the

motivational properties of stimuli rather than to


the elicitation of emotional responses.

In the counterconditioning paradigm the

introduction of anxiety-competing responses


presumably enables people to tolerate higher
levels of threat without responding anxiously.

Some suggestive evidence that this does, in fact,

occur is furnished by Davison’s study (1968), in

which subjects in different treatment conditions


were individually yoked, and hence received the
same number, order, content, and duration of

stimulus exposures. Students who were

administered threatening scenes in the context of

deep relaxation signaled distress on 27 percent of

the stimulus presentations, whereas those who

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1395
received the same items without relaxation

registered anxiety on 61 percent of the trials.

Schubot (1966) reports similar differences in


anxiety elicitation, under conditions where

subjects themselves controlled the termination of

aversive scenes, between subgroups that

displayed extreme avoidance behavior. The rate of

anxiety- signaling by markedly avoidant subjects


who received exposure only was three times as
high as that shown by their equally fearful

counterparts for whom exposure was paired with

relaxation. Moreover, the emotional reactions of

the latter group were neutralized more rapidly to


individual scenes and they completed significantly

more items in the hierarchy during the same


period of time. Interestingly, the anxiety-signaling

rates did not differ between the moderately fearful

subgroups who achieved comparable gains in


approach behavior. Consistent with the view

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1396
expressed earlier regarding anxiety responding

during extinction, Schubot found that frequency of

anxiety elicitation was inversely related to degree


of extinction of avoidance behavior.

Assuming that subjects’ imaginal self-


stimulation corresponds closely to scenes

presented by the experimenter, the above findings

indicate that an approach that combines


graduated stimulus presentation with deliberate
elicitation of anxiety-neutralizing responses is

best suited for reducing avoidance behavior with

minimal stress. In evaluating differential

treatment approaches one must consider not only


the rate at which they modify avoidant behavior,
but also the emotional costs to the client. The

latter criterion is particularly important if a given

method achieves relatively quick results but

drives away many of the participants because it

engenders excessive distress.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1397
Findings of an experiment by Krapfl (1967) are

relevant to several of the issues discussed above.

Snake-phobic subjects received socially


administered desensitization or several forms of

desensitization conducted via tape recordings. In


the semiautomated treatments the aversive
stimuli were presented either in order of

increasing aversiveness as in the standard

procedure, in a descending arrangement from


most to least anxiety- arousing, or in a random

order. Two control groups, one that received no

treatment and another that was presented


pleasant but snake-irrelevant stimuli, were also

included. Behavioral avoidance tests were


administered after five sessions of treatment and

again six weeks later.

Subjects in all treatment conditions achieved

enduring increases in approach behavior and

differed in this respect from the two control

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1398
groups (Figure 7-4). No significant differences

were found among the experimental conditions,

except that the randomized procedure


consistently produced weaker effects than did

procedures employing the ascending aversive

order. Although desensitization that proceeded

from most to least aversive items proved

efficacious on behavioral indices, it elicited


initially a high level of emotional responding and
negative reactions to the procedure. In clinical

applications this method, therefore, runs a higher

risk that clients might terminate their

participation.

TEMPORAL FACTORS IN DESENSITIZATION

If stimulus events are to lose their arousal

capacity through a process of counterconditioning,

then emotion-provoking and emotion-countering

stimuli must be contiguously associated. Melvin &

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1399
Figure 7-4. Mean number of approach responses performed
by subjects in each of four treatment conditions and in
two control conditions. Plotted from data of Krapfl,
1967.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1400
Brown (1964) found that repeated paired
presentations of a positive reinforcer with a

physically noxious stimulus reduced its aversive


power, but temporally dissociated presentations

of these same events did not alter its negative

valence. Moreover, reduction in aversiveness

varied directly with the number of paired


associations. It is evident, however, that the

temporal relationship between stimulus events in

the standard desensitization procedure, while

meeting the association requirement, differs

markedly from that generally considered optimal


for classical conditioning. In the latter case,

conditioned responses are most readily produced


when the CS precedes the UCS by a very brief

interval. By contrast, in the desensitization


procedure, relaxation, which is supposed to serve

the same function as UCS-induced states, is


maintained continuously, whereas the conditioned

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1401
aversive stimuli are presented briefly at irregular

intervals.

If one equates classical conditioning with a

limited set of operations in which stimulus events

are presented episodically and in very close


temporal conjunction, then obviously Wolpe’s

procedure does not fulfill these specific

requirements, nor, incidentally, does a large body


of literature demonstrating classical aversive
conditioning. For example, experiments in which

animals are shocked in compartments painted a

certain color, the CS (i.e., the color cue) is

continuously present and the painful stimulation


is experienced intermittently. Similarly,
conditioned emotionality can be extinguished

when aversive cues and positive stimuli that elicit

antagonistic responses are both continuously

present (Farber, 1948).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1402
In order to establish conditioned responses it

is essential that the effects induced by a UCS occur

in conjunction with the CS. Under circumstances


where the events to be conditioned to the CS are

controlled almost exclusively by the UCS then a


close temporal relationship between these two
sets of stimuli is required. However, in human

learning, emotional responses are generally

elicited not only by an external UCS but also by


symbolic representations of aversive or

pleasurable experiences. The influential role of

self-generated arousal in classical conditioning is


indicated by studies showing that conditioned

responses can be developed by having subjects


merely associate a CS with imagined stimulation in

the absence of the appropriate UCS; conversely,

conditioned responses generally fail to develop,


even though the CS and UCS are presented

repeatedly under optimal temporal contiguity, if

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1403
subjects do not recognize that the two stimuli are

related. Research bearing on these issues and


alternative interpretations of empirical findings

are discussed more fully in the concluding chapter.

To the extent that self-generated stimulation


substitutes for, or supplements, external inputs,

conditioning can occur under a variety of external


temporal arrangements. The question of whether
desensitization outcomes are achieved through

conditioning in the traditional sense should

perhaps be held in abeyance until the

psychological processes underlying classical


conditioning have been more adequately

explicated.

In addition to the temporal issue, questions

also arise about the nature of the conditioned

response. It is apparent from laboratory studies of

classical conditioning that the conditioned

response is rarely, if ever, identical to the behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1404
originally evoked by the unconditioned stimulus.

For example, a painful shock delivered to a

person’s forearm following a tone will typically


elicit autonomic reactions, arm retraction, and

vocalization of pain. After a series of trials,

presentation of the tone alone will most likely

elicit autonomic and central arousal without the

motor and vocal elements. Indeed, if the organism


were so constructed that a conditioned stimulus
had the capacity to create the tissue damage

accompanying physically injurious events, then

learning would have self-destructive rather than

survival value. A conditioned stimulus not only


evokes merely a component part of the original

reaction, but it often activates anticipatory


responses that bear little resemblance to the

unconditioned response. It would, therefore, seem

more plausible to view conditioning outcomes as


reflecting the operation of mediating mechanisms

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1405
rather than the direct coupling of stimuli with

responses evoked by other events. Since

responses to an aversive stimulus contain a self-


generated component one would not expect them

to be identical to those produced by the external


stimulus.

Similarly, after an aversive stimulus has been

repeatedly paired with relaxation, it is exceedingly


unlikely that formerly threatening cues will
promptly evoke muscular flaccidity. Rather, such

cues no longer generate emotional arousal. The

foregoing interpretation of counterconditioning

differs from explanations based on Guthrie’s


theory (1935) or Wolpe’s model (1958), both of
which assume a recoupling of conditioned

responses to stimuli.

INFLUENCE OF SOCIAL REINFORCEMENT AND


COGNITIVE VARIABLES ON DESENSITIZATION

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1406
The studies reviewed thus far investigated the

influence of traditional learning variables on

desensitization processes. This class of


determinants is primarily concerned with

stimulus events, that is, their content, intensity,


valence, frequency, mode of presentation, and
temporal relationship. Learning variables often

exert differential effects on behavior depending

upon concomitant social and cognitive variables.


The relationships obtained between responses

and their programmed stimulus consequences, for

example, may vary considerably when the same


reinforcing stimuli are administered by persons

who vary in prestige (Prince, 1962), attractiveness


(Marder, 1961), sex (Epstein & Liverant, 1963;

Stevenson, 1965), ethnic status (Smith & Dixon,

1968), and friendship (Hartup, 1964; Patterson &


Anderson, 1964). Similarly, informational

variables may be influential in determining

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1407
responsiveness to stimulus presentations. For

these reasons, the behavioral changes effected by


desensitization cannot be solely attributed to the

effects of stimulus pairings.

Not only is learning multiply controlled by


interacting variables, but the willingness to

perform responses that have been acquired can be

affected by a host of motivation-related influences.


Subtle situational demands, self-imposed
achievement pressures, expectations that a given

method will result in beneficial changes, and the

desire to please conscientious therapists are often

invoked as unsuspected determinants of


psychotherapeutic changes. Such factors are most
likely to exercise some control over existing

behaviors that persons can readily perform should

they wish to. On the other hand, motivational

influences alone will not produce response

patterns that are lacking, nor will they meet with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1408
much success in restoring severely inhibited

behaviors that have proved intractable to

repeated social pressure and persuasion. In the


latter case, requisite learning experiences must be

provided.

The prevalent tendency to view multiple

determination of behavior in terms of rival

preferences for certain component variables has


given rise to much unproductive argument and
research. This is particularly true of investigations

that are explicitly designed to negate learning

influences. In an effort to demonstrate that


expectations alone regarding treatment might
account for reductions in avoidance behavior in

desensitization therapy, Efran & Marcia (1967)


administered to snake-fearful students a

pseudotherapy in which they were occasionally

shocked while observing blank slides in a

tachistoscope that they were led to believe

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1409
contained pictures of the phobic stimuli presented

at subliminal levels. Half of these subjects were

assigned to a “low expectancy” condition in which


they were informed that the treatment lacked a

crucial element, whereas subjects in the “high

expectancy” condition were told that the

treatment yielded promising results. To further

enhance the expectancy manipulation, subjects


were shown fictitious polygraph records
indicating that their physiological reactivity to the

“subliminal stimuli” had decreased over the course

of treatment. A control group participated in the

assessment procedures without exposure to any of


the experimental procedures. Unlike previous

researchers, who have tested changes in


avoidance behavior toward live reptiles that have

some realistic threat value, for some unexplained

reason Efran & Marcia used lifeless specimens of


the phobic objects. Lifeless objects are likely to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1410
arouse relatively weak inhibitions that would be

especially susceptible to motivational

manipulations.

On the basis of a partial analysis of the data,

the authors conclude that positive expectations


can reduce fear responses. This finding is

considered especially significant since punishing

subjects for reacting fearfully should, if anything,


increase their avoidance behavior.

In point of fact, the three groups of subjects did

not differ significantly in degree of self-reported


fear experienced during the test of approach
behavior. The authors attribute this negative

finding to a lack of independence between

fearfulness and total amount of approach behavior

performed and to the susceptibility of this

measure to situational demands. The

methodological confounding of the dependent

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1411
variable could have been easily avoided by
measuring the degree of fear associated with each

specific approach response rather than obtaining a


single aggregate rating after the avoidance test

was completed. Considering that expectancy cues,

through which situational demands are most

frequently conveyed (Rosenthal, 1966), were


explicitly manipulated in this study, it seems

illogical to discount the absence of differences on a

measure because of its susceptibility to situational

influences. Statistical comparisons were also made

on measures of approach behavior and interview


ratings of improvement between various

combinations of groups except high and low


expectancy, the conditions of major relevance to

the expectancy hypothesis. Complete analysis of


their published data reveals that experimental

subjects achieved a higher rate of improvement


than the controls, but the high and low expectancy

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1412
groups did not differ significantly from each other

on either of these measures. In direct contrast to

the authors’ conclusion, the results show in fact


that the increases in approach behavior produced

toward the attenuated threats, though in the


predicted direction, were essentially comparable
regardless of whether subjects expected the

treatment to be effective or ineffective. It should

also be noted that “principles of conditioning”


would not lead one to predict that several random

pairings of shock with a blank card interspersed in

a hundred trials should necessarily increase


avoidance of snakes or spiders. Indeed, the

opposite outcome is entirely possible considering


that subjects were led to believe that unconscious

responses to subliminal phobic stimuli would be

followed by painful shocks, whereas the


occurrence of the assumed stimuli did not result in

aversive experiences on 84 of the trials! It remains

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1413
an open question whether the authors’ procedure

is more likely to condition anxiety relief, rather

than aversive reactions, to whatever the subjects


were imagining to the blank slides.

Research in which social and cognitive


variables are studied as components of

counterconditioning procedures can provide

valuable information about the degree to which


these different factors, both singly and in
combination with each other, facilitate extinction

outcomes. If it were found that response to

desensitization was partly determined by induced

expectations and other informational inputs, their


mechanisms of action would still remain to be
explained. Since induced negative expectations

tend to decrease behavioral participation (Kelley,

1950), supposedly cognitive variables might affect

conditioning outcomes mainly through peripheral

processes, by reducing attentiveness and by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1414
eliciting only halfhearted adherence to required

procedures.

A study by Valins & Ray (1967), designed to


demonstrate that cognitive labeling of one’s

internal physiological reactions can affect

avoidance behavior, is likewise marred by

methodological deficiencies that yield data easily

subject to misinterpretations. According to the


authors’ reasoning, to the extent that persons can
be led to believe that they are no longer affected

internally by a feared object, they will consider

their fear to be unwarranted and accordingly


reduce their avoidance behavior. To test this
notion, paid volunteers who rated themselves as

fearful of snakes were shown pictures of


increasingly fearsome snakes as well as slides with

the word “shock” followed by shock stimulation.

One group received false heartbeat feedback

suggesting that their heart rates were unaffected

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1415
by snake stimuli but adversely affected by shocks.

A control group was exposed to the identical tape

recording except that subjects were informed that


they were hearing meaningless sounds rather than

their internal reactions amplified. All subjects

were then tested for approach responses toward a

snake.

In order to provide a meaningful test of the


cognitive labeling hypothesis it is essential to
preselect subjects who, in fact, display emotional

arousal and avoidance behavior toward snakes. It

is of little value, for example, to demonstrate that


subjects who do not fear snakes will perform
approach responses after being informed that they

are internally unaffected by pictures of snakes. On


the other hand, if misinforming fearfully avoidant

subjects that they are no longer internally affected

by conditioned aversive stimuli produces

significant reductions in avoidance behavior, then

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1416
cognitive labeling processes might serve as

contributory influences in desensitization

treatments. Because of the absence of objective


evidence concerning subjects’ initial fears of

snakes, this study provides no basis for judging

whether cognitive labeling is an irrelevant, weak,

or strong variable. Since 44 percent of the control

subjects successfully performed the terminal


approach task, it is evident that a sizable
proportion of the sample was completely fearless

to begin with. Other investigators have similarly

found that approximately 40 percent of subjects

who label themselves as fearful of snakes turn out


to be relatively fearless, much to their surprise,

when administered a behavioral test.

Contrary to the authors’ conclusion that

“cognitions about internal reactions are important

modifiers of behavior,” the heart-rate feedback

and the control group did not differ significantly in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1417
approach behavior. When subjects who reported

that they had previously touched a snake were

excluded from the sample, a difference emerged;


but the effect cannot necessarily be attributed to

cognitive labeling, because the independent

variable in the reconstituted groups includes both

labeling and self-selection influences. A number of

unknown factors associated with accuracy of past


recall may enter in as plausible determinants.
Results of a second experiment also provide

limited evidence, because subjects’ initial

fearfulness was never objectively assessed, and

treatment effects were not evaluated by the


amount of snake approach behavior but by the

amount of money required to induce subjects


merely to touch a snake, a relatively weak

behavioral requirement.

Unlike desensitization, which eliminates fear

arousal through nonreinforced reexposures to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1418
subjective threats, the cognitive approach

discussed above attempts to produce behavioral

changes by mislabeling existing emotional


responsiveness. Any fear reduction resulting from

deceptive feedback is apt to be short-lived unless

the mislabeling occasions genuine changes in

persons’ anticipatory arousal reactions. If our

assumption is correct that conditioned stimuli


generate emotional effects partly through an
intervening self-arousal mechanism, then persons

who are led to believe that they are no longer

frightened by threatening events may

subsequently reduce fear-arousing cognitions in


response to these situations and thus diminish

emotional responsiveness. A test of the self-


arousal theory would require measurement of

physiological and self-evaluative responses to

conditioned aversive stimuli prior to, and after,


cognitive mislabeling of internal states.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1419
It should be remembered that cognitive claims

have been mined many times with disappointing

therapeutic yields. In the case of persons who


display relatively weak inhibitions, erroneous

explanations for physiological arousal to fear-


provoking situations may lower their fear to the
point where they can perform desired behavior. It

is doubtful, however, that strong fears and

inhibitions can be eliminated through either


mislabeling internal reactions or attributing them

to erroneous sources. A severe acrophobic, for

example, may be temporarily misled into believing


that his fear is no longer physiologically justified,

but he is likely to encounter unnerving internal


feedback when confronted with actual dreaded

heights. There is little reason to expect that

auspicious cognitions induced through deceptive


labeling can substitute for corrective learning

experiences in the stable modification of human

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1420
behavior.

Leitenberg, Agras, Barlow, & Oliveau (1969)


provide evidence that therapeutically oriented

instructions and social reinforcement may

enhance the favorable response changes


accompanying desensitization treatment. Snake-

phobic students who were administered this

procedure in the guise of an experiment on


visualization achieved some reduction in
avoidance behavior. The behavioral changes,

however, were much greater for subjects who had

been told that they were receiving a form of

therapy that is successful in reducing fears and


were praised for completion of items in the
hierarchy. These social variables would not

account for the success of self-administered

desensitization (Melamed & Lang, 1967; Krapfl,

1967) unless, as the authors suggest, self-observed

signs of progress assume a similar reinforcing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1421
function.

TRANSFER OF EXTINCTION EFFECTS

In the standard desensitization procedure

emotional responses are extinguished to symbolic

representations of fear-provoking situations. The

treatment is not only directed toward attenuated


forms of actual threats, but a relatively limited set

of aversive stimuli is usually neutralized. Thus, for


example, a person with a widespread social phobia

may be desensitized to a dozen or so imagined

situations which cannot possibly encompass the


wide variety of interpersonal circumstances that
provoke anxiety. Under these conditions

extinction effects must generalize from thinking to

acting and to situations that may contain some

aversive elements which were never neutralized.

Results of laboratory studies reviewed in

preceding sections amply demonstrate that the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1422
effects of symbolic desensitization do exert a
significant influence on behavioral functioning.

This is shown not only in improved performance


but also in the fact that the number of hierarchical

items successfully neutralized is positively

correlated with degree of reduction in phobic

behavior (Davison, 1968; Lang, Lazovik, &


Reynolds, 1965). However, the extent of

behavioral transfer is somewhat less than is

frequently claimed on the basis of clinical

observations.

Agras (1967) compared progress in

desensitization and reduction in GSR responses to

imagined test scenes with reports of performance


in the actual feared situation by a small group of

severely agoraphobic clients. Parallel changes


were obtained on all measures, but improved

performance in the real-life situations lagged


behind extinction of anxiety to symbolic stimuli.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1423
However, no consistent relationship was found

between extinction of GSR responses to test items

and reported behavioral change. Hoenig & Reed


(1966) likewise found only partial

correspondence between degree of reduction in

phobic behavior and extinction of GSR responding

when the phobic cues were represented by word

labels, by imagination, and by the actual stimulus


objects. These results are somewhat at variance
with those reported by Rachman (1966), who

tested subjects’ self-reported anxiety to actual

phobic stimuli immediately after they had been

desensitized to imaginal representations of


identical situations. Immediate generalization of

anxiety reduction, as measured by fear reports,


was found in 82 percent of the tests.

Unfortunately, neither of the preceding studies

involves a systematic test of avoidance behavior.


Considering that avoidance behavior appears to be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1424
influenced more by central than by autonomic

mediators, reliance upon GSR responsiveness as

the sole indicant of emotional arousal leaves much


to be desired.

The apparent discrepancies in findings


probably depend, in part, on differences in

severity of phobic disorders, on whether one

measures transfer in terms of improved


performance or decreases in subjective distress,
and on variations in the test procedures

themselves. In an experimental design in which

the same subjects are repeatedly tested with real

stimuli following neutralization of imagined


counterparts, any observed changes reflect the
combined effects of performance extinction and

symbolic desensitization. A precise appraisal of

generalization unconfounded by test-produced

changes would require desensitizing different

groups of subjects to different levels in the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1425
hierarchy of anxiety-provoking stimuli and then

measuring their avoidance behavior in the actual

feared situation that corresponds to their highest


neutralized item. It would also be of interest to

investigate generalization systematically as a

function of degree of similarity between the

imagined stimuli that are desensitized and those

encountered in the real-life situation.

From knowledge of stimulus generalization


one would not expect symbolic desensitization

alone to exert vast transfer effects on instrumental

responding. The degree of generalization will


depend upon, among other factors such as
similarity of mediators, the number of stimulus

elements that different situations have in common.


The traditional desensitization procedure involves

too limited a sample of aversive stimulus

elements, and the threats are neutralized in too

attenuated a form to produce complete extinction

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1426
of emotional responses to threatening events

encountered in actual life circumstances. The

more that imagined treatment stimuli differ from


their actual counterparts, the greater the transfer

decrements. In accord with theoretical

expectation, the overall evidence of laboratory

studies (Agras, 1967; Bandura, Blanchard, &

Ritter, 1969; Davison, 1968) shows that symbolic


desensitization significantly reduces both
subjective distress and avoidance behavior, but

the number of approach responses that subjects

can perform behaviorally is generally less than the

number that have been successfully desensitized


in imagination. Moreover, new approach

responses are usually accompanied by relatively


high anxiety when first performed.

It has sometimes been erroneously concluded,

because of evidence that persons experience

anxiety while performing responses which have

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1427
ceased to be threatening in symbolic form, that

desensitization procedures do not achieve

behavioral changes through extinction of the


arousal potential of aversive stimuli. Since some

transfer loss is operative in symbolic

desensitization, this procedure is more likely to

produce anxiety decrements rather than complete

anxiety extinction. It has been repeatedly shown in


laboratory studies that control subjects manifest
undiminished anxiety when they perform their

pretest approach responses a second time,

whereas the same responses have lost much of

their anxiety-provoking capacity for matched


subjects after they have undergone desensitization

treatment. To the extent that emotional arousal is


reduced below the threshold which would activate

avoidance responses, people will be able to engage

in approach behavior, although with some residual


anxiety.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1428
In clinical practice symbolic desensitization is

typically supplemented, either deliberately or

unwittingly, with other procedures that tend to


facilitate transfer effects. Symbolic desensitization

is most often combined with performance


extinction in which clients are urged to perform
formerly inhibited behavior in carefully selected

naturalistic situations as their fears extinguish to

equivalent imagined threats. Even though change


agents may not prescribe appropriate

performance tasks, most people nevertheless

eventually engage in approach behavior as their


avoidant tendencies gradually weaken through

treatment.

The desire to please the change agent and

others may induce individuals to venture fear-

provoking behavior. Positive social reinforcement

and other rewarding outcomes accruing from

successful performance of previously inhibited

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1429
activities may further extinguish any residual

anxieties. In some cases desensitization is also

supplemented by modeling procedures which, in


themselves, can produce substantial vicarious

extinction of emotional arousal. Thus, for example,

in the frequently quoted case by Jones (1924),

extinction of the boy’s animal phobia was achieved

not only by feeding him his favorite food in the


presence of gradually increasing anxiety-arousing
stimuli, but also by having him observe the

positive response of other children as they played

with the feared animal. Abrupt increases in

approach behavior were associated with each of


several modeling experiences.

In laboratory investigations, of course, these


various “extraneous” influences are intentionally

excluded. Because clinical outcomes are usually

obtained by diverse combinations of methods,

results are difficult to evaluate and to compare

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1430
with changes produced by single procedures

under laboratory conditions. Nevertheless,

findings bearing on transfer effects question the


wisdom of relying solely upon symbolic

desensitization for eliminating behavioral

inhibitions and conditioned anxiety. When such

methods are employed for clinical purposes they

should be supplemented with graduated


performance tasks, positive reinforcement of
approach behavior to overcome initial reluctance

of phobic persons to re-expose themselves to

feared situations, and modeling procedures to

further augment change in behavior. The use of


supplementary procedures to obtain more

consistent extinction outcomes will be discussed


more fully later.

COMPARATIVE EFFICACY OF
COUNTERCONDITIONING PROCEDURES

A number of experiments have been

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1431
specifically designed to compare outcomes of
desensitization with those obtained by other

methods of change. Paul (1966) reports a


methodologically sophisticated study in which he

compared, with appropriate controls, the relative

success of desensitization and interview

approaches for modifying debilitating


performance anxiety in college students who

experienced high distress in public speaking

situations. The students were initially

administered a series of personality

questionnaires measuring both generalized


emotional responsiveness and apprehension

about speaking before an audience. Those who


received high scores on the pretest measures

participated in a relatively stressful situational


test in which they were asked to deliver an

impromptu speech before an unfamiliar audience


including several clinical psychologists who, the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1432
subjects were informed, would be evaluating their

performance. Immediately prior to the test

speeches the students’ pulse rates and palmar


sweating were measured; in addition, their actual

speech performance was rated in terms of


customary behavioral indicants of anxiety. On the
basis of these scores, students were randomly

assigned from stratified blocks to different

treatment conditions and control groups.

One group of students received insight-

oriented psychotherapy in which self-

understanding and insight into the psychological

determinants of their speech problems were


pursued through conventional interpretive
techniques. In order to assess the degree of change

resulting from effects of social interaction and

expectation of beneficial outcomes, a second group

of students was assigned to an “attention-placebo”

condition. During each session the latter subjects

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1433
were administered a placebo with strong

suggestions that the drug which they had received

effectively reduces the occurrence of anxiety in


stressful situations. Following the administration

of placebos subjects performed a supposedly

stressful task that, in fact, produced feelings of

drowsiness. For students in the

counterconditioning group, relaxation was


progressively associated with public speaking
items on a temporal anxiety hierarchy, graded

from reading about a speech two weeks before

presentation to delivering a speech before a large

audience. Students in a no-treatment control


group merely participated in all of the assessment

procedures.

The therapy was limited to five sessions

distributed over a period of six weeks. Five

practicing clinicians, who had considerable

experience in the use of insight-oriented

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1434
treatment approaches, administered each of the

three therapeutic procedures; this controlled for

possible variability stemming from differences in


therapists’ characteristics. Following completion

of the treatment series, the subjects’ degree of

physiological arousal, behavior dysfunctioning,

and self-reported distress were measured in the

threatening speech test situation; approximately


six weeks later the original set of personality
questionnaires was also readministered.

The percentage of students in each group who

exhibited decreases in emotional behavior of a


specified magnitude, as objectively assessed in the
standardized test situation, is summarized

graphically in Figure 7-5. Statistical analyses of a


variety of measures, including magnitude of

change as well as the percentage of cases

displaying decrements in emotionality, reveal that

subjects in all three treatment conditions showed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1435
1436
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 7-5. Percent of subjects in each of the four conditions who displayed decreases in anxiety as measured by
behavior ratings, self-reports of emotional disturbance, and measures of physiological arousal. Drawn
from data of Paul, 1966.
significantly less overt behavior indicative of

anxiety, and reported less distress in the

situational test as compared to the no-treatment


control group. However, only the subjects

receiving the counterconditioning treatment

achieved a significant reduction in physiological

arousal relative to the controls. Additionally, the

counterconditioning group proved consistently


superior on all measures to subjects in the insight
and the attention-placebo conditions, which did

not differ significantly from each other. Follow-up

data similarly disclosed that the students treated

by means of counterconditioning reported


experiencing less anxiety related to giving

speeches than did students in either the other


treatment or the control groups.

It is also of interest that the therapists, who in

their regular clinical practice favored insight-

oriented methods, not only rated subjects treated

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1437
by the desensitization procedure as having

improved to a greater degree, but indicated a

significantly better prognosis for them as well.


Their prognostications were borne out by an

additional follow-up assessment (Paul, 1967), in

which all subjects were readministered the

personality tests approximately two years after

the formal experiment was completed.


Desensitization yielded the highest percentage of
subjects (85 percent) who showed decrements

(from pre-therapy ratings) in speech anxiety two

years later, followed by insight (50 percent),

placebo (50 percent), and non-treated controls


(22 percent).The corresponding percentages of

improvement on generalization measures of


interpersonal anxiety were 36, 25, 25, and 18

percent for the four groups, respectively. Not only

did desensitized cases maintain their gains over


time, but none showed increases in performance

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1438
anxiety or any evidence of substitute forms of

deviant behavior. These follow-up results,

although most impressive, should be accepted


with caution considering that they are based

entirely upon self-report measures.

The foregoing project was later extended (Paul

& Shannon, 1966) by administering a group

desensitization treatment to students, selected


from a waiting list, who, as controls of the earlier
study, had shown no reduction in anxiety during

the waiting interval. Students in this condition

participated in nine sessions during which their

emotional responses were neutralized to a


common hierarchy of public speaking situations.
An additional control group, matched for sex, age,

class, and equated on personality test scores, was

also included. In order to determine whether

elimination of speech fright improves academic

functioning, students’ grade point averages for the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1439
semester prior to, and following, treatment were

obtained.

Group desensitization produced significant


reductions in self-reported anxiety with respect to

speaking and other interpersonal situations, and

increases in extroversion. Additional comparisons

involving these same measures show group

desensitization to be equally effective as


individual desensitization, but superior to the
insight and placebo treatments. Moreover,

students treated with group desensitization

showed a modest gain, whereas nontreated


controls suffered a substantial loss, in grade point
average. The latter finding is somewhat surprising

because one would not expect most academic


grades to be determined to any appreciable degree

by amount of public participation. The extensive

generalization of favorable changes revealed also

in the personality tests— assuming that they are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1440
manifested in actual social behavior as well—may

occur because verbal communication figures

prominently in virtually all social and intellectual


activities. The noted improvements were

successfully maintained as revealed by a two-year

follow-up study (Paul, 1968a). Group differences

in academic performance provide even more

impressive testimony for the lasting benefits


accompanying desensitization treatment. Two
years after the project was completed, 90 percent

of the students who received group

desensitization had either graduated or were

completing their studies in good standing,


whereas 60 percent of the nontreated controls had

dropped out of school. The grade point averages


for students in the group desensitization and

control conditions in the follow-up semester were

3.5 and 2.4, respectively.

The beneficial effects of desensitization on

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1441
academic performance are further corroborated

by Mann & Rosenthal (1969) with elementary

school children. Compared to nontreated controls,


children who suffered from examination anxiety

showed significant changes in test anxiety and

reading achievement scores after receiving either

individual or group desensitization. Interestingly,

participant observers benefited to the same


degree as did direct recipients of the treatment
procedures.

Another comparative test of the efficacy of

counterconditioning methods for modifying


diverse forms of phobic disorders is provided by
Lazarus (1961), who employed an experimental

design in which the behavioral outcomes of group


desensitization were compared with those of

conventional group psychotherapy. The

experiment included acrophobics,

claustrophobics, cases of impotence, and clients

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1442
who each exhibited a different type of phobic

reaction. The participants were matched in pairs

on the basis of age, sex, and severity of the phobic


behavior, and were randomly assigned to

desensitization and interpretive treatment

conditions. In addition, a third group of phobic

clients, who received interpretive treatment plus

relaxation at the conclusion of each session, was


subsequently added in order to assess the possible
effects of relaxation per se on avoidance behavior.

The same person served as the therapist for all

three treatment conditions.

Only clients who exhibited severe phobic


behavior, as measured by actual behavioral tests,

were selected for the experiment. In order to


minimize the possible influence of preliminary

relationship experiences, the relevant anxiety

hierarchies were constructed from clients’ written

responses to questionnaire items, rather than

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1443
from personal interviews. The clients who

participated in the group desensitization were

treated in small, separate, homogeneous groups.


For the acrophobic clients, a common stimulus

hierarchy was constructed utilizing primarily a

physical proximity dimension beginning with a

scene in which a subject is looking down from a

height of about 10 feet, and terminating with


highly anxiety-provoking items. The
claustrophobic anxiety hierarchy represented a

stimulus continuum in which the degree of spatial

constriction and ventilation were varied

simultaneously from scenes depicting the client


“sitting in a large and airy room with all the

windows open” to “sitting in front of an open fire


in a small room with the doors and windows shut.”

Finally, the stimulus hierarchy items constructed

for the impotent men described progressively


intimate sexual situations ranging from sitting

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1444
close to a woman to pre-coital love-making in the

nude.

During desensitization sessions, a deep level of

relaxation was induced, following which the

participants were all asked to visualize the scene


which had been presented to them, and to signal

the psychotherapist whenever any stimulus item

proved disturbing. The rate and duration of


presenting the hierarchical items were paced
according to the most anxious group-member.

Clients who participated in the interpretive

treatment condition received a traditional form of

group psychotherapy which highlighted personal


exploration of feelings and interpersonal
relationships, permissiveness toward and

acceptance of emotional expressions, and the

development of insight into the origins and the

factors underlying their phobic disorders. These

clients received a mean of 22 sessions, the same

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1445
number as their matched counterparts in the

desensitization groups.

The therapeutic outcomes for acrophobics and


claustrophobics were objectively assessed by

situational tests administered one month

following the termination of the treatment

sessions. The acrophobics were required to climb

a fire escape to a height of approximately 50 feet,


then to accompany the experimenter in an
elevator to the roof of the building eight stories

above the street, from where for two minutes they

were to count the automobiles passing below. A


similarly rigorous objective criterion was adopted
as evidence that claustrophobic reactions were

successfully extinguished: The subjects were


required to remain in the cubicle with the movable

screen a few inches away without experiencing

any disturbance for a period of five minutes. A

second observer was present during the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1446
situational tests to provide a reliability check on

whether the client had successfully met the

tolerance criteria. For obvious reasons, the


impotent men were not subjected to a situational

test, nor was the behavior of the four clients in the

mixed group objectively measured. The

improvement rates in these cases were based on

reports of significant behavioral change. Although


capacity performance tests are less subject to
extraneous influences than self-report measures,

results of this study would have been more

definitive had the assessments been conducted by

a tester who had no knowledge of the conditions


to which the subjects were assigned rather than by

the therapist himself.

Phobic behavior was completely extinguished

in 13 of the 18 clients who received

desensitization treatment, whereas the

interpretive and interpretive-plus-relaxation

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1447
treatment successfully modified the phobic

responses in only 2 out of 17 cases. Additional

evidence for the effectiveness of


counterconditioning is provided by the finding

that 10 of the 15 clients whose phobias were

essentially unmodified by the interpretive

procedures were successfully treated by group

desensitization within ten sessions. A follow-up


study conducted at varying intervals after the
termination of the treatment program revealed

that 80 percent of cases who were successfully

treated by means of counterconditioning

procedures maintained their behavioral changes,


according to a stringent criterion in which even

the recurrence of weak phobic responses was


rated as a relapse. Gelder & Marks (1968)

similarly found that a group of phobic clients who

had not responded to 18 months of group


psychotherapy showed significant reductions in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1448
phobic behavior after a few months of

desensitization treatment.

Although most of the laboratory studies

discussed in earlier sections were primarily

designed to isolate the contribution of component


variables in the desensitization procedure, their

results nevertheless have some bearing on the

efficacy of this approach. In evaluating outcomes a


distinction should be drawn between behavioral
improvement and complete elimination of

avoidance behavior. The findings generally

disclose that a relatively brief program of

desensitization involving five to ten sessions


produces improved performances in virtually all
participants. In the study by Bandura, Blanchard,

& Ritter (1969), for example, 90 percent of the

subjects who received desensitization treatment

displayed increases in approach behavior that

exceeded the performances of their matched

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1449
nontreated controls. However, depending on the

stringency of the criterion test, only between 30

and 50 percent of the subjects achieve complete


extinction as evidenced by their ability to perform

the terminal approach tasks in behavioral tests

(Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter, 1968; Davison,

1968; Lang, Lazovik, & Reynolds, 1965; Schubot,

1966). It should be noted, of course, that these


improvement rates are based on very brief, time-
limited therapies in which all subjects are tested

after several treatment sessions regardless of the

number of hierarchical items to which they have

been desensitized. The actual therapeutic limits of


this particular form of counterconditioning can be

best established by studies in which behavioral


improvement and complete extinction rates are

objectively measured after subjects have been

thoroughly desensitized to the entire set of


anxiety-arousing stimuli. In evaluating the efficacy

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1450
of treatment procedures the incidence of terminal

performances should be of major concern to

discourage the development of complacency about


methods that consistently achieve performance

gains but leave many of the participants


behaviorally incapacitated to some degree.

There are a number of clinical reports that

present outcome data in the form of therapists’


judgments of their success rates. Wolpe (1958)
and Lazarus (1960, 1963b) state that between 75

and 90 percent of the clients whom they have

treated were “markedly improved or completely

recovered.” Hain, Butcher, & Stevenson (1966)


report that desensitization was effective in 78
percent of the cases and that improvements often

occurred in areas of occupational, sexual, and

social functioning beyond the specifically treated

phobias. Follow-up studies, conducted at intervals

ranging from six months to several years after the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1451
termination of treatment, with few exceptions

disclose that clients not only maintain their gains,

but many display additional positive changes in


behavior. In another paper Wolpe (1964)

reaffirms the efficacy of desensitization therapy

for modifying complex behavior disorders.

Somewhat less favorable outcomes than those

given above have been published by Cooper


(1963) and Marks & Gelder (1965) in
retrospective comparisons of clients treated by

“behavior therapy” and “psychotherapy” made in

terms of general judgments of improvement rates


from case notes. In a spirited rejoinder Wolpe &
Lazarus (1966) discount the discrepant results as

due to “the fledgling efforts of novices who have


learned the rudiments of systematic

desensitization [p. 159].” Conflicting data of this

sort are not at all surprising as long as they are not

erroneously considered as measures of behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1452
outcome but are understood instead as differences

between therapists’ judgmental responses (which

rarely correlate perfectly with clients’ actual


behavior functioning). Indeed, one would expect

diminishing correspondence between actual

behavior and subjective ratings as one moves from

objective measures of clients’ behavior to their

own self-assessments, from clients’ verbal reports


of performance changes to therapists’ judgments
of improvement, from therapists’ inferences based

on clients’ self-reports to information that

happens to get recorded in case notes, and from

case notes of undetermined reliability to


retrospective global ratings made by still another

set of judges who never had any contact with the


client. Major differences in the types of case notes

kept by therapists of behavioral and

psychodynamic persuasions further preclude any


meaningful comparison between success rates.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1453
Much progress can be made in reducing fruitless

controversies about the relative superiority of

rival methods by abandoning the outcome


numbers game in which therapists’ judgments of

their clients’ verbal reports of their behavioral


changes are evaluated against a legendary
baseline figure of two-thirds improvement.

These types of quasi-outcome data have, at


best, only suggestive value. Apart from the
subjective and general nature of clinical ratings,

the therapeutic interventions are exceedingly

varied, including assertion training, graded

reexposure to feared situations, anxiety-relief


procedures, aversive counterconditioning, role
playing, symbolic desensitization, verbal

prompting of desired response patterns, social

reinforcement of behavioral changes, and a host of

unrecognized treatment factors a: well as a variety

of unmeasured environmental influences. It is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1454
consequently impossible to identify which

treatment variables are responsible for observed

changes, even if the outcome figures were valid


and the behavioral modifications were

attributable to the therapeutic interventions. Of

much greater concern, however, is the fact that

subjective evaluations of treatment outcomes lend

themselves readily to unwarranted claims of


efficacy by their proponents and to premature
rejection of potentially promising approaches by

their theoretical rivals.

Proof of the efficacy of procedures of treatment


must rest on objective assessment of behavioral
changes and evidence of lawful covariation

between specific learning variables and


designated outcomes, rather than on general

judgments of improvement in “neurotic illness,”

achieved through the use of varied combinations

of treatment procedures. Laboratory experiments

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1455
and controlled individual studies involving well-

defined manipulations of treatment variables and

objective measurements of behavioral changes


will eventually yield reliable information

regarding the critical parameters in

counterconditioning methods. Research of this

kind should not only greatly increase

understanding of conditioning processes, but it


also provides the basis for refinements in
treatment procedures. The laboratory studies

reviewed earlier represent an encouraging

advance in this direction.

Innumerable single case reports are of interest


not because they validate anything but because

they illustrate how counterconditioning principles


can be applied to exceedingly diverse anxiety

disorders. Most of these studies include detailed

reports, often independently verified, of the

modifications achieved in clients’ behavioral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1456
functioning. However, in treatments involving the

combined use of different procedures,

desensitization is obviously not the sole


determinant of observed changes. It is also

important to bear in mind that case reports may

convey an overly favorable impression of the

efficacy of a given treatment method because of

selective publication of successful cases.

APPLICABILITY OF DESENSITIZATION TREATMENTS

Desensitization procedures have been utilized

to extinguish countless circumscribed, but


partially incapacitating, phobias including
avoidance of places and activities that might result

in contact with feared animals, birds, reptiles, and

insects (Clark, 1963; Cooke, 1966; Friedman,

1966; Ramsay, Barends, Brenker, & Kruseman,

1966); fearful avoidance of automobiles, airplanes,

and other types of transports (Kraft & Al-Issa,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1457
1965a; Lazarus, 1960; Rosenthal, 1967; Wolpe,
1962); fear of water (Bentler, 1962); storms

(Costello, 1963); aversion to heat that prevented


the person from washing in warm water and from

drinking or eating hot foods (Kraft & Al-Issa,

1965b); and dread of atomic disasters that

resulted in avoidance of radios, television, movies,


newspapers, conversations, and other forms of

communication that might bring news of

international disharmony (Ashem, 1963); school

phobias (Chapel, 1967); anxiety reactions to

hypodermic injections and the use of sanitary pads


(Rachman, 1959), and to hospitals and

ambulances (Lazarus & Rachman, 1957);


hyperesthesia of taste and touch (Beyme, 1964);

autonomic disorders (Cohen & Reed, 1968); and


persistent apprehensions about illness, physical

injury, and death (Rifkin, 1968; Wolpe, 1961).

In addition to isolated phobias, desensitization

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1458
methods have been applied to the modification of

pervasive behavioral dysfunctions resulting from

incapacitating obsessions and compulsions


(Haslam, 1965; Walton & Mather, 1963b), from

claustrophobias and agoraphobias, from

articulatory disorders (Gray, England, & Mohoney,

1965; Lazarus & Rachman, 1957; Rosenthal, 1968;

Walton & Mather, 1963a), from recurrent


nightmares (Geer & Silverman, 1967) and
insomnia (Geer & Katkin, 1966), and from chronic

alcoholism (Kraft & Al-Issa, 1967a). Finally,

complex interpersonal problems have been

eliminated by extinguishing clients’ anxieties


associated with sexual intimacy, aggressive and

hostile behavior, close social relationships, social


disapproval and rejection, failure to meet external

or self-imposed achievement demands, and fear of

persons in positions of authority (Hain, Butcher, &


Stevenson, 1966; Kraft & Al-Issa, 1967b; Madsen &

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1459
Ullmann, 1967; Wolpe, 1958; Wolpe & Lazarus,

1966).

It is commonly assumed, especially in

psychiatric theorizing, that grossly deviant

behavior is primarily a function of biochemical


determinants, whereas deviations of lesser

magnitude are governed by experiential factors.

Granted that physiological variables can


contribute significantly to behavioral variability,
such evidence does not justify a dichotomous

theory of psychopathology, particularly in view of

the absence of any objective criteria as to where

the line of demarcation should be drawn between


so-called “neurotic” and “psychotic” response
patterns. In keeping with the dichotomous thesis,

Wolpe (1958) questions whether schizophrenics

can profit from desensitization therapy. This

position implies, among other things, that persons

who are considered to be psychotic are incapable

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1460
of classical conditioning. Contrary to this view,

laboratory studies provide evidence that

schizophrenics not only exhibit emotional


conditionability (O’Connor & Rawnsley, 1959;

Vinogradov, 1962), but apparently they differ

little, if at all, in this respect from groups judged to

be normal (Howe, 1958; Spence & Taylor, 1953).

Favorable outcomes have been achieved in the few


instances where counterconditioning procedures
were applied to emotional behavior exhibited by

individuals diagnosed as schizophrenic (Cowden &

Ford, 1962; Zeisset, 1968).

The ease with which symbolic desensitization


is achieved will depend in part on secondary

factors such as clients’ attentiveness,


cooperativeness in visualizing verbally presented

scenes, and facility in inducing anxiety-inhibiting

responses, in addition to variations in their

conditionability. Cowden & Ford (1962), for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1461
example, encountered considerable difficulty in

persuading their clients to carry out relaxation

exercises. It is possible, therefore, that grossly


deviant personalities who present severe

cognitive deficits, inadequate attending behavior,

and irrelevant associations to verbal stimuli,

would prove less responsive to exclusively

symbolic forms of counterconditioning therapy.


Instances in which these types of limiting factors
are operative may require, at least in early stages

of treatment, greater reliance upon graduated

reexposure to actual threats along with stress-

reducing stimuli that can be externally controlled.

The mere presence of anxiety and avoidance

behavior does not necessarily mean that


conditioned emotionality is the central problem. A

person who lacks requisite behaviors for coping

effectively with the social, intellectual, and

vocational demands of his environment will be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1462
repeatedly subjected to punishing experiences.

Under these circumstances, feared situations are,

in fact, aversive and hence the emotional arousal is


not unrealistic. In cases of this type behavioral

deficits constitute the major problem, whereas the

emotional component is a secondary consequent.

A treatment devoted solely to extinguishing

emotional responses would be at best a


temporarily effective means of producing a
relaxed incompetent. On the other hand, a

treatment that established behavioral

competencies would substantially decrease the

punitiveness of the client’s social environment and


thus achieve stable reductions in fearfulness.

A response induction program may be


necessary even when anxiety disorders do not

originate in behavioral deficits. Persons who suffer

from unrealistic or disproportionate fears tend to

avoid engaging in fear-provoking activities. This

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1463
often results in a spiraling process where fear and

avoidance prevent further elaboration of

interpersonal skills, and marginal competencies, in


turn, render threatening situations even more

anxiety-arousing. In most cases, therefore, a

combined treatment aimed at extinguishing

unwarranted fears and at instilling capabilities

would yield best results.

Since removal of unwarranted avoidance


behavior enables people to participate in

potentially rewarding activities, approach

responses, once they have been restored, are likely


to be effectively maintained by their favorable
consequences. However, the initial behavioral

changes are sometimes impeded by the existing


advantages derived from disabling phobias. As a

result of their phobic conditions, people may gain

exemptions from certain responsibilities, they may

reduce unpleasant vocational and familial

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1464
demands, and they may achieve considerable

control over the behavior of others. Where such

hindering contingencies exist, unless the rewards


that contribute to the maintenance of avoidance

behavior are withdrawn, desensitization alone is

unlikely to bring about much behavioral

improvement.

Identification of the Stimulus Determinants


of Emotional Behavior

Procedures that effect behavioral changes

primarily through response consequences

ordinarily do not present any major diagnostic

problems. An agent of change who has at his


disposal an effective means for evoking the
desired behavior and sufficiently powerful

incentives can, given adequate control over the


environment, achieve substantial behavioral

modifications by overriding undetermined

maintaining conditions. In contrast, stimulus-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1465
oriented treatments, especially in their present

stage of development, require considerable

diagnostic ingenuity. In order to achieve any


measure of success with classical extinction

procedures the primary stimulus determinants of

emotional behavior must be accurately identified

and neutralized. In current practice the selection

of anxiety sources is based upon informally


collected data, from interviews, case histories, and
various personality tests, most of which were

originally constructed for entirely different

purposes. Although no reliability studies have

been conducted in which different therapists


select from the same protocols what they consider

to be the critical sources of anxiety, it would come


as no surprise to find low consensus, particularly

in cases involving multiform problems.

If the efficacy of counterconditioning methods

is to be maximized, the present informal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1466
assessment approaches must be replaced by more

efficient and reliable procedures. In many cases, of

course, the eliciting stimuli are readily identifiable.


In others, however, the occurrence of emotional

responses is dependent upon stimuli that lack

distinctiveness, or upon particular patterns and

sequences of events which are difficult to discern.

However, individuals are rarely continuously


anxious; rather, they tend to display such
reactions only at certain times and under

particular circumstances. Covariations between

stimulus events and emotional responding can,

therefore, be best identified through careful


analysis of regular variations in the onset and

magnitude of emotional behavior. After the


pattern in anxiety responses has been determined,

one can isolate common features in situations in

which the behavior typically occurs. The major


controlling stimuli cannot always be identified

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1467
solely through systematic examination of the

objective characteristics of environmental events

which may, in fact, be highly dissimilar. Rather, the


common determinants are often revealed in

detailed accounts of the thoughts and subjective


reactions that clients experienced in anxiety-
producing situations.

New assessment approaches are particularly


needed for isolating stimulus determinants when
only gross covariant relationships are noted.

Information of this kind can be obtained by

systematic behavioral measurement of emotional

reactions, both in the presence and in the absence


of specific contextual and social cues which appear
to be regularly correlated with variations in the

observed anxiety responses. Because of the

countless and complex varieties of learning

histories represented by clinic populations, a

highly flexible stimulus exploration procedure is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1468
required. When utilization of physical events is too

cumbersome and impractical, suspected fear-

provoking situations can be easily presented in


verbal or pictorial forms. Emotional responses to

these potential threats might be measured

behaviorally, physiologically, through verbal

reports, or a combination of these methods.

Before turning to other issues of stimulus


specification, it is necessary to clarify several
common misconceptions about the range of

applicability of counterconditioning procedures.

In most polemical discussions of psychotherapy,


behavioral and psychodynamic approaches are
usually presented as rival methods of treatment

suitable for different types of anxiety conditions.


Advocates of psychodynamic methods typically

assume that desensitization is essentially limited

to simple “monosymptomatic” disorders under the

control of clear-cut stimuli, whereas

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1469
psychodynamic procedures are suitable to more

complex and pervasive anxiety problems.

Behaviorally oriented therapists, on the other


hand, contend that desensitization procedures are

applicable to any dysfunction involving anxiety,

and view psychodynamic approaches as having

proved ineffective in modifying either simple or

complex conditions.

Greatest progress would be achieved in


developing efficacious treatment approaches if

these ill-defined partisan labels were retired from

further use. Much time has been spent fruitlessly


in attempts to define what constitutes “behavior
therapy” and “psychotherapy.” A more productive

and less confusing approach to the understanding


of social influence processes is to focus on the

basic mechanisms through which behavioral

changes are produced. These mechanisms are

undoubtedly brought into play to varying degrees

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1470
by conditions created either deliberately or

unwittingly by change agents in influence

attempts arbitrarily designated behavior therapy,


psychotherapy, counseling, reeducation, or some

other appellation. In each of the foregoing

enterprises change agents model certain attitudes

and response patterns. One might, therefore,

analyze these various activities in terms of the


behavior the change agents are modeling, its
functional value for the recipients, and the extent

to which conditions that facilitate modeling are

present. Similarly, one might examine

reinforcement influences, which are operative in


all social situations, to determine what behavior is

being reinforced, with what frequency, and by


what means in different systems designed to

modify psychological functioning.

Because of the ambiguities and erroneous

impressions associated with the terms “behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1471
therapy” and “psychotherapy,” their continued use

beclouds most of the fundamental issues in this

field. For example, the rate at which a given


behavior can be extinguished depends upon the

nature of its maintaining conditions rather than

upon whether it is “monosymptomatic” or part of

a multiform problem. In fact, many so-called

simple disorders, such as tics, that are


immediately self-reinforcing because their
occurrence is tension-reducing, are unusually

resistant to change. Therefore, some apparently

simple behaviors fail to respond well to treatment,

whereas many complex disorders are readily


modifiable (Lazarus, 1963a; Meyer & Crisp, 1966).

Moreover, whether emotional responses are


limited or diffuse is often determined by the

pervasiveness of emotion-arousing cues rather

than by conditions existing within the individual.


Those for whom snakes are threatening will

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1472
exhibit a highly circumscribed phobic disorder

because reptiles are rarely or never encountered

in urban settings. By contrast, when anxiety has


been conditioned to stimuli that appear in a

variety of frequently encountered situations,


persons will experience pervasive or “free-
floating” anxiety (Wolpe, 1958). Similarly, if our

unperturbed snake-phobics were to become

residents in a reptile-infested locale, they too


would exhibit diffuse and disabling anxiety

disorders.

Discussions of desensitization often create the

impression that this procedure is principally


applicable to stimulus events that are easily
specifiable and nonsocial in character; whereas

anxieties arising from aggression, dependency,

sex, and other interpersonal sources are made to

seem the exclusive domain of psychodynamic

approaches. In actuality, desensitization methods

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1473
are not restricted, either on theoretical or practical

grounds, to any particular set of emotion-arousing

stimuli. Indeed, a counterconditioning form of


treatment could be employed to neutralize the

negative valence of Oedipal fantasies in clients for

whom this might constitute a problem.

It would perhaps be more accurate to say that

the applicability of desensitization treatment is


mainly limited by therapists’ ingenuity in
identifying sources of anxiety, particularly when

the crucial stimulus determinates are obscure.

This task is complicated by the absence of any


objective criteria for determining the appropriate
events for treatment. Let us consider, for example,

a female agoraphobic who is unable to venture


outside the household. Should one desensitize her

to progressively farther anxiety-arousing

excursions from the home? One might argue that

her phobic behavior arises from a morbid fear of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1474
sexual encounters, apprehensions about

abandonment and helpless exposure to crowds, or

some other sources, and it is these contents that


must be emphasized in the treatment. To take

another example, should a snake-phobic be

desensitized to progressively closer interactions

with reptiles or to genital concerns on the

assumption that “the sight of snakes provokes


penis emotions [Fenichel, 1945, p. 48]”?
Laboratory studies of desensitization furnish some

evidence that claustrophobia can be successfully

eliminated by neutralizing individuals’ emotions

to cues of increasing space constriction (Lazarus,


1961) without focusing on their fears of being “left

alone with dangerous impulses and fantasies


[Cameron, 1963, p. 286]”; acrophobics have lost

their fear of heights through utilizing elevation

hierarchies (Lazarus, 1961) rather than fears of


“falling in self-esteem” or “self-destructive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1475
impulses [Cameron, 1963, p. 280]”; and countless

snake-phobics have been cured by being

desensitized to reptile rather than phallic stimuli.


In view of these interesting findings, it would be

most instructive to compare the degree to which


phobic behavior is extinguished when
desensitization is directed toward either the

phobic stimuli themselves or the hypothesized

internal threats. A desensitization procedure


combined with objective measurement of changes

in avoidance behavior provides an excellent

means of testing different theories regarding the


stimulus determinants of emotional response

patterns.

As illustrated in the preceding examples,

psychodynamic formulations assume that

anxieties are internally generated by arousal of

unconscious impulses which are then displaced

and projected onto environmental objects.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1476
External phobic cues are therefore regarded as

pseudoevocative stimuli. Contrary to this

interpretation, successful neutralization of


emotional responses to phobic stimuli not only

produces stable decreases in avoidance behavior

without the emergence of new deviant responses,

but it is often accompanied by reductions in

anxiety in other areas of functioning (Bandura,


Blanchard, & Ritter, 1969; Lang, Lazovik, &
Reynolds, 1965; Paul, 1967). The latter findings

make the view that avoidance behavior is

controlled by displaced and projected stimulus

valences appear of questionable validity, or


alternatively, suggest that neutralization of

external projective stimuli is one of the most


powerful means currently available for

extinguishing the arousal properties of

unconscious internal events.

The above discussion is not meant to imply

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1477
that stimulus determinants are always inferable

from the content of deviant responses. In many

cases emotional behavior is under multiple


stimulus control in which some of the evocative

cues, because of peculiar conditioning histories,

may be thematically remote. Also, complex social

behaviors characteristically depend upon

interrelated activities, each governed by


somewhat different stimuli. A given performance
may consequently be inhibited or disrupted by

anxiety arising from thematically different

component functions that are not readily evident.

The operation of these more intricate stimulus


determinants is best illustrated by applications of

desensitization procedures to the modification of


diverse sexual disorders.

According to Bond & Hutchison (1960) the

most frequent classes of exposure-eliciting stimuli

for sexual exhibitionists are stress experiences

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1478
provoking inadequacy and females who possess

certain physical characteristics that have been

endowed by the exhibitionist with unusually high


sexual valence. The authors therefore employed

both sexual and devaluation hierarchies in

treating a 25-year-old male who presented a long

history of persistent genital exposure leading to

24 charges of indecent exposure including eleven


prison convictions. The client had undergone a
variety of treatments without benefit, including

individual and group therapy, carbon dioxide

abreaction therapy, moralistic exhortations under

hypnosis; finally, in desperation, he resorted to a


specially designed chastity belt that his wife

locked in the morning and unlocked at night. Even


these physical restraints failed to control the

client’s behavior as he was once again arrested for

indecent assault as he attempted to grasp the legs


and breasts of a young woman while wearing his

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1479
chastity belt.

Three hierarchies of exposure-provoking


stimuli were constructed for the desensitization

treatment. One set of stimuli was graded on the

basis of the age and physical appearance of the


females, ranging from older women who

minimally provoked exposure to young attractive

females. These stimuli were presented in each of


four settings in which genital exposure frequently
occurred (i.e., in department stores, on beaches, on

sidewalks, and in automobiles). In addition, a

separate hierarchy was constructed on the basis of

washroom situations, since they served as the


most potent contextual stimuli for exhibitionism.
The third stimulus dimension contained social

situations giving rise to feelings of inadequacy.

These sets of eliciting stimuli were then

progressively paired with hypnotically induced

relaxation over a period of 30 sessions. The client

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1480
was also instructed to practice relaxation and to

initiate this chain of responses by the word “relax.”

As treatment progressed the client became less


emotionally aroused by provocative females, his

exhibitionistic urges and sexual fantasies

diminished in frequency and intensity, and he

displayed increasing voluntary control over his

exposure behavior on occasions when he


experienced some degree of emotional arousal. As
the client made continued improvement, he was

able to participate in group activities involving

close heterosexual contacts without experiencing


any tension or urges to expose himself. He showed
no exhibitionistic behavior for a period of 13

months following the termination of therapy


(Bond & Hutchison, 1964). Subsequently, the

client exposed himself on a few occasions to

women in washrooms in response to severe

financial and vocational stresses, but provocative

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1481
females in public places such as parks, streets, and

department stores no longer elicited sexual

exhibitionism.

It is of interest that in the above case deviant

sexual behavior was partly controlled by

nonsexual stress events. Wolpe (1958) similarly

reports the successful treatment of a pharmacist

who suffered from impotence by desensitizing him


to essentially nonsexual cues. This client, who had
experienced satisfactory sexual relationships with

several different girls, suddenly became impotent

when he attempted intercourse with a virgin


girlfriend who yielded reluctantly to his insistent
pressures. During this unsuccessful seduction the

client found himself thinking about a disturbing


childhood event in which he overheard his parents

having intercourse; the mother’s protestations and

weeping had apparently succeeded in conditioning

aggressive and brutal meanings to the sexual act.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1482
This conditioning was undoubtedly facilitated by

the marked hypersensitivity to aggressive and

physical injury cues that the boy exhibited long


before witnessing the parental sexual episode. By

the time the client had entered psychotherapy he

was sexually incapacitated, not only by

generalization from the parental situation to the

virginal girlfriend, but also by a secondary


generalization from her to a subsequent girlfriend
who bore a physical resemblance to her. On the

basis of a learning analysis of the impotence, the

anticipated pain and physical injury cues

occurring during defloration were considered to


be the critical events producing the sexual

inhibitions. The client was therefore desensitized


to several stimulus dimensions involving physical

injury, vocalization of pain and suffering, and

violent verbal interchanges resembling the


parents’ endless arguments. Complete sexual

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1483
responsiveness was restored after aggressive and

pain cues lost their capacity to evoke anxiety.

Further suggestive evidence that in some cases

inhibition of heterosexual behavior may be

maintained primarily by anxiety attached to social


and physical contact cues rather than to sexual

stimuli per se is provided by Stevenson & Wolpe

(1960) in the treatment of a pedophile and two


homosexuals. All three clients displayed markedly
passive, submissive, and withdrawn behavior that

apparently stemmed from authoritarian control by

tyrannical parents. As a consequence of this

aversive social training a broad class of


interpersonal responses was inhibited except
toward little girls in one case, and toward

nurturant nonthreatening male companions in the

other two. The treatment strategy in all three

cases consisted essentially of training in socially

assertive behavior. The fact that these clients

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1484
readily adopted exclusively heterosexual patterns

of behavior and experienced them as positively

reinforcing after assertive responses had been


developed suggests that the deviant sexuality was

a function of generalized interpersonal anxieties,

rather than of specific sexual origin.

The covariant relationship between

assertiveness and sexuality noted in the preceding


cases is convincingly demonstrated by Kahn
(1961) in a laboratory experiment with

infrahuman subjects. Two groups of mice were

trained in either socially aggressive or submissive


behavior. Both groups were then tested for sexual
responsiveness toward virgin females known to be

in estrus. The social training in aggression and


submission had a striking differential effect on the

mating responses of the males. Whereas the

aggressively trained animals immediately pursued

the females, copulated with them, and remained

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1485
sexually active throughout the test session, not a

single submissive animal did so. On the infrequent

occasions when the submissives approached their


female companions, they did so very hesitantly

and then quickly reverted to the avoidant

response pattern.

The findings of the latter experiment may be

interpreted in several ways. To the extent that


aggression is an important component of sexual
behavior (Ford & Beach, 1951), then any increase

or inhibition of aggressive responses would be

associated with a corresponding enhancement or


inhibition of sexuality. Since, however, the animals
failed to initiate any sexual responses, including

preparatory ones, any anxiety-evoking stimuli


accompanying either aggressive or sexual

behavior could not have been generated in the test

situation. Therefore, a second and perhaps more

likely explanation of these findings is that the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1486
previously acquired physical contact anxieties

motivated and reinforced generalized avoidance

responses that precluded the occurrence of any


sexual behavior, even if such responses were only

weakly inhibited.

The discussion thus far has illustrated how

anxiety arising from nonsexual sources can

control the inhibition of appropriate heterosexual


behavior. Actually, it is possible to delineate
several different stimulus determinants of sexual

deviance, each requiring a somewhat different

treatment strategy. First, there is the behavioral


syndrome, to which reference has already been
made, wherein social and physical contact arouse

anxiety reactions, but sexual stimuli per se may be


positively valenced. In these cases, a program of

treatment utilizing modeling and reinforcement

procedures designed to foster and disinhibit

interpersonal approach tendencies is likely to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1487
result in a corresponding enhancement of

heterosexual behavior.

A second frequently encountered pattern is


one in which a person has little or no

apprehension about close social interactions, but

sexual cues, particularly those associated with

coital performance, are negatively valenced.

Impotence, frigidity and other specific sexual


inhibitions are common complaints associated
with the latter syndrome. For problems of this

sort, the neutralization of sex-related stimuli,

through some form of desensitization procedure,


would be the method of choice. An example is
provided by Lazarus & Rachman (1957), who

successfully treated a case of impotence of recent


origin by eliminating anxiety to pre-coital scenes.

In instances where the sexual inhibitions are less

strongly established, impotence can be effectively

modified by having the client follow a self-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1488
administered program of desensitization in actual

sexual situations (Wolpe, 1958). The latter

procedure is described more fully in a later section


of this chapter.

Individuals who are more severely

incapacitated because they respond with strong

anxiety to close interpersonal contact as well as

specific cues would benefit most from a treatment


program combining both desensitization of
interpersonal and sexual cues and training in

interpersonal competencies.

Finally, it should be realized that deviant


sexual behavior is sometimes maintained by
substantial positive reinforcement, whereas

anxiety mechanisms, which may have figured

prominently in the genesis of the disorder,

currently play a minor role. Many homosexual

patterns of behavior are, in fact, sustained by the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1489
positive rewards deriving from homosexual
alliances (Henry, 1941; Hooker, 1961). If the

person should desire to change his sexual


orientation, differential reinforcement procedures

designed to reduce the strong positive valence of

homoerotic stimuli and to enhance responsiveness

to heterosexual cues would constitute the


appropriate therapeutic strategy in such cases.

These methods are discussed in some detail in the

next chapter.

The necessity for identifying the controlling

stimuli and determining the functional value of

behavioral dysfunctions before selecting the

method of treatment is well illustrated in a clinical


study reported by Lazarus (1963b). Sixteen

women with chronic frigidity were administered


the standard desensitization procedure on the

assumption that frigidity represents conditioned


avoidance maintained by sexually generated

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1490
anxiety. The desensitization was conducted along

stimulus dimensions of increasing intimacy and

other sexually inhibiting stimuli that were unique


to each case. Significant improvements in sexual

relationships, as reported by the women and

corroborated by their husbands, were achieved in

9 of the 16 women for whom frigidity appeared to

be determined by sexual anxieties. The majority of


the others, most of whom displayed intense and
generalized hostile attitudes toward men,

terminated therapy after several interviews. This

subgroup of women evidently required a

treatment program aimed at reducing hostile


behavior. Where hostility toward men results

from a sense of inadequacy and submission, a


program of assertion training would not only

reduce exploitation, which a submissive person is

likely to incur, but at the same time increase


feelings of self-esteem and self-worth. In addition

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1491
to establishing new response orientations toward

men through modeling and positive

reinforcement, desensitization procedures might


be employed to decrease excessive hostility to

inevitable evocative situations. To achieve greatest


gains it may also be necessary to reduce hostility-
generating behavior on the part of the husband.

ANXIETY RESPONSE DECREMENTS AS A FUNCTION


OF NEUTRALIZING PRIMARY AND
GENERALIZATION STIMULI

The introductory chapter discussed how

emotional responses elicited by a particular

stimulus tend to generalize spontaneously to a


wide variety of cues falling on the same physical or
semantic stimulus dimension. It has also been

demonstrated in numerous laboratory studies

(Bass & Hull, 1934; Hoffeld, 1962; Hovland, 1937)

that extinction effects also generalize to stimuli at

all points on the generalization gradient. Whether

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1492
extinguishing emotional responses to the primary
conditioned stimulus is more effective in

neutralizing the entire range of similar cues than


desensitizing the individual to generalization

stimuli represents a question of considerable

theoretical and practical importance.

Many theories assume that stable and


widespread behavioral changes will result only if

the prototypic conditioning involving the primary


stimulus object is modified. Consequently, a

considerable amount of time is typically devoted

to diagnostic exploration and reconstruction of the

client’s social history before any therapeutic

interventions are attempted. If it were found that


neutralizing a generalization stimulus had about

as much effect on the extinction gradient as would


result from extinguishing emotional responses to

the original conditioned stimulus, then it would


make little difference at which point on the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1493
stimulus continuum the change agent began the

counterconditioning process. Unfortunately,

laboratory experiments necessary to resolve this


issue have not yet been conducted. A few

investigations have been reported, however, in

which the magnitude of emotional responses to

the CS originally employed in the establishment of

conditioned responses is assessed as a function of


extinguishing emotional reactions to
generalization stimuli located at various distances

from the CS. Findings from these studies (Bass &

Hull, 1934; Hoffeld, 1962; Hovland, 1937)

consistently demonstrate that neutralizing any


relevant stimulus, whether adjacent to, or remote

from, the CS, has the effect of reducing somewhat


the emotional responsiveness to cues at all points

on the generalization gradient. The anxiety

decrement, however, becomes progressively


smaller the farther the test stimuli are removed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1494
from the stimulus selected for extinction.

The foregoing findings suggest that positive


outcomes can be achieved to some degree by

deconditioning any stimulus possessing anxiety-

arousing properties, but that the greatest benefits


will be derived from focusing on the particular

events that the change agent wishes to neutralize,

regardless of whether they constitute the original


or the generalized stimuli. On the basis of these
findings there is no reason to expect that

desensitizing the primary conditioned stimulus

would have a more widespread effect on

generalization cues than neutralizing the latter


stimuli directly. Thus, for example, if a given
individual’s anxiety responses to persons in

authority primarily represent generalization from

earlier punishing experiences with his parents,

more substantial and rapid benefits would be

derived from neutralizing emotional responses to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1495
authority figures whom he currently fears than to

his parents.

BASIC STIMULUS DIMENSIONS

After the stimuli to be counterconditioned

have been selected, they must be scaled in terms

of their emotion-eliciting potential if one employs


a graduated approach. In setting up the stimulus

hierarchies, the potency of aversive cues can be


varied on several dimensions. When emotional

responses are elicited by nonsocial events they

can be ordered in terms of physical proximity to


the feared objects. The use of a proximity
dimension is illustrated in Wolpe’s (1962)

treatment of a woman suffering from a severe

automobile phobia that originated from a collision

at a crossroad. An anxiety hierarchy was

constructed involving highway scenes in which

cars made progressively closer advances to the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1496
client’s automobile as it approached a highway
intersection.

Stimulus hierarchies have been constructed in


terms of a temporal dimension for treating fears of

public speaking (Paul, 1966), separation anxieties

(Lazarus, 1960), and apprehensions about


examinations (Emery & Krumboltz, 1967). A
symbolic-reality dimension is frequently employed

in setting up the anxiety stimulus continuum. A


claustrophobic series may range from reading

about others being confined in small enclosures to

imagining oneself “trapped” in a stalled elevator

for progressively longer periods of time (Wolpe,

1961); a snake-phobic series may vary from


writing the word “snake” to handling plastic

specimens, to holding a live nonpoisonous reptile


(Lazovik & Lang, 1960).

Another effective way of grading cues is by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1497
varying the number of emotion-provoking elements
in the total stimulus complex. In Wolpe’s

treatment of the automobile phobia described


above the anxiety- arousing value of traffic scenes

was controlled by varying the speed of the

automobiles, the trustworthiness of drivers in the

approaching cars, the presence or absence of


traffic signs and signals, and the characteristics of

the person driving the client’s car.

For many persons who seek treatment,

relatively complex social stimuli or interpersonal

responses themselves serve as the primary

sources of anxiety. In scaling the emotion-arousing

properties of social cues, the nature and intensity


of the behavior of others can be utilized as the basis

for graduating threats. Thus, for example, a person


who was greatly disturbed by displays of

aggression was gradually desensitized to a


stimulus hierarchy which ranged from a situation

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1498
in which men engaged in a mild argument to one

in which the participants physically attacked each

other (Wolpe, 1958); similarly, a graded series of


devaluation situations was set up for a

gynecologist who was extremely sensitive about

any criticism or rejection from others (Wolpe,

1962). An intensely jealous male was treated by

utilizing a series of jealousy-provoking


hierarchies, each of which depicted varying
degrees of friendly interactions between the

client’s fiancée and several rival males (Wolpe,

1958).

In the illustrations presented thus far the


determinants of emotional responses are

primarily external social or situational cues. If a


person has been repeatedly punished for

displaying a particular form of behavior, the

tendency to perform these social responses

becomes, through their association with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1499
punishment, a stimulus for anxiety. Thus, for

example, if punishment has occurred frequently in

conjunction with aggressive behavior, its


expression will elicit anticipatory emotional

reactions. Similarly, if sexual behavior is

associated with punishment, sex responses

gradually acquire anxiety-evoking properties.

Both of the emotional reactions considered are


response-correlated or self-generated. The
stimulus hierarchies in the treatment of such

classes of anxiety disorders would contain

increasing intensities of the negatively valenced

social responses, ranging from attenuated forms


that are likely to elicit relatively mild anxiety to

more forceful expressions capable of arousing


emotional responses of high magnitude.

Although the preceding discussion has

highlighted individual dimensions on which

emotion-eliciting cues can be ordered, in many

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1500
cases the stimulus hierarchies are constructed by

varying cues on several dimensions

simultaneously. Moreover, many psychological


problems are multiform in character and,

consequently, a number of different stimulus

hierarchies containing both environmental or

response-produced cues may have to be

constructed for a given individual in order to


encompass the full range of his anxiety responses.
The more generalized the emotional behavior, the

greater the need for multiple hierarchies.

Neutralization of Threats in Symbolic or


Realistic Forms

For reasons of ease, economy, and flexibility,


counterconditioning is typically directed toward

symbolic representations of actual threats.


Symbolic presentation obviates the practical

inconvenience and encumbrance of graded

physical presentations; it also allows the client to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1501
terminate fear-arousing stimuli without making

actual avoidance responses by simply thinking of

something else. As illustrated in the preceding


section, persons may be desensitized to imagery of

aggressive behavior, sexual intimacy, social

rejection, or any other type of emotion-provoking

situation. The desensitization procedure devised

by Wolpe, therefore, represents a form of cognitive


counterconditioning where both the aversive
events and the opposing positive condition are

verbally induced and sustained through covert

self-stimulation rather than through paired

presentation of the physical stimuli themselves.


Unlike direct forms of deconditioning in which, for

example, a feared rabbit appears in temporal


conjunction with positive consummatory

responses (Jones, 1924), the subject is instructed

to visualize the rabbit and to imagine himself


eating a delicious culinary treat. In view of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1502
heavy reliance of Wolpe’s method upon symbolic

processes, it is surprising to find this approach

described critically as being concerned only with


peripheral skeletal processes (Murray, 1963;

Shoben, 1963).

The utilization of symbolized aversive events is

predicated on the assumption that stimuli in this

form possess emotion-arousing properties


analogous to their real-life counterparts. If this
were not the case, the symbolic method would

afford little opportunity to decrease the arousal

potential of actual threats, and hence, there would

be no appreciable treatment effects to transfer


from imagined to real situations. Results of several
studies demonstrate that thoughts do have arousal

capabilities. Miller (1950) found that emotional

responses conditioned to overt verbalizations

generalized extensively to their cognitive

equivalents so that thoughts of the negatively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1503
valenced events generated strong GSR

responsiveness, whereas thoughts of responses

that had never been punished failed to elicit any


emotional arousal. Barber & Hahn (1964)

measured subjective discomfort and physiological

responses (heart rate, frontalis muscle tension,

and reduction in skin resistance) in subjects who

received either a painful cold stressor or merely


imagined the unpleasant experience. They found
that imagined painful stimulation produced

subjective distress and physiological responses

similar to those induced by actual painful

stimulation. In a study directly relevant to the


desensitization procedure, Grossberg & Wilson

(1968) found that instructions to visualize fearful


scenes generated significantly more autonomic

arousal than instructions to imagine neutral

situations.

Individuals who are unable, for one reason or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1504
another, to visualize threatening stimuli vividly, or

for whom imagined scenes fail to evoke emotional

reactions, will most likely derive little benefit from


an exclusively cognitive form of

counterconditioning treatment. It is not entirely

clear why, in some cases, symbolic stimuli have

not spontaneously acquired, through

generalization, some emotion-arousing potential


from their actual counterparts to which
conditioned responses were originally established.

This phenomenon may partly reflect the outcome

of a particular form of discrimination training that

markedly influences the generalization gradient.


Under conditions where thoughts, feelings and

verbalizations are accepted or even encouraged


but corresponding overt actions are punished,

emotional conditioning is apt to be confined to the

actual activities. This type of differential


reinforcement of verbalizations and actions is, in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1505
fact, often recommended in popular books on

child- rearing (Baruch, 1949), and widely

practiced socially.

Since therapists can exercise only indirect

control over client’s self-stimulation, requisite


treatment conditions are sometimes difficult to

create and to manage in a cognitively mediated

desensitization. Weinberg & Zaslove (1963), for


example, report that individuals occasionally
attenuated the aversiveness of situations

presented for their visualization by incorporating

protective elements. Lazovik & Lang (1960)

similarly found that a phobic subject, who derived


little benefit from desensitization, tended to
modify the presented scene by simultaneously

visualizing herself in a comfortable situation.

Emotional responses can be successfully protected

from extinction by introduction of discriminative

safety cues (Solomon, Kamin, & Wynne, 1953). In

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1506
addition to stimulus-attenuating alterations,

individuals sometimes generate excessively high

levels of arousal by adding unintended aversive


elements to a presented scene. In an effort to

increase the affective value of nonarousing

imagined stimuli, and to minimize stimulus

modifications in undesired directions, clients who

present these types of problems are often


instructed to verbalize aloud what they are
visualizing (Wolpe, 1958).

Aversive stimuli can be more precisely

controlled, the potency of counterconditioning


methods can be augmented, and problems of
transfer of extinction effects to real-life situations

can be largely obviated by utilizing actual anxiety-


provoking objects or situations. These benefits

accrue because emotional responsiveness is

extinguished to the actual stimuli that exercise

strong control over avoidance behavior under

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1507
naturalistic conditions rather than to symbolic

events that may possess weaker arousal potential

because of their dissimilarity to the primary


instigators. In keeping with this assumption,

Strahley (1966) demonstrated that phobic

subjects who were required to interact with the

feared object achieved greater reduction in fear

and avoidance behavior than subjects who


received symbolic desensitization.

A few studies have been reported in which a

graded series of tangible stimuli was employed in

the desensitization paradigm. Clark (1963) treated


a 31-year-old woman who, for more than 25 years,
had exhibited a seriously incapacitating phobia of

feathers and birds. Although she was able to


visualize scenes involving birds without displaying

much affective arousal, she was unable to venture

out of doors to situations where there was any

possibility that birds might be encountered (e.g.,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1508
parks, zoos, outdoor walks, or the seaside); she

responded with marked anxiety to down pillows,

cushions, and feathered hats, and suffered from


persistent “anxiety dreams of people throwing

feathers and birds swooping.” The treatment

sessions were conducted in the following manner:

After feelings of calm relaxation were hypnotically

induced the therapist first presented a feather at a


distance and gradually brought it closer so long as
the client showed neither subjective disturbance

nor GSR deflections. Repeated stimulus

presentations were interspersed with suggestions

of relaxation and calmness. In this manner the


woman was deconditioned to a wide variety of

increasingly disturbing physical stimuli that


included feathers of all shapes and sizes, bags full

of feathers, stuffed birds with wings folded and

outstretched, and finally, caged live birds.

As the client displayed increasing tolerance of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1509
feathers, she was encouraged to perform a

corresponding graded series of tasks in real-life

situations to further augment the deconditioning


and generalization process. After 20 treatment

sessions she was “completely undisturbed by

sleeping on feather pillows, could have handfuls of

feathers flung at her, could plunge her hands into a

bag of down and no longer feared going outdoors


or birds in the garden [p. 65].”

Freeman & Kendrick (1960) similarly

employed a physical stimulus dimension in

treating a woman who was terrified of cats and


responded anxiously toward a wide variety of
furry objects. The hierarchical items included

pieces of material graded in texture and


appearance which ranged from velvet to catlike

fur, toy kittens, pictures of cats, a live kitten, and

finally a large full-grown cat. In addition to adults

who are unable to produce emotion-arousing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1510
imagery, it is also difficult to employ symbolic

desensitization with young children.

Consequently, applications of these methods to


young age groups typically involve carefully

graded exposures to actual feared objects (Bentler,

1962; English, 1929; Jones, 1924; Lazarus, 1960).

Aversive stimuli have occasionally been

presented in other tangible forms when imaginal


procedures were either ineffective or inapplicable.
Friedman (1966) successfully treated a deaf mute

who was incapacitated due to a severe dog phobia

by employing pictorial stimuli of dogs arranged in


increasing size and ferocity. Results of a study by
Leon (1967) suggest that avoidance behavior may

be more durably eliminated by neutralizing


aversive stimuli in pictorial than in imaginal

forms. Seager & Brown (1967) extinguished a

severe wind phobia by altering the fear-provoking

capacity of auditory stimuli. In the latter case, tape

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1511
recordings of wind noises progressing from mild

breezes to blustery storms were carefully adjusted

in volume and turbulence to the client’s emotional


responses, as monitored physiologically

throughout treatment.

Counterconditioning of emotional responses

poses no serious problems as long as physical

stimuli can be managed with ease. Even if the


actual objects cannot be introduced into the
treatment situation because of their size and

complexity the desensitization can be conducted

in naturalistic settings in which the critical stimuli


regularly occur. The procedural problems become
considerably more difficult, however, in cases

where emotional arousal is primarily generated by


complex social situations or by the person’s own

behavior. It would be of considerable interest in

this connection to experiment with graded

pictorial stimuli (Bandura & Menlove, 1968) or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1512
tape-recorded social interaction sequences.

Preliminary evidence (Bandura, Blanchard, &

Ritter, 1969) indicates that these more tangible


modes of stimulus presentation, particularly if

combined with modeling cues, can extinguish

anxiety more thoroughly than when subjective

threats are cognitively reinstated. These findings

suggest that it would be advantageous to devise


graduated film sequences for objects and social
situations which are common sources of anxiety.

SELF-ADMINISTERED DESENSITIZATION IN
NATURALISTIC SITUATIONS

In the treatment strategies discussed thus far

the change agent manages the presentation of

both the emotion-provoking and the anxiety-

competing stimuli so that responses to the latter


cues prevail over the former. To the extent that a

person can be trained to manage skillfully these


two sets of events in his everyday experiences, he

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1513
can achieve some degree of self-directed
desensitization.

Graded performance tasks have been


successfully employed to some extent in

conjunction with symbolic counterconditioning.

Indeed, because individuals often show


insufficient improvements in performance
following complete symbolic desensitization,

Meyer (1966) has adopted a procedure in which


clients are required to perform behaviors under

optimal real-life circumstances after anxiety has

been thoroughly extinguished to the

corresponding imagined situations. The relative

superiority of this type of approach is


corroborated empirically by Garfield, Darwin,

Singer, & McBrearty (1967). These authors found


that avoidance behavior was more extensively

reduced by symbolic desensitization combined


with graded performance tasks than by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1514
desensitization alone.

A further illustration of how performance

extinction can supplement even


counterconditioning involving realistic stimuli is

provided by Clark (1963) in the treatment of the

bird phobia described earlier. After a particular

aversive stimulus had been successfully

neutralized (e.g., a single feather, a bag full of


feathers, a stuffed bird) the client took the objects
home. Similarly, when emotional reactions to

stuffed birds were extinguished, visits were

arranged to an aviary and a museum containing a


varied array of stuffed birds. In later stages of
treatment, following desensitization to a live bird,

the client visited, with the reassuring support of


her family, a park full of domesticated ducks and

other fowl. As a precaution against possible

negative reconditioning by premature exposure or

the occurrence of unanticipated threats, the client

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1515
was instructed to retire from the situation or to

terminate approach behavior if it should become

emotionally disturbing.

Considering the transfer decrements from

imagined to real-life situations, carefully selected

and well-timed performance tasks should be

included as an integral part of desensitization. In

this type of treatment strategy the formal


desensitization is principally used to reduce
anxiety reactions sufficiently to enable clients to

perform desired responses in previously feared

situations where the major extinction of emotional


responding takes place.

Self-regulated desensitization can serve not

only as an important supplement to symbolic

desensitization but as a method of treatment in its

own right. Hutchison (1962) successfully treated

an electronics technician with a long history of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1516
exhibitionism in 26 sessions by training the client
to perform a set of relaxation responses

immediately following the occurrence of stimuli


that typically preceded genital exposure. The first

few interviews were devoted to identifying the

essential covariations between social events and

the exhibitionistic responses. In this particular


case, the critical determinants involved criticism

from his supervisor or his wife, and feelings of

inadequacy connected with his vocational and

personal achievements. The client received

training in relaxation until he was capable of


inducing rapid and deep muscular relaxation.

Thus, by performing the relaxation responses


immediately following the occurrence of

experiences eliciting exposure, he was able to gain


full control over his exhibitionism. A follow-up

study conducted one year after the termination of


therapy revealed that genital exposure had been

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1517
completely eliminated.

Wolpe (1958) reports considerable success


with self-conducted desensitization of impotence

for males who are sexually responsive, but for

whom the anxiety produced by coital performance


gives rise to premature ejaculations, or an inability

to achieve and maintain an erection. Briefly, the

procedure is as follows. The individual first


receives training in progressive relaxation as an
aid in counteracting anxieties elicited in the sexual

situation. In addition, to ensure beneficial

outcomes he is advised to engage in sexual

behavior only when he has a strong positive desire


to do so, and under the most favorable
circumstances. He is then instructed to lie in bed

with his partner in a relaxed way, but to confine

the sexual activity initially to caresses and

preliminary love play. In order to avoid any

possible reinforcement of anxiety, no attempt at

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1518
intercourse is made until sexual inhibitions have

been sufficiently reduced. As deconditioning

proceeds, the client is likely to exhibit a gradual


increase in sexual responsiveness, and eventually

coitus can be attempted after adequate erections

have been achieved. Additional examples of the

use of relaxation by individuals in the self-

management of chronic anxiety reactions are


provided by Jacobson (1964), and by Haugen,
Dixon, & Dickel (1958).

The extent to which a stable change in

behavior can be produced by deliberate utilization


of self-induced responses that compete with
anxiety and supplant it depends upon whether

they serve primarily to reduce distress or to


neutralize the crucial eliciting stimuli. In

applications of relaxation by Haugen, Dixon, &

Dickel (1958), for example, people are simply

instructed to practice muscular relaxation, but

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1519
otherwise the therapists seem to show little

interest in the stimulus determinants of emotional

responses. Thus, a client who achieves sufficient


relaxation to counteract anxiety may gain

temporary relief, but if the eliciting cues are

absent during this process he will still remain

vulnerable to the disturbing stimuli because their

arousing properties have in no way been altered.


Any deconditioning that may result from an
exclusively response-directed program of

relaxation will, therefore, depend on the fortuitous

contiguous occurrence of eliciting stimuli and

anxiety-competing responses. On the other hand,


in the prescribed desensitization programs of

Hutchison (1962) and Wolpe (1958), clients are


encouraged to induce deep muscular relaxation or

other competing responses while exposed to the

crucial emotion-provoking stimuli. Under these


temporal conditions, the motivational properties

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1520
of stimulus events can be significantly modified.

If the controlling stimuli for emotional


responses have not been identified,

deconditioning effects may still be achieved to

some degree provided the competing responses


occur in close temporal relationship. The onset of

emotional arousal can serve as a cue signifying the

presence of the enigmatic eliciting events.


Provided the individual is able to discriminate
changes in his arousal and to induce positive

responses of sufficient strength immediately at the

onset of arousal, the self-produced incompatible

responses may coincide with the stimuli eliciting


anxiety, thereby ensuring the temporal
prerequisites for reconditioning. By contrast,

when persons merely engage in relaxation for a

given period of time, usually at the end of the day,

the eliciting and neutralizing events are essentially

uncorrelated.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1521
In most social situations people obviously

cannot lapse into complete muscular relaxation.

This problem, however, does not impose serious


limitations on the utility of relaxation, since a

certain degree of stress reduction can be achieved


through the selective relaxation of muscle groups
that are not in use at any given moment (Jacobson,

1964; Wolpe, 1958). Furthermore, positive

imagery and pleasant activities that can be easily


engaged in may serve as even more effective

stress reducers.

Under free-responding circumstances, the use

of positive events in a fear-arousing situation may


hasten elimination of avoidance responses
because the positive cues enable the individual to

expose himself to threats for longer durations,

rather than because of their direct

counterconditioning effects. Nelson (1966) found

that animals willingly entered a feared situation

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1522
twice as frequently and remained there

approximately three times as long when food was

present in the situation as they did when it was


absent. However, animals that were confined in

the feared compartment for an equivalent period

of time without food showed a similar amount of

fear reduction. The influential role of duration of

exposure on extinction is further corroborated by


Proctor (1968) who presented motion pictures of
snakes of either 5 or 20 seconds duration to

snake-phobic subjects in a desensitization

paradigm. The longer exposure produced the

greater reduction in avoidance behavior. The


comparative data reported by Nelson conflict with

Davison’s (1968) finding that students who


received only exposure to aversive stimuli

experienced more distress and less avoidance

extinction than subjects who had the same


exposure paired with relaxation. Positive events

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1523
most likely serve both as incentives for self-

exposure to aversive situations and as fear

reducers.

Self-administered desensitization has several

important limitations as a sole method of


treatment. In the first place, individuals often do

not have sufficient control over the incidence and

intensity of aversive stimuli and the social context


in which they occur (Wolpe, 1958). On certain
occasions it may therefore be difficult to marshal

competing responses of sufficient strength to

contravene adverse emotional experiences.

Second, people who exhibit strong avoidance


tendencies are inclined to shun feared situations
even though objectively they may be relatively

innocuous. Finally, in cases involving severe and

widely generalized inhibitions, the extinction of

anxiety may have to commence, under controlled

conditions, at the symbolic end of the stimulus

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1524
generalization continuum. After emotional

responses to imagined threats have been

substantially reduced, the individual is in a more


favorable position to attempt previously inhibited

behavior in progressively more fearful situations.

Antagonistic Activities in
Counterconditioning

In order to achieve counterconditioning,

aversive stimuli that ordinarily evoke emotional


responses are introduced in the presence of

incompatible activities. Although relaxation has

received greatest attention, a wide variety of

operations have been employed for inducing


antagonistic response tendencies. The earliest
application of this learning principle (Jones, 1924)

relied upon appetizing foods. This particular case


involved a young boy who exhibited severe

anxiety responses of unknown origin toward

animals, and a host of furry objects including fur

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1525
coats, cotton wool, fur rugs, and feathers. In

testing the boy’s reactions to varied fear-

provoking stimuli a rabbit elicited the strongest


emotional response, and it was therefore selected

as the stimulus to be neutralized.

Counterconditioning was achieved by feeding

the boy in the presence of initially weak, but

gradually increasing, anxiety-arousing stimuli.


While he was eating his favorite food, a rabbit in a
cage was placed in the room at a sufficient

distance not to arouse emotional reactions more

powerful than the positive consummatory


activities. Each day the caged rabbit was brought
nearer to the table without evoking the customary

anxiety responses, and eventually, released from


the cage. During the final stage of treatment, the

boy not only displayed no fear at having the rabbit

placed on the feeding table or even in his lap, but

spontaneously verbalized a fondness for the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1526
animal which previously had terrified him. Further

objective tests revealed that the anxiety extinction

effects had generalized to all the other furlike


objects that he had previously feared. Some

therapists (Bettelheim, 1950), working within a

psychodynamic framework, have made extensive

use of appetizing foods in counteracting anxiety

responses of emotionally prone children


whenever they are about to be exposed to
potentially fear-arousing situations.

That food might serve as an effective anxiety

neutralizer gains support from the suggestive


evidence cited earlier (John, 1961) that the
reticular formation possesses reciprocally

inhibitory arousal systems that mediate


conditioned defensive and alimentary activities.

During alimentary activation, response to aversive

stimuli is essentially eliminated. It is generally

assumed that counterconditioning procedures

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1527
employing food are limited in applicability to

children in whom emotional arousal cannot be

reduced through verbal means. Actually, adults


could easily conduct an entire course of

desensitization on their own by systematically

pairing real, pictorial, or imaginal aversive stimuli

with gratifying eating activities. Such

contingencies may, in fact, be more easily self-


administered than relaxation-based
desensitization. Whether this type of approach has

counterconditioning capabilities remains to be

demonstrated.

It has been shown that relaxation, which is


most often employed as the stress reducer in

desensitization of adults, increases tolerance of


aversive stimuli and can facilitate the elimination

of avoidance behavior. However, its mode of

influence is not well understood. Because of the

frequent equation of anxiety with autonomic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1528
reactivity, the research in this area has been

almost exclusively concerned with the autonomic

effects of relaxation. It has been reported that


deep muscular relaxation decreases blood

pressure (Jacobson, 1939), pulse rate (Jacobson,

1940), and GSR responsiveness (Clark, 1963). In a

more systematic evaluation of the relative

physiological effectiveness of relaxation,


Grossberg (1965) compared changes in heart rate,
palmar conductance, and forehead muscle tension

in college students who either performed

relaxation exercises to tape- recorded

instructions, listened to music they considered


relaxing, or were simply asked to relax as best

they could without any external aids. No


significant differences were obtained among the

three groups. The author suggests, however, that

these results be accepted with reservation because


active muscle contraction during the training

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1529
exercises elevates heart rate and lowers skin

resistance, which confound the autonomic effects

of relaxation.

Paul (1969) conducted a series of studies in

which the autonomic effects of muscular


relaxation are compared with those accompanying

hypnotic suggestions of drowsiness and relaxation

and a control condition in which subjects were


simply told to relax. A variety of physiological
changes are measured including tonic forearm

muscle tension, heart rate, respiratory rate, skin

conductance, and anxiety differential. Both

relaxation training and hypnotic suggestion


reduce physiological responsiveness to stressful
imagery, but relaxation achieves somewhat

greater decrements.

The above studies demonstrate that relaxation

induction procedures can decrease physiological

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1530
responsiveness. It has been further shown by
Grings & Uno (1968), by means of a “compound

stimulus transfer” design, that the presentation of


an aversive stimulus in conjunction with

relaxation reduces the arousal capabilities of the

threatening cue. Subjects were separately trained

to respond emotionally to a colored light and to


relax whenever the word “now” was projected on

a screen. In subsequent tests subjects displayed

the strongest autonomic responses to the colored

fear cue alone, the weakest response when the

fear and relaxation cues were presented


simultaneously as a compound stimulus, and

emotional response of intermediate magnitude to


a compound stimulus containing the fear cue and a

neutral word cue. Paul (1968b) reports a positive


relationship (r = .50) between a composite

physiological index of degree of relaxation and


reduction in stress response. However, a tape-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1531
recorded relaxation procedure was less effective

than a socially administered one, both in inducing

relaxation and in attenuating arousal to stressful


imagery.

A muscular relaxation procedure includes at


least three distinct components: calming

suggestions of tranquility and relaxation, positive

imagery, and tensing and relaxing various muscle


groups. The effects of these components need to
be studied separately to determine whether the

muscular activity per se is a significant contributor

to reductions in arousal level. It seems unlikely

that physiological research will clarify behavioral


counterconditioning effects to any great extent
until a viable theory is advanced regarding the

nature and locus of the mechanisms which control

emotional behavior. Since evidence strongly

supports a central rather than a peripheral view of

anxiety, it would be especially important to study

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1532
the effects that relaxation, or any other anxiety-

neutralizing activity that might be employed in

counterconditioning, have upon central arousal


mechanisms.

Lazarus & Abramovitz (1962) have

occasionally relied upon positive imagery for

modifying fearful behavior of children for whom

relaxation was not feasible. This procedure is


identical with the standard method of
desensitization except that the graded aversive

stimuli are presented in the context of strong

positive ideations. The child is interviewed on his


areas of interest and his idols, usually drawn from
television, movies, fiction, or the child’s own

imagination. “The child is then asked to close his


eyes and told to imagine a sequence of events

which is close enough to his everyday life to be

credible, but within which is woven a story

concerning his favorite hero [p. 192].” After a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1533
sufficient degree of positive affect has been

created, the therapist introduces into the narrative

the lowest item in the hierarchy and the child is


instructed to signal if he feels afraid, unhappy or

uncomfortable. When the child registers

disturbance, the threatening element is

immediately withdrawn, and the positive imagery

is further enhanced. This procedure is continued


until the most phobic item has been neutralized. In
most cases arousal-reducing imagery can be

presented and controlled more effectively in

discrete conditioning trials rather than in the form

of a continuing narrative. Pleasant imagery and


mollifying thoughts are often used in this manner

with adults to enhance the tranquilizing effects of


relaxation procedures. No attempts have been

made, however, to assess the physiological effects

of positive imagery, or to determine whether it


accelerates extinction of avoidance behavior.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1534
Those who often mediate pleasant experiences

or the reduction of discomfort in others are likely

to acquire positive properties; consequently, the


mere presence of such an individual will elicit

positive affective responses that can serve as


anxiety neutralizers. Frequent social contact, even
though unaccompanied by nurturant functions,

may also endow others with positive valence

(Cairns, 1966; Homans, 1961). That familiar social


stimuli can function as anxiety reducers has been

clearly shown with both humans and infrahuman

subjects. Mason (1960) found that responses


indicative of emotional disturbance were

exhibited less frequently to stressful situations by


monkeys in the presence of peers than in the

company of adult monkeys (whom they had rarely

seen since birth), other animals, or when they


were left alone in the situation. The influence of

familiarity on social stress reduction receives

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1535
further support from a study by Kissel (1965)

conducted with college students. A friend’s

company was found to be more effective in

diminishing autonomic arousal to induced failure


than that of a stranger, whose presence had no
distress-reducing value.

There is reason to expect from the above

laboratory findings that relationship-induced


responses can serve to mitigate emotional arousal
to some extent. Wolpe (1958), in fact, contends

that favorable outcomes achieved by traditional

interview methods primarily derive from

inadvertent counterconditioning of anxiety by


positive responses evoked in the client-therapist
relationship. This interpretation is consistent with

Shoben’s (1949) conceptualization of the

treatment process as one in which symbolically

reinstated anxiety is counterconditioned through

association with comfort reactions made by the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1536
therapeutic relationship. Although this type of

deconditioning is possible, the outcome data

reviewed in the introductory chapter suggest that


it does not occur with any degree of consistency.

The high dropout rates and frequent displays of

avoidance behavior by clients who continue in

treatment suggest that therapists are more likely

to arouse anxiety than comfort reactions. Many of


the widely prescribed therapist-role behaviors
would, in fact, be clearly contraindicated if judged

primarily in terms of their comfort-inducing

function. Therapists of psychoanalytic persuasion,

for example, strive hard to maintain a high degree


of ambiguity on the assumption that ambiguity

facilitates and intensifies transference responses.


Controlled studies (Bordin, 1958; Dibner, 1958),

on the other hand, demonstrate that the amount of

anxiety experienced by a client varies positively


with the ambiguity of the therapist. Thus, while

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1537
ambiguity may facilitate generalization of

inappropriate response patterns, it is antithetical

to counterconditioning.

The fact that strength of relationship-produced

responses cannot be easily controlled or rapidly


increased if necessary to counteract strong

emotional arousal places additional limitations on

the extent to which relationship variables alone


can create the requisite conditions for successful
desensitization. Positive relationship-induced

responses may thus serve as an important adjunct

to, but not a reliable substitute for, more powerful

anxiety-neutralizing procedures. Even if the


necessary competing comfort reactions were
strongly established, desensitization outcomes

would remain unpredictable if the introduction of

emotionally disturbing contents was primarily left

to the vagaries of clients, rather than carefully

regulated by psychotherapists.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1538
Some of the procedures employed in

counterconditioning treatments, such as

relaxation and pleasant imagery, for the explicit


purpose of inducing strong comfort reactions, can

also enhance the anxiety-reducing cue value of the


therapist. An early hypnosis study conducted by
Estabrooks (1930) demonstrated that even a

person who, in the experiences of subjects, had

been indirectly associated with relaxation


acquired deactivation properties. During early

phases of this experiment, subjects’ level of

autonomic arousal decreased only as hypnosis


was induced. However, after they had been

hypnotized a number of times, the mere presence


of the apparatus operator before the induction of

hypnosis produced much the same decrease in

arousal level. As would be expected, this


phenomenon did not occur with a subject who had

had a negative experience with the operator

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1539
sometime prior to the experimental session.

The rate at which emotional behavior is


extinguished by therapeutic agents who vary in

their stress-reducing properties has not been

systematically investigated. Informal observations


of cases where parents serve as therapists in

eliminating their children’s phobic behavior

(English, 1929) have suggestive value and are in


accordance with experimental findings. A 7-
month-old child developed marked fear of a

stuffed cat on the basis of a sudden aversive

experience. In an effort to extinguish this fear a

therapist presented the stuffed animal repeatedly,


but each time the child responded with
withdrawal, trembling, and frightened wails.

When the cat was later offered in the presence of

both parents, she accepted it hesitantly but

continued to exhibit some apprehensiveness. A

brief experience during which the child handled

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1540
the toy while she was held in her mother’s arms

effectively eliminated the residual fear responses;

thereafter, the child readily accepted the stuffed


animal and played happily with it. The differential

distress shown by the child to the feared object

when the parents were present and absent is

similar to Liddell’s (1950) findings with

infrahuman subjects that the presence of a mother


increases her offspring’s tolerance of stressful
stimuli. Although the relative efficacy of different

persons for mitigating emotional disturbance was

not explored, Bentler (1962) reports a case in

which a mother completely extinguished an


aquaphobia in her infant daughter by reexposing

her to progressively larger amounts of water in


the context of close maternal contact

supplemented by a highly prized flotilla of toys.

Most parents similarly function as effective


anxiety reducers in modifying their children’s

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1541
fears of loud noises, heights, darkness, animals,

and other common fear-provoking situations

(Jersild & Holmes, 1935).

Pharmacologic agents that decrease emotional

arousal have also been occasionally employed in


cases where psychological procedures for

inducing competing activities have proved either

ineffective or not feasible for various reasons


(Friedman, 1966; Walton & Mather, 1963b). Brady
(1966) has made extensive use of short-acting

barbiturates (e.g., metho-hexitone sodium) in

conjunction with relaxation instructions as a quick

means of producing deep muscular relaxation.


This method was applied with notable success in
the treatment of severe frigidity in women who

rarely engaged in coitus because it caused them

considerable physical pain, revulsion and anxiety.

After they were desensitized to individualized sex-

anxiety hierarchies they not only became

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1542
considerably more sexually responsive but found

intercourse erotically pleasurable. In a later paper

Brady (1967) reaffirms the efficacy of


desensitization based on drug-induced relaxation

with a variety of anxiety disorders. Friedman

(1968) reports similarly high success rates in the

treatment of impotence by drug-assisted

desensitization.

The foregoing results are sufficiently


promising to warrant systematic comparative

evaluations of the degree to which, if any, drug-

produced effects facilitate the counterconditioning


process. There is some laboratory evidence to
indicate that the beneficial tranquilizing effects of

drugs may be partially offset by their retarding


effects on learning (Cole & Gerard, 1959; Mitchell

& Zax, 1959; Schneider & Costiloe, 1957). Not only

can conditioning be impeded but, if results from

animal experimentation are applicable to humans,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1543
changes induced during a drugged state may have

limited transfer value. Barry, Etheredge, & Miller

(1965) found that varying dosages of amobarbital


sodium enabled animals to resume a food-

producing response that was previously inhibited

by fear conditioning, but the reduction of

behavioral inhibition failed to transfer to the

normal nondrugged state. A study by Sherman


(1967) suggests, however, that the transfer
decrement might be obviated by a treatment

procedure involving progressive reduction in

dosage of the fear-reducing drug. Amobarbital

sodium restored previously inhibited behavior in


animals during the sedated state; however,

subjects who experienced abrupt withdrawal of


the drug exhibited a precipitous decrement in

performance to the level of the saline control

group, whereas those who continued to receive


the drug in progressively smaller amounts showed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1544
a monotonic increase in responsiveness.

Apparently, a gradual drug-withdrawal treatment

can facilitate permanent extinction of fear-


mediated behavior although, as the author notes,

the relative superiority of this approach cannot be


determined without data from a group that is
deconditioned under a constant drug dose for an

equivalent period of time before the drug is

abruptly withdrawn.

The question remains as to why organisms that

repeatedly perform feared responses, which are

intermittently rewarded during a drugged state,

fail to display some degree of permanent fear


extinction. One interpretation, favored by Barry,
Etheredge, & Miller (1965), assumes that lasting

extinction does occur, but transfer of therapeutic

effects is impeded by marked stimulus change

resulting from the shift from sedated to normal

states. Considering that avoidance behavior is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1545
extensively controlled by external stimuli, it seems

improbable that changes in internal stimulation

adequately explain why animals may show a


dramatic loss from 200 rewardable responses

while under the influence of the drug to only 3

scattered responses in the same situation under

nondrugged conditions (Sherman, 1967). An

alternative interpretation would involve learning


rather than generalization deficits. Drugs,
particularly in higher dosages, may produce not

only transient deactivation but also impairment of

learning functions. Behavioral changes which are

primarily chemically induced would not be


expected to persist after pharmacologic recovery.

On the other hand, optimal drug dosages that


produce beneficial tranquilizing effects without

adversely affecting learning processes might

augment extinction of the anxiety-arousing


potential of aversive stimuli. Even if comparative

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1546
evaluations showed this to be the case,

pharmacologic aids should be primarily confined

to persons who do not benefit from exclusively


psychological procedures. In view of the

unpleasant effects of intravenous injections and


the potential dangers of drug dependency with
repeated usage, the extinction gains would have to

be substantial to justify frequent use of drugs as

adjuncts to standard counterconditioning


procedures.

It should also be noted in passing that persons

who routinely consume “tranquilizing” drugs will

not necessarily experience progressive extinction


of emotional responses. Achievement of lasting
deconditioning outcomes requires both the

presence of a tranquil or positive emotional state

of sufficient strength, and judicious reexposures to

crucial anxiety-evoking stimuli. These learning

requisites are rarely obtained in experiences of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1547
everyday life. On the contrary, persons are often

prematurely confronted with excessively

threatening situations that result in the


reinforcement of avoidant behaviors rather than

in their extinction. The more severely

incapacitated cases, on the other hand, are usually

too heavily sedated to be capable of much

reconditioning.

Physiological Accompaniments of
Emotional Behavior

Theories of personality and psychotherapy

generally differentiate among types of “impulses”

or emotional states as though they represented


distinct forms of physiological arousal. Thus, in
one case a person is assumed to be suffering from

“repressed hostile impulses” and is therefore


encouraged to express verbal or physical

aggression designed to discharge the troublesome

hostile affective state. In another case, “anxiety”

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1548
may be regarded as the major emotional condition

that presumably reflects a somewhat different

form of physiological arousal. If emotional


behaviors which are designated as anger, fear, or

euphoria were controlled by separate

physiological events, then different types of

treatments might be required to extinguish

diverse types of emotional behavior.

Physiological studies disclose that the varied


array of emotions that people experience

phenomenologically are not accompanied by a

corresponding diversity in physiological response


patterns. In the procedures commonly used
individuals are subjected to fear- or anger-

provoking stimulation during which changes in


numerous physiological responses are

simultaneously recorded. Interpretation of these

findings is complicated by lack of independent

evidence that the two stimuli are of comparable

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1549
aversiveness. It is consequently difficult to

determine whether obtained differences are

attributable to dissimilarities in the qualitative


features or in the relative intensities of the

aversive stimuli. Moreover, as Duffy (1962) has

noted, unless it has been shown that dissimilar

stimuli within the same emotional class produce

identical patterns of physiological arousal, the


generality of findings yielded by a single fear
stimulus and a single anger stimulus is open to

question.

Based on manipulations in which subjects


experienced increasing shock while the
experimenter expressed alarm about a dangerous

high-voltage short circuit in the apparatus, and in


which they also received rude sarcastic treatment

by an assistant, Ax (1953) found some subtle

differences in physiological reactions to the fear

and anger provocation. Data reported by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1550
Schachter (1957) using analogous manipulations

disclose essentially similar cardiovascular

responsiveness to fear and anger stimulation, but


they both differ significantly from reactions to

pain induced by a cold pressor test. However, on

the basis of subjective categorization of the data

the author extracts greater physiological

specificity than the actual findings warrant. It


seems unlikely that small differences in an
otherwise identical pattern of physiological

reactivity are sufficiently discriminable, if at all, to

serve as the cues for differentiating among

different emotional states.

Results of both physiological and psychological

studies support the conclusion that a common


diffuse state of physiological arousal mediates

diverse forms of emotional behavior and that

different emotional states are identified and

discriminated primarily in terms of external

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1551
stimuli rather than internal somatic cues. Among

the situational cues that help to label a given state

of physiological arousal, the inducing stimulus


conditions undoubtedly play an influential role.

Thus, for example, visceral arousal that is

generated by threatening stimulation is likely to

be interpreted as fear or anxiety; arousal

produced by obstacles and thwarting activities of


frustrating agents will tend to be experienced as
anger; and arousal resulting from highly

pleasurable stimulation will be identified as joy or

euphoria. In a study designed to identify the

characteristics of situations that might serve as


cues for differentiating among emotions Hunt,

Cole, & Reis (1958) found that students were


inclined to rate environmental events as

provoking fear when they were threatening, as

anger when frustrating agents figured


prominently, and as sorrow when desired objects

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1552
were irretrievably lost.

Even the same inducing operation and its


associated physiological arousal may result in

differential emotions depending on the form of

affective modeling cues which serve to define the


appropriate emotional behavior under conditions

of ambiguity. According to the theory of emotion

advanced by Schachter (1964) when a person


experiences a state of physiological arousal and
cannot clearly identify its source, the same

emotional condition may be interpreted as anger,

euphoria, anxiety, or some other type of feeling

depending upon the nature of external influences.


The interaction between modeling, cognitive, and
physiological determinants of emotional state is

revealed in an experiment by Schachter & Singer

(1962) which proceeded in the following manner.

One group of college students received injections

of epinephrine, a sympathetic stimulant, and was

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1553
at the same time correctly informed of its physical

side effects. A second group of subjects was

similarly administered epinephrine but was


uninformed about its side effects, while other

students received a placebo injection of saline

solution. Immediately after the experimental

manipulation of physiological arousal, al subjects

were sent to a room where they were exposed to


the experimenter’s confederate, supposedly
another subject, who displayed considerable anger

and verbal aggression toward the experimental

procedure. Subjects in the epinephrine-

uninformed group displayed more anger than


students in either the epinephrine-informed or

placebo condition, which did not differ from each


other. In another phase of this experiment, four

treatments were employed, the three described

above and one in which subjects were injected


with epinephrine and deliberately misinformed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1554
concerning its side effects so they had no adequate

explanation for their aroused state. The

confederate in this phase behaved in an


extraordinarily euphoric manner, for example,

flying paper airplanes, hoola-hooping, and playing


basketball with equipment contained in the room.
Subjects who experienced physiological arousal

and were misinformed or uninformed concerning

the basis for their reactivity displayed a great deal


of euphoria, whereas subjects who were equally

aroused but for whom an accurate explanation

was available, and the nonaroused placebo group,


were little affected by the behavior of the

confederate.

In a related experiment, Schachter & Wheeler

(1962) extended the range of autonomic arousal

by administering either epinephrine, a placebo

saline solution, or chlorpromazine, a sympathetic

depressant, to different groups of subjects. After

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1555
receiving their injections all subjects viewed a

slapstick comedy film. The epinephrine-injected

group showed a greater amount of amusement,


assessed both by self-reports and behavioral

ratings, than placebo subjects, while the

chlorpromazine-injected group was least affected

by the comical displays.

Further research conducted by Nisbett &


Schachter (1966) showed that emotional states
induced by environmental stimuli are manipulable

to some extent, as was demonstrated with drug-

induced arousal. Students were administered


either weak or severe electric shocks after
receiving a placebo pill. Half the subjects within

each condition were led to believe that the side


effects accompanying the drug were similar to

shock- produced emotional reactions, whereas the

remaining subjects were correctly informed that

shock evokes arousal symptoms such as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1556
palpitations and tremors. Students who received

weak shock and attributed their autonomic

arousal to the pill tolerated more painful


stimulation and reported less pain than those who

interpreted their arousal as due to shock.

However, ascribing arousal to an artificial source

had no influence on pain tolerance when the

shocks were severe. The latter findings indicate


that arousal states are less susceptible to
relabeling when the controlling stimuli are

apparent and powerful.

Taken together, the studies demonstrate that


emotional states are partly a function of the
degree of physiological arousal, but that social and

cognitive variables may play a crucial role in


determining both the nature and intensity of the

emotions experienced, particularly when

individuals cannot accurately label the source of

their aroused condition. Thus, the same state of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1557
physiological arousal can be experienced as

euphoria, anger, or some other type of emotional

condition depending upon defining cognitions and


the affective reactions of others to arousal-

inducing situations.

The preceding findings have several important

therapeutic implications. It is not necessary to

reduce specific types of physiological arousal in


order to modify different forms of affective
behavior. To the extent that counterconditioning

procedures can successfully neutralize the arousal

potential of valenced stimulus events, then the


method should be applicable not only to problems
of anxiety but also to emotions experienced as

hostility, jealousy, grief, or by some other name. It


should also be possible to decrease positively

labeled emotions by this means if the theory of

nonspecific visceral control is valid. Finally, some

emotional problems might result from mislabeling

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1558
of feeling states; given this, the person would need

to be taught to discriminate accurately the

determinants of his arousal states.

People often experience anxiety arousal

without being able to identify the evocative

stimuli. In interpretive therapies such emotional

responses are frequently attributed to

hypothetical causes operating at an unconscious


level. If emotional arousal can be reduced to some
extent by misattributing the reactions to non-

emotional sources, then it is conceivable that

neutral stimuli could become invested with fear-


arousing properties if they are erroneously
interpreted as the source of anxiety reactions.

Summary

In this chapter the principle of

counterconditioning has been discussed in

relation to the modification of emotional behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1559
by neutralizing the arousal potential of
threatening stimuli. The reconditioning process is

achieved by inducing activities incompatible with


emotional responses in the presence of anxiety-

arousing stimuli. This mode of behavior change is

based on the fact that classically conditioned

effects can exert mediating control, principally


through central mechanisms, over instrumentally

learned behavior.

Three sets of variables, some necessary and

others merely facilitative, have been singled out as

especially relevant to counterconditioning

processes. First, one must select an anxiety-

neutralizing stimulus capable of eliciting


competing events of sufficient strength to

predominate over responses characteristically


evoked by emotion-arousing cues. In practice,

muscular relaxation, appetizing foods, positive


imagery, relationship-induced affective responses,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1560
and pharmacological agents that decrease

emotional arousal have been employed as anxiety

neutralizers. These types of competing activities


have been shown to increase tolerance of aversive

stimuli, accelerate the rate of desensitization, and

generally facilitate extinction of severe avoidance

behavior.

The second class of variables pertains to


emotion-provoking events. The issues here
concern accurate identification of the stimulus

determinants of emotional behavior, and the

forms and intensities in which arousal stimuli are


neutralized. Counterconditioning treatments are
typically directed toward symbolic

representations of actual threats because, in the


latter form, they can be easily controlled and the

method can be applied to an almost infinite variety

of anxiety sources. However, in cases where

symbolic stimuli lack arousal capacity or requisite

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1561
conditions for achieving desensitization cannot be

adequately induced through verbal means, actual

threats are presented in physical, pictorial, or


auditory modes.

In most applications of the

counterconditioning principle, the aversive events

are initially administered in attenuated forms so

that the emotional responses to be counteracted


are relatively weak and can therefore be readily
extinguished. As weak items lose their anxiety-

producing value, progressively more threatening

stimuli, which presumably have been weakened


through generalization of extinction effects, are
gradually introduced. Although stimulus

graduation is not a necessary condition for


achieving desensitization, it permits greater

control over the change process and produces

minimal anxiety elicitation as compared to

approaches involving repeated confrontation with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1562
stimuli having high threat value.

The third variable pertains to the temporal

prerequisites for the occurrence of


counterconditioning outcomes. Both the anxiety-

neutralizing and the aversive events must be

contiguously associated. The mechanism of

conditioning is conceived as including both

mediational and association processes.

Numerous laboratory investigations and

controlled individual studies utilizing the symbolic


desensitization paradigm with relaxation show

this approach to be effective for extinguishing the


emotional arousal capacity of aversive stimuli and
for reducing avoidance behavior. Moreover,

generalized improvements in behavioral

functioning often result from specifically induced

changes. However, more refined analyses of

degree of transfer of extinction effects from

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1563
symbolic stimuli to real-life situations disclose
some generalization decrement. Not only is the

number of approach responses that persons can


perform behaviorally generally less than the

number that have been successfully neutralized in

symbolic form, but restored approach behavior is

usually accompanied by moderate anxiety when


first performed. This transfer decrement partly

reflects the inherent limitations of working

exclusively with symbolic counterparts which

rarely encompass the full range of aversive

elements contained in real-life situations.


Counterconditioning alone is also likely to effect

limited behavioral improvements in conditions


where anxious responding is a realistic

consequence of behavioral deficits, or where the


rewards associated with restored functioning are

outweighed by the advantages of remaining


behaviorally incapacitated.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1564
Symbolic desensitization might primarily serve

to decrease the anxiety-producing value of

aversive stimuli below the threshold for activating


avoidance behavior, thus enabling persons to

engage, though somewhat anxiously, in new


approach behavior. This provides the opportunity
for further extinction of residual anxiety and

avoidance behavior in naturalistic settings. Also, in

cases involving severe anxiety disorders,


deconditioning may have to commence with

symbolic stimuli that are far enough removed

from real threats to evoke less intense reactions.


After emotional responses to imaginal stimuli

have been substantially reduced, the individual is


better prepared for encounters with the

corresponding real-life situations. Emotional

behavior can be most thoroughly extinguished by


supplementing symbolic desensitization with

graded performance tasks, positive reinforcement

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1565
of desired approach behavior, and appropriate

modeling procedures. It is possible that the

efficacy of desensitization itself can be further

enhanced, and transfer problems reduced, by the


use of more tangible threats along with more
powerful competing activities.

The behavioral changes achieved by

desensitization operations cannot be attributed


solely to the conditioning of competing responses
to fear-arousing stimuli through repetitive paired

association. Other mechanisms are also operative.

Some reduction in avoidance behavior

undoubtedly results from selection of explicit


behavioral objectives and positive reinforcement
of progressive advances toward the chosen goal.

The resultant changes also partly reflect the

influence of exposure to aversive stimuli

independently of the effects of the explicitly

programmed competing activities. For this reason,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1566
the multiform procedure combining graduated

exposure, anxiety-neutralizing events, and

positive reinforcement is generally more effective


in extinguishing avoidance behavior than the

separate components alone.

Although counterconditioning methods have

been primarily employed to extinguish “anxiety,”

evidence that diverse emotional behaviors are


mediated by a common diffuse state of
physiological arousal indicates that this approach

may be applicable to other emotional conditions

as well. Furthermore, counterconditioning


procedures can be utilized not only to neutralize
aversive events but also to attach negative

valences to positive stimuli which are potentially


harmful. The principles governing these aversive

forms of counterconditioning are discussed in the

following chapter.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1567
REFERENCES

Agras, W. S. Transfer during systematic


desensitization therapy. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1967, 5, 193-199.

Ashem, B. The treatment of a disaster phobia by


systematic desensitization. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1963, 1, 81-84.

Ax, A. F. The physiological differentiation between


fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 1953, 15, 433-442.

Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. The relative


efficacy of desensitization and modeling
approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and
attitudinal changes, Stanford University, 1969,
13, 173-199.

Bandura, A., & Menlove, F. L. Factors determining


vicarious extinction of avoidance behavior
through symbolic modeling. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1968, 8, 99-108.

Barber, T. X., & Hahn, K. W., Jr. Experimental studies in


“hypnotic” behavior: Physiological and subjective
effects of imagined pain. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 1964, 139, 416-425.

Barry, H., in., Etheredge, E. E., & Miller, N. E.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1568
Counterconditioning and extinction of fear fail to
transfer from amobarbital to nondrug state.
Psychopharmacologia, 1965, 8, 150-156.

Baruch, D. W. New ways in discipline. New York:


McGraw-Hill, 1949.

Bass, M. J., & Hull, C. L. The irradiation of a tactile


conditioned reflex in man. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 1934, 17, 47-65.

Bentler, P. M. An infant’s phobia treated with


reciprocal inhibition therapy. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 1962, 3, 185-189.

Bettleheim, B. Love is not enough. Glencoe, Ill.: Free


Press, 1950.

Beyme, F. Hyperesthesia of taste and touch treated by


reciprocal inhibition. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1964, 2, 7-14.

Black, A. H. The extinction of avoidance responses


under curare. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 519-524.

Black, A. H. Heart rate changes during avoidance


learning in dogs. Canadian Journal of Psychology,
1959,13, 229-242.

Black, A. H. Transfer following operant conditioning in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1569
the curarized dog. Science, 1967,155, 201-203.

Bond, I. K., & Hutchison, H. C. Application of reciprocal


inhibition therapy to exhibitionism. Canadian
Medical Association Journal, 1960, 83, 23-25.

Bond, I. K., & Hutchison, H. C. Application of reciprocal


inhibition therapy to exhibitionism. In H. J.
Eysenck (Ed.), Experiments in behaviour therapy.
New York: Pergamon, 1964. pp. 80-86.

Bordin, E. S. Inside the therapeutic hour. In E. A.


Rubinstein & M. B. Parloff (Eds.), Research in
psychotherapy. Washington, D. C.: American
Psychological Association, 1958. pp. 235-246.

Bower, G., & Grusec, T. Effect of prior Pavlovian


discrimination training upon learning an operant
discrimination. Journal of Experimental Analysis
of Behavior, 1964, 7, 401-404.

Brady, J. P. Brevital-relaxation treatment of frigidity.


Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966, 4,71-77.

Brady, J. P. Brevital-relaxation treatment of neurosis.


Proceedings of Symptomatic Higher Nervous
Activity, Madrid, Spain, September 9, 1967.

Cairns, R. R. Attachment behavior of mammals.


Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 409-426.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1570
Cameron, N. Personality development and
psychopathology: A dynamic approach. Boston:
Houghton Mifflin, 1963.

Chapel, J. L. Treatment of a case of school phobia by


reciprocal inhibition. Canadian Psychiatric
Association Journal, 1967, 12, 25-28.

Clark, D. F. The treatment of monosymptomatic


phobia by systematic desensitization. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1963, 1, 63-68.

Cohen, S. I., & Reed, J. L. The treatment of “nervous


diarrhoea” and other conditioned autonomic
disorders by desensitization. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1968, 114, 1275-1280.

Cole, J. O., & Gerard, R. W. (Eds.), Psychopharmacology:


Problems in evaluation, Washington, D. C.:
National Academy of Sciences—National
Research Council, 1959.

Cooke, G. The efficacy of two desensitization


procedures: An analogue study. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 17-24.

Cooke, G. Evaluation of the efficacy of the components


of reciprocal inhibition psychotherapy. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 1968, 73, 464-467.

Cooper, J. E. A study of behaviour therapy in thirty

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1571
psychiatric patients. Lancet, 1963, 23,411-415.

Costello, C. G. The essentials of behaviour therapy.


Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 1963, 8,
162-166.

Cowden, R. C., & Ford, L. I. Systematic desensitization


with phobic schizophrenics. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 1962, 119, 241-245.

Davison, G. C. Systematic desensitization as a


counterconditioning process. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 1968, 73, 91-99.

Dibner, A. S. Ambiguity and anxiety. Journal of


Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1958, 56, 165-
174.

Donner, L. Effectiveness of a pre-programmed group


desensitization treatment for test anxiety with
and without a therapist present. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University, 1967.

Duffy, E. Activation and behavior. New York: Wiley,


1962.

Efran, J. S., & Marcia, J. E. The treatment of fears by


expectancy manipulation: An exploratory
investigation. Proceedings of the 75th Annual
Convention of the A.P.A. Washington, D. C.:
American Psychological Association, 1967. pp.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1572
239-240.

Emery, J. R., & Krumboltz, J. D. Standard versus


individualized hierarchies in desensitization to
reduce test anxiety. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 1967,14, 204-209.

English, H. B. Three cases of the “conditioned fear


response.” Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1929, 24, 221-225.

Epstein, R., & Liverant, S. Verbal conditioning and sex-


role identification in children. Child Development,
196.3, 34, 99-106.

Estabrooks, G. H. The psychogalvanic reflex in


hypnosis. Journal of General Psychology, 1930, 3,
150-157.

Farber, I. E. Response fixation under anxiety and non-


anxiety conditions Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1948, 38, 111-131.

Fenichel, O. The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. New


York: Norton, 1945

Folkins, C. H., Lawson, K., Opton, E. M., Jr., & Lazarus,


R. S. Desensitization and the experimental
reduction of threat. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology 1968, 73, 100-113.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1573
Ford, C. S., & Beach, F. A. Patterns of sexual behavior.
New York: Harper 1951.

Freeman, H. L., & Kendrick, D. C. A case of cat phobia:


Treatment by method derived from experimental
psychology. British Medical Journal 1960, 2, 497-
502.

Friedman, D. Treatment of a case of dog phobia in a


deaf mute by behavior therapy. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 141.

Friedman, D. The treatment of impotence by brevital


relaxation therapy. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1968, 6, 257-261.

Gale, D. S., Sturmfels, G., & Gale, E. N. A comparison of


reciprocal inhibition and experimental extinction
in the psychotherapeutic process. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 149-155.

Garfield, Z. H., Darwin, P. L., Singer, B. A., & McBrearty,


J. F. Effect of “in vivo” training on experimental
desensitization of a phobia. Psychological Reports,
1967, 20, 515-519.

Geer, J. H., & Katkin, E. S. Treatment of insomnia using


a variant of systematic desensitization. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 1966, 7,161-164.

Geer, J. H., & Silverman, I. Treatment of a recurrent

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1574
nightmare by behavior- modification procedures.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1967, 72, 188-
190.

Gelder, M. G., & Marks, I. M. Desensitization and


phobias: A cross-over study. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1968,114, 323-328.

Gray, B. B., England, G., & Mohoney, }. L. Treatment of


benign vocal nodules by reciprocal inhibition.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1965, 3, 187-
193.

Grings, W. W., & Lockhart, R. A. Galvanic skin response


during avoidance learning. Psychophysiology,
1966, 3, 29-34.

Grings, W. W., & Uno, T. Counterconditioning: Fear


and relaxation. Psychophysiology, 1968, 4, 479-
485.

Grossberg, J. M. The physiological effectiveness of


brief training in differential muscle relaxation.
Unpublished manuscript, San Diego State College,
1965.

Grossberg, J. M., & Wilson, H. Physiological changes


accompanying the visualization of fearful and
neutral situations. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1968,10, 124-1.33.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1575
Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of learning. New York:
Harper, 1935.

Hain, J. D., Butcher, R. H. G., & Stevenson, I. Systematic


desensitization therapy: An analysis of results in
twenty-seven patients. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1966, 112,295-307.

Hartup, W. W. Friendship status and the effectiveness


of peers as reinforcing agents. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 1964, 1, 154-162.

Haslam, M. T. The treatment of an obsessional patient


by reciprocal inhibition. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1965, 2, 213-216.

Haugen, G. B., Dixon, H. H., & Dickel, H. A. A therapy for


anxiety tension reactions. New York: Macmillan,
1958.

Henry, G. W. Sex variants. New York: Hoeber, 1941. 2


vols.

Hoenig, J., & Reed, G. F. The objective assessment of


desensitization. British Journal of Psychiatry,
1966, 112, 1279-128.3.

Hoffeld, D. R. Primary stimulus generalization and


secondary extinction as a function of strength of
conditioning. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1962, 55, 27-31.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1576
Homans, G. C. Social behavior: Its elementary forms.
New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1961.

Hooker, E. The homosexual community. Proceedings


of the XIV International Congress of Applied
Psychology. Copenhagen: Munksgoord, 1961.

Hovland, C. I. The generalization of conditioned


responses: I. The sensory generalization of
conditioned responses with varying frequencies
of tone. Journal of General Psychology, 19.37, 17,
125-148.

Howe, E. S. GSR conditioning in anxiety states,


normals, and chronic functional schizophrenic
subjects. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1958, 56, 183-189.

Hunt, J. McV., Cole, M. W., & Reis, E. E. S. Situational


cues distinguishing anger, fear, and sorrow.
American Journal of Psychology, 1958, 71, 136-
151.

Hutchison, H. C. Reciprocal inhibition therapy as an


alternative to psychotherapy in the treatment of
certain neurotic conditions. Paper read at the
meeting of the American Psychological
Association, St. Louis, 1962.

Jacobson, E. Variation of blood pressure with skeletal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1577
muscle tension and relaxation. Annals of Internal
Medicine, 1939, 12, 1194-1212.

Jacobson, E. Variation of blood pressure with skeletal


muscle tension and relaxation. II. The heart beat.
Annals of Internal Medicine, 1940, 13, 1619-1625.

Jacobson, E. Anxiety and tension control. Philadelphia:


Lippincott, 1964.

Jersild, A. T., & Holmes, F. B. Methods of overcoming


children’s fears. Journal of Psychology, 1935,1, 75-
104.

John, E. R. High nervous functions: Brain functions


and learning. Annual Review of Physiology, 1961,
23, 451-481.

Jones, M. C. A laboratory study of fear: The case of


Peter. Pedagogical Seminary, 1924, 31, 308-315.

Kahn, M. W. The effect of socially learned aggression


or submission on the mating behavior of C57
mice. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1961, 98,211-
217.

Kelley, H. H. The warm-cold variable in first


impressions of persons. Journal of Personality,
1950,18,431-439.

Kimble, G. A., & Kendall, J. W., Jr. A comparison of two

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1578
methods of producing experimental extinction.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1953, 45, 87-
90.

Kissel, S. Stress-reducing properties of social stimuli.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965,
2, 378-384.

Kraft, T., & Al-Issa, I. The application of learning


theory to the treatment of traffic phobia. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 1965, 111, 277-279. (a)

Kraft, T., & Al-Issa, I. Behaviour therapy and the recall


of traumatic experiences—a case study.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1965, 3, 55-58.
(b)

Kraft, T., & Al-Issa, I. Alcoholism treated by


desensitization: a case report. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1967, 5, 69-70. (a)

Kraft, T., & Al-Issa, I. Behavior therapy and the


treatment of frigidity. American Journal of
Psychotherapy, 1967, 21, 116-120. (b)

Krapfl, J. E. Differential ordering of stimulus


presentation and semiautomated versus live
treatment in the systematic desensitization of
snake phobia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Missouri, 1967.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1579
Lacey, J. I. Individual differences in somatic response
patterns. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1950, 43, 338-350.

Lacey, J. I. Somatic response patterning and stress:


Some revisions of activation theory. In M. H.
Appley & R. Trumbull (Eds.), Psychological stress:
Issues in research. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1967. pp. 14-42.

Lang, P. J. Fear reduction and fear behavior: Problems


in treating a construct. In J. M. Shlien (Ed.),
Research in psychotherapy, Vol. III. Washington, D.
C.: American Psychological Association, 1968. pp.
90-102.

Lang, P. J., & Lazovik, A. D. Experimental


desensitization of a phobia. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1963, 66, 519-525.

Lang, P. J., Lazovik, A. D., & Reynolds, D. J.


Desensitization, suggestibility, and pseudo-
therapy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965,
70, 395-402.

Lazarus, A. A. The elimination of children’s phobias by


deconditioning. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), Behavior
therapy and the neuroses. New York: Pergamon,
1960. pp. 114-122.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1580
Lazarus, A. A. Group therapy of phobic disorders by
systematic desensitization. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1961, 63, 504-510.

Lazarus, A. A. The results of behaviour therapy in 126


cases of severe neurosis. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1963, 1, 69-79. (a)

Lazarus, A. A. The treatment of chronic frigidity by


systematic desensitization. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease, 1963, 136, 272-278. (b)

Lazarus, A. A. Crucial procedural factors in


desensitization therapy. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1964, 2, 65-70.

Lazarus, A. A., & Abramovitz, A. The use of “emotive


imagery” in the treatment of children’s phobias.
Journal of Mental Science, 1962, 108, 191-195.

Lazarus, A. A., & Rachman, S. The use of systematic


desensitization in psychotherapy. South African
Medical Journal, 1957, 31, 934-937.

Lazovik, A. D., & Lang, P. J. A laboratory


demonstration of systematic desensitization
psychotherapy. Journal of Psychological Studies,
1960, 11,238-247.

Leitenberg, H., Agras, W. S., Barlow, D. H., & Oliveau, D.


C. Contribution of selective positive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1581
reinforcement and therapeutic instructions to
systematic desensitization therapy. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 74, 113-118.

Leon, PI. V. Reciprocal inhibition: An evaluation of


group procedures with “normal” snake phobic
subjects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Tennessee, 1967.

Liddell, PI. Some specific factors that modify tolerance


for environmental stress. Research Publication of
the Association for Nervous and Mental Disease,
1950, 29, 155-171.

Madsen, C. H., Jr., & Ullmann, L. P. Innovations in the


desensitization of frigidity. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1967, 5, 67-68.

Mann, J., & Rosenthal, T. L. Vicarious and direct


counterconditioning of test anxiety through
individual and group desensitization.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Arizona,
1969.

Marder, M. The effects of experimenter attractiveness


and negative reinforcement on verbal
conditioning. Dissertation Abstracts, 1961, 22,
1243.

Marks, I. M., & Gelder, M. G. A controlled retrospective

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1582
study of behaviour therapy in phobia patients.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 1965, 111, 561-573.

Mason, W. A. Socially mediated reduction in emotional


responses of young rhesus monkeys. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 60, 100-
104.

Melamed, B., & Lang, P. J. Study of the automated


desensitization of fear. Paper read at the meeting
of the Midwestern Psychological Association,
Chicago, May, 1967.

Melvin, K. B., & Brown, J. S. Neutralization of an


aversive light stimulus as a function of number of
paired presentations with food. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1964,
58, 350-353.

Meyer, V., & Crisp, A. H. Some problems in behaviour


therapy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1966,112,
367-381.

Migler, B., & Wolpe, J. Automated self-desensitization:


a case report. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
1967, 5, 133-135.

Miller, H. R. The role of control of aversive stimulus


termination in the systematic desensitization of
snake phobic subjects. Unpublished doctoral

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1583
dissertation, University of Missouri, 1967.

Miller, N. E. Learnable drives and rewards. In S.


Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental
psychology. New York: Wiley, 1950. pp. 435-472.

Mitchell, L. E., & Zax, M. The effects of chlorpromazine


on GSR conditioning. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1959, 59, 246-249.

Moore, N. Behaviour therapy in bronchial asthma: A


controlled study. Journal of Psychosomatic
Research, 1965, 9, 257-276.

Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and the symbolic


processes. New York: Wiley, 1960.

Murray, E. J. Learning theory and psychotherapy:


Biotropic versus sociotropic approaches. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 1963, 10, 250-255.

Nelson, F. Effects of two counterconditioning


procedures on the extinction of fear. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1966,
62, 208-213.

Nisbett, R. E., & Schachter, S. Cognitive manipulation


of pain. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
1966, 2, 227-236.

Notterman, J. M., Schoenfeld, W. N., & Bersh, P. J. A

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1584
comparison of three extinction procedures
following heart rate conditioning. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1952, 47, 674-
677.

O’Connor, N., & Rawnsley, K. Two types of


conditioning in psychotics and normals. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, 58, 157-
161.

Patterson, G. R., & Anderson, D. Peers as social


reinforcers. Child Development, 1964,35, 951-
960.

Paul, G. L. Insight vs. desensitization in psychotherapy.


Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1966.

Paul, G. L. Insight versus desensitization in


psychotherapy two years after termination.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1967, 31, 333-
348.

Paul, G. L. Two-year follow-up of systematic


desensitization in therapy groups. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 1968,73, 119-130. (a)

Paul, G. L. Personal communication. 1968. (b)

Paul, G. L. Physiological effects of relaxation training


and hypnotic suggestion. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 1969, 74, 425-437.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1585
Paul, G. L., & Shannon, D. T. Treatment of anxiety
through systematic desensitization in therapy
groups. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1966, 71,
124-135.

Poppen, R. L. Counterconditioning of conditioned


suppression. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Stanford University, 1968.

Prince, A. I. Relative prestige and the verbal


conditioning of children. American Psychologist,
1962,17, 378 (abstract).

Proctor, S. Duration of exposure to items and


pretreatment training as factors in systematic
desensitization therapy. Unpublished
manuscript, Indiana University, 1968.

Rachman, S. The treatment of anxiety and phobic


reactions by systematic desensitization
psychotherapy. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology. 1959, 58, 259-263.

Rachman, S. Studies in desensitization—I: The


separate effects of relaxation and desensitization.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1965, 3, 245-
251

Rachman, S. Studies in desensitization—III: Speed of


generalization. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1586
1966, 4, 7-15.

Rachman, S. The role of muscular relaxation in


desensitization therapy. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1968, 6, 159-166.

Ramsay, R. W., Barends, J., Brenker, J., & Kruseman, A.


Massed versus spaced desensitization of fear.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 205-
207.

Rescorla, R. H., & Solomon, R. L. Two-process learning


theory: Relationships between Pavlovian
conditioning and instrumental learning.
Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 151-182.

Rifkin, B. G. The treatment of cardiac neurosis using


systematic desensitization. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1968, 6, 239-241.

Rosenthal, R. Experimenter effects in behavioral


research. New York: Appleton- Century-Crofts,
1966.

Rosenthal, T. L. Stimulus modality and aerophobia:


cautions for desensitization therapy. American
Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1967, 9, 269-274.

Rosenthal, T. L. Severe stuttering and maladjustment


treated by desensitization and social influence.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1968, 6, 125-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1587
130.

Schachter, J. Pain, fear, and anger in hypertensives


and normotensives: A psychophysiological study.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 1957, 19, 17-29.

Schachter, S. The interaction of cognitive and


physiological determinants of emotional state. In
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology, Vol. I. New York: Academic
Press, 1964. pp. 49-80.

Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. Cognitive, social, and


physiological determinants of emotional state.
Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 379-399.

Schachter, S., & Wheeler, L. Epinephrine,


chlorpromazine, and amusement. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962, 65, 121-
128.

Schneider, R. A., & Costiloe, J. P. Effects of centrally


acting drugs on conditioning in man; the
inhibiting and facilitating effects of
chlorpromazine, amobarbital, and
methylphenidylacetate on the conditioned
galvanic skin response. American Journal of the
Medical Sciences, 1957, 233, 418-423.

Schubot, E. D. The influence of hypnotic and muscular

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1588
relaxation in systematic desensitization of phobic
behavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Stanford University, 1966.

Seager, C. P., & Brown, B. H. An indicator of tension


during reciprocal inhibition. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1967, 113, 1129-1132.

Sherman, A. R. Therapy of maladaptive fear-motivated


behavior in the rat by the systematic gradual
withdrawal of a fear-reducing drug. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 121-129.

Shoben, E. I., Jr. Psychotherapy as a problem in


learning theory. Psychological Bulletin, 1949, 46,
366-392.

Shoben, E. J., Jr. The therapeutic object: Men or


machines? Journal of Counseling Psychology,
1963, 10, 264-268.

Smith, E. W. L., & Dixon, T. R. Verbal conditioning as a


function of race of the experimenter and
prejudice of the subject. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 1968, 4, 285-301.

Solomon, R. L., & Brush, E. S. Experimentally derived


conceptions of anxiety and aversion. In M. R.
Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation:
1956. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1589
1956. pp. 212-305.

Solomon, R. L., Kamin, L. J., & Wynne, L. C. Traumatic


avoidance learning: The outcomes of several
extinction procedures with dogs. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1953, 48, 291-
302.

Solomon, R. L., & Turner, L. H. Discriminative classical


conditioning in dogs paralyzed by curare can
later control discriminative avoidance responses
in the normal state. Psychological Review, 1962,
69, 202-219.

Spence, K. W., & Taylor, J. A. The relation of


conditioned response strength to anxiety in
normal, neurotic, and psychotic subjects. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 195.3, 45, 265-272.

Stevenson, H. W. Social reinforcement of children’s


behavior. In L. P. Lipsitt & C. C. Spiker (Eds.),
Advances in child development and behavior. Vol.
2. New York: Academic Press, 1965. pp. 97-126.

Stevenson, I., & Wolpe, J. Recovery from sexual


deviations through overcoming of non-sexual
neurotic responses. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 1960, 116, 737-742.

Strahley, D. F. Systematic desensitization and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1590
counterphobic treatment of an irrational fear of
snakes. Dissertation Abstracts, 1966, 27, 973B.

Terrace, H. S. Stimulus control. In W. K. Honig (Ed.),


Operant behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1966. pp. 271-344.

Trapold, M. A., & Winokur, S. Transfer from classical


conditioning and extinction to acquisition,
extinction, and stimulus generalization of a
positively reinforced instrumental response.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 73,
517-525.

Valins, S., & Ray, A. A. Effects of cognitive


desensitization on avoidance behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 7, 345-
350.

Vinogradov, N. V. Ob okhranitel’ nom i “zastoinom”


tormozheinii u bol’ nykh shizofreniei. [Protective
and “stagnant” inhibition in schizophrenic
patients.] Zhurnal vysshei nervnoi diatel’ nosti
imeni, 1962, 12, 426-431.

Walton, D., & Mather, M. D. The relevance of


generalization techniques to the treatment of
stammering and phobic symptoms. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1963, 1, 121-125. (a)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1591
Walton, D., & Mather, M. D. The application of learning
principles to the treatment of obsessive-
compulsive states in the acute and chronic
phases of illness. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1963, 1, 163-174. (b)

Weinberg, N. H., & Zaslove, M. “Resistance” to


systematic desensitization of phobias. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 1963, 19, 179-181.

Wolpe, J. Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition.


Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1958.

Wolpe, J. The systematic desensitization treatment of


neuroses. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
1961, 132, 189-203.

Wolpe, J. Isolation of a conditioning procedure as the


crucial psychotherapeutic factor: a case study.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1962, 134,
316-329.

Wolpe, J. Behaviour therapy in complex neurotic


states. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1964, 110,
28-34.

Wolpe, J., & Lazarus, A. A. Behavior therapy techniques.


New York: Pergamon, 1966.

Wolpin, M., & Raines, J. Visual imagery, expected roles


and extinction as possible factors in reducing fear

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1592
and avoidance behavior. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1966, 4, 25-37.

Wynne, L. C., & Solomon, R. L. Traumatic avoidance


learning: Acquisition and extinction in dogs
deprived of normal peripheral autonomic
function. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 1955,
52, 241-284.

Zeisset, R. M. Desensitization and relaxation in the


modification of psychiatric patients’ interview
behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1968
73, 18-24.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1593
8

Aversive Counterconditioning
In the preceding chapter it was shown how
threatening events can be neutralized by

associating them with positive experiences. Some

forms of psychological dysfunctions reflect a

converse problem, resulting from the fact that

certain activities or objects, which are potentially

harmful or socially prohibited, have acquired


unusually potent reinforcing value for the
individual. These deviations generally take the

form of addiction to various drugs or intoxicants,


or marked sexual attraction to inappropriate

stimuli as manifested in transvestism, fetishism,

exhibitionism, homosexuality, and other aberrant


sexual expressions. Attempts are sometimes made

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1594
to control such behavior by developing
conditioned aversion to the positively reinforcing

stimuli by pairing them contiguously with

negative experiences. A variety of methods has


been devised for producing conditioned aversion.

These have been applied mainly to persons who


wish to gain control over intractable behavior
which can produce serious long-term

consequences for them.

The classical conditioning approach to the


elimination of aberrant response patterns should

be distinguished from aversive instrumental

conditioning, discussed in Chapter 5, in which

response tendencies are inhibited by having

punishing consequences follow the occurrence of


the behavior. In the classical conditioning
paradigm, aversive events are stimulus correlated

for the purpose of altering the valence of stimuli,

whereas in the instrumental procedure, negative

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1595
outcomes are response correlated so as to inhibit

the performance of deviant responses.

Punishment that is made contingent on the


appearance of undesired behavior may

temporarily suppress its occurrence, but if the

positive stimuli that evoked the behavior are

absent during the punishment they are likely to

retain their attractiveness. Although the classical


and the instrumental approaches can be easily
differentiated operationally, the latter procedure

often produces some classical conditioning effects.

When a given behavior is punished, stimuli arising

from the punished response itself, and


environmental events present at the time, may

become endowed with negative properties. Thus,


for example, in treatment programs in which

aversive stimulation is presented to alcoholics in

the act of drinking alcoholic beverages, and to


transvestites while they are putting on ladies’

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1596
undergarments, the designation of the procedure

is somewhat arbitrary.

Development of Conditioned Aversion and


Avoidance

The basic process involved in the development

of conditioned aversion and avoidance has been

discussed in some detail in Chapter 5. Briefly, an


object or an activity that is repeatedly associated

with aversive experiences acquires some of the

negative properties of the aversive stimulus. As


long as the negatively conditioned stimuli retain

their aversive effects, the individual will be


inclined to avoid them.

In most laboratory investigations of

conditioned aversion, formerly neutral stimuli are

endowed with negative properties. Of greater

relevance to issues of behavioral change, however,

are studies showing that aversion can be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1597
established to strongly preferred stimuli (Garcia,
Kimeldorf, & Koelling, 1955; Masserman, 1943;

Peacock & Watson, 1964). Although the


phenomenon has been demonstrated, there has

been no systematic research to determine the

optimal conditions for creating persistent

aversions.

UNCONDITIONED STIMULI IN AVERSIVE


COUNTERCONDITIONING

Until recent years, most applications of

aversive counterconditioning have utilized

nauseous pharmacological agents to create the


requisite negative conditions. The specific

procedures followed in this form of therapy are

well illustrated by Lavin, Thorpe, Barker,

Blakemore, & Conway (1961) in their treatment of


a male transvestite. The client, a 22-year-old

married truck driver, frequently dressed in


women’s clothing, a pattern of behavior that began

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1598
in early childhood when he occasionally wore his
sister’s dresses. After puberty he experienced

orgasms when dressed in female apparel, and


masturbated to the accompaniment of transvestite

fantasies. Cross-sex dressing had become a

powerful sexual reinforcer, but the fear of serious

legal consequences, since he occasionally


appeared in public in feminine attire, and

pressures from his wife, led the client to seek

treatment.

This case illustrates careful application of

several of the requirements for successful

counterconditioning. In selecting the conditioned

stimulus it is necessary to determine what specific


aspects of the stimulus events are most positively

valenced. In this particular case, for example, it


was noted that the texture or feel of women’s

clothing produced no sexual excitement, but


viewing himself in a mirror dressed in feminine

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1599
apparel was highly stimulating. It was decided,

therefore, to employ a series of colored slides of

the client in various stages of female dress,


ranging from panties only to fully clothed, in order

to condition negative reactions to all aspects of

cross-sex dressing. In addition, a tape recording of

the client describing the transvestite behavior

depicted (e.g., “I have now put on and am wearing


a pair of ladies’ panties”) accompanied each slide.
The auditory cues were primarily designed to

augment the conditioned stimulus, to ensure the

presence of the stimulus when the client might not

be attending to the pictorial displays, and to


facilitate generalization effects.

Intramuscular injections of apomorphine


hydrochloride served as the unconditioned

stimulus, although occasionally emetine

hydrochloride was substituted after the client’s

tolerance to apomorphine had increased. As soon

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1600
as he reported feeling nauseous, a slide was

projected onto the screen and simultaneously the

tape recorder was started; the pictorial and


auditory stimuli were promptly terminated after

emesis. The client received 66 emetic trials, one

every two hours, for six days. As aversive

conditioning progressed, transvestism assumed

sufficiently unpleasant properties that the client


no longer derived any erotic satisfaction from
cross-sex dressing. A series of follow-up,

interviews with both the client and his wife

revealed complete cessation of transvestism, and

continued indifference to clothes that had


previously excited him.

Later sections of this chapter review numerous


studies in which emetic drugs, usually

apomorphine or emetine, have been employed to

create aversion. There are, however, many

disadvantages in pharmacological procedures that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1601
place certain limitations on their utility and

applicability.

Stimuli contiguous with the onset of aversive


experiences gradually acquire negative properties,

whereas stimuli associated with reduction or

termination of discomfort may actually acquire a

palliative function (Mowrer, 1960). One must,

therefore, exercise precise control over the timing


and sequence of stimulus variables in order to
ensure that positively reinforcing events are

accruing negative, rather than discomfort-relief,

value. The optimal temporal requirements are


difficult to achieve with the use of biochemical
agents because of the gradual, and often

unpredictable, onset of physiological reactions. As


a consequence, the presentation of conditioned

stimuli is typically delayed until some time after

persons begin to exhibit nauseous reactions or

other signs of mounting discomfort. In addition to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1602
the temporal issue, it has been shown (Fromer &

Berkowitz, 1964) that aversive stimuli with a

gradual onset produce significantly weaker


aversion responses than those with a sudden

onset. Since there is no effective way of

terminating abruptly drug- induced aversive

states, the nauseous reactions are needlessly

prolonged. Moreover, because they also tend to


subside gradually, the therapist lacks reliable
criteria for timing the withdrawal of conditioned

stimuli to prevent their association with the

reduction of discomfort.

Apart from the problems created by


inadequate control over the rate of onset,

duration, intensity, and recovery time of drug


activity, undesirable physiological side-effects are

sometimes produced, requiring the administration

of additional medicinals. Some of the actions of

drugs that accompany, but are unrelated to, the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1603
effects for which they are being administered may,

of course, impair the conditioning process itself.

Central depressants have been shown to decrease


conditioning, whereas stimulant drugs facilitate

the formation of conditioned responses (Franks,

1966). Emetine is, therefore, generally favored

because apomorphine has sedative effects. When

the latter drug is employed, stimulants such as


dexamphetomine sulphate (Lavin et ah, 1961) or
caffeine (Freund, 1960) are usually administered

to counteract its depressant action. The ideal

pharmacological agent would be a stimulant drug

that produces brief and readily controllable emetic


responses and is free of adverse side effects.

Although emetine is more suitable since it does


not possess depressant properties, its frequent

administration may result in cardiovascular

sequelae (Barker, Thorpe, Blakemore, Lavin, &


Conway, 1961). Finally, the use of pharmacological

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1604
methods necessitates hospitalization and imposes

certain limitations on the manner in which the

conditioned stimuli can be presented.

Aversive electrical stimulation has been

increasingly employed in aversion treatment,


mainly because it permits precise control over

conditioning variables (Rachman, 1965). Aversive

shocks can be presented and terminated abruptly;


they can be easily varied in duration and intensity;
also, except in the case of cardiac disorders, they

produce no adverse physiological effects. This

increased control makes it possible not only to

arrange optimal temporal relationships between


conditioned and unconditioned stimuli but also to
conduct numerous aversion trials within the same

session. The treatment can even be self-

administered in the naturalistic situations in

which the problem behavior is evoked.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1605
During conditioning trials moderately severe

shocks are administered either to the forearm

(Kushner & Sandler, 1966) or to the feet


(Blakemore, Thorpe, Barker, Conway, & Lavin,

1963) of the client in conjunction with the elicitors


of the undesired behavior. A number of shock
trials are presented during each of the sessions,

which are scheduled over a period of a week or

two.

It is sometimes difficult to introduce into

treatment the stimulus events for deviant

behavior in the forms and intensities in which they

are ordinarily encountered in everyday situations.


Consequently, aversion reactions may be
conditioned to verbal, pictorial, or imaginal

representations of the actual stimulus objects,

with the hope that sufficient transfer will occur to

inhibit approach tendencies to the real-life

counterparts. McGuire & Vallance (1964) have

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1606
devised a portable electric stimulation apparatus

that makes it possible for the client, after some

preliminary training, to carry out his own


counterconditioning in naturalistic settings

whenever he feels impelled to perform the deviant

behavior.

The utilization of the self-conditioning

procedure is illustrated in the treatment of a 25-


year-old graduate student who had been
masturbating to fetishistic fantasies with

considerable guilt about three times a day for 10

years. The client began aversion therapy after


having participated in conventional treatment,
which had reduced neither his fetishistic

masturbatory behavior nor the attendant guilt


feelings. In the initial phase of treatment, the client

was asked to produce the usual fantasies and to

signal by raising his hand when the fetish objects

were clearly visualized, at which time a shock was

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1607
administered. At later sessions, when the client

was unable to conjure up the sexually provocative

fantasies, photographs of persons attired in the


fetishistic clothing were employed as the

conditioned stimuli. In addition, he was

encouraged to use the conditioning apparatus at

home whenever he felt instigated to masturbate.

The fetishistic fantasies were completely


eliminated within a short period, the incidence of
masturbatory behavior was substantially reduced,

and on the occasions when the client did

masturbate, for the first time in his life this

behavior was accompanied by heterosexual


fantasies. The authors report favorable outcomes

in the use of the self-conditioning procedure for


reducing obsessional ruminations, obesity,

smoking, and alcoholism. Similarly, Wolpe (1965)

was able to achieve temporary control over drug-


addictive behavior in a physician by having him

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1608
administer to himself a shock from a portable

apparatus whenever craving for the drug

appeared.

In the methods discussed thus far, aversive

counterconditioning is accomplished through


repeated association of pharmacologically created

nausea or unpleasant electrical stimulation. A

much more interesting approach, that has


numerous advantages over noxious physical
stimuli, involves symbolically induced aversion. In

this form of counterconditioning, positively

valenced events are repeatedly paired with strong

feelings of nausea and emetic responses which are


verbally induced. The negative verbal contents are
usually drawn from disagreeable, painful, or

revolting experiences that have previously arisen

either in connection with the pleasurable objects

and activities or in other contexts. As in the other

paradigms, the conditioning trials are continued

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1609
until the formerly positive stimuli alone elicit

feelings of revulsion.

Miller (1959, 1963) provides several


illustrations of the successful use of symbolic

aversion methods in modifying homosexuality and

alcoholism. Most homosexuals have experienced

specific disgust reactions, at one time or another,

in intimate relationships with certain male


partners. During treatment hypnotically revivified
nauseous reactions, which the client has

experienced in previous homosexual contacts, are

repeatedly associated with visualized homosexual


practices involving current male companions. One
client, for example, had felt strong revulsion to the

smell and taste of urine and stale perspiration


while performing fellatio with an uncircumcised

male. These past experiences were, therefore,

employed as aversive verbal stimuli in the

treatment. The author reports that after several

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1610
sessions the client became nauseated by his male

lovers and eventually broke off all homosexual

contacts.

Avoidance responses established in this

manner are typically reinforced by supplementary

conditioning trials at monthly intervals for the

first year. In addition to attaching negative valence

to homoerotic responses and love objects,


symbolic counterconditioning procedures are also
employed to enhance the positive reinforcing

value of heterosexual stimuli. In the preceding

illustrative case the client participated


concurrently in a number of sessions in which
certain feminine attributes that he found desirable

were hypnotically augmented and associated with


women. Although on the few occasions when the

client had formerly dated girls he had selected

masculine types, following the

counterconditioning treatment he was attracted

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1611
to, and dated, women possessing notable feminine

attributes.

In aversion treatment of alcoholism, which is


reviewed later, verbally revivified hangover

experiences are contiguously associated with the

smell and taste of alcoholic beverages. Essentially

similar symbolic aversion techniques have been

applied on a limited basis to the treatment of


obesity (Cautela, 1966), alcoholism (Abrams,
1964; Anant, 1967), addictions, and sexual

perversions (Kolvin, 1967). The major advantages

of this technique are that it has no adverse side


effects, it is highly adaptable, and people can be
taught to administer the treatment to themselves

in the naturalistic situations in which their


problem behavior is apt to arise.

PROCESS GOVERNING CONDITIONED AVERSION

The manner in which aversive procedures are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1612
employed and the durability of resultant aversions
are likely to be influenced to a considerable extent

by one’s conceptualization of the mechanism

through which aversive stimulation produces its


effects. Most traditional accounts of

counterconditioning convey the impression that,


as a result of paired association with negative
experiences, formerly positive stimuli directly and

automatically evoke aversive reactions. The

temporal relationship between stimulus events is


therefore considered to be a major determinant of

the strength and durability of conditioned

aversions. As will be shown in the following

chapter, this nonmediational view is at variance

with certain experimental findings. It has been


demonstrated, for example, that both conditioned
autonomic and avoidance responses promptly

disappear when shock electrodes are removed or

subjects are merely informed that a given stimulus

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1613
will no longer be accompanied by painful

stimulation. Moreover, conditioned emotional

responses can be established cognitively without


the immediate support of external aversive

stimulation. Since in aversion treatments the


negative experiences associated with pleasurable
activities are arbitrarily rather than intrinsically

related to the behavior, individuals can readily

discriminate that in everyday life the same


activities will not only be unaccompanied by

unpleasant consequences but may, in fact, prove

highly rewarding. Given cognitive control over


conditioning effects and markedly different

situational contingencies, one might expect


conditioned aversions to be readily extinguishable

and to show little transfer from treatment to real-

life situations. On the other hand, there is


considerable evidence that established revulsions

usually generalize across situations and that they

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1614
can be relatively long lasting.

An alternative view of counterconditioning


effects is that external stimuli acquire the capacity

to activate a self-stimulation mechanism which, in

turn, produces the aversive reactions. Thus, for


example, after a person has repeatedly

experienced strong nausea in conjunction with

alcoholic beverages the mere sight or smell of


alcohol leads him to revivify his past nauseous
experiences. In this conceptualization aversive

reactions are, in large part, self-induced rather

than automatically evoked. If the aversive self-

stimulation established through


counterconditioning is potent enough, a person
may be able to counteract the disposition to

engage in deviant behavior by symbolically

reinstating nauseous reactions whenever the need

arises.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1615
The manner in which counterconditioning is

applied will differ in several important respects

depending upon whether it is viewed as a form of


automatic immunization or as a technique of self-

control. In the former case, methods are favored


that permit precise management and split-second
timing of stimulus events. In the latter approach,

on the other hand, the procedures are designed to

develop strong and readily recreatable aversions


to certain objects or activities. For this purpose,

verbal and pharmacological agents may

regenerate more natural and symbolically


reproducible aversions than electric shock, the

physiological manifestations of which are


relatively subtle. An optimal procedure might

initially involve the combined use of verbal

induction and either emetic drugs or shocks to


create vivid aversive reactions. In subsequent

sessions, however, verbal stimuli alone would be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1616
used as the conditioning agent although they

might be paired occasionally with emetic drugs or


shock to preserve their potency. After an aversive

self-stimulation system had been established in

sufficient strength, individuals would be


instructed to avoid engaging in the deviant

behavior by deliberately inducing nauseous


reactions. When conditioned aversions are
regarded as self-induced reactions rather than as

automatic products of stimulus pairings, the

change agent assumes greater responsibility for

arranging positive incentives to ensure that


individuals utilize this potentially effective means

of self-control. In a comprehensive treatment


program this practice would, of course, be used in

conjunction with other methods of self-control, as

well as procedures designed to reduce the

instigation to engage in the deviant behavior.

When some persons who have undergone

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1617
aversive treatment later revert to their deviant

activities these outcomes are often attributed to

deficiencies inherent in the conditioning


procedure itself. Faulty timing and sequencing of

stimulus events, selection of inadequate aversive

agents, and conditionability deficits are typically

invoked as explanatory factors. Similarly,

recommendations for enhancing the efficacy of


this method single out conditioning variables.
These include the use of intermittent schedules of

reinforcement, continuation of the trials for a

sufficient period to ensure overlearning, and

inclusion of booster treatments at periodic


intervals after the formal program has been

discontinued (Eysenck, 1963). There exists


suggestive evidence (Voegtlin, Lemere, Broz, &

O’Hollaren, 1942) that conditioned aversion and

avoidance can be successfully maintained through


periodic reconditioning trials. Portable devices

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1618
that permit self-administration of aversive stimuli

and the judicious use of symbolically generated

consequences would undoubtedly also reduce the


disposition to engage in deviant behavior.

However, there is little reason to expect that


intermittent reinforcement would increase the
durability of aversive reactions. Partial

reinforcement retards the rate of extinction by

reducing discriminability of the occasions when


customary consequences will or will not occur

(Spence, 1966). Administration of reinforcements

in an unpredictable manner may produce stable


conditioned responses during treatment, but the

conditions of reinforcement prevailing in


treatment situations and in everyday life differ

markedly and are easily distinguishable. The

situational change would ordinarily result in a


rapid decrement in responsiveness unless

cognitive functions were utilized in a self-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1619
reinforcing system that could endure long after

conditioning influences were discontinued.

Induced aversions are generally less durable

than approach behavior that has been restored by

eliminating inhibitions and anxieties. This


differential reversion rate is most likely

attributable to the different maintaining

contingencies associated with these two forms of


behavior. In the case of behavioral conditions
involving fear, removal of avoidance responses

enables people to engage in many potentially

rewarding activities which, in turn, further

strengthen the newly established behavior. Initial


behavioral changes are thus apt to set in motion a
positive reciprocal influence process. By contrast,

sexual, appetitive, and addictive behavior produce

powerful immediate reinforcement, whereas any

aversive consequences that do occur are typically

long delayed. These types of controlling conditions

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1620
result in behavior that is not only refractory to

change but also easily reinstatable because of the

positive reinforcing effects that accompany its


recurrence. Aversion treatments attempt to

forestall the recurrence of deviant behavior by

developing immediate aversive reactions to the

stimuli that typically evoke the behavior.

The major value of aversion procedures is that


they provide a means of achieving control over
injurious behavior for a period of time during

which alternative, and more rewarding, modes of

response can be established and strengthened.


Used by itself, this method may bring about only
temporary suppression of deviant tendencies. The

answer to the reduction of reversion rates,


therefore, lies in the scope of the treatment

program employed rather than in variables

operating within the conditioning paradigm. This

issue will be discussed more fully in the context of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1621
the modification of specific behavior disorders.

STIMULUS EVENTS IN AVERSION TREATMENTS

The consequences of selecting inappropriate

stimulus events for aversion treatment are

considerably more serious than those for

desensitization approaches. In the latter case,


deconditioning an irrelevant stimulus merely

prolongs treatment, whereas the former


procedures can establish needless aversions and

avoidant behaviors. Although the risks associated

with aversive counterconditioning may be greater,


in the types of behavior disorders that are
customarily treated by these methods the

rewarding objects are easily identified (e.g.,

alcoholic beverages, specific drugs, fetishistic

objects, cross-sex apparel, homoerotic stimuli).

The major decisions, therefore, concern the

variety of conditioned stimuli and the forms in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1622
which they will be presented.

If inadequately applied, aversive procedures

can have generalized inhibitory effects. Control of


generalization is therefore an important issue. A

change agent is faced with the task of presenting

aversive stimuli sufficiently intense to arouse


strong aversion, while simultaneously confining
generalization to a particular class of activities. To

achieve this dual objective, the stimulus events


must be distinctive and carefully delimited.

Applications of aversion therapy to the

modification of homosexuality will serve to

illustrate the latter point. Typically, pictures of

nude, semi-nude, and fully clothed males are


contiguously associated with unpleasant

experiences. In these instances, aversions are


established to a general stimulus class so that

some undesired generalization may occur, but the


most relevant stimulus events (i.e., specific

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1623
homosexual practices) are not included in the

conditioning process. Hence, there is no assurance

that the negative conditioning will necessarily


transfer extensively from pictures of men to

specific homosexual responses toward them.

Clearly, stronger but circumscribed transfer would

be achieved if the stimulus presentations

contained all aspects of homosexuality that the


treatment is designed to eliminate.

The problem of negative transfer can be even

more complicated in cases where the existing

behavior is entirely satisfactory, but the evoking


stimuli are so bizarre and disturbing to others that
attempts are made to nullify their arousal

capabilities through aversive procedures. This


situation arises most frequently in the treatment

of heterosexual fetishists and transvestites who, in

order to obtain an erection and to engage in sexual

intercourse, must wear their wives’ clothing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1624
(Blakemore et al., 1963), rubberized mackintoshes

(Oswald, 1962), be kicked with rubber boots or

high-heeled shoes (Marks, Rachman, & Gelder,


1965). Such unique modes of erotic arousal create

serious marital conflicts, even conditioning

revulsions in sexual partners. A similar problem

exists when efforts are made to negate the sex-

arousing value of self-flagellating, fetishistic, and


sadistic fantasies that arouse masturbatory or
heterosexual behavior without suppressing the

behavior itself. It is of considerable import that in

many of the published cases more appropriate

sexual fantasies emerge as the arousal potential of


bizarre elicitors is eliminated, and desirable sexual

behavior is either maintained at its original level


or further enhanced (Blakemore et al., 1963;

Cooper, 1963; Kushner & Sandler, 1966; Marks &

Gelder, 1967; McGuire & Vallance, 1964; Oswald,


1962; Raymond, 1956).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1625
The direction and extent of generalization of

aversion effects can be regulated through a

program of differential reinforcement in which


undesirable events are repeatedly associated with

negative experiences, while the desired ones are


paired with either rewarding or no adverse
consequences. Verbal labeling can also be utilized

effectively both to delimit what is being negatively

conditioned and to enhance the most relevant


elements in the stimulus complex. To continue

with the above example, pictorial stimuli could be

supplemented with descriptions of the client,


recorded in his voice, engaging in anal and

interfemoral intercourse, oral-genital relations,


mutual manual masturbation, and other forms of

erotic responsiveness to males.

It seems reasonable to expect that, as in the

case of desensitization, the more inclusive the

representation of relevant stimuli in the aversion

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1626
program, and the more similar they are to real-life

events, the more potent will be the

counterconditioning outcomes. This practice is


generally followed in the treatment of fetishism,

transvestism, and the various addictions because,

in each of these disorders, the attractive stimuli

can be easily included in the treatment

procedures. To further enhance the visual stimuli,


tape recordings are sometimes played in which
clients describe themselves engaging in the

deviant behavior, or they are instructed to develop

mental imagery involving the undesired activities.

In aversion treatment of compulsive gambling


behavior, Barker & Miller (1966) and Goorney

(1968) employed newspaper items, sound


broadcasts, televised presentations, and actual

gambling devices that ordinarily served as the

evocative stimuli.

There is some evidence based on the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1627
modification of alcoholism (Lemere & Voegtlin,

1940; Quinn & Henbest, 1967) and sexual

deviations (Marks & Gelder, 1967) that when a


single stimulus is employed in aversive

counterconditioning, the resultant aversion

reactions may be highly specific to that particular

stimulus class. Consequently, a wide sampling of

stimuli is employed when, as in the treatment of


alcoholism, a generalized aversion is required.

Sexual Deviance

There exists considerable cross-cultural

evidence (Ford & Beach, 1951) that in societies in


which homosexuality and transvestism are
socially disapproved, sexual inversions rarely

occur; by contrast, cross-sex dressing, anal

intercourse, oral-genital contacts, and mutual

masturbation are regularly practiced by most

members of societies in which such behavior is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1628
fully sanctioned and positively reinforced.

Whereas in our own society inflicting pain on a

sexual partner is regarded as a sadistic perversion,


in other cultures pain cues, resulting from

aggression that normally accompanies coital

activities, serve as powerful sexual reinforcers

(Holmberg, 1946; Malinowski, 1929). There are

also wide cultural variations in the physical


attributes and adornments that become culturally
conditioned sexual stimuli. What has been

endowed with erotic arousal properties in one

society—corpulence, skinniness, upright

hemispherical breasts or long pendulous ones,


shiny white teeth or black pointed ones, deformed

ears, nose, or lips, broad pelvis and wide hips or


narrow pelvis and slim hips, light or dark skin

color—may be neutral or highly repulsive to

members of another social group. These cross-


cultural data showing the range of preferred

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1629
sexual reinforcers are striking testimony of the

influential role of social learning in the

development of sexual behavior that may be


judged deviant by some social group.

Although our society imposes severe social and


legal prohibitions against some forms of behavior

that are believed to have sexual implications,

certain members may nevertheless experience


unusual reinforcement and modeling influences
serving to promote and to maintain deviant sexual

behavior. A major obstacle to the understanding of

human sexual deviance is that, for ethical reasons,

experimentation designed to identify the


conditions governing sexual phenomena cannot be
conducted. Consequently, the search for the

relevant controlling variables must rely on

naturalistic studies. A number of clinical reports

have been published containing data that illustrate

the social learning processes whereby culturally

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1630
inappropriate stimuli and responses acquire

unusually strong sexual reinforcing properties.

Litin, Giffin, & Johnson (1956) describe the


development of transvestism in a young boy who

continually dressed up in his mother’s clothes,

including cosmetics and jewelry, exhibited almost

complete feminine-role behavior, and even

adopted a girl’s name which his mother suggested.


Cross-sex dressing first appeared following an
episode in which the mother, in response to her

son’s comment that she looked pretty in her new

shoes, hugged him and offered him her old shoes.


He wore these shoes daily, eliciting considerable
maternal approbation. The mother continued to

encourage and reward sex-inappropriate behavior


with demonstrations of affection and approval,

while the grandmother and neighbors

supplemented the mother’s training in

transvestism by supplying the boy with generous

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1631
quantities of old shoes, hats, purses, bridal veils,

and other female apparel. When the boy’s inverted

behavior met with disapproval from other


persons, the mother attempted some

discrimination training by informing her son, “You

must never dress like that in front of company,

only in front of the family.” In a study of the

mothers and wives of 32 transvestites, Stoller


(1967) found that the subjects were initiated into
transvestism by being dressed in girls’ clothing or

highly rewarded whenever they dressed

themselves in feminine apparel. The transvestite

behavior was further elaborated by mothers and


wives who devoted many hours to teaching the

subjects how to dress in feminine clothing, how to


apply cosmetics, and how to behave as women.

Litin, Giffin, & Johnson (1956) depict how a

mother actively conditioned voyeuristic behavior

in her son by sleeping with him, and by being

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1632
physically and verbally seductive while appearing

nude before him. When the boy was six years old,

the mother had shown him her vagina a number of


times, but she later discontinued her physically

seductive behavior after the son suggested that

they engage in sexual intercourse. The boy’s

strongly established voyeuristic behavior had

generalized to the maid and other persons;


eventually he was apprehended by the police
while peeping from a ladder into neighborhood

bedrooms.

Generalization of strongly reinforced


homoerotic responses is illustrated in the case of a
16-year-old girl whose mother would lie in bed

with her, encouraging mutual stroking of the


breasts, and other erotic play. The mother sought

psychiatric advice when she became jealous of her

daughter’s homosexual attachment to a teacher.

Homosexual patterns of behavior are not always

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1633
fostered in such a blatant fashion. In many

instances, the father serves as an inadequate

model for masculine behavior and the distribution


of social power within the family constellation

promotes cross-sex modeling (Whitener & Nikelly,

1964). Sex-appropriate behavior is nonrewarded

and, when the parents encourage peer

relationships, homosexual associates tend to be


favored (Fleck, 1960; Kolb & Johnson, 1955).

A mother’s active reinforcement of deviant

sexual behavior is again evident in a 17-year-old

exhibitionist described by Giffin, Johnson, & Litin


(1954). The mother often showered with her son,
engaged in endless sex discussions, enjoyed

exhibiting herself to him, and delighted at looking


at the boy’s nude body, particularly his “beautiful

masculine endowment.” A dress fetish was

similarly conditioned in a ten-year-old boy by a

mother who responded demonstratively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1634
whenever the son stroked her dress or

complimented women on their attire (Johnson,

1953).

That erotic experiences can endow formerly

neutral stimuli with sexual arousal properties is

supported by results of an interesting study by

Rachman (1966), designed to create a mild fetish

under laboratory conditions. After a photograph of


women’s boots was repeatedly associated with
slides of sexually stimulating nude females,

subjects exhibited sexual arousal (as measured by

penile volume changes) to the boots alone, and


generalized the conditioned sexual responses to
other types of black shoes. In accord with these

findings, McGuire, Carlisle, & Young (1965) report


that deviant sexuality often develops through

masturbatory conditioning in which aberrant

sexual fantasies are endowed with strong erotic

value through repeated association with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1635
pleasurable experiences from masturbation. The

details of their interpretive scheme, and its

therapeutic implications, are discussed more fully


later.

The foregoing cases represent a small sample

of those documented in the reports to which

reference has been made. Three social learning

variables emerge from these naturalistic studies as


important determinants of deviant sexual
behavior. The first consideration involves the

degree to which parents themselves model

homosexual, transvestite, fetishistic, or


exhibitionistic behavioral patterns in either
blatant or attenuated forms. Second, once the

responses are elicited, either by direct instigation


or modeling, they are endowed with exaggerated

sexual significance and strong positive valence.

This may derive from repeated association with

relatively intense, affectionate demonstrativeness,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1636
with close physical intimacy, or from

masturbatory conditioning. Indeed, for many of

the children, inappropriate cues and responses


had acquired strong positive valence, and in some

cases had resulted in well-developed patterns of

sexual behavior long before the onset of

pubescence. Third, parents tend to maintain

children’s deviant sexual responses on an


instrumental basis over a long period, both
through direct and vicarious reinforcement.

Sexual responses that have acquired strong

positive valence can also become self-reinforcing


through their stress-reducing capabilities. The
diminution of aversive emotional states by

engaging in sexual behavior may reflect the


operation of two somewhat different

psychological processes. First, sexual activities can

produce sufficiently intense pleasurable

experiences to contravene feelings of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1637
apprehension or frustration. Secondly,

performance of sexual behavior also changes the

stimulus situation by temporarily directing the


person’s attention away from stress-provoking

events. This attentional shift may in itself produce

substantial anxiety relief.

Evidence of the stress-reducing function of

deviant sexual responses is disclosed in a report


published by Cooper (1963). The client
characteristically experienced heightened sexual

arousal in response to tactual sensations from

silken garments. Dressed in women’s clothing, he


would frequently masturbate to the point of
orgasm by straddling a chair. Although initially the

transvestite behavior served a sexual function, it


later acquired, through an accidental contingency,

generalized anxiety-terminating value. One day

while experiencing considerable apprehension

regarding a scholastic examination, the client

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1638
discovered that cross-sex dressing resulted in an

abrupt diminution in anxiety. Thereafter, a wide

range of stressful situations elicited transvestite


behavior. Several other authors (Bond &

Hutchison, 1964; Conn, 1954) have noted a similar

behavioral sequence in which mounting tension is

abruptly reduced by the execution of deviant

sexual responses. These observational data would


seem to indicate that deviant sexual behavior of an
unusually persistent character is probably

maintained not only by sexual reinforcement but

also by its tension-reducing effects.

In applications of aversive counterconditioning


to sexual disorders, attempts are made to reverse

the sexual arousal value of appropriate and


inappropriate stimuli through differential

conditioning procedures.

EFFICACY OF CONDITIONED AVERSION


APPROACHES WITH SEXUAL DISORDERS

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1639
There have been no controlled studies of the

relative efficacy of aversion treatments for

modifying deviant sexual behavior, but numerous


case studies are available which have some

evidential value. These reported outcomes, while


very interesting, must be accepted with
reservation for several reasons. Many of the

treatment programs involve a combination of

methods, which makes it difficult to assess the


relative contribution of the aversion procedures.

Moreover, changes in sexual response patterns are

typically measured in terms of clients’ self-reports,


although substantiating information is obtained

from spouses and close associates whenever


possible.

Several objective tests of sexual arousal have

been developed which are beginning to be

employed to measure progress during treatment,

the degree of alteration in sexual arousal patterns

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1640
at the completion of therapy, and their stability

over time. One of the laboratory procedures,

originally devised by Freund (1963; Freund,


Sedlacek, & Knob, 1965), measures, by means of a

transducer, penile volume changes in response to

pictures of males and females of varying ages, or

to other erotic objects. Several validational studies

(Freund, 1967a, b; McConaghy, 1967) have shown


that this measure can successfully differentiate
between persons with homo- or hetero-erotic

preferences for adults, adolescents, and children.

Another quantitative index of the attraction


value of visual stimuli is provided in terms of
changes in pupillary size. In this technique (Hess,

1968) a subject’s eye is filmed at the rate of two


frames per second while he views test items

alternated with control patterns equated for

brightness. The film is later magnified and the

diameter of the pupil size is measured manually

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1641
frame by frame or electronically with photocells. It

has been shown that pupillary dilation occurs in

response to stimuli that have high interest value,


whereas negatively valenced stimuli produced

pupillary constriction. Findings of a pilot study

(Hess, Seltzer, & Shlien, 1965), in which pupillary

responses of homosexuals and heterosexuals were

measured to pictures of males and females, verify


that this procedure can differentiate sexual
preferences with considerable accuracy. All

heterosexuals displayed larger responses to

pictures of women than to pictures of men,

whereas homosexuals showed the reverse pattern.


The authors suggest that standard sets of pictorial

stimuli could be devised to provide reliable


measures of sexual attraction. However, aversive

conditioning may be difficult to evaluate through

pupillometric measures because pupillary


responses could represent either sexual interest

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1642
or anxiety arousal conditioned to sexual stimuli.

This interpretive problem does not arise with

respect to erectile responses.

Although it is customary to question the value

of case studies, because sexual deviations are


exceedingly resistant to change, favorable results

cannot be casually dismissed. Deviant sexual

patterns rarely change “spontaneously,” and they


have proved equally unresponsive to the planned
efforts of psychotherapists employing varied

strategies (Curran & Parr, 1957; Woodward,

1958). The fact that most persons receiving

aversion therapy have undergone traditional


forms of treatment for extended periods of time
without any degree of success makes it unlikely

that the behavioral modifications accompanying

aversive counterconditioning are attributable to

interpersonal and cognitive variables that are

prominent in conversational therapies. The nature

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1643
and brevity of the treatment further limit the

opportunity for social factors to exert a strong

influence on the outcomes.

The individual case method can be effectively

employed to assess the functional relationship

between treatment variables and behavioral

changes. It was previously shown how

intrasubject replication furnishes a means of


evaluating the role of reinforcement variables in
behavioral processes. In their detailed

measurement of changes accompanying aversive

counterconditioning, Marks & Gelder (1967;


Gelder & Marks, 1969) provide an excellent model
that other researchers would do well to emulate. A

large number of transvestites and fetishists


received aversion treatment in which shocks were

administered while they carried out their deviant

behavior or imagined themselves performing the

same activities. In each case, the process of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1644
treatment was studied by recording changes in

sexual arousal through the use of a penis

transducer. The clients’ attitudes to the objects of


their deviation, to sexual intercourse, to

themselves, and to the therapist were also

measured by the evaluative scales of the semantic

differential. In addition, progressive changes in the

latency of sexual images and accompanying


erectile responses were recorded. Different
aspects of the clients’ deviant behavior were

modified one at a time to determine whether the

observed changes reflected the operation of

general factors present in any treatment or the


specific counterconditioning procedures.

Figure 8-1 depicts changes during the course


of aversion therapy in both the frequency and

latency of erectile responses of a transvestite to

different feminine garments that he regularly used

in cross-dressing. At the beginning of treatment he

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1645
1646
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 8-1. Changes in the frequency and latency of erectile responses of a transvestite to
different feminine garments as they were included serially in aversive
counterconditioning. Marks & Gelder, 1967.
was highly aroused sexually by all of the items.

The aversive procedures were then applied to one

garment at a time beginning with panties. After


approximately 20 trials the client no longer

showed any erectile responses to panties, but he

responded with undiminished sexual excitement

to the remaining articles of clothing. As the other

items were serially counterconditioned they also


lost their sex- arousing capacity. It is important to
note, however, that the client maintained high

sexual responsiveness to appropriate heterosexual

stimuli after deviant sources of sexual arousal

were eliminated. The specific sequence of


behavioral changes obtained by repeated

application of aversive stimulation provides


convincing demonstrations that the alteration in

sexual arousal was indeed produced by the

conditioning procedure. In the cases studied,


sexual reconditioning was followed by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1647
corresponding elimination of deviant sexual

desires and activities.

The findings of the above study not only attest

to the efficacy of aversive conditioning, but they

also help to clarify the stimulus function of


symbolic processes and their modification. Prior

to treatment, imagery of the deviant behavior

elicited strong erectile responses. As these


fantasies were repeatedly associated with
unpleasant experiences the latency of the images

increased until eventually they could be produced

only with considerable difficulty (Figure 8-1).

Moreover, the erections which originally


accompanied the fantasies diminished gradually
with successive trials and eventually disappeared

even when the images could be produced. The

selective effect of the treatment is again shown in

the fact that clients were able to conjure up

arousing heterosexual imagery with ease.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1648
The pattern of attitudinal change

corresponded closely to the modifications

achieved in sexual arousal. Sexually attractive


garments were selectively devalued as the specific

objects were negatively conditioned, whereas self-


attitudes, and general attitudes toward sexual
intercourse and toward therapists, changed little

during treatment. The consistent selective changes

achieved in erectile responses, in the arousal


properties of deviant fantasies, in attitudes, and in

overt sexual behavior, would indicate that in cases

where aversive conditioning is adequately applied


the outcomes are at least partly attributable to the

conditioning experiences.

Findings of controlled individual studies by

Barlow, Leitenberg, & Agras (1969), in which a

pedophile and a homosexual were treated with

symbolic aversion in a replicative design, lend

further support to the contribution of contiguous

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1649
experiences to observed changes. The clients’

autonomic and evaluative responses to deviant

sexual stimuli and the frequency of their sexual


urges were measured during periods when

sexually arousing imagery was successively paired

with verbally induced nausea or occurred alone.

Deviant sexual arousal declined sharply during the

aversive conditioning phase, it gradually increased


when sexually arousing scenes were repeatedly
presented without any aversive experiences, and it

was virtually eliminated after the aversive

contingency was reinstated.

Aversion therapy has produced many marked


and enduring changes in transvestism (Blakemore

et ah, 1963; Cooper, 1963; Glynn & Harper, 1961;


Lavin et al., 1961; Marks & Gelder, 1967;

Morgenstern, Pearce, & Rees, 1965); in fetishism

(Kushner & Sandler, 1966; Marks, Rachman, &

Gelder, 1965; McGuire & Vallance, 1964; Oswald,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1650
1962; Raymond, 1956; Raymond & O’Keeffe, 1965;

Thorpe, Schmidt, Brown, & Castell, 1964); in

exhibitionism (Evans, 1967; Kushner & Sandler,


1966); and in homosexuality (Costello, 1963;

Freund, 1960; James, 1962; Max, 1935; Miller,

1963; Thorpe et al., 1964). In other cases involving

similar disorders, aversion therapy effected

temporary cessations or reductions in deviant


sexual behavior (Clark, 1963a, b; Freund, 1960;
Oswald, 1962; Thorpe, Schmidt, & Castell, 1963).

Still other clients have derived little benefit from

this mode of treatment (Freund, 1960; Solyom &

Miller, 1965).

It is difficult to identify the factors responsible

for the differential efficacy of aversion methods


because the cases involve variations in motivation

for change, in aversive stimuli, in the extent to

which heterosexual attractions are established, in

length of follow-up, in degree and duration of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1651
homosexual behavior, and in the extent to which

favorable conditions for heterosexual activities

exist within the environment. The influential role


of some of these factors is revealed in studies

containing a sufficient number of cases to compare

outcome rates. Freund (1960), for example, found

that for homosexuals who were self-referred,

aversion therapy produced enduring heterosexual


orientations in approximately 45 percent of the
cases, and 16 percent adopted predominantly

heterosexual patterns for at least some period of

time. By contrast, only 6 percent of those who

were coerced into treatment by legal authorities


and relatives became exclusively heterosexual.

These data, in accord with the self-arousal


interpretation of counterconditioning presented

earlier, show how motivational factors may

counteract the effects that customarily result from


aversive stimulus pairing. Reluctant individuals

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1652
can impede the development of aversions by

failing to attend to the attractive stimuli and to

produce accompanying imagery. They can give


fake signals at crucial points in the procedure

where the therapist usually depends upon


guidance from the client. They could even reverse
the direction of counterconditioning by conjuring

up heterosexual imagery while undergoing

unpleasant stimulation. Finally, if aversion


reactions were established, they could be easily

extinguished by clients’ repeatedly engaging in

homosexual activities. Whatever the reasons


might be, the differential outcomes associated

with the desire to modify one’s sexual orientation


reaffirm the view that unless individuals are

committed to the selected objectives, their

behavior is likely to nullify the effects of change


programs.

McGuire, Carlisle, & Young (1965) advance the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1653
interesting thesis that in some cases deviant

sexual preferences are developed through

masturbatory conditioning. According to the


authors, three factors usually figure prominently

in this form of sexual learning. First, as a result of

unpleasant heterosexual experiences or feelings of

physical and social inadequacy, the person comes

to believe that he cannot achieve a normal sex life.


Second, the person usually has a sexual experience
that is not sufficient by itself to establish deviant

erotic preferences, but it stimulates a fantasy for

later masturbation. The major conditioning is

assumed to occur in relation to symbolic


representations. As the person repeatedly

masturbates to the fantasy as his exclusive sexual


outlet, the pleasurable experiences from

masturbation endow the deviant fantasy with

increasing erotic value. This is essentially the


same mechanism through which Rachman (1966)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1654
conditioned sexual excitement to shoes except

that instead of seductive photographs, the

arousing events are more powerful orgasmic


experiences.

The authors document their thesis with


extensive case data obtained from a large number

of sexual deviants. In one of these cases, for

example, a 17-year-old male was highly sexually


stimulated at seeing a girl dressed only in her
underwear. Thereafter, he frequently masturbated

to the mental imagery of the scantily clad girl.

Eventually the memory of the girl’s characteristics

faded, but advertisements and shop-window


displays of women’s undergarments continued to
serve as strong masturbatory fantasies. After a

period of several years the erotic potential of these

fetishistic objects had increased to the point

where he no longer showed interest in girls, but

rather derived his sexual stimulation almost

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1655
entirely from women’s underwear which he

bought or stole. This helps to explain how

outlandish fantasies can acquire powerful sexual


valence through contiguous association with

masturbatory experiences (Marks, Rachman, &

Gelder, 1965; Mees, 1966) and, once established,

why they are so refractory to extinction. Other

case reports by McGuire and his associates depict


a similar process in which aberrant sexual
fantasies are selectively reinforced; they

eventually become able to provoke corresponding

homosexual, exhibitionistic, and voyeuristic

behavior. The prevalence of masturbatory


conditioning in aberrant sexuality is further

shown by Evans (1968), who found that among a


large group of sexual deviates 79 percent used

deviant fantasy while masturbating.

In instances where erotic fantasies serve as

evocative stimuli for deviant sexual activities,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1656
control over the behavior may be achieved by

eliminating either the aberrant fantasies or their

arousal properties. Evans (1967) reports


surprisingly high success in the treatment of seven

exhibitionists by aversive conditioning of imaginal

stimulus events. In this procedure, clients are

presented with image-producing phrases that

depict either normal heterosexual behavior or


exhibitionistic activities. Vivid imagery of the
deviant behavior is associated with shock, which

the client can terminate by changing to a slide

describing normal sexual responses. Of the seven

exhibitionists who finished treatment, five no


longer experience any urge to expose themselves

and have completely ceased exhibitionistic


behavior, while the remaining two have reduced

their frequency of genital exposure from a pre-

therapy high of 28 per month to 2 episodes per


month. In a subsequent study Evans (1968) found

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1657
that the speed of aversive conditioning was

related to the content of masturbatory fantasies.

Exhibitionists who utilized normal masturbatory


fantasies discontinued exposure behavior within

about 4 weeks of treatment, whereas those who


engaged in exhibitionistic masturbatory fantasies
required approximately 24 weeks before they

ceased the deviant activities. Results obtained by

Mees (1966) in modifying a sadistic fantasy


suggest that aberrant erotic fantasies of long

standing can be most effectively eliminated if, in

addition to aversive conditioning, normal


heterosexual imagery is induced and reinforced.

DIFFERENTIAL CONDITIONING OF SEXUAL


RESPONSIVENESS

In many of the cases cited above, therapists


have tried not only to create aversions to
inappropriate objects, but also to develop

attraction to heterosexual stimuli. A variety of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1658
differential conditioning techniques has been
employed for this purpose. In one such approach,

which is probably minimally effective,


homosexuals, in addition to receiving aversive

conditioning trials, are shown pictures of nude or

scantily clad females several hours after

administration of testosterone (Freund, 1960;


James, 1962). Gonadal hormones may increase

sexual arousal, but they do not determine its

quality or direction. Indeed, attempts to treat

homosexuals by administrations of large amounts

of androgen simply increased their homoerotic


desires and behavior (Ford & Beach, 1951; Perloff,

1965). It remains a question, therefore, whether


any positive conditioning is achieved by this

method.

Masturbatory conditioning has also been

employed as a means of increasing the erotic


arousal properties of heterosexual stimuli. As part

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1659
of an aversion-relief method, Thorpe, Schmidt,

Brown, & Castell (1964) had clients masturbate to

photographs of attractive females and utilize these


images in masturbatory fantasies. In addition,

shock stimulation was presented in conjunction

with phrases describing deviant sexual practices,

whereas descriptions of heterosexual behavior

occurred with shock termination. The authors


report that heterosexual fantasies of high arousal
value can be strongly established in this manner.

In an earlier study, Thorpe, Schmidt, & Castell

(1963) found that masturbatory conditioning

alone did not eliminate homosexual fantasies, but


a method combining positive and negative

conditioning eventually replaced homoerotic


fantasies with heterosexual ones. In the absence of

comparative data of the sexual responses of

individuals receiving either aversive trials alone or


the combined procedure, there is no way of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1660
determining the degree to which the positive

component facilitated the change in sexual

interest.

A differential conditioning strategy was

similarly employed by Davison (1968) in treating


a college student whose sexual activities were

confined to masturbation evoked by fantasies of

inflicting injury on women. The client was


instructed initially to utilize the sadistic fantasy to
induce sexual arousal but to masturbate while

looking at pictures of captivating belles from

Playboy magazine. After conventional sexual

stimuli had acquired sex-arousing value the


sadistic fantasy was paired with nauseous imagery
and supplanted by normal masturbatory fantasies.

Consistent with the interpretation of

counterconditioning as partly reflecting self-

control processes, the client was later able to

reinstate and to eliminate sadistic fantasies at will

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1661
through masturbatory conditioning with sadistic

or normal erotic fantasies.

In a third approach a differential conditioning


procedure is employed in which presentations of

pictures of nude males are accompanied by

electric shock, while looking at projected pictures

of nude females is reinforced by termination of

continuous electrical stimulation (Solyom & Miller,


1965). This particular method is predicated on the
assumption that stimuli associated with aversion

relief will acquire positive properties, it is unclear,

however, how pain-relief experiences can endow


related stimuli with sex arousal value.
Nevertheless, Solyom and Miller found, during

treatment of a group of homosexuals, that


plethysmograph responses to female pictures

became progressively greater, while responses to

male pictures remained essentially unchanged.

This suggests that the differential conditioning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1662
produced a change from predominantly

homosexual to bisexual responsiveness. However,

these findings are difficult to interpret because, as


the authors themselves acknowledge, their

plethysmograph measure does not differentiate

between sexual and anxiety arousal. Erotic arousal

can be most validly assessed in terms of penile

erectile responses as demonstrated by Bancroft,


Jones, & Pullan (1966), who measured changes in
several object preferences in a pedophile

throughout the course of aversion therapy. This

measure makes it possible to conduct systematic

investigations of the relative efficacy of different


conditioning procedures for altering erotic

preferences. It also provides an objective criterion


for determining the duration of treatment, thus

safeguarding against either premature

termination, or needless prolongation, of the


conditioning sessions.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1663
It should be emphasized here that conditioning

of sexual attraction to appropriate objects

constitutes only part of a broader treatment


objective. Persons who have engaged in deviant

sexual practices for a long time must develop not


only heterosexual attraction but also intricate
patterns of heterosexual behavior. This may

require, among other things, acquisition of new

speech patterns, dress styles, courtship behaviors,


modes of sexual stimulation that are closely

associated with heterosexual coitus, and many

other aspects of sex-role behavior. To the extent


that such behavioral changes enable persons to

engage in rewarding intimate interactions, the


resultant positive experiences will exert a

powerful influence on the further development of

heterosexual feelings and preferences.

CONDITIONING OF AVERSIONS AND


REINFORCEMENT OF ALTERNATIVE MODES OF
BEHAVIOR

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1664
The existence of desirable modes of sexual

behavior is probably a major determinant of the

durability of changes induced through aversive


counterconditioning. Enduring modification of

sexual deviance through aversion methods will


therefore be governed, not so much by the
magnitude of negative properties conditioned to

previously attractive stimuli but rather by the

availability of alternative means of securing sexual


gratification. Because the sexual taboos that

prevail in the culture extend to treatment

situations as well, it is exceedingly difficult to


produce am to reinforce desired patterns of sexual

behavior. Consequently, treatment interventions


are mainly directed toward reducing the incidence

of deviant behavior, but the appearance and

continued maintenance of heterosexual responses


are left to fortuitous circumstances.

Treatment by aversion methods exclusively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1665
generally achieves favorable outcomes with

persons who display bisexual responsiveness or

engage in heterosexual intercourse, even through


they rely upon fetishistic objects for erotic arousal.

On the other hand, aversion treatment alone

appears to be much less successful in cases where

the deviant behavior constitutes the sole means of

obtaining sexual gratification. This differential


efficacy is well illustrated by Oswald’s (1962)
treatment of two rubberized-clothing fetishists. In

both cases aversion to rubberized garments was

successfully established, but one of the clients

subsequently reverted to the deviant behavior.

In the first case, rubberized garments

primarily served as arousal stimuli for exclusively


heterosexual patterns of behavior. The client was

prompted to seek treatment because of marital

conflicts arising from his wife’s refusal to wear her

rubberized mackintosh in bed. The erotic arousal

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1666
properties of the fetishistic stimuli were rapidly

eliminated through a series of counterconditioning

trials, in each of which the client donned a


mackintosh while experiencing apomorphine-

induced nausea supplemented by a recorded tape

suggesting similar reactions. A 21-month post-

therapy evaluation is summarized as follows: “He

feels quite indifferent to rubberized clothes and


finds it hard to believe how he could ever have had
this interest in them. His career has prospered

extremely well by his own efforts and talents, and

his wife confirms that they are normal and happy

in their general and sexual life [p. 201].”

The second case, a 32-year-old military recruit,

experienced heightened sexual arousal whenever


he tied himself up tightly in black shiny rubber.

This fetishistic behavior apparently originated in

an early experience in which a group of boys

seized the client, tied a groundsheet over his head

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1667
and masturbated him. “Since then he had made a

practice of tying himself up with rubber

groundsheets, a rubber hood and ropes. He came


for treatment partly because he feared he might

encompass his own death, as he had recently had

difficulty in releasing himself [p. 201].” Except for

one occasion when the client had sexual

intercourse at a specialized brothel equipped with


rubber strait-jackets, hoods, thongs, and rubber
zip suits, masturbation served as his predominant

form of sexual behavior. The counterconditioning

sessions proceeded along lines similar to the case

cited above. Following the administration of


apomorphine, the client tied himself up in

groundsheets or donned a frogman’s suit. In


addition, on two successive days he received

several injections of testosterone and was

furnished with books featuring photographs of


females.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1668
It is apparent from follow-up reports that

rubberized garments had temporarily lost their

erotic value, but the reinstatement of fetishistic


behavior would be expected from the limited

scope of the treatment, aside from the fact that the


deviant behavior was instrumental in eventually
gaining the client a military discharge.

He tried out his rubber articles a week after


leaving hospital, found they held no interest
for him and discarded them. He went out to
dances and other social events for the first
time in years. After 6 months he relapsed
and a further 4 months later made known his
deviation to the Service authorities and was
invalided. He told me at that time that he
intended to live in London where there were
others who shared his interests. He had been
back to the brothel which, he pointed out,
advertises in a well-known week-end
publication available at any bookstall, under
the guise of a rubber-clothing store. He had
formed a friendship with a male homosexual
(but not had sexual relations with him),
whom he had first noticed wearing a black,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1669
shiny, rubber mackintosh in Hyde Park one
fine summer’s evening [p. 202].

The conditioning of aversion to inappropriate

sexual stimuli can assist in promoting


heterosexual behavior provided these alternative

responses already exist in the client’s repertoire,


and sufficient positive reinforcement is available
in everyday situations to maintain them. The latter

conditions were clearly present in the first case


reviewed, but the second client exhibited only a

weakly developed heterosexual repertoire, which

evidently was further extinguished during the


period following treatment when he became more

active socially.

The mere absence of heterosexual behavior in

itself does not necessarily indicate a behavioral


deficit. A person may have developed some

capability and desire for culturally approved

forms of sexuality, but these tendencies are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1670
strongly inhibited because of heterosexual
anxieties. It is therefore important to distinguish

between developmental deficits and inhibitory


effects in devising supplemental treatment

programs to aversion therapy. The inclusion of

anxiety extinction procedures is particularly

important in modifying deviant sexual behavior


that is, in part, maintained by fear of heterosexual

involvements. Cooper (1963) reports a case of this

type that illustrates the combined use of aversive

therapy and self-directed desensitization in the

treatment of a 25-year-old pharmacist whose


central problems featured transvestism and

impotence.

The discussion of the necessity for

supplemental programs has dealt thus far with


conditions in which heterosexual repertoires are

either minimal or strongly inhibited. The scope of


the interventions may also need to be extended

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1671
when changes in behavior cannot be readily

achieved and sustained due to inadequate sexual

reinforcement. This problem is revealed in the


treatment of a transvestite reported by Glynn &

Harper (1961). After a series of aversion trials in

which the client experienced apomorphine-

induced nausea while attired in women’s clothes,

he exhibited revulsion at the sight of the female


clothing, he no longer felt any desire to wear it,
and he declined a request to do so. While aversive

counterconditioning successfully eliminated the

transvestite behavior, the client’s marital

relationship provided absolutely no sexual


gratifications. Although he had been married for

four years, the marriage had never been


consummated, due largely to his wife’s frigidity.

The wife’s marked sexual inhibitions were

therefore treated by the standard desensitization


procedure with considerable success, as evidenced

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1672
by the fact that “she is now pregnant and the

marriage is happily stabilized.” A seven-month

follow-up study disclosed no transvestism nor any


desire on the client’s part to engage in cross-sex

dressing. Had the treatment in this case been


confined to negative conditioning of transvestite
responses, it is highly probable that any attempted

heterosexual behavior would have been rapidly

extinguished and transvestism might have


regained its sexually rewarding function.

When deviant sexual behavior is less strongly

established, it may be possible to dispense with

aversive counterconditioning by substituting a


treatment program based on extinction of
heterosexual anxieties coupled with positive

reinforcement of desired alternative behaviors.

Existing sociosexual repertoires can in this way be

further developed and strengthened until

eventually they become more rewarding than the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1673
deviant tendencies. It will be recalled from clinical

studies reviewed earlier (Bond & Hutchison, 1964;

Stevenson & Wolpe, 1960) that successful


outcomes were, in fact, achieved with

exhibitionists and homosexuals treated by

desensitization methods alone. These findings

suggest the need for systematic comparative

studies of the relative efficacy of aversive


counterconditioning, desensitization, and
reinforcement procedures, utilized singly or in

combination, with persons exhibiting varying

dispositions for sexually deviant behavior.

Modification of Symbolic Activities

Although aversive counterconditioning has

been most extensively employed in the treatment

of alcoholism and sexual deviations, a number of

investigators have also attempted to eliminate

unusually persistent ruminations by this method.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1674
Perturbing fantasies containing sexual, aggressive,

and other elements have proved exceedingly

refractory to change. Because of their limited


accessibility, these types of ideational phenomena

are not readily amenable to experimental

investigation. As a consequence, the conditions

governing the occurrence and persistence of

emotionally disturbing thoughts remain obscure.


There exists some research evidence (Eriksen &
Kuethe, 1956) to suggest, however, that thoughts

can be brought under control by applying aversive

contingencies. In this particular experiment,

students were instructed to produce associations


to fifteen words, and their associations to five of

these words were followed by shock. Associations


that were accompanied by the aversive experience

declined rapidly, while the frequency of

nonpunished associations remained unchanged


over succeeding trials (Figure 8-2). The decrement

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1675
Figure 8-2. Percentage of first-trial responses repeated on
succeeding trials as a function of punishment. Eriksen
& Kuethe, 1956.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1676
in associations occurred whether or not subjects

were aware of the punishment contingency.

It is, of course, easier to eliminate relatively

neutral ideations than the highly valenced forms

appearing in treatment. Wolpe (1958) reports


varying degrees of success in removing

obsessional ruminations by associating their

occurrence with electric shock. Employing


essentially the same procedure, McGuire &
Vallance (1964) successfully treated a 29- year-old

teacher who was plagued by intrusive thoughts

concerning his wife’s fidelity, dating back to an

ambiguous remark originally made by his mother.


Although he realized that these thoughts had no
basis in reality, nevertheless this knowledge was

of little avail in controlling the obsessional

ruminations. In treatment, shocks were

contiguously associated with thoughts about his

mother making the uncomplimentary remark and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1677
its implications. During the second and third

sessions, the client controlled the timing and

intensity of shock administration and, thereafter,


he continued the conditioning process at home

with a portable apparatus. Within a relatively brief

time the obsessional ruminations were eliminated,

a change that was accompanied by a general

diminution of anxiety.

The elimination of disturbing thoughts in the


course of aversion therapy is best revealed by

Marks, Rachman, & Gelder (1965), who

demonstrated that with successive aversive trials


the latency of perverted thoughts increased until
eventually they could not be produced at all. In the

previously reviewed studies involving more


extensive measurement of changes Marks &

Gelder (1967) found that during reduction of

deviant sexual fantasies, erotic arousal

accompanying the imagery also diminished

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1678
progressively. In approximately half the cases,

however, aversive conditioning reduced the

arousal potential of deviant sexual fantasies, but it


did not eliminate the mental imagery. The reasons

for the differential susceptibility to inhibition are

not apparent. At this point it is worth noting that

aversion therapy provides an excellent

opportunity for studying the extent to which


thought processes are amenable to reinforcement
control. It also furnishes a means of investigating

symbolic control of overt behavior.

In a number of studies, strongly established


behavior has been enduringly eliminated by
aversive conditioning of symbolic events alone.

Evans (1968) and Thorpe and his associates


(1964), for example, modified sexual disorders by

endowing verbal and imaginal representations of

the deviant activities with negative qualities

through association with shock. Agras (1967) also

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1679
successfully applied the symbolic conditioning

paradigm to aggressively destructive behavior in a

chronic schizophrenic, who had to be physically


restrained because of uncontrollable tendencies to

smash any glass in sight. The client participated in

a series of sessions in which he was asked to

visualize himself breaking glass, whereupon he

was administered a painful electric shock. As the


treatment progressed the latency of the
destructive imagery increased and eventually he

lost all urge to smash glass. A follow-up study

revealed that, except for one minor incident, the

destructive behavior never appeared again.

Imaginal counterconditioning may be of

greatest value in modifying disorders in which


symbolic events possessing high arousal potential

exert substantial control over behavior. If the

internal elicitors are eliminated the related actions

should decrease in frequency. Some investigators

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1680
have assumed, however, that symbolic stimuli can

be substituted for real events without sacrificing

effectiveness. If this assumption were valid, then


the method would have wide applicability, since

any event, no matter how complex, can be easily

visualized. On the basis of generalization

principles one would expect actual stimuli to

produce better results than imagined ones.

OTHER ADDICTIVE BEHAVIORS

Another behavioral problem to which aversive

forms of treatment have been applied is cigarette


addiction. Preliminary case studies report
relatively high abstinence rates accompanying

treatments in which the act of smoking is

contiguously paired with aversive electrical

stimulation (McGuire & Vallance, 1964), drug-

induced nausea (Raymond, 1964), or a

disagreeable mixture of smoke and hot air (Wilde,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1681
1964). These favorable outcomes contrast with
those of Koenig & Masters (1965), who compared

changes in smoking behavior in groups of students


who received either desensitization, aversive

counterconditioning, or supportive counseling.

Very few of their subjects discontinued the habit,

and degree of reduction in smoking was found to


be unrelated to the type of treatment

administered.

In an experimental study employing

appropriate controls, Stollak (1968) had little

success in modifying obesity by pairing

descriptions of fattening foods with shock

stimulation. It would appear from the enduring


weight reductions obtained through self-

management of contingencies by Harris (1969)


and Stuart (1967), that a broad program of self-

control can be highly effective, whereas aversive


conditioning alone is likely to yield unimpressive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1682
results.

Alcoholism

A wide variety of “neurotic personality


disturbances” have been proposed as the

underlying determinants of chronic alcoholism.

Among the more widely accepted interpretations


are those advanced by psychoanalytic theory,

according to which alcoholism derives from latent

homosexuality related to fixations on “passive-

narcissistic aims.” Oral dependent needs and


characterological structures are thus frequently

invoked as the decisive predisposing factors in the


excessive use of alcohol. Self-destructive drives,
feelings of inferiority, unconscious needs to

dominate, and a host of other factors including

excessive mothering, insufficient mothering,

emotional immaturity, and introverted

psychoneuroses have also been proposed as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1683
determinants of alcoholism.

In contrast to the widespread claim for a

prealcoholic personality, comparative studies of


alcoholics and nonalcoholics (Sutherland,

Schroeder, & Tordella, 1950; Syme, 1957) have

generally failed to identify any specific personality

traits or “underlying dynamics” that would clearly

differentiate alcoholics from other deviant groups,


or for that matter, from persons judged to be
“normal.” Even if some consistent personality

correlates of alcoholism had been obtained, it

would be impossible, without longitudinal studies,


to determine whether the given personality
patterns represented the cumulative

consequences or the causes of chronic


intoxication. It is nevertheless evident from a large

body of empirical findings and knowledge of

behavior maintenance mechanisms that the search

for personality dynamics that supposedly control

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1684
excessive drinking is a futile pursuit. Just as

persons who differ markedly in personality

attributes can learn to use tobacco excessively, so,


given appropriate social-learning conditions,

persons who possess diverse personality

characteristics can be taught heavy drinking of

alcoholic beverages. In fact, it has been shown

repeatedly that no matter what deviant behavior


is singled out for study, it is usually found in a
wide variety of personality types. A much more

fruitful approach to the understanding of

alcoholism would be to investigate the learning

contingencies specifically associated with drinking


behavior and the reinforcement mechanisms

maintaining self-intoxication.

EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON EMOTIONAL AROUSAL


AND REACTIVITY

Psychodynamic theories have generally


emphasized the symbolic value of alcohol in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1685
gratifying “oral” or “passive-dependent” needs, but
remarkably little attention has been paid to the

pharmacological properties of ethanol which,


under certain conditions, make it a powerful

positive reinforcer.

One set of experiments that has direct bearing


on the reinforcing qualities of ethanol is concerned
with its effects on both autonomic arousal and

reactivity. In these studies subjects’ physiological


responses are measured prior to and following the

ingestion of alcohol, with basal conductance level

and magnitude of GSR responses to specific

stressor stimuli typically serving as indices of

emotional responsiveness. The findings generally


show that alcohol in small doses has no consistent

effects (Docter & Perkins, 1961; McDonnell &


Carpenter, 1959), but it can produce substantial

reduction in affective arousal when taken in


moderate to large quantities (Carpenter, 1957;

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1686
Greenberg & Carpenter, 1957). A comparison of

alcohol, meprobamate, and a placebo by Lienert &

Traxel (1959) further reveals that alcohol and the


tranquilizer are equally effective in reducing GSR

responses to disturbing verbal stimuli. Moreover,

subjects who had exhibited high emotionality, as

assessed several weeks prior to the experimental

session, were tranquilized by alcohol to a greater


degree than those who had previously displayed
low arousal.

It is sometimes mistakenly assumed (Chafetz &

Demone, 1962) that reinforcement principles


cannot adequately account for alcoholism because
the devastating social and physical consequences

of chronic drinking far outweigh its temporary


relief value. This argument overlooks the fact that

behavior is more powerfully controlled by its

immediate, rather than delayed, consequences,

and it is precisely for this reason that persons may

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1687
persistently engage in immediately reinforcing,

but potentially self-destructive, behavior. Future

adverse consequences, reinstated symbolically in


the present, may be sufficiently strong to inhibit

drinking behavior when instigation for escape is

relatively weak. On the other hand, it is

unreasonable to expect thoughts of future effects

to exert much of an inhibitory influence in persons


who experience a high level of aversive
stimulation and who present a well-established

stress-alcoholism response pattern.

EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON AVOIDANCE AND


ESCAPE RESPONSES

Further evidence for the stress-reducing

properties of alcohol is provided in experiments

with animals designed to study disinhibitory


effects and extinction of avoidance responses.

Suggestive findings regarding the fear-reducing


effects of alcohol were originally reported by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1688
Masserman & Yum (1946) in a study in which cats
that had learned to perform complex

manipulations to secure food subsequently


inhibited the instrumental manipulatory and

approach responses after they had been shocked

at the goal. Administration of small doses of

alcohol, however, promptly restored the approach


manipulations designed to obtain the food

rewards. In addition, the cats developed a

preference for milk cocktails containing 5 percent

alcohol to plain milk during the series of shock

trials, but reverted to their original preference for


nonalcoholic drinks after the aversive stimulation

had been discontinued and emotional responses


were completely extinguished. In a partial

replication of the Masserman and Yum study,


Smart (1965) further confirmed the anxiety-

mitigating effects of alcohol.

In order to test whether alcohol reduces

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1689
punishment-induced avoidance behavior or

increases approach tendencies in an approach-

avoidance conflict, Conger (1951) designed an


experiment in which he trained one group of

animals to approach the lighted end of the alley for

food, and a second group to avoid the lighted end

of the alley to escape electric shocks. Compared to

the behavior of sober controls, which had been


administered placebo injections, the avoidance
responses of subjects that had received alcohol

injections showed a substantial reduction in

strength, but approach responses seemed

unaffected.

A few experiments involving human subjects

have also demonstrated the disinhibiting effects of


alcohol on verbal expressions of sexual and

aggressive behavior in social drinking situations

(Bruun, 1959; Clark & Sensibar, 1955). Among

humans, however, the same dose of alcohol may

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1690
have diverse effects because individuals differ in

the types of responses inhibited, the strength of

inhibitions, and variations in social conditions


which, in part, serve to define and to control

appropriate behavior.

Numerous studies have been concerned with

the influence of ethanol on escape and avoidance

responses tested in a variety of aversive


conditioning situations involving no rewarded
competing responses. In these experiments

animals are initially taught to perform responses

which either avert the onset of aversive


stimulation or terminate it after its occurrence.
Changes in the rate of avoidance and escape

responses as a function of the administration of


ethanol are then assessed relative to the

performances of control groups, which receive

either water or solutions containing other types of

drugs. Ethanol in moderate doses produces more

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1691
rapid extinction of fear-mediated responses

(Kaplan, 1956; Pawlowski, Denenberg, & Zarrow,

1961), and it reduces the rate of responses


designed to postpone the occurrence of aversive

stimuli (Hogans, Moreno, & Brodie, 1961; Sidman,

1955). Moreover, the capacity of alcohol to reduce

emotional behavior is similar to that of other

drugs possessing central depressant properties


(Korpmann & Hughes, 1959).

The withdrawal of positive reinforcers

following a period of reward generally produces

aversive effects that lead to the suppression of


associated responses. Further support for
interpreting the behavioral effects of ethanol in

terms of emotion-reducing processes is furnished


by experiments concerned with the reinstatement

of responses following their inhibition through

frustrative nonreward. Under these conditions,

animals administered ethanol are more persistent

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1692
than those given a placebo in performing

nonreinforced behavior (Barry, Wagner, & Miller,

1962), and they increase their rate of response in


the presence of stimuli signifying nonreward

(Blough, 1956; Miller, 1961).

The experimental data reviewed so far, based

on the forced administration of moderate doses of

ethanol, strongly indicate that alcohol can produce


significant decrements in both autonomic arousal
and emotional behavior generated by aversive

environmental conditions. Investigations

concerned with variables governing the voluntary


intake of alcohol also contribute to an
understanding of the development and

maintenance of self-intoxication. This research is


reviewed next.

DETERMINANTS OF VOLUNTARY ALCOHOL


CONSUMPTION

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1693
The self-selection method has been extensively

employed in studies designed to identify the

stimulus determinants of alcohol consumption. In


these investigations animals are typically provided

with a choice between an alcoholic solution and


one or more other liquids; the base level of
voluntary alcohol intake is then compared with

the amount consumed under various

environmental conditions.

Findings of studies utilizing a forced alcohol

regimen, in which the animals’ entire liquid intake

is restricted over a period of time to solutions

containing various concentrations of ethanol,


reveal that alcohol per se has no strong inherently
reinforcing properties. Under such conditions,

animals consume small, nonintoxicating amounts

of alcohol, but they readily revert to drinking

other liquids when these later become available

(Korman & Stephens, 1960; Richter, 1956). The

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1694
positive reinforcing value of alcohol, as inferred

from increases in alcohol consummatory

responses, can be substantially enhanced,


however, by certain physiological and

psychological conditions. Although animals which

metabolize ethanol rapidly consume greater

amounts of alcohol in free-choice situations than

those who exhibit lower metabolic rates, studies of


intraindividual variations reveal that nutritional
deficiencies, endocrinal factors such as insulin,

and drugs which produce liver damage increase

voluntary ethanol intake (Mardones, 1960;

Rodgers & McClearn, 1962). However, studies of


human alcoholism, though complicated by

ambiguities regarding cause and effect, have failed


to yield any reliable differences between

alcoholics and nonalcoholics in genetic and

endocrinological characteristics (Lester, 1966).

Findings of laboratory studies comparing

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1695
voluntary intake of alcohol before, during, and

after aversive stimulation throw some light on one

of the possible mechanisms underlying alcohol


consumption. It will be recalled that Masserman &

Yum (1946) found that animals who had initially

preferred plain milk to an alcoholic milk solution

developed a preference for alcohol during periods

of shock-induced stress, but reverted to


nonalcoholic drinks following termination of
aversive stimuli and fear extinction. Clark & Polish

(1960) measured the intake of water and a

solution of 20 percent alcohol by monkeys before,

during, and after avoidance training in which each


response briefly postponed the occurrence of

electric shocks. Although there was relatively little


change in water intake across phases, alcohol

consumption increased during, and decreased

following, the avoidance conditioning sessions.

The effects of aversive stimulation on alcohol

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1696
consumption are apt to be more prolonged when

punishment is administered on a noncontingent

and unpredictable basis. Casey (1960), for


example, studied the relative intake of water and

an alcohol solution as a function of aversive

shocks programmed according to a variable

interval schedule. Under such conditions of

uncertainty, the animals drank somewhat larger


amounts of alcohol for the period of stress, but the
greatest increments in voluntary alcohol

consumption occurred during the following

month, after the shocks had been discontinued. On

the other hand, in a second group of animals


provided with a free choice of water or a solution

of reserpine (which has long-delayed effects), the


same experimental manipulations failed to

increase the attraction of the latter drug. These

differential findings suggest that the relatively


rapid absorption of alcohol, and the attendant

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1697
reduction in aversive arousal, may partly

contribute to its effectiveness as a positive

reinforcer under conditions of aversive


stimulation. Moreover, generalized emotionality

may further enhance its reinforcing effects (Korn,


1960).

SOCIAL LEARNING OF DRINKING BEHAVIOR

The research discussed above indicates that


excessive alcohol consumption is maintained

through positive reinforcement deriving from the

central depressant and anesthetic properties of

alcohol. Persons who are repeatedly subjected to


environmental stress are, consequently, more

prone to consume anesthetic doses of alcohol than

those who experience less stress and for whom,

therefore, alcohol has only weak reinforcing value.

In many cases, also, excessive drinking may


primarily serve to relieve the aversive effects of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1698
boredom.

Prolonged and heavy use of alcoholic

beverages produces alterations in the metabolic


system which provide the basis for a second

maintaining mechanism that is quite independent

of the original functional value of alcohol. That is,


withdrawal of alcohol elicits exceedingly aversive
physiological reactions consisting of

tremulousness, nausea, vomiting, marked


weakness, diarrhea, fever, hypertension, excessive

perspiration, and insomnia (Isbell, Fraser, Wikler,

Belleville, & Eiseman, 1955; Mendelson & La Dou,

1964). After the person thus becomes physically

dependent on alcohol, he is compelled to consume


large quantities of liquor both to alleviate

distressing physical reactions and to avoid their


recurrence. Since the ingestion of intoxicants

promptly terminates physiologically generated


aversive stimulation, drinking behavior is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1699
automatically and continuously reinforced. After

pharmacological addiction occurs, the major part

of the alcoholic’s time and resources becomes


devoted to maintaining a continuously high level

of self-intoxication.

Although aversion reduction and other

positive reinforcements which typically

accompany social drinking may account


adequately for the maintenance of inebriety, an
adequate theory of alcoholism must invoke

additional social-learning variables since,

obviously, most people who are subject to


stressful experiences do not become alcoholics. It
has been customary to summon internal

determinants in the form of neurotic personality


disturbances and underlying pathologies as the

differentiating antecedent variables. The

inadequacy of theories of alcoholism, which

emphasize the role of personality traits and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1700
internal dynamics, becomes readily evident in the

marked cultural and subcultural differences in the

incidence of alcoholism. Indeed, based on the


theory that a “neurosis” is instrumental in the

development of chronic alcoholism, one would be

forced to conclude that Jews, Mormons, Moslems,

Italians, Chinese, and members of other cultural

groups which present exceedingly low rates of


addictive drinking are lacking in neuroses, oral
deprivations, self-destructive tendencies, latent

homosexuality, indulgent mothering, and

inadequacy feelings, whereas the latter pernicious

conditions must be highly prevalent among the


Irish, who surpass all other ethnic groups in

chronic alcoholism (Chafetz & Demone, 1962;


McCarthy, 1959; Pittman & Snyder, 1962).

Perhaps the most striking evidence that

alcoholism primarily represents a learned pattern


of behavior, rather than a manifestation of a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1701
particular type of predisposing underlying

pathology, is provided by the extraordinarily low

rates of alcoholism among Jews, who experience


no less, and in all probability more, psychological

stress than members of other ethnic groups noted


for their drinking (Glad, 1947; Snyder, 1958).
These ethnic and subcultural differences in the use

of intoxicants point to the importance of the

prealcoholic social learning of drinking behavior


in the development of alcoholism.

The social-learning variables take several

forms. At the most general level they are reflected

in the cultural norms that define the


reinforcement contingencies associated with the
use of alcohol. There is considerable evidence that

the consumption of alcohol is significantly

influenced by the drinking mores of given social

groups. Members of cultures that are highly

permissive toward the use of intoxicants, or even

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1702
consider drinking to be emulative behavior,

display a higher incidence of drunkenness than

individuals reared in cultures that, for religious


and other reasons, demand sobriety. Similarly,

within heterogeneous cultures such as our own,

the prevalence of chronic intoxication varies as a

function of the types of social-learning conditions

that are associated with class status, religious


affiliation, racial and ethnic background,
occupation, and urban or rural residence.

Although cultural and subgroup mores

obviously play an influential role in determining


the extent of excessive drinking, normative
injunctions alone do not explain either the

relatively low incidence of addictive drinking in


social groups that positively sanction the use of

alcoholic beverages, or the occurrence of chronic

alcoholism in cultures prohibiting intoxicants.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1703
Cultural and subgroup mores are to a large

extent transmitted through the modeling behavior

of socializing agents; consequently, one cannot


assume that members of a particular class

undergo equivalent learning experiences. Studies


of the family background of alcoholics generally
reveal an unusually high incidence of familial

alcoholism (Fort & Porterfield, 1961; Lemere,

Voegtlin, Broz, O’Hollaren, & Tupper, 1942b; Wall,


1936). It might be argued that these data support

a genetic interpretation of alcoholism, but the

pattern of drinking behavior being modeled and


the range of circumstances in which it occurs are

of greater importance than exhibition of some


drinking or complete abstinence by family

members. For example, in Italian and Jewish

households, use of dilute alcoholic beverages,


particularly wine, is approved under clearly

circumscribed conditions but negatively

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1704
sanctioned if consumed in either intoxicating

amounts or inappropriate situations. When the


use of alcohol is thus restricted primarily to

mealtimes or forms an integral part of religious

ceremonies and other social and familial practices,


alcohol consumption may be brought under

sufficiently narrow stimulus control to ensure


moderation (Bales, 1946; Glad, 1947; Snyder,
1958). On the other hand, in familial situations

where alcohol is consumed extensively in a large

variety of circumstances and is a preferred

response to monotony or stress, a similar type of


drinking pattern is likely to be transmitted to

growing offspring. Although drinking behavior is


initially most often acquired under nonstress

conditions, a habitual social drinker will

experience stress reduction on many occasions.

Once alcohol consumption is thus intermittently


reinforced, it will be readily elicited under

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1705
frustrative or aversive conditions. Therefore,
alcoholism typically results from habituation after

prolonged heavy social drinking acquired within

the context of familial alcoholism.

The relationship of stress to alcoholism is


perhaps strongest among alcoholics who are

members of subcultural groups that negatively

sanction the consumption of intoxicating


beverages, and whose parents have practiced total

abstinence. The social-learning history of


alcoholism under these conditions has never been

adequately documented, but there is some

evidence to suggest that in these cases the


drinking pattern is originally acquired under
highly stressful conditions and then generalizes to

less acute emotional circumstances (Fort &

Porterfield, 1961). In addition, outside the family,


peer models who drink may play an influential

role in transmitting drinking behavior (Skolnik,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1706
1957).

In the foregoing analysis of alcoholism,

aversive stimulation and its quick reduction


through the depressant action of alcoholic

beverages were assigned a central role in the

development and maintenance of addictive

drinking. It should be emphasized, however, that

conflict, boredom, frustration, and other stressful


conditions may elicit a wide variety of reactions
including aggression, dependency, withdrawal,

somatization, regression, apathy, autism,

inebriety, or constructive coping behaviors.


Persons who exhibit the latter stress response
pattern will typically be judged “normal”; in

contrast, “neuroses,” “deep-seated personality


disturbances,” and other disease processes are

frequently invoked as explanatory factors when

persons have acquired one or more of the former

patterns of coping behavior. These assumed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1707
pathologies represent essentially pseudo-

explanations since the main evidence for their

existence is the behavior that they are called on to


explain.

From a social-learning point of view, alcoholics

are people who have acquired, through differential

reinforcement and modeling experiences, alcohol

consumption as a widely generalized dominant


response to aversive stimulation. Therapeutic
attention would therefore be most profitably

directed toward reducing the level of aversive

stimulation experienced by individuals, and


toward eliminating alcohol stress responses either
directly or, preferably, by establishing alternative

modes of coping behavior. Given more effective


and rewarding means of dealing with

environmental demands, individuals will have less

need to resort to self-anesthetization against

everyday experiences.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1708
It is entirely possible that the stress

component in alcoholism has been assigned

excessive weight largely because investigations


involving psychological variables have been

essentially confined to aversive conditioning


procedures, to the relative neglect of other
potentially significant determinants of alcohol

consumption. Moreover, although physiological

conditions and environmental stress have been


shown to increase alcohol intake, the amount

consumed seldom exceeds the animals’ oxidative

capacity. In contrast, Lester (1961) found that


animals on a variable interval schedule of positive

reinforcement maintained a steady and prolonged


state of self-intoxication accompanied by signs of

overt drunkenness, behavioral impairment, and

the development of metabolic tolerance analogous


to human alcoholism. Since intermittent food

reward can hardly be considered a highly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1709
distressing situation, evidently factors other than

stress reduction were primarily responsible for

the self-maintained inebriation. The findings from


the latter study point to the need for experimental

investigations of other psychological variables that


may be expected eventually to produce addictive
drinking.

Since alcoholism often arises in rewarding


social interactions, operant drinking, in which
alcohol consumption primarily serves an

instrumental rather than a reinforcing function,

warrants detailed examination. In this process a

person drinks in order to obtain a variety of


reward; deriving from social interactions with
imbibing companions. Prolonged heavy drinking

leads to the development of physiological

tolerance and dependence on alcohol which, in

turn, necessitates increased alcohol intake. Thus,

in advanced stages biochemical, stress-reduction,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1710
and social-reinforcement mechanisms may

contribute to the maintenance of addictive

drinking.

Regardless of the theory of alcoholism and

psychotherapy to which one may subscribe, the

elimination or drastic modification of alcoholic

behavior is obviously an objective of considerable

import. One of the behavioral approaches to this


problem has relied on the conditioning of aversive
properties to alcoholic beverages. In the following

sections the value and limitations of this mode of

therapy, and the conditions under which


alternative or supplementary procedures are
essential for the successful modification of

alcoholism, are considered in detail.

CONDITIONED-AVERSION THERAPY

The first systematic application of aversive

counterconditioning to the modification of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1711
alcoholism was reported by Kantorovich (1934).
Twenty alcoholics participated in 5 to 18 sessions

in which cards containing the names of drinks, and

actual sight of bottles of vodka, wine, and beer,


and the smell and taste of these various alcoholic

beverages were successively paired with electric


shocks. A control group of 10 alcoholics received
hypnotic suggestions and medication. Of the 20

clients in the experimental group, 17 acquired

stable aversion reactions to alcohol, and 14


remained totally abstinent when subsequently

evaluated at periods ranging from 3 weeks to 20

months. In contrast, all but one of the controls

reverted to their customary alcoholic ways within

a few days following their discharge from the


hospital.

Although Kantorovich’s procedure aroused

little interest, aversion therapies employing

pharmacological agents have been widely applied

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1712
to the treatment of alcoholism. Except for minor

variations, the conditioning procedures are

generally patterned after the methods originally


devised by Voegtlin and his associates (Lemere,

Voegtlin, Broz, O’Hollaren, & Tupper, 1942a;

Voegtlin, 1940) at a sanitarium devoted

exclusively to the treatment of alcoholism. The

treatment consists essentially of associating the


sight, smell, taste, and thought of alcohol with
drug-induced nausea in 4 to 7 brief sessions

distributed over a period of about ten days.

On the morning preceding the treatment, the


client is administered only liquids and a stimulant
drug (e.g., benzedrine sulphate) designed to

augment the conditioning process. The sessions


are conducted in a semi-darkened, soundproof

room from which all extraneous auditory, visual,

and olfactory stimuli have been excluded. In front

of the client’s chair is a table containing a varied

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1713
array of liquors including bourbon, scotch, gin,

brandy, rum, beer, and wine, spotlighted so as to

focus attention on the liquors. The client is first


given a glass of tepid water containing oral

emetine, and immediately thereafter an injection

of an emetine-pilocarpine-ephedrine mixture.

Emetine is utilized as the aversion-producing

agent primarily because its emetic action is more


sustained and it does not have the sedative effect
of apomorphine.

Just prior to the onset of nausea the client is

poured an ounce of straight whiskey and asked to


smell it, to sip it, and to taste it thoroughly. This
same procedure is repeated several times with

whiskey either taken straight or mixed with warm


water to afford easy emesis. The rationale for

relying on whiskey exclusively in the initial

session and at the beginning of each subsequent

session is that it produces greater gastric irritation

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1714
than beer or wine and therefore serves to facilitate

the emetic reaction. Kant (1944) has seriously

questioned the wisdom of using the conditioned


stimulus in order to enhance the unconditioned

response, since this procedure runs a high risk of

reinforcing drinking behavior. If 4 to 6 ounces of

alcohol are ingested before emesis occurs, large

quantities of alcohol are likely to be absorbed.


Under these conditions, the immediate reinforcing
effects of alcohol may reduce, or even outweigh,

the effectiveness of subsequent aversive

experiences. Considering the difficulties in

precisely timing the onset of emetic responses,


this factor may partly account for variations in

strength of conditioned aversion developed by


different investigators supposedly using the same

method. While Voegtlin has taken necessary

precautions to avoid alcohol absorption by the


client during treatment, it is not clear whether

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1715
other therapists have paid as close attention to

this important point in the technique.

The procedural changes recommended by Kant

(1944, 1945) obviate the alcohol absorption

problem without detracting from the efficacy of


the treatment. During the first two sessions just

prior to and during nausea, the client is asked to

look at, smell, and taste the different alcoholic


beverages, but then to spit them out. In
subsequent sessions, the client is requested to

drink some alcohol at the height of nausea. Only

during the terminal sessions, when alcohol itself

has acquired the capacity to produce rapid


emptying of the stomach, is the client encouraged
to take several drinks.

It is important to include all varieties and types

of alcoholic beverages as conditioned stimuli in

order to establish the most stable and generalized

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1716
aversion responses. Lemere & Voegtlin (1940), for
example, report several cases in which aversion

persisted to the class of beverages originally


counterconditioned, but the client began to drink

intoxicants toward which aversion reactions had

never been established. Subsequent treatment

involving these beverages produced total


abstinence. Quinn & Henbest (1967) report a

similar specificity of aversion in most cases in

which negative properties were conditioned to

whiskey alone. Although beer, wine, and whiskey

are used, greatest attention is usually directed


toward the particular type of intoxicant that the

client most prefers. The conditioning trials are


continued until the alcoholic stimuli alone elicit

nauseous reactions, and ingestion of the different


varieties of liquor produces prompt emesis. At the

conclusion of the treatment the client is instructed


that he must, in the future, abstain totally from all

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1717
alcoholic beverages.

There have been some variations in the


conditioning procedure originally developed by

Voegtlin. Miller, Dvorak, & Turner (1960) report

that excellent aversions to alcohol can be


developed by group applications of this method.

The authors report that simultaneous presence of

several persons undergoing treatment frequently


produces contagious emesis, thereby facilitating
the negative conditioning process.

Many European therapists have employed a


counterconditioning method devised by Feldmann
(DeMorsier & Feldmann, 1950), in which

apomorphine serves as the UCS, and treatment

sessions continue at 2 to 4 hour intervals until

complete aversion has been achieved toward all

varieties of alcoholic beverages.

The search continues for an unconditioned

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1718
stimulus which would have sufficiently strong
aversion-producing properties without

disagreeable side effects, but which at the same

time permitted the administration of


counterconditioning methods on an out-patient

basis. Miller (1959), for example, reports


considerable success in the treatment of
alcoholism with hypnotically induced aversion.

The client is hypnotized and instructed to

reexperience vividly his worst hangover, including


the general malaise, headache, nausea, and

vomiting. With the onset of nausea and emesis the

client is asked to smell and taste alcoholic

beverages including whiskey, wine, and beer. In

addition to the conditioning trials, the client is also


given direct suggestions that, in the future, the
taste or smell of alcohol will promptly evoke

disagreeable nauseous feelings. Both Miller and

Strel’chuk (1957), who has similarly experimented

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1719
with verbal induction methods under hypnosis,

maintain that hypnotically induced aversion

responses are more stable than those produced by


emetic drugs, although no comparative data are

furnished to support this claim.

EFFICACY OF AVERSION THERAPY

Numerous outcome studies have been


reported indicating the rates of abstinence from
alcoholic beverages that follow the application of

aversive counterconditioning. In evaluating the

outcomes of any form of treatment for alcoholism,

it is important to bear in mind several


qualifications. Precise assessment of a person’s

alcohol intake would necessitate continuous

monitoring of his activities. Since this is obviously

both ethically objectionable and impractical,

outcomes are typically measured in terms of self-


reports of drinking behavior; ratings by persons

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1720
who are well acquainted with the client; public
records of intoxication; and various indirect

indices based on adequacy of social, physical, and


occupational functioning (Hill & Blane, 1967).

It has been widely agreed, on the assumption

that alcoholics can never resume a controlled and


less excessive pattern of drinking, that complete
abstinence from alcoholic beverages should be the

main objective of any remedial program.


Consequently, the efficacy of behavioral

approaches is typically evaluated in terms of

duration of sobriety achieved among its clients. In

recent years several investigators (Davies, 1962;

Kendell, 1965) have reported that a small


percentage of alcoholics with a long history of

addictive drinking have been able to drink in


moderation after treatment. If one employed a

measure of change in drinking behavior rather


than the stringent criterion of total abstinence, the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1721
proportion of cases deriving some benefit from a

therapeutic program would be somewhat higher.

However, the validity of more refined


improvement ratings is open to question,

considering that assessment of pretreatment

drinking behavior is usually based on

retrospective reports rather than on direct

measurement of amount and pattern of alcohol


consumption. Mello & Mendelson (1965) have
developed a sensitive measure of drinking

behavior that could be employed to study changes

in alcoholic intake. Participants are given free

access to an operant conditioning device on which


they can work for either alcohol or monetary

reinforcement. After performing a certain number


of responses a small amount of alcohol is

dispensed or money points are recorded on a

counter, depending upon which of these


reinforcers is selected. This procedure permits

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1722
detailed examination of the pattern of alcohol

consumption over a specified period.

Although a major purpose of treatment should

be to modify the problem behavior for which

clients seek help, the efficacy of a given method of


treatment can be best evaluated in terms of its

total consequences. This is particularly true of

chronic alcoholism, which has profound adverse


effects on social, marital, occupational, and other
areas of functioning. However, in emphasizing the

value of measuring multiple outcomes, therapists

too often discount the relevance of the abstinence

criterion (Hill & Blane, 1967). This reordering of


criteria is often accompanied by enumeration of
hazardous consequences that can result from

cessation of drinking. Assessment of drinking

behavior is therefore largely neglected in favor of

inferred psychological changes that can make any

form of therapy look good even though it has

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1723
failed to achieve its intended objective. To

safeguard against perpetuation of weak methods

on the basis of extraneous criteria, evaluation


research should include assessment of drinking

behavior, regardless of whatever other outcome

criteria one wishes to invoke.

Table 8-1 Abstinence Rates Obtained by Aversion Therapy


Investigator Number Aversive Complete Duration of
of Cases Stimulus Abstinence Follow-up
(%)
Edlin, 63 Emetine 30 3-10
Johnson, months
Hletko, &
Heilbrunn
(1945)
Kant (1945) 31 Emetine 80 Unspecified
Lemere & 4096 Emetine 51 1–10 years
Voegtlin
(1950)
Miller, 10 Emetine 80 8 months
Dvorak, &
Turner
(1960)
Shanahan & 24 Emetine 70 9 months
Hornick
(1946)
Thimann 275 Emetine 51 3-7 years
(1949)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1724
Wallace 31 Emetine 42 4-17
(1949) months
DeMorsier 150 Apomorphine 46 8-31
& Feldmann months
(1950)
Mestrallet & 183 Apomorphine 41
Lang (1959)
Ruck Apomorphine 50 1.5 years
(1956)
Kantorovich 20 Electric 82 3 weeks-20
(1934) shock months
Blake 25 Electric 23 12 months
(1967) shock
37 Electric 48 12 months
shock with
relaxation
training
Miller 24 Verbally 83 9 months
(1959) induced
aversion
Anant 26 Verbally 96 8-15
(1967) induced months
aversion
Ashem & 15 Verbally 40 6 months
Donner induced
(1968) aversion

Table 8-1 summarizes the percentages of

complete abstinence obtained by different


investigators employing aversive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1725
counterconditioning. The figures in the table
generally do not include cases whose drinking

status was unknown because they could not be


located in subsequent follow-up studies. These

methods, of course, are not applied under

psychologically sterilized conditions. The

conditioning events are socially administered,


clients are undoubtedly given some practical

suggestions for more constructive means of coping

with their life situations, and they are probably

socially reinforced for maintaining sobriety. It is

also undoubtedly true that by the time alcoholics


appear for aversion therapy they have been

recipients of considerable wise counsel,


impassioned appeals by significant people in their

lives, repeated admonitions, rewards, and a


variety of remedies, to no avail. Treatment

outcomes are frequently attributed to common


social influences as though these were

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1726
encountered for the first time in the treatment

situation.

The reported variability in abstinence rates

most likely reflects the differential time intervals

at which the various investigators conducted their


follow-up evaluations. In general, abstinence rates

are extremely high in the period immediately

following treatment; the incidence of reversions to


drinking is greatest between 6 and 12 months;
thereafter abstinence declines gradually with

increasing duration. The fact, however, that some

variation in outcomes is found even when similar

procedures and follow-up intervals are involved,


suggests that the differences may also be partly
attributable to inadequate implementation of

requisite conditioning procedures, differences in

sample characteristics, and variations in the extent

to which environmental contingencies are

unfavorable for maintaining sobriety. The

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1727
conspicuous absence of any controlled

experimentation in this area makes it impossible

to determine the degree to which treatment


outcomes may be differentially affected by the

nature of the aversive stimulus, the number and

distribution of conditioning sessions, clients’

resources for alternative modes of response to

stress, and environmental contingencies


associated with drinking behavior.

The outcome data reported by Voegtlin and his

associates merit some discussion since they reflect

the most judicious and extensive application of the


principle of counterconditioning to the treatment
of alcoholism. Except for instances where the

therapy was contraindicated for physical reasons


(4 percent) and cases who refused to accept

treatment after detoxication (5 percent), aversion

therapy was offered to all applicants without

further selection. Consequently, an extremely wide

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1728
age range, varied socioeconomic levels, and

practically all occupational groups are

represented. Statistical analyses of follow-up data


from approximately 3000 cases treated over a

period of ten years reveal numerous significant

correlates of abstinence (Voegtlin & Broz, 1949).

Clients under 25 contributed the lowest rates of

sobriety (23 percent), whereas the incidence of


abstinence increased with each succeeding age
interval. Although occupational status, in itself, did

not appear to represent an important source of

variance, clients who presented a history of

unemployment and frequent job changes proved


to be considerably less responsive (21 percent)

than those with relatively stable employment


histories (71 percent). Similarly, a considerably

smaller proportion of charity cases (20 percent)

remained abstinent compared to middle class (49


percent) or wealthy (62 percent) participants.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1729
Continued association with drinking companions

accounted for a number of reversions to

alcoholism. It is not surprising, therefore, that


clients who joined abstinence clubs maintained

much higher sobriety rates (87 percent) than


those who refused to associate with such groups
(40 percent).

Of particular interest is the finding that clients’


willingness to, and actual participation in, periodic
reconditioning sessions during the year

immediately following treatment (when most

alcoholic reversions occur) significantly increased

the probability of continued abstinence (Voegtlin


et al., 1942). Of a total number of 155 clients who
initially agreed to participate in the post-therapy

program, 91 percent remained abstinent during

the year of the study, whereas the corresponding

figure for 73 clients who refused to volunteer for

follow-up sessions was 71 percent. In order to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1730
provide a further comparison group, every fourth

case was not offered the opportunity to receive

additional conditioning trials following the


completion of the basic treatment. The latter

control group yielded a 70 percent abstinence

rate.

It is difficult to determine from these data the

influence of motivation to change per se, since


some of the clients who initially volunteered for
additional sessions failed to return; conversely, an

unspecified number of control cases, who

subsequently learned of the program, voluntarily


requested and received the supplemental
treatment. It is clear from the within-groups

analysis, however, that abstinence rates are


positively related to the number of supplementary

conditioning sessions (Table 8-2). Based on the

overall findings of this study, an alcoholic who is

favorably disposed toward continued periodic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1731
treatment has an 86 percent chance of remaining

abstinent for at least one year.

Table 8-2 Percentage of Abstinence as a Function of the


Number of Supplementary Conditioning Sessions
Number of Number of Cases Percentage of Abstinence
Supplementary
Sessions

None 88 74
One 113 80
Two 57 95
Three 20 90
Four or more 7 100

Aversive counterconditioning is thus a simple,


brief, economical, and relatively effective method

for producing aversion to alcohol for at least a

limited period, and for continued total abstinence


in approximately 50 percent of the clients. The

aversion form of therapy offers the additional

advantages of ready acceptance by clients and


wide applicability. Contraindications primarily

include certain physical disorders such as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1732
gastrointestinal ulceration or hemorrhage, hernia,
hepatic cirrhosis, cardiac conditions, and coronary

disease. Despite this relatively favorable showing


aversion treatment of alcoholism has never been

widely accepted, and with the advent of

disulfiram, applications of counterconditioning

procedures have further declined. Although


disulfiram therapy relies upon aversive

contingencies for the maintenance of sobriety, as

we shall see next, this approach is aimed primarily

at suppressing drinking behavior and does not

necessarily alter the positive valence of alcoholic


beverages.

DISULFIRAM REGIMEN

In 1948, Hald & Jacobsen reported


experiments in which they found that persons

who had ingested Antabuse or disulfiram


(Tetraethylthiuram disulphide) for a period of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1733
time experienced intensely aversive physiological
reactions when they subsequently drank even

small amounts of alcohol. The disulfiram-alcohol


reactions (Bowman, Simon, Hine, Macklin, Crook,

Burbridge, & Hanson, 1951) consist initially of

disagreeable warmth or flushing in the face,

conjunctiva and tachycardia occurring


approximately 5 to 20 minutes after the intake of

alcohol, followed during the next 20 to 50 minutes

by headache, dyspnea, dizziness, nausea and

vomiting, chest pains, physical weakness, pallor,

and hangover symptoms. These reactions, which


usually persist for 1 to 2 hours, apparently result

from the action of disulfiram in blocking the


oxidation of alcohol at the stage of acetaldehyde.

Because of the slow elimination of the disulfiram


substance, a single dose can render a person

physiologically sensitized to alcoholic beverages


for a relatively long time. Reports of encouraging

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1734
results obtained through this method, along with

its simplicity, led to the widespread use of

Antabuse treatment for alcoholism.

The therapeutic regimen generally proceeds

along the following lines: On the first day


following detoxication the client is orally

administered 1 to 2 grams of disulfiram, and is

given progressively diminishing dosages on the


next three days. After the primary intolerance for
alcohol has been established, the client is then

given one or more test trials of alcohol in order to

determine the optimum maintenance dosage of

Antabuse. The dosage is adjusted individually to


the level where the characteristic, unpleasant side
effects of the drug are reduced to a minimum, but

the dosage is still adequate to produce sufficiently

intense reactions to deter further drinking. The

reactions to the test doses also serve to impress

upon the client the serious physical consequences

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1735
of ingesting even small amounts of alcohol while

on disulfiram. Following discharge, the client is

placed on a maintenance dosage which usually


varies from ⅛ to &frac12: gram tablet of

disulfiram taken each day either before breakfast

or in the evening (Bowman et al., 1951; Child,

Osinski, Bennett, & Davidoff, 1951).

Because of the violent physiological reactions


that can be elicited by alcohol when disulfiram is
present in the body, the primary intolerance to

alcohol and the maintenance dose are generally

established during a brief period of hospitalization


with the client under careful observation.
However, Martensen-Larsen (1953), who has

written authoritatively about this mode of


therapy, describes a therapeutic regimen that may

be conducted on an outpatient basis. The initial

dosage of disulfiram, calculated on the basis of 15

milligrams per kilogram of body weight, is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1736
administered to the client during his first

consultation visit. After the first treatment, the

size of the optimum maintenance dose is regulated


during a period of several months in accordance

with the individual’s response to alcohol test trials

and reported side effects.

Considerable statistical data are available

related to the efficacy of the disulfiram regimen. In


general, the abstinence rates associated with
follow-up periods of varying duration are

essentially of the same magnitude as those

obtained by aversive counterconditioning


(Bourne, Alford, & Bowcock, 1966; Bowman et al.,
1951; Brown & Knoblock, 1951; Child et al., 1951;

Epstein & Guild, 1951; Hoff & McKeown, 1953;


Jacobsen, 1950; Shaw, 1951). Despite the

voluminous statistical data relating to the

counterconditioning and disulfiram approaches,

there is a paucity of comparative investigations in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1737
which the relative efficacy of different modes of

therapy is systematically assessed within the same

experimental design.

The problem of evaluating different treatment

approaches is further complicated by the fact that

psychotherapists employing traditional interview

procedures generally confine their reports to

prescriptive statements concerning the proper


conduct of therapy or elaborate accounts of
psychodynamic processes, but they generally fail

to cite objective data regarding the efficacy of this

type of psychotherapeutic enterprise.


Questionnaire surveys further reveal that
psychotherapists are reluctant to treat alcoholics

because of their aversive disruptive behavior


(Hollingshead, 1956; Robinson & Podnos, 1966).

When interview methods are applied, the clinical

reports convey the impression that successful

outcomes are disappointingly low. Although there

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1738
are no adequate data available for estimating

precisely the base rates of change in addictive

drinking without psychotherapeutic interventions,


the average figures quoted are generally on the

order of 10 to 15 percent.

Comparative experimental studies of

conditioned aversion and disulfiram therapies are

particularly essential, because these two


approaches have proved to be most efficacious for
modifying and controlling chronic drinking

behavior. However, unlike counterconditioning

methods which involve few risks and


contraindications, potentially serious physical
effects may result from the use of disulfiram

should the client ingest moderate or large


amounts of alcohol while on the drug. Apart from

the physiological reactions to alcohol, a number of

unpleasant side effects of disulfiram have also

been noted; these include drowsiness, nausea,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1739
headache, unpleasant taste and body odor,

gastrointestinal disturbances, and sometimes

decreased sexual potency. These attendant


reactions may, in themselves, be sufficiently

disturbing to lead clients to terminate medication.

There is some evidence, however, that such side

effects can be substantially reduced by decreasing

the daily maintenance dose (Child et al., 1951;


Martensen-Larsen, 1953). The disulfiram
treatment regimen is also generally

contraindicated for clients suffering from

cardiovascular disorders, cirrhosis of the liver,

nephritis, diabetes, epilepsy, advanced


arteriosclerosis, and in cases of pregnancy.

It is possible that eventually an effective anti-


alcohol agent will be discovered that produces few

unpleasant side effects. Ferguson (1956), for

instance, reports a drug, citrated calcium

carbimide (CCC), whose action is similar to that of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1740
disulfiram in inhibiting acetaldehyde metabolism,

but which is free of some of its disagreeable

features. A preliminary experiment in which


different groups of alcoholics were treated with

CCC and with disulfiram revealed that fewer

subjects in the CCC group discontinued medication

of their own accord (Armstrong & Kerr, 1956).

In view of the possible physical manifestations


associated with disulfiram and the inconvenience
of continuous self-medication, the selection of this

mode of therapy over the shorter, safer, and more

economical counterconditioning methods would


be justified only if the pharmacological approach
were shown to yield significantly higher rates of

successful outcomes. In a study comparing the


relative efficacy of aversive counterconditioning,

Antabuse, group hypnotherapy, and milieu

therapy, Wallerstein (1957) found that Antabuse

was most efficacious according to an aggregate

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1741
rating based on degree of abstinence, general

social adjustment, “subjective feelings of

difference,” and changes in “personality structure.”


Results for the conditioning group, however, are at

such marked variance with those achieved by

other investigators that the findings of this

experiment must be accepted with reservation.

Yanushevskii (1959) analyzed the follow-up data


on 2000 alcoholics who had received either
medication, psychotherapy, hypnosis,

apomorphine-counterconditioning, or disulfiram

in a Moscow clinic. Conditioned aversion and

disulfiram proved superior to the other


procedures, but both of these therapeutic

approaches produced essentially similar


abstinence rates. Since there is no way of

determining what selective criteria were

employed in assigning cases to the different


treatment groups, these results have only

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1742
suggestive value. In view of the limited and

conflicting findings, any conclusion regarding the

relative efficacy of the methods under discussion


must be deferred until adequate empirical data

become available.

It should be emphasized that modifications in

drinking behavior produced by aversive

counterconditioning and by anti-alcohol drugs are


achieved through entirely different mechanisms.
In the case of disulfiram, abstinence is maintained

on a chemical basis. As long as the pills are taken

regularly, the potent physiological consequences

of drinking serve as a powerful deterrent.


However, the conditioning of aversive properties
to alcoholic beverages is precluded by the

relatively long temporal interval between the

ingestion of alcohol on the one hand, and the onset

of the aversive consequences on the other.

Consequently, alcohol retains its positive value

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1743
and the client is able to drink within several days

after terminating medication. Many alcoholics, in

fact, will take disulfiram intermittently and go on


drinking sprees during periods when their

physiological tolerance for alcohol has been

restored. The duration and degree of abstinence is,

therefore, contingent on the duration and

regularity with which medication is used


(Jacobsen, 1950).

The temporal prerequisites for aversive

conditioning are also absent from methods in

which nauseants are added to alcoholic beverages.


Under these conditions a person will refrain from
drinking emeticized cocktails but retain his strong

attraction to unmedicated alcoholic drinks. In


addition to physiologically induced restraints

against the use of alcohol, physical prevention

methods were also employed as a means of

ensuring sobriety in the early history of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1744
treatment of alcoholism. Persons were given

posthypnotic suggestions that they would develop

arm paralysis whenever they attempted to drink


alcoholic beverages. This mode of therapy not only

resulted in a considerable amount of spilt liquor,

but also fostered the acquisition of highly

ingenious drinking styles.

Unlike the preceding approaches, by creating


aversion reactions to the smell, taste, and thought
of alcohol, counterconditioning procedures

directly reduce the positive value of intoxicants

and, therefore, do not require the continuance of


externally imposed deterrents to drinking.

MULTIFORM TREATMENT OF ALCOHOLISM

Aversive counterconditioning alone has


proved most successful with alcoholics who have

developed their habituation by way of prolonged

heavy social drinking, and who possess sufficient

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1745
personal resources to derive adequate
gratifications from sober behavior (Thimann,

1949; Voegtlin & Broz, 1949). It is sometimes

erroneously assumed by critics of treatment


programs aimed at the direct modification of

drinking behavior that these approaches are based


on the premise that alcohol is the sole problem of
the alcoholic. Quite to the contrary, they assume

that psychological functioning involves a

reciprocal influence process in which the


characteristics of behavior are important

determiners of the way the environment responds

to it; as a person changes, so does his

environment. Sustained abstinence is therefore

largely ensured not by the fact that liquor has


been endowed with negative properties, but
because elimination of drinking behavior removes

the adverse consequences of chronic inebriation

and creates new reinforcement contingencies with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1746
respect to a broad range of behavior. The

restoration of physical well-being and the positive

experiences derived from improved social,


marital, and financial functioning can reinforce

sobriety and reduce aberrant tendencies. For this


reason, “neuroses” and grossly deviant behaviors
often disappear after alcoholism has been brought

under control (Jellinek, 1962; Thompson &

Bielinski, 1953). Evidently, the risks of aversion


therapy, even when employed as the sole method

of treatment, are minor compared to the hazards

of chronic inebriety.

The fact that 40 to 60 percent of the alcoholics


who receive aversion therapy eventually resume
excessive drinking after a period of abstinence

clearly reveals that, in certain cases, this method

must be supplemented with, or replaced by, other

programs if sobriety is to be maintained.

Traditional interpretive therapy is generally

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1747
assumed to be best suited for such cases, a belief

that persists despite evidence that, of the various

treatments available, interview approaches have


proved least successful in modifying chronic

drinking behavior.

Development of Behavioral Competencies.

Alcoholics whose drinking behavior has been

temporarily controlled are unlikely to remain


abstinent for long if they lack the behavioral
competencies for securing gratifications while

sober. Individuals deficient in educational

achievements and satisfying vocational skills and


those who have failed, for one reason or another,
to acquire interpersonal adroitness will be

subjected to considerable negative experiences. As


alcohol is increasingly used to provide escape

from an unrewarding existence, the alcoholic’s

initially minimal competencies typically undergo

further deterioration, resulting in even greater

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1748
aversive experiences and avoidance. Therefore,

alcoholics who present behavioral deficits require

a treatment program employing modeling and


reinforcement procedures designed to establish

behavioral competencies. It is assumed that as

positively reinforcing behaviors are developed

they will compete with, and eventually replace,

alcoholic escape and avoidance.

In a pilot project Narrol (1967) employed


reinforcement principles to promote vocational

activities in chronic, hospitalized alcoholics. A

simulated economy was devised in which points


earned for performance of work assignments were
used to purchase commissary items, clothing,

hospital leaves, recreational opportunities, and


room and board on wards that varied in the

comforts and freedom they provided. All members

in this project devoted approximately twice as

much time to their work assignments as did

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1749
alcoholics for whom the hospital privileges were

not made contingent upon work performance. In

accord with previous findings, this project


demonstrates that a simulated economy will

control behavior as long as the contingencies

remain in effect. To test the therapeutic efficacy of

this type of program, reinforcement practices

would need to be applied over a long period of


time, and extended to a wide range of behaviors,
including drinking behavior under conditions

where alcoholic beverages are readily available.

Modification of Self-Reinforcement Patterns.


There is a third class of alcoholics who experience
a great deal of aversive stimulation, not because of

behavioral deficits or unavailability of rewarding


resources, but because they impose exceedingly

severe performance demands on themselves.

Events and accomplishments considered worthy

of self-approval by most persons are viewed by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1750
alcoholics who have set themselves high standards

for self-reinforcement as marginal or inadequate

performances. Consequently, such persons not


only deny themselves warranted gratifications but

they also engage in a great deal of self-castigation,

from which they periodically escape through

alcoholic intoxication. The primary objective in the

treatment of alcoholics who are escaping from


self-generated aversive consequences would
involve the lowering of standards for self-

reinforcement, rather than the elimination of

behavioral deficits.

Desensitization of Stress-Provoking Situations.


Under conditions where drinking behavior is

strongly controlled by relief from aversive


stimulation, a desensitization form of treatment

would constitute the method of choice. Kraft and

Al-Issa (1967a, b) report success in modifying

alcoholism by desensitizing clients to stressful

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1751
interpersonal situations that typically provoked

them to drink. Although the sample sizes are too

small to draw reliable conclusions, Blake (1967)


found that alcoholics who received aversion

therapy combined with relaxation training were

better able to maintain complete abstinence over a

one-year period than those who were

administered aversive counterconditioning alone.


Had relaxation been deliberately employed to
neutralize sources of tension and anxiety the

differences might have been even more marked.

The discussion thus far has focused on the


individual methods best suited for modifying
different conditions which may exert control over

drinking behavior. In many cases, achievement of


stable changes in alcoholism requires a

combination of treatment procedures in which

people are desensitized to situations they find

stressful; they acquire rewarding patterns of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1752
behavior which will become prepotent over

alcoholic self-anesthetization; they develop

aversions to alcoholic beverages; and they are


taught other self-control techniques for

forestalling drinking in their natural environment.

Social-Systems Approach to the Treatment of

Widespread Alcoholism. Counterconditioning, or

any other individual treatment approach, is of


little value in modifying the drinking behavior of
Skid Row alcoholics. The impoverished personal

resources of these disadvantaged persons, and the

deviant reinforcement contingencies existing


within the Skid Row milieu, serve as powerful
influences in shaping a more or less irreversible

alcoholic destiny (Pittman & Gordon, 1958).

The Skid Row subculture provides social aliens

a refuge from an otherwise demoralizing and

unrewarding existence, in which few demands are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1753
made of its members, anonymity is respected,
failures and deviant tendencies are not

condemned, and achievement strivings and other


behaviors that are actively reinforced in the larger

society are either nonrewarded or disapproved

(Jackson & Connor, 1953). Within this milieu, in

addition to the low level of aversive control and


the adoption of anti-achievement standards, the

major positive reinforcements center around

drinking behavior. Social prestige is largely

contingent on being a good drinking partner and

on the ability to get enough to drink without


having to resort to gainful employment. Similarly,

most interpersonal rewards occur during the


mutual sharing of alcohol in drinking cliques or

“bottle gangs.” These patterns of reinforcement


not only promote continuous inebriation, but by

attaching negative values to societal norms and


demands, they also establish barriers to reentry

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1754
into the larger community.

Except for repeated incarceration for public


intoxication and conspicuous violation of other

legal codes, Skid Row alcoholics receive little or no

constructive attention. For the most part, they are


left to themselves to control their own behavior

according to their own deviant norms. Therefore

the rehabilitation of Skid Row alcoholics must


involve an extensive resocialization process that
can be achieved only in a markedly different

environment. If they are to be successful

participants in the larger society, alcoholics must

acquire, among other things, a new set of


incentives and behavioral norms, a wide variety of
social competencies, and esteem- and income-

producing vocational skills. Social organizations

such as Alcoholics Anonymous (1952) may

provide some of the learning conditions necessary

for the attainment of resocialization objectives,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1755
but these programs fail to reach those who, on the

basis of an extended extinction history, have little

reason to share the societal values, and who are


therefore unlikely to seek rehabilitation

voluntarily. Since any radical change in the

alcoholic’s social activities will inevitably lose him

the gratifications associated with the drinking

subculture, willingness to undergo relatively


extensive behavioral modifications cannot be
obtained without the provision of more rewarding

alternatives.

To accomplish fundamental changes in the


behavior of persons from a deviant subculture, it
is necessary to create social systems that provide

the necessary conditions for learning new styles of


life. Such a system must teach new skills; it must

furnish exemplary role models; and it must

embody a set of reinforcement contingencies that

will counteract deviant activities and promote

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1756
more constructive modes of behavior. It is

interesting to note, as dramatically illustrated by

the Synanon approach to the treatment of drug


addiction (Yablonsky, 1965), that these types of

social systems often prove most successful when

they are developed by persons for their own

rehabilitation.

Ethical Considerations in Aversion Therapy

The use of aversive procedures in the

modification of human behavior generally meets


with either a cool or a hostile reception on the part

of professional psychotherapists. In some cases


aversive techniques are applied in an ethically
objectionable manner that justifies censorious

reactions. For example, exceedingly noxious

procedures are occasionally employed even

though they produce no greater changes than

stimuli in much weaker intensities (Campbell,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1757
Sanderson, & Laverty, 1964; Clancy, Vanderhoof, &

Campbell, 1967; Hsu, 1965). Oswald (1962) has

promulgated an ill-conceived procedure in which


clients not only receive massed aversion trials, but

are also subjected to disparaging personal

remarks played endlessly on a recorded tape. The

rationale for these needless personal assaults,

some of which are recorded in the therapist’s


voice, is apparently based on Sargent’s (1957)
impressions that social conversions are facilitated

by intense emotional crises. Considering that the

verbalizations were “designed deliberately to

disturb the patient emotionally,” it is


understandable that a number of the clients

exhibited suspicious animosity, unplugged tape


recorders, and refused to submit themselves to

this inexorable ordeal. It should be emphasized

that the brainwashing prescription, which is


antithetical to practices derived from learning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1758
principles, is better designed to instill marked

aversion to therapeutic agents than to stimulus

events that evoke deviant behavior in clients.

Many of the applications of

counterconditioning based on nauseous drugs


employ massed aversion trials in which the

procedures are continuously administered at two-

hour intervals over a period of several days.


Raymond, who originally devised this method, has
subsequently questioned the necessity for such a

rigorous regimen (Raymond, 1964). Apart from

ethical considerations, massed aversive

experiences can produce many undesirable side


effects that seriously obstruct progress. With
repeated administrations of pharmaceuticals,

physical tolerance develops and drugs become less

effective. Therapists are, therefore, forced to use

increasingly larger dosages or less desirable

emetic mixtures in order to induce sufficient

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1759
emesis (Cooper, 1963). Moreover, persons who

are repeatedly subjected to unpleasant

experiences without any rewarding respites and


opportunities for positive relationships with the

therapists administering the aversive procedures

are likely to develop resentment, antipathy to the

entire treatment situation, and escape behavior.

Many clients who have completed a course of


aversion therapy may need to return occasionally
for supplementary sessions if they find themselves

reverting to their formerly deviant activities. One

effect of a massive aversive experience is to

reinforce strong avoidance of the treatment


situation itself on future occasions, even though a

limited reconditioning experience might yield


highly beneficial results. For these reasons, a

substantial amount of positive reinforcement

should be incorporated into aversive conditioning


procedures.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1760
If the major purpose of aversion experiences is

to provide clients with a means of exercising

control over harmful behavior, then clients should


play an active role in practicing self-control

techniques in the presence of progressively


stronger evocative stimuli, rather than serving
merely as passive recipients of stimulus pairings.

Thus, for example, in the treatment of alcoholism,

after persons have been taught how to self-induce


nauseous feelings, they should be exposed for

increasingly longer periods to social and stress

situations that involve high instigation to drinking


behavior. In this type of program self-control

techniques are developed, repeatedly tested, and


adequately reinforced.

The fact that some applications of aversive

procedures contain objectionable features does

not warrant a blanket indictment of the

responsible use of aversion therapy (Allchin,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1761
1964; Matthews, 1964) under special

circumstances demanding a more drastic form of

therapeutic intervention, any more than one


would be justified in condemning the use of

surgical and dental procedures with patients

willing to undergo a brief painful experience in

order to alleviate more deleterious and long-

lasting suffering. The brief discomfort occasioned


by a program of aversion therapy is minor
compared to the repeated incarceration, social

ostracism, serious disruption of family life, and

self-condemnation resulting from uncontrollable

injurious behavior. It is a therapist’s responsibility


to provide clients with information about the

treatment alternatives available to them and


which outcomes are most likely to result from

each choice. Given this knowledge, it should be the

client’s right to decide what types of treatment, if


any, he wishes to undergo.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1762
As noted earlier, aversion therapy has proved

least effective with sexual deviants who are

coerced into treatment in an attempt to change


their behavior in the direction of conformity with

more conventional practices. In cases where their


conduct threatens the welfare of others, they have
the choice of either altering their injurious

behavior or having their freedom revoked. There

are other forms of sexual activities, however,


which are also legally prohibited, such as cross-sex

dressing and homosexuality involving consenting

adults, that generally have no adverse


consequences for others. As sexual mores undergo

further changes private consensual sexual


behavior between adults will eventually be

legalized. Nevertheless, deviant sexual practices

will continue to be subjected to social ridicule and,


hence, to serve as a source of emotional

disturbance. After threat of criminal sanctions is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1763
removed, persons who seek to alter their sexual

orientation will do so under motivational

conditions that are more favorable for achieving


behavioral changes.

Summary

This chapter is primarily concerned with


classical aversion treatment of exceedingly

persistent behavior that is maintained by


inappropriate, potentially harmful, or culturally

prohibited positive reinforcers. This stimulus-

oriented approach attempts to establish control


over behavior by endowing formerly attractive
stimuli or symbolic representations of deviant

activities with negative properties through

contiguous association with aversive experiences.

These negative experiences are typically induced

by administering nauseous pharmacological

agents or unpleasant shocks, or they may take the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1764
form of symbolically revivified feelings of

revulsion.

Aversive counterconditioning is not regarded


as a process in which aversive reactions become

directly and automatically attached to formerly

positive stimuli. Rather, the counterconditioning

procedure establishes an aversive self-stimulation

mechanism which enables persons to counteract


the disposition to engage in deviant behavior by
symbolically reinstating nauseous reactions

previously experienced in treatment. The most

direct evidence that conditioned aversions


represent, in large part, self-induced reactions,
rather than automatic products of stimulus

pairings, is provided by laboratory studies


demonstrating that classically conditioned

responses are amenable to symbolic control.

Viewed from this perspective, aversive

counterconditioning creates a means of self-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1765
control rather than automatic immunity to

addictive or rewarding stimuli.

Aversive procedures have been applied most


extensively to harmful addictive behaviors and to

various types of sexual aberrations. Treatment

programs aimed at modifying sexual disorders

usually involve differential conditioning of sexual

responsiveness, wherein aversion is developed to


fetishistic, transvestite, or homoerotic stimuli,
while concurrently erotic arousal properties are

conditioned to heterosexual stimuli. The summary

of results, based mainly on single case studies,


indicates that this mode of therapy can, in addition
to eliminating deviant sexuality, assist in

promoting heterosexual behavior, provided that


these alternative responses already exist in the

person’s repertoire and that environmental

conditions are favorable for maintaining them. On

the other hand, an exclusively aversive form of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1766
therapy is much less effective in cases where

heterosexual behaviors are either lacking or

strongly inhibited. In the latter conditions,


aversive counterconditioning must be

supplemented with procedures designed to

develop behavioral patterns that will enable

persons to engage in rewarding heterosexual

interactions.

Similarly variable outcomes have been


obtained in the counterconditioning treatment of

chronic alcoholism. Aversive procedures alone

have proved most successful with alcoholics who


possess sufficient personal resources to derive
adequate gratifications from sober behavior. In

most cases, however, the treatment must also be


directed toward the conditions that control

drinking behavior. This might involve

development of behavioral competencies to the

point where sober behavior is sufficiently

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1767
reinforcing to predominate over anesthetic

avoidance; the lowering of standards for self-

reinforcement that result in self-generated


aversive consequences; and, in cases where

excessive drinking is controlled by relief from

interpersonal stresses, the desensitization of

primary sources of tension and anxiety.

Of particular interest are studies


demonstrating that symbolic events, which may
serve as important internal elicitors of deviant

behavior, are modifiable through aversive

conditioning. When imagery possessing affective


value is repeatedly paired with negative
experiences, the symbolic events not only lose

their arousal potential but they are less frequently


self-generated. The imaginal counterconditioning

is generally accompanied by reductions in the

corresponding behavior.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1768
The major value of aversive procedures is that

they provide a rapid means of achieving control

over injurious behavior for a period during which


alternative, and more rewarding, modes of

behavior can be established. A treatment which


addresses itself to both stimulus and response
events is most likely to yield uniformly favorable

results because it not only alters the valence of

stimuli that evoke deviant behavior, but also


creates reinforceable response patterns.

REFERENCES

Abrams, S. An evaluation of hypnosis in the treatment


of alcoholics. American Journal of Psychiatry,
1964, 120, 1160-1165.

Agras, W. S. Behavior therapy in the management of


chronic schizophrenia. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 1967, 124, 240-243.

Alcoholics Anonymous. Twelve steps and twelve


traditions. New York: Alcoholics Anonymous
Publishing Co., 1952.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1769
Allchin, W. H. Behaviour therapy. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1964, 110, 108.

Anant, S. S. A note on the treatment of alcoholics by a


verbal aversion technique. Canadian Psychologist,
1967, 8, 19-22.

Armstrong, J. D., & Kerr, H. T. A new protective drug in


the treatment of alcoholism. Canadian Medical
Association Journal, 1956, 74, 795-797.

Ashem, B., & Donner, L. Covert sensitization with


alcoholics: a controlled replication. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1968, 6, 7-12.

Bales, R. F. Cultural differences in rates of alcoholism.


Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1946, 6,
480-499.

Bancroft, J. H. J., Jones, H. G., & Pullan, B. R. A simple


transducer for measuring penile erection, with
comments on its use in the treatment of sexual
disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966,
4, 239-241.

Barker, J. C., & Miller, M. Aversion therapy for


compulsive gambling. Lancet, 1966,1, 491-492.

Barker, J. C., Thorpe, J. G., Blakemore, C. B., Lavin, N. I.,


& Conway, C. G. Behaviour therapy in a case of
transvestism. Lancet, 1961, 1, 510.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1770
Barlow, D. H., Leitenberg, H., & Agras, W. S. The
experimental control of sexual deviation through
manipulation of the noxious scene in covert
sensitization. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
1969, 74, 596-601.

Barry, H., Wagner, A. R., & Miller, N. E. Effects of


alcohol and amobarbital on performance
inhibited by experimental extinction. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1962,
55, 464-468.

Blake, B. G. A follow-up of alcoholics treated by


behaviour therapy. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1967, 5, 89-94.

Blakemore, C. B., Thorpe, J. G., Barker, J. C., Conway, C.


G., & Lavin, N. I. The application of faradic
aversion conditioning in a case of transvestism.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1963,1, 29-34.

Blough, D. S. Technique for studying the effects of


drugs on discrimination in the pigeon. Annals of
the New York Academy of Sciences, 1956, 65, 334-
344.

Bond, I. K., & Hutchison, H. C. Application of reciprocal


inhibition therapy to exhibitionism. In H. J.
Eysenck (Ed.), Experiments in behaviour therapy.
New York: Pergamon, 1964. pp. 80-86.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1771
Bourne, P. G., Alford, J. A., & Bowcock, J. Z. Treatment
of Skid Row alcoholics with disulfiram. Quarterly
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1966, 27, 42-48.

Bowman, K. M., Simon, A., Hine, C. H., Macklin, E. A.,


Crook, G. H., Bur- bridge, N., & Hanson, K. A
clinical evaluation of tetraethylthiuram
disulphide (Antabuse) in the treatment of
problem drinkers. American Journal of Psychiatry,
1951, 107, 832-838.

Brown, C. T., & Knoblock, E. C. Antabuse therapy in the


Army. A preliminary report of fifty cases. U. S.
Armed Forces Medical Journal, 1951, 2, 191-202.

Bruun, K. Significance of role and norms in the small


group for individual behavioral changes while
drinking. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
1959, 20, 53-64.

Campbell, D., Sanderson, R. E., & Laverty, S. G.


Characteristics of a conditioned response in
human subjects during extinction trials following
a single traumatic conditioning trial. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964, 68, 627-
6.39.

Carpenter, J. A. Effects of alcoholic beverages on skin


conductance: An exploratory study. Quarterly
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1957, 18, 1-18.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1772
Casey, A. The effect of stress on the consumption of
alcohol and reserpine. Quarterly Journal of
Studies on Alcohol, 1960, 21, 208-216.

Cautela, J. R. Treatment of compulsive behavior by


covert sensitization. Psychological Record, 1966,
16, 33-41.

Chafetz, M. E., & Demone, H. W., Jr. Alcoholism and


society. New York: Oxford University Press, 1962.

Child, G. P., Osinski, W., Bennett, R. E., & Davidoff, E.


Therapeutic results and clinical manifestations
following the use of tetraethylthiuram disulfide.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 1951,107, 774-
780.

Clancy, J., Vanderhoof, E., & Campbell, P. Evaluation of


an aversive technique as a treatment of
alcoholism. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 28, 476-485.

Clark, D. F. Fetishism treated by negative


conditioning. British Journal of Psychiatry,
1963,109, 404-407. (a)

Clark, D. F. Treatment of fetishism by negative


conditioning—A further note. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1963,109,695-696. (b)

Clark, R., & Polish, E. Avoidance conditioning and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1773
alcohol consumption in rhesus monkeys. Science,
1960, 132, 223-224.

Clark, R. A., & Sensibar, M. R. The relationship


between symbolic and manifest projections of
sexuality with some incidental correlates. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, 50, 327-
334.

Conger, J. J. The effects of alcohol on conflict behavior


in the albino rat. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 1951,12, 1-29.

Conn, J. H. Ilypno-synthesis. IV. Hypnotherapy of the


sex offender. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 1954, 2, 13-26.

Cooper, A. J. A case of fetishism and impotence treated


by behaviour therapy. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1963, 109, 649-652.

Costello, C. G. The essentials of behaviour therapy.


Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 1963, 8,
162-166.

Curran, D., & Parr, D. Homosexuality: An analysis of


100 male cases seen in private practice. British
Medical Journal, 1957, 1, 797-801.

Davies, D. L. Normal drinking in recovered alcohol


addicts. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1774
1962, 23, 94-104.

Davison, G. C. Elimination of a sadistic fantasy by a


client-controlled counterconditioning technique:
A case study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73,
84-90.

DeMorsier, G., & Feldmann, H. Le traitement


biologique de l’alcoolisme chronique par
l’apomorphine. Etude de 200 cas. (The biological
treatment of chronic alcoholism with
apomorphine. Study of 200 cases.) Schweizer
Archiv fur Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 1950, 65,
472-473.

Docter, R. F., & Perkins, R. B. The effects of ethyl


alcohol on autonomic and muscular responses in
humans. I. Dosage of 0.5 milliliter per kilogram.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1961, 22,
374-386.

Edlin, J. V., Johnson, R. H., Hletko, P., & Heilbrunn, G.


The conditioned aversion treatment of chronic
alcoholism. Preliminary report. Archives of
Neurology and Psychiatry, 1945, 53, 85-87.

Epstein, N. B., & Guild, J. Further clinical experience


with TEDT in the treatment of alcoholism.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1951, 12,
366-380.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1775
Eriksen, C. W., & Kuethe, J. L. Avoidance conditioning
of verbal behavior without awareness: A
paradigm of repression. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1956, 53, 203-209.

Evans, D. R. An exploratory study into the treatment


of exhibitionism by means of emotive imagery
and aversive conditioning. Canadian Psychologist,
1967, 8,162.

Evans, D. R. Masturbatory fantasy and sexual


deviation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1968,
6, 17-19.

Eysenck, H. J. Behaviour therapy, extinction, and


relapse in neurosis. British Journal of Psychiatry,
1963, 109, 12-18.

Ferguson, J. K. W. A new drug for alcoholism


treatment. 1. A new drug for the treatment of
alcoholism. Canadian Medical Association Journal,
1956, 74, 793-795.

Fleck, S. Family dynamics and origin of schizophrenia.


Psychosomatic Medicine. 1960, 22, 333-344.

Ford, C. S., & Beach, F. A. Patterns of sexual behavior.


New York: Harper, 1951.

Fort, T., & Porterfield, A. L. Some backgrounds and


types of alcoholism among women. Journal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1776
Health and Human Behavior, 1961, 2, 283-292.

Franks, C. M. Conditioning and conditioned aversion


therapies in the treatment of the alcoholic.
International Journal of Addiction, 1966, 1, 61-96.

Freund, K. Some problems in the treatment of


homosexuality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), Behaviour
therapy and the neuroses. New York: Pergamon,
1960. pp. 312-325.

Freund, K. A laboratory method for diagnosing


predominance of homo- or hetero-erotic interest
in the male. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
1963, 1, 85-93.

Freund, K. Diagnosing homo- or heterosexuality and


erotic age-preference by means of a
psychophysiological test. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1967, 5,209-228. (a)

Freund, K. Erotic preference in pedophilia. Behaviour


Research and Therapy, 1967, 5,209-348. (b)

Freund, K., Sedlacek, F., & Knob, K. A simple


transducer for mechanical plethysmography of
the male genital. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1965, 8, 169-170.

Fromer, R., & Berkowitz, L. Effect of sudden and


gradual shock onset on the conditioned fear

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1777
response. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, 1964, 57, 154-155.

Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, F. J., & Koelling, R. A. Conditioned


aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to
gamma radiation. Science, 1955, 122, 157-158.

Gelder, M. G., & Marks, I. M. Aversion treatment in


transvestism and transsexualism. In R. Green
(Ed.), Transsexualism and sex reassignment.
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1969 (in press).

Giffin, M. W., Johnson, A. M., & Litin, E. M. Antisocial


acting out: 2. Specific factors determining
antisocial acting out. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 1954, 24, 668-684.

Glad, D. D. Attitudes and experiences of American-


Jewish and American-Irish male youth as related
to differences in adult rates of inebriety.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1947, 8,
406-472.

Glynn, J. D., & Harper, P. Behaviour therapy in


transvestism. Lancet, 1961, 1, 619.

Goorney, A. B. Treatment of a compulsive horse race


gambler by aversion therapy. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1968, 114, 329-333.

Greenberg, L. A., & Carpenter, J. A. The effect of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1778
alcoholic beverages on skin conductance and
emotional tension. I. Wine, whisky, and alcohol.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1957, 18,
190-204.

Hald, J., & Jacobsen, E. A drug sensitizing the organism


to ethyl alcohol. Lancet, 1948, 2, 1001-1004.

Harris, M. B. A self-directed program for weight


control: A pilot study. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 1969 (in press).

Hess, E. H. Pupillometric assessment. In J. M. Shlien


(Ed.), Research in psychotherapy. Washington, D.
C.: American Psychological Association, 1968. pp.
573-583.

Hess, E. H., Seltzer, A. L., & Shlien, J. M. Pupil response


of hetero- and homosexual males to pictures of
men and women: A pilot study. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 70, 165-168.

Hill, M. J., & Blane, H. T. Evaluation of psychotherapy


with alcoholics. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 1967, 28, 76-104.

Hoff, E. C., & McKeown, C. E. An evaluation of the use


of tetraethylthiuram disulfide in the treatment of
560 cases of alcohol addiction. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 1953,109, 670-673.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1779
Hogans, A. F., Moreno, C. M., & Brodie, D. A. Effects of
ethyl alcohol on EEG and avoidance behavior of
chronic electrode monkeys. American Journal of
Physiology, 1961, 201,434-436.

Hollingshead, A. B. Views on the etiology of


alcoholism. In H. D. Kruse (Ed.), Alcohol as a
medical problem. New York: Harper, 1956. pp. 56-
67.

Holmberg, A. R. The Siriono. Unpublished doctoral


dissertation, Yale University, 1946.

Hsu, J. J. Electroconditioning therapy of alcoholics. A


preliminary report. Quarterly Journal of Studies
on Alcohol, 1965, 26, 449-459.

Isbell, H., Fraser, H. F., Wikler, A., Belleville, R. E., &


Eiseman, A. J. An experimental study of the
etiology of “Rum fits” and delirium tremens.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1955, 16,
1-33.

Jackson, J. K., & Connor, R. The skid road alcoholic.


Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1953, 14,
468-486.

Jacobsen, E. Biochemical methods in the treatment of


alcoholism, with special reference to Antabuse.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 1950,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1780
43, 519-526.

James, B. Case of homosexuality treated by aversion


therapy. British Medical Journal, 1962,1, 768-770.

Jellinek, E. M. Phases of alcohol addiction. In D. J.


Pittman & C. R. Snyder (Eds.), Society, culture, and
drinking patterns. New York: Wiley, 1962. pp.
356-368.

Johnson, A. M. Factors in the etiology of fixations and


symptom choice. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 1953,
22, 475-496.

Kant, F. Further modifications in the technique of


conditioned reflex treatment of alcohol addiction.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1944, 5,
229-232.

Kant, F. The use of conditioned reflex in the treatment


of alcohol addicts. Wisconsin Medical Journal,
1945, 44, 217-221.

Kantorovich, N. V. An attempt of curing alcoholism by


associated reflexes. Novoye Refleksologii nervnoy i
Fiziologii Sistemy, 1928, 3, 436-445. Cited by
Razran, G. H. S., Conditioned withdrawal
responses with shock as the conditioning
stimulus in adult human subjects. Psychological
Bulletin, 1934, 31,111-143.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1781
Kaplan, H. S. The effects of alcohol on fear extinction.
Dissertation Abstracts, 571-572.

Kendell, R. E. Normal drinking by former alcohol


addicts. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
1965, 26, 247-257.

Koenig, K. P., & Masters, J. Experimental treatment of


habitual smoking. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1965, 3, 235-243.

Kolb, L. C., & Johnson, A. M. Etiology and therapy of


overt homosexuality. Psychoanalytic Quarterly,
1955, 24, 506-515.

Kolvin, I. “Aversive imagery” treatment in


adolescents. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
1967, 5, 245-248.

Korman, M., & Stephens, H. D. Effects of training on


the alcohol consummatory response in rats.
Psychological Reports, 1960, 6, 327-331.

Korn, S. J. The relationship between individual


differences in the responsivity of rats to stress
and intake of alcohol. Quarterly Journal of Studies
on Alcohol, 1960, 21, 605-617.

Korpmann, E., & Hughes, F. W. Potentiating effect of


alcohol on tranquilizers and other central
depressants. AM A Archives of General Psychiatry,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1782
1959, 1,7-11.

Kraft, T., & Al-Issa, I. Alcoholism treated by


desensitization: A case report. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 69-70. (a)

Kraft, T., & Al-Issa, I. Desensitization and reduction in


cigarette consumption. Journal of Psychology,
1967, 67,323-329. (b)

Kushner, M., & Sandler, J. Aversion therapy and the


concept of punishment. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1966, 4, 179-186.

Lavin, N. I., Thorpe, J. G., Barker, J. C., Blakemore, C. B.,


& Conway, C. G. Behaviour therapy in a case of
transvestism. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 1961, 133, 346-353.

Lemere, F., & Voegtlin, W. L. Conditioned reflex


therapy of alcoholic addiction: Specificity of
conditioning against chronic alcoholism.
California and Western Medicine, 1940, 53, 268-
269.

Lemere, F., & Voegtlin, W. L. An evaluation of the


aversion treatment of alcoholism. Quarterly
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1950, 11, 199-204.

Lemere, F., Voegtlin, W. L., Broz, W. R., O’Hollaren, P.,


& Tupper, W. E. Conditioned reflex treatment of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1783
chronic alcoholism: VII. Technic. Diseases of the
Nervous System, 1942, 3, 243-247. (a)

Lemere, F., Voegtlin, W. L., Broz, W. R., O’Hollaren, P.,


& Tupper, W. E. The conditioned reflex treatment
of chronic alcoholism: VIII. A review of six years’
experience with this treatment of 1526 patients.
Journal of the American Medical Association, 1942,
120, 269-270. (b)

Lester, D. Self-maintenance of intoxication in the rat.


Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1961, 22,
223-231.

Lester, D. Self-selection of alcohol by animals, human


variation, and the etiology of alcoholism.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1966, 27,
395-438.

Lienert, G. A., & Traxel, W. The effects of


meprobamate and alcohol on galvanic skin
response. Journal of Psychology, 1959, 48, 329-
334.

Litin, E. M., Giffin, M. E., & Johnson, A. M. Parental


influence in unusual sexual behavior in children.
Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 1956, 25, 37-55.

McCarthy, R. (Ed.) Drinking and intoxication. Glencoe,


Ill.: Free Press, 1959.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1784
McConaghy, N. Penile volume change to moving
pictures of male and female nudes in
heterosexual and homosexual males. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 43-48.

McDonnell, G. J., & Carpenter, J. A. Anxiety, skin


conductance, and alcohol. A study of the relation
between anxiety and skin conductance and the
effect of alcohol on the conductance of subjects in
groups. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
1959, 20, 38-52.

McGuire, R. J., Carlisle, J. M., & Young, B. G. Sexual


deviations as conditioned behaviour: A
hypothesis. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
1965, 2, 185-190.

McGuire, R. J., & Vallance, M. Aversion therapy by


electric shock: A simple technique. British
Medical Journal, 1964, 1, 151-153.

Malinowski, B. The sexual life of savages in north-


western Melanesia. New York: Harcourt, Brace,
1929.

Mardones, J. Experimentally induced changes in the


free selection of alcohol. International Review of
Neurobiology, 1960, 2, 41-76.

Marks, I. M., & Gelder, M. G. Transvestism and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1785
fetishism: Clinical and psychological changes
during faradic aversion. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1967,113,711-729.

Marks, I. M., Rachman, S., & Gelder, M. G. Methods for


assessment of aversion treatment in fetishism
with masochism. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1965, 3, 253-258.

Martensen-Larsen, O. Five years’ experience with


disulfiram in the treatment of alcoholics.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1953, 14,
406-418.

Masserman, J. H. Behavior and neurosis. Chicago:


University of Chicago Press, 1943.

Masserman, J. H., & Yum, K. S. An analysis of the


influence of alcohol on experimental neurosis in
cats. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1946, 8, 36-52.

Matthews, P. C. Behaviour therapy. British Journal of


Psychiatry, 1964, 110, 108.

Max, L. W. Breaking up a homosexual fixation by the


conditioned reaction technique: A case study.
Psychological Bulletin, 1935, 32, 734 (Abstract).

Mees, H. L. Sadistic fantasies modified by aversive


conditioning and substitution: A case study.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 317-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1786
320.

Mello, N. K., & Mendelson, J. H. Operant analysis of


drinking patterns of chronic alcoholics. Nature,
1965, 206, 43-46.

Mendelson, J. H., & La Dou, J. Experimentally induced


chronic intoxication and withdrawal in
alcoholics. Part 2, Psychophysiological findings.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1964,
Supplement No. 2, 14-39.

Mestrallet, A., & Lang, A. Indications, techniques et


resultats du traitement par l’apomorphine de
l’alcoolisme psychiatrique. (Indications,
technique, and results in apomorphine therapy of
psychiatric alcoholism). Le Journal de Medecine de
Lyon, 1959, 40, 279-285.

Miller, E. C., Dvorak, A., & Turner, D. W. A method of


creating aversion to alcohol by reflex
conditioning in a group setting. Quarterly Journal
of Studies on Alcohol, 1960, 21, 424-431.

Miller, M. M. Treatment of chronic alcoholism by


hypnotic aversion. Journal of the American
Medical Association, 1959, 171, 1492-1495.

Miller, M. M. Hypnotic-aversion treatment of


homosexuality. Journal of the National Medical

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1787
Association, 1963, 55, 411-415.

Miller, N. E. Some recent studies of conflict behavior


and drugs. American Psychologist, 1961,16, 12-24.

Morgenstern, F. S., Pearce, J. F., & Rees, W. L.


Predicting the outcome of behaviour therapy by
psychological tests. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1965, 2, 191-200.

Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and the symbolic


processes. New York: Wiley, 1960.

Narrol, H. G. Experimental application of


reinforcement principles to the analysis and
treatment of hospitalized alcoholics. Quarterly
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1967, 28, 105-115.

Oswald, I. Induction of illusory and hallucinatory


voices with considerations of behaviour therapy.
Journal of Mental Science, 1962, 108, 196-212.

Pawlowski, A. A., Denenberg, V. H., & Zarrow, M. X.


Prolonged alcohol consumption in the rat. 2.
Acquisition and extinction of an escape response.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1961, 22,
232-240.

Peacock, L. J., & Watson, J. A. Radiation-induced


aversion to alcohol. Science, 1964,143, 1462-
1463.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1788
Perloff, W. H. Hormones and homosexuality. In J.
Marmor (Ed.), Sexual inversion. New York: Basic
Books, 1965. pp. 44-69.

Pittman, D. J., & Gordon, C. W. Revolving door: A study


of the chronic police case inebriate. Glencoe, Ill.:
Free Press, 1958.

Pittman, D. J., & Snyder, C. R. (Eds.) Society, culture,


and drinking patterns. New York: Wiley, 1962.

Quinn, J. T., & Henbest, R. Partial failure of


generalization in alcoholics following aversion
therapy. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
1967, 28, 70-75.

Rachman, S. Aversion therapy: Chemical or electrical?


Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1965, 2, 289-
299.

Rachman, S. Sexual fetishism: An experimental


analogue. Psychological Record, 293-296.

Raymond, M. J. Case of fetishism treated by aversion


therapy. British Medical Journal, 1956, 2, 854-
857.

Raymond, M. J. The treatment of addiction by aversion


conditioning with apomorphine. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1964, 1, 287-291.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1789
Raymond, M., & O’Keeffe, K. A case of pin-up fetishism
treated by aversion conditioning. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 1965, 111, 579-581.

Richter, C. P. Production and control of alcoholic


craving in rats. In Neuropharmacology,
Proceedings of the third conference. New York:
Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation, 1956. pp. 39-146.

Robinson, L. H., & Podnos, B. Resistance of


psychiatrists in treatment of alcoholism. Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1966, 143, 220-
225.

Rodgers, D. A., & McClearn, G. E. Mouse strain


differences in preference for various
concentrations of alcohol. Quarterly Journal of
Studies on Alcohol, 1962, 23, 26-33.

Ruck, F. Alkoholentziehungskur mit Hilfe eines


bedingten Reflexes
(Apomorphinentziehungskur). (Conditioned
reflex treatment of alcoholism.) Psychiatrie,
Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie, 1956, 8,
88-92.

Sargent, W. Battle for the mind: A physiology of


conversion and brainwashing. London:
Heinemann, 1957.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1790
Shanahan, W. M., & Hornick, E. J. Aversion treatment
of alcoholism. Hawaii Medical Journal, 1946, 6,
19-21.

Shaw, I. A. The treatment of alcoholism with


tetraethylthiuram disulfide in a state mental
hospital. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
1951, 12, 576-586.

Sidman, M. Technique for assessing the effects of


drugs on timing behavior. Science, 1955,122, 925.

Skolnik, J. H. The stumbling block. A sociological study


of the relationship between selected religious
norms and drinking behavior. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Yale University, 1957.

Smart, R. G. Effects of alcohol on conflict and


avoidance behavior. Quarterly Journal of Studies
on Alcohol, 1965, 26, 187-205.

Snyder, C. R. Alcohol and the Jews: A cultural study of


drinking and sobriety. Glencoe, Ill.: Free Press,
1958.

Solyom, L., & Miller, S. A differential conditioning


procedure as the initial phase of the behaviour
therapy of homosexuality. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1965, 3, 147-160.

Spence, K. W. Cognitive and drive factors in the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1791
extinction of the conditioned eye blink in human
subjects. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 445-458.

Stevenson, I., & Wolpe, J. Recovery from sexual


deviations through overcoming of non-sexual
neurotic responses. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 1960, 116, 737-742.

Stollak, G. E. Weight loss obtained under various


experimental procedures. Unpublished
manuscript, Indiana University, 1968.

Stoller, R. J. Transvestites’ women. American Journal


of Psychiatry, 1967, 124, 333-339.

Strel’chuk, I. V. C novykh sovremennykh metodakh


lecheniya bol’nykh alkogolizmom. [New
contemporary methods of treating patients with
alcoholism.] Sovietskaia Meditsina, 1957, 21 (5),
26-33.

Stuart, R. B. Behavioral control of overeating.


Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 357-
365.

Sutherland, E. H., Schroeder, H. G., & Tordella, C. L.


Personality traits and the alcoholic. A critique of
existing studies. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 1950, 11, 547-561.

Syme, L. Personality characteristics and the alcoholic.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1792
A critique of current studies. Quarterly Journal of
Studies on Alcohol, 1957, 18, 288-301.

Thimann, J. Conditioned-reflex treatment of


alcoholism. II. The risks of its application, its
indications, contraindications, and
psychotherapeutic aspects. New England Journal
of Medicine, 1949, 241, 406-410.

Thompson, G. N., & Bielinski, B. Improvement in


psychosis following conditioned reflex treatment
for alcoholism. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 1953, 117, 537-543.

Thorpe, J. G., Schmidt, E., Brown, P. T., & Castell, D.


Aversion-relief therapy: A new method for
general application. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1964, 2, 71-82.

Thorpe, J. G., Schmidt, E., & Castell, D. A comparison of


positive and negative (aversive) conditioning in
the treatment of homosexuality. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 196.3, 1, 357-362.

Voegtlin, W. L. The treatment of alcoholism by


establishing a conditioned reflex. American
Journal of Medical Science, 1940, 199, 802-810.

Voegtlin, W. L., & Broz, W. R. The conditioned reflex


treatment of chronic alcoholism. X. An analysis of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1793
3125 admissions over a period of ten and a half
years. Annals of Internal Medicine, 1949, 30, 580-
597.

Voegtlin, W. L., Lemere, F., Broz, W. R., & O’Hollaren, P.


Conditioned reflex therapy of chronic alcoholism.
IV. A preliminary report on the value of
reinforcement. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 1942, 2, 505-511.

Wall, J. H. A study of alcoholism in men. American


Journal of Psychiatry, 1936, 92, 1389-1401.

Wallace, J. A. The treatment of alcoholism by the


conditioned-reflex method. Journal of the
Tennessee Medical Association, 1949, 42, 125-128.

Wallerstein, R. S. (Ed.) Hospital treatment of


alcoholism; a comparative, experimental study.
New York: Basic Books, 1957.

Whitener, R. W., & Nikelly, A. G. Sexual deviation in


college students. Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
1964, 34, 486-492.

Wilde, G. J. S. Behaviour therapy for addicted cigarette


smokers: A preliminary investigation. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1964, 2, 107-109.

Wolpe, J. Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition.


Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1958.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1794
Wolpe, J. Conditioned inhibition of craving in drug
addiction: A pilot experiment. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1965, 2, 285-288.

Woodward, M. The diagnosis and treatment of


homosexual offenders. British Journal of
Delinquency, 1958, 9, 44-59.

Yablonsky, L. The tunnel back: Synanon. New York:


Macmillan, 1965.

Yanushevskii, I. K. Effektivnost’ protwoolykogol’ nogo


techeniya po dannym katamneza. [The
effectiveness of antialcohol treatment according
to follow-up data.] Zhurnal nevropatologii i
psikhiatrii, 1959, 59, 693-696.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1795
9

Symbolic Control of
Behavioral Changes
Both complex behaviors and even relatively

simple performances that have generally been

assumed to represent direct linkages between

external stimuli and overt responses are

extensively controlled by symbolic processes.

These higher level activities involve, among other


things, strategic selection of the stimuli to which
attention is directed, symbolic coding and

organization of stimulus inputs, and acquisition,


through informative feedback, of mediating

hypotheses or rules which play an influential role

in regulating response selection.

The introductory chapter considered in some

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1796
detail questions regarding variables that govern

the occurrence of symbolic events, the different

forms that mediators may take, and the conditions


under which they exercise cue function in

directing overt actions. The present chapter is

focused on the extent to which behavioral change


processes are affected by persons’ awareness of

stimuli impinging upon them, responses that they

are exhibiting, consequences that their behavior


incurs, and the contingencies that exist among the
latter events. In addition, relationships between

attitudinal and behavioral changes are reviewed


with particular reference to the development of

self-regulatory mechanisms.

Role of Awareness of Contingencies in


Behavioral Change

A number of different theories have been


suggested concerning the functional role of

symbolic activities, which is usually subsumed

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1797
under the general term “awareness,” in the

behavioral change process. The main alternative

views are depicted in a simplified form in Figure


9-1, following the schematization of Farber (1963)

and Spielberger & DeNike (1966).

According to the nonmediational theory of

learning (Skinner, 1953; Thorndike, 1933),

reinforcing consequences act directly and


automatically to strengthen preceding overt
responses. While learning occurs independently of

awareness, a person may eventually recognize the

reinforcement contingencies from the high output


of correct responses. In this view, however,
awareness is a resultant rather than a

precondition of change.

The independent response systems theory

(Verplanck, 1962), which is a more recent version

of the foregoing position, treats awareness as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1798
Figure 9-1. Schematization of the functional relationship
between awareness and response change. Dashed lines
represent temporally contiguous events, arrows denote
causal relationships, and plus signs designate the
magnitude of response change.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1799
merely a verbal operant rather than as a factor
that controls performance. Since verbal and

nonverbal classes of behavior represent


independent response systems, almost any type of

relationship can be obtained between these two

sets of events, depending upon the manner in

which their governing reinforcement


contingencies are arranged. Thus, verbalizations

and actions will be congruent under conditions

where the same contingencies are applied to

verbal statements and the corresponding

instrumental responses. On the other hand,


verbalizations and actions can be made to diverge

through the application of conflicting


reinforcement to these two forms of response.

Since reinforcing stimuli are assumed to exert


automatic control over behavior independently of

their effects upon awareness, this theoretical


position also represents a nonmediational model

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1800
of behavioral change.

According to the cognitive view (Dulany, 1962,


1968; Spielberger & DeNike, 1966), which stands

in marked contrast to the preceding formulations,

awareness is considered a prerequisite for


learning and improvement in performance. The

information conveyed by reinforcing stimuli,

rather than their response-strengthening effects,


is highlighted in this point of view. It is assumed
that, in the course of observing the differential

consequences associated with different types of

responses emitted, subjects test various

hypotheses about the required response class and


eventually figure out what they are supposed to
do. The acquired information, in turn, gives rise to

intentions or self-instructions to produce the

correct responses, the strength of the tendency

depending upon subjects’ valuation of the

contingent incentives. The magnitude of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1801
performance gains is thus primarily a function of

the accuracy of the guiding hypothesis and the

incentive value of the consequences. However, no


conditioning can presumably occur without either

partial or correct symbolic representation of the

reinforcement contingencies.

The reciprocal interaction theory (Farber,

1963; Postman & Sassenrath, 1961), on the other


hand, assumes that awareness is both a
consequent and a condition of behavioral change.

According to this view, a certain amount of

learning can take place from the automatic action


of aftereffects, independently of subjects’
understanding of the basis on which

reinforcements are administered. During the


learning process, however, subjects not only make

overt responses, but they also develop thoughts or

hypotheses about the responses required to

obtain reinforcement. These self-generated rules

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1802
serve as discriminative stimuli for directing

instrumental actions in essentially the same way

as external stimuli control behavior. Accurate


hypotheses are likely to be accompanied by

correct overt responses, whereas erroneous

hypotheses tend to coincide with inappropriate

performances. Consequently, symbolic events are

selectively strengthened, maintained, or


extinguished by the differential reinforcements
administered for the more distally occurring overt

behavior. The emergence of awareness may, of

course, be facilitated by performance gains which

make the contingencies more obvious. Once the


correct hypothesis is established, it can result in a

substantial increase of appropriate responding,


given adequate incentive conditions.

The acquisition of rules and their functions in

regulating performance are typically studied in

concept identification and other forms of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1803
discrimination learning. In these paradigms

subjects must categorize different stimuli on the

basis of some common property that the


experimenter has arbitrarily selected as relevant

for the classification. In more complex situations

the correct responses are defined in terms of a

combination of attributes rather than a single

common element. Under these circumstances,


subjects must abstract the relevant stimulus
dimensions and formulate a rule about how the

different attributes combine to specify the

appropriate behavior (Bourne, 1966; Shepard,

Hovland, & Jenkins, 1961).

Numerous experiments have been conducted

in which subjects are asked to state the rules they


employed for making responses. When hypotheses

are measured after subjects have performed and

experienced the outcome, it is difficult to

determine whether the responses were derived

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1804
from a rule or the rule was inferred from the

correct responses. This interpretive problem does

not arise when rule statements are recorded prior


to performance and measures are obtained of the

degree to which they control trial-by-trial

responding. Studies of this type show that rules

can be strengthened either by direct

reinforcement or indirectly through response


outcomes and that they serve as a primary
determinant of overt behavior (Dulany &

O’Connell, 1963; O’Connell & Wagner, 1967).

However, under conditions where stimuli are

more complex and verbal control over responding


is not explicitly encouraged, accurate performance

often occurs in the absence of adequate verbalized


rules (Hislop & Brooks, 1969).

Several different approaches, some of which

were discussed earlier, have been employed in

experimental analyses of the role of symbolic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1805
activities in behavioral change processes. These

have included investigations of verbal

conditioning rates as a function of awareness of


response-reinforcement contingencies,

mediational control of classical conditioning and

extinction, the occurrence of semantic

generalization in which a common cognitive

associate of heterogeneous stimuli provides the


basis for generalization, covert verbal control of
problem-solving activities, and the influence of

recognition and discrimination of weak stimuli

upon discriminative nonverbal behavior.

The methods of assessing symbolic activities


have been equally varied. In some studies

awareness is manipulated instructionally through


explicit descriptions of the response-

reinforcement contingencies given prior to the

conditioning series. More frequently, however,

awareness is inferred from observations about the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1806
experiment reported by subjects either at periodic

intervals during the course of conditioning or in

interviews conducted immediately following the


experimental session. In other cases, awareness is

manipulated indirectly by arranging conditions

that either facilitate or impede recognition of

contingencies during the acquisition process.

Verbal Conditioning as a Function of


Awareness

Innumerable studies employing paradigms of

verbal conditioning have been conducted in order

to determine whether response consequences

increase performance primarily by effecting


voluntary symbolic control over available
responses or through a process of automatic

response strengthening. Although the issue of


whether learning can occur without awareness is

by no means settled (Farber, 1963; Kanfer, 1968;

Postman & Sassenrath, 1961), most experiments

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1807
fail to obtain performance gains in the absence of

accurate or at least partially correct hypotheses

regarding the reinforcement contingencies


(Adams, 1957; Dulany, 1962; Spielberger &

DeNike, 1966). Subjects who are able to describe

the responses required for reinforcement

generally show a substantial increase in

appropriate responses, whereas exposure to


reinforcement contingencies is relatively
ineffective in modifying the behavior of subjects

who remain unaware.

Experiments utilizing post-acquisition


measures of awareness furnish inconclusive
results, since it is entirely possible that subjects

may initially condition without awareness and


later recognize the reinforcement principle

employed when it is made more apparent by the

increased output of correct responses. In order to

establish whether awareness precedes or follows

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1808
behavioral change it is therefore necessary to

assess subjects’ speculations about the

experimental contingencies at periodic intervals


during the acquisition process. DeNike (1964), for

example, asked college students, who were

reinforced for human noun responses in a word-

naming task, to write down their “thoughts about

the experiment” after each block of 25 words


during conditioning. On the basis of the written
reports, approximately a third of the subjects were

judged to have gained awareness of the

contingency at different points in the series, while

the remainder were categorized as unaware. A


control group of subjects, who were reinforced on

a random basis for 10 percent of their responses,


was also included. As can be seen from Figure 9-2,

aware subjects displayed a substantial increase in

human noun responses, whereas unaware


subjects, like the control group, showed no

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1809
Figure 9-2. (A) Mean percent human noun responses given
by aware, unaware, and control groups in the verbal
conditioning task. (B) Mean percent of correct
responses given by subjects in the aware group prior to
and after verbalization of the reinforcement
contingency. Spielberger & DeNike, 1966.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1810
performance gains whatsoever. Of considerably

greater interest, however, is the temporal

relationship obtained between the emergence of


awareness and the occurrence of large

performance increments. Subjects displayed no


significant rise in the number of critical responses
before they were able to report the reinforcement

contingency, but they markedly increased their

output of reinforced responses after they had


discerned the contingency governing the

administration of social rewards (Figure 9-2).

Considering, however, that each block contained


25 trials, it is entirely possible that awareness may

still have resulted from behavior change during


the block in which the contingency was discerned.

A stringent test of mediational control of

performance changes would require a trial-by-


trial inquiry.

Results of experiments conducted by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1811
investigators who differ widely in their views

regarding the role of awareness in learning

consistently demonstrate that symbolic


representation of the conditions of reinforcement

has a strong facilitative effect upon overt

performance. The empirical data diverge,

however, on the question of whether any learning

can take place without symbolic mediation. The


findings of Dulany, DeNike, & Spielberger may be
contrasted with those of Hirsch (1957), Philbrick

& Postman (1955), and Sassenrath (1962), who

likewise have analyzed performance curves as a

function of the temporal appearance of awareness,


and find small but significant improvements in

performance prior to correct statement of


contingencies, particularly among subjects who

eventually developed complete awareness (Figure

9-3). Evidence of verbal conditioning without


awareness is usually dismissed by staunch

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1812
Figure 9-3. Average number of correct responses given on
blocks of words at varying distances from the point at
which the principle was first verbalized correctly.
Philbrick & Postman, 1955.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1813
adherents to cognitive explanations as probably

either the result of insensitive methods for

gauging awareness, or the operation of partially


correct hypotheses.

Although virtually all investigators subscribe,


albeit uneasily, to the same operational definition

of awareness (i.e., correct verbalization of

response-reinforcement contingencies), a number


of factors may lead to inaccuracies and
inconsistencies in composing aware and unaware

groups of subjects. First, awareness is usually

treated as an all-or-none phenomenon, when in

fact it may vary in accuracy from a correct


determination, through partially correlated
hypotheses, to highly misleading notions about

why the subject is being rewarded. As Adams

(1957) has noted, partially correct formulations

(e.g., a subject believes that the experimenter is

interested in comments about people when

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1814
actually familial references is the correct response

class) can produce some increases in performance.

Because criteria of awareness are somewhat


arbitrary, the types of relationships obtained

between events are partly dependent upon the

stringency of definitions employed by different

investigators.

A second major complication in the assessment


of awareness arises from the fact that a number of
other variables, quite apart from the amount of

relevant information possessed by the subject,

may contribute to the types of verbal reports that


are obtained.

Awareness is frequently inferred from

responses to a series of progressively more

suggestive interview questions. Therefore, the

number of subjects judged to be aware is

determined to some extent by the number and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1815
nature of informative cues conveyed by the
interview probes. The more intensive the

questioning the less the likelihood that partially


aware subjects will be erroneously categorized as

unaware, but also the greater the danger that the

assessment procedure itself may induce

recognition of the correct contingency that did not


exist at the time of conditioning. Thus, Levin

(1961) found evidence of learning without

awareness when subjects’ discernment of

contingencies was estimated from a brief

interview, whereas categorization of the same


subjects on the basis of their replies to a more

extended specific inquiry yielded a peculiar set of


results, in which subjects who were unaware of

both the contingency and the reinforcer displayed


as much conditioning as subjects who were fully

aware, and a higher rate of response than a group


that was only aware of the reinforcing stimulus.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1816
While this irregular pattern of relationships does

not support the common assumption that

comprehensive assessments furnish more valid


measures, it should be noted that early studies of

verbal conditioning, which reported relatively


high incidences of learning without awareness
(Krasner, 1958), generally relied upon brief

examinations that may not have been sufficiently

sensitive to detect partial degrees of awareness.


The unreliability of post-acquisition reports of

awareness is further underscored by Weinstein &

Lawson (1963), who found that interviews of the


kind widely employed in this line of research

yielded complete awareness in only half the


subjects who had been fully informed, midway

through the experiment, of the contingencies and

the entire purpose of the study. Based upon


criteria utilized in earlier experiments,

approximately half the sample would have been

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1817
misclassified and their improved performances

interpreted as evidence for learning without

awareness.

In addition to the influence of the type of

measurement procedures employed, the


likelihood that subjects will report awareness is

reduced if they are examined by a female or a

person of low status (Krasner, Ullmann, Weiss, &


Collins, 1961), if a negative valence is attached to
the reinforced class of responses (Krasner &

Ullmann, 1963), if they feel hostile toward the

experimenter (Weiss, Krasner, & Ullmann, 1960),

and if they obtained information about the


contingencies spuriously (Levy, 1967). Moreover,
there is some evidence (Rosenthal, Persinger,

Vikan-Kline, & Fode, 1963) that experimenters

who are biased to expect a high incidence of

awareness obtain it more frequently than those

who assume it to be a relatively uncommon

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1818
phenomenon.

It is possible to control, to some extent, for the

operation of experimenter bias by relying upon


written reports rather than interview procedures,

which provide greater opportunities for

inadvertent influence of respondents’ accounts.

Also, reluctance to disclose provisional judgments

about the experiment, and intentional distortions,


might be effectively counteracted by the provision
of positive incentives which would maximize

verbalization of the information that subjects do

possess. It is evident from the data cited above


that if much importance is to be attached to
studies of symbolic mediation of learning based

upon information provided by subjects, then


extensive research is needed to identify the

variables influencing reported awareness with a

view toward further improving the accuracy of

such measures.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1819
Because of the numerous problems associated

with phenomenological data, some researchers

have recommended that awareness be relegated


to the status of a dependent variable and treated

as a verbal operant. This type of approach


decisively solves a technical problem at the
expense of a potentially influential independent

variable which, under some conditions, can exert

more powerful discriminative control over


behavior than reinforcement variables (Ayllon &

Azrin, 1964; Dulany, 1968; Kaufman, Baron, &

Kopp, 1966).

Granted all the deficiencies of verbal reports as


indicants of subjects’ level of awareness, findings
based on the temporal relationship between

awareness and performance nevertheless indicate

that one can predict with considerably greater

accuracy performance increments during the

course of conditioning by taking into account

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1820
subjects’ hypotheses than if self-instructional

influences are disregarded.

The host of methodological and interpretive


problems associated with post-acquisition

measures of awareness can be easily avoided by

the use of research procedures in which

knowledge about reinforcement contingencies is

experimentally induced rather than inferred from


subjects’ verbal reports. Experiments in which
subjects are informed of the appropriate

responses and their consequences prior to

conditioning disclose substantial symbolic


regulation of overt performances.

Situations of social conditioning contain a

number of different elements of which a person

might become aware. These separable events

include the environmental cues eliciting his

behavior, the class of responses considered

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1821
appropriate to the situation, the occurrence and
scheduling of reinforcements, and the contingent

relation between the latter classes of events. Some


of the experiments in which awareness is

manipulated experimentally have been specifically

designed to compare the relative efficacy of insight

into the different aspects of the behavioral


influence process. Dulany (1962), for example,

found that students who were informed about the

correct response-reinforcement contingency and

those who received only response instructions

markedly increased their output of correct


responses compared both to their baseline rates

and to the performance of control groups who


were apprised only of the reinforcing events or

given no information. The latter groups, in fact,


exhibited no significant improvements in

performance. In this study, knowledge of the


desired behavior was the critical determinant,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1822
since the addition of reinforcing consequences in

the form of either tones, “Um-hmms,” or omission

of electric shocks did not augment the change


process.

In a subsequent study Dulany (1968) had


students perform a verbal conditioning task in a

chamber kept at a temperature of 110° and

utilized a contingent stream of air of either 70°,


100°, or 150° as positive, neutral, or negative
reinforcers for different groups of subjects.

Students within each of the reinforcement

conditions were also given different reinforcement

instructions: that the stream of air signified a


correct response, an incorrect response, or had
nothing to do with their performance. Dulany

found that reinforcement instructions exercised

greater control over conditioning performance

than did the nature of the reinforcing

consequences.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1823
The findings of Dulany are, in large part,

corroborated by Kaufman, Baron, & Kopp (1966),

who provided students with complete or minimal


information about the required response, and

either accurate or erroneous knowledge


concerning the schedule according to which
rewarding consequences would be administered.

One group of students was accurately informed

that rewards would be forthcoming each minute


on the average (variable interval schedule),

whereas other groups were misled into believing

that their behavior would be reinforced either on a


fixed interval of one minute, or after they had

performed 150 responses on the average (variable


ratio schedule).

Inspection of Table 9-1 reveals that knowledge

about the required behavior markedly increased

subjects’ rate of responding. Even more

impressive, however, is the finding that illusory

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1824
schedules governed students’ responsiveness in

much the same way as they do in reality: Fixed

interval instructions produced very low rates,


variable ratio instructions maintained an

extremely high response output, and variable

interval instructions generated intermediate rates

of response. Alleged schedules thus outweighed

the influence of the program of reinforcement that


was actually imposed on students’ behavior. A
further study disclosed that reinforcement had

little effect upon performance rate under

conditions of minimal response specification, but

students who had received variable ratio


instructions combined with monetary rewards

were approximately twice as productive as those


who were furnished the same schedule

information without any reinforcing

consequences. Instructional influences can be


equally powerful in regulating responsiveness

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1825
under aversive contingencies. Simply exposing

subjects to punishing contingencies proved to be

an extremely inefficient way of altering behavior,


whereas instructions about the appropriate

behavior and its consequences immediately


produced stable and discriminated avoidance
behavior (Baron & Kaufman, 1966; Scobie &

Kaufman, 1969).

Table 9-1 Median Number of Responses Performed per


Minute as a Function of Information about the
Required Response and Alleged Schedules of
Reinforcement (adapted from Kaufman, Baron, & Kopp,
1966)
Response Rate
Experimental Condition First 30 Min. Last 30 Min.

No schedule information
Minimal response instruction 17 17
Complete response instruction 161 161
Schedule information
Variable interval 88 43
Fixed interval 5 7
Variable ratio 250 269

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1826
It is noteworthy that among persons who have

achieved complete awareness the extent to which

this knowledge continues to govern their behavior


during extinction depends upon the type and

schedule of reinforcement applied during the


acquisition process (Hirsch, 1957).

The experiments of Ayllon & Azrin (1964), in

which highly persistent behavior of psychotics


was modified in naturalistic settings, reveal that
when discrepancies exist between verbally

represented contingencies and actual

consequences, instructional influences lose their

potency over time, and behavior comes more


extensively under the control of the prevailing
conditions of reinforcement. These data

underscore the necessity for exercising

considerable caution in generalizing about the

relative efficacy of cognitive and reinforcement

variables on the basis of exceedingly brief

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1827
experiments conducted with cooperative college

sophomores who are offered trivial rewards for

performing inconsequential responses that


require little expenditure of effort, under

conditions where they are expected to participate

in experiments in partial fulfillment of course

requirements.

The findings of investigations in which


awareness is experimentally induced are in accord
with those furnished by studies relying upon

report-inferred measures. We return now to the

major controversial issue: Is awareness a


prerequisite for behavioral change? Proponents of
cognitive theories have been unable to find any

evidence of verbal conditioning in the absence of


correct or correlated hypotheses, whereas

Postman and his colleagues report, on the basis of

experiments involving more complex

reinforcement contingencies, that a significant

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1828
amount of learning can take place prior to

verbalization of the basis for reinforcement. These

divergent conclusions do not appear to be


attributable to any major differences in the

definition and assessment of awareness. Nor can

they be accounted for in terms of the operation of

partially correct hypotheses, since the use of

partially relevant hypotheses in the concept


learning task did not facilitate performance
(Hirsch, 1957; Postman & Sassenrath, 1961), and

the phenomenon is evident even when awareness

is defined to include partially correct

verbalizations (Sassenrath, 1962).

Some additional suggestive evidence of

behavioral change without awareness is furnished


by investigations involving more complex tasks,

such as probability learning, in which persons

predict alternative events or outcomes that vary in

their frequency of occurrence. In these situations,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1829
persons’ choice behavior gradually adjusts to the

event probabilities even though the vast majority

of subjects are not only unable to state the


probability rules, but frequently entertain quite

erroneous hypotheses (Goodnow & Postman,

1955).

Discrepancies in results may, in part, result

from the complexity of the principle governing the


administration of reinforcement and the response
restrictions imposed by the nature of the learning

task. Studies in which verbalization is

accompanied by dramatic performance gains have


generally involved relatively explicit response
classes or simple discrimination tasks in which

subjects are asked to construct sentences by


selecting one of several personal pronouns or

verbs printed on cards. When the critical response

class is unambiguous and the response

alternatives are severely curtailed, both

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1830
awareness and “learning” are most likely to occur

as a one-trial event rather than as an incremental

process.

Considering the feeble and inconsequential

nature of the reinforcers employed in most verbal

conditioning experiments, one might seriously

question whether reinforcement processes, which

presumably govern automatic response-


strengthening effects, are even operative in most
of the studies that have been reviewed. This issue

is, of course, of little or no concern to researchers

who are quite content with a circular empirical


law of effect. Nevertheless, it is important to
distinguish between the informational and

incentive properties of feedback stimuli, both of


which may enhance correct responding (Keller,

Cole, Burke, & Estes, 1965). Flashing fights, buzzer

noises, and ambiguous guttural sounds can convey

to subjects adequate information for altering their

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1831
behavior, but it is exceedingly doubtful that such

feedback events have much rewarding value as

determined independently of response changes on


the conditioning task.

In view of the fact that verbal conditioning

procedures are designed to augment the

production of existing responses under conditions

where both response and reinforcing events are


highly salient, the findings may have more
relevance to issues of social conformity than to

conditioning. Although the verbal conditioning

paradigm is adequate to demonstrate the


facilitative role of awareness upon performance, it
is poorly suited to throw much light on the more

basic theoretical issue of whether awareness is a


prerequisite for learning or performance change.

The question of whether learning must be

consciously mediated can be answered most

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1832
decisively by studies in which the reinforced
responses are not observable to the performer, or

the reinforcing events are devoid of informational


cues but of sufficient rewarding value to activate

mechanisms regulating reinforcement effects.

Either of the latter conditions would effectively

preclude recognition of the contingency employed.


The studies discussed earlier, in which correct

responding in animals was significantly increased

by intravenous presentation of nutritive solutions,

would seem to dispute the radical cognitive view.

Such reinforcing events are not observable and,


therefore, convey no information to the subject.

There is evidence that covert responses, such


as invisibly minute thumb contractions, can be

successfully conditioned in adult humans without


their observation of the rewarded responses

(Hefferline & Keenan, 1963; Hefferline, Keenan, &


Harford, 1959; Sasmor, 1966). These studies

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1833
provide further convincing demonstrations of how

persons learn to respond in accordance with

reinforcement contingencies without appropriate


symbolic mediation. In the latter experiments

subjects are equipped with several sets of

electrodes, ostensibly to measure their ability to

relax. A visually imperceptible thumb contraction

of a preselected magnitude, detected by the


experimenter through electromyographic
amplification, is then selected for modification

through reinforcement either in the form of

monetary rewards or termination of aversive

stimulation. Responses in the chosen amplitude


category increase substantially during

reinforcement and decline abruptly after


reinforcement is withdrawn. As might be

expected, none of the subjects could identify the

response that produced reinforcement.

Apart from the laboratory findings, it is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1834
difficult to believe that planaria, goldfish, and

other lower organisms, which lack the anatomical

structures for adequate symbolic representation


of environmental events, are totally unaffected by

response consequences until they have accurately

cognized their experimenter’s contingencies.

Implicit mediators would, of course, assume an

important role in governing performance in tasks


that require response on the basis of relatively
complicated principles or rules.

The overall evidence would seem to indicate

that learning can take place without awareness,


albeit at a slow rate, but that symbolic
representation of response-reinforcement

contingencies can markedly accelerate


appropriate responsiveness. The validity of this

view, which assumes a reciprocal interaction

between awareness and performance gains, seems

even more probable when one realizes the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1835
limitations of paradigms of verbal conditioning for

elucidating the role of symbolic activities in

behavioral change processes.

Interactive Effects of Cognitive and


Incentive Variables

Although awareness of environmental

contingencies is usually accompanied by some


gains in performance relative to baseline rates, the

absolute level of responsiveness after the


contingencies have been either ascertained or

divulged remains comparatively low. That is,

performance increases are generally in the order

of 20-30 percent, which can hardly be considered


a massive outpouring of correct responses. It is
also extremely likely that if the experiments were

extended beyond the usual single session,


symbolic control, in the absence of supporting

incentives, would decrease over time and the

desired behavior might eventually return to its

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1836
original level. Moreover, even in short-term

situations embodying high demand

characteristics, a significant number of aware


subjects never do show any change in their

behavior (Farber, 1963). Hence, the findings of

verbal conditioning studies, rather than

demonstrating the potency of symbolic control, in

fact illustrate the limitations of approaches that


rely primarily upon cognitive variables to effect
behavioral changes. The experiments do provide

considerable evidence, however, that awareness

combined with incentive-related variables can

exert a powerful influence over behavior.

Spielberger, Bernstein, & Ratliff (1966)

compared the response rate of aware and


unaware subjects during an initial phase of the

experiment in which “Mm-hmm” served as the

reinforcer, and after an effort was made to bolster

the incentive value of the utterance by challenging

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1837
subjects to produce as many “Mm-hmms” as they

possibly could. Students who remained unaware

throughout both phases of the study showed no


evidence of conditioning; those who discovered

the contingency prior to the incentive

manipulation displayed moderate improvements

in performance followed by extremely high

response rates under the heightened motivational


conditions; a third group of subjects, who became
aware after the incentive manipulation, exhibited

an intermediate level of responding (Figure 9-4).

Studies in which reinforcing properties of the


feedback events are evaluated by subjects
(Spielberger, Berger, & Howard, 1963;

Spielberger, Levin, & Shepard, 1962) rather than


being varied independently likewise disclose that,

among aware subjects, those who prize the

reinforcers show a high output of criterion

behavior. By contrast, aware subjects who are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1838
Figure 9-4. Mean percent of correct responses given during
the baseline (A), low-incentive (B), and high-incentive
(C) phases of the experiment by subjects who either
discovered the reinforcement contingency prior to or
after the incentive manipulation or remained unaware
throughout the experiment. Spielberger, Bernstein, &
Ratliff, 1966.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1839
indifferent to, or annoyed by, experimenters’

guttural utterances may perform some correct

responses in order to confirm their speculations,


but otherwise they are about as unproductive as

their unaware counterparts.

In addition to the influence of incentives

specifically associated with the desired behavior,

more generalized sets may determine the extent to


which persons will behave in accordance with
their knowledge of social contingencies. Farber

(1963) and Holmes (1967) found that aware

subjects with cooperative dispositions displayed a


sharp increase in reinforced responses, but
nonconforming aware subjects showed relatively

little change in behavior and did not differ in this


respect from subjects who remained unaware.

While awareness typically facilitates

performance, where the correct responses carry

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1840
negative connotations, awareness may exert
inhibitory effects upon performance, as shown in

the study by Ekman, Krasner, & Ullmann (1963).


Among aware subjects, those who were led to

believe that the verbal conditioning task exposed

personal debilities exhibited fewer responses

during reinforced trials compared to their baseline


rates, whereas those who were informed that the

task measured empathy and warmth toward

people showed a substantial increment in

reinforced responses. On the other hand, groups of

unaware students who received the same negative


and benign sets displayed relatively small

response gains and did not differ from each other.

It is apparent from the research thus far

reviewed that awareness of reinforcement


contingencies has greater behavioral

consequences under laboratory conditions than


appears to be the case in naturalistic or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1841
psychotherapeutic situations. This difference may

be attributable to several factors. Based upon the

findings previously discussed, one would not


expect development of insight into social

contingencies to produce much change in behavior

if the customary incentives are weak, delayed, or

only sporadically applied, as is often true in

realistic circumstances. Second, the responses


chosen in experimental studies (e.g., plural
responses, verbs, personal pronouns, emotion-

arousing words) are readily available within

subjects’ repertoires, and the task is primarily a

matter of response selection rather than response


acquisition. In most behavioral change programs,

on the other hand, individuals must develop


behavior requisite to bring them into contact with

prevailing contingencies, rather than merely

gaining information about what one would have to


do in order to obtain reinforcement. Acquired

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1842
insights, no matter how valid they may be, have

limited utility for individuals who lack the

necessary performance skills. The case is


analogous to informing English monolingual

students in verbal conditioning experiments that


the criterion responses are Hindustani adjectives.

Symbolic Control of Classical Conditioning


Phenomena

Consistent with findings from studies of

instrumental conditioning, results of numerous


investigations of classical conditioning (Grings,

1965) reveal extensive mediational control of


conditioned autonomic responses. This process

has been demonstrated in several different ways.


In one experimental approach to the problem,
subjects are informed that the CS will sometimes

be followed by shock; they are then given a sample

shock or a single confirmation trial during the

acquisition series when autonomic responses to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1843
the CS are measured. The studies (Bridger &

Mandel, 1964; Cook & Harris, 1937; Dawson &

Grings, 1969) show that autonomic responses are


readily conditioned through association of

stimulus events with anticipatory emotional

responses.

Data of this sort have led to suggestions that a

distinction be drawn between genuine


conditioning and perceptual or relational learning
(Grings, 1965; Razran, 1955). Implicit in this

dichotomy is the assumption that conditioned

emotional responses established through


instructional manipulations represent a “pseudo-
conditioning” phenomenon. An alternative

conceptual scheme, which has the potential of


elucidating the process without proliferating

unnecessary varieties of learning, would hold that

conditioning based upon the occurrence of actual

versus imagined events mainly involves

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1844
differences in the locus of the emotion-producing

stimulus rather than in the governing mechanism.

It is extremely unlikely that verbal association


of events alone is sufficient to establish

conditioned responses, since a stimulus capable of

activating the autonomic responses is also

required. There is consistent evidence from

studies in which autonomic responses are


continuously recorded during desensitization
sessions (Clark, 1963; Mackay & Laverty, 1963),

and from controlled laboratory investigations

(Barber & Hahn, 1964), that imagined aversive


events can produce emotional effects analogous to
the actual Occurrence of aversive stimulation.

These findings indicate that subjects can acquire


conditioned responses in the absence of an

externally administered UCS to the extent that

prior instructions lead them to generate fear-

producing thoughts in conjunction with the CS. On

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1845
the other hand, verbal instruction alone would not

be expected to produce any conditioning in

subjects who did not engage in appropriate covert


self-stimulation. According to this self-arousal

formulation, instructional conditioning may

represent a variant of the basic conditioning

paradigm in which autonomic responses are

cognitively induced rather than directly elicited by


aversive stimuli under the experimenter’s control.

The foregoing conceptualization implies that,

rather than representing a simple process in

which external stimuli are directly and


automatically connected to overt responses,
classical conditioning is partly mediated through

symbolic activities. In the mediational


interpretation, the CS elicits covert symbolic

activities that produce autonomic responses. Some

suggestive evidence for the influential role of self-

stimulation in instructional conditioning is

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1846
provided by Dawson (1966), who found that the

degree of belief in instructions that shock would

follow a certain signal and the amount of


anticipation of shock were positively correlated

with extent of autonomic conditioning.

The influential role of mediational variables in

classical conditioning is also supported by other

lines of evidence. Fuhrer & Baer (1965), for


example, demonstrated that subjects who
recognized the CS-UCS relationship while

undergoing differential conditioning of GSR

responses demonstrated considerable autonomic


conditioning. In contrast, subjects who remained
unaware of the stimulus contingencies did not

respond differently to auditory stimuli that were


associated with shock and to those never paired

with aversive stimulation. Dawson & Grings

(1969) have likewise shown that masked CS-UCS

pairings, which impeded recognition of the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1847
stimulus contingency, were not sufficient to

condition discriminative autonomic responses.

In order to ascertain if covariations in symbolic


behavior and conditioned responses do involve a

causal relationship, Chatterjee & Eriksen (1962)

conducted an experiment in which awareness of

the stimulus contingencies was manipulated in

advance. One group of subjects was informed that


a shock would only follow one particular word in a
chain association task, but that aversive

stimulation would be discontinued at a clearly

designated point in the experiment. A second


group was told that although shock would always
follow a particular word in the list, each of the

remaining words would be paired once with


aversive stimulation and eventually shocks would

cease altogether. A third group was instructed that

a certain number of shocks would be administered

during the experiment without implying a regular

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1848
contingency. Subjects in the first two groups, all of

whom discerned the correct word-to-shock

relationship, displayed conditioned heart rate


responses, whereas subjects who received

minimal information and remained unaware

evidenced no conditioning. The controlling power

of symbolic events is further shown by evidence

that aware subjects exhibited strong autonomic


responses to the critical stimulus but they did not
generalize these responses inappropriately along

either semantic or physical dimensions. Moreover,

those who were informed when the extinction

phase commenced showed a prompt and virtually


complete loss of conditioned responses before

experiencing any nonreinforced presentations of


the conditioned stimulus.

In accord with the above finding, the most

striking evidence of symbolic control of classically

conditioned responses is provided by studies in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1849
which extinction of autonomic responsiveness is

compared in subjects who are told that there will

be no further aversive stimulation and in those


who remain uninformed. Induced awareness of

the change in stimulus contingencies generally

results in rapid and practically complete

disappearance of conditioned responses to the CS

(Cook & Harris, 1937; Grings & Lockhart, 1963;


Wickens, Allen, & Hill, 1963). The decrement is
most sudden and dramatic when subjects who,

despite assurances to the contrary, suspect that

they might continue to be shocked are excluded

from the analysis (Bridger & Mandel, 1965). On


the other hand, under circumstances where

arousal level is maintained and the operation of


cognitive factors is curtailed by disguising the

conditioning procedures (Spence, 1966),

extinction proceeds at a comparatively slow rate


after reinforcement has been discontinued.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1850
Although a strong causal relationship has been

established between cognitive variables and rate

of classical conditioning and extinction, it should


not be concluded that all conditioned responses

are necessarily consciously mediated. It seems


extremely unlikely that in the case of interoceptive
conditioning either the conditioned stimulus (e.g.,

intestinal distention) or the unconditioned

stimulus (e.g., chemical stimuli presented


internally) are symbolically represented. In these,

and other experiments employing internal

stimulative procedures (Razran, 1961), the


contingencies are undoubtedly operating below

the threshold of awareness.

There is also some evidence to indicate that

the strength of symbolic control partly depends on

the conditions under which emotional behavior

was originally acquired. Bridger & Mandel (1964)

found autonomic conditioning was similar

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1851
regardless of whether the CS was associated with

threat of shock alone or with threat and shock

stimulation (Figure 9-5). However, emotional


responses established on the basis of actual

painful experiences were less susceptible to

cognitive control. Whereas threat- conditioned

responses were promptly abolished by removal of

electrodes and information that shocks would be


discontinued, emotional responses produced by
painful stimulation were much more resistant to

extinction. The latter findings lend support to the

view that conditioned responses typically contain

dual components (Bridger & Mandel, 1965). One


of the component parts, which is produced by the

self-arousal mechanism, is readily manipulable by


varying emotion-provoking cognitions. On the

other hand, the nonmediated component is

directly evoked by external stimulus events and


requires disconfirming experiences for its

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1852
1853
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 9-5. Mean GSR responses during acquisition and extinction for groups of subjects for whom the
CS was associated with either threat of shock alone or threat plus shock. The pseudo-conditioning
curves show the subjects’ GSR responses to control stimuli that were never paired with either
threat or shock. The latter data provide a control for the effects of general arousal and orienting
mechanisms. Bridger and Mandel, 1964.
extinction.

A study by Mandel & Bridger (1967) of the


interaction between cognitive influences and

stimulus contingencies lends further validity to

the view that conditioning outcomes typically


reflect the operation of both associative processes

and symbolic generative mechanisms. Subjects

who were informed that no further negative


reinforcement would occur showed marked
decrements in conditioned autonomic responding;

nevertheless the rate with which the response

extinguished differed depending on the order in

which conditioned and unconditioned stimuli


were presented during the acquisition period and
on the temporal interval between these stimulus

events.

The nonmediational theory of classical

conditioning assumes that, in order for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1854
conditioning to occur, the associated stimulus
events must at least be registered in the nervous

system of the organism. Therefore, in studies


assessing the role of awareness in conditioning, it

would be of considerable value to obtain evidence

that there has, in fact, been input from the

conditioned stimulus. It is not inconceivable that


in experiments employing masking procedures, in

which subjects’ attention is diverted to irrelevant

features of the task, the conditioned stimuli may

not be registered in a sufficiently consistent

manner to produce stable conditioned responses.


Hernandez-Peon, Scherrer, & Jouvet (1956)

provide evidence, based upon neurophysiological


studies, that attention focused on a particular

stimulus simultaneously reduces afferent signals


activated by other sensory stimuli. The evoked

auditory potential in the cochlear nucleus of eats


to a loud auditory stimulus was virtually

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1855
eliminated when they gazed at mice, attentively

sniffed fish odors, or received electric shocks that

distracted their attention. Horn (1960) has


demonstrated a similar diminution of neural

responses to a light flash during active attention to


other visual and auditory cues. Although there is
some dispute about whether the attenuation of

sensory signals results from mechanisms

operating at the periphery or at more central


levels, there is no doubt that neural responses to

afferent input can be substantially reduced by

attending behavior directed toward irrelevant


stimulus events.

Even in the absence of an experimentally


induced diverting set, some subjects may choose

to attend closely to extraneous stimuli, and thus

fail to achieve appropriate registration,

recognition, or conditioning. Under these

circumstances, the absence of learning may be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1856
erroneously attributed to lack of conscious

recognition when, in fact, it derives from deficient

stimulus registration. The most decisive


demonstration that awareness is a necessary

precondition for learning would require evidence

that, despite adequate stimulus registration,

classical conditioning does not occur without

awareness of the stimulus contingency.

Implications of Symbolic Control for


Behavioral Modification

The therapeutic potential of symbolic

processes has not been fully exploited although,

contrary to common belief, behavioral therapies


rely heavily upon effects which are cognitively
produced. This is particularly true of

desensitization treatments in which imagined


stimulus events are characteristically employed to

evoke emotional responses that ordinarily occur

to the actual stimuli. In some variants of this

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1857
approach emotion-neutralizing responses are

likewise induced symbolically. It is true that the

therapist exercises a certain degree of


instructional control over the timing and duration

of clients’ implicit activities, but no deconditioning

effects are likely to ensue unless clients engage in

appropriate cognitive representation of the

suggested sequence of events. As noted in an


earlier chapter, most desensitization methods
represent a form of cognitive counterconditioning

in which either the anxiety responses, the anxiety-

neutralizing responses, or both sets of events are

in large part symbolically controlled.

A similar higher-order conditioning process is

involved in the modification of addictive or


compulsive forms of behavior by means of aversive

cognitive counterconditioning (Cautela, 1966;

Miller, 1959, 1963). In the application of this

procedure, individuals typically visualize the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1858
objects to which they are markedly attracted and

immediately revivify strong nauseous reactions.

Preliminary results based upon individual case


studies, which were reviewed in the preceding

chapter, indicate that conditioned aversions and

avoidance responses can be established in this

manner.

The material discussed earlier attests to the


fact that the most rapid and enduring changes in
instrumental behavior are achieved when

knowledge of contingencies is supplemented with

appropriate reinforcing consequences. In


interview approaches interpretations of probable
contingencies and suggestions for preferable

courses of action are offered repeatedly, but


favorable outcomes are rarely arranged. On the

other hand, practitioners utilizing reinforcement

procedures carefully plan the necessary

behavioral consequences, but often fail to specify

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1859
the basis for the reinforcement. It is apparent from

the influential role played by cognitive variables in

change processes that in an optimal treatment


program change agents should designate the

conditions of reinforcement in addition to

arranging the requisite response consequences.

There is a further potential application of

knowledge of symbolic control that is well worth


exploring. It has been amply demonstrated that
behavior is partly regulated by its immediate

consequences. Extensions of this principle to the

phenomenon of self-regulation (Bandura & Perloff,


1967; Ferster, Nurnberger, & Levitt, 1962) provide
evidence that people can exercise a certain degree

of control over their own behavior by arranging


favorable contingencies for themselves. Extending

this notion of self-management a step further, it is

entirely possible that individuals may be able to

control and alter their behavior by symbolically

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1860
produced consequences.

Many forms of behavior that eventually create

adverse social or physiological effects are strongly


maintained by their immediately reinforcing

effects. If the remote consequences could be

moved forward, or if other types of negative

outcomes were applied to the incipient precursors

of the behavior, its occurrence might be


significantly reduced. In most cases, this
rearrangement of consequences is difficult to

achieve by manipulating actual reinforcing events.

However, there is some suggestive evidence that


symbolized outcomes possess reinforcing
properties that are similar to their physical

equivalents. Weiner (1965) found that both


imagining aversive consequences and the actual

occurrence of the same negative events reduced

responding compared to a condition involving no

feedback, although imagined outcomes produced

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1861
somewhat weaker reductive effects. These

findings add credence to the view that overt

behavior can be partly regulated by covert self-


reinforcement operations.

Possible applications of this form of behavioral

control are discussed briefly by Homme (1965) in

a paper concerned with the conditioning of

implicit responses. In dealing with such problems


as excessive cigarette smoking, overeating, and
other activities which produce immediate and

automatic reinforcing effects, the individual

selects numerous aversive consequences of the


behavior which can be employed as covert
negative reinforcers. When he wishes to smoke,

for example, he immediately symbolizes the


aversive effects of smoking, or revivifies other

negative experiences. To the extent that

sufficiently strong aversive consequences of

smoking can be created by anti-smoking ideations,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1862
smoking may be significantly reduced. Such

implicitly produced consequences, if effective at

all, are likely to exert greatest controlling power


when applied to weaker incipient forms of the

behavior than when the response tendency is

quite compelling, or after the behavior has already

been performed.

In the above examples cognitive activities are


employed in a contingent manner as reinforcing
events to reduce the incidence of overt behaviors.

Often certain trains of thought produce strong

emotional responses or disruptive effects upon


behavior, in which case the problem becomes one
of controlling the symbolic events themselves. In

its less extreme but more prevalent form, this type


of dysfunction tends to hamper persons’ efficiency

and productivity. As Dollard & Miller (1950) point

out, productive and creative work requires, among

other things, sustained attention to the task at

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1863
hand. Any interruptions from without, or intrusion

by irrelevant thoughts from within, temporarily

halt ongoing activities. Whereas the physical


environment can be arranged so as to minimize

external distractions, attainment of control of

one’s thoughts presents a much more challenging

problem.

The most direct and effective means of


reducing emotion-arousing intrusive thoughts is
to modify their eliciting conditions by the types of

procedures discussed in preceding chapters.

Nevertheless, ruminations about upsetting


experiences inevitably occur in everyday life and
hence persons must develop effective means of

ideational self-control which can be utilized


whenever the need arises. By far the most

prevalent, as well as the most futile, approach to

this problem is to advise others simply to banish

disturbing thoughts from their minds.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1864
If a person is to achieve greater voluntary

control over his thought processes he must

manipulate variables which are capable of eliciting


and sustaining competing ideational activities.

This can be achieved in several different ways. The


simplest is by effecting changes in attentional
responses. That is, perturbing trains of thought

can be promptly turned off by directing one’s

attention to absorbing events which elicit


superseding cognitive activities. Indeed, this form

of self-control, in which thought-produced arousal

is diminished by engrossment in absorbing


literary material, televised programs, vocational

and avocational pursuits, and other engaging


projects, is widely practiced inadvertently.

Individuals could undoubtedly exercise greater

self-control over their thought processes and


attendant emotional responses through more

deliberate use of prepotent activities which are

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1865
kept in reserve for this very purpose.

Although the discussion thus far has


highlighted the possible tranquilizing effects of

attentional changes, they can serve a preventive

function as well. In many cases, a relatively weak


external stimulus may elicit a particular train of

thought which, through its associative

connections, activates further ideational contents


capable of generating strong emotional responses.
By interrupting this associative sequence in its

early stages, the occurrence of thought-produced

arousal may be forestalled altogether.

Assuming that symbolic activities obey the

same psychological laws as overt behavior, it

should be possible to influence significantly the

nature, incidence, and potency of covert events.

The difficulties in detecting the presence of

implicit responses present a major obstacle to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1866
their control by reinforcement practices if one
adheres to the conventional paradigm in which an

external agent monitors the occurrence of the


desired behavior, imposes the contingencies, and

administers the reinforcers. However, as Homme

(1965) points out, the occurrence or absence of

covert events can be easily and reliably detected


by the person doing the thinking. Consequently,

such responses are most readily conditioned

through self-reinforcement operations. In this type

of approach implicit responses are self-monitored,

the contingencies are self-prescribed, and the


consequences self-produced.

Homme suggests that Premack’s (1965)


differential-probability hypothesis (i.e., any highly

preferred activity has reinforcing capabilities)


might be utilized to good advantage in the

contingency arrangement and selection of self-


reinforcers. That is, the strength and incidence of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1867
certain classes of thoughts can be increased by

making preferred activities contingent upon their

occurrence. Presumably depressive, infuriating,


and other vexatious ruminations could be

displaced by self-reinforcement of more

constructive lines of thought. However, in view of

the dearth of information regarding the

conditionability of implicit events, development of


efficacious treatment methods must await
thoroughgoing analysis of thought control

processes as influenced by external or self-

monitored reinforcement operations, by

attentional changes, and by other self-manipulable


factors.

Discrepancy between Response Systems


and the Unconscious

The discussion thus far has emphasized

cognitive control of autonomic and instrumental

behavior under conditions where both the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1868
environmental stimulus supports and the

reinforcing events are easily recognizable. Of

equal interest, particularly for theories of


personality, is the observation that persons

sometimes display discriminative autonomic or

motor responsiveness without conscious

recognition of eliciting stimuli that are too weak

for reliable identification. Traditional explanations


of such phenomena have tended to invoke a
potent psychic entity in the form of an

“unconscious mind” which supposedly possesses

sensitive discriminating capacities. According to

this interpretation, the unconscious mind readily


perceives threatening stimuli which occur below

the level of awareness, and the ego mobilizes


various defensive mechanisms to cope with them

and keep them out of awareness.

Considerable research has been conducted

over the years, principally by Eriksen (1958,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1869
1960), to evaluate the empirical status of

subliminal discrimination and conditioning.

Results of these rigorously designed


investigations, along with other findings, lend

support to an alternative conception of the

characteristics of unconscious processes.

In the typical paradigm subjects are presented

a series of neutral and negatively valenced visual


stimuli at about threshold value, and observers’
nonverbal responses (autonomic or motor), as

well as verbal responses to the stimuli, are

recorded concurrently. A major controversy arose


in connection with evidence originally reported by
Lazarus & McLeary (1951) that subjects

frequently displayed conditioned autonomic


responses to aversive stimuli in the absence of

correct verbal recognition of the stimulus

sequence. The authors interpreted these data as

demonstration of unconscious discrimination.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1870
Although the findings were not disputed, their

explanation as support for the existence of a

“subception” process was vigorously challenged


by Eriksen (1958) and Goldiamond (1958) on

methodological grounds.

In studies of this sort, the definition and

method of establishing thresholds provide one

major source of error. In most instances the


threshold is defined as the stimulus value at which
verbal recognition is about 50 percent correct.

Hence, a subject can, in fact, discriminate at better

than chance level a stimulus below the arbitrary


statistical threshold. Therefore, occurrence of
conditioned autonomic responses at slightly better

than chance expectancy does not represent a


compelling demonstration of unconscious

discrimination. Of much greater significance is

evidence (Eriksen, 1960) that subliminal

conditioning or discrimination rarely occurs when

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1871
the relevant stimuli are below recognition level.

A second methodological problem arises

because verbal reports can be influenced by


nonsensory factors. Subjects are generally

reluctant to admit the presence of a faint stimulus

when they are unsure, a negative response bias

that is likely to raise the verbal threshold

artifactually. A cautious person would obtain a


verbal threshold much higher than is, in fact, the
case, thus producing a large, spurious subliminal

effect. Feigned nonrecognition is most likely to

occur when subjects are presented socially


censured content such as taboo sexual words.

In addition to the influence of propriety and

subjective confidence upon verbal reports, verbal

responses do not convey the finer discriminations

made by subjects when they are required to

classify their perceptual experiences in terms of a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1872
few discrete verbal categories. Pseudo-subliminal
effects may arise from the differential accuracy of

continuous autonomic responses and discrete


categorical responses at the verbal level. The fact,

however, that discrepancies in response systems

exist even when subjects are allowed a more

refined set of verbal responses, and motor


responses are substituted for an autonomic

measure (Eriksen, 1957), indicates that

discriminative behavior can occur without

accurate verbal labeling of the relevant stimulus

events.

As Eriksen points out, the question of whether

a more sensitive discriminatory mechanism exists


at an unconscious level can be answered most

directly by comparing the thresholds of different


response systems. This procedure involves

conditioning an autonomic response to a


supraliminal stimulus and then comparing the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1873
incidence of accurate verbal recognition and

concurrent autonomic responsiveness to the

stimulus presented above and below threshold.


Using this method, along with a verbal response

measure that controlled for negative reporting

bias, Dulany & Eriksen (1959) found that

physiological and verbal response systems were

equally insensitive to low stimulus values, but


verbal response was the superior discriminator at
intermediate and high levels of stimulation.

Although the above findings provide no

evidence for unconscious responding to


threatening stimuli too weak to produce
awareness, nevertheless discrepancies between

simultaneous responses to environmental cues do


occur and hence require explanation. The latter

phenomenon, rather than being attributed to the

regulating influence of egos, superegos, or other

psychic agents operating within an unconscious

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1874
mind, can perhaps be more plausibly and

parsimoniously conceptualized in terms of

discrepancy between partially independent


response systems, a view for which Eriksen

provides considerable empirical support.

When two responses appear more or less

concomitantly with a series of sensory stimuli, the

nature of the relationship between these events


remains obscure. It may be that conscious
recognition of threatening stimuli evokes the

autonomic responses; the occurrence of

discriminative autonomic responses may produce


correct verbal recognition; or the two modes of
response may be elicited independently by the

environmental stimulus. In an effort to unravel the


relationships among variables in “subception”

studies, Eriksen conducted a series of experiments

in which verbal recognition responses and

concurrent motor or autonomic responses were

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1875
each correlated with the eliciting stimuli, with the

influence of the concomitant response variable

partialled out statistically. By this method of


analysis it is possible to determine what

relationship, if any, exists between autonomic

responses and sensory stimuli when differences in

awareness are eliminated. Results of these

experiments consistently show that verbal and


autonomic modes of behavior represent parallel
response systems that are both reactive to sensory

stimulation and are partially independent of each

other. The findings furthermore provide no

evidence that persons respond autonomically or


motorically in a more sensitive and accurate

manner than they do at the conscious verbal level.

Variables that affect symbolic, physiological,

and motor response systems differentially would

be expected to reduce the degree of correlation

between different classes of reactions. To the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1876
extent that certain factors impair the accuracy of

measurement of one class of behavior without

affecting another, response disparities will obtain


even though the two systems are, in fact, highly

congruent. Extraneous variables are most likely to

exercise a high degree of control when persons are

responding to stimuli that are too weak to

override the effects of more salient characteristics


of the situation.

Genuine disparities among different classes of

behavior can be produced through the application

of differential reinforcement. Thus, if hostile


thoughts and verbalizations are approved or
permissively accepted but overt aggressive actions

are consistently punished, persons will readily


verbalize aggressive feelings without exhibiting

any of their motor equivalents. Similarly, by

reversing the reinforcement contingencies one

could effectively inhibit cognitive representations

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1877
of behavioral manifestations. It should likewise be

possible to produce other patterns of correlations

by varying systematically the contingencies of


reinforcement into which the responses enter.

Most of the material in the preceding sections

treats unconscious phenomena as internal and

environmental events which are not represented

in awareness. Some theories, however, consider it


important to distinguish between the
preconscious and the unconscious. In this

distinction the preconscious comprises mental

elements which are readily accessible to


consciousness by directing the subject’s attention
to them. On the other hand, the unconscious

includes elements which are relatively


inaccessible to awareness and which can be made

conscious only by removing strong resistance,

preferably through interpretive treatment.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1878
In view of the questionable validity of

psychotherapists’ speculations about unconscious

contents, accessibility to consciousness, as


inferred from degree of resistance, is an

exceedingly unreliable criterion upon which to


build a structural theory of mental functioning. If
an individual refuses to acknowledge certain

thoughts or impulses suggested to him by his

therapist, the question remains whether the


resistance reveals repressed contents or justifiable

incredulity in the face of erroneous

interpretations. For example, it would be a


mistake to conclude that a person who vigorously

opposed interpretations that certain of his deviant


behaviors represented derivative manifestations

of a clandestine and powerful “zoognick,” did in

fact possess an unconscious “zoognick” held in


check by strong repressive forces. As noted earlier,

when the interpretive process is viewed from a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1879
framework of social influence, the degree of

opposition shown by clients, and the amount and


type of elements which eventually emerge from

their hypothesized unconscious depend, in large

part, upon psychotherapists’ prestige, credibility,

belief systems, and other extraneous factors.

Although one might seriously dispute whether

the existence of unconscious psychic events can be


reliably established through psychotherapeutic
methods, there is no doubt that thoughts and

other implicit activities can be effectively

inhibited. Thought inhibition is traditionally

attributed to the mechanism of repression, which


is believed to operate largely at unconscious
levels. It is further assumed, particularly in

psychoanalytic formulations, that repressed

elements not only maintain a dynamic life of their

own, but are charged with a cathexis of drive

energy which continuously presses for discharge

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1880
either directly or in derivative form. Opposing

psychic agents which assume defensive and

prohibitive functions maintain the repression by


expending a portion of the psychic energy at their

disposal.

In behavior theory the phenomenon ascribed

to repression is conceptualized in terms of

processes of avoidance conditioning. If certain


thoughts are repeatedly associated with painful
experiences, they gradually become endowed with

aversive properties. Since thoughts are private

events, they can be affected only indirectly by


reinforcing operations. One way in which this
might come about is through the process of

response generalization. For example, the


experiment by Miller (1951), which was discussed

earlier, demonstrated that anxiety attached to a

spoken word tends to generalize to the thought of

the word. Also, since thoughts typically precede or

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1881
accompany overt responses, implicit events may

acquire the capacity to arouse anxiety through

their close temporal proximity to punished


instrumental acts.

Once certain thoughts come to function as

conditioned aversive stimuli, their occurrence

generates anxiety and their elimination allays it.

As shown in the experiment by Eriksen & Kuethe


(1956), inhibition of anxiety-provoking thoughts is
usually achieved through simultaneous

occurrence of competing ideational activities.

Students were asked to associate to a series of


words, and they were punished by electric shock
every time they responded with associations given

initially to five arbitrarily selected words.


Punished associations declined rapidly, whereas

nonpunished word associations remained

unchanged as items were repeated. Some

indications that punishment eliminated responses

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1882
at the implicit level rather than merely produced

deliberate word substitution are shown by the fact

that replacement of initially dominant associates


occurred in students who were unaware of the

basis for shocks or that their associations had

changed; the inhibitory effects also persisted on a

somewhat different association task in which the

threat of aversive stimulation was removed.

In social-learning interpretations of
repression, incompatible responses rather than

psychic agents are considered to be the inhibitory

forces. It should also be noted that the various


mechanisms of defense are defined, for the most
part, by the characteristics of the behavior that

competes with and supersedes negatively


valenced tendencies. For example, if a person who

is instigated to hostility exhibits positive thoughts

and actions toward the thwarting agent he is

engaging in reaction formation; if he should

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1883
become irritated at a blameless person he is

exhibiting displacement; if he avoids unpleasant

thoughts by becoming preoccupied with


competing ideational content and attendant rituals

he is resorting to obsessive-compulsive defenses.

Social-learning theory not only makes no

appeal to prohibitive psychic agents in accounting

for the inhibitory process, but it differs from


psychodynamic approaches also in the
assumptions made about the nature of inhibited

response tendencies. While formerly punished

responses may retain their capacity to generate


emotional effects if they are activated, it is not
assumed that they lead a dynamic existence within

an unconscious mind, that they possess a drive


energy which must be reduced periodically, that

they press continuously for discharge in one guise

or another, or that they require unceasing

restraint to confine them in the unconscious

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1884
domain. Rather, it is assumed that inhibited

behavior propensities remain inert unless

activated by appropriate stimulation. Under


circumstances where the incompatible controlling

responses to the same stimuli are clearly

dominant and therefore readily evoked, the

punished behavior is unlikely to reach even the

incipient level. On the other hand, when punished


responses are strongly established and competing
tendencies are not completely dominant, inhibited

responses may be aroused to the point where they

generate conflict and anxiety. A repressed

element, according to this view, does not have a


qualitatively different nature from any response

that has been superseded by an alternative


pattern of behavior.

Apart from its more secure empirical status,

the concurrent response system model has many

advantages over formulations which assume the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1885
existence of an unconscious mind. The former

conceptualization does not lend itself to pseudo-

explanations in which a descriptive label for


response disparities is reified and assigned causal

properties—for example, discrepancy between

symbolic and motor or physiological responses to

the same stimulus events is given the descriptive

label of “unconscious,” which is then converted


into an internal agency that exercises powerful
control over behavior. The multiple-response-

systems interpretation of phenomena designated

as unconscious also encourages systematic

exploration of the variables which give rise to


response disparity. Finally, the theory has

important treatment implications. Given the


existence of independent but partially correlated

modes of response to significant stimuli, the

psychotherapist can profitably concentrate his


efforts on direct modification of the classes of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1886
response that the treatment is designed to alter,

rather than embarking on a protracted search for

unconscious causative agents that one can predict


in advance will prove to be highly concordant with

the therapist’s particular theoretical predilections.

COGNITIVE EFFECTS OF SUBLIMINAL STIMULATION

The studies reviewed in the preceding section


investigated unconscious phenomena mainly in
terms of discrimination of weak stimuli. Klein and

his collaborators (Klein, Spence, Holt, &

Gourevitch, 1958; Spence, 1964; Spence &

Holland, 1962) have employed a somewhat


different approach to the problem. The effects of

subliminal stimulation are measured, not on

autonomic or recognition responses, but in terms

of indirect indicators of cognitive changes.

In most of these experiments neutral rather


than threatening stimuli are presented at levels

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1887
which apparently are sufficiently intense to
register and evoke some response in the central

nervous system, but the sensory input is either too

faint or too brief to produce full recognition or


correct identification of the stimulus events. In

other studies a verbal stimulus, which is presented


briefly well above identification level, is imbedded
in other competing stimuli and, therefore, may not

be recalled even though it has been fully

registered. Numerous indirect measures of


unconscious activation have been employed, such

as the inclusion of similar stimulus elements in

fantasy production (e.g., stories given to projective

tests, drawing, dreams); increased tendency to

give related responses to items in a word-


association test; cognitive elaboration of a neutral
stimulus in terms of the affective tone of preceding

subliminal cues; and arousal of responses

belonging to the same associative network.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1888
Two conflicting hypotheses have been

proposed regarding the characteristics of

subliminal stimulation. According to the continuity


view of perceptual processes, the effects of

subliminal stimuli are similar to those of


recognized stimuli; since the magnitude of
influence varies with the intensity of input,

however, the former are less potent. The opposing

theory, on the other hand, assumes that the effects


of subliminal stimulation are more diffuse and

pronounced than those produced by supraliminal

stimuli because, in the former case, the restricting


effects of consciousness upon thought are

removed. This formulation, which was originally


advanced by Freud (1953), assumes that

preconscious and conscious influences obey

different laws, with stimuli which operate outside


of awareness being less bound by logical and

reality-oriented controls.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1889
These alternative views cannot be evaluated

empirically because it has yet to be reliably

demonstrated that subliminal stimulation does in


fact influence cognitive activities to any

appreciable degree. Research of this type is


plagued by even more difficult methodological
problems than the approaches to unconscious

phenomena reviewed earlier. As was true of the

research on “perceptual defense,” the question of


whether one is dealing with a genuine

phenomenon or with a methodological artifact is

of continuing concern. Interpretations of findings


always remain in doubt because subjects who

display subliminal effects may be partially aware


of the stimulus patterns before reporting them so

that the stimulation is actually at or above

recognition threshold. Since subliminal stimuli


rarely appear in cognitive performances in direct

form, investigators must search for indirect,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1890
distorted, or remote symbolic representations in

order to obtain at least a few scorable responses

for most subjects. To complicate matters further,

no objective criteria are available for identifying


distorted or transformed representations, and

high inter-scorer reliability is often difficult to


achieve.

Even if one includes the altered guises that


unrecognized sensory input presumably takes, the
effect of subliminal stimulation upon cognitive

functioning is exceedingly weak and, in some

cases, it is wiped out completely when sources of

artifact such as associative clustering effects are


controlled (Worell & Worell, 1966), and base rates
of indirect manifestations are obtained from

subjects who have never been exposed to the

subliminal stimuli ( Johnson & Eriksen, 1961). It

would appear from the elusive and scanty yield of

research in this area that subliminal activation

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1891
must play a relatively inconsequential role in

regulating human behavior. Whereas recognizable

stimuli assume a powerful behavior-directing


function, nonrecognized stimuli have, at best,

weak, inconsistent, and fragmentary psychological

effects.

Attitudinal Consequences of Behavioral and


Affective Changes

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ATTITUDINAL AND


BEHAVIORAL CHANGES

In most instances change agents who strive to

alter people’s attitudes are not interested in

attitudes per se. Although the influence

procedures are designed to change evaluations

and preferences concerning certain persons,

commodities, or activities, the principal aim is to

modify behavior. Thus, for example, the intent of


persuasive efforts is to get people to buy
particular brands of merchandise, to vote for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1892
certain candidates, to engage in or to abandon
smoking, or to pursue various other courses of

action. The attitude-change approach is selected as


a means of influencing behavior when the desired

behavior cannot be directly elicited and reinforced

for practical or other reasons.

It is widely assumed that attitudes are


important determinants of overt actions and

consequently that any changes brought about in


the attitudinal domain will have widespread

effects upon subsequent behavior. It is further

believed that altered response patterns that are

accompanied by correspondingly altered attitudes

will be more stable over time than behavior that is


induced directly without cognitive supports. For

these and other reasons, the development of


beneficial attitudes is often regarded as a major

objective of social change endeavors. If it is


demonstrated that attitudinal changes do, in fact,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1893
have significant behavioral consequences, it would

be of considerable import to devise efficacious

procedures that might be specifically directed


toward the modification of attitudes.

A voluminous literature has accumulated over

the years concerning attitude change through

exposure to persuasive communications, which,

because of their extensive mass application,


constitute the prevailing influence technique
explored in this field. Considering the multitude of

publications and the powerful controlling

functions assuredly conferred upon attitudes, it is


surprising to find that a thorough search of this
literature by Festinger (1964) yielded a dearth of

studies in which the influence of attitude change


upon behavior had been specifically investigated.

Nor has the yield of pertinent studies increased

noticeably in more recent years. The available

data, though admittedly meager, disclose that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1894
changes in attitudes produced by persuasive

communications generally have little or no effect

upon overt actions (Fleishmann, Harris, & Burtt,


1955; Levitt, 1965; Maccoby, Romney, Adams, &

Maccoby, 1962). In contrast to these findings,

Greenwald (1965a) reports a positive, but low,

correlation between attitudinal and behavioral

changes. However, for subjects who express a


prior commitment counter to the influence
attempt, persuasive communications alter

attitudes but produce no significant change in

behavior (Greenwald, 1965b). These apparently

conflicting results may partly reflect differences in


the nature and importance of the behavior

undergoing change, the time elapsing between


assessment of changes in attitudes and behavior,

and the order in which these two sets of events are

measured. There is some reason to expect (Cohen,


1964) that changes in these two forms of response

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1895
will be more highly correlated when attitudes are

measured following, rather than prior to,

performance of discrepant behavior.

The failure to obtain consistent and high

relationships may be interpreted in several ways.


It is possible that responses to questionnaires and

self-ratings, which constitute the dependent

variables in most social- psychological research,


are unreliable indicants of people’s actual opinions
and attitudes. It has been shown (Schanck, 1932),

for example, that privately held attitudes often

differ markedly from those that are publicly

espoused. Persuasive communications may thus


elicit compliance to implied situational demands
without significantly affecting people’s private

attitudes.

A second explanation, suggested by Festinger

(1964), is that attitudinal changes resulting from

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1896
persuasive influences are relatively unstable and
will, therefore, disappear unless the

corresponding overt behavior is sustained by


adequate consequences. This view assumes that

attitudes produce temporary performance

changes; however, when environmental

contingencies do not support the new activities


individuals revert to their old behavior and the

newly established attitudes are similarly altered to

coincide with the actions.

The relative modifiability of attitudes and

actions, and the degree of correspondence

obtained between changes in these two sets of

events, may vary with the affective and social


consequences accompanying the behavior. A given

social influence might produce analogous changes


in both attitude and action when persons are

indifferent to, or favorably disposed toward,


performing the advocated activities. Most

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1897
attempts to control consumer behavior through

persuasive communications would fall in this

category. Thus, for example, a person who is


considering purchasing a television set may be

prompted to buy an expensive color console after

being convinced by persuasive commercials that it

provides more pleasurable viewing than a black-

and-white set. The process is more complicated,


however, when persons resist advocated behavior
that they can perform because it results in self-

devaluation. This is illustrated by situations in

which people are induced to behave in ways that

conflict with their beliefs. Obstacles to change also


arise when individuals are amenable to engaging

in desired activities but are unable to do so


because of strong fears and inhibitions. In the

latter instances, a weak method may alter

responses that are readily susceptible to change,


such as verbal evaluations, but fail to modify overt

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1898
behavior that is rendered intractable by its

adverse consequences. A relatively powerful

influence would be required to achieve correlative


changes in different types of response systems.

Unlike the inconsistent effects of persuasive


communications, desensitization and modeling
approaches are capable of producing correlated

changes in refractory behavior and attitudes.

These positive relationships range from r = .39


when changes in attitudes are measured by the

semantic differential technique to r = .59 and r =

.72 for change scores based on a variety of attitude


scales (Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter, 1969;

Blanchard, 1969).

Correlated changes, when they do occur, may

reflect the operation of several different

mechanisms. According to most contemporary

attitude theories, some of which will be reviewed

later, there is a drive to maintain consistency

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1899
among beliefs, feelings, and actions. A change in

any one of the components will, therefore,

engender congruous modifications in the other


constituents. In these consistency models, changes

in attitudes or behavior are treated, not as

consequent events, but as causal variables that

affect other forms of behavior. An alternative

interpretation is that environmental influences


have similar but independent effects on feelings,
beliefs, and behavior. In this view, consistencies of

belief and behavior represent correlated coeffects

rather than outcomes of a process in which

modification of one type of behavior produces


changes in other forms of responding.

Definitive tests of the parallel effects and


consistency explanations of change processes are

precluded by the absence of a methodology that

would permit simultaneous measurement of

beliefs, affect, and actions. If incongruity creates

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1900
an internal stimulus for psychological change then

a sequential testing procedure confounds the

effects of external influences and the consistency


drive. Conversely, a given environmental influence

could have similar consequences on different

classes of response that would be erroneously

ascribed to the operation of a consistency drive.

These alternative formulations perhaps should be


regarded as complementary rather than
conflicting. Under most conditions, powerful

stimulus events produce diverse psychological

changes, and performance of new behavior is

likely to have additional cognitive and emotional


consequences.

In many respects, the question of whether


attitudes regulate overt behavior might be

considered a pseudo-issue created by arbitrary

distinctions between different types of response.

An attitude is variously defined as a disposition to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1901
behave favorably or unfavorably toward a given

object (Brown, 1965); an organization of valenced

cognitions combined with a predilection for


motive arousal (Newcomb, Turner, & Converse,

1965); an affective evaluative response toward an

object (Rosenberg, 1960); or an implicit

anticipatory mediating response (Doob, 1947).

Like most implicit tendencies, attitudes are


characteristically inferred from various forms of
overt behavior rather than identified by some

independent criterion. Consequently, if self-ratings

were treated as a class of behavior rather than

assigned special status as indicants of an internal


mediator which is given substance and endowed

with influential regulatory powers, then the issue


of the relationship of attitude to behavior might be

more meaningfully conceptualized as a problem of

correlation between different response systems.


From this point of view, there exists no intrinsic

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1902
relationship between these two sets of responses,

since they can be either highly correlated or

dissociated depending upon their respective


contingencies of reinforcement.

The differentiation between attitudes and


overt actions disappears completely when the

former are primarily inferred from nonverbal

behavior, as is generally the case in


interpretations of naturalistic phenomena. For
example, a person who displays antagonistic

responses or actively avoids members of a given

ethnic group is believed to have a negative

attitude, whereas he is assumed to possess a


positive attitude if he exhibits approving amicable
reactions. In such circumstances, the issue of

whether attitudes influence behavior reduces to

the meaningless question of whether a particular

response pattern determines itself!

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1903
It should be emphasized that the foregoing

discussion is not intended to minimize the role of

symbolic mediators in human functioning, but


rather to call attention to the conceptual

difficulties created by a lack of independent


measures of covert tendencies designated as
attitudes. As a result, it is often difficult to

ascertain whether investigators are dealing with

superfluous abstractions from behavior, with


coeffects of operations of social influence, or with

causally related events.

Much of the ambiguity that prevails in this field

might be reduced and the theoretical issues more


precisely delineated if the indeterminate concept
of “attitude” were abandoned altogether. Rather,

the basic issue posed earlier might be rephrased

as follows: To what degree, and under what

conditions, do changes brought about in either

cognitive, affective, or motor classes of behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1904
have reciprocal effects? It might be argued that

there would be some value in retaining the

attitude construct, apart from its shorthand


labeling function, if it were used to represent the

higher organization processes that are inferred

from specific behavioral manifestations. However,

in view of the substantial evidence (Bandura &

Walters, 1963; Mischel, 1968) that human


behavior is markedly specific and extensively
regulated by discriminative cues, reinforcement

contingencies, and other external events, there is

reason to question the utility of theoretical

formulations that invoke unitary mediators that


cannot possibly serve as determinants of

heterogeneous responses that are not


intercorrelated to any appreciable degree.

In an effort to account for behavioral

specificity within an attitude theory framework,

Rokeach (1966) has advanced the view that social

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1905
behavior is determined by sets of interacting

attitudes—one activated by the attitude objects,

and the other elicited by the situation. He


rightfully argues that in many instances situational

influences, which have been neglected in attitude

theory, may outweigh the response-eliciting

effects of the attitude object. Hence, individuals

often display quite different attitudes toward the


same attitude object in different social situations.
A theory that predicts attitudinal responses on the

basis of both subject and situational variables

would undoubtedly have greater predictive power

than one relying solely on subjects’ evaluations of


the attitude object in undefined contexts. The

controlling properties of situations primarily


reflect differences in reinforcement contingencies

as they apply to attitudinal behaviors expressed in

diverse social contexts. One might achieve even


greater predictive efficacy by treating attitudes as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1906
evaluative responses that are under reinforcement

and discriminative stimulus control, as is any

other form of behavior.

Strategies of Attitude Change

BELIEF-ORIENTED APPROACH

Three general approaches can be employed


either singly or in various combinations to induce

attitudinal changes. The informational or belief-

oriented approach attempts to effect modifications

in people’s attitudes by altering their beliefs about

the attitude object through exposure to various


forms of persuasive communications. It is

assumed that people can be induced to change

their evaluations of an attitude object by

presenting them with new information about its

characteristics.

Most of the research generated by this

informational approach (Cohen, 1964; Hovland &

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1907
Janis, 1959; Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953;
Rosenberg et al., 1960) has been expressly

designed to isolate the conditions under which a

given communication will have its maximal effect


upon recipients’ attitudes. Three general sets of

variables, namely the nature of the communicator,


the communication, and the recipients, have been
most extensively investigated. Studies of the

persons being influenced have generally been

concerned with their personality characteristics,


the level of their intelligence or sophistication, the

nature of their pre-existing attitudes, and the

strength of their commitment to a given position.

The effects of communicator variables in

enhancing attitudinal modification are typically


analyzed in terms of attributes such as expertness,
trustworthiness, prestige, impartiality, social

power, and concealment of the persuader’s

manipulative or propagandistic intent. The form

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1908
and organization of persuasive arguments, which

can also significantly influence attitude formation

and change, involves such matters as the optimal


order of presenting weak and major arguments,

the sequence of supporting and opposing


arguments, the degree of explicitness with which
conclusions are stated, the amount of repetition,

the degree of discrepancy between the subject’s

views and the ones advocated, the affective


properties of the contents, and whether the

influence program relies upon a one-sided

presentation or also includes some consideration


and refutation of counterarguments. Research

findings show that the effects of these different


variables rarely produce simple effects; rather

their direction and magnitude are dependent in

part upon the simultaneous influence of other


factors. For example, the amount of attitude

change may increase as a direct function of degree

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1909
of discrepancy of opinions advocated by a highly

respected source, whereas a less credible

persuader may exert a decreasing influence the


more divergent his opinions (Aronson, Turner, &

Carlsmith, 1963; Bergin, 1962). To complicate


matters further, a given variable may have
differential immediate and long-term effects upon

attitudes. With the passage of time, relevant

content may be retained but the source forgotten,


thus reducing initial credibility influences

(Hovland & Weiss, 1951; Hovland, Lumsdaine, &

Sheffield, 1949; Kelman & Hovland, 1953).

Most of the preceding investigations of


persuasive communications have been primarily
guided by a set of empirical principles rather than

a systematic theory. However, these principles are

organized around the basic assumption that

attitude change is governed to a large extent by

anticipations conveyed through communications

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1910
of rewarding and punishing consequences for

certain courses of action. A competent or

prestigious communicator is generally more


influential than a less competent one because the

former’s behavioral recommendations, if executed,

are more likely to result in favorable outcomes. As

noted below, the content of communications often

includes incentive references or is expressly


designed to alter the valence of the attitude object.

Although belief changes can be induced by

exposure to communication stimuli, there is little

evidence that mere presentation of information


about the attitude object alters people’s behavior
toward it to any great extent. Higher-order

conditioning processes are therefore frequently


employed to augment the potency of persuasive

communications. One method, which relies upon

the phenomenon of vicarious reinforcement,

increases the likelihood that an observer will

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1911
respond in the recommended manner by depicting

reinforcing consequences accruing to a

performing model. In positive appeals,


performance of the behavior suggested by the

communicator results in a host of rewarding

effects. Thus, smoking a certain brand of cigarettes

or using a particular hair lotion wins the loving

admiration of voluptuous belles, enhances job


performance, masculinizes one’s self-concept,
actualizes individualism and authenticity,

tranquilizes irritable nerves, invites social

recognition and amicable responsiveness from

total strangers, and arouses affectionate reactions


in spouses. Laboratory studies (Bandura, 1968)

disclose that, according to their nature, depicted


consequences to a performer not only facilitate or

inhibit response tendencies, but their effects may

outweigh the previously acquired value system of


the viewers (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1912
Negative appeals, on the other hand, portray

the adverse consequences which result from

failure to comply with a communicator’s


behavioral recommendations. Although vicarious

punishment may inhibit existing response


dispositions to some extent, it is a less reliable
procedure for producing desired attitudes and

corresponding patterns of behavior. Display of

noxious or revolting outcomes tends to arouse


strong emotional responses which may give rise to

avoidance of disturbing material and associated

recommendations (Janis, 1967) or endow the


attitude object itself with negative valence.

Belief changes achieved by persuasive


arguments may temporarily increase the

likelihood of advocated courses of action, but it is

doubtful that this type of approach can by itself

produce enduring effects unless favorable

incentive conditions, which govern persistence of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1913
induced changes, are arranged as well. That is, if a

person were to act on his beliefs, the effects of

adverse outcomes would eventually negate the


influence of persuasive communications. An

uncomely brunette, for example, who has been

persuaded that “blondes have more fun” may dye

her hair a flaxen tint, but if her dismal dating

plight should remain unchanged she is likely to


discard the belief and revert to her natural hue.

Some research indicates that susceptibility to

counterinfluence and rate of extinction of newly

established beliefs can be temporarily attenuated


by preparatory communications (Janis & Herz,
1949, cited in Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953;

McGuire, 1964). These serve to stimulate


rehearsal of refutations of opposing arguments or

to instill expectations in recipients that although

they will at first encounter failure experiences and

other adverse outcomes, if they adhere to their

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1914
convictions they will eventually achieve success. A

person who has been led to anticipate unfavorable

initial outcomes may discount subsequent failures


for a time. However, where discrepancies exist

between assumed and actual schedules of

reinforcement, both his behavior and his beliefs

probably will gradually adjust to existing

reinforcement conditions.

Because of ease of application, persuasion


techniques presented through verbal or pictorial

devices are widely employed on a mass basis in

efforts to control consumer behavior, to influence


voting choices, and to indicate either positive or
negative evaluative responses toward particular

attitude objects and social issues. The efficacy of


mass persuasion methods is often diminished,

however, by the limited control that influence

agents can exercise over people’s attention to

communication stimuli, and by lack of direct

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1915
means of immediately reinforcing the audience

members for performing the recommended

behavior. On the other hand, under conditions


where selected communication stimuli are capable

of attracting and holding viewers’ attention and

the advocated actions do, in fact, result in

favorable consequences, mass appeals may initiate

lasting changes in people’s beliefs and behavior.

AFFECT-ORIENTED APPROACH

A second general strategy for inducing

attitudinal changes involves an affect-oriented


approach. In this paradigm, both evaluations of,
and behavior toward, particular attitude objects

are modified by altering their affective properties.

These emotional changes are typically achieved

through procedures based upon the principle of

classical conditioning. As shown in preceding

chapters, attitudinal and behavioral reversals can

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1916
be produced by contiguous association of objects
that are highly positive in valence with noxious

experiences in aversive forms of


counterconditioning, or by pairing subjectively

distressing stimuli with positively reinforcing

events in desensitization operations. The most

convincing demonstrations of transfer effects of


emotional reconditioning are furnished by studies

in which the affective properties of attitude

objects are independently measured, usually in

terms of appropriate physiological indices, with

adequate controls for nonspecific social influences


(Marks & Gelder, 1967).

Although the use of association principles to


facilitate attitudinal changes has been widespread,

there has been surprisingly little research into the


effectiveness of this approach. There is some

evidence that evaluative responses can be altered


by presenting persuasive messages or objects

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1917
contiguously with appetizing foods (Janis, Kaye, &

Kirschner, 1965; Razran, 1938), unpleasant odors

(Razran, 1940), or sexually arousing stimuli


(Smith, 1968). In an effort to determine whether

extraneous gratification facilitates attitudinal

change by a conditioning mechanism or by

creating a positive attitude toward the donor,

Dabbs & Janis (1965) compared the attitudinal


effects of food consumption occurring
contiguously or noncontiguously with exposure to

persuasive messages under two different

endorsement conditions. For half the subjects the

experimenter positively endorsed the messages


while for the remaining subjects he personally

disagreed with the conclusions advocated by the


communication. Neither the contiguity nor the

endorsement variable alone produced a significant

effect, but contiguous food combined with positive


endorsement increased acceptance of unpopular

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1918
opinions. These findings, however, must be

accepted with reservation because attitude

measures are of questionable validity when


obtained by the same person who positively or

negatively endorses the opinions being rated.

In naturalistic influence situations the method

that is most frequently employed to induce

changes in the affective value of an object involves


higher-order associations of symbolic stimuli. In
this procedure, the names and attributes of

attitude objects are paired with verbal stimuli or

pictorial presentations likely to evoke in listeners

strong emotional responses on the basis of prior


first-order conditioning. In several laboratory
investigations of this learning process (Insko &

Oakes, 1966; Staats & Staats, 1957), formerly

neutral nonsense syllables have been contiguously

associated with emotionally toned adjectives. The

syllables take on negative valence through

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1919
repetitive pairings with adjectives having negative

connotations (e.g., ugly, dirty), whereas these

same items are evaluated as pleasant after they


have been associated with positively conditioned

words such as beautiful, tasty, and happy. Pre-

existing attitudinal responses toward familiar

names of persons and nations have also been

significantly altered through conditioning


methods utilizing emotional words as the
evocative stimuli (Staats & Staats, 1958).

A study by Das & Nanda (1963) further reveals

that developed conditioned evaluative responses


tend to generalize along previously established
associative networks, thus resulting in widespread

effects. After nonsense syllables had been


contiguously associated with the names of two

aboriginal tribes, favorable and unfavorable

attitudes were developed toward the syllables. In

a subsequent test subjects ascribed positive and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1920
negative attributes to the tribes in accordance

with the evaluative responses conditional to their

corresponding nonsense syllables.

It should be noted in this context that, unlike

laboratory analogues of attitudinal learning in

which single emotional words are presented in

discrete trials, in real-life situations considerably

more intense emotional reactions are typically


elicited in audiences by the cumulative impact of
long series of emotionally toned descriptions or

pictorial presentations.

The above studies, though relevant to the issue


of attitudinal modification through affective
manipulations, would have greater implications

had they included more extensive assessment of

emotional changes. Of much greater import would

be evidence that exposure to communication

stimuli does, in fact, endow attitude objects with

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1921
emotion-arousing properties, and that alterations
in the affective domain are associated with

corresponding changes in individuals’ overt


behavior toward the objects in question.

Another method of inducing affective changes

that have considerable behavioral consequences


relies upon modeling processes (Bandura, 1968).
This outcome is achieved by associating attitude

objects or their descriptions with affective


modeling cues capable of arousing in viewers

analogous emotional responses. Attitudinal

modification through modeling is illustrated in an

ingenious experiment by Duncker (1938). In an

initial test of food preferences children chose


powdered chocolate with a pleasant lemon flavor

over a very sweet sugar with a disagreeable


medicinal taste. Later, a story was read to the

children in which a stalwart astute hero abhorred


a sour-tasting foodstuff similar to the children’s

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1922
preferred food and enthusiastically relished a

sweet-tasting substance. The reactions of the

admired hero reversed the children’s initial food


preference, as measured immediately after the

story session and in six successive tests in which

the children chose between powdered chocolate

and medicated sugar. Moreover, brief recall of the

story reinstated the experimentally induced


preferences that had declined gradually over time.
More recently, Carlin (1965) found that young

children showed a greater preference for deferred

gratification after they saw an adult model display

positive affective reactions while waiting for


delayed rewards than they did after they observed

the model express negative emotional reactions


and devalue the goal object during the imposed

delay period.

In the foregoing studies both evaluative

judgments and emotional responses were

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1923
modeled. The observed changes therefore cannot

be attributed solely to the influence of affective

modeling cues. There is some reason to believe


from evidence provided by Culbertson (1957) that

the modeling of preferences and beliefs without

strong affective displays can alter attitudes.

Observers who witnessed others express

favorable attitudes toward integration


subsequently exhibited a decrease in prejudicial
attitudes.

The potency of modeling for inducing

attitudinal changes is further demonstrated in the


experiment by Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter
(1969) that was previously described. Snake-

phobic subjects were administered eight


evaluative dimensions of the semantic differential

technique, and six attitude scales on which they

rated how much they would like or loathe

different types of encounters with reptiles.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1924
Subjects were then given factual information

about the characteristics and habits of snakes in

order to control and to assess the possible


influence of incidental information before any

treatment procedures were applied. After the test

for snake avoidance behavior, the attitude

measures were again administered. In the next

phase of the experiment subjects received either


systematic desensitization, symbolic modeling,
live modeling combined with guided participation,

or no treatment. Following completion of the

treatment series the attitude measures were again

administered prior to, and immediately after, the


snake avoidance test.

The results are summarized graphically in


Figure 9-6. Subjects’ loathing of reptiles was not

altered in the slightest by factual information and

exposure to the test snake. The refractory quality

of these negative attitudes is further shown by the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1925
1926
www.freepsychotherapybooks.org
Figure 9-6. Attitudinal changes for subjects who received either one of the three treatment procedures or served
as untreated controls. The numeral 1 indicates subjects’ attitudes prior to the behavioral test,
and the numeral 2 shows their attitudes immediately after the test of avoidance behavior. Bandura,
Blanchard, and Ritter, 1969.
control subjects, whose evaluative reactions

remained unchanged across repeated

assessments. Both symbolic modeling and


desensitization, which successfully extinguished

negative emotional responses to snake stimuli,

produced extensive attitudinal changes. The

treatment condition that neutralized the anxiety-

arousing properties of snakes and enabled


subjects to interact with the repugnant attitude
object without any adverse consequences

achieved the greatest modification in attitudinal

behavior. In a study designed to assess the relative

influence of information, modeling, and guided


contact in the latter method, Blanchard (1969)

found that modeling accounted for approximately


80 percent of the attitude change. Information, on

the other hand, increased subjects’ emotional

arousal to modeling displays and had, if anything,


a slightly adverse effect.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1927
The attitudinal consequences of affective

change are also disclosed in desensitization

studies involving more general attitudes dealing


with sex, aggression, and other interpersonal

contents. These findings indicate that the cognitive


evaluative component of attitudes can be
substantially modified through direct

manipulation of the affective properties of the

attitude object without involving informational


references of a favorable or unfavorable sort.

Essentially similar results are reported by

Rosenberg (1960), who has shown that a negative


affect induced through post-hypnotic suggestions

produces a corresponding change in beliefs about


the attitude object.

BEHAVIOR-ORIENTED APPROACH

The third approach to the modification of


attitudes, which is frequently employed in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1928
experimental social psychology (Brehm & Cohen,
1962; Festinger, 1957), relies upon a behavior-

oriented strategy. Change programs conducted


within a social-learning framework likewise favor

this type of approach, although they receive little

mention in discussions of attitude theory because

until recently the cognitive consequences that


undoubtedly accompany behavioral modifications

have rarely been systematically assessed. Before

specific experimental findings bearing on

behavioral approaches are discussed, the

conceptual scheme underlying most of this


research will be presented briefly.

Investigations of the process of attitude change


have, in large part, been guided by various models

of cognitive consistency. Among the more


prominent theoretical positions are those of

congruity (Osgood & Tannenbaum, 1955), balance


(Abelson & Rosenberg, 1958; Heider, 1958), and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1929
cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1957). Although

these formulations differ somewhat in the types of

events that are interrelated and the methods used


to disrupt internal equilibrium, they have in

common the view that a person’s cognitions about

himself and his environment are organized into an

internally consistent system. It is further assumed,

albeit implicitly, that there exists a strong drive for


self-consistency. Consequently, the introduction of
new information that contradicts existing

attitudes or beliefs creates an aversive

motivational state that instigates the individual to

eliminate or reduce it by making cognitive


adjustment designed to achieve a new mental

equilibrium. These consistency doctrines thus


assume that disruption of internal congruity

between cognitive elements constitutes a basic

determinant of attitude change.

In laboratory investigations the requisite

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1930
cognitive disequilibrium is usually created

through exposure to persuasive communications

which counter subjects’ initial attitudes. Research


stimulated by the cognitive dissonance model is

especially relevant to the issue under discussion

because, unlike the other consistency enterprises,

the method most often employed to induce

attitudinal changes involves getting a person to


engage in attitude-discrepant behavior under
conditions of minimal external inducement.

There are several reasons for selecting change

in behavior as a primary mode of attitude change.


First, it is much easier to arrange reinforcement
contingencies for altering specific overt actions

than for changing personal convictions, which


have a more private character and are often more

difficult to define. By skillful management of

incentives a person can be induced to take

progressively more favorable actions toward

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1931
attitude objects. Second, diverse opinions usually

exist about possible effects of engaging in certain

forms of behavior. Consequently, such beliefs are


more readily modifiable than cognitive

representations of the behavior itself, which,

because of its objective status, is more firmly fixed.

Thus, for example, it is easier to alter one’s

opinions about the effects of smoking than to deny


that one is, in fact, smoking, or to discontinue
smoking altogether. Third, in many cases behavior

is so powerfully maintained by its immediate

consequences that any induced cognitive

modification is likely to exert, at most, weak and


transitory influence upon corresponding actions. A

psychotherapist, for example, who contracted to


cure chronic alcoholism or debilitating compulsive

rituals by exposing his clients to discrepant

information about the physiological hazards of


excessive drinking or the irrationality of needless,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1932
arduous compulsions would, in a short time, suffer

insolvency. Obviously, in instances where

behavior is highly resistant to change,


modification of response consequences is essential

for effecting enduring alterations in performance


to which attitudes would eventually be expected
to adapt.

In the prototypic dissonance experiment,


subjects’ attitudes toward a particular issue or
object are assessed through self-ratings, after

which they are prompted, in one way or another,

to engage in behavior which contradicts their

private views. The same rating scales are later


readministered, and the change scores are taken
to represent the degree of attitude alteration.

These studies (Brehm & Cohen, 1962; Cohen,

1964; Festinger, 1957) demonstrate that induced

behavioral changes typically produce a

corresponding modification in subjects’ attitudes.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1933
After it was demonstrated that behavioral change

has attitudinal consequences, subsequent research

was primarily concerned with identifying the


variables governing the amount of attitudinal

change effected by performance of discrepant

behavior. The conditions selected for investigation

are based upon the general assumption that the

less compelling the reasons for engaging in the


contradictory behavior, the greater the dissonance
and hence, the more attitude change is required to

reduce it. Thus, persons who engage in attitude-

discrepant behavior because of large rewards or

strong coercive pressures have ample external


justification for their actions and presumably,

therefore, experience little dissonance and change


of attitude. On the other hand, it is assumed that

those who behave contrary to their private

opinions under conditions of minimal external


inducement are obliged to discover new

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1934
attractions in the disagreeable activity to justify to

themselves their voluntary performance of

inconsistent actions.

According to dissonance theory, inconsistent

action will produce the greatest amount of attitude


change under conditions where small incentives,

just sufficient to get the person to comply, are

employed; there are minimal threats or coercive


inducements; few reasons are given for taking the
discrepant stand; the person receives a high

degree of choice in committing himself to the

counterattitudinal performance; there is high

expenditure of effort in the attainment of the goal


object or in the enactment of the discrepant
behavior; the inducing agent is viewed

unfavorably; and the person being influenced

displays high self-esteem. It should be noted here

that in naturalistic situations it is ordinarily no

easy task to get people to perform personally

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1935
repugnant actions for any length of time under

such unfavorable incentive conditions.

Evaluation of the major theoretical issues and


voluminous empirical findings bearing on

dissonance-arousing variables goes beyond the

scope of this book. For the interested reader,

detailed reviews are available elsewhere (Abelson,

Aronson, McGuire, Newcomb, Rosenberg &


Tannenbaum, 1968; Chapanis & Chapanis, 1964;
Elms, 1967; Feldman, 1966). The empirical studies

have generally yielded conflicting results;

consequently, the precise conditions under which


induced discrepant performance will have
greatest effect on attitudes still remain somewhat

obscure. A major difficulty in verifying derivations


from dissonance theory and in drawing

conclusions from experimental data arises

because there exists no independent measure of

the degree to which the postulated state of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1936
dissonance has been aroused by a given

procedure. Since the induction operations

typically involve a complex set of events,


experimental outcomes are open to numerous

alternative explanations, which complicate

interpretation. For a graphic illustration of the

ambiguity concerning the independent variables

in forced compliance studies, the reader is


referred to the spirited debate between Aronson
(1966) and Rosenberg (1966) who interpret the

same experimental manipulation as having

created opposite amounts of cognitive dissonance!

One theoretical issue, because of its obvious


relevance to the role of incentives in change

processes, warrants discussion in this context. It is


widely believed that experimental findings

concerning the effects of incentives upon attitude

change brought about by divergent behavior

contradict derivations from “conventional

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1937
reinforcement theory.” In fact, because of

inadequate application of incentives, results of

those studies are of limited relevance to


reinforcement principles. Moreover, as will be

discussed later, contrary to common belief both

dissonance and reinforcement theory offer the

same nonobvious implications.

In these experiments subjects are induced to


write essays, enact prescribed roles, or otherwise
publicly espouse a set of opinions that contradict

their private feelings and beliefs. Some subjects

are offered small monetary incentives (15¢, 50¢)


for assuming the discrepant position, while others
are promised more generous rewards ($5, $20).

Several experiments (Brehm & Cohen, 1962;


Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959) report an inverse

relationship between size of monetary incentive

and attitude change; other investigations have

yielded both positive and inverse relationships

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1938
(Carlsmith, Collins, & Helmreich, 1966; Linder,

Cooper, & Jones, 1967), no incentive effects of

statistically significant magnitude (Elms & Janis,


1965; Janis & Gilmore, 1965; Nuttin, 1966), or

evidence that higher monetary incentives produce

greater degrees of attitude change (Collins, 1969;

Rosenberg, 1965).

The conditions governing the relationship


between incentives and attitude change cannot be
reliably identified unless data are presented for

two other critical relationships, namely, the

amount of counterattitudinal behavior engaged in


as a function of different magnitudes of reward,
and the degree to which variations in amount of

discrepant behavior are associated with extent of


attitude change. In experiments where the amount

and quality of counterattitudinal performance are

measured, outcomes are often uninterpretable

because the material rewards, which supposedly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1939
serve as external inducements for discrepant

behavior, are applied in such a loose contingency

that their incentive function is virtually


obliterated. Rewards are offered for any

performance subjects choose to display, but

otherwise the incentives are not explicitly made

contingent upon the number, persuasiveness, and

elaborated quality of arguments.

Even experiments conducted by proponents of


incentive principles have limited bearing on

incentive theory because rewards are offered

without specific performance requirements. If


incentives facilitate attitude change because they
motivate individuals to generate positive

arguments counter to their own beliefs (Janis,


1968), then subjects should be rewarded on the

basis of the number of favorable arguments that

they produce. An adequate test of the predictive

efficacy of incentive theory would also require

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1940
independent evidence that variations in reward

actually produce a differential number and variety

of arguments. When rewards are provided without


regard to response output there is no reason to

expect them to have any consistent behavioral or

attitudinal effects. This is borne out by the actual

findings. A number of limiting conditions under

which the consistency theories hold have been


proposed, including freedom of choice,
commitment, public or private performance,

anticipated consequences of influencing others in

the counterattitudinal direction, and self-

devaluative consequences. However, none of these


explanations adequately reconciles all the

divergent results.

The positive influence of incentives is also

frequently nullified in dissonance experiments by

introducing monetary rewards in the context of

inordinate social pressures upon subjects to

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1941
perform the disagreeable task. In the procedure

most often employed, a flustered experimenter

explains to an unwitting subject that an


unexpected emergency has arisen because the

regular assistant has just phoned saying that he

will be unable to conduct the study with the next

subject, who has already arrived and is waiting for

his scheduled session. Would the subject be


willing, for a small or a large fee, to substitute for
the absent assistant by informing the waiting

subject that a boring task is interesting and

enjoyable? It is hardly surprising that, given such

compelling reasons, the same amount of


discrepant behavior is enacted regardless of

incentive size (Carlsmith, Collins, & Helmreich,


1966; Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959), and subjects

are willing to perform the disagreeable task even

without any monetary rewards at all (Nuttin,


1966). Indeed, given this “sudden, unexpected,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1942
and pressing” crisis, and urgent appeals to the

subject to help the experimenter “out of a jam,”

none of the experimental conditions, regardless of


the appended fee, can be considered as providing

insufficient justification for compliance. By


contrast, when the inordinate social pressures are
absent and the monetary rewards serve as the

main justification for developing

counterattitudinal arguments, as in essay-writing


situations, increased incentives often produce

increasing amounts of attitude change (Carlsmith,

Collins, & Helmreich, 1966). Other investigators


(Elms, 1967; Janis & Gilmore, 1965; Rosenberg,

1966) have therefore attributed the effects of


differential payments to arousal of resentment,

suspicion, and other interfering emotional

responses rather than to their intended positive


incentive value.

Although there is abundant evidence that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1943
performance of counter- attitudinal behavior can

be a highly efficacious means of altering existing

attitudes, divergent findings regarding


contributory conditions suggest that more than

one mediating process is probably involved. Some

of these alternative mechanisms are discussed

below.

Individuals undergo considerable social


training to be logical and consistent in their
beliefs. To the extent that contradictory beliefs

engender critical reactions from others and other

negative consequences, inconsistency may become


an aversive condition that instigates emotional
arousal and cognitive modifications designed to

remove the source of discomfort. Hence,


dissonance processes may be involved to some

extent under conditions where people have

voluntarily committed themselves to perform

disagreeable behavior with weak external

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1944
inducement and are therefore compelled to

modify their beliefs to justify their contradictory

actions. Because of the many limiting conditions


under which dissonance effects are believed to

occur, the phenomenon could not be highly

prevalent. Dissonance reduction must, therefore,

be only one of several processes activated by

counterattitudinal performance.

Whenever a given action has been rewarded,


reinforcement effects tend to generalize across

similar classes of behavior, with the result that the

incidence of corresponding verbal responses is


likewise increased to some degree (Lovaas, 1961).
Cognitive equivalents of the reinforced overt

behavior are also affected in a similar manner


(Miller, 1951) even though they have never been

directly involved in the reinforcement

contingency. Thus, in situations where counter-

attitudinal behavior is contingently rewarded,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1945
analogous changes in the cognitive domain may

partly reflect a response generalization process.

A third interpretation of role-enactment


effects, advanced by Janis & King (1954), Janis &

Gilmore (1965), and Rosenberg (1965),

emphasizes the self-persuasive consequences of

recalling and developing numerous positive

arguments. According to this point of view,


favorable incentive conditions are likely to
produce a greater amount of improvisation and

more persuasively elaborated arguments

upholding the opposed point of view. In the course


of role-playing the person becomes influenced by
the merits of his own convincing arguments.

Although there is some evidence that degree of

attitude change is positively related to amount and

quality of counterattitudinal behavior, incentive

size alone has no consistent effects upon either

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1946
improvisation or attitude change (Janis & King,
1954; Janis & Gilmore, 1965; Kelman, 1953;

Rosenberg, 1965). However, Janis has shown that


large incentives furnished by a favorable source

produce better quality of performance and greater

modification in attitudes than do small monetary

rewards offered by an unfavorable sponsor for


taking a contradictory stand. Rosenberg (1966)

also offers the interesting proposition that self-

persuasive consequences of behavioral rehearsal

may depend upon the performer’s psychological

set. As in the case of influences from external


sources, a person who labels his

counterattitudinal advocacy as manipulative and


deceptive may be considerably more resistant to

his own persuasive arguments than if he


undertakes the task with a positive self-searching

orientation. This factor, if operative, might account


for some of the conflicting results. Bern (1967)

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1947
similarly argues that the self-persuasive effects of

observing one’s own behavior may be partly

determined by the stimulus conditions under


which it occurs.

To the extent that attitudinal changes are


partly governed by the amount of discrepant

behavior engaged in, the selection of incentive

magnitude as the critical variable for testing


predictions from dissonance and reinforcement
theory was an unfortunate choice, because

variations in amount of reward have no consistent

effects upon performance by human subjects

(Bruning, 1964; Elliott, 1966; Lewis & Duncan,


1961). This is analogous to manipulating a
variable that has no uniform effect upon the

amount of dissonance arousal. In order to furnish

a critical test of reinforcement theory, it is

necessary to vary an incentive property that has

reliable behavioral consequences, since the only

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1948
reason for employing rewards is to alter the

incidence of the crucial behavior. In view of

evidence that variable, intermittent reinforcement


results in higher performance than the same

rewards administered on a fixed schedule, a more

appropriate incentive variable, from the

standpoint of reinforcement theory, would be the

pattern in which counterattitudinal behavior is


rewarded. For most incentive characteristics, the
supposedly rival theories predict the same

outcome, though for different reasons. Consider,

for example, situations in which counterattitudinal

behavior is generously reinforced on a fixed-


interval schedule in one case, and much less

frequently on a variable-ratio schedule in a


contrasting treatment. The less favorable incentive

condition would be expected to produce more

attitudinal change because, according to


dissonance theory, it provides less justification

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1949
and hence greater dissonance, and according to

reinforcement theory, because it generates more

self-persuasive behavior.

It should also be noted in passing that, as far as

behavior change programs are concerned, there


are no reinforcement theories that prescribe the

use of excessive rewards. On the contrary, as

explained in Chapter 4, it is most advantageous for


several reasons to employ incentive conditions
just sufficient to elicit the desired behavior. First,

the aim is to produce enduring alterations in

behavior, and partially reinforced behavior is most

resistant to extinction. Second, in a well-designed


program artificial, external inducements, initially
required to elicit the desired responsiveness, are

gradually reduced as the behavior produces

natural and self-evaluative reinforcing

consequences. Since reductions in incentives

generate disruptive emotional effects, a change

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1950
agent would be inviting unnecessary trouble by

the use of needlessly large rewards.

The discussion thus far has focused on internal


readjustments prompted by inconsistent action

and alternative mediating processes that might

account for the phenomenon. A fourth mechanism

mediating role-enactment effects—an

experimental consequences process—highlights


the fact that a change in behavior provides a
person with a variety of new experiences with the

attitude object. Information gained from these

new social interactions and observations can, in


itself, produce substantial reorganization of
attitudes (Kelman, 1961). Thus, for example, a

prejudiced person who has been induced to


behave positively toward members of a minority

group may adopt a more favorable attitude not so

much because of stress created by intrapsychic

inconsistency, but because close positive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1951
associations with minority groups furnish

additional evaluative knowledge and rewarding

outcomes for the participants. Direct experimental


consequences of behavioral change, depending

upon their nature, may far outweigh the influence

of intraspychic tensions in initiating and

maintaining attitudinal changes.

Another important aspect of this process


concerns the effects of induced behavioral
modifications upon the social environment of a

monitoring membership group. If a person

behaves in a discrepant manner he may be


virtually forced into association with the outgroup
through ostracism. Under these circumstances

“consistency” may be enforced and maintained


through external, social mediation rather than

intrapsychic compromises. Experiential

consequences of behavioral change are likely to

play a major role in determining how long induced

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1952
attitudinal changes will endure.

MODIFICATION OF SELF-ATTITUDES

Of particular relevance to social approaches for

developing and modifying attitudes is the evidence

provided by Breer & Locke (1965) that task

experiences can exert strong influence upon


performers’ attitudes. In these studies individuals

are either differentially rewarded or experience


differential success for performing tasks in two

different ways. After the performance experiences,

subjects’ preferences for similar activities and


more abstract values only indirectly related to the
tasks themselves are measured. The overall

results, based upon numerous investigations of

attitudes toward individualism, equalitarianism,

theism, and achievement, show that significant

attitudinal changes can be induced by providing

individuals with successful task experiences. For

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1953
example, college students who worked better in
groups than alone became more collectivistic in

their attitudes, whereas subjects who experienced


greater success when performing tasks

independently adopted a more individualistic

orientation. These studies also provide some

evidence that attitudes induced by success tend to


generalize to related types of activities and to

abstract preferences.

Change agents are often concerned not only

with altering individual’s evaluations of different

forms of behavior but in modifying their self-

attitudes as well. Indeed, in some schools of

psychotherapy, such as the client-centered


approach (Rogers, 1959), self-concept changes are

routinely selected as one of the primary treatment


objectives. According to this point of view, self-

attitudes can be modified most effectively through


intrapsychic exploration under conditions where

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1954
the change agent displays empathy, noncontingent

positive regard, and genuineness. The individual’s

difficulties presumably stem from the fact that


experiences that are incompatible with his faulty

self-conception are consistently denied or

inadequately symbolized. Self-examination in a

positive, non- evaluative relationship will lead him

to attend to warded-off experiences and accept


them as part of himself; this, in turn, produces
increased feelings of self-worth, self-acceptance,

and greater freedom of action. This approach is

predicated on the basic assumption that the

person already has developed highly competent


repertoires of behavior, most of which are

inherently satisfying, but which are neither


accepted nor actualized because of the faulty self-

evaluative contingencies that he has adopted from

misguided socialization agents.

Undoubtedly many competent people do

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1955
experience a great deal of self-generated distress

and many self-imposed constraints as a result of

adherence to ill-advised or excessively high


standards of self-reinforcement. To the extent that

a change agent differentially reinforces realistic

standard-setting behavior and elicits emulation of

more lenient self- evaluative standards as

conveyed through his comments and actions, the


client’s habitual self-attitudes are likely to undergo
change. However, results of outcome studies

presented earlier indicate that this objective may

not be too readily achieved on the basis of the

types of conditions prescribed by the client-


centered approach.

In many cases, of course, unfavorable self-


attitudes stem from behavioral deficits and are

repeatedly reinforced through failure experiences

occasioned by the person’s inability to meet

realistic cultural expectations. It is obvious that for

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1956
such persons no amount of self-exploration will

yield esteem-producing vocational skills, academic

capabilities, interpersonal competencies, and


rewarding avocational proficiencies which would

support realistic positive self-evaluations. Here

the primary concern must be with self-

development rather than self-exploration.

Evidence that attitudes are significantly influenced


by rewarding performance feedback indicates that
enduring positive self-evaluations can be most

effectively achieved by arranging optimal

conditions for the individual to acquire the

requisite competencies. On the other hand, the


likelihood is exceedingly small that favorable self-

attitudes, however induced, could survive in the


face of discontinuing performance experiences.

“Internalization” and Persistence of


Behavioral Changes

It is generally assumed that when a change in

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1957
behavior is accompanied by a set of congruent

attitudes the behavior has become effectively

internalized. After this state of integration is


achieved, a person’s conduct is presumably guided

by inner values rather than by compliance with

external demands and outcomes. As a result of

reciprocal support of attitudes and behavior,

internalized response tendencies are presumed to


be more stable and enduring over time, even
under relatively unfavorable conditions of

external reinforcement, than compliant behavior

without personal conviction. This view, if true,

would appear on casual inspection to dispute the


principle that behavior is regulated by its

consequences. This apparent contradiction arises


because the latter proposition has often been

interpreted, by both its ardent proponents and its

critics, to mean that behavior is governed by


situational contingencies. In fact, as will be

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1958
explained later, even so-called internalized

behavior remains under reinforcement feedback

control, although it may be relatively independent


of externally occurring consequences.

Before discussing alternative mechanisms that


have been advanced to account for phenomena

subsumed under the term internalization, it is

appropriate to question what, if anything, is


internalized in the organism. It is perhaps
misleading to talk of behavior being internalized

since, after response patterns have been acquired,

it is doubtful that they can undergo any further

interiorization. The major issues, therefore, are


less concerned with the locus of behavior than with
the nature of its controlling conditions.

In evaluating theories of internalization and

self-regulating processes, it is important to

distinguish between the reinforcing and the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1959
discriminative functions of stimuli. Behavior is
controlled not only by its reinforcing

consequences, but also by environmental stimuli


which signify the types of outcomes that are likely

to follow certain courses of action. A great deal of

human behavior that appears to be internally

directed is, in fact, under the control of such


discriminative cues. Sometimes the controlling

stimulus events can be readily identified because

of their distinctive properties, as in the case of a

motorist who waits patiently at a red signal light

on a deserted street without an automobile,


pedestrian, or traffic officer in sight. While this

motorist is exhibiting remarkable control,


nevertheless his behavior is clearly externally

regulated. This example, incidentally, illustrates


some of the problems inherent in definitions of

internalization which ignore the behavior-


directing function of cues, and in which the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1960
primary criterion is the occurrence of behavioral

control in the absence of social surveillance.

In most instances the controlling external

stimuli are not as easily identified, and

consequently internal controlling agents tend to


be invoked as explanatory factors. The case of a

dog who refrains from lounging on an inviting sofa

though unobserved, which is sometimes cited as


an example of internalized control, might serve to
illustrate the latter point. Some time ago we

trained our spaniel through differential

reinforcement to keep clear of all lounge chairs

except one old recliner that soon became the dog’s


semi-permanent abode. Our conditionable spaniel
exhibited a finely developed “superego” until one

day when my wife rearranged the furniture. Upon

entering the living room I was greeted by the

tranquil scene of our socialized canine snoozing

contentedly on a new chair located in the area

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1961
previously occupied by the threadbare recliner. It

suddenly became apparent that our dog’s behavior

was regulated by irrelevant spatial cues rather


than by an internal governor.

It has been repeatedly demonstrated in

research with infrahuman organisms and with

human subjects that cues regularly correlated

with reinforcement eventually gain control over


the associated behavior. Hence, in situations
where reliable discriminative stimuli are present

it is reasonable to question what has been

internalized, and why it is necessary to invoke an


internal agency which supposedly regulates the
observed behavior. The fact that other types of

subtle stimuli such as temporal cues (Ferster &


Skinner, 1957; Sidman, 1966) can exert

considerable discriminative control over the form,

rate, and pattern of responding makes it

exceedingly difficult to rule out external stimulus

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1962
determinants.

Although there are extensive stimulus

supports for behavior, one must nevertheless


explain differences in the responses of individuals

to what appear to be essentially the same cues. To

return to the example of our lonesome motorist, a

systematic field study would undoubtedly disclose

that under deserted traffic conditions some


motorists would wait dutifully for the green light,
others might stop momentarily and then continue

on their way, while still others are likely to

disregard the traffic signal completely. A


noninternalist would most likely argue that the
signal light, by itself, does not adequately define

the total controlling environment. Whether or not


a particular individual will transgress depends

upon a large number of other stimulus variables

(e.g., restraining influence of other passengers,

time pressures on the motorist, his subjective

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1963
estimates of the probability of being caught and

the gravity of the consequences that might follow,

etc.) each of which may exercise some degree of


control over his behavior. It is conceivable that in

many cases consideration of all the relevant

stimulus events operative in a given situation

would disclose that behavior appearing to be

internally governed is, in large part, under the


control of complex patterns of multiple stimuli,
which are rarely identical either over time or

across individuals.

Systematic social analyses would undoubtedly


also reveal that persons often persist in behavior
that receives little or no social support (Bateson,

1961), they forego rewarding activities and


objects which are readily available and socially

permitted, they impose upon themselves highly

unfavorable performance demands (Bandura &

Perloff, 1967), and their actions may be highly

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1964
refractory even to severe external consequences

(Farber, Harlow, & West, 1957). These and other

data indicate that regulatory mechanisms can be


established that render behavior partially

independent of specific situational contingencies

and outcomes.

One can differentiate several different types of

“intrinsic” reinforcement control. As noted in


Chapter 4, response patterns can be effectively
maintained without social or natural support by

their intrinsic sensory consequences. Artificial

incentives and a great deal of social surveillance


may initially be required, for example, to induce
children to acquire the necessary skills to play the

piano; but after proficiency is achieved keyboard


performances are likely to be engaged in for their

melodic feedback. Other activities may similarly

be self-reinforced through their intrinsic sensory

feedback. It should be noted, however, that the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1965
reinforcing values of most visual or auditory

stimuli generated by behavioral sequences must

themselves be developed through a process of


differential reinforcement. There is nothing

inherently rewarding about a skillfully executed

Wagnerian aria, an abstract painting, or a tuba

solo.

Response patterns may also be partly


maintained by anticipatory consequences. Studies
reported earlier show that behavior can be

sustained by imagined rewards or punishments.

This process is also vividly illustrated in the case,


cited in Chapter 1, of the patient who tenaciously
performed arduous bizarre rituals designed to

forestall dreadful, hellish torture, even though his


atonement rituals were consistently and severely

punished by ward personnel. In this instance

imagined aversive consequences had such

overpowering effects on the patient’s behavior

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1966
that it became relatively autonomous of external

reinforcements.

The third mechanism by which behavior can


become largely independent of situational

contingencies and outcomes involves a process in

which response patterns are largely controlled by

their self-evaluative consequences. As discussed in

detail elsewhere, people adopt certain standards


of behavior and generate self-rewarding or self-
punishing consequences depending upon how

their behavior compares to their self-prescribed

demands. It is this self-imposition of contingencies


that probably serves as the basis for the notion
that values govern conduct. Under conditions

where self-evaluative and externally occurring


consequences conflict, as when a given pattern of

behavior is socially rewarded but personally

devalued, the inhibiting effects of anticipatory self-

criticism may prevail over external rewards.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1967
Conversely, positive self-reinforcement may

maintain, in some strength, behavior that is

nonrewarded or negatively sanctioned by societal


agents whose behavioral standards are

repudiated.

Although in both of the “internalization”

mechanisms discussed above behavior is

internally regulated by self-generated


consequences, the types of outcomes produced
differ in at least one important respect. In the first

case, behavior is controlled by anticipatory

representation of response consequences


administered by external agents. Consequently, in
situations which involve little risk that

transgressive behavior will be detected, or when


anticipated aversive outcomes are mild, people

may readily transgress. In the second case, a

person is deterred from behaving counter to his

standards of conduct by anticipatory self-

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1968
punishing responses. Since the person’s own self-

demands and self-respect serve as his main guides

and deterrents, behavior that is under the latter


form of self-control is apt to be less affected by

variations in specific situational contingencies. It

might be noted in passing that the distinction

drawn between types of self-generated

consequences is similar to the common


differentiation of fear-controlled and conscience-
controlled behavior.

The preceding remarks are not intended to

imply, of course, that self-reinforcement standards


do not require some degree of social support.
Persons tend to affiliate with others who share

similar behavioral norms and mutually reinforce


adherence to the standards they have adopted.

Those who choose a small, select reference group

that does not share the values of the vast majority

may appear “inner-directed,” whereas in actuality

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1969
they are very much dependent on the real or

imagined approval and disapproval of a few

individuals whose judgments are highly valued.

Stabilization of Behavioral Changes through


Development of Self-Regulatory Functions

By far the most important but most neglected

aspect of behavioral change processes is the


appropriate generalization of established patterns

of behavior to new situations and their persistence


after the original controlling conditions have been

discontinued. The generalization and persistence

of behavior can be facilitated by three different

means. These include transfer training, alteration


of the reinforcement practices of the social
environment, and the establishment of self-

regulatory functions. In cases where newly


established or disinhibited response patterns

either relieve subjective distress or are favorably

received within the naturalistic environment, the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1970
altered behavior will be adequately sustained

without special maintenance procedures.

Occasional negative experiences in the context of


many neutral or rewarding experiences with

formerly threatening events are usually ineffective

in reinstating fear responding. On the other hand,

where behavior that is rewarding and positively

self- evaluative must be counteracted, and where


customary environmental contingencies provide
only weak support for alternative modes of

behavior, the development of self-regulatory

functions is essential if induced behavioral

changes are to transfer and to endure to any


significant degree. This issue is best exemplified in

the treatment of antisocial personalities.

The most fundamental changes would clearly

be accomplished by altering the contingency

structure and reinforcement practices prevailing

in the deviant subculture. This would require

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1971
modification of the behavior of individuals who

exert a strong controlling influence over their

fellow members in the social system. However,


owing to the individualistic bias of treatment

approaches and the inordinate difficulties

encountered in efforts to gain adequate control

over antisocial groups, the common procedure is

to remove a transgressor from his usual


environment and to subject him to some type of
social influence.

Severe antisocial behavior can be controlled in

residential centers through differential


reinforcement. Moreover, the resultant
conforming behavior is likely to persist as long as

the institutional sanctions remain in effect. The


residents may, in fact, come to behave

irreproachably and even to perform obligingly

whatever behavior is expected of them in order to

make conditions in the institution as pleasant as

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1972
possible and to expedite their release. A beneficent

incentive system in a treatment center may thus

extract considerable prosocial behavior from


delinquents, but such persons often revert to their

usual antisocial patterns whenever supervisory

staff members are no longer present. The

attraction of the deviant subculture can be

reduced by having members acquire alternative


rewarding patterns of behavior and adopt new
standards for self- evaluation.

Findings of studies reviewed in preceding

sections and earlier chapters suggest several


procedures that might be successful in developing
self-regulatory functions. First, the desired

patterns of behavior and standards for self-


reinforcement should be adequately exemplified

by change agents. Second, an explicit set of

performance requirements linked with a graded

system of incentives should be instituted, such

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1973
that progressive adoption of more advanced

behavior brings increased freedom, privileges, and

access to rewarding activities. However, provision


of exemplary models and positive inducements for

behavioral change may not alone be sufficient for

transmitting self-monitoring reinforcement

systems to avowedly antisocial persons.

After participants adopt new patterns of


behavior on the basis of their utilitarian value, the
next phase in the program may require direct

training in self-reinforcement. This is achieved by

gradually transferring evaluative and reinforcing


functions from change agents to the individual
himself. Rewards are now made contingent not

only upon occurrence of desired behavior, but also


accurate evaluation of one’s own performances.

Although at this stage the person judges when his

behavior warrants reward according to the

prevailing contingency structures, others still

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1974
serve as the reinforcing agents. After accurate self-

evaluative behavior is well established, the

reinforcing function is likewise transferred so that


the individual both evaluates his own behavior

and reinforces himself accordingly. In addition, the

artificial material rewards are gradually reduced

as the person’s behavior is brought increasingly

under the control of self-administered and


symbolic consequences. The ultimate aim of the
training in self-reinforcement is to produce a level

of functioning at which participants can control

their own behavior with minimum external

constraints and artificial inducements.

Another means of instilling self-regulatory

functions is to provide ample opportunities for


participants to enact role behaviors toward peers

that are ordinarily performed by regular change

agents. Specifically, this entails delegating

progressively more of the standard-setting,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1975
evaluative, and reinforcement functions to

members of the group as they progress in the

program. The members themselves, with staff


guidance, thus become the contingency managers.

In order to enhance participants’ willingness to

adopt new role behaviors, increased privileges

and rewards are associated with increased

responsibility for guiding member behavior.


Active participation in decision-making,
application of rewards and sanctions for

regulating the behavior of peers in accord with

institutional standards, and performance of other

counterattitudinal behaviors would be expected to


exert greater influences on values and preferences

than a program in which contingencies are simply


imposed on covertly resistant members. It might

also be supposed that those who willingly

implement reinforcement contingencies


advocated by a social agency for modifying the

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1976
behavior of their associates will similarly alter

their own standards of self-reinforcement in the

practiced direction.

When individuals function as change agents for

members of their own group they not only achieve


modifications that might otherwise be strongly

resisted, but they also advance the treatment of

their subordinates by providing models for


desirable modes of behavior. One might expect
peers to be emulated to a much greater extent

than staff because peers are less socially distant,

have more opportunity to exhibit desired

response patterns, and are likely to evoke weaker


resistive tendencies. Additionally, individuals are
less apt to be ostracized for adopting the behavior

of their peers.

Just as self-regulatory functions are socially

transferable and conditionable, they are also

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1977
extinguishable unless given a sufficient amount of
social support. Various social groups, which differ

considerably in their behavioral standards and


reinforcement practices, are potentially available

to individuals. The groups with which they choose

to affiliate largely determine the role models and

contingency system to which they will be exposed


and, consequently, the direction in which their

behavior will be further modified. Therefore,

attention to factors governing the selection of

reference groups is of critical importance,

particularly in cases where induced behavioral


changes are discordant with the conduct

advocated and reinforced by the individuals’


former associates. Generalization and

maintenance of personality changes can, therefore,


be best ensured if the program instills in

participants behavioral competencies and self-


reinforcement standards that are likely to exert

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1978
decisive influence on association preferences.

After a person has adopted new standards for


self-evaluation, a group’s pressures for conformity

to conflicting behavioral requirements are likely to

be resisted. Instead, when the advocated conduct


is not in accord with self-prescribed standards, the

individual may attempt to alter the demands, he

may remain a marginal member, or, if the rewards


for membership are insufficient, he may
discontinue his association with the group.

If persons are to affiliate with new social


groups, they must acquire at least some of the
requisite behaviors for securing approval and

recognition that will be necessary to sustain their

active involvement. Otherwise, they will be unable

to meet successfully the demands of their new

social environment and will either eventually

withdraw from or be rejected by the group. Many

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1979
rehabilitative programs, for example, concentrate
on producing radical changes in offenders’

behavior that will lose them the social and


material rewards associated with a deviant career,

but relatively little attention is devoted to

providing them the means for obtaining adequate

substitute gratifications.

The way in which affiliation processes govern

the course of behavior change is revealed in


studies (Ellis & Lane, 1963; Krauss, 1964)

investigating the sources of high educational

aspirations among lower-class children. In the

families of such children the parents cannot

themselves provide satisfactory models of class-


typed habits of speech, customs, and social skills

required to win acceptance from upper middle-


class peers. The parents characteristically initiate

the upward mobility process by attaching positive


value to educational achievements; admired

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1980
teachers further reinforce, by approval and

example, college ambitions in lower-class youth;

and selective association with college-oriented


peers provides the social-learning conditions for

the gradual acquisition of attitudes, belief systems,

and complex behavioral repertoires necessary for

achieving the desired socioeducational status.

Bandura & Walters (1959) have similarly shown


that adolescents tend to choose close associates
who share similar value systems and behavioral

norms; peer group members, in turn, serve to

reinforce and to uphold the standards of behavior

that the boys adopted.

Summary

Several theories have been proposed

concerning the role of symbolic processes in the

regulation of behavior. These range from

nonmediational views that assume that

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1981
reinforcing consequences modify behavior directly

and automatically, to cognitive formulations that

consider symbolic representation of contingencies


a prerequisite for learning and performance

change. A reciprocal-interaction theory seems best

able to order the divergent findings bearing on

this issue. According to this view, reinforcing

consequences can alter behavior independently of


awareness, but individuals eventually infer, from
observation of their behavior and its differential

outcomes, the correct reinforcement rules which

partly control subsequent responding.

In studies of both instrumental and classical


conditioning persons who discern the

contingencies governing the administration of


rewards and punishments typically display

significant increments in learning or performance,

whereas unaware subjects generally show few or

no conditioning effects. Interpretation of these

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1982
findings, however, remains unclear because the

studies lack the data necessary for determining

whether conditioning failures in the absence of


awareness are attributable to inadequate central

registration of sensory inputs or to lack of

recognition of contiguous stimulus events that

have registered and evoked neural responses.

Experiments designed so that subjects cannot


observe either their own responses or the
occurrence of reinforcing events—thus precluding

recognition of contingencies—reveal that

conditioning can occur, albeit much less reliably,

on a nonmediated basis. The overall findings seem


to indicate that awareness is a powerful

facilitative factor, but it may not be a necessary


and certainly is not a sufficient condition for

behavioral change. Awareness in itself is unlikely

to produce response changes unless persons


possess the necessary performance skills and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1983
unless adequate incentives are provided to elicit

and to sustain appropriate responsiveness.

Symbolic activities not only augment the

efficacy of reinforcement operations, but they are

also increasingly employed to generate emotional


effects that constitute the major reinforcing

consequences in behavioral modification

programs. In symbolic desensitization both the


aversive stimuli and the emotion-neutralizing
responses are in large part symbolically induced.

Similarly, in aversive cognitive

counterconditioning, avoidance responses toward

addictive objects are established by contiguous


association of symbolic representations of
positively valenced stimuli with thought-produced

nauseous reactions. Imagined consequences may

also be employed instrumentally as covert

reinforcers either to strengthen or to reduce the

incidence of overt behavior. Perturbing trains of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1984
thought often disrupt psychological functioning, in

which case the problem becomes one of

controlling symbolic events themselves. Self-


control of thought processes can be achieved by

redirecting attention to absorbing activities that

elicit competing cognitions and by self-

reinforcement of more constructive lines of

thought.

The issue of mediational control of behavior is


also frequently raised in the context of attitude

theory. Although it is commonly assumed that

attitudinal changes have widespread and


stabilizing influences upon overt actions, induced
alterations in attitudes in fact generally have few

enduring effects upon behavior unless they


receive sufficient reinforcement support. On the

other hand, direct modification of the affective

properties of attitude objects and performance of

attitude-discrepant behavior produce stable

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1985
corresponding changes in attitudes. This process

of cognitive accommodation to affective and

behavioral changes has been variously attributed


to striving for cognitive consistency, to response

generalization of reinforcement effects, to the self-

persuasive influence of counterattitudinal

behaviors, and to new experiential consequences

resulting from the induced behavioral changes. It


still remains to be established whether
environmental influences have similar but

independent effects on feelings, beliefs, and

behavior, or whether a change in one of these

components engenders congruous modifications


in the other constituents.

When regulatory symbolic processes are


combined with self-generated consequences,

behavior may become “internalized” or partially

independent of situational contingencies and

outcomes. Several different types of “intrinsic”

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1986
reinforcement control can be distinguished.

Behavior may be sustained by its inherent sensory

feedback, by anticipatory outcomes, or by self-


evaluative consequences.

The establishment of self-monitoring

reinforcement systems is essential if induced

behavioral changes are to generalize and endure,

particularly where social environments provide


either weak support for new modes of behavior or
conflicting patterns of reinforcement. Stabilization

of changes is most likely to be ensured when the

standards adopted for self-reinforcement result in


selective association with persons who share
similar behavioral norms, thus providing social

support for one’s own system of self-evaluation.

REFERENCES

Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J., Newcomb,


T. M., Rosenberg, M. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H.
Theories of cognitive consistency: A sourcebook.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1987
Chicago: Rand McNally, 1968.

Abelson, R. P., & Rosenberg, M. J. Symbolic psycho-


logic: A model of attitudinal cognition. Behavioral
Science, 1958, 3, 1-13.

Adams, J. K. Laboratory studies of behavior without


awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 1957, 54, 393-
405.

Aronson, E. The psychology of insufficient


justification: An analysis of some conflicting data.
In S. Feldman (Ed.), Cognitive consistency. New
York: Academic Press, 1966. pp. 109–133.

Aronson, E., Turner, J. A., & Carlsmith, J. M.


Communicator credibility and communication
discrepancy as determinants of opinion change.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963,
67, 31–36.

Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. Reinforcement and


instructions with mental patients. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 327-
331.

Bandura, A. Social-learning theory of identificatory


processes. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of
socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand
McNally, 1968.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1988
Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. The relative
efficacy of desensitization and modeling
approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and
attitudinal changes, Stanford University, 1969,
13, 173-199.

Bandura, A., & Perloff, B. Relative efficacy of self-


monitored and externally imposed reinforcement
systems. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1967, 7, 111-116.

Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. Vicarious


reinforcement and imitative learning. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 601-
607.

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression.


New York: Ronald Press, 1959.

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Social learning and


personality development. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston, 1963.

Barber, T. X., & Hahn, K. W., Jr. Experimental studies in


“hypnotic” behavior: Physiological and subjective
effects of imagined pain. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 1964, 139, 416-425.

Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. Human, free-operant


avoidance of time out from monetary

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1989
reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, 557-565.

Bateson, G. (Ed.) Perceval’s narrative: A patient’s


account of his psychosis, 1830-1832. Stanford
University Press, 1961.

Bem, D. J. Self-perception: An alternative


interpretation of cognitive dissonance
phenomena. Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 183-
200.

Bergin, A. E. The effect of dissonant persuasive


communications upon changes in a self-referring
attitude. Journal of Personality, 1962, 30, 423-
438.

Blanchard, E. B. The relative contributions of


modeling, informational influences, and physical
contact in the extinction of phobic behavior.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford
University, 1969.

Bourne, L. E., Jr. Human conceptual behavior. Boston:


Allyn & Bacon, 1966.

Breer, P. E., & Locke, E. A. Task experience as a source


of attitudes. Homewood, Ill.: Dorsey, 1965.

Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. Exploration in cognitive


dissonance. New York: Wiley, 1962.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1990
Bridger, W. H., & Mandel, I. J. A comparison of GSR
fear responses produced by threat and electric
shock. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 1964, 2,
31-40.

Bridger, W. H., & Mandel, I. J. Abolition of the PRE by


instructions in GSR conditioning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1965, 69, 476-482.

Brown, R. W. Social psychology. New York: Free Press,


1965.

Bruning, J. L. Effects of magnitude of reward and


percentage of reinforcement on a lever
movement response. Child Development, 1964,
35, 281-285.

Carlin, M. T. The effects of modeled behavior during


imposed delay on the observer’s subsequent
willingness to delay rewards. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1965.

Carlsmith, J. M., Collins, B. E., & Helmreich, R. L.


Studies in forced compliance: I. The effect of
pressure for compliance on attitude change
produced by face-to-face role playing and
anonymous essay writing. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 1-13.

Cautela, J. R. Treatment of compulsive behavior by

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1991
covert sensitization. Psychological Record, 1966,
16, 33–41.

Chapanis, N. P., & Chapanis, A. Cognitive dissonance:


Five years later. Psychological Bulletin, 1964, 61,
1-22.

Chatterjee, B. B., & Eriksen, C. W. Cognitive factors in


heart rate conditioning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1962, 64, 272-279.

Clark, D. F. The treatment of monosymptomatic


phobia by systematic desensitization. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1963, 1, 63-68.

Cohen, A. R. Attitude change and social influence. New


York: Basic Books, 1964.

Collins, B. E. The effect of monetary inducements on


amount of attitude change produced by forced
compliance. In A. C. Elms (Ed.), Role playing,
reward, and attitude change. Princeton, N. J.: Van
Nostrand, 1969, 209-223.

Cook, S. W., & Harris, R. E. The verbal conditioning of


the galvanic skin reflex. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1937, 21, 202-210.

Culbertson, F. M. Modification of an emotionally held


attitude through role playing. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1957, 54, 230-233.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1992
Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Janis, I. L. Why does eating while
reading facilitate opinion change? An
experimental inquiry. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 1965,1, 133-144.

Das, J. P., & Nanda, P. C. Mediated transfer of attitudes.


Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963,
66, 12-16.

Dawson, M. E. Comparison of classical conditioning


and relational learning. Unpublished master’s
thesis, University of Southern California, 1966.

Dawson, M. E., & Grings, W. W. Comparison of classical


conditioning and relational learning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1969 (in press).

De Nike, L. D. The temporal relationship between


awareness and performance in verbal
conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1964, 68, 521-529.

Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. Personality and


psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950.

Doob, L. W. The behavior of attitudes. Psychological


Review, 1947, 54, 135–156.

Dulany, D. E. The place of hypotheses and intentions:


An analysis of verbal control in verbal
conditioning. In C. W. Eriksen (Ed.), Behavior and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1993
awareness—a symposium of research and
interpretation. Durham, N. C.: Duke University
Press, 1962. pp. 102-129.

Dulany, D. E. Awareness, rules, and prepositional


control: A confrontation with S-R behavior
theory. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal
behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood
Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1968. pp. 340-387.

Dulany, D. E., Jr., & Eriksen, C. W. Accuracy of


brightness discrimination as measured by
concurrent verbal responses and GSRs. Journal of
Abnormal arid Social Psychology, 1959, 59, 418–
423.

Dulany, D. E., Jr. & O’Connell, D. C. Does partial


reinforcement dissociate verbal rules and the
behavior they might be presumed to control?
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
1963, 2, 361-372.

Duncker, K. Experimental modification of children’s


food preferences through social suggestion.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1938,
33, 489-507.

Ekman, P., Krasner, L., & Ullmann, L. P. Interaction of


set and awareness as determinants of response
to verbal conditioning. Journal of Abnormal and

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1994
Social Psychology, 1963, 66, 387-389.

Elliott, R. Reaction time and heart rate as functions of


magnitude of incentive and probability of
success: A replication and extension. Journal of
Experimental Research in Personality, 1966, 1,
174-178.

Ellis, R. A., & Lane, W. C. Structural supports for


upward mobility. American Sociological Review,
1963, 28, 743-756.

Elms, A. C. Role playing, incentive, and dissonance.


Psychological Bulletin, 1967, 68, 132-148.

Elms, A. C., & Janis, I. L. Counter norm attitudes


induced by consonant versus dissonant
conditions of role-playing. Journal of
Experimental Research in Personality, 1965, 1, 50-
60.

Eriksen, C. W. Prediction from and interaction among


multiple concurrent discriminative responses.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1957, 53,
353-359.

Eriksen, C. W. Unconscious processes. In M. R. Jones


(Ed.) Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln:
University of Nebraska Press, 1958. pp. 169-227.

Eriksen, C. W. Discrimination and learning without

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1995
awareness: A methodological survey and
evaluation. Psychological Review, 1960, 67, 279-
300.

Eriksen, C. W., & Kuethe, J. L. Avoidance conditioning


of verbal behavior without awareness: A
paradigm of repression. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1956, 53, 203-209.

Farber, I. E. The things people say to themselves.


American Psychologist, 18, 185-197.

Farber, I. E., Harlow, H. F., & West, L. J. Brainwashing,


conditioning, and DDD (debility, dependency, and
dread). Sociometry, 1957, 20, 271-285.

Feldman, S. Cognitive consistency. New York:


Academic Press, 1966.

Ferster, C. B., Nurnberger, J. I., & Levitt, E. B. The


control of eating. Journal of Mathetics, 1962,1, 87-
109.

Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. Schedules of


reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1957.

Festinger, L. A theory of cognitive dissonance.


Evanston, Ill.: Row, Peterson, 1957.

Festinger, L. Behavioral support for opinion change.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1996
Public Opinion Quarterly, 28, 404-417.

Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. Cognitive


consequences of forced compliance. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, 58, 203-
210.

Fleishmann, E., Harris, E., & Burtt, H. Leadership and


supervision in industry: An evaluation of a
supervisory training program. Columbus: Ohio
State University, Bureau of Educational Research,
1955.

Freud, S. The interpretation of dreams. London:


Hogarth, 1953.

Fuhrer, M. J., & Baer, P. E. Differential classical


conditioning: Verbalization of stimulus
contingencies. Science, 1965, 150, 1479-1481.

Goldiamond, I. Indicators of perception: I. Subliminal


perception, subception, unconscious perception:
An analysis in terms of psychological indicator
methodology. Psychological Bulletin, 1958, 55,
373-412.

Goodnow, J. J., & Postman, L. Probability learning in a


problem-solving situation. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1955, 49, 16-22.

Greenwald, A. G. Behavior change following a

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1997
persuasive communication. Journal of Personality,
1965, 33,370-391. (a)

Greenwald, A. G. Effects of prior commitment on


behavior change after a persuasive
communication. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1965,
29, 595-601. (b)

Grings, W. W. Verbal-perceptual factors in the


conditioning of autonomic responses. In W. F.
Prokasy (Ed.), Classical conditioning: A
symposium. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,
1965. pp. 71-89.

Grings, W. W., & Lockhart, R. A. Effects of “anxiety-


lessening” instructions and differential set
development on the extinction of GSR. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 292-299.

Hefferline, R. F., & Keenan, B. Amplitude-induction


gradient of a small-scale (covert) operant. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6,
307-315.

Hefferline, R. F., Keenan, B., & Harford, R. A. Escape


and avoidance conditioning in human subjects
without their observation of the response.
Science, 1959,130, 1338-1339.

Heider, F. The psychology of interpersonal relations.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1998
New York: Wiley, 1958.

Hernández-Péon, R., Scherrer, H., & Jouvet, M.


Modification of electric activity in cochlear
nucleus during “attention” in unanesthetized cats.
Science, 1956,123, 331-332.

Hirsch, J. Learning without awareness and extinction


following awareness as a function of
reinforcement. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1957, 54, 218-224.

Hislop, M. W., & Brooks, L. R. Suppression of concept


learning by verbal rules. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, in press.

Holmes, D. S. Verbal conditioning, or problem-solving


and cooperation? Journal of Experimental
Research in Personality, 1967, 2, 289-294.

Homme, L. E. Perspectives in psychology: XXIV.


Control of coverants, the operants of the mind.
Psychological Record, 1965, 15, 501-511.

Horn, G. Electrical activity of the cerebral cortex of the


unanesthetized cat during attentive behavior.
Brain, 1960, 83, 57-76.

Hovland, C. I., & Janis, I. L. (Eds.), Personality and


persuasibility. New Haven: Yale University Press,
1959.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 1999
Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H.
Communication and persuasion: Psychological
studies of opinion change. New Haven: Yale
University Press, 1953.

Hovland, C. I., Lumsdaine, A. A., & Sheffield, F. D.


Experiments on mass communication. Princeton,
N. J.: Princeton University Press, 1949.

Hovland, C. I., & Weiss, W. W. The influence of source


credibility on communication effectiveness.
Public Opinion Quarterly, 1951, 15, 635-650.

Insko, C. A., & Oakes, W. F. Awareness and the


“conditioning” of attitudes. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 487-496.

Janis, I. L. Attitude change via role-playing. In R. P.


Abelson, E. Aronson, W. J. McGuire, T. M.
Newcomb, & P. H. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Theories of
cognitive consistency: A sourcebook. Chicago:
Rand McNally, 1968.

Janis, I. L. Effects of fear arousal on attitude change:


Recent developments in theory and experimental
research. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology. Vol. III. New York:
Academic Press, 1967. pp. 167-224.

Janis, I. L., & Gilmore, J. B. The influence of incentive

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2000
conditions on the success of role playing in
modifying attitudes. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1965,1, 17-27.

Janis, I. L., Kaye, D., & Kirschner, P. Facilitating effects


of “eating-while- reading” on responsiveness to
persuasive communications. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 1, 181–
186.

Janis, I. L., & King, B. T. The influence of role playing


on opinion change. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1954, 49, 211-218.

Johnson, H., & Eriksen, C. W. Preconscious perception:


A re-examination of the Poetzl phenomenon.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961,
62, 497-503.

Kanfer, F. H. Verbal conditioning: A review of its


current status. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton
(Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior
theory. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall,
1968. pp. 254-290.

Kaufman, A., Baron, A., & Kopp, R. E. Some effects of


instructions on human operant behavior.
Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1966, 1,
243-250.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2001
Keller, L., Cole, M., Burke, C. J., & Estes, W. K. Reward
and informational values of trial outcomes in
paired-associate learning. Psychological
Monographs, 1965, 79 (12, Whole No. 605).

Kelman, H. C. Attitude change as a function of


response restriction. Human Relations, 1953, 6,
185-214.

Kelman, H. C. The induction of action and attitude


change. Proceedings of the XIV International
Congress of Applied Psychology. Copenhagen:
Munksgaard, 1961.

Kelman, H. C., & Hovland, C. I. Reinstatement of the


communicator in delayed measurement of
opinion change. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1953, 48, 327-335.

Klein, G. S., Spence, D. P., Holt, R. R., & Gourevitch, S.


Cognition without awareness: Subliminal
influences upon conscious thought. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1958, 57, 255-
266.

Krasner, L. Studies of the conditioning of verbal


behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 1958, 55, 148-
170.

Krasner, L., & Ullmann, L. P. Variables affecting report

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2002
of awareness in verbal conditioning. Journal of
Psychology, 1963, 56, 193-202.

Krasner, L., Ullmann, L. P., Weiss, R. L., & Collins, B. J.


Responsivity to verbal conditioning as a function
of three different examiners. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 1961, 17,411-415.

Krauss, I. Sources of educational aspirations among


working-class youth. American Sociological
Review, 1964, 29, 867-879.

Lazarus, R. S., & McCleary, R. A. Autonomic


discrimination without awareness: A study of
subception. Psychological Review, 1951, 58, 113-
122.

Levin, S. M. The effects of awareness on verbal


conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1961, 61, 67-75.

Levitt, T. Industrial purchasing behavior: A study of


communications effects. Boston: Harvard
University Press, 1965.

Levy, L. H. Awareness, learning, and the beneficent


subject as expert witness. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1967, 6, 365-370.

Lewis, D. J., & Duncan, C. P. Effects of variable


magnitude of reward on a lever-pulling response.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2003
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1961, 62,
203-205.

Linder, D. E., Cooper, J., & Jones, E. E. Decision freedom


as a determinant of the role of incentive
magnitude in attitude change. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 6, 245-
254.

Lovaas, O. I. Interaction between verbal and


nonverbal behavior. Child Development, 1961, 32,
329-336.

Maccoby, N., Romney, A. K., Adams, J. S., & Maccoby, E.


E. “Critical periods” in seeking and accepting
information. Stanford: Stanford University
Institute for Communication Research, 1962.

Mackay, H. A., & Laverty, S. G. GSR changes during


therapy of phobic behavior. Unpublished
manuscript, Queen’s University, Ontario, 1963.

Mandel, I. J. & Bridger, W. H. Interaction between


instructions and ISI in conditioning and
extinction of the GSR. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1967, 74, 36-43.

Marks, I. M., & Gelder, M. G. Transvestism and


fetishism: Clinical and psychological changes
during faradic aversion. British Journal of

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2004
Psychiatry, 1967,113, 711-729.

McGuire, W. J. Inducing resistance to persuasion. In L.


Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology. Vol. I. New York: Academic Press,
1964. pp. 191-229.

Miller, M. M. Treatment of chronic alcoholism by


hypnotic aversion. American Medical Association
Journal, 1959,171, 1492-1495.

Miller, M. M. Hypnotic-aversion treatment of


homosexuality. Journal of the National Medical
Association, 1963, 55, 411-415.

Miller, N. E. Learnable drives and rewards. In S. S.


Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental
psychology, New York: Wiley, 1951. pp. 435-472.

Mischel, W. Personality and assessment. New York:


Wiley, 1968.

Newcomb, T. M., Turner, R. H., & Converse, P. E. Social


psychology: The study of human interaction. New
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965.

Nuttin, J. M., Jr. Attitude change after rewarded


dissonant and consonant “forced compliance.”
International Journal of Psychology, 1966, 1, 39-
57.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2005
O’Connell, D. C., & Wagner, M. V. Extinction after
partial reinforcement and minimal learning as a
test of both verbal control and PRE in concept
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1967, 73, 151-153.

Osgood, C. E., & Tannenbaum, P. H. The principle of


congruity in the prediction of attitude change.
Psychological Review, 1955, 62, 42-55.

Philbrick, D. B., & Postman, L. A further analysis of


“learning without awareness.” American Journal
of Psychology, 1955, 68, 417-424.

Postman, L., & Sassenrath, J. The automatic action of


verbal rewards and punishments. Journal of
General Psychology, 1961, 65, 109-136.

Premack, D. Reinforcement theory. In D. Levine (Ed.),


Nebraska symposium on motivation: 1965.
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1965. pp.
123-180.

Razran, G. PI. S. Conditioning away social bias by the


luncheon technique. Psychological Bulletin, 1938,
35, 693.

Razran, G. H. S. Conditioned response changes in


rating sociopolitical slogans. Psychological
Bulletin, 1940, 37, 481.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2006
Razran, G. Conditioning and perception. Psychological
Review, 1955, 62, 83-95.

Razran, G. The observable unconscious and the


inferable conscious in current soviet
psychophysiology. Psychological Review, 1961,
68, 81-147.

Rogers, C. R. A theory of therapy, personality, and


interpersonal relationships, as developed in the
client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.),
Psychology: A study of a science. Vol. III.
Formulations of the person and the social context.
New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959. pp. 184-256.

Rokeach, M. Attitude change and behavioral change.


Public Opinion Quarterly, 1966, 30, 529-550.

Rosenberg, M. J. An analysis of affective-cognitive


consistency. In M. J. Rosenberg, C. I. Hovland, W. J.
McGuire, R. P. Abelson, & J. W. Brehm (Eds.),
Attitude organization and change: An analysis of
consistency among attitude components. New
Haven: Yale University Press, 1960. pp. 15-64.

Rosenberg, M. J. When dissonance fails: On


eliminating evaluation apprehension from
attitude measurement. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1965,1, 28-42.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2007
Rosenberg, M. J. Some limits of dissonance: Toward a
differentiated view of counter-attitudinal
performance. In S. Feldman (Ed.), Cognitive
consistency. New York: Academic Press, 1966. pp.
135-170.

Rosenberg, M. J., Hovland, C. I., McGuire, W. J., Abelson,


R. P., & Brehm, J. W. Attitude organization and
change: An analysis of consistency among attitude
components. New Haven: Yale University Press,
1960.

Rosenthal, R., Persinger, G. W., Vikan-Kline, L. I., &


Fode, K. L. The effect of experimenter outcome-
bias and subject set on awareness in verbal
conditioning experiments. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 275-283.

Sasmor, R. M. Operant conditioning of a small-scale


muscle response. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, 69-85.

Sassenrath, J. M. Transfer of learning without


awareness. Psychological Reports, 1962, 10, 411-
420.

Schanck, R. L. A study of a community and its groups


and institutions conceived of as behavior of
individuals. Psychological Monographs, 1932, 43
(2, Whole No. 195).

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2008
Scobie, S. R., & Kaufman, A. Intermittent punishment
of variable interval and variable ratio responding.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
1969, 12,137-147.

Shepard, R. N., Hovland, C. I., & Jenkins, H. M. Learning


and memorization of classifications. Psychological
Monographs, 1961, 75 (13, Whole No. 517).

Sidman, M. Avoidance behavior. In W. K. Honig (Ed.),


Operant behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1966. pp. 448-498.

Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York:


Macmillan, 1953.

Smith, G. H. The influence of a female model on


perceived characteristics of an automobile. Paper
read at the meeting of the American
Psychological Association, San Francisco, August,
1968.

Spence, D. P. Conscious and preconscious influences


on recall: Another example of the restricting
effects of awareness. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1964, 68, 92-99.

Spence, D. P., & Holland, B. The restricting effects of


awareness: A paradox and an explanation.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962,

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2009
64, 163-174.

Spence, K. W. Cognitive and drive factors in the


extinction of the conditioned eye blink in human
subjects. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 445-458.

Spielberger, C. D., Berger, A., & Howard, K.


Conditioning of verbal behavior as a function of
awareness, need for social approval, and
motivation to receive reinforcement. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 241-
246.

Spielberger, C. D., Bernstein, I. H., & Ratliff, R. G.


Information and incentive value of the
reinforcing stimulus in verbal conditioning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 71, 26-
31.

Spielberger, C. D., & De Nike, L. D. Descriptive


behaviorism versus cognitive theory in verbal
operant conditioning. Psychological Review, 1966,
73, 306-326.

Spielberger, C. D., Levin, S. M., & Shepard, M. C. The


effects of awareness and attitude toward the
reinforcement of the operant conditioning of
verbal behavior. Journal of Personality, 1962, 30,
106-121.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2010
Staats, A. W., & Staats, C. K. Attitudes established by
classical conditioning. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1958, 57, 37-40.

Staats, C. K., & Staats, A. W. Meaning established by


classical conditioning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1957, 54, 74-80.

Thorndike, E. L. A theory of the action of the after-


effects of a connection upon it. Psychological
Review, 1933, 40, 434-439.

Verplanck, W. S. Unaware of where’s awareness:


Some verbal operants—notates, monents, and
notants. In C. W. Eriksen (Ed.), Behavior and
awareness. Durham, N. C.: Duke University Press,
1962. pp. 130-158.

Walters, R. H., & Parke, R. D. Influence of response


consequences to a social model on resistance to
deviation. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 1964, 1, 269-280.

Weiner, H. Real and imagined cost effects upon


human fixed-interval responding. Psychological
Reports, 1965, 17, 659-662.

Weinstein, W. K., & Lawson, R. The effect of


experimentally-induced “awareness” upon
performance in free-operant verbal conditioning

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2011
and on subsequent tests of “awareness.” Journal
of Psychology, 1963, 56, 203-211.

Weiss, R. L., Krasner, L., & Ullmann, L. P. Responsivity


to verbal conditioning as a function of emotional
atmosphere and pattern of reinforcement.
Psychological Reports, 1960, 6, 415-426.

Wickens, D. D., Allen, C. K., & Hill, F. A. Effects of


instruction and UCS strength on extinction of the
conditioned GSR. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1963, 66, 235-240.

Worell, L., & Worell, J. An experimental and


theoretical note on conscious and preconscious
influences on recall. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 119-123.

www.freepsychotherapybooks.org 2012

You might also like