Theory and Practice 1 Assignment
Theory and Practice 1 Assignment
Theory and Practice 1 Assignment
Mark Sheet
I declare that this assignment is the result of my own research and that where appropriate, I have
acknowledged the work of others.
YES
General Comments:
Autism Spectrum Disease (ASD) in people with autism spectrum disorder causes restriction in
hormonal abilities that affects self - expression, speech intelligence and proficiencies in general
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Wing & Gould, 1979). Notably the understanding
of the neurological mechanisms involved in a condition like autism has been the objective of the
majority of studies undertaken in the most recent years. Consequently, majority of diagnoses are
still made based on behavioural analysis and tests, despite the fact that the biological makeup of
individuals contributes significantly in the build up of the disorder (Smalley, et al., 1988).
Notably a complex relationship exists between the presence of autistic children in the school
system and the emerging tendency toward the notion of inclusive education, as such this
relationship has a number of implications for parents, autistic children and education authorities
as well (Seach, et al., 2005). Additionally, because autistic children develop differently from
their neurotypical peers, they frequently struggle to succeed in conventional educational
environments (Jordan, 2005). Furthermore, due to the magnitude and intricacy composition of
autism, many people who have direct contact with them have reported feeling confused and
underqualified to satisfy these children's requirements (Starr, Foy, & Kramer, 2001).
However, it has been well-documented that parents of autistic children are frustrated and
dissatisfied with the school-based services (Spann, Kohler, & Soenksen, 2003). According to a
recent study, autism has a strong genetic component that stems from human physiology, despite
the fact that previous research efforts frequently focused on the behavioural aspects of autism
(Bailey, et al., 1998; Baron-Cohen, et al., 1998; Happe, 1999). Because of this, The most
probable and complete explanation of this condition relies on the opinion of many experts that
the mental mechanisms dictate the social and communicative problems that contribute to autism.
(Baron-Cohen, 1989; Frith, 1991; Hill, 2004).
To further explain this disorder, a number of cognitive theories have been developed; The
theory-of-mind deficiency is probably one of the foremost well-known hypotheses that has
dominated the majority of behavioral study to date (Premack & Woodruff, 1978). The theory of
mind has dominated most studies because It provides the best image possible of what an autistic
spectrum disorder seems like.
1
Autism and Cognitive Theory as a Concept
With no known cause or treatment, autism is a persistent persisting condition that lasts a lifetime
and is majorly discovered at infancy between the 0-3 years (Lovannone, et al., 2003). Wing
(1981) originated the term autism spectrum disorders (ASD) to capture the symptoms categories
of autistic disorder, high functioning autism, and other related characteristics. Although
intellectual ability varies overboard the autism spectrum, the underlying deficiencies in social
and non-social areas seems general in nature, thus informing the benchmarks for its diagnostic
criteria (Leslie & Frith, 1988).
Additionally, according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders- Text
Revision (DSM-IV-TR), Constrictive, monotonous, and predictable patterns of behaviour,
preferences, and aspirations all contribute to these fundamental difficulties in societal
engagement and information sharing evolution (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). No
behavioral health theory has been put forward so far that can fully explain all of these elements.
However, other cognitive models have been created recently in an effort to explain various
aspects of these fundamental deficits in individuals with autism (Baron-Cohen, 1989; Leslie &
Frith, 1988). The mind theory is a key concept that is extensively debated.
In order to discus in depth the behavioural pattern of autistic persons as it relates to cognitive
theory, In order to comprehend autism spectrum disorder properly, it is necessary to take into
account the numerous behavioral hypotheses developed over time. Three major lines of thought
in cognitive theories were reviewed in this study. They are discussed as follows
Theory of Mind: Frith and Frith (1999) asserts that social existence depends on a part of social
awareness, while social intelligence entails recognising another person's mental state and
responding appropriately to it. When tested with false belief and the unexpected transfer tasks,
people with autism spectrum disorders demonstrate a deficit in the theory of mind, failing to
comprehend the emotions and mental states of the other person and consequently failing to
connect with them as well as failing at socialising (Baron-Cohen, et al., 1985).
For instance, at my previous job, I gave the Sally-Anne task to a specific group of newly
admitted pupils. The scenario illustration is shown to the kids and it can be performed by
2
puppets, toys, or actual people. The other character, Anne, moves the ball into her box when the
first character, Sally, leaves it in her basket and leaves. The kids are asked where Sally should
look for her ball when she returns at the conclusion of this role play or demonstration. Sally
should look in the basket, which describes what Sally genuinely thinks as she is not aware that
the ball has been moved, is the response that the majority of typically developing kids will give.
The answer that Sally will look for the ball in the box, where the ball actually is, is typically
given by my autistic pupils because they are unable to comprehend how others think. Because of
this, it has been assumed that Sally has a theory of mind deficit based on her inability to
comprehend her incorrect belief. We now know more about the characteristics of the disabilities
in autism thanks to this innovative account. The Theory of Mind, however, has several
drawbacks. Although it explains a significant trait seen in people with Autism, this theory falls
short of offering a convincing justification for the prevalence of repetitive and stereotypical
behaviours (Happe, 1999). This introduces the second idea that accounted for the occurrence of
these behaviours.
Theory of Executive Dysfunction: Knowing what the basic concept of "executive function"
entails is necessary to comprehend why executive dysfunction offers an insight for the
abnormalities in autism. The phrase "executive function" refers to a variety of high-level
cognitive abilities that control behaviour and guide it toward predetermined goals, especially in
novel situations (Norman & Shallice, 1986). (Russell, 1998). Welsh and Pennington (1988)
included the ability to retain a suitable problem-solving set for the accomplishment of a specific
vision to their definition of executive function. Planning and monitoring behaviour, setting
priorities, stopping automatic behaviours, and keeping knowledge in working memory while
using them are some behaviours that can be categorised as executive functions (Russell, 1998).
Numerous relevant research that has received validation from scientific evidence have
demonstrated that individuals with autism do exhibit abnormalities in executive tasks that require
working memory, inhibition, and set-shifting. (Pennington, et al., 1996). Additionally, some of
the theoretical limits of the theory of mind are regarded to be alternatives to executive
dysfunction. In their experiment, Using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and the Tower of Hanoi
as benchmarks, Ozonoff, Rogers, and Pennington (1991) established that all of the individuals
with Autism/ Asperger's syndrome performed badly on these tests, but not all of them performed
3
badly on the theory-of-mind task. It is still unclear whether executive dysfunction and theory of
mind impairment are the primary hypotheses underlying autism, despite the fact that executive
dysfunction offers an alternative explanation for theory of mind deficit (Russell, 1998). One odd
non-social characteristic of autism has been the prevalence of individuals who score better than
average in math, music, art, and occasionally jigsaw puzzles. According to Rimland & Hill
(1984) and Happé (1999), it affected almost one in ten autistic individuals. Weak Central
Coherence (WCC) was thus proposed by Frith (Frith & Happé, 1994) as a theory to account for
this phenomenon.
Weak Central Coherence: The habitual propensity to integrate the local incoming information
for high-level meaning, according to Frith (1989), is the fundamental coherence of non-social
features. Processing information as a unit as opposed to in parts is a natural instinct. For instance,
Bartlett (1932) hypothesised through his experiment that people were likely to remember the
idea of the story but rapidly forgot and/or had trouble remembering the detail: Normally, a
person wouldn't analyse a scenario like this in such detail. In any typical situation, a person had a
strong propensity to form an overall perception of the situation, and from there they built the
most likely detail. (1932; Bartlett)
According to Hermelin and O'Connor (1967), young children and individuals with mental health
issues who are not autistic also exhibit this predilection for global processing. According to Frith
(1989), people with autism had problems with the way their brains naturally processed the
world's information. The idea of the weak central coherence theory can also be comparable to
Kanner's (1943) diagnosis of autism, which was accompanied by a predisposition for detail-
focused processing that was clinically discovered in autistic individuals in relation to their
reluctance to change. This frequent trait of autism, according to Kanner, is the inability to
experience wholes without paying close attention to the individual pieces (Happé, 1999).
The weak central coherence would apply to both the positive assets and deficiencies in autism,
according to Frith's (1989) prediction. In other words, activities demanding a global integration
of information are projected to be poorly performed by a weak central coherence, while tasks
where it is desirable to pay attention to local information are predicted to be well performed
(Happe, 1999). Weak central coherence has been demonstrated at three levels: perceptual,
visuospatial-constructional, and verbal-semantic (Happé, 1999).
4
Happé (1999) proposed that people with autism were given the typical visual illusions and asked
to distinguish between them, taking into account the perceptual coherence impairment
discovered in the preceding tasks. Some of these illusions were broken down into to-be-judged
figures and contexts that caused them. The idea was that since people with autism tend to
concentrate more on the specific pieces of things, they might be less susceptible to those visual
illusions. The findings revealed that the autism subject group concentrated more on the aspects
that will be judged without merging them into the overall illusion-producing figure. It has also
been shown in other studies that local-level processing at the perceptual level is biased. The
McGurk effect, which favours visual perception over audible perception, is another factor that is
diminished in autism (de Gelder, et al; 1991).
Shah and Frith (1983) used the Wechsler Block Design Task (Wechsler, 1974, 1981) to show
that inadequate coherence at the visuospatial-constructional level was caused by segmentation
abilities. They hypothesised that people with autism evaluated the designs in terms of their
components pieces. Additionally, they discovered that people with both high- and low-
functioning autism performed better on the Embedded Figures Test (Witkin, Oltman, Raskin and
Karp, 1971). Additionally, reading comprehension scores tend to be lower for those with autism
than reading accuracy levels (Loker and Rutter, 1969; Frith and Snowling, 1983).
Since findings suggest that the effects are apparent in the sections where the top-down semantic
signals are prominent, the current investigation will focus on another lower-level language
processing and voice perception where the influence of vocabulary development has been proven
(Ganong, 1980). The Ganong Effect is what is occurring here.
5
Theory of Mind as a Model
Theory of mind (TOM), also known as mentalizing, is the ability to take into account one's own
and other people's mental states when interpreting and predicting behavior (Leslie & Frith,
1988). According to reports, children with autism have a TOM that is underdeveloped, meaning
they have trouble understanding other people's ideas and feelings, may find it difficult to
understand, and frequently find other people's behaviour incomprehensible (Baron-Cohen, 1989;
Ozonoff & Miller, 1995). Additionally, recent neuroimaging studies have shown that people with
autism display much reduced activation in the brain regions that are crucial for mentalizing in
neurotypical people (Castelli, et al., 2002).
As it enables people to create and sustain fulfilling social connections and encourages
community adaptability across time, developing suitable social skills is a crucial aspect of typical
development (Cacioppo, 2002). A wide range of underlying cognitive capabilities known as
social cognition must develop in order for social skills to emerge, which is a challenging
psychological development (Beauchamp and Anderson, 2010). The ability to attribute mental
states to oneself and others, such as desires, knowledge, beliefs, and intentions in order to predict
behavior, was first described by Premack in the 1970s, and since then, TOM has been a major
focus of developmental and social psychology as well as speech therapy (Premack & Woodruff,
1978, Byom & Turkstra, 2012).
It was accepted that children needed to be able to build diverse theoretical depictions of reality,
move between them, and identify their own states from those of others using a variety of cues in
order to demonstrate flexible and explicit TOM (Wimmer & Perner, 1983). Since then,
developmental cognitive science has researched this area more than any other (Sabbagh &
Paulus, 2018). More recently, the discipline of social neuroscience has paid attention to TOM as
a social cognitive construct, which has led to the creation of a substantial body of mainstream
literature about the neural circuits underpinning TOM (Gallagher & Frith, 2003; Frith & Frith,
2006; Blakemore, 2008; Bellerose et al., 2011; Bird & Viding, 2014).
Children who have good TOM typically exhibit signs of social adaptability, including that of
improved communication skills, more fulfilling social interactions, an increase in group appeal,
and strong educational accomplishment (Binnie, 2005; Fink et al., 2015; Slaughter, 2015;
Slaughter et al., 2015; Imuta et al., 2016). On the other hand, in a number of conditions and
6
contexts characterised by disturbed social functioning, such as autism spectrum disorders, lower
TOM has been seen. (Yirmiya et al., 1998; Shaked and Yirmiya, 2004; Senju, 2012; Chung et
al., 2014; Kimhi, 2014; Leekam, 2016), language impairment (Stanzione and Schick, 2014),
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Bora & Pantelis, 2016), Tourette's syndrome (Eddy &
Cavanna, 2013), childhood maltreatment (Luke and (Payne et al., 2016) and Fragile X syndrome
(Turkstra et al., 2014).
The theory of mind has dominated most studies since it provides the most accurate description of
the nature of an autistic spectrum condition by a wide margin. According to Rajendran and
Mitchell (2007), The failure of autistic individuals to comprehend and take into account other
people's mental states or points of view can be used to explain the theory-of-mind profiled in
many of the behavioural symptoms of autism in social, communicative, and imaginative
activities (Jarrold et al., 2000).
While the theory of mind can explain the majority of the behavioral, social, and cognitive issues
these individuals exhibit, it is unable to explain a number of behaviours that are characteristic of
autism, such as the fixation on similarity, monotonous behaviors, and difficulties switching their
attention from one task to another. According to some researchers, these symptoms were similar
to those of a certain kind of brain injury.
Investigations into the function of TOM in therapeutic settings and normative development are
still ongoing. The use of validated, developmentally appropriate assessment techniques is
necessary to advance this understanding because social cognition is now included in the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders' assessment criteria. (DSM-V; American
Psychiatric Association, 2013). Despite the multitude of measures that have been produced to
investigate TOM (particularly in the field of cognitive science), it is challenging to select the
appropriate measure for a given therapeutic or research objective. There are many difficulties in
evaluating TOM, some of which are connected to the various and diverse definitions and
conceptualizations of TOM that have been presented.
(Premack and Woodruff, 1978; Wimmer and Perner, 1983; Leslie, 1987; Tager-Flusberg &
Sullivan, 2000; Abu-Akel & Shamay-Tsoory, 2011, Dennis et al., 2013, Bird & Viding, 2014;
Westby, 2014; Asakura and Inui, 2016, and Happé et al., 2017), the variable manifestations of
7
TOM at various developmental stages (Wellman et al (Mayes et al., 1996; Brune, 2001; Hutchins
et al., 2008a; Carlson et al., 2013; Hiller et al., 2014).
Depending on the assessment tool used, a variety of characteristics are targeted by the
multidimensional phenomenon known as TOM (German & Cohen, 2012). Each definition or
theory provides a slightly distinct idea of its specificity (Premack and Woodruff, 1978; Wimmer
& Perner, 1983; Leslie, 1987; Tager-Flusberg & Sullivan, 2000; Abu-Akel & Shamay-Tsoory,
2011; Dennis et al., 2013; Bird & Viding, 2014; Westby, 2014; Asakura & Inui, 2016; Happé et
al., 2017) However, it is commonly acknowledged that TOM refers to a collection of cognitive
abilities that allow rationalisation about emotional or cognitive mental states, such as beliefs or
emotions.
The self to other model of empathy (SOME) is especially used in most studies as a framework to
define TOM and establish the eligibility and limitation standards for the literature search. The
complete model called the SOME is based on scientific information from clinical and
neuroimaging investigations (Bird & Viding, 2014). Instead of concentrating only on internal
TOM processes, it shows how social cognitive structures like TOM interact to influence
empathic behaviour. A crucial distinction made by SOME is between TOM and empathy: The
difference between TOM and empathy is that TOM is the cognitive representation of one's own
and other people's mental states, whereas empathy is the emotional contagion caused by
exposure to another person's emotion while being aware that the other person is also
experiencing that emotion (Bird & Viding, 2014).
The emotional cue categorisation system, i.e. the lower sensory mechanism in charge of
processing and categorising cues signalling affective states, such as facial emotions and voice
tones, is distinguished from TOM in the model. The SOME model also asserts that TOM is
separate from a scenario understanding system that processes situational signals and infers or
associates estimated emotional states of others depending upon situational cues, for example,
black denotes morning and melancholy (Bird & Viding, 2014). Because of this, the model is
helpful for drawing distinctions between TOM and other social cognitive categories that are
strongly connected to it; in the current review, it was utilised to separate primary TOM measures
from those that are more distantly related to TOM.
8
The TOM construct should be kept separate from other abilities because, while they may be
based on TOM or rely on it, other social cognitive processes are better suited to represent them.
Along with doing this, a specific definition of TOM must be used to identify and specify the
right evaluation tools. For instance, many apparently prosocial and self-promoting behaviours
rely on TOM, but they may be evaluated more thoroughly with the help of specialised tests that
highlight collaboration, adherence to social norms, lying, and deceptive interpersonal methods
(Baurain & Nader-Grosbois, 2013; Slaughter, 2015).
To limit the search to the most straightforward kinds of TOM, developmental concerns should
also be taken into consideration. Determining which infant social abilities are regarded early,
direct expressions of TOM which are different cognitive antecedents that allow TOM to form, is
still up for debate (Carlson et al., 2013). Early social skills like emulation, gaze following,
figuring, and joint attention may reflect, at most, more fully automated and inherent
manifestations of awareness of mental states. While the question of the first measurable
manifestations of TOM has not yet been resolved ideally or practically, current literature as well
as majority of authors suggest that this is the scenario (Carlson et al., 2013).
Thus, it is believed that these abilities serve as stepping stones for later-developing TOM
abilities, which are the focus of the present review and reflect an unambiguous, cohesive,
versatile, and theoretical knowledge of mental processes. In conclusion, this review defines
TOM to set it apart from empathy, categorization of affective and environmental signals, early
quasi cognitive structures of mental states, like peer interaction and emulation, and more
sophisticated social skills, like cooperate or modification techniques.
9
The Developmental Trajectory of TOM and Associated Measurement Tools
It is not unexpected that a wide range of models and indicators have been devised to explore the
construct given the various definitions and concepts of TOM. Despite the wide range of mental
states that kids must learn to comprehend (including emotions, knowledge, intents, beliefs, and
wants), there seems to be a preponderance of tests meant to gauge just one form of mental state:
erroneous assumptions (Hedger & Fabricius, 2011; Hiller et al., 2014).
Wimmer and Perner (1983) first developed the false belief paradigm, which has since been
modified and used in a variety of scenarios (Wellman et al., 2001). Typically, a brief story that
highlights a difference between reality and a character's perspective is offered to children. For
instance, in the change of location paradigm known as the Sally and Ann task, a child is offered
two dolls with the names Sally and Ann (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). Before leaving, Sally puts
her marble in a basket. Ann takes the marble out of the basket and puts it in a box. When Sally
comes back, the youngster is asked where she would seek for the marble.
Children must answer "in the basket" even though they are aware that the marble is actually in
the box in order to pass this activity. This type of situation allows researchers to gauge a child's
ability to comprehend that a person's mental state is not just a reflection of reality and suggests
that the child is capable of developing a theory about another person's mental content, or theory
of mind.
The concept that TOM develops during this time period is supported by the finding that children
often complete false belief paradigms successfully between the ages of 3 and 5 (Wellman et al.,
2001). TOM, however, exhibits a more extensive and complex developmental pattern, as
revealed by the application of a wider range of metrics and techniques (Wellman et al., 2011).
Implicit, non-verbal, and simplified measurements in particular revealed that some TOM abilities
may already be present in infancy, a conclusion that could not be established using traditional
assessments due to the extraneous elements inherent to the tests. These tests were made to be
used with pre-verbal infants that were very young (Slaughter, 2015).
According to this research, children may recognise others' intentions around the age of 12
months, grasp others' wishes around the age of 18 months, and can demonstrate some
understanding of erroneous beliefs as early as 15 months of age by using implicit methods
10
including watching imitation behaviours, violation-of-expectation paradigms, and eye gaze
tracking (Repacholi & Gopnik, 1997; Poulin-Dubois et al., 2007),(Onishi & Baillargeon, 2005;
Southgate et al., 2007; Senju, 2012). Many experts disagree on how to interpret these findings;
some argue that implicit tasks are reliable and viable ways to evaluate TOM (Carruthers, 2013;
Powell et al., 2018), while others argue that there isn't enough evidence to justify their usage.
Between the ages of 5 and 6, children become increasingly adept at grasping second order false
belief assignments, such as "Ann thinks that Sally thinks the marble is in the basket," and during
adolescence they develop a more sophisticated comprehension of sarcasm, faux-pas (social
gaffes), and white lies (Miller, 2009). During a range of TOM activities, neuroimaging studies
also show longitudinal changes in patterns of brain activation, indicating prolonged development
well into adulthood and into adolescence (Blakemore, 2008, 2012).
When taken as a whole, these data show that TOM cannot be viewed as a unitary concept and
must be understood in the context of its constant evolution. In order to accurately document a
complex and quickly evolving cognitive capacity, there is also a need to underscore the
significance of depending on a variety of TOM measures that are trustworthy, valid, and
sensitive to developmental changes.
Even though there has been made a significant stride in understanding of both normal and
abnormal TOM (Wellman et al., 2001; Gallagher & Frith, 2003; Vuadens, 2005; Poletti &
Adenzalo, 2013; Kimhi, 2014; Imuta et al., 2016), it is still challenging to draw firm conclusions
about its function in average progress and clinical circumstances. These difficulties could be the
11
result of empirical flaws in the TOM assessment criteria (Hiller et al., 2014; Henry et al., 2016).
The fact is, unsystematic, inadequate, and unsophisticated terms have been used to describe the
psychometric requirements of TOM assessments (Mayes et al., 1996; Brune, 2001; Hutchins et
al., 2008a; Carlson et al., 2013; Hiller et al., 2014).
The over-representation of false belief understanding as the only TOM measure is one of the
fundamental deficiencies of TOM appraisal, as it is a fact that only a small number of TOM
indicators have scientifically confirmed psychometric features (Hutchins et al., 2008a; Hiller et
al., 2014; Ziatabar Ahmadi et al., 2015).
As far as research is aware, there has not been a thorough assessment done to describe the
characteristics of the TOM measures that are currently used for young children. Non-systematic
reviews of TOM measures have been published for adults (Henry et al., 2015), middle childhood
(Sprung, 2010), clinical populations (Sprung, 2010), middle childhood and adolescence (Henry
et al., 2015), and clinical populations (Sprung, 2010). (Hayward and Homer, 2017). These
evaluations highlight the usefulness of a number of TOM indicators for analysing social efficacy
in clinical contexts and typical development while providing fascinating insights into their use,
but they are unstructured and do not take into account instruments designed for infants, toddlers,
and preschoolers.
A systematic review of TOM measures for preschoolers was undertaken by Ziatabar Ahmadi et
al. (2015), although they limited the scope to papers presenting the creation and verification of
complex assessments made up of a variety of TOM tasks. Their review excludes single task
measures (such as single false belief tests) since individual task variables make up the majority
of measurements used in TOM research (Hiller et al., 2014). In addition, Ziatabar Ahmadi et al.
(2015)'s review is limited to studies that only seek to analyse the psychometric properties of
TOM indicators, neglecting other types of scientific inquiries (e.g., longitudinal, outcome or
prediction papers).
Conclusion
The main goal of this study was to rigorously assess one cognitive hypothesis used to explain
behaviours in autistic people. Hence the theory of mind concept of cognitive theory was the
12
chosen theory which was discussed extensively amongst the various cognitive theories identified.
The theory of mind has been the subject of the majority of research since it provides the clearest
picture possible of the characteristics of an autistic spectrum disease. Although TOM as a theory
offers an innovative description of the understanding of the nature of the impairments in Autism,
a critical investigation of the theory's review demonstrated that this is not truly the case a s there
are however certain drawbacks. Even while it explains a significant trait exhibited in people with
autism, TOM falls short of offering a convincing justification for the occurrence of repetitive and
stereotypical behaviours.
13
References
References
Abu-Akel, A., and Bailey, A. L. (2001). Indexical and symbolic referencing: what role do they
play in children's success on theory of mind tasks? Cognition 80, 263–281. doi: 10.1016/S0010-
0277(00)00149-9
Adrian, J. E., Clemente, R. A., Villanueva, L., and Rieffe, C. (2005). Parent-child picture-book
reading, mothers' mental state language and children's theory of mind. J. Child Lang. 32, 673–
686. doi: 10.1017/S0305000905006963
Adrien, J., Rossignol, C., Barthelemy, C., and Jose, C. (1995). Development and functioning of a
“theory of mind” in autistic and normal children. Approch. Neuropsychol. Apprent. l'Enfant 7,
188–196.
Allen, J. R., and Kinsey, K. (2013). Teaching theory of mind. Early Educ. Dev. 24, 865–876.
doi: 10.1080/10409289.2013.745182
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM−5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous, X., and Warden, D. (2008). Cognitive and affective
perspective-taking in conduct-disordered children high and low on callous-unemotional
traits. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 2:16. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-2-16
Aronson, J. N., and Golomb, C. (1999). Preschoolers' understanding of pretense and presumption
of congruity between action and representation. Dev. Psychol. 35, 1414–1425. doi:
10.1037/0012-1649.35.6.1414
Astington, J. W., Pelletier, J., and Homer, B. (2002). Theory of mind and epistemological
development: the relation between children's second-order false-belief understanding and their
ability to reason about evidence. New Ideas Psychol. 20, 131–144. doi: 10.1016/S0732-
118X%2802%2900005-3
Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., and Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test
of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger
syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 38, 813–822. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-
7610.1997.tb01599.x
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., and Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of
mind”? Cognition 21, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8
Beauchamp, H. M. (2017). Neuropsychology's social landscape: common ground with social
neuroscience. Neuropsychology 31, 981–1002. doi: 10.1037/neu0000395
Bennett, M., and Galpert, L. (1993). Children's understanding of multiple desires. Int. J. Behav.
Dev. 16, 15–33. doi: 10.1177/016502549301600102
Biedermann, F., Frajo-Apor, B., and Hofer, A. (2012). Theory of mind its relevance in
schizophrenia. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 25, 71–75. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503624
14
Bird, G., and Viding, E. (2014). The self to other model of empathy: providing a new framework
for understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymia. Neurosci.
Biobehav. Rev. 47, 520–532. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.021
Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 267–277. doi:
10.1038/nrn2353
Blakemore, S. J. (2012). Imaging brain development: the adolescent brain. Neuroimage 61, 397–
406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.080
Bora, E., Yücel, M., and Pantelis, C. (2009). Theory of mind impairment: a distinct trait-marker
for schizophrenia spectrum disorders and bipolar disorder? Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 120, 253–264.
doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01414.x
Brune, M. (2001). Social cognition and psychopathology in an evolutionary perspective. Current
status and proposals for research. Psychopathology 34, 85–94. doi: 10.1159/000049286
Cacioppo, J. T. (2002). Social neuroscience: understanding the pieces fosters understanding the
whole and vice versa. Am. Psychol. 57, 819–831. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.11.819
Callaghan, T. C., Rochat, P., and Corbit, J. (2012). Young children's knowledge of the
representational function of pictorial symbols: development across the preschool years in three
cultures. J. Cogn. Dev. 13, 320–353. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.587853
Cassidy, J., Parke, R. D., Butkovsky, L., and Braungart, J. M. (1992). Family-peer connections:
the roles of emotional expressiveness within the family and children's understanding of
emotions. Child Dev. 63, 603–618. doi: 10.2307/1131349
Cermolacce, M., Lazerges, P., Da Fonseca, D., Fakra, E., Adida, M., Belzeaux, R., et al. (2011).
Théorie de l'esprit et schizophrénie. [Theory of mind and schizophrenia.]. L'Encephale Rev.
Psychiatr. Clin. Biol. Ther. 37, S117–S122. doi: 10.1016/S0013-7006(11)70037-9
Comte-Gervais, I., Giron, A., Soares-Boucaud, I., and Poussin, G. (2008). Assessment of social
intelligence in children with specific language impairment: presentation of an assessing
scale. L'Evol. Psychiatr. 73, 353–366. doi: 10.1016/j.evopsy.2008.02.004
Cornish, K., Rinehart, N., Gray, K., and Howlin, P. (2010). Comic Strip Task. Melbourne, VIC:
Monash University Developmental Neuroscience and Genetic Disorders Laboratory and Monash
University Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology.
Crowe, L., Beauchamp, M., Catroppa, C., and Anderson, V. (2011). Social function assessment
tools for children and adolescents: a systematic review from 1988 to 2010. Clin. Psychol.
Rev. 31, 767–785. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.008
Cutting, A. L., and Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and
family background: individual differences and interrelations. Child Dev. 70, 853–865. doi:
10.1111/1467-8624.00061
Davis, P. E., Meins, E., and Fernyhough, C. (2011). Self-knowledge in childhood: relations with
children's imaginary companions and understanding of mind. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 29, 680–686.
doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02038.x
15
Davis, T. L. (1998). “Children's understanding of false beliefs in different domains: affective
versus physical,” in Poster Presented at the Conference on Human Development (Mobile, AL).
Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers:
contextual validation. Child Dev. 57, 194–201. doi: 10.2307/1130651
Denham, S. A., Zoller, D., and Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of preschoolers' emotion
understanding. Dev. Psychol. 30, 928–936. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.928
Dennis, M., Simic, N., Bigler, E. D., Abildskov, T., Agostino, A., Taylor, H., et al. (2013).
Cognitive, affective, and conative theory of mind (TOM) in children with traumatic brain
injury. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 25–39. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.006
Dore, R. A., and Lillard, A. S. (2015). Theory of mind and children's engagement in fantasy
worlds. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 34, 230–242. doi: 10.1177/0276236614568631
Dörrenberg, S., Rakoczy, H., and Liszkowski, U. (2018). How (not) to measure infant theory of
mind: testing the replicability and validity of four non-verbal measures. Cogn. Dev. 46, 12–30.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.01.001
Ebersbach, M., Stiehler, S., and Asmus, P. (2011). On the relationship between children's
perspective taking in complex scenes and their spatial drawing ability. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 29,
455–474. doi: 10.1348/026151010X504942
Eddy, C. M., and Cavanna, A. E. (2013). Altered social cognition in Tourette syndrome: nature
and implications. Behav. Neurol. 27, 15–22. doi: 10.1155/2013/417516
Edelstein, W., Keller, M., and Wahlen, K. (1984). Structure and content in social cognition:
conceptual and empirical analyses. Child Dev. 55, 1514–1526. doi: 10.2307/1130021
Feshbach, N. D., and Cohen, S. (1988). Training affect comprehension in young children: an
experimental evaluation. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 9, 201–210. doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(88)90023-8
Filippova, E., and Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in
discourse irony understanding. Child Dev. 79, 126–138. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01115.x
Fink, E., Begeer, S., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., and de Rosnay, M. (2015). Friends,
friendlessness, and the social consequences of gaining a theory of mind. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 33,
27–30. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12080
Flavell, J. H. (1968). The Development of Role-Taking and Communication Skills in Children.
New York, NY: Wiley.
Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., and Flavell, E. R. (1981). Young children's knowledge
about visual perception: further evidence for the level 1–level 2 distinction. Dev. Psychol. 17,
99–103. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.17.1.99
Ford, R. M., Driscoll, T., Shum, D., and Macaulay, C. E. (2012). Executive and theory-of-mind
contributions to event-based prospective memory in children: exploring the self-projection
hypothesis. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 111, 468–489. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.10.006
Galende, N., de Miguel, M. S., and Arranz, E. (2011). The role of physical context, verbal skills,
non-parental care, social support, and type of parental discipline in the development of TOM
16
capacity in five-year-old children. Soc. Dev. 20, 845–861. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
9507.2011.00625.x
Gallagher, H. L., and Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind'. Trends Cogn.
Sci. 7, 77–83. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6
Garner, P. W., Curenton, S. M., and Taylor, K. (2005). Predictors of mental state understanding
in preschoolers of varying socioeconomic backgrounds. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 29, 271–281. doi:
10.1177/01650250544000053
Garner, P. W., Jones, D. C., and Miner, J. L. (1994). Social competence among low-income
preschoolers: emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Dev. 65, 622–
637. doi: 10.2307/1131405
Happé, F., Cook, J. L., and Bird, G. (2017). The structure of social cognition: in(ter)dependence
of sociocognitive processes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 243–267. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-
010416-044046
Happé, F., and Frith, U. (2014). Annual research review: towards a developmental neuroscience
of atypical social cognition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 55, 553–557. doi:
10.1111/jcpp.12162
Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters'
thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J.
Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 129–154. doi: 10.1007/BF02172093
Harris, P. L., Donnelly, K., Guz, G. R., and Pitt-Watson, R. (1986). Children's understanding of
the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Dev. 57, 895–909. doi:
10.2307/1130366
Hedger, J. A., and Fabricius, W. V. (2011). True belief belies false belief: recent findings of
competence in infants and limitations in 5-year-olds, and implications for theory of mind
development. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 2, 429–447. doi: 10.1007/s13164-011-0069-9
Henry, J. D., Cowan, D. G., Lee, T., and Sachdev, P. S. (2015). Recent trends in testing social
cognition. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 28, 133–140. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000139
Heyes, C. (2014). False belief in infancy: a fresh look. Dev. Sci. 17, 647–659. doi:
10.1111/desc.12148
Hiller, R. M., Weber, N., and Young, R. L. (2014). The validity and scalability of the theory of
mind scale with toddlers and preschoolers. Psychol. Assess. 26, 1388–1393. doi:
10.1037/a0038320
Hirai, M., Muramatsu, Y., Mizuno, S., Kurahashi, N., Kurahashi, H., and Nakamura, M. (2013).
Developmental changes in mental rotation ability and visual perspective-taking in children and
adults with Williams syndrome. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:856. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00856
Hoddenbach, E., Koot, H. M., Clifford, P., Gevers, C., Clauser, C., Boer, F., et al. (2012).
Individual differences in the efficacy of a short theory of mind intervention for children with
autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Trials 13:206. doi: 10.1186/1745-6215-
13-206
17
Hogrefe, G.-J., Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1986). Ignorance versus false belief: a
developmental lag in attribution of epistemic states. Child Dev. 57, 567–582. doi:
10.2307/1130337
Hughes, C., Adlam, A., Happe, F., Jackson, J., Taylor, A., and Caspi, A. (2000). Good test–retest
reliability for standard and advanced false-belief tasks across a wide range of abilities. J. Child
Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 41, 483–490. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00633
Hughes, C., Soares-Boucaud, I., Hochmann, J., and Frith, U. (1997). Social behaviour in
pervasive developmental disorders: effects of informant, group and “theory-of-mind”. Eur. Child
Adolesc. Psychiatry 6, 191–198. doi: 10.1007/BF00539925
Iannotti, R. J. (1978). Effect of role-taking experiences on role taking,empathy, altruism, and
aggression. Dev. Psychol. 14, 119–124. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.14.2.119
Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B., and Ruffman, T. (2016). Theory of mind and
prosocial behavior in childhood: a meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1192–1205. doi:
10.1037/dev0000140
Jin, X., Li, P., He, J., and Shen, M. (2017). Cooperation, but not competition, improves 4-year-
old children's reasoning about others' diverse desires. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 157, 81–94. doi:
10.1016/j.jecp.2016.12.010
Killen, M., Lynn Mulvey, K., Richardson, C., Jampol, N., and Woodward, A. (2011). The
accidental transgressor: morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition 119, 197–215. doi:
10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.006
Kim, Y.-S., and Phillips, B. (2014). Cognitive correlates of listening comprehension. Read. Res.
Q. 49, 269–281. doi: 10.1002/rrq.74
Kimhi, Y. (2014). Theory of mind abilities and deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Top. Lang.
Disord. 34, 329–343. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000033
Knafo, A., Zahn-Waxler, C., Davidov, M., Van Hulle, C., Robinson, J. L., and Rhee, S. H.
(2009). Empathy in early childhood: genetic, environmental, and affective contributions. Ann. N.
Y. Acad. Sci. 1167, 103–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04540.x
Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., and Perst, H. (2011). Infants' joint attention skills predict
toddlers' emerging mental state language. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1207–1219. doi: 10.1037/a0024808
Kulke, L., von Duhn, B., Schneider, D., and Rakoczy, H. (2018). Is implicit theory of mind a real
and robust phenomenon? Results from a systematic replication study. Psychol. Sci. 29, 888–900.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617747090
Lackner, C., Sabbagh, M. A., Hallinan, E., Liu, X., and Holden, J. J. (2012). Dopamine receptor
D4 gene variation predicts preschoolers' developing theory of mind. Dev. Sci. 15, 272–280. doi:
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01124.x
Laranjo, J., Bernier, A., Meins, E., and Carlson, S. M. (2010). Early manifestations of children's
theory of mind: the roles of maternal mind-mindedness and infant security of
attachment. Infancy 15, 300–323. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00014.x
18
Lecce, S., Bianco, F., Devine, R. T., Hughes, C., and Banerjee, R. (2014). Promoting theory of
mind during middle childhood: a training program. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 126, 52–67. doi:
10.1016/j.jecp.2014.03.002
Leekam, S. (2016). Social cognitive impairment and autism: what are we trying to
explain? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150082. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0082
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of “theory of mind”. Psychol.
Rev. 94, 412–426. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.94.4.412
Martin, A. K., Robinson, G., Dzafic, I., Reutens, D., and Mowry, B. (2014). Theory of mind and
the social brain: implications for understanding the genetic basis of schizophrenia. Genes Brain
Behav. 13, 104–117. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12066
Masangkay, Z. S., McCluskey, K. A., McIntyre, C. W., Sims-Knight, J., Vaughn, B. E., and
Flavell, J. H. (1974). The early development of inferences about the visual percepts of
others. Child Dev. 45, 357–366. doi: 10.2307/1127956
Mayes, L. C., Klin, A., Tercyak, K. P. Jr., Cicchetti, D. V., and Cohen, D. J. (1996). Test-retest
reliability for false-belief tasks. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 37, 313–319. doi:
10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01408.x
McDonald, S. (2013). Impairments in social cognition following severe traumatic brain injury. J.
Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 19, 231–246. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712001506
McKown, C., Russo-Ponsaran, N. M., Johnson, J. K., Russo, J., and Allen, A. (2016). Web-
based assessment of children's social-emotional comprehension. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 34, 322–
338. doi: 10.1177/0734282915604564
Moll, H., and Tomasello, M. (2007). How 14- and 18-month-olds know what others have
experienced. Dev. Psychol. 43, 309–317. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.309
Muris, P., Steerneman, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., Horselenberg, R., van den Hogen, T.,
et al. (1999). The TOM test: a new instrument for assessing theory of mind in normal children
and children with pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29, 67–80. doi:
10.1023/A:1025922717020
Nader-Grosbois, N., Houssa, M., and Mazzone, S. (2013). How could theory of mind contribute
to the differentiation of social adjustment profiles of children with externalizing behavior
disorders and children with intellectual disabilities? Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 2642–2660. doi:
10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.010
Onishi, K. H., and Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false
beliefs? Science 308, 255–258. doi: 10.1126/science.1107621
Oppenheimer, L., and Rempt, E. (1986). Social cognitive development with moderately and
severely retarded children. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 7, 237–249. doi: 10.1016/0193-
3973%2886%2990032-8
O'Reilly, K., Peterson, C. C., and Wellman, H. M. (2014). Sarcasm and advanced theory of mind
understanding in children and adults with prelingual deafness. Dev. Psychol. 50, 1862–1877. doi:
10.1037/a0036654
19
Payne, J. M., Porter, M., Pride, N. A., and North, K. N. (2016). Theory of mind in children with
neurofibromatosis type 1. Neuropsychology 30, 439–448. doi: 10.1037/neu0000262
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., and Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief:
the case for a conceptual deficit. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 5, 125–137. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-
835X.1987.tb01048.x
Perner, J., and Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that…”: attribution of second-
order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 39, 437–471. doi:
10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7
Peskin, J., Prusky, C., and Comay, J. (2014). Keeping the reader's mind in mind: development of
perspective-taking in children's dictations. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 35, 35–43. doi:
10.1016/j.appdev.2013.11.001
Peterson, C. C., Garnett, M., Kelly, A., and Attwood, T. (2009). Everyday social and
conversation applications of theory-of-mind understanding by children with autism-spectrum
disorders or typical development. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 18, 105–115. doi:
10.1007/s00787-008-0711-y
Poulin-Dubois, D., and Yott, J. (2014). Executive functions and theory of mind understanding in
young children: a reciprocal relation? Psychol. Franc. 59, 59–69. doi:
10.1016/j.psfr.2013.11.002
Powell, L. J., Hobbs, K., Bardis, A., Carey, S., and Saxe, R. (2018). Replications of implicit
theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands. Cogn. Dev. 46, 40–50. doi:
10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.10.004
Premack, D., and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav.
Brain Sci. 1, 515–526. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512
Reed, T. (1994). Performance of autistic and control subjects on three cognitive perspective-
taking tasks. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 53–66. doi: 10.1007/BF02172212
Repacholi, B. M., and Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and
18-month-olds. Dev. Psychol. 33, 12–21. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12
Ribordy, S. C., Camras, L. A., Stefani, R., and Spaccarelli, S. (1988). Vignettes for emotion
recognition research and affective therapy with children. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 17, 322–325.
doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp1704_4
Ricard, M., Girouard, P. C., and Decarie, T. G. (1999). Personal pronouns and perspective taking
in toddlers. J. Child Lang. 26, 681–697. doi: 10.1017/s0305000999003943
Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., Koops, W., Stegge, H., and Oomen, A. (2001). Preschoolers'
appreciation of uncommon desires and subsequent emotions. Brit. J. Dev. Psychol. 19, 259–274.
doi: 10.1348/026151001166065
Riggio, M. M., and Cassidy, K. W. (2009). Preschoolers' processing of false beliefs within the
context of picture book reading. Early Educ. Dev. 20, 992–1015. doi:
10.1080/10409280903375685
20
Ruffman, T., and Olson, D. (1989). Children's ascriptions of knowledge to others. Dev.
Psychol. 25, 601–606. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.4.601
Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., and Tidswell, T. (1991). The “windows task” as a measure
of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects. Br. J. Dev. Psychology 9, 331–349.
doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1991.tb00881.x
Sabbagh, M. A., and Paulus, M. (2018). Replication studies of implicit false belief with infants
and toddlers. Cogn. Dev. 46, 1–3. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.07.003
Schult, C. A. (2002). Children's understanding of the distinction between intentions and
desires. Child Dev. 73, 1727–1747. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00502
Scott, R. M., and Baillargeon, R. (2017). Early false-belief understanding. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21,
237–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.012
Senju, A., Southgate, V., Snape, C., Leonard, M., and Csibra, G. (2011). Do 18-month-olds
really attribute mental states to others? A critical test. Psychol. Sci. 22, 878–880. doi:
10.1177/0956797611411584
Shaked, M., and Yirmiya, N. (2004). Matching procedures in autism research: evidence from
meta-analytic studies. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 35–40. doi:
10.1023/B:JADD.0000018072.42845.83
Slaughter, V., Imuta, K., Peterson, C., and Henry, J. D. (2015). Meta-analysis of theory of mind
and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Dev. 86, 1159–1174. doi:
10.1111/cdev.12372
Slick, D. J. (2006). “Psychometrics in neuropsychological assessment,” in A Compendium of
Neuropsychological Tests, eds E. Strauss, E. M. S. Sherman, and O. Spreen (Oxford: Oxford
University Press), 3–32.
Smogorzewska, J., Szumski, G., and Grygiel, P. (2019). The children's social understanding
scale: an advanced analysis of a parent-report measure for assessing theory of mind in Polish
children with and without disabilities. Dev. Psychol. 55, 835–845. doi: 10.1037/dev0000673
Snodgrass, C., and Knott, F. (2006). Theory of mind in children with traumatic brain
injury. Brain Injury 20, 825–833. doi: 10.1080/02699050600832585
Southgate, V., Senju, A., and Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false
belief by 2-year-olds. Psychol. Sci. 18, 587–592. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x
Sprong, M., Schothorst, P., Vos, E., Hox, J., and van Engeland, H. (2007). Theory of mind in
schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br. J. Psychiatry 191, 5–13. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.035899
Sprung, M. (2010). Clinically relevant measures of children's theory of mind and knowledge
about thinking: non-standard and advanced measures. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 15, 204–216.
doi: 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2010.00568.x
Stanzione, C., and Schick, B. (2014). Environmental language factors in theory of mind
development: evidence from children who are deaf/hard-of-hearing or who have specific
language impairment. Top. Lang. Disord. 34, 296–312. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000038
21
Steinmann, E., Schmalor, A., Prehn-Kristensen, A., Wolff, S., Galka, A., Mohring, J., et al.
(2014). Developmental changes of neuronal networks associated with strategic social decision-
making. Neuropsychologia 56, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.025
Stewart, E., Catroppa, C., and Lah, S. (2016). Theory of mind in patients with epilepsy: a
systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 26, 3–24. doi: 10.1007/s11065-015-
9313-x
Tager-Flusberg, H., and Sullivan, K. (1994). Predicting and explaining behavior: a comparison
of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 35, 1059–1075.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01809.x
Tager-Flusberg, H., and Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: evidence
from Williams syndrome. Cognition 76, 59–90. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00069-X
Tahiroglu, D., Moses, L. J., Carlson, S. M., Mahy, C. E., Olofson, E. L., and Sabbagh, M. A.
(2014). The children's social understanding scale: construction and validation of a parent-report
measure for assessing individual differences in children's theories of mind. Dev. Psychol. 50,
2485–2497. doi: 10.1037/a0037914
Tardif, T., Wellman, H. M., and Cheung, K. M. (2004). False belief understanding in Cantonese-
speaking children. J. Child Lang. 31, 779–800. doi: 10.1017/S0305000904006580
Tashakkori, A., and Teddlie, C. (Eds.). (2010). SAGE Handbook of Mixed Methods in Social &
Behavioral Research. 2nd Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Taylor, M. (1988). Conceptual perspective taking: children's ability to distinguish what they
know from what they see. Child Dev. 59, 703–718. doi: 10.2307/1130570
Terwee, C. B., Bot, S. D. M., de Boer, M. R., van der Windt, D. A. W. M., Knol, D. L., Dekker,
J., et al. (2007). Quality criteria were proposed for measurement properties of health status
questionnaires. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 60, 34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.03.012
Veneziano, E., and Christian, H. (2006). Etats Internes, Fausse Croyance et Explications Dans
les Récits: Effets de l'étayage chez les Enfants de 4 à 12 ans. (Langage et l'Homme), 117–138.
Vera-Estay, E., Dooley, J.J., and Beauchamp, M.H. (2015). Cognitive underpinnings of moral
reasoning in adolescence: the contribution of executive functions. J. Moral Educ. 44, 17–33. doi:
10.1080/03057240.2014.986077
Viranyi, Z., Topal, J., Miklosi, A., and Csanyi, V. (2006). A nonverbal test of knowledge
attribution: a comparative study on dogs and children. Anim. Cogn. 9, 13–26. doi:
10.1007/s10071-005-0257-z
Vuadens, P. (2005). The anatomical bases of the theory of mind: a review. Schweiz. Arch.
Neurol. Psychiatr. 156, 136–146. doi: 10.4414/sanp.2005.01600
Walz, N. C., Yeates, K. O., Taylor, H., Stancin, T., and Wade, S. L. (2010). Theory of mind
skills 1 year after traumatic brain injury in 6- to 8-year-old children. J. Neuropsychol. 4, 181–
195. doi: 10.1348/174866410X488788
22
Wang, Y., Liu, H., and Su, Y. (2014). Development of preschoolers' emotion and false belief
understanding: a longitudinal study. Soc. Behav. Pers. 42, 645–654. doi:
10.2224/sbp.2014.42.4.645
Wellman, H. A., Cross, D., and Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind development:
the truth about false belief. Child Dev. 72, 655–684. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00304
Whitehouse, A., and Hird, K. (2004). Is grammatical competence a precondition for belief-desire
reasoning? Evidence from typically developing children and those with autism. Adv. Speech
Lang. Pathol. 6, 39–51. doi: 10.1080/14417040410001669480
Williamson, R. A., Brooks, R., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2015). The sound of social cognition:
Toddlers' understanding of how sound influences others. J. Cogn. Dev. 16, 252–260. doi:
10.1080/15248372.2013.824884
Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining
function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–
128. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
Yagmurlu, B., Berument, S. K., and Celimli, S. (2005). The role of institution and home contexts
in theory of mind development. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 26, 521–537. doi:
10.1016/j.appdev.2005.06.004
Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., and Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Meta-analyses comparing
theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and
normally developing individuals. Psychol. Bull. 124, 283–307. doi: 10.1037/0033-
2909.124.3.283
Ziatabar Ahmadi, S. Z., Jalaie, S., and Ashayeri, H. (2015). Validity and reliability of published
comprehensive theory of mind tests for normal preschool children: a systematic review. Iran. J.
Psychiatry 10, 214–224.
23