FINALThesisofShawnaScott Sept252012
FINALThesisofShawnaScott Sept252012
FINALThesisofShawnaScott Sept252012
net/publication/244989364
CITATIONS READS
3 2,093
1 author:
Shawna Scott
Saskatchewan Health Authority
8 PUBLICATIONS 14 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Shawna Scott on 03 June 2014.
by
A Thesis
Submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies
through the Department of Psychology
in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for
the Degree of Master of Arts at the
University of Windsor
2012
Parenting Styles
by
Shawna Scott
APPROVED BY:
______________________________________________
Dr. K. Babb
Department of Psychology
______________________________________________
Dr. B. Barrett
Department of Social Work and Women’s Studies
______________________________________________
Dr. J. Hakim-Larson, Advisor
Department of Psychology
______________________________________________
Chair of Defense
DECLARATION OF ORIGINALITY
I hereby certify that I am the sole author of this thesis and that no part of this
I certify that, to the best of my knowledge, my thesis does not infringe upon
anyone’s copyright nor violate any proprietary rights and that any ideas, techniques,
quotations, or any other material from the work of other people included in my thesis,
material that surpasses the bounds of fair dealing within the meaning of the Canada
Copyright Act, I certify that I have obtained a written permission from the copyright
owner(s) to include such material(s) in my thesis and have included copies of such
I declare that this is a true copy of my thesis, including any final revisions, as
approved by my thesis committee and the Graduate Studies office, and that this thesis has
not been submitted for a higher degree to any other University or Institution.
iii
ABSTRACT
Parents’ meta-emotion philosophy (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1996) includes their
thoughts and feelings about emotions. The Meta-Emotion Interview (Katz & Gottman,
emotion socialization practices. Based on the interview, long and short form Likert-type
measures have been developed (see Gottman & DeClaire, 1997; Hakim-Larson, Parker,
Lee, Goodwin, & Voelker, 2006; Paterson, Babb, Camodeca, Goodwin, Hakim-Larson,
Voelker, & Gragg, 2012). The purpose of the present study was to evaluate the construct
validity of the questionnaires in conjunction with the original interview. Archival data
included 33 mothers with at least one child between ages 3 to 5. When mothers’ scores
on the questionnaires correlated with dimensions scores on the interview, the coefficients
were in the expected directions, suggesting further evidence for the construct validity of
the long and short forms. Additional findings and study implications are discussed.
iv
DEDICATION
Mom, you are a source of solace and unconditional love. Dad, you always bring humour
into my life when I need a boost. To my siblings Ryan, Stephanie, Jamie, Christopher,
and Tyler: as the eldest of six children, I am supposed to be the leader of the pack. One
day, I hope you will all realize that you are the ones who inspire me. Dereck, you have
encouraged me every step of the way. My life would certainly be quite different without
you, and I thank you for bringing out the best in me. To my friends who have stood by
v
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I thank Dr. Julie Hakim-Larson for being a remarkable supervisor and teacher
throughout this research process. I appreciate her for providing me with support and
positivity at just the right moments. I would also like to express appreciation to my
committee members, Dr. Kimberley Babb and Dr. Betty Barrett, for their invaluable
guidance and sincere desire for this study to succeed. Next, I would like to thank the
Ashley, Brianne, Silvia, Kristen, Gillian, Erin, Aman, and Melissa all remained dedicated
to the noble task of transcribing dozens of lengthy interviews. If it were not for all of
you, I would still be listening to audio cassette recordings at this very moment. I am also
vi
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ABSTRACT ....................................................................................................................... iv
DEDICATION .....................................................................................................................v
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ............................................................................................... vi
CHAPTER
I. INTRODUCTION......................................................................................................1
III. METHOD
Participants .........................................................................................29
Procedure ............................................................................................29
Measures .............................................................................................31
IV. RESULTS
Overview of Results ...........................................................................48
Data Screening ....................................................................................48
vii
Preliminary Analyses ..........................................................................49
Main Analyses ....................................................................................55
Additional Analyses............................................................................63
Examples from Mothers’ and Fathers’ Meta-Emotion Interviews .....69
V. DISCUSSION
Construct Validity of the Emotion-Related Parenting Styles Self-Test
– Likert (ERPSST-L) ................................................................74
Construct Validity of the Emotion-Related Parenting Styles (ERPS) 76
Limitations and Directions for Future Research .................................78
REFERENCES ..................................................................................................................86
APPENDICES...................................................................................................................95
APPENDIX A
Inter-Rater Reliability for Coding of Meta-Emotion Interviews
(MEIs) for Coding Training, as Measured by Intra-Class
Correlation Coefficients ............................................................95
APPENDIX B
Inter-Rater Reliability for Coding of Meta-Emotion Interviews
(MEIs) for Present Study, as Measured by Intra-Class
Correlation Coefficients ............................................................96
APPENDIX C
Comparison of Scores on Meta-Emotion Interview Dimensions
between High and Low Emotion Coaching Groups, based on
Emotion-Related Parenting Styles Self-Test – Likert (ERPSST-
L) Scores ...................................................................................97
APPENDIX D
Comparison of Scores on Meta-Emotion Interview Dimensions
between High and Low Emotion Coaching Groups, based on
Emotion-Related Parenting Styles (ERPS) Scores ...................98
viii
LIST OF TABLES
Table 8 Means, Standard Deviations, and Cronbach’s Alpha Coefficients for the
Emotion-Related Parenting Styles Self-Test – Likert (ERPSST-L)..........43
Table 10 Means, Standard Deviations, and Cronbach’s Alpha Coefficients for the
Emotion-Related Parenting Styles (ERPS)………....................................47
Table 12 Zero-Order Correlations between Child Age, Child Sex, Family Income,
and Scores on the Meta-Emotion Interview, Emotion-Related Parenting
Styles Self-Test – Likert (ERPSST-L), and Emotion-Related Parenting
Styles (ERPS)............................................................................................53
ix
Table 14 One-Tailed Partial Correlations between Meta-Emotion Interview Child
Dimensions and Emotion-Related Parenting Styles Self-Test – Likert
(ERPSST-L) Subscales Controlling for Child Age, Child Sex, and Family
Income…………….……………...............................................................58
x
LIST OF FIGURES
xi
CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION
Parents teach their children how to express, cope with, and respond to emotions in
thoughts and feelings about emotions and has been found to translate into emotion
socialization practices. The Meta-Emotion Interview (Katz & Gottman, 1986) has been
the principal way to measure this construct. In this interview, parents describe their past
and current experiences with the emotions of sadness, anger, and fear; goals in teaching
their children about these emotions; and understanding how their children express and
cope with emotions. This interview produces continuous scores on dimensions including
parents’ awareness, acceptance, and regulation of their own emotions, as well as the
three questionnaire versions were developed based on the framework by Gottman, Katz,
and Hooven (1996): the true-false version (Gottman & DeClaire, 1997), the long form
Likert scale called the Emotion-Related Parenting Styles Self-Test – Likert (ERPSST-L;
Gottman & DeClaire, 1997, modified by Hakim-Larson, Parker, Lee, Goodwin, &
Voelker, 2006), and the short form Likert scale called the Emotion Related Parenting
Styles (ERPS; Gottman & DeClaire, 1997, modified by Paterson, Babb, Camodeca,
Each item on the ERPSST-L describes one of the four emotion-related parenting styles
1
originally identified by Gottman, Katz, and Hooven (1996): emotion coaching, laissez-
questionnaire that is a subset of the items in the ERPSST-L. The ERPS produces
To date, these two measures have been evaluated in terms of internal validity,
convergent validity, and some preliminary construct validity. The purpose of the present
study was to test the construct validity of both the ERPSST-L and the ERPS. Construct
validity would be demonstrated if scores on the ERPSST-L and ERPS correlate with
theory.
The following sections in the present paper include reviews of the process of
parenting styles. The Meta-Emotion Interview and three measures of parental meta-
emotion will be described, followed by the objectives, rationale, and hypotheses of the
present study.
2
CHAPTER II
REVIEW OF LITERATURE
development of children. For young children, parents are considered to be the most
socialization was largely influenced by Haim Ginott (1965), who emphasized that
socializers can teach children to understand emotions by using empathy and respectful
can influence parenting behaviours just like parenting behaviours can influence children
(Saarni, 1999). For example, a mother may adjust her interactions with her child based
may adjust her interactions with her child based on how she perceives her child’s
tendencies in dealing with emotions (Fabes, Eisenberg, Karbon, Bernzweig, Speer, &
Carlo, 1994). To illustrate this bidirectional process, Fabes et al. (1994) assessed
dyads during a storytelling task. Child age differences were found in which mothers of
the young children were more likely to attempt to induce a positive mood in the children
in order to minimize their unpleasant responses. Because younger children are often
thought to have emotional skills that are less advanced compared to older children, child
age influenced parenting behaviours. It is also relevant to note that this tendency
3
primarily occurred when mothers of young children believed that the child would likely
become emotionally aroused in the storytelling task. Additionally, this finding by Fabes
et al. (1994) suggested that parental attitudes and beliefs do play a role in a parent’s
beliefs of parents, can be direct or indirect (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Cumberland, 1998),
and the recognition of the need to examine these factors has been increasing (Dunsmore,
Her, Halberstadt, & Perez-Rivera, 2009). Direct socialization involves the behaviours of
the socializer that reflect his or her cognitions and goals related to emotions (Eisenberg,
Cumberland, & Spinrad, 1998). The three main ways parents directly socialize emotional
however, it is not a direct reflection of the socializer’s beliefs and goals related to
emotion (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Cumberland, 1998). Thus, emotion socialization occurs
experience of emotions (Gottman et al., 1996). Measuring this construct can be useful in
better understanding how one responds to negative emotions in self and others. A
negative emotion is not necessarily one that is bad or maladaptive. The term negative
emotion is used to describe the emotions that are typically unpleasant (e.g., sadness,
4
anger, and fear). Positive emotions are those that are typically pleasant (e.g., happiness).
way to understand how parents react during potentially stressful situations based on their
own traits, the nature of the situation, and traits of the child (Hakim-Larson, Dunham,
(Jäger &Bartsch, 2006). Jäger and Bartsch (2006) pointed out that there is a need to
emotion and their style of communicating emotions with their children (Gottman et al.,
1996). Gottman and colleagues (1996) defined parental meta-emotion philosophy as ―an
organized set of feelings and thoughts about one’s own emotions and one’s children’s
when examining a parent’s verbal and nonverbal emotion socialization practices and
philosophy is associated with socialization behaviour when mothers interact with their
children ages 4-5 (Gottman et al., 1996). Katz, Gottman, and Hooven (1996) posited that
the exploration of parental meta-emotion philosophy can aid in understanding the relation
5
Eisenberg (1996) brought up an important question: how does a parent’s meta-
acceptability of emotions and belief about active emotion socialization. These two
dimensions can better explain the construct of meta-emotion. Under the framework of
meta-emotion theory, one may recognize how a parent’s understanding and awareness of
emotions can translate into socialization practices (Hakim-Larson et al., 2006; Katz,
Gottman et al. (1996) also suggested that a parent’s emotional awareness and
coaching can relate to his or her parenting behaviours, but may also lead directly to child
outcome. For instance, Gottman and colleagues (1996) found that children of emotion
coaching parents at age five were predicted to be rated as socially competent by teachers
at age eight.
coaching parents are high in emotional awareness, acceptance, regulation (Gottman &
DeClaire, 1997), and coaching (Gottman et al., 1996) of their children’s emotions. For
labelling and validating emotions, offering guidance for emotion regulation, and by
6
teaching problem-solving skills (Gottman & DeClaire, 1997). Characteristically, this
parenting style is used by parents who have a healthy relationship with their spouse.
These parents also feel comfortable with their own emotions and support their children,
Gottman and his colleagues (1996), outcomes for emotion-coached children are positive
in that they experience less stress and illness, have better self-regulation skills, higher
levels of academic achievement, and more positive relationships with peers. Children of
parents who adopt an emotion-coaching parenting style tend to develop strong emotion
are typically high in emotional awareness and acceptance but low in emotional regulation
and coaching of their children’s emotions (Gottman & DeClaire, 1997). Compared to
parents. Laissez-faire parents rarely set limits on behaviour and are unlikely to teach
that negative emotions are harmful (Gottman et al., 1996). Such parents are low in
emotional awareness, acceptance, coaching, and regulation (Gottman & DeClaire, 1997).
They would much rather avoid addressing negative emotions at all. Children of emotion
dismissing parents may face difficulty in solving socio-emotional problems and may
learn that emotions such as sadness, anger, and fear are wrong to experience and express,
7
Emotion disapproving parenting. This is considered the harshest of the four
styles in that criticism and punishment may be used when the child expresses
disapproved emotions. Parents using this style are low in emotional awareness,
acceptance, regulation, and coaching (Gottman & DeClaire, 1997). It is important to note
that a disapproving parenting style can lead to particular difficulties for children.
Children may be less emotionally and socially competent (Gottman & DeClaire, 1997)
and tend to have elevated anxiety and poor emotion regulation (Lagacé-Séguin & Coplan,
2005).
styles differ from general parenting styles. Baumrind (1971) established four general
general parenting styles, emotion-related parenting styles determine how parents set
guidelines related to the emotional experiences of the child. Eisenberg (1996) supported
the notion that there is, for example, a distinction between parental derogation (related to
8
establish a structured environment and provide praise and approval in response to a
taped interview that begins by evaluating parents’ past and present experience with
sadness, anger, and fear. Parents are then asked to describe their children’s experience
with those emotions. Next, parents look at a list of emotions and discuss an emotion they
prefer, an emotion they dislike the most, as well as the emotion with which the child has
the most difficulty. Finally, parents verbally summarize what they are trying to teach
their children about emotions, in general. This interview often has been used by
researchers in the last decade due to its ability to generate rich data on parental meta-
emotion philosophy.
settings and has been used to assess associations between emotion-related parenting
styles and child outcome of social skills and adjustment. For example, the Meta-Emotion
Interview was used to evaluate emotion socialization processes and child outcome in
understanding for boys and girls, some components of emotional understanding differed
emotion socialization and the internalizing behaviour of boys. For girls, emotional
understanding was a mediator between maternal emotion socialization and social skills.
Additionally, they found that emotion regulation mediated emotion socialization in boys’
9
adjustment, but not for girls. The Meta-Emotion Interview also has been used to assess
the relation between meta-emotion philosophy and child outcome in families with
domestic violence (e.g., Katz, Hunter, & Klowden, 2008; Katz & Windecker-Nelson,
2006). One interesting finding from the study by Katz and colleagues (2008) was that
emotion coaching in mothers may actually function as a buffer for children exposed to
peer provocation in a more adaptive, less negative manner when compared to children of
mothers low in emotion coaching. In another study, the Meta-Emotion Interview was
used to evaluate the emotion regulation of children, as well as the emotion socialization
of mothers who physically maltreat their children and mothers who do not physically
maltreat their children (Shipman, Schneider, Fitzgerald, Sims, Siwsher, & Edwards,
2007). Shipman and colleagues (2007) found that mothers who physically maltreated
their children tended to use less emotion coaching when their children displayed negative
According to Shipman et al. (2007), mothers who physically maltreated their children
emotions may be invalidated and fewer adaptive emotion regulation strategies may be
Interview has been described as the gold-standard measure of the construct of parental
(2001), construct validity ―comprises the evidence and rationales indicating the degree to
10
which data from an assessment instrument measures the targeted construct; includes all
evidence bearing on the measure and encompasses all types of validity‖ (p. 239). In
other words, construct validity demonstrates how well variables of interest represent the
construct (Cherulnik, 2001). According to Clark and Watson (1995), construct validity is
Meta-emotion also has been studied by examining parents’ scores on the Meta-
Emotion Interview and peer relations among children with conduct problems (Katz &
Windecker-Nelson, 2004). Even for children who are aggressive, more positive play
with peers occurred when mothers were higher in emotion awareness and coaching.
Meta-emotion also has been examined in families with older children and
adolescents. In one study, the Meta-Emotion Interview was used to examine the relation
temperament (Yap, Allen, Leve, & Katz, 2008). Yap et al. (2008) found that maternal
during mothers’ interactions with their adolescent-aged children. Further, the Meta-
families of adolescents with depressive symptomatology (Katz & Hunter, 2007). The
sample contained thirty dyads of adolescents and their mothers. Results suggested that
adolescents of mothers who scored high in acceptance of their own emotions were more
higher in self-esteem. Overall, these findings suggest that the Meta-Emotion Interview
11
As interest in meta-emotion philosophy was generated, Gottman and DeClaire
(1997) wrote a parenting book called Raising an Emotionally Intelligent Child: The Heart
measure – entitled ―A Self-Test: What Style of Parent are you?‖ – parents could assess
and determine their primary emotion-related parenting style. This measure was later
Parenting Styles Self-Test – Likert (ERPSST-L; Gottman & DeClaire, 1997, modified by
Hakim-Larson et al., 2006) and also a short form Likert-type questionnaire called
Paterson, Babb, Camodeca, Goodwin, Hakim-Larson, Voelker, & Gragg, 2012). The
construct validity of the long form and the short form has yet to be tested in conjunction
This is an 81-item true/false self-report measure (Gottman & DeClaire, 1997) that
on this measure include, ―I think it’s good for kids to feel angry sometimes‖ and ―I think
sadness is okay as long as it’s under control‖ (p. 42-48). An average score for each scale
is calculated. A parent’s primary emotion-related parenting style is the one that results in
the highest average score. Scoring produces a continuous score on each of the emotion-
12
related parenting styles. All parents have the four emotion-related parenting styles to
some extent, but it is a matter of degree. Typically, average Likert-type scores for each
consistency was found to be from .33 to .87 over the four parenting styles, with the
laissez-faire scale as the weakest. Social desirability was found to relate to variables, and
was controlled for in analyses. Test-retest reliability after two to three months was good,
suggesting that a parents’ primarily endorsed style is somewhat stable over time.
Evidence for test-retest reliability and internal consistency reliability were found (Lee,
Hakim-Larson, & Voelker, 2000). Hakim-Larson and colleagues (2006) found that the
emotion coaching parenting style was endorsed most often (in 91 out of 100 parents from
the first administration) and the remaining nine were laissez-faire. Lee (1999) used a
confirmatory factor analysis to test the construct validity of the ERPSST-T/F measure.
Construct validity would have been supported if four parenting style constructs were
represented by the ERPSST-T/F. However, Lee (1999) found the model fit of the
dichotomous response formats have been criticized extensively in the literature due to
their tendency to be less reliable and less stable than ones with multiple choices. Though
the ERPSST-T/F was useful in the sense that it was quick to administer, it required
reconstruction due to its poor psychometric properties. This led to the development of
13
Emotion-Related Parenting Styles Self-Test – Likert (ERPSST-L)
2006, reproduced with permission of Simon & Schuster) was developed by converting
the true/false measure (ERPSST-T/F) into a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = always false, 2
= mostly false, 3 = somewhat true/false, 4 = mostly true, and 5 = always true). All 81
items from the ERPSST-T/F remained on this scale to measure parents’ typical style of
teaching their children about emotion. Average Likert-type scores for each subscale are
parenting style. A parent’s primary emotion-related parenting style is the subscale that
to .91), showing improvement over the ERPSST-T/F. After controlling for social
desirability and parent gender, Hakim-Larson and colleagues (2006) found support for
Halberstadt Cassidy, Stifter, Parke, & Fox, 1995), attitudes toward children’s emotional
expressiveness (Saarni, 1985), and ability to cope with negative emotions (Fabes,
Eisenberg, & Bernzweig, 1990). Partial correlation analyses in the study by Hakim-
Larson et al. (2006) produced statistically significant positive correlations between scores
14
encouragement; positive correlations between scores on the laissez-faire subscale and
Mills, Freeman, Clara, Elgar, Walling, and Mak (2007) to examine parents’ proneness to
behaviour. The sample included 198 mothers and fathers of preschoolers. Mills et al.
(2007) used a principal components analysis and produced the expected four components
of the ERPSST-L. Because they wanted a measure of parents’ negative approach to the
child, they only used the disapproval scale for their main analyses. They found that this
scale had a significant, positive correlation with measures of spousal overprotection, self-
criticism, criticism towards spouse, guilt, shame, worry about danger, worry about
scale of the ERPSST-L, producing alphas of .88 for mothers and .85 for fathers (Walling,
Mills, & Freeman, 2007). In this study of parenting cognitions and parental use of
psychological control, Walling et al. (2007) found that for fathers of girls the disapproval
of negative emotions predicted a parent’s use of guilt/shame induction. This finding did
not occur for fathers of boys. In addition, they also found that maternal and paternal
15
sensitivity to hurtful messages and disapproval of negative emotions were related to an
increased use of psychological control (i.e., parental intrusion and manipulation of their
specific emotion-related parenting styles. Recently, the ERPSST-L was transformed into
a short-form measure called the Emotion-Related Parenting Styles (ERPS). Despite the
utility of the ERPSST-L, no study to date has tested the construct validity of the
The ERPS (Gottman & DeClaire, 1997, modified by Paterson et al., 2012) is a 20-
item short-form questionnaire. Items on the ERPS are a subset of selected items from the
long form ERPSST-L. The ERPS produces scores on four different emotion-related
socialization.
The sample used by Paterson et al. (2012) consisted of 107 mothers of children
measure were satisfactory in both samples, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .70 to
.80. The authors found support for convergent validity in that the ERPS subscales
correlated in the expected directions with subscales of the Coping with Children’s
Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES) and subscales of the Parent Attitude toward
Children’s Expressiveness Scale (PACES). The relation between scales on the ERPSST-
16
L and ERPS, as found by Paterson and colleagues (2012) are found in Table 1. The
ERPS was later revised to include gender-neutral language, removing any pronouns from
Emotion coaching. Like the emotion coaching subscale from the ERPSST-L, the
emotion coaching subscale of the ERPS is used to assess a parent’s acceptance of his or
her child’s emotional expression and desire to teach the child about emotions (e.g.,
―When my child is sad, I try to help the child explore what is making him or her sad‖).
Paterson et al. (2012) found a positive correlation between the emotion coaching subscale
and that of the ERPSST-L, r = .75, p < .001. Thus, descriptors of emotion coaching
appear to remain quite consistent with the emotion coaching subscale of the ERPSST-L.
which parents reject their children’s experience of negative emotions (e.g., ―When my
child gets angry, my goal is to get him or her to stop‖). Paterson et al. (2012) found
evidence for collapsing the dismissing and disapproving styles to produce the parental
rejection subscale. They found that this subscale correlated with the dismissing (r = .68,
p < .001) and disapproving (r = .71, p < .001) subscales of the ERPSST-L.
scale tend to accept negative emotions but provide little guidance in helping the child
work through those emotions (e.g., ―I think it’s good for kids to feel angry sometimes‖).
Paterson et al. (2012) reported that parental acceptance positively correlated with the
ERPSST-L’s emotion coaching (r = .67, p < .001) and laissez-faire (r = .32, p < .001)
subscales, and negatively correlated with dismissing (r = -.27, p < .001) and disapproving
17
Table 1
L) and the Emotion-Related Parenting Styles (ERPS) adapted from Paterson et al. (2012)
ERPS Subscales
Permission to reproduce nonsignificant coefficients from the original data set was granted
by A. Paterson (personal communication, September 14, 2012). *p < .05, **p < .01,
18
Feelings of uncertainty/ineffectiveness in emotion socialization. This subscale
is unique to the ERPS in that parents who endorse this subscale typically feel uncertain or
ineffective with regards to handling their child’s experience of negative emotions (e.g.,
―When my child is angry, I’m not quite sure what he or she wants me to do‖). This
coaching subscale (r = -.24, p = .001), but positively correlated with the laissez-faire (r =
.38, p < .001), dismissing (r = .47, p < .001), and disapproving (r = .45, p < .001)
Aside from Paterson et al. (2012), there has been one other attempt to adapt the
measure called the Maternal Emotional Styles Questionnaire (MESQ). Parents rated their
disagree). This measure was found to be internally consistent, correlated with the Meta-
Emotion Interview, and established convergent validity with parental goals that are
philosophies. There are two known published studies that included the MESQ as a
measure. In the first, Lagacé-Séguin and Gionet (2009) found that parental meta-
emotion, as assessed by the MESQ, and temperament were predictors of coping skills for
adolescents aged 10-13 years. In a second study that used the MESQ, Baker, Fenning,
and Crnic (2010) examined relations among various parental emotion socialization
19
mothers and 76 fathers) of 8-year-old children. They found that paternal emotion
coaching attitudes, which were associated with the social competence of the children,
predicted their reactions to child emotion, emotional expression in the family, and use of
an emotion-coaching approach. The 20-item ERPS differs from the 22-item MESQ in
several ways. First of all, the ERPS assesses four theoretical meta-emotion philosophies,
Additionally, the development of the ERPS began with a smaller pool of items (81) than
Testing the Construct Validity of the Long Form (ERPSST-L) and Short Form
(ERPS)
The purpose of the present study was to test the construct validity of both the
ERPSST-L (long form) and the ERPS (short form) in conjunction with the original Meta-
Emotion Interview. Though the ERPSST-L and ERPS scales may be useful to
researchers, there is a need to determine if scores on these scales correlate with scores on
the Meta-Emotion Interview in the expected directions. In order to test the construct
validity of the long form questionnaire, scores on the ERPSST-L were compared to
emotions, as well as the parents’ awareness, acceptance, coaching, and regulation of their
the anticipated direction of correlation. Finding these predicted relationships would show
that construct validity has been established (Crano & Brewer, 2002), and the ERPSST-L
20
parental meta-emotion philosophy, it is optimal to examine whether scores on the new
The next aim of the present study was to evaluate the construct validity of the
short form ERPS by comparing subscale scores to the dimension scores of the Meta-
Emotion Interview. If scores correlate in the expected direction, it would provide support
for the construct validity of the ERPS as a short-form measure of parental meta-emotion.
Study Rationale
A summary of all measures used in the present study and their subscales is
evaluate the ERPSST-L because ―such self-report measures take less time and fewer
resources to administer and score than lengthy, structured interviews, and could
Larson et al. (2006) also stated ―future studies on the ERPSST-L will need to include a
comparison of parents’ scores on the four self-report scales to the scores obtained from
the coding of the meta-emotion interview as originally developed by Gottman and his
colleagues‖ (p. 248). The objective of the present study is to meet this very need.
structured interviews are also susceptible to data loss due to technical difficulties and
insufficient prompting by interviewers, as was the case in a study that used the Meta-
problems, data analysis can be time and cost prohibitive. As found in the present study,
transcribing one Meta-Emotion Interview can take anywhere from five to twelve hours.
21
Meta-Emotion Interview “A Self-Test: What Style of Parent are
A parent is interviewed. You?”
Experiences related to sadness, Emotion-Related Parenting Styles Self-Test
anger, and fear pertaining to – True/False (ERPSST-T/F)
self and child are explored. A parent completes an 81-item dichotomous
______________________ measure
_______________________
Parent Dimensions
◦ Awareness ◦ Emotion coaching
◦ Acceptance ◦ Laissez faire
◦ Regulation ◦ Dismissing
◦ Disapproving
Child Dimensions
◦ Awareness
◦ Acceptance Emotion-Related Parenting Styles Self-Test
◦ Coaching – Likert (ERPSST-L)
◦ Regulation A parent completes a measure with 81-items
on a 5-point Likert-type scale
_______________________
◦ Emotion coaching
◦ Laissez faire
◦ Dismissing
◦ Disapproving
◦ Emotion coaching
◦ Parental rejection of negative emotion
◦ Parental acceptance of negative emotion
◦ Feelings of uncertainty/ineffectiveness in
emotion socialization
22
Further, Meta-Emotion Interview coders are required to complete extensive
training, which involves the use of manuals and audio tapes for approximately 20 hours
(Cunningham et al., 2009). Once practice tapes are completed and adequate inter-rater
reliability has been established, the Meta-Emotion Interviews need to be coded; this can
also be a lengthy process. Cunningham and colleagues (2009) reported that each
meta-emotion researchers will save time and resources. Validating self-report measures
If the ERPSST-L and ERPS are found to have good construct validity, they may
Because these are quick self-report measures, they may also be useful in pre and post-test
Hypotheses
Cunningham and colleagues (2009), the maternal emotion socialization process did not
differ for mothers of boys and mothers of girls in an African American sample.
However, they found that emotion socialization practices related to emotion regulation
for boys but not for girls. Thus, in the present study it was expected that main study
variables would differ significantly for mothers of boys and mothers of girls.
This hypothesis pertains to the expected direction of correlations between the four Meta-
23
acceptance of child’s emotions, parent coaching of child’s emotions, and parent’s account
of child’s regulation of emotion) and the four ERPSST-L subscale scores (emotion
each of these variables, with a higher score indicating greater endorsement. On specific
construct validity analyses, the focus was on the Meta-Emotion Interview child
specifically on the parent’s own management of his or her emotions. Due to the
major hypotheses for this study regarding the relation between the Meta-Emotion
Interview and the ERPSST-L. These hypotheses are all based on meta-emotion theory
parenting styles, like the ERPSST-L does. Gottman, Katz, and Hooven (1997) stated
Interview dimensions has never been explained in the literature and specific procedures
are not available. Instead of classifying emotion-related parenting styles within the Meta-
24
Table 2
ERPSST-L Subscales
Emotion Laissez- Dismissing Disapproving
Coaching Faire
Awareness Positive Positive Negative Negative
of Child’s Correlation Correlation Correlation Correlation
Emotions (Gottman & (Hakim- (Gottman & (Hakim-
DeClaire, Larson et al., DeClaire, Larson et al.,
1997, p. 63; 2006, p. 1997, p. 50; 2006, p. 231)
Hakim- 230) Hakim-
Larson et al., Larson et al.,
2006, p. 230) 2006, p. 231)
Meta- Acceptance Positive Positive Negative Negative
Emotion of Child’s Correlation Correlation Correlation Correlation
Interview Emotions (Gottman & (Hakim- (Gottman & (Gottman &
Dimensions DeClaire, Larson et al., DeClaire, DeClaire,
1997, p. 63; 2006, p. 1997, p. 50) 1997, 1997, p.
Hakim- 230) 51)
Larson et al.,
2006, p. 230)
Account of Positive Negative Negative Negative
Child’s Correlation Correlation Correlation Correlation
Regulation (Gottman & (Hakim- (Gottman & (Gottman &
of DeClaire, Larson et al., DeClaire, DeClaire,
Emotions 1997, p. 52; 2006, p. 1997, p. 56) 1997,
Hakim- 230) p. 51)
Larson et al.,
2006, p. 230)
Coaching Positive Negative Negative Negative
of Child’s Correlation Correlation Correlation Correlation
Emotions (Gottman et (Gottman & (Gottman & (Gottman &
al., 1996, p. DeClaire, DeClaire, DeClaire,
244) 1997, p. 50) 1997, p. 50) 1997, p. 51)
25
Hypotheses related to the construct validity of the ERPS short form. It was
expected that the ERPS would establish construct validity in conjunction with the original
(refer to Table 3). Consistent with the hypothesized findings for the ERPSST-L long
form, it was expected that the emotion coaching subscale of the ERPS would show a
positive correlation with the Meta-Emotion Interview child dimensions just as was
expected for the emotion coaching dimension of the ERPSST-L. Second, it was expected
that the parental rejection of negative emotion subscale would show a negative
parental acceptance of negative emotion was found to correlate highly with the ERPSST-
L’s emotion coaching subscale (Paterson et al., 2012), it was expected that parental
acceptance would show a positive correlation with the Meta-Emotion Interview child
dimensions, just as the emotion coaching subscale did. Further, acceptance of emotion is
a large component of emotion coaching. Finally, it was expected that the feelings of
with child dimensions of the Meta- Emotion Interview. The rationale for this hypothesis
is that parents high in uncertainty/ ineffectiveness may feel incompetent with regards to
emotion socialization and may, therefore, avoid or struggle with being involved in
their children.
expected directions with the Meta-Emotion Interview subscales. This was expected
26
Table 3
Hypothesized Relations between the Emotion-Related Parenting Styles (ERPS) and the Meta-Emotion Interview
ERPS Subscales
Emotion Parental Parental Rejection Uncertainty/
Coaching Acceptance Ineffectiveness
27
because meta-emotion theory is used to code the Meta-Emotion Interview, and the
emotion theory. In having a small sample size, it is possible for there to be nonsignificant
correlations between some variables. This is likely to happen on scales that have low
internal consistency.
It has been found that mothers and fathers differ in parenting practices and
emotional expression (e.g., Katz, Gottman, & Hooven, 1996). Hakim-Larson et al.
(2006) found that mothers were more likely to report higher self-expressiveness and
expressive encouragement than fathers. Fathers were significantly more likely to adopt a
dismissing parenting style. Additionally, Gottman et al. (1996) found that mothers
reported greater emotional awareness and coaching than fathers. Due to having a small
sample size in the present study, the focus on the present study was on maternal meta-
emotion philosophy.
28
CHAPTER III
METHOD
Participants
Archival data collected from the fall of 2000 to the fall of 2002 were used for the
children, University of Windsor internal research grant). Participants were recruited from
the Psychology Department participant pool (i.e., a group of undergraduate students who
mid-size university in southwestern Ontario. Spouse pairs were excluded from the
final sample included 36 parents (33 mothers, 3 fathers). Mothers (Mage = 30.97 years,
SD = 5.83) were used in all analyses but fathers were only incorporated as additional
analyses in order to inform considerations for future studies. All mothers had a child who
was between ages 3 and 5 years old (Mage = 3.91 years, SD = .84). In the event that a
parent had more than one child within this age range, the parent was asked to report on
the oldest child that fit into the study design. Complete information on participant
demographics is in Table 4.
Procedure
Parents first completed a consent form and were asked to bring home a package
containing a background information form, two counterbalanced measures, and items for
a storytelling task not used in the present study. The two counterbalanced measures
29
Table 4
Participant Demographics
Mothers Fathers
(n = 33) (n = 3)
Frequency (% Frequency (%
Feature Description of total) of total)
Age of Parent Mage = 30.97 Mage = 33.67
years years
SD = 5.83 SD = 6.81
Minage = 21 Minage = 26
Maxage = 45 Maxage = 39
Marital Status Common-Law 5 (15.2%) 1 (33.3%)
Married 17 (51.5%) 2 (66.7%)
Separated/Divorced/Widowed 3 (9.1%) 0 (0%)
Single, never married 8 (24.2%) 0 (0%)
Ethnicity Caucasian 25 (75.8%) 3 (100%)
Middle Eastern 1 (3%) 0 (0%)
Native/Aboriginal 2 (6.1%) 0 (0%)
Other 2 (6.1%) 0 (0%)
No response 3 (9.1%) 0 (0%)
Annual Family Less than $10,000 1 (3.0%) 0 (0%)
Income $11,000 to 20,000 9 (27.3%) 0 (0%)
$21,000 to 30,000 2 (6.1%) 1 (33.3%)
$31,000 to 40,000 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
$41,000 to 50,000 3 (9.1%) 0 (0%)
$51,000 to 60,000 2 (6.1%) 0 (0%)
$61,000 to 70,000 2 (6.1%) 0 (0%)
Greater than $70,000 12 (36.4%) 2 (66.7%)
No response 2 (6.1%) 0 (0%)
Employment Currently employed 18 (54.5%) 3 (100%)
Status Not currently employed 15 (45.5%) 0 (0%)
Birthplace Canada 30 (90.9%) 3 (100%)
Outside Canada 3 (9.1%) 0 (0%)
Highest level of Some college/university or less 13 (39.4%) 2 (66.7%)
education College/university graduate or 20 (60.6%) 1 (33.3%)
more
Use of Yes 8 (24.2%) 1 (33.3%)
counselling No 25 (75.8%) 1 (66.7%)
services for self
Age of Target 3 years 13 (39.4%) 0 (0%)
Child 4 years 10 (30.3%) 2 (66.7%)
5 years 10 (30.3%) 1 (33.3%)
Sex of Target Female 16 (48.5%) 2 (33.3%)
Child Male 17 (51.5%) 1 (66.7%)
30
reasons for reading that was not used in the present study. Parents returned the materials
to the researchers or a researcher picked up the materials from the family’s home.
Next, participants were invited back to the university to complete the Meta-
Emotion Interview (Katz & Gottman, 1986). Interviewers included five members of the
administering the Meta-Emotion Interview. The duration of this interview varied from
one to two hours, and participants were offered $10 in compensation for participating in
the entire study. Those parents who were students at the university received bonus points
Measures
This section begins with a description of the Meta-Emotion Interview, the long
form, and the short form measures used in the present study. Scoring procedures and the
psychometric properties from past studies are discussed for each measure. The inter-rater
reliability, mean scores and standard deviations, and internal consistency reliability for all
by the interview. The first three dimensions pertain to the parent: awareness of emotions,
child’s emotions, parent’s coaching of child’s emotions, and parent’s account of child’s
emotional regulation. All seven dimensions are further described below. Table 5
31
Table 5
Awareness of 9 9 to 36 18 to 72
child’s emotions
Acceptance of 13 13 to 65 26 to 130
child’s emotions
Coaching of 11 11 to 55 22 to 110
child’s emotions
Regulation of 9 9 to 45 18 to 90
child’s emotions
Note. Most dimension items are scored on a scale from 1 to 5 (1 = strongly disagree, 2 =
disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree, and DK = don’t know). Some items
could only be rated on a scale from 1 to 4, while others could be rated on a scale from 1
to 5.
32
Dimensions of the Meta-Emotion Interview that pertain to the parent. Three of
the seven dimensions of the Meta-Emotion Interview pertain to the parent. They are
dimension to examine the degree to which a parent is aware of his or her own emotional
important dimension in that Gottman and colleagues (1996) stated, ―we found that only
people who are aware of emotion and can differentially talk about the nuances of emotion
and emotion intensity find emotional expression to be acceptable‖ (p. 267). Those who
are high in this dimension consciously experience emotions, while those who are low in
this dimension might prefer to avoid noticing negative emotions. Such parents might see
the ―passage of time‖ as a means of resolving issues of sadness or anger (p. 267).
attitudes toward emotion regarding their own level of comfort accepting emotions. There
are 17 items on this dimension (e.g., ―Parent feels comfortable with their expression of
this emotion‖).
remediation techniques. There are 12 items on this dimension. (e.g., ―This emotion is
difficult to get over‖). Difficulty in emotion regulation has been associated with physical
and mental health problems and difficulties with marital relations (Gottman, Katz, &
Hooven, 1997).
33
Parent’s responses to child’s emotions. There are four dimensions of the Meta-
Emotion Interview that pertain to the child. They are parent’s awareness, acceptance, and
coaching of the child’s emotions, and parent’s perception of the child’s ability to regulate
emotions.
(e.g., ―Parent knows cause of child’s emotion‖). This dimension evaluates a parent’s
wants child to know it’s OK to have this feeling‖). It measures a parent’s responses (both
direct and indirect) to his/her child’s emotional expression. For instance, responses can
Parent coaching of child’s emotions. This dimension contains 11 items that tap
into a parent’s ability to show respect for and comfort the child during emotional
experiences, as well as teach age-appropriate strategies for the child to soothe his or her
own emotion (e.g., ―When child is upset, parent talks about situation, emotion‖).
Additionally, these parents aim to teach their children about the world of emotions. The
researcher can evaluate a parent’s ability to recognize his or her child’s ability to get over
the emotion. To score high on this dimension, a parent may recognize that his or her
child can self-regulate the emotion and/or identify remediation strategies that are
34
effective for the child. There are 9 items on this dimension (e.g., ―This emotion is
the Meta-Emotion Interview has been found to be adequate. For example, emotion
coaching parents are more likely to use scaffolding and praising but less derogation than
parents of other styles (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1997). Internal consistency, as
measured by Cronbach’s alpha, has been found to be moderate (Katz & Windecker-
Nelson, 2004). Katz and Windecker-Nelson (2004) found the inter-rater reliability to
range from r = .57 to r = .82 among dimensions, while Cunningham and colleagues
With the exception of one study (Cunningham et al., 2009), the transcribing of the Meta-
Emotion Interview prior to coding has rarely been reported in the literature. Typically,
other researchers only use the audio-recording of the interview when coding (e.g., Katz et
al., 2008). Transcription was beneficial because the transcripts included line numbers
that were used to document where codes occurred. Line numbers were useful in
resolving coding discrepancies. When interviews were transcribed in the present study,
between the interviewer and the interviewee, general descriptions were used instead of
35
Coding of the Meta-Emotion Interviews. After receiving ethics clearance to use
archival data, the author completed the Meta-Emotion Coding System Coding Training
by Katz, Mittman, and Embry (n.d.). The Meta-Emotion Coding System Coding
Training Manual (Katz, Mittman, & Embry, n.d.) was used to assist in making coding
decisions, and coding sheets were used to record scores. The coding system contains a
these dimensions, and possible codes for each item. Coding training involved listening to
Interview transcripts were unavailable for coding training. The duration of training was
2009).
When coding the interviews from the present study, the coder first listened to the
audio-taped interview in order to code items related to the parent’s tone of voice (e.g., in
terms of interest in the questions being asked, hesitation, and uncertainty). Next, the
coder coded each interview by listening to the tape and following along with the
transcript. The Meta-Emotion Interview was scored on sadness, anger, and the
combination scores of sadness and anger. Interview questions related to fear were
excluded because the ERPSST-L only contains items related to sadness and anger.
During coding of the Meta-Emotion Interviews, it is possible that a code may not
be applicable on an item (e.g., if the interviewer never asks the corresponding question).
In this case, a DK (don’t know) code would be provided. It is important to note that for
the Meta-Emotion Interview, scoring instructions require that ―don’t know‖ (DK)
responses are given the average score for the dimension, and the total score is adjusted.
36
For example, if the total score for Awareness of Child’s Emotions is 22 but the parent
responded with DK for 2 of the 9 items, the score is calculated as follows: 22 + (22/7)(2)
= 28.28. It is important to note that even though dimensions may have the same number
of items it is possible for the range of scores to differ due to the computation of DK
responses. For example, the awareness of child’s emotions dimension has 9 items but the
scale only ranges from 1 to 4, making the maximum score 36. In contrast, the regulation
of child’s emotions dimension has 9 items but the scale can range from 1 to 5, making the
The coder was blind to information about the parents, their children, and their
scores on other measures. However, the coder was aware of participant gender based on
the context of the interview and the audio recorded voices of the participants.
training and in the coding of the Meta-Emotion Interviews from the present study, inter-
rater reliability was calculated. Gottman and colleagues (1996) and a research assistant
in Katz’s lab (A. Maliken, personal communication, July 11, 2012) recommended using
for both rating differences and the correlation between raters (McGraw & Wong, 1996).
A two-way random Intraclass Correlation Coefficient was selected (i.e., both rater effects
and item effects are random) in order to compare ratings between the primary researcher
and each of the other raters on each dimension score. An Intraclass Correlation
Coefficient ranges from 0.00 to 1.00, and an Intraclass Correlation Coefficient of .70 or
37
Inter-rater reliability: Meta-Emotion Interview coding training. The primary
researcher coded fifteen Meta-Emotion Interviews from the Coding Training System
(Katz, Mittman, & Embry, n.d.). To calculate inter-rater reliability for the coding
training, dimension scores computed by the primary coder were first compared to scores
in the training manual (Katz, Mittman, & Embry, n.d.). The primary coder’s scores also
were compared to training scores computed by two independent coders who were trained
in the same procedure. The first author of the Coding Training System stated that
reliability tapes should be completed until coders are confident they understand the
dimensions being coded (L. F. Katz, personal communication, June 13, 2011). Results of
Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for training interviews (Appendix A) indicate that the
inter-rater reliability between the primary researcher and the developers of the Meta-
Emotion Interview was .90 on average, ranging from .68 to .97. Between the primary
coder and these two other independent coders, the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient on
the training interviews was .71 on average, ranging from .34 to .97.
Intraclass Correlation Coefficient exceeding the .70 minimum on the training data), the
coding of the Meta-Emotion Interviews from the present study began. Interviews with
mothers (n = 33) and fathers (n = 3) were coded. To calculate inter-rater reliability for
the present study, scores computed by the primary coder were compared to scores
computed previously by two other trained, independent raters. In the present study, there
were five batches of seven to eight randomly-ordered interviews, and two interviews
from each batch were tested for inter-rater reliability. Inter-rater reliability, calculated on
38
30.30% of the sample, was found to be reliable (average r = .80, with a range of .13 to
1.0). The poor inter-rater reliability of .13 was on the dimension for parent awareness of
In the case of a discrepancy between two coders’ dimension scores, item codes
were compared in order to reach a final decision. For subsequent analyses, dimension
scores from the primary coder were used, as per a recommendation from a research
assistant from Dr. Katz’s lab (A. Maliken, personal communication, July 11, 2012).
Also, the primary coder in the present study had the highest inter-rater reliability with the
Gottman lab.
Means and standard deviations for the Meta-Emotion Interview. Findings from
the present study are reported in Table 6. In order to allow for the recognition of
dimensions and child dimensions separately; additionally, they recommended that the
dimensions be examined differentially by emotion type. Thus, in the present study the
child and parent dimensions were analyzed separately. Emotions of sadness and anger
were examined separately; they also were examined together, referred to as ―combined,‖
in order to stay consistent with the literature. The means and standard deviations of the
Meta-Emotion Interview dimensions were not available from other research publications
because summary scores have often been used. These summary scores represent
aggregate variables, combining parent dimension scores with their corresponding child
dimension scores (e.g., an overall awareness score is created by adding scores on parents’
awareness of their own emotions with parent’ awareness of their children’s emotions).
39
Table 6
40
Thus, dimension scores in the present study could not be compared to dimension scores
DeClaire, 1997, modified by Hakim-Larson et al., 2006). This measure contains 81 items
related to parental meta-emotion about sadness and anger, measured on a 5-point Likert-
type scale (1 = always false, 2 = mostly false, 3 = somewhat true/false, 4 = mostly true,
Each statement on the ERPSST-L represents one of four parenting styles: emotion
coaching (23 items), laissez-faire (10 items), dismissing (25 items), and disapproving (23
items), and higher scores represent greater endorsement of that parenting style. Table 7
ERPSST-L. Means, standard deviations, and internal consistency reliability also were
calculated for the ERPSST-L (refer to Table 8). Each mother’s average Likert-type score
for each subscale was used in later analyses. To calculate an average Likert-type score,
the item responses for each individual scale were summed and then divided by the
having a predominant style of emotion coaching using the ERPSST-T/F version in their
analysis. The scale with the highest average score for each participant was designated
81.8% (n = 27) of the mothers in the current study sample using the ERPSST-L were
41
Table 7
(ERPSST-L)
Laissez-Faire 10 10 to 50 1 to 5
Dismissing 25 25 to 125 1 to 5
Disapproving 23 23 to 115 1 to 5
Note. On the ERPSST-L, parents rated their level of agreement on a scale ranging from 1
(always false), 2 (mostly false), 3 (somewhat true/false), 4 (mostly true), and 5 (always
true).
42
Table 8
Means, Standard Deviations, and Cronbach’s Alpha Coefficients for the Emotion-Related
Note. Each mother’s average Likert-type score for each subscale was used in later
analyses.
43
predominately laissez-faire, and 3% (n = 1), had an equal score on emotion coaching and
disapproving style, although some mothers had scores on these scales that were higher
Parenting Styles (ERPS). Though participants did not directly complete the ERPS,
subscale scores may be computed by extracting corresponding scores from the 20 items
subscale score on the ERPS, scores on items 34, 75, 29, 35, and 64 from the ERPSST-L
calculate a parental rejection of negative emotion subscale score, scores on items 3, 11,
14, 41, and 66 (corresponds to items 1, 4, 10, 11, and 14 on the ERPS, respectively) were
on items 38, 31, 73, 72, and 39 of the ERRPSST-L were summed. This corresponds to
items 2, 5, 9, 12, and 16 on the ERPS, respectively. Finally, a score on the feelings of
summing scores on items 53, 77, 48, 76, and 78 of the ERPSST-L (corresponds to items
7, 13, 17, 18, and 20 on the ERPS, respectively). As explained in Table 9, each ERPS
subscale score can range from 5 (low endorsement of that parenting style) to 25 (high
endorsement of that parenting style). Average scores on each subscale were calculated
and used in analyses. On both the raw scores and average subscale scores, a higher score
44
Table 9
Emotion Coaching 5 5 to 25 1 to 5
Parental Rejection 5 5 to 25 1 to 5
Parental Acceptance 5 5 to 25 1 to 5
Uncertainty/Ineffectiveness 5 5 to 25 1 to 5
Note. On the ERPS, parents rated their level of agreement on a scale ranging from 1
45
Means, standard deviations, and internal consistency reliability for the ERPS.
Finally, means, standard deviations, and internal consistency reliability were also
calculated on the ERPS. Refer to Table 10 for this information. Each mother’s average
Likert-type score for each subscale was used in later analyses. To calculate an average
Likert-type score, the item responses for each individual scale were summed and then
divided by the number of items for that scale. Mothers were classified according to
ERPS scale with the highest score; this represented their predominant emotion-related
parenting style. When examined using the ERPS, 78.8% (n = 26) of the mothers were
negative emotion group, and 6.1% (n = 2) were equally emotion coaching and accepting.
46
Table 10
Means, Standard Deviations, and Cronbach’s Alpha Coefficients for the Emotion-Related
score
Note. Each mother’s average Likert-type score for each subscale was used in later
analyses.
47
CHAPTER IV
RESULTS
Overview of Results
Analyses are divided into five main sections. The first section includes data
screening procedures related to missing data and statistical outliers. Next are the
preliminary analyses, including the assessment of attrition in the sample, testing for
assumptions to be used in the main analyses, and the identification of control variables.
The third section for main analyses consists of testing the construct validity of the
ERPSST-L long form, and testing the construct validity of the ERPS via zero-order
correlations and partial correlations. The fourth section includes additional analyses,
such as the comparing high emotion coaching and low emotion coaching groups,
examining the relation between Meta-Emotion Interview parent dimensions and child
emotion (sadness and anger). The final section ends with examples from mothers’ and
fathers’ Meta-Emotion Interviews. Only the final section contains data pertaining to
fathers.
Data Screening
Missing data. Prior to the main analyses, variables were examined in order to
identify missing data. Missing data were not found in items for the Meta-Emotion
Interview dimensions. However, missing data points on ERPSST-L items were identified
on five cases, also creating missing data points on the ERPS. When data points are
missing at random, as was the case in the present data set, one option is to use a mean
substitution for the missing values (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2012). Mean substitutions were
48
used for each missing data point, as overall assumptions and testing outcomes were not
impacted.
Outliers. Data were then examined for outliers, using a z-score of 3.29 as a
cutoff, as recommended by Field (2009). One outlier (z = -3.89) was identified for the
awareness of child sadness dimension, and another outlier (z = -3.49) was identified for
the awareness of child (combined sadness and anger) dimension. Both outliers were from
the same case. With outliers included in the data set, kurtosis was elevated on these
dimensions (z = 3.81 and z = 4.23, respectively) and skewness was within the acceptance
range. Upon further inspection, it was determined these outliers were sampled from the
target population and the case’s other scores did not indicate a pattern of a response set.
In order to reduce the impact of this variable, a score change was implemented, as
recommended by Tabachnick and Fidell (2012) and Field (2009). To implement the
score change for each variable, the two outliers were substituted with a raw score that
was one unit smaller than the lowest score on that variable. In doing so, the impact of
Preliminary Analyses
respondents but not the nonrespondents, or vice versa, it may indicate that the sample is
not representative or that a confounding variable might be present. Thus, attrition was
assessed in the present study because 49 parents completed the ERPSST-L and
questionnaire package but 10 did not return for the Meta-Emotion Interview (note that
two Meta-Emotion Interview tape were inaudible and spouse pairs were removed,
49
reducing the sample size to 36 mothers and fathers). Thus, ―respondents‖ refers to
participants who completed the study questionnaires and returned to complete the Meta-
significant differences between these groups may warrant looking further into
confounding variables.
parent sex, child age, number of children, marital status, and race) and scores on the
normality, interval-level data, and independence of observations were met (Field, 2009,
Assumptions. After using score substitution to resolve the problem with two
outliers, skewness and kurtosis values were converted into z-scores by dividing each
skewness and kurtosis value by its respective standard error, as recommended by Field
(2009). For small to moderate samples, an alpha level of .001 (i.e., an absolute value of
3.29) can be used to indicate a normal distribution (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2012).
Skewness and kurtosis values all met the assumption of normality with the exception of
3.85). Across all other variables, the range of skewness was from z = -2.93 to z = 1.80;
the range of kurtosis was from z = -1.57 to z = 2.64. Frequency histograms were visually
50
Table 11
51
inspected for normality. In general, histograms represented the normal bell curve;
however, the histogram for Meta-Emotion Interview parent awareness of sadness was
negatively skewed. On nearly all dimensions for the Meta-Emotion Interviews, skewness
values were negative, indicating that data contained scores in the higher range. In dealing
philosophy, such as parent awareness of sadness, were anticipated. One option to repair
the skewness of the parent awareness of sadness variable was to use a data
planned analyses (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2012), particularly so when only transforming
one variable. Analyses proceeded without using a data transformation but with the
used to test if hypothesized control variables (i.e., child age, child gender, and annual
(ERPS). Though child age and gender were not significantly related to ERPSST-L scores
in a study by Hakim-Larson et al. (2006), they were checked in the present sample. In
order to preserve the sample size for correlational analyses, cases were excluded on a
pairwise (analysis-by-analysis) basis. Complete findings are located in Table 12, and
52
Table 12
Zero-Order Correlations between Child Age, Child Sex, Family Income, and Scores on
the Meta-Emotion Interview, Emotion-Related Parenting Styles Self-Test – Likert
(ERPSST-L), and Emotion-Related Parenting Styles (ERPS).
53
Child age. Results of two-way correlational analyses indicated that child age was
positively related to maternal awareness of her own anger, r(31) = .40, p = .021 and was
trending towards significance with regards to the total emotion coaching score from the
Child sex. Independent sample t-tests were used to determine if mean scores on
main variables differed by child sex. On average, daughters were rated higher in terms of
their ability to regulate sadness and anger (M = 62.63, SD = 5.69) as compared to sons (M
= 58.44, SD = 8.93). This difference was not statistically significant, t(31) = 1.62, p =
Family income. When data were collected, annual family income was reported
indicated that income bracket could be split at $51, 000. Thus, a high income group (n =
16) and a low income group (n = 15) were formed based on this median split.
bracket were better able to accept their own sadness (M = 61.07, SD = 5.08) than mothers
of a lower income bracket (M = 56.30, SD = 6.16), t(29) = -2.36, p = .025, effect size of
r(29) = .42. Mothers of a higher income bracket reported being better able to regulate
their own sadness (M = 43.70, SD = 2.89) than mothers of a lower income bracket (M =
40.35, SD = 4.59), t(29) = -2.45, p = .021, effect size of r(29) = .30. On average, mothers
31.74, SD = 2.65) than mothers of a lower income (M = 35.49, SD = 4.12), t(29) = 3.03, p
54
In summary, child age, child sex, and family income correlated with some of the
main variables in the present study. Those three variables, therefore, were controlled for
Main Analyses
The purpose of the present study was to test the construct validity of the long
form and short form questionnaires by comparing subscale scores to those of the
between variables. Correlational analyses are considered appropriate for continuous data
that are normally distributed. In 1955, correlation matrices were identified by Cronbach
and Meehl as appropriate for testing construct validity. They stated, ―If two tests are
(Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). The use of correlations to test the construct validity of a
measure has been used in the literature (e.g., Paterson et al., 2012). It is highly
recommended that researchers report effect sizes and confidence intervals in addition to
the correlation coefficients in testing for construct validity (e.g., Thompson et al., 2005).
Construct validity of the ERPSST-L long form. The first purpose of the
present study was to test the construct validity of the ERPSST-L long form. Construct
validity would be demonstrated if scores on the long form correlate with scores on the
Meta-Emotion Interview in the expected directions. It was hypothesized that the emotion
coaching scale would positively correlate with all four child dimensions (i.e., awareness
emotions, and coaching of child’s emotions). It was expected that the laissez-faire scale
would positively correlate with awareness and acceptance, but would correlate negatively
55
with regulation and coaching. Finally, it was anticipated that the dismissing and
ERPSST-L, zero-order correlations were conducted as shown in Table 13. Greater use of
a dismissing style was related to lower ratings on child dimensions related to the
acceptance of emotions; the coaching of child anger, r(31) = -.44, p = .005; and the
regulation of child anger, r(31) = -.30, p = .048. Greater use of a disapproving style was
negatively related to the acceptance of child anger, r(31) = .44, p = .005 and the
regulation of child anger, r(31) = -.41, p = .009. Statistically significant relations were
not found between the Meta-Emotion Interview dimensions and the ERPSST-L emotion
ERPSST-L correlated with child age, child sex, and family. The removal of the effect of
these three variables was abbreviated and referred to as a subscripted x. Thus, one-tailed
partial correlations were computed to examine the ERPSST-L scores in relation to scores
on the Meta-Emotion Interview while controlling for child age, child sex, and family
income (refer to Table 14). These analyses were one-tailed because a specific direction
was anticipated for each analysis based on literature. A statistically significant relation
between emotion coaching and the child’s regulation of anger (r-a) was found, rEC,r-a.x(26)
= .35, p = .035. In contrast, mothers who scored high on the dismissing (DI) scale tended
to rate low on child regulation of anger, rDI,r-a.x(26) = -.37, p = .026. Similarly, mothers
who scored high on the disapproving (DA) scale tended to have their children rate low on
their ability to regulate anger, rDA,r-a.x(26) = -.37, p = .028. When statistically significant,
56
Table 13
57
Table 14
58
the dismissing and disapproving scales of the ERPSST-L negatively correlated with the
mothers who scored high on the dismissing scale tended to score low on coaching of
child sadness (c-s), rDI,c-s.x(26) = -.37, p = .028; low on coaching of child anger, rDI,c-a.x(26)
= -.45, p = .009; and low on the coaching of both sadness and anger, rDI,c-sa.x (26) =-.47, p
correlations found between emotion coaching (EC) and acceptance of child sadness (ac-s),
rEC,ac-s.x(26) = .02, p = .460; child regulation of sadness (r-s), rEC,r-s.x(26) = .01, p = .491;
and the coaching of child anger (c-a), rEC,c-a.x(26) = -.03, p = .447. No significant
correlation was found between the laissez-faire scale of the ERPSST-L and the Meta-
Emotion Interview dimensions. Though there were some nonsignificant correlations, all
Construct validity of the ERPS short form. It was expected that both the ERPS
emotion coaching scale and the parental acceptance of negative emotion scale would
positively correlate with ratings on all child dimensions of the Meta-Emotion Interview
regulation of emotions, and coaching of child’s emotions). Next, it was expected that the
would negatively correlate with ratings on the Meta-Emotion Interview child dimensions.
59
Zero-order correlations (Table 15) revealed that some scores on the ERPS
correlated with scores on the Meta-Emotion Interview. For example, greater parental
rejection of negative emotion was associated with lower scores on emotion coaching of
child anger, r(31) = -.30, p = .047. On the other hand, greater parental acceptance of
negative emotion was related to greater coaching of child sadness, r(31) = .32, p = .036.
relation to scores on the Meta-Emotion Interview while controlling for child age, child
sex, and family income (refer to Table 16). As done previously, the removal of the effect
of child age, child sex, and family income is represented by a subscripted x. Hypotheses
were partially supported. It was found that mothers who reported greater use of emotion
coaching (ec), as measured by the ERPS, tended to be rated high in child regulation of
combined sadness and anger (r-sa), rec,r-sa.x(26)= .35, p = .035. Mothers who scored high in
parental rejection (pr) tended to score low in the coaching of child anger, rpr,c-a.x(26) =
-.32, p = .050. Additionally, mothers who scored high on parental acceptance (pa) tended
to score high in awareness of child anger, rpa,aw-a.x(26) = .36, p = .032. Mothers who
scored high in uncertainty/ineffectiveness (ui) reported that their children had difficulty
regulating anger, rui,r-a.x(26) = -.36, p = .028. Further, there was a marginal negative
and anger, rui,r-sa.x(26)= -.30, p = .058. In contrast to this, both mothers who were high in
emotion coaching and high in acceptance of their children’s emotions were rated higher
scores on their children’s ability to regulate anger ,rec,r-a.x(26) = .34, p = .037 and rpa,r-
a.x(26) = .33, p = .042, respectively. Between the Meta-Emotion Interview and the ERPS,
60
Table 15
61
Table 16
62
Additional Analyses
After conducting the study’s main analyses, follow-up analyses were conducted.
The first analysis involved examining emotion coaching dichotomously (high vs. low).
The second additional analysis involved examining the relation between Meta-Emotion
Interview child dimensions (i.e., awareness, acceptance, regulation, and coaching) and
Third, a maternal experience variable was constructed and tested in order to explore other
Emotion coaching by high and low groups. It was unexpected that the emotion
coaching subscale of the ERPSST-L and ERPS would have zero correlation with many
Meta-Emotion Interview child dimensions. To further explore the nature of the zero-
correlation, two emotion coaching groups were created: relatively high emotion coaching
(using an absolute cutoff of 1SD above the mean) and relatively low emotion coaching
(using an absolute cutoff of 1SD below the mean). These cutoff standards were created
for emotion coaching scores according to the ERPSST-L and then for emotion coaching
High and low emotion coaching group differences using ERPSST-L cutoffs.
Cases with an ERPSST-L emotion coaching score 1SD or greater above the mean (i.e., a
score equal to or higher than 98.99) were included in the high emotion coaching group (n
= 6); cases with an emotion coaching score 1SD or greater below the mean (i.e., a score
equal to or lower than 77.17) were included in the low emotion coaching group (n = 4).
63
Independent samples t-tests for all mother-child dyads were conducted to test for
differences between ERPSST-L high and low emotion coaching groups on the Meta-
indicate that no statistically significant group differences were found (all ps > .05).
High and low emotion coaching group differences using ERPS cutoffs. Those
relatively high in emotion coaching (n = 8), according to the ERPS, had a score equal to
or greater than 23.52. Those relatively low in emotion coaching (n = 4) had a score of
18.52 or lower.
Independent samples t-tests for all mother-child dyads were conducted to test if
differences between ERPS high and low emotion coaching groups were present on any of
the Meta-Emotion Interview parent and child dimensions. Findings were similar to that
of the ERPSST-L, in that no statistically significant differences were found between low
and high in emotion coaching in terms of Meta-Emotion Interview dimension scores (all
scores on the Meta-Emotion Interview parent dimensions relate to scores on the Meta-
Emotion Interview child dimension after controlling for child age, child gender, and
annual family income (together, the removal of the effect of these three variables is
cases per predictor (Field, 2009), and the sample size for the present study was just below
this mark. Being that there would be limited power in such an analysis, other methods of
64
Partial correlation analyses were used to examine the relation between parenting
in Table 17, it was found that mothers who were highly aware of their own sadness,
anger, and the combination of the two were also highly aware of their children’s sadness,
anger, and combined. Additionally, mothers who were highly accepting of their own
sadness (M-a-s) were also highly aware of sadness in their children, rM-as,aw-s.x(26) = .54, p
= .003. Third, mothers who scored high in the ability to regulate their own anger (M-r-a)
also scored high in coaching of their child’s anger, rM-r-a,ca.x(26) = .40, p = .033. Due to
the absence of statistically significant correlations in some instances, the expectation that
The role of maternal experience. To better examine maternal factors that may
variable was created. This variable incorporated the age of the mother’s oldest child
(range = 1 to 22) in order to estimate how many years the mothers have taken on this
role. A score of one was assigned if the oldest child was ages 0 to 6, a score of two was
assigned for ages 7 to 12, a score of three was assigned for ages 13-17, and a score of
four was assigned for ages 18-22. Number of children (range = 1 to 4) was incorporated
in the maternal experience variable, whereby one point was scored for each child in the
family. Maternal experience scores were computed by adding scores on the two variables
previously described (range = 2 to 8). The role of maternal experience was assessed in
order to better delineate parent-related factors in the meta-emotion process. This variable
65
Table 17
Two-Tailed Partial Correlations between Meta-Emotion Interview Parent Dimensions and Child Dimensions, Controlling for Child
Age, Child Sex, and Family Income
Parent Dimensions
Parent Awareness Parent Acceptance Parent Regulation
Sadness Anger Combined Sadness Anger Combined Sadness Anger Combined
Child Dimensions
Awareness of Child
Sadness .58***. .50** .61*** .54** .16 .44* -.11 -.08 -.12
Anger 58*** .56** .64*** .34 .04 .23 -.17 .17 .04
Combined .66*** .60*** .71*** .49** .11 .37 -.16 .06 -.04
Acceptance of Child
Sadness .01 .13 .07 .17 .01 .11 .01 .06 .05
Anger .27 .27 .30 .19 .26 .30 .02 .27 .23
Combined .19 .24 .24 .21 .18 .25 .02 .21 .18
Child Regulation
Sadness .17 .14 .18 .17 .03 .12 .20 -.34 -.16
Anger .01 .03 .02 -.01 .21 .15 .18 .24 .28
Combined .09 .09 .10 .08 .16 .16 .23 -.01 .11
Coaching
Sadness .26 .27 .30 -.02 .16 .10 -.15 .34 .19
Anger .38* .35 .41* .11 .26 .24 -.17 .40* .23
Combined .38* .36 .42* .07 .25 .22 -.18 .43* .25
Note. ―Combined‖ refers to the summed scores of sadness and anger for that dimension. * p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
66
is unique in that it does not appear on the Meta-Emotion Interview and provides another
aspect of maternal factors that may relate to one’s emotion-related parenting style. By
using this method, mothers who scored relatively high on maternal experience (e.g., a
mother of four children, with the oldest child being 22 years) could be compared to
mothers scoring relatively low on maternal experience (e.g., a mother of one child who is
present study, overall scores on maternal experience ranged from 2 to 8. The average
Partial correlation analyses were conducted between maternal experience and the
removing the effect of child age, child sex, and family income. Findings, as shown in a
column of Table 18, suggest that the relation between maternal experience(me) and
determine if maternal experience scores related to maternal age. Findings suggest that
greater maternal age was related to greater maternal experience scores, r(31) = .66, p <
.001.
Though not tested directly in the present study, cohort effects in terms of maternal
age may be related to maternal level of experience. As such, partial correlation analyses
were conducted (as shown in Table 18) between maternal experience and the Meta-
removing the effect of child age, child sex, family income, and maternal age (ma).
67
Table 18
Maternal Experience
Controlling for child age, Controlling for child age,
child sex, and family child sex, family income,
income and maternal age
Parent Awareness
Sadness .19 .21
Meta- Anger .10 .06
Emotion Combined .17 .17
Interview Parent Acceptance
Variables Sadness -.17 -.26
Anger -.33 -.14
Combined -.34 -.25
Parent Regulation
Sadness -.41* -.24
Anger -.19 -.08
Combined -.36 -.18
Child Awareness
Sadness .27 .28
Anger .28 .26
Combined .31 .31
Child Acceptance
Sadness -.36 -.56**
Anger -.08 -.10
Combined -.22 -.32
Child Regulation
Sadness .01 .05
Anger -.02 .02
Combined -.01 .04
Child Coaching
Sadness .04 .14
Anger .04 .13
Combined .05 .15
ERPSST-L Emotion Coaching .11 .05
Variables Laissez Faire .20 .19
Dismissing -.04 -.06
Disapproving -.11 -.09
ERPS Emotion Coaching .22 .15
Variables Parental Rejection -.19 -.16
Parental Acceptance -.01 -.12
Uncertainty/Ineffectiveness .15 .23
Note. ―Combined‖ refers to the summed scores of sadness and anger for that dimension.
*p < .05, **p < .01.
68
Findings suggest that greater maternal experience (me) was associated with a lower
acceptance of child sadness, rme,ac-s.x,ma(23) = -.56, p = .003. All other partial correlations
present study, statistically significant findings were found between the Meta-Emotion
Interview and ERPSST-L and ERPS. These findings involved Meta-Emotion Interview
dimensions related to child anger, maternal use of dismissing and disapproving emotion-
related parenting style subscales of the ERPSST-L, and the parental rejection of child
equal number of questions related to sadness and anger. This was not the case with the
ERPSST-L and ERPS. Sadness-specific content (i.e., items that contained the words
―sad‖ or ―sadness‖) were found on 32/81 items, whereas anger-specific content (i.e.,
items that contained the words ―anger,‖ ―angry,‖ or ―mad‖) were found on 48/81 items on
the ERPSST-L. A greater number of items related to anger also were found, with 9/20
related to sadness and 11/20 related to anger. Because more questions on the ERPSST-L
and ERPS pertained to anger, it is possible that the questionnaires assess anger better than
sadness. A second consideration is that children’s display of anger is often met with
al., 2007). Thus, it should not be surprising that in the present study many dimensions of
and the study of meta-emotion as a whole. In coding the Meta-Emotion Interviews, some
69
response patterns were acknowledged and were deemed notable for representing the
and ―I‖ referred to interviewer. Notable perspectives for mothers will first be discussed
demonstrated a preference for their children to be soothed before getting involved. This
P: And so I’ll tell him ―Okay if you don’t want to talk right now. Because you
are so hyper. You’re so angry. Go. Go calm down. Then come back and talk‖
Some mothers received a low score in showing the child respect towards his or
her emotional experiences. Under the child coaching dimension, this refers to Meta-
Emotion Interview item F1: ―P shows respect for child’s experience of emotions.‖
P: When I see him pouting, I always say oh my god, is this my son? He looks
so ugly, his face looks so ugly. He’s (cautious?) and cute. You know. He likes
that, so he’s like (stretches?) his face. He goes, ―Mom, this is your son. I’m
not ugly.‖ You know. I’m like okay! And that’s the extent of it, really.
P: I just tell him he has an ugly face. (Laughs). And I tell him it’s not worth
70
it. Like what are you angry about, not getting a toy? Or not being able to
coaching, and regulation), the fathers described a variety of their strategies and
One item on the dimension for child acceptance of emotion states ―P uses a mental
(analytical) approach to C’s emotions,‖ in which a low score is provided if the parent
uses this approach. Two of the three fathers whose interviews were scored used a
rational approach in dealing with their children’s sadness, while all of the fathers used a
rational approach in dealing with their children’s anger. The fathers, therefore, lost
points on the acceptance dimension for using a rational approach, which is described as
the parent teaching the child to approach the emotion in a way to figure it out or analyze
P: I guess that it’s okay to feel angry but that she should really think about
what she’s getting angry about. And you know. Ask herself whether it is
really worth it. You know. Because there’s always, you know sometimes
she’ll just get frustrated and angry and I’ll tell her you know, (child’s name)
there’s more than one way to skin a cat. You know. Or something to that
them head on and getting angry about them. And I’m just trying to teach her
that it’s okay to get angry about certain things. But uh, it’s not always the best
71
way of going about it. And you know. Working through a problem is better
All fathers in the present study specifically stated that they want their children to
I: OK. If you could sum it up, what are you trying to teach (daughter’s name)
P: That it’s not bad to have any feeling. And, it’s alright to even express your
feelings. As her dad I hope she can tell me whenever she’s having any of
these feelings.
P: But I, I think there’s probably still a lesson to be learned. And I'm really
having a hard time describing exactly what that is. But I think there’s
probably something that you know, makes your heart good. Makes you a
better person when under certain circumstances you experience sadness. And
it’s going to be hard for me to pinpoint it if you want me to get more direct
than that. But I really feel that there is something there. Something to be said
72
Some parents, such as this father, specifically stated they do not use distraction
techniques with their children. Under the child acceptance dimension, this refers to
P: Mm. We just talk. Talk about it. We’ll discuss it. I ask her questions and
I’ll let her tell me what’s on her mind. And I don’t say ―Let’s go get ice
cream‖ or whatever.
Emotion-related parenting styles can differ greatly from parent to parent, even
comments from the Meta-Emotion Interview was helpful in further exploring how one’s
73
CHAPTER V
DISCUSSION
The aim of the present study was to evaluate the construct validity of the
Findings partially supported the construct validity of the ERPSST-L and ERPS in that
direction.
and this may have been a reflection of a small, homogenous sample. Lack of construct
measure that is not present in the construct) and/or deficiency (variance in the construct is
not captured by the measure). Threats to construct validity (Cherulnik, 2001, p. 67) were
considered and examined in the present study. If threats to construct validity are not
addressed, construct validity may not be found. A lack of construct validity might also
indicate that there is a problem with the theory, measurement strategy, item content, or
the construct might not be specified very well (Westen & Rosenthal, 2005).
(ERPSST-L)
It was expected that scores on the long form would correlate with scores on the
74
between scores on the emotion coaching subscale and ratings on all Meta-Emotion
When correlation coefficients were statistically significant, they also were in the
emotion coaching and ratings on the child’s ability to regulate anger. The significance of
emotion socialization pertaining to anger is later discussed. Next, it was expected that the
laissez-faire scale would positively correlate with the child awareness and child
acceptance dimensions and would negatively correlate with the child regulation and
coaching dimensions. This expectation was not confirmed, as correlations between this
scale and the Meta-Emotion Interview child dimensions were very small or had a zero
correlation. This may be due to the fact that the laissez-faire scale had an internal
consistency reliability that was lower (α = .59) relative to the other subscales. Thus, the
results linked to the construct validity for the laissez-faire scale should be interpreted
with caution. Scores on the dismissing scale were expected to negatively correlate with
all of the Meta-Emotion Interview child dimensions. When results were statistically
particular, high scores on the child acceptance dimensions and coaching dimensions were
associated with lower scores on the dismissing subscale. Scores on the disapproving
dimensions. The disapproving subscale negatively correlated with the acceptance and
regulation of child anger. Overall, when statistically significant relations were found
between the ERPSST-L subscales and the Meta-Emotion Interview dimensions, they also
75
Construct Validity of the Emotion-Related Parenting Styles (ERPS)
Another goal of the present study was to test the construct validity of the short
dimensions. Results partially supported the hypotheses (refer to Table 3). It was
expected that scores on the emotion coaching scale would positively correlate with all
child regulation of anger and the regulation of combined sadness and anger. Next, it was
predicted that parental rejection of negative emotion would correlate negatively with all
Meta-Emotion Interview child dimensions. In the present study, mothers who were rated
as high in parental rejection of negative emotion also were rated as being low in the
coaching of child anger. As was the case for the emotion coaching subscale, it was
expected that parental acceptance of negative emotion would correlate positively to all
acceptance of negative emotion tended to score high in awareness and regulation of child
anger. Finally, it was expected that mothers who feel uncertain or ineffective in emotion
socialization also would be rated as having low scores on all Meta-Emotion Interview
child dimensions. Findings suggest that mothers who scored highest in uncertainty and
ineffectiveness had children who were rated as having difficulty regulating anger. This
may exemplify a situation in which a mother of a child who has great difficulty
regulating his or her anger may feel inadequate or unsuccessful in helping the child deal
with anger. The finding for the uncertainty/ineffectiveness scale is in contrast to the
finding that scores on the ERPS emotion coaching and parental acceptance scales
positively correlated with scores on child regulation of anger. Though not tested directly,
76
it can be suggested that a child’s ability to regulate anger may have a bidirectional effect
Interview were not statistically significant. This may have been related to the fact that
this subscale had a relatively low internal consistency reliability of .67. Additionally, it is
important to note that the uncertainty/ineffectiveness scale was designed in a study that
The relation between parent dimensions and child dimensions of the Meta-
Emotion Interview also was explored. The expectation that Meta-Emotion Interview
dimensions was only partially supported. The strongest findings were found between the
statistically significant, positive relation between child awareness dimensions and parent
awareness dimensions. The association between the parent and child dimensions of the
Meta-Emotion Interview have been explored in other studies. For example, Hunter and
colleagues (2011) administered the Meta-Emotion Interview to 148 mothers and 106
fathers. They found that both mothers’ and fathers’ scores on the parent dimensions of
the Meta-Emotion Interview positively related to scores on the child dimensions. They
also found that mothers’ scores on the parent dimensions positively correlated with
fathers’ scores on the parent dimensions. In summary, though the relation between
parent dimensions and child dimensions of the MEI was only partially supported in the
small sample used in the present study, it has been clearly supported in other research
77
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
The first limitation of the present study is the small sample size (N = 33 mothers).
Small sample size limits statistical power, which is why trends often were found when
relations were not always statistically significant. Statistical power also was reduced
when multiple control variables were used. A limitation of the present study is the
exclusion of fathers from analyses. Fathers were not included in the main analyses of the
present study because there were so few. The current study focused on maternal meta-
how spouse pairs and their children contribute to the many multi-directional processes at
Second, the sample was homogenous in terms of ethnicity and level of education.
External validity also may be limited because the sample in the present study was quite
of the mothers were classified as predominately emotion coaching by the ERPSST-L, and
78.8% (n = 26) of the mothers were classified as predominately emotion coaching by the
negative emotions, or uncertain/ineffective. The group, therefore, was not diverse and
on past research, the results from an ANOVA indicated that Meta-Emotion Interview
scores did not differ based on either an ERPSST-L or ERPS emotion coaching or non-
78
emotion coaching status. Preliminary findings in the present study indicate that in order
worthy of exploration in future studies. Future studies should incorporate a more diverse
sample and also should consider other variables, such as maternal experience.
Good (1990), were evaluated in the present study. The first threat to address is
inconsequential research hypotheses. In this study, results have the potential to answer an
ERPSST-L and ERPS hold construct validity in conjunction with the original Meta-
Emotion Interview. The second threat, ambiguous research hypotheses, also was
considered. The hypotheses in the present study were stated with specific directions and
research hypotheses and statistical tests. In this study, both the hypotheses and statistical
tests accentuate the direction and strength of the relations as assessed through correlation.
The final threat is diffuse statistical hypotheses and tests, which is when too many
analyses are conducted per hypothesis. In the present study, an effort was made to avoid
The measures used in the present study also pose limitations. One limitation of
the Meta-Emotion Interview is that the replacement of ―don’t know‖ scores with
items with a possible score range from 1 to 4), a mother received a score of 4 on two
items but was coded ―don’t know‖ for the remaining seven items. After mean
79
substitution, that mother received a perfect score of 36 on that dimension, which is likely
responsibility of the interviewer to ensure that the parent continues to discuss only the
target child. However, on the questionnaire, it is possible for a parent to deviate from this
P: Now if we’re talking about my other son it’s different. It’s a whole
Another parent discussed a limitation of both the questionnaire and the possible
P: ….. Like I said with the, with the questionnaire. I found that it was kind of
difficult to answer because there are different situations for when he’s feeling
angry. And then there’s different situations for when he’s feeling sadness.
Gathering the full picture of emotion socialization in families may be best done
80
Questionnaires, such as the ERPSTT-L and ERPS, are helpful in quickly getting a sense
Hooven (2006) has expanded to areas beyond parent-child relationships. Over the past
decade, there have been some promising developments in the area of meta-emotion. The
construct of meta-emotion also has been explored in marital relationships (e.g., Schwab,
2001; Yoshimoto, 2005) and has been used to enhance an understanding of relational
explored in children and adolescents. For instance, Taylor and Carrère (2002) established
a meta-emotion interview and coding system for children ages 7 to 8, known as the
Family Health Project Child Meta-Emotion Interview. For older children, Windecker-
Nelson and Katz (2004) published the Child–Adolescent Meta-Emotion Coding System.
This measure has been used to investigate emotional competence and risky behaviour of
adolescents (Hessler & Katz, 2010), to assess emotion regulation and physiological
responses during peer provocation (Hessler & Katz, 2007), and to investigate the emotion
competence of children who have been exposed to domestic violence (Katz, Hessler, &
Annest, 2007). In a study by Hunter and colleagues (2011), 75 depressed and 77 healthy
adolescents completed the Child and Adolescent Meta-Emotion Interview (Katz &
Interview. They found that dimensions of the Child and Adolescent Meta-Emotion
Interview were found to significantly correlate with parent dimensions of the Meta-
adolescence. With both a measure for child meta-emotion and adolescent meta-emotion,
81
it may be possible to follow a child’s meta-emotion longitudinally. Perhaps meta-
first is social desirability, which is a research concern particularly for face valid
questionnaire is that results may not necessarily inform what occurs in the home.
Respondents may describe emotion coaching techniques, for example, that they would
ideally use, not ones that are typically implemented. It also would be useful to examine
the ERPSST-L and ERPS in more longitudinal studies, in order to examine the stability
the dimension scores should be examined separately for each emotion as well as a
combined scale. As previously discussed, the most statistically significant findings in the
present study involved dimensions related to anger. Does anger, as compared to sadness,
play a special role in parental meta-emotion philosophy? Are there other possible
explanations for this relation? Displays of anger in children tend to be met with more
negative consequences from parents than are displays of sadness (Shipman et al., 2007).
Anger may draw for more of the dismissing and disapproving emotion-related parenting
82
styles. However, when mothers coach adolescents through anger, the adolescents tend to
have better anger regulation and less externalizing behaviour (Shortt, Stoolmiller, Smith-
Shine, Eddy, & Sheeber, 2010). Thus, the context and nature of the emotion may inform
the emotion socialization process. In a study by Shipman and colleagues (2007), it was
found that mothers who physically maltreat their children tended to use more invalidation
towards child anger as compared to child sadness or fear. Emotion type may play a role
these expressions of anger in children may more likely be met with invalidating parental
behaviours such as minimization (Shipman et al., 2007). Thus, when parental response to
child anger is evaluated, the dismissing and disapproving responses tend to be utilized to
consideration is that when working with clinical samples, depressed adolescents may
display more intense anger than healthy adolescents (Sheeber, Allen, Leve, Davis, Shortt,
& Katz, 2009). In a case where parents of adolescents with intense anger are tested, it
may vary for different emotions. For example, a parent of a child with intense anger may
be highly disapproving of anger but may also score high in emotion coaching of sadness.
As discussed earlier, more questions on the ERPSST-L and ERPS targeted anger as
compared to sadness. The imbalance of questions for sadness and anger on the ERPSST-
L and ERPS may be one explanation for the differential findings; however, past research
may be informative as well. As described earlier, anger may draw for more of the
83
respond to expressions of anger with more negative consequences than they would for
researchers, but it can also be a valuable experience for parents. In the Meta-Emotion
study. Some parents expressed that it is important to socialize positive emotions as well.
about reading stories about emotions. I don’t know if I really ever read
anything in particular dealing with a particular problem. But this study has
open my eyes to that. Well maybe when there is a problem we’ll get a book.
And read it together. Because I read to her everyday but not just stories that
I: Yes. It sounds like you have unconsciously addressed some of these issues.
The parent in the previous excerpt indicated that emotion socialization through
only sadness and anger were explored in the present study. Both Gottman and colleagues
(1996) and Cowan (1996) proposed that it would be valuable to use the Meta-Emotion
Interview to assess positive emotions. Recently, the interview has been adapted to
evaluate other emotions such as pride, love, and affection. This measure, known as the
84
Interview, was developed by Katz and Carrère (as cited in Doohan, Carrère, & Taylor,
2004). As previously described, parents may be high in acceptance for some emotions
but not others. It is, therefore, important to apply measures of emotion-related parenting
One part of the present study involved examining the relation between scores on
Meta-Emotion Interview parent dimensions and scores on child dimensions. The purpose
of this was to inform, but not necessarily to confirm, factors that may be involved in the
bidirectional exchange of emotion socialization between mothers and their children. For
example, mothers high in awareness of their own sadness were also rated as being well-
aware of their child’s sadness. Thus, parental factors, such as emotional awareness, can
inform the way they perceive and accordingly interact with their children. Katz, Maliken,
and Stettler (2012) urged researchers to explore bidirectional relations between parent
A major strength of the present study was that it fulfilled the need to test the
construct validity of the ERPSST-L and ERPS using the original meta-emotion interview
scores. Testing the construct validity of measures for meta-emotion philosophy is just the
summary, construct validity for the ERPSST-L and ERPS was partially supported in the
direction. The further validation of time-efficient and user-friendly measures, such as the
ERPSST-L and ERPS, may encourage more research concerning the construct of meta-
emotion philosophy.
85
REFERENCES
mothers and fathers: Coherence among behaviors and associations with parent
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507. 2010.00585.x
Monograph, 4, 1-103.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale
Crano, W. D., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). Principles and methods of social research:
Second edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. (pp. 33–113).
Cunningham, J. N., Kliewer, W., & Garner, P. W. (2009). Emotion socialization, child
86
emotion understanding and regulation, and adjustment in urban African American
DeOliveira, C. A., Moran, G., & Pederson, D. R. (2005). Understanding the link between
doi:10.1080/14616730500135032
Doohan, E., Carrère, S., & Taylor, M. (2004). The meta-emotion interviews and coding
Dunsmore, J. C., Her, P., Halberstadt, A. G., & Perez-Rivera, M. B. (2009). Parents’
whose time has come: Comment on Gottman et al. (1996). Journal of Family
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Cumberland, A. (1998). The socialization of emotion:
doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0904_17
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Bernzweig, J. (1990). The Coping with Children’s
87
Negative Emotions Scale: Description and scoring. Unpublished scale,
University, Tempe.
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Karbon, M., Bernzweig, J., Speer, A. L., & Carlo, G. (1994).
Field, A. P. (2009). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS (3rd Ed.) (pp. 113–359). London:
Sage.
Gottman, J., & DeClaire, J. (1997). Raising an emotionally intelligent child: The heart of
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and
the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families
Hakim-Larson, J., Dunham, K., Vellet, S., Murdaca, L., & Levenbach, J. (1999). Parental
doi:10.1037/h0087069
Hakim-Larson, J., Parker, A., Lee, C., Goodwin, J., & Voelker, S. (2006). Measuring
88
parental meta-emotion: Psychometric properties of the Emotion-Related Parenting
doi:10.1207/s15566935eed1702_2
Halberstadt, A. G., Cassidy, J., Stifter, C. A., Parke, R. D., & Fox, N. A. (1995). Self-
Hessler, D. M., & Katz, L. F. (2007). Children’s emotion regulation: Self-report and
27–38. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.27
Hessler, D. M., & Katz, L. F. (2010). Brief report: Associations between emotional
246. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.04.007
Hunter, E. C., Katz, L. F., Shortt, J. W., Davis, B., Leve, C., Allen, N. B., & Sheeber, L.
depressed and healthy adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 428–
441. doi:10.1007/s10964-010-9545-2
Jäger, C., & Bartsch, A. (2006). Meta-emotions. Grazer Philosophische Studien. 73, 179–
204.
89
manuscript, University of Washington.
Katz, L. F., Gottman, J. M., & Hooven, C. (1996). Meta-emotion philosophy and family
Katz, L. F., Hessler, D. M., & Annest, A. (2007). Domestic violence, emotional
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507. 2007.00401.x
Katz, L. F., & Hunter, E. C. (2007). Maternal meta-emotion philosophy and adolescent
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00388.x
Katz, L. F., Hunter, E., & Klowden, A. (2008). Intimate partner violence and children’s
Katz, L. F., Maliken, A. C., & Stettler, N. M. (2012). Parental meta-emotion philosophy:
Katz, L. F., Mittman, A., & Embry, L. (n.d.). The Meta Emotion Coding System Coding
doi:10.1023/B:JACP.0000030292.36168.30
Katz, L. F., & Windecker-Nelson, B. (2006). Domestic violence, emotion coaching, and
90
child adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 20(1), 56–67.
doi:10.1037/0893-3200.20.1.56
Lagacé-Séguin, D. G., & Coplan, R. J. (2005). Maternal emotional styles and child social
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507. 2005.00320.x
Lee, C. H. (1999). The Parenting Styles Self-Test: Reliability and construct validity.
Lee, C. H., Hakim-Larson, J., & Voelker, S. (2000, June). The Parenting Styles Self-Test:
McGraw, K. O., & Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about some intraclass
989X.1.1.30
Mills, S. L., Freeman, W. S., Clara, I. P., Elgar, F. J., Walling, B. R., & Mak, L. (2007).
Parent proneness to shame and the use of psychological control. Journal of Child
91
Paterson, A. D., Babb, K. A., Camodeca, A., Goodwin, J., Hakim-Larson, J., Voelker, S.,
& Gragg, M. (2012). Emotion-Related Parenting Styles (ERPS): A short form for
23, 583-602.
Sheeber, L. B., Allen, N. B., Leve, C., Davis, B., Shortt, J. W., & Katz, L. F. (2009).
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02148.x
Shine, S., & Wampler, K. S. (1997). The role of emotion in the family: Building a theory.
Shipman, K. L., Schneider, R., Fitzgerald, M. M., Sims, C., Swisher, L., & Edwards, A.
Shortt, J. W., Stoolmiller, M., Smith-Shine, J. N., Eddy, J. M., & Sheeber, L. (2010).
808. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02207.x
92
Smith, G. T. (2005). On construct validity: Issues of method and measurement.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2012). Using Multivariate Statistics 6th ed. Upper
Taylor, M.G., & Carrère S. (2002). The child meta-emotion interview and coding system
Walling, B. R., Mills, R. S. L., & Freeman, W. S. (2007). Parenting cognitions associated
with the use of psychological control. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 16,
642–659. doi:10.1007/s10826-006-9113-2
Wampold, B. E., Davis, B., & Good III, R. H. (1990). Hypothesis validity in clinical
Association.
Westen, D., & Rosenthal, R. (2005). Improving construct validity: Cronbach, Meehl, and
3590.17.4.409
Yap, M. B. H., Allen, N. B., Leve, C., & Katz, L. F. (2008). Maternal meta-emotion
doi:10.1037/a0013104
93
Yoshimoto, D. K. (2005). Marital meta-emotion: Emotion coaching and dyadic
94
APPENDIX A
Inter-Rater Reliability for Coding of Meta-Emotion Interviews (MEIs) for Coding Training, as Measured by Intra-Class Correlation
Coefficients
95
APPENDIX B
Inter-Rater Reliability for Coding of Meta-Emotion Interviews (MEIs) for Present Study,
as Measured by Intra-Class Correlation Coefficients
96
APPENDIX C
Parent Awareness
Sadness 43.11(3.63) 41.46(4.74) t(8) = -.63, p = .548
Anger 44.61(4.19) 42.75(3.59) t(8) = -.74, p = .490
Combined 87.72(7.71) 84.21(7.65) t(8) = -.71, p = .499
Parent Acceptance
Sadness 55.76(6.90) 56.76(6.19) t(8) = .23, p = .822
Anger 56.17(7.17) 56.12(5.46) t(8) = -.01, p = .991
Combined 111.93(11.70) 112.88(6.25) t(8) = .15, p = .887
Parent Regulation
Sadness 41.38(6.57) 41.23(2.55) t(8) = -.04, p = .968
Anger 39.30(7.16) 41.48(6.70) t(8) = .48, p = .642
Combined 80.67(10.61) 82.71(8.29) t(8) = .32, p = .756
Child Awareness
Sadness 33.79(4.13) 34.60(2.13) t(8) = .36, p = .731
Anger 34.02(2.58) 32.28(3.18) t(8) = -.96, p = .368
Combined 67.81(6.50) 66.88(5.09) t(8) = -.24, p = .817
Child Acceptance
Sadness 46.51(4.28) 46.08(5.71) t(8) = -.14, p = .894
Anger 40.64(2.18) 39.31(8.82) t(8) = -.30, p = .785
Combined 87.15(4.58) 85.39(13.76) t(8) = -.25, p = .819
Child Regulation
Sadness 32.24(4.78) 33.00(4.24) t(8) = .26, p = .804
Anger 27.87(7.19) 24.38(6.97) t(8) = -.76, p = .468
Combined 60.11(10.68) 57.38(10.70) t(8) = -.40, p = .702
Child Coaching
Sadness 42.67(1.60) 41.49(1.97) t(8) = -1.05, p = .326
Anger 35.12(4.33) 36.55(5.30) t(8) = .47, p = .651
Combined 77.79(5.43) 78.04(6.83) t(8) = .07, p = .950
Note. High emotion coaching group = cases (n = 6) with an ERPSST-L emotion coaching
score 1SD or greater above the mean (i.e., a score equal to or higher than 98.99). Low
emotion coaching group = cases (n = 4) with an ERPSST-L emotion coaching score 1SD
or greater below the mean (i.e., a score equal to or lower than 77.17).
97
APPENDIX D
Parent Awareness
Sadness 43.92(3.53) 41.75(5.06) t(8) = -.88, p = .402
Anger 44.93(3.59) 42.36(3.15) t(10) = -1.21, p = .254
Combined 88.86(6.91) 84.11(7.59) t(10) = -1.09, p = .302
Parent Acceptance
Sadness 57.74(6.89) 53.62(4.49) t(10) = -1.07, p = .308
Anger 56.87(6.45) 53.89(5.86) t(10) = -.78, p = .456
Combined 114.61(11.26) 107.51(3.49) t(10) = -1.21, p = .250
Parent Regulation
Sadness 40.97(5.62) 38.46(1.77) t(10) = -.86, p = .412
Anger 40.11(6.89) 40.23(5.89) t(10) = .031, p = .976
Combined 81.08(9.59) 78.69(6.37) t(10) = -.45, p = .665
Child Awareness
Sadness 34.34(3.64) 34.03(2.32) t(10) = -.15, p = .883
Anger 34.39(2.30) 33.13(2.95) t(10) = -.46, p = .430
Combined 68.73(5.76) 67.16(5.00) t(10) = -.46, p = .653
Child Acceptance
Sadness 46.96(4.21) 46.34(5.91) t(10) = -.21, p = .837
Anger 41.67(4.69) 38.63(8.50) t(10) = -.82, p = .434
Combined 88.63(7.53) 84.97(13.54) t(10) = -.61, p = .553
Child Regulation
Sadness 33.18(4.40) 29.63(2.56) t(10) = -1.47, p = .171
Anger 28.64(6.80) 23.52(5.64) t(10) = -1.29, p = .225
Combined 61.82(9.94) 53.14(5.94) t(10) = -1.59, p = .144
Child Coaching
Sadness 43.00(1.49) 42.27(2.24) t(10) = -.68, p = .512
Anger 36.60(4.58) 38.94(4.46) t(10) = .84, p = .420
Combined 79.60(5.69) 81.22(6.35) t(10) = .45, p = .665
Note. High emotion coaching group = cases (n = 6) with an ERPS emotion coaching
score 1SD or greater above the mean (i.e., a score equal to or higher than 23.52).
Low emotion coaching group = cases (n = 4) with an ERPS emotion coaching score 1SD
or greater below the mean (i.e., a score equal to or lower than 18.52).
98
VITA AUCTORIS
2000-2004
ON
99