Schmitt Consumerpsychologybrands 2012
Schmitt Consumerpsychologybrands 2012
Schmitt Consumerpsychologybrands 2012
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Wiley is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of
Consumer Psychology
Received 28 June 201 1; received in revised form 16 September 201 1; accepted 17 September 201 1
Available online 19 October 2011
Abstract
This article presents a consumer-psychology model of brands that integrates empirical studies and individual constructs (such as bran
rization, brand affect, brand personality, brand symbolism and brand attachment, among others) into a comprehensive framework. The
tinguishes three levels of consumer engagement (object-centered, self-centered and social) and five processes (identifying, experi
integrating, signifying and connecting). Pertinent psychological constructs and empirical findings are presented for the constructs within
cess. The article concludes with research ideas to test the model using both standard and consumer-neuroscience methods.
© 201 1 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
2000);
Over the past two decades, we have learned a lot about the parent-brand memory structures (Morrin, 1999); the
strength of association between the brand's parent category
consumer judgments of brands and the processes that underlie
specific brand-related phenomena, from brand extensions and to
the extension category (Herr, Farquhar, & Fazio, 1996); de-
gree
global branding to brand equity. The empirical literature on of congruence (Maoz & Tybout, 2002; Sood & Dreze,
2006); relatedness of the categories (Herr et al., 1996); sub-
brands is vast and detailed, demonstrating and testing highly
domain-specific effects. But we have neglected investigating
branding (Milberg, Park, & McCarthy, 1997); brand name sug-
gestiveness (Sen, 1999); brand breadth (Sheinin & Schmitt,
"the big picture" - identifying how specific empirical findings
1994); brand specific associations (Broniarczyk & Alba, 1994);
add up to a broader understanding of how consumers perceive
brands. To be sure, domain-specific theorizing and testing
brand
is extension typicality (Boush & Loken, 1991); intervening
valuable and should continue. However, research on brands
extensions (Keller & Aaker, 1992); positive affect (Adaval,
2003); brand attachment (Fedorikhin, Park, & Thomson, 2008);
may benefit from a broader perspective that integrates various
mood
empirical findings into a comprehensive framework on the psy-(Barone, Miniard, & Romeo, 2000); and involvement
chology of brands. (Barone, 2005). What is missing from the literature is an analysis
Consider the domain of brand extensions. The literature on of how brand extension research contributes to our overall under-
brand extensions alone has amassed more than a hundred stud- standing of the consumer psychology of brands.
ies in leading journals and has identified numerous factors that Not all of our research has been narrow and purely empiri-
affect how consumers feel toward a given brand extension.cally-focused. We have also been quite inventive in generating
These factors have included: overall fit (Aaker & Keller, new constructs - for example, brand personality (Aaker, 1997),
1990; Bottomley & Holden, 2001; Milberg, Sinn, & Goodstein, brand relationships (Foumier, 1998), brand community (Muniz
2010); type of brand (e.g., prestige or functional) (Monga && O'Guinn, 2001), self-brand connections (Escalas, 2004),
John, 2010); brand knowledge (Broniarczyk & Alba, 1994);brand attachment (Thomson, Maclnnis, & Park, 2005), and
the presence of explanatory links (Bridges, Keller, & Sood,brand experience (Brakus, Schmitt, & Zarantonello, 2009).
* The author thanks Michel Pham for discussions of the model, and Matt Quint and David Rogers for comments on the manuscript. The article was prepared, in
part, while the author was a visiting professor at Nanyang Technological University in Singapore, directing the Institute on Asian Consumer Insight.
E-mail address: BHSl@columbia.edu.
1057-7408/$ - see front matter © 201 1 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved,
doi: 1 0. 1 0 1 6/j .jcps.20 1 1 .09.005
'dentifying
voter-brand re/af/00s
ßtaod association
C*
o>
^ <P
^
Sq
§ t ^ ? BIa"a ' Sq %.
I / / ^.9»'«%, 4 ' %
o j? t, % % S- °-
° I g I05 <?/ft,ll %i
g 05 ! ~I §I é o o ~
W /
% ' "'"""cu
' ue -V ue V*'' ■>'
'V
x%„
'
^ /
X
' ^
Object-cent
Self-center
Social enga
know at brand
least the nam
nam
times, this 2003;
may Schm
be en
brand and its categor
There is on
Brown, 1990; Nedung
tifying an a
type, the has been
categorizatiothe
lished sions (e.g.,
verbally ment K
physical sumers
proximity are
(the
aging or webfor existing
site), thro
soon after new
a produc
product sh
& Malter, be categori
2005). There c
the speaking,
distinction tw
between
of the and
name or the
logo ext
of
Simmons, & Netemey
fit, degree
Brand specific
awareness is dyn
an
task in breadth,
which a consumaff
sented with the catego
trieval Brand associations
cues may be self
& Srull, 1982). Two key
studied are the
To engage with prod
a brand in self-relevant ways, consumers
Chattopadhyay,
identify information that is relevant to them. 198
The unique set
In addition, recall
of brand associations that a brand strategist aspiresand to create
by or maintain in the consumer's
linguistic mind constitutes a brand's iden-
characte
tions and tity (Aaker, 1996). This information may include, among
through lex
Brands as informational cues Brands may be used to signify not only individual selves;
they may also be used to represent a group, a society, or culture.
The accumulated information and knowledge about a brand As cultural symbols, they can stand for nations (McDonald's),
can be used in a functional-rational way as informational cues. generations (the Gap), and cultural values (Marlboro, Harley-
Price and quality of a brand are the most widely used types of Davidson). As exemplary symbols that are worthy of admira-
informational signals, signifying that a brand is a value, premi- tion and respect; they can assume the role of cultural icons
um or luxury brand (Zeithaml, 1988). and assume mythic qualities (Holt, 2004).
In a competitive marketplace, brands can be used by firms to Consumer culture theory has illuminated the socio-cultural
inform consumers about product positions in the marketplace symbolism and ideology of brands (Amould, 2005). Sociolog-
(Erdem & Swait, 1998). Brands can do so especially well ically speaking, brands are social representations (Moscovici
when the signal that they convey is clear and consistent, and, & Markova, 1998). They allow groups and communities to
most importantly, credible (that is, truthful and dependable). communicate, behave and orient themselves. Consumers may
also use brands to enact archetype myths, e.g., the archetype
Brands as identity signals of the hero, rebel or lover (Woodside, Sood, & Miller, 2008).
Following Jungian analysis, archetypes are universally under-
When the self is engaged, then the brand can be a signal for a stood, recurring symbols in the collective unconscious that ap-
consumer's personal identity, to both the consumer and to others. pear in cultural myths, literature and art, and, in contemporary
Psychological research has shown that the self consists of stable consumer culture, as brand stories. Finally, brands can provide
knowledge structures (so-called "self-schemas") that organize in- symbolic security, for example, for materialistic consumers
coming self-related information and help people make sense of who experience existential insecurities, e.g., fear of death
themselves in their environments (Markus, 1977). People vary (Rindfleisch, Burroughs, & Wong, 2009).
in their tendency to possess particular self-schemas, and this vari-
ation leads to differential attitudes and behaviors toward objects,
including brands (Markus, 1983; Markus & Sentis, 1982). Con- Connecting
sumers with a strong masculine self-schema described fragrance
brands in more accentuated gendered terms and held sharply dif- Finally, the model distinguishes three psychological con-
ferent brand preferences than those with weaker masculine self- structs to indicate various ways of connecting with a brand
schemas (Markus & Sentis, 1982). that differ in strength and affect the consumer's interaction
The part of the self that is defined by brands is referred to as with a brand: brand attitude (resulting from object-centered en-
self-brand identity. The self-brand connection scale measures gagements with brands), brand attachment (resulting from self-
the strength of the link between the self and a particular brand centered engagements), and brand community (resulting from
(Escalas, 2004). There are also individual differences among interpersonal and socio-cultural engagements).
Brand attitude They tend to form around publicly consumed goods rather
than those consumed in private.
Brand attitudes are psychological tendencies to evaluate ob- Brand community includes a sense of emotional involve-
jects along a degree of favor or liking. Attitudes toward brands,ment and connection with the group. Yet, brand communities
or ads, have been central constructs in consumer psychology are not only providing emotional bonds. They create shared
for a long time (MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986; Mitchell &goals among members, who may engage in joint actions to ac-
Olson, 1981). Recently, following dual-processing theories in complish these collective goals (Bagozzi & Dholakia, 2006).
psychology, a distinction has been drawn between implicitThey provide help and support, recommendations, and interac-
and explicit attitudes (Gawroński & Bodenhausen, 2006). The tion with like-minded consumers, as well as participation in
basis of implicit attitudes is seen in associative processes that community activities. The meaningfulness of the community
are activated automatically with little cognitive capacity or ex-rests in sharing such activities and experiences (McAlexander,
plicit intention to evaluate an object. For brands, they may be Schouten, & Koenig, 2002). As social entities, brand communi-
the result of a classical conditioning process, e.g., by pairing ties also have their own norms and rituals, and consumers tend
sensory images with brands (Grossman & Till, 1998). Explicit to agree with the community's objectives, norms, and rituals
attitudes, in contrast, are evaluative judgments that are derived(Algesheimer, Dholakia, & Herrmann, 2005).
through a reflective system and the resulting propositions are
subject to syllogistic inferences that assess their validity.
Positive attitudes express a relatively weak connection with aFuture research
brand. They are generalized dispositions to behave toward a
brand, and they may lead to simple preference and purchase inten- The consumer-psychology model of brands presented here
tion. But attitudes are often not stable over time, and the attitude-provides an integrative framework for organizing research on
behavior link is weak and subject to numerous moderator effectsbrands and guiding further empirical studies. Future research
(Park & Maclnnis, 2006). should test the validity of the core constructs and core assump-
tions of the model, regarding the three layers of engagement,
Brand attachment the five brand related processes, and, most importantly, regarding
the allocation of constructs to these layers and processes.
For self-related engagement, brand attachment seems to be the As mentioned earlier, the model proposes that the five psy-
essential construct that expresses a consumer's connection with chological
a brand processes do not necessarily occur in a partic-
brand. Brand attachment provides stronger connections thanular order. Although I proceeded didactically from identifying
brand attitudes (Thomson et al., 2005). Attachment was originallyrelevant information and experiencing the brand, to integrating
used in developmental psychology in the realm of parent-infant
information and experiences, to signifying and connecting with
relationships to define a strong bond between a child and caretak-the brand, some of these processes may occur in alternative or-
ders. For example, after trial, a consumer may have formed a
er (Bowlby, 1979). After childhood, attachment can manifest it-
self in romantic relationships (Hazan & Shaver, 1994), kinships,positive attitude (e.g., toward a consumer electronics brand)
and friendships (Trinke & Bartholomew, 1997; Weiss, 1988). that he only subsequently confirms through a brand concept
In the realm of consumer psychology, consumers can form(e.g., as an "innovative brand"). Or the consumer may endow
emotional attachments to gifts, collectibles, places of residence,a brand with a personality ("exciting") and subsequently search
and, in particular, brands (Thomson et al., 2005). Brand attach-for associated features and benefits that confirm it. Or, after be-
ment and brand attitude have distinct conceptual properties and coming part of a brand community, a consumer may judge her
formation processes, and, therefore, different behavioral impli- brand relationship to be a "friendship." Future research should
cations (Park, Maclnnis, Priester, Eisingerich, & Iacobucci, investigate under what conditions the processes of the model
2010). Brand attachment predicts consumer intentions to per- occur sequentially from identifying to connecting and when
form behaviors that use significant resources, such as time, they occur in a different sequence.
money, and reputation, better than brand attitudes. Attachment There should also be tests concerning whether the three layers
may be viewed as an antecedent of true loyalty (Foumier of & engagement are psychologically distinct. Is there, as proposed,
Yao, 1997). a relatively basic object-centered and functionally-guided con-
sumer engagement process that includes simple categorization
Brand community and multi-sensory processing that can be summarized in a
brand concept that serves as an informational cue for brand atti-
Moving to the interpersonal and socio-cultural engagementtudes? Is there a self-centered engagement process that includes
level, researchers have examined consumer connections with brand associations, affect and personality and serves as an identi-
brand communities. A brand community is "a specialized,ty signal that leads to attachment toward the brand? Finally, can
non-geographically bound community, based on a structured we verify an interpersonal and social engagement process
set of social relationships among users of a brand" (Muniz & where consumers examine brands contextually and process
O'Guinn, 2001). Brand communities are explicitly commercialinter-brand relations, create their own experiences through partic-
and, unlike other communities (such as neighborhood groups,ipation, relate to brands as cultural symbols and view themselves
collectors or social clubs), not necessarily tied to geography. as part of a brand community?
Conclusion
In testing the model or parts of it, we should allow for mod-
erator effects. Particular emphasis should be placed on modera-
While research
tors that put differential emphasis on one engagement in the
layer orbranding area has usually been narrow
and not integrative,
another - for example, independent and interdependent some have been waiting for a comprehen-
selves
(Ng & Houston, 2006; Swaminathan et al., sive2007) or self-
consumer-psychology model of brands - a "blueprint of
brand engagement (Sprott et alģ, 2009). brand knowledge, as comprehensive while also as parsimoni-
ous as possible, that would provide the necessary depth and
Consumer neuroscience breadth of understanding of consumer behavior and marketing
activity" (Keller, 2003). I feel that the model that I presented
Most of the research that I am suggesting for testinghere
thehas achieved this kind of comprehensive, yet parsimoni-
model can be done using standard research techniques such
ous, as
blueprint of brands from a consumer psychology perspec-
tive. The model, developed primarily for an academic audience,
experiments, surveys and causal modeling. However, because
we proposed that the five processes of the model might summarizes
not be and integrates existing constructs and findings and
stepwise but widely distributed, newly emerging consumerintends
in- to stimulate more systematic future research. By draw-
ing the attention of practitioners to the key psychological fac-
sight techniques may be useful as well. For example, consumer
neuroscience addresses consumer relevant problems tors usingunderlying brand effects, the model may also provide a
bridge from theory to business practice and inspire managers
methods originally used for brain research (Hubert & Kenning,
to create functionally useful, psychologically meaningful and
2008). Brain imaging techniques examine activations as they
occur spontaneously and simultaneously throughout the culturally
entire relevant brands.
brain and may thus help to test and refine the proposed model
(Shiv et al., 2005; Yoon, Gutchess, Feinberg, & Polk, 2006).
Consumer neuroscience on brands has provided some References
pre-
liminary evidence that is consistent with the model. Consider
Aaker, D. (1991). Managing brand equity: Capitalizing on the value of a brand
the well known "Coke-Pepsi" brain study by McClure et al.
name. New York: The Free Press.
(2004). While observing the typical "Pepsi challenge" Aaker,
effectD. (1996). Building strong brands. New York: The Free Press.
that consumers prefer Pepsi in a blind test over Coke, the au-J. L. (1997). Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Re-
Aaker,
thors also observed stronger activations in brain regions associ-
search, 34, 447-356.
ated with emotions when participants were shown a Coke Aaker, J. L. (1999). The malleable self: The role of self-expression in persua-
logo
sion. Journal of Marketing Research, 36,45-51.
than a Pepsi logo. Importantly, the effect could be reversed with
Aaker, D. (2004). Brand portfolio strategy. New York: The Free Press.
patients whose ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an emotion area,
Aaker, J., Benet-Martínez, V., & Garolera, J. (2001). Consumption symbols as
was damaged: they preferred Pepsi to Coke both in a blind taste
carriers of culture: A study of Japanese and Spanish brand personality con-
test and in a test that featured brand information (Koenigs & Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 492-508.
structs.
Tranel, 2008). These and other results suggest that brandAaker,
choiceJ. L., Foumier, S., & Brasei, S. A. (2004). When good brands do bad.
Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 1-16.
can be driven by sensory information in some contexts, but also
Aaker, D. A., & Keller, K. L. (1990). Consumer evaluations of brand exten-
by experienced emotions (Esch et al., 2012-this issue). More-
sions. Journal of Marketing, 54, 27-41.
over, results from a series of brain scanning studies suggest
Adaval, R. (2003). How good gets better and bad gets worse: Understanding the
that brands may be processed differently for extracting informa-
impact of affect on evaluations of known brands. The Journal of Consumer
tion for functional decision-making, personal meaningResearch,
and 30, 352-367.
Agarwal, M. K., & Rao, V. R. (1996). An empirical comparison of consumer-
inter-personal relevance. For example, the prefrontal cortex
based measures of brand equity. Marketing Letters, 3, 237-247.
seems to be differentially engaged for value versus prestige
Aggarwal, P. (2004). The effects of brand relationship norms on consumer atti-
brands; moreover, the medial prefrontal cortex and precuneus
tudes and behavior. The Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 87-101.
seem to be involved in the processing of luxury brands (associ-
Aggarwal, P., & Law, S. (2005). Role of relationship norms in processing brand
information. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 453-464.
ated with self-centered cognitions) whereas the left superior
Aggarwal, P., & McGill, A. L. (2007). Is this car smiling at me? Schema con-
frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex are activated for
gruity as basis for evaluating anthropomorphized products. Journal of Con-
value brands that are more likely to follow rather objective,
sumer Research, 34, 468-479.
functional choice (Schaefer, 2009). Finally, abstract, semantic
Akaoui, J. (2007). Brand experience on the pitch: How the sponsors fared in the
brand concepts can be neurologically distinguished from affec-
World Cup. Journal of Advertising Research, 47, 147-157.
Alba, J. &
tive, self-relevant brand concepts (Ratnayake, Broderick, W., & Chattopadhyay, A. (1985). Effects of context and part-category
cues on recall of competing brands. Journal of Marketing Research, 22,
Mitchell, 2010). 340-349.
In future consumer-neuroscience research, the model Alba,
shouldJ. W., Hutchinson, W. J., & Lynch, J. (1991). Memory and decision mak-
be tested more comprehensively and rigorously, e.g., by manip-
ing. In Thomas S. Robertson, & Harold H. Kassarijian (Eds.), Handbook of
ulating levels of engagement, rather than measuring them
Consumer Behavior (pp. 1-49). NJ: Englewood Cliffs.
through brands or category types. This could be done byAlgesheimer,
prim- R., Dholakia, U. M., & Herrmann, A. (2005). The social influence
of brand community: Evidence from European car clubs. Journal of Market-
ing the levels of engagement and giving individuals simple
ing, 69, 19-34.
tasks related to the five processes of identifying, experiencing,
Algom, D., & Cain, W. S. (1991). Remembered odors and mental mixtures:
integrating, signifying and connecting, while measuring activa-
Tapping reservoirs of olfactory knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psy-
tions in brain regions. chology. Human Perception and Performance, 17, 1 104-1 119.
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Guido, G. (2001). Brand personality: How
Hubert, M., & Kenning, P. (2008). A current overview of consumer neurosci-
to make the metaphor fit? Journal of Economic Psychology, 22, 377-395. ence. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 7, 272-292.
Carroll, B. A., & Ahuvia, A. C. (2006). Some antecedents and outcomes of Janiszewski, C., & van Osselaer, S. M. J. (2000). A connectionist model of
brand love. Marketing Letters, 1 7, 79-89. brand-quality associations. Journal of Marketing Research, 37, 331-350.
Chaplin, L. N., & Roedder John, D. (2005). The development of self-brand con-Keller, K. L. (1987). Memory factors in advertising: The effect of advertising
nections in children and adolescents. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, retrieval cues on brand evaluations. The Journal of Consumer Research,
119-129. 74,316-333.
Keller,
Chaudhuri, A., & Holbrook, M. B. (2001). The chain of effects from brand K. L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing consumer-
trust
and brand affect to brand performance: The Role of Brand Loyalty. Journal
based brand equity. Journal of Marketing, 57, 1-22.
of Marketing, 65, 81-93. Keller, K. L. (2002). Branding and brand equity. In Bart Weitz, & Robin
Wensley (Eds.), Handbook of Marketing (pp. 151-178). London: Sage
Delgado-B, E., Munuera- Aleman, J. L., & Yagoe-Guillin, M. J. (2003). Devel-
Publications.
opment and validation of a brand trust scale. International Journal of Mar-
ket Research, 45, 335-353. Keller, K. L. (2003). Brand synthesis: The multidimensionality of brand knowl-
Dimoile, C. V., & Yalch, R. F. (201 1). The mere association effect and brand
edge. Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 595-600.
evaluations. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21, 24-37. Keller, K. L., & Aaker, D. A. (1992). The effects of sequential introduction of
Erdem, T., & Swait, J. (1998). Brand equity as a signaling phenomenon. brand
Jour- extensions. Journal of Marketing Research, 29, 35-50.
nal of Consumer Psychology, 7, 131-157. Keller, K. L., & Lehman, D. R. (2006). Brands and branding: Research findings
Escalas, J. E. (2004). Narrative processing: Building consumer connectionsand to
future priorities. Marketing Science, 25, 740-759.
brands. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 168-179. Kirmani, A. (2009). The self and the brand. Journal of Consumer of Business,
19, 271-275.
Esch, F. -R., Moli, T., Schmitt, B., Eiger, C., Neuhaus, C., & Weber, B. (2012).
Koenigs, M., & Tranel, D. (2008). Prefrontal cortex damage abolishes brand-
Brands on the brain: Do consumers use declarative information or experi-
enced emotions to evaluate brands? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22,changes in cola preference. Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosci-
cued
75-85 this issue. ence, 3, 1-6.
Farquhar, P. H., & Herr, P. M. (1993). The dual structure of brand associations.
Kreuzbauer, R., & Malter, A. J. (2005). Embodied cognition and new product
In David A. Aaker, & Alexander L. Biel (Eds.), Brand equity & advertising: design: Changing product form to influence brand categorization. Journal
Advertising's role in building strong brands (pp. 263-279). Hillsdale, NJ: of Product Innovation Management, 22, 165-176.
Laurence Erlbaum Associates. Leclerc, F., Schmitt, B. H., & Dubé, L. (1994). Foreign branding and its effects
Fedorikhin, A., Park, C. W., & Thomson, M. (2008). Beyond fit and attitude:on product perceptions and attitudes. Journal of Marketing Research, 31,
The effect of emotional attachment on consumer responses to brand exten- 264-270.
sions. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18(4), 281-291. Lowrey, T. M., Shrum, L. J., & Dubitsky, T. M. (2003). The Relation between
Foumier, S. (1998). Consumers and their brands: Developing relationship theo- brand-name linguistic characteristics and brand-name memory. Journal of
ry in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 343-373. Advertising, 32, 7-17.
Foumier, S., & Yao, J. L. (1997). Reviving brand loyalty: A reconceptualization
Lynch, J. G., & Srull, T. K. (1982). Memory and attentional factors in consumer
within the framework of consumer-brand relationships. International Jour-choice: Concepts and research methods. Journal of Consumer Research, 9,
nal of Research in Marketing, 14(5), 451-472. 18-37.
Zeithami,
Yorkston, E., & Menon, G. (2004). A sound idea: Phonetic effects V. A. names
of brand (1988). Consumer perceptions of price, quality,
means-end
on consumer judgments. The Journal of Consumer Research, model and synthesis of evidence. Journal of Mar
31, 43-51.
Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (2004). The role of auditory cues2-22.
in modulating the
perceived crispness and staleness of potato chips. Journal of S.,
Zhang, Sensory Stud-
& Schmitt, B. H. (2001). Creating local brands in multilingual inter-
ies , 19, 347-363. national marketing. Journal of Marketing Research , 38, 313-325.