Differential Susceptibility To Environmental Influences: Jay Belsky
Differential Susceptibility To Environmental Influences: Jay Belsky
Differential Susceptibility To Environmental Influences: Jay Belsky
Jay Belsky
University of California, Davis & King Abdulaziz University
USA
Evidence that adverse rearing environments exert negative effects particularly on children and adults
presumed ―vulnerable‖ for temperamental or genetic reasons may actually reflect something else:
heightened susceptibility to the negative effects of risky environments and to the beneficial effects of
supportive environments. Building on Belsky’s (1997, 2005; Belsky & Pluess, 2009) evolutionary-inspired
differential susceptibility hypothesis stipulating that some individuals, including children, are more
affected—both for better and for worse—by their environmental exposures and developmental
experiences, recent research consistent with this claim is reviewed. It reveals that in many cases, including
both observational field studies and experimental intervention ones, putatively vulnerable children and
adults are especially susceptible to both positive and negative environmental effects. In addition to
reviewing relevant evidence, unknowns in the differential-susceptibility equation are highlighted.
Key words: early childhood education and care, preschool, policy, ethnic minority children, special needs
children
15
Jay Belsky
16
Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences
17
Jay Belsky
18
Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences
Kooy-Hofland, van der Kooy, Bus, van and reward mechanisms and one
Ijzendoorn, & Bonsel, 2012). In other variant of this polymorphism, the 7-
words, only the putatively ―vulnerable‖ repeat allele, has been linked to lower
—those manifesting or likely to dopamine reception efficiency, van
manifest high levels of negativity— IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg
experienced developmental enhancement (2006) predicted this allele would
as a function of the interventions cited. moderate the association between
Similar results emerge among older maternal unresolved loss or trauma and
children, as Scott and O’Connor’s (2012) infant attachment disorganization.
parenting intervention resulted in the Having the 7-repeat DRD4 allele
most positive change in conduct among substantially increased risk for
emotionally dysregulated children (i.e., disorganization in children exposed to
loses temper, angry, touchy). maternal unresolved loss/trauma, as
expected, consistent with the diathesis-
Genes as Plasticity Factors stress framework; yet when children
Perhaps nowhere has the diathesis- with this supposed ―vulnerability gene‖
stress framework informed person-X- were raised by mothers who had no
environment interaction research more unresolved loss, they displayed significantly
than in the study of GXE interaction. less disorganization than agemates
Recent studies involving measured without the allele, regardless of
genes and measured environments also mothers’ unresolved-loss status
document both for better and for worse (Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn,
environmental effects--in the case of 2011).
susceptible individuals—as it turns out. Similar results emerged when the
Here I consider evidence pertaining to interplay between DRD4 and observed
two specific candidate genes before parental insensitivity in predicting
turning attention to research examining externalizing problems was studied in a
multiple genes at the same time. group of 47 twins (Bakermans-
Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, 2007).
DRD4 Children carrying the 7-repeat DRD4
One of the most widely studied allele raised by insensitive mothers
genetic polymorphisms in research displayed more externalizing behaviors
involving measured genes and than children without the DRD4 7-
measured environments pertains to a repeat (irrespective of maternal
particular allele (or variant) of the sensitivity), whereas children with the
dopamine receptor gene, DRD4. 7-repeat allele raised by sensitive
Because the dopaminergic system is mothers showed the lowest levels of
engaged in attentional, motivational, externalizing problem behavior
19
Jay Belsky
20
Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences
allele (l/l). The short allele has but best—showing least problems—
generally been associated with reduced when encountering few or none (e.g.,
expression of the serotonin transporter Wilhelm et al., 2006). Calling explicit
molecule––which is involved in the attention to such a pattern of results,
reuptake of serotonin from the synaptic Taylor and associates (2006) reported
cleft—and thus considered to be related that young adults homozygous for
to depression, either directly or in the short alleles (s/s) manifested greater
face of adversity. Indeed, the short depressive symptomatology than
allele has often been conceptualized as individuals with other allelic variants
a ―depression gene‖. when exposed to early adversity (i.e.,
Caspi and associates (2003) were the problematic childrearing history), as
first to show that the 5-HTTLPR well as many recent negative life events,
moderates effects of stressful life events yet the fewest symptoms when they
during early adulthood on depressive experienced a supportive early
symptoms, as well as on probability of environment or recent positive
suicide ideation/attempts and of major experiences. The same for-better-and-
depression episode at age 26 years. for-worse pattern of results concerning
Individuals with two s alleles proved depression are evident in Eley et al.’s
most adversely affected whereas effects (2004) research on adolescent girls who
on l/l genotypes were weaker or were and were not exposed to risky
entirely absent. Of special significance, family environments.
however, is that carriers of the s/s allele The effect of 5-HTTLPR in moderating
scored best on the outcomes just environmental influences in a manner
mentioned when stressful life events consistent with differential susceptibility is
were absent, though not by very much. not restricted to depression and its
Multiple research groups have symptoms. It also emerges in studies of
attempted to replicate Caspi et al.’s anxiety (Stein, Schork & Gelernter, 2008)
(2003) findings of increased vulnerability and ADHD, particularly ADHD which
to depression in response to stressful persists into adulthood (Retz et al.,
life events for individuals with one or 2008). In all these cases, emotional
more copies of the s allele, with many abuse in childhood (Stein et al., 2008) or
succeeding (see below), but certainly a generally adverse childrearing
not all (e.g., Surtees et al., 2006; Risch et environment (Retz et al., 2008), it
al., 2009). The data presented in quite a proved to be those individuals carrying
number of studies indicates, however, short alleles who responded to
that individuals carrying short alleles developmental or concurrent experiences
(s/s, s/l) did not just function most in a for-better-and-for-worse manner,
poorly when exposed to many stressors, depending on the nature of the
21
Jay Belsky
22
Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences
23
Jay Belsky
24
Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences
25
Jay Belsky
African American Youths: Testing the Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1998).
DRD4 Hypothesis. Journal of Family Mothering, fathering, and infant
Psychology, 24(5), 513-521. doi: negativity as antecedents of boys’
10.1037/a0020835 externalizing problems and inhibition
Beach, S. R. H., Lei, M. K., Brody, G. H., at age 3: Differential susceptibility to
Simons, R. L., Cutrona, C., & Philibert, rearing influence? Development and
R. A. (2012). Genetic Moderation of Psychopathology, 10, 301–319.
Contextual Effects on Negative Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond
Arousal and Parenting in African- Diathesis-Stress: Differential Susceptibility
American Parents. Journal of Family to Environmental Influences.
Psychology, 26(1), 46-55. doi: Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885-908.
10.1037/a0026236 Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2013a). Beyond
Beaver, K. M., Sak, A., Vaske, J., & Nilsson, Risk, Resilience and Dysregulation:
J. (2010). Genetic risk, parent-child Phenotypic Plasticity and Human
relations, and antisocial phenotypes in Development. Development and
a sample of African-American males. Psychopathology, 25, 1243-1261.
Psychiatry Research, 175, 160-164 Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2013b). Genetic
Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility Moderation of Early Child Care Effects
to rearing influence: An evolutionary on Behavior Problems and Social
argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182– Competence across Childhood: A
186. Developmental Analysis. Child
Belsky, J. (2005). Differential susceptibility Development, 84, 1209-1225.
to rearing influence: An evolutionary Blair, C. (2002). Early intervention for low
hypothesis and some evidence. In B. birth weight preterm infants: The role
Ellis & D. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of of negative emotionality in the
the social mind: Evolutionary psychology specification of effects. Development and
and child development (pp. 139–163). Psychopathology, 14, 311–332.
New York: Guilford. Boyce, W.T. & Ellis, B. (2005). Biological
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & sensitivity to context: I. An
van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For evolutionary-developmental theory of
better and for worse: Differential the origins and functions of stress
Susceptibility to environmental influences. reactivity. Development and
Current Directions in Psychological Psychopathology, 17, 271–301.
Science, 16(6), 300-304. Brody, G. H., Beach, S. R. H., Chen, Y.-F.,
Belsky, J. & Beaver, K. M. (2011). Obasi, E., Philibert, R. A., Kogan, S. M.,
Cumulative-genetic plasticity, parenting & Simons, R. L. (2011). Perceived
and adolescent self-control/regulation. discrimination, serotonin transporter
Journal of Child Psychology and linked polymorphic region status, and
Psychiatry, 52(5), 619-626. the development of conduct problems.
26
Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences
27
Jay Belsky
Smyke, A. T., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., Hankin, B. L., Nederhof, E., Oppenheimer,
& Zeanah, C. H. (2012). Genetic C. W., Jenness, J., Young, J. F., Abela, J.
sensitivity to the caregiving context: R. Z., … Oldehinkel, A. J. (2011).
The influence of 5httlpr and BDNF Differential susceptibility in youth:
val66met on indiscriminate social evidence that 5-HTTLPR x positive
behavior. Physiology and Behavior, parenting is associated with positive
106(5), 728-735. affect `for better and worse'.
Eley, T. C., Hudson, J. L., Creswell, C., Translational Psychiatry, 1, e44.
Tropeano, M., Lester, K. J., Cooper, Kegel, C. A. T., Bus, A. G., & Van
P., ... Collier, D. A. (2012). Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential
Therapygenetics: the 5HTTLPR and Susceptibility in Early Literacy
response to psychological therapy. Instruction Through Computer Games:
Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 236-241. doi: The Role of the Dopamine D4 Receptor
10.1038/mp.2011.132 Gene (DRD4). Mind, Brain, And
Eley, T. C., Sugden, K.., Corsico, A., Education, 5(2), 71-78.
Gregory, A. M., Sham, P., McGuffin, P., Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2012). Child
… Craig, I. W. (2004). Gene- temperament moderates effects of
environment interaction analysis of parent-child mutuality on self-
serotonin system markers with regulation: a relationship-based path
adolescent depression. Molecular for emotionally negative infants. Child
Psychiatry, 9(10), 908-915. Development, 83(4), 1275-1289.
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Klein Velderman, M., Bakermans-
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Kranenburg, M.J., Juffer, F., & van
IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential IJzendoorn, M.H. (2006). Effects of
susceptibility to the environment: an attachment-based interventions on
evolutionary--neurodevelopmental maternal sensitivity and infant
theory. Development and Psychopathology, attachment: Differential susceptibility
23(1), 7-28. of highly reactive infants. Journal of
Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C., & Yirmiya, N. Family Psychology, 20, 266–274.
(1999). Mother-infant affect synchrony Knafo, A., Israel, S., & Ebstein, R. P. (2011).
as an antecedent of the emergence of Heritability of children's prosocial
self-control. Developmental Psychology, behavior and differential susceptibility
35, 223–231. to parenting by variation in the
Fortuna, K., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., dopamine receptor D4 gene.
Mankuta, D., Kaitz, M., Avinun, R., Development and Psychopathology, 23(1),
Ebstein, R. P., & Knafo, A. (2011). 53-67.
Differential Genetic Susceptibility to Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., & Joy, M.E.
Child Risk at Birth in Predicting (2007). Children’s fearfulness as a
Observed Maternal Behavior. PLoS moderator of aprenting in early
ONE, 6(5), e19765. socialization. Developmental Psychology,
28
Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences
29
Jay Belsky
30
Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences
hassles. Genes, Brain & Behavior, 7, 403-410. Wilhelm, K., Mitchell, P. B., Niven, H.,
Van IJzendoorn, M.H., Belsky, J., & Finch, A., Wedgwood, L., Scimone, A.,
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. (2012). … Schofield, P. R. (2006). Life events,
Serotonin Transporter Genotype first depression onset and the
5HTTLPR as a Marker of Differential serotonin transporter gene. British
Susceptibility? A Meta-Analysis of Journal of Psychiatry, 188, 210-215.
Child and Adolescent Gene-by- Zuckerman, M. (1999). Vulnerability to
Environment Studies. Translational psychopathology: A biosocial model.
Psychiatry 2, e147. Washington: American Psychological
Association.
31