Introduction To Research Methods: April 2024
Introduction To Research Methods: April 2024
net/publication/379899185
CITATIONS READS
0 4,450
1 author:
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Samih Salah Mohammed on 16 October 2024.
Copyright Notice
Ethnicity and Nationalism in World Politics
Author: Samih Salah
Published by: International Relations Network
This work is protected by copyright. No part of this book may be reproduced,
stored, or transmitted in any form or by any means without prior written
permission from the author, except in the case of brief quotations in reviews or
articles.
Please note that this book has not been assigned an official ISBN number.
All rights reserved © 2024 Samih Salah.
Introduction to Research Methods Page |3
Copyright
The following chapters have been adapted from Principles of Sociological Inquiry, which was
adapted by the Saylor Academy without attribution to the original authors or publisher,
as requested by the licensor, is licensed under a CC BY-NC-SA 3.0 License.
Introduction to Research Methods Page |4
Preface
more approachable manner, the book aims to alleviate fears and facilitate
a more engaging learning experience.
Furthermore, the book recognizes the historical significance of
methodology and its evolution over time.
In summary, this book provides a practical and step-by-step guide to
conducting research, focusing on engaging students and simplifying
complex concepts. It offers a fresh approach to research methods, aimed
at making the subject more accessible and enjoyable for beginners.
Additionally, it acknowledges the historical significance of methodology,
adding depth and context to the content.
The book explores the nature of research as a creative and systematic
endeavor to expand knowledge. It involves collecting, organizing, and
analyzing information to enhance understanding, often building upon
previous work. Research serves various purposes, including exploration,
description, and explanation, catering to the researcher's curiosity and
desire for knowledge.
Social research is described as a systematic attempt to broaden our
understanding of the world using socially accepted methods. It examines
people as both subjects and benefactors, contributing to the development
of knowledge and addressing societal challenges.
The book emphasizes the importance of a methodological strategy,
starting with a research question and making informed decisions
accordingly. It introduces three main research strategies: survey research,
case study, and experimentation.
Survey research involves collecting standardized information from groups
of people, often through questionnaires and structured interviews. Case
study focuses on developing detailed knowledge about complex subjects,
utilizing various data collection techniques and studying cases in their
context. Experimentation measures the effect of one variable on another,
typically involving sample selection, variable control, and hypothesis
testing.
Overall, this book equips readers with practical tools and techniques to
conduct research effectively, while recognizing the historical and
methodological significance behind the process.
SAMIH SALAH.
Khartoum ,2019.
Introduction to Research Methods Page |8
Content
Chapter I
Introduction to Research Methods
1.1 What are the Research Methods?
1.2 Basis for political and practical decisions
1.3 qualitative & quotative research
1.4 The process of undertaking Research
1.5 where do research Ideas came from
1.6 Undertaking key Research concepts and terms
1.7 Ontology & epistemology
1.8 Ontology
1.9 Exercise
1.10 Epistemology
1.11 Research paradigms In Social Research
1.12 Inductive Approaches to Research
1.13 Deductive research approaches
Summary
References
Chapter II
Ethics in Research
Chapter III
Developing a Research Question
Chapter IV
Measurement and Units of Analysis
4.1 Reliability
4.2 Validity
4.3 Complexities in Measurement
4.4 units of analysis and units of observation
4.5 Independent and development variables
4.6 Extraneous Variables
4.7 Rival Plausible Explanations
Summary
Key Takeaway
References
Chapter V
The Literature Review
Chapter VI
Data Collection strategies
6.1 Experiments
6.1.1 Random Assignation
6.2 Nonexperimental Research
6.2.1 Cross-sectional research
6.2.2 Correlational Research
6.2.3 Observational Research
6.3 Quasi-Experiments
6.4 Internal Validity
Summary
References
Chapter VII
Sampling Techniques
7.1 Sampling
7.2 Population versus samples
7.3 Probabilistic and Non-Probabilistic Sampling Techniques
7.4 Who Sampled, How Sampled, and for what Purpose..
Summary
References
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 10
Chapter VIII
Data collection Methods: survey Research.
Chapter X
Qualitative Data Collections & Analysis Methods
Chapter XI
Quantitative Interview Techniques & Consideration.
Chapter XII
Quantitative Interview Techniques & Considerations
Chapter XIII
Filed Research: A Qualitative Research Technique
Chapter XIII
Unobtrusive Research: Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches
Chapter XIV
The Research Proposal
Chapter XV
Sharing Your Research
Chapter XVI
Reading and Understanding Social Research
I
Introduction to Research Methods
Interpret
Analyze the Communication
The the finding
data
Data
Figure 1.1
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 27
Figure 1.2
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 28
The origin of research ideas stems from various sources. Researchers draw
inspiration from a range of places, such as replicating, clarifying, or
challenging previous research, as well as resolving conflicting results. New
technology, like the impact of Facebook or Twitter on society, can spark
research ideas. Serendipitous findings that pique a researcher's curiosity,
anomalies that defy expectations, or the desire to explore further
something commonly believed to be known can also drive research. This
is sometimes referred to as common sense research, where history,
tradition, or the practical needs of a field, such as public safety, provide
researchers with problem-solving opportunities.
Research is ubiquitous, arising from agencies seeking to achieve specific
goals, concerns about policy changes, or individuals making observations
or questioning the world around them. Typically, research begins with
broad "why" or "how" questions, but it undergoes an iterative process
that requires refinement.
As the motivations behind research projects vary, so do the types of
research questions. Research can be exploratory, descriptive, relational,
explanatory, or transformative, each with distinct methods and objectives.
Therefore, it is crucial to identify the research project's objectives to
determine the most appropriate research method. The subsequent step
involves formulating a research question, which will be further explored
in Chapter 2.
strategy has its own data collection and analysis approaches associated
with the various methodological approaches you choose. Figure 1.1 is
intentioned to provide a general overview of the research concept. You
may want to keep this figure handy as you read through the various
chapters.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 30
Figure 1.3
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 31
8.Ontology
Ontology, derived from the Greek word meaning "the study, theory, or
science of being," focuses on understanding the nature of reality or "what
is" (Saunders, Lewis, & Thornhill, 2009). It delves into profound and
challenging questions, encompassing topics such as: - The purpose of life:
Ontology explores the fundamental question of why we exist and what
gives meaning to human existence. - Existence beyond our universe: It
investigates the possibility of entities or phenomena that exist outside the
boundaries of our known universe. - Categorization: Ontology explores
the classification and categorization of entities, phenomena, or concepts
based on their inherent characteristics or attributes. - Objective reality: It
examines the concept of whether there is an external reality that exists
independently of our subjective experiences or perceptions. - The
meaning of the verb "to be": Ontology delves into the philosophical and
linguistic implications of the verb "to be" and its role in expressing
existence, identity, and essence. These questions within ontology invite
deep contemplation and philosophical inquiry, acknowledging that they
may not have definitive or universally agreed-upon answers. Ontology is
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 32
10. Exercise
In social science, there are several predominant paradigms, each with its
own unique ontological and epistemological perspective. We will look at
some of the most common social scientific paradigms that might guide
you in starting to think about conducting your research. The first
paradigm we will consider, positivism, is probably the framework that
comes to mind for many of you when you think of science. Positivism is
guided by the principles of objectivity, knowability, and deductive logic.
Deductive logic is discussed in more detail in the section that follows. The
positivist framework operates from the assumption that society can and
should be studied empirically and scientifically. Positivism also calls for a
value-free sociology, one in which researchers aim to abandon their biases
and values in a quest for objective, empirical, and knowable truth. An
Interpretivist paradigm suggests that it is necessary for researchers to
understand the differences amongst humans as social actors. The
emphasis is on conducting research among people, as opposed to objects.
As Saunders et al. observe, the reference to social actors’ bears noting.
They use the analogy of the theatre, where actors interpret, in a specific
way, the parts they play. They relate this to the same way in which people
interpret their social roles in relationship and how they then give meaning
to those roles. Similarly, people interpret the social roles of others in
accordance with their own meanings of those roles. Figure 1.3 provides
an example of two students, each from a difference academic field of
study, and how they might approach their research in their respective
fields.
Gather data
Develop theory
Inductive Reasoning
Figure 1.4
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 39
Theorizing /hypothesis
Hypothesis
supported or
not.
Figure 1.6
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 40
study found that sisters play a significant role in shaping boys' early
comprehension of menstruation, that it creates a sense of separation
between boys and girls, and that young men develop more mature
attitudes towards menstruation as they enter adulthood and form
romantic relationships. This research exemplifies the inductive approach
by starting with specific observations and constructing a general theory
based on the patterns identified in the collected data.
Another example of inductive research is the study conducted by Kristin
Ferguson and colleagues (2011) on enhancing empowerment and
leadership among homeless youth in agency and community settings. The
researchers collected
empirical data through focus groups with 20 young people residing in a
homeless shelter. By analyzing the data, they aimed to gain insights into
effectively addressing the needs of homeless youth. Based on their
analysis, Ferguson and her colleagues developed a set of
recommendations for applied interventions targeting this population.
Additionally, they formulated hypotheses that could guide future
investigations on the topic. While the researchers did not test these
hypotheses in their study, their work provided a foundation for further
research and concluded with a set of testable hypotheses, which is typically
where deductive investigations begin. This study exemplifies the inductive
approach by starting with specific data and generating recommendations
and hypotheses for future research.
Theorizing Hypothesis
/hypothesize Analyze Data
supported
or not
EXERCISES
1. For a hilarious example of logic gone awry, check out the following clip
from Monty Python and Holy Grail: Do the townspeople take an
inductive or deductive approach to determine whether the woman in
question is a witch? What are some of the different sources of knowledge
(recall Chapter 1 "Introduction") they rely on?
2. Think about how you could approach a study of the relationship
between gender and driving over the speed limit. How could you learn
about this relationship using an inductive approach? What would a study
of the same relationship look like if examined using a deductive approach?
Try the same thing with any topic of your choice. How might you study
the topic inductively? Deductively?
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 45
References
Aylesworth, G. (2015). Postmodernism. In Stanford encyclopedia of
philosophy. Retrieved from
https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/postmodernism/ Palys, T., &
Atchison, C. (2014). Research decisions: Quantitative, qualitative, and
mixed methods approaches (5th ed.). Toronto, Canada: Nelson
Education. Paradigm. (2018). In Macmillan dictionary. Retrieved from
http://www.macmillandictionaryblog.com/paradigm Saunders, M.,
Lewis, P., & Thornhill, A. (2009). Research methods for business students
(5th ed.). Essex, England: Pearson Education. Saylor Academy. (2012).
Principles of sociological inquiry: Qualitative and quantitative methods.
Retrieved from
https://resources.saylor.org/wwwresources/archived/site/textbooks/P
rinciples%20of%20Sociological%20Inquiry.pdf
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 46
II
ETHICS IN RESEARCH
Learning Objectives
• Define the term human participants, in terms of research.
• Explain how history has now defined a moral imperative for ethics in
Research.
• List ethical principles that must underpin all research.
• Describe why ethics review boards came into existence and explain their
role in the research process.
• Discuss the importance and implications of researcher integrity.
The National Council on Ethics in Human Research (NCEHR) is a
Canadian national agency that was established in 1989 with the primary
objective of promoting and enhancing the protection and well-being of
human research participants.
The NCEHR plays a crucial role in ensuring that researchers in Canada
adhere to ethical guidelines and standards when conducting studies
involving human participants. Its overarching goal is to safeguard the
rights, welfare, and dignity of individuals involved in research endeavours.
To achieve its mission, the NCEHR has a range of key objectives:
1. Establishing Ethical Standards: The NCEHR develops comprehensive
guidelines and ethical frameworks that serve as a reference for researchers,
providing them with clear principles and values to guide their research
involving human participants.
2. Reviewing Research Protocols: The NCEHR conducts thorough
reviews of research protocols to assess their ethical implications. Through
this process, the agency ensures that proposed studies meet the necessary
ethical requirements and protect the rights and well-being of participants.
3. Education and Training: The NCEHR offers educational resources,
training programs, and guidance to researchers, ethics review boards, and
other stakeholders involved in research. These initiatives aim to enhance
understanding and compliance with ethical principles in studies involving
human participants.
4. Collaborating with Ethics Review Boards: The NCEHR collaborates
closely with research ethics review boards across Canada to promote
consistency and adherence to ethical standards. This collaborative effort
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 47
lacks validity. What exactly constitutes "a problem" with alcohol? For
some, it could be a single regrettable or embarrassing moment resulting
from excessive consumption. For others, the threshold for a "problem"
might be different; perhaps an individual has had numerous embarrassing
drunken incidents but still manages to fulfill daily responsibilities, and thus
does not perceive themselves as having a problem. Due to the substantial
variation in what each respondent considers problematic, our measure of
alcoholism is unlikely to yield useful or meaningful results if our objective
is to objectively understand the prevalence of alcoholism among our
research participants.
At its core, validity relies on social agreement. One effective way to ensure
the validity of your measures is to discuss them with others. To illustrate,
think of validity as you would a portrait. Some portraits accurately depict
the person they aim to represent, while others, like caricatures or stick
drawings, are less precise. While a portrait may not capture every detail of
a person's appearance, what matters is how closely it approximates the
intended representation. The same principle applies to the validity of
measures. No measure can be exact, but some measures are more accurate
than others.
6.Summary
Research is serious business. Not only must the conduct of research be
undertaken in a manner such that it abides by society’s ethical standards,
researchers must personally have a strong set of moral standards.
Researchers must ensure that their participants (human and animal) are
treated ethically, and that, in the case of human participants, their
confidentiality is maintained. They must also apply ethical principles in
the design of their studies, as well as the collection, analysis and
presentation of the data. Overall, an ethic of research involving both
human and animal participants should include two essential components:
1) the selection and achievement of morally acceptable ends, and;
2) morally acceptable means to the ends. The first component is directed
at defining acceptable ends in terms of the benefits of the research for a
given set of participants, for associate groups, and for the purposes of
advancing knowledge. The second component is directed at ethically
appropriate means of conducting research.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 53
References
CHAPTER III
DEVELOPING A RES EARCH QUESTION
Learning Objectives
If you concluded that the first statement is normative and the second
statement is empirical, you are correct. Normative statements express an
opinion or judgment, while empirical statements rely on real-world
observations and data. Although normative statements can underlie or
influence empirical statements, sociologists primarily focus on answering
empirical questions that can be addressed through real-world
experiences.
As you can see, there is much to think about and many decisions to be
made as you begin to define your research question and your research
project. Something else you will need to consider in the early stages is
whether your research will be exploratory, descriptive, or explanatory.
Each of these types of research has a different aim or purpose,
consequently, how you design your research project will be determined
in part by this decision. In the following paragraphs we will look at these
three types of research.
Exploratory research Researchers conducting exploratory research are
typically at the early stages of examining their topics. These sorts of
projects are usually conducted when a researcher wants to test the
feasibility of conducting a more extensive study; he or she wants to figure
out the lay of the land with respect to the particular topic. Perhaps very
little prior research has been conducted on this subject. If this is the case,
a researcher may wish to do some exploratory work to learn what method
to use in collecting data, how best to approach research participants, or
even what sorts of questions are reasonable to ask. A researcher wanting
to simply satisfy his or her own curiosity about a topic could also conduct
exploratory research. Conducting exploratory research on a topic is often
a necessary first step, both to satisfy researcher curiosity about the subject
and to better understand the phenomenon and the research participants
in order to design a larger, subsequent study.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 57
3.4 hypotheses
References
Adjei, J.K. (n.d.). Research methods. Retrieved from African Virtual
University website: https://oer.avu.org/handle/123456789/490
Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development. (2016). PISA
2015: Results in focus. https://doi.org/10.1787/aa9237e6-en Saylor
Academy. (2012). Principles of sociological inquiry: Qualitative and
quantitative methods. Retrieved from
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 62
CHAPTER IV
MEASUREMENT AND UNITS OF ANALYSIS
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
•Differentiate between validity and reliability. • Explain the difference
between internal and external validity. • Examine the difference between
a variable and an attribute.
• Define and provide examples for each of the four level of
measurement: nominal, ordinal, interval, ratio.
• Explain the difference between the independent and dependent
variable.
• Describe an extraneous variable and explain how it can threaten
research findings.
• Discuss what is meant by a rival plausible explanation.
• Explain what a hypothesis is and in what situations creating a
hypothesis is a suitable approach.
How can we ensure the quality of our measures? Without some form of
assurance regarding the quality of our measures, we cannot be confident
that our findings hold any significance, or at the very least, that they
convey the intended meaning. In the realm of social science, when
researchers measure concepts, their objective is to attain reliability and
validity in their measures. These two dimensions of measurement quality
are the primary focus of the initial section in this chapter. We will first
delve into reliability and subsequently explore validity. For the purpose of
this section, let's envision that our interest lies in measuring the concepts
of alcoholism and alcohol intake. What are some potential challenges that
may arise when attempting to measure these concepts, and how can we
address and overcome them?
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 63
4.1 Reliability
4.2 Validity
You should now have an idea about how to assess the quality of your
measures. But measurement is a complex process, and some concepts are
more complex than others. Measuring a person’s political party affiliation,
for example, is less complex than measuring her or his sense of alienation.
In this section we will consider some of these complexities in
measurement. First, we will examine the various levels of measurement
that exist, and then we will consider a couple of strategies for capturing
the complexities of the concepts we wish to measure. Levels of
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 66
comparison to another. For example, it would not make sense to say that
50 degrees is half as hot as 100 degrees. Ratio measurement Finally, at the
ratio level, attributes are mutually exclusive and exhaustive, attributes can
be rank ordered, the distance between attributes is equal, and attributes
have a true zero point. With these variables, we can say what the ratio of
one attribute is in comparison to another. Examples of ratio-level
variables include age and years of education. We know, for example, that
a person who is 12 years old is twice as old as someone who is six years
old.
potential unit of analysis that social scientists might wish to say something
about. Organizations include entities like corporations, colleges and
universities, and even night clubs. At the organization level, a study of
students’ cell phone addictions might ask, “How do different colleges
address the problem of cell phone addiction?” In this case, our interest
lies not in the experience of individual students but instead in the campus-
to-campus differences in confronting cell phone addictions. A researcher
conducting a study of this type might examine schools’ written policies
and procedures, so his unit of observation would be documents.
However, because he ultimately wishes to describe differences across
campuses, the college would be his unit of analysis. Social phenomena are
also a potential unit of analysis. Many sociologists study a variety of social
interactions and social problems that fall under this category. Examples
include social problems like murder or rape; interactions such as
counselling sessions, Facebook chatting, or wrestling; and other social
phenomena such as voting and even cell phone use or misuse. A
researcher interested in students’ cell phone addictions could ask, “What
are the various types of cell phone addictions that exist among students?”
Perhaps the researcher will discover that some addictions are primarily
centred on social media such as chat rooms, Facebook, or texting, while
other addictions centre on single-player games that discourage interaction
with others. The resultant typology of cell phone addictions would tell us
something about the social phenomenon (unit of analysis) being studied.
As in several of the preceding examples, however, the unit of observation
would likely be individual people. Finally, a number of social scientists
examine policies and principles, the last type of unit of analysis we will
consider here. Studies that analyze policies and principles typically rely on
documents as the unit of observation. Perhaps a researcher has been hired
by a college to help it write an effective policy against cell phone use in
the classroom. In this case, the researcher might gather all previously
written policies from campuses all over the country, and compare policies
at campuses where the use of cell phones in classroom is low to policies
at campuses where the use of cell phones in the classroom is high.
apprehended. This individual is still on the loose. How do you think this
incident will affect the members of the community and the local
businesses? How might this incident affect how your survey participants
fill out the survey, as it relates to their feelings related to the centre? How
might their survey answers differ, had the survey taken place before this
incident, when there had been no such incidents? It is quite likely that this
event will impact or “colour” the responses of your participants. In other
words, there is now a strong likelihood that you have an RPE as to why
the research participants have reacted negatively to the safe injection
center. RPEs are serious, and while it is true that careful research design
can eliminate threats to internal validity, the incident as outlined in the
previous paragraph demonstrates how an RPE can sink a research project.
As a researcher you spent a lot of time designing and planning your
research, but essentially the findings are null, in this case, because you are
not getting the true feelings of the community. Their feelings will have
been negatively influenced by this recent incident. The researcher must
decide how significant and how likely it is that the RPE influenced the
results, in order to decide whether or not to scrap the research project.
While the preceding is an example of a blatant RPE, some are less
obvious. Researchers must always consider the likelihood that an RPE
explains the results of their findings when analyzing data. Less blatant
RPEs (i.e. weather, postal strikes, a new government policy, recent media
attention to an incident related to your research) must be discussed in the
limitations section of the research finding.
References
Explore Psychology. (2019). What is an extraneous variable? Retrieved
from https://www.explorepsychology.com/what-is-an-extraneous-
variable/ Palys, T., & Atchison, C. (2014). Research decisions:
Quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods approaches (5th ed.).
Toronto, Canada: Nelson Education. Saylor Academy. (2012). Principles
of sociological inquiry: Qualitative and quantitative methods. Retrieved
from
https://www.saylor.org/site/textbooks/Principles%20of%20Sociologic
al%20Inquiry.pdf.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 72
CHAPTER V
THE LITERATURE REVIEW
Learning Objectives
How have the various researchers defined key terms that are relevant to
your topic? Have the definitions of any of the key terms evolved over
time? What are the different theories that have been examined and applied
to this topic? How, if at all, have the various theories applied to this topic
evolved over time? What methodologies have been used to study this
topic? Have the methodologies evolved over time? In addition to thinking
about these questions, you should be taking notes during this process. It
canbe helpful to keep these notes in an Excel file, e.g., your notes should
include the following information: If the article is empirical, write down
the results of the research study in one or two sentences of your own
words, e.g., “people who are between ages 18 – 35 are more likely to own
a smart phone than those in an age range above or below.” It is also a
good idea to take note of the methods, research design, number of
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 74
participants, and details of the sample used in the study. Sometimes, you
may even want to write down the names of the statistical procedures used
to analyze the data or even some of the statistics, depending on your
assignment. If the article is a review of previous research, look for the
main points. It may be helpful to read or skim the whole article, look away,
and ask yourself what you felt was the main idea. Write down any
limitations or gaps you notice, anything that seems to contradict
something you read elsewhere, or just anything that you think is important
or interesting (Adjei, n.d.). When reading through your sources, remember
that you are looking for the “big picture,” not a collection of random,
separate articles (an annotated bibliography). You are also not trying to
prove a point (an essay). You are looking for common themes and
patterns in the research as a whole. You are also looking to see how the
various pieces of research are linked, if at all. As part of this process, you
also want to identify research gaps or areas that require further research
related your topic (Adjei, n.d.). In this regard, you cannot be expected to
be an expert on your topic. A suggestion for finding gaps is to read the
conclusion section of the academic journal articles and conference
proceedings your search has uncovered. Researchers often identify gaps
in the research in their conclusion. They may even suggest areas for future
research. However, remember, if a researcher suggested a gap 10 years
ago, it is likely that the gap has now been addressed. To find a gap, look
at the most recent research your literature review has uncovered (within
2-3 years of the current date). At this point in your search of the literature,
you may realize that your research question needs to change or adapt. This
is a fairly common occurrence, since when you first develop a research
question, you cannot be sure what the status of the research area is until
you undertake your review of the literature related to this topic. Finally, it
is worth mentioning that it is very likely you will not include all of the
resources you have read in your literature review. If you are asked to
include 20 resources in your literature review, e.g., expect to read
approximately 30.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 75
Articles from professional journals should be used with caution for your
literature review. This is because articles in trade journals are not usually
peer reviewed, even though they may appear to be. A good way to find
out is to read the “About Us” section of the professional journal, which
should state whether or not the papers are peer reviewed. You can also
find out by Googling the name of the journal and adding “peer reviewed”
to the search.
But again, this type of website- based material should be used with caution
and sparingly.
To help you frame and write your literature review, think about these five
c’s (Callahan, 2014):
1. Cite the material you have referred to and used to help you define the
research problem that you will study.
2. Compare the various arguments, theories, methods, and findings
expressed in the literature.For example, describe where the various
researchers agree and where they disagree. Describe the similarities and
dissimilarities in approaches to studying related research problems.
3. Contrast the various arguments, themes, methods, approaches, and
controversies apparent and/or described in the literature. For example,
describe what major areas are contested, controversial and/or still in
debate.
4. Critique the literature. Describe which arguments you find more
persuasive and explain why. Explain which approaches, findings, and
methods seem most reliable, valid, appropriate, and/or most popular and
why. Pay attention to the verbs you use to describe what previous
researchers have stated (e.g., asserts, demonstrates, argues, clarifies, etc.).
5. Connect the various research studies you reviewed. Describe how your
work utilizes, draws upon, departs from, synthesizes, adds to or extends
previous research studies.
References
Adjei, J. K. (n.d.). Research methods. Retrieved from African Virtual
University website: https://oer.avu.org/handle/123456789/490
Callahan, J. L. (2014). Writing literature reviews: A reprise and update.
Human Resource Development Review, 13(3), 271-275.
doi:10.1177/1534484314536705 Edited volume. (2019, October 15). In
Wikipedia. Retrieved December 10, 2019, from
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Edited_volume Palys, T., & Atchison, C.
(2014). Research decisions: Quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods
approaches (5th ed.). Toronto, Canada: Nelson Education. Thomas, J.
(2012, September 26). Literature review vs. essay
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 81
CHAPTER VI
DATA COLLECTION STRATEGIES
Learning Objectives
• Differentiate between the various data collection strategies for
experimental, non-experimental and experimental research.
• Differentiate the experimental and the control group.
• Define random assignation and explain its importance in an experiment.
• Explain how internal validity can be affected by research design.
Researchers have a variety of data collection strategies to choose from.
Selecting the appropriate data collection strategy is a crucial decision for
the researcher. If an unsuitable strategy is chosen, it is probable that the
researcher will be unable to address the research question(s) effectively.
In the upcoming sections, we will explore three types of data collection
strategies: experiments, quasi-experiments, and non-experimental
approaches.
6.1 Experiments
Example 1
In a study on PTSD, a total of 100 police officers from the Winnipeg
Police Department were included as participants and randomly assigned
to either an experiment group or a control group. To assess their levels of
PTSD, all participants, regardless of their group assignment, underwent
an identical pre-test. The results of the pre-test revealed no significant
differences in reported symptoms related to PTSD between the
experiment and control groups. Subsequently, participants in the
experiment group were instructed to watch a video showcasing scenic
travel routes in Manitoba, while the control group did not receive any
specific stimulus. Afterward, both groups underwent a post-test to
reevaluate their reported symptoms associated with PTSD. Upon
analyzing the post-test scores, the researchers discovered that individuals
who were exposed to the experimental stimulus (the video on car
accidents) reported greater symptoms of PTSD compared to those in the
control group. As you can see from Example 1, the dependent variable is
reported levels of PTSD symptoms (measured through the pre- and post-
test) and the independent variable is visual exposure to trauma (video).
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 83
Example 2
As part of a comprehensive study on depression, participants were
randomly assigned to either an experiment group or a control group. To
assess their levels of depression, all participants underwent a pre-test. The
results of the pre-test indicated no significant differences in depression
between the experiment and control groups. Subsequently, participants in
the experiment group were instructed to read an article that highlighted
the severity and pervasiveness of prejudice against their racial group. After
the intervention, depression scores were measured during the post-test
phase. The researchers discovered that individuals who received the
experimental stimulus (the article emphasizing prejudice against their
racial group) reported greater levels of depression compared to those in
the control group .
However, one problem with coin flipping and other strict procedures for
random assignment is that they are likely to result in unequal sample sizes
in the different conditions. Unequal sample sizes are generally not a
serious problem, and you should never throw away data you have already
collected to achieve equal sample sizes. However, for a fixed number of
participants, it is statistically most efficient to divide them into equal-sized
groups. It is standard practice, therefore, to use a kind of modified
random assignment that keeps the number of participants in each group
as similar as possible. One approach is block randomization. In block
randomization, all the conditions occur once in the sequence before any
of them is repeated. Then they all occur again before any of them is
repeated again. Within each of these “blocks,” the conditions occur in a
random order. Again, the sequence of conditions is usually generated
before any participants are tested, and each new participant is assigned to
the next condition in the sequence. When the procedure is computerized,
the computer program often handles the random assignment, which is
obviously much easier. You can also find programs online to help you
randomize your random assignation. For example, the Research
Randomizer website will generate block randomization sequences for any
number of participants and conditions (Research Randomizer). Random
assignation is not guaranteed to control all extraneous variables across
conditions. It is always possible that, just by chance, the participants in
one condition might turn out to be substantially older, less tired, more
motivated, or less depressed on average than the participants in another
condition. However, there are some reasons that this may not be a major
concern. One is that random assignment works better than one might
expect, especially for large samples. Another is that the inferential
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 85
in other ways. For example, the healthy food eating group may also be
more likely to exercise and get more sleep, both of which increase memory
function. We would not know then what the effect of healthy eating is, in
isolation, upon memory ability, because there may be other variables (e.g.
exercise, sleep) that factor into memory ability.
6.8 summary
References
Adjei, J. K. (n.d.). Research methods. Retrieved from African Virtual
University website: https://oer.avu.org/handle/123456789/490
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-
experimental designs for research. Boston, USA: Houghton Mifflin.
Crump, M. J., Price, P. C., Jhangiani, R., Chiang, I-C.A., & Leighton, D.
C. (2017). Research methods for psychology (3rd ed.). Retrieved from
http://www.saylor.org/books/ Humphreys, L. (1970). Tearoom Trade:
A Study of Homosexual Encounters in Public Places. Transaction
Publishers. McCoy, S. K., & Major, B. (2003). Group identification
moderates emotional response to perceived prejudice. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(8), 1005–1017.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203253466 Palys, T., & Atchison, C.
(2014). Research decisions: Quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods
approaches (5th ed.). Toronto, Canada: Nelson Education.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 93
CHAPTER VII
SAMPLING TECHNIQUES
Learning Objectives
• Differentiate between the population and the sample.
• Describe the difference between homogenous and heterogeneous
samples.
• Differentiate between probabilistic and non-probabilistic sampling.
• Explain what is meant by representativeness and generalizability.
• Discuss sampling error, and differentiate between a random sampling
error and a system sampling error.
• Explain the importance of knowing the who, the how, and the why for
the purpose of sampling. All research projects involve gathering specific
data from specific sources in specific places at specific times (Palys &
Atchison, 2014). Also known as sampling, the necessity of sampling
occurs because we simply cannot gather all data from all sources at all
places and all times. In other words, we must make choices when we
design our research projects. This chapter focuses on sampling techniques
as another level of choice to be made by the researcher.
7.1 Sampling
Sampling is the process of selecting observations that will be analyzed for
research purposes. To put it another way, sampling has to do with
selecting some subset of one’s group of interest and drawing conclusions
from that subset. Sampling is an integral part of any research project. The
question is not if you will sample, but how you will sample. The answer
to that question usually is dependent on the methods you use and the
objectives of the study. Sampling can apply to people or objects, and is
most important when these people or objects (your units of analysis) are
heterogeneous (have different characteristics). If people (or objects) are
homogeneous, or the same in terms of a specific characteristic of study,
any sample will do, since everyone you sampled would be the same on
that characteristic. However, when there is diversity or heterogeneity,
sampling becomes highly relevant to the study, since a researcher will want
to ensure that his/her sample reflects that variability in the population.
How we sample and whom we sample shapes the sorts of conclusions we
are able to draw.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 94
If you had all the money and resources in the world, you could potentially
sample the whole population. However, money and resources usually limit
sampling, and furthermore all members of a population may not actually
be identifiable in a way that allows you to sample. As a result, researchers
take a sample, or a subgroup of people (or objects) from the population
and study that instead of the population. In social scientific research, the
population is the cluster of people, events, things, or other phenomena in
which you are most interested. It is often the “who” or “what” that you
want to be able to say something about at the end of your study.
Populations in research may be rather large, such as “the Canadian
people,” but typically they are more focused than that. For example, a
large study, for which the population of interest really is the Canadian
people, will likely specify which Canadian people, such as adults over the
age of 18 or citizens or legal residents. One of the most surprising and
often frustrating lessons students of research methods learn is that there
is a difference between one’s population of interest and one’s study
sample. While there are certainly exceptions,
more often than not, a researcher’s population and the sample are not the
same. A sample is the cluster of people or events, for example, from or
about which you will actually gather data. Some sampling strategies allow
researchers to make claims about populations that are much larger than
their actual sample with a fair amount of confidence. Other sampling
strategies are designed to allow researchers to make theoretical
contributions rather than to make sweeping claims about large
populations. We will discuss both types of strategies later in this chapter.
As mentioned previously, it is quite rare for a researcher to gather data
from their entire population of interest. This might sound surprising or
disappointing until you think about the kinds of research questions that
sociologists typically ask. For example, suppose we wish to answer the
following research question: “How do men’s and women’s college
experiences differ, and how are they similar?” Would you expect to be
able to collect data from all college students across all nations from all
historical time periods? Unless you plan to make answering this research
question your entire life’s work (and then some), the answer is probably
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 95
“no.” So then, what is a researcher to do? Does not having the time or
resources to gather data from every single person of interest mean having
to give up your research interest? Absolutely not. It just means having to
make some hard choices about sampling, and then being honest with
yourself and your readers about the limitations of your study based on the
sample from whom you were able to actually collect data. Click on this
link to help you better understand how to get from the theoretical
population (to whom you want to generalize) to your sample (who will
actually be in your study)
https://www.socialresearchmethods.net/kb/sampterm.php Now having
said this, there are certainly times when it is possible to access every
member of the population. This happens when the population is small,
accessible, and willing to participate, or the researcher has access to
relevant records. For example, suppose that a university dean wants to
analyse the final graduating scores for all students enrolled in the
university’s health sciences program, for 2015 to 2019. The dean wants to
know if there is a trend toward an average increase in final graduating
scores in health sciences, over this time period, as she suspects. Since the
dean is only interested in her particular university and only those students
who graduated from health sciences from 2015 to 2019, she can easily use
the whole population. In this case, the population is the records of final
graduating scores for all students enrolled in the university’s health
sciences program from 2015 to 2019.
Example:
Random and systemic errors Consider the study of playground conditions
for elementary school children. You would need a sampling frame (or list
from which you sample) and select from that. Random sampling error
would occur by chance and could not be controlled, but systemic error
would be possible. Let us say that the list is designed in such a way that
every 5th school is a private school. If you were to randomly sample every
5th school on the list, you would end up with a sample exclusively from
private schools! Sampling error just means that an element of the
population is more likely to be selected for the sample than another (in
this case, the private schools are more likely to be sampled than the public
schools). Why is this discussion of error important? The use of the right
techniques for sampling gives researchers the best chances at minimizing
sampling error, and thus the strongest ability to say their results are
reflective of the population. Research is done to benefit society in some
way, so it is important that research results reflect what we might expect
to see in society. Sample size also impacts sampling error. Generally, the
bigger the sample, the smaller the error. However, there is a point of
diminishing returns where only small reductions in error occur for
increases in size. Cost and resources usually also prohibit very large
samples, so ultimately the sample size is dependent upon a variety of
factors, of which sampling error is only one Probability sampling
techniques. There are a variety of probability samples that researchers may
use. For our purposes, we will focus on four: simple random samples,
systematic samples, stratified samples, and cluster samples (see Table 6.1
for a summary of these four techniques). Simple random samples are the
most basic type of probability sample, but their use is not particularly
common. Part of the reason for this may be the work involved in
generating a simple random sample. To draw a simple random sample, a
researcher starts with a list of every single member, or element, of his or
her population of interest. This list is sometimes referred to as a sampling
frame. Once that list has been created, the researcher numbers each
element sequentially and then randomly selects the elements from which
he or she will collect data. To randomly select elements, researchers use a
table of numbers that have been generated randomly. There are several
possible sources for obtaining a random number table. Some statistics and
research methods textbooks offer such tables as appendices to the text.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 99
Perhaps a more accessible source is one of the many free random number
generators available on the Internet. A good online source is the website
Stat Trek (https://stattrek.com/), which contains a random number
generator that you can use to create a random number table of whatever
size you might need. As you might have guessed, drawing a simple random
sample can be quite tedious. Systematic sampling techniques are
somewhat less tedious but offer the benefits of a random sample. As with
simple random samples, you must be able to produce a list of every one
of your population elements. Once you have done that, to draw a
systematic sample you would simply select every kth element on your list.
But what is “k”, and where on the list of population elements does one
begin the selection process? The symbol “k” is your selection interval or
the distance between the elements you select for inclusion in your study.
To begin the selection process, you would need to figure out how many
elements you wish to include in your sample. Let us say you want to
interview 25 students from the Law program at your college or university.
You do some research and discover that there are 150 students currently
registered in the program. In this case, your selection interval, or k, is 6.
To arrive at 6, simply divide the total number of population elements by
your desired sample size. To determine where on your list of population
elements to begin selecting the names of the 25 students you will
interview, select a random number between 1 and k, and begin there. If
we randomly select 3 as our starting point, we would begin by selecting
the third student on the list and then select every sixth student from there.
you randomly select 2 as your starting point and select every seventh day
after that, you will wind up with a total of 4 days on which to conduct
your observations. But what happens is that you are now observing on
the second day of the week, being Tuesdays. As you have probably figured
out, that is not such a good plan if you really wish to understand how
public spaces in your city or town are used. Weekend use probably differs
from weekday use, and that use may even vary during the week.
(presumably) willing and able human guinea pigs. But that luxury comes
at the cost of sample representativeness. One study of top academic
journals in psychology found that over two-thirds (68%) of participants
in studies published by those journals were based on samples drawn in
the United States (Arnett, 2008). Further, the study found that two-thirds
of the work that derived from US samples published in the Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology were based on samples made up
entirely of American undergraduates taking psychology courses. These
findings certainly beg the question: what do we actually learn from social
scientific studies and about whom do we learn it? That is exactly the
concern raised by Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan (2010), authors of the
article “The Weirdest People in the World?” In their article, Henrich et al.
point out that behavioural scientists very commonly make sweeping
claims about human nature based on samples drawn only from WEIRD
(Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic) societies, and
often based on even narrower samples, as is the case with many studies
relying on samples drawn from college classrooms. As it turns out, many
robust findings about the nature of human behaviour when it comes to
fairness, cooperation, visual perception, trust, and other behaviours, are
based on studies that excluded participants from outside the United
States, and sometimes excluded anyone outside the college classroom
(Begley, 2010). This raises questions about what we really know about
human behaviour as opposed to U.S. resident or U.S. undergraduate
behaviour. Of course, not all research findings are based on samples of
WEIRD folks like college students. But even then, it would behoove us
to pay attention to the population on which studies are based and the
claims that are being made about to whom those studies apply. In the
preceding discussion, the concern is with researchers making claims about
populations other than those from which their samples were drawn. A
related, but slightly different, potential concern is sampling bias. Bias in
sampling occurs when the elements selected for inclusion in a study do
not represent the larger population from which they were drawn. For
example, a poll conducted online by a newspaper asking for the public’s
opinion about some local issue will certainly not represent the public since
those without access to computers or the internet, those who do not read
that paper’s website, and those who do not have the time or interest will
not answer the question. Another thing to keep in mind is that, just
because a sample may be representative in all respects that a researcher
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 102
thinks are relevant, there may be relevant aspects that didn’t occur to the
researcher when she was drawing her sample. So how do you know when
you can count on results that are being reported? While there might not
be any magic or always-true rules you can apply, there are a couple of
things you can keep in mind as you read the claims researchers make about
their findings. First, remember that sample quality is determined only by
the sample actually obtained, not by the sampling method itself. A
researcher may set out to administer a survey to a representative sample
by correctly employing a random selection technique, but if only a handful
of the people sampled actually respond to the survey, the researcher will
have to be very careful about the claims he can make about his survey
findings. Second, researchers may be drawn to talking about implications
of their findings as though they apply to some group other than the
population actually sampled.
Though this tendency is usually quite innocent, it is all too tempting a way
to talk about findings; consumers of those findings have a responsibility
to be attentive to this sort of (likely unintentional) bait and switch. Third,
keep in mind that a sample that allows for comparisons of theoretically
important concepts or variables is certainly better than one that does not
allow for such comparisons. In a study based on a non-representative
sample, for example, we can learn about the strength of our social theories
by comparing relevant aspects of social processes.
6.8 Summary
Researchers simply do not have the resources to draw data from all
sources, at all times, and in all places. Therefore, they must make
important decisions regarding their sources. This chapter has focused on
sampling methods, including the most popular probabilistic and non-
probabilistic techniques. It concluded by discussing the importance of
thinking about who is sampled, when, how, and for what purposes, as well
as the importance of ensuring the sample actually reflects the population.
The next step in the research process is to determine which data collection
methods are best to help you answer you research questions. Data
collection is the focus of the next chapter.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 103
References
CHAPTER VIII
DATA COLLECTION METHODS: SURVEY RESEARCH
Learning Objectives
• Define survey research.
• Differentiate between a survey and a questionnaire.
• Identify the strength and weakness of survey research.
• Distinguish between the various types of surveys.
• Identify the various ways to administer a survey, and understand the
limitations of each survey method of administration.
• Describe the characteristics of an effective survey question.
• Describe the characteristics of an effective survey. In this chapter we
will cover the collection of research data through the survey methods. It
is most likely that you have participated in a survey at one time or another;
accordingly, you probably have a fairly good idea of what a survey is.
However, constructing a good survey requires more technique than meets
the eye. Survey design takes a great deal of thoughtful planning, and often
many rounds of revision, to get it just right, but it is worth the effort. As
you will learn in this chapter, there are many benefits to choosing survey
research as your method of data collection; specifically: what a survey is,
what the benefits and drawbacks of this method are, how to construct a
survey, and what to do with survey data once you have it in hand.
1. Cost-effectiveness.
2. Generalizability.
3. Reliability.
4. Versatility.
As with all methods of data collection, survey research also comes with a
few drawbacks. First, while one might argue that surveys are flexible in
the sense that they can ask any number of questions on any number of
topics, the fact that the survey researcher is generally stuck with a single
instrument for collecting data (the questionnaire) means that surveys
could also be described as inflexible. For example, suppose you mail a
survey out to 1,000 people and then discover, as responses start coming
in, that your phrasing on a particular question seems to be confusing a
number of respondents. At this stage, it is too late to change the question
for the respondents who have not yet returned their surveys (however, if
you conduct a pilot study first, you should avoid such a situation). When
conducting in-depth interviews, on the other hand, a researcher can
provide respondents further explanation if they are confused by a
question, and can tweak their questions as they learn more about how
respondents seem to understand them. Validity can also be a problem
with surveys. Survey questions are standardized; thus, it can be difficult to
ask anything other than very general questions that a broad range of
people will understand. Because of this, survey results may not be as valid
as results obtained using methods of data collection that allow a researcher
to more comprehensively examine the topic being studied. Potential
drawbacks to survey research include:
1. Inflexibility; and
2. Validity.
References
Babbie, E. (2010). The practice of social research (12th ed.). Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth. McKay, S. (2015, April 13). Are you using a questionnaire or
survey to collect data? Retrieved from
https://www.surveygizmo.com/resources/blog/taking-the-question-
out-of-questionnaires/ Palys, T., & Atchison, C. (2014). Research
decisions: Quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods approaches (5th
ed.). Toronto, Canada: Nelson Education. Saylor Academy. (2012).
Principles of sociological inquiry; Qualitative and quantitative methods.
Washington, DC: Saylor Academy. Retrieved from
https://www.saylor.org/site/textbooks/Principles%20of%20Sociologic
al%20Inquiry.pdf Saylor Academy. (2012). Principles of sociological
inquiry; Qualitative and quantitative methods. Washington, DC: Saylor
Academy. Retrieved from
https://www.saylor.org/site/textbooks/Principles%20of%20Sociologic
al%20Inquiry
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 113
CHAPTER VIII
ANALYSIS OF SURVEY DATA.
Learning Objectives
For those who will be conducting manual data entry, there probably is not
much to be said about this task that will make you want to perform it
other than pointing out the reward of having a database of your very own
analyzable data. We will not get into too many of the details of data entry,
but we will mention a few programs that survey researchers may use to
analyze data once it has been entered. The first is SPSS, or the Statistical
Package for the Social Sciences (http://www.spss.com/). SPSS is a
statistical analysis computer program designed to analyze just the sort of
data quantitative survey researchers collect. It can perform everything
from very basic descriptive statistical analysis to more complex inferential
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 115
statistical analysis. SPSS is touted by many for being highly accessible and
relatively easy to navigate (with practice). Excel, which is far less
sophisticated in its statistical capabilities, is relatively easy to use and suits
some researchers’ purposes just fine.
You will see in Table 9.4 that a couple of the financial security response
categories have been collapsed from five to three Researchers sometimes
collapse response categories on items such as this in order to make it easier
to read results in a table. You will also see that the variable “gender” was
placed in columns and “financial security” is displayed in rows. Typically,
values that are contingent on other values are placed in rows (a.k.a.
dependent variables), while independent variables are placed in columns.
This makes it pretty simple to compare independent variable across
categories. Reading across the top row of our table, we can see that around
44% of men in the sample reported that they are not financially secure
while almost 52% of women reported the same. In other words, more
women than men reported that they are not financially secure. You will
also see in the table that the total number of respondents for each category
of the independent variable is in the table’s bottom row. This is also
standard practice in a bivariate table, as is including a table heading
describing what is presented in the table. Researchers interested in
simultaneously analyzing relationships among more than two variables
conduct multivariate analysis. If we hypothesized that financial security
declines for women as they age but increases for men as they age, we
might consider adding age to the preceding analysis. To do so would
require multivariate, rather than bivariate, analysis. We will not go into
detail here about how to conduct multivariate analysis of quantitative
survey items, but we will return to multivariate analysis in Chapter 16
“Reading and Understanding Social Research”.
Summary
Chapter 9 has focused on the analysis of quantitative data associated with survey data.
It is not the intention of this introductory chapter to delve too deeply into quantitative
analysis. As such, this chapter has focused briefly on univariate data analysis. If you are
interested in learning more about the analysis of quantitative survey data, we encourage
you to take some courses in statistics. The quantitative data analysis skills you will gain
in a statistics class could serve you quite well, should you find yourself seeking
employment one day.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 119
References
Babbie, E. (2010). The practice of social research (12th ed.). Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth. McKay, S. (2015, April 13). Are you using a questionnaire or survey
to collect data? Retrieved from
https://www.surveygizmo.com/resources/blog/taking-the-question-
out-of-questionnaires/ Palys, T., & Atchison, C. (2014). Research decisions:
Quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods approaches (5th ed.). Toronto,
Canada: Nelson Education. Saylor Academy. (2012). Principles of sociological
inquiry: Qualitative and quantitative methods. Washington, DC: Saylor
Academy. Retrieved from
https://www.saylor.org/site/textbooks/Principles%20of%20Sociologic
al%20Inquiry.pdf
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 120
CHAPTER X
QUALITATIVE DATA COLLECTION & ANALYSIS
METHODS
Learning Objectives
• Describe the circumstances under which it is suitable to use the interview
technique for data collection. • Explain semi-structured interview
• Identify the characteristics of an open-ended question.
• Describe an interview guide.
• Identify the challenges associated with interviewing.
• Explain what a focus group is and identify the situations where
conducting a focus group is valuable.
• Describe when it is appropriate to utilize videography as a data collection
method.
• Identify the pros and cons of videography as a data collection method.
• Explain what a code is and describe the coding process.
• Describe the differences between inductive and deductive coding.
• Describe the two types of inductive coding (descriptive and interpretive)
and compare those to the two deductive coding (open and focused/axial
coding) techniques.
List the various steps involved in analyzing qualitative data.
• Describe an oral history.
• Identify the strengths and weaknesses of qualitative
10.1 Interview Research Interviewing is a qualitative research technique
and a valuable skill. Interviews are used by market researchers to learn
how to sell their products; journalists use interviews to get information
from a whole host of people, from VIPs to random people on the street.
From the social scientific perspective, interviews are a method of data
collection that involves two or more people exchanging information
through a series of questions and answers. The questions are designed by
a researcher to elicit information from interview participant(s) on a
specific topic or set of topics. Typically interviews involve an in-person
meeting between two people, an interviewer and an interviewee. But as
you will discover in this chapter, interviews need not be limited to two
people, nor must they occur in person.
10.2 When should qualitative data collection be used?
Interviews are an excellent way to gather detailed information. They also
have an advantage over surveys. For example, with a survey, if a
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 121
Videography
Like an interview, videography can be an effective means for collecting
data, both during researcher/ participant interviews and during focus
groups. However, videography can also be employed to collect data in
more natural settings and, therefore, is a popular tool for those
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 126
1. Ensure that the study meets with ethical guidelines for human
participant’s research.
2. Describe all details of the procedure of the study.
3. Comply with all legal requirements for recording in real environments.
4. Obtain legal consent for video recording.
5. Ensure all privacy and confidentiality issues related to the preservation
of participants’ identification, and identifiable video data storage are
addressed. 6. Complete and comply with all local regulations regarding
eligibility for human subject research.
7. Submit IRB application and gain final approval in order to start the
project. Participants and Sampling
1. Determine the number of participants you need.
2. Determine the unit of analysis and sampling frame that will most
effectively help answer your research question (e.g.: Do you need a certain
number of participants? How will you recruit your participants? Will you
randomly recruit the participants or will they have certain eligibility
requirements, such as people within a certain age range? Will participants
be paid?).
3. Inform all participants about the benefits and risks of your study.
4. Conduct the recruitment as planned in the IRB.
5. Get informed consent from all people who agree to participate in the
study.
6. Decide on all technical specifications of the equipment you need.
7. Choose an appropriate high-quality camera or cameras.
8. Choose the best audio recording style (built into camera or separate).
9. Determine the camera layout of the room; get the best angle to ensure
a clear view of the participants.
10. Establish a protocol for recording the interactions.
11. Maximize the captured area by adjusting the camera angle.
12. Create protocols to link the data.
13. Sync the audio and video data for the analysis.
14. Determine protocols for storing video recordings.
15. Secure the hard drives for privacy protection.
16. Back up the data.
17. Train all researchers, camera persons, interviewers, and other
members of the research team. Data analysis Review the quality of all data.
Identify the software you will be using to analyze the data. Clearly
distinguish the research questions and analyze accordingly. Create coding
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 128
Summary
This chapter has focused on collecting and analyzing qualitative data. We
explored some of the more traditional methods, such as interviews and
focus groups, for collecting qualitative data. We also explored less popular
methods such as oral histories and videography. Analyzing qualitative data
requires time and commitment. If possible, the researcher who undertakes
the analysis and write-up of the data should complete the transcription, in
order to be totally immersed in the data. Time spent in these processes
should result in a study that produces valuable, in-depth data that numbers
alone (i.e., quantitative methods) cannot explain.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 134
References
Assan, O. & Montague, E. (2015). Using video-based observation
research methods in care health encounters to evaluate complex
interactions. Inform Primary Care, 21(4), 161-170.
doi:10.14236/jhi.v21i4.72. Babbie, E. (2010). The practice of social
research (12th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Fontana, A., & Frey, J. H.
(2003). The interview: From structured questions to negotiated text. In
N. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.). Collecting and interpreting qualitative
materials (2nd ed.), (pp. 61- 106). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. Glaser,
B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory:
Strategies for qualitative research. London, England: Aldine Transaction.
Jewitt, C. (2012). An introduction to using video for research. NCRM
Working paper 03/12. Retrieved from
http://eprints.ncrm.ac.uk/2259/4/NCRM_workingpaper_0312.pdf.
Kumarapeli, P., & de Lusignan, S. (2013). Using the computer in the
clinical consultation: Setting the stage reviewing, recording, and taking
actions: Multi-channel video study. Journal of the American Medical
Informatics Association, 20(e1), e67-e75. doi: 10.1136/amiajnl-2012-
001081. Leong, A., Koczan, P., de Lusignan, & Sheeler, I. (2006). A
framework for comparing video methods used to assess the clinical
consultation: A qualitative study. Medical Informatics and the Internet in
Medicine, 31(4), 255-265. doi:10.1080/14639230600991668. Lofland, J.,
& Lofland, L. H. (1995). Analyzing social settings: A guide to qualitative
observation and analysis (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Palys, T.,
& Atchison, C. (2014). Research decisions: Quantitative, qualitative, and
mixed methods approaches (5th ed.). Toronto, Canada: Nelson
Education. Patton, M. Q. (2001). Qualitative research and evaluation
methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Payne, G., & Payne, J. (2004). Hawthorne effect. Key concepts in social
research. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781849209397.n22. Reinharz,
S. (1992). Feminist methods in social research. London, England: Sage
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 135
CHAPTER XI
QUANTITATIVE INTERVIEW TECHNIQUES &
CONSIDERATIONS
Learning Objectives
• Describe a standardized interview.
• Explain how quantitative interviews differ from qualitative interviews.
• Explain how to analyze quantitative interview data.
• Identify the main issues that qualitative and quantitative interviewers
should consider.
• Describe the options that interviewers have for balancing power
between themselves and interview participants.
• Describe and define rapport.
• Define the term, “probe”, and describe how probing differs in
qualitative and quantitative interviewing.
Quantitative interviews are similar to qualitative interviews in that they
involve some researcher/respondent interaction; however, the process
of conducting and analyzing findings from quantitative interviews differs
in several ways from that of qualitative interviews. Each approach comes
with its own unique set of strengths and weaknesses. We will explore
these differences in the following sections.
Summary
Many of the considerations related to quantitative interviews are similar
to those of qualitative interviews. While both types of interviews involve
some researcher/ respondent interaction, the process of conducting the
interview, and collecting and analyzing the findings, differ in a few key
ways.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 143
References
Babbie, E. (2010). The practice of social research (12th ed.). Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth. Busse, B., & Fuchs, M. (2012). The components of landline
telephone survey coverage bias: The relative importance of no-phone and
mobile-only populations. Quality & Quantity, 46(4), 1209-1225.
doi:10.1007/ s11135-011-9431-3. Holbrook, A. L., Green, M. C., &
Krosnick, J. A. (2003). Telephone versus face-to-face interviewing of
national probability samples with long questionnaires. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 67, 79-125. Retrieved from
http://www.business.illinois.edu/shavitt/BA_531/
Holbrook%20Green%20and%20Krosnick%202003.pdf Karnieli-Miller,
O., & Strier, R., & Pessach, L. (2009). Power relations in qualitative
research. Qualitative Health Research, 19(2), 279-289.
doi:10.1177/1049732308329306 Ryan, P., & Dundon, T. (2008). Case
research interviews: Eliciting superior quality data. International Journal
of Case Method Research & Application, xx(4), 443-450. Retrieved from
https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/7bb1/f3e62f354c06d917ccbe79f0906
1ed7cbb27.pdf? Saylor Academy. (2012). Principles of sociological
inquiry; Qualitative and quantitative methods. Washington, DC: Saylor
Academy. Retrieved from
https://www.saylor.org/site/textbooks/Principles%20of%20Sociologic
al%20Inquiry.pdf
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 144
CHAPTER XII
FIELD RESEARCH A QUA LITATIVE RESEARCH TECHNIQUE
Learning Objectives
• Define field research.
• Define ethnography.
• Explain the conditions under which it is appropriate to undertake field
research.
• Identify the pros and cons of field research.
• Explain what is meant by “getting in” in the context of field research If
we wanted to know who conducts more of the housework in households,
how could we find the answer? One way might be to interview people and
simply ask them. That is exactly what Arlie Hochschild did in her study
of “the second shift”, her term for the work that goes on in the home
after the day’s work for pay is completed. Hochschild (1989) interviewed
50 heterosexual, married couples with children to learn about how they
did, or did not, share the work of the second shift. Many of these couples
reported to her that they shared the load of the second shift equally,
sometimes dividing the house into areas that were “her” responsibility and
those that were “his.” Hochschild was not satisfied with just people’s
personal accounts of second-shift work. She chose to observe 12 of these
couples in their homes as well, to see for herself just how the second shift
was shared. What Hochschild discovered was that even those couples
who claimed to share the second shift did not have as equitable a division
of duties as they had professed. For example, one couple who told
Hochschild during their interview that they shared the household work
equally had explained that the wife was responsible for the upstairs
portion of the house and the husband took responsibility for the
downstairs portion. Upon conducting observations in this couple’s home,
however, Hochschild discovered that the upstairs portion of the house
contained all the bedrooms and bathrooms, the kitchen, the dining room,
and the living room, while the downstairs included a storage space and
the garage. This division of labour meant that the woman actually carried
the weight of responsibility for the second shift. Without a field research
component to her study, Hochschild might never have uncovered these
and other truths about couples’ behaviours and sharing (or not sharing)
of household duties. Overall, there are two reasons for doing research in
the field. The first is that from a qualitative perspective, behaviour only
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 145
have asked, or of the range of field research projects that have been
conducted over the years, but they do provide a snapshot of the kinds of
work win which sociological field researchers engage.
The major benefits of field research are: 1. It yields very detailed data. 2.
It emphasizes the role and relevance of social context. 3. It can uncover
social facts that may not be immediately obvious, or of which research
participants may be unaware. On the other hand, the fact that field
researchers collect very detailed data does come at a cost. Because a field
researcher’s focus is so detailed, it is, by necessity, also somewhat narrow.
Field researchers simply are not able to gather data from as many
individuals as, say, a survey researcher can reach. Indeed, field researchers
generally sacrifice breadth in exchange for depth. Related to this point is
the fact that field research is extremely time intensive. Field research can
also be emotionally taxing. It requires, to a certain extent, the development
of a relationship between a researcher and her participants. However, if
interviews and field research both require relationship development, you
might say that interviews are more like casual dating while field research
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 148
more ground, and therefore collect more data, than you can on your own.
Having collaborators in any research project, but especially field research,
means having others with whom to share your trials and tribulations in
the field. However,
120
collaborative research comes with its own set of challenges, such as
possible personality conflicts among researchers, competing
commitments in terms of time and contributions to the project, and
differences in methodological or theoretical perspectives (Shaffir,
Marshall, & Haas, 1979). When considering something that is of interest
to you, consider also whether you have possible collaborators. How might
having collaborators shape the decisions you make about where to
conduct participant observation? This section began by asking you to
think about limitations that might shape your field site decisions. But it
makes sense to also think about the opportunities—social, geographic,
and otherwise—that your location affords. Perhaps you are already a
member of an organization where you would like to conduct research.
Maybe you know someone who knows someone else who might be able
to help you access a site. Perhaps you have a friend you could stay with,
enabling you to conduct participant observations away from home.
Choosing a site for participation is shaped by all these factors—your
research question and area of interest, a few limitations, some
opportunities, and sometimes a bit of being in the right place at the right
time. Choosing a role As with choosing a research site, some limitations
and opportunities beyond your control might shape the role you take once
you begin your participant observation. You will also need to make some
deliberate decisions about how you enter the field and who you will be
once you are in. In terms of entering the field, one of the earliest decisions
you will need to make is whether to be overt or covert. As an overt
researcher, you enter the field with your research participants having some
awareness about the fact that they are the subjects of social scientific
research. Covert researchers, on the other hand, enter the field as though
they are full participants, opting not to reveal that they are also researchers
or that the group they’ve joined is being studied. As you might imagine,
there are pros and cons to both approaches. A critical point to keep in
mind is that whatever decision you make about how you enter the field
will affect many of your subsequent experiences in the field. As an overt
researcher, you may experience some trouble establishing rapport at first.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 151
Having an insider at the site who can vouch for you will certainly help,
but the knowledge that subjects are being watched will inevitably (and
understandably) make some people uncomfortable and possibly cause
them to behave differently than they would, were they not aware of being
research subjects. Because field research is typically a sustained activity
that occurs over several months or years, it is likely that participants will
become more comfortable with your presence over time. Overt
researchers also avoid a variety of moral and ethical dilemmas that they
might otherwise face. As a covert researcher, “getting in” your site might
be quite easy; however, once you are in, you may face other issues. Some
questions to consider are: 1. How long would you plan to conceal your
identity? 2. How might participants respond once they discover you’ve
been studying them? 3. How will you respond if asked to engage in
activities you find unsettling or unsafe? Researcher, Jun Li (2008)
struggled with the ethical challenges of “getting in” to interview female
gamblers as a covert researcher. Her research was part of a post-doctoral
fellowship from the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre to study
female gambling culture. In response to these ethical aspects, she changed
her research role to overt; however, in her overt role female gamblers were
reluctant to “speak their minds” to her (p. 100). As such, she once again
adjusted her level of involvement in the study to one who participated in
female gambling culture as an insider and observed as an outsider. You
can read her interesting story at the following link:
https://nsuworks.nova.edu/tqr/vol13/iss1/8. Beyond your own
personal level of comfort with deceiving participants and willingness to
take risks, it is possible that the decision about whether or not to enter the
field covertly will be made for you. If you are conducting research while
associated with any federally funded agency (and even many private
entities), your institutional review board (IRB) probably will have
something to say about any planned deception of research subjects. Some
IRBs approve deception, but others look warily upon a field researcher
engaging in covert participation. The extent to which your research site is
a public location, where people may not have an expectation of privacy,
might also play a role in helping you decide whether covert research is a
reasonable approach. Having an insider at your site who can vouch for
you is helpful. Such insiders, with whom a researcher may have some prior
connection or a closer relationship than with other site participants, are
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 152
called key informants. A key informant can provide a framework for your
observations, help translate what you observe, and give you
121
important insight into a group’s culture. If possible, having more than one
key informant at a site is ideal, as one informant’s perspective may vary
from another’s. Once you have made a decision about how to enter your
field site, you will need to think about the role you will adopt while there.
Aside from being overt or covert, how close will you be to participants?
In the words of Fred Davis (1973), [12] who coined these terms in
reference to researchers’ roles, “will you be a Martian, a Convert, or a bit
of both”? Davis describes the Martian role as one in which a field
researcher stands back a bit, not fully immersed in the lives of his subjects,
in order to better problematize, categorize, and see with the eyes of a
newcomer what’s being observed. From the Martian perspective, a
researcher should remain disentangled from too much engagement with
participants. The Convert, on the other hand, intentionally dives right into
life as a participant. From this perspective, it is through total immersion
that understanding is gained. Which approach do you feel best suits you?
In the preceding section we examined how ascribed and achieved statuses
might shape how or which sites are chosen for field research. They also
shape the role the researcher adopts in the field site. The fact that the
authors of this textbook are professors, for example, is an achieved status.
We can choose the extent to which we share this aspect of our identities
with field study participants. In some situations, sharing that we are
professors may enhance our ability to establish rapport; in other field sites
it might stifle conversation and rapport-building. As you have seen from
the examples provided throughout this chapter, different field researchers
have taken different approaches when it comes to using their social
locations to help establish rapport and dealing with ascribed statuses that
differ from those of their “subjects Whatever role a researcher chooses,
many of the points made in Chapter 11 “Quantitative Interview
Techniques” regarding power and relationships with participants apply to
field research as well. In fact, the researcher/researched relationship is
even more complex in field studies, where interactions with participants
last far longer than the hour or two it might take to interview someone.
Moreover, the potential for exploitation on the part of the researcher is
even greater in field studies, since relationships are usually closer and lines
between research and personal or off-the-record interaction may be
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 153
format. We have already noted that carefully paying attention while in the
field is important; so is what goes on immediately upon exiting the field.
Field researchers typically spend several hours typing up field notes after
each observation has occurred. This is often where the analysis of field
research data begins. Having time outside of the field to reflect upon your
thoughts about what you have seen and the meaning of those
observations is crucial to developing analysis in field research studies.
Once the analytic field notes have been written or typed up, the field
researcher can begin to look for patterns across the notes by coding the
data. This will involve the iterative process of open and focused coding
that is outlined in Chapter 10, “Qualitative Data Collection & Analysis
Methods.” As mentioned in Section 12.4 “Field Notes”, it is important to
note as much as you possibly can while in the field and as much as you
can recall after leaving the field because you never know what might
become important. Things that seem decidedly unimportant at the time
may later reveal themselves to have some relevance. As mentioned in
Chapter 10, analysis of qualitative data often works inductively. The
analytic process of field researchers and others who conduct inductive
analysis is referred to as grounded theory (Glaser & Strauss, 1967;
Charmaz, 2006).
The goal when employing a grounded theory approach is to generate
theory. Its name not only implies that discoveries are made from the
ground up but also that theoretical developments are grounded in a
researcher’s empirical observations and a group’s tangible experiences.
Grounded theory requires that one begin with an open-ended and open-
minded desire to understand a social situation or setting and involves a
systematic process whereby the researcher lets the data guide her rather
than guiding the data by preset hypotheses. As exciting as it might sound
to generate theory from the ground up, the experience can also be quite
intimidating and anxiety-producing, since the open nature of the process
can sometimes feel a little out of control. Without hypotheses to guide
their analysis, researchers engaged in grounded theory work may
experience some feelings of frustration or angst. The good news is that
the process of developing a coherent theory that is grounded in empirical
observations can be quite rewarding, not only to researchers, but also to
their peers, who can contribute to the further development of new
theories through additional research, and to research participants who
may appreciate getting a bird’s-eye view of their every day.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 155
Summary
This chapter focused on a qualitative research method known as field
research. It involves participant observation, interviewing, and document
or artifact analysis. Field research can gather very detailed data; however,
as such, field researchers often sacrifice breadth for depth as it relates to
their findings.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 156
References
Birch, P., Vickers, M. H., Kennedy, M., & Galovic, S. (2017). Wellbeing,
occupational justice and police practice: An ´affirming environment´?
Police Practice & Research, 18(1), 26-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
15614263.2016.1205985 Harrison, T. R., Yang, F., Anderson, D., Morgan,
S. E., Muhamad, J. W., Solle, N.S., …Kobetz, E. (20017). Resilience,
culture change, and cancer risk reduction in a fire rescue organization:
Clean gear as the new badge of honor. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis
Management, 25, 171-181. doi:10.1111/1468-5973.12182 Hochschild, A.
(1989). The second shift: Working parents and the revolution at home (1st
ed.). New York, NY: Viking. Humphreys, L. (1970). Tearoom trade:
Impersonal sex in public places. London, UK: Duckworth. Li, J. (2008).
Ethical challenges in participant observation: A reflection on
ethnographic fieldwork. The Qualitative Report, 13(1), 100-115.
Retrieved from https://nsuworks.nova.edu/tqr/vol13/iss1/8 Lorenzo,
R. A., & Lins, B. A. (2018). The act of listening to “battered” women: An
ethnographic comparison of police and emergency responses in Sao
Paulo, Brazil. Latin American Research Review, 53(1), 96-110. doi:
https://doi.org/10.25222/larr.348 Palys, T., & Atchison, C. (2014).
Research decisions: Quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods
approaches (5th ed.). Toronto, Canada: Nelson Education. Paretta, L. T.,
Catalano, A. (2013). What students really do in the library: An
observational study. The Reference Library, 54(2), 157-167.
doi:10.1080/02763877.2013.755033 Saylor Academy. (2012). Principles
of sociological inquiry; Qualitative and quantitative methods. Washington,
DC: Saylor Academy. Retrieved from
https://www.saylor.org/site/textbooks/Principles%20of%20Sociologic
al%20Inquiry.pdf Shaffir, W., Marshall, V., & Haas, J. (1980). Competing
commitments: Unanticipated problems of field research. Qualitative
Sociology, 2(3), 56-71. White, C. & Jha, S. (2018). Towards an
interdisciplinary approach to wellbeing: Life histories and self-
determination theory in rural Zambia. Social Science & Medicine, 212,
153-160. doi:10.1016/ j.socscimed.2018.07.026 Wood, J. D., Watson, A.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 157
C., & Fulambarker, A. J. (2017). The “gray zone” of police work during
mental health encounters: Findings from an observational study in
Chicago. Police Quarterly, 20(1), 81-105. doi:10.1177/1098611116658875
CHAPTER XIII
UNOBTRUSIVE RESEARCH: QUALITATIV
AND QUANTITATIVE APPROACHES
Learning Objectives
• Define unobtrusive research methods and explain when it is suitable to
employ this type of research method.
• Outline the benefits and the drawbacks of using unobtrusive research
methods.
• Define the Hawthorne effect.
• Explain the difference between primary and secondary data sources.
• Explain the various methods for conducting unobtrusive research.
• Describe some of the advantages and disadvantages of analyzing other
people’s data.
• Describe three measures of reliability in unobtrusive research.
• Define ethnomethodology and conversation analysis. Unobtrusive
research refers to methods of collecting data that do not interfere with the
subjects under study (because these methods are not obtrusive). Both
qualitative and quantitative researchers use unobtrusive research methods.
Unobtrusive methods share the unique quality that they do not require
the researcher to interact with the people he or she is studying. It may
seem strange that sociology, a discipline dedicated to understanding
human social behaviour, would employ a methodology that requires no
interaction with human beings. However, humans create plenty of
evidence of their behaviours: they write letters to the editor of their local
paper; they create various sources of entertainment for themselves, such
as movies and televisions shows; they consume goods; they walk on
sidewalks; and they lie on the grass in public parks. All these activities
leave something behind: printed papers, recorded shows, trash, and worn
paths. These are all potential sources of data for the unobtrusive
researcher. Sociologists interested in history are likely to use unobtrusive
methods, which are also well suited to comparative research. Historical
comparative research is “research that focuses either on one or more cases
over time (the historical part) or on more than one nation or society at
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 158
one point in time (the comparative part)” (Esterberg, 2002, p. 129). While
not all unobtrusive researchers necessarily conduct historical,
comparative, or even some combination of historical and comparative
work, unobtrusive methods are well suited to such work. In this chapter,
we will examine content analysis as well as analysis of data collected by
others. Both types of analysis use data that do not require direct
interaction with human subjects, but the particular type and source of data
for each type of analysis differs. We will explore these similarities and
differences in the following sections, after we look at some of the pros
and cons of unobtrusive research methods. As is true of the other research
types we have examined thus far, unobtrusive research has both strengths
and weaknesses.
Physical trace
Content is not the only sort of data that researchers can collect
unobtrusively. Unobtrusive researchers might also be interested in
analyzing the evidence that humans leave behind that tells us something
about who they are or what they do. This kind evidence includes the
physical traces left by humans and the material artifacts that tell us
something about their beliefs, values, or norms. Fire and police will
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 163
Summary
Chapter 13 focused on unobtrusive research, which enables researchers
to gather data without interfering or interacting with the research subjects.
Unobtrusive methods can be utilized in both qualitative and quantitative
research methodologies. Overall, it is a cost-effective manner of
undertaking research, however, it can suffer from validity issues, data
availability, and the challenge of accounting for the social context in which
the data was produced.
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 168
References
Babbie, E. (2010). The practice of social research (12th ed.). Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth. Esterberg, K. G. (2002). Qualitative methods in social
research. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill. Garfinkle, H. (1967). Studies in
ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Gubrium, J. F.,
& Holstein, J. A. (2000). Analyzing interpretative practice. In N. Denzin
& Y.S. Lincoln (Eds.), The handbook of qualitative research (2nd ed.),
(pp. 487-508). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. Heritage, J. C.
(1984). Garfinkel & ethnomethodology. Cambridge: UK. Polity Press.
Krippendorff, K. (2004a). Content analysis: An introduction to its
methodology (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Krippendorf, K.
(2004b). Reliability in content analysis: Some common misconceptions
and recommendations. Human Communication Research, 30(3), 411-433.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2004.tb00738.x Lombard, M.,
Snyder-Duch, J., & Campenella Bracken, C. (2010). Practical resources for
assessing and reporting intercoder reliability in content analysis research
projects. Retrieved from
http://matthewlombard.com/reliability/#How%20should%20research
ers%20calculate%20intercoder%20reliability%20What%20software%20i
s%20available Palys, T., & Atchison, C. (2014). Research decisions:
Quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods approaches (5th ed.).
Toronto, Canada: Nelson Education. Patton, M. Q. (2015). Qualitative
research & evaluation methods: Integrating theory and practice (4th ed.).
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. Saylor Academy. (2012).
Principles of sociological inquiry: Qualitative and quantitative methods.
Washington, DC: Saylor Academy. Retrieved from
https://www.saylor.org/site/textbooks/Principles%20of%20Sociologic
al%20Inquiry.pdf Schutt, R. K. (2012). Investigating the social world: The
process and practice of research. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE
Publications. Sheppard, V. A., & Fennell, D. A. (2019, August). Progress
in public sector tourism policy: Toward an ethic for non-human animals.
Tourism Management, 73, 134-142. doi:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2018.11.017
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 169
CHAPTER XIV
THE RESEARCH PROPOSAL
Learning Objectives
• Describe what a research proposal is.
• Discuss the goals of a research proposal.
• List the various components of a research proposal.
• Identify the 15 steps of writing a research proposal.
In this chapter, we will focus on the components of writing an effective
research proposal. We will begin by discussing what a research proposal
is, what its goals are, and the various components of a research proposal.
We will also examine a 15-step approach to writing a research proposal.
14.1 What are the Goals of a Research Proposal?
The research proposal has a set of specific goals: 1. To present and
justify the need to study a research problem. 2. To present a practical
way in which the proposed research study should be undertaken. 3. To
demonstrate that the design elements and procedures being set forth to
study the research problem meet with the governed standards within the
predominant discipline in which the problem resides. Regardless of the
research problem being investigated and the methods chosen to study
that problem, all research proposals must address the following
questions: 1. What do you plan to accomplish? Be clear and succinct in
defining the research problem and what it is you are proposing to
research. 2. Why do you want to do it? In addition to detailing your
research design, you must also conduct a thorough review of the
literature and provide convincing evidence that the topic is worthy of
study. Be sure you answer the “so what?” question. 3. How are you
going to do it? Make sure that what you propose to do is doable. In
other words, make sure you have the time, the resources and, most
importantly, the stamina to undertake what you are proposing to do.
14.2 Writing the Research Proposal
As with writing any academic paper, research proposals are generally
organized in the same manner across most social science disciplines. The
length of a research proposal depends upon the audience for whom the
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 170
Citations and references As with any scholarly research paper, you must
cite the sources you used in composing your research proposal. In a
research proposal, this can take two forms: a reference list or a
bibliography. A reference list lists the literature you referenced in the body
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 171
of your research proposal. All references in the reference list must appear
in the body of the research proposal. Remember, it is not acceptable to
say “as cited in …” As a researcher you must always go to the original
source and check it for yourself. Many errors are made in referencing,
even by top researchers, and so it is important not to perpetuate an error
made by someone else. While this can be time consuming, it is the proper
way to undertake a literature review. In contrast, a bibliography, is a list
of everything you used or cited in your research proposal, with additional
citations to any key sources relevant to understanding the research
problem. In other words, sources cited in your bibliography may not
necessarily appear in the body of your research proposal. Make sure you
check with your instructor to see which of the two you are expected to
produce. Overall, your list of citations should be a testament to the fact
that you have done a sufficient level of preliminary research to ensure that
your project will complement, but not duplicate, previous research efforts.
For social sciences, the reference list or bibliography should be prepared
in American Psychological Association (APA) referencing format.
Usually, the reference list (or bibliography) is not included in the word
count of the research proposal. Again, make sure you check with your
instructor to confirm.
Summary
Research proposals take a lot of time to prepare, even after one has
undertaken the literature review. As the research proposal serves as the
map for your research study, it is critical to take your time in researching,
thinking, and writing your research proposal. At the end of the day, you
want to leave the readers of your research proposal feeling, “Wow, this is
an exciting idea and I cannot wait to see how it turns out!” To help you
make sure your research proposal is clearly and logically written, here are
some common mistakes to avoid: Failure to develop a coherent and
persuasive argument for undertaking the proposed research. Failure to be
concise; not making the purpose clear and being “all over the map.”
Failure to cite landmark (significant) pieces of work in your literature
review. Failure to set forth the contextual boundaries of your research
(i.e., time, place, people, etc.). Failure to stay focused on the research
problem (i.e., going off on unrelated tangents). Sloppy or imprecise
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 172
References
Martin, C. H., & Fleming, V. A. (2010). 15-step model for writing a
research proposal. British Journal of Midwifery, 18(2), 791-798.
doi:10.12968/bjom.2010.18.12.791 Krathwohl, D. R. (2005). How to
prepare a dissertation proposal: Suggestions for students in education and
the social and behavioral sciences. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University
Press. Retrieved from
http://libguides.usc.edu/writingguide/researchproposal Sheppard, V. A.,
& Williams, P. W. (2016, September). Factors strengthening tourism
resort resilience. Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Management, 28, 20-
30. doi:1 0.1016/j.jhtm.2016.04.006 Whitmore, R., & Knafl, K. (2005).
The integrative review: Updated methodology. Journal of Advanced
Nursing, 52(5), 546-553. Retrieved from
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.465.9393&r
ep=rep1&type=pdf
Introduction to Research Methods P a g e | 173