EBSCO-FullText-02_12_2024
EBSCO-FullText-02_12_2024
EBSCO-FullText-02_12_2024
The purpose of this article is to discuss issues of language, specifically African American Vernacular
English (AAVE), as it relates to the reading performance of African American children. Previous research
on the science of reading provides a research-based framework that is a starting point for evidence-based
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
research that can be used to improve the reading outcomes of African American children. School
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
psychology literature is limited in its inclusion of issues posed by deficit perspectives of AAVE with
Black children and reading achievement. Given that practicing school psychologists spend significant
portions of their time conducting assessments related to identifying and remediating reading problems, an
increased awareness on how AAVE can impact African American children’s reading performance is
necessary. Implications for research and practice will be discussed, such as inclusion of instruments to
differentiate between reading difficulties and dialect differences (i.e., Diagnostic Evaluation of Language
Variation).
The quest for literacy has been an ongoing struggle for African technical skills (Aaronson & Mazumder, 2011; Albritton, 2012).
Americans (Span, 2005). For instance, antiliteracy laws in several While the value that African American families place on education
states (e.g., Alabama, Georgia, Missouri, Virginia) prohibited both is not in question, opportunity gaps have resulted from the structural
enslaved and emancipated African Americans from learning how to barriers (e.g., Jim Crow laws) that have consistently been placed in
read. Teaching African Americans how to read was met with harsh front of African American populations. Within the U.S. educational
consequences such as whippings, fines, and jail time (Cornelius, system, opportunity gaps have manifested themselves as the lack of
1991). In spite of these barriers, African Americans continued to progress on measures of academic performance on measures of
deliberately seek out educational opportunities to develop schools national reading assessments (i.e., National Assessment of Educa-
and universities. In the face of segregated schools, African tional Progress).
Americans helped to establish Rosenwald schools and Historically To improve these outcomes, policymakers have supported
Black Colleges and Universities in order to develop literacy and research that indicates there are five essential components of
This article was published Online First November 10, 2022. teacher and educator perceptions of Black children relate to their emotional
Kyanna P. Johnson https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9378-1237 and academic well-being.
Scott L. Graves Jr. https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7680-422X SHANYE PHILLIPS is a doctoral student in the school psychology program at
Marcel Jacobs https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5491-1246 The Ohio State University. Her current interests include understanding how
KYANNA P. JOHNSON is a doctoral student studying school psychology at The parent involvement and gendered racial socialization influences behavioral
Ohio State University. Her interests center understanding cultural relativism in outcomes in schools.
regard to African American children and their behaviors in the classroom. MARCEL JACOBS is a second-year doctoral student in the school psychology
SCOTT L. GRAVES Jr. is an associate professor in the school psychology program at The Ohio State University. His research interests include
program at The Ohio State University and the director of the Positive Youth understanding how constructs such as motivation and academic self-efficacy
Development Lab. His research agenda is focused on identifying strengths in improve outcomes for Black youth.
African American children that lead to positive social–emotional and The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.
academic outcomes. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Scott L. Graves
MARK A. JONES Jr. is a doctoral student in the school psychology program Jr., School Psychology, Ohio State University, 305 Annie and John Glenn
at The Ohio State University. He is currently interested in researching how Avenue, Columbus, OH 43210, United States. Email: graves.388@osu.edu
7
8 JOHNSON, GRAVES, JONES, PHILLIPS, AND JACOBS
issues that has been neglected in the study of the SoR is the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
demonstrate inadequate language and literacy skills, essentially literature base on this topic among linguistic researchers (Charity
viewing AAVE users as intellectually inferior. In comparison, et al., 2004). Previous studies suggest an increased likelihood to
the difference perspective legitimizes the use of this form of English, struggle with reading skills for students who do not decrease their
as academics point out that students have different speech patterns use of AAVE after first grade; however, this is not the case for
but still follow rules of language (e.g., semantics, syntax). The children who frequently code-switch (Craig & Washington, 2004,
differences that exist in nonmainstream American English (NMAE) 2006). This code-switching is frequently referred to as the dialect
are due to its users’ social contexts. Harris and Schroeder (2013) shifting–reading achievement hypothesis, which states that African
state that African American children who communicate with AAVE American children who use AAVE but shift to use ME in literacy
follow similar developmental trends in language learning as White tasks will perform better than students who do not shift their dialect
children that speak ME. Although the working memory structure of (Craig et al., 2009).
African American children that use AAVE is similar to that of ME Research consistently supports the dialect shifting–reading
users (Harris & Schroeder, 2013), challenges may still exist with achievement hypothesis. For instance, Terry et al. (2016) examined
information processing during code-switching in environments that how the continual use of AAVE in second grade relates to achieve-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
consider AAVE to be inappropriate. ment. Their results showed that the more students used AAVE, the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Given the normalized expectation of speaking ME, typical school lower their overall reading performance (e.g., comprehension,
professionals tend to look at AAVE speakers from a deficit per- vocabulary, language, and nonrepetition skills). Additionally, stu-
spective (Harris & Schroeder, 2013). However, the use of AAVE dents who frequently used AAVE but increased their dialect-shift
should be regarded as a strength. In fact, individuals who frequently over the course of the year saw significant improvements in their
use AAVE are described in the speech pathology literature as reading comprehension. A more recent study conducted by Puranik
bidialectal, which indicates that these individuals can code-switch et al. (2020) found that the high dialect density scores of AAVE
and have the ability to use ME dialect or AAVE depending on the resulted in negative impacts in reading and writing estimates for
context. This type of dialectical variation was seen in Terry et al.’s elementary and middle school students, including slowed literacy
(2010) examination of dialect variation and literacy skills by school growth. In their examination of dialect density on the growth of
context in 1st grade. Their results indicated that students’ measured reading in African American children, Washington et al. (2018)
vocabulary and NMAE were shown to have a negative and nonlin- found that dialect density decreases about 5% each year of school
ear relationship. Explicit teaching of dialect shifting is beneficial for between kindergarten and second grade, which is related to an
children who speak dialects other than ME such as AAVE. Research increase in reading comprehension. Similar associations between
by Johnson et al. (2017) indicated that Dialect Awareness, a dialectical variation and reading were found in Terry et al.’s (2012)
program that teaches dialect shifting, is effective in promoting examination of strong-to-moderate users of AAVE. Their findings
dialect shifting in students who speak NMAE. Students who indicated that children decreased their use of AAVE over the course
received lessons through this program were more likely to use of first and second grade, and this was related to increased growth in
“school English” when expected and had benefits in proximal and reading comprehension.
distal measures of literacy including narrative writing, morphosyn- While the findings of AAVE and the negative correlation with
tactic awareness, and reading comprehension. reading achievement are substantial, methodological limitations
Researchers in speech-language pathology and other related fields may limit some of the findings. For instance, while Dexter et al.
have taken strides in acknowledging differences in vernacular (2018) found a similar relationship between AAVE use in their
dialects. A technical report provided by the American Speech– kindergarten and second grade longitudinal examination of reading
Language–Hearing Association (ASHA) states that “no dialectical outcomes, the best predictor of reading outcomes was disordered
variety of American English is a disorder or a pathological form of language development. AAVE and disordered language develop-
speech or language,” and that practitioners should demonstrate ment share common features according to these authors, and this fact
competencies that prevent discriminatory practices against those may be one reason why African American children are overrepre-
who exhibit linguistic variation (American Speech-Language- sented in the language disorder population. Although it would be
Hearing Association, 2003). ASHA has since provided scopes of beneficial for school professionals to routinely include measures of
practice for audiologists and speech–language pathologists (SLPs) disordered language development and AAVE in their assessment
that seek to improve services that influence communication in ways practices for reading growth, this is not the case.
that respect cultural differences. Resources are also available from
the association that provides information on phonemic systems that
Assessing for AAVE
follow various languages as well as information on ethical con-
siderations of serving diverse clients. These specific advances, In their analysis of school psychologists’ perceptions of identify-
however, have not taken hold in the field of school psychology ing reading disabilities, Machek and Nelson (2010) found that
in the same manner. More research should be conducted to under- psychologists supported the use of individualized IQ tests in addi-
stand the impact of linguistic variation in reading performance and tion to response to intervention; however, there was no mention of
the educational practices of school psychologists, especially as it any measure related to AAVE assessment or cultural considerations.
relates to African American children. In fact, very little of the currently published literature that examines
how reading disabilities should be conceptualized or actual practices
by school psychologists have any specific discussion or concern for
AAVE and Reading Performance
AAVE issues (see Fish & Margolis, 1988; Machek & Nelson, 2007;
While research on AAVE and its relation to academic achieve- Maki & Adams, 2019; Nelson & Machek, 2007; Sotelo-Dynega &
ment in the field of school psychology is limited, there exists a great Dixon, 2014). Wheeler and colleagues are some of the few
10 JOHNSON, GRAVES, JONES, PHILLIPS, AND JACOBS
researchers to have looked into these issues (Wheeler, 2016; assessments, considering African American children have consis-
Wheeler et al., 2012). In their analysis of AAVE in reading tently received below-average and low scores due to language
assessment and instruction, Wheeler et al. (2012) discusses the differences. Additionally, language-focused interventions are
implications of not factoring dialect into assessment, which may impacted by test bias as African American children in this review
lead to inappropriate intervention plans and consequently slowed were less likely to produce higher posttest scores.
academic growth in students. Furthermore, most norm-referenced
standardized tests have not been designed to differentiate social
School Psychologists Assessment and Language
dialects from language impairments. School psychologists should
look to the literature in the speech pathology research base to help in Research has indicated that language can have a significant
their understanding of AAVE assessment issues. impact on children’s performance on measures of reading
In Hendricks and Diehm’s (2020) survey of assessment and (Washington et al., 2018). Once thought to be exclusive to the
intervention practices for students who speak AAVE, they explored domain of speech and language pathology, this issue also affects the
the type and frequency of assessment and intervention practices field of school psychology. Recent research in the field of school
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
used by SLPs in two U.S. states that work with students who speak psychology indicates that assessments commonly used to assess
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
AAVE. Results indicated that modified scoring of standardized students’ cognitive and academic abilities have varying degrees of
assessments and selecting different intervention strategies were linguistic demand. Linguistic demand is composed of two main
the most commonly reported clinical practices used when working dimensions: the complexity and verbosity of assessment instructions
with students who speak AAVE. However, these practices are not (Cormier et al., 2014). For instance, Cormier et al. (2016) examined
universally used by all SLPs, as half of SLPs reported using modified the linguistic demands of the oral subtests found in the Wechsler
or alternative assessment practices the majority or some of the time Intelligence Scales for Children, Fourth and Fifth Editions (WISC-
for students who speak AAVE. SLPs that use modified or alternative IV and WISC-V). Cormier et al. (2018) relatedly conducted a
assessments tend to do so less often with students who speak AAVE systematic examination of linguistic demand across multiple cogni-
(Hendricks & Diehm, 2020). Knowledge of linguistic features of tive assessment instruments (i.e., Kaufman Assessment Battery for
AAVE was a significant predictor of the frequency with which SLPs Children, 2nd ed.; WISC-V; Woodcock–Johnson IV Test of Cog-
reported implementing modified or alternative assessment and nitive Abilities). The results from these studies have indicated that
intervention practices. SLPs with the highest levels of knowledge cognitive assessment directions are not uniform across cognitive
of AAVE utilize different clinical practices than those with lower assessment instruments. As such, the language of the examinee
levels of knowledge of AAVE. As such, school psychologists should could cause their results to be inadvertently deflated. This poses an
work to understand the potential barriers to implementing modified issue as school psychologists could unintentionally use cognitive
assessment practices with students who speak AAVE. assessment instruments that possess higher linguistic demands for
The use of modified assessment practices has been shown to be African American students who utilize AAVE more consistently
beneficial when used with students who speak AAVE. Craig and than MAE. This might make it more difficult for students to
Washington (2000) investigated 48 language-impaired and typically understand tasks that might be presented to them on various
developing African American students’ performance on five infor- measures of intelligence, as these standardized assessments utilize
mal language assessment measures. Three of these measures were MAE. Thus, Black students in this instance may struggle to perform
derived from child-centered free play language samples, whereas at an age- or grade-appropriate level on these measures if they are
two of the measures examined language comprehension and re- unable to comprehend the tasks presented to them. The lack of
sponses to requests for information using “wh-” questions (i.e., assessment practices that account for differences related to the use of
“who,” “what,” “when,” “where”), as well as responses to probes of AAVE could increase the chances that African American students
passive and active sentences. Results indicated that children with experience a mismatch of support and services, which could lead to
language impairments scored significantly lower on each measure troublesome outcomes. For instance, African American students
than typically developing peers. The modified measures had excel- may be identified as special education students needing intensive
lent sensitivity and specificity. As such, these modified assessment support, although this level of support is not necessary for their
measures show promise in being culturally fair. Given that school academic success. Previous research has identified that African
psychologists are ethically responsible for using culturally fair American students are disproportionately represented in special
assessment practices, they should become familiar with the use education and often lag behind peers in these programs (Graves
of informal language assessments to provide fairness in assessment & Ye, 2017). Additionally, these practices could directly impact the
when working with students who speak AAVE. nature of expectations teachers have on African American students,
As indicated by the American Psychological Association (2022) thus contributing to the academic opportunity gap and subjecting
as “the tendency of scores to systematically over- or under-estimate African American students to adverse educational outcomes (e.g.,
the true performance of individuals to whom a test was adminis- school dropout, school failure). Considering school psychologists
tered, particularly because they are members of specific groups,” test are ethically obligated to engage in culturally fair assessment
bias is a posed threat among Black individuals due to their minority practices with all students, these potential results are problematic.
status and perceived intellectual inferiority. Standardized testing has A recent examination by Graves et al. (2021) found that the factor
historically produced unfair results for Black individuals, resulting structure of the WISC-V in African American children was not
in ostracism in integral components of success, such as rejection in invariant by race. The subtests that were not invariant (i.e., Picture
the workforce (Brunn-Bevel & Byrd, 2015). A review of dialect Span and Figure Weights) had the highest linguistic loading (i.e., the
differences in African American children conducted by Stockman highest loading factors from subtests in the visual spatial reasoning
(2010) discusses the need to reduce negative bias in these and fluid reasoning domains were analyzed and were found to load
AFRICAN AMERICAN VERNACULAR 11
higher on a four-factor solution for African American children, Language Screening Test (Fluharty 2); and the Washington–Craig
compared to the five-factor model of fit proposed by test publishers), Language Screener. Each of the three screeners includes African
meaning these subtests measured domains differently for Black American children in their standardization samples and involve
children. The linguistic characteristics of assessment instruments grammar, phonology, and vocabulary comprehension. An important
used in several studies have been based on the Culture–Language finding in this study was that screeners were inconsistent in their
Interpretive Matrix (C-LIM; Flanagan et al., 2013). Ortiz (2019) identification of students with learning disorders, with 46% of the
introduced the C-LIM as a tool that psychologists can use to evaluate participants either failed one or two of the three screening tools.
the validity of test scores for English learners. More specifically, the Also, students shown to have high rates of AAVE were not at greater
C-LIM was designed for students with English as their second risk for failing the screenings than those with lower rates. This
language, yet this does not apply to the majority of African Amer- discussion of the DELV and the DELV screening provides an
icans in the United States. The C-LIM has been demonstrated to example of a tool that can be used by school professionals to
have limited diagnosed utility based on independent evaluations understand language variation in Black children. It should be
(Styck & Watkins, 2013). There is a significant gap in the school acknowledged that additional research is necessary given the lack
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
psychology research base as it relates to how language can influence of full measurement invariance in Terry et al. (2017) because one
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
outcomes such as reading for African American youth. specific tool is recommended as best practices.
speech or language impairment educational classification each year Cormier, D. C., McGrew, K. S., & Ysseldyke, J. E. (2014). The influences of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
for at least a decade (2004–2014). As schools in the United States linguistic demand and cultural loading on cognitive test scores. Journal of
began to integrate, teachers who were unfamiliar with African Psychoeducational Assessment, 32(7), 610–623. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0734282914536012
American children’s communication patterns viewed their language
Cormier, D. C., Wang, K., & Kennedy, K. E. (2016). Linguistic demands of
abilities as inferior to those of White children. This perspective of
the oral directions for administering the WISC-IV and WISC-V. Canadian
inferiority has made it difficult for some to differentiate between Journal of School Psychology, 31(4), 290–304. https://doi.org/10.1177/
appropriate differences in language and language disorders, which 0829573516643314
greatly impacts assessment results. These deficit views of AAVE Cornelius, J. (1991). When I can read my title clear: Literacy, slavery, and
persist today; however, research has suggested a shift to view AAVE religion in the antebellum south. University of South Carolina Press.
as difference rather than a deficit (Harris & Schroeder, 2013). Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2000). An assessment battery for
However, additional research is needed in the field of school identifying language impairments in African American children. Journal
psychology on methods such as curriculum-based measurement of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2), 366–379. https://
to account for dialect differences in children to ensure they are doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4302.366
not penalized for their language variation. Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2004). Grade-related changes in the
production of African American English. Journal of Speech, Language,
and Hearing Research, 47(2), 450–463. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-
References 4388(2004/036)
Aaronson, D., & Mazumder, B. (2011). The impact of Rosenwald schools on Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2006). Malik goes to school: Examining
Black achievement. Journal of Political Economy, 119(5), 821–888. the language skills of African American students from preschool—5th
https://doi.org/10.1086/662962 grade. Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410615602
Albritton, T. J. (2012). Educating our own: The historical legacy of HBCUs Craig, H. K., Zhang, L., Hensel, S. L., & Quinn, E. J. (2009). African
and their relevance for educating a new generation of leaders. The Urban American English-speaking students: An examination of the relationship
Review, 44(3), 311–331. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11256-012-0202-9 between dialect shifting and reading outcomes. Journal of Speech, Lan-
American Psychological Association. (2022). Test bias. In APA dictionary of guage, and Hearing Research, 52(4), 839–855. https://doi.org/10.1044/
psychology. Retrieved May 16, 2022, from https://dictionary.apa.org/test- 1092-4388(2009/08-0056)
bias Dexter, C. A., Johnson, A., Bowman, M., & Barnett, D. (2018). Using
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2003). American kindergarten language, dialect variation, and child behavior to predict second
English dialects [Technical report]. https://www.asha.org/policy grade reading ability in African American Children. Reading Psychology,
Aston, C., & Brown, D. L. (2021). Progress or setback: Revisiting the current 39(8), 763–786. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711.2018.1538650
state of assessment practices of Black children. Contemporary School Fish, M. C., & Margolis, H. (1988). Training and practice of school psychol-
Psychology, 25(2), 140–148. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40688-020-00308-7 ogists in reading assessment and intervention. Journal of School Psychol-
Aukerman, M., & Chambers Schuldt, L. (2021). What matters most? Toward ogy, 26(4), 399–404. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-4405(88)90048-9
a robust and socially just science of reading. Reading Research Quarterly, Flanagan, D. P., Ortiz, S. O., & Alfonso, V. C. (2013). Essentials of cross-
56(S1), S85–S103. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.406 battery assessment. Wiley.
Ballis, B., & Heath, K. (2019). The long-run impacts of special education Grant, S. D., Oka, E. R., & Baker, J. A. (2009). The culturally relevant
(EdWorkingPaper: 19-151). https://doi.org/10.26300/a6df-4266 assessment of Ebonics-speaking children. Journal of Applied School Psy-
Ballis, B., & Heath, K. (2021). Direct and spillover effects of limiting chology, 25(2), 113–127. https://doi.org/10.1080/15377900802484588
minority student access to Special Education (EdWorkingPaper: 21-364). Graves, S., Jr., Smith, L. V., & Nichols, K. (2021). Is the WISC-V a fair test
https://doi.org/10.26300/m2m5-0t83 for Black children: Factor structure in an urban public school sample.
Benson, N. F., Floyd, R. G., Kranzler, J. H., Eckert, T. L., Fefer, S. A., & Contemporary School Psychology, 25(2), 157–169. https://doi.org/10
Morgan, G. B. (2019). Test use and assessment practices of school psychol- .1007/s40688-020-00306-9
ogists in the United States: Findings from the 2017 national survey. Journal Graves, S. L., Jr., & Ye, F. F. (2017). Are special education labels accurate
of School Psychology, 72, 29–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2018.12.004 for Black children? Racial differences in academic trajectories of youth
Bland-Stewart, L., Elie, M., & Townsend, A. (2013). Using the DELV to diagnosed with specific learning and intellectual disabilities. The Jour-
determine language difference vs. a language disorder in dialectal speak- nal of Black Psychology, 43(2), 192–213. https://doi.org/10.1177/
ers. Online Journal of Education Research, 2(1), 1–10. https://www.resea 0095798416636280
rchgate.net/profile/Martine-Elie-2/publication/338254038_Using_the_ Harris, T., & Graves, S. (2010). The influence of cultural capital transmission
DELV_to_Determine_Language_Difference_vs_a_Language_Disorder_ on reading achievement in African American fifth grade boys. The Journal of
in_Dialectal_Speakers/links/5e0b0625a6fdcc28374aef26/Using-the-DE Negro Education, 79(4), 447–457. https://www.jstor.org/stable/41341088
AFRICAN AMERICAN VERNACULAR 13
Harris, Y. R., & Schroeder, V. M. (2013). Language deficits or differences: for Effective Intervention, 37(4), 243–250. https://doi.org/10.1177/15345
What we know about African American vernacular English in the 21st 08411413760
century. International Education Studies, 6(4), 194–204. https://doi.org/ Puranik, C., Branum-Martin, L., & Washington, J. A. (2020). The relation
10.5539/ies.v6n4p194 between dialect density and the codevelopment of writing and reading in
Hendricks, A. E., & Diehm, E. A. (2020). Survey of assessment and African American children. Child Development, 91(4), e866–e882. https://
intervention practices for students who speak African American English. doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13318
Journal of Communication Disorders, 83, Article 105967. https://doi.org/ Rickford, J. R. (2016). Labov’s contributions to the study of African American
10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.105967 Vernacular English: Pursuing linguistic and social equity. Journal of
Horton-Ikard, R., & Miller, J. F. (2004). It is not just the poor kids: The use of Sociolinguistics, 20(4), 561–580. https://doi.org/10.1111/josl.12198
AAE forms by African-American school-aged children from middle SES Robinson, G. C., & Norton, P. C. (2019). A decade of disproportionality: A
communities. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37(6), 467–487. state-level analysis of African American students enrolled in the primary
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.02.001 disability category of speech or language impairment. Language, Speech,
Johnson, L., Terry, N. P., Connor, C. M., & Thomas-Tate, S. (2017). The and Hearing Services in Schools, 50(2), 267–282. https://doi.org/10.1044/
effects of dialect awareness instruction on nonmainstream American 2018_LSHSS-17-0149
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
English speakers. Reading and Writing, 30(9), 2009–2038. https:// Seymour, H. N., Bland-Stewart, L., & Green, L. J. (1998). Difference versus
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9764-y deficit in child African American English. Language, Speech, and Hear-
Labov, W. (1968). A study of the non-standard English of Negro and Puerto- ing Services in Schools, 29(2), 96–108. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461
Rican speakers in New York City (Vol. I). Columbia University. https:// .2902.96
eric.ed.gov/?id=ED028423 Sotelo-Dynega, M., & Dixon, S. G. (2014). Cognitive assessment practices:
Louden, M. L. (2000). African-Americans and minority language mainte- A survey of school psychologists. Psychology in the Schools, 51(10),
nance in the United States. The Journal of Negro History, 85(4), 223–240. 1031–1045. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.21802
https://doi.org/10.2307/2668543 Span, C. M. (2005). Learning in spite of opposition: African Americans and
Machek, G. R., & Nelson, J. M. (2007). How should reading disabilities be their history of educational exclusion in antebellum America. Counter-
operationalized? A survey of practicing school psychologists. Learning points, 131, 26–53. https://www.jstor.org/stable/42977282
Disabilities Research and Practice, 22(2), 147–157. https://doi.org/10 Stockman, I. J. (2010). A review of developmental and applied language
.1111/j.1540-5826.2007.00239.x research on African American children: From a deficit to difference
Machek, G. R., & Nelson, J. M. (2010). School psychologists’ perceptions perspective on dialect differences. Language, Speech, and Hearing Ser-
regarding the practice of identifying reading disabilities: Cognitive assess- vices in Schools, 41(1), 23–38. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2009/
ment and response to intervention considerations. Psychology in the 08-0086)
Schools, 47(3), 230–245. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20467 Styck, K. M., & Watkins, M. W. (2013). Diagnostic utility of the culture-
Maki, K. E., & Adams, S. R. (2019). A current landscape of specific learning language interpretive matrix for the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for
disability identification: Training, practices, and implications. Psychology Children—Fourth edition among referred students. School Psychology
in the Schools, 56(1), 18–31. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.22179 Review, 42(4), 367–382. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2013
McNamara, K. M., Walcott, C. M., & Hyson, D. (2019). Results from .12087460
the NASP 2015 membership survey, part two: Professional practices in Sullivan, A. L., & Field, S. (2013). Do preschool special education services
school psychology [Research report]. National Association of School make a difference in kindergarten reading and mathematics skills?: A
Psychologists. https://www.nasponline.org/research-and-policy/nasp-resea propensity score weighting analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 51(2),
rch-center/member-surveys 243–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2012.12.004
Milner, H. R., IV. (2020). Disrupting racism and whiteness in researching a Tatum, A. W., Johnson, A., & McMillon, D. (2021). The state of Black male
science of reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 55(S1), S249–S253. literacy research, 1999–2020. Literacy Research: Theory, Method, and
https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.347 Practice, 70(1), 129–151. https://doi.org/10.1177/23813377211038368
Moland, C. W., & Oetting, J. B. (2021). Comparison of the diagnostic Terry, N. P., Connor, C. M., Johnson, L., Stuckey, A., & Tani, N. (2016).
evaluation of language variation–screening test risk subtest to two other Dialect variation, dialect-shifting, and reading comprehension in second
screeners for low-income prekindergartners who speak African American grade. Reading and Writing, 29(2), 267–295. https://doi.org/10.1007/
English and live in the urban south. American Journal of Speech- s11145-015-9593-9
Language Pathology, 30(6), 2528–2541. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_A Terry, N. P., Connor, C. M., Petscher, Y., & Conlin, C. R. (2012). Dialect
JSLP-20-00270 variation and reading: Is change in nonmainstream American English use
National Reading Panel (U.S.), National Institute of Child Health and Human related to reading achievement in first and second grades? Journal of
Development (U.S.). (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(1), 55–69. https://doi.org/
assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implica- 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/09-0257)
tions for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups. National Institute of Terry, N. P., Connor, C. M., Thomas-Tate, S., & Love, M. (2010).
Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health. Examining relationships among dialect variation, literacy skills, and
Nelson, J. M., & Machek, G. R. (2007). A survey of training, practice, and school context in first grade. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
competence in reading assessment and intervention. School Psychology Research, 53(1), 126–145. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-
Review, 36(2), 311–327. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2007.12087948 0058)
Ortiz, S. O. (2019). On the measurement of cognitive abilities in English Terry, N. P., Petscher, Y., & Rhodes, K. T. (2017). Psychometric analysis of
learners. Contemporary School Psychology, 23(1), 68–86. https://doi.org/ the diagnostic evaluation of language variation–screening test: Extension
10.1007/s40688-018-0208-8 to low-Income African American pre-kindergarteners. Assessment for
Pearson, B. Z., Conner, T., & Jackson, J. E. (2013). Removing obstacles for Effective Intervention, 42(3), 176–185. https://doi.org/10.1177/15345084
African American English-speaking children through greater understand- 16679402
ing of language difference. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 31–44. Washington, J. A., Branum-Martin, L., Sun, C., & Lee-James, R. (2018). The
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028248 impact of dialect density on the growth of language and reading in African
Petscher, Y., Connor, C. M., & Al’Otaiba, S. (2012). Psychometric analysis American children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
of the diagnostic evaluation of language variation assessment. Assessment 49(2), 232–247. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0063
14 JOHNSON, GRAVES, JONES, PHILLIPS, AND JACOBS
Wheeler, R. (2016). “So much research, so little change”: Teaching Wolfram, W. (2004). The grammar of urban African American vernacular
standard English in African American classrooms. Annual Review of English. In B. Kormann, C. Upton, K. Burridge, E. W. Schneider, & R.
Linguistics, 2(1), 367–390. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics- Mesthrie (Eds.), A handbook of varieties of English: Phonology, mor-
011415-040434 phology and syntax (pp. 111–132). Mouton de Gruyter.
Wheeler, R., Cartwright, K., & Swords, R. (2012). Factoring AAVE into
reading assessment and instruction. The Reading Teacher, 65(6), 416–
425. https://doi.org/10.1002/TRTR.01063
Winford, D. (2015). The origins of African American Vernacular English: Received October 1, 2021
Beginnings. In S. Lanehart (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of African Revision received June 28, 2022
American language (pp. 85–104). Oxford University Press. Accepted July 21, 2022 ▪
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.