The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
of Northern Colorado
Volume 1 | Number 2 Article 2
January 2012
Recommended Citation
Lowe, Elizabeth Deborah (2012) "The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System – Tower Test Resilience to Response Bias," Ursidae:
The Undergraduate Research Journal at the University of Northern Colorado: Vol. 1 : No. 2 , Article 2.
Available at: http://digscholarship.unco.edu/urj/vol1/iss2/2
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Scholarship & Creative Works @ Digital UNC. It has been accepted for inclusion in Ursidae:
The Undergraduate Research Journal at the University of Northern Colorado by an authorized editor of Scholarship & Creative Works @ Digital UNC.
For more information, please contact Jane.Monson@unco.edu.
Lowe: The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
1
Abstract
Neuropsychological tools are used to gain more accurate insight about an individual’s level of
functioning (cognitive, behavioral, executive, etc.), and to make more exact diagnoses; therefore,
valid neuropsychological tools are necessary for precise evaluation. Valid neuropsychological
assessment relies upon the individual putting forth maximum effort during testing. While the
literature is rich when describing methods of detecting incomplete effort, it is sparse when
identifying instruments resistant to such response bias. The goal of this study was to determine
whether or not effort affects performance on the D-KEFS Tower Test by comparing the results
with the Test of Memory Malingering (a neuropsychological assessment designed to measure
effort). Thirty-nine neurologically intact college students from a medium sized Rocky Mountain
university introductory subject pool were asked to participate. The participants in the
experimental group were given a vignette explaining that they had been in a car accident. The
participants were then asked to pretend that they had suffered a brain injury and were having
memory problems. The participants in the control group were asked to do their best. A blind
examiner administered the D-KEFS Tower Test and The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM)
to both groups. Data analysis shows that there was a significant difference between the two
groups’ performance on the TOMM, but no significant difference between the scores on the
Tower Test. These results suggest that the D-KEFS Tower Test is relatively resilient to
incomplete effort.
Key Terms
behavioral, language, and executive dysfunction of an individual (Malik, Turner, & Sadler,
2007). The data obtained from neuropsychological evaluation provide insight that may lead to
important factor to keep in mind when interpreting the results is that these tools are sensitive to
influences such as age, education, sex, and cultural background. All of these elements challenge
the validity of the neuropsychological evaluation. Thus, they should be addressed before any
neuropsychological evaluation – and, therefore, the validity of the results – is response bias.
Response bias occurs when individuals do not put forth maximum effort during
neuropsychological evaluation. Response bias skews the results so that they no longer accurately
reflect the abilities of the individual. According to Dunn (2006), manifestations of response bias
can be arranged on a spectrum. On one end, incidental response bias includes behaviors such as
fatigue. These behaviors may occur because the test is tiring, or because individuals are
distracted by environmental factors, such as noise or the temperature of the room. These
behaviors are categorized by unintentional factors. The next integer on the continuum of
response bias is the exaggeration of existing symptoms, or the reporting of symptoms that were
once present but no longer exist. These behaviors are more intentional than the first cluster.
Even more severe and intentional than the exaggeration of present or previous symptoms –
residing on the extreme end of the response bias spectrum – is the deliberate fabrication of
symptoms for personal gain, known as malingering. An example of this deliberate malingering
would be an individual who was in an accident and feigns dysfunction in order to receive a
significant insurance pay out. Malingering results in purposeful lack of effort; therefore,
malingering occurs in 5-30% of cases, and additional cases of response bias occur equally as
often. Their research suggests that some form of response bias (like fatigue or distraction) can
occur in up to 60% of cases in neuropsychological testing. With such high rates, measures of
detecting response bias are vital for accurate assessment of an individual’s functioning.
http://digscholarship.unco.edu/urj/vol1/iss2/2 2
Lowe: The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
3
There are two ways to detect response bias (Dunn, 2006). The first method is to use an
assessment that is so simple and that most individuals, including those who are actually
impaired, will easily perform well. Since the task is so simple, poor performance would be a
reflection of response bias. The second strategy employed to assess response bias is to use an
assessment known as a Symptom Validity Test (Dunn, 2006). These tests appear to measure
memory while actually assessing effort. Individuals who put forth maximum effort on these
tests, even those with cognitive deficits, will get all or almost all of the items correct; low scores
(below 50%) reflect poor performance, which suggests that response bias has occurred. These
The major neuropsychological measures designed to assess effort include the Rey 15-
Item Memory Test (Dunn, 2006), one of the oldest measures of response bias, and the Test of
considered to be “one of the most frequently used” and most accurate test used for detecting
minimal effort (Sharland & Gfeller, 2007). The TOMM is disguised as a memory test, and
malingering individuals tend to score very lowly. As the TOMM is simple enough that
individuals can score 50% by chance, abnormally low scores are a reflection of deliberate
Much research has been conducted to determine what kind of tools can detect
malingering, as well as other forms of response bias. However, the literature is sparse in regards
to research that investigates whether there are instruments that are resistant to response bias in
any form. If such instruments do exist, an individual may malinger or become fatigued, but his
or her performance on the test would be no different than if maximum effort was put forth, under
ideal testing conditions. Though the response bias is existent, the conclusions drawn from an
One area to investigate is whether tasks that rely upon implicit memory are resilient to
response bias. Research suggests that when a task involves implicit memory, an individual
improves by simply doing it. If this is the case, people may not be able to fake poor performance
Executive Function Systems Tower Test (TT), which is designed to assess several different
executive functions, “including spatial planning, rule learning, inhibition of impulsive and
perseverative responding, and the ability to establish and maintain the instructional set” (Delis,
Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001), as well as to measure an individual’s ability to problem solve (Chan,
Chen, Cheung, Chen, & Cheung, 2004). The TT is composed of a series of nine items, each one
progressively more difficult than the last. While completing each item, participants inadvertently
Research indicates that traumatic brain injury (TBI) impairs abilities including problem
solving, cognitive flexibility, and executive functioning (Leon-Carrion et al., 1998)—the very
abilities that the TT assesses. Further research has been conducted to investigate how patients
suffering from TBI perform on the TT. According to Chan et al. (2004), patients with TBI took
longer to initiate the first move, and they also took longer to complete each problem than
individuals without TBI. While research has been conducted to see how individuals with actual
brain injuries perform on the TT, little research has explored whether performance on the TT is
http://digscholarship.unco.edu/urj/vol1/iss2/2 4
Lowe: The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
5
Recently, Doyle and Dunn (2010) attempted to investigate this issue; they sought to
determine if the D-KEFS TT was resilient to effort. In their study, Doyle and Dunn administered
all nine items of the TT, then had each participant repeat two of the moderately difficult items.
The expectation was that the performance of the control group (the participants who put forth
maximum effort) would be consistent with the norms, and that their performance on the repeated
items would improve. The expectation of the experimental group (the participants who were
malingering) was that they would perform inconsistently with the norms, and that their
performance would remain stagnant on the repeated items. The results were inconsistent with
the hypotheses. Therefore, the results indicated that the TT was resilient to effort. Resilience to
effort or malingering–a type of response bias–may imply that the measure is also resistant to
other forms of response bias. However, the study lacked an objective assessment verifying the
validity of the participant’s performance. Doyle and Dunn (2010) only used a follow-up
questionnaire asking the participants to rate themselves: how well did they think they followed
the directions they had been given. Perception is subjective. Participants could think that they
had effectively malingered when in reality they had not. This current study is designed to
address the confound in Doyle and Dunn’s (2010) research, using the TOMM to objectively
We hypothesized that the performance of the control group on the TT and TOMM would
be consistent with the norms outlined in the Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System and Test
indicative that participants were putting forth maximum effort. We expected that the
performance of the experimental group on the TT would be atypically poor, and that malingering
would be represented by abnormally low scores on the total achievement score. We also
expected that the experimental group would perform poorly on the TOMM, showing low scores
on each of the trials, indicating that participants were not putting forth maximum effort,
malingering instead. If the control group’s performance is consistent with norms, and the
experimental group’s performance is abnormally low and significantly different than the control,
Method
Participants
In order to determine how well the Tower Test detects malingering in comparison to the
were asked to complete both the D-KEFS Tower Test as well as the TOMM. (Refer to Table 1
for demographic information.) Both males and females were randomly assigned into the control
or experimental groups. The participating students received credit in their psychology class as
compensation. However, participating in this study was only one way to receive that credit;
Table 1
Demographics
n
Gender
Female 30
Male 18
Total 48
Age
17 2
18 26
19 13
20 5
21 2
http://digscholarship.unco.edu/urj/vol1/iss2/2 6
Lowe: The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
7
Total 48
Measures
The D-KEFS Tower Test was given to both a control and an experimental group. The TT
consists of a flat wooden board that holds three equidistant pegs, as well as eight wooden disks
of increasing size. In each of the nine items, up to eight disks were arranged on the board in a
predetermined configuration. The participants transferred the disks one at a time, placing only
smaller disks on top of larger disks, to build a tower that replicated the tower in a picture. Each
item has a set arrangement to begin with and a counterpart that the final tower must emulate.
Performance on the Tower Test was determined by a single score which was calculated
using the scoring system provided by the D-KEFS manual. Each individual item received a
score based on whether or not the tower was correct, how many moves the participant made, and
how long the participant took to complete the item. Once the score for each item was
determined, all nine numbers were added together. The sum was the overall achievement score
To verify that the participants were following instructions, it was necessary to use an
objective measure which would detect effort. In this study, we employed the TOMM. The
TOMM is a pictorial memory test that consists of three trials: Trial 1, Trial 2, and Retention.
Each trial is made up of 50 items. In the first trial, the participant was shown each of the 50
pictures for three seconds each. Once all the pictures were shown, the participants were shown a
series of 50 recognition panels. Each panel contained one of the original pictures and one novel
image. The participants identified which image they had seen before. The second trial was
administered immediately after the first. The participants were shown the original 50 pictures a
second time, in a different order. Again, the first series of panels was followed by a series of
recognition panels, this time with different novel images. The participants identified which
image they recognized. The second trial was followed by a 15-minute delay when the examiner
administered the Tower Test to the participant. At the end of the delay, the retention trial was
administered. This time, the original pictures were not presented. Instead, the retention trial was
composed of only 50 recognition panels. Each panel held one of the original images as well as a
new novel image, and participants identified which image they recognized as having seen before.
Performance on the TOMM is represented by three scores: each score is the sum of correct
The materials used for this study were not limited to the tests themselves. The
directions for the control group, the experimental vignette, as well as a follow-up questionnaire,
and a debriefing form. Additionally, a quarter was used to randomly assign individuals to the
control or experimental conditions. The protocol employed during this study consisted of forms
for the TOMM (for the examiner to record which responses were correct and incorrect on each
trial) as well as a form for the TT (for the examiner to record for each item, the length of time,
number of moves, and whether the final product was correct or incorrect).
Procedure
In order to conduct this study, there were two different types of people helping to perform
the study. The first was an experimenter who greeted the participants and briefed them. The
second was an examiner who was blind to the true nature of the study and to which group each
participant was assigned. The second examiner administered the two tests.
The experimenter asked the participants to read and sign the informed consent, then to
complete a short questionnaire asking their age, year in school, major, and whether they had ever
http://digscholarship.unco.edu/urj/vol1/iss2/2 8
Lowe: The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
9
suffered a brain injury. While the participants completed the questionnaire, the experimenter
flipped a coin to determine whether the participant would be in the control or experimental
group. The experimenter told those who were randomly assigned to the control group that they
would be taking two neuropsychological tests, then directed them to do their best on both. The
experimenter read a vignette to those assigned to the experimental group asking participants to
pretend that they had been in a car accident; if they could prove they had suffered a brain injury,
the insurance company would pay a settlement of $50,000. The experimental participants were
asked to feign symptoms of a brain injury, including memory problems and poor concentration
while taking two neuropsychological tests. They were also asked not to discuss these
instructions with the experimenter conducting the two tests so that the examiner collecting data
could remain blind. All participants were given the opportunity to ask questions, and then taken
The lead experimenter left the participant with the blind examiner who began by
conducting the first and second trials of the TOMM, followed by all items of the TT, and then
the retention trial of the TOMM. Upon completion of the tests, the participant returned to the
first experimenter who provided a follow-up questionnaire consisting of three questions: how
well did you remember the directions, how motivated were you to follow the directions, and how
well did you do as a participant? The participants were debriefed and were provided with
Once the data were collected, the tests were scored so that the results could be analyzed
and interpreted. An alpha level of .05 was set, and an independent samples t-test was computed
Results
Once all of the data was collected and scored for each of the 48 participants, it was
analyzed. Of the 48 participants, the data for two participants were not used because they
admitted to having suffered a head injury, leaving 46 data points to analyze. In the analysis, an
independent samples t-test was run to see if there was a difference between the control and
experimental groups’ performance on the TT and on the TOMM. The hypothesis was that there
would be a difference between the performances of the two groups on the TT, as well as between
The t-test showed several different things. First, on the TT, there was no statistically
significant difference (t(44)=-0.48, p>.05) between the experimental group (M=16.86, SD=2.35)
and the control group (M=16.48, SD=2.93). Second, the two groups’ performances on the first
trials of the TOMM (control: M=48.48, SD=3.43; experimental: M=32.76, SD=10.72) were
significantly different: t(44)=6.95, p<0.00. Third, when the performance of the control
(M=49.20, SD=2.77) and experimental groups (M=35.24, SD=13.26) were compared on the
second trial of the TOMM, the t-test showed that there was a statistically significant difference:
t(44)=5.144, p<0.00. Finally, when the means of the groups’ performances on the TOMM
SD=14.14), a statistically significant difference was found between groups: t(43)=5.94, p<0.00.
The results of the independent samples t-test are inconsistent with the hypothesis: the
control and experimental groups performed differently on the TT, and there were marked
differences between the groups’ performances on each trial of the TOMM. The statistically
significant differences between the groups on each trial of the TOMM suggest that the
Discussion
http://digscholarship.unco.edu/urj/vol1/iss2/2 10
Lowe: The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
11
This study sought to address the confound in Doyle and Dunn’s (2010) research: to verify
that the participants in the experimental group were indeed malingering during the administration
of the TT by including the TOMM in order to determine whether or not the TT is resilient to
response bias. Participants completed the TT and the TOMM; some were asked to put forth
maximum effort on both, while others were asked to malinger (a form of response bias). We
expected that there would be a statistically significant difference between the control and
experimental groups’ performance on the TOMM as well as the TT. The data analysis reflected
that there was a statistically significant difference between the control and experimental groups’
performance on the TOMM (supporting the hypothesis). However, contrary to the hypothesis,
there was not a statistically significant difference between the control and experimental groups’
performance on the TT. This shows that the participants in both groups were following
instructions: those in the control group were trying their best, and those in the experimental
group were malingering. Since the participants in the experimental group were malingering, and
the results show that there was no difference between the two groups’ performance on the TT, it
has been concluded that the results suggest that the TT is resilient to effort.
Doyle and Dunn (2010) found that there was a statistically significant difference between
the experimental and control groups’ performance on the TT, which is inconsistent with the
results of the present study. However, Doyle and Dunn also found that when the groups repeated
two of the items, neither group improved on one of the items, and only the control group
improved on the other. Despite the difference between the control and experimental groups’
performance on the TT, both groups performed above the level that D-KEFS specified as
indicative of impairment. These findings suggest that individuals could not fake traumatic brain
injury on the TT. The findings of the present study are consistent with those of Doyle and Dunn
(2010), as both the control and experimental groups performed above the D-KEFS determined
level of impairment.
The finding that the TT may be relatively resilient to intentional response bias may imply
that it is also resilient to more incidental forms of response bias such as fatigue or exaggeration
of symptoms. However, more research needs to be conducted in this area. Future research
should be conducted to investigate whether the TT is resilient to other forms of response bias
(fatigue and distraction). Future research should also explore whether other neuropsychological
tools, such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task or Trail Making Task, are also resilient to
may be worthwhile to consider adding a component of implicit memory to the task in an attempt
Acknowledgements
I would like to thank my family for their unconditional love and support. Your
encouragement has given me the motivation to keep going when I haven’t thought I could
continue. I would also like to thank my friends for their support as well: having study buddies
into the early hours of the morning made those long nights bearable. Finally, I would like to
thank my advisor, Dr. Dunn, for answering every single one of my questions, for directing me
through this complex process, for criticizing draft after draft after draft in order to ensure that I
did not settle for mediocrity. And most of all, for giving me the good tissues when nothing
Author’s Note
In May 2011, a poster version of this work was presented at the Association for Psychological
http://digscholarship.unco.edu/urj/vol1/iss2/2 12
Lowe: The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System
13
References
Chan, R. C. K., Chen, E. Y. H., Cheung, E. F. C., Chen, R. Y. L., & Cheung, H. K. (2004).
traumatic brain injury. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clinical Neurosci, 254, 236-241.
Delis, D., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. San
Doyle, Z., & Dunn, T.M. (2010, May). Response Bias on the Tower Test of the D-KEFS.
Leon-Carrion, J., Alarcon, J. C., Revuelta, M., Murillo-Cabezas, F., Domingue-Roldan, J. M.,
94, 75-83.
Malik, A., Turner, M. E., & Sadler, C. (2007). Neuropsychological Evaluation. Retrieved
[http://www.medscape.com], 1. Available:
http://emedicine.medscape.com/article/317596-overview
Sharland, M. J., & Gfeller, J. D. (2007). A survey of neuropsychologists’ beliefs and practices
with respect to the assessment of effort. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 213-
223.
Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). TOMM: Test of Memory Malingering. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-
http://digscholarship.unco.edu/urj/vol1/iss2/2 14