Conover PeopleOrganizePolitical 1984
Conover PeopleOrganizePolitical 1984
Conover PeopleOrganizePolitical 1984
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Midwest Political Science Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to American Journal of Political Science
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
How People Organize the Political World:
A Schematic Model *
A schematic model of political belief systems based on recent research in social and
cognitive psychology is developed. We show that schema theory has the ability to bridge
the gaps among the major competing approaches to the study of mass belief systems as
well as provide direction for new research on the structure and functioning of political
beliefs. An initial test of the theory, done with Q methodology, is reported and the results
provide consistent support for the general predictions of schema theory as applied to
political beliefs.
In the study of belief systems there has recently been a sharp increase
in the number of writings critical of "paradigmatic" understandings, and
there is a growing lack of consensus in the literature about the meaning
of key concepts (Bennett, 1977). To some extent, this confusion has been
generated by research from basically two different perspectives. Some
theorists advocate a "sociological" view that emphasizes the "social"
origins of mass belief systems and focuses on the constraint or structure
among specific issue positions (e.g., Converse, 1964; Nie, Verba, and
Petrocik, 1976). Others offer a "psychological" perspective that stresses
the individualistic origins of belief systems and that concentrates on the
structure between specific issue positions and more general political ideas
(e.g., Lane, 1973; Marcus, Tabb, and Sullivan, 1974; Jackson and Marcus,
1975; and Thomas, 1978). Despite their differences, these two perspectives
have shared a common concern with the question of whether people
think ideologically. Yet, recently even this shared focus has come under
fire. Kinder (1982), for one, has argued that, since the great bulk of
research indicates that most people do not think ideologically, we should
abandon our focus on the ideology question and concentrate instead on
how people actually think about politics.
In this paper we do just that. Specifically we propose a schematic
model of how people organize their beliefs about the political world.
While this model by no means resolves the debate between the sociological
and psychological approaches to political belief systems, it does provide
a basis for integrating certain aspects of the two frameworks. More
*An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 1982 Annual Meeting of the
American Political Science Association, the Denver Hilton Hotel, September 2-5, 1982.
The order of the authors' names is alphabetical; we each share equally in any credit that
might be due. We would like to thank Bruce Campbell and several anonymous referees for
their helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
96 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
important, it also leads us to consider phenomena generally not dealt
with by the traditional literature on mass belief systems. Once we have
outlined our approach we proceed to specify a means of testing our
theory that draws heavily on Q methodology. Finally, we review our
initial efforts to test some of the aspects of the model.
Schema Theory
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD 97
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
98 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
affect may well stimulate the development of schemas, heighten their
saliency, and influence their use (Fiske, 1982; Markus and Smith, 1981).
I Sears and Citrin (1982) suggest precisely this in their conceptualization of a tax-revolt
schema.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD 99
2This is not to suggest that all people have organized ways of looking at all facets of
the political world. Some people are "aschematic" (i.e., they lack organized beliefs) with
respect to certain areas of politics. But, saying that someone is aschematic is quite different
from arguing that they have an organized, but isolated, way of looking at that part of the
political world.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
IOO Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
Political Schemas
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD IOI
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
102 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
of content (i.e., the elements) for each domain and at differing levels of
abstraction, and then allow the respondents to identify, through some
sort of rating task, the nature of the schemas that are most relevant to
their own particular way of looking at that portion of the political world.
We pursue this research strategy through the use of Q methodology.
Q methodology is a general approach to the study of attitudes,
beliefs, and preferences that is based on an examination of relationships
among people rather than among variables (for a review of Q method-
ology see Brown, 1980). Q analysis emphasizes the discovery of patterns
that characterize some subset of the respondents instead of examining
extent to which an hypothesized construct or relationship describes all
members of the sample. Theory or prior research guides the selection of
stimuli for the analysis, but the ways in which subjects can respond to
the stimuli are left largely unstructured. The central data-collection
instrument in Q methodology is the Q sort in which subjects are presented
with a large number of stimuli or statements from a particular domain.
They are then instructed to divide the statements into a number of
categories ranging from, for example, most strongly disagree through
neutral to most strongly agree. The next step is to compute a correlation
matrix among the Q sorts, or in other words, among people. To simplify
the data matrix and examine common patterns of sorting the stimuli, the
correlation matrix is factor analyzed and the initial factors rotated to a
final solution. Two strategies are available to interpret the results: the
factor loadings point to the individuals who best define the factors, and
the factor scores indicate which statements or stimuli are most character-
istic of each factor (Brown, 1980).
From our perspective, Q methodology provides an excellent means
of assessing political schemas. As researchers, we are able to specify the
possible range of political schemas by identifying the nature of stimuli
present in different domains and at varying levels of abstraction. The
individual is then allowed to reveal through the rating procedure the
manner in which he or she personally structures those stimuli. By asking
the respondents to rate the statements according to whether they agree
strongly, are neutral, or disagree strongly we have allowed them to reveal
both the affective and cognitive structure they lend to that domain of
stimuli: the critical dimensions of affective structure-valence and inten-
sity (Fiske, 1981)-are explicit in their ratings, while the essential elements
of cognitive organization are implicit in the overall pattern of the ratings.3
In addition, unlike more traditional ways of measuring beliefs Q meth-
3While we argue that Q methodology does reveal elements of both cognitive and
affective structure, it does not do so in a way that allows us to easily separate the two.
This is not a major concern for us, however, since our interest lies in assessing the overall
(both cognitive and affective) patterns of belief organization.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD 103
odology can identify the stimuli from a domain that are most salient to
an individual.
While it might be interesting to identify idiosyncratic forms of
structure, it is probably more useful to specify common patterns of
schematic organization since one function of patterns of political belief
is the communication of shared meaning in society. In this regard, the
factors represent a common way of structuring the stimuli from a specific
domain; in essence they define schemas shared among some portion of
the respondents. The nature of these shared schemas can be determined
by examining the factor scores and thereby identifying the statements or
stimuli most useful in defining the schema. Furthermore, given that the
factor loadings are the correlations between each individual Q sort and
the general pattern defined by the factor, they can be interpreted as
representing the extent to which a particular schema characterizes an
individual's responses to the stimuli making up that domain. Significant
positive loadings can be taken as an indication that the individual has
the shared schema defined by that factor; significant negative loadings
can be interpreted as meaning that the person has a schema that is the
mirror image of that defined by the factor. Finally, if a subject fails to
have a statistically significant loading on any of the factors derived for
that Q sort it would indicate that the person did not have a well-developed
schema for that area-at least not one that was shared by some other
subjects in the analysis. We use the term aschematic to refer to such an
individual.
Data
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
104 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD 105
Findings
4The specific issue position items were drawn from National Election Studies of the
Center for Political Studies (CPS) and National Opinion Research Center (NORC) General
Social Surveys.
5 We used the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) factor program in our
analysis.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 1
/o oof N oof
Number Sample Sa
Loading Loading Loading
Schema on Factor Positive Negative Aschem
I.A.Basic human philosophy 15
Altruistic-positive (F1l) 29 47.5 1.7 r1
B. Hobbesian-Freudian (F12) 10 16.9 0 r1
C. Individualistic (F13) 11 18.6 0 r14
D. Altruistic-complex (F14) 6 8.5 1.7
II.A.Ideological principles 15.
Neoconservativism (F21) 30 49.2 1.7
B. Free-market conservativism (F22) 14 22.0
C. Democratic socialism (F23) 10 16.9 0
III. Economic beliefs 8.
A. Value of free enterprise (F31) 28 45.8 1
B. Society's responsibility for inequities (F32) 15 25
C. Value of self-reliance (F33) 17 27.1 1.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 1 continued
% of % of
Number Sample Sam
Loading Loading Loading 0
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
io8 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
in others there are viewpoints that mix liberal and conservative ideas.
Moreover, at least 100/ of the respondents (6 people) significantly load
on each factor, and in many instances fully a quarter of the sample share
the schema.6 Thus by no means are the schemas we have identified purely
idiosyncratic structures.
Second, an examination of the correlations among the factors within
a Q sort provides some idea of the distinctiveness of these schemas. A
high correlation among two factors indicates that the schemas defined by
the factors are related or share a good deal in common. Alternatively, a
low correlation between factors implies that the two schemas are relatively
independent; that is, they have very little meaning in common. As detailed
in Table 1, in most cases the correlations among the derived factors range
from being moderate to nonexistent. The one major exception to this
pattern occurs in the economic beliefs Q sort. There we find two related,
but distinct, conservative schemas (r = .44 for factors 1 and 3) as well
as one liberal schema. Thus in most Q sorts, the various schemas can be
described as being relatively independent or neutral with regard to one
another.
Third, there is only one really bipolar factor on which substantial
numbers of people load both positively and negatively. That factor-
F63-can be thought of as defining two schemas that are mirror images
of one another; one is an isolationism-nonmilitaristic schema while the
other one (defined by negative loadings on the factor) is an internation-
alism-militaristic schema. For the remaining factors, there are relatively
few negative loadings, suggesting that each schema defines a single,
distinct perspective (as opposed to two perspectives).7 This lack of
bipolarity is critical because it casts serious doubts on traditional concep-
tualizations of political belief systems, which tend to assume that people
structure their beliefs in terms of one or two bipolar structures-typically
labeled liberal-conservative dimensions (Asher, 1980). Furthermore, this
lack of bipolarity is quite consistent with the recent findings that people
do not view either political parties (Weisberg, 1980) or liberal-conservative
labels (Conover and Feldman, 1981) from a bipolar perspective.
Fourth, it is interesting to note that for most of the Q sorts there
are a number of individuals who do not load on any factor (i.e.,
aschematics). They are lowest for the Q sorts dealing with what one
would expect to be very familiar information: social and racial beliefs.
Similarly, the number of aschematics is highest in an area where
6 Several people in each Q sort had significant loadings on two of the extracted factors.
This indicates that their schema for that domain combines two different ways of structuring
the relevant political information and beliefs.
7 This lack of bipolarity is also evident in the correlations among factors: there is only
one negative correlation, which is not sizeable.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD 1O9
9Similarly, a more diverse sample might also be useful in exploring the determinants
of various schemas. Given the relative homogeneity of our sample, such an investigation
would not have been very fruitful in this case. However, even a preliminary analysis using
this sample does indicate that people holding different schemas do differ significantly in
the groups they identify with and the values they hold.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Iio Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 2
The Structure of Political Schemas
Ideologic
Basic Human Philosophy Principles Eco
Schema F11 F12 F13 F14 F21 F22 F23 F31 F32 F3
1. Basic human philosophy
A. Altruistic-positive (Fll)
B. Hobbesian-Freudian (F12)
C. Individualistic (F13)
D. Altruistic-complex (F14)
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 2 continued
Ideologica
Basic Human Philosophy Principles Econom
Schema Fll F12 F13 F14 F21 F22 F23 F31 F32 F33
IV. Racial beliefs
A. Liberal integrationism (F41) .27** -.11 -.31** .04 -.28** .07 .38** -.08 .39** -.3
B. Conservative integrationism (F42) -.19* .04 .43* -.13 .28** .06 -.52** .11 -.41**
V. Social beliefs
A. Religious-liberal sex roles (F51) .30** -.32* .12 .40** .32** -.40** -.10 -.21** .34
B. Liberal sex roles-nonreligious (F52) -.03 .05 -.22** -.26** -.17* .06 .38** -.08 .11
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD II3
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
I14 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
tially liberal orientation, and several viewpoints that mix liberal and
conservative ideas. Furthermore, people relate schemas to one another in
very different ways, depending on the nature of their schemas. For some
people, two domains may be very related to one another, while for others
they are quite independent.
12 The four issues were chosen because they tend to represent the middle range of
correlation; they are not the issues most correlated with the schemas, but neither are they
the ones least correlated.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 3
The Structure of Political Schemas and Specific Issue Po
Government Improv
Schema Standard of Living Integration
I. Basic human philosophy
A. Altruistic-positive (Fll) -.17 -
B. Hobbesian-Freudian (F12) .0
C. Individualistic (F13) .26**
D. Altruistic-complex (F14) -.43** -
II. Ideological principles
A. Neoconservativism (F21) .00 .1
B. Free-market conservativism (F22)
C. Democratic socialism (F23) -.55* 3
III. Economic beliefs
A. Value of free enterprise (F31) .33
B. Society's responsibility for inequities (F32) -.65
C. Value of self-reliance (F33) .53** .
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 3 continued
Government Improv
Schema Standard of Living Integration
IV. Racial beliefs
A. Liberal integrationism (F41) -.35**
B. Conservative integrationism (F42) .4
V. Social beliefs
A. Religious-liberal sex roles (F51) -.23
B. Liberal sex roles-nonreligious (F52) -.1
VI. Foreign-affairs beliefs
A. Internationalism-nonmilitaristic (F61) -.53*
B. Nationalism-ethnocentric (F62) *34* .4
C. Isolationism-nonmilitaristic (F63) -.21* -
NOTE: Entries are Pearson product-moment correlations.
* = (p < .1). ** = (p - .05).
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD II7
mation, while liberal stands on the issues are related to so-called liberal
schemas. Next, the pattern of significant relationships for a particular
schema tends to be consistent across issues (i.e., liberal schemas tend to
be associated with liberal issue positions). The one major exception to
this pattern is the religious/liberal sex-roles schema; people having that
schema tend to have a liberal position on the standard-of-living issue and
a conservative position on marijuana use. Yet such a pattern makes sense
when it is recognized that this schema combines a conservative religious
perspective with a liberal view of some, but not all, social matters.
Finally, those schemas most relevant to the domain of an issue tend
to have some of the strongest correlations with the issue positions. Thus,
for example, the three economic belief schemas are all strongly related
to stands on the standard-of-living issue, and the racial beliefs schemas
are the strongest correlates of positions on school integration. This is not
to say, however, that there are not strong correlations between certain
schemas and issues basically outside their domain. Indeed, there are
substantial correlations between the foreign affairs schemas and all three
domestic issues. On one hand, such correlations may indicate real
substantive links between domains traditionally thought to be independ-
ent. It is certainly plausible that basic beliefs about our own culture vis-
a-vis others (i.e., feelings of nationalism and ethnocentrism) may well be
related to issues of race and economics within our society. On the other
hand, these correlations may be spurious ones created by the relationship
of the foreign affairs schemas to other very basic schemas. To test this
possibility, however, we must shift our attention away from the structural
linkages between political schemas and specific issue positions to a
multivariate analysis and a more traditional focus on our ability to predict
specific issue positions.
We ran stepwise regression analyses in which we regressed the
subjects' stands on the 11 issues on their factor loadings for the 17
schemas (see Table 4). As can be seen, we do very well in predicting
specific issue positions for most of the economic issues and the defense
spending issue; the multiple R's are quite large for those issues, particu-
larly when one takes into account the relatively low reliabilities of the
issue questions.13 The schemas do slightly less well in predicting positions
on the two racial issues, and the least well of all on the four social issues.
But this poor performance on the social issues is not altogether unex-
pected; because the subjects are relatively similar for many of the
13 Analysis by Erikson (1979), for one, indicates that the issue-position scales may
contain a substantial degree of random measurement error. Therefore if an issue scale had
a reliability of .7, for example, an estimated R2 from a regression on that scale of .4 (40o1
explained variance) would actually represent .4/.7, or 57% of the systematic variation in
the issue scale explained.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 4
Multiple Regression Analysis: Specific Issue Positions on Polit
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 4 continued
A. Religious-liberal
V. Social beliefs
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
120 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD 121
Conclusions
APPENDIX
Listed are the 17 schemas and, for illustrative purposes, the two statements with the
most extreme positive-factor scores and the two statements with the most extreme negative
scores. It is critical to remember that the naming of the factors was based on the full array
of positive and negative rankings of statements; in some instances the meaning of the
factors may not be at all clear from the limited number of statements presented. A complete
record on the results of the six factor analyses can be obtained by request from the authors.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
122 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
1. BASIC HUMAN PHILOSOPHY
A. Altruistic-positive: People are seen as being basically good, trusting, and willing to help
each other; altruism is considered important.
+ 1. You can't put a price tag on human life.
+2. If you act in good faith toward people, most all of them will reciprocate with fairness
toward you.
- 1. All in all, it is better to be important and dishonest than to be humble and honest.
-2. An individual's responsibility for the welfare of others extends no further than the
boundaries of his or her immediate circle of friends.
B. Hobbesian-Freudian: A view of people as self-interested, untrustworthy, and difficult
to understand.
+ 1. It's a rare person who will go against the crowd.
+ 2. People are too complex to ever be fully understood.
- 1. The average person has an accurate understanding of the reasons for their behavior.
- 2. People usually tell the truth, even when they know they would be better off lying.
C. Individualistic: Success and failure in life is a matter of personal initiative; people
deserve what they get.
+ 1. If people try hard enough they can usually reach their goals.
+ 2. You can't put a price tag on human life.
- 1. All in all, it is better to be important and dishonest than to be humble and honest.
-2. Our success in life is pretty much determined by forces outside our own control.
D. Altruistic-complex: People are basically self-interested and difficult to understand, but
they still have an obligation to the community as a whole.
+ 1. You can't put a price tag on human life.
+ 2. Honesty is the best policy in all cases.
- 1. All in all, it is better to be important and dishonest than to be humble and honest.
- 2. The average person has an accurate understanding of the reasons for their behavior.
II. IDEOLOGICAL PRINCIPLES
A. Neoconservativism: Big government is seen as a necessary evil needed to maintain law
and order; existing customs and institutions are important.
+ 1. The maintenance of law and order is essential to the sound development of society.
+ 2. Society can be improved by ideas.
- 1. There should be no interference with business and trade.
-2. A better society can only be realized through a radical change of the present social
structure.
B. Free-market conservativism. A basic antigovernment view that stresses the need for
freedom in order to pursue business interests.
+ 1. A great deal of government interference can only lead to bureaucracy and economic
stagnation.
+ 2. If freedom of enterprise is restricted other freedoms will disappear.
- 1. A great deal of government interference leads to planning and therefore a more
efficient economy.
- 2. Efficient, large-scale production requires government intervention.
C. Democratic socialism. A view in which change is perceived as a necessary and positive
element in society. The existence of social classes is viewed as a detriment to society.
+ 1. Society can be improved by ideas.
+ 2. The maintenance of law and order is essential to the sound development of society.
- 1. The existence of social classes is necessary for the welfare of all.
-2. In present-day society, social classes no longer form an important social conflict.
III. ECONOMIC BELIEFS
A. Value of free enterprise. A focus on the positive value of competition, big business, and
profit-making.
+ 1. Private ownership of property is as important to a good society as freedom.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD I23
+ 1. Some people who don't get ahead in life tend to blame the system, when they really
have only themselves to fault.
+ 2. Competition leads to better performance and a desire for excellence.
- 1. The poor are poor because the wealthy and powerful keep them poor.
-2. Competition, whether in school, work, or business is often wasteful and destructive.
IV. RACIAL BELIEFS
A. Liberal integrationism. A view that favors integration at both a public (social) level as
well as at a personal (individual) level.
+ 1. There is nothing wrong with blacks and whites being close personal friends
+ 2. It is certainly proper for blacks and whites to be acquaintances.
- 1. A person should not invite a member of another race to dinner at his or her home.
-2. There should be no blacks serving on the city council.
B. Conservative integrationists. A view that supports integration at the public level, but
opposes it at a close personal level. Also, there is some tendency not to perceive the
existence of discrimination.
+ 1. People should be willing to take orders from a black police officer.
+ 2. It is certainly proper for blacks and whites to be acquaintances.
- 1. It is quite all right for blacks and whites to date each other.
-2. When two qualified people, one black and the other white, are considered for the
same job, the black won't get the job no matter how hard he or she tries.
V. SOCIAL BELIEFS
A. Religious-liberal sex roles. An emphasis on the importance of religious faith, with some
tendency towards liberal sexual attitudes.
+ 1. There is nothing wrong with a married woman working even if she has a husband
capable of supporting her.
+ 2. Religious commitment gives life a purpose it would not otherwise have.
- 1. The Bible is not the actual word of God; it is simply an ancient book of fables,
legends, and history recorded by humans.
- 2. There is no survival of any kind after death.
B. Liberal sex roles-nonreligious. Definitely nonreligious with an emphasis on liberal sexual
attitudes.
+ 1. There is no reason why a man should lose respect for a woman if they have sexual
relations before marriage.
+2. If you lead a good and decent life it is not necessary to go to church.
- 1. Every explanation of man and the world is incomplete unless it takes account of
God's will.
-2. The story of creation as recorded in Genesis is literally true.
VI. FOREIGN AFFAIRS BELIEFS
A. Internationalism-nonmilitaristic. A stress on world-as opposed to U.S.-interests and
the avoidance of war whenever possible.
+ 1. All human beings are of equal importance.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
124 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
+2. International disputes should be settled without war.
- 1. We should be willing to fight for our country whether it is in the right or wrong.
-2. The best way to ensure peace is through military strength.
B. Nationalism-ethnocentric. A basically nationalistic view with some militaristic overtones.
+ 1. It is only natural and right for people to feel that their country is better than any
other.
+ 2. All human beings are of equal importance.
- 1. Our country is probably no better than many others.
-2. The United States should limit itself to defensive weapons only.
C. Isolationism-nonmilitaristic. A heavy emphasis on staying uninvolved in world affairs,
with nonmilitaristic overtones.
+ 1. George Washington's advice to stay out of agreements with foreign powers is just as
wise now as it was when he was alive.
+ 2. We shouldn't risk our happiness and well-being by getting involved with other countries.
- 1. The United States should go out of its way to lend a helping hand to all countries.
- 2. The biblical command against killing does not apply to warfare.
REFERENCES
Asher, Herbert B. 1980. Presidential elections and American politics. 2nd ed. Homewood,
Ill.: Dorsey.
Aaxelrod, Robert. 1973. Schema theory: An information processing model of perception
and cognition. American Political Science Review 67 (December 1973): 1248-66.
Bennett, W. Lance. 1977. The growth of knowledge in mass belief studies: An epistemolog-
ical critique. American Journal of Political Science 21 (August 1977): 465-500.
Brown, Steven R. 1980. Political subjectivity: Applications of Q methodology in political
science. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press.
Cantor, Nancy, and Walter Mischel. 1979. Prototypes in person perception. In Advances in
experimental social psychology, vol. 12. edited by Leonard Berkowitz, pp. 3-52. New
York: Academic.
Cattell, R. B. 1965. Factor analysis: An introduction to essentials. (I) the purpose and
underlying models, (II) the role of factor analysis in research. Biometrics 21 (March-
June 1965): 190-215, 405-35.
Conover, Pamela Johnston, and Stanley Feldman. 1980. Belief system organization in the
American electorate: An alternate approach. In The electorate reconsidered, edited by
John C. Pierce and John L. Sullivan, pp. 49-67. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage.
. 1981. The origins and meaning of liberal/conservative self-identifications. American
Journal of Political Science 25 (November 1981): 617-45.
Converse, Philip E. 1964. The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In Ideology and
Discontent, edited by David Apter, pp. 202-61. New York: John Wiley.
Erikson, Robert S. 1979. The SRC panel data and mass political attitudes. British Journal
of Political Science 9 (January 1979): 89-114.
Fiske, Susan T. 1981. Social cognition and affect. In Cognition, social behavior and the
environment, edited by John H. Harvey, pp. 227-64. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
. 1982. Schema-triggered affect: Applications to social perception. In Affect and
cognition, edited by Margaret Syndor Clark and Susan T. Fiske, pp. 55-78. Hillsdale,
N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Fiske, Susan T., and Donald R. Kinder. 1981. Involvement, expertise, and schema use:
Evidence from political cognition. In Personality, Cognition, and Social Interaction,
edited by Nancy Cantor and John F. Kihlstrom, pp. 171-90. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
HOW PEOPLE ORGANIZE THE POLITICAL WORLD 125
Fiske, Susan T., and Patricia W. Linville. 1980. What does the schema concept buy us?
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6 (December 1980): 543-57.
Graber, Doris A. 1982. Have I heard this before and is it worth knowing?: Variations in
political information processing. Paper prepared for delivery at the 1982 Annual
Meeting of the American Political Science Association, Denver, Colorado, September
1-5.
Hastie, Reid. 1981. Schematic principles in human memory. In Social cognition: The
Ontario symposium, vol. 1., edited by E. Tory Higgins, C. Peter Herman, and Mark
P. Zanna, pp. 39-88. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Jackson, Thomas H., and George E. Marcus. 1975. Political Competence and Ideological
Constraint. Social Science Research 4 (June 1975): 93-111.
Kinder, Donald R. 1982. Enough already about ideology: The many bases of American
public opinion. Paper prepared for delivery at the 1982 Annual Meeting of the
American Political Science Association, Denver, Colorado, September 1-4.
Knoke, David. 1979. Stratification and the dimensions of American political orientations.
American Journal of Political Science 23 (November 1979): 772-91.
Lane, Robert E. 1973. Patterns of political belief. In Handbook of Political Psychology,
edited by Jeanne Knutson, pp. 83-116. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.
Lodge, Milton, and John C. Wahlke. 1982. Politicos, apoliticos, and the processing of
political information. International Political Science Review 3 (January 1982): 131-50.
Marcus, George, David Tabb, and John L. Sullivan. 1974. The application of individual
differences scaling to the measurement of political ideologues. American Journal of
Political Science 18 (May 1974): 405-20.
Markus, Hazel. 1977. Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 35 (February 1977): 63-78.
Markus, Hazel, and Jeanne Smith. 1981. The influence of self-schemas on the perception
of others. In Personality, cognition, and social interaction, edited by Nancy Cantor
and John H. Kihlstrom, pp. 233-62. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Neisser, Ulric. 1976. Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of cognitive
psychology. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Nie, Norman, Sidney Verba, and John R. Petrocik. 1976. The changing American voter.
Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Ostrom, Thomas M., John B. Pryor, and David D. Simpson. 1981. The organization of
social information. In Social cognition: The Ontario symposium, vol. 1, edited by E.
Tory Higgins, C. Peter Herman, and Mark P. Zanna, pp. 3-38. Hillsdale, N.J.:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rumelhart, David E., and Andrew Ortony. 1977. The representation of knowledge in
memory. In Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge, edited by R. C. Anderson,
R. J. Spiro, and W. E. Montague, pp. 99-135. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Sears, David O., and Jack Citrin. 1982. Tax revolt: Something for nothing in California.
Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Taylor, Shelley E. 1981. The interface of cognitive and social psychology. In Cognition,
social behavior and the environment, edited by John H. Harvey, pp. 189-211. Hillsdale,
N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Taylor, Shelley E., and Jennifer Crocker. 1981. Schematic bases of social information
processing. In Social cognition: The Ontario symposium, vol. 1, edited by E. Tory
Higgins, C. Peter Herman, and Mark P. Zanna, pp. 89-134. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Taylor, Shelley E. and Susan T. Fiske. 1981. Getting inside the head: Methodologies for
process analysis in attribution and social cognition. In New Directions in Attribution
Research, vol. 3, pp. 459-524, edited by J. H. Harvey, W. Ickes and R. F. Kidd.
Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
126 Pamela Johnston Conover, Stanley Feldman
Thomas, Dan B. 1978. Political belief systems and ideo-affective resonance: The structuring
principle revisited. Experimental Study of Politics 6 (1978): 34-89.
Weisberg, Herbert. 1980. A multidimensional conceptualization of party identification.
Political Behavior 2 (1980): 33-60.
This content downloaded from 172.96.140.89 on Wed, 18 Dec 2024 03:52:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms