Lezak, M. (1983). the Problem of Assessing Executive Functions
Lezak, M. (1983). the Problem of Assessing Executive Functions
International Journal of
Psychology
Publication details, including instructions for
authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pijp20
To cite this article: Muriel D. Lezak (1982): The Problem of Assessing Executive
Functions, International Journal of Psychology, 17:1-4, 281-297
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.
Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan,
sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is
expressly forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any
representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to
date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be
independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable
for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages
whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection
with or arising out of the use of this material.
International Journal of Psychology 17 (1982) 281-297 28 1
North-Holland Publishing Company
The capacities for formulating goals, planning, and carrying out plans effectively - the executive
Downloaded by [University of Calgary] at 04:27 24 June 2013
functions - are essential €or independent, creative, and socially constructive behavior. Although
they tend to be vulnerable to brain impairment, they are often overlooked in neuropsychological
and neurological examinations. Reasons why there are few formalized examination procedures for
evaluating executive functions are suggested. Prefrontal contributions and the importance of other
brain areas (e.g., subcortical, right hemisphere) to executive functions are discussed. Assessment
techniques are presented for evaluating four categories of executive capacities: (1) goal formula-
tion, (2) planning, (3) carrying out goal-directed plans, and (4) effective performance. The
Tinkertoy Test", which can provide information about these capacities, is described in some detail.
Need for further exploration in this area is emphasized.
Author's address: Muriel D. Lezak, Oregon Health Sciences University and Veterans Adminis-
tration Medical Center, Portland, OR 97201, USA.
cognitive phenomena.
A look at some of the important differences between executive and
cognitive functions may help us appreciate why systematic measure-
ment of. the executive functions has lagged so far behind. It also may
suggest some techniques for measuring executive functions and some
dimensions that could be assessed.
One distinction between cognitive and executive functions lies in the
kinds of question each class of functions calls for. Cognitive functions
concern what and how much knowledge, skill, and intellectual equip-
ment a person may possess. When assessing cognitive functions for
neuropsychological purposes we ask such questions as “What are the
patient’s intellectual strengths and weaknesses?”, “What abilities have
remained intact or are particularly well-developed or deteriorated?”,
and “How well can he perform this task compared to that?” Executive
functions have to do with how a person goes about doing something or
whether he does it at all. Questions dealing with executive functions ask
how well the patient maintains a performance rate, how consistently
and effectively he self-corrects, how responsive is he to changes in the
demands of the task, or does he start and stop activities by himself and
if so, how appropriately, and so on.
Our habits of conceptualizing and observing behavior are much more
suited to dealing with questions of cognitive deficits than impaired
executive functions. The familiar three-dimensional classification
scheme for cognitive functions enables us to analyze a person’s intel-
lectual behavior in terms of verbal and configural components; sensory
modalities; perceptual, response, memory, or concept formation and
reasoning activities. Using this scheme, we can make fine discrimina-
tions between different kinds of aphasic disturbances, of perceptual
M.D.Lezak / Assessing executioe functions 283
behalf may not be aware that something is missing from the patient’s
behavioral repertoire.
This aspect of the problem of assesssing executive functions was
demonstrated by Hebb who found that loss of even considerable
amounts of frontal lobe tissue had little effect on the scores patients
earned on many highly structured tests of cognitive knowledge and
skills (1942). In reporting his examination of a near-blind 15-year-old
following surgical removal of between 40 to 50% of his right hemis-
sphere and 20% of the left due to multiple abscesses Hebb noted that
on most of the tests he gave (excluding those that required vision), his
patient performed well within the expected range for a young man of
Downloaded by [University of Calgary] at 04:27 24 June 2013
his age, and better than expected on recall of digits forward and
backward. Hebb concluded that the patient had “unusually good reten-
tion of subjective clarity, responsiveness, memory, and apparent
coherence of thought processes”. He described the patient’s psychologi-
cal status as “exceptionally good’,, basing this in part on his observa-
tion that the young man “seemed normally alert and responsive, and
quite cooperative’. Yet Hebb made nothing of his observation of the
patient’s “inactivity, and apparent willingness to do nothing for rather
long periods”, an observation that suggests seriously impaired executive
functions with a probable future of chronic social dependency.
The most refined and thoughtful observations and formulations
concerning the nature of impaired executive functions have been made
of patients with frontal lobe damage (Damasio 1979; Hecaen and
Albert 1978; Luria 1966, 1973; Seron 1978). As a rule, patients who
have had significant injury or disease of the prefrontal region of the
brain, particularly if the orbital or medial regions have been damaged,
undergo behavioral and personality changes that prevent them from
conducting their lives in a normal, socially responsible manner. Those
with mild injuries may experience changes in drive, in the intensity,
stability, or flexibility of response, or in social sensitivity that di-
minishes their capacity to function as fully and interact socially as they
once had. Since many of the most handicapping changes these patients
undergo involve one or more of their executive capacities, executive
functions generally have been ascribed to the frontal lobes.
However, damage to other areas of the brain can also interfere with
executive functions. Such deficits are part of the clinical picture of
many disorders involving subcortical regions, particularly when limbic
structures are involved, as can be the case in anoxic conditions (Falicki
M.D. Lerak / Assessing executive functions 285
ways in which executive functions break down and showing how such
breakdowns interfere with the normal expression of behavior. This
review is intended to be illustrative and provocative; it is far from
exhaustive. I hope that others can use it as a point of departure for their
own explorations into this most subtle and most central realm of
human activity.
1. Goal formulation
with motivation and with awareness of self and how one’s surroundings
impinge on oneself. Goal-directed motivation differs from the simple
arousal states that spur infants, impulsive adults, and subhuman
animals. Simple arousal states lead automatically to reactive or instinc-
tive activity. In contrast, persons capable of goal formulation not only
can conceptualize their needs and desires before acting upon them but
can entertain motives that may be far removed from organismic drive
states and much more complex than are impulsive acts or automatic
responses to physiological needs or environmental stimuli. The ability
to create motives out of past experiences, out of an appreciation of
physically or temporally distant needs, or out of one’s imagination
requires self-awareness at a number of levels including awareness of
internal states, an experiential sense of self, and self-consciousness
vis-a-vis the social and objective environment. It also requires the
ability to identify those aspects of one’s surroundings that may have
personal relevance.
Persons who lack capacity to formulate goals for themselves may be
unable to initiate activities excepting elementary responses to internal
stimuli such as bladder pressure, or to external stimuli, such as scratch-
ing at a mosquito. It simply does not occur to them to do anything. In
extreme cases, persons fully capable of complex activities, such as using
table utensils in the socially prescribed manner, may not eat what is set
before them without continuing explicit direction and guidance. Less
impaired persons may eat or drink what they see but even when hungry
will not seek nourishment spontaneously.
A surgeon who suffered cardiac arrest with secondary hypoxia during minor elective
surgery drives a delivery truck for his cousin’s business. He can make deliveries
M.D.Lerak / Assessing executive functions 287
anywhere within his home town so long as he has explicit instructions about where to
go and what to do, but he is unable to handle unexpected situations. When the family
he lives with occasionally leaves him alone on a weekend, he may go for as long as two
days without eating or drinking anything that requires even minimal search or prepara-
tion, although he can make coffee and simple meals when reminded.
can make sense out of his perceptions. The Cookie Jar picture from the
Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (Goodglass and Kaplan 1972)
is an excellent one for this purpose because it is a simple line drawing
of a number of familiar character types (e.g., mother, mischievous boy)
engaged in familiar activities in a familiar setting. The subject can
devise a single, integrated story that shows he takes account of the
important elements in the picture; he can simply describe the picture on
a piecemeal basis which raises questions about his ability to integrate
what he sees; or he may talk about only one or two items. In
disregarding the rest of the picture he may be demonstrating an
impaired capacity to attend systematically to what he sees.
2. Planning
Several capacities are necessary for planning. Not least of these is the
capacity for sustained attention. In order to plan, a person must also be
able to deal objectively with himself in relation to the environment, and
to view the environment objectively, i.e., to take the abstract attitude
(Walsh 1978b). Planning also requires the abilities to think of alterna-
tives, to weigh and make choices, and to evolve a conceptual framework
or structure which can serve to direct activity.
Defective planning can be identified in patients who have no diffi-
culty formulating goals but lose track of their intentions or activities,
do not generate plans, or come up with plans that are unrealistic if not
simply silly. It also appears, often in a more subtle manner, in a
patient’s inability to create a conceptual structure which can give him
the blueprint and time schedule, so to speak, for carrying out a plan.
M.D. Lezak / Assessing executive functions 289
A 35-year old mechanic who had sustained a head injury in a 15-foot fall had not
returned to work in the three years since the accident. In a compensation examination
he reported that he prepares dinner on the days his wife works. When asked to describe
a sample menu, he gave one and included some alternatives, which sounded reasonable.
He also indicated that he did the shopping. When I later asked his wife about his
cooking, she explained that he accompanies her when she shops and that he always
prepares the identical meal exactly the way she had taught him.
A 22-year-old man who had successfully completed two years of college before
sustaining a severe head injury in a motor vehicle accident wanted to leave his parents’
home and live in his own apartment. He knew what was needed to make the move,
continued to talk and pester his parents about it, but was unable to undertake even the
simple step of walking to apartment buildings in his neighborhood to make inquiries.
Table 1
Items used in the Tinkertoy Test.
were also compared to see whether the differences between the patient
and control groups might be due to cognitive deficits. This comparison
yielded significant differences between the groups on both the np
( t = 3.58, df = 27, p < 0.01) and the comp ( t = 4.58, df = 27, p < 0.001)
scores. The lower Tinkertoy Test scores of the patients whose cognitive
performances were relatively intact suggest that this test measures more
than cognitive abilities. Moreover, patients with high Block Design
scores (nine or better) and those with Block Design scores of eight or
less did not differ significantly in tendencies to achieve'high or low np
scores (30 or more, 29 or less) using Fisher's exact probabilities test
(Finney et al. 1963). These data suggest that level of performance on
the Tinkertoy Test is not dependent on constructional ability.
Table 2
Scoring for complexity.
n >20= 1, >30=2, 4
>40=3, =50=4
nome +=1
mar mobile= 1, moving parts = 1
vm X2-1, X4=2
3d 3-dimcnsional= 1
stand free-standing= 1
mode constmtion(s) any combination of pieces= 1
error one or more error -1
---------
Highest score possible 12
Lowest score possible -1
M.D. k z a k / Assessing exenrtioe functions 293
Table 3
Comparisons between groups on np and complexity scores.
Measure Group
Patient Control
Dependent Non-dependent F
“P
Mean (S.D.) 13.5 (9.46) 30.24 (1 1.32) 42.2 (10.03) 26.91‘
Range 0 to 42 9 to 50 23 to 50
Cornplexiv
Mean (S.D.) 2.22 (2.10) 5.47 (1.77) 7.8 (1.99) 28.27‘
Downloaded by [University of Calgary] at 04:27 24 June 2013
Range -1 to8 2 to 9 5 to 12
4. Effective performance
* * *
References
Albert, M.L., 1978. ‘Subcortical dementia’. In: R. Katzman, R.D.Terry and K.L. Bick (eds.),
Alzheimer’s disease: senile dementia and related disorders. New York: Raven Press.
Arlien-Mor& P., P. Bruhn, C. Gylensted and B. Melgaard, 1979. Chronic painters’ syndrome.
Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 60, 149-156.
Bowen, F.P., 1976. ‘Behavioral alterations in patients with basal ganglia lesions’. In: N.D. Yahr
(ed.), The basal gangha. New York: Raven Press.
Christensen, A-L., 1979. Luria’s neuropsychological investigation. Copenhagen: Munksgaard.
Cohen, J., 1957a. Factor analytically based rationale for Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Journal
of Consulting Psychology 21, 45 1-457.
Cohen, I., 1957b. The factorial structure of the WAIS between early adulthood and old age.
Journal of Consulting Psychology 21, 283-290.
Damasio, A., 1979. ‘The frontal lobes’. In: K.M. Heilman and E. Valenstein (eds.), Clinical
neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Falicki, Z. and B. Sep-Kowalik, 1969. Psychic disturbances as a result of cardiac arrest. Polish
Medical Journal 8,200-206.
Finney, D.J., R. Latscha, B.M. Bennett and P. Hsu, 1963. Tables for testing significance in a 2 X 2
contingency table. Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press.
Fuld, PA., 1978. ‘Psychological testing in the differential diagnosis of the dementias’. In: R.
Katzman, R.D. Terry and K.L. Bick (eds.), Alzheimer’s disease: senile dementia and related
disorders, Vol. 7: Aging. New York: Raven Press.
Gainotti, G., C. Caltagirone, C. Masullo and G. Miceli 1980. ‘Patterns of neuropsychologic
impairment in various diagnostic groups of dementia’. In: L. Amaducci, A.N. Davison and P.
Antuono (eds.), Aging of the brain and dementia. New York: Raven Press.
Goodglass, H. and E. Kaplan, 1972. Assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia:
Lea and Febiger.
296 M .D.Lezak / Assessing executive functions
Gregersen, P., S. Middelsen, H. Klausen et al. 1978. [A chronic cerebral syndrome in painters.
Dementia due to inhalation of cryptogenic origin?] Ugeskr. Laeg. 140, 1638-1644.
Guilford, J.P., 1969. The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hebb, D.O., 1942. The effect of early and late brain injury upon test scores, and the nature of
normal adult intelligence. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 85,275-292.
Hkaen, H. and M.L. Albert, 1978. Human neuropsychology. New York: Wiley.
Jefferson, J.W., 1976. Subtle neuropsychiatric sequelae of carbon monoxide intoxication. American
Journal of Psychiatry 133, 961-964.
Lezak,M.D., 1976. Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lezak, M.D., 1979. Behavioral concomitants of configurational disorganization..Paper presented at
the Seventh Annual Meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, New York City.
Lezak, M.D., 1980. Assessing initiating, planning, and executive capabilities. Paper presented at
the Annual Meeting of the Australian Society for Brain Impairment, Sydney, Australia,
October.
Downloaded by [University of Calgary] at 04:27 24 June 2013
Luria, A.R.,1966. Higher cortical functions in man, New York: Basic Books.
Luria, A.R., 1973. The working brain: an introduction to neuropsychology (trans. B. Haigh). New
York: Basic Books.
McFie, J., 1975. Assessment of organic intellectual impairment. London: Academic Press.
Messerli, P., X. Seron and R. Tissot, 1979. Quelques aspects des troubles de la programmation
dans le syndrome frontal. Archives Suisse de Neurologie, Neurochirurgie et de Psychiatne 125,
23-35.
Milner, B., 1964. Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. The frontal granular cortex and
behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Milner, B., 1974. 'Hemisphere specialization: scope and limits'. In: F.O. Schmitt and F.G. Worden
(eds.), The neurosciences third study program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Muramoto, O., Y. Kuru, M. Sugishita and Y. Toyokura. 1979. Pure memory loss with hippo-
campal lesions. A pneumoencephalographc study. Archives of Neurology 36, 54-56.
Osterrieth; P.A., 1944. Le test de copie d'une figure complexe. Archives de Psychologie 30,
206-356.
pillon, B., J.L. Signoret and F. Lhermitte, 1977. Troubles de la pen& spatiale et syndrome
amnisique consicutifs A une enctphalopathie anoxique. h a l e s de Medkine Interne. (Paris)
128, 269-274.
Porteus, S.D.,1959. The maze test and clinical psychology. Palo Alto, CA: Pacific Books.
Rey, A., 1941. L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'enckphalopathie traumatique. Archives de
Psychologie 28, No. 112.
Russell, E.W., 1979. Three patterns of brain damage on the WAIS. Journal of Clinical Psychology
35,611-620.
Seron, X., 1978. Analyse neuropsychologique des lesions prefrontales c h a l'homme. L'Annk
Psychologiq~e78, 183-202.
Smith, A., 1960. Changes in Porteus M a x scores of brain-operated subjects after an eight year
interval. Journal of Mental Science 106, 967-978.
Terman, L.M. and M.A. Mcrrill, 1973. The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (1972 Norms
Edition). Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Teuber, H.L., W.S. Battersby and M.B. Bender, 1951. Performance of complex visual tasks after
cerebral lesions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases 114,413-429.
Tsushima, W.T. and W.S. Tome, 1977. Effects of paint sniffing on neuropsychological test
performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 86, 402-407.
Walsh, K.W., 1978a. 'Frontal lobe problems'. In: G,V. Stanley and K.W. Walsh (eds.), Brain
impairment. Proceedings of the 1976 Brain Impairment Workshop. Parkville: Neuropsychology
Group, Department of Psychology, University of Melbourne.
Walsh, K.W., 1978b. Neuropsychology. New York: Churchill Livingstone/Longman.
M.D.Lezak / Assessing executioe functions 291
Etre capable de formuler des buts, elaborer des plans et les executer effectivement sont “les
fonctions executives” essentieUes a l’existence de conduites autonomes, creatives et socialement
constructives. Bien que ces fonctions soient vulnkables en cas d’atteintes cerebrales, elles sont
souvent neghgizs dans les examens neurologiques et neuropsychologiques. Les raisons de cette
carence au Nveau de 1’8valuation formelle sont evcquks. La contribution des aires prefrontales e t
d’autres aires cerebrales (par ex. les structures souscorticales, himispheriques droites) aux “fonc-
tions exkutives” est tgalement discutte. On presente des procMQ d’evaluation pour quatre
catkgories de capacitt exkutives: (1) la formulation d’un but; (2) la planification de I’action; (3)
l’exhtion orientte du plan; et (4) la performance effectivement realisk.On d k r i t en outre et en
detail le Tinkertoy test@, qui semble capable de fournir des informations en relation avec ces
capacitis. Enfin, la nkessite &explorations futures dans ce domaine est soulignk.
Downloaded by [University of Calgary] at 04:27 24 June 2013