Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp
The light-from-above prior is intact in autistic
children
Abigail Croydon a,⇑,1, Themelis Karaminis a,b,⇑,1, Louise Neil a, David Burr c,d,
Elizabeth Pellicano a,d
a
Centre for Research in Autism and Education (CRAE), Department of Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of
Education, University College London, London WC1H 0NU, UK
b
Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, Erasmusplein 1, 6525 HT Nijmegen, The Netherlands
c
Institute of Neuroscience, National Research Council (CNR), 56100 Pisa, Italy
d
School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, Perth, Western Australia 6009, Australia
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 19 November 2016
Revised 10 April 2017
Keywords:
Perception
Autism
Bayesian priors
Light-from-above
Development
Bias
a b s t r a c t
Sensory information is inherently ambiguous. The brain disambiguates this information by anticipating or predicting the sensory
environment based on prior knowledge. Pellicano and Burr (2012)
proposed that this process may be atypical in autism and that
internal assumptions, or ‘‘priors,” may be underweighted or less
used than in typical individuals. A robust internal assumption used
by adults is the ‘‘light-from-above” prior, a bias to interpret
ambiguous shading patterns as if formed by a light source located
above (and slightly to the left) of the scene. We investigated
whether autistic children (n = 18) use this prior to the same degree
as typical children of similar age and intellectual ability (n = 18).
Children were asked to judge the shape (concave or convex) of a
shaded hexagon stimulus presented in 24 rotations. We estimated
the relation between the proportion of convex judgments and
stimulus orientation for each child and calculated the light source
location most consistent with those judgments. Children behaved
similarly to adults in this task, preferring to assume that the light
source was from above left, when other interpretations were compatible with the shading evidence. Autistic and typical children
used prior assumptions to the same extent to make sense of shading patterns. Future research should examine whether this prior is
⇑ Corresponding authors at: Centre for Research in Autism and Education (CRAE), Department of Psychology and Human
Development, UCL Institute of Education, University College London, London WC1H 0NU, UK (A. Croydon), Centre for Language
Studies, Radboud University, Erasmusplein 1, 6525 HT Nijmegen, The Netherlands (T. Karaminis).
E-mail addresses: a.croydon@easynet.co.uk (A. Croydon), themkar@gmail.com (T. Karaminis).
1
These authors contributed equally to this work.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.005
0022-0965/Ó 2017 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc.
This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
114
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
as adaptable (i.e., modifiable with training) in autistic children as it
is in typical adults.
Ó 2017 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open
access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by/4.0/).
Introduction
In human vision, the complex dimensions of a visual scene—the shapes of objects, their spatial
arrangement, and their material properties—are reduced to flat patterns of excitation of the cones
and rods of the retina. Information entering the brain is inherently ambiguous, compatible with a
range of interpretations. Visual input, therefore, is ‘‘underspecified” for the task of providing the reliable and stable awareness of the environment that we experience. Consequently, perception has long
been considered as a process of ‘‘unconscious inference” (Helmholtz, 1866/1911), in which existing
knowledge is spontaneously and automatically deployed to interpret the meaning of sensory signals.
Expectations based on experience of how the material environment works—for example, of faces being
convex or of light sources being overhead—feed into the construction of a percept by the brain.
The framework provided by Helmholz (1866/1911) was extended to characterize perceptual inferences as ‘‘hypotheses” or informed speculations using noisy and limited data (Gregory, 1980). Perceptual decisions are made possible by comparing the probability of the sensory evidence and prior
experience. The Bayesian framework has since supplied an established mathematical model for perceptual decision making under conditions of uncertainty. In Bayesian terms, if both sensory signals
and knowledge-based hypotheses are represented as probability distributions, techniques of statistical inference can be used to locate the combined point of maximal probability, the ‘‘best guess” interpretation (Knill, Kersten, & Yuille, 1996). In Bayesian perceptual inference, the prior probability
distribution, or ‘‘prior,” represents a ‘‘baseline” understanding of the likelihood of particular environmental conditions on the basis of past experience (Gregory, 1980).
A number of visual priors have been established and are thought to improve overall perceptual efficiency by weighting perceptual hypotheses in a broadly reliable way. For example, a prior for convexity reflects the predominance of convex, over concave, objects in the world (Langer & Bülthoff, 2001;
Sun & Perona, 1996). The statistical inference calculation implies a trade-off between the image data
and the prior probability, such that perception will be more prior driven when ambiguity in the sensory input is high. In some circumstances, prior-driven expectations will be misguided, resulting in
visual illusions. For example, perceiving a hollow mask as a convex mask implies the operation of
overriding expectations of convexity in faces. Hence, when problems of object perception are resolved
on a probabilistic basis, the optimal solution may still be ‘‘inaccurate” (Kersten & Yuille, 2003). Importantly, in the context of this research, Bayesian priors envisage dynamic connections among perceptual inference, experience of the environment, and behavior (Lee, Yang, Romero, & Mumford, 2002).
Atypicalities in sensation and perception are highly characteristic of autism (Baranek, David, Poe,
Stone, & Watson, 2006; Leekam, Nieto, Libby, Wing, & Gould, 2007; Mottron, Dawson, Soulieres,
Hubert, & Burack, 2006; Simmons et al., 2009). Although difficulties in social communication are considered hallmarks of autism, sensory reactivity, including hypersensitivity (e.g., to light or touch) and
hyposensitivity (e.g., to pain), was noted during the first description of autism (Kanner, 1943). Sensory
reactivity has since been shown to be present in the majority of autistic children and adults (BenSasson et al., 2009; Leekam et al., 2007; Simmons et al., 2009), to be pervasive and persistent across
development (Crane, Goddard, & Pring, 2009; McCormick, Hepburn, Young, & Rogers, 2016), and to
have a substantial impact on the lives of autistic people (e.g., Dickie, Baranek, Schultz, Watson, &
McComish, 2009; Grandin, 2009; Williams, 1994).
In the perceptual domain, the majority of scientific studies have reported atypical processing in
aspects of visual perception and visual attention ranging from characteristically nonsocial stimuli
and tasks, such as discrimination of chromatic stimuli (e.g., Franklin et al., 2010), cast shadow
(Becchio, Mari, & Castiello, 2010), static gratings (e.g., Bertone, Mottron, Jelenic, & Faubert, 2005), mov-
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
115
ing dots (e.g., Milne et al., 2002; Pellicano, Gibson, Maybery, Durkin, & Badcock, 2005), and complex
objects (e.g., ‘‘Greebles”) (e.g., Behrmann, Thomas, & Humphreys, 2006; Davies, Bishop, Manstead, &
Tantam, 1994), to social stimuli, including faces (e.g., Dalton et al., 2005; Kemner & van Engeland,
2006), eye gaze (e.g., Elsabbagh et al., 2009), and biological motion (e.g., Blake, Turner, Smoski,
Pozdol, & Stone, 2003; Klin, Lin, Gorrindo, Ramsay, & Jones, 2009; for a review, see Simmons et al.,
2009). Theories of autistic perception have explained these findings in terms of a ‘‘detail-focused” perceptual style (Frith & Happé, 1994), generally enhanced perceptual functioning (Mottron et al., 2006),
or reduced generalization (Plaisted, 2001).
Building on these accounts, Pellicano and Burr (2012) suggested that it is not sensory processing
itself that is atypical in autism but rather the interpretation of the sensory input. Specifically, drawing
on the tools of Bayesian theory, they proposed that the internal priors of autistic people are underweighted or less used than in typical individuals. Attenuated priors might result in enhanced perception in some contexts and reduced performance in others, depending on whether the task draws more
heavily on sensory input itself or on successful perceptual prediction using prior knowledge. This idea
was expanded by several related Bayesian and neurobiological accounts that proposed possible atypicalities in predictive processing in autism (Brock, 2012; Friston, Lawson, & Frith, 2013; Lawson, Rees,
& Friston, 2014; Sinha et al., 2014; van Boxtel & Lu, 2013; Van de Cruys et al., 2014).
Evidence supporting the hypothesis of attenuated priors in autism comes from studies showing
that autistic children and adults show reduced adaptation, a form of experience-dependent plasticity
in which neural systems fine-tune to the current visual environment according to the previous context. There is now considerable evidence for reduced adaptation in autism for high-level visual attributes, both social stimuli (e.g., faces: Pellicano, Jeffery, Burr, & Rhodes, 2007; biological motion: van
Boxtel, Dapretto, & Lu, 2016) and nonsocial stimuli (e.g., numerosity: Turi et al., 2015), as well as
for other sensory modalities (e.g., touch: Tommerdahl, Tannan, Holden, & Baranek, 2008; audition:
Lawson, Aylward, White, & Rees, 2015); audiovisual calibration: Turi, Karaminis, Pellicano, & Burr,
2016).
More recently, Karaminis et al. (2016) demonstrated atypicalities in prior knowledge in autism
more formally, in the context of temporal reproduction, using a Bayesian computational model for
central tendency (Cicchini, Arrighi, Cecchetti, & Burr, 2012). The computational model proposed that
central tendency reflects the integration of noisy temporal estimates with prior knowledge representations of a mean stimulus. This integration serves to reduce overall error and, crucially, is flexible; the
noisier the sensory estimates, the greater the reliance on prior knowledge. Karaminis and colleagues
contrasted the performance of autistic and typical children completing a time interval reproduction
task (measuring central tendency) and a temporal discrimination task (assessing temporal resolution)
to the predictions of the Bayesian model. Computational simulations suggested that central tendency
in autistic children was much less than that predicted by computational modeling given the poor temporal resolution of these children. Autistic children presented with a much less flexible use of priors
across development compared with typically developing children.
In the current study, we provided another test of the account of Pellicano and Burr (2012) using a
well-established perceptual expectation, namely the ‘‘light-from-above” prior. The patterns of light
and shade on and around an object provide uncertain cues for the brain to determine its shape and
position in space. To resolve this ambiguity, the visual system must assume a light source location
(Adams, 2007; Gerardin, de Montalembert, & Mamassian, 2007; Mamassian & Landy, 2001). The
light-from-above prior weights inference toward assuming that a light source is located above the
scene observed, as is most commonly experienced. The presence of a light-from-above prior is demonstrated when an image lit from above is rotated by 180°, so that shading previously perceived as indicating convexity will indicate concavity (and vice versa). Simple rotation changes interpretation (e.g.,
Mamassian & Goutcher, 2001). A perceptual bias toward light from above is also demonstrated in
visual search; observers can identify shapes compatible with light from above significantly faster than
the same shapes reversed (i.e., compatible with light from below), whereas shapes suggesting a light
source from the side are recognized both more slowly and less accurately (Kleffner & Ramachandran,
1992). A further characteristic of the light-from-above prior is that it is biased slightly to the left of
vertical (Gerardin et al., 2007; Mamassian & Goutcher, 2001; Sun & Perona, 1996; Thomas, Nardini,
& Mareschal, 2010), although there is considerable individual variation in the location of the prior
116
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
(e.g., Adams, 2007; Champion & Adams, 2007; Mazzilli & Schofield, 2013; Morgenstern, Murray, &
Harris, 2011). While the preference for an overhead light source is assumed to have an environmental
origin, the source of the leftward bias is not well understood (see Mamassian & Goutcher, 2001, for a
discussion). The light-from-above prior strongly influences depth perception and assists in reconstructing three dimensions (objects/scenes) from two-dimensional retinal images.
Here, we examined the use of this robust and well-characterized light-from-above prior in autistic
children. We also investigated age-related differences in the use of the prior in autistic and typical
children. To address these aims, we assessed autistic and typical children of similar age and ability
on a shape judgment task designed to reveal individual differences in implicit judgments of light
source location. Specifically, we adapted the seven-hexagon stimulus developed by Andrews,
Aisenberg, d’Avossa, and Sapir (2012), which was used to demonstrate an effect of cultural differences
(reading direction) on location of the prior and, therefore, was thought to be sufficiently sensitive to
potential group-level variations. Limited shading information in the stimulus delivered a high level of
ambiguity about depth, ensuring that perceptual inference was required to resolve shape and a light
source location would need to be assumed.
We sought to investigate whether autistic children would resolve the ambiguous shape-fromshading information in our stimulus using an assumption of light from above. Specifically, we tested
whether autistic children would show the bias in stable conditions designed to encourage reliance on
prior experience of lighting conditions. An attenuated light-from-above prior, in line with the hypothesis of Pellicano and Burr (2012), might result in more mixed shape judgments according to rotation of
the stimulus by autistic children than by typical children, with fewer compatible with light from
above. Because priors are thought to smooth over neural noise, attenuated priors in the autistic group
might also lead to less stable perception of convexity or concavity, leading to more varied (or less confident) interpretations. Our test purposefully excluded signals that might compete with the lightfrom-above prior, such as a visible light source (Morgenstern et al., 2011), to determine whether
the prior is fundamentally intact and develops similarly in autistic children compared with typical
children.
Method
Participants
In total, 18 autistic children (16 boys) and 18 typical children (12 boys), all between 7 and 14 years
of age, took part in this study. Children were recruited via community contacts. All autistic children
had been previously diagnosed with an autism spectrum condition by independent clinicians and
scored above the threshold for an autism spectrum disorder on either the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Second Edition (ADOS-2) or the Lifetime version of the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) (see Table 1 for scores). All typically developing children scored below the cutoff for
autism on the SCQ (score of 15; Rutter, Bailey, & Lord, 2003), reflecting the absence of clinically significant autistic features. All children had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, as reported
by their parents.
The groups were matched in terms of age, t(34) = 0.06, p = .95, verbal IQ, t(34) = 0.94, p = .35, performance IQ, t(34) = 0.88, p = .39, and full-scale IQ, t(34) = 0.38, p = .70, as measured by the Wechsler
Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence–Second Edition (WASI-2; Wechsler, 2011) (see Table 1 for scores).
All children obtained full-scale IQ scores of 70 or above and, thus, were considered to be cognitively
able.
An additional 15 typical adults (14 female; 20–35 years of age), recruited from the university, were
tested to establish parameters for adult performance in our task. One additional adult was tested but
excluded from the analysis because the estimated light source direction (see ‘‘Measurements” section
below) lay more than 2 standard deviations to the left of the group average. Removing this outlier did
not change the results reported here.
117
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
Table 1
Participant characteristics.
n (boys:girls)
Age (years)
Verbal IQa
Performance IQa
Full-scale IQa
SCQb
ADOS-2c
ADOS-2c CSSd
Typical children
Autistic children
18 (12:6)
10.14 (1.76)
105.94 (15.36)
99.11 (19.83)
102.94 (13.46)
4.59 (3.32)
18 (16:2)
10.19 (2.44)
101.11 (15.32)
104 (12.95)
104.72 (14.41)
20.29 (7.15)
10.65 (3.87)
6.23 (1.92)
Note. Values are means (and standard deviations) except in the first row.
a
Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (WASI-2; Wechsler, 2011).
b
Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter et al., 2003).
c
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Second Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012).
d
ADOS-2 Calibrated Severity Score (CSS; Gotham et al., 2009; Hus & Lord, 2014).
Ethics statement
The study was conducted in accordance with the principles laid out in the Declaration of Helsinki.
Ethical approval was granted by the faculty research ethics committee of the university (FPS456). Parents of all children gave their informed written consent prior to the participation of their children in
the project, and children gave their verbal assent.
Stimuli
Following Andrews et al. (2012), the stimulus comprised seven tessellated gray-scale hexagons on
a gray-scale background (RGB = 127, 127, 127), with shading on the inner and outer edges of the
shapes (see Fig. 1) providing ambiguous cues to depth. Each shape was 2.5° of visual angle, with an
intermediate level of blur. The stimulus was rotated by 360° in 15° increments, compatible with different light source locations. We presented 24 rotations in a randomized order in order to estimate the
light source direction most consistent with the judgments of convexity and concavity by children.
Each rotation was presented five times, yielding a total of 120 trials.
Stimuli were presented on a Dell Precision laptop screen with 1366 768 pixel resolution at a
refresh rate of 60 Hz and mean luminance of 60 cd/m2. All children viewed the stimuli binocularly
at a distance of approximately 57 cm from the screen.
Fig. 1. Stimulus with light and shadow as though lit from directly above (A) and rotated + 150° to maximize the appearance of
concavity in the central hexagon according to mean adult priors (B).
118
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
Fig. 2. Examples of slides introducing the shape judgment task. Panel D shows the stimulus rotated 150° and is followed by an
animation of the bee filling the central cell with honey.
Procedure
The experiment was written in Matlab using the Psychophysics Toolbox extensions (Brainard,
1997; Kleiner, Brainard, & Pelli, 2007). We measured the implicit judgments of light source location
made by children in the context of a child-friendly computer game. During the introduction phase,
the cover story was introduced showing colored cartoon honeycomb cells and an animated bee. Children were asked to help the bee decide which ‘‘cells” needed to be filled with honey (Fig. 2A and B).
Children were told that they would collect points for their decisions and win a small gift with more
than 500 points. Dummy points were also provided to maintain children’s interest. To encourage children to distinguish between cells they perceived as empty and those they saw as full, they were also
asked not to ‘‘waste” honey when cells were ‘‘full.” We also showed the bee with a telescope looking at
the central hexagon (Fig. 2C) to ensure that answers were given for this cell only.
After the introduction to the story, children were taken through the demonstration phase. The cartoon honeycomb was replaced with the hexagon stimulus, which appeared at a rotation of 150° so that
the central cell would be most likely to be perceived as concave, according to the known mean prior
for adults (Fig. 2D). Children were told that this needed filling with honey, which was demonstrated
by the bee in an animation. Two demonstration trials followed, using the exact procedure followed in
later test trials. The stimulus appeared with the prompt, ‘‘Does this one need filling?” Color-coded
‘‘yes” and ‘‘no” prompts appeared below the question to the left and right of the screen, respectively,
in order to match the location of the response keys (letters ‘‘a” and ‘‘l” on the keyboard). These keys
were also labeled ‘‘Y” and ‘‘N” using sticky labels matched in color to the font of the on-screen
response options (yellow for yes and blue for no). Children were asked to press a key to indicate their
response, with left (yes) indicating perceived concavity and right (no) indicating perceived convexity.
The first demonstration trial again showed the rotation most likely to be perceived as concave, and
children were invited to respond. A ‘‘yes” response was required to proceed. The second demonstration trial showed the stimulus rotated 330°, the position most likely to be perceived as convex. This
required a ‘‘no” response and was followed by the caption ‘‘No! That one’s full!” Children could repeat
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
119
the demonstration trials if required, but there were no such further trials in order to avoid influencing
subsequent responses (Stone, Kerrigan, & Porrill, 2009). All children produced correct responses for
both trials.
The test phase commenced immediately following the demonstration phase. An animated bee hovered on the screen (2 s) as a fixation point. Children saw the honeycomb stimulus in one of the 24 rotations, the order of which was randomized. They answered the question ‘‘Does this one need filling?”
by pressing the ‘‘yes” or ‘‘no” key according to their interpretation of the stimulus shape. Trials were
self-paced, so that each response triggered the next fixation bee. Children completed three blocks of
40 test trials, yielding a total of 120 trials. Dummy scores and general encouragement were provided
at the end of each block.
General procedure
Children were tested individually in a quiet room at the university. The room was lit only by the
computer screen, so that no environmental cues were available to influence children’s perceptions
of lighting direction (Morgenstern et al., 2011). Testing on the experimental task lasted 10–15 min.
The WASI-2 (and the ADOS-2 for autistic children) was administered to children in later sessions.
Measurements
For each participant, we first calculated the proportion of convex judgments (‘‘no” judgments in our
game) for each stimuli direction. Following Andrews et al. (2012), the relation between the proportion
of convex judgments (‘‘no” judgments in our game) and stimulus orientation was estimated for each
participant using a multivariate logistic regression:
pðCjhÞ ¼ 1=ð1 þ e
f ðhÞ
Þ;
ð1Þ
where for a given stimulus direction h, f(h) was a series of sine and cosine functions,
f ðhÞ ¼ a0 þ a1 cos h þ b1 sin h:
ð2Þ
We then estimated the light source direction implied by the judgments of each participant as
Olight
source
¼ tan 1 ðb1 =a1 Þ:
ð3Þ
Results
Fig. 3 shows example results from one autistic and one typical child participant. The plots show
two-dimensional psychometric functions on polar axes, plotting percentage convex as a function of
rendered orientation. In both cases, the judgment tends to be convex for orientations left of vertical
and concave for orientations to the right; this is consistent with a light-from-above interpretation,
specifically with a light-from-above left interpretation, in line with previous research (Gerardin
et al., 2007; Sun & Perona, 1996; Thomas et al., 2010). The average orientation [estimated from Eq.
(3)] is indicated by the orange circle: 32° for the autistic child and 17° for the typical child.
Fig. 4 shows group averages for the estimated light source direction biases for the three participant
groups. As expected (cf. Gerardin et al., 2007; Sun & Perona, 1996; Thomas et al., 2010), average light
source biases were to the left of vertical (autistic children: M = 12.67°, SD = 12.51; typical children:
M = 12.50°, SD = 7.87; adults: M = 12.06°, SD = 16.72) and were significantly lower than zero for all
groups [autistic children: t(17) = 4.44, p < .001; typical children: t(17) = 6.72, p < .001; adults: t(14)
= 2.79, p = .01]. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with group (autistic children, typical children, or adults) as a between-participants factor revealed no significant effect of group on the magnitude of the light bias, F(2, 48) = 0.10, p = .99. We also examined the data by performing a Bayesian oneway ANOVA using JASP software (Version 0.8.0.0; JASP Team, 2016) and estimating a Bayes factor
using Bayesian information criteria (Wagenmakers, 2007). The Bayes factor allowed for a comparison
of the fit of our data under the null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis. The estimated Bayes
120
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
Fig. 3. Example data for one autistic participant and one typical child participant. Magenta and cyan dots indicate the
proportion of convex judgments for the orientation values tested for one autistic participant (left) and one typical participant
(right). Blue lines indicate the predictions of the fitted bimodal distribution. Yellow dots show the estimate of the light source
biases for these two participants. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the
Web version of this article.)
Fig. 4. Light source bias estimates for autistic children, typical children, and typical adults estimated from Eq. (3). Negative
values correspond to biases to the left of the vertical. Red bands indicate median values; boxes extend from lower to upper
quartile values; and whiskers show the full range of values. (For interpretation of the reference to color in this figure legend, the
reader is referred to the Web version of this article.)
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
121
factor (null/alternative) suggested that our results were 6.56:1 in favor of the null hypothesis, that is,
6.56 times more likely to occur under a model without an effect of group on the magnitude of the lightfrom-above bias rather than a model with an effect of group. Our data, therefore, provided substantial
evidence (Wetzels et al., 2011) that adults, typical children, and autistic children interpreted the shape
of the stimulus in a similar way in that they used a light-from-above prior to a similar degree.
To investigate potential age-related differences in the formation of the prior, we examined the relationship between chronological age and the magnitude of the light bias. There was no significant relationship between age and bias for either group (autistic: r = .24, p = .33; typical group: r = .03,
p = .90). There was also no significant relationship with ability, as measured by full-scale IQ scores
on the WASI-2, for either group of children (autistic: r = .10, p = .95; typical group: r = .23, p = .36),
or with autistic symptomatology, as measured by the ADOS-2 severity scores of children (r = .13,
p = .61) (Gotham, Pickles, & Lord, 2009; Hus & Lord, 2014).
We also examined whether the judgments of younger children were influenced by a preference for
convexity, as suggested by Thomas et al. (2010), by conducting a Pearson correlation analysis between
participants’ percentage of convex judgments (across all responses) and age. However, we found no
significant relationship between age and preference to interpret the shape as convex for either group
of children (typical: r = .05, p = .84; autistic: r = .02, p = .93).
Discussion
This study investigated whether autistic and typical children apply a light-from-above prior similarly to interpret shape from shading in conditions where shading cues are ambiguous. The mean
light-from-above prior for the children seen in this study was approximately 13° (above and slightly
to the left). We found no significant difference in assumed light source location between autistic and
typical children. Judgments of depth by autistic children were influenced by a light-from-above prior—
that is, they preferentially assumed a light source above and to the left of the stimuli—to a similar
extent as those by typical children. Given that all children reported that it was easy to decide whether
the cell should be filled, it appears that they used their priors to help resolve noise and ambiguity and
achieve stable percepts. This finding is consistent with a recent report suggesting that priors for eye
gaze direction—where gaze is more likely to be perceived as direct in conditions of uncertainty—are
intact in autistic adults (Pell et al., 2016).
In our task, environmental conditions other than rotation were held constant, so that inference
relied on participants’ existing priors, summing their long-term experience of lighting conditions.
We used a task that had previously shown subtle differences between groups whose long-term (cultural) experience of reading direction differed (Andrews et al., 2012), suggesting that it should have
been possible to detect differences based on long-term experience between our groups if they were
in fact present.
Similar mean levels might nevertheless conceal different group levels of adaptation in the short
term to prevailing environmental conditions. Experimental evidence has demonstrated that changes
in prevailing environmental conditions can modify light priors in the short term. In the case of typical
adults, the light-from-above prior adapts rapidly (Adams, Graf, & Ernst, 2004; Adams, Kerrigan, & Graf,
2010; Champion & Adams, 2007), so that training with haptic feedback can change an individual light
prior location by approximately 10° with 1.5 h of training (Adams et al., 2004). Participants’ visual
judgments on a separate task after training were recalibrated in line with the new prior (Adams
et al., 2010; Champion & Adams, 2007), indicating that the prior had been temporarily updated. Similarly, learning an association between a context and its lighting conditions led to contextually appropriate calibration of the light prior (Kerrigan & Adams, 2013), in line with Bayesian updating of the
prior probability distribution by context. Flexible adaptation to prevailing conditions was not tested
in the current study but is a worthy avenue for future research and a more direct test of the account
of Pellicano and Burr (2012).
It is also important to consider precisely how prior knowledge acts on perceptual inference.
Although evidence strongly suggests that an assumed light source position is represented early in
the visual system and acts on early inference (Champion & Adams, 2007; Lee et al., 2002;
122
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
Mamassian, Jentzsch, Bacon, & Schweinberger, 2003), perceptual computation also appears to be an
interactive process, so that activity in the early visual cortex may nevertheless take into account
behavioral experience and higher order perceptual saliency (Lee, 2003; Lee et al., 2002). Champion
and Adams (2007) found evidence of both early and late influence of the light-from-above prior on
perceptual inference. Although visual haptic training modified the light-from-above prior used in a
judgment of shape task, the prior assessed in a visual search task was not affected by the same training
(Champion & Adams, 2007). The discrepant effects of the training environment were interpreted as
evidence that although training did not touch the ‘‘quick and dirty” process of early inference, it influenced later additional stages of visual processing, where recent experience with the world is taken
into account. Because our task did not require accounting for recent experience, the mean priors we
found may reflect the influence of the prior on the initial stage of visual processing alone, leaving open
the possibility of reduced influence of priors during later stages of visual processing for autistic children. Future research should test how far the priors of children adapt to prevailing environmental
conditions.
Our findings also show that the two groups of children performed similarly to adults. Furthermore,
we found no age-related changes in the use of the light-from-above prior, at least in children between
7 and 14 years of age, suggesting that the prior develops early—before 7 years. This would be consistent with recent findings showing that typical children use prior knowledge in magnitude estimations
from an early age, in particular for temporal (Karaminis et al., 2016) and spatial (Sciutti, Burr, Saracco,
Sandini, & Gori, 2014) interval reproduction. If learning an internal model of the sensory environment
is key to the statistical inference process involved in Bayesian perception, and also is critical to learning beyond perception (Fiser, Berkes, Orbán, & Lengyel, 2010), early acquisition of priors, especially
robust ones like these, would make sense developmentally. However, Karaminis et al. (2016) found
differences between autistic and typical development in time interval reproduction. Autistic children
showed significantly less precision in their estimations than matched typical children, with computational simulations suggesting less use of prior knowledge than expected to compensate for this level of
imprecision. This result implies differences in the flexible deployment of priors to improve the precision/reliability of estimates. Our task showed no differences between typical and autistic children in
the use or development of the light-from-above prior, at least in controlled conditions where competing information is limited (cf. Morgenstern et al., 2011), but the findings of Karaminis and colleagues
are another reason to study the flexibility of the light-from-above prior (cf. Adams et al., 2010) in
autistic children.
Our findings are in contrast to other research reporting developmental increases in responses consistent with light-from-above prior (Stone, 2011; Stone & Pascalis, 2010; Thomas et al., 2010),
although the evidence is not clear-cut. Thomas et al. (2010) found an overall effect of age on children’s
interpretation of ambiguous stimuli, so that trials answered assuming light from above increased
across childhood. Yet this result was complicated by the measurement of two distinct priors—one
for convexity and one for light from above. When the two priors biased interpretation in the same
direction, there was little change in use of the light-from-above prior over development; when the priors conflicted, younger children (4- and 5-year-olds) preferred a convex interpretation, whereas older
children and adults preferred to assume light from above if light could be interpreted as from above
and to the left rather than as above right. However, the number of younger children was small (n = 7),
with some performing at around chance levels. Stone and Pascalis (2010) showed children aged 4–10
years geometric shapes and photographic stimuli, either upright or rotated 180°, reporting increasing
levels of interpretation consistent with light from above by age group. Yet the results for symbolic
images—where ambiguity is higher and, therefore, the potential influence of the prior is stronger—
did not clearly support this interpretation. In particular, the regression analysis for concave stimuli
(if lit from above) against age was nonsignificant, whereas that for convex symbols (if lit from above)
indicated greater use of the prior for children aged around 7 or 8 years. Overall, these results point to
the difficulty of disentangling the influence of light from above and convexity priors and the difficulty
of interpreting which factors influenced the performance of younger children.
In sum, we have established that the light-from-above prior is similar in strength in a shape judgment task in school-age autistic and typical children of similar age and ability. Furthermore, contrary
to previous reports (Stone & Pascalis, 2010; Thomas et al., 2010), our results suggest a stable level of
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
123
use of the prior across this age range. Although our methods were developmentally sensitive, with
performance not being confounded with linguistic demands (concave and convex), our sample sizes
were relatively small and we may have had insufficient power to detect age-related changes in the
use or strength of the prior; this is especially important given that priors vary widely between individuals (e.g., Adams, 2007; Champion & Adams, 2007; Mazzilli & Schofield, 2013; Morgenstern
et al., 2011). One outstanding question is whether the light-from-above prior is just as adaptable as
it is in adults for typically developing children—but especially for children on the autism spectrum.
Acknowledgments
This work was generously supported by a grant from the U.K. Medical Research Council awarded to
E.P. and D.B. (MR/J013145/1) and also by the European Science Council (ERC advanced grant
‘‘ECSPLAIN” awarded to M. C. Morrone). Research at the Centre for Research in Autism and Education
(CRAE) is also supported by the Clothworkers’ Foundation and Pears Foundation. We are very grateful
to all the young people and their parents who took part in this research. We would also like to thank
Ayelet Sapir for providing us with the stimulus used in Andrews et al. (2012).
References
Adams, W. J. (2007). A common light-prior for visual search, shape, and reflectance judgments. Journal of Vision, 7(11),
11.1–11.7.
Adams, W. J., Graf, E. W., & Ernst, M. O. (2004). Experience can change the ‘‘light-from-above” prior. Nature Neuroscience, 7,
1057–1058.
Adams, W. J., Kerrigan, I. S., & Graf, E. W. (2010). Efficient visual recalibration from either visual or haptic feedback: The
importance of being wrong. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 14745–14749.
Andrews, B., Aisenberg, D., d’Avossa, G., & Sapir, A. (2012). Cross-cultural effects on the assumed light source direction: Evidence
from English and Hebrew readers. Journal of Vision, 13(13). http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/13.13.2.
Baranek, G. T., David, F. J., Poe, M. D., Stone, W. L., & Watson, L. R. (2006). Sensory experiences questionnaire: Discriminating
sensory features in young children with autism, developmental delays, and typical development. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 47, 591–601.
Becchio, C., Mari, M., & Castiello, U. (2010). Perception of shadows in children with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE, 5,
e10582.
Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 10, 258–264.
Ben-Sasson, A., Hen, L., Fluss, R., Cermak, S. A., Engel-Yeger, B., & Gal, E. (2009). A meta-analysis of sensory modulation
symptoms in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1–11.
Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism
depends on stimulus complexity. Brain, 128, 2430–2441.
Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in
children with autism. Psychological Science, 14, 151–157.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.
Brock, J. (2012). Alternative Bayesian accounts of autistic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 573–574.
Champion, R. A., & Adams, W. J. (2007). Modification of the convexity prior but not the light-from-above prior in visual search
with shaded objects. Journal of Vision, 7(13), 10.1–10.10.
Cicchini, G., Arrighi, R., Cecchetti, L. M. G., & Burr, D. (2012). Optimal encoding of interval timing in expert percussionists. Journal
of Neuroscience, 3, 1056–1060.
Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2009). Sensory processing in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 13, 215–228.
Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., ... Ellipsis Davidson, R. J. (2005).
Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 519–526.
Davies, S., Bishop, D., Manstead, A. S., & Tantam, D. (1994). Face perception in children with autism and Asperger’s syndrome.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1033–1057.
Dickie, V. A., Baranek, G. T., Schultz, B., Watson, L. R., & McComish, C. S. (2009). Parent reports of sensory experiences of
preschool children with and without autism: A qualitative study. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 63, 172–181.
Elsabbagh, M., Volein, A., Holmboe, K., Tucker, L., Csibra, G., Baron-Cohen, S., ... Ellipsis Johnson, M. H. (2009). Visual orienting in
the early broader autism phenotype: Disengagement and facilitation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 637–642.
Fiser, J., Berkes, P., Orbán, G., & Lengyel, M. (2010). Statistically optimal perception and learning: From behavior to neural
representations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 119–130.
Franklin, A., Sowden, P., Notman, L., Gonzalez-Dixon, M., West, D., Alexander, I., ... Ellipsis White, A. (2010). Reduced chromatic
discrimination in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Science, 13, 188–200.
Friston, K. J., Lawson, R., & Frith, C. D. (2013). On hyperpriors and hypopriors: Comment on Pellicano and Burr. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 17, 1.
Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond ‘‘theory of mind”. Cognition, 50, 115–132.
Gerardin, P., de Montalembert, M., & Mamassian, P. (2007). Shape from shading: New perspectives from the Polo Mint stimulus.
Journal of Vision, 7(11), 13.1–13.11.
124
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
Gotham, K., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2009). Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 693–705.
Grandin, T. (2009). Visual abilities and sensory differences in a person with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 15–16.
Gregory, R. L. (1980). Perceptions as hypotheses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 290,
181–197.
Helmholtz, H. V. (1911). Concerning the perceptions in general. In Treatise on physiological optics. New York: Continuum.
(Original work published 1866).
Hus, V., & Lord, C. (2014). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Module 4: Revised algorithm and standardized severity
scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 1996–2012.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
Karaminis, T., Cicchini, G. M., Neil, L., Cappagli, G., Aagten-Murphy, D., Burr, D., & Pellicano, E. (2016). Central tendency effects in
time interval reproduction in autism. Scientific Reports, 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep28570.
Kemner, C., & van Engeland, H. (2006). ERPs and eye movements reflect atypical visual perception in pervasive developmental
disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 45–54.
Kerrigan, I. S., & Adams, W. J. (2013). Learning different light prior distributions for different contexts. Cognition, 127, 99–104.
Kersten, D., & Yuille, A. (2003). Bayesian models of object perception. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 150–158.
Kleffner, D. A., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1992). On the perception of shape from shading. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, 18–36.
Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. (2007). What’s new in PsychToolbox-3? Perception, 36, 1–16.
Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies
rather than biological motion. Nature, 459, 257–261.
Knill, D. C., Kersten, D., & Yuille, A. (1996). Introduction: A Bayesian formulation of visual perception. In D. C. Knill & W. Richards
(Eds.), Perception as Bayesian inference (pp. 1–21). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Langer, M. S., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2001). A prior for global convexity in local shape-from-shading. Perception, 30, 403–410.
Lawson, R. P., Aylward, J., White, S., & Rees, G. (2015). A striking reduction of simple loudness adaptation in autism. Scientific
Reports, 5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep16157.
Lawson, R. P., Rees, G., & Friston, K. J. (2014). An aberrant precision account of autism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. http://
dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00302.
Lee, T. S. (2003). Computations in the early visual cortex. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 97(2), 121–139.
Lee, T. S., Yang, C. F., Romero, R. D., & Mumford, D. (2002). Neural activity in early visual cortex reflects behavioral experience
and higher-order perceptual saliency. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 589–597.
Leekam, S. R., Nieto, C., Libby, S. J., Wing, L., & Gould, J. (2007). Describing the sensory abnormalities of children and adults with
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 894–910.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition
(ADOS-2). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Mamassian, P., & Goutcher, R. (2001). Prior knowledge on the illumination position. Cognition, 81, B1–B9.
Mamassian, P., Jentzsch, I., Bacon, B. A., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2003). Neural correlates of shape from shading. NeuroReport, 14,
971–975.
Mamassian, P., & Landy, M. S. (2001). Interaction of visual prior constraints. Vision Research, 41, 2653–2668.
Mazzilli, G., & Schofield, A. J. (2013). A cue-free methods to probe human shape from shading. Perception, 42, 932–940.
McCormick, C., Hepburn, S., Young, G. S., & Rogers, S. J. (2016). Sensory symptoms in children with autism spectrum disorder,
other developmental disorders, and typical development: A longitudinal study. Autism, 20, 572–579.
Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in
children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 255–263.
Morgenstern, Y., Murray, R. F., & Harris, L. R. (2011). The human visual system’s assumption that light comes from above is
weak. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 12551–12553.
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update and
eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 27–43.
Pell, P. J., Mareschal, I., Calder, A. J., von dem Hagen, E. A., Clifford, C. W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Ewbank, M. P. (2016). Intact priors for
gaze direction in adults with high-functioning autism spectrum conditions. Molecular Autism, 7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/
s13229-016-0085-9.
Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes ‘‘too real”: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 16, 504–510.
Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual
pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43, 1044–1053.
Pellicano, E., Jeffery, L., Burr, D., & Rhodes, G. (2007). Abnormal adaptive face-coding mechanisms in children with autism
spectrum disorder. Current Biology, 17, 1508–1512.
Plaisted, K. C. (2001). Reduced generalization in autism: An alternative to weak central coherence. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N.
Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 149–169). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Sciutti, A., Burr, D., Saracco, A., Sandini, G., & Gori, M. (2014). Development of context dependency in human space perception.
Experimental Brain Research, 232, 3965–3976.
Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., & Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders.
Vision Research, 49, 2705–2739.
Sinha, P., Kjelgaard, M. M., Gandhi, T. K., Tsourides, K., Cardinaux, A. L., Pantazis, D., & Ellipsis Held, R. M. (2014). Autism as a
disorder of prediction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 15220–15225.
Stone, J. V. (2011). Footprints sticking out of the sand: 2. Children’s Bayesian priors for shape and lighting direction. Perception,
40, 175–190.
Stone, J. V., Kerrigan, I. S., & Porrill, J. (2009). Where is the light? Bayesian perceptual priors for lighting direction. Proceedings of
the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276, 1797–1804.
A. Croydon et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 161 (2017) 113–125
125
Stone, J., & Pascalis, O. (2010). Footprints sticking out of the sand: 1. Children’s perception of naturalistic and embossed symbol
stimuli. Perception, 39, 1254–1260.
Sun, J., & Perona, P. (1996). Shading and stereo in early perception of shape and reflectance. Perception, 26, 519–530.
Thomas, R., Nardini, M., & Mareschal, D. (2010). Interactions between ‘‘light-from-above” and convexity priors in visual
development. Journal of Vision, 10(8). http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/10.8.6.
Tommerdahl, M., Tannan, V., Holden, J. K., & Baranek, G. T. (2008). Absence of stimulus-driven synchronization effects on
sensory perception in autism: Evidence for local underconnectivity? Behavioral and Brain Functions, 4. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1186/1744-9081-4-19.
Turi, M., Burr, D. C., Igliozzi, R., Aagten-Murphy, D., Muratori, F., & Pellicano, E. (2015). Children with autism spectrum disorder
show reduced adaptation to number. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112,
7868–7872.
Turi, M., Karaminis, T., Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2016). No rapid audiovisual recalibration in adults on the autism spectrum.
Scientific Reports, 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep21756.
van Boxtel, J. J., Dapretto, M., & Lu, H. (2016). Intact recognition, but attenuated adaptation, for biological motion in youth with
autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 9, 1103–1113.
van Boxtel, J. J., & Lu, H. (2013). A predictive coding perspective on autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. http://
dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00019.
Van de Cruys, S., Evers, K., Van der Hallen, R., Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., de Wit, L., & Wagemans, J. (2014). Precise minds in
uncertain worlds: Predictive coding in autism. Psychological Review, 121, 649–675.
Wagenmakers, E. J. (2007). A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14,
779–804.
Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence – Second Edition (WASI-II). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson.
Wetzels, R., Matzke, D., Lee, M. D., Rouder, J. N., Iverson, G. J., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2011). Statistical evidence in experimental
psychology: An empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 291–298.
Williams, D. (1994). Somebody somewhere. New York: Doubleday.