Author's personal copy
Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only.
Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.
This article was originally published in the International Encyclopedia of the Social
& Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy
is provided by Elsevier for the author’s benefit and for the benefit of the
author’s institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including
without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific
colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator.
All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including
without limitation commercial reprints, selling or
licensing copies or access, or posting on open
internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or
repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission
may be sought for such use through Elsevier’s
permissions site at:
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial
From Read, D., 2015. Kinship, Formal Models of. In: James D. Wright (editor-in-chief),
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Vol
13. Oxford: Elsevier. pp. 53–60.
ISBN: 9780080970868
Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. unless otherwise stated. All rights reserved.
Elsevier
Author's personal copy
Kinship, Formal Models of
Dwight Read, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Abstract
Kinship involves social interactions and forms of social organization based on systems of culturally constructed social
relations expressed linguistically through the kin terms constituting a kinship terminology. The organization and structure for
these social relations expressed in a kinship terminology is formally modeled here as being part of an axiomatic theory in
which a few, primary kinship concepts are the equivalent of axioms in a mathematical theory. The cultural knowledge
embedded in a kinship terminology therefore is part of a cultural theory – analogous to a mathematical theory – that
expresses the culturally salient kinship properties derived from the primary kinship concepts.
Introduction: The Domain of Kinship
Kinship involves social interactions and forms of social organization based on systems of culturally constructed social
relations expressed linguistically through the kin terms
constituting a kinship terminology. Formal modeling of
structural patterning in the social relations expressed through
kin terms began with Lewis Henry Morgan’s systematic study of
kinship terminologies in the mid-1800s. Since then, the analysis of kinship systems has been central to anthropological
theorizing, though some aspects such as what constitutes the
cultural criteria by which one person is the kin of another
person are still topics of considerable disagreement. Regardless,
all societies have a corpus of terms – a kinship terminology –
that expresses for culture bearers their cultural concepts
regarding kinship. A kin term referentially designates, in
a culturally meaningful and mutually understood manner, the
kin relation one person may have to another person, thereby
identifying those who are kin to speaker and to each other.
The British anthropologist Rodney Needham usefully
divided the domain of kinship into three aspects: (1)
behavior – what people actually do, (2) rules – what people say
are the rules or jural restrictions on behavior, and (3) categories
and relations among categories. These divisions have content
varying from less to more abstract and the third is the focus
here for the formal modeling of kinship systems.
Modeling of categories and relations among categories has
generally considered one of two related analytical frameworks:
(1) the structure and organization of a kinship terminology or
(2) the implication of prescriptive marriage rules for the
structural integration of social units such as families, lineages,
clans, or even whole societies. The first framework will be the
focus of this article. For this framework, it has generally been
assumed – though incorrectly – that kin terms are primarily
linguistic labels for categories of genealogical relations. While
kin terms have this function, it is secondary to the categories
being derived from the implications of the primary kinship
concepts embedded in a kinship terminology.
The organization and structure of the kinship relations
expressed through a kinship terminology will be formally
modeled by considering a few, primary kinship concepts to be
the equivalent of axioms in a mathematical theory. The cultural knowledge embedded in a kinship system therefore
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Volume 13
incorporates a cultural theory – analogous to a mathematical
theory – that expresses the culturally salient kinship properties
derived from these primary kinship concepts. These concepts
have roots both in (without being determined solely by or
limited to) the biological facts of reproduction and in culturally
defined marriage systems that define conditions for offspring to
be fully recognized as social members of a society or a social
stratum. Marriage rules – the cultural stipulation of the social
facts sufficient for assigning a complete social identity to the
offspring of a woman – structure the interconnections among
social groups either positively through prescriptive marriage
rules or negatively through incest taboos.
Primary Kinship Concepts
We begin by identifying culturally salient concepts of relatedness that are the axioms for cultural theories of kinship relations. These concepts derive from the universally recognized
statuses of motherhood, fatherhood, and spousehood, each having
different, culturally specific criteria for its implementation. In
its default mode, motherhood involves ‘mothering’ – the
positive caring, nurturing, and feeding behaviors that a female
directs toward those she recognizes as her offspring – but
‘mothering,’ hence motherhood, is not limited to behaviors
directed in this manner and can arise in conjunction with other
practices such as adoption, suckling, and/or coresidence,
among others.
Similarly, what constitutes fatherhood is culture-specific
and the status can, but need not always, be initiated through
cultural recognition of, and the meaning assigned to, a male’s
biological role in a woman becoming pregnant. In some societies, the biological role of semen in pregnancy is not given
importance in local theories of reproduction and pregnancy is
culturally attributed to other actions a man engages in that
relate to establishing the social identity of a newborn.
Motherhood (fatherhood) leads to the concept of a mother
relation (father relation) through associating mothering behavior
with the dyad of two interacting individuals that is the locus for
the behavior associated with motherhood. Cognizing a mother
relation in this sense is not just a human capacity but occurs
among the macaques (Dasser, 1988). The father relation in
human societies, however, does not have a counterpart among
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.43114-4
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 53–60
53
Author's personal copy
54
Kinship, Formal Models of
nonhuman primates since the status of fatherhood does not
exist for most nonhuman primate species.
We can define the idea of a mother relation (father relation)
through the ensemble of behaviors, BMo, associated with
motherhood (BFa, associated with fatherhood). We define
a mother relation, M, as follows.
Definition of Mother Relation M: For female person s and
person t, let sMt (read ‘s is the mother of t’) be true when t is
recognized as the target for s when engaging in the mothering
behaviors in BMo; that is, s takes on the motherhood status vis-avis t (see left side of Figure 1 in gray) by virtue of engaging in the
behaviors making up BMo with t, the recipient of those behaviors.
A father relation, F, may be defined in a similar manner using
the behaviors in BFa.
There are several boundary conditions included in the
cultural instantiation of these two relations: (1) if sMt or sFt is
true, then s s t, (2) for no persons s, t is it the case that sMt and
sFt are simultaneously true, and (3) for each person, u there is
presumed to be a person s and a person t such that sMu and tFu.
Another condition, corresponding to the assertion that a single
female has the primary responsibility for the care and wellbeing of a child (Schneider, 1961), is that for persons s, t, and u,
sMt and uMt are simultaneously true only when u ¼ s.
Since both of these statuses require that there be a person
who is the recipient of the behaviors, it follows that there is
a reciprocal daughter relation and son relation for the mother and
father relations definable from the perspective of the recipient
of the BMo and BFa behaviors. The daughter relation can be
defined as follows.
Definition of Daughter Relation D: For persons s and t, let sDt
(read ‘s is the daughter of t’) be true when s is female and either
tMs or tFs is true.
A son relation may be defined in a similar manner.
The M and F relations are conceptually independent since s
can be recognized as the mother of t without a person being
recognized as father of t, or vice versa. However, cultural
kinship systems are bilateral, meaning that the mother relation
and the father relation are both axiomatic for generating
kinship concepts. By itself, this would lead to conceptually
disconnected sets of kinship relations. Matrilateral relations
(those who are related to speaker through a sequence of one or
more relations beginning with the mother relation and then
followed by a parental or sibling relation when there is more
than one relation in the sequence) and patrilateral relations
(defined similarly except beginning with the father relation)
would lack a conceptual connection.
The cultural institution of marriage conceptually forms
a connection between the mother relation and the father
relation, thereby removing disconnectedness. Through
marriage, a spousehood status (see top part of Figure 1) is
defined using culture-specific behavioral criteria that determine a symmetric spouse relation in a manner analogous to
how the mother relation is determined from the motherhood
status. Associating the social identity of offspring with
Spousehood
Mother
Perso
Person
s
Marriage
act
Person
erson
t
Father
Fatherhood
Fat
Motherhood
hood
Person
n
s
Spouse
relation
Mothering
behaviors
Mother
relation
Father
relation
Person
u
Person
P
t
Fathering
behaviors
Person
u
Child
Mediation structure
Figure 1 Motherhood and fatherhood are statuses determined by behaviors directed to another person (dashed arrows). Spousehood is a status
determined by a marriage act (dashed arrow). Each is the basis for identifying when a relation holds between two persons. The three relations form
a mediation structure (El Guindi, Fadwa, Read, Dwight, 1979. Mathematics in structural theory. Current Anthropology 20, 761–790; Read, Dwight,
2010a. Mathematical representation of cultural constructs. In: Kronenfeld, D., Bernardo, G., De Munck, V.C., Fischer, M.D. (Eds.), A Companion to
Cognitive Anthropology. Wiley-Blackwell Publishing, Malden, MD) for the three categories (shown by boxes) labeled mother, father, and child with the
indicated gender attributes. The structural opposition between the mother category and the father category is expressed through the spouse relation
and each of the categories in opposition is linked to a mediating category (bottom box) through the mother and father relations, respectively. Persons
s, t, and u may be categorized as indicated by the dotted arrows, thereby conceptually making s mother of u and t father of u and, reciprocally, u is
child of s and u is child of t. The child category (lower box) has both male and female attributes as required for a mediating category. The spouse
categorization conceptually makes s spouse of t and t spouse of s. Generally the biological sex of the person being categorized agrees with the gender
of the category, though in some societies such as the LoDagaba of Ghana, Africa, a male person – a mother’s brother – is categorized as a kind of
mother and correspondingly is referred to by the term madeb whose literal meaning is ‘male mother’ (Goody, Jack, 1959. The mother’s brother and
the sister’s son in West Africa. The Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 89, 61–86). In several African societies
such as the Nandi of Kenya, female fatherhood arises through female husbands (Oboler, Regine S., 1980. Is the female husband a man? Woman/
woman marriage among the Nandi of Kenya. Ethnology 19, 69–88). Note: Terms, symbols, and arrows in gray refer to phenomena in the material
domain and those in black refer to concepts in the cultural domain.
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 53–60
Author's personal copy
Kinship, Formal Models of
marriage ensures that there is at least a presumed, if not
actual, spouse relation between a person who culturally
instantiates the mother relation, if any, and a person who
culturally instantiates the father relation, if any, for an
offspring to have a complete social identity.
The mother, father, and child categories and the spouse,
mother, and father relations are conceptually integrated
through a mediation structure (El Guindi and Read, 1979; Read,
2010a). A mediation structure consists of an open, mediating
category having a pair of attributes with opposite values that is
linked to a pair of closed categories, each of which has associated with it one of the pair of opposite-valued attributes
associated with the mediating category (see central part of
Figure 1 in black). The child category has the pair of opposite
attributes, male, female, and is open since it categorizes the
outcome of a biological birth. The mother category has the
attribute female, the father category has the attribute male, and
each is linked to the child category through the mother relation
and the father relation, respectively. They are closed as there are
culture-specific criteria for being culturally recognized as
a mother or a father. The mother and father categories are, then,
categories in opposition and conceptually one goes from the
mother category to the child category using the mother relation
and then from the child category to the father category using
the inverse of the father relation, and vice versa. This link is
culturally enabled through the institution of marriage and the
associated spouse relation defined by mother of child of father is
spouse is father of child of mother.
More succinctly, the spouse relation expresses the opposition between the mother and father categories by the computations, spouse of mother is father and spouse of father is mother.
These computations map the female gender of mother to the
male gender of father. The derived kinship system based on
mother, father, and spouse will be bilateral. Also, it follows that
a person can be categorized as father without reference to the
biological facts of reproduction, as occurs in groups such as the
Tiwi of Australia where father is the man currently married to
mother regardless of his biological relationship to her child. Yet
another implication is that the axiomatic foundation of kinship
can neither be reduced to biological nor to cultural properties
alone, as it necessarily incorporates both.
(a)
Family Structure
Kinship systems universally recognize a sibling relation derived
from the biological fact that multiple offspring can be produced
through reproduction. While each offspring may be categorized
as child in the mediating structure shown in Figure 1, siblinghood
status arises through behaviors between offspring framed by
having at least one person in common categorized as mother or
father. A sibling relation may be defined as follows.
Definition of Sibling Relation SB: For persons s and t, let sSBt
(read ‘s is the sibling of t’) be true when there is a u and a v such
that both of uMs and uMt and both of vFs and vFt are true. The
sibling relation expands the mediating structure (Figure 2(a))
into what we will call a family structure (see Figure 2(b)). It
should be noted that this structure neither defines nor determines the empirical form of actual families. Rather, it illustrates
the structural relationships among the axiomatic kinship relations that are integral to the concept of family.
Next we begin the construction of a kinship domain by first
constructing a Family Space derived from the family structure.
Then we add a Genealogical Space (GS) and a Kin Term Space
and finally we indicate the structural relationships among these
spaces.
Family Space
Kin terms identify a kinship relation between speaker and
target person, whereas the primary kinship relations introduced
above are from the perspective of an observer noting the
behavior between the two members of a dyad. Shifting from an
observer to an actor or agent perspective corresponds to
introducing the universal concept of self into the family
structure and changing, for example, the relation, ‘s is mother of
t,’ into, ‘s is my mother,’ with t categorized as self. Introducing
self transforms the Family Structure of relations into a Family
Space of positions and their connections to self.
Two Family Spaces are possible, depending on whether the
sibling relation is derived from the parent and child relations
(see Figure 3(a)), or is primary (see Figure 3(b)). Both
concepts regarding sibling relations are empirically found in
(b)
Mother
Spouse
relation
Father
Mother
Spouse
relation
Mother relation
Mother
relation
Father
Father relation
Father
relation
Child
Child
Sibling
relation
Mediating structure
55
Child
Family structure
Figure 2 (a) The mediating structure from Figure 1. (b) Modified mediating structure with a sibling relation. The structure incorporates all of the
elements for the concept of a family. The structure is that of a nuclear family, but actual families have empirical form depending on the cultural
instantiation of the categories in the family structure.
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 53–60
Author's personal copy
56
Kinship, Formal Models of
(a)
(b)
Mother
Sister
Mother
Father
Self
Brother
Sister
Self
Spouse
Daughter
Father
Brother
Spouse
Son
Daughter
Son
Family space
Figure 3 (a) and (b) Solid arrows (black or gray) are the primary relations. Spouse is in a third dimension and connected to self by the spouse
relation (double headed arrow). Daughter of spouse is daughter and son of spouse is son (dashed arrows). The mother and father positions are connected with the spouse relation (dotted, double-headed arrow). (a) Family Space based on constructing the sibling relations through parent: sister is
child of parent and brother is son of parent (dashed arrows). (b) Family Space when the sibling relation is a primary relation (double-headed arrows).
human societies (see Marshall, 1983 for ethnographic examples of axiomatic sibling relations).
Genealogical Space
The GS consists of genealogical paths constructed from the
relations in the Family Space through (1) each relation in the
Family Space is a genealogical path and (2) if P and Q are
genealogical paths, then the product PQ defined through
concatenation is a genealogical path. GS consists of all possible
products of the form P ¼ P1, P2, . Pn, 1 n < N, where each
Pi, 1 i n, is a relation in the Family Space.
For persons s and t, a genealogical claim connecting s to t
consists of a list of persons, beginning with s and ending with t,
such that all adjoining persons in the list satisfy one of the
relations in the Family Space. A genealogical path, P, may
(potentially) be instantiated recursively over a group G of
persons as a genealogical claim from person s to some person t
in G as follows. Instantiate self with s and find s1 in G (if any)
such that s P1 s1 is true. If n ¼ 1, set t ¼ s1 and the list, [s, t], is the
desired genealogical claim. Otherwise, recursively instantiate
self with s1 and find s2 in G (if any) such that s1 P2 s2 is true. If
n ¼ 2, set t ¼ s2 and [s, s1, t] is the desired genealogical claim.
Otherwise, continue recursively in this manner. If there is
a person si in G with si 1 Pi si true for all i, 1 i n, then the
genealogical path P determines the genealogical claim [s, s1,
s2, ., sn 1, t] connecting s and t ¼ sn. Conversely, each genealogical claim corresponds to a genealogical path.
Kin Term Space and Kin Term Maps
The size of the GS scales with kn, where k is the number of
relations used to form genealogical paths and n is the length of
a genealogical path, hence GS is cognitively complex even for
short genealogical paths. In all societies, this complexity is
reduced to categories of genealogical paths corresponding to
the 15–30 kin terms in a kinship terminology. Unlike folk
taxonomies, though, kin relations can be computed directly by
culture bearers from other kin terms using the knowledge they
have of their kinship terminology as a system of interconnected
terms. As reported in numerous ethnographic accounts, the
calculations have the form of a kin term product (see Figure 4)
defined as follows.
Definition of a Kin Term Product: If person s (properly) refers
to person t using the kin term L and person t (properly) refers to
person u using the kin term K, then the kin term M, if any, that
Person t
Kin term K
(Father)
Kin term L
(Mother or father)
Person s
Person u
Kin term M
(Grandfather)
Kin term product: K o L = M
(father o (mother or father) =
(father o mother) or (father o father) = grandfather)
Figure 4 Definition of the kin term product, K o L ¼ M, for the kin
term L used by person s to refer to person t and the kin term K used
by person t to refer to person u. The product is illustrated by the kin
terms in parentheses. The English kin term grandfather is determined by
the product of the kin term father with the kin term parent ¼ (mother,
father).
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 53–60
Author's personal copy
Kinship, Formal Models of
person s uses to (properly) refer to person u is the product of
the kin terms K and L: M ¼ K o L.
The kin term product makes it possible to generate the kin
terms in a kinship terminology starting with the kinship relations in the Family Space. The lexeme K is a (primary) kin term
corresponding to the Family Space relation R when there is
cultural agreement that person s may (properly) refer to person
t by K when tRs is true. For example, English speakers agree that
it is proper for s to refer to t as ‘my parent’ when either ‘t is my
mother’ (tMs) or ‘t is my father’ (tFs) is true for s, so parent
becomes a primary kin term for English speakers. A new kin
term may be constructed from a primary kin term K and an
already identified kin term L by letting the kin term product K o
L determine the relation person s has to person u when s refers
to person t by the term L and t refers to u by the term K (see
Figure 4). Naming this kin term product incorporates it as a kin
term in the kinship terminology. The same name may be used
for more than one product as part of the kinship concepts
embedded in the kinship terminology. For English speakers, for
example, grandfather is the name for the kin term product father
o mother and it is also the name for the kin term product father o
father (see Figure 4).
We visually display the structure of a kinship terminology
with a kin term map (see Figure 5) made by letting each kin term
be a node and drawing an arrow corresponding to a primary
kin term (with a different arrow form for each primary kin
term) from a kin term K to the kin term L when L is the product
of the primary term with the kin term K. The kin term maps for
the American/English kinship terminology and the terminology of the Shipibo Indians, a horticultural group in the
eastern part of Peru, are shown in Figure 5. Structural differences in their respective kinship terminologies are immediately
apparent. The Shipibo terminology has symmetry between
matrilateral and patrilateral kin reflected in the symmetry of the
women’s designs used to decorate pottery, clothing, faces, and
other parts of the body that relate to therapeutic aspects of
curing and bewitching shamanistic practices (Roe, 2004). The
57
American terminology has a contrast between lineal and
collateral kin reflected in the practice of a woman and her
children taking on the name of her husband even though lineal
descent groups are not culturally recognized.
Generating a Kinship Terminology
The procedure for generating new kin terms through the kin
term product suggests that kinship terminologies may share,
despite their structural differences (see Figure 5), a common
scheme for the generation of a kinship terminology as an
abstract algebra consisting of the ordered triplet, <T, o, S>,
where T is the set of kin terms including self, o is the kin term
product interpreted as a binary operation over T with self an
identity element (0 is included in T to ensure that o is closed
over T), and S is a set of ethnographically validated structural
equations giving the algebra its particular structure as displayed
in the kin term map. Analysis of a wide variety of kinship
terminologies leads to the following construction schema
expressed as a sequence of steps (see Leaf and Read (2012) for
a more detailed presentation; examples of generating a terminology can be found in Read (1984) (American terminology),
(2010b) (Dravidian terminology), (2012) (Kung San terminology), Read and Behrens (1990) (English, Shipibo, and
Trobriand terminologies), Bennardo and Read (2007) (Tongan
terminology); and Leaf and Read (2012) (Kariera and Punjabi
terminologies)).
Step 1: Generate an ascending structure of kin terms based on
either the generating set (1) A ¼ {self, P}, where P is an
ascending primary term or (2) A ¼ {self, P, G}, where G is
a primary sibling term, along with the structural equations
G o G ¼ G (‘sibling of sibling is sibling’) and P o G ¼ P
(‘parent of sibling is parent’) included in S. Inclusion of
a primary sibling term accounts for the defining structural
properties of the terminologies that Morgan distinguished
Figure 5 (a) Kin term map of the American/English kinship terminology based on parent, child, and spouse as the primary terms. (b) Kin term map
of the Shipibo Indians of eastern Peru based in ea (‘self’), papa (‘father’), tita (‘mother’), and bake (‘child’) as the primary kin terms. Affinal kin terms
have not been included.
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 53–60
Author's personal copy
58
Kinship, Formal Models of
as classificatory terminologies. For some terminologies, either
the structural equation An ¼ 0 or An ¼ An 1, n > 2, that
limits producing new kin terms in an ascending direction is
included in S, where An ¼ A o A o . o A (n times).
Step 2: Generate an isomorphic, descending structure of kin
terms based on either the generating set (1) D ¼ {self, C},
where C is a descending primary term or (2) D ¼ {self,
C, G*}, where G* is a primary sibling term distinct from G.
(The sibling terms G and G* have interpretation as ascending
sibling and descending sibling, respectively, though they are
usually referred to as older sibling and younger sibling.)
Include in S, the isomorphic version of any structural
equation included in Step 1.
Step 3: Form a single structure with generating set G ¼ A W D.
Include in S, the structural equation P o C ¼ self (‘parent of
child is self’ for consanguineal relations) that defines P and
C to be reciprocal kin terms. If G, G* are in G, then include
the structural equations G o G* ¼ self ¼ G* o G (‘ascending
sibling of descending sibling is self is descending sibling of
ascending sibling’) that define G and G* to be reciprocal kin
terms.
Step 4: Introduce ‘sex marking’ of kin terms either by (1)
adding sex marking elements m and f to S that are rightidentities in S or (2) making an isomorphic copy of the
structure generated in Step 3, but with a new term, self*,
replacing self in the isomorphic copy. The structure and its
isomorphic copy are interpreted as male-marked and
female-marked terms, respectively. These two structures are
joined to form a single structure of both male-marked and
female-marked kin terms. Different ways the structures may
be joined correspond to major structural differences among
the classificatory terminologies.
Step 5: Introduce affinal terms by either (1) adding a spouse
primary term, Sp, to S, along with appropriate structural
equation such as Sp o P ¼ P, Sp o Sp ¼ Sp, and so on or (2)
defining a kin term in S to also be an affinal term through
a marriage rule, such as the prescriptive marriage rule in
many Australian societies specifying that a man marries one
of the women he refers to by such-and-such a kin term (e.g.,
the kin term for a cross-cousin).
Step 6: Introduce terminology specific kin term rules that
locally modify the structure determined from Steps 1–5.
Step 7: Introduce any relevant culture-specific kin term
distinctions that arise from usage of the terminology, such
as the term with translation, ‘younger brother of mother,’ in
the Tongan terminology due to inheritance rules.
The terminologies generated in this manner using the
primary concepts of a kinship terminology have been found to
have structure isomorphic to the structure shown in a kin term
map for that terminology, hence the schema presented here can
be considered to determine a theory explanatory of the properties of kinship terminologies.
Relations among the Family Space, the GS,
and the Kin Term Space
The relations among the Family Space, the GS, and the Kin
Term Space are shown in Figure 6 and derived as follows. Both
the GS and the Kin Term Space are constructed from the relations in the Family Space, hence there is a natural mapping, h,
of the GS into the Kin Term Space defined as follows. Map each
path of length 1 to its corresponding primary kin term; for
example, for the English terminology, h: M / mother. For all
other paths, map the relations in the genealogical path to the
kin term product of the corresponding primary kin terms and
reduce this product to a kin term in the Kin Term Space. Thus
for the genealogical path [M, B, D], h:[M, B, D] / daughter o
brother o mother ¼ daughter o uncle ¼ cousin. (Note: products of
kin terms are written from right to left to allow a product such
as mother o father to be read as the kin term product ‘mother of
father,’ whereas a genealogical path is written from left to right
to allow a list such as [F, M] to be read as the claim, ‘ego’s
father’s mother.’)
The mapping is a homomorphism since for genealogical paths
P ¼ [P1, ., Pn] and Q ¼ [Q1, ., Qm], h:PQ ¼ h:[P1, ., Pn,
Q1, ., Qm] ¼ h:Qm o . o h:Q1 o h:Pn o . o h:P1 ¼ h:[Q1, .,
Qm] o h:[P1, ., Pn] ¼ h:Q o h:P. It is a surjective (onto) mapping
since any kin term, K, in the Kin Term Space may be written as
a product of primary kin terms, hence the genealogical path
determined by a sequence of relations constructed with each
relation in the sequence mapped by h to the corresponding
primary term in the product definition of K will be mapped to
K by h.
Predicted genealogical definitions of kin terms are given by
the inverse mapping, h 1, and the predictions match empirical
observations. The mapping, h, it should be noted, ensures that
computations with kin terms will be consistent with genealogical computations.
Formal Representation of Computations over the GS
Most symbolic systems devised for representing genealogical
paths are essentially notation systems. One of the more elaborated symbolic representations of this kind is the Grafik
symbol system (Atkins, 1974), but it does not represent
a computational system for doing computations with genealogical relations. The latter has been developed formally by
distinguishing between genealogical claims that conceptually
link one person to another through a chain of persons connected by parent and/or child relations and an abstract positional schema for expressing the genealogical path embedded
in a genealogical claim (Lehman and Witz, 1974). Culture
bearers do not just make genealogical claims; they also do
genealogical computations with the genealogical relations that
provide the connections among the adjacent persons in
a genealogical claim. Lehman and Witz define an abstract
Primary Genealogical Space (PGS) as the multiplicative semigroup generated from four filiation symbols Pm, Pf, mQ, fQ that
represent the relations ‘has male parent,’ ‘has female parent,’ ‘is
child of male person,’ and ‘is child of female person,’ respectively, and are derived from the upward lineation symbol P and
its inverse downward lineation symbol Q, all subject to the
structural equations (Px)(yQ) ¼ 0 ¼ (xQ)(Px), where x, y ¼ m
(male) or f (female) and x s y (1974: p. 119). (Their PGS is
essentially what is being called GS here.) The first equation says
that a male (female) parent cannot also be a female (male)
person having a child and the second eliminates genealogical
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 53–60
Author's personal copy
Kinship, Formal Models of
Genealogical space
Family space (a and b)
Made up of positions connected
to self (see Figure 3):
(1) the mother and father positions are determined by the
mother and father relations,
(2) the daughter and son positions are determined by the
reciprocals of the mother
and father relations,
(3) the spouse position is determined by the spouse relation; the spouse relation
links the mother and father
positions,
(4) (a) the sister and brother
positions are derived from
the product of the mother or
father relation with the
daughter or son relation,
or
(b) the sister and brother
positions are determined by
primary sister and brother
relations.
59
Consists of genealogical paths P = [P1, P2,
n ] and combinations of paths using the
concatenation operation, where each Pi, 1
i n, is a relation in the Family Space.
Homomorphic mapping h
h:P (= [P1, P2
K,
n])
where h:Pn
h:P2 o h:P1 is
reduced to the kin term K in the Kin
Term Space
T
Kin term categories
determined by h–1
Kin term space
Kin terms are constructed using kin term
products of the primary kinship terms that
identify the kinship concepts corresponding
to the positions in the Family Space. Prodtions that determine the structure of the Kin
Term Space in accordance with cultural kinship concepts.
Figure 6 Relationships among the Family Space, the Genealogical Space (GS), and Kin Term Spaces. The GS may be mapped homomorphically
onto a Kin Term Space, implying that the structure of the GS is carried over to a Kin Term Space. Genealogical definitions are determined by the
inverse of the homomorphism mapping from GS onto Kin Term Space.
claim loops that go from a person to a child and back to the
starting person. They show that from PGS a positional schema
can be defined that formally represents the process of genealogical reckoning.
Summary
The kinship domain has been formally represented here as
being composed of a Family Space of positions defined with
respect to self, a GS of genealogical paths defined as sequences
of relations from the Family Space, and a Kin Term Space
defined through kin term products of the primary kin terms
determined from the positions and relations in the Family
Space and subject to equations that structurally express kinship
concepts structuring a particular kinship terminology. The
formal representation establishes that the GS can be mapped
homomorphically onto a Kin Term Space, meaning that
calculations made with genealogical paths will be consistent
with computations in a Kin Term Space.
The formal representation relates to empirical observations
through the systematic elicitation of kin terms from culture
bearers (Leaf, 2006) conducted by asking about kin term
products of the primary kin terms with the primary kin terms
and then products with any elicited kin terms. The elicitation
establishes that a terminology is conceptually bounded since
any sequence of kin term products with a primary kin term
eventually terminates, either because the next product does not
yield a new kin term or it yields a previously elicited term
(or possibly a variant on a previously elicited term such as
great . great grandparent in English).
The homomorphism from the GS onto a Kin Term Space
and the algebraic structure of the Kin Term Space together
imply that the terminology provides a symbolic way, through
the kin term product, to compute kinship relations in a manner
consistent with genealogical reckoning. Thus the relationship
between calculating with genealogical paths and the symbolic
kin term computations with the kin term product is analogous
to the relationship between calculating with counting numbers
(where a counting number represents the cardinality of a set of
objects) and symbolic number computations using the addition operation with the natural numbers. In both situations,
a formal representation makes evident how new concepts are
generated from a few concepts taken as self-evident, namely the
counting number 1 and the idea of a successor number in the
case of arithmetic and the positions and relations of the Family
Space and the idea of the kin term product as a way to construct
a new kinship relation in the case of kinship systems. The
formal representation lays the foundation for exploring the
interplay of kinship with other domains whose cultural properties are expressed using kinship concepts.
Bibliography
Atkins, John R., 1974. Grafik: a multipurpose kinship metalanguage. In: Ballonoff, P.
(Ed.), Genealogical Mathematics. Mouton, Paris.
Bennardo, Giovanni, Read, Dwight, 2007. Cognition, algebra, and culture in the
Tongan kinship terminology. Journal of Cognition 7, 49–88.
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 53–60
Author's personal copy
60
Kinship, Formal Models of
Dasser, Verena, 1988. Mapping social concepts in monkeys. In: Byrne, R.W.,
Whiten, A. (Eds.), Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of
Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Oxford University Press, New York.
El Guindi, Fadwa, Read, Dwight, 1979. Mathematics in structural theory. Current
Anthropology 20, 761–790.
Goody, Jack, 1959. The mother’s brother and the sister’s son in West Africa. The
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 89,
61–86.
Leaf, Murray, 2006. Experimental analysis of kinship. Ethnology 45, 305–330.
Leaf, Murray, Read, Dwight, 2012. The Conceptual Foundation of Human Society and
Thought: Anthropology on a New Plane. Lexington Books, Lanham, MD.
Lehman, Frederick K., Witz, Klaus, 1974. Prolegomena to a formal theory of kinship.
In: Ballonoff, P. (Ed.), Genealogical Mathematics. Mouton, Paris.
Marshall, Mac (Ed.), 1983. Siblingship in Oceania. University Press of America,
Lanham, MD.
Oboler, Regine S., 1980. Is the female husband a man? Woman/woman marriage
among the Nandi of Kenya. Ethnology 19, 69–88.
Read, Dwight, 1984. An algebraic account of the American kinship terminology.
Current Anthropology 25, 417–449.
Read, Dwight, 2010a. Mathematical representation of cultural constructs. In:
Kronenfeld, D., Bernardo, G., De Munck, V.C., Fischer, M.D. (Eds.), A Companion
to Cognitive Anthropology. Wiley-Blackwell Publishing, Malden, MD.
Read, Dwight, 2010b. The generative logic of Dravidian language terminologies.
Mathematical Anthropology and Cultural Theory 3 (7).
Read, Dwight, 2012. How Culture Makes Us Human: Primate Evolution and the
Formation of Human Societies. Left Coast Press, Walnut Creek, CA.
Read, Dwight, Behrens, Clifford, 1990. KAES: an expert system for the algebraic analysis
of kinship terminologies. Journal of Quantitative Anthropology 2, 353–393.
Roe, Peter, 2004. At play in the fields of symmetry: design structure and shamanic
therapy in the Upper Amazon. In: Washburn, D.K., Crowe, D.W. (Eds.), Symmetry
Comes of Age: The Role of Pattern in Culture. University of Washington Press,
Seattle, WA.
Schneider, David M., 1961. Introduction: the distinctive features of matrilineal descent
groups. In: Schneider, D.M., Gough, K. (Eds.), Matrilineal Kinship. University of
California Press, Berkeley, CA.
Relevant Websites
http://www.ausanthrop.net/research/kinship/ – research, resources and documentation on the study of kinship.
http://kaes.anthrosciences.net/ – University of Kent, Centre for Social Anthropology
and Computing.
http://sourceforge.net/projects/kam/ – Kinship Algebra Modeller.
http://www.umanitoba.ca/faculties/arts/anthropology/kintitle.html – Kinship and Social
Organization: An Interactive Tutorial.
International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 53–60