Book Reviews — Comptes rendus de lecture
JOHN A. MILLS
Control: A History of Behavioral Psychology
New York: New York University Press, 1998,
246 pages (ISBN 0-8147-5611-5, US$37.50, Hardcover)
Reviewed by KURT DANZIGER
Another history of behaviourism? The title of John
Mills' book promises more than that, and it is a
promise richly fulfilled by the text. Behaviourism in the
narrow sense may now be a topic of essentially
historical interest, but behavioural science is quite
another matter. American (including Canadian)
psychologists, as Mills reminds us, "are trained to think
behavioristically from their earliest undergraduate
years, usually without being made aware, or realizing,
that this is the case" (p. 1). Within this academic
culture, behaviouristic assumptions and precepts have
acquired a self-evident status that goes with low
visibility.
If better vision is desired, Mills' book will supply the
necessary corrective. It does so by stepping outside the
present and examining the stations on the path followed by behaviourist thought and practice in its
conquest of the discipline. Although these broader
effects are a constant presence, the examination is
conducted with scrupulous attention to specific historical evidence. Mills not only engages die considerable
secondary literature in fruitful discussion but also
marshals important facts and insights gained from his
own archival research on the unpublished correspondence, diaries, and seminar notes of key figures.
Without such evidence, who would have suspected, for
example, that behind the apparent aridity of Hull's
neobehaviourist system there lurked a determination
to offer a homegrown American alternative to die alien
import of Gestalt psychology?
But if such revelations are to become more dian
historical curiosities they need to be seen in broader
perspective. And Mills' book provides the basis for dial
too. To continue with the example, the Americanism
of the behavioural approach emerges in several different contexts and at a number of levels. Not only were
there early and persisting links between this approach
and the politics of American Progressivism, but in its
ultimate values and commitments behavioural science
Canadian Psychology/Psychologic canadienne, 40:3
was deeply beholden to the pragmatic variants of
positivism current in North America rather than to the
logical positivism imported from Europe.
Such insights into die social and intellectual context
of behaviourism and its progeny are not new, but Mills
certainly offers the most comprehensive, accessible,
and elegantly argued account of the topic that is
currendy available. Moreover, he balances contextualist
history with detailed analyses of significant issues that
arose in the course of psychological research in the
behaviourist mode. Here you will find all you ever
wanted to know but were afraid to ask about such
topics as Hull's goal gradient hypodiesis or Skinnerian
autoclitics.
Mills' most important contribution, however, lies in
his masterly analysis of the presuppositions dial characterize die behavioural approach. In contrast to the
hasty polemics that have sometimes disfigured this
area, Mills presents what are clearly the fruits of a
lifetime of study devoted to the issues. Repeatedly, he
bends over backwards to make the strongest possible
case for diis or diat behaviouralist position and shows
that, nevertheless, it is both logically and empirically
indefensible. Ultimately, these positions represent the
product of culturally sanctioned acts of faith.
Though die specific theoretical and practical
contexts might have varied historically, Mills identifies
certain unshakable and interrelated features that have
always characterized behavioural psychology. First, a
technological imperative dictates that theorizing
should be guided and circumscribed by potential
practical applications. This goes hand in hand with an
antispeculative attitude that conveniendy conceals a
pragmatist version of positivism which constitutes
behavioural psychology's own florid philosophical
basis. It is also linked to a conception of science and its
practice ("prediction and control") that Mills regards
as mighty strange when measured against die way the
natural sciences have generally conducted their business. As for the specific content of its psychological
dieories, behavioural psychology has consistentiy based
that on an implicit and taken-for-granted theory of
human nature of the utilitarian variety.
Unfortunately, these tendencies do not represent a
closed chapter in the history of American psychology.
In the final pages of his book, Mills indicates that
reports of behaviourism's demise appear to have been
gready exaggerated. He is sceptical of the notion of a
more recent paradigm change and hints at some
remarkable affinities between what often passes for
Comptes rendus de lecture 273
cognitivism in psychology and the logical behaviourism
of the post-World War H period. At the same lime, his
book
contains
abundant
evidence
that
the
more
specific behaviourist, hypotheses have, on the whole,
had a remarkably brief shelf life. There is therefore a
distinction
ephemera
to be
and
made between these behaviourist
the
much
more
stable
ideological
commitments that have underpinned the enterprise of
behavioural psychology for almost a century. It is a
distinction
that
might have been
spelled out
explicitly in this book; however, in the
more
light of the
historical evidence presented, readers are not likely to
miss it. For anyone whose training or goals go beyond
a
technician's interest in psychological science
this
book will be an indispensable resource.
John Mills is Professor emeritus of Psychology at the University of Saskatchewan.
Kurt Danziger is Professor emeritus of Psychology at
York University. He is author of Constructing ike Subject
{Cambridge University Press, 1990) and Naming the Mind
(Sage Publications, 1997).