Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
www.elsevier.com/locate/tra
Driving by choice or necessity?
Susan Handy
a,*
, Lisa Weston
b,1
, Patricia L. Mokhtarian
c,2
a
c
Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
b
Community and Regional Planning Program, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, University of California Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
Received 3 March 2004; received in revised form 4 August 2004; accepted 7 September 2004
Abstract
From just about all accounts, Americans are driving more than ever, not just to work but to shopping, to
school, to soccer practice and band practice, to visit family and friends, and so on. Americans also seem to
be complaining more than ever about how much they drive—or, more accurately, how much everyone else
drives. However, the available evidence suggests that a notable share of their driving is by choice rather
than necessity. Although the distinction between choice and necessity is not always so clear, it is important
for policy makers. For necessary trips, planners can explore ways of reducing the need for or length of the
trip or ways of enhancing alternatives to driving. For travel by choice, the policy implications are much
trickier and touch on basic concepts of freedom of choice. This paper first develops a framework for exploring the boundary between choice and necessity based on a categorization of potential reasons for and
sources of ‘‘excess driving’’, and then uses in-depth one-on-one interviews guided by this framework to
characterize patterns of excess driving. This research contributes to a deeper understanding of travel behavior and provides a basis for developing policy proposals directed at reducing the growth in driving.
Ó 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
*
Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 530 752 5878; fax: +1 530 752 3350.
E-mail addresses: slhandy@ucdavis.edu (S. Handy), lisa.weston@mail.utexas.edu (L. Weston), plmokhtarian@
ucdavis.edu (P.L. Mokhtarian).
1
Tel.: +1 512 471 1922; fax: +1 512 471 0716.
2
Tel.: +1 530 752 7062; fax: +1 530 752 7872.
0965-8564/$ - see front matter Ó 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.tra.2004.09.002
184
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
1. Introduction
From just about all accounts, Americans are driving more than ever. Data from the Federal
Highway Administration suggests that kilometers of vehicle travel (VKT) on roads in the US
has been increasing at an average rate of 2.7% per person per year, from 8710 VKT per person
per year in 1970 to 15,686 in 2000 (Handy, 2002). They are driving more not just to work but
to shopping, to school, to soccer practice and band practice, to visit family and friends, and
so on. According to data from the Nationwide Household Travel Survey, trips for social and recreation purposes accounted for 13.4% of distance traveled by car in the US in 2001 and 13.7% of
car trips; visits to friends and relatives accounted for another 10.8% of distance traveled by car
and 6.7% of car trips. Americans also seem to be complaining more than ever about how much
they drive—or, more accurately, how much everyone else is driving: congestion regularly tops
the list of issues of greatest concern to residents of metropolitan areas in the US (e.g., Nguyen,
2003).
Yet the available evidence suggests that a notable share of their driving may be by choice rather
than necessity. Although many reasonable explanations for why Americans are forced to drive so
much have been put forth (e.g., the spread of the suburbs, the lack of good transit alternatives),
the categories of travel increasing the most are those that tend to be more flexible and optional.
Americans made 86.5 more trips for social and recreation purposes, 68.9 more trips for shopping,
85.9 more for family and personal business, and 17.5 more for visiting per year on average in 2001
than in 1990 (Liss et al., 2003). Other evidence suggests more directly that a significant amount of
driving takes place by choice. In an attitudinal survey in the UK, drivers reported that less than
50% of their annual driving was ‘‘essential,’’ with around 10% rated as ‘‘not at all important’’ and
another 18% as ‘‘not very important’’ (Jones and Sloman, 2003). Handy and Clifton (2001) found
that as much as 50% of driving associated with trips to the supermarket in Austin could be attributed to the choice to shop at stores other than the one closest to home.
The distinction between choice and necessity is not always clear, however. What if that more
distant supermarket offers some product, service, or quality that the closest supermarket does
not? What about driving the kids to school or to soccer practice? The distinction between driving
by choice and by necessity is further complicated by the fact that day-to-day decisions about travel are shaped by long-term decisions about residential location, job location, and activity participation. Each one of these decisions involves some degree of choice, although some individuals
have more choices than others, depending on their constraints of income, social ties, knowledge,
etc. But once these decisions are made, they create a certain necessary level of daily driving and
may considerably narrow the flexibility in trip frequency, destination, mode, and route.
The distinction between choice and necessity, though not always clear, is important for policy
makers. For necessary trips, planners can explore ways of reducing the need for or length of the
trip or ways of enhancing alternatives to driving, and everyone benefits if the planners are successful. For driving by choice, the policy implications are much trickier and touch on basic concepts
of freedom of choice: ‘‘What we need to do is make certain that weÕre able to get [energy] resources . . . into the hands of consumers so they can make the choices that they want to make as they
live their lives day to day,’’ proclaimed White House spokesman Ari Fleischer in May 2001 (White
House, 2001). An understanding of the boundary between driving by choice and driving by necessity can help to clarify these philosophical issues and define the policy alternatives.
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
185
The goal of the research summarized in this paper was to explore the choices that individuals
and households make about driving, in particular, the boundary between driving by choice and
driving by necessity, and through this exploration both contribute to a deeper understanding of
travel behavior and provide a basis for developing policy proposals directed at reducing the
growth in vehicular travel. The paper first proposes a conceptual framework for categorizing what
might be called ‘‘excess’’ driving by the reasons for excess driving and the source of excess driving.
The paper then summarizes results from a series of in-depth interviews conducted in May and
June 2003 in Austin, TX, that used this framework to characterize patterns of excess driving. Finally, the paper concludes with a discussion of potential policy implications and questions for further research.
2. Proposed framework
As noted, the distinction between driving by choice and driving by necessity is not entirely clear.
One way to clarify this distinction is to ask, in what ways are people driving more than they really
need to, thereby generating what might be called ‘‘excess’’ driving? As a starting point, excess driving is defined here as driving beyond that required for household maintenance given choices about
residential location, job location, and activity participation. The required level of driving can be
defined more specifically as the minimum number of trips using the shortest routes to the closest
destinations possible and using modes other than the car as often as possible. Excess driving is
then defined as driving above and beyond the required level and can be generated by the choice
of longer routes, farther destinations, greater use of the car, and more frequent trips than the minimum required. Of course, these minimum requirements can be difficult to define, particularly the
minimum requirements for destination and frequency. Each individual has her own set of minimum requirements, given her own needs and constraints, that is not readily observable. To complicate matters further, even seemingly necessary trips—the commute to work, a trip to the
grocery store—might involve some element of choice with respect to route, mode, destination,
or frequency that contributes to excess driving.
A series of reasons for the four sources of excess driving (more frequently, longer route, more by
car, farther destination) differ with respect to the degree of conscious choice involved (Table 1).
Driving purely for the sake of driving is clearly a choice, as are driving because of the value of
activities while driving and driving for the sake of variety. Excess driving because of habit or poor
planning does not result from a conscious choice as much as it does from a lack of conscious
thought. Misperceptions and lack of information, in contrast, are unconscious influences that
may lead to excess driving. Excess driving can thus be intentional (as is the case for driving for
the sake of driving, for the value of activities while driving, or for the sake of variety) or unintentional (as is the case for excess driving because of habit, poor planning, misperceptions, and lack of
information). Similarly, these potential reasons for excessive driving differ in their degree of apparent ‘‘rationality’’: from the standpoint of traditional assumptions in travel behavior theory, driving
for the sake of driving and other intentional excessive driving appear ‘‘irrational,’’ in that individuals are consciously choosing not to minimize their travel time (Goodwin and Hensher, 1978).
Within the conscious choice category are the value that drivers derive from the act of
driving itself and the value that drivers derive from the activities they can participate in while
186
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
Table 1
Typology of excess driving
Reason for excess driving
Intentional
Unintentional
Sources of excess driving
More frequently
Longer route
More
by car
Farther
destination
A2 (e.g.) to
spend more
time in the car
B2
A3
A4
Value of activities
while driving
Variety seeking
A1 (e.g.) a
Sunday drive
in the country
B1
B3
B4
C1
C2
C3
C4
Lack of
conscious
thought
Habit
Poor planning
D1
E1
D2
E2
D3
E3
D4
E4
Unconscious
influence
Misperceptions
Lack of information
F1
G1
F2
G2
F3
G3
F4
G4
Conscious choice
Value of driving
itself
driving—watching the scenery, listening to the radio, getting out of the house, clearing oneÕs head,
etc. Mokhtarian et al. (2001) found significant evidence for these motivations in a majority of their
sample, although participants in their study found it difficult to distinguish between the value of
travel itself and the value of the activities while traveling. The ‘‘positive utility’’ of driving might
lead to the choice of longer routes (cells A2 or B2 in Table 1) and farther destinations (A4 or B4)
than are necessary, either to extend the time spent driving or to enable more time for activities that
one enjoys while driving. The positive utility of driving might also generate more driving trips
than are necessary (A1 or B1), driving trips that are purely optional, such as a Sunday drive in
the country. As Mokhtarian and Salomon (2001) have articulated, for these kinds of trips, driving
itself is the purpose of the trip; Heinze (2000) calls such trips ‘‘original demand,’’ in contrast to
derived demand. Even for some trips that involve a destination, driving is the primary purpose
of the trip, and the destination is of secondary importance, nothing more than an excuse for
the drive. The positive utility of driving might also contribute to more use of the car than is necessary (A3 or B3). Given that the car is often faster than other modes, any positive utility to driving is likely to be frosting on the cake, so to speak, when choosing between modes.
A desire for variety also falls into the category of conscious choice. Ratner et al. (1999) have
found evidence that individuals sometimes switch away from their preferred option to less-preferred options purely for the sake of variety. With respect to travel behavior, variety-seeking
behavior may influence the choice of routes (C2), the choice of destinations (C4), and the frequency of trips (C1), leading to excess driving. As Mokhtarian and Salomon (2001) have noted,
it is difficult to distinguish between the positive utility of travel and variety seeking as motives,
that is, whether the motivation is a desire for more time in the car or for variety in scenery. It
can also be difficult to distinguish between pure variety seeking behavior and variable needs that
might lead an individual to choose different destinations on different occasions. In the latter case,
variable needs lead to variable levels of minimum required driving, which affects the level of driving that could be defined as excess. It is also possible that variety seeking leads to different mode
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
187
choices on occasion (C3); when driving is the usual mode, variety seeking would tend to reduce
rather than increase excess driving.
The remaining factors that can contribute to excess driving are not intentional: habit, poor
planning, misperceptions, and lack of information. With these factors, drivers do not consciously
choose to drive more than they need to, but they could make different choices that would reduce
their driving. The Travel BlendingÒ Program, described by Rose and Ampt (2001), focuses on the
potential for reducing driving by raising awareness and providing information. In a pilot study,
driving declined by about 10% after participants were made aware of alternatives to their current
patterns of driving. A review of other voluntary travel behavior change programs in UK found
evidence of similar declines in driving (Jones and Sloman, 2003). These programs address all four
of these sources of excess driving.
Excess driving due to habit perhaps involves a larger element of choice than excess driving due
to poor planning, misperceptions, and lack of information. Garling and Axhausen (2003) define
habitual behavior as that which involves no or little deliberation and no formation of intention—
it involves little in the way of conscious thinking; choices evolve into habitual behavior over time
and are only infrequently re-evaluated (Heggie, 1979). Individuals may regularly travel longer
routes (D2), visit more distant destinations (D4), and drive rather than use other modes (D3)
without thinking about their choices, even when they are aware of alternatives. Habit may also
contribute to more frequent trips than necessary for a particular activity (D1). For many people,
choices made by habit may come to seem a necessity, although in some cases, behavior that appears to be a matter of habit is actually forced on individuals by institutional constraints (Jones,
1978). The key to undoing habitual behavior is to get individuals to deliberate their choices once
again (Bamberg et al., 2003; Fujii and Kitamura, 2003; Garling and Axhausen, 2003; Goodwin
and Hensher, 1978; Heggie, 1979). The willingness to reconsider habitual behavior may depend
on a variety of characteristics of the individual, including gender (Matthies et al., 2002).
For excess driving due to poor planning, choice also plays a role, though a lesser one; a lack of
conscious thought is the main culprit. Poor planning for an activity such as grocery shopping may
lead to additional trips for a particular activity (E1). Poor planning with respect to trip chaining
may lead to more frequent trips (E1), more distant destinations (E4), or longer routes (E2). Because transit service has a fixed schedule, poor planning might also lead to more use of the car
than is necessary (E3). A factor closely related to that of poor planning is poor anticipation of
needs. If an individual accurately anticipates future needs for goods or activities, these needs
can be met as a part of an existing trip, thereby eliminating the need for a later trip.
The final two categories, misperceptions and lack of information, represent unconscious influences on excess driving. In neither case can the individual make different choices without some
help. Mistakes in perception are behind many of the discrepancies between actual behavior and
economic theory, particularly when individuals are choosing between unfamiliar alternatives
(McFadden, 2002). Misperceptions might include an individualÕs incorrect belief about what is
the shortest route (F2), the closest destination (F4), or the quickest mode (F3). In this case, the
individual believes he is making the choice that minimizes driving when he is not. Misperceptions
about the availability of activities at particular times and locations might also lead to extra trips
(F1).
Lack of information may have similar effects. In this case, the individual simply does not know
about other alternatives that would minimize driving. Again, he believes he is making the choices
188
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
about route (G2), destination (G4), mode (G3), and frequency (G1) that minimize driving. As
Heggie (1979) notes, individuals generally have limited information about alternative modes,
routes, and destinations. Efforts by public agencies to provide information about routes, destinations, and modes to the general public aim at these sources of excess driving. For example, parking information systems in European cities that direct drivers to the nearest available parking help
to reduce excess driving in congested areas. However, simply providing information is not always
enough. As noted by Sharps and Martin (2002), individuals often make decisions without making
use of important information, even when that information is readily available.
2.1. Gray areas
This framework leaves many significant ‘‘gray areas,’’ where it is hard to pin down exactly what
constitutes driving by choice versus driving by necessity. Two factors in particular that contribute
to levels of driving but were excluded from the definition of excess driving presented earlier may
merit further consideration: assessment of destination attractiveness and choice of activities.
According to travel behavior theory, individuals choose the option that provides them with the
greatest utility. For destination choices, researchers assume that utility is determined by the cost
of reaching the destination and the attractiveness of the destination. In our definition of excess
driving, the individualÕs assessment of the attractiveness of potential destinations was taken as
a given. However, some of the qualities that contribute to the attractiveness of potential destinations are clearly more important than others. Should all qualities contributing to the attractiveness of a destination qualify as contributing to the necessity of that choice and thus to the
necessity of travel? Or should the more expendable factors, the ones with higher cross-elasticities
with travel costs, be considered in defining excess travel? Kemperman et al. (2002) suggest that the
utility of a destination can decline over time simply as a result of repeated visits; if so, is the variety
offered by a more distant destination a necessary or unnecessary quality? Mayo et al. (1988), for
example, found that ‘‘the far-off destination has a special allure about it simply because it is far
off,’’ at least for vacation travel—should that count as a necessary quality? The difficulty is in
knowing where to draw the line.
In addition, excess driving was defined earlier as driving above and beyond the minimum required, given choices about residential location, job location, and activity participation. Residential location and job location are relatively inflexible in the short run and may be highly
constrained even in the long run and so seem appropriate to exclude from consideration in defining excess travel. However, choices about activity participation may be quite flexible, at least for
some activities on some occasions. It may be appropriate, then, to also consider activity choice to
some extent in defining excess driving. Again, the challenge is in deciding where to draw the line.
What appears to be a question of choice to an observer may be perceived as a matter of necessity
by the individual. Finding an objective way to make these distinctions may simply be impossible.
3. Methods
Although qualitative techniques do not yield statistically significant results, they are ideally suited for exploratory research such as this (Clifton and Handy, 2003). As a first step toward testing
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
189
and refining the proposed framework, we held three focus groups at the University of Texas at
Austin in May and June 2002. The purpose of the focus groups was to look for evidence of excess
travel, test alternative ways of asking about excess travel, and identify other issues or themes related to excess travel. The results of the focus groups provided a basis for the development of an
interview guide, which we used to conduct in-depth one-on-one interviews in Austin, TX in May,
June, and July 2003. In the interviews, we looked for further evidence of excess driving and used
the results to begin to assess which categories of excess driving are most pervasive and how excess
driving varies across the population. The following discussion is based on the results of the oneon-one interviews.
Participants were recruited through an email message sent to a random sample of university
employees, including faculty, staff, and student employees drawn from the published directory.
Interviews were held on campus (or at satellite facilities for the university) at a time and location
chosen by the participants, who were offered a $10 gift certificate at the university bookstore as an
incentive. Three research assistants were trained to conduct the interviews, which were audiotaped
and later transcribed. Using these transcripts, the research assistants coded the comments of the
participants according to the framework described above to identify examples for each of the cells
in Table 1. The coding was reviewed by two of the authors for consistency. A total of 43 interviews were completed over a period of eight weeks.
University employees are diverse, though not necessarily representative of the population of
Austin as a whole. Most obviously, all participants are employed, at least part-time; several
participants were also students (Table 2). The participants ranged in age from 23 to 67, with
Table 2
Characteristics of interview participants
Number
Percent
Gender
Male
Female
18
25
41.9
58.1
Household structure
One adult and no children
One adult and one or more children
Two adults and no children
Two adults and one or more children
Three or more adults
10
2
15
13
2
23.3
4.7
34.9
30.2
4.7
Employment status
Full-time
Part-time
36
7
83.7
16.3
Student status
Full-time
Part-time
Not a student
3
4
35
7.0
9.3
81.4
Mean
SD
43.9
27.8
11.0
17.7
Age
Length of commute (min)
190
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
Table 3
Interview questions
Reason for excess travel
Sample questionsa
Value of driving itself
Remember Star Trek, how Captain Kirk could beam himself instantly from place
to place? If you could beam yourself up for all your trips, would you do it?
Do you ever go for a drive just for the sake of driving?
If you could not [do the things you like to do while driving], would you still
enjoy driving? Why or why not?
Value of activities
while driving
What do you like to do while you drive?
Do you ever go for a drive to be able to [do this]? Can you give me an example?
Do you ever pick a route because it is better for [the things you like to
do while driving]? Can you give me an example?
Variety seeking
Do you ever choose a different driving route because youÕre bored with the old one?
Can you give me an example?
Do you ever choose to take the bus or walk or use some mode other than
driving just for a change? Can you give me an example?
Do you ever drive somewhere just for a change of scenery? Can you give me an example?
Do you ever pick a store or restaurant or other place farther from home
than your usual one just for a change of pace? Can you give me an example?
Habit
OK, let me ask about your drive to work. Do you have a usual route you take?
Is this the shortest possible route? Do you ever think about taking a different route?
Now let us talk about grocery shopping for one minute. Do you have a usual store
where you do most of your food shopping? Is there a closer store where
you could grocery shop?
Tell me more about your grocery shopping routine. Do you go on a particular
day every week?
Do you think you could make do with grocery shopping less frequently than this?
Poor planning
Do you ever have to run to the store because you forgot something
or ran out of something?
When was the last time this happened and can you tell me a little about it?
Do you ever make a trip to the store because you decide you need something
and cannot wait until the next trip? When was the last time this happened
and can you tell me a little about it?
Do you ever stop to do your shopping on the way home from work? Do you think you
could do more of this than you do?
Are there any other ways you think a little planning could
help you reduce how much you drive?
Misperceptions and
lack of information
Can you think of a time you went somewhere and found it closed
and had to go home? Can you describe what happened?
Can you think of a time you drove more than you needed to because you did not
know about a shorter route?. . . a closer store or other kinds of destination?
Can you describe what happened?
What do you know about the options for taking the bus to work?
a
The actual interview guide included additional follow-up questions to these.
an average age of 43.9 years, and 58.1% were women. The majority of participants live with at
least one other adult, though these households are about equally split between those that have
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
191
children and those that do not. Commute length ranged from 5 to 90 min, with an average length
of 27.8 min; many participants live outside of the City of Austin. One indication that the participants may not be representative of the population of the region as a whole is that 12 out of the 43
participants explicitly mentioned listening to National Public Radio (NPR) while driving, considerably higher than the overall market share for NPR of around 5% (Clemetson, 2004).
The interview guide consisted of three sections: In the first section, the interviewer asked the
participants to recount their trips on the previous day. For each trip, the interviewer then asked
about possible alternatives in terms of mode, destination, and route, and whether the trip was necessary. The purpose of this section was to assess the flexibility of travel choices and identify examples of excess driving. In the second section, the interviewers asked the participant a series of
questions designed to elicit examples of each type of excess driving, as outlined in Table 1. Examples of these questions are included in Table 3. Some types of excess driving are easier to directly
identify; for others, more indirect questions were used. Thus, the results may to some degree reflect differences in the effectiveness of our questions. The third section included three questions
intended to assess the participantsÕ own perspectives on the extent to which they drive by necessity
and by choice, as well as their general feelings about owning a car.
4. Results
The interviews offered interesting illustrations of the proposed framework but also demonstrated the complexity of identifying excess driving and distinguishing between driving by choice
and necessity. We first discuss findings from the first and second sections of the interview guide on
the different categories of excess driving (see Table 1). Following this discussion we review the results from the third section of the interview guide. Counts and statistics are provided as a way of
summarizing the results and identifying patterns only; they do not provide an accurate portrait of
excess driving for the population as a whole.
4.1. Evidence on types of excess driving
4.1.1. Value of driving itself
In the interviews, we looked for evidence of the value of driving itself as well as the impact of
this value, if any, on the amount of driving. The value of driving proved difficult to separate from
the value of activities while driving. When asked whether they ever went for a drive for the sake of
driving, many participants said they had (Table 4). But participants often then gave examples of
going for drives to see the wildflowers or other scenery, in other words, for the sake of activities
while driving rather than for the sake of driving itself. One way we approached this problem was
to ask participants if they would still enjoy driving if they could not do the things they enjoy doing
while driving, such as looking at the scenery or listening to the radio; few said that they would.
Thirty-three out of 43 participants said that they would ‘‘beam up’’ rather than drive if they could
(‘‘Hell, yes!’’ said one participant). But 10 participants said they would not want to ‘‘beam up’’ for
all of their trips and noted that they would miss traveling if they did; three of these participants
explicitly mentioned that they would give up their driving trips but not their biking and walking
trips, but seven others said that they would miss driving itself. A few comments suggest that at
192
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
least for some people, there really is something about driving itself that they value (Table 5, Comments 1–5). Many participants noted that while they now rarely go for a drive for the sake of driving, they more often did when they were younger, suggesting that the value of driving itself may
depend on lifecycle stage (Comments 6 and 7).
Table 4
Number of participants with examples of excess driving
Reason for excess driving
Sources of excess driving
More
frequently
Value of driving itself
Value of activities while driving
Variety seeking
Habit
Poor planning
Misperceptions
Lack of information
A1
B1
C1
D1
E1
F1
G1
14
15
14
14
30
21
4
Longer route
More by car
Farther
destination
A2
B2
C2
D2
E2
F2
G2
A3
B3
C3
D3
E3
F3
G3
A4
B4
C4
D4
E4
F4
G4
1
17
20
1
0
12
19
0
0
13
2
3
1
0
0
0
13
2
0
2
5
Table 5
Examples of comments
Reason for excess travel
Value of driving itself
Examples
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Value of activities
while driving
8
9
10
11
12
13
‘‘I enjoy driving. I love driving. I just enjoy it.’’
‘‘One does trips sometimes for the sake of traveling, rather than for the sake
of getting somewhere.’’
‘‘I could have flown [to Tulsa] but, you know, it was something that I used to do,
the drive, and I have been itching for a road trip, so I decided to drive.’’
‘‘You can experience the wind, the smells [in my MG convertible]. If you pass
flowers you will actually smell them . . .’’
‘‘It wasnÕt a very long drive, but it had a spirit of adventure—you know, pack
everything in the car and go, even though it doesnÕt take very long.’’
‘‘And I think when youÕre younger, too, you also like driving more.’’
‘‘If I was driving in a Corvette that might be a different thing, but I donÕt have
anything exciting that I am driving in and IÕm 37 years old . . . now being married,
responsibility with kids . . .’’
‘‘I wanted to hear the rest of this book that I was listening to. So, I got in the car
and drove to the store and bought something and came back. But it was
really an unnecessary trip.’’
‘‘I guess [I would miss] the scenery, not seeing all the new construction,
keeping up with the latest in the roadÕs changes [if I Ôbeamed upÕ for all my trips].’’
‘‘My wife . . . likes to look at the houses in the neighborhood so she will take more
of an indirect route, where I am very much more, what is the best route to get.’’
‘‘I usually watch the traffic.’’
‘‘When I was driving her home [before we got married], weÕd just take a long drive.
She didnÕt know Austin very well, so it was a pretty good trick.’’
‘‘I gave up the radio for Lent . . . I pray while driving . . . I mean itÕs not the
best way to pray, but, you know, itÕs an option.’’
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
193
Table 5 (continued)
Reason for excess travel
Variety seeking
Examples
14
15
16
17
Habit
18
19
20
21
22
23
Poor planning
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
‘‘I donÕt know, just sometimes I want to go some place different.’’
‘‘Now my husband is just the opposite—heÕll try to get out for any reason.’’
‘‘When I was younger . . . I had a lot of roommates, and so the only place
I actually had any real privacy was in my car.’’
‘‘I also think itÕs a good thing to vary your route occasionally, especially to
and from work, just to keep from getting in the rut of driving on autopilot . . .
something different, a break from the normal.’’
‘‘I just have a habit of buying a weekÕs worth of stuff.’’
‘‘I go everyday . . . I could [shop less than once per day] by eating canned peas
for the rest of my life, but IÕm not going to . . . My can opener is for cat food.’’
‘‘You know there is something shorter but you take the way you know
because it works.’’
‘‘I am sure [I chose driving yesterday] out of habit . . . [I used to have reasons to
drive] now itÕs just kind of out of habit.’’
‘‘I just love HEB. ItÕs just my store. I never think about going anywhere else.’’
‘‘I have a collection of restaurants that I go to and some are
further away than others.’’
‘‘Poor planning is the main thing that makes us have to drive, basically.’’
‘‘I could plan better to do more things in one trip rather than making a
trip and then making another trip and then making another trip.
It is probably because of not planning.’’
‘‘We plan pretty well because we donÕt like driving around.’’
‘‘I plan to try to be as efficient with the car [as possible].’’
‘‘. . . I was in the army several years in army transportation corps . . . it
probably has had some effect on me as far as driving around and planning
because that is what I did in the army.’’
‘‘If I need to go get groceries and maybe need to pick up something else from
another store, IÕll do it on one trip. Rather do that than a bunch
of separate trips.’’
‘‘IÕm actually pretty conscientious about planning my routes so IÕm making
a circuit or driving in a pattern that makes sense.’’
‘‘. . . I could be home and say, ÔOh, we need that,Õ then IÕll run back since
it is just a mile down the road.’’
‘‘I tend to, when I drive, I do multiple errands. When I lived in Austin,
I used to do what I call Ôspoke drivingÕ—just go to a store, come back,
just jump in the car and go do one thing and come back. Because
I donÕt live convenient to things now, when I drive I plan everything.’’
‘‘ItÕs pretty difficult—itÕs not like we can go to the corner . . . You try
not forgetting things . . . We donÕt have the luxury to say ÔIÕll run out and
do thisÕ or ÔIÕll run out and do thatÕ. We have to really plan what we do
and make the trip count.’’
‘‘I am writing my dissertation and I think, ÔIÕve got to have some Pepperidge
Farm cookies,Õ and it wonÕt go away till I go there.’’
‘‘IÕve lost about 30 pounds over the last year and half because we have
stopped [running to the store for ice cream] as much.’’
‘‘Maybe three weeks ago, the girls wanted some ice cream and I had to go
get them some. I didnÕt have to, but, you know, they give you that, ÔDad.Õ ’’
(continued on next page)
194
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
Table 5 (continued)
Reason for excess travel
Examples
37
38
39
40
Misperceptions
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
Lack of information
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
‘‘. . . my little girl said that she needed a tea party, and it became—became a big
deal . . . And so we just made a special trip to the store to buy her fancy tea.
I figured if happiness could be acquired at HEB for a dollar and
a quarter, why not?’’
‘‘IÕm good at telling the boys no.’’
‘‘Well, definitely if I did more planning in terms of taking the bus to work.
That would certainly reduce some driving.’’
‘‘. . . if I planned more the work drive, I could almost eliminate that.
So, if I plan more in the morning I could do that.’’
‘‘I am not sure that I took the shortest route. I perceive it to be
the shortest route.’’
‘‘. . . sometimes you think you know but you donÕt.’’
‘‘I get lost or distracted or whatever.’’
‘‘I think I wandered. . . So I am not very reasonable.
Why did I do that? I donÕt know.’’
‘‘I picked up the groceries but forgot to pick up the prescription.
I unloaded the groceries at home and didnÕt find it.
I had to go back to the store in Austin.’’
‘‘So they close on Mondays and weÕve forgotten that like three or four times.
So weÕve made that mistake before.’’
‘‘I didnÕt know on Sundays that they closed at 6 . . . I should have called
to see and make sure that they were open before I went.’’
‘‘I had heard a rumor that it had closed but someone else told me that
it hadnÕt. So I decided to believe the person that said that it hadnÕt.’’
‘‘. . . I am going to some new destination and I really donÕt know
how to get there very well.’’
‘‘Yeah, almost every drive I did when I first came to Austin
[I drove farther than I needed to].’’
‘‘I am the queen of not knowing where I am.’’
‘‘If I might have thought ahead and called and found out
what the bus routes were . . .’’
‘‘I donÕt know anything about the bus timing and how to get there.’’
‘‘IÕm trying this experiment; IÕm gonna ride the bus. IÕm gonna get
it down and see if I can do it.’’
‘‘I could have taken the bus, but it takes too long . . . I am not willing
to experiment with it . . .’’
Whatever the source of enjoyment, the level of enjoyment depended on several factors. First,
participants enjoyed driving more or less depending on the destination of the trip. In general, trips
associated with optional activities were more enjoyable than trips associated with going to work,
although the transition time between home and work was also important for some participants.
Another important factor influencing the enjoyment of driving is the conditions in which the drive
occurs, in particular, levels of traffic. This factor may partly explain the lack of enjoyment of driving to work. Rather than the increase in travel time that results from congestion, participants
seemed to be reacting to the fact of traffic itself and the frustrations associated with not being able
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
195
to move freely. Several participants complained about the behavior of other drivers on the road.
Not surprisingly, then, participants talked about the enjoyment of driving in the country rather
than the city. This enjoyment undoubtedly has to do with scenery (as discussed below) but is also
tied to traffic conditions.
The most obvious contribution of the value of driving itself to excess driving is on the generation of additional driving trips (A1 in Table 4), although these are infrequent. Only one participant pointed to the value of driving itself as leading to the choice of a longer route (A2). On the
other hand, some participants noted that they will sometimes take longer, more scenic routes in
order to enjoy driving more—the reverse direction of causality. We found no examples of the
value of driving itself leading to the choice to use the car over other possible modes. We also
found no examples of the choice of further destinations than necessary because of the value of
driving itself, although participants sometimes noted that they will stop somewhere when going
for a drive for the sake of driving. In this case, the destination is ancillary to the drive itself.
4.1.2. Value of activities while driving
Activities while driving seem to provide more value to participants than driving itself. The interviews produced examples of several different kinds of activities the participants enjoy while driving, and everyone seemed to enjoy something about driving on at least some occasions.
Participants most often mentioned watching the scenery and listening to the radio or to tapes
as things they enjoy doing while driving (Comments 8 and 9). Talking with friends and family
members was also frequently mentioned. Others mentioned the importance of having time to oneself or time to think. Several participants noted that they enjoy what might be called ‘‘checking
things out’’—seeing what is going on in the community, watching progress on construction projects, exploring a new neighbourhood (Comments 9 and 10). Only one participant confessed to
regularly talking on a cell phone while driving. More unusual activities included courting (Comment 12) and praying (Comment 13).
The contribution of these different activities to excess travel varies considerably, however.
Looking at the scenery, getting to know a community, and sight-seeing clearly generate additional
trips (B1) and often contribute to the choice of longer routes (B2). But other activities—checking
things out, talking, thinking—do not and instead seem to be more of a way of compensating
for the negatives of driving, of making the time spent driving more useful and enjoyable. The
compensation value seemed especially clear for listening to tapes or to the radio (Comment
8). Although deriving some positive utility from such activities while driving does not always
lead to excess driving, a little positive utility may be enough to lessen the motivation to reduce
driving.
4.1.3. Variety seeking
Participants had less trouble identifying excess driving associated with a desire for variety. Most
participants said that they will sometimes take a different route just for a change of pace, and
nearly half of participants said that they sometimes take a longer route (C2). A third of participants agreed that they sometimes take a trip just to get out of the house or for a change of scenery
(C1). These trips were sometimes motivated by curiosity but also sometimes by a desire to get
away from home or just to do something (Comments 14–16). One participant suggested that varying oneÕs route (C2) is important for getting out of a rut (Comment 17). The impact of a desire for
196
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
variety on mode choice (C3) is more complicated, however. Most commonly, participants who
usually drive said that they will sometimes choose to walk, leading to a decrease in driving and
suggesting that driving for these trips is not always necessary. A few participants who usually
walk, bike, or take transit said that they will sometimes decide to drive just for a change of pace,
in this case leading to an increase in driving. Determining the impact on excess travel of the choice
of farther destinations because of a desire for variety was also complicated. If the farther destination truly offers a quality that a closer destination does not, then the additional driving involved in
getting there would not fall into our definition of excess driving. However, several respondents
suggested that they sometimes chose more distant destinations, most often a restaurant, because
they felt like getting away or wanted to visit a particular area.
4.1.4. Habit
The interview questions designed to identify excess driving due to habit focused on two relatively habitual trips, the trip to work and the trip to the grocery store. Habit seemed to have
its clearest impact on trip frequency (D1). A number of participants said they grocery shop once
a week or more out of habit (Comment 18) and, when pressed, said they could make fewer trips
than they do, at least with better planning. Thus, excess driving due to habit was difficult to
separate from excess driving due to poor planning. On the other hand, two participants who
grocery shop almost every day offered justifications that suggest an element of need: a preference for fresh food in one case (Comment 19), and an enjoyment of being with people in the
other. Although driving is the regular mode for almost all participants, only two said they could
use an alternative mode but continue to drive out of habit (D3; Comment 21). Only two participants said that they use a more distant store out of habit when a closer one would do (D4;
Comment 22). Of course, it is possible that other participants are not consciously aware of their
habitual behavior, or rationalize it as being necessary or desirable rather than just an automatic
pattern.
The route to work is evidently a matter of habit for participants (D2), many of whom said they
had experimented with different routes before finding the one they liked best. Some participants
admitted that their habitual route might be somewhat longer in terms of distance than the alternatives, but it was either shorter in terms of time or involved fewer traffic lights or less traffic or
was otherwise more comfortable for them. Many participants make it a habit to sometimes use
alternative routes depending on the time of day or if traffic is particularly bad that day, but
the alternative route was shorter or not significantly longer than the usual route in most cases.
Thus, habitual routes did not seem to generate excess driving, although one participant suggested
the possibility (Comment 20).
4.1.5. Poor planning
Participants split about equally into those who confessed to driving more than they needed to
because they are poor planners (Comments 24–25) and those who claimed to have largely minimized the driving they do because they are good planners (Comments 26–30). These differences
seemed to be a matter of personality as much as of demographics, although parents often expressed a need to be efficient because of time pressures associated with family responsibilities.
Location also plays a role: participants who live farther away from stores make a point of planning their trips to minimize their driving (Comments 32 and 33).
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
197
When asked about grocery shopping, participants admitted to extra driving because of poor
planning in two ways: First, they did not always plan their shopping well enough to make it
through to their next regular shopping trip, leading to extra trips beyond their regular shopping
trips (E1) or to the lack of any kind of regular shopping schedule. Nearly three-quarters of participants could name such an occasion, generally within the last week or two. Many participants
confessed to extra trips to deal with an emergency, take care of unanticipated needs, or satisfy
cravings (Comments 34 and 35). A number of participants gave examples of trips to satisfy the
demands of their children (Comments 36 and 37), although one mother said she resists such pressure (Comment 38). Trips to deal with an emergency can be classified as necessary, but trips to
satisfy cravings or the demands of children are less clearly a matter of necessity. However, not
all participants chose to make an extra trip when faced with such a situation. In particular, participants who lived farther from a grocery store often said that they simply make do or do without. Others said that they will make an extra stop on the way home from somewhere else or go to
a nearby store rather than the usual store. Those who live very close to a store are more likely to
make extra trips (Comment 31), though in these cases the participant sometimes chooses to walk
rather than drive.
Second, participants often did not stop on the way home but instead went home first and then
back out, thus missing an opportunity to reduce driving through trip chaining (E1). Participants
were about equally split between those who make an effort to do all their shopping on the way
home from work or other locations, so as to eliminate the need to go back out again, and those
who choose to get home more quickly and deal with shopping later. Many participants said they
consciously plan their errands so as to maximize what they accomplish on one trip and so as to
minimize the amount of driving involved (Comments 27–30). Over a quarter of respondents
claimed to be such good planners that their shopping trips are as efficient as they can be. However,
only three participants said that with better planning, they could use a mode other than driving
for at least some of their trips (E3; Comments 39 and 40).
4.1.6. Misperceptions
Questions in this section focused on situations that participants could recall in which they drove
more than they needed to because of misperceptions. Because participants may not always be
aware of such situations, the results may underestimate the amount of excess driving generated.
Into this category, we put examples that reflected misperceptions, forgetfulness, and other mental
mistakes. In contrast to excess driving due to a lack of information, discussed below, these examples represent situations in which participants had the information necessary for driving less but
did not use it or in which they believed the wrong thing. As two participants acknowledged, what
they believe is not necessarily accurate (Comments 41 and 42). The most common examples were
trips to a destination that the participant believed would be open at that time but was closed, leading to a second trip at a later time (F1; Comment 46); in these cases, as a number of participants
noted, a phone call ahead of time would have eliminated the extra driving (Comment 47). Several
participants also mentioned examples of forgetting to pick something up or leaving something
behind, necessitating a return trip as well (Comment 45). Others described mental lapses that
led to longer trips than necessary (F2; Comments 43 and 44), or relying on the wrong person
for information (Comment 48). However, most participants claimed that such incidents are rare
or extremely rare.
198
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
4.1.7. Lack of information
Questions in this section focused on situations that participants could recall in which they drove
more than they needed to because of a lack of information. As was the case for misperceptions,
participants do not always know when they are driving more than they need to because of a lack
of full or accurate information; the results may thus underestimate the extent of excess driving.
Lack of information is difficult to separate from misperceptions; in this category, we included
examples where the participant had no information or the wrong information for a reason not
of their own doing. In one of the most common examples, participants took longer routes to
get to a new destination because they were unfamiliar with the area (Comments 49–51). In addition, many participants said that they simply do not know enough about bus service to consider
taking transit (Comments 52–53); it was not clear from the interviews whether bus service would
in fact be a viable option for these participants, however. Participants also suggested the importance of experiential knowledge with respect to riding the bus: Some are willing to experiment to
see if it works for them (Comment 54) and some are not (Comment 55).
4.2. Driving by choice or necessity?
Although we did not measure the amount of excess driving as a part of this study, we can use
the number of different types of excess driving for which participants provided evidence as an indicator of the extent of excess driving. On average, participants provided evidence of 5.2 different
types of excess driving. Every participant offered at least one example of at least one type of excess
driving, and some offered examples of as many as nine different types (Fig. 1). Another indicator is
whether participants had any flexibility in their trips on the previous day to drive less than they
did. When asked, 27 out of 43 participants said they had some flexibility to not make the trip, use
a shorter route, use a mode other than driving, or go to a closer destination for at least one of their
trips. Both indicators suggest that participants engaged in a measurable level of excess driving,
though how much in either absolute or proportional terms cannot be quantified based on the
interviews.
When asked directly about whether they drive more than they need to, 19 participants said
‘‘yes’’ and 24 participants said ‘‘no’’ (Table 6). Those saying no were slightly older and had longer
commutes on average than those saying yes, perhaps reflecting an association between efforts to
8
Number of Participants
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
Number of Types of Excess Drivingu
Fig. 1. Frequency distribution for number of types of Excess Driving.
199
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
Table 6
Want and need comparisons
Number
Percent female
Age
Length of commute (min)
Average number of types of excess drivinga
Percent of participants who make trips for cravings
Percent of participants who say owning car is freedom
a
More than you want
More than you need
Yes
No
Yes
No
34
61.8
43.2
28.7
4.8
21
59
9
44.4
46.3
24.4
6.8
33
89
19
57.9
40.3
23.9
6.1
37
58
24
58.3
46.8
31.0
4.5
13
71
Between group difference is statistically significant.
minimize driving and both age and residential location. Those saying yes provided evidence of 6.1
different types of excess driving on average and 37% admitted to making extra trips to satisfy cravings. Those saying no provided evidence of 4.5 different types, a statistically significant difference
from those saying yes, and only 13% admitted to making extra trips to satisfy cravings. It is interesting that even those not driving more than they need to provided some evidence of excess driving
according to our definitions. This inconsistency suggests either some discrepancy between our definitions and theirs or an ability on the part of participants to discount in their own minds the extent of their excess driving. As a result, a conservative assessment of excess driving would focus on
the difference between those not driving more than they need to and those that do, rather than on
absolute levels.
When asked whether they drive more than they want to, 34 participants said ‘‘yes’’ and only
nine said ‘‘no’’ (Table 6). A much higher share of those saying yes were female compared to those
saying no—61.2% versus 44.4%, respectively; average age and average commute distance did not
vary significantly. This result points to the possibility that women have a lower preference for
driving overall. Those saying they drive more than they want provided evidence of 4.8 different
types of excess driving on average, compared to 6.8 different types for those saying they do not
drive more than they want. Similarly, only 21% of those driving more than they want confessed
to extra trips to satisfy cravings, compared to 33% for those not driving more than they want.
These seemingly contradictory results, where those who are happy with the amount of driving
they do drive more than those who are not, also point to fundamental differences in preferences
towards driving: those who do not drive more than they want to must have a much higher tolerance for driving than those who do drive more than they want.
We also asked participants which statement better characterizes how they feel about their cars:
‘‘Owning a car gives me freedom,’’ or ‘‘Owning a car is a significant burden.’’ Nearly two-thirds of
respondents agreed with the first statement, while nearly a third said that both statements are true
for them; only two participants said that owning a car is solely a burden for them. Participants
mentioned the ability to go where they want when they want to as providing an important sense
of freedom. ‘‘Driving is a necessity to make my life happen,’’ said one participant. ‘‘I feel like I get
freedom because if I did not have a car, my social life on the weekends would be very curtailed,’’
said another. The feeling of freedom was clearly tied to driving by choice, while the feeling of burden was tied to driving by necessity. As one participant put it, ‘‘Freedom is when itÕs a choice . . .
200
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
Table 7
Driving more than you want versus driving more than you need
Driving more than you need to
Driving more than you want to
Yes
No
Total
Yes
No
14
5
32.6%
11.6%
20
4
46.5%
9.3%
34
9
79.1%
20.9%
Total
19
44.2%
24
55.8%
43
100.0%
The burden is when you have to commute to work.’’ This sentiment was implicit in the comment
of another participant, ‘‘I hate errand driving. I like open road drives.’’ For the participants, the
freedom associated with owning a car outweighs the burdens: ‘‘It can be a burden, too, but it is a
gladly accepted burden.’’
Interestingly, participants who said that they do not drive more than they need to were more
likely to say that owning a car gives them freedom but is not a burden, compared to participants
who said they do drive more than they need to (Table 6). In other words, those driving more than
they need to also feel a greater burden of owning a car. This result is somewhat surprising, in that
those driving more than they need to may do so at least partly out of choice. It makes more sense
if the extra driving increases the burden of owning a car because of costs associated with additional wear-and-tear. Participants not driving more than they want to were more likely to say that
owning a car gives them freedom but is not a burden, compared to participants driving more than
they want to. In other words, those driving more than they want to also feel a greater burden of
owning a car. This result is not surprising.
Cross-tabulating whether participants drive more than they need to and whether they drive
more than they want to yields interesting results (Table 7). Nearly a third of participants fall into
the category of driving more than they need to but also driving more than they want to. These
participants are apparently driving by choice to some degree but they are not happy about the
driving they do out of necessity: they prefer to reduce certain kinds of driving. Close to half of
participants fall into the category of not driving more than they need to but still driving more than
they want to. These participants do not drive by choice and they would also like to reduce the
driving they do out of necessity: they prefer to reduce all kinds of driving. Only nine participants
said that they do not drive more than they want to, and these split almost equally between those
that said they drive more than they need to and those that do not. The former group is driving to
some degree by choice, while the latter group is not, but both groups are satisfied with the situation. The differences between these four groups point to different policy approaches.
5. Conclusions
Although these findings are exploratory, they begin to suggest the need for a combination of
policies to address both individual- and community-level concerns (Table 8). For those not driving more than they need to but driving more than they want to, policies must work to further reduce the need for driving. Policies that provide alternatives to driving or that reduce the length of
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
201
Table 8
Policy implications
Driving more than you need to
Driving more than you want to
Yes
No
Yes
Alternatives to driving, voluntary travel
behavior change programs, and Intelligent
Transportation Systems
Alternatives to driving, including
transit, bike/ped, land use policies
No
Pricing policies or additional road capacity
Alternatives to driving, and
pricing policies
driving trips would help. Such policies might include improved transit services and improved bicycle and pedestrian infrastructure. Land use policies that bring activities closer together could increase the viability of walking and biking and also reduce necessary driving distances. These
policies would also benefit those driving more than they need to and more than they want to,
as would approaches such as voluntary travel behavior change programs, which help households
one-on-one to reduce their driving, and certain applications of Intelligent Transportation Systems
(ITS), particularly Advanced Traveler Information Systems (ATIS).
For those not driving more than they need to or than they want to, issues arise at the community level though not at the individual level. These individuals have little internal motivation to
reduce driving, yet the driving that they do out of necessity has impacts on the community. Policies that provide alternatives to driving or that reduce necessary driving distances might lead to
less driving for these individuals and thus benefits for the community, assuming that individuals
do not make up for a reduction in necessary driving with an increase in driving by choice. Pricing
policies that increase the cost of driving would also encourage these individuals to take advantage
of these alternatives and drive less if they can. Finally, for those driving more than they need to
but not more than they want to, two different policy approaches are possible. If the larger goal is
to reduce environmental impacts and manage congestion, then pricing policies could help to discourage driving by choice. If the larger goal is to accommodate the desires of individuals to drive
more than they need to, then additional road capacity may be appropriate. The fact that few of
the participants in this study fall into this category supports a move towards less traditional transportation policies to address both individual- and community-level concerns.
Proposed transportation projects are often evaluated in terms of the travel-time savings they
will yield and the estimated value of those savings to travelers. Although this study did not directly address the concept of the value of travel-time savings, it provides useful insights into
the challenges of measuring it. In particular, the results show that not all travel-time savings
would be equally valued by drivers. Most obviously, drivers would value a reduction in time spent
on necessary driving more than time spent driving by choice. On the other hand, drivers mostly
dislike driving in congested traffic, so that any reduction in time spent driving in heavy traffic
would be valued more than an equal savings in time spent driving in uncongested conditions
by both those driving by choice and those driving by necessity. An approach to measuring the
value of travel-time savings that is sensitive to such differences might influence the decision-making process for transportation projects in significant ways.
202
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
Besides providing evidence of excess driving, this initial exploration of the question of driving
by choice versus necessity suggests the need for further research to categorize the potential sources
of excess driving, develop effective techniques for identifying excess driving, and quantify both the
amount of excess driving and the contribution of various explanatory factors. The results from the
in-depth interviews can serve as the basis for the development of a survey instrument to address
these issues quantitatively. Future efforts should focus on ways of distinguishing between the value of driving itself and the value of activities while driving and on ways of searching for excess
driving caused by habit, misperceptions, and lack of information. The gray areas associated with
destination choice and activity choice merit further consideration, as do the even grayer areas
associated with residential location choice and job choice. In addition, an exploration of the decision-making processes underlying excess driving might yield important new insights into travel
behavior. A better understanding of the magnitude of excess driving and its sources will help
in the formulation of policies designed to slow the growth in vehicular travel.
Acknowledgment
This research was supported by a grant from the Southwest Region University Transportation
Center, which is funded 50% with general revenue funds from the State of Texas.
References
Bamberg, S., Rolle, D., Weber, C., 2003. Does habitual car use not lead to more resistance to change of travel mode?
Transportation 30, 97–108.
Clemetson, L., 2004. NPRÕs Growing Clout Alarms Member Stations. New York Times, New York.
Clifton, K.J., Handy, S.L., 2003. Qualitative methods in travel behaviour research. In: Jones, D., Stopher, P. (Eds.),
Transport Survey Quality and Innovation. Elsevier, Oxford.
Fujii, S., Kitamura, R., 2003. What does a one-month free bus ticket do to habitual drivers? Transportation 30, 81–95.
Garling, T., Axhausen, K.W., 2003. Introduction: habitual travel choice. Transportation 30, 1–11.
Goodwin, P.B., Hensher, D.A., 1978. The transport determinant of travel choices: an overview. In: Hensher, D.A.,
Dalvi, Q. (Eds.), Determinants of Travel Choice. Praeger, New York.
Handy, S.L., 2002. Accessibility- vs. mobility-enhancing strategies for addressing automobile dependence in the US.
European Conference of Ministers of Transport, Paris.
Handy, S.L., Clifton, K.J., 2001. Local shopping as a strategy for reducing automobile travel. Transportation 28 (4),
317–346.
Heggie, I.G., 1979. Behavioural dimensions of travel choice. In: Hensher, D.A., Dalvi, Q. (Eds.), Determinants of
Travel Choice. Praeger, New York.
Heinze, G.W., 2000. Transport and leisure: growth as opportunity. In: ECMT Round Table 111, Paris, pp. 5–51.
Jones, P.M., 1978. Destination choice and travel attributes. In: Hensher, D.A., Dalvi, Q. (Eds.), Determinants of Travel
Choice. Praeger Publishers, New York, pp. 266–311.
Jones, P.M., Sloman, L., 2003. Encouraging behavioural change through marketing and management: what can be
achieved? Paper presented at the 10th International Conference on Travel Behaviour Research, Lucerne,
Switzerland.
Kemperman, A., Borgers, A., Timmermans, H., 2002. Incorporating variety seeking and seasonality in stated
preference modeling of leisure trip destination choice. Transportation Research Record 1807, 67–76.
Liss, S., McGuckin, N., Gross, B., 2003. Changes in the Purpose of Travel over Time. Federal Highway
Administration.
S. Handy et al. / Transportation Research Part A 39 (2005) 183–203
203
Matthies, E., Kuhn, S., Klockner, C.A., 2002. Travel mode choice of women: the result of limitation, ecological norm,
or weak habit? Environment and Behavior 34 (2), 163–177.
Mayo, E.J., Jarvis, L.P., Xander, J.A., 1988. Beyond the gravity model. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science
16 (3&4), 23–29.
McFadden, D.L., 2002. The path to discrete-choice models. Access 20, 2–7.
Mokhtarian, P.L., Salomon, I., 2001. How derived is the demand for travel? Some conceptual and measurement
considerations. Transportation Research A 35, 695–719.
Mokhtarian, P.L., Salomon, I., Redmond, L.S., 2001. Understanding the demand for travel: itÕs not purely ÔderivedÕ.
Innovation 14 (4), 355–380.
Nguyen, T., 2003. Orange County Residents Name Traffic Congestion No. 1 Concern in New Survey. Orange County
Transportation Authority.
Ratner, R.K., Kahn, B.E., Kahneman, D., 1999. Choosing less-preferred experiences for the sake of variety. Journal of
Consumer Research 26 (1), 1–15.
Rose, G., Ampt, L., 2001. Travel blending: an Australian travel awareness initiative. Transportation Research D 6,
95–110.
Sharps, M.J., Martin, S.S., 2002. ÔMindlessÕ decision making as a failure of contextual reasoning. The Journal of
Psychology 136 (3), 272–282.
White House, 2001. May 7th Press Briefing by Ari Fleischer.