DOCUMENT RESUME
ED 356 141
SE 052 979
AUTHOR
TITLE
Cavallo, Ann Liberatore
The Retention of Meaningful Understanding of Meiosis
and Genetics.
PUB DATE
NOTE
92
PUB TYPE
EDRS PRICE
DESCRIPTORS
IDENTIFIERS
31p.; Paper presented at a poster session at the
Annual Conference of the National Association for
Research in Science Teaching (Boston, MA, March 22,
1992). For related documents, see SE 052 977-978.
Reports
Research/Technical (143)
Speeches /Conference Papers (150)
MF01/PCO2 Plus Postage.
Academic Achievement; *Biology; *Cognitive Style;
*Concept Formation; Educational Research; Genetics;
Grade 10; High Schools; High School Students;
Learning Motivation; *Retention (Psychology); Rote
Learning; *Science Education; Science Instruction;
Scientific Concepts; Secondary School Science;
Standardized Tests
"Meaningful Learning; Meiosis; *Mental Models; New
York; Punnett Square Method
ABSTRACT
This study investigated the retention of meaningful
understanding of the biological topics of meiosis, the Punnett square
method and the relations between these two topics. This study also
explored the predictive influence of students' general tendency to
learn meaningfully or by rote (meaningful learning orientation),
prior knowledge of meiosis, instructional treatment (students told
relations between concepts, students asked to construct relations
themselves) and all interactions of these variables on the retention
of meaningful understanding of the topics. A 50-item Likert
instrument taken by the students and teacher ratings of their
students' approach to learning (meaningful, rote) were used in
combination as a measure of students' meaningful learning
orientation. The mental model technique was used to assess students'
meaningful-level and rote-level understandings of the topics.
Students were given a pre-test mental model on meiosis (prior
knowledge variable) and were randomly assigned to the two
instructional treatments. Results of correlations indicated that
students' attainment of meaningful understanding as measured
immediately after instruction was significantly and positively
related with their retention of meaningful understanding. Stepwise
multiple regressions revealed that students' retention of meaningful
understanding of meiosis was predicted by student's meaningful
learning orientation and prior knowledge of meiosis. The interactions
of meiosis and meaningful learning orientation best predicted both
student' retention of meiosis and the conceptual relation between the
topics. None of the predictor variables (prior knowledge, meaningful
learning orientation, and treatment) nor the interaction terms
predicted students' retention of meaningful understanding of the
Punnett square method. (Author/PR)
THE RETENTION OF MEANINGFUL UNDERSTANDING
OF MEIOSIS AND GENETICS
ANN LIBERATORE CAVALLO
SCIENCE EDUCATION CENTER
THE UNIVERSITY OF OKLAHOMA
NORMAN, OKLAHOMA 73019
U.S DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION
Office of Educational Research and Improvement
PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS
MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BY
EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION
CENTER IERICI
:XTnis document has been reproduced as
received Iron, the person or organization
originating it
Minor changes have been made to improve
reproduction duality
Points of view or opinions slated in this dOcu
merit do not necessarily represent official
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)."
OF RI position or Policy
Paper presented at a poster session at the Annual Conference of the National
Association for Research in Science Teaching, Boston, MA, March 22, 1992.
(
THE RETENTION OF MEANINGFUL UNDERSTANDING OF MEIOSIS AND GENETICS
Ann L. Cavallo
Assistant Professor
Science Education Center
The University of Oklahoma
Norman, Oklahoma 73019
ABSTRACT
This study investigated the retention of meaningful understanding of the
biological topics of meiosis, the Punnett square method and the relations between
these two topics. This study also explored the predictive influence of students' general
tendency to learn meaningfully or by rote (meaningful learning orientation), prior
knowledge of meiosis, instructional treatment (students told relations between
concepts, students asked to construct relations themselves) and all interactions of
these variables on the retention of meaningful understanding of the topics. A 50-item
Likert instrument taken by the students and teacher ratings of their students' approach
to learning (meaningful, rote) were used in combination as a measure of students'
meaningful learning orientation. The mental model technique (Cavallo, 1991;
Mosenthal & Kirsch, 1991) was used to assess students' meaningful-level and rote-
level understandings of the topics. Students were given a pre-test mental model on
meiosis (prior knowledge variable) and were randomly assigned to the two
instructional treatments. Immediately following the instructional treatments, students
were given a mental model test on meiosis, the Punnett square method and on the
procedural and conceptual relations between these two topics. After four weeks, the
students were again given the mental model tests. Results of correlations indicated
that students' attainment of meaningful understanding as measured immediately after
instruction was significantly and positively related with their retention of meaningful
understanding. Stepwise multiple regressions revealed that students' retention of
meaningful understanding of meiosis was predicted by students' meaningful learning
orientation and prior knowledge of meiosis. Students' meaningful retention of the
conceptual and procedural relations between the two topics was best predicted only
by their prior knowledge of meiosis. The interaction of meiosis and meaningful
learning orientation best predicted both students' retention of meiosis and the
conceptual relation between the topics. None of the predictor variables (prior
knowledge, meaningful learning orientation, treatment) nor the interaction terms
predicted students' retention of meaningful understanding of the Punnett square
method.
4
The Retention of Meaningful Understanding of Meiosis and Genetics
INTRODUCTION
A valued outcome of student learning in science is the formulation of sound
conceptual knowledge about the world and the way it works. To accomplish this goal,
learning science should involve the construction of knowledge and allow students to
create new ideas from what is already known. Learning should not only be concerned
with knowing information about science, but should involve the formulation of relations
among the conceptual and theoretical knowledge of science (Driver & Erickson, 1983).
Ausubel (1963, 1968) describes the formulation of relationships between theoretical
concepts in making sense of the world as "meaningful learning."
Meaningful learning is a constructive process in which the learner strives to
build an understanding of the information and observations which make up the "body
of knowledge" of science (Pines & West, 1986). In meaningful learning, new concepts
are most often "subsumed" by existing concepts and organized in a hierarchical
framework (Ausubel, 1963). The hierarchical relationships individuals make between
concepts often result in the formulation of sound understandings and enable learners
to appl- meaningfully learned concepts to new situations or problems (Ausubel,
Novak, & Hanesian, 1978). Furthermore, students that learn concepts meaningfully
are thought to retain their network of understanding over a period of time (Bodolus,
1986; Cliburn, 1988; Pankratius, 1987).
In contrast with meaningful learning is rote learning, in which new knowledge
may be attained by verbatim memorization and incorporated into a person's
5
knowledge structure without connecting it to information or frameworks previously
acquired (Ausubel, 1963; Baird, 1986; Novak, 1988). Concepts learned by rote are
not "subsumed" and do not fit within one's conceptual structure in any sensible way.
Thus, a conceptual framework of understanding is not developed and therefore does
not serve as the basis for new understanding. Since rote learners are said to
formulate weak cognitive frameworks or none at all, they are thought not to retain
information learned over time (Novak, 1988).
The meaningful and/or rote learning of meiosis and the relationship of meiosis
with genetics has warranted particular interest in science education (Browning, 1988;
Cho, Kahle & Nord land, 1985; Kinnear, 1983; Stewart, 1982,1983; Stewart & Dale,
1989). Research in this area has shown that students are able to use Punnett square
diagrams to solve genetics problems successfully, but with little or no understanding of
how the use of this tool relates with meiosis and fertilization of gametes (Cho, Kahle, &
Nord land, 1985; Kinnear, 1983; Stewart, 1982, 1983). One important indication of the
prior research was that some students do tend to learn meiosis and the Punnett
square method by forming relationships between concepts, but others tend to learn
these concepts by rote (Stewart & Dale, 1989).
Previous reports of the current study explored factors which may help explain
why some students attain meaningful understandings of these biological concepts
whereas others attain only rote-level understandings (Cavallo, 1991; Cavallo & Schafer,
in preparation). The earlier study used a technique called "mental modeling"
(Mosenthal & Kirsch, 1991; Mosenthal & Kirsch, in press) to measure students' rotelevel and meaningful-level understandings of meiosis, the Punnett square method and
the relationship between these topics (see Cavallo, 1991). The major findings were
6
that students generally tended to learn concepts meaningfully or by rote (meaningful
learning orientation) (Donn, 1989; Entwistle & Ramsden, 1983), and this tendency was
positively related with the level of meaningful understanding they attained (Cavallo,
1991). Prior knowledge of meiosis was also found to be related with students'
attainment of meaningful understanding of meiosis and of genetics topics (Cavallo,
1991), which was similar to findings of a study by Stewart and Dale (1989). Two
instructional treatments, one based on Ausubel's (1963) reception learning model
(students told relationships between meiosis and genetics concepts) and one based
on Osborne & Wittrock's (1985) generative learning model (students asked to
construct relationships themselves), were found not.to be significant factors in
students' attainment of meaningful understanding (Cavallo, 1991).
The current research is an extension of the previous research by Cavallo (1991)
and primarily focussed on exploring the extent to which students retained meaningful
understandings of meiosis, the use of the Punnett square method in genetics, and the
procedural and conceptual relations between these topics over time. This research
also investigated factors (meaningful learning orientation, prior knowledge, instructional
treatment) which may be related with the retention of meaningful understanding of
these biological concepts. The purposes of this study were as follows:
1. To determine the relationship between the level of meaningful understanding
acquired immediately after instruction and the level of meaningful understanding
retained after four weeks on the biological topics of meiosis, the Punnett square
method and the relations between these topics.
7
2. To explore the relationship of students' meaningful learning orientation, prior
knowledge of meiosis, and instructional treatment (students told relationships, students
asked to construct relationships themselves) with retention of meaningful
understanding of meiosis, the Punnett square method and the relations between these
topics.
3. To investigate the relative predictive influence of students' meaningful learninc
orientation, prior knowledge, instructional treatment and all interactions of these
variables on students' retention of meaningful understanding of meiosis, the Punnett
square method and the relationship between these topics.
METHODOLOGY
Assessment of Meaningful Understanding
The mental model technique (Kirsch & Mosenthal, in press; Mosenthal & Kirsch,
1991; Mosenthal & Kirsch, in press) was used to measure students' meaningful
understandings of the three biology topics. The mental model assessment is an openended question assessment designed to reveal in detail, students' understandings of
any given topic. The mental model assessment technique requires that students
provide a comprehensive written description of their understanding of a particular
topic. The knowledge that students express is parsed into individual, informationbearing propositions, which are then mapped on a template or grid designed to
represent the nature of their understanding. The result of the mental model procedure
8
is a qualitative description and quantitative measure of learners' meaningful or rote
understandings of any given topic.
In this study, the understandings assessed were of meiosis, the Punnett square
method, and of the procedural and conceptual relations between these two topics
The procedural relations are indicated by students' understanding of how meiosis and
Punnett squares "work" together. For example, an understanding of the procedural
relations would be evident in students' explanations of how meiosis works (i.e., there
are two cell divisions, in the first phase chromosomes replicate, etc.) in relationship
with how the Punnett square method is used (i.e., write the genes from the sperm cell
on one side of the diagram, etc.). An understanding of the conceptual relations would
be represented by students' explanations of connections between what is involved in
meiosis (i.e., genes of chromosomes in gametes) and why meiosis occurs (i.e.,
number of chromosomes and associated genes reduced to haploid number in
preparation for fertilization) and its relationship with what is represented by using
Punnett squares (i.e., genes of gametes from meiosis are used in Punnett squares to
show fertilization) and why Punnett square diagrams are used (i.e., to predict possible
genes for certain traits in offspring). The criteria for measuring the conceptual
relations between meiosis and the Punnett square method were based on research by
Cho, Kahle and Nord land (1985).
Instrumentation and Procedures
This study was carried out in tenth grade biology classrooms (N =140) during
regular instructional periods. Students were given the Learning Approach
Questionnaire (LAO), a 50-item Likert-scale instrument for measuring students'
iJ
9
tendency to learn meaningfully or by rote (Donn,1989; Edmonson, 1989; Entwistle &
Ramsden, 1983; Novak, Kerr, Donn, & Cobern, 1989). In addition, the four teachers
of the students in the sample were asked to rate their students on a scale ranging
from meaningful to rote according to specific criteria described during a series of
training sessions by the researcher (Cavallo, 1991). Based on a composite
(meaningful learning orientation score) of the self-reported LAO scores and teacher
ratings, the students were identified as either meaningful, mid-range, or rote with
respect to the way they learn.
Students were given a mental model pre-test to assess their prior knowledge of
meiosis. Meiosis was used since it was considered an important underlying topic for
the meaningful understanding of genetics topics in research by Stewart and Dale
(1989). Students had been given instruction on meiosis prior to the study by their
respective teachers but they had not been instructed on genetics nor on the use of
Punnett square diagrams. Students were also given mental model pre-tests on the
Punnett square method and the relationship between meiosis and the Punnett square
method. As expected, students had no prior knowledge of either of these topics.
Students were randornly assigned to one of two auto-tutorial printed
instructional treatments. In both treatments, the presentation of meiosis, fertilization,
the inheritance of traits and the Punnett square method wer, identical. One treatment
(reception treatment) provided information, then highlighted, and answered and
explained questions specifically relating meiosis and the use of the Punnett square
method. In Treatment 2 (generative treatment), the same information and highlighted
questions were provided but the students were to construct the answers and
10
explanations relating the topics themselves. The instruction used in this study was
patterned after computer-assisted instruction developed by Browning (1988).
After the administration of the instructional treatments, students were given the
same three-question mental model test as was given in the pre-test (meiosis, the
Punnett square method, the relations between the topics). Four weeks later, the
students were again given the mental model test on these topics.
RESULTS
Students' pre-test, post-test and retention mental model tests were scored
using the parsing method described by Mosenthal & Kirsch, (1990) and detailed in a
previous study (Cavallo, 1991). Pre-test mental model scores were obtained for
meiosis and these scores were used as the prior knowledge variable. Post-test and
retention mental model scores were obtained from students' explanations of:
1)
meiosis, 2) the use of the Punnett square method, 3) the procedural relations between
meiosis and Punnett squares, 4) the conceptual relations between meiosis and
Punnett squares.'
Results of the data analyses are reported as follows.
The third mental model test question asked students to
describe the relationship between meiosis and the use of the
1
Punnett square method.
were scored in two ways.
Students' explanations on this question
One score represented their understanding
of the procedural relations, the second score represented their
understanding of the conceptual relations.
11
The relationship between U&Ieats2 meaningful understandings
square method and the relations between these topics after instruction and their
retention of meaningful understanding four weeks after instruction
Students' mental model post-test scores obtained immediately after the
instruction were correlated with retention mental model test scores taken four weeks
later. The results of the correlations are presented in Table 1.
12
TABLE 1. CORRELATION MATRIX OF STUDENTS' POST - INSTRUCTION TEST
SCORES AND RETENTION TEST SCORES.
RETENTION TEST SOORES
MEIOSIS
PUNNETT
SQUARES
PROCEDURAL
RELATION
CONCEPTUAL
RELATION
POST-TEST
SCORES
MEIOSIS
.50**
.27**
.19
.33**
PUNNEI I
.44**
.42**
.33**
.37**
PROCEDURAL
RELATION
.32**
.29**
.43**
.50**
CONCEPTUAL
.30**
.28**
.38**
.47**
SQUARES
RELATION
< .05, **.p < 01.
3
13
As observed from the correlation matrix shown in Table 1, the attainment of
meaningful understanding of each of the topics is significantly and positively correlated
with students' retention of meaningful understanding of the same topic. Thus if
students attained a meaningful understanding as measured immediately after
instruction they likely retained a meaningful understanding of that same topic four
weeks later. Additionally, with the exception of meiosis and the procedural relation
between meiosis and Punnett squares, students' meaningful understanding each topic
is correlated with retention of any of the other topics. In other words, if students
tended to attain a meaningful understanding of a topic (i.e., meiosis) they also tended
to retain meaningful understanding of any of the other topics (i.e., Punnett squares).
The meaningful attainment-meaningful retention relationship is consistent across the
different topics.
The relative importance of meaningul learning orientation, prior knowledge and
treatment on students' attainment of meaningful understanding
Correlations and stepwise multiple regression analyses were conducted to
determine the best predictor (prior knowledge of meiosis, meaningful learning
orientation and treatment) of students' retention of meaningful understanding of
meiosis, the Punnett square method and the relations between these topics. The
results of the correlations are reported in Table 2. Results of the stepwise regression
analyses are summarized in Table 3.
A
11
TABLE 2. CORRELATIONS OF STUDENTS' MEANINGFUL LEARNING
ORIENTATION, PRIOR KNOWLEDGE AND TREATMENT WITH RETENTION TEST
SCORES.
RETENTION TEST SCORES
MEIOSIS
PUNNETT
SQUARES
PROCEDURAL
RELATION
CONCEPTUAL
RELATION
MEANINGFUL
LEARNING
ORIENTATION
.25*
.03
.01
.06
PRIOR
.30"
.09
.25*
.21*
KNOWLEDGE
(MEIOSIS)
TREATMENT
< .05,
.04
-.10
.11
< .01.
5
-.06
15
ON STUDENTS' MEANINGFUL
REGRESSIONS
MULTIPLE
TABLE 3. STEPWISE
THE
SQUARE METHOD AND
PUNNETT
RETENTION OF MEIOSIS, THE
MEANINGFUL
WITH STUDENTS'
RELATIONS,
CONCEPTUAL
PROCEDURAL AND
PREDICTOR
AND TREATMENT AS
KNOWLEDGE
PRIOR
LEARNING ORIENTATION,
VARIABLES.
RETENTION
TEST
MEIOSIS
PREDICTOR VARIABLES
(SIGNIFICANT 9. <.05)
PRIOR KNOWLEDGE
MEANINGFUL LEARNING
VARIANCE
.09
.04
F
8.38
4.50
.005
.037
ORIENTATION
PUNNETT
SQUARES
PROCEDURAL
RELATION
CONCEPTUAL
RELATION
WERE SIGNIFICANT
NONE OF THESE VARIABLES
PREDICTORS
PRIOR KNOWLEDGE
PRIOR KNOWLEDGE
.06
.04
5.80
4.03
.0181
.0478
16
The correlation and regression analyses indicate that meaningful learning
orientation was an important factor only for students' meaningful retention of meiosis.
Prior knowledge of meiosis was a significant factor in predicting students' retention of
meiosis and the procedural and conceptual relations between the topics. None of the
predictor variables seemed to influence students' retention of meaningful
understanding of Punnett square diagrams.
The influence of all interactions of prior knowledge, meaningful learning orientation and
instructional treatment as .redictors of students' retention of meaningful understandin
of meiosis, the Punnett square method in genetics and the relations between these
topics
Interaction terms were created by multiplying the predictor variables (prior
knowledge, meaningful learning orientation, treatment) in every way possible. The
interaction terms included the following: 1) prior knowledge x meaningful learning
orientation, 2) prior knowledge x treatment, 3) meaningful learning orientation x
treatment, and 4) prior knowledge x meaningful learning orientation x treatment.
These four interaction terms were correlated with retention test scores, and entered as
predictor variables of retention scores in stepwise multiple regression analyses. Table
4 shows correlations between the interaction terms and retention scores for meiosis,
Punnett squares and the procedural and conceptual relations between the topics.
Table 5 summarizes the results of stepwise multiple regressions analyses with the
interaction terms used as predictor variables of retention test scores.
rr:
17
TABLE 4. CORRELATIONS OF STUDENTS' PRIOR KNOWLEDGE, MEANINGFUL
LEARNING ORIENTATION AND TREATMENT INTERACTION TERMS WITH
RETENTION TEST SCORES.
RETENTION TEST SCORES
MEIOSIS
PUNNETT
SQUARES
PROCEDURAL
RELATION
CONCEPTUAL
RELATION
PRIOR
KNOWLEDGE X
MEANINGFUL
LEARNING
ORIENTATION
.38-
.09
.20
.21*
.21
.08
.07
-.04
.13
.14
-.07
-.04
.26*
.11
.08
-.02
PRIOR
KNOWLEDGE X
TREATMENT
MEANINGFUL
LEARNING
ORIENTATION
X TREATMENT
PRIOR
KNOWLEDGE X
MEANINGFUL
LEARNING
ORIENTATION
X TREATMENT
< .05, **p < .01.
18
TABLE 5. STEPWISE MULTIPLE REGRESSIONS ON STUDENTS' MEANINGFUL
RETENTION TEST SCORES OF MEIOSIS, THE PUNNETT SQUARE METHOD AND
THE PROCEDURAL AND CONCEPTUAL RELATIONS, WITH ALL POSSIBLE
INTERACTIONS BETWEEN STUDENTS' MEANINGFUL LEARNING ORIENTATION,
PRIOR KNOWLEDGE AND TREATMENT AS PREDICTOR VARIABLES.
RETENTION
TEST
PREDICTOR VARIABLES
(SIGNIFICANT <.05)
VARIANCE
MEIOSIS
MEANINGFUL LEARNING
ORIENTATION X PRIOR
KNOWLEDGE
.14
PUNNETT
SQUARES
NONE OF THESE VARIABLES WERE SIGNIFICANT
PREDICTORS
PROCEDURAL
RELATION
NONE OF THESE VARIABLES WERE SIGNIFICANT
PREDICTORS
CONCEPTUAL
RELATION
MEANINGFUL LEARNING
ORIENTATION X PRIOR
KNOWLEDGE
.04
L.
14.25
4.09
.0003
.0462
19
The stepwise multiple regression analyses using the interaction terms as
predictor variables revealed that the interaction between prior knowledge of meiosis
and meaningful learning orientation explained the variance in retention test scores for
meiosis and the conceptual relation between the two topics. The regression lines for
meiosis and conceptual relation test scores were calculated and plotted in Figures 1
and 2.
20
Figure 1. Regression lines for retention test scores of meiosis by prior knowledge for meaningful learners,
mid-range learners, and rote learners. This graph represents the interaction of students' meaningful
learning orientation and prior knowledge in predicting means of retention mental model scores of meiosis.
KEY: o = rote learners
* = mid-range learners
x = meaningful learners
3
2.8
M
e
2.6
o
2.4
S
i
2.2
S
2
R
e
1.8
e
1.6
n
t
1.4
o
1.2
n
1
0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
0
2
1
Prior Knowledge of Meiosis
21
3
271
Figure 2. Regression lines for the conceptual relation between meiosis and Punnett squares retention test
by prior knowledge for meaningful learners, mid-range learners, and rote learners. This graph represents
the interaction of students' meaningful learning orientation and prior knowledge in predicting means of
retention mental model scores of the conceptual relation.
KEY: o = rote learners
* = mid-range learners
x = meaningful learners
3.6
C
o
3.4
n
c
3.2
e
P
3
t
u
2.8
a
I
2.6
R
2.4
e
I
2.2
a
t
2
i
o
n
1.8
1.6
R
e
t
1.4
e
1.2
n
t
1
i
o
0.8
n
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
2
1
Prior Knowledge of Meiosis
22
3
22
The slopes of the regression lines for meiosis retention indicate that meaningful,
mid-range and rote learners appeared to have approximately equivalent retention of
meiosis with low prior knowledge. With high prior knowledge however, meaningful
and mid-range learners seem to have retained more meaningful understandings than
rote learners.
The slopes of the regression lines for the conceptual relation indicate that, as
with meiosis, all three learner groups retained about the same level of understanding
of the conceptual relation with low prior knowledge. With high prior knowledge,
meaningful learners retained more meaningful understandings than mid-range or rote
learners.
DISCUSSION
The results of this study generally revealed that if students meaningfully
understood meiosis, the Punnett square method and the relations between these
topics after initial instruction, meaningful understanding was likely to be retained after a
period of time. Conversely, if students attained rote knowledge of these topics after
initial instruction, they were likely to have retained only rote-level knowledge of these
topics after the four weeks. In other words, students with rote knowledge were not
likely to have developed meaningful understandings of these topics over time.
Furthermore, with the exception of meiosis and the procedural relation between the
two topics, students' attainment of meaningful understanding of one topic was
r
23
positively related with their retention of the other topics.
This finding seems to
indicate a pattern of attainment and subsequent retention of rote, mid-range and
meaningful understandings of different topics among students. More research would
be needed to clarify this relationship and the possible existence of learning patterns
among students.
The results of this study also indicate that prior meaningful understanding of
meiosis was important for the meaningful retention of meiosis, and of the relationships
between meiosis and the use of Punnett square diagrams in genetics. In addition to
prior knowledge, a meaningful learning orientation was also important for the retention
of meaningful understanding of meiosis. This finding implies that retaining meaningful
understanding of a topic such as meiosis, which is not directly experienced or
observable, may require more than prior knowledge. Meaningful retention of meiosis,
and possibly similarly abstract or intangible topics, may also require a tendency to
actively create meaning from those prior understandings.
It was interesting to find that none of the predictor variables predicted the
retention of understanding of Punnett square diagrams. This may be a reflection of
the nature of the topic as a tool, or perhaps algorithm, to solve problems. It may not
be conceptually related with other topics by students, thus meaningful learning
orientation and prior knowledge would have little to do with retention of the topic.
Alternatively, this finding may indicate that there may be other variables, such as
logical thinking ability, which have a role in the retention of the Punnett square method.
Other variables such as logical thinking ability should be researched to determine a
24
24
possible influence on the retention of problem solving-oriented topics such as the
Punnett square method.
The interaction between meaningful learning orientation and prior knowledge
significantly predicted students' retention of meaningful understanding of meiosis and
the conceptual relations between meiosis and the Punnett square method. From the
correlations and regression lines it appeared that both meaningful and mid-range
learners retained more meaningful understandings of meiosis with high prior
knowledge than did rote learners. Meaningful prior understanding of meiosis may
have helped meaningful and mid-range learners retain meaningful understandings of
meiosis. However, meaningful prior understanding of meiosis did not seem to be a
factor in the meaningful retention of meiosis among rote learners. It is also apparent
from the regressions lines that with low prior knowledge, retention of meiosis is about
equal for all three learner groups.
High prior knowledge seemed to be important for the meaningful retention of
the conceptual relations between meiosis and the Punnett square method for all three
learner groups. However, meaningful learners with high prior knowledge of meiosis
seem to have retained greater, more meaningful understandings of the conceptual
relations between meiosis and Punnett squares than rote or mid-range learners. For
all three learner groups, retention of the conceptual relations appears to have been
approximately equal with low prior knowledge. These findings indicate that prior
meaningful understanding of meiosis may serve as an "anchor" for formulating
conceptual relationships with new information on the Punnett square method (i.e., that
25
the letters used represent genes on chromosomes, that "filling in" the boxes
represents fertilization, that 'he genes used on the outside of the boxes represent
genes of the sperm and egg cell, and that the. chromosomes with genes within the
sperm and egg cell are there as a result of meiosis). If meaningful learners, who tend
to actively make connections and formulate relationships between ideas, have high
prior knowledge of an underlying topic, conceptual relations between the topics may
be particularly meaningfully retained. Thus, the tendency to actively formulate
relationships between ideas, along with a sound foundation of relevant prior
knowledge appears to be important for retaining inter-related, meaningful
understandings of the conceptual relations between these topics in biology.
IMPLICATIONS AND SIGNIFICANCE
This study advances current understanding of students' attainment and
retention of sound, inter-related understandings of science topics. Importantly, this
research provides information on students' retention of meaningful understanding, and
on variables which may relate with retention, particularly, relevant prior knowledge and
students' meaningful learning orientation. With information obtained from this
research, educators may be better prepared to help students achieve and retain
meaningful understandings of science. Finally, the results of this study provide a
foundation for future inquiry and research on student learning and retention.
2
26
REFERENCES
Ausubel, D. P., (1963). The Psychology of Meaningful Verbal Learning. New York:
Grune & Stratton, Inc.
Ausubel, D. P., (1968). A subsumption theory L f meaningful verbal learning and
retention. In R. G. Kuhlen (Ed.), Studies in Educational Psychology, Boston:
Blaisdell Publishing Company.
Ausubel, D. P., Novak, J. D., & Hanesian, H., (1978). Educational Psychology: A
Cognitive View, 2nd Edition, New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Baird, J. R. (1986). Improving learning through enhanced metacognition: A
classroom study. European Journal of Science Education, 8(3), 263-282.
Bodolus, J. E., (1986). The use of a concept mapping strategy to facilitate meaningful
learning for ninth grade students in science (Doctoral dissertation, Temple
University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 2230, 48/09A.
27
Browning, M., (1988). The effects of meiosis/genetics integration and instructional
sequence on college biology student achievement in genetics. Paper presented
at the Annual Meeting of the National Association for Research in Science
Teaching, Lake of the Ozarks, MO. (Eric Document Reproduction Service ED
291 578).
Cavallo, A. L., (1991). The relationship between students' meaningful learning
orientation and their mental models of meiosis and genetics. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Syracuse University.
Cho, H., Kahle, J. B., & Nord land, F. H., (1985). An investigation of high school
biology textbooks as sources of misconceptions and difficulties in genetics:
Some suggestions for teaching genetics. Science Education, 69(5), 707-719.
Cliburn, J. W., Jr., (1986). An Ausubelian approach to instruction: The use of concept
maps as advance organizers in a junior college anatomy and physiology course
(Doctoral dissertation, The University of Southern Mississippi). Dissertation
Abstracts International, 852, 47/09A.
Donn, S. (1989). Epistemological Issues in Science Education. Paper presented at the
Annual Meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching,
San Francisco, California.
28
Driver, R., & Erickson, G., (1983). Theories-in-action: Some theoretical and empirical
issues in the study of students' conceptual frameworks in science. Studies in
Science Education, 10, 37-60.
Edmonson, K. M., (1989). Differences and similarities between males' and females'
conceptions of scientific knowledge and their orientations to learning. Paper
presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Association for Research in
Science Teaching, San Francisco, California.
Entwistle, N., & Ramdsen, P., (1983). Understanding Student Learning. London:
Croom Helm.
Kinnear, J., (1983). Identification of misconceptions in genetics and the use of
computer simulations in their corrections. Proc. Int. Seminar: Misconceptions in
Science and Mathematics. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.
Kirsch, I. S., & Mosenthal, P. B., (in press). Understanding mimetic documents
through "knowledge modeling". Journal of Reading Behavior.
Mosenthal, P. B., & Kirsch, I. S., (in press). More mimetic documents: Procedural
schematics. Journal of Reading.
29
Mosenthal, J. B., & Kirsch, I. S., (1991). Mimetic documents: Process schematics.
Journal of Reading, 3_4(5) 390-397.
Novak, J. D., (1988). Learning science and the science of learning. Studies in Science
Education, 15, 77-101.
Novak, J. D., Kerr, P., Donn, S., & Cobern, W., (1989). Epistemological issues in
science education. Symposium presented at the Annual Conference of the
National Association for Research in Science Teaching, San Francisco, CA.
Osborne, R. & Wittrock, M., (1985). The generative learning model and its implications
for science education. Studies in Science Education, 12, 59-87
Pankratius, W. J., (1987). Building an organized knowledge base: Concept mapping
and achievement in secondary school physics. (Doctoral dissertation, Georgia
State University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1024, 49/03A.
Pines, A. L. & West, L. H. T., (1986). Conceptual understanding and science learning:
An interpretation of research within a sources-of-knowledge framework.
Science Education, 70(5), 583-604.
30
Stewart, J. H., (1982). Difficulties experienced by high school students when learning
basic Mendelian genetics, The American Bioloay Teacher, 44(2), 80-89.
Stewart, J. H., (1983). Student problem solving in high school genetics. Science
Education, 67, 523-540.
Stewart, J. H., & Dale, M., (1989). High school students' understanding of
chromosome/gene behavior during meiosis. Science Education, 73(4),
501-521.