Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Stress, Arousal, and Sleep

Stress is considered to be an important cause of disrupted sleep and insomnia. However, controlled and experimental studies in rodents indicate that effects of stress on sleep–wake regulation are complex and may strongly depend on the nature of the stressor. While most stressors are associated with at least a brief period of arousal and wakefulness, the subsequent amount and architecture of recovery sleep can vary dramatically across conditions even though classical markers of acute stress such as corticosterone are virtually the same. Sleep after stress appears to be highly influenced by situational variables including whether the stressor was controllable and/or predictable, whether the individual had the possibility to learn and adapt, and by the relative resilience and vulnerability of the individual experiencing stress. There are multiple brain regions and neurochemical systems linking stress and sleep, and the specific balance and interactions between these systems may ultimately determine the alterations in sleep–wake architecture. Factors that appear to play an important role in stress-induced wakefulness and sleep changes include various monominergic neurotransmitters, hypocretins, corticotro-pin releasing factor, and prolactin. In addition to the brain regions directly involved in stress responses such as the hypothalamus, the locus coeruleus, and the amygdala, differential effects of stressor controllability on behavior and sleep may be mediated by the medial prefrontal cortex. These various brain regions interact and influence each other and in turn affect the activity of sleep–wake controlling centers in the brain. Also, these regions likely play significant roles in memory processes and participate in the way stressful memories may affect arousal and sleep. Finally, stress-induced changes in sleep-architecture may affect sleep-related neuronal plasticity processes and thereby contribute to cognitive dysfunction and psychiatric disorders.

Stress, Arousal, and Sleep Larry D. Sanford, Deborah Suchecki and Peter Meerlo Abstract Stress is considered to be an important cause of disrupted sleep and insomnia. However, controlled and experimental studies in rodents indicate that effects of stress on sleep–wake regulation are complex and may strongly depend on the nature of the stressor. While most stressors are associated with at least a brief period of arousal and wakefulness, the subsequent amount and architecture of recovery sleep can vary dramatically across conditions even though classical markers of acute stress such as corticosterone are virtually the same. Sleep after stress appears to be highly influenced by situational variables including whether the stressor was controllable and/or predictable, whether the individual had the possibility to learn and adapt, and by the relative resilience and vulnerability of the individual experiencing stress. There are multiple brain regions and neurochemical systems linking stress and sleep, and the specific balance and interactions between these systems may ultimately determine the alterations in sleep–wake architecture. Factors that appear to play an important role in stress-induced wakefulness and sleep changes include various monominergic neurotransmitters, hypocretins, corticotropin releasing factor, and prolactin. In addition to the brain regions directly involved in stress responses such as the hypothalamus, the locus coeruleus, and the amygdala, differential effects of stressor controllability on behavior and sleep may be mediated by the medial prefrontal cortex. These various brain regions interact and influence each other and in turn affect the activity of sleep–wake controlling centers in the brain. Also, these regions likely play significant roles in memory processes and L. D. Sanford (&) Department of Pathology and Anatomy, Eastern Virginia Medical School, P.O. Box 1980, Norfolk, VA 23507, USA e-mail: SanforLD@evms.edu D. Suchecki Departamento de Psicobiologia, Universidade Federal de Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo, Brazil P. Meerlo Center for Behavior and Neurosciences, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands Ó Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014 Curr Topics Behav Neurosci (2015) 25: 379–410 DOI 10.1007/7854_2014_314 Published Online: 23 May 2014 379 380 L. D. Sanford et al. participate in the way stressful memories may affect arousal and sleep. Finally, stress-induced changes in sleep-architecture may affect sleep-related neuronal plasticity processes and thereby contribute to cognitive dysfunction and psychiatric disorders. Keywords Stress differences Fear   Controllability  Predictability  Vulnerability  Individual  Arousal  Sleep disturbance  Insomnia  Psychopathology Contents 1 2 Introduction........................................................................................................................ Complex Effects of Stress on Sleep Architecture............................................................ 2.1 Effects of Acute Stress ............................................................................................. 2.2 Repeated or Chronic Stress ...................................................................................... 3 Effects on Sleep May Vary with Specific Stress Parameters.......................................... 3.1 Stressor Controllability ............................................................................................. 3.2 Stressor Predictability ............................................................................................... 3.3 Stress-Related Learning and Sleep .......................................................................... 3.4 Fear Extinction and Sleep ........................................................................................ 3.5 Stressor Resilience and Vulnerability to Sleep Disturbance................................... 4 Stress Mediators as an Important Cause of Arousal and Sleep Disturbance.................. 4.1 Hypocretin/Orexin .................................................................................................... 4.2 Corticotropin Releasing Hormone............................................................................ 4.3 Prolactin .................................................................................................................... 4.4 Monoamines.............................................................................................................. 5 Brain Regions Linking Stress, Arousal and Sleep........................................................... 5.1 Amygdala and Stress-Induced Alterations in Arousal and Sleep........................... 5.2 REM Regulatory Regions, Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Stressor Control .......... 6 Stress, Sleep and Neuronal Plasticity: Implications for Stress-Related Disorders ......... References................................................................................................................................ 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 390 391 393 394 394 395 398 399 401 1 Introduction Stress is generally viewed as a major cause of disrupted sleep. Traumatic life events often result in sleep disturbances that may include insomnia or subjective sleep problems (Lavie 2001) and the persistence of these disturbances may be predictive of the future development of emotional and cognitive disorders (Chang et al. 1997; Koren et al. 2002; Neckelmann et al. 2007). Understandably, given the practical and ethical concerns, few controlled and experimental studies on severe stress and its effect on subsequent sleep have been done in human subjects. As such, most of the available data on stress and sleep have come from studies in laboratory rodents (Pawlyk et al. 2008). Importantly, the large body of animal studies based on a wide variety of experimental stress models indicates that effects of stress on sleep may be far more complex than a simple and prolonged increase of wakefulness. The impact Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 381 of stress on sleep may vary with specific characteristics of a stressor (e.g., duration, intensity, controllability, and predictability) and with characteristics of the individual experiencing stress (e.g., individual stress coping strategies, relative resilience, and vulnerability). In addition, any stressful situation provides an opportunity for learning, and the success or failure of an organism in developing an adaptive coping strategy can influence post-stress sleep and behavior. Subsequently, evoking stress-related memories can impact sleep and behavior in much the same fashion as the original stress. There are significant overlaps of the neural circuitry and neurochemistry underlying the stress response and that regulating arousal and sleep. Thus, it is not surprising that the interaction between stress and sleep is implicated in a variety of disease processes and psychiatric disorders. However, it is important to note that even significant stress can be experienced without producing permanent or pathological changes. The stress response engages the physiological and behavioral resources needed to cope with a challenge followed by a return to normalcy when the situation is resolved. Indeed, the purpose of the stress response is to restore homeostasis (Johnson et al. 1992; Chrousos 2009). In this review, we will discuss the complex effects of stress on sleep, the stress parameters that appear to be important in determining post-stress sleep, and the neurochemical and neuroanatomical substrates important in regulating the relationship between stress and sleep. Lastly, we will discuss factors linking stress and sleep in the genesis of stress-related disorders. 2 Complex Effects of Stress on Sleep Architecture Much of our knowledge of the relationship between stress and sleep is based on animal models, which allow for controlled studies on the consequences of both acute and chronic stress. A wide variety of experimental paradigms have been used to assess effects of acute stress on sleep, including social defeat stress (Meerlo et al. 1997, 2001a; Meerlo and Turek 2001), restraint or immobilization (Rampin et al. 1991; Meerlo et al. 2001b), footshocks (Smith 1995; Palma et al. 2000; Sanford et al. 2005), water immersion (Smith 1995), cold exposure (Palma et al. 2000; Tiba et al. 2004, 2008), ether exposure (Roky et al. 1995; Bodosi et al. 2000), cage changes (Tang et al. 2004, 2005b), exposure to novel environments (Tang et al. 2004, 2005b), and exposure to novel objects (Schiffelholz and Aldenhoff 2002; Tang et al. 2004, 2005b). Studies aimed at chronic stress are often based on repetition of a stressful stimulus [i.e., intermittent footshock: (Kant et al. 1995)] or alternating presentations of different noxious stimuli [i.e., tilted cage, wet cage, food deprivation, etc. (Cheeta et al. 1997; Gronli et al. 2004)] over a prolonged period of time. Despite the obvious variation in the nature of the stimuli applied, their use in studies on stress is motivated by the assumption that these stimuli and conditions are, to some degree, aversive to the animals. In many cases this assumption is supported by data showing activation of the classical neuroendocrine stress 382 L. D. Sanford et al. systems, i.e., the sympatho-adrenal axis and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. Indeed the major similarity between these models in terms of stress appears to be an increase in the plasma levels of the stress hormones adrenaline and corticosterone, although one has to keep in mind that such elevations occur in response to virtually any kind of challenge and not exclusively to aversive stimuli (Koolhaas et al. 2011). Importantly, though the outcome in many studies on stress and sleep is discussed in terms of general stress effects, the experimental paradigms that are used may have stimulus-specific effects as well. 2.1 Effects of Acute Stress Stress is generally considered to be a functional response of the brain and the body to challenges that humans and other animals may face. Coping with environmental challenges requires alertness and, since stress is a state of physiological activation and arousal, by definition, it inhibits sleep. Indeed, exposing animals to stressors is invariably associated with at least a short-lasting increase in wakefulness. Consistent with this aspect of the stress response, several of the classical neuropeptides and hormones involved in the stress response are known to promote wakefulness (see Sect. 4). After the initial phase of arousal, sleep architecture is often altered; but in ways that may vary among stressors. In addition to inhibiting all sleep, some stressors may have a more pronounced and prolonged inhibiting effect on rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, that is, once the animals fall asleep there may be a prolonged period of time with non-REM (NREM) sleep but little or no REM sleep. This has been shown, for example, in rodents after exposure to severe social stress (Meerlo and Turek 2001) and exposure to multiple presentations of inescapable footshock stress (Adrien et al. 1991; Liu et al. 2003; Sanford et al. 2003a, b, c). The initial period of stress-induced wakefulness and sleep disruption is most often followed by a rebound to compensate for the NREM and/or REM sleep that was lost. However, how much of the lost sleep is compensated and which sleep state varies widely between studies and stressors. For example, in rodents exposed to acute social stress, the initial loss of REM sleep is largely compensated during the subsequent recovery period (Meerlo and Turek 2001). However, such a REM sleep rebound may not occur after exposure to inescapable footshock stress in fear conditioning paradigms (Sanford et al. 2003a, b, c, 2010) or learned helplessness paradigms (Adrien et al. 1991). Intriguingly, after some stressors, animals appear to gain more sleep than was actually lost during and immediately following the stress. For example, controlled studies in laboratory rats and mice showed that exposure to acute social stress, i.e., a 1 h interaction with an aggressive male conspecific, is followed by deeper or longer NREM sleep than a similar period of non-stressful sleep deprivation (Meerlo et al. 1997; Meerlo and Turek 2001). On the other hand, other studies have shown that acute immobilization or restraint stress is often followed by a Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 383 selective increase in REM sleep (Rampin et al. 1991; Meerlo et al. 2001b). The latter has long been the basis for the widely held belief that stress causes an increase in REM sleep, which we now know is clearly not a general feature of all stressors. It has not been established whether the stressor-specific increases in one sleep state or another reflect disturbances in sleep regulation or perhaps functional adaptations that evolved to deal with and recover from different stress conditions. Indeed, it has been argued that acute social stress may represent a form of intense wakefulness associated with increased brain activity, which would require a higher than normal need for recovery sleep (Meerlo et al. 1997). Similarly, the increase in REM sleep after e.g., immobilization stress has been suggested to be an adaptive coping response that may also serve the purpose of recovery (Suchecki et al. 2012). However, it remains unknown why some stressors are not followed by a complete compensation for the sleep that was lost, whereas other stressors are followed by more sleep than was lost. Nor is it understood why some stressors seem to promote NREM sleep whereas others are followed by an increase in REM sleep. This complex variation in the effects of different stressors on sleep may depend on the nature of the stressors and the specific effects they have on physiology and brain function. The variation in outcome may also be modulated by the way stressors are perceived by the individual, e.g., in terms of controllability and predictability see (Sects. 3.1 and 3.2), and whether or not a successful coping strategy is developed (Sect. 3.3). Overall, the finding that, in most animal models of acute stress, the arousing and sleep-inhibiting effects of stressors are rapidly overcome and are sometimes followed by increased sleep during the recovery phase may seem at odds with the general notion that stress is a major cause of sleep disturbance and insomnia in humans. One explanation for this apparent inconsistency is that in laboratory rodents the physiological activation and arousal disappear quite rapidly upon termination of the stressor and return to the home cage, whereas human beings may carry their problems and stress with them. That is, the ‘‘stress’’ responsible for prolonged sleep disruption in humans may be a cognitive and emotional phenomenon that is not necessarily always associated with an acute challenge. Humans may suffer from stress based on memories of past events as well as worries and expectations about the future. In that respect, compared to some animals, the human brain may be more capable of turning a single acute stressor or life event that occurred in the past, or even one pending in the future, into a persistent and chronic stress state. 2.2 Repeated or Chronic Stress With chronic stress, prolonged activation of the same behavioral, physiological, and metabolic processes beneficial for coping with an acute stressor can become detrimental (Chrousos 1998). Chronic stress has been reported to be a factor in the 384 L. D. Sanford et al. disruption of sleep in a variety of situations including individuals lacking social support in the work environment (Gadinger et al. 2009; Nomura et al. 2009), children and adolescents exposed to traumatic events (Charuvastra and Cloitre 2009), and burnout patients (Armon et al. 2008). Chronic stress has also been viewed as a risk factor for the development of insomnia (Cartwright and Wood 1991). A number of experimental studies in laboratory rats have applied a model of so-called chronic mild stress, which consists of exposing animals to a mixture of noxious stimuli, once or twice a day, for periods up to 3 or 4 weeks (e.g. Cheeta et al. 1997, Gronli et al. 2004). The stimuli include tilting of the cage, temporary exposure to a wet or soiled cage, food and water deprivation, exposure to prolonged periods of continuous lighting, and even stroboscopic lighting in one of the studies (Cheeta et al. 1997). The most significant finding in these studies was an increase in the amount of REM sleep the day after 3 or 4 weeks of treatment. The overall amount of sleep tended to be somewhat increased as well, which may in part reflect a rebound due to sleep loss during the actual stress exposure. Unfortunately, some of the stimuli applied, in particular the continuous or stroboscopic lighting, may have effects on sleep that have little to do with stress per se, for example through alterations in circadian function. Chronic stress as discussed here partly relies on direct stimulation of the animals, which may explain some of the changes in sleep, whereas stress-related sleep disturbances in humans often appear to be of a more psychological nature. It may very well be that the physiological and neurobiological mechanisms resulting in disrupted sleep due to repeated presentations of actual stressors are quite different from those involved in psychological stress in humans. Although, these studies are important first steps toward developing relevant models for stress-related sleep disturbances and insomnia, perhaps research on the relationship between stress and sleep would gain by models that are based more on psychological factors; for instance, conditioned fear and arousal in which animals anticipate the occurrence of adverse events. Such an approach may have more resemblance with the psychological stress in humans, and may allow us to study central mechanisms by which sleep is disrupted and how such disturbances could best be treated. 3 Effects on Sleep May Vary with Specific Stress Parameters While the importance of specific stress parameters and individual differences in stress sensitivity is generally well established, there has been very limited research on these issues with respect to their influence on sleep. Indeed, to date, much of the work on stress and sleep has been primarily descriptive and focused on effects of different types of stressors. The variable outcomes in terms of sleep Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 385 produced by different stressors clearly indicate that the observed changes are not simply a generalizable stress effect. One should thus be careful with the interpretation and extrapolation of findings from these types of studies and perhaps even not refer to the sleep changes as simple stress effects, as sleep after a stressful event can be modified as a consequence of specific stimuli or conditions. Indeed, in our view, experimental paradigms that manipulate specific stress parameters (e.g., duration, intensity, controllability, and predictability) and paradigms that consider organismal variables (e.g., learning and memory, resilience, and vulnerability) have considerably more potential for providing actual insight into the complex relationship between stress and sleep. In this section, we will provide an overview of how a number of these factors can modulate the stressinduced changes in sleep. 3.1 Stressor Controllability Recent work has examined changes in sleep after controllable and uncontrollable stress, and of memories associated with each parameter, using a simple yoked control paradigm. In this paradigm, animals receive equal amounts of footshock, but one of the yoked pair can terminate the footshock simply by moving to the safe side of the shuttlebox. The actions of the yoked animal cannot alter shock presentation. Even though both animals receive equal shock, sleep in the post-shock period can be dramatically different. As demonstrated in Fig. 1, animals trained with controllable stress [escapable shock (ES)] can show significant enhancements of REM sleep, whereas their yoked controls that receive uncontrollable stress [inescapable shock (IS)] show significant reductions in REM sleep (Sanford et al. 2010). Returning the animals to the shock context without presenting footshock is also followed by increased REM in the controllable stress condition and decreased REM in the uncontrollable stress condition. Importantly, upon return to the shock context, both groups of mice show enhanced freezing, the primary behavioral indicator of fear memory (e.g. Blanchard and Blanchard 1969; Phillips and LeDoux 1992; Paylor et al. 1994). Training with ES and IS also elicits similar acute physiological stress responses as indicated by increased levels of plasma corticosterone (Shors et al. 1989) and increased body temperature (stress-induced hyperthermia) (Yang et al. 2011a). Thus, in this model, controllable and uncontrollable stress (and reminders of controllable and uncontrollable stress) result in similar activation of the acute stress response and behavioral indices of fear but directionally different alterations in REM. Controllability over a stressor does not simply dampen the changes in sleep but rather, it may result in qualitatively different changes. This work extends the findings based on the standard conditioned fear paradigm and further demonstrates that post-stress changes in sleep are not a simple function of the physical stress that an animal receives. 386 L. D. Sanford et al. (a) (b) Total REM 90 **** **** 60 30 0 **** 10 5 0 (c) (d) REM Episodes 120 **** 80 **** REM Episode dur ation 1.2 *** **** Minutes Counts **** **** Percent Minutes **** REM/ TST 15 40 ** 0.8 *** 0.4 ES IS 0 Base ST1 ST2 Con 0 Base ST1 ST2 Con Fig. 1 REM sleep parameters plotted as 20 h totals for baseline (Base), two shock training days (ST1, ST2), and context (Con) in a study comparing the effects of controllable (modeled by escapable shock or ES) and uncontrollable (modeled by inescapable shock or IS) stress. a Total REM sleep. b REM sleep percentage (total REM sleep time/total sleep time). c Number of REM sleep episodes. d REM sleep episode duration. Significant differences between ES and IS: **, p \ 0.01; ***, p \ 0.001; ****, p \ 0.0001 (Tukey test). Significant differences compared to Base (open symbols) are indicated by dark circles or squares for the ES and IS groups. Reprinted with permission from (Sanford et al. 2010) 3.2 Stressor Predictability Predictability is an important factor in the effects of stress and a preference for predictability has been repeatedly demonstrated (French et al. 1972; Gliner 1972; Miller et al. 1974). For example, animals given the opportunity to determine whether shocks delivered to them will be signaled or unsignaled typically choose to spend their time in the signaled conditions regardless of whether the shock is escapable or inescapable [reviewed in (Badia et al. 1979)]. The strong behavioral effects suggest that predictability may also have a role in the modulating effects of stress on sleep. In fact, stressor predictability is a significant component in shock avoidance training in a shuttlebox, a paradigm in which laboratory rats are signaled of imminent shock and can learn to prevent shock from being delivered. Variants of this paradigm have often been used in studies of learning and sleep and have typically found increases in the amount of REM sleep at various latencies after training that have been interpreted as indicating a role for REM sleep in memory consolidation (e.g. Smith and Lapp 1986; Datta 2000). Unfortunately, the Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 387 potential role of predictability in modulating sleep after stress has received very little attention. One study of stressor predictability in mice examined sleep after training with signaled escapable shock (SES) and signaled inescapable shock (SIS) (Yang et al. 2011a). Compared to mice experiencing SIS, those experiencing SES showed significantly increased REM sleep after each of two shock training sessions whereas compared to mice experiencing SES, those experiencing SIS showed significantly increased NREM sleep after both shock sessions. Interestingly, groups receiving either SES or SIS showed reduced REM sleep in response to cue presentation alone. In another study, mice exposed to either predictive or nonpredictive auditory cues during training with ES also showed post-stress increases in REM sleep (Machida et al. 2013). However, a subgroup of mice (around 35 %) trained with the predictive auditory cue failed to improve their escape performance from the first to second day of training. Those mice that did not improve also did not show enhanced REM on either shock training day, suggesting a learning component in the alterations in REM sleep. It is useful to compare these results to those obtained using non-signaled escapable and IS used to model controllable and uncontrollable stress as described above (Sanford et al. 2010). Without predictive cues, the relative differences in post-stress REM after escapable and IS were more pronounced. Contexts associated with non-signaled escapable and IS also produced directionally different changes in REM similar to those seen when shock was presented (Sanford et al. 2010), whereas predictive cues associated with either escapable or IS produced similar reductions in REM sleep. While at this point the data are limited, these findings suggest that contexts and auditory cues associated with different shock training conditions may carry different, and potentially competing, types of information regarding the stressful situations. This difference is more pronounced in training with ESs as both contextual and cued fear associated with uncontrollable stress have similar effects on sleep in mice and both reduce REM (Sanford et al. 2003a, c). Thus, competing cued and contextual information associated with ESs may have interacted during training resulting in competing influences on REM, thereby suggesting that stressor predictability and controllability may interact in complex ways to modulate the changes in subsequent sleep. 3.3 Stress-Related Learning and Sleep In addition to producing direct physiological effects, stressful situations provide an opportunity for learning as the individual responds to the stressor and seeks to use available information to cope with the ongoing challenge the stressor imposes. While in humans this could involve a variety of activities, including higher order cognitive processing; in rodents, the simplest behavioral responses to a stressor may be avoidance or escape attempts. In this case, stress-related parameters such 388 L. D. Sanford et al. as controllability and predictability may provide useful information that shapes avoidance and escape behaviors thereby facilitating successful coping. By comparison, stressors that are uncontrollable or occur unpredictably do not provide information that can guide the animal to learn successful avoidance or escape behaviors. In these situations, the animal may still engage in escape attempts, but its behavior will not alter the presentation of the stressor or facilitate coping. The impact of stressor controllability and predictability on behavior are central to a number of well-established learning paradigms that are motivated by stressful events. Of these, fear conditioning and related paradigms are beginning to demonstrate that the learning options an animal has in a stressful situation play a significant role in determining the impact of stress. Experimental fear conditioning is a learning paradigm in which an animal makes an association between an uncontrollable stressor (usually footshock) and previously neutral cues (typically auditory) or contextual information (the test box and experimental room). Afterwards, presenting the fearful cues and contexts alone elicit physiological and behavioral fear responses similar to those produced by the initial uncontrollable stressor. Fear conditioned alterations in sleep are now also established though these can vary with the amount of training and with the strain of rats or mice that is studied. In agreement with the data in previous sections showing that uncontrollable footshocks reduce REM sleep, the primary and most consistent effect of extensive training with inescapable footshock is a marked reduction in REM sleep that occurs both after the shock training and after presentation of shock associated fearful cues and contexts (Sanford et al. 2003a, c; Tang et al. 2005d). This reduction in REM sleep has been reported across species and across strains (Sanford et al. 2001, 2003a, c) and can occur without the rebound or recovery REM sleep that has been reported for most stressors (Sanford et al. 2003a, c). Changes in NREM sleep in fear conditioning studies appear to be less consistent. Some studies have reported increases in (light) NREM sleep (Adrien et al. 1991), whereas others have shown strain-dependent reductions in NREM sleep after shock training and fearful contexts (Sanford et al. 2003a). There also may be relatively less NREM sleep EEG delta power (slow wave activity) in animals that show greater fear conditioned changes in sleep (Tang et al. 2006). Critically, these studies demonstrate that fear conditioned memories of stressful events can produce mostly the same changes in sleep as those produced by the stressful event itself and indicate the importance of learning in both the immediate and lasting effects that stress can have on sleep. 3.4 Fear Extinction and Sleep Fear extinction is another important type of stress-related learning. While fear conditioning can be involved in the long term, negative effects of stress, it also can underlie adaptive behavior that occurs only so long as the fear-inducing stimulus is predictive of, or associated with, an aversive event (Kishimoto et al. 2000; Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 389 Pitman et al. 2001). Repeated presentation of a fearful cue or context without shock results in fear extinction, a type of new learning that inhibits subsequent fear behavior without erasing the original memory for fear conditioning (Bouton 2004). It is the failure of extinction that has been linked to stress-related psychopathology, particularly posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Myers and Davis 2007). The processes that make fear behaviors resistant to extinction remain mostly unknown though there appears to be a relationship between fear extinction and post-training REM sleep. Post-training REM sleep deprivation has been reported to impair extinction (as indicated by freezing) for light cues (Silvestri 2005), but not for auditory cues (Fu et al. 2007) previously paired with shock. REM sleepdeprived rats did show greater spontaneous recovery of freezing on a second day with presentation of the fearful auditory cue alone. Neither of these studies found that post-training REM sleep deprivation significantly altered contextual fear extinction learning or spontaneous recovery of freezing on a second day of testing (Silvestri 2005; Fu et al. 2007). However, sleep (both NREM and REM) following extinction of contextual fear does return to normal, whereas rats that continued to show fear exhibited reductions in REM sleep (Wellman et al. 2008). 3.5 Stressor Resilience and Vulnerability to Sleep Disturbance Genetic differences in vulnerability and resilience are recognized as important factors in the development of stress-related pathology. For example, approximately 20–30 % of individuals who experience traumatic events may develop PTSD whereas others do not appear to suffer significant long-lasting effects (Cohen et al. 2003; Kerns et al. 2004). Attempts to develop animal models that better represent individual differences in clinical populations have included the selection of low and high responders to stressors in outbred rat strains (Cohen et al. 2003; Kerns et al. 2004). There is also evidence that differences in vulnerability are a factor in the impact of stress on sleep, but the significance of individual differences has not been fully appreciated in either studies of stress or in studies of sleep in general (Irmis et al. 1971, 1974; Tang et al. 2007). Some of the best evidence for the role in resilience and vulnerability in the impact of stress on sleep comes from studies comparing inbred strains of rodents, which are genetically identical within strain but which vary genetically and phenotypically across strain. Work in mice and rats has demonstrated that strains that exhibited greater anxiety-like behaviors in response to challenges in wakefulness exhibited correspondingly greater and longer duration alterations in sleep after training with IS and after fearful cues (Sanford et al. 2003c) and contexts (Sanford et al. 2003a) associated with IS. In general, vulnerable mouse strains (e.g., BALB/ cJ mice compared to more resilient C57BL/6 J mice) also showed greater decreases in sleep after stressful situations with unlearned responses, including exposure to an 390 L. D. Sanford et al. open field (Tang et al. 2004), cage change, and novel objects placed in the home cage (Tang et al. 2005b). Moreover, BALB/cJ mice also do not show a significant REM sleep increase during recovery from restraint stress, whereas C57BL/6 J mice do (Meerlo et al. 2001b). In addition to genetic determinants of individual resilience and vulnerability, environmental factors and prior experiences with stress also play a major role in shaping future responses to stressful challenges. One such factor is neonatal stress that can be induced by disruptions of mother–infant relationship and that can have repercussions for adult behavior (Levine 2005) though the specific effects of maternal separation on sleep have varied across studies. For example, 3 h of maternal separation from postnatal days 2–14 in rats has been reported to increase both spontaneous baseline REM sleep and cold-stress-induced changes in REM sleep in males (Tiba et al. 2004) and females (Tiba et al. 2008). Similarly, neonatal rats maternally separated for 3 h and exposed to chronic mild stress as adults were reported to show longer sleep time, more episodes of REM sleep, and more episodes of NREM sleep transitioning to REM sleep (Mrdalj et al. 2013). By comparison, 6 h of neonatal maternal deprivation reduced the time spent in REM, without changes in NREM sleep when the rats attained adulthood (Feng et al. 2012). In addition to differences in experimental procedure, another important aspect that may differentiate these studies is the strain of rats used. While Feng and co-workers used SpragueDawley rats, the other studies used Wistar rats, which display more maternal behavior upon reunion with their litters (Lehmann and Feldon 2000), possibly buffering potential harmful effects of the separation procedure. 4 Stress Mediators as an Important Cause of Arousal and Sleep Disturbance The regulation of sleep and arousal involves multiple neurotransmitter systems as well as excitatory and inhibitory amino acids, peptides, purines, and neuronal and non-neuronal humoral (i.e., cytokines and prostaglandins) modulators (Steiger and Holsboer 1997; Steiger et al. 1998; Jones 2005; Steiger 2007; Luppi 2010; Espana and Scammell 2011). Many of these same neurotransmitters and neuromodulators are also influenced by and/or mediate the effects of stress and are likely involved in the effects of stress on sleep. This section will briefly review some of the major neurochemical systems that link stress and sleep. 4.1 Hypocretin/Orexin Hypocretin-1 and -2 are a set of neuropeptides that are derived from the same precursor gene and produced by neurons located in the lateral hypothalamus. The hypocretins are also called orexins as they were independently discovered by two Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 391 research groups in 1998 and separately named as hypocretins (de Lecea et al. 1998) or orexins (Sakurai et al. 1998). The hypocretin containing neurons have widespread projections throughout the brain and play a role in a variety of functions including autonomic control, neuroendocrine function, and feeding. Numerous studies have also linked hypocretin to the regulation of the sleep–wake cycle, particularly the induction and maintenance of wakefulness (Kilduff and Peyron 2000; Sutcliffe and de Lecea 2002). Indeed, the hypocretin system activates various well-known wake-active and arousal promoting centers in the brain, including the histaminergic tuberomammilary nucleus, the noradrenergic locus coeruleus (LC), the serotonergic dorsal raphe, and the cholinergic cell clusters in the brainstem and basal forebrain (Peyron et al. 1998b). Impaired hypocretin transmission is a core pathophysiological factor of narcolepsy, a disease characterized by uncontrollable onset of sleep (Nishino et al. 2000; Kornum et al. 2011). Several lines of evidence indicate that hypocretins/orexins may also play a role in the behavioral arousal and neuroendocrine activation associated with stress (Winsky-Sommerer et al. 2005). A close and bidirectional relationship exists between the hypocretin system and the HPA axis. Hypocretins stimulate the activity of the HPA axis in a dose-dependent manner, an effect that seems to be mediated at the hypothalamic level (Kuru et al. 2000; Samson et al. 2002) but not at the adrenal level (Jaszberenyi et al. 2000). Under stressful conditions, a dual hypocretin-1/ hypocretin-2 receptor antagonist does not interfere with corticosterone secretion but does reverse the immediate waking effect of novelty and social stressors (Steiner et al. 2013). In turn, hypothalamic corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) containing neurons project directly to the lateral hypothalamus hypocretin containing neurons, where CRH1 and 2 receptors are abundantly expressed (Winsky-Sommerer et al. 2004). Indeed, studies in mice have shown that exposure to footshock and restraint stress causes an activation of the lateral hypothalamic hypocretin neurons, an effect that is mediated by CRH (Winsky-Sommerer et al. 2005). 4.2 Corticotropin Releasing Hormone CRH is a major mediator of central nervous system responses to stressors (Koob and Bloom 1985; Heinrichs et al. 1995; Koob 1999). Intracerebroventricular (ICV) administration of CRH in rats produces many of the signs associated with anxiety in humans, including increased wakefulness (Ehlers et al. 1986; Marrosu et al. 1990; Chang and Opp 1998), altered locomotor activity, and an exaggerated startle response (Swerdlow et al. 1986; Heilig et al. 1994). By comparison, CRH antagonists attenuate behavioral responses to stress (e.g. Aloisi et al. 1999; Basso et al. 1999; Deak et al. 1999). CRH may not only play an important role in stress-induced wakefulness and arousal, it may also be partly responsible for changes in sleep architecture during the subsequent recovery phase (Gonzalez and Valatx 1997). However, the few studies examining the role of CRH in stress-induced alterations in sleep have 392 L. D. Sanford et al. yielded conflicting data. This is exemplified with the work on restraint stressinduced increases in REM sleep. The ICV administration of the broad CRH antagonist a-helical CRH9–41 prior to restraint stress prevents the subsequent increase in REM, but does not alter spontaneous REM, NREM, or wakefulness in non-stressed rats (Gonzalez and Valatx 1997). In contrast, other investigators found no effect of restraint stress applied at the beginning of the dark period on subsequent sleep, and also found no effect of the CRH antagonist, astressin, on sleep after restraint (Chang and Opp 2002). By comparison, restraint exposure at the onset of the light period increases wakefulness and decreases both NREM and REM, and ICV administration of astressin attenuates the increase in wakefulness over a 5 h-period immediately after the end of restraint but does not alter arousal during the period when restraint was applied (Chang and Opp 2002). There may have been differences in the procedures used for restraint [e.g., whether or not it was conducted in the home cage (Chang and Opp 2002)] that could have produced different results in these studies. A recent study (Kimura et al. 2010) examined baseline and recovery sleep after sleep deprivation in conditional mouse mutants that overexpress CRH in the entire central nervous system or only in the forebrain, including limbic structures. In baseline recordings, homozygous mice with either global or forebrain overexpression of CRH showed increased REM compared to controls and both homozygous and heterozygous mice with global overexpression of CRH showed enhanced recovery REM sleep after 6 h sleep deprivation. However, repeated ICV administration of CRH during prolonged REM sleep deprivation in rats inhibits the expected REM rebound (Machado et al. 2010). Enhanced REM sleep recovery, but not NREM sleep recovery, was blocked by oral administration of the CRH receptor type 1 (CRHR1) antagonist, DMP696, 1 h prior to the end of sleep deprivation. Peripheral stress hormone levels were not elevated during baseline and did not differ across genotypes after sleep deprivation. The authors concluded that enhanced REM sleep in these mice was most likely induced through the activation of CRHR1. Consistent with this conclusion is a report that repeated administration of a-helical CRH9–41 in rats over 10 h of sleep deprivation also reduced the amount of REM sleep recovery (Gonzalez and Valatx 1998). However, there is a separate line of research that demonstrates an inhibiting effect of CRH on REM sleep. Fear conditioning with IS, an uncontrollable stressor, and the presentation of fearful contexts and cues associated with IS are followed by significant reductions in REM that occur in the first few hours after exposure (Sanford et al. 2003a, c). In mice, ICV administration of CRH enhances the reduction in REM sleep following fearful contexts, whereas ICV administration of the non specific CRH antagonist, astressin, attenuates fear-induced reductions in REM (Yang et al. 2009). Training with ES, and reminders of ES, can produce significant enhancements in REM sleep (Sanford et al. 2010). Microinjections of either saline or astressin prior to training produce similar, significant enhancements in post-stress REM sleep relative to a non-shocked handling control condition, whereas the increases in REM sleep are blocked by pretreatment with CRH (Yang Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 393 et al. 2011b). The effect of CRH seems to be relatively specific for REM sleep as changes in NREM sleep and wakefulness were minimal. One potential explanation for differences across studies is that administration of a CRH antagonist simply blocked the initiation of neural processes that would have led to a subsequent increase in REM sleep. 4.3 Prolactin A variety of studies have indicated that prolactin can promote REM sleep. Both systemic and ICV injection of prolactin enhances REM sleep in rats (Roky et al. 1995), whereas administration of a prolactin antiserum reduces the amount REM sleep without affecting NREM sleep (Obal et al. 1992). Also, the amount of REM sleep was found to be reduced in prolactin-deficient mice, which could be reversed by prolactin replacement (Obal et al. 2005). Several studies in laboratory rodents have shown that the plasma level of prolactin increases in response to a wide variety of stressors, including restraint stress and ether exposure (Lenox et al. 1980; Meerlo et al. 2001b) suggesting that it may play a role in the effects of stress on sleep. A comparative study on different strains of mice showed that C57BL/6 J mice and BALB/cJ mice had similar corticosterone responses to restraint stress; however, the effects on prolactin and subsequent sleep were quite different. Restraint stress caused a concomitant increase in prolactin and REM sleep in the C57BL/6 J mice, but not in BALB/cJ mice, which supports the idea that prolactin might be involved in the mechanism underlying restraint stressinduced REM sleep (Meerlo et al. 2001b). Direct evidence for prolactin as a mediator of stress-related increases in REM sleep comes from a study in rats showing that an ether exposure-induced increase in REM sleep could be blocked by hypophysectomy and by ICV administration of an antiserum to prolactin (Bodosi et al. 2000). Other data implicating prolactin in stress-induced alterations in sleep come from a study examining post-stress sleep in REM sleep-deprived rats subsequently submitted to single or repeated sessions of footshock (Machado et al. 2008). REM sleep rebound was greater in the REM sleep-deprived rats that received multiple sessions of footshock, and the increase was associated with higher levels of prolactin (Machado et al. 2008). Together these studies suggest that stressful stimuli and conditions that are associated with strong increases in prolactin levels may be followed by sleep with increased amounts of REM sleep. The precise mechanism of these effects of prolactin remains to be clarified but may involve a direct stimulatory effect of prolactin on cholinergic neurons in the mesopontine tegmental area involved in REM-sleep induction (Takahashi et al. 2000). 394 L. D. Sanford et al. 4.4 Monoamines Serotonin (5-HT) containing neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN), noradrenaline (NA) containing neurons in the LC, and histamine containing neurons in the tuberomammillary nucleus are wake-active and act directly on cortical and subcortical regions to promote wakefulness (Jones 2005). The 5-HT and NA systems are strongly stress-reactive (see discussion below) whereas there has been less work on the role of the histaminergic system. However, it is involved with the regulation of the stress response as central administration of histamine produces increases in adrenocorticotropin and corticosterone (Rudolph et al. 1979; Knigge and Warberg 1991) and blocking histamine synthesis or administration of antagonists block ACTH, beta-endorphin and prolactin responses to some stressors (Rudolph et al. 1979; Seltzer et al. 1986; Knigge and Warberg 1991; Kjaer et al. 1993; Fleckenstein et al. 1994). There also appears to be heterogeneity in specific histaminergic cells groups with respect to responding to different stressors (Miklos and Kovacs 2003). Interestingly, each of these systems has been implicated in the enhancement of REM sleep that typically follows restraint stress. The increase is not found in 5-HT1A knockout mice (Boutrel et al. 2002; Popa et al. 2006) or in mice lacking the 5-HT transporter (Rachalski et al. 2009). Administration of the serotonin synthesis inhibitor para-chlorophenylalanine (Sinha 2006), neurotoxic destruction of noradrenergic cells in LC (Gonzalez et al. 1995), and administration of the histamine H1 receptor antagonist, chlorpheniramine, also prevent the increase in REM sleep induced by restraint in rats (Rojas-Zamorano et al. 2009). However, the actual cause of the attenuation of the REM sleep increase is not yet fully understood. As indicated for CRH, alterations in these systems prior to stress could simply alter the intensity of some elements of the stress response such that the processes that result in the post-restraint increase in REM sleep are not engaged. 5 Brain Regions Linking Stress, Arousal and Sleep As discussed in the above section on stress mediators, there are several points of overlap in the neural regions/systems involved in stress and those directly involved in arousal. This section will focus on the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), two regions not typically considered as direct regulators of arousal and sleep but which play significant roles in mediating the effects of stress on sleep and arousal (see Fig. 2). Stress, Arousal, and Sleep Hippocampus 395 mPFC Amygdala BLA CNA BNST LC DRN PVN Emotional Stress LH Pituitary Adrenal glands Fear-Induced Sympathetic Activation Arousal/ Sleep Fig. 2 This diagram illustrates the principal circuitry (shaded) that we are discussing in this section along with some of their connections to other regions involved in stress and sleep. In this figure, emotional stress would act on the amygdala which would be regulated by the hippocampus (contextual information) and the mPFC (perceived stressor control). BLA would act on CNA and the BNST to regulate the peripheral stress axis via PVN. Output from CNA would also impact LC and DRN, which have roles in regulating REM sleep and arousal as well as in regulating PVN. Both CNA and LC are involved in regulating fear-induced sympathetic activation via effects on LH. This diagram is necessarily incomplete, but illustrates the central role of the amygdala in controlling the stress axis, fear responses, and important components of the arousal system. Heavyweight arrows indicate presumed critical connections for mediating the effects of stress on sleep. Lightweight arrows indicate other connections that may play a role in regulating responses. Dashed arrows indicate indirect pathways. BLA basolateral nucleus of the amygdala, BNST bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, CNA central nucleus of the amygdala, DRN dorsal raphe nucleus, LC locus coeruleus, LH lateral hypothalamus, mPFC medial prefrontal cortex, PVN paraventricular nucleus 5.1 Amygdala and Stress-Induced Alterations in Arousal and Sleep Several lines of research have demonstrated that the amygdala is a significant modulator of sleep. The majority of research on the role of the amygdala in regulating sleep has focused on its influence on REM sleep (e.g. Sanford et al. 1995, 396 L. D. Sanford et al. 1998, 2002; Calvo et al. 1996; Zhu et al. 1998; ); however, a number of studies indicate that the amygdala can influence all sleep–wakefulness states (Sanford et al. 1995; 1998, 2006; Zhu et al. 1998). This influence most likely involves amygdalar projections to thalamic, hypothalamic, and brainstem target regions (Amaral et al. 1992) that are involved in the control of sleep and arousal. These include direct projections via the central nucleus of the amygdala [CNA; e.g. (Krettek and Price 1978; Inagaki et al. 1983; Price et al. 1987; Semba and Fibiger 1992; Peyron et al. 1998a)] and the lateral division of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis [BNST; reviewed in (Amaral et al. 1992; Davis and Whalen 2001)], the source of the major descending outputs of the amygdala to brainstem regions linked to the regulation of REM sleep. The amygdala is important in the regulation of behavioral, physiological, and neuroendocrine responses to stress (Roozendaal et al. 1991a, b; Bohus et al. 1996) and it appears to be a vital interface between stressful events and their impact on sleep and arousal. The BNST is an important relay for the influence of the amygdala on the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus (PVN) (Forray and Gysling 2004), the final common pathway for information influencing the HPA axis (Pacak and Palkovits 2001; Herman et al. 2004) and a key site for integrating neuroendocrine, autonomic, and behavioral responses to stress (Chrousos 1998). GABA-ergic neurons in BNST can directly inhibit PVN and reduce ACTH secretion (Herman et al. 2004). By comparison, CNA has minimal direct projections to PVN (Prewitt and Herman 1998) and lesions of CNA do not directly influence PVN activation (Prewitt and Herman 1997) though CNA can influence PVN via trans-synaptic pathways through the dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus and BNST (Prewitt and Herman 1998). CNA does play a role in regulating the effects of stress on sleep, whereas a possible role for BNST has not been established. Inhibition of the CNA suppresses REM sleep whereas its activation [e.g., with electrical stimulation (Smith and Miskiman 1975)] can promote REM sleep in some situations. For example, functional inactivation of CNA with microinjections of the GABAA agonist, muscimol, produces a relatively selective decrease in REM sleep whereas blocking GABAergic inhibition with the GABAA antagonist, bicuculline, enhances REM sleep (Sanford et al. 2002). Functional lesions of the CNA by TTX, which inactivates both cell bodies and fibers of passage also decrease REM sleep and reduce arousal (Tang et al. 2005c). The decrease in REM sleep can occur without recovery (Tang et al. 2005c), a finding also reported for training with IS and fearful cues and contexts. That stress-induced inactivation of CNA is involved in stress-induced reductions in REM sleep is also suggested by the lack of Fos activation in CNA after conditioned fear (Liu et al. 2003). Functionally, this hypothesis is supported by findings that bicuculline microinjected into CNA attenuates footshock-induced reductions in REM sleep whereas inactivation of CNA with muscimol did not (Liu et al. 2009). However, it should be noted that findings that activation of CNA promotes and inactivation of CNA reduces REM sleep appear at odds with the prevailing conventional view that CNA activation is responsible for regulating fear Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 397 responses via projections to the periaqueductal gray and other brainstem areas [Reviewed in (Duvarci et al. 2011)]. In fact, CNA neurons do fire in response to footshock stress (Rosenkranz et al. 2006) and in response to conditioned stimuli (Duvarci et al. 2011). However, CNA is inhibited by stimulation of the basal and lateral nuclei of the amygdala (Rosenkranz et al. 2006) both of which show high Fos expression after footshock (Liu et al. 2003). Thus, it is possible that CNA activation during fearful/stressful events does regulate fear behavior in wakefulness, but subsequently, with certain stressors, can be inhibited to decrease REM sleep in the post-stress period. The involvement of the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) in the control of sleep is indicated by reports that bilateral electrolytic and chemical lesions of BLA increase NREM sleep and total sleep time in rats (Zhu et al. 1998) and that bilateral chemical lesions of the amygdala produce more consolidated sleep in chair restrained Rhesus monkeys (Benca et al. 2000). Electrical and chemical stimulation of BLA also increase low voltage, high frequency activity in the cortical EEG and decrease NREM sleep and total sleep time, respectively (Dringenberg and Vanderwolf 1996; Zhu et al. 1998). In general, the evidence suggests that CNA is more involved in the regulation of REM sleep than that of NREM sleep and that by comparison, BLA has a greater role in the regulation of NREM sleep and arousal. However, it is important to note that BLA regulates CNA output and therefore likely controls its influences on REM. Fibers from BLA also course through CNA on to the BNST which has brainstem targets similar to those of CNA (Davis and Whalen 2001), thereby providing an additional pathway by which BLA can influence brainstem regions. Indeed, it was recently found that microinjections into BLA of the Group II metabotropic glutamate (mGlu) receptor agonist, LY379268, selectively reduced REM sleep without significantly altering wakefulness or NREM sleep (Dong et al. 2012). By comparison, microinjection of LY379268 into CNA did not significantly alter sleep. Thus, group II mGlu receptors may influence specific cells in BLA that control descending outputs (possibly via CNA or BNST) that in turn regulate REM sleep generator regions in the brainstem. The amygdala (including extended amygdala) is a critical region for the central effects of CRH, and it appears to mediate a number of the anxiogenic effects of CRH as evidenced by intra-amygdala microinjections of CRH agonists and antagonists [Reviewed in (Davis and Whalen 2001)]. For example, local application of CRH or urocortin (Sajdyk et al. 1999) into the BLA in rats produces dose-dependent increases in anxiety behaviors. CRH in the amygdala also plays a significant role in regulating stress-induced alterations in sleep. It was reported that microinjections of the CRHR1 antagonist, antalarmin, into CNA in rats block fear-induced reductions in REM sleep and attenuate Fos expression in regions important in stress and REM sleep regulation including the PVN, LC, and DRN (Liu et al. 2011). Similarly, bilateral microinjections of antalarmin into BLA in rats do not alter spontaneous sleep, but do block the reduction in REM sleep produced by inescapable footshock (Wellman et al. 2013). Further, microinjecting antalarmin into BLA prior to shock training also blocked the 398 L. D. Sanford et al. subsequent effects of contextual fear on REM sleep, but did not block fear memory or behavior as indicated by freezing. These data indicate that CRH receptors within the CNA and BLA are important in the regulation of stress- and fear-induced alterations in REM sleep, and also suggest that BLA plays a role in modulating how stressful memories influence sleep. The hippocampus is also likely to be involved. Information regarding fear conditioned contexts is first processed in the hippocampus and followed by BLA with output through CNA [reviewed in (LeDoux 2000)] and possibly BNST (Davis and Whalen 2001). 5.2 REM Regulatory Regions, Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Stressor Control Stress often has a prominent effect on REM sleep (Sanford et al. 2003a, b, c; Jha et al. 2005). Thus, it is not surprising that brain regions directly implicated in the regulation REM sleep have significant roles in mediating the stress response. These include the LC and DRN. LC noradrenergic neurons and DRN serotonergic neurons are virtually silent during REM sleep and their activation may inhibit its generation (Steriade and McCarley 1990). LC and DRN also have connections to the PVN. PVN receives a large noradrenergic projection from brainstem A1 and A2 groups and a smaller projection from LC (Dunn et al. 2004). However, lesions of LC do reduce ACTH and corticosterone responses to acute stress (Ziegler et al. 1999), and there are suggestions that LC may impact PVN indirectly via limbic structures [reviewed in (Herman and Cullinan 1997)]. DRN has collateral serotonergic projections to CNA and PVN (Petrov et al. 1992, 1994), and 5-HT agonists enhance PVN activity as indicated by increased corticosterone levels and Fos expression (Mikkelsen et al. 2004). Indirect pathways may also play a role in serotonergic regulation of PVN (Herman and Cullinan 1997). As discussed above, stressor controllability appears to be an important parameter in the effects of stress on sleep. Brainstem noradrenergic and serotonergic regions play important roles in stressor controllability. For example, IS in rats produced sustained increases in NA turnover in various brain regions regardless of stress duration, whereas with ES, NA utilization was reduced after rats had learned the coping response (Tsuda et al. 1987). IS also activates DRN serotonergic neurons to a greater degree than ES thereby increasing 5-HT in DRN and in target areas (Amat et al. 1998a, b; Hammack et al. 2002; Bland et al. 2003a, b). Administration of CRH into DRN produced behavioral changes like those seen with IS, whereas microinjection of a nonselective CRH antagonist blocked the behavioral changes normally seen with IS (Hammack et al. 2002, 2003a, b). The application of CRH to LC also increases NA release (Van Bockstaele et al. 1998). The alterations in 5-HT and NA after inescapable and ES are consistent with their putative roles as inhibitory modulators of REM sleep and with the respective increases and decreases in REM sleep after controllable and uncontrollable stress. However, the evaluation of controllability requires assessment and evaluation of Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 399 information at the cortical level (Maier et al. 2006). The mPFC has been found to be a critical region in the perception of control and in mediating the consequences of stress (Maier et al. 2006; Smith and Vale 2006; Akirav and Maroun 2007). For example, blocking activation of the ventral mPFC with muscimol did not alter escape behavior in rats presented with IS, but blocking ventral mPFC in rats presented with ES produced failure in escape learning and greater fear conditioning (Maier et al. 2006). By comparison, activation of ventral mPFC with picrotoxin prior to IS promoted later escape learning in rats provided an opportunity to escape shock in a shuttlebox (Maier et al. 2006). Unfortunately, the role of the mPFC in mediating the effects of stressor controllability on sleep has not been examined. However, part of the influence of mPFC is enacted through its effects on the DRN and possibly LC (Maier et al. 2006), providing a potential substrate for regulating alterations in REM sleep. For example, consistent with the discussion above, activation of mPFC inhibits DRN (Maier et al. 2006; Smith and Vale 2006). The prelimbic mPFC also has robust but restricted projections to the BLA and CNA, whereas the infralimbic mPFC projects to the medial, basomedial, cortical nuclei as well as to the CNA (Vertes 2004). There are projections from the mPFC to GABAergic neurons in the intercalated nuclei which have inhibitory control over CNA output, but there are conflicting reports regarding their specific origin within mPFC (see Vertes 2004). However, these projections from the mPFC to brainstem regulatory regions and the amygdala provide a substrate by which stressor controllability could influence REM sleep. 6 Stress, Sleep and Neuronal Plasticity: Implications for Stress-Related Disorders Both NREM and REM sleep have putative roles in regulating neuronal plasticity and synaptic strength (Benington and Frank 2003; Tononi and Cirelli 2006; Meerlo et al. 2009; Havekes et al. 2012). Stress-induced changes in sleep and sleep architecture might lead to alterations in these plasticity processes and ultimately brain function. In fact, some of the changes in plasticity and brain function traditionally linked to stress may in part be related to alterations in sleep. Work on stress and plasticity has distinguished the effects of acute and chronic stress. Acute stress can impact functional plasticity whereas chronic stress can differentially alter structural plasticity across brain regions. For example, chronic stress results in dendritic atrophy and reductions in spine density in the hippocampus (Magarinos and McEwen 1995a, b; Magarinos et al. 1997; Sandi et al. 2003; Stewart et al. 2005) and prefrontal cortex (Wellman 2001; Cook and Wellman 2004; Radley and Morrison 2005; Liston et al. 2006; Radley et al. 2006). Similar types of chronic stress produce increased dendritic arborization and increased spine density in BLA spiny neurons (Mitra et al. 2005; Vyas et al. 2006) and spine down-regulation in the medial amygdala (Bennur et al. 2007). Some stress-induced changes in the 400 L. D. Sanford et al. hippocampus (Sandi et al. 2003; Stewart et al. 2005) and prefrontal cortex appear to be reversible whereas those in the amygdala are not [Reviewed in (Christoffel et al. 2011)]. Acute restraint, tail shock, and environmental stress impair long-term potentiation (LTP) in the hippocampus (Foy et al. 1987) and acute environmental stress can enhance long-term depression (Xu et al. 1997). However, stress-induced impairment in hippocampal LTP was significantly less in rats allowed to escape footshock than in yoked controls receiving identical shock, but not allowed to escape (Shors et al. 1989). This control mediated attenuation occurred even after a week of daily training sessions with relatively intense shock (30 trials, 1 mA, 1.5 s mean duration). Although both sleep and stress can impact neuronal plasticity, their potential interactions in mediating alterations in plasticity have been minimally explored. The presence of interactions is indicated by the strong effects that stress can have on sleep as well as the demonstrated and hypothesized roles each has in mediating various aspects of plasticity. Importantly, post-stress sleep may have an adaptive function in coping with stress. This is suggested by the directionally different post-stress changes in REM sleep that occur following uncontrollable and controllable stress (Sanford et al. 2010) and the normalization in REM sleep that occurs following fear extinction versus the continued suppression of REM sleep in animals that still show fear (Wellman et al. 2008). These differences suggest that sleep and specific stress parameters may interact in mediating synaptic plasticity associated with stressrelated learning and memory and/or the emotional valence of the memory. Indeed, a variety of authors have made suggestions consistent with this hypothesis, e.g., REM sleep functions to weaken unwanted memory traces in the cortex (Crick and Mitchison 1983); intact REM sleep aids in the processing of memory for trauma (Mellman et al. 2002, 2007); and REM sleep may play an important role in consolidating memories for aversive events and for ‘‘decoupling’’ those memories from their emotional charge (Nishida et al. 2009; Walker 2009). Best et al. proposed that pyramidal neurons in the hippocampus change from a firing pattern that supports LTP in wakefulness to one that supports depotentiation during REM sleep; thereby putatively ‘‘resetting’’ the hippocampus after memories have been transferred to the frontal cortex and clearing the way for the formation of future memories (Best et al. 2007). If true, reductions in REM sleep induced by IS, particularly those that occur without recovery REM sleep (Sanford et al. 2003a, c), could impair this process. Impairment could also involve stress-induced enhancement of NA and 5-HT which facilitate LTP and may impede depotentiation [Reviewed in (Best et al. 2007)]. By comparison, enhancements of REM sleep and corresponding decreased activity in noradrenergic and serotonergic regions could facilitate the adaptive processing of strong memories. Post-stress NREM sleep may also be important. This is suggested by reports that rats trained with IS in an intense learned helplessness paradigm show increased light NREM sleep as well as decreased REM sleep (Adrien et al. 1991). Sleep disturbances both before (Bryant et al. 2010) and after (Lavie 2001) traumatic events have been linked to the development of stress-related pathology. However, it is important to note that stress and the temporary alterations in sleep Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 401 associated with it typically do not give rise to persisting or detrimental effects. This suggests that being able to distinguish normal stress responses from those that can lead to pathology is likely key to fully understanding the processes leading to stressrelated disorders. As stress-induced dysregulation of neuronal plasticity and remodeling of neural circuits are implicated in a variety of psychiatric disorders (McEwen 2007; Christoffel et al. 2011) understanding the role sleep plays in mediating the effects of stress on neuronal plasticity also may be critical for understanding how stress comes to produce persisting and pathological changes in the brain. Acknowledgments The contribution of L.D.S. was supported by NIH research grants MH64827 and EVMS institutional funds. D.S. was supported by research grants from FAPESP (98/14303-3) and Associação Fundo de Incentivo à Pesquisa and she is the recipient of a research fellowship from CNPq (Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico). References Adrien J, Dugovic C, Martin P (1991) Sleep-wakefulness patterns in the helpless rat. Physiol Behav 49:257–262 Akirav I, Maroun M (2007). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex-amygdala circuit in stress effects on the extinction of fear. Neural Plast 2007: 30873 Aloisi AM, Bianchi M, Lupo C, Sacerdote P, Farabollini F (1999) Neuroendocrine and behavioral effects of CRH blockade and stress in male rats. Physiol Behav 66(3):523–528 Amaral D, Price J, Pitkanen A, Carmichael S (1992) Anatomical organization of the primate amydaloid complex. In: Aggleton J (ed) The Amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction. Wiley-Liss, Inc, New York, pp 1–66 Amat J, Matus-Amat P, Watkins LR, Maier SF (1998a) Escapable and inescapable stress differentially alter extracellular levels of 5-HT in the basolateral amygdala of the rat. Brain Res 812(1–2):113–120 Amat J, Matus-Amat P, Watkins LR, Maier SF (1998b) Escapable and inescapable stress differentially and selectively alter extracellular levels of 5-HT in the ventral hippocampus and dorsal periaqueductal gray of the rat. Brain Res 797(1):12–22 Armon G, Shirom A, Shapira I, Melamed S (2008) On the nature of burnout–insomnia relationships: a prospective study of employed adults. J Psychosom Res 65(1):5–12 Badia P, Harsh J, Abbott B (1979) Choosing between predictable and unpredictable shock conditions: data and theory. Psychol Bull 86(5):1107–1131 Basso AM, Spina M, Rivier J, Vale W, Koob GF (1999) Corticotropin-releasing factor antagonist attenuates the ‘‘anxiogenic-like’’ effect in the defensive burying paradigm but not in the elevated plus-maze following chronic cocaine in rats. Psychopharmacology 145(1):21–30 Benca RM, Obermeyer WH, Shelton SE, Droster J, Kalin NH (2000) Effects of amygdala lesions on sleep in rhesus monkeys. Brain Res 879(1–2):130–138 Benington JH, Frank MG (2003) Cellular and molecular connections between sleep and synaptic plasticity. Prog Neurobiol 69(2):71–101 Bennur S, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, Pawlak R, Strickland S, McEwen BS, Chattarji S (2007) Stress-induced spine loss in the medial amygdala is mediated by tissue-plasminogen activator. Neuroscience 144(1):8–16 Best J, Diniz Behn C, Poe GR, Booth V (2007) Neuronal models for sleep–wake regulation and synaptic reorganization in the sleeping hippocampus. J Biol Rhythms 22(3):220–232 402 L. D. Sanford et al. Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC (1969) Crouching as an index of fear. J Comp Physiol Psychol 67(3):370–375 Bland ST, Hargrave D, Pepin JL, Amat J, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2003a) Stressor controllability modulates stress-induced dopamine and serotonin efflux and morphine-induced serotonin efflux in the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:1589–1596 Bland ST, Twining C, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2003b) Stressor controllability modulates stressinduced serotonin but not dopamine efflux in the nucleus accumbens shell. Synapse 49(3):206–208 Bodosi B, Obal F Jr, Gardi J, Komlodi J, Fang J, Krueger JM (2000) An ether stressor increases REM sleep in rats: possible role of prolactin. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 279(5):R1590–R1598 Bohus B, Koolhaas J, Luiten P, Korte S, Roozendaal B, Wiersma A (1996) The neurobiology of the central nucleus of the amygdala in relation to neuroendocrine outflow. Prog Brain Res 107:447–460 Bouton ME (2004) Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn Mem 11(5):485–494 Boutrel B, Monaca C, Hen R, Hamon M, Adrien J (2002) Involvement of 5-HT1A receptors in homeostatic and stress-induced adaptive regulations of paradoxical sleep: studies in 5-HT1A knock-out mice. J Neurosci 22(11):4686–4692 Bryant RA, Creamer M, O’Donnell M, Silove D, McFarlane AC (2010) Sleep disturbance immediately prior to trauma predicts subsequent psychiatric disorder. Sleep 33(1):69–74 Calvo J, Simón-Arceo K, Fernández-Mas R (1996) Prolonged enhancement of REM sleep produced by carbachol microinjection into the amygdala. NeuroRep 7:577–580 Cartwright RD, Wood E (1991) Adjustment disorders of sleep: the sleep effects of a major stressful event and its resolution. Psychiatry Res 39(3):199–209 Chang FC, Opp MR (1998) Blockade of corticotropin-releasing hormone receptors reduces spontaneous waking in the rat. Am J Physiol 275(3 Pt 2):R793–R802 Chang FC, Opp MR (2002) Role of corticotropin-releasing hormone in stressor-induced alterations of sleep in rat. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 283(2):R400–R407 Chang PP, Ford DE, Mead LA, Cooper-Patrick L, Klag MJ (1997) Insomnia in young men and subsequent depression. The Johns Hopkins precursors study. Am J Epidemiol 146(2):105–114 Charuvastra A, Cloitre M (2009) Safe enough to sleep: sleep disruptions associated with trauma, posttraumatic stress, and anxiety in children and adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 18(4):877–891 Cheeta S, Ruigt G, van Proosdij J, Willner P (1997) Changes in sleep architecture following chronic mild stress. Biol Psychiatry 41(4):419–427 Christoffel DJ, Golden SA, Russo SJ (2011) Structural and synaptic plasticity in stress-related disorders. Rev Neurosci 22(5):535–549 Chrousos GP (1998) Stressors, stress, and neuroendocrine integration of the adaptive response. Ann N Y Acad Sci 851:311–335 Chrousos GP (2009) Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature Rev Endocrinol 5(7):374–381 Cohen H, Zohar J, Matar M (2003) The relevance of differential response to trauma in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 53(6):463–473 Cook SC, Wellman CL (2004) Chronic stress alters dendritic morphology in rat medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurobiol 60(2):236–248 Crick F, Mitchison G (1983) The function of dream sleep. Nature 304(5922):111–114 Datta S (2000) Avoidance task training potentiates phasic pontine-wave density in the rat: a mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci 20(22):8607–8613 Davis M, Whalen PJ (2001) The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry 6(1):13–34 de Lecea L, Kilduff TS, Peyron C, Gao X, Foye PE, Danielson PE, Fukuhara C, Battenberg EL, Gautvik VT, Bartlett FS 2nd, Frankel WN, van den Pol AN, Bloom FE, Gautvik KM, Sutcliffe JG (1998) The hypocretins: hypothalamus-specific peptides with neuroexcitatory activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95(1):322–327 Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 403 Deak T, Nguyen KT, Ehrlich AL, Watkins LR, Spencer RL, Maier SF, Licinio J, Wong ML, Chrousos GP, Webster E, Gold PW (1999) The impact of the nonpeptide corticotropin-releasing hormone antagonist antalarmin on behavioral and endocrine responses to stress. Endocrinology 140(1):79–86 Dong E, Wellman LL, Yang L, Sanford LD (2012) Group II metabotropic glutamate receptors in the basal amygdala regulate rapid eye movement sleep. Brain Res 1452:85–95 Dringenberg HC, Vanderwolf CH (1996) Cholinergic activation of the electrocorticogram: an amygdaloid activating system. Exp Brain Res 108(2):285–296 Dunn AJ, Swiergiel AH, Palamarchouk V (2004) Brain circuits involved in corticotropinreleasing factor-norepinephrine interactions during stress. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1018:25–34 Duvarci S, Popa D, Pare D (2011) Central amygdala activity during fear conditioning. J Neurosci 31(1):289–294 Ehlers CL, Reed TK, Henriksen SJ (1986) Effects of corticotropin-releasing factor and growth hormone-releasing factor on sleep and activity in rats. Neuroendocrinology 42(6):467–474 Espana RA, Scammell TE (2011) Sleep neurobiology from a clinical perspective. Sleep 34(7):845–858 Feng P, Hu Y, Vurbic D, Guo Y (2012) Maternal stress induces adult reduced REM sleep and melatonin level. Dev Neurobiol 72(5):677–687 Fleckenstein AE, Lookingland KJ, Moore KE (1994) Histaminergic neurons mediate restraint stress-induced increases in the activity of noradrenergic neurons projecting to the hypothalamus. Brain Res 653(1–2):273–277 Forray MI, Gysling K (2004) Role of noradrenergic projections to the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in the regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Brain Res Rev 47(1–3):145–160 Foy MR, Stanton ME, Levine S, Thompson RF (1987) Behavioral stress impairs long-term potentiation in rodent hippocampus. Behav Neural Biol 48(1):138–149 French D, Palestine D, Leeb C (1972) Preference for a warning in an unavoidable shock situation: replication and extension. Psychol Rep 30:72–74 Fu J, Li P, Ouyang X, Gu C, Song Z, Gao J, Han L, Feng S, Tian S, Hu B (2007) Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation selectively impairs recall of fear extinction in hippocampusindependent tasks in rats. Neuroscience 144(4):1186–1192 Gadinger MC, Fischer JE, Schneider S, Fischer GC, Frank G, Kromm W (2009) Female executives are particularly prone to the sleep-disturbing effect of isolated high-strain jobs: a cross-sectional study in German-speaking executives. J Sleep Res 18(2):229–237 Gliner JA (1972) Predictable versus unpredictable shock: preference behavior and stomach ulceration. Physiol Behav 9(5):693–698 Gonzalez MM, Valatx JL (1997) Effect of intracerebroventricular administration of alpha-helical CRH (9–41) on the sleep/waking cycle in rats under normal conditions or after subjection to an acute stressful stimulus. J Sleep Res 6(3):164–170 Gonzalez MM, Valatx JL (1998) Involvement of stress in the sleep rebound mechanism induced by sleep deprivation in the rat: use of alpha-helical CRH (9–41). Behav Pharmacol 9(8):655–662 Gonzalez MM, Debilly G, Valatx JL, Jouvet M (1995) Sleep increase after immobilization stress: role of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus system in the rat. Neurosci Lett 202(1–2):5–8 Gronli J, Murison R, Bjorvatn B, Sorensen E, Portas CM, Ursin R (2004) Chronic mild stress affects sucrose intake and sleep in rats. Behav Brain Res 150(1–2):139–147 Hammack SE, Richey KJ, Schmid MJ, LoPresti ML, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2002) The role of corticotropin-releasing hormone in the dorsal raphe nucleus in mediating the behavioral consequences of uncontrollable stress. J Neurosci 22(3):1020–1026 Hammack SE, Pepin JL, DesMarteau JS, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2003a) Low doses of corticotropinreleasing hormone injected into the dorsal raphe nucleus block the behavioral consequences of uncontrollable stress. Behav Brain Res 147(1–2):55–64 Hammack SE, Schmid MJ, LoPresti ML, Der-Avakian A, Pellymounter MA, Foster AC, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2003b) Corticotropin releasing hormone type 2 receptors in the dorsal 404 L. D. Sanford et al. raphe nucleus mediate the behavioral consequences of uncontrollable stress. J Neurosci 23(3):1019–1025 Havekes R, Vecsey CG, Abel T (2012) The impact of sleep deprivation on neuronal and glial signaling pathways important for memory and synaptic plasticity. Cell Signal 24(6): 1251–1260 Heilig M, Koob G, Ekman R, Britton K (1994) Corticotropin-releasing factor and neuropeptide Y: role in emotional integration. Trends Neurosci 17:80–85 Heinrichs SC, Menzaghi F, Merlo Pich E, Britton KT, Koob GF (1995) The role of CRF in behavioral aspects of stress. Ann N Y Acad Sci 771:92–104 Herman JP, Cullinan WE (1997) Neurocircuitry of stress: central control of the hypothalamopituitary-adrenocortical axis. Trends Neurosci 20(2):78–84 Herman JP, Mueller NK, Figueiredo H (2004) Role of GABA and glutamate circuitry in hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical stress integration. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1018:35–45 Inagaki S, Kawai Y, Matsuzaki T, Shiosaka S, Tohyama M (1983) Precise terminal fields of the descending somatostatinergic neuron system from the amygdaloid complex of the rat. J Hirnforsch 24(3):345–356 Irmis F, Lâat J, Radil-Weiss T (1971) Individual differences in hippocampal EEG during rhombencephalic sleep and arousal. Physiol Behav 7(1):117–119 Irmis F, Lâat J, Radil-Weiss T (1974) Individual (constitutional) differences in sleep patterns in rats. Behav Res Therapy 12:245–249 Jaszberenyi M, Bujdoso E, Pataki I, Telegdy G (2000) Effects of orexins on the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal system. J Neuroendocrinol 12(12):1174–1178 Jha SK, Brennan FX, Pawlyk AC, Ross RJ, Morrison AR (2005) REM sleep: a sensitive index of fear conditioning in rats. Eur J Neurosci 21(4):1077–1080 Johnson EO, Kamilaris TC, Chrousos GP, Gold PW (1992) Mechanisms of stress: a dynamic overview of hormonal and behavioral homeostasis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 16(2):115–130 Jones BE (2005) From waking to sleeping: neuronal and chemical substrates. Trends Pharmacol Sci 26(11):578–586 Kant GJ, Pastel RH, Bauman RA, Meininger GR, Maughan KR, Robinson TN 3rd, Wright WL, Covington PS (1995) Effects of chronic stress on sleep in rats. Physiol Behav 57(2):359–365 Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW 3rd, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS (2004) Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303(5660):1023–1026 Kilduff TS, Peyron C (2000) The hypocretin/orexin ligand-receptor system: implications for sleep and sleep disorders. Trends Neurosci 23(8):359–365 Kimura M, Muller-Preuss P, Lu A, Wiesner E, Flachskamm C, Wurst W, Holsboer F, Deussing JM (2010) Conditional corticotropin-releasing hormone overexpression in the mouse forebrain enhances rapid eye movement sleep. Mol Psychiatry 15(2):154–165 Kishimoto T, Radulovic J, Radulovic M, Lin CR, Schrick C, Hooshmand F, Hermanson O, Rosenfeld MG, Spiess J (2000) Deletion of CRHR2 reveals an anxiolytic role for corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor-2. Nat Genet 24(4):415–419 Kjaer A, Knigge U, Madsen EL, Soe-Jensen P, Bach FW, Warberg J (1993) Insulin/hypoglycemiainduced adrenocorticotropin and beta-endorphin release: involvement of hypothalamic histaminergic neurons. Endocrinology 132(5):2213–2220 Knigge U, Warberg J (1991) The role of histamine in the neuroendocrine regulation of pituitary hormone secretion. Acta Endocrinol 124(6):609–619 Koob GF (1999) Corticotropin-releasing factor, norepinephrine, and stress. Biol Psychiatry 46(9):1167–1180 Koob G, Bloom F (1985) Corticotropin-releasing factor and behavior. Fed Proc 44:259–263 Koolhaas JM, Bartolomucci A, Buwalda B, de Boer SF, Flugge G, Korte SM, Meerlo P, Murison R, Olivier B, Palanza P, Richter-Levin G, Sgoifo A, Steimer T, Stiedl O, van Dijk G, Wohr M, Fuchs E (2011) Stress revisited: a critical evaluation of the stress concept. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35(5):1291–1301 Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 405 Koren D, Arnon I, Lavie P, Klein E (2002) Sleep complaints as early predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder: a 1-year prospective study of injured survivors of motor vehicle accidents. Am J Psychiatry 159:855–857 Kornum BR, Faraco J, Mignot E (2011) Narcolepsy with hypocretin/orexin deficiency, infections and autoimmunity of the brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21(6):897–903 Krettek JE, Price JL (1978) Amygdaloid projections to subcortical structures within the basal forebrain and brainstem in the rat and cat. J Comp Neurol 178(2):225–254 Kuru M, Ueta Y, Serino R, Nakazato M, Yamamoto Y, Shibuya I, Yamashita H (2000) Centrally administered orexin/hypocretin activates HPA axis in rats. NeuroReport 11(9):1977–1980 Lavie P (2001) Sleep disturbances in the wake of traumatic events. N Engl J Med 345:1825–1832 LeDoux JE (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:155–184 Lehmann J, Feldon J (2000) Long-term biobehavioral effects of maternal separation in the rat: consistent or confusing? Rev Neurosci 11(4):383–408 Lenox RH, Kant GJ, Sessions GR, Pennington LL, Mougey EH, Meyerhoff JL (1980) Specific hormonal and neurochemical responses to different stressors. Neuroendocrinology 30(5):300–308 Levine S (2005) Developmental determinants of sensitivity and resistance to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30(10):939–946 Liston C, Miller MM, Goldwater DS, Radley JJ, Rocher AB, Hof PR, Morrison JH, McEwen BS (2006) Stress-induced alterations in prefrontal cortical dendritic morphology predict selective impairments in perceptual attentional set-shifting. J Neurosci 26(30):7870–7874 Liu X, Tang X, Sanford LD (2003) Fear-conditioned suppression of REM sleep: relationship to Fos expression patterns in limbic and brainstem regions in BALB/cJ mice. Brain Res 991(1–2):1–17 Liu X, Yang L, Wellman LL, Tang X, Sanford LD (2009) GABAergic antagonism of the central nucleus of the amygdala attenuates reductions in rapid eye movement sleep after inescapable footshock stress. Sleep 32(7):888–896 Liu X, Wellman LL, Yang L, Ambrozewicz MA, Tang X, Sanford LD (2011) Antagonizing corticotropin-releasing factor in the central nucleus of the amygdala attenuates fear-induced reductions in sleep but not freezing. Sleep 34(11):1539–1549 Luppi PH (2010) Neurochemical aspects of sleep regulation with specific focus on slow-wave sleep. World J Biol Psychiatry 11(Suppl 1):4–8 Machado RB, Tufik S, Suchecki D (2008) Chronic stress during paradoxical sleep deprivation increases paradoxical sleep rebound: association with prolactin plasma levels and brain serotonin content. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33(9):1211–1224 Machado RB, Tufik S, Suchecki D (2010) Modulation of Sleep Homeostasis by Corticotropin Releasing Hormone in REM Sleep-Deprived Rats. Int J Endocrinol 2010:326151 Machida M, Yang L, Wellman LL, Sanford LD (2013) Effects of stressor predictability on escape learning and sleep in mice. Sleep 36:421–430 Magarinos AM, McEwen BS (1995a) Stress-induced atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal CA3c neurons: comparison of stressors. Neuroscience 69(1):83–88 Magarinos AM, McEwen BS (1995b) Stress-induced atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal CA3c neurons: involvement of glucocorticoid secretion and excitatory amino acid receptors. Neuroscience 69(1):89–98 Magarinos AM, Verdugo JM, McEwen BS (1997) Chronic stress alters synaptic terminal structure in hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94(25):14002–14008 Maier SF, Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, Watkins LR (2006) Behavioral control, the medial prefrontal cortex, and resilience. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8(4):397–406 Marrosu F, Gessa GL, Giagheddu M, Fratta W (1990) Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) increases paradoxical sleep (PS) rebound in PS-deprived rats. Brain Res 515(1–2):315–318 McEwen BS (2007) Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev 87(3):873–904 Meerlo P, Turek FW (2001) Effects of social stimuli on sleep in mice: non-rapid-eye-movement (NREM) sleep is promoted by aggressive interaction but not by sexual interaction. Brain Res 907(1–2):84–92 406 L. D. Sanford et al. Meerlo P, Pragt BJ, Daan S (1997) Social stress induces high intensity sleep in rats. Neurosci Lett 225(1):41–44 Meerlo P, de Bruin EA, Strijkstra AM, Daan S (2001a) A social conflict increases EEG slowwave activity during subsequent sleep. Physiol Behav 73(3):331–335 Meerlo P, Easton A, Bergmann BM, Turek FW (2001b) Restraint increases prolactin and REM sleep in C57BL/6 J mice but not in BALB/cJ mice. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 281(3):R846–R854 Meerlo P, Mistlberger RE, Jacobs BL, Heller HC, McGinty D (2009) New neurons in the adult brain: the role of sleep and consequences of sleep loss. Sleep Med Rev 13(3):187–194 Mellman TA, Bustamante V, Fins AI, Pigeon WR, Nolan B (2002) REM sleep and the early development of posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 159(10):1696–1701 Mellman TA, Pigeon WR, Nowell PD, Nolan B (2007) Relationships between REM sleep findings and PTSD symptoms during the early aftermath of trauma. J Trauma Stress 20(5):893–901 Mikkelsen JD, Hay-Schmidt A, Kiss A (2004) Serotonergic stimulation of the rat hypothalamopituitary-adrenal axis: interaction between 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1018:65–70 Miklos IH, Kovacs KJ (2003) Functional heterogeneity of the responses of histaminergic neuron subpopulations to various stress challenges. Eur J Neurosci 18(11):3069–3079 Miller RR, Daniel D, Berk AM (1974) Successive reversals of a discriminated preference for signaled tailshock. Anim Learn Behav 2(4):271–274 Mitra R, Jadhav S, McEwen BS, Vyas A, Chattarji S (2005) Stress duration modulates the spatiotemporal patterns of spine formation in the basolateral amygdala. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102(26):9371–9376 Mrdalj J, Pallesen S, Milde AM, Jellestad FK, Murison R, Ursin R, Bjorvatn B, Gronli J (2013) Early and later life stress alter brain activity and sleep in rats. PLoS One 8(7):e69923 Myers KM, Davis M (2007) Mechanisms of fear extinction. Mol Psychiatry 12(2):120–150 Neckelmann D, Mykletun A, Dahl AA (2007) Chronic insomnia as a risk factor for developing anxiety and depression. Sleep 30(7):873–880 Nishida M, Pearsall J, Buckner RL, Walker MP (2009) REM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memory. Cereb Cortex 19(5):1158–1166 Nishino S, Ripley B, Overeem S, Lammers GJ, Mignot E (2000) Hypocretin (orexin) deficiency in human narcolepsy. Lancet 355(9197):39–40 Nomura K, Nakao M, Takeuchi T, Yano E (2009) Associations of insomnia with job strain, control, and support among male Japanese workers. Sleep Med 10(6):626–629 Obal F Jr, Kacsoh B, Alfoldi P, Payne L, Markovic O, Grosvenor C, Krueger JM (1992) Antiserum to prolactin decreases rapid eye movement sleep (REM sleep) in the male rat. Physiol Behav 52(6):1063–1068 Obal F Jr, Garcia-Garcia F, Kacsoh B, Taishi P, Bohnet S, Horseman ND, Krueger JM (2005) Rapid eye movement sleep is reduced in prolactin-deficient mice. J Neurosci 25(44):10282–10289 Pacak K, Palkovits M (2001) Stressor specificity of central neuroendocrine responses: implications for stress-related disorders. Endocr Rev 22(4):502–548 Palma BD, Suchecki D, Tufik S (2000) Differential effects of acute cold and footshock on the sleep of rats. Brain Res 861(1):97–104 Pawlyk AC, Morrison AR, Ross RJ, Brennan FX (2008) Stress-induced changes in sleep in rodents: Models and mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32(1):99–117 Paylor R, Tracy R, Wehner J, Rudy J (1994) DBA/2 and C57BL/6 mice differ in contextual fear but not auditory fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 108:810–817 Petrov T, Krukoff TL, Jhamandas JH (1992) The hypothalamic paraventricular and lateral parabrachial nuclei receive collaterals from raphe nucleus neurons: a combined double retrograde and immunocytochemical study. J Comp Neurol 318(1):18–26 Petrov T, Krukoff TL, Jhamandas JH (1994) Chemically defined collateral projections from the pons to the central nucleus of the amygdala and hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus in the rat. Cell Tissue Res 277(2):289–295 Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 407 Peyron C, Petit JM, Rampon C, Jouvet M, Luppi PH (1998a) Forebrain afferents to the rat dorsal raphe nucleus demonstrated by retrograde and anterograde tracing methods. J Neurosci 82(2):443–468 Peyron C, Tighe DK, van den Pol AN, de Lecea L, Heller HC, Sutcliffe JG, Kilduff TS (1998b) Neurons containing hypocretin (orexin) project to multiple neuronal systems. J Neurosci 18(23):9996–10015 Phillips RG, LeDoux JE (1992) Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 106(2):274–285 Pitman RK, Shin LM, Rauch SL (2001) Investigating the pathogenesis of posttraumatic stress disorder with neuroimaging. J Clin Psychiatry 62(Suppl 17):47–54 Popa D, El Yacoubi M, Vaugeois JM, Hamon M, Adrien J (2006) Homeostatic regulation of sleep in a genetic model of depression in the mouse: effects of muscarinic and 5-HT1A receptor activation. Neuropsychopharmacology 31(8):1637–1646 Prewitt CM, Herman JP (1997) Hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical regulation following lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala. Stress 1(4):263–280 Prewitt CM, Herman JP (1998) Anatomical interactions between the central amygdaloid nucleus and the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus of the rat: a dual tract-tracing analysis. J Chem Neuroanat 15(3):173–185 Price J, Russchen F, Amaral D (1987). The limbic region. II: the amygdaloid complex. In: Swanson L (ed) Handbook of chemical neuroanatomy. Integrated systems of the CNA, Part I. Elsevier, New York, pp 279–375 Rachalski A, Alexandre C, Bernard JF, Saurini F, Lesch KP, Hamon M, Adrien J, Fabre V (2009) Altered sleep homeostasis after restraint stress in 5-HTT knock-out male mice: a role for hypocretins. J Neurosci 29(49):15575–15585 Radley JJ, Morrison JH (2005) Repeated stress and structural plasticity in the brain. Ageing Res Rev 4(2):271–287 Radley JJ, Rocher AB, Miller M, Janssen WG, Liston C, Hof PR, McEwen BS, Morrison JH (2006) Repeated stress induces dendritic spine loss in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 16(3):313–320 Rampin C, Cespuglio R, Chastrette N, Jouvet M (1991) Immobilisation stress induces a paradoxical sleep rebound in rat. Neurosci Lett 126(2):113–118 Rojas-Zamorano JA, Esqueda-Leon E, Jimenez-Anguiano A, Cintra-McGlone L, Mendoza Melendez MA, Velazquez Moctezuma J (2009) The H1 histamine receptor blocker, chlorpheniramine, completely prevents the increase in REM sleep induced by immobilization stress in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 91(3):291–294 Roky R, Obâal F Jr, Valatx JL, Bredow S, Fang J, Pagano LP, Krueger JM (1995) Prolactin and rapid eye movement sleep regulation. Sleep 18(7):536–542 Roozendaal B, Koolhaus J, Bohus B (1991a) Attenuated cardiovascular, neuroendocrine, and behavioral responses after a single footshock in central amygdaloid lesioned male rats. Phys Behav 50:771–775 Roozendaal B, Koolhaus J, Bohus B (1991b) Central amygdala lesions affect behavioral and autonomic balance during stress in rats. Phys Behav 50:777–781 Rosenkranz JA, Buffalari DM, Grace AA (2006) Opposing influence of basolateral amygdala and footshock stimulation on neurons of the central amygdala. Biol Psychiatry 59(9):801–811 Rudolph C, Richards GE, Kaplan S, Ganong WF (1979) Effect of intraventricular histamine on hormone secretion in dogs. Neuroendocrinology 29(3):169–177 Sajdyk TJ, Schober DA, Gehlert DR, Shekhar A (1999) Role of corticotropin-releasing factor and urocortin within the basolateral amygdala of rats in anxiety and panic responses. Behav Brain Res 100(1–2):207–215 Sakurai T, Amemiya A, Ishii M, Matsuzaki I, Chemelli RM, Tanaka H, Williams SC, Richarson JA, Kozlowski GP, Wilson S, Arch JR, Buckingham RE, Haynes AC, Carr SA, Annan RS, McNulty DE, Liu WS, Terrett JA, Elshourbagy NA, Bergsma DJ, Yanagisawa M (1998). Orexins and orexin receptors: a family of hypothalamic neuropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors that regulate feeding behavior. Cell 92(5):1 page following 696 408 L. D. Sanford et al. Samson WK, Taylor MM, Follwell M, Ferguson AV (2002) Orexin actions in hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus: physiological consequences and cellular correlates. Regul Pept 104(1–3):97–103 Sandi C, Davies HA, Cordero MI, Rodriguez JJ, Popov VI, Stewart MG (2003) Rapid reversal of stress induced loss of synapses in CA3 of rat hippocampus following water maze training. Eur J Neurosci 17(11):2447–2456 Sanford LD, Tejani-Butt SM, Ross RJ, Morrison AR (1995) Amygdaloid control of alerting and behavioral arousal in rats: involvement of serotonergic mechanisms. Arch Ital Biol 134(1):81–99 Sanford LD, Nassar P, Ross RJ, Schulkin J, Morrison AR (1998) Prolactin microinjections into the amygdalar central nucleus lead to decreased NREM sleep. Sleep Res Online 1(3):109–113 Sanford LD, Silvestri AJ, Ross RJ, Morrison AR (2001) Influence of fear conditioning on elicited ponto-geniculo-occipital waves and rapid eye movement sleep. Arch Ital Biol 139(3):169–183 Sanford LD, Parris B, Tang X (2002) GABAergic regulation of the central nucleus of the amygdala: implications for sleep control. Brain Res 956(2):276–284 Sanford L, Yang L, Tang X (2003a) Influence of contextual fear on sleep architecture in mice: a strain comparison. Sleep 26:527–540 Sanford LD, Fang J, Tang X (2003b) Sleep after differing amounts of conditioned fear training in BALB/cJ mice. Behav Brain Res 147(1–2):193–202 Sanford LD, Tang X, Ross RJ, Morrison AR (2003c) Influence of shock training and explicit fearconditioned cues on sleep architecture in mice: strain comparison. Behav Genet 33(1):43–58 Sanford LD, Xiao J, Liu X, Yang L, Tang X (2005) Influence of avoidance training (AT) and AT cues on sleep in C57BL/6 J (B6) and BALB/cJ (C) mice. Sleep (Abstract Supplement) 28:A6 Sanford LD, Yang L, Liu X, Tang X (2006) Effects of tetrodotoxin (TTX) inactivation of the central nucleus of the amygdala (CNA) on dark period sleep and activity. Brain Res 1084(1):80–88 Sanford LD, Yang L, Wellman LL, Liu X, Tang X (2010) Differential effects of controllable and uncontrollable footshock stress on sleep in mice. Sleep 33(5):621–630 Schiffelholz T, Aldenhoff JB (2002) Novel object presentation affects sleep–wake behavior in rats. Neurosci Lett 328(1):41–44 Seltzer AM, Donoso AO, Podesta E (1986) Restraint stress stimulation of prolactin and ACTH secretion: role of brain histamine. Physiol Behav 36(2):251–255 Semba K, Fibiger HC (1992) Afferent connections of the laterodorsal and the pedunculopontine tegmental nuclei in the rat: a retro- and antero-grade transport and immunohistochemical study. J Comp Neurol 323(3):387–410 Shors TJ, Seib TB, Levine S, Thompson RF (1989) Inescapable versus escapable shock modulates long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Science 244(4901):224–226 Silvestri AJ (2005) REM sleep deprivation affects extinction of cued but not contextual fear conditioning. Physiol Behav 84(3):343–349 Sinha RK (2006) P-CPA pretreatment reverses the changes in sleep and behavior following acute immobilization stress rats. Journal Physiol Sci: JPS 56(1):123–129 Smith C (1995) Sleep states and memory processes. Behav Brain Res 69(1–2):137–145 Smith C, Lapp L (1986) Prolonged increases in both PS and number of REMS following a shuttle avoidance task. Physiol Behav 36(6):1053–1057 Smith CT, Miskiman DE (1975) Increases in paradoxical sleep as a result of amygdaloid stimulation. Physiol Behav 15(1):17–19 Smith SM, Vale WW (2006) The role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in neuroendocrine responses to stress. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8(4):383–395 Steiger A (2007) Neurochemical regulation of sleep. J Psychiatr Res 41(7):537–552 Steiger A, Holsboer F (1997) Neuropeptides and human sleep. Sleep 20(11):1038–1052 Steiger A, Antonijevic IA, Bohlhalter S, Frieboes RM, Friess E, Murck H (1998) Effects of hormones on sleep. Horm Res 49(3–4):125–130 Steiner MA, Sciarretta C, Brisbare-Roch C, Strasser DS, Studer R, Jenck F (2013) Examining the role of endogenous orexins in hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis endocrine function using Stress, Arousal, and Sleep 409 transient dual orexin receptor antagonism in the rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38(4):560–571 Steriade M, McCarley R (1990) Brainstem control of wakefulness and sleep. Plenum Press, New York Stewart MG, Davies HA, Sandi C, Kraev IV, Rogachevsky VV, Peddie CJ, Rodriguez JJ, Cordero MI, Donohue HS, Gabbott PL, Popov VI (2005) Stress suppresses and learning induces plasticity in CA3 of rat hippocampus: a three-dimensional ultrastructural study of thorny excrescences and their postsynaptic densities. Neuroscience 131(1):43–54 Suchecki D, Tiba PA, Machado RB (2012) REM sleep rebound as an adaptive response to stressful situations. Frontiers Neurol 3:41 Sutcliffe JG, de Lecea L (2002) The hypocretins: setting the arousal threshold. Nat Rev Neurosci 3(5):339–349 Swerdlow N, Geyer M, Vale W, Koob G (1986) Corticotropin-releasing factor potentiates acoustic startle in rats: blockade by chlordiazepoxide. Psychopharmacology 88:147–152 Takahashi K, Koyama Y, Kayama Y, Yamamoto M (2000) The effects of prolactin on the mesopontine tegmental neurons. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 54(3):257–258 Tang X, Xiao J, Liu X, Sanford LD (2004) Strain differences in the influence of open field exposure on sleep in mice. Behav Brain Res 154(1):137–147 Tang X, Liu X, Yang L, Sanford LD (2005a) Rat strain differences in sleep after acute mild stressors and short-term sleep loss. Behav Brain Res 160(1):60–71 Tang X, Xiao J, Parris BS, Fang J, Sanford LD (2005b) Differential effects of two types of environmental novelty on activity and sleep in BALB/cJ and C57BL/J mice. Physiol Behav 85:419–429 Tang X, Yang L, Liu X, Sanford LD (2005c) Influence of tetrodotoxin inactivation of the central nucleus of the amygdala on sleep and arousal. Sleep 28(8):923–930 Tang X, Yang L, Sanford LD (2005d) Rat strain differences in freezing and sleep alterations associated with contextual fear. Sleep 28(10):1235–1244 Tang X, Yang L, Sanford LD (2006) Spectral EEG power after uncontrollable shock (US) and fearful context (FC): variability amongst mouse strains. Sleep 29:A11 Tang X, Yang L, Sanford LD (2007) Individual variation in sleep and motor activity in rats. Behav Brain Res 180(1):62–68 Tiba PA, Tufik S, Suchecki D (2004) Effects of maternal separation on baseline sleep and cold stress-induced sleep rebound in adult Wistar rats. Sleep 27(6):1146–1153 Tiba PA, Tufik S, Suchecki D (2008) Long lasting alteration in REM sleep of female rats submitted to long maternal separation. Physiol Behav 93(3):444–452 Tononi G, Cirelli C (2006) Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev 10(1):49–62 Tsuda A, Ida Y, Tsujimaru S, Satoh H, Nishimura H, Tanaka M (1987) Stressor controllability and brain noradrenaline turnover in rats. Yakubutsu Seishin Kodo 7(3):363–374 Van Bockstaele EJ, Colago EE, Valentino RJ (1998) Amygdaloid corticotropin-releasing factor targets locus coeruleus dendrites: substrate for the co-ordination of emotional and cognitive limbs of the stress response. J Neuroendocrinol 10(10):743–757 Vertes RP (2004) Differential projections of the infralimbic and prelimbic cortex in the rat. Synapse 51(1):32–58 Vyas A, Jadhav S, Chattarji S (2006) Prolonged behavioral stress enhances synaptic connectivity in the basolateral amygdala. Neuroscience 143(2):387–393 Walker MP (2009) The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1156:168–197 Wellman CL (2001) Dendritic reorganization in pyramidal neurons in medial prefrontal cortex after chronic corticosterone administration. J Neurobiol 49(3):245–253 Wellman LL, Holbrook BD, Yang L, Tang X, Sanford LD (2008) Contextual fear extinction eliminates sleep disturbances found following fear conditioning in rats. Sleep 31:1035–1042 Wellman LL, Ambrozewicz MA, Yang L, Machida M, Sanford LD (2013) Basolateral amygdala and the regulation of fear conditioned changes in sleep: role of corticotropin releasing factor. Sleep 36:471–480 410 L. D. Sanford et al. Winsky-Sommerer R, Yamanaka A, Diano S, Borok E, Roberts AJ, Sakurai T, Kilduff TS, Horvath TL, de Lecea L (2004) Interaction between the corticotropin-releasing factor system and hypocretins (orexins): a novel circuit mediating stress response. J Neurosci 24(50):11439–11448 Winsky-Sommerer R, Boutrel B, de Lecea L (2005) Stress and arousal: the corticotrophinreleasing factor/hypocretin circuitry. Mol Neurobiol 32(3):285–294 Xu L, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ (1997) Behavioural stress facilitates the induction of long-term depression in the hippocampus. Nature 387(6632):497–500 Yang L, Tang X, Wellman LL, Liu X, Sanford LD (2009) Corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) modulates fear-induced alterations in sleep in mice. Brain Res 1276:112–122 Yang L, Wellman LL, Ambrozewicz MA, Sanford LD (2011a) Effects of stressor predictability and controllability on sleep, temperature, and fear behavior in mice. Sleep 34(6):759–771 Yang L, Wellman LL, Tang X, Sanford LD (2011b) Effects of corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) on sleep and body temperature following controllable footshock stress in mice. Physiol Behav 104(5):886–892 Zhu GQ, Zhong MK, Zhang JX, Zhao LZ, Ke DP, Wang M, Shi L (1998) Role of basolateral amygdaloid nuclei in sleep and wakeful state regulation. Sheng Li Xue Bao 50(6):688–692 Ziegler DR, Cass WA, Herman JP (1999) Excitatory influence of the locus coeruleus in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis responses to stress. J Neuroendocrinol 11(5):361–369