Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
Larry D. Sanford, Deborah Suchecki and Peter Meerlo
Abstract Stress is considered to be an important cause of disrupted sleep and
insomnia. However, controlled and experimental studies in rodents indicate that
effects of stress on sleep–wake regulation are complex and may strongly depend on
the nature of the stressor. While most stressors are associated with at least a brief
period of arousal and wakefulness, the subsequent amount and architecture of
recovery sleep can vary dramatically across conditions even though classical
markers of acute stress such as corticosterone are virtually the same. Sleep after
stress appears to be highly influenced by situational variables including whether the
stressor was controllable and/or predictable, whether the individual had the possibility to learn and adapt, and by the relative resilience and vulnerability of the
individual experiencing stress. There are multiple brain regions and neurochemical
systems linking stress and sleep, and the specific balance and interactions between
these systems may ultimately determine the alterations in sleep–wake architecture.
Factors that appear to play an important role in stress-induced wakefulness and sleep
changes include various monominergic neurotransmitters, hypocretins, corticotropin releasing factor, and prolactin. In addition to the brain regions directly involved
in stress responses such as the hypothalamus, the locus coeruleus, and the amygdala,
differential effects of stressor controllability on behavior and sleep may be mediated
by the medial prefrontal cortex. These various brain regions interact and influence
each other and in turn affect the activity of sleep–wake controlling centers in the
brain. Also, these regions likely play significant roles in memory processes and
L. D. Sanford (&)
Department of Pathology and Anatomy, Eastern Virginia Medical School,
P.O. Box 1980, Norfolk, VA 23507, USA
e-mail: SanforLD@evms.edu
D. Suchecki
Departamento de Psicobiologia, Universidade Federal de Sao Paulo,
Sao Paulo, Brazil
P. Meerlo
Center for Behavior and Neurosciences, University of Groningen,
Groningen, The Netherlands
Ó Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014
Curr Topics Behav Neurosci (2015) 25: 379–410
DOI 10.1007/7854_2014_314
Published Online: 23 May 2014
379
380
L. D. Sanford et al.
participate in the way stressful memories may affect arousal and sleep. Finally,
stress-induced changes in sleep-architecture may affect sleep-related neuronal
plasticity processes and thereby contribute to cognitive dysfunction and psychiatric
disorders.
Keywords Stress
differences Fear
Controllability Predictability Vulnerability Individual
Arousal Sleep disturbance Insomnia Psychopathology
Contents
1
2
Introduction........................................................................................................................
Complex Effects of Stress on Sleep Architecture............................................................
2.1 Effects of Acute Stress .............................................................................................
2.2 Repeated or Chronic Stress ......................................................................................
3 Effects on Sleep May Vary with Specific Stress Parameters..........................................
3.1 Stressor Controllability .............................................................................................
3.2 Stressor Predictability ...............................................................................................
3.3 Stress-Related Learning and Sleep ..........................................................................
3.4 Fear Extinction and Sleep ........................................................................................
3.5 Stressor Resilience and Vulnerability to Sleep Disturbance...................................
4 Stress Mediators as an Important Cause of Arousal and Sleep Disturbance..................
4.1 Hypocretin/Orexin ....................................................................................................
4.2 Corticotropin Releasing Hormone............................................................................
4.3 Prolactin ....................................................................................................................
4.4 Monoamines..............................................................................................................
5 Brain Regions Linking Stress, Arousal and Sleep...........................................................
5.1 Amygdala and Stress-Induced Alterations in Arousal and Sleep...........................
5.2 REM Regulatory Regions, Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Stressor Control ..........
6 Stress, Sleep and Neuronal Plasticity: Implications for Stress-Related Disorders .........
References................................................................................................................................
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
390
391
393
394
394
395
398
399
401
1 Introduction
Stress is generally viewed as a major cause of disrupted sleep. Traumatic life events
often result in sleep disturbances that may include insomnia or subjective sleep
problems (Lavie 2001) and the persistence of these disturbances may be predictive
of the future development of emotional and cognitive disorders (Chang et al. 1997;
Koren et al. 2002; Neckelmann et al. 2007). Understandably, given the practical and
ethical concerns, few controlled and experimental studies on severe stress and its
effect on subsequent sleep have been done in human subjects. As such, most of the
available data on stress and sleep have come from studies in laboratory rodents
(Pawlyk et al. 2008). Importantly, the large body of animal studies based on a wide
variety of experimental stress models indicates that effects of stress on sleep may be
far more complex than a simple and prolonged increase of wakefulness. The impact
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
381
of stress on sleep may vary with specific characteristics of a stressor (e.g., duration,
intensity, controllability, and predictability) and with characteristics of the individual experiencing stress (e.g., individual stress coping strategies, relative resilience, and vulnerability). In addition, any stressful situation provides an opportunity
for learning, and the success or failure of an organism in developing an adaptive
coping strategy can influence post-stress sleep and behavior. Subsequently, evoking
stress-related memories can impact sleep and behavior in much the same fashion as
the original stress.
There are significant overlaps of the neural circuitry and neurochemistry
underlying the stress response and that regulating arousal and sleep. Thus, it is not
surprising that the interaction between stress and sleep is implicated in a variety of
disease processes and psychiatric disorders. However, it is important to note that
even significant stress can be experienced without producing permanent or pathological changes. The stress response engages the physiological and behavioral
resources needed to cope with a challenge followed by a return to normalcy when
the situation is resolved. Indeed, the purpose of the stress response is to restore
homeostasis (Johnson et al. 1992; Chrousos 2009).
In this review, we will discuss the complex effects of stress on sleep, the stress
parameters that appear to be important in determining post-stress sleep, and the
neurochemical and neuroanatomical substrates important in regulating the relationship between stress and sleep. Lastly, we will discuss factors linking stress and
sleep in the genesis of stress-related disorders.
2 Complex Effects of Stress on Sleep Architecture
Much of our knowledge of the relationship between stress and sleep is based on
animal models, which allow for controlled studies on the consequences of both acute
and chronic stress. A wide variety of experimental paradigms have been used to
assess effects of acute stress on sleep, including social defeat stress (Meerlo et al.
1997, 2001a; Meerlo and Turek 2001), restraint or immobilization (Rampin et al.
1991; Meerlo et al. 2001b), footshocks (Smith 1995; Palma et al. 2000; Sanford
et al. 2005), water immersion (Smith 1995), cold exposure (Palma et al. 2000; Tiba
et al. 2004, 2008), ether exposure (Roky et al. 1995; Bodosi et al. 2000), cage changes
(Tang et al. 2004, 2005b), exposure to novel environments (Tang et al. 2004, 2005b),
and exposure to novel objects (Schiffelholz and Aldenhoff 2002; Tang et al. 2004,
2005b). Studies aimed at chronic stress are often based on repetition of a stressful
stimulus [i.e., intermittent footshock: (Kant et al. 1995)] or alternating presentations
of different noxious stimuli [i.e., tilted cage, wet cage, food deprivation, etc. (Cheeta
et al. 1997; Gronli et al. 2004)] over a prolonged period of time.
Despite the obvious variation in the nature of the stimuli applied, their use in
studies on stress is motivated by the assumption that these stimuli and conditions
are, to some degree, aversive to the animals. In many cases this assumption is
supported by data showing activation of the classical neuroendocrine stress
382
L. D. Sanford et al.
systems, i.e., the sympatho-adrenal axis and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
(HPA) axis. Indeed the major similarity between these models in terms of stress
appears to be an increase in the plasma levels of the stress hormones adrenaline
and corticosterone, although one has to keep in mind that such elevations occur in
response to virtually any kind of challenge and not exclusively to aversive stimuli
(Koolhaas et al. 2011). Importantly, though the outcome in many studies on stress
and sleep is discussed in terms of general stress effects, the experimental paradigms that are used may have stimulus-specific effects as well.
2.1 Effects of Acute Stress
Stress is generally considered to be a functional response of the brain and the body
to challenges that humans and other animals may face. Coping with environmental
challenges requires alertness and, since stress is a state of physiological activation
and arousal, by definition, it inhibits sleep. Indeed, exposing animals to stressors is
invariably associated with at least a short-lasting increase in wakefulness. Consistent with this aspect of the stress response, several of the classical neuropeptides
and hormones involved in the stress response are known to promote wakefulness
(see Sect. 4).
After the initial phase of arousal, sleep architecture is often altered; but in ways
that may vary among stressors. In addition to inhibiting all sleep, some stressors may
have a more pronounced and prolonged inhibiting effect on rapid eye movement
(REM) sleep, that is, once the animals fall asleep there may be a prolonged period of
time with non-REM (NREM) sleep but little or no REM sleep. This has been shown,
for example, in rodents after exposure to severe social stress (Meerlo and Turek
2001) and exposure to multiple presentations of inescapable footshock stress
(Adrien et al. 1991; Liu et al. 2003; Sanford et al. 2003a, b, c).
The initial period of stress-induced wakefulness and sleep disruption is most
often followed by a rebound to compensate for the NREM and/or REM sleep that
was lost. However, how much of the lost sleep is compensated and which sleep
state varies widely between studies and stressors. For example, in rodents exposed
to acute social stress, the initial loss of REM sleep is largely compensated during
the subsequent recovery period (Meerlo and Turek 2001). However, such a REM
sleep rebound may not occur after exposure to inescapable footshock stress in fear
conditioning paradigms (Sanford et al. 2003a, b, c, 2010) or learned helplessness
paradigms (Adrien et al. 1991).
Intriguingly, after some stressors, animals appear to gain more sleep than was
actually lost during and immediately following the stress. For example, controlled
studies in laboratory rats and mice showed that exposure to acute social stress, i.e.,
a 1 h interaction with an aggressive male conspecific, is followed by deeper or
longer NREM sleep than a similar period of non-stressful sleep deprivation
(Meerlo et al. 1997; Meerlo and Turek 2001). On the other hand, other studies
have shown that acute immobilization or restraint stress is often followed by a
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
383
selective increase in REM sleep (Rampin et al. 1991; Meerlo et al. 2001b). The
latter has long been the basis for the widely held belief that stress causes an
increase in REM sleep, which we now know is clearly not a general feature of all
stressors. It has not been established whether the stressor-specific increases in one
sleep state or another reflect disturbances in sleep regulation or perhaps functional
adaptations that evolved to deal with and recover from different stress conditions.
Indeed, it has been argued that acute social stress may represent a form of intense
wakefulness associated with increased brain activity, which would require a higher
than normal need for recovery sleep (Meerlo et al. 1997). Similarly, the increase in
REM sleep after e.g., immobilization stress has been suggested to be an adaptive
coping response that may also serve the purpose of recovery (Suchecki et al.
2012). However, it remains unknown why some stressors are not followed by a
complete compensation for the sleep that was lost, whereas other stressors are
followed by more sleep than was lost. Nor is it understood why some stressors
seem to promote NREM sleep whereas others are followed by an increase in REM
sleep. This complex variation in the effects of different stressors on sleep may
depend on the nature of the stressors and the specific effects they have on physiology and brain function. The variation in outcome may also be modulated by the
way stressors are perceived by the individual, e.g., in terms of controllability and
predictability see (Sects. 3.1 and 3.2), and whether or not a successful coping
strategy is developed (Sect. 3.3).
Overall, the finding that, in most animal models of acute stress, the arousing and
sleep-inhibiting effects of stressors are rapidly overcome and are sometimes followed by increased sleep during the recovery phase may seem at odds with the
general notion that stress is a major cause of sleep disturbance and insomnia in
humans. One explanation for this apparent inconsistency is that in laboratory
rodents the physiological activation and arousal disappear quite rapidly upon termination of the stressor and return to the home cage, whereas human beings may
carry their problems and stress with them. That is, the ‘‘stress’’ responsible for
prolonged sleep disruption in humans may be a cognitive and emotional phenomenon that is not necessarily always associated with an acute challenge. Humans
may suffer from stress based on memories of past events as well as worries and
expectations about the future. In that respect, compared to some animals, the human
brain may be more capable of turning a single acute stressor or life event that
occurred in the past, or even one pending in the future, into a persistent and chronic
stress state.
2.2 Repeated or Chronic Stress
With chronic stress, prolonged activation of the same behavioral, physiological,
and metabolic processes beneficial for coping with an acute stressor can become
detrimental (Chrousos 1998). Chronic stress has been reported to be a factor in the
384
L. D. Sanford et al.
disruption of sleep in a variety of situations including individuals lacking social
support in the work environment (Gadinger et al. 2009; Nomura et al. 2009),
children and adolescents exposed to traumatic events (Charuvastra and Cloitre
2009), and burnout patients (Armon et al. 2008). Chronic stress has also been
viewed as a risk factor for the development of insomnia (Cartwright and Wood
1991).
A number of experimental studies in laboratory rats have applied a model of
so-called chronic mild stress, which consists of exposing animals to a mixture of
noxious stimuli, once or twice a day, for periods up to 3 or 4 weeks (e.g. Cheeta
et al. 1997, Gronli et al. 2004). The stimuli include tilting of the cage, temporary
exposure to a wet or soiled cage, food and water deprivation, exposure to prolonged periods of continuous lighting, and even stroboscopic lighting in one of the
studies (Cheeta et al. 1997). The most significant finding in these studies was an
increase in the amount of REM sleep the day after 3 or 4 weeks of treatment. The
overall amount of sleep tended to be somewhat increased as well, which may in
part reflect a rebound due to sleep loss during the actual stress exposure. Unfortunately, some of the stimuli applied, in particular the continuous or stroboscopic
lighting, may have effects on sleep that have little to do with stress per se, for
example through alterations in circadian function.
Chronic stress as discussed here partly relies on direct stimulation of the
animals, which may explain some of the changes in sleep, whereas stress-related
sleep disturbances in humans often appear to be of a more psychological nature.
It may very well be that the physiological and neurobiological mechanisms
resulting in disrupted sleep due to repeated presentations of actual stressors are
quite different from those involved in psychological stress in humans. Although,
these studies are important first steps toward developing relevant models for
stress-related sleep disturbances and insomnia, perhaps research on the relationship between stress and sleep would gain by models that are based more on
psychological factors; for instance, conditioned fear and arousal in which animals
anticipate the occurrence of adverse events. Such an approach may have more
resemblance with the psychological stress in humans, and may allow us to study
central mechanisms by which sleep is disrupted and how such disturbances could
best be treated.
3 Effects on Sleep May Vary with Specific Stress
Parameters
While the importance of specific stress parameters and individual differences in
stress sensitivity is generally well established, there has been very limited
research on these issues with respect to their influence on sleep. Indeed, to date,
much of the work on stress and sleep has been primarily descriptive and focused
on effects of different types of stressors. The variable outcomes in terms of sleep
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
385
produced by different stressors clearly indicate that the observed changes are not
simply a generalizable stress effect. One should thus be careful with the interpretation and extrapolation of findings from these types of studies and perhaps
even not refer to the sleep changes as simple stress effects, as sleep after a
stressful event can be modified as a consequence of specific stimuli or conditions.
Indeed, in our view, experimental paradigms that manipulate specific stress
parameters (e.g., duration, intensity, controllability, and predictability) and paradigms that consider organismal variables (e.g., learning and memory, resilience,
and vulnerability) have considerably more potential for providing actual insight
into the complex relationship between stress and sleep. In this section, we will
provide an overview of how a number of these factors can modulate the stressinduced changes in sleep.
3.1 Stressor Controllability
Recent work has examined changes in sleep after controllable and uncontrollable
stress, and of memories associated with each parameter, using a simple yoked
control paradigm. In this paradigm, animals receive equal amounts of footshock,
but one of the yoked pair can terminate the footshock simply by moving to the safe
side of the shuttlebox. The actions of the yoked animal cannot alter shock presentation. Even though both animals receive equal shock, sleep in the post-shock
period can be dramatically different. As demonstrated in Fig. 1, animals trained
with controllable stress [escapable shock (ES)] can show significant enhancements
of REM sleep, whereas their yoked controls that receive uncontrollable stress
[inescapable shock (IS)] show significant reductions in REM sleep (Sanford et al.
2010). Returning the animals to the shock context without presenting footshock is
also followed by increased REM in the controllable stress condition and decreased
REM in the uncontrollable stress condition. Importantly, upon return to the shock
context, both groups of mice show enhanced freezing, the primary behavioral
indicator of fear memory (e.g. Blanchard and Blanchard 1969; Phillips and
LeDoux 1992; Paylor et al. 1994). Training with ES and IS also elicits similar
acute physiological stress responses as indicated by increased levels of plasma
corticosterone (Shors et al. 1989) and increased body temperature (stress-induced
hyperthermia) (Yang et al. 2011a). Thus, in this model, controllable and uncontrollable stress (and reminders of controllable and uncontrollable stress) result in
similar activation of the acute stress response and behavioral indices of fear but
directionally different alterations in REM. Controllability over a stressor does not
simply dampen the changes in sleep but rather, it may result in qualitatively
different changes. This work extends the findings based on the standard conditioned fear paradigm and further demonstrates that post-stress changes in sleep are
not a simple function of the physical stress that an animal receives.
386
L. D. Sanford et al.
(a)
(b)
Total REM
90
****
****
60
30
0
****
10
5
0
(c)
(d)
REM Episodes
120
****
80
****
REM Episode dur ation
1.2
***
****
Minutes
Counts
****
****
Percent
Minutes
****
REM/ TST
15
40
**
0.8
***
0.4
ES
IS
0
Base
ST1
ST2
Con
0
Base
ST1
ST2
Con
Fig. 1 REM sleep parameters plotted as 20 h totals for baseline (Base), two shock training days
(ST1, ST2), and context (Con) in a study comparing the effects of controllable (modeled by
escapable shock or ES) and uncontrollable (modeled by inescapable shock or IS) stress. a Total
REM sleep. b REM sleep percentage (total REM sleep time/total sleep time). c Number of REM
sleep episodes. d REM sleep episode duration. Significant differences between ES and IS: **,
p \ 0.01; ***, p \ 0.001; ****, p \ 0.0001 (Tukey test). Significant differences compared to
Base (open symbols) are indicated by dark circles or squares for the ES and IS groups. Reprinted
with permission from (Sanford et al. 2010)
3.2 Stressor Predictability
Predictability is an important factor in the effects of stress and a preference for
predictability has been repeatedly demonstrated (French et al. 1972; Gliner 1972;
Miller et al. 1974). For example, animals given the opportunity to determine
whether shocks delivered to them will be signaled or unsignaled typically choose
to spend their time in the signaled conditions regardless of whether the shock is
escapable or inescapable [reviewed in (Badia et al. 1979)]. The strong behavioral
effects suggest that predictability may also have a role in the modulating effects of
stress on sleep. In fact, stressor predictability is a significant component in shock
avoidance training in a shuttlebox, a paradigm in which laboratory rats are signaled of imminent shock and can learn to prevent shock from being delivered.
Variants of this paradigm have often been used in studies of learning and sleep and
have typically found increases in the amount of REM sleep at various latencies
after training that have been interpreted as indicating a role for REM sleep in
memory consolidation (e.g. Smith and Lapp 1986; Datta 2000). Unfortunately, the
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
387
potential role of predictability in modulating sleep after stress has received very
little attention.
One study of stressor predictability in mice examined sleep after training with
signaled escapable shock (SES) and signaled inescapable shock (SIS) (Yang et al.
2011a). Compared to mice experiencing SIS, those experiencing SES showed
significantly increased REM sleep after each of two shock training sessions
whereas compared to mice experiencing SES, those experiencing SIS showed
significantly increased NREM sleep after both shock sessions. Interestingly,
groups receiving either SES or SIS showed reduced REM sleep in response to cue
presentation alone. In another study, mice exposed to either predictive or nonpredictive auditory cues during training with ES also showed post-stress increases
in REM sleep (Machida et al. 2013). However, a subgroup of mice (around 35 %)
trained with the predictive auditory cue failed to improve their escape performance
from the first to second day of training. Those mice that did not improve also did
not show enhanced REM on either shock training day, suggesting a learning
component in the alterations in REM sleep.
It is useful to compare these results to those obtained using non-signaled
escapable and IS used to model controllable and uncontrollable stress as described
above (Sanford et al. 2010). Without predictive cues, the relative differences in
post-stress REM after escapable and IS were more pronounced. Contexts associated with non-signaled escapable and IS also produced directionally different
changes in REM similar to those seen when shock was presented (Sanford et al.
2010), whereas predictive cues associated with either escapable or IS produced
similar reductions in REM sleep.
While at this point the data are limited, these findings suggest that contexts and
auditory cues associated with different shock training conditions may carry different, and potentially competing, types of information regarding the stressful
situations. This difference is more pronounced in training with ESs as both contextual and cued fear associated with uncontrollable stress have similar effects on
sleep in mice and both reduce REM (Sanford et al. 2003a, c). Thus, competing
cued and contextual information associated with ESs may have interacted during
training resulting in competing influences on REM, thereby suggesting that
stressor predictability and controllability may interact in complex ways to modulate the changes in subsequent sleep.
3.3 Stress-Related Learning and Sleep
In addition to producing direct physiological effects, stressful situations provide an
opportunity for learning as the individual responds to the stressor and seeks to use
available information to cope with the ongoing challenge the stressor imposes.
While in humans this could involve a variety of activities, including higher order
cognitive processing; in rodents, the simplest behavioral responses to a stressor
may be avoidance or escape attempts. In this case, stress-related parameters such
388
L. D. Sanford et al.
as controllability and predictability may provide useful information that shapes
avoidance and escape behaviors thereby facilitating successful coping. By comparison, stressors that are uncontrollable or occur unpredictably do not provide
information that can guide the animal to learn successful avoidance or escape
behaviors. In these situations, the animal may still engage in escape attempts, but
its behavior will not alter the presentation of the stressor or facilitate coping.
The impact of stressor controllability and predictability on behavior are central
to a number of well-established learning paradigms that are motivated by stressful
events. Of these, fear conditioning and related paradigms are beginning to demonstrate that the learning options an animal has in a stressful situation play a
significant role in determining the impact of stress.
Experimental fear conditioning is a learning paradigm in which an animal makes
an association between an uncontrollable stressor (usually footshock) and previously neutral cues (typically auditory) or contextual information (the test box and
experimental room). Afterwards, presenting the fearful cues and contexts alone
elicit physiological and behavioral fear responses similar to those produced by the
initial uncontrollable stressor. Fear conditioned alterations in sleep are now also
established though these can vary with the amount of training and with the strain of
rats or mice that is studied. In agreement with the data in previous sections showing
that uncontrollable footshocks reduce REM sleep, the primary and most consistent
effect of extensive training with inescapable footshock is a marked reduction in
REM sleep that occurs both after the shock training and after presentation of shock
associated fearful cues and contexts (Sanford et al. 2003a, c; Tang et al. 2005d).
This reduction in REM sleep has been reported across species and across strains
(Sanford et al. 2001, 2003a, c) and can occur without the rebound or recovery REM
sleep that has been reported for most stressors (Sanford et al. 2003a, c). Changes in
NREM sleep in fear conditioning studies appear to be less consistent. Some studies
have reported increases in (light) NREM sleep (Adrien et al. 1991), whereas others
have shown strain-dependent reductions in NREM sleep after shock training and
fearful contexts (Sanford et al. 2003a). There also may be relatively less NREM
sleep EEG delta power (slow wave activity) in animals that show greater fear
conditioned changes in sleep (Tang et al. 2006). Critically, these studies demonstrate that fear conditioned memories of stressful events can produce mostly the
same changes in sleep as those produced by the stressful event itself and indicate
the importance of learning in both the immediate and lasting effects that stress can
have on sleep.
3.4 Fear Extinction and Sleep
Fear extinction is another important type of stress-related learning. While fear
conditioning can be involved in the long term, negative effects of stress, it also can
underlie adaptive behavior that occurs only so long as the fear-inducing stimulus
is predictive of, or associated with, an aversive event (Kishimoto et al. 2000;
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
389
Pitman et al. 2001). Repeated presentation of a fearful cue or context without
shock results in fear extinction, a type of new learning that inhibits subsequent fear
behavior without erasing the original memory for fear conditioning (Bouton 2004).
It is the failure of extinction that has been linked to stress-related psychopathology,
particularly posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Myers and Davis 2007).
The processes that make fear behaviors resistant to extinction remain mostly
unknown though there appears to be a relationship between fear extinction and
post-training REM sleep. Post-training REM sleep deprivation has been reported
to impair extinction (as indicated by freezing) for light cues (Silvestri 2005), but
not for auditory cues (Fu et al. 2007) previously paired with shock. REM sleepdeprived rats did show greater spontaneous recovery of freezing on a second day
with presentation of the fearful auditory cue alone. Neither of these studies found
that post-training REM sleep deprivation significantly altered contextual fear
extinction learning or spontaneous recovery of freezing on a second day of testing
(Silvestri 2005; Fu et al. 2007). However, sleep (both NREM and REM) following
extinction of contextual fear does return to normal, whereas rats that continued to
show fear exhibited reductions in REM sleep (Wellman et al. 2008).
3.5 Stressor Resilience and Vulnerability to Sleep
Disturbance
Genetic differences in vulnerability and resilience are recognized as important
factors in the development of stress-related pathology. For example, approximately
20–30 % of individuals who experience traumatic events may develop PTSD
whereas others do not appear to suffer significant long-lasting effects (Cohen et al.
2003; Kerns et al. 2004). Attempts to develop animal models that better represent
individual differences in clinical populations have included the selection of low and
high responders to stressors in outbred rat strains (Cohen et al. 2003; Kerns et al.
2004). There is also evidence that differences in vulnerability are a factor in the
impact of stress on sleep, but the significance of individual differences has not been
fully appreciated in either studies of stress or in studies of sleep in general (Irmis
et al. 1971, 1974; Tang et al. 2007).
Some of the best evidence for the role in resilience and vulnerability in the
impact of stress on sleep comes from studies comparing inbred strains of rodents,
which are genetically identical within strain but which vary genetically and phenotypically across strain. Work in mice and rats has demonstrated that strains that
exhibited greater anxiety-like behaviors in response to challenges in wakefulness
exhibited correspondingly greater and longer duration alterations in sleep after
training with IS and after fearful cues (Sanford et al. 2003c) and contexts (Sanford
et al. 2003a) associated with IS. In general, vulnerable mouse strains (e.g., BALB/
cJ mice compared to more resilient C57BL/6 J mice) also showed greater decreases
in sleep after stressful situations with unlearned responses, including exposure to an
390
L. D. Sanford et al.
open field (Tang et al. 2004), cage change, and novel objects placed in the home
cage (Tang et al. 2005b). Moreover, BALB/cJ mice also do not show a significant
REM sleep increase during recovery from restraint stress, whereas C57BL/6 J mice
do (Meerlo et al. 2001b).
In addition to genetic determinants of individual resilience and vulnerability,
environmental factors and prior experiences with stress also play a major role in
shaping future responses to stressful challenges. One such factor is neonatal stress
that can be induced by disruptions of mother–infant relationship and that can have
repercussions for adult behavior (Levine 2005) though the specific effects of
maternal separation on sleep have varied across studies. For example, 3 h of maternal
separation from postnatal days 2–14 in rats has been reported to increase both
spontaneous baseline REM sleep and cold-stress-induced changes in REM sleep in
males (Tiba et al. 2004) and females (Tiba et al. 2008). Similarly, neonatal rats
maternally separated for 3 h and exposed to chronic mild stress as adults were
reported to show longer sleep time, more episodes of REM sleep, and more episodes
of NREM sleep transitioning to REM sleep (Mrdalj et al. 2013). By comparison, 6 h
of neonatal maternal deprivation reduced the time spent in REM, without changes in
NREM sleep when the rats attained adulthood (Feng et al. 2012). In addition to
differences in experimental procedure, another important aspect that may differentiate these studies is the strain of rats used. While Feng and co-workers used SpragueDawley rats, the other studies used Wistar rats, which display more maternal
behavior upon reunion with their litters (Lehmann and Feldon 2000), possibly buffering potential harmful effects of the separation procedure.
4 Stress Mediators as an Important Cause of Arousal
and Sleep Disturbance
The regulation of sleep and arousal involves multiple neurotransmitter systems as
well as excitatory and inhibitory amino acids, peptides, purines, and neuronal and
non-neuronal humoral (i.e., cytokines and prostaglandins) modulators (Steiger and
Holsboer 1997; Steiger et al. 1998; Jones 2005; Steiger 2007; Luppi 2010; Espana
and Scammell 2011). Many of these same neurotransmitters and neuromodulators
are also influenced by and/or mediate the effects of stress and are likely involved in
the effects of stress on sleep. This section will briefly review some of the major
neurochemical systems that link stress and sleep.
4.1 Hypocretin/Orexin
Hypocretin-1 and -2 are a set of neuropeptides that are derived from the same
precursor gene and produced by neurons located in the lateral hypothalamus. The
hypocretins are also called orexins as they were independently discovered by two
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
391
research groups in 1998 and separately named as hypocretins (de Lecea et al.
1998) or orexins (Sakurai et al. 1998). The hypocretin containing neurons have
widespread projections throughout the brain and play a role in a variety of functions including autonomic control, neuroendocrine function, and feeding.
Numerous studies have also linked hypocretin to the regulation of the sleep–wake
cycle, particularly the induction and maintenance of wakefulness (Kilduff and
Peyron 2000; Sutcliffe and de Lecea 2002). Indeed, the hypocretin system activates various well-known wake-active and arousal promoting centers in the brain,
including the histaminergic tuberomammilary nucleus, the noradrenergic locus
coeruleus (LC), the serotonergic dorsal raphe, and the cholinergic cell clusters in
the brainstem and basal forebrain (Peyron et al. 1998b). Impaired hypocretin
transmission is a core pathophysiological factor of narcolepsy, a disease characterized by uncontrollable onset of sleep (Nishino et al. 2000; Kornum et al. 2011).
Several lines of evidence indicate that hypocretins/orexins may also play a role
in the behavioral arousal and neuroendocrine activation associated with stress
(Winsky-Sommerer et al. 2005). A close and bidirectional relationship exists
between the hypocretin system and the HPA axis. Hypocretins stimulate the activity
of the HPA axis in a dose-dependent manner, an effect that seems to be mediated at
the hypothalamic level (Kuru et al. 2000; Samson et al. 2002) but not at the adrenal
level (Jaszberenyi et al. 2000). Under stressful conditions, a dual hypocretin-1/
hypocretin-2 receptor antagonist does not interfere with corticosterone secretion but
does reverse the immediate waking effect of novelty and social stressors (Steiner
et al. 2013). In turn, hypothalamic corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) containing neurons project directly to the lateral hypothalamus hypocretin containing
neurons, where CRH1 and 2 receptors are abundantly expressed (Winsky-Sommerer
et al. 2004). Indeed, studies in mice have shown that exposure to footshock and
restraint stress causes an activation of the lateral hypothalamic hypocretin neurons,
an effect that is mediated by CRH (Winsky-Sommerer et al. 2005).
4.2 Corticotropin Releasing Hormone
CRH is a major mediator of central nervous system responses to stressors (Koob
and Bloom 1985; Heinrichs et al. 1995; Koob 1999). Intracerebroventricular (ICV)
administration of CRH in rats produces many of the signs associated with anxiety in
humans, including increased wakefulness (Ehlers et al. 1986; Marrosu et al. 1990;
Chang and Opp 1998), altered locomotor activity, and an exaggerated startle
response (Swerdlow et al. 1986; Heilig et al. 1994). By comparison, CRH antagonists attenuate behavioral responses to stress (e.g. Aloisi et al. 1999; Basso et al.
1999; Deak et al. 1999).
CRH may not only play an important role in stress-induced wakefulness and
arousal, it may also be partly responsible for changes in sleep architecture during
the subsequent recovery phase (Gonzalez and Valatx 1997). However, the few
studies examining the role of CRH in stress-induced alterations in sleep have
392
L. D. Sanford et al.
yielded conflicting data. This is exemplified with the work on restraint stressinduced increases in REM sleep. The ICV administration of the broad CRH
antagonist a-helical CRH9–41 prior to restraint stress prevents the subsequent
increase in REM, but does not alter spontaneous REM, NREM, or wakefulness in
non-stressed rats (Gonzalez and Valatx 1997). In contrast, other investigators
found no effect of restraint stress applied at the beginning of the dark period on
subsequent sleep, and also found no effect of the CRH antagonist, astressin, on
sleep after restraint (Chang and Opp 2002). By comparison, restraint exposure at
the onset of the light period increases wakefulness and decreases both NREM and
REM, and ICV administration of astressin attenuates the increase in wakefulness
over a 5 h-period immediately after the end of restraint but does not alter arousal
during the period when restraint was applied (Chang and Opp 2002). There may
have been differences in the procedures used for restraint [e.g., whether or not it
was conducted in the home cage (Chang and Opp 2002)] that could have produced
different results in these studies.
A recent study (Kimura et al. 2010) examined baseline and recovery sleep
after sleep deprivation in conditional mouse mutants that overexpress CRH in the
entire central nervous system or only in the forebrain, including limbic structures.
In baseline recordings, homozygous mice with either global or forebrain overexpression of CRH showed increased REM compared to controls and both
homozygous and heterozygous mice with global overexpression of CRH showed
enhanced recovery REM sleep after 6 h sleep deprivation. However, repeated
ICV administration of CRH during prolonged REM sleep deprivation in rats
inhibits the expected REM rebound (Machado et al. 2010). Enhanced REM sleep
recovery, but not NREM sleep recovery, was blocked by oral administration of
the CRH receptor type 1 (CRHR1) antagonist, DMP696, 1 h prior to the end of
sleep deprivation. Peripheral stress hormone levels were not elevated during
baseline and did not differ across genotypes after sleep deprivation. The authors
concluded that enhanced REM sleep in these mice was most likely induced
through the activation of CRHR1. Consistent with this conclusion is a report that
repeated administration of a-helical CRH9–41 in rats over 10 h of sleep deprivation also reduced the amount of REM sleep recovery (Gonzalez and Valatx
1998).
However, there is a separate line of research that demonstrates an inhibiting
effect of CRH on REM sleep. Fear conditioning with IS, an uncontrollable stressor,
and the presentation of fearful contexts and cues associated with IS are followed by
significant reductions in REM that occur in the first few hours after exposure
(Sanford et al. 2003a, c). In mice, ICV administration of CRH enhances the
reduction in REM sleep following fearful contexts, whereas ICV administration of
the non specific CRH antagonist, astressin, attenuates fear-induced reductions in
REM (Yang et al. 2009). Training with ES, and reminders of ES, can produce
significant enhancements in REM sleep (Sanford et al. 2010). Microinjections of
either saline or astressin prior to training produce similar, significant enhancements
in post-stress REM sleep relative to a non-shocked handling control condition,
whereas the increases in REM sleep are blocked by pretreatment with CRH (Yang
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
393
et al. 2011b). The effect of CRH seems to be relatively specific for REM sleep as
changes in NREM sleep and wakefulness were minimal. One potential explanation
for differences across studies is that administration of a CRH antagonist simply
blocked the initiation of neural processes that would have led to a subsequent
increase in REM sleep.
4.3 Prolactin
A variety of studies have indicated that prolactin can promote REM sleep. Both
systemic and ICV injection of prolactin enhances REM sleep in rats (Roky et al.
1995), whereas administration of a prolactin antiserum reduces the amount REM
sleep without affecting NREM sleep (Obal et al. 1992). Also, the amount of REM
sleep was found to be reduced in prolactin-deficient mice, which could be reversed
by prolactin replacement (Obal et al. 2005).
Several studies in laboratory rodents have shown that the plasma level of prolactin increases in response to a wide variety of stressors, including restraint stress
and ether exposure (Lenox et al. 1980; Meerlo et al. 2001b) suggesting that it may
play a role in the effects of stress on sleep. A comparative study on different strains
of mice showed that C57BL/6 J mice and BALB/cJ mice had similar corticosterone
responses to restraint stress; however, the effects on prolactin and subsequent sleep
were quite different. Restraint stress caused a concomitant increase in prolactin and
REM sleep in the C57BL/6 J mice, but not in BALB/cJ mice, which supports the
idea that prolactin might be involved in the mechanism underlying restraint stressinduced REM sleep (Meerlo et al. 2001b). Direct evidence for prolactin as a
mediator of stress-related increases in REM sleep comes from a study in rats
showing that an ether exposure-induced increase in REM sleep could be blocked by
hypophysectomy and by ICV administration of an antiserum to prolactin (Bodosi
et al. 2000). Other data implicating prolactin in stress-induced alterations in sleep
come from a study examining post-stress sleep in REM sleep-deprived rats subsequently submitted to single or repeated sessions of footshock (Machado et al.
2008). REM sleep rebound was greater in the REM sleep-deprived rats that
received multiple sessions of footshock, and the increase was associated with higher
levels of prolactin (Machado et al. 2008).
Together these studies suggest that stressful stimuli and conditions that are
associated with strong increases in prolactin levels may be followed by sleep with
increased amounts of REM sleep. The precise mechanism of these effects of
prolactin remains to be clarified but may involve a direct stimulatory effect of
prolactin on cholinergic neurons in the mesopontine tegmental area involved in
REM-sleep induction (Takahashi et al. 2000).
394
L. D. Sanford et al.
4.4 Monoamines
Serotonin (5-HT) containing neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN), noradrenaline (NA) containing neurons in the LC, and histamine containing neurons
in the tuberomammillary nucleus are wake-active and act directly on cortical and
subcortical regions to promote wakefulness (Jones 2005). The 5-HT and NA
systems are strongly stress-reactive (see discussion below) whereas there has been
less work on the role of the histaminergic system. However, it is involved with the
regulation of the stress response as central administration of histamine produces
increases in adrenocorticotropin and corticosterone (Rudolph et al. 1979; Knigge
and Warberg 1991) and blocking histamine synthesis or administration of antagonists block ACTH, beta-endorphin and prolactin responses to some stressors
(Rudolph et al. 1979; Seltzer et al. 1986; Knigge and Warberg 1991; Kjaer et al.
1993; Fleckenstein et al. 1994). There also appears to be heterogeneity in specific
histaminergic cells groups with respect to responding to different stressors (Miklos
and Kovacs 2003).
Interestingly, each of these systems has been implicated in the enhancement of
REM sleep that typically follows restraint stress. The increase is not found in
5-HT1A knockout mice (Boutrel et al. 2002; Popa et al. 2006) or in mice lacking
the 5-HT transporter (Rachalski et al. 2009). Administration of the serotonin
synthesis inhibitor para-chlorophenylalanine (Sinha 2006), neurotoxic destruction
of noradrenergic cells in LC (Gonzalez et al. 1995), and administration of the
histamine H1 receptor antagonist, chlorpheniramine, also prevent the increase in
REM sleep induced by restraint in rats (Rojas-Zamorano et al. 2009). However,
the actual cause of the attenuation of the REM sleep increase is not yet fully
understood. As indicated for CRH, alterations in these systems prior to stress
could simply alter the intensity of some elements of the stress response such that
the processes that result in the post-restraint increase in REM sleep are not
engaged.
5 Brain Regions Linking Stress, Arousal and Sleep
As discussed in the above section on stress mediators, there are several points of
overlap in the neural regions/systems involved in stress and those directly involved
in arousal. This section will focus on the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex
(mPFC), two regions not typically considered as direct regulators of arousal and
sleep but which play significant roles in mediating the effects of stress on sleep and
arousal (see Fig. 2).
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
Hippocampus
395
mPFC
Amygdala
BLA
CNA
BNST
LC
DRN
PVN
Emotional
Stress
LH
Pituitary
Adrenal
glands
Fear-Induced
Sympathetic
Activation
Arousal/
Sleep
Fig. 2 This diagram illustrates the principal circuitry (shaded) that we are discussing in this section
along with some of their connections to other regions involved in stress and sleep. In this figure,
emotional stress would act on the amygdala which would be regulated by the hippocampus
(contextual information) and the mPFC (perceived stressor control). BLA would act on CNA and
the BNST to regulate the peripheral stress axis via PVN. Output from CNA would also impact LC
and DRN, which have roles in regulating REM sleep and arousal as well as in regulating PVN. Both
CNA and LC are involved in regulating fear-induced sympathetic activation via effects on LH. This
diagram is necessarily incomplete, but illustrates the central role of the amygdala in controlling the
stress axis, fear responses, and important components of the arousal system. Heavyweight arrows
indicate presumed critical connections for mediating the effects of stress on sleep. Lightweight
arrows indicate other connections that may play a role in regulating responses. Dashed arrows
indicate indirect pathways. BLA basolateral nucleus of the amygdala, BNST bed nucleus of the stria
terminalis, CNA central nucleus of the amygdala, DRN dorsal raphe nucleus, LC locus coeruleus,
LH lateral hypothalamus, mPFC medial prefrontal cortex, PVN paraventricular nucleus
5.1 Amygdala and Stress-Induced Alterations in Arousal
and Sleep
Several lines of research have demonstrated that the amygdala is a significant
modulator of sleep. The majority of research on the role of the amygdala in regulating sleep has focused on its influence on REM sleep (e.g. Sanford et al. 1995,
396
L. D. Sanford et al.
1998, 2002; Calvo et al. 1996; Zhu et al. 1998; ); however, a number of studies
indicate that the amygdala can influence all sleep–wakefulness states (Sanford et al.
1995; 1998, 2006; Zhu et al. 1998). This influence most likely involves amygdalar
projections to thalamic, hypothalamic, and brainstem target regions (Amaral et al.
1992) that are involved in the control of sleep and arousal. These include direct
projections via the central nucleus of the amygdala [CNA; e.g. (Krettek and Price
1978; Inagaki et al. 1983; Price et al. 1987; Semba and Fibiger 1992; Peyron et al.
1998a)] and the lateral division of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis [BNST;
reviewed in (Amaral et al. 1992; Davis and Whalen 2001)], the source of the major
descending outputs of the amygdala to brainstem regions linked to the regulation of
REM sleep.
The amygdala is important in the regulation of behavioral, physiological, and
neuroendocrine responses to stress (Roozendaal et al. 1991a, b; Bohus et al. 1996)
and it appears to be a vital interface between stressful events and their impact on
sleep and arousal. The BNST is an important relay for the influence of the amygdala
on the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus (PVN) (Forray and Gysling 2004), the
final common pathway for information influencing the HPA axis (Pacak and
Palkovits 2001; Herman et al. 2004) and a key site for integrating neuroendocrine,
autonomic, and behavioral responses to stress (Chrousos 1998). GABA-ergic
neurons in BNST can directly inhibit PVN and reduce ACTH secretion (Herman
et al. 2004). By comparison, CNA has minimal direct projections to PVN (Prewitt
and Herman 1998) and lesions of CNA do not directly influence PVN activation
(Prewitt and Herman 1997) though CNA can influence PVN via trans-synaptic
pathways through the dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus and BNST (Prewitt and
Herman 1998).
CNA does play a role in regulating the effects of stress on sleep, whereas a
possible role for BNST has not been established. Inhibition of the CNA suppresses
REM sleep whereas its activation [e.g., with electrical stimulation (Smith and
Miskiman 1975)] can promote REM sleep in some situations. For example, functional inactivation of CNA with microinjections of the GABAA agonist, muscimol,
produces a relatively selective decrease in REM sleep whereas blocking GABAergic
inhibition with the GABAA antagonist, bicuculline, enhances REM sleep (Sanford
et al. 2002). Functional lesions of the CNA by TTX, which inactivates both cell
bodies and fibers of passage also decrease REM sleep and reduce arousal (Tang et al.
2005c). The decrease in REM sleep can occur without recovery (Tang et al. 2005c), a
finding also reported for training with IS and fearful cues and contexts.
That stress-induced inactivation of CNA is involved in stress-induced reductions in REM sleep is also suggested by the lack of Fos activation in CNA after
conditioned fear (Liu et al. 2003). Functionally, this hypothesis is supported by
findings that bicuculline microinjected into CNA attenuates footshock-induced
reductions in REM sleep whereas inactivation of CNA with muscimol did not (Liu
et al. 2009). However, it should be noted that findings that activation of CNA
promotes and inactivation of CNA reduces REM sleep appear at odds with the
prevailing conventional view that CNA activation is responsible for regulating fear
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
397
responses via projections to the periaqueductal gray and other brainstem areas
[Reviewed in (Duvarci et al. 2011)]. In fact, CNA neurons do fire in response to
footshock stress (Rosenkranz et al. 2006) and in response to conditioned stimuli
(Duvarci et al. 2011). However, CNA is inhibited by stimulation of the basal and
lateral nuclei of the amygdala (Rosenkranz et al. 2006) both of which show high
Fos expression after footshock (Liu et al. 2003). Thus, it is possible that CNA
activation during fearful/stressful events does regulate fear behavior in wakefulness, but subsequently, with certain stressors, can be inhibited to decrease REM
sleep in the post-stress period.
The involvement of the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) in the
control of sleep is indicated by reports that bilateral electrolytic and chemical
lesions of BLA increase NREM sleep and total sleep time in rats (Zhu et al. 1998)
and that bilateral chemical lesions of the amygdala produce more consolidated
sleep in chair restrained Rhesus monkeys (Benca et al. 2000). Electrical and
chemical stimulation of BLA also increase low voltage, high frequency activity in
the cortical EEG and decrease NREM sleep and total sleep time, respectively
(Dringenberg and Vanderwolf 1996; Zhu et al. 1998).
In general, the evidence suggests that CNA is more involved in the regulation of
REM sleep than that of NREM sleep and that by comparison, BLA has a greater
role in the regulation of NREM sleep and arousal. However, it is important to note
that BLA regulates CNA output and therefore likely controls its influences on
REM. Fibers from BLA also course through CNA on to the BNST which has
brainstem targets similar to those of CNA (Davis and Whalen 2001), thereby
providing an additional pathway by which BLA can influence brainstem regions.
Indeed, it was recently found that microinjections into BLA of the Group II
metabotropic glutamate (mGlu) receptor agonist, LY379268, selectively reduced
REM sleep without significantly altering wakefulness or NREM sleep (Dong et al.
2012). By comparison, microinjection of LY379268 into CNA did not significantly
alter sleep. Thus, group II mGlu receptors may influence specific cells in BLA that
control descending outputs (possibly via CNA or BNST) that in turn regulate REM
sleep generator regions in the brainstem.
The amygdala (including extended amygdala) is a critical region for the central
effects of CRH, and it appears to mediate a number of the anxiogenic effects of
CRH as evidenced by intra-amygdala microinjections of CRH agonists and
antagonists [Reviewed in (Davis and Whalen 2001)]. For example, local application of CRH or urocortin (Sajdyk et al. 1999) into the BLA in rats produces
dose-dependent increases in anxiety behaviors. CRH in the amygdala also plays a
significant role in regulating stress-induced alterations in sleep.
It was reported that microinjections of the CRHR1 antagonist, antalarmin, into
CNA in rats block fear-induced reductions in REM sleep and attenuate Fos
expression in regions important in stress and REM sleep regulation including the
PVN, LC, and DRN (Liu et al. 2011). Similarly, bilateral microinjections of antalarmin into BLA in rats do not alter spontaneous sleep, but do block the reduction in
REM sleep produced by inescapable footshock (Wellman et al. 2013). Further,
microinjecting antalarmin into BLA prior to shock training also blocked the
398
L. D. Sanford et al.
subsequent effects of contextual fear on REM sleep, but did not block fear memory or
behavior as indicated by freezing. These data indicate that CRH receptors within
the CNA and BLA are important in the regulation of stress- and fear-induced
alterations in REM sleep, and also suggest that BLA plays a role in modulating how
stressful memories influence sleep. The hippocampus is also likely to be involved.
Information regarding fear conditioned contexts is first processed in the hippocampus and followed by BLA with output through CNA [reviewed in (LeDoux
2000)] and possibly BNST (Davis and Whalen 2001).
5.2 REM Regulatory Regions, Medial Prefrontal Cortex
and Stressor Control
Stress often has a prominent effect on REM sleep (Sanford et al. 2003a, b, c; Jha
et al. 2005). Thus, it is not surprising that brain regions directly implicated in the
regulation REM sleep have significant roles in mediating the stress response.
These include the LC and DRN. LC noradrenergic neurons and DRN serotonergic
neurons are virtually silent during REM sleep and their activation may inhibit its
generation (Steriade and McCarley 1990). LC and DRN also have connections to
the PVN. PVN receives a large noradrenergic projection from brainstem A1 and
A2 groups and a smaller projection from LC (Dunn et al. 2004). However, lesions
of LC do reduce ACTH and corticosterone responses to acute stress (Ziegler et al.
1999), and there are suggestions that LC may impact PVN indirectly via limbic
structures [reviewed in (Herman and Cullinan 1997)]. DRN has collateral serotonergic projections to CNA and PVN (Petrov et al. 1992, 1994), and 5-HT
agonists enhance PVN activity as indicated by increased corticosterone levels and
Fos expression (Mikkelsen et al. 2004). Indirect pathways may also play a role in
serotonergic regulation of PVN (Herman and Cullinan 1997).
As discussed above, stressor controllability appears to be an important parameter
in the effects of stress on sleep. Brainstem noradrenergic and serotonergic regions
play important roles in stressor controllability. For example, IS in rats produced
sustained increases in NA turnover in various brain regions regardless of stress
duration, whereas with ES, NA utilization was reduced after rats had learned the
coping response (Tsuda et al. 1987). IS also activates DRN serotonergic neurons to a
greater degree than ES thereby increasing 5-HT in DRN and in target areas (Amat
et al. 1998a, b; Hammack et al. 2002; Bland et al. 2003a, b). Administration of CRH
into DRN produced behavioral changes like those seen with IS, whereas microinjection of a nonselective CRH antagonist blocked the behavioral changes normally
seen with IS (Hammack et al. 2002, 2003a, b). The application of CRH to LC also
increases NA release (Van Bockstaele et al. 1998).
The alterations in 5-HT and NA after inescapable and ES are consistent with
their putative roles as inhibitory modulators of REM sleep and with the respective
increases and decreases in REM sleep after controllable and uncontrollable stress.
However, the evaluation of controllability requires assessment and evaluation of
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
399
information at the cortical level (Maier et al. 2006). The mPFC has been found to
be a critical region in the perception of control and in mediating the consequences
of stress (Maier et al. 2006; Smith and Vale 2006; Akirav and Maroun 2007). For
example, blocking activation of the ventral mPFC with muscimol did not alter
escape behavior in rats presented with IS, but blocking ventral mPFC in rats
presented with ES produced failure in escape learning and greater fear conditioning (Maier et al. 2006). By comparison, activation of ventral mPFC with
picrotoxin prior to IS promoted later escape learning in rats provided an opportunity to escape shock in a shuttlebox (Maier et al. 2006).
Unfortunately, the role of the mPFC in mediating the effects of stressor controllability on sleep has not been examined. However, part of the influence of
mPFC is enacted through its effects on the DRN and possibly LC (Maier et al.
2006), providing a potential substrate for regulating alterations in REM sleep. For
example, consistent with the discussion above, activation of mPFC inhibits DRN
(Maier et al. 2006; Smith and Vale 2006). The prelimbic mPFC also has robust but
restricted projections to the BLA and CNA, whereas the infralimbic mPFC projects to the medial, basomedial, cortical nuclei as well as to the CNA (Vertes
2004). There are projections from the mPFC to GABAergic neurons in the
intercalated nuclei which have inhibitory control over CNA output, but there are
conflicting reports regarding their specific origin within mPFC (see Vertes 2004).
However, these projections from the mPFC to brainstem regulatory regions and the
amygdala provide a substrate by which stressor controllability could influence
REM sleep.
6 Stress, Sleep and Neuronal Plasticity: Implications
for Stress-Related Disorders
Both NREM and REM sleep have putative roles in regulating neuronal plasticity
and synaptic strength (Benington and Frank 2003; Tononi and Cirelli 2006;
Meerlo et al. 2009; Havekes et al. 2012). Stress-induced changes in sleep and sleep
architecture might lead to alterations in these plasticity processes and ultimately
brain function. In fact, some of the changes in plasticity and brain function traditionally linked to stress may in part be related to alterations in sleep.
Work on stress and plasticity has distinguished the effects of acute and chronic
stress. Acute stress can impact functional plasticity whereas chronic stress can differentially alter structural plasticity across brain regions. For example, chronic stress
results in dendritic atrophy and reductions in spine density in the hippocampus
(Magarinos and McEwen 1995a, b; Magarinos et al. 1997; Sandi et al. 2003; Stewart
et al. 2005) and prefrontal cortex (Wellman 2001; Cook and Wellman 2004; Radley
and Morrison 2005; Liston et al. 2006; Radley et al. 2006). Similar types of chronic
stress produce increased dendritic arborization and increased spine density in BLA
spiny neurons (Mitra et al. 2005; Vyas et al. 2006) and spine down-regulation in the
medial amygdala (Bennur et al. 2007). Some stress-induced changes in the
400
L. D. Sanford et al.
hippocampus (Sandi et al. 2003; Stewart et al. 2005) and prefrontal cortex appear to
be reversible whereas those in the amygdala are not [Reviewed in (Christoffel et al.
2011)]. Acute restraint, tail shock, and environmental stress impair long-term
potentiation (LTP) in the hippocampus (Foy et al. 1987) and acute environmental
stress can enhance long-term depression (Xu et al. 1997). However, stress-induced
impairment in hippocampal LTP was significantly less in rats allowed to escape
footshock than in yoked controls receiving identical shock, but not allowed to escape
(Shors et al. 1989). This control mediated attenuation occurred even after a week of
daily training sessions with relatively intense shock (30 trials, 1 mA, 1.5 s mean
duration).
Although both sleep and stress can impact neuronal plasticity, their potential
interactions in mediating alterations in plasticity have been minimally explored. The
presence of interactions is indicated by the strong effects that stress can have on sleep
as well as the demonstrated and hypothesized roles each has in mediating various
aspects of plasticity. Importantly, post-stress sleep may have an adaptive function in
coping with stress. This is suggested by the directionally different post-stress
changes in REM sleep that occur following uncontrollable and controllable stress
(Sanford et al. 2010) and the normalization in REM sleep that occurs following fear
extinction versus the continued suppression of REM sleep in animals that still show
fear (Wellman et al. 2008). These differences suggest that sleep and specific stress
parameters may interact in mediating synaptic plasticity associated with stressrelated learning and memory and/or the emotional valence of the memory. Indeed, a
variety of authors have made suggestions consistent with this hypothesis, e.g., REM
sleep functions to weaken unwanted memory traces in the cortex (Crick and
Mitchison 1983); intact REM sleep aids in the processing of memory for trauma
(Mellman et al. 2002, 2007); and REM sleep may play an important role in consolidating memories for aversive events and for ‘‘decoupling’’ those memories from
their emotional charge (Nishida et al. 2009; Walker 2009).
Best et al. proposed that pyramidal neurons in the hippocampus change from a
firing pattern that supports LTP in wakefulness to one that supports depotentiation
during REM sleep; thereby putatively ‘‘resetting’’ the hippocampus after memories
have been transferred to the frontal cortex and clearing the way for the formation of
future memories (Best et al. 2007). If true, reductions in REM sleep induced by IS,
particularly those that occur without recovery REM sleep (Sanford et al. 2003a, c),
could impair this process. Impairment could also involve stress-induced enhancement of NA and 5-HT which facilitate LTP and may impede depotentiation
[Reviewed in (Best et al. 2007)]. By comparison, enhancements of REM sleep and
corresponding decreased activity in noradrenergic and serotonergic regions could
facilitate the adaptive processing of strong memories. Post-stress NREM sleep may
also be important. This is suggested by reports that rats trained with IS in an intense
learned helplessness paradigm show increased light NREM sleep as well as
decreased REM sleep (Adrien et al. 1991).
Sleep disturbances both before (Bryant et al. 2010) and after (Lavie 2001)
traumatic events have been linked to the development of stress-related pathology.
However, it is important to note that stress and the temporary alterations in sleep
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
401
associated with it typically do not give rise to persisting or detrimental effects. This
suggests that being able to distinguish normal stress responses from those that can
lead to pathology is likely key to fully understanding the processes leading to stressrelated disorders. As stress-induced dysregulation of neuronal plasticity and
remodeling of neural circuits are implicated in a variety of psychiatric disorders
(McEwen 2007; Christoffel et al. 2011) understanding the role sleep plays in
mediating the effects of stress on neuronal plasticity also may be critical for
understanding how stress comes to produce persisting and pathological changes in
the brain.
Acknowledgments The contribution of L.D.S. was supported by NIH research grants MH64827
and EVMS institutional funds. D.S. was supported by research grants from FAPESP (98/14303-3)
and Associação Fundo de Incentivo à Pesquisa and she is the recipient of a research fellowship
from CNPq (Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico).
References
Adrien J, Dugovic C, Martin P (1991) Sleep-wakefulness patterns in the helpless rat. Physiol
Behav 49:257–262
Akirav I, Maroun M (2007). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex-amygdala circuit in stress
effects on the extinction of fear. Neural Plast 2007: 30873
Aloisi AM, Bianchi M, Lupo C, Sacerdote P, Farabollini F (1999) Neuroendocrine and behavioral
effects of CRH blockade and stress in male rats. Physiol Behav 66(3):523–528
Amaral D, Price J, Pitkanen A, Carmichael S (1992) Anatomical organization of the primate
amydaloid complex. In: Aggleton J (ed) The Amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion,
memory, and mental dysfunction. Wiley-Liss, Inc, New York, pp 1–66
Amat J, Matus-Amat P, Watkins LR, Maier SF (1998a) Escapable and inescapable stress
differentially alter extracellular levels of 5-HT in the basolateral amygdala of the rat. Brain Res
812(1–2):113–120
Amat J, Matus-Amat P, Watkins LR, Maier SF (1998b) Escapable and inescapable stress differentially
and selectively alter extracellular levels of 5-HT in the ventral hippocampus and dorsal
periaqueductal gray of the rat. Brain Res 797(1):12–22
Armon G, Shirom A, Shapira I, Melamed S (2008) On the nature of burnout–insomnia relationships:
a prospective study of employed adults. J Psychosom Res 65(1):5–12
Badia P, Harsh J, Abbott B (1979) Choosing between predictable and unpredictable shock
conditions: data and theory. Psychol Bull 86(5):1107–1131
Basso AM, Spina M, Rivier J, Vale W, Koob GF (1999) Corticotropin-releasing factor antagonist
attenuates the ‘‘anxiogenic-like’’ effect in the defensive burying paradigm but not in the
elevated plus-maze following chronic cocaine in rats. Psychopharmacology 145(1):21–30
Benca RM, Obermeyer WH, Shelton SE, Droster J, Kalin NH (2000) Effects of amygdala lesions
on sleep in rhesus monkeys. Brain Res 879(1–2):130–138
Benington JH, Frank MG (2003) Cellular and molecular connections between sleep and synaptic
plasticity. Prog Neurobiol 69(2):71–101
Bennur S, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, Pawlak R, Strickland S, McEwen BS, Chattarji S (2007)
Stress-induced spine loss in the medial amygdala is mediated by tissue-plasminogen activator.
Neuroscience 144(1):8–16
Best J, Diniz Behn C, Poe GR, Booth V (2007) Neuronal models for sleep–wake regulation and
synaptic reorganization in the sleeping hippocampus. J Biol Rhythms 22(3):220–232
402
L. D. Sanford et al.
Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC (1969) Crouching as an index of fear. J Comp Physiol Psychol
67(3):370–375
Bland ST, Hargrave D, Pepin JL, Amat J, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2003a) Stressor controllability
modulates stress-induced dopamine and serotonin efflux and morphine-induced serotonin
efflux in the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:1589–1596
Bland ST, Twining C, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2003b) Stressor controllability modulates stressinduced serotonin but not dopamine efflux in the nucleus accumbens shell. Synapse
49(3):206–208
Bodosi B, Obal F Jr, Gardi J, Komlodi J, Fang J, Krueger JM (2000) An ether stressor increases
REM sleep in rats: possible role of prolactin. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol
279(5):R1590–R1598
Bohus B, Koolhaas J, Luiten P, Korte S, Roozendaal B, Wiersma A (1996) The neurobiology of
the central nucleus of the amygdala in relation to neuroendocrine outflow. Prog Brain Res
107:447–460
Bouton ME (2004) Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn Mem 11(5):485–494
Boutrel B, Monaca C, Hen R, Hamon M, Adrien J (2002) Involvement of 5-HT1A receptors in
homeostatic and stress-induced adaptive regulations of paradoxical sleep: studies in 5-HT1A
knock-out mice. J Neurosci 22(11):4686–4692
Bryant RA, Creamer M, O’Donnell M, Silove D, McFarlane AC (2010) Sleep disturbance
immediately prior to trauma predicts subsequent psychiatric disorder. Sleep 33(1):69–74
Calvo J, Simón-Arceo K, Fernández-Mas R (1996) Prolonged enhancement of REM sleep produced
by carbachol microinjection into the amygdala. NeuroRep 7:577–580
Cartwright RD, Wood E (1991) Adjustment disorders of sleep: the sleep effects of a major
stressful event and its resolution. Psychiatry Res 39(3):199–209
Chang FC, Opp MR (1998) Blockade of corticotropin-releasing hormone receptors reduces
spontaneous waking in the rat. Am J Physiol 275(3 Pt 2):R793–R802
Chang FC, Opp MR (2002) Role of corticotropin-releasing hormone in stressor-induced alterations
of sleep in rat. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 283(2):R400–R407
Chang PP, Ford DE, Mead LA, Cooper-Patrick L, Klag MJ (1997) Insomnia in young men and
subsequent depression. The Johns Hopkins precursors study. Am J Epidemiol 146(2):105–114
Charuvastra A, Cloitre M (2009) Safe enough to sleep: sleep disruptions associated with trauma,
posttraumatic stress, and anxiety in children and adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N
Am 18(4):877–891
Cheeta S, Ruigt G, van Proosdij J, Willner P (1997) Changes in sleep architecture following
chronic mild stress. Biol Psychiatry 41(4):419–427
Christoffel DJ, Golden SA, Russo SJ (2011) Structural and synaptic plasticity in stress-related
disorders. Rev Neurosci 22(5):535–549
Chrousos GP (1998) Stressors, stress, and neuroendocrine integration of the adaptive response.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 851:311–335
Chrousos GP (2009) Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature Rev Endocrinol 5(7):374–381
Cohen H, Zohar J, Matar M (2003) The relevance of differential response to trauma in an animal
model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 53(6):463–473
Cook SC, Wellman CL (2004) Chronic stress alters dendritic morphology in rat medial prefrontal
cortex. J Neurobiol 60(2):236–248
Crick F, Mitchison G (1983) The function of dream sleep. Nature 304(5922):111–114
Datta S (2000) Avoidance task training potentiates phasic pontine-wave density in the rat: a
mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci 20(22):8607–8613
Davis M, Whalen PJ (2001) The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry 6(1):13–34
de Lecea L, Kilduff TS, Peyron C, Gao X, Foye PE, Danielson PE, Fukuhara C, Battenberg EL,
Gautvik VT, Bartlett FS 2nd, Frankel WN, van den Pol AN, Bloom FE, Gautvik KM, Sutcliffe JG
(1998) The hypocretins: hypothalamus-specific peptides with neuroexcitatory activity. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 95(1):322–327
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
403
Deak T, Nguyen KT, Ehrlich AL, Watkins LR, Spencer RL, Maier SF, Licinio J, Wong ML,
Chrousos GP, Webster E, Gold PW (1999) The impact of the nonpeptide corticotropin-releasing
hormone antagonist antalarmin on behavioral and endocrine responses to stress. Endocrinology
140(1):79–86
Dong E, Wellman LL, Yang L, Sanford LD (2012) Group II metabotropic glutamate receptors in
the basal amygdala regulate rapid eye movement sleep. Brain Res 1452:85–95
Dringenberg HC, Vanderwolf CH (1996) Cholinergic activation of the electrocorticogram: an
amygdaloid activating system. Exp Brain Res 108(2):285–296
Dunn AJ, Swiergiel AH, Palamarchouk V (2004) Brain circuits involved in corticotropinreleasing factor-norepinephrine interactions during stress. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1018:25–34
Duvarci S, Popa D, Pare D (2011) Central amygdala activity during fear conditioning. J Neurosci
31(1):289–294
Ehlers CL, Reed TK, Henriksen SJ (1986) Effects of corticotropin-releasing factor and growth
hormone-releasing factor on sleep and activity in rats. Neuroendocrinology 42(6):467–474
Espana RA, Scammell TE (2011) Sleep neurobiology from a clinical perspective. Sleep
34(7):845–858
Feng P, Hu Y, Vurbic D, Guo Y (2012) Maternal stress induces adult reduced REM sleep and
melatonin level. Dev Neurobiol 72(5):677–687
Fleckenstein AE, Lookingland KJ, Moore KE (1994) Histaminergic neurons mediate restraint
stress-induced increases in the activity of noradrenergic neurons projecting to the hypothalamus. Brain Res 653(1–2):273–277
Forray MI, Gysling K (2004) Role of noradrenergic projections to the bed nucleus of the stria
terminalis in the regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Brain Res Rev
47(1–3):145–160
Foy MR, Stanton ME, Levine S, Thompson RF (1987) Behavioral stress impairs long-term
potentiation in rodent hippocampus. Behav Neural Biol 48(1):138–149
French D, Palestine D, Leeb C (1972) Preference for a warning in an unavoidable shock situation:
replication and extension. Psychol Rep 30:72–74
Fu J, Li P, Ouyang X, Gu C, Song Z, Gao J, Han L, Feng S, Tian S, Hu B (2007) Rapid eye
movement sleep deprivation selectively impairs recall of fear extinction in hippocampusindependent tasks in rats. Neuroscience 144(4):1186–1192
Gadinger MC, Fischer JE, Schneider S, Fischer GC, Frank G, Kromm W (2009) Female
executives are particularly prone to the sleep-disturbing effect of isolated high-strain jobs: a
cross-sectional study in German-speaking executives. J Sleep Res 18(2):229–237
Gliner JA (1972) Predictable versus unpredictable shock: preference behavior and stomach ulceration.
Physiol Behav 9(5):693–698
Gonzalez MM, Valatx JL (1997) Effect of intracerebroventricular administration of alpha-helical
CRH (9–41) on the sleep/waking cycle in rats under normal conditions or after subjection to
an acute stressful stimulus. J Sleep Res 6(3):164–170
Gonzalez MM, Valatx JL (1998) Involvement of stress in the sleep rebound mechanism induced by
sleep deprivation in the rat: use of alpha-helical CRH (9–41). Behav Pharmacol 9(8):655–662
Gonzalez MM, Debilly G, Valatx JL, Jouvet M (1995) Sleep increase after immobilization stress:
role of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus system in the rat. Neurosci Lett 202(1–2):5–8
Gronli J, Murison R, Bjorvatn B, Sorensen E, Portas CM, Ursin R (2004) Chronic mild stress
affects sucrose intake and sleep in rats. Behav Brain Res 150(1–2):139–147
Hammack SE, Richey KJ, Schmid MJ, LoPresti ML, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2002) The role of
corticotropin-releasing hormone in the dorsal raphe nucleus in mediating the behavioral
consequences of uncontrollable stress. J Neurosci 22(3):1020–1026
Hammack SE, Pepin JL, DesMarteau JS, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2003a) Low doses of corticotropinreleasing hormone injected into the dorsal raphe nucleus block the behavioral consequences of
uncontrollable stress. Behav Brain Res 147(1–2):55–64
Hammack SE, Schmid MJ, LoPresti ML, Der-Avakian A, Pellymounter MA, Foster AC,
Watkins LR, Maier SF (2003b) Corticotropin releasing hormone type 2 receptors in the dorsal
404
L. D. Sanford et al.
raphe nucleus mediate the behavioral consequences of uncontrollable stress. J Neurosci
23(3):1019–1025
Havekes R, Vecsey CG, Abel T (2012) The impact of sleep deprivation on neuronal and glial
signaling pathways important for memory and synaptic plasticity. Cell Signal 24(6):
1251–1260
Heilig M, Koob G, Ekman R, Britton K (1994) Corticotropin-releasing factor and neuropeptide
Y: role in emotional integration. Trends Neurosci 17:80–85
Heinrichs SC, Menzaghi F, Merlo Pich E, Britton KT, Koob GF (1995) The role of CRF in
behavioral aspects of stress. Ann N Y Acad Sci 771:92–104
Herman JP, Cullinan WE (1997) Neurocircuitry of stress: central control of the hypothalamopituitary-adrenocortical axis. Trends Neurosci 20(2):78–84
Herman JP, Mueller NK, Figueiredo H (2004) Role of GABA and glutamate circuitry in
hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical stress integration. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1018:35–45
Inagaki S, Kawai Y, Matsuzaki T, Shiosaka S, Tohyama M (1983) Precise terminal fields of the
descending somatostatinergic neuron system from the amygdaloid complex of the rat. J Hirnforsch
24(3):345–356
Irmis F, Lâat J, Radil-Weiss T (1971) Individual differences in hippocampal EEG during
rhombencephalic sleep and arousal. Physiol Behav 7(1):117–119
Irmis F, Lâat J, Radil-Weiss T (1974) Individual (constitutional) differences in sleep patterns in
rats. Behav Res Therapy 12:245–249
Jaszberenyi M, Bujdoso E, Pataki I, Telegdy G (2000) Effects of orexins on the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal system. J Neuroendocrinol 12(12):1174–1178
Jha SK, Brennan FX, Pawlyk AC, Ross RJ, Morrison AR (2005) REM sleep: a sensitive index of
fear conditioning in rats. Eur J Neurosci 21(4):1077–1080
Johnson EO, Kamilaris TC, Chrousos GP, Gold PW (1992) Mechanisms of stress: a dynamic
overview of hormonal and behavioral homeostasis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 16(2):115–130
Jones BE (2005) From waking to sleeping: neuronal and chemical substrates. Trends Pharmacol
Sci 26(11):578–586
Kant GJ, Pastel RH, Bauman RA, Meininger GR, Maughan KR, Robinson TN 3rd, Wright WL,
Covington PS (1995) Effects of chronic stress on sleep in rats. Physiol Behav 57(2):359–365
Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW 3rd, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS (2004) Anterior
cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303(5660):1023–1026
Kilduff TS, Peyron C (2000) The hypocretin/orexin ligand-receptor system: implications for sleep
and sleep disorders. Trends Neurosci 23(8):359–365
Kimura M, Muller-Preuss P, Lu A, Wiesner E, Flachskamm C, Wurst W, Holsboer F, Deussing JM
(2010) Conditional corticotropin-releasing hormone overexpression in the mouse forebrain
enhances rapid eye movement sleep. Mol Psychiatry 15(2):154–165
Kishimoto T, Radulovic J, Radulovic M, Lin CR, Schrick C, Hooshmand F, Hermanson O,
Rosenfeld MG, Spiess J (2000) Deletion of CRHR2 reveals an anxiolytic role for
corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor-2. Nat Genet 24(4):415–419
Kjaer A, Knigge U, Madsen EL, Soe-Jensen P, Bach FW, Warberg J (1993) Insulin/hypoglycemiainduced adrenocorticotropin and beta-endorphin release: involvement of hypothalamic
histaminergic neurons. Endocrinology 132(5):2213–2220
Knigge U, Warberg J (1991) The role of histamine in the neuroendocrine regulation of pituitary
hormone secretion. Acta Endocrinol 124(6):609–619
Koob GF (1999) Corticotropin-releasing factor, norepinephrine, and stress. Biol Psychiatry
46(9):1167–1180
Koob G, Bloom F (1985) Corticotropin-releasing factor and behavior. Fed Proc 44:259–263
Koolhaas JM, Bartolomucci A, Buwalda B, de Boer SF, Flugge G, Korte SM, Meerlo P, Murison R,
Olivier B, Palanza P, Richter-Levin G, Sgoifo A, Steimer T, Stiedl O, van Dijk G, Wohr M,
Fuchs E (2011) Stress revisited: a critical evaluation of the stress concept. Neurosci Biobehav
Rev 35(5):1291–1301
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
405
Koren D, Arnon I, Lavie P, Klein E (2002) Sleep complaints as early predictors of posttraumatic
stress disorder: a 1-year prospective study of injured survivors of motor vehicle accidents. Am
J Psychiatry 159:855–857
Kornum BR, Faraco J, Mignot E (2011) Narcolepsy with hypocretin/orexin deficiency, infections
and autoimmunity of the brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21(6):897–903
Krettek JE, Price JL (1978) Amygdaloid projections to subcortical structures within the basal
forebrain and brainstem in the rat and cat. J Comp Neurol 178(2):225–254
Kuru M, Ueta Y, Serino R, Nakazato M, Yamamoto Y, Shibuya I, Yamashita H (2000) Centrally
administered orexin/hypocretin activates HPA axis in rats. NeuroReport 11(9):1977–1980
Lavie P (2001) Sleep disturbances in the wake of traumatic events. N Engl J Med 345:1825–1832
LeDoux JE (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:155–184
Lehmann J, Feldon J (2000) Long-term biobehavioral effects of maternal separation in the rat:
consistent or confusing? Rev Neurosci 11(4):383–408
Lenox RH, Kant GJ, Sessions GR, Pennington LL, Mougey EH, Meyerhoff JL (1980) Specific
hormonal and neurochemical responses to different stressors. Neuroendocrinology 30(5):300–308
Levine S (2005) Developmental determinants of sensitivity and resistance to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30(10):939–946
Liston C, Miller MM, Goldwater DS, Radley JJ, Rocher AB, Hof PR, Morrison JH, McEwen BS
(2006) Stress-induced alterations in prefrontal cortical dendritic morphology predict selective
impairments in perceptual attentional set-shifting. J Neurosci 26(30):7870–7874
Liu X, Tang X, Sanford LD (2003) Fear-conditioned suppression of REM sleep: relationship to
Fos expression patterns in limbic and brainstem regions in BALB/cJ mice. Brain Res
991(1–2):1–17
Liu X, Yang L, Wellman LL, Tang X, Sanford LD (2009) GABAergic antagonism of the central
nucleus of the amygdala attenuates reductions in rapid eye movement sleep after inescapable
footshock stress. Sleep 32(7):888–896
Liu X, Wellman LL, Yang L, Ambrozewicz MA, Tang X, Sanford LD (2011) Antagonizing
corticotropin-releasing factor in the central nucleus of the amygdala attenuates fear-induced
reductions in sleep but not freezing. Sleep 34(11):1539–1549
Luppi PH (2010) Neurochemical aspects of sleep regulation with specific focus on slow-wave
sleep. World J Biol Psychiatry 11(Suppl 1):4–8
Machado RB, Tufik S, Suchecki D (2008) Chronic stress during paradoxical sleep deprivation
increases paradoxical sleep rebound: association with prolactin plasma levels and brain
serotonin content. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33(9):1211–1224
Machado RB, Tufik S, Suchecki D (2010) Modulation of Sleep Homeostasis by Corticotropin
Releasing Hormone in REM Sleep-Deprived Rats. Int J Endocrinol 2010:326151
Machida M, Yang L, Wellman LL, Sanford LD (2013) Effects of stressor predictability on escape
learning and sleep in mice. Sleep 36:421–430
Magarinos AM, McEwen BS (1995a) Stress-induced atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal
CA3c neurons: comparison of stressors. Neuroscience 69(1):83–88
Magarinos AM, McEwen BS (1995b) Stress-induced atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal
CA3c neurons: involvement of glucocorticoid secretion and excitatory amino acid receptors.
Neuroscience 69(1):89–98
Magarinos AM, Verdugo JM, McEwen BS (1997) Chronic stress alters synaptic terminal
structure in hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94(25):14002–14008
Maier SF, Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, Watkins LR (2006) Behavioral control, the medial
prefrontal cortex, and resilience. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8(4):397–406
Marrosu F, Gessa GL, Giagheddu M, Fratta W (1990) Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF)
increases paradoxical sleep (PS) rebound in PS-deprived rats. Brain Res 515(1–2):315–318
McEwen BS (2007) Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the
brain. Physiol Rev 87(3):873–904
Meerlo P, Turek FW (2001) Effects of social stimuli on sleep in mice: non-rapid-eye-movement
(NREM) sleep is promoted by aggressive interaction but not by sexual interaction. Brain Res
907(1–2):84–92
406
L. D. Sanford et al.
Meerlo P, Pragt BJ, Daan S (1997) Social stress induces high intensity sleep in rats. Neurosci Lett
225(1):41–44
Meerlo P, de Bruin EA, Strijkstra AM, Daan S (2001a) A social conflict increases EEG slowwave activity during subsequent sleep. Physiol Behav 73(3):331–335
Meerlo P, Easton A, Bergmann BM, Turek FW (2001b) Restraint increases prolactin and REM
sleep in C57BL/6 J mice but not in BALB/cJ mice. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol
281(3):R846–R854
Meerlo P, Mistlberger RE, Jacobs BL, Heller HC, McGinty D (2009) New neurons in the adult
brain: the role of sleep and consequences of sleep loss. Sleep Med Rev 13(3):187–194
Mellman TA, Bustamante V, Fins AI, Pigeon WR, Nolan B (2002) REM sleep and the early
development of posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 159(10):1696–1701
Mellman TA, Pigeon WR, Nowell PD, Nolan B (2007) Relationships between REM sleep findings
and PTSD symptoms during the early aftermath of trauma. J Trauma Stress 20(5):893–901
Mikkelsen JD, Hay-Schmidt A, Kiss A (2004) Serotonergic stimulation of the rat hypothalamopituitary-adrenal axis: interaction between 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors. Ann N Y Acad Sci
1018:65–70
Miklos IH, Kovacs KJ (2003) Functional heterogeneity of the responses of histaminergic neuron
subpopulations to various stress challenges. Eur J Neurosci 18(11):3069–3079
Miller RR, Daniel D, Berk AM (1974) Successive reversals of a discriminated preference for
signaled tailshock. Anim Learn Behav 2(4):271–274
Mitra R, Jadhav S, McEwen BS, Vyas A, Chattarji S (2005) Stress duration modulates the
spatiotemporal patterns of spine formation in the basolateral amygdala. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 102(26):9371–9376
Mrdalj J, Pallesen S, Milde AM, Jellestad FK, Murison R, Ursin R, Bjorvatn B, Gronli J (2013)
Early and later life stress alter brain activity and sleep in rats. PLoS One 8(7):e69923
Myers KM, Davis M (2007) Mechanisms of fear extinction. Mol Psychiatry 12(2):120–150
Neckelmann D, Mykletun A, Dahl AA (2007) Chronic insomnia as a risk factor for developing
anxiety and depression. Sleep 30(7):873–880
Nishida M, Pearsall J, Buckner RL, Walker MP (2009) REM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the
consolidation of human emotional memory. Cereb Cortex 19(5):1158–1166
Nishino S, Ripley B, Overeem S, Lammers GJ, Mignot E (2000) Hypocretin (orexin) deficiency
in human narcolepsy. Lancet 355(9197):39–40
Nomura K, Nakao M, Takeuchi T, Yano E (2009) Associations of insomnia with job strain,
control, and support among male Japanese workers. Sleep Med 10(6):626–629
Obal F Jr, Kacsoh B, Alfoldi P, Payne L, Markovic O, Grosvenor C, Krueger JM (1992) Antiserum
to prolactin decreases rapid eye movement sleep (REM sleep) in the male rat. Physiol Behav
52(6):1063–1068
Obal F Jr, Garcia-Garcia F, Kacsoh B, Taishi P, Bohnet S, Horseman ND, Krueger JM (2005) Rapid
eye movement sleep is reduced in prolactin-deficient mice. J Neurosci 25(44):10282–10289
Pacak K, Palkovits M (2001) Stressor specificity of central neuroendocrine responses: implications
for stress-related disorders. Endocr Rev 22(4):502–548
Palma BD, Suchecki D, Tufik S (2000) Differential effects of acute cold and footshock on the
sleep of rats. Brain Res 861(1):97–104
Pawlyk AC, Morrison AR, Ross RJ, Brennan FX (2008) Stress-induced changes in sleep in
rodents: Models and mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32(1):99–117
Paylor R, Tracy R, Wehner J, Rudy J (1994) DBA/2 and C57BL/6 mice differ in contextual fear
but not auditory fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 108:810–817
Petrov T, Krukoff TL, Jhamandas JH (1992) The hypothalamic paraventricular and lateral
parabrachial nuclei receive collaterals from raphe nucleus neurons: a combined double
retrograde and immunocytochemical study. J Comp Neurol 318(1):18–26
Petrov T, Krukoff TL, Jhamandas JH (1994) Chemically defined collateral projections from the
pons to the central nucleus of the amygdala and hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus in the
rat. Cell Tissue Res 277(2):289–295
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
407
Peyron C, Petit JM, Rampon C, Jouvet M, Luppi PH (1998a) Forebrain afferents to the rat dorsal
raphe nucleus demonstrated by retrograde and anterograde tracing methods. J Neurosci
82(2):443–468
Peyron C, Tighe DK, van den Pol AN, de Lecea L, Heller HC, Sutcliffe JG, Kilduff TS (1998b)
Neurons containing hypocretin (orexin) project to multiple neuronal systems. J Neurosci
18(23):9996–10015
Phillips RG, LeDoux JE (1992) Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued
and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 106(2):274–285
Pitman RK, Shin LM, Rauch SL (2001) Investigating the pathogenesis of posttraumatic stress
disorder with neuroimaging. J Clin Psychiatry 62(Suppl 17):47–54
Popa D, El Yacoubi M, Vaugeois JM, Hamon M, Adrien J (2006) Homeostatic regulation of sleep
in a genetic model of depression in the mouse: effects of muscarinic and 5-HT1A receptor
activation. Neuropsychopharmacology 31(8):1637–1646
Prewitt CM, Herman JP (1997) Hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical regulation following
lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala. Stress 1(4):263–280
Prewitt CM, Herman JP (1998) Anatomical interactions between the central amygdaloid nucleus
and the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus of the rat: a dual tract-tracing analysis. J Chem
Neuroanat 15(3):173–185
Price J, Russchen F, Amaral D (1987). The limbic region. II: the amygdaloid complex. In: Swanson L
(ed) Handbook of chemical neuroanatomy. Integrated systems of the CNA, Part I. Elsevier, New
York, pp 279–375
Rachalski A, Alexandre C, Bernard JF, Saurini F, Lesch KP, Hamon M, Adrien J, Fabre V (2009)
Altered sleep homeostasis after restraint stress in 5-HTT knock-out male mice: a role for
hypocretins. J Neurosci 29(49):15575–15585
Radley JJ, Morrison JH (2005) Repeated stress and structural plasticity in the brain. Ageing Res
Rev 4(2):271–287
Radley JJ, Rocher AB, Miller M, Janssen WG, Liston C, Hof PR, McEwen BS, Morrison JH
(2006) Repeated stress induces dendritic spine loss in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb
Cortex 16(3):313–320
Rampin C, Cespuglio R, Chastrette N, Jouvet M (1991) Immobilisation stress induces a
paradoxical sleep rebound in rat. Neurosci Lett 126(2):113–118
Rojas-Zamorano JA, Esqueda-Leon E, Jimenez-Anguiano A, Cintra-McGlone L, Mendoza
Melendez MA, Velazquez Moctezuma J (2009) The H1 histamine receptor blocker,
chlorpheniramine, completely prevents the increase in REM sleep induced by immobilization
stress in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 91(3):291–294
Roky R, Obâal F Jr, Valatx JL, Bredow S, Fang J, Pagano LP, Krueger JM (1995) Prolactin and
rapid eye movement sleep regulation. Sleep 18(7):536–542
Roozendaal B, Koolhaus J, Bohus B (1991a) Attenuated cardiovascular, neuroendocrine, and
behavioral responses after a single footshock in central amygdaloid lesioned male rats. Phys
Behav 50:771–775
Roozendaal B, Koolhaus J, Bohus B (1991b) Central amygdala lesions affect behavioral and
autonomic balance during stress in rats. Phys Behav 50:777–781
Rosenkranz JA, Buffalari DM, Grace AA (2006) Opposing influence of basolateral amygdala and
footshock stimulation on neurons of the central amygdala. Biol Psychiatry 59(9):801–811
Rudolph C, Richards GE, Kaplan S, Ganong WF (1979) Effect of intraventricular histamine on
hormone secretion in dogs. Neuroendocrinology 29(3):169–177
Sajdyk TJ, Schober DA, Gehlert DR, Shekhar A (1999) Role of corticotropin-releasing factor and
urocortin within the basolateral amygdala of rats in anxiety and panic responses. Behav Brain
Res 100(1–2):207–215
Sakurai T, Amemiya A, Ishii M, Matsuzaki I, Chemelli RM, Tanaka H, Williams SC, Richarson JA,
Kozlowski GP, Wilson S, Arch JR, Buckingham RE, Haynes AC, Carr SA, Annan RS, McNulty
DE, Liu WS, Terrett JA, Elshourbagy NA, Bergsma DJ, Yanagisawa M (1998). Orexins and
orexin receptors: a family of hypothalamic neuropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors that
regulate feeding behavior. Cell 92(5):1 page following 696
408
L. D. Sanford et al.
Samson WK, Taylor MM, Follwell M, Ferguson AV (2002) Orexin actions in hypothalamic
paraventricular nucleus: physiological consequences and cellular correlates. Regul Pept
104(1–3):97–103
Sandi C, Davies HA, Cordero MI, Rodriguez JJ, Popov VI, Stewart MG (2003) Rapid reversal of
stress induced loss of synapses in CA3 of rat hippocampus following water maze training. Eur
J Neurosci 17(11):2447–2456
Sanford LD, Tejani-Butt SM, Ross RJ, Morrison AR (1995) Amygdaloid control of alerting and
behavioral arousal in rats: involvement of serotonergic mechanisms. Arch Ital Biol
134(1):81–99
Sanford LD, Nassar P, Ross RJ, Schulkin J, Morrison AR (1998) Prolactin microinjections into
the amygdalar central nucleus lead to decreased NREM sleep. Sleep Res Online 1(3):109–113
Sanford LD, Silvestri AJ, Ross RJ, Morrison AR (2001) Influence of fear conditioning on elicited
ponto-geniculo-occipital waves and rapid eye movement sleep. Arch Ital Biol 139(3):169–183
Sanford LD, Parris B, Tang X (2002) GABAergic regulation of the central nucleus of the
amygdala: implications for sleep control. Brain Res 956(2):276–284
Sanford L, Yang L, Tang X (2003a) Influence of contextual fear on sleep architecture in mice:
a strain comparison. Sleep 26:527–540
Sanford LD, Fang J, Tang X (2003b) Sleep after differing amounts of conditioned fear training in
BALB/cJ mice. Behav Brain Res 147(1–2):193–202
Sanford LD, Tang X, Ross RJ, Morrison AR (2003c) Influence of shock training and explicit fearconditioned cues on sleep architecture in mice: strain comparison. Behav Genet 33(1):43–58
Sanford LD, Xiao J, Liu X, Yang L, Tang X (2005) Influence of avoidance training (AT) and AT
cues on sleep in C57BL/6 J (B6) and BALB/cJ (C) mice. Sleep (Abstract Supplement) 28:A6
Sanford LD, Yang L, Liu X, Tang X (2006) Effects of tetrodotoxin (TTX) inactivation of the
central nucleus of the amygdala (CNA) on dark period sleep and activity. Brain Res
1084(1):80–88
Sanford LD, Yang L, Wellman LL, Liu X, Tang X (2010) Differential effects of controllable and
uncontrollable footshock stress on sleep in mice. Sleep 33(5):621–630
Schiffelholz T, Aldenhoff JB (2002) Novel object presentation affects sleep–wake behavior in
rats. Neurosci Lett 328(1):41–44
Seltzer AM, Donoso AO, Podesta E (1986) Restraint stress stimulation of prolactin and ACTH
secretion: role of brain histamine. Physiol Behav 36(2):251–255
Semba K, Fibiger HC (1992) Afferent connections of the laterodorsal and the pedunculopontine
tegmental nuclei in the rat: a retro- and antero-grade transport and immunohistochemical
study. J Comp Neurol 323(3):387–410
Shors TJ, Seib TB, Levine S, Thompson RF (1989) Inescapable versus escapable shock modulates
long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Science 244(4901):224–226
Silvestri AJ (2005) REM sleep deprivation affects extinction of cued but not contextual fear
conditioning. Physiol Behav 84(3):343–349
Sinha RK (2006) P-CPA pretreatment reverses the changes in sleep and behavior following acute
immobilization stress rats. Journal Physiol Sci: JPS 56(1):123–129
Smith C (1995) Sleep states and memory processes. Behav Brain Res 69(1–2):137–145
Smith C, Lapp L (1986) Prolonged increases in both PS and number of REMS following a shuttle
avoidance task. Physiol Behav 36(6):1053–1057
Smith CT, Miskiman DE (1975) Increases in paradoxical sleep as a result of amygdaloid
stimulation. Physiol Behav 15(1):17–19
Smith SM, Vale WW (2006) The role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in neuroendocrine responses to stress. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8(4):383–395
Steiger A (2007) Neurochemical regulation of sleep. J Psychiatr Res 41(7):537–552
Steiger A, Holsboer F (1997) Neuropeptides and human sleep. Sleep 20(11):1038–1052
Steiger A, Antonijevic IA, Bohlhalter S, Frieboes RM, Friess E, Murck H (1998) Effects of
hormones on sleep. Horm Res 49(3–4):125–130
Steiner MA, Sciarretta C, Brisbare-Roch C, Strasser DS, Studer R, Jenck F (2013) Examining the
role of endogenous orexins in hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis endocrine function using
Stress, Arousal, and Sleep
409
transient dual orexin receptor antagonism in the rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology
38(4):560–571
Steriade M, McCarley R (1990) Brainstem control of wakefulness and sleep. Plenum Press, New
York
Stewart MG, Davies HA, Sandi C, Kraev IV, Rogachevsky VV, Peddie CJ, Rodriguez JJ,
Cordero MI, Donohue HS, Gabbott PL, Popov VI (2005) Stress suppresses and learning
induces plasticity in CA3 of rat hippocampus: a three-dimensional ultrastructural study of
thorny excrescences and their postsynaptic densities. Neuroscience 131(1):43–54
Suchecki D, Tiba PA, Machado RB (2012) REM sleep rebound as an adaptive response to
stressful situations. Frontiers Neurol 3:41
Sutcliffe JG, de Lecea L (2002) The hypocretins: setting the arousal threshold. Nat Rev Neurosci
3(5):339–349
Swerdlow N, Geyer M, Vale W, Koob G (1986) Corticotropin-releasing factor potentiates acoustic
startle in rats: blockade by chlordiazepoxide. Psychopharmacology 88:147–152
Takahashi K, Koyama Y, Kayama Y, Yamamoto M (2000) The effects of prolactin on the
mesopontine tegmental neurons. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 54(3):257–258
Tang X, Xiao J, Liu X, Sanford LD (2004) Strain differences in the influence of open field
exposure on sleep in mice. Behav Brain Res 154(1):137–147
Tang X, Liu X, Yang L, Sanford LD (2005a) Rat strain differences in sleep after acute mild
stressors and short-term sleep loss. Behav Brain Res 160(1):60–71
Tang X, Xiao J, Parris BS, Fang J, Sanford LD (2005b) Differential effects of two types of
environmental novelty on activity and sleep in BALB/cJ and C57BL/J mice. Physiol Behav
85:419–429
Tang X, Yang L, Liu X, Sanford LD (2005c) Influence of tetrodotoxin inactivation of the central
nucleus of the amygdala on sleep and arousal. Sleep 28(8):923–930
Tang X, Yang L, Sanford LD (2005d) Rat strain differences in freezing and sleep alterations
associated with contextual fear. Sleep 28(10):1235–1244
Tang X, Yang L, Sanford LD (2006) Spectral EEG power after uncontrollable shock (US) and
fearful context (FC): variability amongst mouse strains. Sleep 29:A11
Tang X, Yang L, Sanford LD (2007) Individual variation in sleep and motor activity in rats.
Behav Brain Res 180(1):62–68
Tiba PA, Tufik S, Suchecki D (2004) Effects of maternal separation on baseline sleep and cold
stress-induced sleep rebound in adult Wistar rats. Sleep 27(6):1146–1153
Tiba PA, Tufik S, Suchecki D (2008) Long lasting alteration in REM sleep of female rats
submitted to long maternal separation. Physiol Behav 93(3):444–452
Tononi G, Cirelli C (2006) Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev 10(1):49–62
Tsuda A, Ida Y, Tsujimaru S, Satoh H, Nishimura H, Tanaka M (1987) Stressor controllability
and brain noradrenaline turnover in rats. Yakubutsu Seishin Kodo 7(3):363–374
Van Bockstaele EJ, Colago EE, Valentino RJ (1998) Amygdaloid corticotropin-releasing factor
targets locus coeruleus dendrites: substrate for the co-ordination of emotional and cognitive
limbs of the stress response. J Neuroendocrinol 10(10):743–757
Vertes RP (2004) Differential projections of the infralimbic and prelimbic cortex in the rat.
Synapse 51(1):32–58
Vyas A, Jadhav S, Chattarji S (2006) Prolonged behavioral stress enhances synaptic connectivity
in the basolateral amygdala. Neuroscience 143(2):387–393
Walker MP (2009) The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1156:168–197
Wellman CL (2001) Dendritic reorganization in pyramidal neurons in medial prefrontal cortex
after chronic corticosterone administration. J Neurobiol 49(3):245–253
Wellman LL, Holbrook BD, Yang L, Tang X, Sanford LD (2008) Contextual fear extinction
eliminates sleep disturbances found following fear conditioning in rats. Sleep 31:1035–1042
Wellman LL, Ambrozewicz MA, Yang L, Machida M, Sanford LD (2013) Basolateral amygdala
and the regulation of fear conditioned changes in sleep: role of corticotropin releasing factor.
Sleep 36:471–480
410
L. D. Sanford et al.
Winsky-Sommerer R, Yamanaka A, Diano S, Borok E, Roberts AJ, Sakurai T, Kilduff TS, Horvath
TL, de Lecea L (2004) Interaction between the corticotropin-releasing factor system and
hypocretins (orexins): a novel circuit mediating stress response. J Neurosci 24(50):11439–11448
Winsky-Sommerer R, Boutrel B, de Lecea L (2005) Stress and arousal: the corticotrophinreleasing factor/hypocretin circuitry. Mol Neurobiol 32(3):285–294
Xu L, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ (1997) Behavioural stress facilitates the induction of long-term
depression in the hippocampus. Nature 387(6632):497–500
Yang L, Tang X, Wellman LL, Liu X, Sanford LD (2009) Corticotropin releasing factor (CRF)
modulates fear-induced alterations in sleep in mice. Brain Res 1276:112–122
Yang L, Wellman LL, Ambrozewicz MA, Sanford LD (2011a) Effects of stressor predictability
and controllability on sleep, temperature, and fear behavior in mice. Sleep 34(6):759–771
Yang L, Wellman LL, Tang X, Sanford LD (2011b) Effects of corticotropin releasing factor
(CRF) on sleep and body temperature following controllable footshock stress in mice. Physiol
Behav 104(5):886–892
Zhu GQ, Zhong MK, Zhang JX, Zhao LZ, Ke DP, Wang M, Shi L (1998) Role of basolateral
amygdaloid nuclei in sleep and wakeful state regulation. Sheng Li Xue Bao 50(6):688–692
Ziegler DR, Cass WA, Herman JP (1999) Excitatory influence of the locus coeruleus in
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis responses to stress. J Neuroendocrinol 11(5):361–369