International Journal of Art Therapy
Formerly Inscape
ISSN: 1745-4832 (Print) 1745-4840 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rart20
Looking together: Joint attention in art therapy
Jonathan Isserow
To cite this article: Jonathan Isserow (2008) Looking together: Joint attention in art therapy,
International Journal of Art Therapy, 13:1, 34-42, DOI: 10.1080/17454830802002894
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17454830802002894
Published online: 03 Jun 2008.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 2317
View related articles
Citing articles: 14 View citing articles
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rart20
Download by: [University of London]
Date: 23 November 2016, At: 20:44
International Journal of Art Therapy, June 2008; 13(1): 3442
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Looking together: Joint attention in art therapy
JONATHAN ISSEROW
Abstract
This paper examines the phenomenon of looking together in art therapy. It argues that the triangular relationship on which much of
art therapy theory is premised takes as a priori the capacity of the patient to look together with the therapist at the art object. This
capacity, however, cannot be taken as a given but is based on early childhood development, emerging out of the motherinfant
relationship where in ordinary growth, the infant’s looking at the mother develops into looking with her. This paper explores the
development of joint attention skills in relation to art therapeutic practice drawing together child developmental and psychoanalytic
perspectives.
Keywords: Joint attention skills, primary and secondary subjectivity, blind infants, triangular relationship, autistic spectrum disorder,
theory of mind
Introduction
This paper aims to investigate the use and function of
the eyes within art therapy. It is particularly interested
in how both patient1 and art therapist look together at
the art object, in an attempt to share the feeling and
possible meaning that its making may hold. The act of
looking together at the object is of vital importance for
art therapy as it joins up the vertices of the triangle
between patient, image and therapist, enabling the
structure of the art therapeutic relationship.
‘Looking together’ is often such a routine phenomenon within art therapy that the behaviour itself
can easily go by unobserved. It is only when working
with clients where this capacity for joint attention is
missing or compromised that its importance is
brought to the fore. Far from being a given, this
capacity to look together, co-ordinating attention and
sharing affect around a third object is premised on a
series of significant developmental milestones
achieved within infancy (Bakeman & Adamson, 1984).
Drawing on child developmental research and
psychoanalytic theory, this paper explores how joint
attention emerges between mother and infant in
ordinary development. This understanding will then
be used to inform the absence and presence of joint
attention within two contrasting clinical vignettes: the
first with a profoundly autistic young woman; the
second with an ordinary developing adolescent boy.
The paper concludes with some reflections on using
child developmental research to approach the familiar
art therapy concept of triadic relating in a new way.
Triadic relating within art therapy
The theoretical formulation of the triadic relationship
between client, image and therapist has long been a
source of lively investigation (Case, 2000; Dalley,
Rifkind, & Terry, 1993; Schaverien, 2000; Wood,
1984, 1990). Lines of inquiry have explored the interrelational dynamic between the three constituents and
how they may bring about understanding of and
change within the client. Case (2000, p. 27) suggests
that this conceptualisation has been a useful one,
having an outer-world function of distinguishing the
profession from other forms of therapy. It also has an
inner-world function of exploring the dynamic intrarelationship within the art therapy room, generating
much art therapy theory.
Wood (1990) has conceptualised art within the
triangular relationship as being akin to a third person
in the room. Dalley et al. (1993) have further
personified this third person, writing extensively about
the reciprocal, informing and reflexive inter-relationship between the ‘three voices of art therapy’.
Additional conceptualisations include the Winnicottian triangle of mother, child and transitional object
(1958/1971), superimposed onto the art therapy
triangle (Wood, 1984). While moving theoretically and
developmentally along, the mother, father, child
Correspondence: Roehampton University, School of Human and Life Sciences, Arts and Play Therapies, Whitelands College, Holybourne Avenue,
London SW15 4JD, UK. Tel: 020 8392 3118. E-mail: j.isserow@roehampton.ac.uk
1745-4832 (print)/1745-4840 (online) # 2008 British Association of Art Therapists
DOI: 10.1080/17454830802002894
Looking together
triangular relationship also brings irresistibly to mind
the classic Oedipal constellation (Britton, 1989; Freud,
1905; Klein, 1928). Case has argued that art therapy
enables the fostering of the ‘third position of the
Oedipal triangle, in that bringing in the medium
creates a triangular interaction’ (2006, p. 207).
Other writers have explored the particular dynamics of transference, counter-transference and
aesthetic counter-transference within the triangular
relationship (Case, 2000; Schaverien, 2000; Simon,
1995; Wood, 1990). Case (2000) has challenged the
simplicity of the triangular shape arguing for more
complex forms to match the multiple and refracted
projections within the art therapy room. Schaverien
(2000) has explored how different emphases on the
triangular axis may determine the kind of therapy
being practiced (i.e. whether it is art therapy, art
psychotherapy or analytic art psychotherapy).
Back to basics
The above theories describe how the patient, therapist
and art object may be linked up from a variety of
perspectives. However, behind this rich and complex
body of knowledge lies an a priori assumption which
supposes that the patient can use their eyes to look
together with the therapist at the art object. The
capacity to look together and share feelings in a
linked-up triangular way on which the above
theories are premised though, has its developmental
precursors in infancy. This capacity is a result of a
fundamental developmental milestone in the growth
of the child’s mind. Its significance marks the
achievement of a group of emotional and cognitive
abilities that are known collectively within child
development research as joint attention skills.
These skills are characterised by the development of
a range of socially embedded behaviours and include
the infant giving, showing and pointing things out to
others; often looking back and forth between objects
and the other’s eyes; requesting objects to play with
and initiating games (Hobson, 1993, p. 266).
Damarell (1999) has written about the absence of
the idea of joint attention within art therapy theory,
suggesting that ‘an important opportunity to recognise
that the triangular relationship involves two pairs of
eyes and a target object’ (1999, p. 45) has been missed.
Damarell goes on to suggest that this omission may be
as a result of the concept of ‘joint attention’ emerging
from the field of developmental psychology, a theoretical framework not often drawn on within art
therapy literature, despite its potential to add significant support and insight. Notable exceptions to
this can be found in the work of Damarell (1999);
Evans and Dubowski (2001); Hosea (2006); and Case
(2006).
35
My interest in joint attention skills has emerged out
of my work at a school for profoundly autistic young
people and a contrasting experience of working within
a Child and Family Consultation Service. Children at
the school often avoid eye contact, becoming mesmerised by inanimate objects which seemed to
function as autistic objects (Tustin, 1981) denying any
knowledge of a separate ‘live object’ (Alvarez, 1992)
within the room. The capacity to look together at an
object in any shared or joined up manner seems like a
developmental quantum leap away. This is in striking
contrast to the children at the Consultation Service
who are able, in a more ordinary manner, to look at
me and with me at the art object they have produced.
This striking contrast in experience of relating
between these two contexts has prompted me to ask:
What might be the developmental conditions within
the infant which enable triadic relating to occur?
What are the developmental implications should this
capacity be compromised? How can this understanding inform the work of art therapy whose
premise, broadly speaking, is one of an overlap of coordinated looking, thinking and feeling around the art
object? It is to this development of joint attention skills
that this paper now turns.
Joint attention skills
Joint attention skills are a group of behaviours which
usually develop within the latter part of the infant’s
first year of life and include the sharing of attentional
focus and affect around a common object (Scaife &
Brunner, 1975). Joint attention episodes are triadic in
nature and involve the alternating co-ordination of
attention to the object and to the parent. It is socially
embedded, emerging before language, and is clearly a
part of proto-communication (Newson & Newson,
1975). It not only includes the sharing of attention
with another but also includes the monitoring and
shaping of the other’s attention around an object. This
is often achieved through the use of pointing, gesture
and referential eye contact. An example of this can be
Figure 1.
36
J. Isserow
seen in Figure 1 where a young child and her mother
share the experience of looking together at an event or
object outside the picture plane. It is possible to
speculate that their enjoyment is as much to do with
the sharing of the experience, as the event itself.
The development of the capacity to look and feel
together has its roots in the infant’s earliest relationship (Bates, 1979). Prior to the relative sophistication
of triadic relating, infants are engaged almost exclusively with dyadic interactions which include a
significant amount of time where the infant and his or
her care-giver look at each other. Fraiberg suggests
that the motherinfant ‘engagement of the eyes is part
of the universal code of the human fraternity, which is
read as a greeting and an acknowledgement to the
‘‘other’’ long before it can have meaning for the
infant’ (1974, p. 210). This visual acknowledgement
and experience of mutual gazing between mother and
infant is central to the establishment of the infant’s
attachment to his or her mother or primary care-giver
(Holmes, 1993).
The perceptual nature of the eyes forms one of the
main sensory domains where the infant’s intersubjective experience of the other is shaped. Looking at each
other provides a conduit for the sharing of affect,
which along with other psychophysiological care such
as holding, protecting and containing enables the
motherinfant relationship to grow (Holmes, 1993).
The meeting of eyes between infant and mother
provides a point of mind-to-mind connection and
connectedness. Frith suggests that the infant’s communication and social competency is acquired
through the understanding of the ‘language of the
eyes’. The sharing of mental states underpins the
varied meaning that the gaze may take. ‘If there were
no mental states, then a language of the eyes would
not exist’ (Frith, 1989, p. 143).
Stern (1985) has closely followed how reciprocal eye
contact between mother and infant is dependent on
the primary care-giver’s capacity for sensitive attunement. This includes allowing for moments of stimulation as well as rest between periods of interactions
where the infant is given the space to self-regulate
before re-engaging with the other. Trevarthen’s idea of
‘primary inter-subjectivity’ (1979) encapsulates this
behaviour at this period and refers to the infant’s
capacity to attend to only one person at a time in an
inter-affective manner.
At around six months of age the infant begins to
orientate himself not only to the object but also to his
care-giver’s attitude to the object. Trevarthen calls this
joint sharing of interest in another object ‘secondary
intersubjectivity’ (1979), requiring a flexibility of
orientation to begin to see things from the other’s
point of view. Through the intimate inter-affective
experience of the mother’s mind, the infant begins to
realise that other people have minds separate to his
own (Baron-Cohen, 2004; Hobson, 2002). From here
the infant is gradually able to construct a theory about
the existence of other minds. With this ‘theory of
mind’ (Baron-Cohen, 2004) the infant becomes aware
that other people have thoughts and attitudes to
objects and events in and of their own right. As such,
the other’s thoughts and attitudes towards the third
object become as interesting as the object itself.
The theory of mind hypothesis has also influenced
recent psychoanalytic and attachment theory research
into borderline personality disorder. Fonagy, Gergely,
Jurist, and Target (2004) have proposed that joint
attention and associated mirroring activity between
care-giver and baby are central to the development of
mentalisation. Mentalisation is broadly defined as the
ability to appreciate the existence of mental states
both in ourselves and in others, and that these internal
intentional states influence the behaviour of people in
the world. This ‘knowledge’ about minds enables an
understanding of emotions, thoughts, feelings and
intentions which in turn underpins communication
and relationships with others.
Joint attention skills culminate when infants’ interactions become triadic as object-focused attention
becomes embedded in social contexts (Bates, 1979;
Harding & Golinkoff, 1979). As the child develops he
uses proto-declarative gesturing to actively share
attention to an event, object or ‘topic’ (Leung &
Rheingold, 1981). Bates (1979) suggests that the
capacity for joint attention becomes consolidated at
around 13 months where the infant readily enters into
nonverbal referential communication with a person
about a present object (Bates, 1979; Harding &
Golinkoff, 1979). Looking together at an object
therefore entails firstly the understanding that other
people have minds separate from one’s own; secondly,
that other minds have thoughts of their own which
can be directed to other objects and events in the
world; and thirdly the capacity to orientate to the
other’s point of view (Hobson, 1993).
It is useful here to differentiate between protodeclarative and proto-imperative pointing. Proto-declarative pointing is used by the infant in an attempt
to shape attention and share an experience with the
other and is marked by the presence of affect. It is
indicative that the child has an understanding of other
people’s intentionality (Diessel, 2006). In contrast, the
intention of proto-imperative pointing by the child is
the achievement of a concrete goal, such as a toy that
is out of reach or a biscuit in the cupboard.
Congenitally blind infants
Having established the role of looking and feeling
together in the growth of joint attention, it is useful
as a corollary to explore the development of joint
attention skills in congenitally blind infants where this
Looking together
perceptual apparatus for looking is either impaired or
compromised. Selma Fraiberg’s longitudinal study of
ten blind infants has pointed to how dependent
primary care-givers are on eye-to-eye contact with
their babies to determine levels of engagement and
recognition. Fraiberg writes about the need for eye
contact in motherinfant relation:
when the eyes do not meet ours in acknowledgement of our
presence, it feels curiously like a rebuff. Certainly, mothers
attribute ‘knowing’ and ‘recognition’ to a baby’s sustained regard
of the face long before he can actually discriminate and recognise
faces . . . (1974, p. 210)
Fraiberg’s work highlights the developmental danger
for blind infants who cannot make an emotional
connection to their primary carer through visual
means. The consequence for the blind infant of not
finding a common sensory domain to share mental
states with his care-giver is perilous and there are a
significant number of blind children who show grave
impairment in their humanobject relationships as a
result, according to Fraiberg.
In order to mitigate against the loss of relational
connectedness as a result of impaired vision with these
children, Fraiberg and her team help care-givers
become aware of and shift their focus to the ‘hand
language’ employed by their blind infants. This hand
language is a means of establishing that vital human
link to another mind within the first months of life.
This idea of ‘hand language’ seems to function
developmentally in a similar way to Frith’s idea of ‘eye
language’, as both serve to share mental states
between infant and mother. When intersubjectivity
cannot be achieved through the immediacy of visual
contact between mother and infant, it needs to be
found in other sensory domains, if developmental
complications are to be avoided. Intersubjectivity
through the tactile delicacy of the hands, rather than
through the dance of the eyes provides such an
alternative route. Interestingly blind infants, who
manage to have an early experience of inter-affectivity,
develop delayed joint attention skills which occur at
around two years of age (Hobson, 2002).
Looking together and autism
In comparison to blind infants, there is another
clinical group that find eye contact problematic, albeit
for very different reasons. Young children and adults
on the autistic spectrum often experience great
difficulties in making eye contact with ‘live company’
(Alvarez, 1992). This difficulty is so marked that
avoidant eye contact is seen as one of the foremost
indicators of early childhood autism (Hobson, 2002, p.
257). Frith (1989, p. 143) argues against the reliability
of this commonly held understanding that children
with autism avoid eye contact. Instead, she suggests
37
that these children do not differentiate their quality of
looking between inanimate objects and people; however, it is the animate objects who complain about this
experience.
Both Frith and Hobson agree that the central
difficulty with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) children seems to be the ‘disruption in these children’s
capacity for reciprocal, affective relatedness with
others’ (Hobson, 1993, p. 258). As such these children
are in danger of never discovering the ‘language of the
eyes’ between mother and child. Hobson (2002, p.
192) suggests that the blind toddlers under two and
autistic toddlers both have difficulties with developing
joint attention skills but for different reasons:
In each case there is something that makes it very difficult for the
young child to relate to someone else’s orientation towards the
world and towards himself . . .The difference between them is
that in the case of blindness, the child cannot see how other
people relate to the world; in the case of the sighted child with
autism, the child cannot see how other people relate to the world.
(Hobson’s emphasis)
While blind infants may be enabled to ‘see’ how
people relate to the world, through the help of a
sensitive care-giver, ASD children face greater difficulties. Lack of inter-affective relatedness compromises the infant’s capacity to understand people as
having minds of their own with their own points of
view about objects and events in the world. The
child’s ability to move out from their single perspective
and ‘step into the other’s shoes’ is also significantly
curtailed. As a result, the development of a mental
picture of the other’s mind is disrupted and their
capacity to look together is significantly under-developed. Within art therapy theory the impact of this
developmental deficit has been explored by Dubowski
(1990), Tipple (1993), Tipple (1994) and Evans and
Dubowski (2001).
Psychoanalytic perspectives on looking together
The use and function of the eyes in early development
has a long history of exploration within psychoanalytic
literature (Freud, 1910; Winnicott, 1971; Wright,
1991). In addition, the capacity to relate triadically
has also been extensively examined within this field.
From a psychoanalytic perspective the external behaviour of joint attention skills can be seen to be
determined by the infant’s internal capacity to see
himself as separate from whole objects as well as able
to think triadically about the inter-relationship between objects. The awareness of being separate
necessitates the awareness that people have relationships to other people and objects from which he can
be excluded (Britton, 1989; Freud, 1905; Klein, 1928).
The manifest capacity to see the connection between
people and objects separate from self is predicated on
38
J. Isserow
the infant’s internal shift from dyadic to triadic (or
Oedipal) relating and the development of ‘triangular
space’ (Britton, 1989) in the child’s mind.
Burhouse (2000, p. 64) suggests that the infant’s
capacity for triangular thinking where the ‘child can
begin to think about the links between himself, his
environment and other minds’ is dependent on the
acknowledgement of being on the outside of a
parental couple. Similarly, the capacity for the patient
to think about the link between himself, the art object
and the art therapist’s mind is dependent on this
earlier Oedipal struggle.
Looking back and forth between the art object and
the therapist’s eyes requires knowledge and toleration
of triadic relating, where two things can be held
simultaneously in mind, requiring the capacity for
‘two track thinking’ (Alvarez & Furgiuele, 1997).
Interestingly, according to Klein (1928), Oedipal
relating emerges towards the end of the second half of
the first year of life, which corresponds closely in time
to the emergence of joint attention skills according to
developmental research (Bates, 1979; Trevarthen,
1979).
Clinical application
Keeping the over-lapping perspectives of psychoanalysis and developmental psychology in mind, it is now
possible to explore how this understanding of joint
attention may inform clinical work. Two contrasting
case vignettes are presented, one with a profoundly
autistic young women; the other with an ordinary
developing adolescent boy. To maintain client confidentiality, all names have been changed.
Mary
I first met Mary when working in a residential school
for children and young people with severe learning
difficulties, located in a rural village in the South-East
of England. Many of the children were diagnosed as
being on the autistic spectrum disorder with challenging behaviour. Mary was coming up to her 17th
birthday when I first met her. She had a slightly
plump, round face, and she looked younger than her
years. This youthful quality was added to by her dark
hair being pulled back into a neat ponytail at the back
of her head. She had been living at the school for
several years and little was known of her early
childhood experience. Her mother kept in tenuous
contact with her carers. Mary was referred to art
therapy as her carers were concerned about her selfinjurious behaviour when she became distressed.
Despite these episodes, Mary was a quiet young
woman.
On meeting Mary I was aware that she was highly
adept at looking everywhere but at me or she would
look straight through me at a black hole somewhere
behind my head. Making direct eye contact seemed to
be too difficult an option for her. She had a tendency
to rock hypnotically in her chair when seated at the
art table or on the balls of her feet when standing in
the room. She would often vigorously bite the back of
her hand when she became particularly agitated. She
never used spoken language but would respond to the
occasional simple requests and verbal prompts after
some encouragement. I worked with Mary for seven
months prior to her leaving the school.
Mary always came compliantly to her weekly
sessions and would mostly sit at the art table gently
rocking in her chair. She would often suck at the palm
of her hand so that it was wet with saliva or allow her
saliva to drench her clothes and the table that she was
sitting at. Repetitively, she would hold this glistening
wet hand up in front of her face, trailing a thread of
saliva from her mouth to her hand, before returning
her hand to her mouth. She would then line up her
eyes, following the glistening thread, linking up her
mouth and her hand. Outside of this idiosyncratic
behaviour, Mary’s visual line of focus had a hollowness
and far-away quality. It mostly felt as if she was in a
world of her own and I was often aware of the pull to
slip into a dull mindlessness of my own when working
with her. In attempts to keep mentally alive within the
sessions, I would suggest that she could use the art
materials. This she would do after much encouragement and would pour out paint into large amorphous
puddles on the paper (Figure 2). She seemed more
interested in the flowing quality of the materials than
what they could do. Much of the work revolved
around her making these large messy paintings within
the boundary of the piece of paper. My comments to
her either about the quality of her use of material or
her use of paint and saliva to blur the distinction
between self and other, never seemed to impact.
Over the course of time Mary’s over-flowing use of
paint and saliva prompted me to suggest that she
make use of the flowing water and containers in the
Figure 2.
Looking together
large aluminium sink located in the corner of the
room. This was done in an attempt to offer both a
practical and psychological way to hold her and her
work.
Mary was able to make greater use of the water and
containers, becoming more enlivened at the sink. She
was eager to place her hands into the flowing water
and would frequently bring her wet hand up to her
mouth. Her eyes often became transfixed by the
flowing water in a more mindless manner reminiscent
of her use of saliva. However there were times when
she would tip out the container or turn on and off the
tap when I spoke to her about the container being
either full or empty. Although materials were all used
in a very concrete manner, there was the occasional
glimmer of a shared experienced when Mary seemed
to briefly become aware of my words and presence in
the room.
Discussion
The striking factor of my time with Mary was the
overriding absence of any shared quality to our work.
She would often fix her stare into the distance and
there was little evidence of coordinated looking
between the image and my eyes in any referential way.
It predominantly felt as if my mind, my thoughts and
my words held no interest for her. Mary’s haunting
lack of eye contact often left me feeling as if I was not
there and it felt like there was little differentiation
between how she related to an inanimate object
compared to a live person in the room. Martin Buber
makes a distinction between ‘IThou’ and ‘IIt’
relating (1958), the latter of which seems to accurately
describe the way that Mary related to me in the art
therapy room. As the work progressed it often felt like
a real struggle to remain mentally alive to her.
Mary’s use of her saliva and flowing quality of the
paint seemed to suggest that its liquid quality was
being used to maintain a fused and primitive state of
mind. The concrete connection between her hand
and mouth seemed to be reinforced by her gaze as she
followed the bridging thread of saliva. It may be
speculated that Mary was using her eyes to obfuscate
the difference between her hand and her mouth.
Possibly even seeing parts of herself as different,
separate but connected, felt terrifying for her. This use
of saliva and paint is reminiscent of Tustin’s conceptualisation of the ‘autistic object’ which ‘is an
object which is experienced as being totally ‘‘me’’’
(1981, p. 118). Any awareness of a ‘not-me’ or of
difference of self and other seems to have been
obviated, as difference would threaten the frightening
possibility of relating. If there is no distinction between
self and other then ‘nothing can get in but more
importantly, nothing can get out’ (1981, p. 118). Her
fixed focus on her saliva suggests that possibly she
39
used her eyes to close off any gaps in experiencing
difference and experiencing the other.
It is possible to speculate that Mary’s lack of
differentiation between self and other curtailed her
experience of me as having a mind of my own which
could be interested in her art work, providing another
perspective to her own. Without an awareness of other
minds Mary would not be able to develop a theory of
mind, and to look at things from another point of
view, all of which are important for the development
of joint attention. I wondered about Mary’s early
experiences of intersubjectivity in light of her difficulty
in sharing mental states. Hobson suggests that ‘autistic
children’s deficient capacity for affectively grounded
interpersonal relations seriously constrains both their
connectedness to and differentiation from other
people, and for this reason impairs their capacity to
symbolize’ (1993, p. 255). Mary seemed to use the art
materials in a very concrete manner to sustain this
lack of connectedness and differentiation.
In working with Mary there was an attempt to
reach her mind through art-making where there
always was the potential for the art materials to take
on a more expressive or symbolic quality. Towards the
end of the work with Mary her use of water in the
containers at the sink had, at times, a quality that
suggested that she was more aware of objects having
an inside and an outside which could be separate
(Bion, 1962). It was at these moments when she
seemed dimly aware of another mind in the room and
became responsive to my words that there was a
greater sense of a shared experience although these
were momentary and fleeting. However, these experiences sustained the work and belief in the art
therapeutic relationship which could be maintained
despite the lack of visual joint attention.
Dyadic relating posed significant difficulties for
Mary. This was evident in her avoidant eye contact
and quality of looking at people as if they were the
same as furniture. Mary’s capacity to relate triadically
seemed to be a considerable developmental achievement away. Working with her highlighted the enormity and importance of the need to establish a
shared experience with the other, in order for a
therapeutic relationship to develop.
Paul
Paul was referred to art therapy as part of a treatment
offered to him and his family within a Child,
Adolescent and Family Consultation Service in the
South-East of England. Paul was 12 years old when I
met him, he was tall for his age, had brown hair and a
pleasant and expressive face. From the start of our
work, Paul was very insightful into and articulate
about his difficulties. Based on a single episode, he
complained of being anxious at school in case he was
40
J. Isserow
sick. This anticipatory anxiety had developed to the
point that it deterred him from attending for extended
periods of time. His background was one of being the
youngest child in the family with two much older
siblings who had already left the family home. Paul’s
parents were also experiencing marital difficulties, a
constant concern for Paul. Another difficulty for Paul
was going to sleep at night, which seemed to
precipitate a great deal of separation anxiety for him.
In an attempt to help him, his mother had begun to
sleep on a spare mattress in his bedroom.
In his sessions Paul was engaging and quick to work
with the art materials. In his first session he chose to
work with clay, looking up at me every now and then
before returning to his interest to shape the clay. Once
it was finished he placed it on the table between us
and we were able to talk together about his experience
of making and about the object itself: a disembodied
head in a state of fright (Figure 3). When I wondered
with Paul if there might be a link between the look of
fright on the clay head and his own feelings, he
became quiet, seemed to think about this for a
moment or two before shrugging his shoulders saying
‘maybe’.
Discussion
Paying close attention to how Paul both coordinated
and alternated his gaze between the art object and
myself hopefully highlights how the act of looking
together often goes by unnoticed. However, it contrast
to Mary, Paul’s looking together seems to be in the
service of object relating. His referential and coordinated eye contact during the making of the clay object
suggests that Paul is aware that I have a mind of my
own. Indeed it is possible that he was using his eyes to
determine what kind of mind he had in the room with
him.
Paul’s ‘maybe’ suggests that at the moment when I
make a connection between what he has made and
how he feels, he can orientate himself towards another
perspective, demonstrating a flexibility of perceptual
orientation. At that moment it seemed as if Paul was
stepping out of his perspective and looking at the clay
head from another’s point of view. Psychoanalytically,
this capacity to reflect on self is founded on the
recognition of the sexual link between the parental
couple (Britton, 1989). The fact that his mother was
now sleeping in his room might indicate that it was
this very issue of Oedipal relating that Paul was
struggling with now as an adolescent, in the face of his
parents’ marital difficulties.
Although this paper is restricted in its focus to how
the art therapeutic relationship is underpinned and
informed by joint attention, it is interesting to note
that both Mary and Paul seemed to be struggling with
the similar issue of separation. Mary’s limited use of
her eyes and rigid focus on her wet hand seemed to
function as a way of denying any sense of separateness
from her surroundings, maintaining a fused state of
mind. As such, her eyes were used to avoid any
awareness of other people and the possibility of
relating to them. Paul on the other hand had a far
greater developed mind seemingly grown from an
early experience of emotional relatedness. Paul was
struggling to use his eyes to gain insight into himself
and begin to ‘look’ at some of his difficult feelings of
being separate, in relation to his artistic production
and the mind of the therapist.
The development of joint attention skills also has
implications for art-making itself. Damarell (1999)
links the infant’s graphic development with the joint
attentional behaviour of proto-declarative pointing,
where there is ‘a transition from pointing to literally
drawing another person’s attention to a phenomenon
in the shared environment’ (p. 45, Damarell’s italics).
Paul’s art object ‘points’ to his difficulties and can be
understood as declarative in nature. While Mary’s use
of paint has the potential for being thought about
together, she seems to display little awareness that she
can ‘point’ to her image as a target for both of us to
think about.
Conclusion
Figure 3.
The paper has attempted to explore the patient’s
capacity to alternate attention to the art object and
the therapist in art therapy by investigating how this
Looking together
capacity for joint attention emerges between mother
and infant in ordinary development. It has argued
that joint attention skills emerge out of the infant’s
intersubjective experience of looking at the primary
care-provider and slowly develops during the latter
part of infancy into looking with her. It is dependent
on the infant’s understanding that other people have
minds separate to their own; that others’ minds have
thoughts of their own which can be directed to objects
and events in the world, as well as the infant
developing the capacity to orientate to the other’s
point of view. It also requires that the infant pays
attention to both social and object aspects of their
surroundings. Psychoanalytically, it is dependent on
the capacity to see self as separate in relation to whole
objects, as well as tolerating being on the outside of
the parental couple enabling the child to think about
the inter-relatedness of objects.
Clearly understanding how patients use their eyes
within sessions is a complex issue.
The implication for art therapy is that the patient
requires the underlying cognitive and emotional skills
(Burhouse, 2000, p. 51) of joint attention should they
be able to look, think and reflect on their own position
and the position of their image within the session.
Looking together at the art object is determined by
the dance of the eyes between patient, therapist and
art object. It requires an interaffective experience,
enabled by the therapist’s sensitive and attuned (Stern,
1985) responses to the patient as well as the patient’s
capacity to share affect with the therapist. Thinking
around what the image may reflect or mirror back
(Winnicott, 1971; Damarell, 1999) to the patient may
need to be supported by the therapist within the
triangular relationship. The development of joint
attention by congenitally blind infants suggest that the
vital element is to have a shared experience between
mother and infant and the need to find alternative
sensory fields for contact, should the visual senses be
impaired. The above work with Mary confirms the
need to establish a shared experience within art
therapy before joint attention can develop. For
patients such as Mary, the tactile qualities of the art
materials can potentially provide an alternate domain
for this shared experience.
The dependence on the use of the eyes and visual
joint attention skills (Scaife & Brunner, 1975) in art
therapy distinguishes it from other forms of therapy.
The development of visual joint attention between the
ordinary developing infant and his mother has and
continues to be extensively investigated by developmental psychology. As such, this body of knowledge
offers another point of view where new links to
existing understanding in art therapy can be made. As
looking together at the physical art object is such a
ubiquitous and distinguishing feature of art therapy, it
seems important to begin to integrate some of this
41
research into our own theoretical thinking and
understanding of the art therapeutic relationship.
Acknowledgements
I would like to express my gratitude to Barrie Damarell
for his thoughtfulness in helping bring this paper into
fruition.
Note
1
In this article ‘patient’ and ‘client’ are used interchangeably. In
addition, pronouns used for client/patient and infant are in the
masculine unless referring to a specific individual. Similarly, the
feminine pronoun is used for ‘primary care-giver’ and ‘therapist’.
This is done to aid comprehension.
References
Alvarez, A. (1992). Live company: Psychoanalytic psychotherapy with
autistic, borderline, deprived and abused children. London:
Routledge.
Alvarez, A., & Furgiuele, P. (1997). Speculations on components in the
infant’s sense of agency: The sense of abundance and the capacity to
think in parentheses. In S. Reid (Ed.), Developments in infant observation:
The Tavistock model (pp. 123139). London: Routledge.
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to people
and objects in motherinfant and peerinfant interactions. Child
Development, 55, 12781289.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2004). Theory of mind and autism: A fifteen year
review. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.),
Understanding other minds (pp. 320). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in
infancy. London and New York: Academic Press.
Bion, W.R. (1962). Learning from experience. London: Heinemann.
Britton, R. (1989). The missing link: Parental sexuality in the Oedipus
complex. In J. Steiner (Ed.), The Oedipus complex today: Clinical
implications (pp. 83101). London: Karnac.
Buber, M. (1958). I and thou. Edinburgh: Clark.
Burhouse, A. (2000). Now we are two, going on three. International
Journal of Infant Observation, 4(2), 5167.
Case, C. (2000). Our Lady of the Queen: Journeys around the maternal
object. In A. Gilroy & G. McNeilly (Eds.), The changing shape of art
therapy: New developments in theory and practise (pp. 1554). London and
Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Case, C. (2006). Imagining animals: Art, psychotherapy and primitive
states of mind. London and New York: Routledge.
Dalley, T., Rifkind, G., & Terry, K. (1993). The three voices of art therapy.
London: Routledge.
Damarell, B. (1999). Just forging, or seeking love and approval? Inscape,
4(2), 4450.
Diessel, H. (2006). Demonstratives, joint attention, and the emergence
of grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(4), 463.
Dubowski, J. (1990). Art versus language: Separate development during
childhood. In C. Case & T. Dalley (Eds.), Working with children in art
therapy (pp. 722). London: Routledge.
Evans, K., & Dubowski, J. (2001). Art therapy with children on the autistic
spectrum: Beyond words. London and Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley
Publishers.
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2004). Affect regulation,
mentalisation and the development of the self. London and New York:
Karnac.
Fraiberg, S. (1974). Blind infants and their mothers: An examination of
their sign system. In M. Lewis & L. Roesenblum (Eds.), The effects of
the infant on the caregiver (pp. 209223). New York and London: Wiley.
Freud, S. (1905). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. London: Penguin
Books.
Freud, S. (1910). Leonardo da Vinci and a memory of his childhood.
London: Penguin Books.
42
J. Isserow
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell
publishers.
Harding, C., & Golinkoff, R. (1979). The origins of intentional
vocalisations in prelinguistic infants. Child Development, 50, 3340.
Hobson, P. (1993). Through feeling and sight to self and symbol. In V.
Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self (pp. 254279). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Hobson, P. (2002). The cradle of thought. London: Macmillan.
Hosea, H. (2006). ‘The Brush’s Footmarks’: Parents and infants paint
together in a small community art therapy group. International Journal
of Art Therapy: Inscape, 11(2), 6978.
Holmes, J. (1993). John Bowlby and attachment theory. London and New
York: Routledge.
Klein, M. (1928). Early stages of the Oedipus conflict. International
Journal of Psycho-Analysis, Volume 9 (pp. 167180). London: Blackwell.
Leung, E., & Rheingold, H. (1981). Development of pointing as a social
gesture. Developmental Psychology, 17, 215220.
Newson, J., & Newson, E. (1975). Intersubjectivity and the transmission
of culture. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 28, 437445.
Scaife, M., & Brunner, J.S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual
attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265266.
Schaverien, J. (2000). The triangular relationship and the aesthetic
countertransference in analytical art psychotherapy. In A. Gilroy &
G. McNeilly (Eds.), The changing shape of art therapy: New developments in
theory and practise (pp. 5583). London and Philadelphia: Jessica
Kingsley Publishers.
Simon, R. (1995). The symbolism of style. London: Routledge.
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from
psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Tipple, R. (1993). Challenging assumptions: The importance of
transference processes in work with people with learning difficulties.
Inscape, summer, 29.
Tipple, R. (1994). Communication and interpretation in art therapy
with people who have a learning disability. Inscape, 2, 3135.
Tustin, F. (1981). Autistic states in children. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and co-operation in early
infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa
(Ed.), Before speech: The beginnings of interpersonal communication (pp. 321
348). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Winnicott, D.W. (1958/1971). Transitional objects and transitional
phenomena. In Playing and reality (pp. 134). London: Tavistock.
Winnicott, D.W. (1971). Mirror-role of mother and family in child
development. In Playing and Reality (pp. 149159). London: Tavistock.
Wood, M. (1984). The child and art therapy: A psychodynamic
viewpoint. In T. Dalley (Ed.), Art as therapy (pp. 6281). London:
Tavistock.
Wood, C. (1990). The triangular relationship (1): The beginnings and
the endings of art therapy. Inscape: Journal of Art Therapy, winter, 713.
Wright, K. (1991). Vision and separation. London: Free Association Books.
Biographical details
Jonathan Isserow is a state registered art therapist who
has worked extensively within child, adolescent and
family psychiatry. He has an MA in Psychoanalytic
Observational Studies from the Tavistock Clinic and is
the Programme Co-ordinator for the MA Art Therapy
training at Roehampton University, London.