Academia.eduAcademia.edu

The influence of music on mood and performance while driving

2012, Ergonomics

Mood can influence our everyday behaviour and people often seek to reinforce, or to alter their mood, for example by turning on music. Music listening while driving is a popular activity. However, little is known about the impact of music listening while driving on physiological state and driving performance. In the present experiment, it was investigated whether individually selected music can induce mood and maintain moods during a simulated drive. In addition, effects of positive, negative, and no music on driving behaviour and physiological measures were assessed for normal and high cognitive demanding rides. Subjective mood ratings indicated that music successfully maintained mood while driving. Narrow lane width drives increased task demand as shown in effort ratings and increased swerving. Furthermore, respiration rate was lower during music listening compared to rides without music, while no effects of music were found on heart rate. Overall, the current study demonstrates that music listening in car influences the experienced mood while driving, which in turn can impact driving behaviour.

This art icle was downloaded by: [ Universit y of Groningen] On: 09 January 2012, At : 00: 50 Publisher: Taylor & Francis I nform a Lt d Regist ered in England and Wales Regist ered Num ber: 1072954 Regist ered office: Mort im er House, 37- 41 Mort im er St reet , London W1T 3JH, UK Ergonomics Publicat ion det ails, including inst ruct ions f or aut hors and subscript ion inf ormat ion: ht t p: / / www. t andf online. com/ loi/ t erg20 The influence of music on mood and performance while driving Marj olein D. van der Zwaag , Joyce H. D. M. West erink a a a b b , Chris Dij kst erhuis , Dick de Waard & Karel A. Brookhuis b , Ben L. J. M. Mulder b b Philips Research Laborat ories, High Tech Campus 34, 5654AE Eindhoven, t he Net herlands b Behavioural and Social Sciences, Universit y of Groningen, Grot e Kruisst raat 2/ 1, 9712TS, Groningen, t he Net herlands Available online: 16 Dec 2011 To cite this article: Marj olein D. van der Zwaag, Chris Dij kst erhuis, Dick de Waard, Ben L. J. M. Mulder, Joyce H. D. M. West erink & Karel A. Brookhuis (2012): The inf luence of music on mood and perf ormance while driving, Ergonomics, 55: 1, 12-22 To link to this article: ht t p: / / dx. doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 00140139. 2011. 638403 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTI CLE Full t erm s and condit ions of use: ht t p: / / www.t andfonline.com / page/ t erm s- and- condit ions This art icle m ay be used for research, t eaching, and privat e st udy purposes. Any subst ant ial or syst em at ic reproduct ion, redist ribut ion, reselling, loan, sub- licensing, syst em at ic supply, or dist ribut ion in any form t o anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warrant y express or im plied or m ake any represent at ion t hat t he cont ent s will be com plet e or accurat e or up t o dat e. The accuracy of any inst ruct ions, form ulae, and drug doses should be independent ly verified wit h prim ary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, act ions, claim s, proceedings, dem and, or cost s or dam ages what soever or howsoever caused arising direct ly or indirect ly in connect ion wit h or arising out of t he use of t his m at erial. Ergonomics Vol. 55, No. 1, January 2012, 12–22 The influence of music on mood and performance while driving Marjolein D. van der Zwaaga,b*, Chris Dijksterhuisb, Dick de Waardb, Ben L.J.M. Mulderb, Joyce H.D.M. Westerinka and Karel A. Brookhuisb a Philips Research Laboratories, High Tech Campus 34, 5654AE Eindhoven, the Netherlands; bBehavioural and Social Sciences, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712TS Groningen, the Netherlands Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 (Received 4 May 2011; final version received 2 November 2011) Mood can influence our everyday behaviour and people often seek to reinforce, or to alter their mood, for example by turning on music. Music listening while driving is a popular activity. However, little is known about the impact of music listening while driving on physiological state and driving performance. In the present experiment, it was investigated whether individually selected music can induce mood and maintain moods during a simulated drive. In addition, effects of positive, negative, and no music on driving behaviour and physiological measures were assessed for normal and high cognitive demanding rides. Subjective mood ratings indicated that music successfully maintained mood while driving. Narrow lane width drives increased task demand as shown in effort ratings and increased swerving. Furthermore, respiration rate was lower during music listening compared to rides without music, while no effects of music were found on heart rate. Overall, the current study demonstrates that music listening in car influences the experienced mood while driving, which in turn can impact driving behaviour. Practitioners Summary: Even though it is a popular activity, little is known about the impact of music while driving on physiological state and performance. We examined whether music can induce moods during high and low simulated drives. The current study demonstrates that in car music listening influences mood which in turn can impact driving behaviour. The current study shows that listening to music can positively impact mood while driving, which can be used to affect state and safe behaviour. Additionally, driving performance in high demand situations is not negatively affected by music. Keywords: music mood induction; demand; simulated drive; respiration rate; heart rate; driving behaviour 1. Introduction In Western society, music listening has become a frequent activity in the background of almost any activity (DeNora 2000, North and Hargreaves 2008). Music research has now started to focus on music listening in these specific everyday life situations to improve the understanding of how music can influence personal experiences and behaviour (DeNora 2003, Juslin and Sloboda 2010). Driving is one of the most popular music listening situational contexts. While driving, people listen to music to attain enjoyment or to feel engaged when driving in solitude (DeNora 2000, Walsh 2010). It also is suggested that music listening distracts from driving and can therefore influence safety (Brodsky 2002). Although the impact of music on driving performance has been given some attention (Dibben and Williamson 2007), its impact on mood and physiological measures has not. Neither has a distinction been made between the respective impacts of the specific types of music such as positive and negative valence music. In the current article, these *Corresponding author. Email: mvanderzwaag@gmail.com ISSN 0014-0139 print/ISSN 1366-5847 online Ó 2012 Taylor & Francis http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2011.638403 http://www.tandfonline.com relationships between music valence and driving demand on mood, physiological measures, and driving performance are studied. 1.1. Music listening The potential of music to influence mood is described as one of the most important functions of music (Juslin and Sloboda 2010, Van der Zwaag and Westerink 2010, Van der Zwaag et al. 2011). Although music is known to influence mood, it is still under discussion whether people perceive the expressed state within the music (cognitivist view) or whether music can actually induce moods in listeners (emotivists view) (Kivy 1989, 1990). Evidence of the fact that music induces emotions is for example found by Kastner and Crowder (1990) who showed that major mode music is perceived as more happy compared to minor mode music. Furthermore, fast tempo music has consistently shown to increase arousal levels compared to slow tempo music (Krumhansl 1997, Van der Zwaag et al. 2011). 13 Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 Ergonomics Support for the emotivist position of the influence of music on emotion comes from the growing body of evidence that music can influence physiological responses and thus body state (for an overview see Hodges (2010)). Heart rate and respiration rate (RR) are the most frequently investigated psycho-physiological responses to music (Hodges 2010). The majority of studies in the music literature have found that arousing music increases heart rate compared to low arousing music (DeJong et al. 1973, Knight and Rickard 2001). Still, others have found that any music, both low and high arousing, increases heart rate (Krumhansl 1997, Iwanaga and Moroki 1999, Rickard 2004). RR is found to increase in high arousing compared to sedative music as well (Iwanaga and Moroki 1999, Krumhansl 1997, Nyklı́cek et al. 1997 Gomez and Danuser 2004). Again, in other studies no difference in RR while listening to different types of music was found (Davis 1992, Van der Zwaag and Westerink 2011a). Hence, inconsistent results are found on heart rate and RR responses to music listening. Note further that these latter studies presented music listening as main task, and thus not as in the background of a concurrent activity, such as driving. Still, it is implied that mood can remain when music is played in the background of a concurrent activity, as Van der Zwaag and Westerink (2011b) showed the persistence of musically induced moods in the background of a distracting task. Several explanations can be given for the inconsistent results found for the influence of music on physiological measures. A first explanation comes from the fact that studied physiological measures are affected by regulatory effects in the autonomic nervous system (ANS) which is primarily responsible for keeping homeostasis (Cacioppo et al. 2000b). As a result, physiological responses are not solely influenced by emotional state via, for example, music listening but additionally via physical activity, cognitive demand, and other psychological constructs (Cacioppo et al. 2000b, Van den Broek and Westerink 2009). Hence, the situational context should be taken into account in interpreting physiological responses to music listening. A second explanation can be found in the fact that most studies in music research differ to a great extent on important methodological aspects, such as the song selection method and the duration of the music presentation. For example, Van der Zwaag and Westerink (2011a) showed that the physiological response patterns to positive and negative music mood induction start to differentiate after an average of 4 min. Hence, the physiological responses to music listening in studies presenting relatively short music excerpts cannot be compared to studies inducing moods with music over longer periods. For the study of physiological responses to music, awareness of these methodological aspects is important, as is the perspective to always describe the impact of music to emotions in relation with personal and situational context (Blacking 1973, Saarikallio and Erkkilä 2007, North and Hargreaves 2008, Sloboda and Juslin 2010). 1.2. Music while driving Music can be beneficial while driving as, for example, the mood-arousal hypothesis predicts that in cases of boredom and drowsiness music can lead to a more optimal arousal level which could benefit driving performance (North and Hargreaves 2008, Shek and Schubert 2009). However, following the distraction hypothesis, music can also take attention away from the driver (Shek and Schubert 2009). This distracting effect of music on driving can be disadvantageous when it decreases safety in case high arousing music is played during high demand road situations (Dibben and Williamson 2007). On the contrary, Wiesenthal et al. (2000) showed that one’s favourite music alleviates stress during high congestion drives and found higher stress levels when comparing no music to favourite music during high congestion drives. Furthermore, it is shown that driver aggression can be tempered with favourite music compared to no music in high demanding rides (Wiesenthal et al. 2003). Explanations for the effects of music listening while performing a concurrent task such as driving often focus on processing capacity in service of the primary task (North and Hargreaves 1999, Dalton and Behm 2007, Pêcher et al. 2009) and assume that listening to music may be arousing and therefore requires mental resources. Following the information–distraction approach, music adds additional irrelevant stimuli to a task which leads to increased cognitive load and thus can impact task performance (Konecni 1982, North and Hargreaves 1999, Recarte and Nunes 2000). Consequently, the more attention a particular music requires the more it competes for processing resources with the primary task of, for example, driving. To illustrate, North and Hargreaves (1999) manipulated cognitive load of participants by exposing them to low or high arousing music by varying tempo and volume in a driving game. They found that high arousing music resulted in worse racing performance defined as slower lap times, while the quickest lap times were recorded when listening to low arousing music. Interestingly, they also found a connection between task demand and music liking, and concluded that competition for processing resources caused particpants to dislike music. Pêcher et al. (2009) mentioned that post-experiment interviews revealed that drivers found happy music the most disturbing Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 14 M.D. van der Zwaag et al. and, combined with behavioural data, took this as support to conclude that listening to happy music resulted in deteriorated driving performance. The impact of in-vehicle music listening on driving speed depends on the road situational context. Reducing speed is found to be used as a compensatory reaction when faced with high load situations, namely it enables the driver to maintain safety margins by decreasing required reaction times (Summala 2005). Furthermore, as mentioned above it can be expected that drivers allocate more attention, and thus mental resources, to positive music which could result in detrimental effects on vehicular control or in a compensatory reaction such as slowing down. Because task performance and music listening might compete for the same mental resources, the impact of musically evoked cognitive demand on performance might be dependent on the cognitive demand of a concurrent task (Konecni 1982, North and Hargreaves 1999). In low demand drive situations, there is less competition for attentional space. Hence, it is likely that mental resources can more easily be divided between listening to music and driving as the limits of mental resources are not reached. Therefore, listening to music will not impact driving performance in these low-demand situations. As lane width is known to influence drivers workload, this variable could be used to manipulate primary task demands when studying the relation between listening to positively and negatively rated music. Results reported in the literature show that when driving in narrow lanes, less manoeuvring space is available for the driver, and more attention is required to prevent driving errors such as drifting out of the driving lane and to maintain personal safety margins (De Waard et al. 1995, Dijksterhuis et al. 2011). This results in smaller deviations from the driver’s preferred lateral position (LP) on the road (De Waard et al. 1995, Dijksterhuis et al. 2011) and a compensatory speed reduction (Godley et al. 2004). In terms of physiological responses, higher demand situations would lead to increased heart rate and RR. 1.3. Expectations The aim of the current study was twofold. First, we wanted to investigate whether musically induced positive and negative moods persist during low and high demand drives. Moreover, we investigated whether the presence of positive compared to negative music influences the resources mobilised, as reflected in the amount of effort invested while driving. We expected that positive and negative valence can be successfully induced during music mood induction (Van der Zwaag and Westerink 2010, 2011a). Furthermore, based on Van der Zwaag and Westerink (2011b), we expect that positive and negative music mood induction will persist while driving. Furthermore, we expect that the presence of positive or negative moods will remain during high demand drives. Secondly, the influence of musically induced mood on driving performance in high (narrow lane width) and low (wide lane width) demanding drives was investigated. Hence, lane width was used to further investigate the relation between listening to positively and negatively rated music and primary task demands. First of all, while driving, we expected increased RRs and heart rate during high compared to low demanding drives. In accordance with Dijksterhuis et al. (2011), less swerving (i.e. reduced variation in LP) was expected in narrow lanes. Furtheremore, we expected that lane width reduction would lower the speed to compensate for the higher amount of resources allocated in the more demanding drive (De Waard et al. 1995, Godley et al. 2004). We expected that music would solely influence driving performance in high demand drives, as in those conditions music competes with the limited amount of mental resources available. 2. Method 2.1. Participants The study had been approved by the local ethics committee and informed consent was obtained from all participants. Nineteen participants, 13 men and 6 women, were paid 45 Euros for participating. Age ranged from 22 to 44 years (mean ¼ 27.5; SD ¼ 5.2) and participants had held their driving licence for 4 to 22 years (mean ¼ 8.8; SD ¼ 4.9). Self reported total mileage driven ranged from 6000 to 700,000 km (median ¼ 45,000; inter-quartile range (IQR) ¼ 77,500 km) and current yearly mileage ranged from 1500 to 6000 km (median ¼ 7000; IQR ¼ 5000). 2.2. Design Three music conditions were included: positive music, negative music and no music (which was included as an extra control condition). In addition, two levels of lane width (wide 3.00 m or narrow 2.50 m) were created in the driving simulator, corresponding to low and high demand drives, respectively (Dijksterhuis et al. 2011). Participants completed four sessions on separate days: one introduction session and three experimental sessions. In each experimental session, one music level was presented and both lane widths were used. This resulted in a within-subject design including two 15 Ergonomics Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 repeated-measures factors: music (3) and lane width (2). The orders of both the music and lane width factors were counterbalanced among participants. 2.3. Music stimuli selection Because music preference is highly personal (Hargreaves and North 2010), songs used as stimuli were selected individually. To do so, participants completed an introductory session prior to the experimental sessions. In this session, participants rated 60 songs on perceived valence and energy levels on 7point Likert scales. The participants did not have to listen to the entire song but were encouraged to sample each song for a few moments and at a few locations within the song to get a good impression of the song. The 60 songs included were selected to have a large range of valence and energy values and were selected from a database containing 1800 songs in total. The songs were selected based on energy and valence labels which were acquired by automatic classification of music characteristics into mood labels (Skowronek et al. 2006, 2007). The order of the song presentation was randomised over participants. After participants had finished the ratings, nine songs were selected per participant per music condition (positive/negative) in such a way that valence ratings differed as much as possible between the positive and negative songs while keeping energy ratings as average as possible. Subsequently, three of the selected songs were used for the music mood induction, three songs for the high demand drive, and three songs for the low demand drive. The duration of the three songs was adjusted, using Audacity (Version 1.2.4), to 8 min, keeping the average duration of each song about equal. This was done by cutting the song to about 2.45 min and fading out the new ending of the song. To check the selected song stimuli on their valence and energy ratings, a repeated-measures ANOVA with music (positive/negative) as within-subject factor on the valence and energy ratings of the selected songs was conducted. Results showed a main effects of Music on both energy and valence ratings: valence F (1,17) ¼ 231.20, p 5 0.001, Z2 ¼ 0.93; energy F (1,17) ¼ 16.04, p 5 0.001, Z2 ¼ 0.49. This confirmed that the selected song stimuli for the two Music conditions were significantly different from each other in valence and energy. The positive songs showed higher valence and energy ratings compared to the negative songs; positive songs mean (SE) valence M ¼ 5.8 (0.17), energy M ¼ 4.7 (0.20), negative songs valence M ¼ 2.3 (1.4), energy M ¼ 3.3 (0.26), on scales running from 1 to 7. 2.4. Simulator and driving conditions The study was conducted using a ST SoftwareÓ driving simulator. This simulator consists of a fixed-base vehicle mock up with functional steering wheel, indicators, and pedals. The simulator was surrounded by three 3200 diagonal plasma screens. Each screen provided a 708 view, leading to a total 2108 view. A detailed description of the functionality of the driving simulator used can be found in Van Winsum and Van Wolffelaar (1993). Participants drove the simulated car (width: 1.65 m) over two sections of uninterrupted two-lane roads (2.50 m or 3.00 m wide lanes), winding through rural scenery, and separated by a small town. Roads in each section consisted mainly of easy curves (about 80%) with a constant radius of 380 m and ranging in length from 120 to 800 m. The road surface was marked on the edges by a continuous line (20 cm wide), in the centre by a broken line (15 cm), and outside the edges a soft shoulder was present. The posted speed limit during the drive was 80 km/h. In addition, a stream of oncoming traffic was introduced with a random interval gap between 2 and 6 s, resulting in 15 passing passenger cars (width: 1.75 m) per minute on average. No vehicles appeared in the participant’s driving lane. 2.5. Measures 2.5.1. Subjective ratings Subjective mood scores of valence (ranging from unpleasant to pleasant) and energy (ranging from tired/without energy to awake/full of energy) and calmness ratings (ranging from tense to calm) were assessed using the UWIST Mood Adjective Checklist (UMACL) (Matthews et al. 1990). This UMACL contains eight unipolar items for each dimension, which start with: ‘right now I am feeling. . .’, and range from 1: ‘not at all’ to 7: ‘very much’. The rating scale mental effort (RSME) was used to assess mental effort (Zijlstra 1993). The RSME is a unidimensional scale, ranging from 0 to 150, used to rate mental effort. In addition to digits, several effort indications (calibrated anchor points) are visible alongside the scale to further guide rating. Indications start with ‘absolutely no effort’ (RSME score of 2) and end with ‘extreme effort’ (RSME score of 112). 2.5.2. Physiology Physiological measures covered ANS reactions in the cardiovascular and respiratory domain. The Portilab data recorder and its accompanying Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 16 M.D. van der Zwaag et al. sensors were used to record these responses with a sample frequency of 250 Hz (version 1.10, Twente Medical Systems International, Oldenzaal, the Netherlands). Physiological measures were assessed continuously during the experiment. The MATLAB programming environment (2009, The Mathworks, Natick, MA) was used for the pre-processing of the respiration signals. Cardiovascular measures were recorded using an electrocardiogram (ECG) using three Ag-AgCl electrodes, which were placed following the standard lead II placement (Stern et al. 2001). R-peaks in the ECG signal were detected automatically, after amplification and filtering of the signal (Butterworth band pass: 0.5– 40 Hz). Subsequently, the distances between successive R peaks, the interbeat intervals (IBI), were calculated. Respiration was recorded by means of a respiration belt (RespitraceTM, Twente Medical systems). To obtain the respiration measures, noise was excluded from the raw signal and movement artifacts were reduced by a 0.005–1.0 Hz band pass IIR filter. The amount of respiration cycles per minute indicated the RR (Wientjes 1992, Grossman and Taylor 2007). 2.5.3. Driving parameters Speed and LP were sampled at 10 Hz. Lateral position is defined as the difference in metres between the centre of the participant’s car and the middle of the (right hand) driving lane. Positive LP values correspond to deviations towards the left-hand shoulder and negative values correspond to deviations toward the right-hand shoulder. The sampled LP values were used to calculate mean LP and the standard deviation of LP, i.e. swerving. 2.6. Procedure Participants were invited four times to the driving simulator facility of the University of Groningen. During the first introductory session, the participants were informed about the experiment, signed an informed consent, drove a 6-min practice drive, and completed the music rating. During the three subsequent experimental sessions, physiological sensors were attached and participants were seated in the simulator chair. Next, physiological baseline values were acquired in a habituation period during which participants watched a Coral Sea diving movie for 8 min (Piferi et al. 2000). Hereafter, participants filled out the UMACL. Then an 8-min music mood induction period started in which the participants were asked to listen to the music. To remain attentive to the music, participants were asked to listen to the music carefully to be able to answer questions regarding the music after the entire experiment. During the control session in which no music was presented, participants were asked to sit and relax for 8 min. The participants were not informed that mood induction took place during these 8 min, as this could bias the results. After 8 min, the participants filled out the UMACL again. Next, the first simulated drive began. The participants were instructed to drive as they would normally drive. After approximately 8 min, participants were instructed to park the car and the music was stopped. During this break participants were asked to complete the UMACL questionnaire and the RSME scale. Next up, the second 8-min drive started which only differed from the first drive in lane width. After completing the ride, participants filled out the UMACL and RSME again. The total duration of each experimental session including instructions and attaching and de-attaching the physiological equipment approximated 70 min. 2.7. Data analysis Data were analysed using SPSS 17 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) with level of significance at p 5 0.05 (two-tailed). The data acquired during the music mood induction period were analysed to confirm that successful mood induction took place using a repeated-measures ANOVA with music (positive/negative/no music) as within-subject variable. Furthermore, the data obtained during the drives were analysed to show the effect of the music on driving using a repeated-measures ANOVA with music (positive/negative/no music) and driving demand (wide/narrow) as within-subject variables. Pairwise comparisons were Bonferroni corrected. 3. Results 3.1. Subjective ratings The reliability of the mood dimensions was determined using the normal and the reverse coded items of the UMACL questions. This rendered Chronbach’s alphas for energy of 0.84, valence of 0.89, and calmness of 0.90. Next, to show whether the baseline values were not different from each other between the three music sessions a repeated-measures MANOVA with music (positive/negative/no music) as within-subject factor was conducted on the valence, energy, and calmness ratings obtained directly after the baseline period, i.e. before the music mood induction. Results do not show a significant multivariate effect of music (F (6,70) ¼ 1.54, p ¼ 0.18, Z2 ¼ 0.12). The baselines ratings were as follows: valence (M ¼ 5.5, SE ¼ 0.18), energy (M ¼ 3.8, SE ¼ 0.13), and calmness (M ¼ 6.0, SE ¼ 0.12). Next, mood reaction scores were obtained by subtracting the values obtained during baseline 17 Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 Ergonomics from the values obtained during the conditions; either mood induction or the two drives. A repeated-measures MANOVA, with the withinsubject factor of music (positive/negative/no music) was conducted on the subjective valence, energy, and calmness reaction scores obtained after the music mood induction. A significant multivariate effect of music was found (F (6,70) ¼ 2.51, p ¼ 0.029, Z2 ¼ 0.177). Univariate tests show significant main effects on valence and energy reaction scores during the mood induction (valence: F(2,36) ¼ 4.21, p ¼ 0.023, Z2 ¼ 0.19, Energy: F(2,36) ¼ 6.0, p ¼ 0.006, Z2 ¼ 0.25, calmness: F(2,36) ¼ 2.22, p ¼ 0.12, Z2 ¼ 0.11). Pairwise comparisons show that positive music results in higher valence and energy reaction scores compared to both the negative music (valence p ¼ 0.042, energy p ¼ 0.042) and the no music conditions (valence p ¼ 0.024, energy p ¼ 0.004). Figure 1 displays the mean energy and valence ratings obtained after the drives. Average and standard errors in parentheses of the calmness ratings were 70.25 (0.15) for positive music, 70.71 (0.17) for negative music, and 70.58 (0.15) for no music. Next, a repeated-measures MANOVA with music (positive/negative/no music) and driving demand (wide/narrow) as within-subject factors was conducted on the valence, energy, and calmness reaction scores obtained after the two drives. Results show a marginally significant multivariate effect of music (F (6,70) ¼ 2.22, p ¼ 0.053, Z2 ¼ 0.160). No significant multivariate effects were found for driving demand (F (3,16) ¼ 0.82, p ¼ 0.50, Z2 ¼ 0.13) or the music with drive demand interaction (F (6,70) ¼ 0.42, p ¼ 0.86, Z2 ¼ 0.04). The univariate results for music show significant main effects on valence and energy reaction scores (valence F (2,36) ¼ 4.33, p ¼ 0.021, Z2 ¼ 0.19; energy F (6,70) ¼ 3.49, p ¼ 0.041, Z2 ¼ 0.16; calmness (F (2,36) ¼ 2.21, p ¼ 0.124, Z2 ¼ 0.11). Pairwise comparisons of music show marginally higher valence reaction scores (p ¼ 0.051) and marginally higher energy reaction scores (p ¼ 0.095) during the positive condition compared to the no music condition irrespective of driving demand. Figure 2 shows the mean energy and valence reaction scores obtained after the drives. Average calmness reaction scores were the following: Mean (standard error) positive wide M ¼ 70.27 (0.14), narrow M ¼ 70.55 (0.18), negative wide M ¼ 70.74 (.20), narrow M ¼ 70.82 (0.23), no music wide M ¼ 70.76 (0.19), narrow M ¼ 70.87 (0.19). To evaluate the perceived amount of mental effort (RSME scores) during the drives, a repeated-measure ANOVA of music (positive/negative/no music) with driving demand (wide/narrow) as within-subject factors was conducted on the RSME ratings. A significant effect of driving demand was found (F (1,18) ¼ 9.12, p ¼ 0.007, Z2 ¼ 0.34). Pairwise comparisons of driving demand reveal higher RSME ratings during the narrow drive (M ¼ 39.37, SE ¼ 5.42) compared to the wide drive (M ¼ 33.10, SE ¼ 4.68). No significant effects of music or the music with driving demand interaction were found; music F (2,36) ¼ 1.96, p ¼ 0.156, Z2 ¼ 0.10; music with driving demand F (2,36) ¼ 0.10, p ¼ 0.907, Z2 ¼ 0.005. Figure 1. Subjective valence and energy reaction scores provided after the music mood induction. The error bars represent +standard error. Figure 2. Subjective valence and energy reaction scores provided after the wide and the narrow drives. The error bars represent +standard error. 3.2. Physiological responses A repeated-measures MANOVA with music (positive/ negative/no music) as within-subject factor was conducted for both the RR and the mean IBI duration obtained during the last 3 min of the baseline period. Results do not show a significant main effect of music 18 M.D. van der Zwaag et al. on RR and on mean IBI, indicating that the baseline RRs and IBI durations did not differ for the different sessions; RR F (2,36) 5 1, p ¼ 0.54, Z2 ¼ 0.04, mean (SE) in breath/min: positive 14.61 (0.95), negative 14.04 (0.73), no music 15.20 (0.76); IBI F (2,36) ¼ 1.18, p ¼ 0.36, Z2 ¼ 0.62, mean (standard error) IBI duration in seconds: positive ¼ 0.874 (0.03), negative ¼ 0.846 (0.03), no music ¼ 0.876 (0.03). Next, physiological reaction scores were created by subtracting the average values obtained during the last 4 min of the baseline period with the values obtained during the induction and the drives. the IBI durations were not different during the induction; F(2,36)51, p ¼ 0.40, Z2 ¼ 0.049, mean (SE) positive 70.01 (.006), negative 70.008 (0.006), no music 70.001 (0.006). Subsequently, a repeatedmeasure ANOVA was conducted with music (positive/ negative/no music) and driving demand (wide/narrow) as within-subject factors on the average IBI durations obtained during the drives. Results show no significant effect for music or driving demand (all p 4 0.05) positive mean (Standard Error) ¼ 70.017 (0.009), negative ¼ 70.019 (0.008), no music ¼ 70.022 (0.007), wide drive ¼ 70.018 (0.07), narrow drive ¼ 70.019 (0.07). Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 3.2.1. Respiration rate A repeated-measures ANOVA with music (positive/ negative/no music) as within-subject variable was conducted on the reaction scores of the RR obtained during the induction. No significant main effects were found showing the RRs did not differ during the induction period (F(2,36)51, p ¼ 0.78, Z2 ¼ 0.013), see Figure 3. Subsequently, a repeated-measure ANOVA was conducted with music (positive/negative/no music) with driving demand (wide/narrow) as withinsubject factors on the RR. Results show a main effect of music; F(2,36) ¼ 3.25, p ¼ 0.050, Z2 ¼ 0.153. Pairwise comparisons show a significantly lower RR during the negative compared to the no music condition (p ¼ 0.046) irrespective of driving demand, see also Figure 3. No significant effects of the driving demand or interaction effect of music with driving demand were found. 3.2.2. Cardiovascular measures 3.3. Driving performance Separate repeated-measures analysis of music (positive/negative/no music) with driving demand (wide/narrow) as within-subject factors was conducted on the mean of the LP, the standard deviation of the lateral position (SDLP, swerving), and the speed. A trend was found for mean LP, and for SDLP a significant main effect of driving demand was found; LP (m) F(1,17) ¼ 3.51, p ¼ 0.082, Z2 ¼ 0.201; LP (sd) F(1,17) ¼ 23.80, p 5 0.001, Z2 ¼ 0.583. During the narrow lane drive, more distance from the centre line is found and the SDLP is smaller compared to the wide lane drive; see also Figures 4 and 5. The results on speed show a marginally significant main effect of music (F(2,13) ¼ 3.18, p ¼ 0.075, Z2 ¼ 0.329). Pairwise comparisons show higher speed during the no music condition compared to the positive music condition (p ¼ 0.023) irrespective of driving demand; the average speed values are illustrated in Figure 6. A repeated-measures ANOVA with music (positive/ negative/no music) as within-subject variable was conducted on the IBI reaction scores of the induction. No significant main effect of Music was found showing 4. Discussion Music listening is a very popular side-activity while driving. However, the influence of music listening on Figure 3. The average respiration rates for induction, the wide, and the narrow drives. The error bars represent + 1 standard error. Figure 4. The average lateral position from the centerline of the road, obtained during the wide (3 m) and narrow (2.50 m) drives. The error bars represent + 1 standard error. 19 Ergonomics 4.1. Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 Figure 5. SDLP (standard deviation of the lateral position), i.e. swerving, obtained during the drives on the wide (3 m) and narrow (2.50 m) roads. The error bars represent + 1 standard error. Figure 6. The speed (km/h) during the drive on the wide (3 m) and on the narrow (2.50 m) roads. The error bars represent+1 standard error. Mood induction through music In line with the literature, in the two music conditions positive or negative moods were induced with music over 8-min periods while listening to music as the primary task (Gendolla 2000). The condition without music induced an equal mood state comparable with the mood induction with negative music. This may be explained in that the music is known to influence time perception causing the music listening situations being perceived shorter in duration (Cassidy and MacDonald 2010). For instance, MacDonald et al. (2003) have shown that waiting in a hospital context with music makes participants less anxious than if they have to wait without music. Hence, a situation in which no music is presented might be perceived as long and boring, thereby inducing a negative mood (Juslin and Sloboda 2010). In line with previous research, RR and heart rate did not vary between positive and negative mood inductions (Gendolla and Brinkman 2005, Silverstrini and Gendolla 2007; Van der Zwaag and Westerink 2011a). This finding can be explained in that mood does not immediately result in action tendencies and thus not in altered physiological responses (Gendolla 2000). In contrast, it has been shown that for other physiological measures than heart rate and RR, such as skin conductance and facial muscle tension, differentiation between moods during music mood induction can be observed (Cacioppo et al. 2000a; Van der Zwaag and Westerink 2011a). 4.2. Effects of music during high and low demand drives the body state and on driving performance is not yet fully understood. In the current study it was investigated whether personally selected positive and negative music influences mood, body state, and driving performance. Results show a successful mood induction by music: the induced moods are congruent with the expected moods based on the song stimuli which were selected per participant (see also Figure 1). Furthermore, music listening as primary activity did not change cardiovascular and respiratory responses. In contrast, lower RRs were found in the conditions where music was presented during drives compared to conditions in where no music was presented at all. Finally, as hypothesised, less swerving was observed in the higher demanding drives irrespective of music presence. Thus, the current findings support that music and driving demand influence mood, physiological state, or driving performance to a certain extent. As expected, (subjective) effort invested in driving was higher while driving in the narrow lanes (Dijksterhuis et al. 2011). Furthermore, as expected while driving, the induced mood persists in terms of valence and energy ratings irrespective of driving demand and music type. The energy ratings increase during the drive regardless of whether the music was positive or negative, which can be attributed to the execution of a concurrent task while driving. The result confirms previous research findings (Van der Zwaag and Westerink 2011b) and also extends the literature as it indicates that moods induced with music can maintain in concurrence of a task irrespective of task demands. Listening to negative music compared to no music while driving results in lower RRs irrespective of driving demand. This finding holds even though subjective mood ratings are equal when listening to negative music compared to not listening to music. This implies that music might be used to unconsciously decrease body stress of the driver as RR has been Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 20 M.D. van der Zwaag et al. linked to arousal (Boiten et al. 1994, Nyklı́cek et al. 1997, Ritz 2004, Homma and Masaoka 2008). For example, Nyklı́cek et al. (1997) have shown that stimulative music leads to faster RRs. This result emphasises the importance of incorporating physiological measurements as they can uncover findings that would remain unnoticed by subjective ratings (Van den Broek and Westerink 2009). Driving performance was influenced by both driving demand and the music played during the drive. With regard to driving parameters, the mean LP and the amount of swerving (SDLP) both decreased during high demand drives. Driving in a narrow lane was as expected associated with a decrease in swerving. This confirms that more effort was put into the lane keeping task to deal with decreased lateral margins. Furthermore, this is in accordance to Summala’s Multiple Monitor Theory (Summala 2005, 2007), which states that mental load increases when less time is available to maintain safety margins. In a more general sense, these results can be seen as an indication that more effort was invested in the driving task to prevent performance degradation (Hockey 1997, 2003) or that the level of effort was matched to the current task demands (Hancock and Warm 1989, Matthews and Desmond 2002). Furthermore, in contrast to Pêcher et al. (2009), in the current study in the no music condition swerving was not decreased, hence music listening did not affect lateral safety. This difference in results can be explained in that Pêcher et al. (2009) altered music periods with silent periods of 1 min each. This alteration could distract the driver and therefore decrease swerving while listening to music. The current results thus show a more ecological valid situation and hence more ecologically valid results. Driving in a narrow lane did not decrease driving speed. This could imply that the high demanding situation was not demanding enough to lead to changes in driving performance. The data do show lower speed during the positive music drives compared to the drives without music. This could be caused by increased engagement in the drive while listening to personally selected positive music which resulted in context appropriate speed choices, instead of having distracting effects (Cassidy and MacDonald 2009). This finding is in line with the increased RRs during the no music condition; faster speed coincides with higher physical effort. Lastly, the current study did not show an interaction between driving demand and music. This could be explained in several ways: first, the music used in this study might not have demanded much attentional resources because it was moderate instead of high arousing (Cassidy and MacDonald 2009). Listening to music, therefore, did not require more than the resources still available next to those required for driving, so no real competition for resources resulted. This in line with Beh and Hirst (1999) who showed that high intensity music, which asks for more resources, did decrease performance during high demand conditions in a driving-related task. Second, experienced drivers participated in the study. Therefore, the additional load added by music listening while driving could have been minimal, as the driving task itself can be expected to be mainly automated. Music listening during novice drivers might, however, result in different outcomes. Taken together, these results indicate that music intensity can be an important factor in predicting the influence of music on decreases in driving performance during high demand driving situations. 4.3. Limitations and future research The song stimuli varied in valence and to a lesser extend in energy levels as well. Note that the selected songs were not the most contrasting songs, i.e. favourite songs would have had the highest valence and energy levels, and sad songs would have the lowest energy and valence levels. From the individual song selection, it appeared impossible to select songs that solely varied in mood valence and having equal energy levels. This implies that valence and energy ratings are not fully independent of each other in inducing mood with music. This result is in line with findings of psychobiological theory of aesthetics (Berlyne 1971). This theory proposes that a Ushaped relationship exists between arousal and liking, i.e. an average arousal level has the optimal liking level, and increasing and decreasing arousal levels would decrease liking levels (Berlyne 1971, Hargreaves and North 2008). To cope with the large individual differences in music liking, individually selected song stimuli were chosen in the current study (Juslin and Sloboda 2010). This method assured that the songs stimuli selected indeed induced the targeted moods. However, this method also resulted in stimuli that were not controlled for other music characteristics such as familiarity, or characteristics inherent to the music as tempo or mode, which could impact mood (Juslin and Sloboda 2010, van der Zwaag et al. 2011). Future research should point out how the lessons learned from this study can be generalised to the impact of music listening to different types of music during a wide variety of driving scenarios. 4.4. Conclusion In the current study, the influence of listening to music on mood, body state, and driving performance during Ergonomics high and low demand drives were investigated. Moods were successfully induced with music, and driving without music was perceived with equal negative feelings as driving with negative valence music. Listening to negative music compared to no music while driving leads to decreased RRs and listening to positive music compared to no music leads to slower driving speed. Furthermore, increased driving demand led to an increased amount of swerving. In the present study music did not impair driving performance as often found in the literature. In contrast, listening to music can even lead to an improved mood and a more relaxed body state which could be beneficial for health in the long run. Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 Acknowledgements This work was supported by the European 7th framework REFLECT project. We would like to acknowledge Wilmer Joling, Joop Clots, and Peter Albronda, for the technical support while setting up this study. References Audacity, (Version 1.2.4). [Computer program]. Available from: http://audacity.sourceforge.net/ [Accessed 6 February 2010]. Beh, H.C. and Hirst, R., 1999. Performance on drivingrelated tasks during music. Ergonomics, 42, 1087–1098. Berlyne, C.E., 1971. Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century, Crofts. Blacking, J., 1973. How musical is man? Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press. Boiten, F.A., Frijda, N.H., and Wientjes, C.J.E., 1994. Emotions and respiratory patterns: review and critical analysis. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 17, 103–128. Brodsky, W., 2002. The effects of music tempo on simulated driving performance and vehicular control. Transportation Research Part F, 4, 219–241. Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L., and Berntson, G., 2000a. Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cacioppo, J.T., et al., 2000b. The psychophysiology of emotion. In: M. Lewis and J.M. Haviland-Jones, eds. Handbook of emotions. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford, 173–191. Cassidy, G.G. and MacDonald, R.A.R., 2009. The effects of music choice on task performance: a study of the impact of self-selected and experimenter-selected music on driving game performance and experience. Musicae Scientiae, 13, 357–386. Cassidy, G.G. and MacDonald, R.A.R., 2010. The effects of music on time perception and performance of a driving game. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51, 455–464. Dalton, B.H. and Behm, D.G., 2007. Effects of noise and music on human and task performance: a systematic review. Occupational Ergonomics, 7, 143–152. Davis, C., 1992. The effects of music and basic relaxation instruction on pain and anxiety of women undergoing inoffice gynaecological procedures. Journal of Music Therapy, 29, 202–216. 21 DeJong, M., Van Mourik, K., and Schellekines, H., 1973. A physiological approach to aesthetic preference – music. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 22, 46–51. De Waard, D., et al., 1995. Effect of road layout and road environment on driving performance, drivers’ physiology and road appreciation. Ergonomics, 38, 1395–1407. DeNora, T., 2000. Music in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DeNora, T., 2003. Music sociology: getting the music into the action. British Journal of Music Education, 20, 165– 177. Dibben, N. and Williamson, V.J., 2007. An exploratory survey of in-vehicle music listening. Psychology of Music, 35, 571–589. Dijksterhuis, C., De Waard, D., and Brookhuis, K.A., 2011. Effects of steering demand on lane keeping behaviour, self-reports, and physiology. A simulator study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43, 1074–1081. Gendolla, G.H.E., 2000. On the impact of mood on behavior: an integrative theory and a review. Review of General Psychology, 4, 378–408. Gendolla, G.H.E. and Brinkmann, K., 2005. The role of mood states in self-regulation effects on action preferences and resource mobilization. European Psychologist, 10, 187–198. Godley, S.T., Triggs, T.J., and Fildes, B.N., 2004. Perceptual land width, wide perceptual road centre markings and driving speeds. Ergonomics, 47, 237–257. Gomez, P. and Danuser, B., 2004. Affective and physiological responses to environmental noises and music. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 53, 91–103. Grossman, P. and Taylor, E., 2007. Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrythmia: relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions. Biological Psychology, 74, 263–285. Hancock, P.A. and Warm, J.S., 1989. A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention. Human Factors, 31, 519–537. Hargreaves, D. and North, A., 2010. Experimental aesthetics and liking for music. In: P. Juslin and J. Sloboda, eds. Handbook of music and emotions: theory, research, applications. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 515–547. Hockey, G.R.J., 1997. Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: a cognitive energetical framework. Biologica Psychology, 45, 73–93. Hockey, G.R.J., 2003. Operator functional state as a framework for the assessment of performance degradation. In: G.R.J. Hockey, A.W.K. Gaillard, and O. Burov, eds. Operator functional state. Amsterdam: IOS Press, 8–23. Hodges, D.A., 2010. Bodily responses to music. In: P.N. Juslin and J.A. Sloboda, eds. Handbook of music and emotion: theory, research, and applications. New York: Oxford University Press, 279–311. Homma, I. and Masaoka, Y., 2008. Breathing rhythms and emotions. Experimental Psychology, 93 (9), 1011– 1021. Iwanaga, M. and Moroki, Y., 1999. Subjective and physiological responses to music stimuli controlled over activity and preference. Journal of Music Therapy, 36, 26–38. Juslin, P.N. and Sloboda, J.A., eds., 2010. Handbook of music and emotion: theory, research, applications. New York: Oxford University Press. Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012 22 M.D. van der Zwaag et al. Kastner, M.P. and Crowder, R.G., 1990. Perception of the major/minor distinction: Iv. Emotional connotation in young children. Music Perception, 8, 189–202. Kivy, P., 1989. Sound sentiment: an essay on the musical emotions. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press. Kivy, P., 1990. Music alone: philosophical reflections on the purely musical experience. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Knight, W. and Rickard, N., 2001. Relaxing music prevents stress-induced increases in subjective anxiety, systolic blood pressure, and heart rate in healthy males and females. Journal of Music Therapy, 38, 254–272. Konecni, V., 1982. Social interaction and musical preference. In: D. Deutsch, ed. The psychology of music. New York: Academic Press, 297–516. Krumhansl, C., 1997. An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 336–353. MacDonald, R.A.R., et al., 2003. An empirical investigation of the anxiolytic and pain reducing effects of music. Psychology of Music, 31, 187–203. Matthews, G. and Desmond, P.A., 2002. Task-induced fatigue states and simulated driving performance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 659–686. Matthews, G., Jones, D.M., and Chamberlain, A., 1990. Refining the measurement of mood: the UWIST mood adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17– 42. North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J., 2008. The social and applied psychology of music. Oxford: Oxford University Press. North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J., 1999. Music and driving game performance. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, 285–292. Nyklı́cek, I., Thayer, J.F., and Van Doornen, L.J.P., 1997. Cardiorespiratory differentiation of emotions. Journal of Psychophysiology, 11, 304–321. Pêcher, C., Lemercier, C., and Cellier, J.-M., 2009. Emotions drive attention: effects on driver’s behaviour. Safety Science, 47, 1254–1259. Piferi, R.L., et al., 2000. An alternative approach for achieving cardiovascular baseline: viewing an aquatic video. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37, 207–217. Recarte, M.A. and Nunes, L.M., 2000. Effects of verbal and spatial-imagery tasks on fixations while driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 31–43. Rickard, N., 2004. Intense emotional responses to music: a test of the physiological arousal hypotheses. Psychology of Music, 32, 371–388. Ritz, T., 2004. Probing the psychophysiology of the airways: Physical activity, experienced emotion, and facially expressed emotion. Psychophysiology, 41 (6), 809–821. Saarikallio, S. and Erkkilä, J., 2007. The role of music in adolescents’ mood regulation. Psychology of Music, 35, 88–109. Shek, V. and Schubert, E., 2009. Background music at worka literature review and some hypotheses. In: C. Stevens et al., eds. Proceedings of the 2nd international conference on music communication science (ICoMCS2). 3–4 December, 2009. Sydney, Australia: HCSNet, University of Western Sydney, 87–91. Silverstrini, N. and Gendolla, G.H.E., 2007. Mood effects on autonomic activity in mood regulation. Psychophysiology, 44, 650–659. View publication stats Skowronek, J., McKinney, M.F., and van de, P.S., 2006. Ground truth for automatic music mood classification. In: Proceedings of the 7th international conference on music information retrieval. Victoria, Canada: University of Victoria, 395–396. Skowronek, J., McKinney, M.F., and van der, P.S., 2007. A demonstrator for automatic music mood estimation. In: A demonstrator for automatic music mood estimation. Vienna, Austria: Austrian Computer Society, 345–346. Sloboda, J.A. and Juslin, P.N., 2010. At the interface between the inner and outer world, psychological perspectives. In: P.N. Juslin and J.A. Sloboda, eds. Handbook of music and emotion: theory, research, and applications. Chapter 4. New York: Oxford University Press, 73–98. Stern, R., Ray, W., and Quigley, K., 2001. Psychophysiological recording. New York: Oxford University Press. Summala, H., 2005. Traffic psychology theories: towards understanding driving behaviour and safety efforts. In: G. Underwood, ed. Traffic and transport psychology theory and application. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 383–394. Summala, H., 2007. Towards understanding motivational and emotional factors in driver behaviour: comfort through satisficing. In: P.C. Cacciabue, ed. Modelling driver behaviour in automotive environments: critical issues in driver interactions with intelligent transport systems. London: Springer, 189–207. Van den Broek, E.L. and Westerink, J.H.D.M., 2009. Considerations for emotion-aware consumer products. Applied Ergonomics, 40 (6), 1055–1064. Van der Zwaag, M.D. and Westerink, J.H.D.M., 2010. Musical induction and persistence of mood. In: S.M. Demorest, S.J. Morrison and P.S. Campbell, eds. Proceedings of the 11th ICMPC, 23–27 August 2010. Seattle, University of Washington, 45–46. Van der Zwaag, M.D. and Westerink, J.H.D.M. 2011a. Physiological patterns to music mood induction. Manuscript submitted for publication. Van der Zwaag, M.D. and Westerink, J.H.D.M. 2011b. Sustaining moods with background music: physiological patterns and subjective experience. Manuscript submitted for publication. Van der Zwaag, M.D., Westerink, J.H.D.M., and Van den Broek, E.L., 2011. Emotional and psychophysiological responses to tempo, mode, and percussiveness. Special issue on Music and Emotion. Musicae Scientiae, 15, 250–269. Van Winsum, W. and Van Wolffelaar, P.C., 1993. GIDS small world simulation. In: J.A. Michon, ed. Generic intelligent driver support. London: Taylor and Francis, 175–191. Walsh, M.J., 2010. Driving to the beat of one’s own hum: automobility and musical listening. Studies in Symbolic Interaction, 35, 201–221. Wientjes, C.J., 1992. Respiration in psychophysiology: methods and applications. Biological Psychology, 34, 179–203. Wiesenthal, D.L., Hennessy, D.A., and Totten, B., 2000. The influence of music on driver stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1709–1719. Wiesenthal, D.L., Hennessy, D.A., and Totten, B., 2003. The influence of music on mild drivers aggression. Transportation Research Part F, 6, 125–134. Zijlstra, F.R.H., 1993. Efficiency in work behavior. A design approach for modern tools. Thesis (PhD). Delft University of Technology, Delft, the Netherlands.