This art icle was downloaded by: [ Universit y of Groningen]
On: 09 January 2012, At : 00: 50
Publisher: Taylor & Francis
I nform a Lt d Regist ered in England and Wales Regist ered Num ber: 1072954 Regist ered office: Mort im er House,
37- 41 Mort im er St reet , London W1T 3JH, UK
Ergonomics
Publicat ion det ails, including inst ruct ions f or aut hors and subscript ion inf ormat ion:
ht t p: / / www. t andf online. com/ loi/ t erg20
The influence of music on mood and performance
while driving
Marj olein D. van der Zwaag
, Joyce H. D. M. West erink
a
a
a b
b
, Chris Dij kst erhuis , Dick de Waard
& Karel A. Brookhuis
b
, Ben L. J. M. Mulder
b
b
Philips Research Laborat ories, High Tech Campus 34, 5654AE Eindhoven, t he Net herlands
b
Behavioural and Social Sciences, Universit y of Groningen, Grot e Kruisst raat 2/ 1, 9712TS,
Groningen, t he Net herlands
Available online: 16 Dec 2011
To cite this article: Marj olein D. van der Zwaag, Chris Dij kst erhuis, Dick de Waard, Ben L. J. M. Mulder, Joyce H. D. M.
West erink & Karel A. Brookhuis (2012): The inf luence of music on mood and perf ormance while driving, Ergonomics, 55: 1,
12-22
To link to this article: ht t p: / / dx. doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 00140139. 2011. 638403
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTI CLE
Full t erm s and condit ions of use: ht t p: / / www.t andfonline.com / page/ t erm s- and- condit ions
This art icle m ay be used for research, t eaching, and privat e st udy purposes. Any subst ant ial or syst em at ic
reproduct ion, redist ribut ion, reselling, loan, sub- licensing, syst em at ic supply, or dist ribut ion in any form t o
anyone is expressly forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warrant y express or im plied or m ake any represent at ion t hat t he cont ent s
will be com plet e or accurat e or up t o dat e. The accuracy of any inst ruct ions, form ulae, and drug doses should
be independent ly verified wit h prim ary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, act ions, claim s,
proceedings, dem and, or cost s or dam ages what soever or howsoever caused arising direct ly or indirect ly in
connect ion wit h or arising out of t he use of t his m at erial.
Ergonomics
Vol. 55, No. 1, January 2012, 12–22
The influence of music on mood and performance while driving
Marjolein D. van der Zwaaga,b*, Chris Dijksterhuisb, Dick de Waardb, Ben L.J.M. Mulderb,
Joyce H.D.M. Westerinka and Karel A. Brookhuisb
a
Philips Research Laboratories, High Tech Campus 34, 5654AE Eindhoven, the Netherlands; bBehavioural and Social Sciences,
University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712TS Groningen, the Netherlands
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
(Received 4 May 2011; final version received 2 November 2011)
Mood can influence our everyday behaviour and people often seek to reinforce, or to alter their mood, for example
by turning on music. Music listening while driving is a popular activity. However, little is known about the impact of
music listening while driving on physiological state and driving performance. In the present experiment, it was
investigated whether individually selected music can induce mood and maintain moods during a simulated drive. In
addition, effects of positive, negative, and no music on driving behaviour and physiological measures were assessed
for normal and high cognitive demanding rides. Subjective mood ratings indicated that music successfully
maintained mood while driving. Narrow lane width drives increased task demand as shown in effort ratings and
increased swerving. Furthermore, respiration rate was lower during music listening compared to rides without music,
while no effects of music were found on heart rate. Overall, the current study demonstrates that music listening in car
influences the experienced mood while driving, which in turn can impact driving behaviour.
Practitioners Summary: Even though it is a popular activity, little is known about the impact of music while driving
on physiological state and performance. We examined whether music can induce moods during high and low
simulated drives. The current study demonstrates that in car music listening influences mood which in turn can
impact driving behaviour. The current study shows that listening to music can positively impact mood while driving,
which can be used to affect state and safe behaviour. Additionally, driving performance in high demand situations is
not negatively affected by music.
Keywords: music mood induction; demand; simulated drive; respiration rate; heart rate; driving behaviour
1.
Introduction
In Western society, music listening has become a
frequent activity in the background of almost any
activity (DeNora 2000, North and Hargreaves 2008).
Music research has now started to focus on music
listening in these specific everyday life situations to
improve the understanding of how music can influence
personal experiences and behaviour (DeNora 2003,
Juslin and Sloboda 2010). Driving is one of the most
popular music listening situational contexts. While
driving, people listen to music to attain enjoyment or
to feel engaged when driving in solitude (DeNora 2000,
Walsh 2010). It also is suggested that music listening
distracts from driving and can therefore influence
safety (Brodsky 2002). Although the impact of music
on driving performance has been given some attention
(Dibben and Williamson 2007), its impact on mood
and physiological measures has not. Neither has a
distinction been made between the respective impacts
of the specific types of music such as positive and
negative valence music. In the current article, these
*Corresponding author. Email: mvanderzwaag@gmail.com
ISSN 0014-0139 print/ISSN 1366-5847 online
Ó 2012 Taylor & Francis
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2011.638403
http://www.tandfonline.com
relationships between music valence and driving
demand on mood, physiological measures, and driving
performance are studied.
1.1. Music listening
The potential of music to influence mood is described
as one of the most important functions of music (Juslin
and Sloboda 2010, Van der Zwaag and Westerink
2010, Van der Zwaag et al. 2011). Although music is
known to influence mood, it is still under discussion
whether people perceive the expressed state within the
music (cognitivist view) or whether music can actually
induce moods in listeners (emotivists view) (Kivy 1989,
1990). Evidence of the fact that music induces
emotions is for example found by Kastner and
Crowder (1990) who showed that major mode music is
perceived as more happy compared to minor mode
music. Furthermore, fast tempo music has consistently
shown to increase arousal levels compared to slow
tempo music (Krumhansl 1997, Van der Zwaag et al.
2011).
13
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
Ergonomics
Support for the emotivist position of the influence
of music on emotion comes from the growing body of
evidence that music can influence physiological responses and thus body state (for an overview see
Hodges (2010)). Heart rate and respiration rate (RR)
are the most frequently investigated psycho-physiological responses to music (Hodges 2010). The majority
of studies in the music literature have found that
arousing music increases heart rate compared to low
arousing music (DeJong et al. 1973, Knight and
Rickard 2001). Still, others have found that any music,
both low and high arousing, increases heart rate
(Krumhansl 1997, Iwanaga and Moroki 1999, Rickard
2004). RR is found to increase in high arousing
compared to sedative music as well (Iwanaga and
Moroki 1999, Krumhansl 1997, Nyklı́cek et al. 1997
Gomez and Danuser 2004). Again, in other studies no
difference in RR while listening to different types of
music was found (Davis 1992, Van der Zwaag and
Westerink 2011a). Hence, inconsistent results are
found on heart rate and RR responses to music
listening. Note further that these latter studies presented music listening as main task, and thus not as in
the background of a concurrent activity, such as
driving. Still, it is implied that mood can remain when
music is played in the background of a concurrent
activity, as Van der Zwaag and Westerink (2011b)
showed the persistence of musically induced moods in
the background of a distracting task.
Several explanations can be given for the inconsistent results found for the influence of music on
physiological measures. A first explanation comes
from the fact that studied physiological measures are
affected by regulatory effects in the autonomic nervous
system (ANS) which is primarily responsible for
keeping homeostasis (Cacioppo et al. 2000b). As a
result, physiological responses are not solely influenced
by emotional state via, for example, music listening but
additionally via physical activity, cognitive demand,
and other psychological constructs (Cacioppo et al.
2000b, Van den Broek and Westerink 2009). Hence,
the situational context should be taken into account in
interpreting physiological responses to music listening.
A second explanation can be found in the fact that
most studies in music research differ to a great extent
on important methodological aspects, such as the song
selection method and the duration of the music
presentation. For example, Van der Zwaag and
Westerink (2011a) showed that the physiological
response patterns to positive and negative music
mood induction start to differentiate after an average
of 4 min. Hence, the physiological responses to music
listening in studies presenting relatively short music
excerpts cannot be compared to studies inducing
moods with music over longer periods. For the study
of physiological responses to music, awareness of these
methodological aspects is important, as is the
perspective to always describe the impact of music to
emotions in relation with personal and situational
context (Blacking 1973, Saarikallio and Erkkilä 2007,
North and Hargreaves 2008, Sloboda and Juslin 2010).
1.2.
Music while driving
Music can be beneficial while driving as, for example,
the mood-arousal hypothesis predicts that in cases of
boredom and drowsiness music can lead to a more
optimal arousal level which could benefit driving
performance (North and Hargreaves 2008, Shek and
Schubert 2009). However, following the distraction
hypothesis, music can also take attention away from
the driver (Shek and Schubert 2009). This distracting
effect of music on driving can be disadvantageous
when it decreases safety in case high arousing music is
played during high demand road situations (Dibben
and Williamson 2007). On the contrary, Wiesenthal
et al. (2000) showed that one’s favourite music
alleviates stress during high congestion drives and
found higher stress levels when comparing no music to
favourite music during high congestion drives.
Furthermore, it is shown that driver aggression can be
tempered with favourite music compared to no music
in high demanding rides (Wiesenthal et al. 2003).
Explanations for the effects of music listening while
performing a concurrent task such as driving often
focus on processing capacity in service of the primary
task (North and Hargreaves 1999, Dalton and Behm
2007, Pêcher et al. 2009) and assume that listening to
music may be arousing and therefore requires mental
resources. Following the information–distraction
approach, music adds additional irrelevant stimuli to a
task which leads to increased cognitive load and thus
can impact task performance (Konecni 1982, North
and Hargreaves 1999, Recarte and Nunes 2000).
Consequently, the more attention a particular music
requires the more it competes for processing resources
with the primary task of, for example, driving. To
illustrate, North and Hargreaves (1999) manipulated
cognitive load of participants by exposing them to low
or high arousing music by varying tempo and volume
in a driving game. They found that high arousing
music resulted in worse racing performance defined as
slower lap times, while the quickest lap times were
recorded when listening to low arousing music.
Interestingly, they also found a connection between
task demand and music liking, and concluded that
competition for processing resources caused
particpants to dislike music. Pêcher et al. (2009)
mentioned that post-experiment interviews revealed
that drivers found happy music the most disturbing
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
14
M.D. van der Zwaag et al.
and, combined with behavioural data, took this as
support to conclude that listening to happy music
resulted in deteriorated driving performance.
The impact of in-vehicle music listening on driving
speed depends on the road situational context. Reducing speed is found to be used as a compensatory
reaction when faced with high load situations, namely
it enables the driver to maintain safety margins by
decreasing required reaction times (Summala 2005).
Furthermore, as mentioned above it can be expected
that drivers allocate more attention, and thus mental
resources, to positive music which could result in
detrimental effects on vehicular control or in a
compensatory reaction such as slowing down.
Because task performance and music listening
might compete for the same mental resources, the
impact of musically evoked cognitive demand on
performance might be dependent on the cognitive
demand of a concurrent task (Konecni 1982, North
and Hargreaves 1999). In low demand drive situations,
there is less competition for attentional space. Hence, it
is likely that mental resources can more easily be
divided between listening to music and driving as the
limits of mental resources are not reached. Therefore,
listening to music will not impact driving performance
in these low-demand situations. As lane width is
known to influence drivers workload, this variable
could be used to manipulate primary task demands
when studying the relation between listening to
positively and negatively rated music. Results reported
in the literature show that when driving in narrow
lanes, less manoeuvring space is available for the
driver, and more attention is required to prevent
driving errors such as drifting out of the driving lane
and to maintain personal safety margins (De Waard
et al. 1995, Dijksterhuis et al. 2011). This results in
smaller deviations from the driver’s preferred lateral
position (LP) on the road (De Waard et al. 1995,
Dijksterhuis et al. 2011) and a compensatory speed
reduction (Godley et al. 2004). In terms of physiological responses, higher demand situations would lead
to increased heart rate and RR.
1.3. Expectations
The aim of the current study was twofold. First, we
wanted to investigate whether musically induced
positive and negative moods persist during low and
high demand drives. Moreover, we investigated
whether the presence of positive compared to negative
music influences the resources mobilised, as reflected in
the amount of effort invested while driving. We
expected that positive and negative valence can be
successfully induced during music mood induction
(Van der Zwaag and Westerink 2010, 2011a).
Furthermore, based on Van der Zwaag and
Westerink (2011b), we expect that positive and
negative music mood induction will persist while
driving. Furthermore, we expect that the presence of
positive or negative moods will remain during high
demand drives.
Secondly, the influence of musically induced mood
on driving performance in high (narrow lane width)
and low (wide lane width) demanding drives was
investigated. Hence, lane width was used to further
investigate the relation between listening to positively
and negatively rated music and primary task demands.
First of all, while driving, we expected increased RRs
and heart rate during high compared to low
demanding drives. In accordance with Dijksterhuis
et al. (2011), less swerving (i.e. reduced variation in LP)
was expected in narrow lanes. Furtheremore, we
expected that lane width reduction would lower the
speed to compensate for the higher amount of
resources allocated in the more demanding drive (De
Waard et al. 1995, Godley et al. 2004). We expected
that music would solely influence driving performance
in high demand drives, as in those conditions music
competes with the limited amount of mental resources
available.
2.
Method
2.1. Participants
The study had been approved by the local ethics
committee and informed consent was obtained from all
participants. Nineteen participants, 13 men and 6
women, were paid 45 Euros for participating. Age
ranged from 22 to 44 years (mean ¼ 27.5; SD ¼ 5.2)
and participants had held their driving licence for 4 to
22 years (mean ¼ 8.8; SD ¼ 4.9). Self reported total
mileage driven ranged from 6000 to 700,000 km
(median ¼ 45,000; inter-quartile range
(IQR) ¼ 77,500 km) and current yearly mileage
ranged from 1500 to 6000 km (median ¼ 7000;
IQR ¼ 5000).
2.2. Design
Three music conditions were included: positive music,
negative music and no music (which was included as an
extra control condition). In addition, two levels of lane
width (wide 3.00 m or narrow 2.50 m) were created in
the driving simulator, corresponding to low and high
demand drives, respectively (Dijksterhuis et al. 2011).
Participants completed four sessions on separate days:
one introduction session and three experimental
sessions. In each experimental session, one music level
was presented and both lane widths were used. This
resulted in a within-subject design including two
15
Ergonomics
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
repeated-measures factors: music (3) and lane width
(2). The orders of both the music and lane width
factors were counterbalanced among participants.
2.3. Music stimuli selection
Because music preference is highly personal (Hargreaves and North 2010), songs used as stimuli were
selected individually. To do so, participants completed an introductory session prior to the experimental sessions. In this session, participants rated 60
songs on perceived valence and energy levels on 7point Likert scales. The participants did not have to
listen to the entire song but were encouraged to
sample each song for a few moments and at a few
locations within the song to get a good impression of
the song. The 60 songs included were selected to
have a large range of valence and energy values and
were selected from a database containing 1800 songs
in total. The songs were selected based on energy
and valence labels which were acquired by automatic
classification of music characteristics into mood
labels (Skowronek et al. 2006, 2007). The order of
the song presentation was randomised over
participants.
After participants had finished the ratings, nine
songs were selected per participant per music
condition (positive/negative) in such a way that
valence ratings differed as much as possible between
the positive and negative songs while keeping energy
ratings as average as possible. Subsequently, three of
the selected songs were used for the music mood
induction, three songs for the high demand drive,
and three songs for the low demand drive. The
duration of the three songs was adjusted, using
Audacity (Version 1.2.4), to 8 min, keeping the
average duration of each song about equal. This
was done by cutting the song to about 2.45 min and
fading out the new ending of the song.
To check the selected song stimuli on their valence
and energy ratings, a repeated-measures ANOVA with
music (positive/negative) as within-subject factor on
the valence and energy ratings of the selected songs
was conducted. Results showed a main effects of Music
on both energy and valence ratings: valence F
(1,17) ¼ 231.20, p 5 0.001, Z2 ¼ 0.93; energy F
(1,17) ¼ 16.04, p 5 0.001, Z2 ¼ 0.49. This confirmed
that the selected song stimuli for the two Music
conditions were significantly different from each other
in valence and energy. The positive songs showed
higher valence and energy ratings compared to the
negative songs; positive songs mean (SE) valence
M ¼ 5.8 (0.17), energy M ¼ 4.7 (0.20), negative songs
valence M ¼ 2.3 (1.4), energy M ¼ 3.3 (0.26), on
scales running from 1 to 7.
2.4.
Simulator and driving conditions
The study was conducted using a ST
SoftwareÓ driving simulator. This simulator consists
of a fixed-base vehicle mock up with functional
steering wheel, indicators, and pedals. The simulator
was surrounded by three 3200 diagonal plasma
screens. Each screen provided a 708 view, leading to a
total 2108 view. A detailed description of the functionality of the driving simulator used can be found in
Van Winsum and Van Wolffelaar (1993).
Participants drove the simulated car (width: 1.65 m)
over two sections of uninterrupted two-lane
roads (2.50 m or 3.00 m wide lanes), winding through
rural scenery, and separated by a small town. Roads in
each section consisted mainly of easy curves
(about 80%) with a constant radius of 380 m and
ranging in length from 120 to 800 m. The road
surface was marked on the edges by a continuous line
(20 cm wide), in the centre by a broken line (15 cm), and
outside the edges a soft shoulder was present. The
posted speed limit during the drive was 80 km/h.
In addition, a stream of oncoming traffic was
introduced with a random interval gap between 2 and
6 s, resulting in 15 passing passenger cars
(width: 1.75 m) per minute on average. No
vehicles appeared in the participant’s driving lane.
2.5. Measures
2.5.1. Subjective ratings
Subjective mood scores of valence (ranging from
unpleasant to pleasant) and energy (ranging
from tired/without energy to awake/full of energy) and
calmness ratings (ranging from tense to calm)
were assessed using the UWIST Mood Adjective
Checklist (UMACL) (Matthews et al. 1990).
This UMACL contains eight unipolar items for each
dimension, which start with: ‘right now I am feeling. .
.’, and range from 1: ‘not at all’ to 7: ‘very much’.
The rating scale mental effort (RSME) was used
to assess mental effort (Zijlstra 1993). The RSME
is a unidimensional scale, ranging from 0 to 150,
used to rate mental effort. In addition to digits, several
effort indications (calibrated anchor points) are
visible alongside the scale to further guide rating.
Indications start with ‘absolutely no effort’
(RSME score of 2) and end with ‘extreme
effort’ (RSME score of 112).
2.5.2. Physiology
Physiological measures covered ANS reactions in
the cardiovascular and respiratory domain. The
Portilab data recorder and its accompanying
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
16
M.D. van der Zwaag et al.
sensors were used to record these responses with a
sample frequency of 250 Hz (version 1.10, Twente
Medical Systems International, Oldenzaal, the Netherlands). Physiological measures were assessed continuously during the experiment. The MATLAB
programming environment (2009, The Mathworks,
Natick, MA) was used for the pre-processing of the
respiration signals.
Cardiovascular measures were recorded using an
electrocardiogram (ECG) using three Ag-AgCl electrodes, which were placed following the standard lead
II placement (Stern et al. 2001). R-peaks in the ECG
signal were detected automatically, after amplification
and filtering of the signal (Butterworth band pass: 0.5–
40 Hz). Subsequently, the distances between successive
R peaks, the interbeat intervals (IBI), were calculated.
Respiration was recorded by means of a respiration
belt (RespitraceTM, Twente Medical systems). To
obtain the respiration measures, noise was excluded
from the raw signal and movement artifacts were
reduced by a 0.005–1.0 Hz band pass IIR filter. The
amount of respiration cycles per minute indicated the
RR (Wientjes 1992, Grossman and Taylor 2007).
2.5.3. Driving parameters
Speed and LP were sampled at 10 Hz. Lateral position is
defined as the difference in metres between the centre of
the participant’s car and the middle of the (right hand)
driving lane. Positive LP values correspond to deviations
towards the left-hand shoulder and negative values
correspond to deviations toward the right-hand
shoulder. The sampled LP values were used to calculate
mean LP and the standard deviation of LP, i.e. swerving.
2.6. Procedure
Participants were invited four times to the driving
simulator facility of the University of Groningen.
During the first introductory session, the participants
were informed about the experiment, signed an
informed consent, drove a 6-min practice drive, and
completed the music rating.
During the three subsequent experimental sessions,
physiological sensors were attached and participants
were seated in the simulator chair. Next, physiological
baseline values were acquired in a habituation period
during which participants watched a Coral Sea diving
movie for 8 min (Piferi et al. 2000). Hereafter, participants filled out the UMACL. Then an 8-min music
mood induction period started in which the participants
were asked to listen to the music. To remain attentive to
the music, participants were asked to listen to the music
carefully to be able to answer questions regarding the
music after the entire experiment. During the control
session in which no music was presented, participants
were asked to sit and relax for 8 min. The participants
were not informed that mood induction took place
during these 8 min, as this could bias the results. After
8 min, the participants filled out the UMACL again.
Next, the first simulated drive began. The
participants were instructed to drive as they would
normally drive. After approximately 8 min,
participants were instructed to park the car and the
music was stopped. During this break participants
were asked to complete the UMACL questionnaire
and the RSME scale. Next up, the second 8-min drive
started which only differed from the first drive in lane
width. After completing the ride, participants filled out
the UMACL and RSME again. The total duration of
each experimental session including instructions and
attaching and de-attaching the physiological
equipment approximated 70 min.
2.7. Data analysis
Data were analysed using SPSS 17 for Windows (SPSS
Inc., Chicago, IL) with level of significance at p 5 0.05
(two-tailed). The data acquired during the music mood
induction period were analysed to confirm that successful
mood induction took place using a repeated-measures
ANOVA with music (positive/negative/no music) as
within-subject variable. Furthermore, the data obtained
during the drives were analysed to show the effect of the
music on driving using a repeated-measures ANOVA
with music (positive/negative/no music) and driving
demand (wide/narrow) as within-subject variables.
Pairwise comparisons were Bonferroni corrected.
3.
Results
3.1. Subjective ratings
The reliability of the mood dimensions was determined
using the normal and the reverse coded items of the
UMACL questions. This rendered Chronbach’s alphas
for energy of 0.84, valence of 0.89, and calmness of
0.90. Next, to show whether the baseline values were
not different from each other between the three music
sessions a repeated-measures MANOVA with music
(positive/negative/no music) as within-subject factor
was conducted on the valence, energy, and calmness
ratings obtained directly after the baseline period, i.e.
before the music mood induction. Results do not show
a significant multivariate effect of music (F (6,70) ¼
1.54, p ¼ 0.18, Z2 ¼ 0.12). The baselines ratings were
as follows: valence (M ¼ 5.5, SE ¼ 0.18), energy
(M ¼ 3.8, SE ¼ 0.13), and calmness (M ¼ 6.0,
SE ¼ 0.12). Next, mood reaction scores were obtained
by subtracting the values obtained during baseline
17
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
Ergonomics
from the values obtained during the conditions; either
mood induction or the two drives.
A repeated-measures MANOVA, with the withinsubject factor of music (positive/negative/no music)
was conducted on the subjective valence, energy, and
calmness reaction scores obtained after the music
mood induction. A significant multivariate effect of
music was found (F (6,70) ¼ 2.51, p ¼ 0.029, Z2 ¼
0.177). Univariate tests show significant main effects
on valence and energy reaction scores during the mood
induction (valence: F(2,36) ¼ 4.21, p ¼ 0.023, Z2 ¼
0.19, Energy: F(2,36) ¼ 6.0, p ¼ 0.006, Z2 ¼ 0.25,
calmness: F(2,36) ¼ 2.22, p ¼ 0.12, Z2 ¼ 0.11). Pairwise comparisons show that positive music results in
higher valence and energy reaction scores compared to
both the negative music (valence p ¼ 0.042, energy
p ¼ 0.042) and the no music conditions (valence
p ¼ 0.024, energy p ¼ 0.004). Figure 1 displays the
mean energy and valence ratings obtained after the
drives. Average and standard errors in parentheses of
the calmness ratings were 70.25 (0.15) for positive
music, 70.71 (0.17) for negative music, and 70.58
(0.15) for no music.
Next, a repeated-measures MANOVA with music
(positive/negative/no music) and driving demand
(wide/narrow) as within-subject factors was conducted
on the valence, energy, and calmness reaction scores
obtained after the two drives. Results show a marginally significant multivariate effect of music (F
(6,70) ¼ 2.22, p ¼ 0.053, Z2 ¼ 0.160). No significant
multivariate effects were found for driving demand (F
(3,16) ¼ 0.82, p ¼ 0.50, Z2 ¼ 0.13) or the music with
drive demand interaction (F (6,70) ¼ 0.42, p ¼ 0.86,
Z2 ¼ 0.04). The univariate results for music show
significant main effects on valence and energy reaction
scores (valence F (2,36) ¼ 4.33, p ¼ 0.021, Z2 ¼ 0.19;
energy F (6,70) ¼ 3.49, p ¼ 0.041, Z2 ¼ 0.16; calmness
(F (2,36) ¼ 2.21, p ¼ 0.124, Z2 ¼ 0.11). Pairwise
comparisons of music show marginally higher valence
reaction scores (p ¼ 0.051) and marginally higher
energy reaction scores (p ¼ 0.095) during the positive
condition compared to the no music condition
irrespective of driving demand. Figure 2 shows the
mean energy and valence reaction scores obtained after
the drives. Average calmness reaction scores were the
following: Mean (standard error) positive wide
M ¼ 70.27 (0.14), narrow M ¼ 70.55 (0.18),
negative wide M ¼ 70.74 (.20), narrow M ¼ 70.82
(0.23), no music wide M ¼ 70.76 (0.19), narrow
M ¼ 70.87 (0.19).
To evaluate the perceived amount of mental effort
(RSME scores) during the drives, a repeated-measure
ANOVA of music (positive/negative/no music) with
driving demand (wide/narrow) as within-subject
factors was conducted on the RSME ratings. A
significant effect of driving demand was found (F
(1,18) ¼ 9.12, p ¼ 0.007, Z2 ¼ 0.34). Pairwise
comparisons of driving demand reveal higher RSME
ratings during the narrow drive (M ¼ 39.37,
SE ¼ 5.42) compared to the wide drive (M ¼ 33.10,
SE ¼ 4.68). No significant effects of music or the music
with driving demand interaction were found; music F
(2,36) ¼ 1.96, p ¼ 0.156, Z2 ¼ 0.10; music with driving
demand F (2,36) ¼ 0.10, p ¼ 0.907, Z2 ¼ 0.005.
Figure 1. Subjective valence and energy reaction scores
provided after the music mood induction. The error bars
represent +standard error.
Figure 2. Subjective valence and energy reaction scores
provided after the wide and the narrow drives. The error bars
represent +standard error.
3.2.
Physiological responses
A repeated-measures MANOVA with music (positive/
negative/no music) as within-subject factor was
conducted for both the RR and the mean IBI duration
obtained during the last 3 min of the baseline period.
Results do not show a significant main effect of music
18
M.D. van der Zwaag et al.
on RR and on mean IBI, indicating that the baseline
RRs and IBI durations did not differ for the different
sessions; RR F (2,36) 5 1, p ¼ 0.54, Z2 ¼ 0.04, mean
(SE) in breath/min: positive 14.61 (0.95), negative
14.04 (0.73), no music 15.20 (0.76); IBI F (2,36) ¼ 1.18,
p ¼ 0.36, Z2 ¼ 0.62, mean (standard error) IBI
duration in seconds: positive ¼ 0.874 (0.03),
negative ¼ 0.846 (0.03), no music ¼ 0.876 (0.03).
Next, physiological reaction scores were created by
subtracting the average values obtained during the last
4 min of the baseline period with the values obtained
during the induction and the drives.
the IBI durations were not different during the
induction; F(2,36)51, p ¼ 0.40, Z2 ¼ 0.049, mean (SE)
positive 70.01 (.006), negative 70.008 (0.006), no
music 70.001 (0.006). Subsequently, a repeatedmeasure ANOVA was conducted with music (positive/
negative/no music) and driving demand (wide/narrow)
as within-subject factors on the average IBI durations
obtained during the drives. Results show no significant
effect for music or driving demand (all p 4 0.05)
positive mean (Standard Error) ¼ 70.017 (0.009),
negative ¼ 70.019 (0.008), no music ¼ 70.022
(0.007), wide drive ¼ 70.018 (0.07), narrow
drive ¼ 70.019 (0.07).
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
3.2.1. Respiration rate
A repeated-measures ANOVA with music (positive/
negative/no music) as within-subject variable was
conducted on the reaction scores of the RR obtained
during the induction. No significant main effects were
found showing the RRs did not differ during the
induction period (F(2,36)51, p ¼ 0.78, Z2 ¼ 0.013),
see Figure 3. Subsequently, a repeated-measure ANOVA was conducted with music (positive/negative/no
music) with driving demand (wide/narrow) as withinsubject factors on the RR. Results show a main effect
of music; F(2,36) ¼ 3.25, p ¼ 0.050, Z2 ¼ 0.153. Pairwise comparisons show a significantly lower RR
during the negative compared to the no music
condition (p ¼ 0.046) irrespective of driving demand,
see also Figure 3. No significant effects of the driving
demand or interaction effect of music with driving
demand were found.
3.2.2. Cardiovascular measures
3.3.
Driving performance
Separate repeated-measures analysis of music
(positive/negative/no music) with driving demand
(wide/narrow) as within-subject factors was conducted
on the mean of the LP, the standard deviation of the
lateral position (SDLP, swerving), and the speed. A
trend was found for mean LP, and for SDLP a
significant main effect of driving demand was found;
LP (m) F(1,17) ¼ 3.51, p ¼ 0.082, Z2 ¼ 0.201; LP (sd)
F(1,17) ¼ 23.80, p 5 0.001, Z2 ¼ 0.583. During the
narrow lane drive, more distance from the centre line is
found and the SDLP is smaller compared to the wide
lane drive; see also Figures 4 and 5. The results on
speed show a marginally significant main effect of
music (F(2,13) ¼ 3.18, p ¼ 0.075, Z2 ¼ 0.329).
Pairwise comparisons show higher speed during the no
music condition compared to the positive music
condition (p ¼ 0.023) irrespective of driving demand;
the average speed values are illustrated in Figure 6.
A repeated-measures ANOVA with music (positive/
negative/no music) as within-subject variable was
conducted on the IBI reaction scores of the induction.
No significant main effect of Music was found showing
4. Discussion
Music listening is a very popular side-activity while
driving. However, the influence of music listening on
Figure 3. The average respiration rates for induction, the
wide, and the narrow drives. The error bars represent + 1
standard error.
Figure 4. The average lateral position from the centerline
of the road, obtained during the wide (3 m) and narrow
(2.50 m) drives. The error bars represent + 1 standard error.
19
Ergonomics
4.1.
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
Figure 5. SDLP (standard deviation of the lateral position),
i.e. swerving, obtained during the drives on the wide (3 m)
and narrow (2.50 m) roads. The error bars represent + 1
standard error.
Figure 6. The speed (km/h) during the drive on the wide (3
m) and on the narrow (2.50 m) roads. The error bars
represent+1 standard error.
Mood induction through music
In line with the literature, in the two music conditions
positive or negative moods were induced with music
over 8-min periods while listening to music as the
primary task (Gendolla 2000). The condition without
music induced an equal mood state comparable with
the mood induction with negative music. This may be
explained in that the music is known to influence time
perception causing the music listening situations being
perceived shorter in duration (Cassidy and
MacDonald 2010). For instance, MacDonald et al.
(2003) have shown that waiting in a hospital context
with music makes participants less anxious than if they
have to wait without music. Hence, a situation in
which no music is presented might be perceived as long
and boring, thereby inducing a negative mood (Juslin
and Sloboda 2010).
In line with previous research, RR and heart rate
did not vary between positive and negative mood
inductions (Gendolla and Brinkman 2005, Silverstrini
and Gendolla 2007; Van der Zwaag and Westerink
2011a). This finding can be explained in that mood
does not immediately result in action tendencies and
thus not in altered physiological responses (Gendolla
2000). In contrast, it has been shown that for other
physiological measures than heart rate and RR, such
as skin conductance and facial muscle tension,
differentiation between moods during music mood
induction can be observed (Cacioppo et al. 2000a;
Van der Zwaag and Westerink 2011a).
4.2. Effects of music during high and low demand
drives
the body state and on driving performance is not yet
fully understood. In the current study it was
investigated whether personally selected positive and
negative music influences mood, body state, and
driving performance. Results show a successful mood
induction by music: the induced moods are congruent
with the expected moods based on the song stimuli
which were selected per participant (see also Figure 1).
Furthermore, music listening as primary activity did
not change cardiovascular and respiratory responses.
In contrast, lower RRs were found in the conditions
where music was presented during drives compared to
conditions in where no music was presented at all.
Finally, as hypothesised, less swerving was observed in
the higher demanding drives irrespective of music
presence. Thus, the current findings support that music
and driving demand influence mood, physiological
state, or driving performance to a certain extent.
As expected, (subjective) effort invested in driving was
higher while driving in the narrow lanes (Dijksterhuis
et al. 2011). Furthermore, as expected while driving,
the induced mood persists in terms of valence and
energy ratings irrespective of driving demand and
music type. The energy ratings increase during the
drive regardless of whether the music was positive or
negative, which can be attributed to the execution of a
concurrent task while driving. The result confirms
previous research findings (Van der Zwaag and
Westerink 2011b) and also extends the literature as it
indicates that moods induced with music can maintain
in concurrence of a task irrespective of task demands.
Listening to negative music compared to no music
while driving results in lower RRs irrespective of
driving demand. This finding holds even though
subjective mood ratings are equal when listening to
negative music compared to not listening to music.
This implies that music might be used to unconsciously
decrease body stress of the driver as RR has been
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
20
M.D. van der Zwaag et al.
linked to arousal (Boiten et al. 1994, Nyklı́cek et al.
1997, Ritz 2004, Homma and Masaoka 2008). For
example, Nyklı́cek et al. (1997) have shown that
stimulative music leads to faster RRs. This result
emphasises the importance of incorporating
physiological measurements as they can uncover
findings that would remain unnoticed by subjective
ratings (Van den Broek and Westerink 2009).
Driving performance was influenced by both
driving demand and the music played during the drive.
With regard to driving parameters, the mean LP and
the amount of swerving (SDLP) both decreased during
high demand drives. Driving in a narrow lane was as
expected associated with a decrease in swerving. This
confirms that more effort was put into the lane keeping
task to deal with decreased lateral margins. Furthermore, this is in accordance to Summala’s Multiple
Monitor Theory (Summala 2005, 2007), which states
that mental load increases when less time is available to
maintain safety margins. In a more general sense, these
results can be seen as an indication that more effort
was invested in the driving task to prevent performance degradation (Hockey 1997, 2003) or that the
level of effort was matched to the current task demands
(Hancock and Warm 1989, Matthews and Desmond
2002). Furthermore, in contrast to Pêcher et al. (2009),
in the current study in the no music condition swerving
was not decreased, hence music listening did not affect
lateral safety. This difference in results can be
explained in that Pêcher et al. (2009) altered music
periods with silent periods of 1 min each. This
alteration could distract the driver and therefore
decrease swerving while listening to music. The current
results thus show a more ecological valid situation and
hence more ecologically valid results.
Driving in a narrow lane did not decrease driving
speed. This could imply that the high demanding
situation was not demanding enough to lead to
changes in driving performance. The data do show
lower speed during the positive music drives compared
to the drives without music. This could be caused by
increased engagement in the drive while listening to
personally selected positive music which resulted in
context appropriate speed choices, instead of having
distracting effects (Cassidy and MacDonald 2009).
This finding is in line with the increased RRs during
the no music condition; faster speed coincides with
higher physical effort.
Lastly, the current study did not show an interaction between driving demand and music. This could be
explained in several ways: first, the music used in this
study might not have demanded much attentional
resources because it was moderate instead of high
arousing (Cassidy and MacDonald 2009). Listening to
music, therefore, did not require more than the
resources still available next to those required for
driving, so no real competition for resources resulted.
This in line with Beh and Hirst (1999) who showed that
high intensity music, which asks for more resources,
did decrease performance during high demand
conditions in a driving-related task. Second,
experienced drivers participated in the study.
Therefore, the additional load added by music listening
while driving could have been minimal, as the driving
task itself can be expected to be mainly automated.
Music listening during novice drivers might, however,
result in different outcomes. Taken together, these
results indicate that music intensity can be an
important factor in predicting the influence of music
on decreases in driving performance during high
demand driving situations.
4.3.
Limitations and future research
The song stimuli varied in valence and to a lesser extend
in energy levels as well. Note that the selected songs were
not the most contrasting songs, i.e. favourite songs
would have had the highest valence and energy levels,
and sad songs would have the lowest energy and valence
levels. From the individual song selection, it appeared
impossible to select songs that solely varied in mood
valence and having equal energy levels. This implies that
valence and energy ratings are not fully independent of
each other in inducing mood with music. This result is in
line with findings of psychobiological theory of
aesthetics (Berlyne 1971). This theory proposes that a Ushaped relationship exists between arousal and liking,
i.e. an average arousal level has the optimal liking level,
and increasing and decreasing arousal levels would
decrease liking levels (Berlyne 1971, Hargreaves and
North 2008).
To cope with the large individual differences in
music liking, individually selected song stimuli were
chosen in the current study (Juslin and Sloboda 2010).
This method assured that the songs stimuli selected
indeed induced the targeted moods. However, this
method also resulted in stimuli that were not
controlled for other music characteristics such as
familiarity, or characteristics inherent to the music as
tempo or mode, which could impact mood (Juslin and
Sloboda 2010, van der Zwaag et al. 2011). Future
research should point out how the lessons learned from
this study can be generalised to the impact of music
listening to different types of music during a wide
variety of driving scenarios.
4.4. Conclusion
In the current study, the influence of listening to music
on mood, body state, and driving performance during
Ergonomics
high and low demand drives were investigated. Moods
were successfully induced with music, and driving
without music was perceived with equal negative
feelings as driving with negative valence music.
Listening to negative music compared to no music
while driving leads to decreased RRs and listening to
positive music compared to no music leads to slower
driving speed. Furthermore, increased driving demand
led to an increased amount of swerving. In the present
study music did not impair driving performance as
often found in the literature. In contrast, listening to
music can even lead to an improved mood and a more
relaxed body state which could be beneficial for health
in the long run.
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the European 7th framework
REFLECT project. We would like to acknowledge Wilmer
Joling, Joop Clots, and Peter Albronda, for the technical
support while setting up this study.
References
Audacity, (Version 1.2.4). [Computer program]. Available
from: http://audacity.sourceforge.net/ [Accessed 6 February 2010].
Beh, H.C. and Hirst, R., 1999. Performance on drivingrelated tasks during music. Ergonomics, 42, 1087–1098.
Berlyne, C.E., 1971. Aesthetics and psychobiology. New
York: Appleton-Century, Crofts.
Blacking, J., 1973. How musical is man? Seattle, WA:
University of Washington Press.
Boiten, F.A., Frijda, N.H., and Wientjes, C.J.E., 1994.
Emotions and respiratory patterns: review and critical
analysis. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 17,
103–128.
Brodsky, W., 2002. The effects of music tempo on simulated
driving performance and vehicular control. Transportation Research Part F, 4, 219–241.
Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L., and Berntson, G., 2000a.
Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Cacioppo, J.T., et al., 2000b. The psychophysiology of
emotion. In: M. Lewis and J.M. Haviland-Jones, eds.
Handbook of emotions. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford,
173–191.
Cassidy, G.G. and MacDonald, R.A.R., 2009. The effects of
music choice on task performance: a study of the impact
of self-selected and experimenter-selected music on
driving game performance and experience. Musicae
Scientiae, 13, 357–386.
Cassidy, G.G. and MacDonald, R.A.R., 2010. The effects of
music on time perception and performance of a
driving game. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51,
455–464.
Dalton, B.H. and Behm, D.G., 2007. Effects of noise and
music on human and task performance: a systematic
review. Occupational Ergonomics, 7, 143–152.
Davis, C., 1992. The effects of music and basic relaxation
instruction on pain and anxiety of women undergoing inoffice gynaecological procedures. Journal of Music
Therapy, 29, 202–216.
21
DeJong, M., Van Mourik, K., and Schellekines, H., 1973. A
physiological approach to aesthetic preference – music.
Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 22, 46–51.
De Waard, D., et al., 1995. Effect of road layout and road
environment on driving performance, drivers’ physiology
and road appreciation. Ergonomics, 38, 1395–1407.
DeNora, T., 2000. Music in everyday life. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
DeNora, T., 2003. Music sociology: getting the music into
the action. British Journal of Music Education, 20, 165–
177.
Dibben, N. and Williamson, V.J., 2007. An exploratory
survey of in-vehicle music listening. Psychology of Music,
35, 571–589.
Dijksterhuis, C., De Waard, D., and Brookhuis, K.A., 2011.
Effects of steering demand on lane keeping behaviour,
self-reports, and physiology. A simulator study. Accident
Analysis and Prevention, 43, 1074–1081.
Gendolla, G.H.E., 2000. On the impact of mood on
behavior: an integrative theory and a review. Review of
General Psychology, 4, 378–408.
Gendolla, G.H.E. and Brinkmann, K., 2005. The role of
mood states in self-regulation effects on action
preferences and resource mobilization. European
Psychologist, 10, 187–198.
Godley, S.T., Triggs, T.J., and Fildes, B.N., 2004. Perceptual
land width, wide perceptual road centre markings and
driving speeds. Ergonomics, 47, 237–257.
Gomez, P. and Danuser, B., 2004. Affective and physiological
responses to environmental noises and music. International
Journal of Psychophysiology, 53, 91–103.
Grossman, P. and Taylor, E., 2007. Toward understanding
respiratory sinus arrythmia: relations to cardiac vagal
tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions. Biological
Psychology, 74, 263–285.
Hancock, P.A. and Warm, J.S., 1989. A dynamic model of
stress and sustained attention. Human Factors, 31,
519–537.
Hargreaves, D. and North, A., 2010. Experimental aesthetics
and liking for music. In: P. Juslin and J. Sloboda, eds.
Handbook of music and emotions: theory, research,
applications. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press,
515–547.
Hockey, G.R.J., 1997. Compensatory control in the
regulation of human performance under stress and high
workload: a cognitive energetical framework. Biologica
Psychology, 45, 73–93.
Hockey, G.R.J., 2003. Operator functional state as a
framework for the assessment of performance
degradation. In: G.R.J. Hockey, A.W.K. Gaillard, and
O. Burov, eds. Operator functional state. Amsterdam:
IOS Press, 8–23.
Hodges, D.A., 2010. Bodily responses to music. In: P.N.
Juslin and J.A. Sloboda, eds. Handbook of music and
emotion: theory, research, and applications. New York:
Oxford University Press, 279–311.
Homma, I. and Masaoka, Y., 2008. Breathing rhythms
and emotions. Experimental Psychology, 93 (9), 1011–
1021.
Iwanaga, M. and Moroki, Y., 1999. Subjective and
physiological responses to music stimuli controlled over
activity and preference. Journal of Music Therapy, 36,
26–38.
Juslin, P.N. and Sloboda, J.A., eds., 2010. Handbook of music
and emotion: theory, research, applications. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Downloaded by [University of Groningen] at 00:50 09 January 2012
22
M.D. van der Zwaag et al.
Kastner, M.P. and Crowder, R.G., 1990. Perception of the
major/minor distinction: Iv. Emotional connotation in
young children. Music Perception, 8, 189–202.
Kivy, P., 1989. Sound sentiment: an essay on the musical
emotions. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.
Kivy, P., 1990. Music alone: philosophical reflections on the
purely musical experience. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University
Press.
Knight, W. and Rickard, N., 2001. Relaxing music prevents
stress-induced increases in subjective anxiety, systolic
blood pressure, and heart rate in healthy males and
females. Journal of Music Therapy, 38, 254–272.
Konecni, V., 1982. Social interaction and musical preference.
In: D. Deutsch, ed. The psychology of music. New York:
Academic Press, 297–516.
Krumhansl, C., 1997. An exploratory study of musical
emotions and psychophysiology. Canadian Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 51, 336–353.
MacDonald, R.A.R., et al., 2003. An empirical investigation
of the anxiolytic and pain reducing effects of music.
Psychology of Music, 31, 187–203.
Matthews, G. and Desmond, P.A., 2002. Task-induced fatigue
states and simulated driving performance. The Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 659–686.
Matthews, G., Jones, D.M., and Chamberlain, A., 1990.
Refining the measurement of mood: the UWIST mood
adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–
42.
North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J., 2008. The social and
applied psychology of music. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J., 1999. Music and driving
game performance. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
40, 285–292.
Nyklı́cek, I., Thayer, J.F., and Van Doornen, L.J.P., 1997.
Cardiorespiratory differentiation of emotions. Journal of
Psychophysiology, 11, 304–321.
Pêcher, C., Lemercier, C., and Cellier, J.-M., 2009. Emotions
drive attention: effects on driver’s behaviour. Safety
Science, 47, 1254–1259.
Piferi, R.L., et al., 2000. An alternative approach for
achieving cardiovascular baseline: viewing an aquatic
video. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37,
207–217.
Recarte, M.A. and Nunes, L.M., 2000. Effects of verbal and
spatial-imagery tasks on fixations while driving. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 31–43.
Rickard, N., 2004. Intense emotional responses to music: a
test of the physiological arousal hypotheses. Psychology
of Music, 32, 371–388.
Ritz, T., 2004. Probing the psychophysiology of the airways:
Physical activity, experienced emotion, and facially
expressed emotion. Psychophysiology, 41 (6), 809–821.
Saarikallio, S. and Erkkilä, J., 2007. The role of music in
adolescents’ mood regulation. Psychology of Music, 35,
88–109.
Shek, V. and Schubert, E., 2009. Background music at worka literature review and some hypotheses. In: C. Stevens
et al., eds. Proceedings of the 2nd international conference
on music communication science (ICoMCS2). 3–4 December, 2009. Sydney, Australia: HCSNet, University of
Western Sydney, 87–91.
Silverstrini, N. and Gendolla, G.H.E., 2007. Mood effects on
autonomic activity in mood regulation. Psychophysiology, 44, 650–659.
View publication stats
Skowronek, J., McKinney, M.F., and van de, P.S., 2006.
Ground truth for automatic music mood classification.
In: Proceedings of the 7th international conference on
music information retrieval. Victoria, Canada: University
of Victoria, 395–396.
Skowronek, J., McKinney, M.F., and van der, P.S., 2007. A
demonstrator for automatic music mood estimation. In:
A demonstrator for automatic music mood estimation.
Vienna, Austria: Austrian Computer Society, 345–346.
Sloboda, J.A. and Juslin, P.N., 2010. At the interface between
the inner and outer world, psychological perspectives. In:
P.N. Juslin and J.A. Sloboda, eds. Handbook of music and
emotion: theory, research, and applications. Chapter 4.
New York: Oxford University Press, 73–98.
Stern, R., Ray, W., and Quigley, K., 2001.
Psychophysiological recording. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Summala, H., 2005. Traffic psychology theories: towards
understanding driving behaviour and safety efforts. In:
G. Underwood, ed. Traffic and transport psychology
theory and application. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 383–394.
Summala, H., 2007. Towards understanding motivational
and emotional factors in driver behaviour: comfort
through satisficing. In: P.C. Cacciabue, ed. Modelling
driver behaviour in automotive environments: critical issues
in driver interactions with intelligent transport systems.
London: Springer, 189–207.
Van den Broek, E.L. and Westerink, J.H.D.M., 2009.
Considerations for emotion-aware consumer products.
Applied Ergonomics, 40 (6), 1055–1064.
Van der Zwaag, M.D. and Westerink, J.H.D.M., 2010.
Musical induction and persistence of mood. In: S.M.
Demorest, S.J. Morrison and P.S. Campbell, eds.
Proceedings of the 11th ICMPC, 23–27 August 2010.
Seattle, University of Washington, 45–46.
Van der Zwaag, M.D. and Westerink, J.H.D.M. 2011a.
Physiological patterns to music mood induction.
Manuscript submitted for publication.
Van der Zwaag, M.D. and Westerink, J.H.D.M. 2011b.
Sustaining moods with background music: physiological
patterns and subjective experience. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
Van der Zwaag, M.D., Westerink, J.H.D.M., and Van den
Broek, E.L., 2011. Emotional and psychophysiological
responses to tempo, mode, and percussiveness. Special issue
on Music and Emotion. Musicae Scientiae, 15, 250–269.
Van Winsum, W. and Van Wolffelaar, P.C., 1993. GIDS
small world simulation. In: J.A. Michon, ed. Generic
intelligent driver support. London: Taylor and Francis,
175–191.
Walsh, M.J., 2010. Driving to the beat of one’s own hum:
automobility and musical listening. Studies in Symbolic
Interaction, 35, 201–221.
Wientjes, C.J., 1992. Respiration in psychophysiology:
methods and applications. Biological Psychology, 34,
179–203.
Wiesenthal, D.L., Hennessy, D.A., and Totten, B., 2000. The
influence of music on driver stress. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 30, 1709–1719.
Wiesenthal, D.L., Hennessy, D.A., and Totten, B., 2003. The
influence of music on mild drivers aggression.
Transportation Research Part F, 6, 125–134.
Zijlstra, F.R.H., 1993. Efficiency in work behavior. A design
approach for modern tools. Thesis (PhD). Delft
University of Technology, Delft, the Netherlands.