Bilingualism: Language and Cognition
http://journals.cambridge.org/BIL
Additional services for Bilingualism:
Language and Cognition:
Email alerts: Click here
Subscriptions: Click here
Commercial reprints: Click here
Terms of use : Click here
Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control
MARCO CALABRIA, FRANCESCA M. BRANZI, PAULA MARNE, MIREIA HERNÁNDEZ and ALBERT COSTA
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition / FirstView Article / August 2013, pp 1 - 14
DOI: 10.1017/S1366728913000138, Published online: 25 April 2013
Link to this article: http://journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S1366728913000138
How to cite this article:
MARCO CALABRIA, FRANCESCA M. BRANZI, PAULA MARNE, MIREIA HERNÁNDEZ and ALBERT COSTA Age-related
effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, Available on CJO 2013
doi:10.1017/S1366728913000138
Request Permissions : Click here
Downloaded from http://journals.cambridge.org/BIL, IP address: 91.240.2.62 on 09 Sep 2013
C Cambridge University Press 2013 doi:10.1017/S1366728913000138
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition: page 1 of 14
Age-related effects over
bilingual language control
and executive control∗
MARCO CALABRIA
Center for Brain and Cognition, Universitat Pompeu
Fabra, Spain
FRANCESCA M. BRANZI
Center for Brain and Cognition, Universitat Pompeu
Fabra, Spain
PAU L A M A R N E
Center for Brain and Cognition, Universitat Pompeu
Fabra, Spain
MIREIA HERNÁNDEZ
Center for Brain and Cognition, Universitat Pompeu
Fabra, Spain & Cognitive Neuropsychology Laboratory,
Department of Psychology, Harvard University,
Cambridge, MA, USA
A L B E RT C O S TA
Center for Brain and Cognition, Universitat Pompeu
Fabra, Spain & Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis
Avançats, Spain
(Received: July 25, 2012; final revision received: March 7, 2013; accepted: March 13, 2013)
The aim of the present study is two-fold. First, we investigate age-related changes to bilingual language control (bLC)
mechanisms across lifespan. Second, we explore the relation between bLC mechanisms and those of the domain-general
executive (EC) system by looking at age effects on these two systems. To do so, we compare the performances of the three age
groups of bilinguals (young, middle-aged and elderly) in a language switching task to those of non-linguistic switching task.
We found an age-related change in the non-linguistic switch cost but not in the language switch cost. Moreover, we did not
find any correlation between the magnitudes of the switch costs. Taken together these results indicate that bLC is not affected
by age as the EC system is, and interestingly, we add new evidence that the bLC mechanisms are not fully subsidiary to those
of the domain-general EC system.
Keywords: bilingualism, aging, executive control, bilingual language control
1. Introduction
Language production is the set of processes that
allows individuals to translate thoughts into speech.
These processes include the selection of a concept
to be expressed, the lexical retrieval of the words
and their morphological properties, and the planning
and the monitoring of the articulatory aspects of the
* This work was supported by grants from the Spanish government (PSI2008-01191, PSI2011-23033, Consolider Ingenio 2010
CSD2007-00012, IMSERSO 48/2010) and the Catalan government
(Consolidat SGR 2009-1521). Marco Calabria was supported by
a postdoctoral fellowship from the Spanish Government (Juan de
la Cierva fellowship). Francesca M. Branzi was supported by a
predoctoral fellowship from the Spanish Government (FPU-20092013). This research was also supported by a Marie Curie International
Outgoing Fellowship within the 7th European Community Framework
Programme awarded to Mireia Hernández. We thank three anonymous
reviewers for their comments.
speech output. Although unimpaired individuals appear
to conduct all these processes effortlessly and with
high reliability, it requires the participation of executive
control (EC) processes (Roelofs & Piai, 2011; Strijkers,
Holcomb & Costa, 2011; Ye & Zhou, 2009). Hence, the
domain-general EC system is constantly interacting with
the language production system to guarantee successful
communication. A particular instance in which this
interaction becomes very apparent is that of bilingual
speech production, since bilingual speakers, beyond
mastering all the processes involved in lexicalization, also
have to learn how to prevent cross-language interference.
That is, bilinguals need not only to select the language in
which they want to conduct verbalization (according to
the communicative setting), but they also need to avoid
the potential interference from the irrelevant language.
Furthermore, on some occasions bilinguals have to
switch between the two languages according to the given
interlocutor. The cognitive processes involved in this
ability are usually referred to as bilingual language control
Address for correspondence:
Albert Costa, Departament de Tecnologies de la Informació i les Comunicacions, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, C/ Tanger, 122-140, 08014-Barcelona,
Spain
costalbert@gmail.com
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
IP address: 91.240.2.62
2
Marco Calabria, Francesca M. Branzi, Paula Marne, Mireia Hernández and Albert Costa
(bLC) (e.g., Abutalebi & Green, 2007; Green, 1986, 1998;
Soveri, Rodriguez-Fornells & Laine, 2011). The goal of
the present investigation is to explore whether and how
bLC is affected by aging. To do so, we compare the
performance of three age groups of bilingual speakers
(young, middle-aged and elderly) on a typical bLC task
called the language switching task.
Most of the current evidence regarding bLC comes
almost exclusively from studies in which young university
students have been tested (Calabria, Hernandez, Branzi
& Costa, 2011; Costa & Santesteban, 2004; Costa,
Santesteban & Ivanova, 2006; Garbin, Costa, Sanjuan,
Forn, Rodriguez-Pujadas, Ventura, Belloch, Hernandez
& Ávila, 2011; Hernandez, Dapretto, Mazziotta &
Bookheimer, 2001; Hervais-Adelman, Moser-Mercer
& Golestani, 2011; Magezi, Khateb, Mouthon, Spierer
& Annoni, 2012). To date, the few studies that have
investigated the effects of aging on the functioning of
the bLC have shown moderate effects. For instance,
Gollan, Sandoval and Salmon (2011) showed that the
number of cross-language intrusions during verbal fluency
tasks increases little with age. Moreover, Weissberger,
Wierenga, Bondi and Gollan (2012) showed a complex
pattern of switching costs in which some of such costs
were affected by aging and others were not. For example,
while language mixing costs were relatively unaffected by
aging, local-switch costs were affected. Thus, the current
evidence is not sufficient to argue in favour of detrimental
effects of aging on bLC.
Furthermore, there are also reasons to suspect agerelated changes over bLC. First, to the extent that
bLC depends on the efficient functioning of the EC
system, one might expect that the decline of the EC
system associated with aging (Greenwood, 2000; Rhodes,
2004; Tisserand & Jolles, 2003; Verhaeghen & Cerella,
2002; Verhaeghen, Steitz, Sliwinski & Cerella, 2003;
Wasylyshyn, Verhaeghen & Sliwinski, 2011) affects the
functionality of the bLC system as well. Indeed, some
authors have proposed that some of the age-related
changes in language production are due to defective
functioning of EC mechanisms, such as the weakness of
the inhibitory control and/or the reduction of working
memory abilities. Second, it has been reported that aging
affects a bilingual’s performance in language production
tasks. For example, Bialystok, Craik and Luk (2008)
reported that bilingual elderly adults compared to young
bilinguals have deficits in lexical access when tested in a
verbal fluency task. Gollan et al. (2011) also found an
increase in cross-language intrusions (e.g., an English
word when speaking Spanish) in elderly bilinguals in
verbal fluency. These results suggest that there are agerelated changes in bilingual language production that are
probably due to a loss in efficiency of bLC.
The available evidence for the age effects of bLC comes
from language switching studies (Gollan & Ferreira, 2009;
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
Hernandez & Khonert, 1999; Weissberger et al., 2012). A
typical example of this task is the following: participants
are required to name a series of pictures in different
language conditions and the language to use in each trial
is cued. There are two kinds of trials: those in which,
in a given trial and in the immediately preceding trial,
the naming language does not change (repeat trials), and
those in which the language changes from one trial to the
successive one (switch trials). Participants are slower and
less accurate on switch trials than repeat trials, thus the
difference in reaction times between these two types of
trials is named language switch cost. Two main findings
have been reported in this context (Gollan & Ferreira,
2009; Hernandez & Khonert, 1999; Weissberger et al.,
2012): (i) elderly bilinguals are overall slower and more
error-prone than young bilinguals; (ii) there is an agerelated increase in the magnitude of switch costs.
In the present study we further explore the age-related
changes on bLC across the lifespan using language
switching in Catalan–Spanish bilinguals of three age
groups: young, middle-aged and elderly adults.
A second goal of this article is to advance our
knowledge of how the bLC relates to the domain-general
EC system. Although at present few can deny that these
two systems interact (for reviews, see Abutalebi & Green,
2007; Hervais-Adelman et al., 2011) we are far from
understanding in which way they do so. Perhaps one
way to gain more knowledge on this issue is to address
whether the performances in linguistic and non-linguistic
switching tasks suffer from a similar decline due to aging.
To assess this issue, in the present study we also test
the three age groups of participants in a non-linguistic
switching task, in which bilinguals are cued to judge a
series of pictures according to two sorting criteria based
on their colour or their shape. Next, we discuss a series of
studies that are relevant in this context.
In a recent article, Prior and Gollan (2011) assessed the
relationship between the EC and bLC by testing bilinguals
and monolinguals using a non-linguistic switching task
and bilinguals in a language switching task. Interestingly,
the group of bilinguals that reported switching languages
frequently showed a smaller language switch cost than the
group of bilinguals that claimed not to switch languages
so frequently. Furthermore, those that switched often
showed a smaller switch cost in the non-linguistic task as
compared to the monolinguals, suggesting a link between
bLC and EC system.
Somewhat in contrast with these observations, Calabria
et al. (2011) failed to observe a correlation between the
magnitude of switch costs observed in language switching
and in non-linguistic switching tasks. The results
revealed that the two types of switch cost were
uncorrelated, suggesting that the bLC mechanisms are
not fully subsidiary to those of the domain-general EC
system.
IP address: 91.240.2.62
Bilingual language control and aging
A recent study by Weissberger et al. (2012) has
addressed the issue of the cross-talk between the bLC
and EC system by studying the performance of elderly
bilinguals. Weissberger et al. (2012) observed an agerelated increase in the magnitude of the language switch
cost, that is, the magnitude of the switch cost was smaller
for younger than elderly bilinguals. Interestingly, however,
the performance in the non-linguistic switch task was
much less affected by aging, showing significant effects
only for error rates. This pattern was interpreted as
revealing differential effects of aging on the bLC and EC
systems.1
1.1 The present study
The aim of the present study is two-fold: first, to further
explore to what extent and how bLC is affected by aging,
and second, to assess the interaction of the bLC and EC
mechanisms across the lifespan.
To achieve these goals, sixty Catalan–Spanish highlyproficient bilinguals of three age groups (young, middleaged and elderly) were tested in a language switching
task and in a non-linguistic switching task (sorting by
colour and shape) (see Calabria et al., 2011). We pay
attention to both the quantitative and qualitative aspects
of the switch costs. Quantitatively, we assess the presence
of correlations between the two tasks in terms of overall
speed and in terms of the magnitude of the switch
costs, paying special attention to how these magnitudes
are affected by aging. The qualitative analysis assesses
whether the pattern of switch costs in the two tasks is
affected by aging. Here, it is not so much important
whether the two tasks elicit the same pattern of switching
costs, but rather whether such a pattern is affected by
aging in both tasks.
The asymmetry of the switch costs is defined as the
degree to which the magnitude of the costs to switch
between two tasks is similar. One variable that affects
switch cost is, for example, the relative difficulty of the
two tasks at hand during the experiment (e.g., Calabria
et al., 2011; Martin, Barcelo, Hernandez & Costa, 2011).
For instance, the switching costs tend to be larger when
1
To some extent, Gollan et al. (2011) reported a similar result by
comparing the age-related effects in verbal fluency task and in an
EC task (flanker task). Gollan et al. (2011) took the cross-language
intrusions as a measure of the defective functioning of the bLC and
the magnitude of the conflict effect as a measure of the efficiency
of the EC system. Then, the authors correlated the two measures in
elderly English–Spanish bilinguals and actually they found a positive
correlation. However, the few number of cross-language intrusions
(about 1%) experienced by elderly people suggested that language
control in bilinguals is not affected with the same severity by aging
as the domain-general EC system is. Consequently, this unequal
decline of bLC and EC systems led the authors to conclude that
the overlapping of bLC and EC mechanisms is to some extent partial.
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
3
switching into the easier task than when switching into
the more difficult one (for theoretical explanation see
the review by Koch, Gade, Schuch & Philipp, 2010).
Similarly, when the task switching involves two languages,
low-proficient bilinguals show asymmetrical switch costs
(i.e., larger switch-costs when switching into the easier
language) which parallel the pattern of the non-linguistic
task-switching paradigms. That is, for low-proficient
bilinguals switching into the less proficient (and hence,
the more difficult task) language (L2) is easier (in terms
of RTs and errors) than switching into the more proficient
(and hence, the easier task) language (L1) (e.g., Meuter
& Allport, 1999). This linguistic asymmetrical switch
cost can be explained in the same manner as domaingeneral asymmetrical switching costs. In fact, Meuter and
Allport (1999) argued that the magnitude of the inhibition
applied to two languages is dependent on the relative
strength of the two languages. Therefore, when the less
proficient L2 needs to be produced, the more proficient
L1 needs to be inhibited more than the other way around.
Thus, an asymmetrical switch cost arises because the
amount of inhibition that needs to be overcome during
the switch into L1 is larger than when switching into
L2. However, several studies conducted with highlyproficient bilinguals revealed no asymmetrical language
switching costs. That is, when highly-proficient bilinguals
are asked to switch between their two proficient languages
(hence little difference in difficulty between the two
tasks), the switching costs are comparable in both
directions (from L1 to L2 and vice versa) (Calabria
et al., 2011; Costa & Santesteban, 2004; Costa et al.,
2006).
Interestingly, this pattern of switching costs in highlyproficient bilinguals is restricted to the linguistic domain.
In a previous study, we found symmetrical switching costs
for highly-proficient young bilinguals when switching
between languages, and asymmetrical switching costs
when they switched from colour to shape, that is, in a nonlinguistic switching task (Calabria et al., 2011). What is
important in the present context is whether these different
patterns of switching costs would vary across the life span.
That is, whether the linguistic switching symmetry and the
non-linguistic switching asymmetry would be affected by
aging.
To recapitulate, we examine the issue of the integrity of
bLC and its relation with the EC system in several ways.
First, we will evaluate the integrity of bLC by looking
at the speed of processing, the accuracy and the magnitude
of the language switch costs in the three age groups. For
instance, a slowing of the reaction times, an increase in
errors and the switch costs across groups would indicate
age-related changes over bLC. Moreover, we will also
look at the qualitative aspect of the language switch cost,
such as the symmetry, in the three age groups of bilinguals
as an index of the efficiency of bLC functioning.
IP address: 91.240.2.62
4
Marco Calabria, Francesca M. Branzi, Paula Marne, Mireia Hernández and Albert Costa
Table 1. Participant characteristics for the three age groups.
Young
Age
Education
Age of L2 acquisition
Middle-aged
Elderly
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
21.8
16.5
1.7
2.2
2.5
2.1
45.7
18.6
1.8
5.1
2.6
2.0
70.5
15.1
1.1
4.0
2.4
1.9
4.0
4.0
3.9
4.0
4.0
0.0
0.0
0.4
0.0
0.0
4.0
4.0
4.0
3.6
4.0
0.0
0.0
0.0
0.5
0.0
4.0
4.0
4.0
2.0
3.1
0.0
0.0
0.0
1.5
1.1
4.0
3.9
4.0
3.9
4.0
0.0
0.3
0.0
0.3
0.0
4.0
4.0
4.0
3.8
4.0
0.0
0.0
0.0
0.4
0.0
4.0
3.7
3.8
3.6
4.0
0.0
0.4
0.3
1.6
0.0
Self-rating
Catalan
Comprehension
Speaking
Pronunciation
Writing
Reading
Spanish
Comprehension
Speaking
Pronunciation
Writing
Reading
Second, to explore the relationship between bLC and
EC, we will examine:
(a) From a quantitative point of view, the magnitude of
linguistic and non-linguistic switch costs and any
potential correlations between the two switch costs.
A similar age-related increase in the switch costs
and significant correlations between switch costs in
linguistic and non-linguistic switching tasks would
suggest similar age-related effects, and to some
extent, demonstrate that the bLC mechanisms are
fully subsidiary to those of the EC system.
(b) From a qualitative point of view, we examine
the pattern of switch costs in terms of the
symmetry/asymmetry within the linguistic and nonlinguistic switching tasks across three age groups. The
presence of similar age-related changes in the pattern
of switch costs in both tasks would suggest that the
mechanisms of bLC are completely subsidiary to the
EC system.
2. Methods
2.1 Participants
Sixty bilinguals took part in the experiment. All
participants were early and highly-proficient Catalan–
Spanish bilinguals with Catalan as L1, having learned
Spanish before the age of six. Their proficiency in the two
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
languages was tested by means of a questionnaire at the
end of the experiment. Each participant self-rated on a
four-point scale the abilities of speaking, pronunciation,
comprehension, writing and reading for each language
(1 = poor, 2 = regular, 3 = good, 4 = perfect).
The whole sample of participants was divided into three
age groups, such as: young (n = 20; mean age = 21.8
years, range = 19–27 years), middle-aged (n = 20; mean
age = 45.7, range = 38–53), and elderly bilinguals (n =
20, mean age = 70.5, range = 62–77). The characteristics
of the three age groups of participants (age, education,
age of acquisition of L2, and language proficiency) are
reported in the Table 1.
2.2 Materials and procedure
The experiment was conducted in a soundproof room.
Participants performed the linguistic and non-linguistic
versions of the tasks in the same session. The experiments
were controlled by the software DMDX (Forster & Forster,
2003), which recorded participants’ vocal and manual
responses. Responses were analysed offline and naming
latencies were measured from the onset of the word
through Checkvocal, a data analysis program for naming
tasks in DMDX (Protopapas, 2007). The order of the two
tasks was counterbalanced across participants, meaning
that half of the participants started with the language
switching task and the other half with the non-linguistic
switching task. Each experiment started with a practice
session of 80 trials.
IP address: 91.240.2.62
Bilingual language control and aging
Linguistic switching task
Eight pictures of objects were selected from Snodgrass
and Vanderwart (1980). Half of them referred
to cognate words (Spanish/Catalan names: Caracol/Cargol (in English, “snail”); Escoba/Escombra
(“broom”); Martillo/Martell (“hammer”); Reloj/Rellotge
(“watch”)), and the other half to non-cognate words
(Calcetín/Mitjó (“sock”); Manzana/Poma (“apple”);
Silla/Cadira (“chair”); Tenedor/Forquilla (“fork”)). Note
that in all analyses the two categories were collapsed since
there was no difference between cognate and non-cognate
words.
Participants were required to name the picture in
Catalan or in Spanish. A Catalan or Spanish flag, which
was presented along with the picture, acted as a cue
to indicate in which language subjects had to name the
picture.
There were two types of trials: (i) those in which
participants were required to name the picture in the same
language as the preceding trial (repeat trial), and (ii) those
in which participants were required to name in the other
language with respect to the previous trial (switch trial).
There were a total of 320 trials divided in two blocks with
160 trials each. The total distribution of trials was: 128
repeat trials in Catalan, 128 repeat trials in Spanish, 32
switch trials in Catalan and 32 in Spanish.
Participants were asked to name the picture as fast as
possible and they were informed that the language to be
used was to be indicated by a flag, presented on the top
of the picture. The pictures were presented in a series
of between three and seven trials and at the end of each
series an asterisk appeared, and the participants pressed
the spacebar to start the next series. At the beginning
of each series a word cue was presented for 1000 ms
indicating in which language participants had to start to
name in (CATALÀ, for Catalan; ESPAÑOL, for Spanish)
and for the other trials of the series the cue appeared along
with the picture to name. The picture appeared for 1700
ms and the timeout to respond was 5000 ms.
Non-linguistic switching task
Three shapes (square, circle, and triangle) and three
colours (green, blue, and red) were selected for the task.
The three shapes were combined with the three colours,
resulting in a total of nine coloured shapes (e.g., green
square, blue square etc.). Participants were presented with
an array containing three shapes, two at the top of the
screen and one at the bottom. They were instructed to
match the shape at the bottom with one of the two at the
top of the display according to two possible criteria (shape
or colour). The criterion was indicated by a cue (COLOR,
for Colour; FORMA, for Shape) appearing in the centre
of the array. As in the linguistic version of the task, there
were two types of trials: repeat and switch trials.
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
5
At the beginning of each series a word cue was
presented for 1000 ms indicating by which rule
participants must start matching each item (COLOR, for
Colour; FORMA, for Shape). Then the array appeared for
2500 ms and the timeout to respond was 3000 ms.
Participants gave the response by pressing one of the
two keys, M or V, according to the position of the matched
picture at the top of the array. Specifically, they had to press
the M key when the correct answer was at the top-right
part of the array and the V key when the correct response
was at the top-left part of the array.
3. Results
First, we analysed the data for RTs and accuracy
(percentage of correct responses). RTs exceeding three
standard deviations above or below a given participant’s
mean were excluded from the analyses. Second, we
performed a distributional analysis of the RTs by fitting
the data to an ex-Gaussian distribution. All the details of
this second analysis are reported below.
3.1 Omnibus ANOVA
We first ran an omnibus ANOVA on RTs including the
following variables: “type of trial” (switch vs. repeat) and
“task version” (linguistic vs. non-linguistic) as withinsubject variables and “group” (young, middle-aged and
elderly) as between-subject factor.
All main effects were significant, that is “task version”
(F(1,57) = 11.21, MSE = 21084.52, p = .001, ηp2 = .16),
“type of trial” (F(1,57) = 230.27, MSE = 1651.42, p <
.0001, ηp2 = .80) and “group” (F(1,57) = 14.26, MSE =
55519.71, p < .0001, ηp2 = .33). The interaction between
“type of trial” and “group” was marginally significant
(F(1,57) = 2.49, MSE = 1651.42, p = .09, ηp2 = .09).
Importantly, the interaction between “task version”
and “group” was significant (F(1,57) = 8.69, MSE =
21084.52, p = .001, ηp2 = .23), revealing that the
performance in the two tasks was affected differently
by age (see Table 2). To further explore how age
affected the switching tasks differently, the results from
the linguistic and the non-linguistic switching task were
further analysed separately.
3.2 Linguistic switching task
The variables considered in the analyses were “type of
trial” (switch vs. repeat) and “response language” (L1
vs. L2) as within-subject factors and “group” (young,
middle-aged and elderly) as between-subject factor which
were included in a repeated-measure ANOVA on naming
latencies and accuracy.
IP address: 91.240.2.62
6
Marco Calabria, Francesca M. Branzi, Paula Marne, Mireia Hernández and Albert Costa
Table 2. Mean RTs and SEs of the linguistic and non-linguistic
switching tasks broken down by conditions and age groups.
L1
Young
Middle-aged
Elderly
Repeat
Switch
Repeat
Switch
Repeat
Switch
L2
Mean
SE
Mean
SE
822
902
877
961
939
1016
21.6
22.5
21.6
22.5
21.6
22.5
837
912
914
974
927
1010
20.0
22.7
20.0
22.7
20.0
22.7
Colour
Young
Middle-aged
Elderly
Repeat
Switch
Repeat
Switch
Repeat
Switch
Mean
SE
Mean
SE
749
866
878
971
992
1138
36.7
42.4
36.8
43.4
36.8
42.4
873
895
1008
1049
1174
1253
39.1
43.1
39.1
43.1
39.1
43.1
Reaction times
The main effect of “group” was significant (F(1,57) =
6.77, MSE = 32742.16, p = .002, ηp2 = .19). Posthoc analysis revealed significant differences only between
young participants and the other two groups (all ps <
.03). The main effect of “type of trial” was significant
(F(1,57) = 167.21, MSE = 2083.70, p <.0001,
ηp2 = .75) indicating that participants responded slower to
switch trials (962 ms) than to repeat trials (886 ms). The
main effect of “response language” was not significant
(F(1,57) = 2.93, MSE = 1803.18, p = .10). The
interaction between “group” and “response language” was
also significant (F(1,57) = 3.22, MSE = 1803.18, p =
.05, ηp2 = .11), indicating that the difference between the
latencies in the two languages were only present for the
middle-aged group.
Interestingly, no other interactions were significant.
First, in quantitative terms, this means that the magnitude
of the linguistic switch cost is not modulated by age.
Indeed, as it can be appreciated in Figure 1, the magnitude
of the switch costs was very similar for all three groups,
and far from ceiling or floor effects. Second, qualitatively,
we can conclude that the pattern of switch costs is not
affected by age, since it appears to be symmetrical across
all ages. That is, switching from L1 into L2 and vice versa
has the same cost irrespective of the age of the bilingual
speakers.
These conclusions are further supported by a
regression analysis in which age and age of L2 acquisition
http://journals.cambridge.org
Shape
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
Figure 1. Performances on the linguistic switching task for
the three age groups of participants. Error bars represent
standard errors.
IP address: 91.240.2.62
Bilingual language control and aging
7
are taken into account. In this analysis the magnitude of
the linguistic switch costs (in L1 and L2, separately, and
also with collapsing both costs) was not modulated by
these two variables, regardless of whether the fit was linear
(all ps > .41), logarithmic (all ps > .91) or quadratic (all
ps > .68).
Accuracy
There was no effect of age in accuracy (“group”:
F(2,57) = 1.89, MSE = 11.37, p = .16). Participants were
less accurate in switch trials (95.9%) than repeat trials
(97.2%) (“type of trial”: F(1,57) = 9.18, MSE = 11.17,
p = .004; ηp2 = .14). No interaction was statistically
significant.
3.3 Non-linguistic switching task
The results of this task were submitted basically to the
same analysis as in the previous task. The variables
considered in the analysis were “group” as a betweensubject factor, “type of trial” (switch vs. repeat) and
“sorting criterion” (colour and form), which were included
as a within-subject factor in a repeated-measure ANOVA
using RTs and accuracy as dependent variables.
Reaction times
The main effect of “group” was significant (F(1,57) =
14.34, MSE = 120466.30, p < .001, ηp2 = .34) (see
Figure 2). Post-hoc analysis showed that the young group
was the faster one and the elderly group the slower one,
and the middle-aged group in the middle of the other two
groups (all ps < .02).
Overall, participants were faster to sort by colour (932
ms) than by shape (1042 ms) (F(1,57) = 167.21, MSE =
2083.70, p < .0001, ηp2 = .75), and faster to respond in
repeat trials (945 ms) compared to switch trials (1028 ms)
(F(1,57) = 94.72, MSE = 4354.45, p < .0001, ηp2 = .79).
Moreover, the interaction between “sorting criterion” and
“type of trial” was significant (F(1,57) = 28.95, MSE =
2665.73, p < .0001, ηp2 = .34), meaning that the switch
cost interacted with criterion. That is, participants showed
higher costs when they switched from shape to colour
(118 ms) (F(1,59) = 101.23, MSE = 4183.64, p < .0001,
ηp2 = .63), than when they switched from colour to shape
(47 ms) (F(1,59) = 21.49, MSE = 3111.93, p < .0001,
ηp2 = .27).
Finally, the non-linguistic switch cost interacted with
the main effect of “group” (interaction between “type of
trial” and “group” : F(2,59) = 2.99, MSE = 4354.45, p =
.05, ηp2 = 0.10), being significantly higher in the elderly
group (112 ms) compared to the other two age groups
(young = 69 ms and middle-aged = 68 ms).
The analysis performed here was the same as the
analysis for the language switching task when age and
age of L2 acquisition are taken into account as continuous
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
Figure 2. Performances on the non-linguistic switching task
for the three age groups of participants. Error bars represent
standard errors.
variables. In this case, participants’ age, but not age of
L2 acquisition (all ps > .42), accounted for a significant
amount of the variance associated with switching cost for
shape (R2 = .13, B = 1.42, p = .004), and combined
cost (R2 = .11, B = 1.13, p = .009), but not for the cost
for colour ( p = .25). Interestingly, the effect of age on
the combined non-linguistic cost was also present when
the data were modelled as logarithmic ( p = .02) and
quadratic ( p = .005), confirming the effect of age on
the non-linguistic switch cost.
Accuracy
There was no effect of age in accuracy (“group”: (F(2,57)
= 0.61, MSE = 18.93, p = .55). Participants were less
accurate in switch trials (89.9%) than repeat trials (93.2%)
(“type of trial”: F(1,57) = 56.87, MSE = 12.14, p < .0001)
and they were less accurate in sorting by shape (90.6%)
than by colour (92.5%) (F(1,57) = 10.22, MSE = 20.84, p
= .002, ηp2 = .15). Also, the interaction between “sorting
criterion” and “type of trial” was significant (F(1,57) =
32.80, MSE = 16.22, p < .0001, ηp2 = .36), revealing
that the difference in accuracy between repeat and switch
trials was only significant when sorting by colour ( p <
.0001) but not by shape ( p > .05).
IP address: 91.240.2.62
8
Marco Calabria, Francesca M. Branzi, Paula Marne, Mireia Hernández and Albert Costa
tau (τ ). The question here is whether the differences
between the groups in the magnitude of the switching
costs (both linguistic and non-linguistic) are captured by
the normal component of the RT distribution or by the
exponential one. This is important since according to some
authors the cognitive processes behind differences in these
components might be different (see Discussion).
The raw data was sorted by type of trial (switch and
repeat) and by age group (young, middle-aged and elderly)
and separately for the two tasks. The parameters of the
ex-Gaussian distribution (µ and τ ) were obtained for each
participant using the quantile maximum likelihood (QML)
estimation procedure in QMPE 2.18 (Cousineau, Brown
& Heathcote, 2004). The estimation results into a value
for each parameter (µ and τ ) and for each participant per
condition.
We then ran repeated-measures ANOVAs separately
for µ and τ , separately for each task.
Figure 3. RT distributions of repeat and switch trials broken
by age groups and task versions.
3.4 Ex-Gaussian analysis
In the present section we present the distribution analysis
we performed on the data. These analyses provide a more
detailed description of the differences in performance
between groups and task, and could potentially help
address the effects of aging over the bLC and the EC
system. A quick look at the distributions (see Figure 3)
gives an overall impression of the differences between
groups. Firstly, for the linguistic task there are differences
on overall RTs both for switch and repeat trials. However,
the general shapes of the distributions are very similar
across groups. Secondly, for the non-linguistic task, one
can appreciate also differences in RTs for switch and
repeat trials. Interestingly, however, for the switch trials
one can see that the tail of the distribution for the elderly
group is more pronounced. In other words, very long RTs
contribute considerably to the overall switch cost for this
group.
To assess whether this visual impression is statistically
meaningful, we performed a distributional analysis
fitting the data to an ex-Gaussian distribution. This
fitting decomposes the overall RT distribution into two
distributions, the normal and the exponential one. The
normal distribution is characterized by two parameters,
such as mu (µ) and sigma (σ ). µ is the mean of the fitted
normal distribution, and σ corresponds to the variance.
The exponential distribution corresponds to the tail of the
RT distribution, and it is characterized by the parameter
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
Linguistic switching task
The variables considered in the analyses were “type of
trial” (switch vs. repeat) and “response language” (L1
and L2) as within-subject factors and “group” (young,
middle-aged and elderly) as between-subject factor.
For µ, the main effect of “group” was significant
(F(1,57) = 13.56, MSE = 24855.77, p < .0001, ηp2 = .32)
and the post-hoc analysis revealed that the young group
had the smaller µ values (721 ms) than the middle-aged
group (811 ms, p = .002) and the elderly group (847 ms,
p < .0001). In other words, the older the participants are,
the slower the normal component of the RT distribution
is.
Participants were slower in the switch trials (851 ms)
than in the repeat trials (742 ms) (“type of trial”: F(1,57)
= 144.06, MSE = 5048.89, p < .0001, ηp2 = .72), but
naming latencies for µ were not modulated by language
(“response language”: F(1,57) = 0.17, MSE = 3680.44,
p = .68). Interestingly, the interaction between “type
of trial” and “response language” was not significant,
meaning that the linguistic switch cost was the same when
switching into the L1 and the L2. Moreover, the nonsignificant interactions with “group” also indicate that the
magnitude of the switch cost for µ was not modulated by
age (see Table 3).
For τ , only the main effect of “type of trial” was
significant (F(1,57) = 18.57, MSE = 4011.32, p < .0001,
ηp2 = .25), indicating that overall the participants had
smaller τ values in the switch trials (111 ms) than in the
repeat trials (146 ms).
Non-linguistic switching task
The variables considered in the analysis were “group” as a
between-subject factor, “type of trial” (switch vs. repeat)
and “sorting criterion” (colour vs. form) as within-subject
factors.
IP address: 91.240.2.62
Bilingual language control and aging
Table 3. Means and SEs of the mu and the tau values in the linguistic
(panel A) and non-linguistic (panel B) switching tasks.
(A)
L1
M U VALUES
Young
Middle-aged
Elderly
TAU VALUES
Young
Middle-aged
Elderly
L2
Mean
SE
Mean
SE
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
664
805
141
747
862
115
797
892
95
17.5
23.8
20.6
17.5
23.7
20.7
17.5
23.8
20.6
687
780
93
767
860
93
788
902
114
17.6
25.1
18.3
17.6
25.1
18.7
17.6
25.1
18.9
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
158
97
−61
130
99
−31
142
124
−18
13.9
16.8
15.2
14.0
16.7
15.5
14.0
16.8
15.4
150
122
−28
157
114
−43
139
108
−31
13.4
16.6
15.2
13.4
16.6
15.4
13.4
16.6
15.3
(B)
Colour
M U VALUES
Young
Middle-aged
Elderly
TAU VALUES
Young
Middle-aged
Elderly
http://journals.cambridge.org
Shape
Mean
SE
Mean
SE
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
565
761
196
644
784
140
781
934
153
24.9
42.1
32.9
24.8
42.0
32.8
24.9
42.2
33.1
701
736
47
796
817
21
968
996
28
29.2
36.6
32.3
29.2
36.8
32.4
29.2
36.8
32.6
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
Repeat
Switch
Switch cost
183
105
−78
234
187
−48
211
204
−7
18.8
23.4
21.0
18.9
23.4
21.1
18.9
23.6
21.3
172
159
−13
222
232
10
206
257
51
21.5
22.9
22.3
21.6
23.0
22.6
21.5
23.0
22.4
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
IP address: 91.240.2.62
9
10
Marco Calabria, Francesca M. Branzi, Paula Marne, Mireia Hernández and Albert Costa
For µ, the main effect of “group” was significant
(F(1,57) = 16.99, MSE = 67932.97, p < .0001, ηp2 =
.73), indicating that the elderly group had higher µ values
(920 ms) than the middle-aged group (761 ms, p = .001)
and the young group (691 ms, p < .0001). Overall the
participants were slower in the switch trials (838 ms) than
in the repeat trial (742 ms) (“type of trial”: F(1,57) =
58.33, MSE = 10686.94, p < .0001, ηp2 = .51), and
slower when they matched for shape (836 ms) than for
colour (745 ms) (“sorting criterion”: F(1,57) = 65.93,
MSE = 8400.26, p < .0001, ηp2 = .54). Moreover, the
interaction between “type of trial” and “sorting criterion”
was also significant (F(1,57) = 50.11, MSE = 4519.96,
p <.0001, ηp2 = .47), indicating that the non-linguistic
switch cost for µ was asymmetrical. However, the nonsignificant interactions with group indicate than for µ the
magnitude of the switch cost was not affected by age.
For τ , the main effect of “group” was significant
(F(1,57) = 5.66, MSE = 19631.93, p = .006, ηp2 =
.16) and post-hoc analysis revealed that the young group
had smaller τ values (154 ms) than the middle-aged group
(218 ms, p = .01) and the elderly group (219 ms, p = .01).
Participants were slower to match for shape (208 ms) than
for colour (187 ms) (“sorting criterion”: F(1,57) = 4.16,
MSE = 6021.14, p = .05, ηp2 = .07), whereas there was
not any effect of “type of trial” (F(1,57) = 1.73, MSE =
6966.63, p = 0.19). However, the interaction between
“type of trial” and “sorting criterion” was significant
(F(1,57) = 10.34, MSE = 5270.70, p = .002, ηp2 =
.15), suggesting that the non-linguistic switch cost was
asymmetrical. Finally, the non-linguistic switch cost was
modulated by age (interaction between “type of trial” and
“group”: F(1,57) = 3.31, MSE = 6966.63, p = .04,
ηp2 = .10). To further explore this interaction we
submitted the τ values for the switch cost to a one-way
ANOVA with group as between-subject factor. Indeed, we
found that only the young group had smaller τ values
(–45 ms) than the elderly group (22 ms, p = .01).
3.4 On the correlation between the two tasks
In the present set of analysis, we further explore the
potential relationship between age and the switching costs
in the two tasks.
The first relevant observation is that overall speed in
both switching tasks is correlated with age; namely the
older the participant is the slower he/she performs the
linguistic task (r = .46, p < .001) and the non-linguistic
task (r = .58, p < .001) (see Figure 4A). However, while
the magnitude of the non-linguistic switch cost positively
correlated with age (r = .29, p = .03) (see Figure 4B), the
magnitude of the language switch cost did not (r = .03,
p = .80).
Additionally, we used a correlation analysis to compare
the magnitude of the switch costs between the linguistic
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
Figure 4. Correlation of individuals’ performances on
overall speed (panel A) and switch costs (panel B) as a
function of age of participants.
and non-linguistic switching tasks. In fact, if we assume
that the switch cost reflects to some extent the efficiency
of the bLC and EC in the same way, we may expect
that the magnitude of the two switch costs (linguistic and
non-linguistic) varies in the same manner in participants.
To do so, we correlated the total switch cost between
the linguistic task and the non-linguistic task (collapsing
language in one case and the sorting criteria in the other
case) for each age group. We first ran the correlation
separately for each group because of the difference in
the variability of the switch cost across groups, especially
in the elderly group. The correlations between the two
switch costs were not significant in any age group (young:
r = −.12, p = .61; middle-aged: r = −.21, p = .35, elderly:
r = .22, p = .34).
In order to gain more statistical power we ran the
analysis with all the participants resulting in a total
number of 60. The switch costs of the two tasks were not
significantly correlated (r = .04, p = .75) (see Figure 5).
4 Discussion and conclusion
The main goal of the present study was to investigate
the age-related changes of bLC. To do so we compared
IP address: 91.240.2.62
Bilingual language control and aging
Figure 5. Correlation of individuals’ performances between
the linguistic and non-linguistic switching tasks.
the performances of three age groups of Catalan–Spanish
highly-proficient bilinguals in the language switching
task. The results show several interesting findings.
First, we found an age-related effect on the overall
speed of processing for elderly adults when compared to
young adults, probably suggesting a general effect of aging
on cognition. However, when we looked at the language
switching cost, we did not find any difference in the
magnitude of such a cost among the three age groups.
Indeed, the age of the participants was not correlated with
the magnitude of the language switch cost. Moreover,
the distributional analysis confirmed that the magnitude
of the language switch cost was not affected by age in
neither the exponential nor normal components. This is
an interesting result because it suggests that the language
control abilities of bilinguals are, to some extent, protected
against the cognitive decline associated with aging.
Second, the pattern of the language switch cost was
symmetrical for the three age groups. As highlighted in the
Introduction, highly-proficient bilinguals generally show
a symmetrical language switch cost, that is the same cost
when switching from L1 into L2 and vice versa (Calabria
et al., 2011; Costa & Santesteban, 2004; Costa et al.,
2006), a pattern of switch cost that is not usually found in
low-proficient bilinguals (e.g., Meuter & Allport, 1999).
This has been explained as a qualitative difference in the
recruitment of the bLC mechanisms related to proficiency.
For instance, low-proficient bilinguals could make use of
inhibitory control to get rid of the interference of L1 when
speaking in L2, that is, to prevent the interference of the
strong language over the weak language. On the other
hand, highly-proficient bilinguals behave differently and
in the same condition they show a symmetrical switch
cost. Regardless of the merits of such an explanation, our
contribution here is the observation that the bLC system
does not seem to be affected by age-related decline. This
appears to be so, from both quantitative and qualitative
points of view, namely in terms of the magnitude of the
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
11
switch cost and in terms of the symmetrical switch costs
for the two languages.
This conclusion contrasts with that reached by
Weissberger et al. (2012), where an aging effect on
the magnitude of language switch costs was observed.
Although quantitative differences were observed in this
study, it is worth noticing that the same symmetrical
pattern of switch costs was observed for young and elderly
individuals. Hence, qualitatively the same pattern was
observed regardless of aging. At present, it is difficult
to account for the differences between the two studies
in terms of the quantitative effects given the many
differences between the two studies. Further research
needs to be conducted to clarify this issue.
The second aim of the study was to explore the
nature of the cross-talk between bLC and the domaingeneral EC system by focusing on age-related changes
in linguistic and non-linguistic switching tasks. In the
non-linguistic switching task participants had to judge a
series of pictures according to two sorting criteria: their
colour, or their shape according to a cue. In this task, we
actually found an age-related change both in the speed
of processing and in the magnitude of the switch cost.
That is, the three age groups were different in overall
speed, with the elderly group the slowest and the young
group the fastest, and the magnitude of the switch cost
was larger for the elderly compared to young adults.2 We
also found that age, considered as a continuum variable,
positively correlated with the magnitude of the nonlinguistic switching cost. These results contrast sharply
with those observed in the linguistic switching task, in
which the magnitude of the language switch cost was not
affected by aging. In fact, the correlation analyses revealed
weak associations between performances in the two tasks.
Although the speed with which the tasks were performed
correlated with age, only the non-linguistic switch cost
was correlated with aging. Crucially, linguistic and nonlinguistic switch costs were uncorrelated for any of the
three groups of participants. That is, the cost of switching
languages cannot be predicted by the cost of switching
tasks.
Taken together, the results from the linguistic and nonlinguistic switching tasks suggest that aging affects, in
2
This result contrasts with that of Weissberger et al. (2012) in which
the age-related effect in non-linguistic switching task was confined to
an increase in errors in the mixed condition. In fact, in many studies of
switching task with elderly adults, the switch cost is not consistently
reported. However, the mixing cost, which is the difference in reaction
times between the repeat trials of the mixed condition and those in
non-mixed one, is the measure that is most sensitive to age effects
(for a recent review see Wasylyshyn et al., 2011). However, it is
noteworthy to say that some other studies have shown age-related
effects of switch cost, for instance in some conditions in which the
task is more demanding in terms of alternative of responses (Reimers
& Maylor, 2005).
IP address: 91.240.2.62
12
Marco Calabria, Francesca M. Branzi, Paula Marne, Mireia Hernández and Albert Costa
a relatively different manner, bLC and the EC systems.
Hence, to the extent that such a differential effect of
aging can be understood as revealing different underlying
mechanisms for the two systems, we should conclude that
the bLC cannot be reduced to a specific instance of the
EC system.
Interestingly, and despite the number of differences
between our study and that of Weissberger et al. (2012),
the authors reached similar conclusions to the ones drawn
above. Indeed, they found an instructive dissociation:
a subset of elderly bilinguals was able to perform
the language switching task but not the non-linguistic
switching task. Thus, the relative sparing of the processes
involved in the bLC in the presence of a deficient EC
system, suggests that the bLC and the EC systems are only
partially shared and that some of the bLC mechanisms
are protected against aging (for similar conclusions see
Gollan et al., 2011).
These conclusions do not necessarily conflict with
the information provided by the neuroimaging literature.
Indeed, there is a growing body of evidence revealing
that bLC and EC share some common neural substrate.
For instance, Abutalebi and Green (2007) suggested
that the same neural regions (the dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex and the caudate
nucleus) are engaged during both language switching
tasks (e.g., Abutalebi, Della Rosa, Ding, Weekes, Costa &
Green, 2013; Abutalebi, Della Rosa, Green, Hernandez,
Scifo, Keim, Cappa & Costa, 2012; Garbin et al., 2011;
Hernandez et al., 2001; for a review see also HervaisAdelman et al., 2011) and non-linguistic switching
tasks (e.g., Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter & Cohen,
2001; Botvinick, Cohen & Carter, 2004). This evidence
supports the hypothesis that the mechanisms for language
control are subsidiary to those of the domain-general
EC. However, there is also evidence going against the
claim of functional overlap between bLC and EC (e.g.,
Abutalebi, Annoni, Zimine, Pegna, Seghier, Lee-Jahnke,
Lazeyras, Cappa & Khateb, 2008). In an fMRI study
those authors demonstrated the existence of a neural
network that is specifically recruited to switch between
two different linguistic registers but not between two
intra-linguistic tasks. This suggests that some of bLC
mechanisms are specific to language and not involved
in any other switching task.
Thus, the issue here is to determine which components
are specific to bLC and which are shared with the
domain-general EC. The fitting of the data to the exGaussian distribution can help provide a tentative answer.
This analysis revealed that the normal and exponential
components of the distribution differentially captured
age related variability for the two tasks. Empirically, the
parameter estimation of these two components (µ and τ )
is usually used as a tool to better describe the distribution
of the RTs. However, some authors suggest that group
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
differences in the parameters also indicate the different
degree to which cognitive processes are recruited during
task execution (for a review see Matzke & Wagenmakers,
2009).
In this context, the results of the ex-Gaussian
distribution analysis may help us to identify which are
the shared and specific processes of the two systems.
This is a complex issue that goes beyond the scope of
this article. However, we can put forward the following
tentative account.
The EC system includes a set of mechanisms, such
as inhibitory control, monitoring, shifting, and working
memory, etc. (e.g., Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki
& Howerter, 2000) and aging affects some of these
EC processes. Actually, in the non-linguistic task we
found that the switch cost increased in the elderly group
(as compared to the young group) and, interestingly,
this relative increase was indexed by the exponential
component of the distribution for switch trials (τ ), and
not by a general shift in the normal component of the
distribution (µ). That is, the larger switch costs for elderly
people do not stem from an overall slowing down in switch
trials, but rather for a disproportionately presence of very
slow RTs in such condition (the exponential component,
τ values). So, then the question is: what cognitive process
leads to such an increment of the exponential component
in the switch trials? One possibility is that inhibitory
control deficits are behind these long RTs, therefore
reducing the ability of elderly people to switch smoothly
between different tasks. Indeed, some researchers suggest
that the exponential component captures the efficiency
of the inhibitory control system (e.g., Penner-Wilger,
Leth-Steensen & LeFevre, 2002; Schmiedek, Oberauer,
Wilhelm, Suss & Wittmann, 2007; Spieler, Balota &
Faust, 1996). Regardless of the merits of this tentative
interpretation of this distributional analysis, what is
relevant here is the contrastive distribution observed in
the language switching task. In this task, there were
no differences in the exponential component of the
distribution neither for the comparison between groups
nor for conditions. Hence, whatever the cognitive process
that is behind the age-related decline in the ability to
switch between non-linguistic tasks, it does not seem to
be involved (at least to the same degree) in the language
switch task.
In accordance with this view, some authors have
argued that in the case of high-proficient bilingualism,
the bLC would not resort in inhibitory mechanisms but
rather in a language-specific selection mechanism that
is built into the linguistic system of the speaker, and
relatively independent of the EC system. Under this
language-specific selection account, one could predict that
a reduction in the efficiency of the inhibitory mechanism
would leave relative unaffected the ability of bilingual
speakers to perform language control.
IP address: 91.240.2.62
Bilingual language control and aging
Interestingly, this view leads to the further hypothesis
that bLC should be affected by aging in low-proficient
bilinguals. Further research should be carried out to test
this hypothesis.
To conclude, our study adds new evidence to
a differential age-related change over bLC and the
domain-general EC system in highly-proficient bilinguals.
Specifically, our results show that bLC is not totally
affected by age despite the fact the EC system was
impaired in elderly bilinguals. Moreover, the increase in
the switch cost during the EC task was not correlated to
switch costs during bLC task. Taken together, this suggests
that the underlying mechanisms of bLC and EC systems
are not totally shared. Further research is needed to explore
in more detail which mechanisms are more affected by
age within the EC system by, for instance, using tasks
that involve the different processes of domain-general EC
system.
References
Abutalebi, J., Annoni, J. M., Zimine, I., Pegna, A. J., Seghier,
M. L., Lee-Jahnke, H., Lazeyras, F., Cappa, S. F., & Khateb,
A. (2008). Language control and lexical competition in
bilinguals: An event-related FMRI study. Cerebral Cortex,
18, 1496–1505.
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Ding, G., Weekes, B., Costa, A.,
& Green, D. W. (2013). Language proficiency modulates
the engagement of cognitive control areas in multilinguals.
Cortex, 49, 905–1011.
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M.,
Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S. F., & Costa, A. (2012).
Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict
monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 2076–2086.
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2007). Bilingual language
production: The neurocognition of language representation
and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 242–275.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control
and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 34, 859–873.
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S.,
& Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive
control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict
monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.
Calabria, M., Hernandez, M., Branzi, F. M., & Costa, A. (2011).
Qualitative differences between bilingual language control
and executive control: Evidence from task-switching.
Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 399.
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual
speech production: Evidence from language switching in
highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of
Memory and Language, 50, 491–511.
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I. (2006). How do highly
proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process?
Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
13
are both functional. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1057–1074.
Cousineau, D., Brown, S., & Heathcote, A. (2004). Fitting
distributions using maximum likelihood: Methods and
packages. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, &
Computers, 36, 742–756.
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display
program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research
Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116–124.
Garbin, G., Costa, A., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Rodriguez-Pujadas,
A., Ventura, N., Belloch, V., Hernandez, M., & Ávila, C.
(2011). Neural bases of language switching in high and
early proficient bilinguals. Brain and Language, 119, 129–
135.
Gollan, T. H., & Ferreira, V. S. (2009). Should I stay or
should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary
language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 35, 640–665.
Gollan, T. H., Sandoval, T., & Salmon, D. P. (2011). Crosslanguage intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the
link between executive control and language selection.
Psychological Science, 22, 1155–1164.
Green, D. W. (1986). Control, activation, and resource: A
framework and a model for the control of speech in
bilinguals. Brain and Language, 27, 210–223.
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexicosemantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
1, 67–81.
Greenwood, P. M. (2000). The frontal aging hypothesis
evaluated. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Society, 6, 705–726.
Hernandez, A. E., Dapretto, M., Mazziotta, J., & Bookheimer, S.
(2001). Language switching and language representation in
Spanish–English bilinguals: An fMRI study. NeuroImage,
14, 510–520.
Hernandez, A. E., & Khonert, K. (1999). Aging and language
switching in bilinguals. Aging, Neuropsychology and
Cognition, 6, 69–83.
Hervais-Adelman, A. G., Moser-Mercer, B., & Golestani, N.
(2011). Executive control of language in the bilingual
brain: Integrating the evidence from neuroimaging to
neuropsychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 234.
Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. M. (2010). The
role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 17, 1–14.
Magezi, D. A., Khateb, A., Mouthon, M., Spierer, L., & Annoni,
J. M. (2012). Cognitive control of language production in
bilinguals involves a partly independent process within
the domain-general cognitive control network: Evidence
from task-switching and electrical brain activity. Brain and
Language, 122, 55–63.
Martin, C. D., Barcelo, F., Hernandez, M., & Costa, A. (2011).
The time course of the asymmetrical “local” switch
cost: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biological
Psychology, 86, 210–218.
Matzke, D., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2009). Psychological
interpretation of the ex-Gaussian and shifted Wald
parameters: A diffusion model analysis. Psychonomic
Bulletin Review, 16, 798–817.
IP address: 91.240.2.62
14
Marco Calabria, Francesca M. Branzi, Paula Marne, Mireia Hernández and Albert Costa
Meuter, R. F. I., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language
switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language
selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25–40.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., &
Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive
functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal lobe”
tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41,
49–100.
Penner-Wilger, M., Leth-Steensen, C., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002).
Decomposing the problem-size effect: A comparison of
response time distributions across cultures. Memory and
Cognition, 30, 1160–1167.
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are
good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and
Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682–691.
Protopapas, A. (2007). CheckVocal: A program to facilitate
checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses
from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 859–
862.
Reimers, S., & Maylor, E. A. (2005). Task switching across
the life span: Effects of age on general and specific switch
costs. Developmental Psychology, 41, 661–671.
Rhodes, M. G. (2004). Age-related differences in performance
on the Wisconsin card sorting test: A meta-analytic review.
Psychology and Aging, 19, 482–494.
Roelofs, A., & Piai, V. (2011). Attention demands of spoken
word planning: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 307.
Schmiedek, F., Oberauer, K., Wilhelm, O., Suss, H. M.,
& Wittmann, W. W. (2007). Individual differences in
components of reaction time distributions and their
relations to working memory and intelligence. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 414–429.
Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized
set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image
http://journals.cambridge.org
Downloaded: 09 Sep 2013
agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory,
6, 174–215.
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2011). Is there
a relationship between language switching and executive
functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group
analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 183.
Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., & Faust, M. E. (1996). Stroop
performance in healthy younger and older adults and
in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Perception
and Performance, 22, 461–479.
Strijkers, K., Holcomb, P., & Costa, A. (2011). Conscious
intention to speak proactively facilitates lexical access
during overt object naming. Journal of Memory and
Language, 65, 345–362.
Tisserand, D. J., & Jolles, J. (2003). On the involvement of
prefrontal networks in cognitive ageing. Cortex, 39, 1107–
1128.
Verhaeghen, P., & Cerella, J. (2002). Aging, executive control,
and attention: A review of meta-analyses. Neuroscience
and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 849–857.
Verhaeghen, P., Steitz, D. W., Sliwinski, M. J., & Cerella, J.
(2003). Aging and dual-task performance: A meta-analysis.
Psychology and Aging, 18, 443–460.
Wasylyshyn, C., Verhaeghen, P., & Sliwinski, M. J. (2011).
Aging and task switching: A meta-analysis. Psychology
and Aging, 26, 15–20.
Weissberger, G. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bondi, M. W., & Gollan, T.
H. (2012). Partially overlapping mechanisms of language
and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology
and Aging, 27, 959–974.
Ye, Z., & Zhou, X. (2009). Executive control in language
processing. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33,
1168–1177.
IP address: 91.240.2.62