Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
1999
…
4 pages
1 file
The phenomenon of clitic replication in Bulgarian involves two linguistic entities: a full-fledged nominal constituent in a particular syntactic function and a co-referent weak pronominal form corresponding to the particular syntactic function. In this paper we present experimental results which support our hypothesis that the nominal material that is replicated by a pronominal clitic in Bulgarian utterances consistently exhibits thematic intonational properties.
Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 2009
Clitic clusters display a complicated interaction of prosodic and syntactic properties which determines their word order and stress patterns. In Bulgarian, short pronouns appear as unstressed verbal enclitics in positive utterances. Proclitic negation attracts the pronouns and forms with them a prosodic unit stressed on the second syllable, the pronoun. Theoretical linguistics characterizes the behaviour of object clitics in terms of “non-trivial chains” (Bošković 2001) containing copies. The overt realisation of a higher or lower copy depends on phonological constraints like enclitisation requirements. In line with the slow-syntax-hypothesis (Burkhardt et al. 2008) and with the assumption that prosody-related processes may also compete for the same limited processing resources of Broca’s aphasics (Avrutin et al. 1999), we test sensitivity to the phonosyntactic constraints negation imposes on the word order of personal and reflexive clitics. Results suggest that the pattern of agrammatic processing of clitic clusters resembles normal comprehension but proceeds in a protracted manner. Employing a self-paced reading task and an experimental design which reduces discourse-related interpretation processes, we also show that the syntactic functions of personal object clitics as syntactic object agreement markers in Bulgarian are relatively preserved in the aphasic group.
1997
The present study of Bulgarian nominal morphosyntax focuses on the placement of the short possessive pronouns and the interrogative particle within the nominal syntactic domain, in correlation with the distribution of the definite article. In other words, I am primarily interested in what might be called "proper" nominal clitics, while predicative clitics that are introduced in the NP by attributively used participles or accompany deverbal nouns remain beyond the scope of this article. After presenting some preliminary assumptions, I will show that a treatment of Bulgarian definite article based on H ALPERN 1995 is quite promising, especially if augmented to adequately cover the relevant data. Setting thus the appropriate context, I will concentrate on the distribution of Bulgarian possessive enclitics which supplies a strong evidence in favour of admitting the existence of a nominal enclitic cluster. I will show that their placement obeys the same mechanism which governs ...
Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today, 2008
This paper discusses clitic reduplication constructions in Bulgarian. In contrast to traditional analyses, it distinguishes clitic doubling proper, which is restricted to clauses with psych and physical perception predicates, from other constructions that involve reduplication of an argument by a clitic, notably, left and right dislocation, focus movement, and hanging topic construction. Several properties of clitic doubling proper are identified, among which obligatory doubling of quantifiers, wh-phrases and focus phrases. These are argued to be the distinguishing features of this construction in Bulgarian, given the cross-linguistic evidence from Romance and other languages.
in press
Istro-Romanian is a ‘historical dialect’ of Romanian, a severely endangered linguistic variety, spoken in the Istrian peninsula (Croatia) as an endogenous language, and in USA and Canada as an exogenous language. Using the data extracted from the available corpora, the paper offers a descriptive account of the main features of pronominal clitics in Istro-Romanian, focusing on empirical phenomena such as interpolation, verb(-auxiliary)-clitic inversion, (absence of) clitic climbing, and the position of clitics with respect to other elements of the verbal cluster. Some parallels with Croatian are also drawn, and the importance of old Romanian/old Romance inheritance is also briefly assessed. Future research will concentrate on more closely determining what plays a more important role in the syntax of Istro-Romanian: preservation of archaic Romanian/Romance features or language contact?
2009
I investigate the phonology of prosodic clitics-independent syntactic words not parsed as independent prosodic words-in Bosnian, Serbian, and Croatian. I ask, first, how clitics are organized into prosodic structures, and second, how this is determined by the grammar. Following Zec (1997, 2005), I look at several clitic categories, including negation, prepositions, complementizers, conjunctions, and second-position clitics. Based on a reanalysis of word accent (Browne and McCawley 1965, Inkelas and Zec 1988, Zec 1999), I argue that in some cases where a preposition, complementizer, or conjunction fails to realize accent determined by a following word, it is not a procliticthat is, prosodified with the following word-but rather a free clitic parsed directly by a phonological phrase. Conversely, the second-position clitics are not always enclitic-that is, prosodified with a preceding word-but are sometimes free. Their second-position word order results not from enclisis, but from the avoidance of free clitics at phrase edges, where they would interfere with the alignment of phonological phrases to prosodic words. vii Regarding the determination of clisis by the grammar, I argue for an interface constraint approach (Selkirk 1995, Truckenbrodt 1995), whereby prosodic structures are built according to general constraints on their well-formedness, and on their interface to syntactic structures. I contrast this with the subcategorization approach, which sees clisis as specified for each clitic (Klavans 1982, Radanović-Kocić 1988, Zec and Inkelas 1990). The comparison across clitic categories provides key support for the interface constraint approach, showing that their prosody depends on their syntactic configurations and phonological shapes, rather than on arbitrary subcategorizations. Prosodic differences across categories are a derivative effect of their configuration in the clause, and of the division of the clause into phonological phrases. The relevance of phonological phrases consists in how their edges discourage some kinds of clisis, blocking, for example, proclisis of complementizers and conjunctions to their complements. Free clisis is disfavored at phrase edges, producing the second-position effect. Thus, the interface constraint approach leads to a unified account of word, phrase, and clitic prosody.
Second position clitics in Serbian can be placed after the first word (1W) or after the first phrase (1P). The factors governing the acceptability of 1W or 1P placement are a matter of some debate. Our claim is that 1W/1P depends on discourse, structural, and prosodic factors. We argue further that in free-constituent order languages like Serbian a distinction has to be made between sentences that are argument-initial and those which are predicate-initial. Previous work has focused on 1W/1P placement in branching phrases. However, the circumstances of clitic placement in non-branching phrases are important for clarifying the discourse factors that govern the 1W/1P distinction. Our procedure of data collection involves two major sources: corpora, and experimental techniques designed to investigate the roles of both context and intonation in clitic placement. A series of converging studies confirms not only the distinction between argument-initial and predicate-initial sentences, but also the role that information structure plays in clitic placement. The picture that emerges is that the neutral cases of clitic placement arise from different sources. The neutral positioning is largely syntactic in the argument case, while primarily prosodic in the predicate instances. In the marked cases of clitic placement for both sentence types, the clitic functions as a morphological marker of Contrast – either Contrastive Focus or Contrastive Topic. A corollary of this result is that the non-branching cases will always show unmarked intonational and/or discourse pattern, since the marked pattern only arises when there is a contrast to mark – in a branching phrase.
This paper discusses clitic reduplication constructions in Bulgarian. In contrast to traditional analyses, it distinguishes clitic doubling proper, which is restricted to clauses with psych and physical perception predicates, from other constructions that involve reduplication of an argument by a clitic, notably, left and right dislocation, focus movement, and hanging topic construction. Several properties of clitic doubling proper are identified, among which obligatory doubling of quantifiers, wh-phrases and focus phrases. These are argued to be the distinguishing features of this construction in Bulgarian, given the cross-linguistic evidence from Romance and other languages.
Vowel devoicing at phrase boundaries is shown to depend on sentence mode (statement, question) and information structure (non-contrastive vs. contrastive and broad vs. narrow focus), which affect the type of nuclear accent and boundary tone. In li-questions, considerably less devoicing of the vowels before the phrase boundary is found than in other conditions, because the rising boundary contour prevents devoicing. In all other conditions, mode and information structure only affect the realisation of the vowel in the two syllables before the phrase boundary if the nuclear accent is realised on the last content word of the phrase, just before the boundary. In these position, the differences in observed vowel devoicing can also be related to the intonational properties of the utterance, namely to the accent type. In particular, the peak alignment of the accent explains the presence or absence of voicing, or better the amount of devoicing, since it is clearly a gradient phenomenon. In general, the earlier the peak, the greater the tendency towards devoicing.
2003
Vowel devoicing at phrase boundaries is shown to depend on sentence mode (statement, question) and information structure (non-contrastive vs. contrastive and broad vs. narrow focus), which affect the type of nuclear accent and boundary tone. In li-questions, considerably less devoicing of the vowels before the phrase boundary is found than in other conditions, because the rising boundary contour prevents devoicing. In all other conditions, mode and information structure only affect the realisation of the vowel in the two syllables before the phrase boundary if the nuclear accent is realised on the last content word of the phrase, just before the boundary. In these position, the differences in observed vowel devoicing can also be related to the intonational properties of the utterance, namely to the accent type. In particular, the peak alignment of the accent explains the presence or absence of voicing, or better the amount of devoicing, since it is clearly a gradient phenomenon. In general, the earlier the peak, the greater the tendency towards devoicing.
Levant, 2024
L’Africa antica dall’età repubblicana ai giulio-claudii. L’Africa Romana, XXII, 2024
Leelanau County, Michigan Stolen Parcels, 2024
Revista del Archivo Fotográfico de Córdoba, 2017
SIGNOS-Investigación en sistemas de gestión, 2019
International journal of data and network science, 2024
Pontifical Council for the Laity Vatican City, Saturday 14 February 2004.
American Ethnologist, 2024
Engineering, Technology & Applied Science Research, 2021
Critical Care Medicine, 2018
International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching, 2021
ACM SIGPLAN Notices, 1989
Kathmandu University medical journal (KUMJ)
El Periplo Sustentable, 2018
Cellulose Chemistry and Technology, 2023