Situating the Zone of Proximal Development
George E. Marsh II, Ed.D.
Director, Institute for Interactive Technology
College of Human & Environmental Sciences
The University of Alabama
gemarsh@ches.ua.edu
John J. Ketterer, Ed.D.
Director, International House Program
Jacksonville State University
jkettere@jsucc.jsu.edu
Abstract
Social constructivist theory has advanced the notion that distance education is inferior, because
effective learning is thought to require immersion in a cognitive apprenticeship under the
guidance of a mentor. Effective learning is said to be situated in activity, context, and culture as a
collaboration in a community of practice. Administrators and practitioners in distance education
are confronted with a challenge to the efficacy of their endeavors. The authors briefly trace the
evolution of social constructivism, the influence of Piaget and Vygotsky, and analyze the effects
of contemporary social constructivism with implications for instructional theory and practice.
Introduction
Administrators of distance education enterprises are frequently moved to defend online
instruction because of criticism and uncertainty about the nature of distance instruction. They are
challenged to counter deeply embedded and frequently unquestioned beliefs that distance
teaching cannot adequately provide a "virtual presence" sufficient to overcome the ontological
fact of distance in terms of space, place, and intimacy. In spite of a substantial body of research
demonstrating that there are no differences in achievement between online and traditional
instruction, the belief persists that a distance education course is inferior to the "real thing" on
campus. The ubiquity of social constructivism among practitioners and theorists, as posture or
pedagogy, has contributed to a fragmentation of approaches and misinterpretation of distance
technology as an instructional tool. Since spatial proximity is a prerequisite of social
constructivism, the theories of cognitive apprenticeship and situated cognition condemn distance
education for lacking the essential components of tangible mentors, peers, and contiguous social
environments. A closer examination of the provenance of these theories may provide both
instructors and distance education administrators with helpful principles for the effective
integration of theory and defensible practice.
Distance education is held to different standards and higher expectations than conventional
teaching. Distance learning is regarded as deficient because it is said to lack social experience
and personal interactions apart from a community and the presence of a mentor. Social
constructivism makes strong claims for deep and active learning. In social constructivism,
students are said to acquire knowledge by participation in authentic tasks situated in an
environment that is permeated with "distributed" knowledge (Gibson, 1979; Lave, 1988; Lave &
Wenger, 1991). Brown and Duguid (2000) regard distance education as a poor substitute for
classroom instruction, apparently because it lacks spatial and social proximity, although,
ironically, they also contend that communities need not be spatially close. In a community a
mentor or master teacher directs students by use of such techniques as modeling, scaffolding,
1 of 11
coaching, and fading. Despite instruction centered on a teacher and communal behaviors, the
learning environment under social constructivism is still considered to be student-centered. The
logic is explained partly by the two elements of evolved social constructivism, situated cognition
and cognitive apprenticeship.
In situated cognition, mental construction is the result of authentic tasks, social interaction, and
collaboration in context. Knowledge is distributed among persons and objects in a particular
social context. Lave and Wenger (1991) argue that learning occurs through participation in action
contexts. Situated cognition has been elaborated as cognitive apprenticeship (Collins, Brown, &
Hollum, 1991; Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989). Cognitive apprenticeship, like apprenticeship
in the trades, is a way to induct a neophyte into a work culture through a sequence of experiences
that culminate in competence.
The rich abundance of metaphors and terminology generated by social constructivists—situated
learning, collaborative learning, contextual learning, apprenticeship, community,
scaffolding—leads to questions about how accurately the language of social constructivism
reflects the epistemological and psychological bases found in the works of Piaget and, in
particular, those of Vygotsky.
Since an emphasis on the importance of culture and context is a valid epistemological concern
that may inform and influence practice, the question of the impact of the cluster of metaphors on
professional practice must be entertained. The metaphorical coherence of a pedagogical
framework or model is not necessarily an empirical validation. Cognitive apprenticeship, situated
learning, and Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development (ZPD) are collapsed into a simple
bromide such that instruction is a classroom technique to guide a student to complete a task. One
is tempted to distinguish between an instructional scheme and an instructional model.
Instructional schemes are staged practices based on loose or nonexistent relationships to
established epistemological and psychological principles. The elements of an instructional model
may yield metaphorical terminology, but do they have an integral and accurate relationship to
known principles? A review of social-constructivist models and the basic tenets of Vygotskian
and Piagetian theories should lead to some preliminary conclusions in this respect.
The Influence and Interplay of Vygotsky and Piaget on Sociocultural Theories
The origins of constructivism have been attributed to many (e.g., Jean Piaget, Lev Semyonovich
Vygotsky, John Dewey, and Giambattista Vico), but the contemporary growth of constructivism
in the classroom owes much to the work of Jerome Bruner (1966) who advocated discovery
learning as an alternative to conventional instruction. Bruner's notion was straightforward—that
students would better learn and retain concepts they discover on their own instead of passively
through rote learning and lectures. Bruner rooted his pedagogy in Piagetian and Vygotskian
principles and extended the work of Vygotsky by employing the concept of scaffolding. With the
passage of time, social constructivist theory now reinforces teacher-centeredness. To understand
how the switch from student-centered to teacher-centered instruction seems to have come full
circle, it is necessary to consider the procession of social constructivism.
Language and psychological processes develop initially as social, interpersonal interactions
among people, which Vygotsky referred to as interpsychological. These are gradually "turned
inward" or incorporated as internal tools, which he referred to as intrapsychological. The infant
and the child learn first learn by imitating the immediate social environment, which is influenced
in different ways by the larger culture. One's psychological processes are always bound to the
culture in some way because each person must use a particular "set" of cultural tools into the
2 of 11
society in which one is born or cast. In the second instance, the child begins to think for himself.
Vygotsky (1978) revealed his basic theory about human cognitive development with this
statement:
...the most significant moment in the course of intellectual development, which gives
birth to the purely human forms of practical and abstract intelligence, occurs when
speech and practical activity, two previously completely independent lines of
development, converge (p. 24).
One significance of this "convergence" is that internal speech is ultimately used for thinking,
reasoning, solving problems, and learning. Vygotsky (1978) said, “The internalization of socially
rooted and historically developed activities is the distinguishing feature of human psychology,
the basis of the qualitative leap from animal to human psychology” (p. 57).
Social constructivists emphasize the importance of the ZPD, which seems to have been elevated
to the status of a quintessential core practice. Gredler and Shields (2004) declare that the
common understanding of the ZPD as an educational model is wrong, because Vygotsky defined
it as an area of maturing psychological processes and a construct for understanding social
influence in ontogenesis. Vygotsky (1978) put it this way:
The zone of proximal development defines those functions that have not yet
matured but are in the process of maturation, functions that will mature
tomorrow but are currently in an embryonic state. These functions could be
termed the "buds" or "flowers" of development rather than the "fruits" of
development. The actual developmental level characterizes mental
development retrospectively, while the zone of proximal development
characterizes mental development prospectively (p. 87).
Vygotsky also said the ZPD is the “distance between the actual developmental level as
determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential development as
determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable
peers" (p. 86). Vygotsky elaborated by describing how a teacher or a more advanced peer might
provide an explanation to enable a child to attain a higher level of achievement with support. The
importance in developmental theory is not the actions of a capable adult or peer but, rather, the
learner's state of cognitive maturation. External stimuli are consequential to a learner "only to the
degree that he can assimilate them by means of his existing structures" (Piaget, 1969, p. 5). This
is in concert with Vygotsky's contention that cognitive development can be assessed within the
ZPD. To Vygotsky, the evaluation of cognitive maturation in the ZPD is the major purpose, but
this has been disregarded in favor of its relevance to pedagogy.
Cognitive apprenticeship, situated cognition, and the ZPD reflect the power hierarchy by
affirming the teacher's role while apparently attempting to also endorse the primary constructivist
principle of self-created knowledge. Cheyne and Tarulli (1999) made this observation:
The social relationship that stands as a model for Vygotsky is markedly
asymmetrical, hierarchical, and organized around the developmental goal of
instrumental control. Moreover, the master-slave, supervisor-subordinate
relationship is itself a social transformation of the worker-tool relation and
hence it is easy for Vygotsky to recover, through reciprocal transformation, the
tool-like instrumentality of inner speech from such social relationships. Social
relationships are based on the same labour-production instrumentality as
3 of 11
action-tool relations. All of this is organized around the issue of control
which, through ontogenesis, becomes transformed from that of an external
agent over a subordinate to one of an internal agent over self and ultimately to
a principle over an instance. . .Vygotsky's metaphor also highlights another
significant ingredient of such dialogue—power. Vygotsky, at least in his view
of the ZPD, clearly portrays a knowledge differential as power differential.
That differential brings into focus the role of the other in the ZPD (para 27).
Distributed knowledge emanates from an authoritarian hierarchy. Knowledge production is
entrusted to the school by the culture with explicit and implicit rules that define the practices and
the agencies of governance. Administrators, teachers, and professors are certified or licensed by
the state or a professional organization, as are the programs that train them; the curriculum is
prescribed, and policies are established to define the extent of control over employees and
students and what students may learn. Professional and/or governmental organizations evaluate
both the work of schools (accreditation) and the knowledge production of those who contribute
to the methodology and pedagogy deemed acceptable in schools (peer review). Education is
governed amid a set of countervailing forces where no individual valence prevails: federal and
state laws, policies, courts, cultural narratives, standards, legal interpretations, professional
agencies, organizations, elected officials, appointed administrators, teachers, parents, students,
and more. The educational institution exists as a compromise among sources of power.
The ZPD, scaffolding, and the apprenticeship are embedded in the power structure as tools of
control, both as a way to initiate neophytes into a community of practice and to define and
deliver the curriculum. While there is a clear similarity between Piaget’s theories of
accommodation and assimilation and Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive maturation as evaluated by
the ZPD, the ZPD seems to be more appealing to the practitioner or theorist as an instructional
device.
The progression of theoretical perspectives coincides with the shifts in pedagogical philosophy
over time. Teachers who found themselves watching students individually engaged with
programmed learning materials repudiated behaviorism. However, banishment of behaviorism by
constructivism, owing to the epistemological view that learning is self-created, (the so-called
psychological constructivism of Piaget), relegated the teacher to the fringes as a guide. Social
constructivism restores the teacher firmly at the head of the instructional process, reinforced by
the social metaphors of situated cognition, cognitive apprenticeship, and the ZPD. The teacher
determines the instructional use of the ZPD by judging “right” and "wrong" answers, and
"correct" and "incorrect" procedures. The ZPD is a deliberate strategy to engage the student,
connect prior understanding with new content or skills, and assist the student to achieve
predetermined outcomes. While the ZPD is popularly regarded as "scaffolding" or "support" to
allow learners to achieve success, it involves a deliberate restriction of a student's choices
(Wood, Bruner, Ross, 1976).
Some constructivist practitioners are hesitant to state goals or objectives for teaching out of
respect for the cardinal principle of constructivism—that knowledge cannot be imposed.
However, their instructional practices reveal intended instructional outcomes, otherwise how
could the ZPD be employed? When the mentor is engaged either in modeling a sequence of
manual skills or mental concepts (making thinking visible), the mentor evidently has goals and
objectives in mind. Feedback along the way (i.e., scaffolding, coaching, fading) is for the purpose
of imposing a specific process and reaching a specific outcome. The language chosen by social
constructivists conforms to constructivist principles but truly emphasizes the instructor's role. In
doing so, practitioners of social constructivism may ignore a major element of the principles of
4 of 11
their epistemological history, or at least the writings of Bruner. The hope expressed in nascent
constructivism of the 1960s was that teaching would surrender didactic practice in the interest of
active, engaged learning.
Locating the ZPD
If conceptual knowledge is a tool, it can be used by the learner apart from the mentor and the
classroom environment. An observation by Vygotsky (1978) has particular importance for work
inside the ZPD:
The central characteristic of elementary functions is that they are totally and directly
determined by stimulation from the environment. For higher functions, the central
feature is self generated stimulation, that is, the creation and use of artificial stimuli
which become the immediate causes of behavior (p. 39).
It is at this point that one must observe that both Vygotsky and Piaget recognized a
developmental juncture at which the individual character of the learner is asserted and the
dialogue central to the learning process becomes discreet rather than only communal. Knowledge
begins in context methodologically and phenomenologically; it survives as truth when individual
mental processes and subjective intentionality sustain it. Like Piaget, Vygotsky believed that
conceptual thinking develops at adolescence when there are logical relationships among
concepts. At this stage, knowledge and thinking are relevant psychological tools—only if they
affect the mind and behaviors of the individual. Unless it can influence thought and behavior, a
psychological tool is not useful for any particular individual. While many tools exist in the
culture, not everyone can use them without proper experiences and mental development.
Psychological tools enable independent mental functions free of the immediate environment for
those who can use them.
A student learns through socialization and individual effort, but the uses of books, e-mail, online
courses, videos, or other artifacts are imbued with social content and context. These tools are
situated, if one accepts the view of distributed knowledge. A book, video, and a computer
environment can be used for meaning-making without continuous, direct, personal contact with a
teacher. Indeed, the Oxford tutorial may be said to be work in the ZPD, where students must
assume an active role in learning, develop self-directed study skills, work independently, all of
which are individual pursuits punctuated by intermittent evaluations by a tutor (Palfreyman,
2002). Vygotsky demonstrated that what is learned in the classroom can be applied elsewhere in
the absence of a teacher. Vygotsky (1987) described the ZPD or collaboration as "invisibly
present" (p. 216), which may be the case with children and adults. Presumably, such
collaboration can also be made over the Internet and used as a referent for further work, invisible
or not. The concept of the ZPD is much different when applied to a mature learner rather than to
a child.
Wilson (2002) argues that human cognition can occur "decoupled" from immediate interaction
with the environment and that large portions of cognitive processing are not situated. "In short,
our ability to form mental representations about things that are remote in time and space, which
is arguably the sine qua non of human thought, in principle cannot yield to a situated cognition
analysis" (Wilson, 2002, p. 625). Brown, Ash, Rutherford, Nakagawa, Gordon, & Campione
(1993) claim that the ZPD may include people and such artifacts as books, videos, wall displays,
scientific equipment, and "a computer environment intended to support intentional learning"
(1993, p. 191).
5 of 11
Social Distance and the ZPD
Saba (2004) rejects the comparison of conventional instruction with distance education, saying
"...comparison of interaction in the context of classroom instruction with the kinds that occur in
distance education becomes difficult, if not irrelevant" (para 2). But deeply entrenched metaphors
are not so easily dismissed. Comparisons of different distance education models with
conventional instruction find few significant differences in achievement (Haythornthwaite,
Kaymer, Robins, & Shoemaker, 2000; Russel, 1999). Regardless of the differences in spatial
proximity, a way to compare programs is to put both "places" on common ground by means of
transactional distance (Moore, 1972; Moore, 1980; Moore & Kearsley, 1996). Transactional
distance is a relative pedagogical construct that envisions the transactions between a teacher and
a learner to operate among three variables: structure, dialog, and learner autonomy. Structure is
the extent of the responsiveness to a learner's individual needs, dialog is the extent to which a
learner and educator are able to communicate with each other, and learner autonomy is a
student's commitment to self-direction and study. Moore proposed that these factors characterize
both distance education and the classroom, and some writers claim to have confirmed the theory
(Chen, 2001; Bischoff, Bisconer, Kooker, & Woods, 1996; Saba & Shearer, 1994).
Programs differ in transactional distance according to the extent of dialogue and structure within
them, and the characteristics of each learner. A distance or online model can have high or low
structure, dialog, and learner autonomy. The dialog between the instructor and the student can be
achieved with technologies now available to all instructors, but teachers and students must learn
how to use them in the ZPD. Multimedia programs, books, and videotaped lectures, a "worked
example," discussion board, or other artifacts can provide the necessary social stimuli for
learning.
When a metaphor becomes specific its applicability narrows, restricting the range of what is
possible. This may be the case with the metaphors of apprenticeship and situated cognition
whose implications are that learning can only occur in a classroom (i.e., spatial proximity). Also,
equating the ZPD with the apprenticeship is false. Seeing instruction only as an apprenticeship
leads to solipsistic excesses. If social constructivism is true to its foundations, then internal
speech and logic are valid cultural tools, and learning can occur with electronic representations of
the instructor as well as with instructors in the flesh. Vygotsky (1930) said:
As an individual only exists as a social being, as a member of some social group
within whose context he follows the road of his historical development, the
composition of his personality and the structure of his behaviour turn out to be a
quantity which is dependent on social evolution and whose main aspects are
determined by the latter. Already in primitive societies, which are only just taking
their first steps along the road of their historical development, the entire
psychological makeup of individuals can be seen to depend directly on the
development of technology, the degree of development of the production forces and
on the structure of that social group to which the individual belongs (para 4).
The relative intimacy of one instructional environment as opposed to another is questionable as
an epistemological trait. This inference is probably related to a misunderstanding of the nature of
knowledge for Piaget, Vygotsky and Bruner—none of whom can be accused of having asserted
that the validity of learning depends on some kind of continuous, intimate social process of
validation. Vygotsky and Piaget agreed that the learning process culminates with the autonomy of
the learner. Independent knowledge is neither place-specific nor situated. If Piaget and Vygotsky
were correct, abstract knowledge is transformed knowledge, neither bounded by nor restricted to
6 of 11
situations in which it was originally constructed. As Vygotsky put it, in contrast to situated
cognition, "becoming a master of one's own behavior" rather than a "slave to the environment"
(Vygotsky, 1978).
Conclusions
Directed thought, which is internal and occurs without communication with another, is social
thought that is increasingly influenced by experience and logic and no longer tied to an
immediate social context. Higher mental processes engage the "social" interactions of inner
speech to create new knowledge. Vygotsky proposed that human beings achieve control over
natural mental functions by bringing socioculturally formed mediating artifacts into thinking
activity (Lantolf & Thorne, 2005). Cultural artifacts mediate human activity and they are always
situated within a human activity system (Petrina, 2004).
Vygotsky's references to artifacts included many kinds of tools (Vygotsky, 1978). Bannon and
Bødker (1991) emphasize the process and the context in which a tool is used is important
because cognition is "socially and historically situated, and it is tied to the physical conditions in
which it takes place" (p.227). Tools are discarded if they are no longer useful. Obviously, many
young people today do not recognize or know the purpose of a “church key” or the horse-hoof
awl on a Swiss army knife, because these tools are obsolete. On the other hand, many children
and youth can use computers and software as tools, something some of their teachers cannot do.
Learners can only employ the new media artifacts inside the ZPD if they have the ability to do so,
and teachers cannot take advantage of them unless they also know how to use them.
It is indefensible to maintain that existing and emerging electronic instructional tools in distance
education lie outside the ZPD. As John-Steiner and Souberman (1978), the editors and translators
of Mind in Society, explained, “Vygotsky argued that the effect of tool use upon humans is
fundamental not only because it has helped them relate more effectively to their external
environment but also because tool use has had important effects upon internal and functional
relationships with the human brain” (p 132-133). The emphasis that social constructivism puts on
historical development implies that online learning and self-study are possible because a learner
has inherited evolved cultural tools deliberately devised for intentional learning. Social
constructivists who would restrict learning only to a classroom appear to assume that the "tools"
of learning, in the Vygotskian sense, are constants not subject to independent use or cultural
evolution. Vygotsky maintained that the psychological processes change with sociocultural
history, evolving with the culture, which is the case with distance education. Seen in this light,
some social constructivists may be guilty of the perennialist error of absolutism.
Technological adaptations address the perceived need for socialization and, properly used, may
be effective artifacts for learning. These include electronic listservs (forums, threaded
discussions); e-mail, instant messaging, chat; video; radio and audio; anchored instruction
(CTGV, 1990, 1993); computer-supported collaborative learning (Bowers & Benford, 1991;
Heilesen, Cudrio, & Cheesman, 2002; Etheris & Tan, 2004); computer supported intentional
learning environment CSILE (Scardamalia, Bereiter, McLean, Swallow, & Woodruff, 1989;
Salovaara & Järvelä, 2003); learning communities (Barab & Duffy 2000); cognitive flexibility
hypertext (Spiro, Feltovich, Jacobson & Coulson, 1992; Godshalk, Harvey, & Moller, 2004);
goal-based scenarios (Schank, 1996; Naidu, Ip, & Linser, 2000); and, case-based learning
(Kolodner, 1993; Tsinakos & Margaritis, 2001). Administrators and practitioners in distance
education continue to create innovations that may also augment the traditional classroom and the
apprenticeship. Mature learners can recognize the ZPD for themselves as an intrapsychological
process. There is no reason to believe that people cannot learn through distance education or
7 of 11
through any other, as yet undiscovered, socially-constructed artifact for that purpose, for it is
obvious that this is exactly what learners do. The ZPD is situated with the mature learner.
References
Bannon. L.J. & Bødker, S. (1991). Beyond the interface: Encountering artifacts in use. In J. Carroll (ed.)
Designing Interaction: Psychology at the human-computer interface. New York: Cambridge U.P, pp.
227-253.
Barab, S. A., & Duffy, T. (2000). >From practice fields to communities of practice. In D. Jonassen, & S.
M. Land. (Eds.). Theoretical Foundations of Learning Environments (pp. 25-56). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bischoff, W. R., Bisconer, S.W., Kooker, B. M., and Woods, L.C. (1996). Transactional distance and
interactive television in the distance education of health professionals. The American Journal of Distance
Education (10)3, 4-19.
Bowers, J. M., & Benford, S. D. (Eds.) (1991). Studies in computer supported cooperative work: Theory,
practice and design. Proceedings of ECSCW 89. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland.
Brown, J.S. & Duiguid, P. (2000). The Social life of Information. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University
Press.
Brown, A.L., Ash, D., Rutherford, M., Nakagawa, K., Gordon, A. & Campione, J.C. (1983). Distributed
in the classroom. In G. Salomon (Ed.), Distributed cognitions: Psychological and educational
considerations (pp.188-228). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Brown, J.S., Collins, A. & Duguid, S. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational
Researcher, 18(1), 32-42.
Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Harvard University Press. Cambridge, MA.
Chen, Y. (2001). Dimensions of transactional distance in the world wide web learning environment: a
factor analysis. British Journal of Educational Technology 32 (4), 459-470.
Cheyne, J.A., & Tarulli, D. (1999). Dialogue, difference, and the “third voice” in the zone of proximal
development. Theory and Psychology, 9, 5-28.
Cole, M. & Wertsch, J. V. (1996). Beyond the individual-social antimony in discussion of Piaget and
Vygotsky. Human Development, 39, pp 250-256.
Collins, A., Brown, J.S., and Newman, S.(1989). Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the craft of reading,
writing and mathematics. In L.B. Resnick (Ed.) Knowing, learning and instruction: Essays in honor of
Robert Claser. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Collins, A., Brown, J.S., & Hollum, A. (1991). Cognitive apprenticeship: Making thinking visible.
American Educator, 6(11), 38-46.
CTGV (1990). Anchored instruction and its relationship to situated cognition. Educational Researcher,
19 (6), 2-10.
CTGV (1993). Anchored instruction and situated cognition revisited. Educational Technology, 33 (3),
52- 70.
8 of 11
Etheris, A.I. & Tan, S.C. (2004). Computer-supported collaborative problem solving and anchored
instruction in a mathematics classroom: an exploratory study. Inernational Journal of Learning
Technology, 1(1): p. 16-39.
Godshalk, V.M., Harvey, D.M. & Moller, L. (2004). The role of learning tasks on attitude change using
cognitive flexibility hypertext systems. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13(4), p. 507-526.
Gibson, J.J. (1979). The Ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Gredler, M., & Shields, C. (2004). Does no one read Vygotsky's words? Commentary on Glassman.
Educational Researcher, 33(4), 21-25.
Haythornthwaite, C., Kaymer, M., Robins, J., & Shoemaker, S. (2000). Community development among
distance learners: temporal and technological dimensions. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication, 6(1). http://www.ascusc.org/jcmc/vol6/issue1/haythornthwaite.html
Heilesen, S.B., Cudrio, M.C. & Cheesman, T.R. (2002). Distributed collaborative teaching and learning
in a groupware environment. http://newmedia.colorado.edu/cscl/166.html
John-Steiner, V. & Souberman, E.(1978). Afterword. In Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in society.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kolodner, J.L. (1993). Case-based reasoning. Morgan Kaufmann, Calif., US.
Lantolf, J.P. & Thorne, S. (2005) SCT and the genesis of L2 development. Oxford: Oxford University
Press. http://www.wisc.edu/english/rfyoung/715/06.internalization_theory.pdf
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and culture in everyday life. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Moore, M. (1972). Learner autonomy: The second dimension of independent learning. Convergence
5(2): 76-88.
Moore, M. (1980). Independent study. In Boyd, R. and J. Apps, (Eds) Redefining the discipline of adult
education. San Francisco: Jossey?Bass.
Moore, M. G. & Kearsley, G. (1996). Distance education: A systems view. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth
Publishing Company.
Naidu, S; Ip, A; Linser, R. (2000) Dynamic goal-based role-play simulation on the web: A case study.
Educational Technology & Society 3(3) p. 190-202.
Palfreyman, D. (2002). OxCHEPS Occasional: Paper No. 1. Oxford Centre for Higher Education Policy
Studies, at New College, Oxford, OX1 3BN, UK. Online: http://www.new.oxford.ac.uk/oxcheps
Petrina, S. (2004). Curriculum and instruction for technology teachers. Online:
http://www.cust.educ.ubc.ca/programs/tsed/research/books/
Piaget, J. (1962). Comments on Vygotsky’s critical remarks concerning “The Language and Thought of
the Child” and “Judgment and Reasoning in the Child by Jean Piaget”. Online:
http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/comment/piaget.htm
9 of 11
Piaget, J. (1969). Science of education and the psychology of the child. New York: Grossman Publishers.
Roschelle, J. (1996). Learning by collaborating: Convergent conceptual change. In T. Koschmann (Ed.),
CSCL: Theory and Practice of an emerging paradigm (pp. 209-248). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Russell, T.L. (1999). The no significant difference phenomenon. Raleigh: North Carolina State
University.
Saba, and Shearer, R. (1994) Verifying key theoretical concepts in a dynamic model of distance
education. The American Journal of Distance Education. 8 (1) 36-59.
Saba, F. (2004). Interaction in distance education. Editor's note. Online:
http://store.distance-educator.com/product_info.php?products_id=83
Salovaara, H. & Järvelä, S. (2003). Students’ strategic actions in computer supported collaborative
inquiry. Learning Environments Research, 6(3), 267-284.
Scardamalia, M., Bereiter, C., McLean, R. S., Swallow, J., & Woodruff, E. (1989). Computer supported
intentional learning environments. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 5, 51-68.
Schank, R. C. (1996). Goal-based scenarios: Case-based reasoning meets learning by doing. In D.B.
Leake (Ed). Case-based reasoning, (p.295—348). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Spiro, R.J., Feltovich, P.J., Jacobson, M.J., & Coulson, R.L. (1992). Cognitive flexibility, constructivism
and hypertext: Random access instruction for advanced knowledge acquisition in ill-structured domains.
In T. Duffy & D. Jonassen (Eds.), Constructivism and the technology of instruction. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Tsinakos, A.A. & Margaritis, K.G. (2001). Employing case based reasoning in asynchronous distance
education. Learning Technology, 3(3). Online:
http://lttf.ieee.org/learn_tech/issues/july2001/index.html#3
Vygotsky, L.S. (1930). Mind and society. Online:
http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/mind/index.htm
Vygotsky, L. S. (1931). The development of thinking and concept formation in adolescence. Online:
http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1931/adolescent/ch10.htm
Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Vygotsky, L.S. (1987). The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky. Vol.1: Problems of general psychology.
Including the volume Thinking and speech. R.W. Rieber & A.S. Carton, Eds., N. Minick, Translators.
NY: Plenum Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1994). Imagination and creativity of the adolescent. In R. Van der Veer & J.Valsiner,
Eds. The Vygotsky reader. Oxford: Blackwell, p. 266-88.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). The history of the development of higher mental functions. In L. S. Vygotsky,
Collected works. Volume 4. New York: Plenum Press.
Wilson. M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9:4, pp.
625-636(12)
Wood, D., Bruner, J., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89-100.
10 of 11
Online Journal of Distance Learning Administration, Volume VIII, Number II Summer 2005
University of West Georgia, Distance Education Center
Back to the Online Journal of Distance Learning Administration Contents
11 of 11