Personality and Individual Differences 71 (2014) 35–38
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Differences
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
Short Communication
Liar liar pants on fire: Cheater strategies linked to the Dark Triad
Holly M. Baughman a,⇑, Peter K. Jonason b, Minna Lyons c, Philip A. Vernon a
a
University of Western Ontario, Canada
University of Western Sydney, Australia
c
University of Liverpool, United Kingdom
b
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 10 July 2014
Received in revised form 17 July 2014
Accepted 18 July 2014
Keywords:
Dark Triad
Psychopathy
Narcissism
Machiavellianism
Deception
Evolutionary psychology
a b s t r a c t
The present study (N = 462) examined the relationship between the Dark Triad traits (i.e., narcissism,
Machiavellianism, and psychopathy) and deception in domain-general and domain-specific contexts.
As predicted, psychopathy and Machiavellianism were linked to the propensity to lie in different
contexts, including mating and academic dishonesty. Psychopathy was related to experiencing more
positive emotions associated with lying and Machiavellianism was associated with increased amount
of cognitive effort associated with deception. Sex differences in deception were partially mediated by
individual differences in the Dark Triad traits. Our findings have important implications for the interpersonal strategies employed by those high on the Dark Triad.
Ó 2014 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
1. Introduction
The Dark Triad (i.e., narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy;
Paulhus & Williams, 2002) is a personality cluster of three distinct
but overlapping traits. Narcissism is defined by dominance,
entitlement, and superiority (Emmons, 1984). Machiavellianism
is characterized by manipulativeness and glib social charm
(Christie & Geis, 1970). Psychopathy is exhibited through high
thrill seeking, impulsivity, and low empathy (Hare, 1985). While
generally viewed as socially maladaptive and pathological, recent
work suggests these traits embody cheater adaptations that are
linked to mating success (Jonason, Li, Webster, & Schmitt, 2009)
and the adoption of a wide range of social influence tactics
(Jonason & Webster, 2012). In this study, we test the ‘‘cheater strategy’’ hypothesis by examining links between the Dark Triad traits
and deception-related behavior, emotions, and cognitions.
Recent work has provided initial support for the various cheater
strategies (i.e., intersexual and intrasexual deception) linked to the
Dark Triad traits (Jonason, Baughman, Lyons, & Vernon, 2014).
However, more detail is warranted. First, most work making the
case for the cheater strategy has not directly tested the hypothesis
(Jonason & Webster, 2012). Second, the work that has directly
tested it (Jonason et al., 2014) relied on a sample of British
undergraduates, on one conceptualization of the Dark Triad traits,
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology, University of
Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada.
E-mail address: hbaughma@uwo.ca (H.M. Baughman).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.019
0191-8869/Ó 2014 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
failed to examine emotional and cognitive aspects of deception,
and did not juxtapose deception in different contexts. Therefore,
we examine the Dark Triad traits and deception in the domaingeneral and domain-specific contexts. We also extend previous
research by delving deeper into the emotional and cognitive
aspects of lying. In addition, we consider domain-specific lying in
evolutionary relevant (i.e., mating) and novel (i.e., academic)
contexts.
We make a number of predictions about the deceptive tactics
related to the Dark Triad traits. We predict that psychopathy and
Machiavellianism will be positively related to the frequency of
lying in the domain-general context. Psychopathy and Machiavellianism share a number of common characteristics (McHoskey,
Worzel, & Szyarto, 1998), including manipulativeness and deceitfulness, which are portrayed by pathological lying and other
deceptive behaviors (Hare, 1985; Christie & Geis, 1970). In other
words, we would expect these individuals to lie more often in
everyday life. However, given the exploitative mating strategy that
is associated with psychopathy but not Machiavellianism (Jonason,
Luévano, & Adams, 2012), we expect lying in the domain-specific
context of mating will be stronger among psychopaths than
Machiavellians.
In addition to understanding how the Dark Triad traits function
in the mating context, we also examine how they function in an
evolutionarily-novel context, namely, academic dishonesty.
Among the traits, psychopathy is the least sensitive to contextual
differences (Jonason & Webster, 2012) and we predict that the
deception tendencies underlying psychopathy should be invariant
36
H.M. Baughman et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 71 (2014) 35–38
across contexts. In other words, those high in psychopathy appear
insensitive to the modulating force of context (or even target) of
their actions and this may extend to lying. However, as individuals
high in Machiavellianism are motivated specifically by power and
status (Christie & Geis, 1970), they may engage less in deception in
mating contexts but more in academic contexts, where status is
earned.
Lying is a cognitively demanding task, especially so when telling elaborate lies. As such, we predict that the Dark Triad traits will
be linked to more cognitive effort expended on lying, regardless of
context. In other words, we would expect individuals high on the
Dark Triad to expend more cognitive resources in order to lie successfully. If deception is an adaptive strategy, we would also expect
these same individuals to (1) believe others are fooled by their lies
and (2) derive some pleasure from lying to others.
Therefore, we assess the relationships between the Dark Triad
traits and perception of cognitive effort expended to lie, degree
of enjoyment derived from lying, and degree to which high Dark
Triad individuals believe (rightly or wrongly) that they convince
others with their lies. We contend these three factors come
together as part of the suite of psychological factors that facilitate
the purported cheater strategy that Dark Triad traits embody
(Jonason et al., 2014).
Previous findings indicate that men lie more than women do
and score higher on the Dark Triad traits (Jonason et al., 2009,
2014). This suggests to us that sex differences in lying might be driven, in part, by individual differences in the Dark Triad traits. We
expect this to be primarily attributable to the effects of Machiavellianism and psychopathy, the ‘‘darker’’ traits of the cluster
(Rauthmann, 2012).
To date there has only been one direct test (that we know of) of
the cheater strategy hypothesis suggested for either psychopathy
on its own (Mealey, 1995) or the Dark Triad traits collectively
(Jonason et al., 2014). In order to further test this hypothesis and
to provide new and interesting details about the deception related
links to the Dark Triad traits we present this brief report. In it we
assess the correlations between the Dark Triad traits and
domain-specific and domain-general lying along with deception
in mating and academic contexts.
2. Method
2.1. Participants and procedure
A sample of 462 Canadian undergraduate students (130 men),
aged between 16 and 80 years (M = 19.49, SD = 4.86), participated
in an online testing session for an introductory psychology course.
Once participants had completed the questionnaires, they were
debriefed and compensated with course credit.
2.2. Measures
The Dark Triad traits were assessed with the 27-item Short-D3
(Jones & Paulhus, 2014). Respondents are asked to rate the degree
to which they agree with statements (1 = Disagree Strongly;
5 = Agree Strongly) reflective of narcissism (e.g., ‘‘I like to be the
center of attention’’), Machiavellianism (e.g., ‘‘It’s not wise to tell
your secrets’’), and psychopathy (e.g., ‘‘I like to get revenge on
authorities’’). Items were averaged into indexes of narcissism
(Cronbach’s a = .70), Machiavellianism (a = .66), and psychopathy
(a = .77).1
1
Psychopathy was correlated with Machiavellianism (r(460) = .49, p < .001) and
narcissism (r(460) = .34, p < .001). Narcissism was correlated with Machiavellianism
(r(460) = .26, p < .001).
We assessed participants’ perceptions of lying with an amended
version of a measure used in previous research (Gonza, Vrij, & Bull,
2001). We added items tapping into the cognitive and emotional
aspects of lying. Participants responded to a number of items on
7-point Likert scales in two contexts (i.e., mating and academic;
see Appendix A). Exploratory factor analyses (with various rotations) yielded an unclear multidimensional structure, so we
adopted a modified Thematic Analysis to reduce the number of
variables and Type I error (Jonason & Buss, 2012). This was accomplished by grouping the items into face-valid composites based on
internal consistency where possible (otherwise we correlated the
two items)2. Items were reduced into four composites for each scenario to assess various aspects of lying across and within-contexts
(varied order; within-participants). First, participants were asked
(1) how often they lie, and (2) how often their lies are detected. Participants reported how likely they would lie in the given situation (2
items; r(460) = .31 and .46, p < .001), with higher scores representing
a greater inclination to lie. They also indicated their emotional state
(8 items; a = .80 and .82), with higher scores being characteristic of
positive emotions (e.g., joy or satisfaction). Participants also indicated how much cognitive effort they would expend in order to lie
successfully (5 items; a = .64 and .71), with higher scores signifying
more effort (e.g., planning, originality). Lastly, participants were
asked the degree to which they believed that the person being lied
to (i.e., partner or lecturer) would believe their lie.
3. Results
In terms of the Dark Triad, men scored higher than women on
Machiavellianism and psychopathy (t’s(460) = 2.53 and 2.46,
p’s < .01, Hedge’s g’s = 0.24 and 0.26, respectively). In the mating
context, men reported a greater inclination to lie, a more positive
emotional state, and an expected positive reaction from their partner than women (t’s(460) = 2.67 to 4.09, p’s < .01, g’s = 0.26 to
0.46). In the academic context, men reported a more positive emotional state in relation to lying than women (t(460) = 3.38, p < .01,
g = 0.36).
We report the associations between the Dark Triad traits and
our various measures of lying in Table 1. All three Dark Triad traits
were positively related to self-reported lying frequency. After controlling for the shared variance among the traits, this association
was entirely attributable to psychopathy and Machiavellianism.
Those who scored high on the Dark Triad traits did not report that
they were better at lying than others, which may raise concerns
about social desirability. Psychopathy had the strongest link with
positive emotions while lying in both contexts. Machiavellianism
was related to planning ahead and constructing a lie to be more
original and to include more detail, particularly in the academic
context.
In order to examine whether the Dark Triad traits mediate sex
differences in lying, we conducted hierarchical regressions (Step
1 included sex of the participant; Step 2 included the Dark Triad
traits). Sex differences in one’s emotional state while lying in the
mating context were partially mediated by the Dark Triad traits
(DR2 = .15; F(4, 413) = 23.58, p < .01), such that the direct effect
(b = .19, p < .01) was reduced (b = .12, p < .01) when the indirect
effects of Machiavellianism (b = .16, p < .01) and psychopathy
(b = .25, p < .01) were added. Further, sex differences in one’s emotional state while lying in the academic context was fully mediated
by the Dark Triad traits (DR2 = .13; F(4, 417) = 17.89, p < .01), such
that the direct effect (b = .14, p < .01) was no longer significant
(b = .09, ns) when the indirect effects of narcissism (b = .14,
p < .01), Machiavellianism (b = .15, p < .01) and psychopathy
2
More details upon request.
H.M. Baughman et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 71 (2014) 35–38
Table 1
Zero-order correlations and standardized regression coefficients for the Dark Triad
traits and domain-specific contexts of deception.
r(b)
*
**
Narcissism
Machiavellianism
Psychopathy
Mating context
Probability of lying
Emotional state
Cognitive effort
Partner’s reaction
.07 (.03)
.25** (.10*)
.18** (.10)
.11* (.08)
.10* (.06)
.34** (.18**)
.15** (.15**)
.13** (.16*)
.13**
.46**
.14**
.06 (
Academic context
Probability of lying
Emotional state
Cognitive effort
Lecturer’s reaction
.14** (.06)
.28** (.14**)
.08 (.02)
.19** (.14**)
.25**
.33**
.28**
.16**
.19** (.09)
.42** (.19**)
.10* ( .04)
.17** (.09)
(.18**)
(.16**)
(.29**)
(.08)
(.09*)
(.25**)
(.03)
.02)
p < .05.
p < .01.
(b = .18, p < .01) were added. Last, we found that narcissism
(b = .09, ns), Machiavellianism (b = .10, ns) and psychopathy
(b = .04, ns) did not account for any unique variance, however we
found evidence for partial mediation (DR2 = .03; F(4, 431) = 4.11,
p < .01), such that the direct effect (b = .11, p < .05) was reduced
when the indirect effect (b = .08, ns) was added.
37
Vernon, 2014). Without thorough longitudinal studies, however,
we cannot be certain. Our results indicate that men adopted cheater strategies more so than women, particularly in the mating context. From an evolutionary perspective, being ‘‘bad’’ comes with
fewer costs and more benefits (i.e., mate acquisition, status attainment) for men than for women (Figueredo et al., 2006), and having
these ‘‘darker’’ personality traits may be more adaptive in men
(Jonason et al., 2009). We also found that sex differences in deception were partially accounted for by individual differences in the
Dark Triad traits, namely Machiavellianism and psychopathy.
The present study is not without limitations. First, our study
relied on a sample of university undergraduates. While this is a
standard sample in mainstream social-personality psychology, it
might be important to extend our research to special populations
such as offenders, who have actively engaged in a more costly
cheater strategy than academic misconduct. Similarly, by relying
on undergraduate students we may have missed important other
contexts where lying might be adaptive or functionally useful, such
as the workplace. In addition, we assessed self-reported lying
rather than actual lying ability. However, assessing actual lying
ability in the high stakes context is a difficult task (Mann, Vrij, &
Bull, 2002). Nevertheless, our findings provide support for the
unique cheater strategies linked to the Dark Triad.
Appendix A.
4. Discussion
Lying questionnaire scenarios
In the present study we adopt an evolutionary framework that
views the Dark Triad traits as adaptive in terms of an exploitative,
self-serving life strategy (Jonason & Webster, 2012; Jonason et al.,
2009). The assertion that the Dark Triad traits are characterized by
underlying cheater strategies has only recently been empirically
supported (Jonason et al., 2014). We sought to replicate previous
work by testing the ‘‘cheater strategy’’ hypothesis in the domaingeneral and domain-specific contexts of deception. We extended
this further by examining high-stakes lying in evolutionary relevant (i.e., mating) and novel (i.e., academics) settings and focused
on the emotional and cognitive aspects of lying.
In the mating context, Machiavellianism was related to the
belief that one’s partner would believe the lie, whereas narcissism
was related to a belief one’s lecturer would believe the lie. Consistent with our predictions, we found that psychopathy was related
to a greater probability of lying and more pleasure derived from
lying; links that were equivalent across the two contexts. This
may reveal the ‘‘darker’’ nature of psychopathy (Rauthmann,
2012) and is consistent with the ‘‘one-size fits all’’ approach these
individuals engage in (Jonason & Webster, 2012). A similar pattern
was detected in narcissism but the links were weaker. In contrast,
Machiavellianism was related to an increase in cognitive effort
while lying in both contexts, but considerably more so for lies
involving academic integrity. It appears that Machiavellianism
may be more associated with various aspects of lying in the context of academics. Machiavellianism may be less directly involved
in mating decisions (Jonason et al., 2012) but, instead, be more
involved with social maneuvering and the attainment of status,
which may be found in academic settings. Importantly, this may
distinguish psychopathy from Machiavellianism. It suggests that
these two traits differ as a function of context more than content;
both may use deception but the reasons, goals, and contexts those
high on either trait employ deception may differ.
We replicated sex differences in deceptive attitudes and tactics;
however, our findings are not fully consistent with most of the
Dark Triad literature, as a significant difference did not emerge
for narcissism. Aside from methodological artifacts, it is possible
that women are increasingly endorsing this trait (Giammarco &
Scenario 1 (mating context)
You and your partner have been in a healthy relationship for
over two years. One day, you receive an email from your ex-partner
asking if you want to meet up for coffee. Although hesitant at first,
you agree to meet with them. You decide not to tell your current
partner about this meeting. While you are having coffee at the
café with your ex-partner, you notice that a friend of your current
partner is looking at you from across the room. Instead of
approaching you to say hello, this friend walks out of the café.
When you return home, your partner says that they heard from a
friend about your meeting with your ex and demands to know your
side of the story.
Scenario 2 (academic context)
It is near to the end of the semester and you have assignments
building up and exams to study for. One important assignment is
due in and you have yet to write it. The marks for the assignment
count for 40% of your overall grade for this course and it is not your
best subject. You are friends with a student who took the same
course the year before you and they offer to give you their assignment to help you out. They got 82% for their essay. You finish the
assignment and hand it in on time, although you have plagiarized
a substantial amount of your friend’s work. You then think nothing
more about it. When the assignments have been marked, your lecturer asks to have a word with you about some of the assignment
he recognizes. He suspects you have plagiarized but you tell him
that the essay was all your own work.
References
Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic
Press.
Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the Narcissistic
Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 291–300.
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., Schneider, S. M. R., Sefcek, J. A., Tal, I.
R., et al. (2006). Consilience and life history theory: From genes to brain to
reproductive strategy. Developmental Review, 26, 243–275.
Giammarco, E. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2014). Vengeance and the Dark Triad: The role of
empathy and perspective taking in trait forgiveness. Personality and Individual
Differences, 67, 23–29.
38
H.M. Baughman et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 71 (2014) 35–38
Gonza, L. F., Vrij, A., & Bull, R. (2001). Impact of individual differences on perceptions
of lying in everyday life and in a high stake situation. Personality and Individual
Differences, 31, 1203–1216.
Hare, R. D. (1985). Comparison of procedures for the assessment of psychopathy.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 7–16.
Jonason, P. K., & Buss, D. M. (2012). Avoiding entangling commitments: Tactics for
implementing a short-term mating strategy. Personality and Individual
Differences, 52, 606–610.
Jonason, P. K., Baughman, H. B., Lyons, M., & Vernon, P. A. (2014). What a tangled
web we weave: The Dark Triad traits and deception. Personality and Individual
Differences, 70, 117–119.
Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., Webster, G. W., & Schmitt, D. P. (2009). The Dark
Triad: Facilitating short-term mating in men. European Journal of Personality, 23,
5–18.
Jonason, P. K., Luévano, V. X., & Adams, H. M. (2012). How the Dark Triad
traits predict relationship choices. Personality and Individual Differences, 53,
180–184.
Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2012). A protean approach to social influence: Dark
Triad personalities and social influence tactics. Personality and Individual
Differences, 52, 521–526.
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief
measure of the Dark Triad personality traits. Assessment, 21, 28–41.
Mann, S., Vrij, A., & Bull, A. (2002). Suspects, lies, and videotape: An analysis of highstake liars. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 365–376.
McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and
Psychopathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 192–210.
Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary
model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 523–599.
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism,
Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36,
556–563.
Rauthmann, J. F. (2012). The Dark Triad and interpersonal perception: Similarities
and differences in the social consequences of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and
psychopathy. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 487–496.