Age-Related Changes in The Anatomy of Language Regions in Autism Spectrum Disorder
Age-Related Changes in The Anatomy of Language Regions in Autism Spectrum Disorder
Age-Related Changes in The Anatomy of Language Regions in Autism Spectrum Disorder
DOI 10.1007/s11682-008-9048-x
Received: 15 December 2007 / Accepted: 17 September 2008 / Published online: 4 October 2008
# Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2008
Introduction
Cortical language regions
Perisylvian regions critical for language functions include
posterior regions, which consist of Heschls gyrus and the
posterior superior temporal gyrus, including the planum
temporale, and frontal areas, which are comprised of the
pars triangularis and pars opercularis. The anatomy of these
regions has been the focus of many studies in normal
healthy adults. Consistent asymmetry patterns of Heschls
gyrus, however, have not been established in normal
subjects. Leftward planum temporale asymmetry has been
found repeatedly and is one of the most consistent
asymmetries found in the human cortex (for review,
Shapleske et al. 1999). Less consistent asymmetry has been
found for the pars triangularis and pars opercularis, with
leftward asymmetry of these areas being found in some
studies (Foundas et al. 1995, 1996, 1998, 2001; Gauger et al.
1997), but not in others (Knaus et al. 2006, 2007; Tomaiuolo
et al. 1999). The pars triangularis and pars opercularis
together make up Brocas area and when measured together,
again some studies have demonstrated leftward asymmetry
(Albanese et al. 1989; Falzi et al. 1982), while others have
not (Robichon et al. 2000; Wada et al. 1975).
Although anatomical asymmetries of some of these
language areas have been found in infant and fetal brains
(Chi et al. 1977; Wada et al. 1975; Witelson and Pallie
1973), studies have indicated that the anatomy and
asymmetry of these critical language regions continue to
develop throughout childhood. Wada et al. (1975) found
greater degree of planum temporale asymmetry in adult
compared to infant brains and suggested that left planum
size increases with age. Cross-sectional and longitudinal
studies have indicated increases in gray matter density of
52
Manual
Voxel-based morphometry
Semi-automated
Semi-automated
Voxel-based morphometry
Semi-automated
Manual
Semi-automated
Volume
Asymmetry
Volume
Asymmetry
Volume
Measurement
Findings
TD = typically developing, LI = language impaired, LN = language normal, SLI = specific language impaired, DLD = developmental language delayed, RH = right-handed, L = left,
R = right, PTR = pars triangularis, HG = Heschls gyrus, PT = planum temporale, POP = pars opercularis
Voxel-based morphometry
Method
Participants
Study
54
55
Table 2 Means and standard deviations of demographic information for each group
ASD
Age
Handedness
K-BIT VIQ
K-BIT NVIQ
K-BIT FSIQ
CELF receptive
CELF expressive
CELF total
ADOS Communication
ADOS social
ADOS total
Controls
Younger
Older
9.50
10.58
100.45
109.25
105.85
100.16
97.42
98.79
3.55
9.80
13.35
14.95
11.22
102.74
105.05
104.79
96.80
90.35
92.95
3.10
8.65
11.75
(1.32)
(15.08)
(20.35)
(13.88)
(17.18)
(16.55)
(16.19)
(16.69)
(1.79)
(2.33)
(3.92)
(2.23)
(13.75)
(21.83)
(13.33)
(17.27)
(21.12)
(19.35)
(21.17)
(1.68)
(3.08)
(4.39)
Younger
Older
9.54 (1.38)
15.15 (9.01)
117.50 (13.03)
113.90 (8.81)
118.40 (9.93)
118.11 (13.83)
115.16 (12.65)
118.37 (11.42)
-
14.93 (2.13)
17.42 (9.08)
119.53 (14.53)
109.95 (11.06)
117.21 (12.12)
113.15 (10.66)
107.60 (11.21)
111.45 (10.05)
-
56
57
Analyses
Behavioral measures A multivariate analysis of variance
(MANOVA) was used to examine differences in IQ
between the ASD and control groups and between the
younger and older groups. Diagnosis and age group were
the independent variables and K-BIT verbal and nonverbal standard scores were the dependent variables.
Similarly, differences in language scores were examined
via MANOVA, with diagnosis and age group as
independent variables and CELF receptive and expressive
standard scores as dependent variables.
Volume To control for differences in total brain size, each
region was calculated as a proportion of total hemisphere
volume for all analyses. To examine differences in volume
between the ASD and control groups and between the
younger and older groups, MANOVA was used with
hemisphere as the within-subject independent variable,
diagnosis and age group as the between-subjects independent variables, and Heschls gyrus, planum temporale, pars
triangularis, and pars opercularis gray matter volumes as the
dependent variables. Since the pars triangularis and pars
opercularis together comprise Brocas area and some studies
have examined these regions together, the pars triangularis
and pars opercularis measures were also combined, creating
a total frontal language region. To examine differences in this
total frontal area, an ANOVA was performed with hemisphere as the within-subject independent variable, diagnosis
and age group as the between-subjects independent variables, and pars triangularis + pars opercularis gray matter
volume as the dependent variable.
58
Results
Behavioral measures The group data on IQ and language
scores are presented in Table 2. For IQ, at the multivariate
level, there was a significant main effect of diagnosis (F2,73 =
8.66, p<.001). The univariate level indicated significantly
higher verbal IQ (F1,74 =17.56, p<.001) scores in controls
than in the ASD group, but no significant group differences
in non-verbal IQ scores. There was no significant difference
between age groups or interaction between diagnosis
and age group. For the CELF, there was a significant
main effect of diagnosis (F2,73 =14.76, p<.001), with no
significant differences between age groups or interaction
between diagnosis and age group. For receptive (F1,74 =
59
Table 3 Means and standard deviations of total hemisphere volume and gray matter volume of each region, proportional to total hemisphere
volume for each group
ROI
Hemi
Total
HG
PT
PTR
POP
PTR+POP
ASD
Left
Right
Left
Right
Left
Right
Left
Right
Left
Right
Left
Right
Controls
Younger (n=20)
Older (n=20)
Younger (n=20)
Older (n=20)
670.94
669.13
.0025
.0019
.0036
.0030
.0017
.0016
.0028
.0029
.0045
.0045
682.93
677.41
.0024
.0021
.0034
.0024
.0015
.0015
.0027
.0028
.0042
.0043
670.70
663.48
.0025
.0021
.0033
.0030
.0013
.0013
.0024
.0027
.0037
.0040
691.87
689.76
.0024
.0021
.0038
.0027
.0013
.0014
.0026
.0026
.0040
.0041
(72.10)
(65.61)
(.00056)
(.00053)
(.00130)
(.00084)
(.00068)
(.00066)
(.00068)
(.00087)
(.00124)
(.00127)
(79.66)
(75.26)
(.00072)
(.00049)
(.00091)
(.00096)
(.00056)
(.00073)
(.00079)
(.00081)
(.00108)
(.00129)
(43.96)
(45.68)
(.00053)
(.00065)
(.00091)
(.00103)
(.00054)
(.00062)
(.00073)
(.00075)
(.00113)
(.00115)
(54.91)
(60.97)
(.00059)
(.00048)
(.00112)
(.00089)
(.00068)
(.00063)
(.00060)
(.00063)
(.00117)
(.00097)
HG = Heschls gyrus, PT = planum temporale, PTR = pars triangularis, POP = pars opercularis
0.005
0.0045
Discussion
There were several interesting findings in this study. In
controls, there was significantly stronger leftward planum
temporale asymmetry in the older group than the younger
group. This was also found in the ASD group. There was
0.004
0.0035
0.003
ROI AQ
0.0025
0.002
0.0015
0.001
0.0005
0
Left
Right
Hemisphere
Controls
HG
PT
PTR
POP
PTR+POP
ASD
Controls
Younger
(n=20)
Older
(n=20)
Younger
(n=20)
Older
(n=20)
.267
.151
.088
.011
.016
.156
.368
.023
.016
.006
.231
.119
.050
.120
.068
.116
.342
.064
.009
.036
(.25)
(.42)
(.36)
(.40)
(.35)
(.24)
(.51)
(.51)
(.33)
(.33)
(.35)
(.43)
(.64)
(.29)
(.35)
(.28)
(.36)
(.59)
(.26)
(.29)
ASD
60
Planum Temporale AQ
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0
Controls
ASD
Group
Younger
Older
References
Abell, F., Krams, M., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R., Friston, K.,
Frackowiak, R., et al. (1999). The neuroanatomy of autism: a
voxel-based whole brain analysis of structural scans. Neuroreport,
10, 16471651. doi:10.1097/00001756-199906030-00005.
Albanese, E., Merlo, A., Albanese, A., & Gomez, E. (1989). Anterior
speech region. Asymmetry and weight-surface correlation.
Neurology, 40, 353362.
Alexander, M. P., Naeser, M. A., & Palumbo, C. (1990). Brocas area
aphasias: aphasia after lesions including the frontal operculum.
Neurology, 40, 353362.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Association.
Aylward, E. H., Minshew, N. J., Field, K., Sparks, B. F., & Singh, N.
(2002). Effects of age on brain volume and head circumference in
autism. Neurology, 59, 175183.
Barta, P. E., Dhingra, L., Royall, R., & Schwartz, E. (1997).
Improving stereological estimates for the volume of structures
identified in three-dimensional arrays of spatial data. Journal of
Neuroscience Methods, 75, 111118. doi:10.1016/S0165-0270
(97)00049-6.
61
Beaton, A. A. (1997). The relation of planum temporale asymmetry
and morphology of the corpus callosum to handedness, gender,
and dyslexia: a review of the evidence. Brain and Language, 60,
255322. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1825.
Bigler, E. D., Tate, D. F., Neeley, E. S., Wolfson, L. J., Miller, M. J.,
Rice, S. A., et al. (2003). Temporal lobe, autism, and macrocephaly. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 24, 2066
2076.
Blanton, R. E., Levitt, J. G., Thompson, P. M., Narr, K. L., CapetilloCunliffe, L., Nobel, A., et al. (2001). Mapping cortical
asymmetry and complexity patterns in normal children. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging Section, 107, 2943. doi:10.1016/
S0925-4927(01)00091-9.
Blumenfeld, H. K., Booth, J. R., & Burman, D. D. (2006). Differential
prefrontal-temporal neural correlates of semantic processing in
children. Brain and Language, 99, 226235.
Boddaert, N., Chabane, N., Gervais, H., Good, C. D., Bourgeois, M.,
Plumet, M. -H., et al. (2004). Superior temporal sulcus
anatomical abnormalities in childhood autism: a voxel-based
morphometry MRI study. NeuroImage, 23, 364369. doi:10.
1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.016.
Carper, R. A., Moses, P., Tigue, Z. D., & Courchesne, E. (2002).
Cerebral lobes in autism: early hyperplasia and abnormal age
effects. NeuroImage, 16, 10381051. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.
1099.
Chi, J. G., Dooling, E. C., & Gilles, F. H. (1977). Left-right
asymmetries of the temporal speech areas of the human fetus.
Archives of Neurology, 34, 346348.
Courchesne, E. (2004). Brain development in autism: early overgrowth followed by premature arrest of growth. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10,
106111. doi:10.1002/mrdd.20020.
Courchesne, E., Karns, C. M., Davis, H. R., Ziccardi, R., Carper, R. A.,
Tigue, Z. D., et al. (2001). Unusual brain growth patterns in early
life in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study. Neurology, 57,
245254.
Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2005). Brain overgrowth in autism
during a critical time in development: implications for frontal
pyramidal neuron and interneuron development and connectivity.
International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 153
170. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2005.01.003.
Dapretto, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (1999). Form and content:
dissociating syntax and semantics in sentence comprehension.
Neuron, 24, 427432. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80855-7.
de Foss, L., Hodge, S. M., Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., Caviness Jr,
V. S., McGrath, L., et al. (2004). Language-association cortex
asymmetry in autism and specific language impairment. Annals
of Neurology, 56, 757766. doi:10.1002/ana.20275.
Dmonet, J. F., Chollet, F., Ramsay, S., Cardebat, D., Nespoulous, J. -L.,
Wise, R., et al. (1992). The anatomy of phonological and semantic
processing in the normal subjects. Brain, 115, 17531768.
doi:10.1093/brain/115.6.1753.
Elliot, C. D. (1990). Differential ability scales: introductory and
technical handbook. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Embick, D., Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., ONeil, W., & Sakai, K. L.
(2000). A syntactic specialization for Brocas area. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 97, 61506154. doi:10.1073/pnas.100098897.
Falzi, G., Perrone, P., & Vignolo, L. A. (1982). Right-left asymmetry
in anterior speech region. Archives of Neurology, 39, 239240.
Foundas, A. L., Eure, K. F., Luevano, L. F., & Weinberger, D. R.
(1998). MRI asymmetries of Brocas area: the pars triangularis
and pars opercularis. Brain and Language, 64, 282296.
doi:10.1006/brln.1998.1974.
Foundas, A. L., Leonard, C. M., Gilmore, R. L., Fennell, E. B., &
Heilman, K. M. (1994). Planum temporale asymmetry and
62
language dominance. Neuropsychologia, 32, 12251231.
doi:10.1016/0028-3932(94)90104-X.
Foundas, A. L., Leonard, C. M., Gilmore, R. L., Fennell, E. B., &
Heilman, K. M. (1996). Pars triangularis asymmetry and
language dominance. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 719722.
doi:10.1073/pnas.93.2.719.
Foundas, A. L., Leonard, C. M., & Heilman, K. M. (1995).
Morphologic cerebral asymmetries and handedness the pars
triangularis and planum temporale. Archives of Neurology, 52,
501508.
Foundas, A. L., Weisberg, A., Browning, C. A., & Weinberger, D. R.
(2001). Morphology of the frontal operculum: a volumetric
magnetic resonance imaging study of the pars triangularis.
Journal of Neuroimaging, 11, 153159.
Gaffrey, M. S., Kleinhaus, N. M., Haist, F., Akshoomoff, N.,
Campbell, A., Courchesne, E., et al. (2007). A typical participation of visual cortex during word processing in autism: an fMRI
study of semantic decision. Neuropsychologia, 45, 16721684.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.008.
Gauger, L. M., Lombardino, L. J., & Leonard, C. M. (1997). Brain
morphology in children with specific language impairment.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR,
40, 12721284.
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D.,
Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical
development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 101, 81748179. doi:10.1073/pnas.0402680101.
Habib, M. (1989). Anatomical asymmetries of the human cerebral
cortex. The International Journal of Neuroscience, 47, 6780.
doi:10.3109/00207458908987419.
Hardan, A. Y., Minshew, N. J., Mallikarjuhn, M., & Keshavan, M. S.
(2001). Brain volume in autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 16,
421424.
Harris, G. J., Chabris, C. F., Clark, J., Urban, T., Aharon, I., Steele, S.,
et al. (2006). Brain activation during semantic processing in
autism spectrum disorders via functional magnetic resonance
imaging. Brain and Cognition, 61, 5468. doi:10.1016/j.
bandc.2005.12.015.
Hazlett, H. C., Poe, M., Gerig, G., Smith, R. G., Provenzale, J., Ross, A.,
et al. (2005). Magnetic resonance imaging and head circumference
study of brain size in autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62,
13661376. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.12.1366.
Herbert, M. R., Harris, G. J., Adrien, K. T., Ziegler, D. A., Makris, N.,
Kennedy, D. N., et al. (2002). Abnormal asymmetry in language
association cortex in autism. Annals of Neurology, 52, 588596.
doi:10.1002/ana.10349.
Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Deutsch, C. K., OBrien, L. M.,
Kennedy, D. N., Filipek, P. A., et al. (2005). Brain asymmetries
in autism and developmental language disorder: a nested wholebrain analysis. Brain, 128, 213226. doi:10.1093/brain/awh330.
Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Deutsch, C. K., OBrien, L. M., Lange,
N., Bakardjiev, A., et al. (2003). Dissociations of cerebral cortex,
subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes in autistic boys.
Brain, 126, 11821192. doi:10.1093/brain/awg110.
Jncke, L., Schlaug, G., Huang, Y., & Steinmetz, H. (1994).
Asymmetry of the planum parietale. Neuroreport, 5, 11611163.
Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004).
Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127, 18111821. doi:10.1093/brain/awh199.
Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., & Just,
M. A. (2006). Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in
pictures with decreased functional connectivity. Brain, 129,
24842493. doi:10.1093/brain/awl164.
63
Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental
disorder (pp. 335364, 3rd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Tomaiuolo, F., MacDonald, J. D., Caramanos, Z., Posner, G.,
Chiavaras, M., Evans, A. C., et al. (1999). Morphology,
morphometry and probability mapping of the pars opercularis
of the inferior frontal gyrus: an in vivo MRI analysis. The
European Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 30333046. doi:10.1046/
j.1460-9568.1999.00718.x.
Tzourio, N., Nkanga-Ngila, B., & Mazoyer, B. (1998). Left planum
temporale surface correlates with functional dominance during
story listening. Neuroreport, 9, 829833. doi:10.1097/00001756199803300-00012.
Volkmar, F. R., & Lord, C. (2007). Diagnosis and definition of autism
and other pervasive developmental disorders. In F. R. Volkmar
(Ed.), Autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed.,
pp. 131). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Wada, J. J., Clarke, R., & Hamm, A. (1975). Cerebral hemispheric
asymmetry in humans. Archives of Neurology, 32, 239246.
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. New
York: Harcourt Association.
Witelson, S. F., & Kigar, D. L. (1992). Sylvian fissure morphology
and asymmetry in men and women: bilateral differences in
relation to handedness in men. The Journal of Comparative
Neurology, 323, 236340. doi:10.1002/cne.903230303.
Witelson, S. F., & Pallie, W. (1973). Left hemisphere specialization for
language in the newborn. Neuroanatomical evidence of asymmetry. Brain, 96, 641646. doi:10.1093/brain/96.3.641.
Zatorre, R. J., Meyer, E., Gjedde, A., & Evans, A. C. (1996). PET
studies of phonetic processing of speech: review, replication, and
reanalysis. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.), 6, 2130.
doi:10.1093/cercor/6.1.21.