Che Pro Wrk5
Che Pro Wrk5
Che Pro Wrk5
Human Brain
Center for Human Evolution
proceedings of workshop 5
March 19-20, 2005
Officers
Walter Kistler
President
Bob Citron
Executive Director
Sesh Velamoor
April 2006
Donna Hines
Board of Advisors
University of Washington
Joseph Coates
University of California
Santa Barbara
Graham Molitor
Advisors Emeriti
Dear Readers:
Making a science of human beings is a touchy matter in our society today and, as
a result, we do not adequately understand what drives the human mind. The brain
has basically two parts: the cortex, which is largely a reservoir of knowledge, and the
limbic system, which controls emotions, drives, and motivations. The more we can
study and come to understand both parts of the brain, the better equipped we will be
going forward into the future of the species.
The Foundation For the Future was established with the mission to increase and
diffuse knowledge concerning the long-term future of humanity, and the Center for
Human Evolution focuses that mission on evolution, an arena of vast application.
In March 2005, the Center for Human Evolution brought together eight noted scholars
to offer their research and perspectives on the evolution of the human brain. I am
pleased to present to you the proceedings from that workshop. This book is a record of
the papers presented, the questions and answers engaged as a result of the papers, and
the scholars views on emerging knowledge of the brain and its implications for the
long-term future.
I hope you will enjoy reading the comments of prominent scholars on these
challenging and important issues.
Sincerely,
Co-operating Organizations
Futuribles
Paris
Institute for
Alternative Futures
Alexandria, VA
World Futures
Studies Federation
Philippines
Walter Kistler
President and Benefactor
Contents
Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix
The Proceedings Sections . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xi
List of Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xiii
Section 1
Section 2
Section 3
Description of Workshop 5 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
Section 4
Workshop Transcripts . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
4.1 Introductory Session . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
4.2 Scholar Presentations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
4.2.1 William H. Calvin Presentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
4.2.2 Terrence Deacon Presentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
4.2.3 Ralph L. Holloway Presentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41
4.2.4 Richard G. Klein Presentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49
4.2.5 Steven Pinker Presentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65
4.2.6 John Tooby Presentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
4.2.7 Endel Tulving Presentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
4.2.8 Ajit Varki Presentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95
4.3 Emerging Knowledge of the Brain and Its Long-term Implications for Humanity . . . . . . . 105
4.4 Closing Session . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Contents
vi
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Acknowledgments
he Foundation For the Future wishes to acknowledge the following persons for their efforts and
contributions in the Center for Human Evolution
Workshop The Evolution of the Human Brain:
The Foundation Board of Advisors for their guidance in planning the Center for Human Evolution
Program. These members currently include Dr. William H. Calvin, Dr. Eric J. Chaisson, Dr. Clement C.P.
Chang, Joseph Coates, Dr. Ricardo Diez-Hochleitner,
Dr. Brian Fagan, Barbara Marx Hubbard, Dr. Donald
C. Johanson, Dr. Michio Kaku, Graham T.T. Molitor, and Dr. Robert Muller. Advisors Emeriti are Dr.
George Bugliarello, Dr. Christian de Duve, and Dr.
Edward O. Wilson.
The eight participants of the workshop The Evolution of the Human Brain, whose broad expertise
and indefatigable seeking after knowledge formed the
basis of the workshop and whose contributions will
be valuable well into the future. These participants
include William H. Calvin, Ph.D.; Terrence Deacon,
Ph.D.; Ralph L. Holloway, Ph.D.; Richard G. Klein,
Ph.D.; Steven Pinker, Ph.D.; John Tooby, Ph.D.; Endel
Tulving, Ph.D.; and Ajit Varki, M.D.
Donna Hines
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
vii
Acknowledgements
viii
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Introduction
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
ix
Introduction
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Section 3
shop 5.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
xi
xii
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
List of Participants
William H. Calvin, Ph.D.
University of Washington
School of Medicine
Seattle, WA
Columbia University
Department of Anthropology
New York, NY
Stanford University
Program in Human Biology
Stanford, CA
Harvard University
Department of Psychology
Cambridge, MA
University of California
Department of Anthropology
Center for Evolutionary Psychology
Santa Barbara, CA
University of Toronto
Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care
Rotman Research Institute
Ontario, Canada
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
xiii
List of Participants
xiv
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Section 1
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Section 2
Activities
Activities of the Center for Human Evolution include
providing funds for: studies in the field of human evolution, publishing the results of research in human
evolution, and hosting seminars and workshops concerning human evolution. Additional activities may
be determined by the Foundations Board of Trustees.
Workshops
The subject of workshops 1 and 2, held in November
1998 and February 1999, respectively, was The Evolution of Human Intelligence. Workshop 3 was held
in November 1999 and convened scholars to discuss
How Evolution Works. Workshop 4, on Cultural
Participants
Each workshop brings together six to ten scholars,
experts in fields related to the evolution topic designated for the workshop. Participation is by invitation
only. Researchers interested in participating in workshops on specific aspects of human evolution may
contact Sesh Velamoor, Deputy Director, Programs.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Section 3
Description of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain was the theme
of Center for Human Evolution Workshop 5. Eight
scholars were invited to participate, specifically
selected to allow for a solid, scientific look at the
human brain and its evolution, both past and future.
The invitees were selected by Walter Kistler, President
of the Foundation, and Sesh Velamoor, Deputy Director, Programs.
Each participant was asked to prepare a paper
for presentation and discussion at the workshop.
Abstracts of these papers were received sufficiently in
advance to allow time for distribution to all attendees
prior to the event. During the workshop, each participant was given approximately one hour to deliver his
presentation and respond to questions and comments
from the other attendees.
In addition to the eight scholars, three officers
of the Foundation For the Future and four outside
observers took part in the discussions.
Publication of Results
Transcripts of all presentations and discussions in
the workshop The Evolution of the Human Brain
are published in this document, which is available in
hard copy upon request to the Foundation as well as
downloadable from the Foundations website at http://
www.futurefoundation.org/programs_pub.htm.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Section 4
Workshop Transcripts
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Introductory Session
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
10
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Im a biological anthropologist Im
really interested in human variability,
particularly brain variability.
Deacon:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
11
12
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
ally part of the push, and so for the last 20 years Ive
also been paying a lot of attention to the paleoclimate
studies. It turns out that the glacially slow ice ages were
punctuated by hundreds of very fast flips big changes
like drought, but everywhere and they flip back even
faster into a warm and wet climate like todays.
In 1998, I was asked to write a cover story for The
Atlantic Monthly, which came out as The Great Climate Flip-flop. 1998 was a time when no one had yet
heard much about whats currently in the news and
in the disaster movies. The effect of flips on human
evolution is what my previous book was about, called
A Brain for All Seasons: Human Evolution and Abrupt
Climate Change.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
13
14
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Scholar Presentations
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
15
16
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
17
years. There is a substantial conservatism in toolmaking styles from the initial invention at 2.5 million years
ago to the first really major improvement at 1.7 million years ago. That isnt to say that the old style didnt
continue. The improvement clearly did not replace it.
Its interesting to note that bigger brains did not necessarily make them cleverer at toolmaking. In other
words: If bigger brains are cleverer, it doesnt really
show up very well in the first 2 million years of toolmaking, so maybe it is growing for some reason other
than toolmaking.
However, reorganization in a brain may have
something to do with it. Let me tell you what I mean
by reorganization. The temporal lobe houses categories in regions that are about objects for tool use,
animate/inanimate, and so on. But the visual area, the
V5 area, that is right in the middle of the temporal
lobe beneath the auditory specializations in monkeys,
has moved all the way back to the occipital parietal
junction in humans. If you have to reorganize something, as anybody who has had to reorganize an office
knows, its sure nice to have some surge space. That is
to say, its nice if you can increase something without
having to downsize something else simultaneously.
So, in any generation, the individuals who by chance
happened to have larger brain size were also the ones
where the reorganization would go more easily. So,
brain size in some sense could be an epiphenomenon
of reorganization. I like to put it this way: We might
be able to take our present brain size and downsize it
back to one-third, and as long we kept the same organization, it might function pretty similarly. I dont
know what size, per se, adds to anything yet.
Protolanguage is certainly one of the things that
brain reorganization might be better for. Protolanguage is the words and short sentences like a
two-year-old has. For short sentences, you dont need
syntax. You can get along pretty well without all the
clues of who the actor is, who the recipient is, and
so forth. Its not that hard a problem. But surely the
growth in sentence complexity would require some
18
brain reorganization.
Sharing is one of these things that, the more items
you share, the more people you share them with, over
longer periods of time all have a growth curve that
keeps on giving. Throwing accuracy is like this too.
No matter how good you are, getting twice as good
has additional payoffs in terms of days per month that
your family can eat a nutritious and relatively sterile source of food. So, all of these have great growth
curves, which is unlike a lot of things in evolution.
Once youve invented a carrying basket, its played a
very important role probably in human evolution, but
it doesnt have this kind of growth automatically.
Staged toolmaking is the idea that you make one
thing and then from it you make something else. For
example, if you create a ledge, you can come along and
strike it, shaving off single-edged razor blades. Thats
an example of what is meant by staged toolmaking.
Blades come in by about 280,000 years ago in Africa
and are well established by 120,000 years ago.
The time frame for the designation anatomically
modern was moved back a few weeks ago to 196,000
years ago instead of 165,000 or so. But there wasnt a
big step up in behavioral complexity then. Everybody,
I think, would agree that images the sort of thing
you see in cave paintings are behaviorally modern.
Certainly if the holes in the snail shells [referring to
slide] were well polished, you would be fairly convinced that these were used for necklaces. Theres also
a lot of red ocher found in various places that people
have argued represents a body decoration such as war
paint. Chimpanzees love to decorate themselves. I
dont think thats the issue. The issue is that its hard to
imagine apes sticking to a task like this long enough,
and that same argument is probably true for our
ancestors up to some point. If these were beads for
necklaces, it does bespeak a notion that humans by
that time had a capacity to maintain agendas, revisit
them, and update them, and so forth, in a way that
was perhaps lacking earlier.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
19
20
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
21
22
problem of quality control, because most of the combinations you make of things are nonsense. All the
elements have to hang together despite the combination being novel so that theres no exact memory of
it to go by. Most are nonsense. But we create quality
every time we speak a sentence weve never spoken
before. This is a routine, everyday occurrence that
even kids of low IQ can do.
Looking at intellect rather than language per
se shows us, first of all, that general cleverness and
creativity may be rather late 50,000 years ago, the
last one percent of post-ape, pre-sapiens evolution. I
like to think of it as something like a new operating
system for old hardware. That is to say: an ability to
handle various things at the same time in a way that
you werent doing very efficiently before.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
23
24
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
think this was going on, like Southern Africa, this was
a low point. Its not as if you can argue big populations.
Furthermore, the climate was constantly flipping
back and forth, so there were always big drought
downsizings in this period. About 16,00015,000
14,000 years ago, the climate was flipping every couple
of centuries. This was a very unstable situation. They
could build up a good population size in the warm
and wet, but then it is crashing back down.
Velamoor:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
25
26
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
27
28
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
more you recruit and the more you can integrate that
help, the better your processing can be.
Alfred Russel Wallace asked: How can you explain
the complexity of human cognition? It goes so far
beyond what you might imagine to be survival value.
The answer to this at the end of the 19th century was
posed by a number of researchers who made a kind
of end-run around Lamarckian-inherited stories to
talk about what is now called the Baldwin effect,
although it was actually discovered by three people
independently James Mark Baldwin, Lloyd Morgan,
and Henry Osborne in fact, all in the same year. The
argument was that you could acquire innate capacities
by virtue of your plasticity: If early-on you had relatively little innate support but a lot of trial-and-error
support for a behavioral adaptation or a phenotypic
adaptation that was simply a physiological adaptation
that had some plasticity to it, it would, in effect, shield
selection. So, individuals who could make it by their
plasticity and flexibility for transmitting information
generation to generation could, in effect, do so long
enough so that spontaneously variants could show up
in the population we would say mutants today. This
could eventually replace this clumsy way of doing it
with a more innate, more streamlined way of doing
it. Baldwin called it organic selection. It has come
to take on his name after a number of critics called it
the Baldwin effect in the 1950s.
A parallel idea Im going to show you that it is, in
fact, a very different idea was proposed in the 1950s
and 1960s by the geneticist Conrad Waddington. He
called it genetic assimilation. But Conrad Waddington didnt do it theoretically he showed it. We
now know what the genetics of his experiment was.
It turns out that although I have based a good part
of my argument on these views at least in my book
The Symbolic Species as I went back to review it, I
realized that they both dont work the way these men
thought they did. What Im going to tell you is some
of the evidence weve gathered to show how they dont
work and to show you that something else much more
interesting shows up.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
29
30
and innate trait; you have to have a very tight genotype/phenotype correlation, one-to-one; and you have
to have something like a hopeful-monster saltational
mutation. In other words, in one step you already get
an adaptive function. You dont work your way up to it,
because, in fact, selection has inhibited that process.
Pinker: There was a computer simulation of evolution
of neuronetworks by Geoffrey Hinton and Steven
Nowlan.
Deacon: Ill talk about the Hinton-Nowlan project
a little bit later because actually we re-ran it under
slightly different, more realistic conditions and it
failed. Ill tell you why.
Pinker: It seemed there was an extra condition there
that wasnt in your list, namely that acquisition of the
trait is not all or none that what could drive selection is how early in ontogeny you master it. So, even
if something is acquired, it could be acquired after
100 trials or after 50 trials or after 10 trials, and the
replacement of learned with innate structure moves
the age of acquisition earlier, with the assumption
that the sooner you get it, the sooner you enjoy the
adaptive benefits.
Deacon: Its part of the cost/benefit problem. In effect,
as you can move things earlier, you lose some of the
costs.
Pinker:
Right.
innate?
Deacon:
Thats right.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
31
32
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
33
34
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
35
Pinker:
36
Yes.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Pinker:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
37
Thats right.
38
engineering and natural selection, which is: Everything that has an effect on an output thats under
selection will come under selection. Throughout the
system, in any complex engineering system, that will
be a lot of things. This system will almost never have a
uniquely causally isolated, single solution that develops independently. Thats not the way any biological
system would look.
Deacon: Absolutely. And thats certainly what this
shows.
Another piece is that this is a challenge to aspects
of the modularity argument, in which you can get
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
39
40
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
41
42
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
One of the things you can do if you go to the comparative record is take a look at the marvelous dataset
that came out of Stephan et al. in 1981 and is still being
used. They had these brains; they got their overall
sizes; they had their body weights for something like
46 different species, including ourselves, of primates;
then they did the histological sectioning, and then
calculated the volumes of different parts of the brain,
and so forth. What Ive put up here is Logbase 10 of
striate cortex versus Logbase 10 of brain weight. We
have to use logs because scientists minds go only in
straight lines. This is the human point here. If you
dont use the human point and you get the regression line for this this being chimpanzee and gorilla
up here you end up with a correlation coefficient
of about .97 or .98. Its extremely high, as one would
expect from these kinds of allometry studies. If you
then ask: What would I expect, then, for a primate of
this brain size, if it were human; what would its striate
cortex be, based on this nonhuman regression line?
What you would come up with is that the regression
line is up here and this distance actually is 121 percent
less than would be expected, so the predicted volume
is 121 percent more, if I could put it that way.
I would suggest that when your predictions are off
by more than 100 percent, its time to think about it
and look at it carefully. I think thats a very important argument for the reduction, speaking relatively
and allometrically, of primary visual striate cortex in
humans. I would translate that, then, into a relative
increase of posterior parietal association cortex that
is, through time.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
43
and published it in the international journal Primatology. As I said, what you get is 121 percent, if you
use all of the 45 primate species. If you just use Old
World and apes, it comes down to 66.6 percent but
thats still a respectable amount. If youre doing the
lateral geniculate body, which, of course, relates to
primary visual striate cortex (area 17), its 146 percent
less than would be expected. If youre doing cerebellar
weights, 6.2 percent. Now, remember that this is log
data; confidence intervals are going in this direction,
narrowing toward the central part of the regression
line, and getting increasingly divergent at the ends of
the distribution. Consequently, 6 percent is possibly
meaningless in any significant statistical sense.
However, consider the diencephalon, which is 54
percent greater than expected, or the septum, which
is 20 percent greater than expected, or the amygdala,
which in humans is, for whatever reason, 53 percent
less than expected. I dont know what to say about that.
Im not sure that 53 percent is truly statistically significant. My intuitive feeling is that differences above
1520 percent should be examined more carefully as
possible candidates of examples of reorganization. I
would want to really look at the difference between
the amygdalas in great apes and humans, and explore
whether, for example, fear responses, or amgydaloid/
cerebrum interconnections play some functional role
in differentiating their respective fear responses.
[Referring to slide] This is not a fried potato or
dumpling. This is Australopithecus afarensis, AL16228, an endocast thats possibly around 3.2 million years
of age. Youre looking directly on the occipital surface
of this endocast these being the occipital poles right
here, the cerebellum line here. The anterior portion
is broken off. The main thing to look at is this groove
that you see here. In all great apes, the lunate sulcus
the anterior boundary of area 17 is always bounded by
the lunate sulcus. Coming down from the pre-central
sulcus to the lunate sulcus is the interparietal sulcus.
That always abuts posteriorly on the lunate sulcus in
great apes. Hopefully, this slide helps. John Gurche
made this rendition for me because the actual photographs are very difficult to interpret.
What you have here, then, is this groove we were
talking about being identified as an interparietal. If
it is interparietal and it is ending here and this is a
great ape pattern, then the lunate sulcus has to be
here. Dean Falks argument was, in fact, that this is
44
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
45
that were important early in hominid evolution, running from area 17, primary visual, which we think is
reduced to increases in asymmetries down in areas 44
and 45 in Brocas region.
Im going to suggest that these basic reorganizational changes took place in the evolution of the
human brain, starting with the reduction of primary
visual striate cortex with Australopithecus afarensis this taking place 3.5 to 3 million years ago, the
evidence being at least AL16228. Now I think you
could add Stw 505 to this and possibly Taung as well.
Somehow at the time of Homo habilis, perhaps 2 to
1.8 million years ago, or at least with early Homo,
youre getting reorganization of the frontal lobe. The
third inferior frontal convolution in Brocas area is
very distinct on KNM-1470, if I were to show you a
lateral view of it. It is identifiable as human and not
as pongid. We do know that there are cerebral asymmetries, left occipital/right frontal petalias probably
in Homo habilis. Certainly you see them in the 1470
endocast, but you find them in all subsequent Homo
erectus endocasts after that. There are three aspects
of cerebral reorganization. Here we have refinement
in cortical organization. I have no evidence for that. I
just assumed that there has been some of that.
its not just simply increase in brain
size that characterizes human brain
evolution. It isnt just reorganization
but theres an interdigitation
through time
46
Probably so.
Now were getting into directional pressures, and so forth. I would think that the pressures for
increasing posterior association cortex were stronger
than. Theres no evidence that reduction, relatively
speaking, in primary visual striate cortex would have
Holloway:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Lots of stories!
Thats 1861.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
47
loway.
48
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
49
50
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
dont have the structure of the brow ridges that Neanderthals do. You can see that she has a lot in the way
of forehead. Her skull is relatively short from fore to
rear, and high from, say, the ear aperture to the top.
His is very long from fore to rear, and relatively short
from here to here.
You can see that the face of this Neanderthal is
mounted way out in front of the rest of the skull, and
particularly the midline of the face, from between the
brow ridges or running from between just above the
nose down between the upper incisor teeth. If you
wanted to be a Neanderthal and your face was absolutely plastic, made of clay, what you would do is put
your fingers in your nose and pull out two inches and
everything would sweep back. The cheekbones would
sweep back. Hers come out this way and then go back,
which is, of course, the case in this room today.
Its a very interesting sort of replacement of the Neanderthals by modern humans in Europe. If you think
about European expansions in the age of exploration
Europeans going to Africa and Australia and the
Americas, and what have you wherever they went,
European culture became dominant. But there was a
lot of interbreeding. We sometimes say the Tasmanians are extinct, for example, but you can still detect
Tasmanian genes without any problem. You cant
detect Neanderthal genes, even in the earliest modern humans. So, that makes it a very different kind
of interaction between human populations of any we
have observed historically, and I think that requires a
very special kind of explanation. Why did these two
population groups fail to interact either culturally
and I can talk about that in more detail later or
biologically? Why was it such a complete replacement
without any interbreeding? When I say complete, if it
was at a very low level, say less than five percent, we
probably wouldnt be able to detect it, but right now
thats the level it would have to be at if there was interbreeding.
Now, where did modern humans come from? They
came from Africa. We have an abundant fossil record
to demonstrate this not, unfortunately, as abundant
as the record that demonstrates the evolution of the
Neanderthals, but we feel its still pretty good. Heres
a skull that is maybe as much as 160,000 years old.
Its certainly more than 50,000 years old. It comes
from Herto in Ethiopia. This is somebody who lived
in Ethiopia at the same time when the Neanderthals
were the sole occupants of Europe. This is essentially
a big, robust, modern skull. Theres nobody around
like this today, but you can see that the face is tucked
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
51
52
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
53
54
and when they arrived in the mail, they had holes like
that. So, I dont know: Maybe these are beads; thats
the interpretation. You can see how irregular the holes
are. They dont show any polish from being strung on
strings. Its possible that they are beads.
One of the things that must be true about being modern
is that new, novel behaviors if they increase the
likelihood of reproduction and survival
should spread.
Theres one thing that I think is critical if youre evaluating this. There are other things from Blombos Cave
that are less compelling than this; this is the most
compelling evidence for art or jewelry. This site is one
of 25 in South Africa that could be expected to show
something like this of how people before 50,000 years
ago were behaving, making art or jewelry in a way
that anticipates later people. But this is the only one
that does. One of the things that must be true about
being modern is that new, novel behaviors if they
enhance fitness, that is, if they increase the likelihood of reproduction and survival those behaviors
should spread. I think they should. In historic times,
and certainly in late prehistoric times, when you have
a major innovation, it spreads pretty quickly, if it, in
fact, allows people to have more kids and their kids to
have more kids. Why has this remained restricted to
this particular site for 25,000 or 30,000 years?
Tooby: Richard, are you saying that you dont think these
are beads that they just happened spontaneously?
Klein: Well, I dont know. You could never prove that
theyre not beads. I could say all kinds of things about
the site; Ive been involved in it.
Tooby: But is there any strong, confirmatory evidence
that they are intentionally manufactured?
Klein:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
55
fur seal that lives on this coast today that they hunted
and collected in large numbers; there are penguins that
were brought back in large numbers; and then there are
coastline birds cormorants, gulls, and so on.
Varki: Was the sea level going up and down? What
was the range?
Calvin: A hundred meters is by 22,000 years ago.
Thats the maximum of water being taken out. In that
period, youve got all these fluctuations, but the ice
doesnt follow them very fast.
Klein: There are a couple things. Its not just sea level
fluctuation. Its the nature of the African topography.
If its very steep, sea level can drop 50 to 60 meters and
the coastline wont be more than.
Varki: My question is: Could there be sites along the
east African coast that are now below sea level?
56
sites. Ive shown only the coastal ones here but there
are a lot of them up in here and they dont show what
Blombos does. They dont have all the things that are
said to be special that Blombos has.
[Referring to slide] This is the site, Ysterfontein,
that Im working now. It may not look like much.
This is a granite cliff here. This forms the base of what
was a rock shelter. When the road was widened here
going down to a small boat harbor, they cut into this
cliff face and truncated what was a rock shelter. Artifacts and shells started falling out and thats how we
discovered the site. This is about seven meters above
present sea level, so that means that it has to be older,
at least the occupation here has to be younger than
115,000 years 115,000 years ago, the sea would have
been lapping right at the base of this shelter. We have
a radio-carbon date from near the top of greater than
46,000 years.
[Referring to slides] Here are some of the shells, the
limpet shells in place. Down here is a fireplace. Theres
a whole stack of them in this site. Thats a common
thing, not only in Middle Stone Age sites in Africa
but in comparable or like-age sites in Europe. People
had full control over fire before 50,000 years ago, or
at least its hard to imagine that they could have built
fires so routinely if they didnt control them, if they
had to look for lightning strikes.
You can go too far in talking about behavioral differences between people before and after 50,000 years
ago. I dont want to brutalize the Neanderthals and
their African contemporaries. They would be very
interesting to have around today. They would be so
like us and, yet, maybe we wouldnt want them in
Harvard or maybe they couldnt survive there. Its
a fortunate thing, in a way, that theyre not around
because it would present a real ethical dilemma.
These are not chimpanzees, before 50,000 years ago.
Whatever they are, they are far more like us than
they are like chimpanzees. The difficulty is trying to
understand in any kind of detail what the nature of
this behavioral evolution really was. We see differ-
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
to put yourself at such great physical risk. So, something happened: Technology changed and it allowed
this change in the animal remains we find in sites.
Now, when you look at Middle Stone Age sites right
before 50,000 years ago, you can tell what season of
the year the site was occupied. In this environment on
the South African coast, it would have been better to
be at the coast in the winter and inland in the summer.
Plant resources are abundant in the summer. Baby
seals, in particular, are abundant on the coast in the
winter. So, you should have a kind of seasonal round.
This is characteristic of all historic hunger-gatherers:
They recognize the different seasons when they could
exploit different resources in different places, and they
moved around. In the Middle Stone Age, it doesnt
look as though they did that. They kept coming back
to these coastal sites. I am not saying that they were
there straight through the year but they didnt focus
on them seasonally. In the Later Stone Age, they were
there at exactly the right time to harvest baby seals.
Theres a particular time of year between August and
October when you can do that.
Why might Middle Stone Age people have been
more restricted in their movements? Well, maybe they
lacked water containers. We have lots of evidence that
Later Stone Age people had them. They made these
canteens out of ostrich eggshells. We find them abundantly in Later Stone Age sites. We dont find them in
Middle Stone Age sites.
The final point is that you can look at certain animal remains tortoises and shellfish, in particular
and you can get an idea of the average size that was
being exploited. When you look at the Middle Stone
Age tortoises and shellfish, theyre absolutely huge.
[Referring to slides] This is a limpet, Patella granatina. Here are a bunch of Later Stone Age samples
and Middle Stone Age samples. You can see that the
Middle Stone Age ones are larger. When gray bars
dont overlap between samples, it means that theyre
statistically significantly different. Sometimes these
Middle Stone Age samples are relatively small so the
gray bar, which is an estimation error in the median,
is very wide. But the Middle Stone Age shells are very
much larger.
I took a bunch of students a long time ago to rocks
that are not being exploited today, and said, Go out
there, pick up as many of these shells as you can in
ten minutes. You can do this without doing any real
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
57
58
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
59
Well, we have the same kind of environmental change that happened, lets say 11,000 years ago,
many, many times before. Roughly the same thing
happened, lets say, 128,000 years ago but no agriculture. I just dont think the capacity to do it was
there. I think whatever was involved in inventing
something like agriculture was a change 40,000 years
ago. Whatever you want to say that change was, that
was the basis for it and everything that follows. I dont
believe that there is an industrial revolution gene or a
computer gene obviously thats not right.
You can look at it a lot of different ways, because
you can take somebody who was born in the Australian outback, an Aboriginal, and, if you get them early
enough, you bring them to Bellevue, WA, or Cambridge, MA, or whatever, they are going to become
just like anybody else there, in terms of speaking English without an accent, if theyre brought early enough;
theyre going to have the same capacity to become
computer programmers, or whatever. To me, that
rules out the possibility that there are special genes
for agriculture or whatever since hunter-gatherers
can become agriculturists or computer programmers
without any problem.
Everything you say could be true but whatever the genetic change is for clearly not agriculture
or computer programming it still could be 200 or
500 genes as opposed to one gene.
Varki:
Pinker:
60
Varki: No. The error bars are big, but you dont see a
lot of means that are 30,000 40,000 50,000. You
tend to see things at 100,000 150,000 200,000.
Klein:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
61
62
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Holloway: You dont have any art; you dont have any
burials; you dont have any red ochre.
Holloway: I was going to add the Australian Aborigines because their social systems have wracked modern
anthropological brains despite some of the most miserable stone tools that you have ever found.
Bill [Calvin] showed his slide with the Greenland ice sheet fluctuations in temperature, which
were pretty dramatic. Its been argued that those
things caused the Neanderthals to become extinct.
Some of those fluctuations, one way or the other,
occurred within a human lifetime. Its hard to imagine people adjusting, but they continued after the
Cro-Magnons appeared and if they had an effect on
the Cro-Magnons, its not clear that it was different
from the effect it had on the Neanderthals. It all looks
the same.
Most of what we call Neanderthal sites are as much
carnivore sites, places where cave bears were living, as
they were human-occupation sites. Thats true of European sites before 40,000 years ago. Then suddenly at
40,000 years you get these modern humans appearing. The cave bear becomes extinct and the sites are
just chock-full of stuff. You no longer get any indication that the people are competing with carnivores for
their living sites. Something very dramatic happened
and it might be that the climate is somehow implicated, but then why 40,000 years ago just at the time
when modern humans appeared? Those rapid fluctuations in temperature were occurring before that.
They had a particular impact on the North Atlantic
region. It seems to me that it would be a remarkable
coincidence to have the Neanderthals disappear just
as modern humans were invading Europe, if climate
was the reason.
Klein:
Holloway: I would have no doubt that they were outcompeted by modern Homo sapiens. I think thats
clear, but the reasons why they were out-competed
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
63
64
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
about in the last five or ten years. Its ethically controversial. There have been a number of jeremiads
against it from Francis Fukuyama, Michael Sandel,
Leon Kass, and other bioethicists. But there also have
been authors welcoming it: Gregory Stock, a past winner of the Kistler Book Award, in fact, and others.
But the common assumption among people who
dread designer babies and people who welcome them
is that it is inevitable, that if current progress in genetics continues, designer babies are around the corner,
leading also, at least among the people who think this
is a bad idea, to the idea that we should intervene and
regulate now, pass laws against genetic enhancement,
perhaps curtail research in human reproductive physiology, in human genetics, and genetic engineering in
general, because left to its own devices it would lead to
germline enhancement, and since thats such a terrible
thing, we had better stop it in its tracks right now.
Im going to challenge the common assumption underlying both the fans and critics of designer babies and
suggest that the genetic enhancement of human nature
is not inevitable. In fact, I think its highly unlikely
in our lifetimes. Its foolish to say that anything will
never happen but one could look at the current state
of the technology and make some guesses. One can
certainly challenge the idea that something is inevitable. Why? Just to give you a preview: the fallibility
of predictions about complex technology in general,
impediments from the science of behavioral genetics,
and impediments from human nature itself.
Let me start off with some of the limitations of predictions about our inevitable future. Heres a quote,
Fifty years hence we shall escape the absurdity of
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
65
66
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
67
Ive read that there is a condition called torsion dystonia caused by a single gene that on average gives
a 10-point boost in IQ but also a 10 percent chance
of being confined to a wheelchair with uncontrollable
muscle spasms. There isnt a lot published on this but
I have heard, at least informally, that most genes that
are associated with higher IQ are also associated with
some enhanced susceptibility to various neurological
disorders.
This combines with another problem, namely, the
impediments to actually doing the research in this area
that would get us from here to there. How could one
try out possible genetic enhancements on an experimental basis, given the risk of a deformed child? Can
anyone even imagine a path toward common use of
genetic enhancement, given that the research is pretty
much undoable?
Another genetic impediment is that most traits
are desirable at intermediate values. Wallis Simpson
famously said you cant be too rich or too thin, but
most traits arent like that. Maybe you cant be too
smart but lets take something like self-confidence
or assertiveness. You dont want a shrinking violet
for your child, but you dont want Vlad the Impaler
either. You want some intermediate level of assertiveness or aggressiveness. Likewise, conscientiousness:
You dont want a psychopath for a child, but you also
dont want children who are so self-abnegating that
they cant do anything in their own interests. You
dont want a child who is completely averse to taking
a risk, who just cowers under a desk the whole time,
68
but you dont want a candidate for the so-called Darwin Awards either.
So, lets say that you had a gene, even hypothetically,
that would enhance risk-taking as a personality trait.
What would be desirable in your child would depend
on what the other 19,999 genes are doing. That is, are
they placing your child below the mean or above the
mean or at the mean? Until we know what all of the
genes are doing, its not clear that any gene that simply
increments a trait is going to be desirable.
The third part is that I think there may be strong
impediments in human nature to enhancing human
nature. In parental psychology, its always pointed out
in these discussions that parents, especially in this
culture of young, urban professionals, would stop at
nothing to help their children. If they drive them to
umpteen lessons after every day of school, arrange
play dates, play Mozart to their pregnant bellies, play
Baby Einstein videos, what would stop them from
inserting genes that would enhance IQ, musical talent, tennis skill, and so on? Well, its true that there
is a strong desire to help children, but there is also, I
would think, a much stronger aversion to harm children and that is a force pulling in the other direction,
given that the effects of genetic enhancement would
not be certain.
If its a hard sell to get people to accept
genetically modified soybeans, it might be
an even harder sell to get them to accept
genetically modified children.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
the contrary. I think that the biggest threat to beneficial technological change is the mindset that would
say that as soon as we start on that path, there will be
a juggernaut, a runaway freight train that will lead us
to horrific ends, so we had better stop it now. We had
better stop the runaway freight train before it gains
momentum. We had better nip this in the bud, which
is definitely the argument of people like Leon Kass
and Francis Fukuyama in trying to curtail progress in
biomedical research. Their position is that the costs
of designer babies ethically would be so huge that we
have to shut down the enterprise now, have a moratorium for five years until we understand it or ten
years before we let it get going again with, I think,
quite disastrous consequences for human health and
human well-being, given the possible benefits of this
research.
The state of ignorance is so huge that any decision you
make right now is bound to be the wrong one but
we shouldnt cut off a whole branch of inquiry based on
assumptions that the worst case is the inevitable case.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
69
70
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Varki:
One of the real challenges is that were talking about the human genome. Lets say that the free
market keeps all but .001 percent of the population
from fiddling with their childrens genomes. Those
genes are in the population. Those genes will proliferate in future generations. The question is really an
open end. We have to be more careful than to just let
it sort itself out. As much as it is right that it will work
that way, this is not just somebodys kids. This is the
genome of the species.
Deacon:
Velamoor: So, it comes down to a question of information and more information and more information,
unbiased as opposed to simply casting prohibitions
by those who presume to know.
Calvin: Just look at how effective information has
been in influencing the people that utilize untried
medical treatments by buying all their drugs in the
health food stores. People will have enthusiasms for
doing things that are irrational by other standards and
theyll try them out. Now, this might not be putting a
particular gene in or taking it out, but it is going to
help bias the population, after a while, because there
are going to be a lot of things you take that will turn
out to produce, for example, more spontaneous abortions in certain gene types than others. You can get
shifts of population out of much more subtle effects.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
71
72
Varki:
dies.
Tooby: What would you need to do to be able to ethically do this kind of thing? It would mean that you
would have to have an inventory with all the functional
interactions and systems in human development and
the brain at such a level that you could do predictive
computations. You could predict in advance the effect
of the intervention and all the costs and benefits on all
the different systems. Thats a kind of science that is a
millennium away, or 500 years away. It certainly falls
beyond anything we have on the horizon.
Regarding what you were saying about why predictions dont pan out, the reason is the rise of
competitive things that are cheaper. With changing
human nature, you have to control the developmental
process throughout the life cycle, which means computations with all the interactions. Computationally,
that is just immensely intricate.
On the other hand, making an intervention in an
adult is something where you dont have to deal with
all the developmental complexity of that. Something
like installing a chip in the brain to have higher intelligence, which is something that is not so far away
whether you could have a chip that would increase
all types of intellectual activity, thats a much more
complicated thing, but having some sort of direct
sensory feed into the Internet or something like that
Im just saying that there are all sorts of installation
technologies that dont require such dauntingly com-
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
To the extent that human nature is controlled by our interactions with the environment,
it is possible to modify human nature with genetic
manipulation of other creatures, starting, of course,
with agriculture and animals, but even designer pets
and strange things like that.
Observer:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
73
Pinker:
Yes.
74
Yes.
Velamoor:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
75
76
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
77
78
There are the two inputs of the co-residence monitoring system and the maternal perinatal association
monitoring system, and we believe there are possibly other things. Some people have speculated about
MHC monitoring from olfaction, but things like that
cant be all that strong, from our dataset, because we
have people in our datasets who are adopted people.
The Kinship Index operates independent of your formal, declarative beliefs because the emotions track
these things like how long you lived with somebody
when you were a child and did you observe them
breastfeeding or taking care of your younger sibling?
Those are the things that govern the intensity of the
emotion and not formal beliefs about whether they
think the person is a sibling or not. That does not control or predict the intensity of these emotions.
So, we have these inputs, which go into something
that integrates them, the Kinship Estimator. The Kinship Estimator produces a Kinship Index for person I
I to my brother, or my sister. Then there are two output systems. We all have a sexual psychology, and its a
complicated one, so there are things like attractiveness
and all sorts of other things sexual value or how
attractive somebody is to you. But one of the things
that affect it is the Kinship Index: Did you receive cues
when you were growing up that they are related to you?
Not any cue at all that theyre related to you? There are
certain ones that are evolutionarily stable, long-term
cues. That has a negative effect on the Sexual Value
Estimator, and the Sexual Value Estimator is passed
out to the programs that guide sexual attraction and
sexual avoidance. People who were raised with a person or who had a period when they saw their mother
taking care of a person, even if they were subsequently
separated from that child, the idea is that when they
are adults, the notion of having sex with that person is
disgusting to them. Its at least uninteresting and, more
often, its positively repugnant.
Theres a separate system that has to do with how
much you are predisposed to spontaneously trade off
your welfare for somebody elses. Thats a variable that
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The question is: How come people see race in the world
when the underlying biology doesnt support it?
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
79
80
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
we know when race is a dimension of their categorization and when its not.
If they make an error and they attribute it to somebody else of the same race, that tells us that they do it
more often than chance, and that tells us that race was
a dimension of variation. On the other hand, if the
pictures show people in different jerseys of basketball
teams, then in this scenario the person might be categorizing by team and not race. So, we can get a measure
of the relative intensity of their disposition to make
categorization errors to categorize people by race, by
gender, by team, by a number of different things.
The underlying idea is: What coalitions is this
person a member of, so that the mind automatically
picks up this information? Theres a variable assigned
to a personal representation that indexes the persons
coalition membership. Theres a specialized coalition-mapping device that infers coalitional alliances.
It sits there in the world, in your mind youre not
aware of it and in the social world it detects acts of
cooperation and acts of conflict. It also sifts and sees if
there are some predictor values. For example, if youre
going to be a well-defined hunter-gatherer, you have
to know: If Im going to propose this in this group, can I
get away with it? Will enough people come in on my side
of the issue? You have to have a map of the social world
in which you can anticipate the likely consequences of
various types of activities, and who is going to come in
on what side of what issue is very important.
was a proxy for coalitional affiliation, then if you created a social world in which coalitional affiliation
no longer maps onto racial affiliation, then subjects
should stop encoding race. The notion is that you
could reduce it, and potentially, if you understood it
enough, maybe eliminate it.
We created an experiment in which the people are
racially mixed, but the conflict that emerges in this
group that theyre seeing the story of doesnt map
onto a racial division. Its two different sports teams.
We didnt know whether we would detect any effect
at all, but in the course of four minutes of this, a very
short period of time, we were quite astonished that the
effects were so big, particularly because theres prior
history among social psychologists: Theyve never
been able to find anything that decreased racial coding at all. But if you put people into this environment
where people are having a dispute but the dispute
doesnt map onto racial alliance, what you find out is
that within that context, subjects seriously reduce the
degree to which they encode race of subjects, so that
they will no longer be mistaking what one person said
for what another person said by race. Racial awareness or race consciousness, as an organizing feature
of personal representation, diminishes.
So, we have a very rich, complexly structured piece
of machinery. When you add the shared appearance
cue, which predicts coalitional alliance, you create an
artificial model race. People spontaneously start to
encode that and the degree to which they encode race
goes down. We created this artificial world, only four
minutes long; it was a striking effect and we now replicate it in a lot of different contexts. The underlying
point Im trying to make is that there are a lot of these
widgets in there. You can find out their properties by
modeling adaptive problems carefully.
Varki: In terms of the group that youre defining as
the ancestral state, if youre studying things that are
universal to human population, I suppose youre
defining this as being some time period before 50,000
or 60,000 years ago, before people started out across
the world. In other words, when you said hunter-gatherers, were you using that as a proxy for what we think
that people were doing between 100,000 and 50,000
years ago? If youre dealing with things that you think
were selected for that would be the same in an Australian Aborigine and a Native American, then you
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
81
82
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
My question was about the broader methodology. In some sense, this is a positive fishing in
which you pick out a couple of things that are your
best guess, then you plot them against your best-guess
competitors.
Deacon:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
83
84
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
What Is Future?
Presentation by Endel Tulving, Ph.D.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
85
86
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The bottom line, then: This hypothesized, hypothetical entity called future that does not exist in the
physical world but it does exist in the more important world of human consciousness. As such, it is very
much a part of reality. It is made possible by chronesthesia, a kind of consciousness that only humans
possess. Without this chronesthetic conciousness, the
future could not exist, nor would culture. Im happy
to tell Walter Kistler that your Foundation owes its
existence, among other things, to something you did
not even know about: chronesthesia.
When I figured out that future plays an important role in shaping human affairs, I sat down one
day and wrote down things that human beings do in
their organized activities. I ended up with a very long
list, organized under category names such as education, science, art, literature, religion, communication,
construction, trade, commerce, banking, and many
others like these. I asked, What do all these activities require? What are things without which none of
those activities could occur? The answer is that they
all require large brains and intelligence and creativity
and inventiveness and abstract problem-solving activ-
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
87
88
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
89
Forgetting is one of these natural phenomena. It also makes possible the study of memory by
psychologists. If there is no forgetting, then psychologists have no business saying anything about
memory.
Tulving:
No.
these phenomena, I think, exist
in warm-blooded animals, but what
needs to be explained is how it got
enormously amplified
Tulving: There is something called episodic-like memory. I have been involved watching it and supporting
90
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
91
Tulving: You are right, but note that you are talking
about mental activity and its contents. You are talking
about the message, and here I have been talking about
the medium. Youre talking about ships and voyages
that the ships are on. Im talking about the water on
which these ships float. Voyages are important, and
water makes them possible; thinking about your own
future (and your own past) is important, and chronesthesia makes it possible.
Tulving:
92
Good point.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
93
94
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
95
96
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
97
98
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The genome, of course, is a diploid set of chromosomes. We know exactly what the genome is. We
know how to go about defining it. A few years ago we
published an article in Science suggesting that while
we knew so much about humans, we needed to know
more about chimpanzees and great apes, and we suggested the term phenome. Its still not in any dictionary
but I just checked this morning and there are 113,000
entries in Google for phenome. Its now being used in
many different ways, but our original definition is:
complete information about an organisms phenotype and relevant environmental influences. Here is
the problem we face: We have the human genome at
a 10X coverage. All that remains to be done is to find
the differences between individuals, mostly. And we
have a huge amount of knowledge about the human
phenome. Over the millennia, weve accumulated so
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
99
100
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
101
junk DNA into account, what are the risks of the same
argument being co-opted by Intelligent Design?
I realize that its an inadequate position, but I
dont stay awake at night worrying about the Intelligent Design people. Sometime maybe in my
lifetime, I hope they will go away.
Varki:
Velamoor:
Varki: I have this view that bad ideas reach a crescendo before they collapse and right now we are
facing a huge crescendo of fundamentalism in all
areas. I think thats the prediction of a collapse. The
question is when it happens, and hopefully it happens
soon. The fact is that we shouldnt go exactly by numbers and percentages; we should go by actual genes.
One of the things the Genome Project has found
is that there are so many differences, they dont know
where to start. Meanwhile, we had a system of 55
genes and we had found eight that were different.
Then we found a few additional differences. It turns
out we found most of them just by nosing around in
a gene-centric way. So, what should happen is people
should pursue their little areas and make inroads into
the genome as a resource, and thats how it will happen, I think.
102
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
103
104
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Following the individual presentations on Day 1, participants returned the following day to focus their
attention on group discussions of critical themes related
to the evolution of the human brain. Rather than adhere
to a set list of questions, the scholars chose to engage in
an open-ended discussion on emerging knowledge.
Varki:
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
105
106
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
son adopts his adult identity. Its when they get a lot
of their values, and when peer-to-peer influences are
likely to be much more significant than parent-tochild. But, again, its an empirical question.
As a sign of how important it is, the fact that weve
looked only at one possible solution probably the
wrong solution suggests that this is something that
we need more research in. In particular, the fact that
an enormous amount of variation in intelligence
and personality is not predictable by either genes or
families (up to half the variation) means that theres
an enormous set of causal processes that we have no
understanding of. I suspect it is some kind of small or
random events in either brain development or early
lifelong experience, for that matter. A broader version
of Question 2 is: What makes us what we are? And
not prejudging it by saying that its neo- or postnatal,
but to find out when in life and from what sources
in life we turn into what we are that is a significant
question.
I think Question 3 is interesting but I think we
should remember that the kind of people around this
table are probably not the best ones to address it, but
rather social historians. There is a lot of interesting
data from experimental societies that were set up
in the late 19th and middle of the 20th century. The
Kibbutz; the socialist societies of the Soviet Union,
China, Cambodia; the various utopian rural communities in the United States; the Oneida community;
and so on. There are very interesting social histories
on how these attempts at reforming human nature
ran up against human nature, which provides a lot of
insight to Question 3.
Question 4: Johns point about whether the kind of
science that many of us think ought to be done will
be unacceptable for political and emotional reasons,
I think, is a very significant issue. Bill mentioned creationism. Many of us around the table in the corridors
and over dinner have talked about postmodernism
within academia as a significant barrier to biological anthropology. The fact that both the right and the
left, large segments of them, would rather see this not
happen is something we have to deal with, which is
why I wrote The Blank Slate.
I would like to add another issue that is perhaps a
sub-issue of Question 3: How flexible are we? Another
way of looking at it is: We know that there has to be
some degree of flexibility because there are cross-cul-
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
107
108
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
109
My point here is that I will be quiet during your discussions. Ill let you change your world. But theres only
one thing that I want you to keep in mind and this
is my contribution to that list there. You have overlooked one of the most basic facts about humanity
and biology in general, and I wish that in your discussions you would at least think about it. The problem
is what we could call individual differences, variety,
Darwinian diversity, the one fact about nature that
drives all of evolution, everything. One of the most
important problems you dont have to have a Ph.D.
in anything to realize it is that theres a tremendous
variety of individuals in every imaginable way, right
here in this room, and this is a highly selected, small
segment of society. Just go outside: in northern Wash-
110
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
111
112
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
113
114
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
115
116
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
117
118
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
119
120
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
121
We have, over the course of the evolution of cultures, developed remarkable tools for educating the
emotions, educating empathy. I think of literature as
a part of that process, of helping us figure out what
its like to be in another mind, in another world, in
another time, in another circumstance. I think those
are incredibly important pieces of the story. I dont
think that all ethical education comes from religion
or from some sort of humanistic explanation of why
we should do things. Most of it, in fact, comes from
the stories we tell or hear or exchange, or the movies
we watch, for that matter.
A very important part of the future of the mind or
brain however you want to look at it, and whether
it is collective or individual doesnt matter in this
story is that we have to spend considerable effort
at understanding how we do this. We have developed
incredible tools for this, but we really just use them
at random. Now, maybe thats the right way; its the
evolutionary way, to some extent. Those that stick
around, stick around, but maybe not always for the
right reasons.
It does seem to me that one of the things that we
need to think about even those of us who are not
artists and literary folk is the role of that. Its a very
big part of our society, a very big part of our world, as
big as religion in some ways. Yet even in the educational system, there is a pretense that it doesnt exist
or that it plays a trivial, fun role just to entertain. I
actually dont think that arts and literature and music
and plays are about entertainment. I think in the
real world theyre about educating the emotions and
empathy. I think its an important part of the future.
Klein: What I hear makes great sense to me. I think
that if there were people sitting in the room whose
backgrounds were in academic economics or political science, they would say that the issues that face
humanity have nothing to do with the brain, really.
Its interesting to learn a lot more about the brain, but
youre being overly reductionist. We have to understand precisely how people operate in groups thats
the only thing that really matters. You want to solve a
problem like global warming or the possibility of antibiotic resistance in lots of bacteria that kind of stuff.
Velamoor: Would everyone agree that an understanding of how groups function or behave is a priority?
122
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
123
124
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
just one aspect of it. There is the possibility that different populations differ somewhat to minor degrees in
the speed at which those processes are completed. It
might be interesting to think along those terms without getting too reductionist about it.
One of the other fragments of thought I had was the
recent tsunami phenomenon, because this provides,
in a sense, an absolute laboratory for ethnic responses
to tragedy and disaster that seem to me, from what
I read in the popular press, are really extremely different responses. There are two aspects to it: Now we
are trying to get a worldwide tsunami system that can
predict or give us warning. When you think about the
fact that there have been tsunamis and earthquakes
and all the rest of it and there has not been this ability already in place I find it mind-boggling that it is
now at 2005 and politicians are still arguing as to how
to set this thing up.
Then there are what we read about the differences
between the Tamils in Sri Lanka and their responses
to the distribution of aid, and how the political processes could impede the easing of terrible conditions
among millions and millions of people. You have the
same thing in Sumatra. Then we read about the Thais
and their ability to sacrifice to provide help for those
in their country. I imagine that there are all sorts of
different explanations for it, but it seems to me that
we could look at this as a natural laboratory showing
how the mind elucidates some aspects about human
differences and human similarities.
What are the consequences insofar
as our inherent natures and our
capacities? Are we coming up
against the limits of our flexibility
relative to the pace?
Velamoor: It seems to me that what Paula described
is a case of a child in fast-forward. The child may have
been 18, but perhaps in a mental sense he has lived a
lifetime. Maybe a proposition that we could entertain
is that we, even though we have gone past age 18 in a
civilizational sense, are also in a fast-forward mode.
What are the consequences insofar as our inherent
natures and our capacities? Can we say that we are in
a fast-forward mode, hastening the process to what
the future holds? Are we coming up against the limits
of our flexibility relative to the pace?
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
125
Calvin:
126
Varki: Youre saying that theres a significant dependence on that external input even at that point.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Yes.
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
127
128
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
129
130
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Closing Session
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
131
132
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
133
134
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
shop?
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
135
136
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Appendix 1
Workshop Agenda
March 19, 2005
Welcome
Walter Kistler, President
William H. Calvin
Steven Pinker
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
137
138
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Appendix 2
Participant Biographies
William H. Calvin . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141
Terrence Deacon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141
Ralph L. Holloway . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 142
Richard G. Klein . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 142
Steven Pinker . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143
John Tooby . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143
Endel Tulving . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144
Ajit Varki . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
139
140
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
William H. Calvin
Terrence Deacon
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
141
Ralph L. Holloway
Richard G. Klein
142
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
Steven Pinker
John Tooby
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5
143
Endel Tulving
Ajit Varki
144
The Evolution of the Human Brain | Center for Human Evolution Proceedings of Workshop 5