546f2ba60cf216f8cfa94f21 PDF
546f2ba60cf216f8cfa94f21 PDF
546f2ba60cf216f8cfa94f21 PDF
discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/9081998
CITATIONS
READS
89
318
21 authors, including:
Sonja L Connor
Kathleen M. Gustafson
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE
Lynn T Singer
Vance Zemon
Yeshiva University
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE
Patricia Mena, Martha Neuringer, Gail Ross, Lynn Singer, Terence Stephenson,
Joanne Szabo, and ***Vance Zemon
*University of Toronto and The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Canada; Ross Products Div, Abbott Labs, Columbus,
Ohio; The Childrens Mercy Hospital, Kansas City, Missouri; University of Louisville & Kosair Childrens Hospital,
Louisville, Kentucky; INTA Univ de Chile, Santiago, Chile; Oregon Health & Sciences University, Portland, Oregon;
#MetroHealth Medical Center, Cleveland, Ohio; **Hunter College, New York, New York; Institute of Child Health, London,
United Kingdom; Weill Medical College, Cornell University, New York, New York; Rainbow Babies & Childrens Hospital,
Cleveland, Ohio; University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom; Arkansas Childrens Hospital, Little Rock,
Arkansas; ***Yeshiva University, Bronx, New York; on behalf of The Ross Preterm Lipid Study Group: R. Carroll and B. Meyer
(The Childrens Mercy Hospital); P. Radmacher and S. Rafail (Kosair Childrens Hospital); A. Blanco Gomez (INTA Univ de
Chile); P. Fisher and S. Escoe (Oregon Health & Sciences University); R. Arendt and M. Davillier (Rainbow Babies &
Childrens Hospital); K. Kennedy (Institute of Child Health); J. Putis (Leeds General Infirmary); S. Newell (St. James Hospital,
Leeds); S. Carlisle (Arkansas Childrens Hospital); C. Broestl, C. Downs, Q. Liang, P. Pollack, W. Qiu, and D. Smart (Ross
Products Division); J. Deeks, S. Sullivan, R. Tressler (Abbott Labs); S. Buckley (Yeshiva University); J. Gordon and L.
Garcia-Quispe (Hunter College); and D. Pinchasik and M. Nesin (Weill Medical College).
ABSTRACT
Background: In a recent meta-analysis, human milk feeding of
low birth-weight (LBW) infants was associated with a 5.2 point
improvement in IQ tests. However, in the studies in this metaanalysis, feeding regimens were used (unfortified human milk,
term formula) that no longer represent recommended practice.
Objective: To compare the growth, in-hospital feeding tolerance, morbidity, and development (cognitive, motor, visual,
and language) of LBW infants fed different amounts of human
milk until term chronologic age (CA) with those of LBW infants fed nutrient-enriched formulas from first enteral feeding.
Methods: The data in this study were collected in a previous
randomized controlled trial assessing the benefit of supplementing nutrient-enriched formulas for LBW infants with arachidonic acid and docosahexaenoic acid. Infants (n 463,
birth weight, 7501,800 g) were enrolled from nurseries located in Chile, the United Kingdom, and the United States. If
Received: September 5, 2002; revised: March 14, 2003; accepted: April 9, 2003.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Deborah L. OConnor, Associate Professor, Department of Nutritional Sciences, University of
Toronto, Senior Associate Scientist, The Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Canada, M5G 1X8 (e-mail:
Deborah_l.oconnor@sickkids.ca).
437
438
OCONNOR ET AL.
date that directly examine the development of LBW infants fed fortified human milk compared with specially
designed nutrient-enriched formulas during and after
hospital discharge.
The purpose of this article is to compare the growth,
in-hospital feeding tolerance, morbidity, and development at 14 months CA of LBW infants fed different
amounts of human milk until term-corrected age (CA)
and then fed nutrient-enriched formulas until 12 months
CA when weaned with those of LBW infants fed
nutrient-enriched formulas from first enteral feeding until 12 months CA.
439
Demographic data
Neonatal, perinatal, and family characteristics of enrolled
infants were obtained from medical records or parental report
as described previously (9). The HOME Inventory (10) and the
vocabulary subtest of the WAIS-R (11) administered to the
biologic mother (if living in the home) were obtained to assess
the quality and quantity of cognitive, social, and emotional
support available to each infant in the home environment and
maternal intelligence, respectively.
Statistical methods
Duration and exclusivity of human milk feeding
Human milk and formula intake data were obtained from
flow sheets during initial hospitalization and from 3-day dietary
440
OCONNOR ET AL.
were dropped from follow-up for any reason other than parental
withdrawal of consent, infant death, or change in family residence precluding follow-up (intent-to-treat analysis). Categorical variables were analyzed using 2 or Cochran-MantelHaenszel tests. Continuous variables were evaluated using
analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). Data obtained at more than
one time point were analyzed using repeated-measures analyses
(SAS PROC MIXED, Cary, NC) (15). In addition, analyses
of continuous outcome variables considered site, randomization
strata (gender and birth weight group [7501,250 g or 1,251
1,800 g]), and the formula assignment to which the infant was
originally randomized as preplanned covariates. For the growth
analyses, the infants size for gestational age (either
appropriate-for-gestational age [AGA] or small-for-gestational
age [SGA]) were included as additional preplanned covariates.
For developmental outcomes, additional preplanned covariates
included size for gestation, gestational age, HOME Inventory,
the vocabulary subtest of the maternal WAIS-R, prenatal smoking, and in-home smoking at hospital discharge. Because of its
previously reported influence on development, chronic lung
disease (yes/no) was included as a covariate in developmental
analyses (1618). Weight at 2 months corrected age was added
to these analyses to serve as a proxy for growth after hospital
discharge (only three infants remained in the hospital at this
time). Because of the small numbers of infants, the Little Rock
(n 24) and New York sites (n 16) were treated as a single
site in the statistical analysis.
Because of the large number of preplanned covariates for
development outcomes, a backward step-wise approach was
used to produce a reduced statistical model. In this approach,
covariates or factors, except the patients feeding group, with a
P value 0.15 were excluded sequentially until a final step
was reached, with only feeding group and covariates with a P
value < 0.15 remaining. Unless there were differences between
the full and reduced models with respect to the presence or
absence of a feeding group effect on developmental outcomes,
results for only the reduced models are presented. All statistical
tests of hypotheses were two-tailed with P 0.05 for main
effects and P 0.10 for interaction effects.
RESULTS
Duration and exclusivity of human milk feeding
Eighty-one percent of the 463 infants completed the
study to 12 months CA. Ninety percent of all infants
enrolled in the study remained on human milk and/or the
assigned study formula feeding until term CA. At 12
months CA, 68% of infants remained on human milk or
the assigned study formula. Forty-three (9%) infants
were predominantly human milk fed until term CA
(PHM-T, human milk feeding group 1). Ninety-eight
(21%) infants were fed 50% of total energy during
initial hospitalization from human milk ( 50% HM,
human milk feeding group 2). Two hundred three (44%)
infants were fed < 50% of total energy during initial
hospitalization from human milk (< 50% HM, human
milk feeding group 3), and 119 (26%) infants were predominantly formula fed until term CA (PFF-T, human
milk feeding group 4). Of the 232 mothers providing any
441
PHM-T
50% HM
<50% HM
PFF-T
7 (16)
36 (84)
11 (11)
87 (89)
13 (6)
190 (94)
13 (11)
106 (89)
20 (47)
23 (53)
58 (59)
40 (41)
114 (56)
89 (44)
59 (50)
60 (50)
39 (91)
4 (9)
8.3 1.2 (43)
81 (83)
17 (17)
8.2 1.0 (97)
160 (79)
43 (21)
8.0 1.4 (202)
87 (73)
32 (27)
7.9 1.5 (119)
41 (95)
2 (5)
0
0
5.5 2.5 (43)
64 (65)
10 (10)
9 (9)
15 (15)
5.1 3.3 (98)
110 (54)
31 (15)
44 (22)
18 (9)
5.0 3.3 (203)
71 (60)
28 (24)
7 (6)
13 (11)
5.0 3.0 (119)
* Values are mean SD (number of participants) unless otherwise noted. Unlike superscript letters within each row denote statistically significant
differences.
A statistically significant human milk feeding group*gender effect was found (P 0.042). Birth weights for male infants were similar among
the four feeding groups, but among female infants, those in the 50% HM group had a lower mean birth weight than did female infants in the PFF-T
group (LS, means SE; 1,188 24 g versus 1,314 20 g).
A statistically significant feeding group effect was found (P 0.015).
the PHM-T or 50% HM groups from the United Kingdom had a university degree, compared with fewer than
10% in the < 50% HM or PFF-T groups. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and the proportion of infants ex-
PHM-T
50% HM
<50% HM
PFF-T
1 (11)
0 (0)
2 (22)
2 (22)
4 (44)
4 (22)
2 (11)
4 (22)
1 (6)
7 (39)
9 (26)
8 (23)
16 (46)
1 (3)
1 (3)
6 (35)
3 (18)
7 (41)
0 (0)
1 (6)
2 (5)
41 (95)
18 (19)
79 (81)
57 (28)
146 (72)
41 (34)
78 (66)
4 (10)
38 (90)
15 (17)
73 (83)
70 (35)
132 (65)
35 (30)
80 (70)
42 (98)
0 (0)
1 (2)
39.4 3.4b (40)
53.2 8.9 (40)
85 (87)
11 (11)
2 (2)
36.9 5.8b (81)
45.0 13.5 (79)
163 (81)
32 (16)
3 (1)
35.0 5.8ab (175)
35.7 12.7 (174)
98 (83)
13 (11)
7 (6)
35.2 5.6a (103)
36.3 15.6 (100)
* Values are mean SD (number of participants) unless otherwise noted. Unlike superscript letters within each row denote statistically significant
differences.
A statistically significant human milk feeding group effect was found (P < 0.01).
UK education equivalents: <3, certificate of education or general CSE below C grade.
UK education equivalents: >3, certificates of education or any O level or general certificates of education grade AC.
A statistically significant human milk feeding group*gender effect was found (P 0.032).
442
OCONNOR ET AL.
posed to cigarette smoke in the home after hospital discharge were greater in the primarily formula-fed groups.
HOME Inventory scores were higher (better) in the
families of infants in the PHM-T and 50% HM groups
than those of infants in the PFF-T group but not the
< 50% HM group. Scores on the verbal subtest of the
WAIS-R were higher in mothers of infants in the PHM-T
group than in those of the < 50% HM or PFF-T groups.
These scores were also higher for mothers of male infants in the 50% HM-T group than for mothers of male
infants predominantly fed formula, but these differences
were not seen among female infants.
Growth
There were consistent differences in the growth of
infants depending on human milk feeding group (Table
3). Predominantly formula-fed infants weighed more
(3548 g) at study entry than did infants in the other
feeding groups. By term CA, a dose-dependent relationship existed between infant weight and the proportion of
energy consumed as infant formula. On average, infants
fed predominantly formula weighed about 500 g or
18.5% more at term CA than did infants fed predominantly human milk. This absolute difference in weight
persisted until 6 months CA, after which the < 50% HM
group, but not the PFF-T group, continued to weigh more
than the PHM-T group until 12 months CA. PFF-T infants also tended to be slightly longer and had larger
PHM-T
50% HM
<50% HM
PFF-T
* Values are mean SD (number of participants) unless otherwise noted. Unlike superscript letters within each row (corrected age of child or visit)
denote statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
A statistically significant feeding group*visit effect (P < 0.0001) was found for all anthropometric measures.
443
PHM-T
50% HM-T
<50% HM-T
PFF-T
Number of subjects
Feedings withheld for at least 1 days (% of infants)
Feedings withheld because of gastric residuals (% of infants)
Days to reach full enteral feeds (100 kcal/kg/d) mean SD
(1st, 3rd quartile)
Suspected cases of NEC (% of infants)
Confirmed cases of NEC (% of infants)
Suspected systemic infection (% of infants)
Confirmed cases of systemic infection (% of infants)
Chronic lung disease (% of infants)
Days from study day 1 to hospital discharge mean SD
(1st, 3rd quartile)
Serious adverse events (SAEs), n (%)
Hospital readmissions n (%)
43
26
14
98
38
18
203
28
18
119
28
18
16 19 (4, 21)a
21
0
33
7
28
15 16 (5, 18)a
21
5
33
11
22
15 15 (4, 21)b
22
1
39
16
27
8 8 (3, 10)b
30
7
33
14
20
42 23 (20, 54)
14 (33)
14 (33)
37 19 (25, 49)
38 (39)
28 (29)
45 25 (28, 58)
93 (46)
85 (42)
37 23 (21, 43)
61 (51)
54 (45)
* Unlike superscript letters within each row denote statistically significant differences.
A statistically significant human milk feeding group effect was found (days to first enteral feeding P < 0.0001; SAEs and hospital readmissions,
P < 0.05).
nantly formula fed. Site (P < 0.0001), absence of maternal prenatal smoking (P 0.028), greater weight at 2
months CA (P < 0.001), higher scores on the HOME
Inventory (P 0.004), and higher maternal scores on
the vocabulary subtest of the WAIS-R (P 0.025) were
statistically significant factors in predicting Teller Acuity
scores (reduced statistical model).
No differences in the Bayley Mental Index or Motor
Index were found among feeding groups. However, a
positive association between duration of human milk
feeding and Bayley Mental Index was found in the full
(P 0.032), but not the reduced, statistical model (P
PHM-T
50% HM-T
<50% HM-T
PFF-T
A statistically significant human milk feeding group*visit effect was found in the full (P 0.033) but not reduced statistical model (P 0.058).
At 9 months CA, PFF-T > 50 50% HM.
** A statistically significant human milk feeding group*birth weight stratum effect was found in the full (P 0.019) and reduced models (P
0.0632). Among infants born >1,250 g, 50% HM > PFF-T.
444
OCONNOR ET AL.
445
REFERENCES
1. Anderson JW, Johnstone BM, Remley DT. Breast-feeding and
cognitive development: a meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 1999;70:
52535.
2. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, et al. A randomised multicentre study
of human milk versus formula and later development in preterm
infants. Arch Dis Child 1994;70:F1416.
3. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, et al. Breast milk and subsequent
intelligence quotient in children born preterm. Lancet 1992;339:
2614.
4. Pollock JI. Mothers choice to provide breast milk and developmental outcome. Arch Dis Child 1989;64:7634.
5. Doyle LW, Rickards AL, Kelly EA, et al. Breastfeeding and intelligence. Lancet 1992;339:7445.
6. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ. Randomised trial of early diet in
preterm babies and later intelligence quotient. BMJ 1998;317:
14817.
7. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Nutrition. Pediatric Nutrition Handbook. Elk Grove Village, IL: American Academy of Pediatrics; 1998.
8. Nutrition Committee of the Canadian Paediatric Society. Nutrient
needs and feeding of preterm infants. Can Med Assoc J 1995;
152:176585.
9. OConnor DL, Hall R, Adamkin D, et al. Growth and development
in preterm infants fed long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids: a
prospective, randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics 2001;108:
35971.
10. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas;
1984.
11. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, rev. San Antonio,
TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1981.
12. Teller DY, McDonald MA, Preston K, et al. Assessment of visual
acuity in infants and children: the acuity card procedure. Dev Med
Child Neurol 1986;28:77989.
13. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. San Antonio, TX:
The Psychological Corporation; 1993.
14. Fenson L, Dale P, Reznick J, et al. MacArthur Communicative
Development Inventories: Users Guide and Technical Manual.
San Diego: Singular Publishing Group; 1993.
15. SAS Institute Inc. SAS/STAT Users Guide. 4 ed. Cary, NC: SAS
Institute, Inc; 1989.
16. Carlson SE, Werkman SH, Tolley EA. Effect of long-chain n-3
fatty acid supplementation on visual acuity and growth of preterm
infants with and without bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Am J Clin
Nutr 1996;63:68797.
17. Singer LT, Siegel AC, Lewis B, et al. Preschool language outcomes of children with history of bronchopulmonary dysplasia and
very low birth weight. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2001;22:1926.
18. Singer L, Yamashita T, Lilien L, et al. A longitudinal study of
developmental outcome of infants with bronchopulmonary dysplasia and very low birth weight. Pediatrics 1997;100:98793.
19. Chan GM. Growth and bone mineral status of discharged very low
birth weight infants fed different formulas or human milk. J Pediatr 1993;123:43943.
20. Wauben IP, Atkinson SA, Shah JK, et al. Growth and body composition of preterm infants: influence of nutrient fortification of
mothers milk in hospital and breastfeeding post-hospital discharge. Acta Paediatr 1998;87:7805.
21. Wheeler RE, Hall RT. Feeding of premature infant formula after
hospital discharge of infants weighing less than 1800 g at birth.
J Perinatol 1996;16:1116.
22. Mattia FR, deRegnier RA. Chronic physiologic instability is associated with neurodevelopmental morbidity at one and two years in
extremely premature infants. Pediatrics 1998;102:E35.
23. Hack M, Breslau N, Weissman B, et al. Effect of very low birth
weight and subnormal head size on cognitive abilities at school
age. N Engl J Med 1991;325:2317.
446
OCONNOR ET AL.
24. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, et al. Early diet in preterm babies and
developmental status at 18 months. Lancet 1990;335:147781.
25. Cooke RJ, Embleton ND, Griffin IJ, et al. Feeding preterm infants
after hospital discharge: growth and development at 18 months of
age. Pediatr Res 2001;49:71922.
26. Morley R. The influence of early diet on later development. J
Biosoc Sci 1996;28:4817.
27. Morley R, Lucas A. Influence of early diet on outcome in preterm
infants. Acta Paediatr Suppl 1994;405:1236.
28. Cunningham AS, Jelliffe DB, Jelliffe EF. Breast-feeding and
health in the 1980s: a global epidemiologic review. J Pediatr 1991;
118:65966.
29. Dewey KG, Heinig MJ, Nommsen-Rivers LA. Differences in morbidity between breast-fed and formula-fed infants. J Pediatr 1995;
126:696702.
30. Woodward A, Douglas RM, Graham NM, et al. Acute respiratory
illness in Adelaide children: breast feeding modifies the effect of
passive smoking. J Epidemiol Community Health 1990;44:22430.
31. Wright AL, Holberg CJ, Martinez FD, et al. Breast feeding and
lower respiratory tract illness in the first year of life. Group Health
Medical Associates. BMJ 1989;299:9469.
32. Frank AL, Taber LH, Glezen WP, et al. Breast-feeding and respiratory virus infection. Pediatrics 1982;70:23945.
33. Savilahti E, Jarvenpaa AL, Raiha NC. Serum immunoglobulins in
preterm infants: comparison of human milk and formula feeding.
Pediatrics 1983;72:3126.
34. Hanson LA, Ahlstedt S, Andersson B, et al. Protective factors in
milk and the development of the immune system Pediatrics 1985;
75:1726.
35. Adebonojo FO. Artificial vs breast feeding: relation to infant
health in a middle class American community. Clin Pediatr (Phila)
1972;11:259.
36. Cushing AH, Anderson L. Diarrhea in breast-fed and non-breastfed infants. Pediatrics 1982;70:9215.
37. Rubin DH, Leventhal JM, Krasilnikoff PA, et al. Relationship
between infant feeding and infectious illness: a prospective study
of infants during the first year of life. Pediatrics 1990;85:46471.
38. Sauls HS. Potential effect of demographic and other variables in
studies comparing morbidity of breast-fed and bottle-fed infants.
Pediatrics 1979;64:5237.