STEPHEN M. COREY What is the relationship between the practical decisions made every day by school people and the more careful methodology dignified by the name, "research"? This question is analyzed by Stephen M. Corey, ex ecutive officer, Horace Mann-Lincoln Institute of School Experimenta tion, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York City.
A GREAT DEAL of educational re
search is conducted by investigators who feel no personal responsibility for the application of their findings to the solution of practical problems. Someone becomes intrigued with an hypothesis which may or may not have impor tant theoretical implications. He pro ceeds to test the hypothesis, generalize from his data, and publish a report. The assumption is that the people who are faced daily with practical educa tional problems will study the results of this research and, as a consequence, make better decisions. Because this has been the common point of view regarding the use of educational research, people who must constantly deal with practical problems have themselves not been disposed to use a research approach to the solu tion of their problems. They tend to think of educational research as the business of experts and hence beyond their talents. They consider themselves as qualified to consume research, but not to engage in it. This article attempts to show the relationship between the way many of us school people go about making prac tical decisions and the more careful 478
methodology dignified by the name
"research." The term is usually reserved to describe only those attempts at prob lem solving or hypothesis testing which result in findings in which great confi dence can be placed. The point will be made later in this article that there is only a relative difference between this "research" and the methodology we usually employ to cope with our day by day instructional or administrative problems. In other words, our casual and subjective methods of making prac tical decisions differ from "research" in degree of precision and eventually in the degree of confidence we can have in the actions we take. Any practicing educator or group of educators can pro gress, by degrees, from their customary method of problem solving which eventuates in actions in which relatively little confidence should be placed, to a methodology resulting in actions in which a higher degree of confidence can be placed. The assumption is that we all want greater confidence in the conse quences of our decisions. FACULTY MFMBKRS CONDUCT 'A CASUAL INQUIRY Let's sit in for a moment on a Educational Leadership
meeting of seven members of a high
school social science department. They are together lor the lirst time at the beginning of the year, and the conver sation goes something like this: : 1 think we ought to do some thing about the fact that pupils in this high school lack dependability. There were four members of my American History class last year who cheated on the final examination. : I agree. They don't have their assignments in on time either. When the assignments do come in, they are sloppy and irresponsible. Half of them lie when they give reasons for absence or handing papers in late. -: Yes, and you should see some of the notes I picked up last spring that my pupils were writing to one another. A lot of them were just filthy. The conversation went on in this vein for some time. Three or four members of the department were res ponsible for most of the talk. There seemed to be general agreement that the boys and girls were lacking in depend ability and in other character traits that the members of the department felt were important. Let's pick up the conversation again. : I'm sure that if we gave more emphasis in American History to the biography of great Americans who were honest and dependable and trustworthy, these boys and girls would see how important these traits are. : I think so too. Take Lincoln, for example. If we could show how dependable Lincoln was, and the price he paid to be honestlike the time he walked a long distance to May, 1952
correct a mistake he had made in
the storethese kids would learn some good lessons. : We ought to emphasize other biographies too. Take Washington. He was known for telling the truth. If we were to emphasize the biog raphy of great Americans and the children were to see how important these good character traits are, they would act differently. These commentsand there were more like themimplied that several members of the department were will ing to hypothesize that if there were more emphasis on the teaching of biog raphy in American History, the boys and girls would improve their char acters. Of course, no one used the word "hypothesize." The conversation went on: : I think we're in agreement about this emphasis upon biography. Is there anything specific we can do about it? : Well, let's just put more stress on biography. Let's see if we can find some biographical novels or use some motion pictures that will emphasi/e the dependability and honesty of great Americans. : I think I'll try that. Every chance I get I'll talk about some of the in timate biographical episodes in the lives of Washington or Jefferson or Lincoln or Theodore Roosevelt or Woodrow Wilson to try to make the importance of dependability and honesty more vital. : Let's be sure to spend some time next spring taking a look at what we've done to see whether or not this emphasis on biography has had any effect. 479
These final remarks represented the
i OIK lusimis the members of this de partment leached regarding a test ot their "hypothesis" that emphasis upon the biography of famous Americans would improve the character of the boys and girls. Someone suggested that .mother look be taken at the situation in the spring to see what the conse quences of this emphasis might be. Let's assume that biography was stressed by the teachers in various de grees throughout a year, and now there is a final departmental meeting. In this meeting many things have been taken care of, such as the quality of the ex aminations used and the distribution of grades, and then someone says: : We've talked several times this year about our emphasis upon the biographical method. Does any one have any opinion as to whether or not this has had an effect upon the dependability or honesty of the boys and girls? : I'm not sure, but a lot of them did seem to be interested whenever attention was paid to biography. : I don't know whether it has made any difference or not. My pupils seemed interested, too, but I didn't notice any difference really in their dependability when it came to hand ing in papers or to preparation for examinations. This conversation went on for a while, and there seemed to be agree ment that the method had worked fairly well. The conversation was summed up by the chairman of the de partment: : Well, let's try it again. I think there's everything to gain and noth ing to lose. Maybe we can get the 480
librarian to buy more good historical
novels that emphasi/e the fine char acteristics ol outstanding Americans. ANALYSIS 01- mi. INQUIRY These discussions by the members of this social science department are illus trative of the way many educational decisions are made and actions taken. In the course of what they did these teachers paid at least some attention to all of the questions that must be con sidered if the problem solving is to be dignified by the name "research." The quality of their consideration of these questions, however, was of a subjective, impressionistic, non-evidential nature, as we will try to show. Definition of the Problem No specific p roblem was identified. Members of the faculty talked about a big problem area. They were worried because the boys and girls did not come up to standards that the teachers ex pected in connection with dependabil ity, conscientiousness, honesty, or gen eral moral behavior. There was no at tempt to delimit this broad problem area. Many subjective impressions were expressed about cheating and similar matters, but no one suggested that facts be procured to see how much cheating took place and under what circum stances. Nobody seemed to be interested in establishing a bench mark that would make it possible to find out whether or not improvement in char acter occurred as a result of emphasis upon the biographical method. Hypothesizing The word "hypothesizing" is being used to describe a prediction that if Educational Leadership
something is done certain consequences
would follow. In this case, the "some thing to be done" involved giving more emphasis to the biographical method. The predicted consequences were gen eral improvement in the character and dependability and honesty of the boys' and girls. The hypothesis was in a vague and general form. No considera tion was given to alternative actions that might represent even better ways of improving character. NT o one seemed to be familiar with attempts that had been made elsewhere to teach social studies so as to improve character, Design of the Test You will recall that somebody said, "Well, let's put more emphasis upon biographies this year." This in a sense was the group's conception of an "ex perimental design" to test its hypothesis. It was a casual, unrigoi ous, offhand sort of design. There was little discussion of what the various teachers meant by "em phasis upon the biographical method," or the amount of emphasis that each would give to this method or the vari ous ways in which emphasis upon the biographical method might be effected. There was no attempt to control this treatment in respect to reading or lecturing or looking at motion pic tures. There was no agreement on the biographies that would be used. Evidence At the autumn meeting, when the decision was reached to ernphasi/e the biographical method, someone said, "Let's be sure to take another look at this matter next spring to see what has happened." This statement implied a low degree of awareness of the iniMay, 1952
portance of objective evidence if the
consequences of an experiment are to be examined to find out whether or not the actions taken led to the conse quences anticipated. No one suggested that any methods be employed to find out rather objectively whether or not honesty or dependability or general morality had been changed. Depend ence was placed entirely upon casual recollection and subjective opinion. Generalizing
-,
At the spring meeting a few minutes
were devoted to taking a backward look at what had been done, and what the consequences seemed to be. Some teach ers had one opinion, and some teachers another. There seemed to be agreement that the plan had worked rather well. This generali/ation led to the decision that, with a few modifications, the same emphasis on biographies should be tried again next year. The "methodology" these social studies teachers employed as they went a lion t coping with a problem that con cerned most of them was not unique. They did what most operating groups of teachers or other kinds of school peo ple do when they attack practical, con crete problems. A MORK Snr.NTiFic APPROACH TO A PROBLEM AREA To provide some contrast, let's take a look at another group of high school social science teachers who were faced with a similar problem. 1 They, however, went about tackling it in a way that was i Sec: Banks. Trcssa. Parley. Edgar S.. Powers, Osc;ir. Vandcniiecr. Floyd, \Valdorf. Robert, and Corey. Stephen M. "We Tested Some Be liefs about tbc Biographical Method," The Sc/iool Review, M arch, 1951, p. 157-163. 481
more sophisticated in respect to re
search methodology. The Problem
These teachers, too, were concerned
in general with the character of their pupils. There was quite a bit of talk about dishonesty and lack of dependa bility, and about other aspects of mo rality. Several members of the group, however, realized that they were talk ing about a problem area and not a problem sufficiently specific to do some thing about. After a great deal of dis cussion, they agreed to use, as a criterion of character, peer judgments within the various class groups. The traits they would concentrate on would be de termination, honesty, stick-to-itiveness, leadership, and hard work. These char acteristics were mentioned most fre quently by pupils when they listed the outstandingly desirable characteristics of those Americans who, they believed, had made significant contributions to the history of their country. This "definition" still left the prob lem a broad one. Some additional pre cision resulted when the group decided to find out something about the pres ent status of the "character" situation by getting sociometric judgments from the youngsters which would establish a "reputation" bench mark. This group's attempts to get some evi dence about its problem and to define it somewhat more precisely is in con siderable contrast to what was done by the first group of social studies teachers. This second group took several steps in the direction of the kind of method ology which characterizes educational "research." 482
Hypothesizing
These teachers, too, were more rig
orous in their hypothesizing. Without going into any detail as to the deriva tion of their predictions regarding the consequences of a "biographical" em phasis, these three hypotheses were agreed upon: A substantial, positive relationship will exist between scores measuring in formation pupils have learned about famous Americans and the extent to which these same persons are admired. The degree of admiration for famous American historical personages will be appreciably increased as a consequence of one semester of instruction in Amer ican .history. A measurable degree of relationship will exist between the degree to which these historical persons are admired and the reputation the pupils have among their peers for behaving in a manner consistent with the traits explaining this admiration. Again these "hypotheses" leave much to be desired if scrutinized from the point of view of the professional edu cational researcher. They do, however, represent substantial improvement over the casual and careless and unimagina tive hypothesizing of the first group of social studies teachers. Design
These teachers talked for quite a
while about the best way for them to test their hunch that emphasis upon the biography of famous Americans would have a benefu ial effect upon the characteras definedof their pupils. In general the design they agreed upon involved, first, trying to find out how Educational Leadership
much boys and girls knew about fa
mous Americans, the extent to which these Americans were admired, and the reputations of the youngsters, a t the be ginning of the year. They then agreed to introduce to all American history classes certain readings that stressed the character of famous Americans. They also selected a limited number of mo tion pictures that all boys and girls would see. Each member of the staff agreed, too, to incorporate in his teach ing, and whenever he could, episodes and incidents from the lives of famous Americans. The plan then was to re peat the sociometric, information and "admiration" tests at the end of the year and compare "before and after" scores. This would enable the teachers to find out how much the boys and girls had learned, the extent to which their admiration for famous Americans had changed, and the extent to which their reputation among their peers for the traits agreed upon had been affected. Again this design was far from pre cise. There was no real agreement among the teachers regarding the time that would be spent emphasizing biog raphy or the teaching methodology that would be employed. No attempts were made to get evidence on a control group. But, be these limitations as evi dent as they may be, the difference be tween this design and the design of the first group of social studies teachers represents substantial movement from subjective, casual, non-scientific inquiry toward careful, thoughtful, and "re search" methodology. Evidence
One major indication of the metho
dological sophistication of the second May, 1952
group of social studies teachers was
their attempt, all along the line, to get evidenceto get facts. First, they pro cured some rather reliable and objective evidence as to the status of the boys and girls in respect to a number of factors at the beginning of the year. These attempts at evidence collecting were repeated again in June. There was much less dependence upon casual, sub jective recall. It is true that the instru ments used to get evidence were of limited reliability. Apart from consid ering the "face" validity of these instru ments, little was done to determine the extent to which they really measured what the teachers wanted to measure. But even with these limitations, the realization that facts were essential to the definition and solution of their problem was a long step forward. 4
Generalizing
At a final departmental meeting at
the end of the year, when the data were in, these teachers took a look at them. They got help from someone who understood statistics and could counsel them in the interpretations. They reached these conclusions: The possession by our pupils of in formation about historical characters has little relationship to their admira tion of these characters. One semester of our instruction in American History with emphasis on bi ographies does not measurably increase the extent to which boys and girls ad mire famous Americans. There is no relationship between the extent to which our boys and girls ad mire famous Americans and the repu tations they have among their peers in 483
respect to the traits that explain their
admirations. These conclusions were rather devas tating. Admittedly they were based upon measuring instruments and a de sign that left much to be desired. But, as these teachers said, these conclusions \\ere based upon better evidence and a lietter design than had previously been available to them. Consequently, con siderable confidence \vas placed in the conclusions. RESEARCH Is NOT AN ABSOLUTE Now let's get back to the major argu ment of this article. Educational re search or research of any sort is not an absolute. The research quality of an investigation is relative. Excellent re search involves a method of inquiry that warrants a high degree of confi dence in the results of the investigation. As research departs in quality from that which warrants high confidence in results it can depart along any one or more dimensions. These dimensions are all inter-related, but problem solving at any level of methodological sophistica tion involves problem definition, hy pothesizing, a design to test the hy potheses, procuring of evidence, and generalizing from this evidence. If the quality of definition, hypothesizing, de
signing, evidence getting and generaliz
ing is high, the research is of excellent quality. This is another way of stating that its consequences justify a great deal of confidence. One difficulty with most of the writ ing and talking about educational re search is the implication that it does represent an "absolute" in methodology. The fact that all attempts at problem solving fall at various points on a con tinuum ranging from casual, impres sionistic, and untested inquiry to high quality research is rarely, if ever, em phasized. This is regrettable because it has led teachers and other school peo ple to value research, but at the same time to view it as having little bearing upon the methods they employ to solve their own problems. The fact is, of course, that confidence is warranted in the decisions made by teachers and other school people in the degree to which these decisions are based on problem solving methods that are closely analogous to those used by the professional educational investigator, and no one has a corner on these methods. They can best be learned by practicing them. To refrain from try ing because of lack of skill precludes improvement, and improvement is what counts.
ASCD's new pamphlet publication:
Bibliography on Supervision and Curriculum Development Prepared by an ASCD Committee under direction of Jolmnye V. Cox, c hairman. An annotated bibliography on outstanding publications in supervision and curriculum develop ment during the past ten years. Price r;o cents Order from:
Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development, NKA
1201 Sixteenth Street, N.W., Washington 6, D. C.
Educational Leadership
Copyright 1952 by the Association for Supervision and Curriculum