Bell 2010
Bell 2010
Bell 2010
Schizophrenia Research
j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / s c h r e s
VA Connecticut Healthcare System, Rehabilitation Research and Development Service, West Haven, CT 06517, USA
Yale University School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, New Haven, CT 06511, USA
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 20 January 2010
Received in revised form 14 March 2010
Accepted 16 March 2010
Available online 18 April 2010
Keywords:
Social attribution
Theory of Mind
Mentalizing
Social cognition
Schizophrenia
Neuropsychology
a b s t r a c t
This is the rst report on the use of the Social Attribution Task Multiple Choice (SAT-MC) to
assess social cognitive impairments in schizophrenia. The SAT-MC was originally developed for
autism research, and consists of a 64-second animation showing geometric gures enacting a
social drama, with 19 multiple choice questions about the interactions. Responses from 85
community-dwelling participants and 66 participants with SCID conrmed schizophrenia or
schizoaffective disorders (Scz) revealed highly signicant group differences. When the two
samples were combined, SAT-MC scores were signicantly correlated with other social cognitive
measures, including measures of affect recognition, theory of mind, self-report of egocentricity
and the Social Cognition Index from the MATRICS battery. Using a cut-off score, 53% of Scz were
signicantly impaired on SAT-MC compared with 9% of the community sample. Most Scz
participants with impairment on SAT-MC also had impairment on affect recognition. Signicant
correlations were also found with neurocognitive measures but with less dependence on verbal
processes than other social cognitive measures. Logistic regression using SAT-MC scores correctly
classied 75% of both samples. Results suggest that this measure may have promise, but
alternative versions will be needed before it can be used in prepost or longitudinal designs.
Published by Elsevier B.V.
1. Introduction
Social functioning decits are among the most prominent
features of schizophrenia and play a large role in the individual's level of disability and the likelihood of relapse. Social
cognition, or how an individual processes, interprets, and
responds to social information, has repeatedly been shown to
be impaired in schizophrenia (Bora et al., 2009; Brune and
Brune, 2005; Corcoran et al., 1995a; Edwards et al., 2002;
Fiszdon et al., 2009; Penn et al., 2008; Silverstein, 1997) and to
be associated with various components of functioning (Cohen
et al., 2009; Couture et al., 2006; Hooker and Park, 2002;
Mueser et al., 1996; Penn et al., 1996; Pinkham and Penn, 2006).
While some studies suggest that social cognition may mediate
the relationship between other variables (e.g. neurocognition)
and functional outcomes, other studies indicate that social
cognition may also account for a unique portion of the variance
in predicting functional outcomes (Addington et al., 2006; Bell
et al., 2009; Brekke et al., 2005; Brekke et al., 2007; Dickinson
et al., 2007; Kee et al., 2003; Meyer and Kurtz, 2009; Vauth et al.,
2004). The interrelationship between social cognition and
functional outcomes has led researchers to suggest that social
cognition may be a good proximal treatment target for interventions aimed at improving functional outcomes in schizophrenia (Horan et al., 2008).
A number of laboratory measures have been developed to
assess social cognitive function. Most of these measures have
focused on narrowly dened social cognitive processes such
as ability to recognize affect, identify interrelationships and
165
166
Table 1
SAT-MC participant characteristics.
(n = 66)
(%)
Gender
Male
40
(60.6)
Female
26
(39.4)
Schizophrenia diagnosis
Disorganized
2
Paranoid
28
Residual
11
Undifferentiated
8
Schizoaffective
16
Psychosis Disorder NOS 1
(3.0)
(42.4)
(16.7)
(12.1)
(24.2)
(1.5)
Ethnicity
African
American
Caucasian
Hispanic
Other
Medications
Atypical
Conventional
Both
None
(%)
40
(60.6)
24
1
1
(36.4)
(1.5)
(1.5)
44
8
5
9
(66.7)
(12.1)
(7.6)
(13.6)
(n = 65)
Mean (SD)
Mean (SD)
PANSS
Total
Positive
Age
64.94 (15.4) Education
15.37 (5.1) Age at 1st
hospitalization
16.20 (6.9) Lifetime
number of
hospitalizations
16.52 (4.8)
6.69 (3.0)
8.75 (3.6)
42.73 (10.4)
12.58 (2.6)
23.45 (9.2)
Negative
Cognitive
Hostility
Emotional Discomfort
SANS
Total
SAPS
Total
8.63 (10.7)
35.18 (19.5)
29.11 (19.0)
167
2.3. Procedures
3.2. Discriminant validity
Following written informed consent, schizophrenia participants were individually administered all measures over several
assessment sessions. Neurocognitive assessments were usually
performed over at least two testing sessions, but additional
breaks were taken if there was concern about the participant's
fatigue or alertness. Symptom evaluations and social cognitive
measures were generally performed on different assessment
days.
The community sample was provided with a description
of the study by their classroom teacher. Group administration
of the SAT-MC, BORRTI, BLERT and Hinting Task was done by
study personnel during a single classroom session. Community participants were not administered any neurocognitive
or symptom measures. For the SAT-MC, a large TV monitor
placed at the front of the 40-person classroom displayed the
task, and each student had a copy of the answer sheet. The
SAT-MC was paused at each question and administration did
not resume until it was clear that all participants had circled a
response. All forms were reviewed to determine that they had
been lled out correctly and with sincere intent (e.g. not all
items scored false).
Fig. 1. SAT-MC correct score distributions for community sample (CS) and
schizophrenia sample (Scz).
168
Table 2
Correlations between SAT-MC correct scores and neurocognitive measures in
the schizophrenia sample.
Mean
3.0
2.7
3.3
2.6
2.1
2.9
3.3
0.38
0.29
0.37
0.18
0.26
0.36
0.47
0.003
0.02
0.003
ns
0.04
0.004
0.000
7.9
9.1
7.6
3.1
2.7
3.1
0.31
0.19
0.26
0.01
ns
0.05
85.6
86.5
87.5
86.7
16.2
17.4
14.5
15.4
0.31
0.25
0.34
0.32
0.02
ns
0.007
0.01
35.5
34.0
34.3
36.0
36.1
41.7
36.3
10.3
12.3
13.7
7.6
12.0
9.8
8.0
0.22
0.23
0.32
0.24
0.18
0.28
0.34
ns
ns
0.01
ns
ns
0.03
0.008
8.3
8.8
7.3
6.5
6.1
8.4
8.7
SD
BORRTI (Reality Distortion, r = 0.04; Uncertainty of Perception, r = 0.10; Hallucinations and Delusions, r = 0.10).
3.5. Classication accuracy
Logistic Regression using SAT-MC scores to predict group
membership (Community Comparison sample vs. Schizophrenia) was highly signicant (Chi-square (1) = 44.6, p b 0.001).
Sensitivity to schizophrenia was 60.6%, specicity was 77.0%,
and overall classication accuracy was 74.8% (Table 3). This
analysis was repeated adding Hinting Task, BLERT, and
Egocentricity scores, which increased classication accuracy
slightly to 78%.
4. Discussion
This is the rst report using the Social Attribution Test
in its multiple choice form as a possible measure of social
Table 3
Logistic regression using SAT-MC scores to predict group membership
(Community sample (CS) vs. Schizophrenia (Scz)).
Observed
Scz
CC
Predicted
Scz
CS
Total
40
12
52
26
73
99
66
85
151
cognition for schizophrenia research. We found that schizophrenia participants had signicantly poorer scores than our
community sample, who had not been screened for psychopathology. Despite this lack of screening, a cut-off score for
impaired functioning on the SAT-MC successfully distinguished
our schizophrenia sample from the community sample with
more than half the schizophrenia sample designated as having
impairment while fewer than 10% of the community sample
participants were so designated. We did not expect to nd
social cognitive decits in all people with schizophrenia and
recognize these decits as a likely and important source of
heterogeneity within the disorder, so it should not be expected
that SAT-MC scores would distinguish all schizophrenia
participants from the community comparison participants. It
is also the case that such decits are related to other disorders
such as Asperger's Syndrome, so it may be that some of the
community comparison participants with poor SAT-MC scores
may have a psychiatric condition that affects social cognition
and thus were not strictly speaking false positives.
Convergent validity for the instrument was sought by
examining its relationship to other social cognitive measures
and to a self-report measure of object relations. For those
measures that were administered to both the community
comparison and the schizophrenia participants, the greatest
amount of shared variance was with our measure of affect
recognition. Most participants with schizophrenia who were
SAT-MC impaired were also BLERT impaired, although many
who were BLERT impaired were not SAT-MC impaired.
Other highly signicant relationships were also found
with our theory of mind task and with the self-report measure of Egocentricity. With the more restricted range of scores
within the schizophrenia sample, the associations were weaker
than for the combined samples but the pattern was similar. In
addition, the Social Cognition Index from the MATRICS battery
was signicantly correlated with SAT-MC scores. These ndings
lend some support to the construct validity of the SAT-MC as a
measure of social cognition, but the associations are moderate
at best. This may be because the SAT-MC is capturing features
of social cognition (e.g. anthropomorphizing, metacognitive
creation of coherent social narrative, and social attribution) that
are related to but distinct from affect recognition, theory of
mind, social problem-solving, or self-experience.
While it is generally agreed that social cognition relies on
some features of neurocognition (e.g. attention, working
memory, problem-solving), it is hoped that a social cognitive
measure would be relatively independent from basic cognition.
This is of particular concern because the Social Cognition Index
of the MATRICS and the Hinting Task have both shown highly
signicant dependence on verbal processes, particularly story
memory (Wexler et al., 2009). The SAT-MC had a modest,
but signicant relationship to Logical Memory I as it did to
a number of verbal and non-verbal tasks. Interestingly, the
strongest relationship was with Matrix Reasoning, suggesting
that correctly interpreting the actions of the geometric gures
in the SAT-MC may share some of the same problem-solving
processes required for solving the geometric sequences in
Matrix Reasoning. Based on the MATRICS index scores, it does
appear that the SAT-MC is relatively independent of Speed of
Processing, Attention and Vigilance, and Verbal and Visual
Learning, and that the total contribution of neurocognition
based on the Neurocognitive Composite is relatively modest.
169
170
References
Addington, J., Saeedi, H., Addington, D., 2006. Facial affect recognition: a
mediator between cognitive and social functioning in psychosis?
Schizophrenia Research 85, 142150.
American Psychiatric Association, Task Force on DSM-IV, 1994. Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-IV. American
Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
Andreasen, N.C., 1984a. Scale for the assessment of negative symptoms
(SANS). University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA.
Andreasen, N.C., 1984b. Scale for the assessment of positive symptoms
(SAPS). University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA.
Bazin, N., Brunet-Gouet, E., Bourdet, C., Kayser, N., Falissard, B., Hardy-Bayle,
M.C., Passerieux, C., Bazin, N., Brunet-Gouet, E., Bourdet, C., Kayser, N.,
Falissard, B., Hardy-Bayle, M.-C., Passerieux, C., 2009. Quantitative
assessment of attribution of intentions to others in schizophrenia using
an ecological video-based task: a comparison with manic and depressed
patients. Psychiatry Research 167, 2835.
Bell, M., Bryson, G., Lysaker, P., 1997. Positive and negative affect recognition
in schizophrenia: a comparison with substance abuse and normal control
subjects. Psychiatry Research. 73 (12), 7382 Nov 14.
Bell, M.D., 1995. Manual for the Bell Object Relations Reality Testing Inventory.
Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles, CA.
Bell, M.D., Lysaker, P.H., Beam-Goulet, J.L., Milstein, R.M., Lindenmayer, J.P.,
1994. Five-component model of schizophrenia: assessing the factorial
171