Lenguas Iroquesas
Lenguas Iroquesas
Lenguas Iroquesas
by
Charles Julian
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
Department of Linguistics
University of Manitoba
Winnipeg
The Iroquoian language family is indigenous to eastern North America. It has both a
Huron, Mohawk, and Tuscarora, among others. The languages are notable for their rich
To date, the history of the Iroquoian languages has been limited to short summaries,
and formal reconstruction of the phonology and morphology of Proto-Iroquoian (PI) has
not been undertaken. This work represents the first systematic attempt to reconstruct PI
languages. The comparative method has been used, but the theoretical disposition of
the work is otherwise neutral and should permit interpretation of the data by
issues of time depth, subgrouping, borrowing, and inheritance. Chapter 2 presents the
that relate the phonological and morphological changes separating each descendant
ii
language from its parent stage. Evolution of Cherokee from PI is described in Chapter 3.
Nottoway (PTN) from PNI is related in Chapter 5, and development of Tuscarora and
(PMO) from PNI is presented in Chapter 10, and the evolution of Mohawk and Oneida
from PMO in Chapters 11 and 12. Development of Onondaga, Cayuga, Seneca, and
Huron from PNI is related in Chapters 13 through 16, and development of Wyandot
Data for this study were gathered from primary sources (dictionaries, grammars,
word lists), and cognate sets upon which reconstructions in the study are based are
iii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This dissertation would not have been possible without the encouragement and
financial support of my family. For advice, inspiration, and insight during my formative
linguistic years I would like to thank my undergraduate mentors, John Colarusso, Anna
Moro, and George Thomas, and my M.A. supervisor, Sheila Embleton. I would like to
Fellowship in the first year of my doctoral studies, and to thank the staff and faculty of
David Pentland, for offering helpful suggestions and valuable insight at all points.
Thanks are due also to the late Blair Rudes, who provided feedback on my earliest draft
versions of Proto-Iroquoian, and to John Steckley, who helped me track down Huron
manuscript sources. Finally I would like to thank all the authors, past and present, of
the grammars, dictionaries, and other forms of primary documentation upon which this
study is based. Anyone who has ever taken time out to record a few obscure words from
an Iroquoian language has helped to preserve vital clues from history that might
iv
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Abstract ……………………………………………………………………………………... ii
Acknowledgements ……………………………………………………………………… .. iv
v
2.2.3 Consonant Clusters ………………………………………………………. 36
vi
4.2.1 Vowels ……………………………………………………………………. 107
vii
7.4 Morphological Changes ……………………………………………………….. 180
viii
11.2 Phonemic Inventory ………………………………………………………….. 212
ix
Chapter 15: Seneca ……………………………………………………………………… 284
x
18.3 Wenro ………………………………………………………………………….. 336
xi
LIST OF MAPS
2 Cherokee xvii
4 Susquehannock xix
5 Laurentian xix
6 Iroquoia xx
7 Huronia xxi
xii
LANGUAGE ABBREVIATIONS
xiii
GRAMMATICAL ABBREVIATIONS
xiv
PRONOMINAL PREFIX ABBREVIATIONS
1 first person
1DU first person dual number
1NS first person non-singular number
1PL first person plural number
1SG first person singular number
1+2DU first person inclusive dual number
1+2NS first person inclusive non-singular number
1+2PL first person inclusive plural number
1+3DU first person exclusive dual number
1+3NS first person exclusive non-singular number
1+3PL first person exclusive plural number
2 second person
2DU second person dual number
2NS second person non-singular number
2PL second person plural number
2SG second person singular number
3 third person
3A third person animate
3I third person indefinite
3DU third person dual number
3DUM third person dual number masculine
3M third person masculine
3NS third person non-singular number
3PL third person plural number
3PLM third person plural number masculine
3SGA third person singular number animate
3SGM third person singular number masculine
.DU at least one argument dual number, neither plural number
.IMP imperative verb
.M at least one male present
.PL at least one argument plural number
.SG both arguments singular number
(A):(B) (A) acting on (B)
(A)&(B) (A) and (B) interacting
xv
Map 1 The Iroquoian Language Family
xvi
Map 2 Cherokee
xvii
Map 3 The Coastal Plain
xviii
Map 4 Susquehannock
Map 5 Laurentian
xix
Map 6 Iroquoia
xx
Map 7 Huronia
xxi
xxii
CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION
1.1 INTRODUCTION
I have written this dissertation with the aim of addressing questions that I myself
had about the Iroquoian language family when I began my research, in particular
questions about Proto-Iroquoian, the ancestor from which all of the languages derive.
My hope is that it can serve as a basis for future diachronic inquiry, and as an
introduction to the developmental history of the language family as a whole for anyone
who might be interested. The bulk of the work consists of sixteen chapters, each
which relate to extinct Iroquoian languages for which some reasonable written record
has survived ( Nottoway, Susquehannock, Laurentian, Huron, and Wyandot ), and four
of which relate to unattested earlier stages in the Iroquoian family tree, which have
1
innovations alone are noted, and continuity from mother language chapter to daughter
Erie, and Neutral ), and two appendices supply 498 cognate sets upon which
reconstructions in this dissertation are based; these cognate sets are referenced
( e.g. PNI * { -õweh } 'authenticative suffix' ¹²⁴, PI * { -hsnuːɹiɁ } 'be fast' ²⁵⁸ ).
I have used IPA to represent phonological forms throughout the text, with a star
Laurentian, and Huron forms from orthographic sources, I have adhered to a 'principle
orthographic forms are assumed to have been continued. Thus, for example, Huron
<i8at> 'il y a q.c. dedans' is taken to represent / ˈiːwaːt /, with lengthening of the
penultimate vowel and long / aː / in the final syllable ( cf. PNI * / ˈiːwaːt /
'it is within' ¹⁵⁹ ), since definitive change of these features in Huron relative to
2
Proto-Northern Iroquoian cannot be discerned from the orthography in which Huron
was recorded.
The Iroquoian language family is indigenous to eastern North America (see Map 1).
historically was spoken in the Allegheny Mountains at the juncture of what are now the
substantially from all other known Iroquoian languages, which, in contrast, belong to
is now northeastern North Carolina; Nottoway, an extinct language once spoken in what
is now eastern Pennsylvania; Laurentian, an extinct language spoken along the northern
shore of the Saint Lawrence River prior to the seventeenth century; the languages of the
3
Five Nations Iroquois Confederacy ( Mohawk, Oneida, Onondaga, Cayuga, and Seneca ),
historically spoken in what is now New York State; Huron, the extinct common
language of the Huron and Petun Confederacies, originally spoken south and east of
Lake Huron in what is now Ontario; and Wyandot, a language that descends from the
Petun dialect of Huron and was spoken at the Wyandotte Reservation in Oklahoma until
the early twentieth century. Several other Northern Iroquoian languages are known to
have existed, but became extinct before they could be sufficiently documented
( see Chapter 18 ).
The most significant studies in Iroquoian historical linguistics to date are Chafe
(1977a) and Michelson (1988), in which the accent pattern of Proto-Lake Iroquoian is
Nations Proper ( note that the reconstructed pattern holds for Proto-Northern Iroquoian
even after evidence from Tuscarora and Huron has been introduced ) and Rudes (1995),
Cherokee and Northern Iroquoian lexical items, and 100 items of reconstructed
4
Lounsbury (1961), which gives a glottochronological estimate of 3,500 to 3,800 years
for the separation of Proto-Iroquoian and Proto-Northern Iroquoian, and posits a set of
peripheral vs. inner Iroquoian isoglosses on the basis of five vocabulary items and one
shared morphological trait. Lounsbury (1978) and Mithun (1979) present summaries of
the family, as well as models for the subgrouping of the Northern Iroquoian languages
and sketches of characteristic sound changes that have occurred in some of the
languages, and Mithun (1999) also presents an overview of the family. Mithun (1982)
examines the diachronic development of particles and interjections across the family,
and Mithun (1986) examines the diachronic development of evidentials. Chafe (1984),
Chafe & Foster (1981), and Mithun (1985) examine internal relations among the
languages. Allophonic and allomorphic properties of Seneca are accounted for using
5
Diachronic loss of the phoneme * / ɹ / in Onondaga is discussed in Woodbury (1981)
Foster (1982), and diachronic aspects of the phonology and morphology of Wyandot are
different Iroquoian languages are assembled in order to discern what flora, fauna, and
other items may have been present in the Proto-Iroquoian and Proto-Northern Iroquoian
homelands. Several studies by Chafe (1964a, 1973, 1976) have examined possible deep
genetic links between the Iroquoian, Siouan, and Caddoan language families, and the
Chafe (1977b) examines the evolution of third person pronominal prefixes in Proto-
Reconstructions in the latter study differ from those found in the present work, but do
Note that although marginally recoverable genetic links between Iroquoian and other
language families remain a real possibility, reconstructions in the present study have
6
been based only on evidence internal to the Iroquoian language family.
The approximate time depth at which Proto-Iroquoian may initially have broken
into Northern and Southern branches is difficult to assess due to uncertain rates of
basis of 37.8% to 34.3% retention of core vocabulary between Cherokee and two
vocabulary across Iroquoian languages ( below ) suggests far lower rates of cognacy
between the two branches of Iroquoian, however, ranging from 24% between Cherokee
languages, where one must take into account the influence that areal innovation and
borrowing of vocabulary from neighbouring languages may have had on present rates
of cognacy among the languages. Generally speaking, the Northern languages have not
borrowed vocabulary from non-Iroquoian languages with the exception of a very few
recent loanwords from European languages, all of which are nouns ( e.g. Modern
7
Mohawk / ˈtsihs / 'cheese' from English, and / akʷaˈtə̃ht / 'my aunt', from French
languages, however, borrowings from neighbouring Iroquoian languages may have been
frequent and unimpeded by restrictions of lexical class. For example, among the core
vocabulary of the Swadesh 100-word list, at least the following lexical items appear to
have been borrowed into Cayuga from Seneca or another Northern Iroquoian language:
{ -hsoht- } 'hand', { -ẽjõhs- } 'heart', { -hskwa- } 'stone', and { -nehs- } 'sand'. Examples
like these can be identified owing to irregular development of reflexes: in the event that
these items had been inherited by Cayuga directly from Proto-Northern Iroquoian, one
but instead one finds slightly deviant forms. In still other cases, borrowed words may be
direct inheritance, can be recognized. In this way several Northern Iroquoian languages
may eventually have come to resemble their immediate neighbours more than their
closest genetic relatives ( see Chafe & Foster 1981 for discussion of potential Five
Previous subgroupings of the Iroquoian language family have not generally drawn a
sharp distinction between synchronic similarity arising from areal influences and
8
borrowing on the one hand, and similarities attributable to shared genetic inheritance
IROQUOIAN IROQUOIAN
SOUTHERN [IROQUOIAN] SOUTHERN IROQUOIAN
Cherokee Cherokee
NORTHERN [IROQUOIAN] NORTHERN IROQUOIAN
[TUSCARORA-NOTTOWAY] TUSCARORA-NOTTOWAY
Tuscarora Tuscarora
Nottoway * Nottoway *
[LAKE IROQUOIAN] PROTO-LAKE IROQUOIAN
Laurentian ? * Laurentian *
FIVE NATIONS & SUSQUEHANNOCK IROQUOIS PROPER
Susquehannock * Susquehannock *
[MOHAWK-ONEIDA] ONEIDA-MOHAWK
Mohawk Mohawk
Oneida Oneida
Onondaga Onondaga
Cayuga SENECA-CAYUGA
Seneca Cayuga
HURONIAN Seneca
HURON-PETUN Huron-Wyandot *
Lorette Huron * Laurentian *
Wyandot *
NEUTRAL ? [ * extinct at time of writing ]
9
The models in (1) are essentially correct in that those Northern Iroquoian languages
which bear the greatest synchronic resemblance to one another and which share the
largest amount of cognate vocabulary have been grouped together. Contrasting with
these models, albeit only to a minor extent, is the genetic model presented in (2), which
* Proto-Iroquoian (Chapter 2)
Cherokee (Chapter 3)
* Proto-Northern Iroquoian (Chapter 4)
* Proto-Tuscarora-Nottoway (Chapter 5)
Tuscarora (Chapter 6)
Nottoway (Chapter 7)
Susquehannock (Chapter 8)
Laurentian (Chapter 9)
* Proto-Mohawk-Oneida (Chapter 10)
Mohawk (Chapter 11)
Oneida (Chapter 12)
Onondaga (Chapter 13)
Cayuga (Chapter 14)
Seneca (Chapter 15)
Huron (Chapter 16)
Wyandot (Chapter 17)
The model in (2) differs from those in (1) primarily in its lack of a separate Proto-Lake
10
Proto-Tuscarora-Nottoway, which would appear to make Proto-Lake Iroquoian formally
innovations seem to separate the Five Nations languages from Huron, which again
Huron, and similarities between Seneca and Cayuga ( of which there are admittedly
many ) are interpreted as recent and the result of areal influence, since few regular
sound changes link them and a handful of significant morphological features separate
them ( including, for example, Cayuga's retention of the non-singular animate prefix,
which is absent from Seneca and all of the remaining Five Nations languages ).
present in more than one of the daughter languages, apparently owing to areal
influence or to chance rather than to genetic inheritance. Twelve such innovations that
are common to at least four of eight Northern Iroquoian languages are presented in (3),
where Δ indicates that the innovation is present in at least one dialect of the language
11
(3) Post-PNI Innovations Common to at Least Four Northern Iroquoian Languages
* h > Ø / _nh ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓
* h > Ø / _sk, st Δ ✓ ✓ ✓
* h > Ø / #_w ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓
* h > Ø / Vː_# ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓
* j > Ø / ts_ ✓ Δ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓
* n > Ø / _sh Δ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓
* n > t / _k, kʷ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓
* ns > ts / _k, kʷ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓
* t > k / _j Δ Δ Δ ✓
* ts > (h)s / _k, kʷ Δ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓
* ts > (h)s / _ɹ Δ ✓ ✓ ✓ Δ ✓ ✓ ✓
* w > Ø / _j Δ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓
From data in (3) one could theoretically posit that Tuscarora, Mohawk, Oneida, Seneca,
Huron, and Wyandot descend from the same ancestral language stage (Proto-Tuscarora-
Iroquoian stages that are accepted in the present work, Proto-Tuscarora-Nottoway and
12
post-PNI phonological innovations that are common to Tuscarora and Nottoway (six),
and to Mohawk and Oneida (thirteen), and which share a feeding order that allows
changes apply ( note that the only instance in Iroquoian where rule re-ordering seems
derivation of attested forms from proto-forms involves sound changes WY-12 and
Proto-Lake Iroquoian and Proto-Five Nations are formally indistinguishable from Proto-
Northern Iroquoian itself, is not a consensus in the field. Marianne Mithun (p.c.) notes
that, "[t]he case for an early separation of Northern Iroquoian into 8 parallel branches,
with subsequent convergence of the Iroquois Proper languages due to contact, would be
strengthened if we could see old, deep differences but new, superficial similarities. At
present, although we do have some evidence of contact, the differences do not appear to
be very old." However, I would contend that old, deep similarities among the Five
Nations languages would first need to be evident in order to allow grouping of the Five
Nations languages as a subgroup to the exclusion of Huron, and that old, deep
13
similarities among the Lake Iroquoian languages would first need to be evident in order
to allow the grouping of the Lake Iroquoian languages to the exclusion of Tuscarora-
Nottoway – that is, a set of early, shared innovations, of similar quality and quantity to
morphological innovations relative to PNI, appear to occur in the languages of the Five
Nations subgrouping or the languages of the Lake subgrouping that would serve to set
them apart from other Northern Iroquoian languages in general ( for discussion of the
development of PI * / u(ː) / in PNI and its implications for a Five Nations subgrouping,
see section 4.3, and for discussion of the development of non-singular third person
pronominal prefixes in PNI and its implications for a Lake Iroquoian subgrouping, see
section 4.4 ).
Although the current work is concerned with direct genetic inheritance ( e.g. the
whether this has been borrowed or inherited, may nonetheless provide an idea of how
long each has been isolated ( culturally or geographically ) from the other languages.
14
The percentage of core vocabulary shared between eight well-attested Iroquoian
Ch. --- 20 23 22 24 19 21 19
Tu. 20 --- 69 65 66 65 65 67
Mo. 23 69 --- 95 85 83 86 76
Oe. 22 65 95 --- 80 82 81 72
Oo. 24 66 85 80 --- 86 93 74
Ca. 19 65 83 82 86 --- 91 71
Se. 21 65 86 81 93 91 --- 76
Hu. 19 67 76 72 74 71 76 ---
It is intriguing to note that each Northern Iroquoian language in (4) shares roughly 20%
of its core vocabulary with Cherokee ( Cayuga and Huron the least at 19%, Onondaga
the most at 24% ), and that although Tuscarora was historically spoken in close
core vocabulary with Cherokee ( 20% ) as other Northern languages. The latter finding
15
would seem to suggest that Tuscarora has been culturally and geographically isolated
from Cherokee for roughly the same amount of time as all of the other Northern
languages have been. Within Northern Iroquoian, Tuscarora shares only about 65% of
its core vocabulary with Huron and the Five Nations languages, which suggests that
Tuscarora has been culturally and geographically isolated from both Huron and the Five
Nations languages for roughly an equal amount of time. Among the Northern languages
in the Great Lakes region, Huron stands apart in sharing only 71-76% of its core
vocabulary with the Five Nations Iroquois languages, which in contrast all share at least
80% and in some cases as much as 95% of their core vocabulary with each other. This
suggests that Huron has been isolated ( culturally, if not geographically ) from all of the
The number of unique core vocabulary items ( that is, instances in which a core
vocabulary item from the Swadesh 100-word list has no known cognate in any other
Iroquoian language ) in eight well-attested Iroquoian languages may also provide some
idea of the degree of developmental isolation each language has had relative to others,
16
(5) Unique Core Vocabulary Items in Iroquoian Languages (Swadesh 100-word list)
Cherokee 69
Tuscarora 16
Huron 12
Cayuga 4
Onondaga 4
Oneida 3
Mohawk 1
Seneca 1
Numbers in (5) are again in keeping with the chronological sequence of isolation
outlined above, in which Cherokee has been isolated from other Iroquoian languages for
the longest period of time, followed by Tuscarora, Huron, and finally by the Five
shared by Cherokee and Proto-Northern Iroquoian can also provide some idea of the
potential age of the Proto-language, though figures in this regard will be lower than
those in (4), owing to the greater propensity of general vocabulary to be replaced over
included in King (1975), which contains 2050 lexical roots, 100 entries (4.88%) have
17
Oklahoma Cherokee, which contains 2000 lexical roots, 98 entries (4.90%) have
identifiable Northern Iroquoian cognates. These figures suggest that just under 5% of all
lexical roots, or slightly less than 1 in 20 vocabulary items, remains cognate between
18
CHAPTER 2: PROTO-IROQUOIAN
2.1 INTRODUCTION
This chapter describes Proto-Iroquoian (PI), the reconstructed language from which
cognate forms common to both daughter languages, but because there are many points
on which Cherokee and PNI do not agree, I have at times had to interpret a feature
found in one of the daughter languages as more archaic than a feature found in the
other. The second half of this approach is admittedly arbitrary, and other researchers
may certainly differ as to whether Cherokee or PNI is more likely to continue a feature
original to PI. In order that the more interpretive portion of my presentation of the
proto-language obscure only minimally the facts that are based on secure cognate forms
in both branches, however, I will here list the more theoretical of my assumptions.
Features of Cherokee ( but not of PNI ) that I have interpreted as archaic include a
distinction between independent verb forms ( verbs that occur in matrix clauses and
accent ) and dependent verb forms ( verbs that occur in subordinate clauses or as
and penultimate accent ). I have also interpreted the animate vs. inanimate distinction
19
found in Cherokee pronominal prefixes ( as opposed to the feminine-indefinite vs.
third person pronominal prefixes, and have interpreted Cherokee's lack of a third person
gender distinction as archaic, with PNI masculine gender pronominal prefixes reflecting
( but not of Cherokee ) that I have interpreted as archaic include lack of distinctive tone
on vowels, and lack of productive modal suffixes, with word-final presence of present,
typologically for their relatively small consonant phoneme inventories, a lack of labial
complex consonant clusters of considerable length ( cf. e.g. the cluster * / -Ɂtskhɹj- / in
PNI * / waɁtskhɹjeːnẽɁts / 'I fell over again' ⁰⁰⁶ ⁰¹¹ ⁰¹⁹ ²⁴⁵ ). The reconstructed phonemic
20
(1) Phonemic Inventory of Proto-Iroquoian
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant ɹ j w
VOWELS
Close iː uː
i u
Mid eː ẽː oː õː
e ẽ o õ
Open aː
a
phonetic voicing to [ dz ] and [ gʷ ] when they precede vowels, and because their
21
lengthening in PNI ( cf. Michelson 1988:55; see also Postal 1968 and Chafe 1970b for
than phonemes is theoretically also possible, but makes fluid diachronic description of
certain sound changes, especially those that pertain to * / ts / but not * / s /, somewhat
more cumbersome.
languages where contrary evidence is lacking, as in the case of Huron and Old
Onondaga; Cayuga and Six Nations Mohawk have the alveolar approximant / ɹ /, as
Akwesasne Mohawk and Oneida have a lateral approximant / l /; all surviving dialects
of Cherokee also have / l /, although Mooney notes that the extinct eastern dialect of
If any phonetic rules pertaining to consonants were present in PI, they are not
22
rule now common in some form to all of the surviving Iroquoian languages, is an areal
innovation that only later spread to both branches of Iroquoian following the separation
2.2.1 VOWELS
'look at' ¹⁷⁹ ( cf. Ch. { -àkàhth- }, PNI * { -atkahthw- } ), and PI * { -hskanẽ- } 'slow' ²⁵²
between Cherokee long / aː / and PNI short / a / in final syllables (cf. Rudes 1995:28),
( cf. Ch. { -àlȁːhsǔːl- }, PNI * { -aɹahso- } ), and on the basis of a regular correspondence
23
between Cherokee long / aː / and PNI long * / aː / in final syllables
PNI * { kahkaːɁ } ).
* / e / in non-final syllables (cf. Rudes 1995:20), as in PI * { -e- } 'go' ²⁰⁹ ( cf. Ch. { -è- },
PNI * { -e- } ), PI * { -je- } 'wake up' ²⁹⁹ ( cf. Ch. { -jè- }, PNI * { -je- } ), and
PI * { -we- } 'speak' ⁴⁷⁵ ( cf. Ch. { -wè- }, PNI * { -we- } ), and where Cherokee long
PI * { aɹeɁ } 'about' ¹⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. { àle̋ː }, PNI * { aɹeɁ } ) and PI * { kuːɹeh } 'acorn' ³⁴⁸
PI * { -eːɹ- } 'believe' ²¹¹ ( cf. Ch. { -èːl- }, PNI * { -eɹ- } ), and PI * { -nsteːɹ- } 'help' ³⁹¹
( cf. Ch. { -hstèːl- }, PNI * { -ansteɹihst- } ), and where Cherokee long / eː / corresponds
'be new' ¹⁸⁹ ( cf. Ch. { àtse̋ːɁ- }, PNI * { atseːɁ } ) and PI * { -kaweː- } 'paddle' ³³⁰
24
Cherokee short / o / and PNI short * / ẽ / in non-final syllables (cf. Rudes 1995:20), as
in PI * { -ẽɁ- } 'fall' ²²¹ ( cf. Ch. { -òɁ- }, PNI * { -ẽɁ- } ), PI * { -kẽ- } 'see' ³³²
( cf. Ch. { -kò- }, PNI * { -kẽ- } ), and PI * { -nẽh- } 'grain' ³⁶⁶ ( cf. Ch. { -nòh- },
PNI * { -nẽh- } ), and on the basis of a regular correspondence between Cherokee long
'water' ¹⁹⁶ ( cf. Ch. { àma̋ː }, PNI * { awẽɁ } ), PI * { kanõːnowẽɁ } 'pipe' ³²³
( cf. Ch. { kànʌ̃ː̀ nòwa̋ː }, PNI * { kanõnawẽɁ } ), and PI * { kihɹjuɁkẽh } 'chipmunk' ³⁴⁰
PI * { tsiːjẽh } 'otter, seal' ⁴⁵⁵ ( cf. Ch. { tsìːjőː }, PNI * { tsijẽh } ), though this is less
common. Note that while Rudes (1995:20) also posits that correspondence of Cherokee
roots meaning 'domesticate' and 'slow', the former root is probably not cognate, and the
ˣ { -hskẽnõ- }, with the latter form likely reflecting a variant in the Northern Iroquoian
Cherokee long / oː / and PNI short * / ẽ / in non-final syllables (cf. Rudes 1995:20), as
25
in PI * { -kẽːh } 'have seen' ³³² ( cf. Ch. { -kòːh- }, PNI * { -kẽh- } ), PI * { -nẽːhskʷ- }
'steal' ³⁶⁸ ( cf. Ch. { -nòːhsk- }, PNI * { -nẽhskw- } ), and PI * { -nẽːj- } 'sand, stone' ³⁶⁹
( cf. Ch. { -nòːj- }, PNI * { -nẽj- } ), and on the basis of a correspondence between
Cherokee long / aː / and PNI long * / ẽː / in final syllables (cf. Rudes 1995:20), as in
'1SG:3' ⁰¹⁹ ( cf. Ch. { tsì- }, PNI * { ke- } ) and PI * { hsi- } '2SG:3' ⁰²² ( cf. Ch. { hì- },
PNI * { hse- } ), and on the basis of a correspondence between Cherokee long / iː / and
PNI short * / e / in final syllables (cf. Rudes 1995:20), as in PI * { -hsnuːɹiɁ } 'be fast' ²⁵⁸
( cf. Ch. { -kȍːti ̋ː }, PNI * { -kaɁteɁ } ), and PI * { -õːtaɹiɁ } 'be a lake' ⁴¹²
( cf. Ch. { ʌ̃ː̀ tàli ̋ː }, PNI * { -õtaɹeɁ } ). Occasionally before consonant clusters beginning
( cf. Ch. { -tíɁl- }, PNI * { -teɁɹ- }, * { -tiɁɹ- } ), in which both short * / e / and short
26
* / i / are reflected in Northern Iroquoian. PI long * / iː / is reconstructed on the basis
syllables (cf. Rudes 1995:20), as in PI * { -nõːtsiː- } 'brain, head' ³⁸⁷ ( cf. Ch. { -nʌ̃ː̀ tsíː- },
PNI * { -nõtsi- } ), PI * { -tsiːɹ- } 'fire' ⁴⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. { -tsìːl- }, PNI * { -tsiɹ- } ), and
PI * { -wiːnõːh } 'young person' ⁴⁷⁸ ( cf. Ch. { -wi ̋ːnâː }, PNI * { -winõːh } ), and on the
syllables (cf. Rudes 1995:28), as in PI * { -iː- } 'exist' ²⁷⁵ ( cf. Ch. { -ìː- }, PNI * { -iːh } ).
( cf. Ch. { -oɁj- }, PNI * { -aɁnj- } ), PI * { -ɹhok- } 'paternal aunt' ⁴²⁵ ( cf. Ch. { -lhòk- },
assumed also to have been preserved as Cherokee long / oː / and as PNI short * / a / in
is assumed also to have been preserved as Cherokee long / oː / and as PNI long * / aː /
27
in final syllables, though no cognates are available.
'body' ³⁰⁴ ( cf. Ch. { -jèːlʌ̃Ɂ̀ - }, PNI * { -jeɹõɁ } ), PI * { -nõhs- } 'house' ³⁸²
( cf. Ch. { -nùːlʌ̃-̀ }, PNI * { -noɹõ- } ), and on the basis of a correspondence between
Cherokee long / aː / and PNI short * / õ / in final syllables (cf. Rudes 1995:20), as in
PI * { iːnõh } 'far' ²⁸¹ ( cf. Ch. { iːnâː }, PNI * { inõh } ); Rudes (1995:20) also posits that
the basis of the reconstructed root meaning 'slow', but see discussion under PI short
(cf. Rudes 1995:20), as in PI * { -kõːhs- } 'face' ³⁴⁵ ( cf. Ch. { -kʌ̃ː̀ hs- }, PNI * { -kõhs- } ),
PI * { -õːnh- } 'be alive' ⁴⁰⁶ ( cf. Ch. { -ʌ̃ː̀ hn- }, PNI * { -õnh- } ), and PI * { -Ɂnjõːhs- }
'nose' ⁴⁹⁴ ( cf. Ch. { -Ɂjʌ̃ː̀ hs- }, PNI * { -Ɂnjõhs- } ), and on the basis of a correspondence
(cf. Rudes 1995:20), as in PI * { -wiːnõːh } 'young person' ⁴⁷⁸ ( cf. Ch. { -wi ̋ːnâː },
PNI * { -winõːh } ).
28
PI short * / u / is reconstructed based on a correspondence between Cherokee short
( cf. Ch. { -nùːl- }, PNI * { -noɹ- } ), and PI * { -õːkuːh- } 'pass through' ⁴⁰³
( cf. Ch. { -ʌ̃ː̀ kùːh- }, PNI * { -õkoh- } ). Again, PI * / õː / is assumed also to have been
syllables, in accordance with reflexes of other PI long vowels, though no cognates are
available.
(cf. Lounsbury 1978:338, Rudes 1995:56), as in PI * { -k- } 'eat' ³¹⁶ ( cf. Ch. { -k- },
PNI * { -k- } ), PI * { -kaweː- } 'paddle' ³³⁰ ( cf. Ch. { -kàwèː- }, PNI * { -kaweː- } ), and
29
PI * { kuːɹeh } 'acorn' ³⁴⁸ ( cf. Ch. { kùːle̋ː }, PNI * { koɹeh } ), as well as where
PI * { ki- } '1SG:3' ⁰¹⁹ ( cf. Ch. { tsì- }, PNI * { ke- } ) and PI * { kiji- } '1SG:3A' ⁰²⁰
( cf. Ch. { tsìː- }, PNI * { khe- } ). Two cognates appear to show the opposite
PI * { kiːɹ } 'dog' ³⁴¹ ( cf. Ch. { kìːlhi ̋ː }, PNI * { tsiɹ } ). In the latter cases a shift in
represent an allomorph of the 3SG:3 pronominal prefix, and the 3SG:3 prefix of
association with other animal names ( cf. PNI * / tsihˈskoɁkoɁ / 'robin' ⁴⁵⁴,
* { wakʷ- } '3:1SG' ⁰²⁵ ( cf. Ch. { ȁːkʷ- }, PNI * { wakʷ- } ) and * { jõːkʷ- } '3A:1SG' ⁰²⁶
( cf. Ch. { ʌ̃ː̏ kʷ- }, PNI * { jõkʷ- } ). A significant number of cognates also show
30
'pick up' ³⁵⁰ ( cf. Ch. { -kì- }, PNI * { -kʷ- } ), PI * { -õːtakʷ- } 'remove from fire' ⁴¹¹
( cf. Ch. { -ʌ̃ː̀ tàkì- }, PNI * { -õtakw- }, * { -õtako- } ), and PI * { -uːkʷ- } 'remove from
water' ⁴⁷¹ ( cf. Ch. { -ùːkì- }, PNI * { -okw- }, * { -oko- } ); such sets are likewise
following vowel / i(ː) /. Previous works (Mithun 1984b:260, Rudes 1995:56) have
as between Ch. / àh(à)wi ̋ː / and a PNI form * / ˈaːkʷ / 'deer' (cf. Rudes 1995:42),
between Ch. / àjʌ̃ː̀ wi ̋ː / and PNI * / ˈõːkʷeh / 'person' ⁴⁰⁵, and between Ch. / nʌ̃ː̀ wôːthi ̋ː /
and PNI * / oˈnõhkʷaɁt / 'medicine' ²⁷⁹. The pairs just noted should not be dismissed as
archaic functional allomorphs that their cognate status can be questioned ( e.g. from a
hypothetical PI form ˣ / õ(ː)kʷih / one would expect a Cherokee reflex ˣ / ʌ̃ː̀ ki ̋ː /, and
31
'put into fire' ⁴⁰⁹ ( cf. Ch. { -ʌ̃ː̀ t- }, PNI * { -õt- } ), and PI * { tawiːhskaɹaɁ } 'flint' ⁴³⁷
PNI * / s / (cf. Rudes 1995:56), as in PI * { -kõːhs- } 'face' ³⁴⁵ ( cf. Ch. { -kʌ̃ː̀ hs- },
PI * { -Ɂnjõːhs- } 'nose' ⁴⁹⁴ ( cf. Ch. { -Ɂjʌ̃ː̀ hs- }, PNI * { -Ɂnjõhs- } ), as well as where
'2SG:3' ⁰²² ( cf. Ch. { hì- }, PNI * { hse- } ) and PI * { hsiji- } '2SG:3A' ⁰²³ ( cf. Ch. { hìː- },
PNI * { hshe- } ).
( cf. Ch. * { tsà- }, PNI * { tsa- } ), PI * { -atseːɁ } 'be new' ¹⁸⁹ ( cf. Ch. { àtse̋ːɁ- },
PNI * { atseːɁ } ), PI * { -tsiːɹ- } 'fire' ⁴⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. { -tsìːl- }, PNI * { -tsiɹ- } ), and
32
( cf. Ch. { kànʌ̃ː̀ nòwa̋ː }, PNI * { kanõnawẽɁ } ), and PI * { -nuːɹ- } 'corn husk' ³⁹²
( cf. Ch. { -nùːl- }, PNI * { -noɹ- } ). In at least one cognate PNI * / n / appears to have
( cf. Ch. { kùːle̋ː }, PNI * { koɹeh } ), PI * { -nuːɹõ- } 'be difficult' ³⁹³ ( cf. Ch. { -nùːlʌ̃-̀ },
PNI * { -noɹõ- } ), and PI * { -tsjoːɹ- } 'tobacco' ⁴⁶⁵ ( cf. Ch. { -tsòːl- }, PNI * { -tsjaɹ- } ).
( cf. Ch. { -kàwèː- }, PNI * { -kaweː- } ), and PI * { -wiːnõːh } 'young person' ⁴⁷⁸
( cf. Ch. { -wi ̋ːnâː }, PNI * { -winõːh } ), and on the basis of a correspondence in
word-initial position between Cherokee / Ø / followed by a long vowel with low falling
tone and PNI * / w /, as in PI * { wakʷ- } '3:1SG' ⁰²⁵ ( cf. Ch. { ȁːkʷ- }, PNI * { wakʷ- } )
and PI * { wantiː- } '3NS:3' ⁰⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. { ȁːnì- }, PNI * { wati- } ). At least two cognates
33
followed: PI * { awẽɁ } 'water' ¹⁹⁶ ( cf. Ch. { àma̋ː }, PNI * { awẽɁ } ), and
development can be seen, for example, in reflexes of PI * { -atawẽ- } 'swim' ¹⁶¹ and
PNI * / j / (cf. Rudes 1995:56), as in PI * { -je- } 'wake up' ²⁹⁹ ( cf. Ch. { -jè- },
PNI * { -je- } ), PI * { tsiːjẽh } 'otter, seal' ⁴⁵⁵ ( cf. Ch. { tsìːjőː }, PNI * { tsijẽh } ), and
PI * { -nẽːj- } 'sand, stone' ³⁶⁹ ( cf. Ch. { -nòːj- }, PNI * { -nẽj- } ), and on the basis of a
vowel with low falling tone and PNI * / j /, as in PI * { jeːtsa- } '3A:2SG' ⁰²⁹
( cf. Ch. { ȅːtsà- }, PNI * { jetsa- } ), PI * { ju- } '3:3SG' ⁰⁵⁴ ( cf. Ch. { ȕː- }, PNI * { jo- } ),
and PI * { juːntiː- } '3:3NS' ⁰⁶¹ ( cf. Ch. { ȕːnì- }, PNI * { joti- } ). A correspondence
between Ch. / Ciː / and PNI * / Che / is here also reconstructed as * / Ciji / in light of
( cf. Ch. { tsìː- }, PNI * { khe- } ) and * { hsiji- } '2SG:3A' ⁰²³ ( cf. Ch. { hìː- },
34
PI * { -ateːhẽːh- } 'be embarrassed' ¹⁶³ ( cf. Ch. { -àtèːhòːh- }, PNI * { -ateːhẽːh- } ),
PI * { -nawẽːh- } 'be melted' ³⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. { -nàwòːh- }, PNI * { -nawẽːh } ), and
PI * { -nẽh- } 'kernel' ³⁶⁶ ( cf. Ch. { -nòh- }, PNI * { -nẽh- } ), and where Cherokee / Ø /
( cf. Ch. { iːnâː }, PNI * { inõh } ), PI * { kuːɹeh } 'acorn' ³⁴⁸ ( cf. Ch. { kùːle̋ː },
PNI * { koɹeh } ), and PI * { tsiːjẽh } 'otter, seal' ⁴⁵⁵ ( cf. Ch. { tsìːjőː }, PNI * { tsijẽh } ).
/ Ɂ / will be found in place of the first / h / of a verb base when certain pronominal
prefixes are present ( e.g. Ch. / ȁːtèːhǒːhska̋ː / 'she's embarrassed', but / kàtèːɁǒːhska̋ː /
( cf. Ch. { -tsòɁ- }, PNI * { -tsaɁw- } ) and PI * { -ẽɁ- } 'drop, fall' ²²¹ ( cf. Ch. { -òɁ- },
position, as in PI * { aɹeɁ } 'about' ¹⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. { àle̋ː }, PNI * { aɹeɁ } ), PI * { awẽɁ }
'water' ¹⁹⁶ ( cf. Ch. { àma̋ː }, PNI * { awẽɁ } ), and PI * { -koɁtiɁ } 'be a lot' ³⁴⁴
35
subject to glottal alternation in Cherokee ( see 3.4 ) so that / h / will be found in place
of the first / Ɂ / of a verb base when certain pronominal prefixes are present
( e.g. Ch. / kòɁòhskáː / 'I'm falling', but / kòhòhska̋ː / 'it's falling' ). Note that Rudes
many are not. In nearly all of these exceptional cases PNI appears to have maintained
the original cluster, since a contrast exists in PNI that is not found in Cherokee
since PNI has both * / Ɂj / and * / Ɂnj /, whereas Cherokee contains only the former,
PNI * { -atkahthw- } ) and PI * { -tkẽː- } 'spoil' ⁴⁴⁶ ( cf. Ch. { -kòː- }, PNI * { -tkẽː- } ).
36
PI * { tniː- } '1+2DU:3' ⁰³⁶ ( cf. Ch. { tìː- }, PNI * { tni- } ). PI * / tw / can be
PNI * { -nehstwaɹ- } ) and PI * { -hstwih } 'be small' ²⁶³ ( cf. Ch. { -hstih },
/ hst / and PNI * / tsn /, as in PI * { tsniː- } '2DU&3' ⁰³¹ ( cf. Ch. { hstìː- },
PNI * / tsj /, as in PI * { -tsjoːɹ- } 'tobacco' ⁴⁶⁵ ( cf. Ch. { -tsòːl- }, PNI * { -tsjaɹ- } ) and
PI * { -tsjõɁt- } 'fish' ⁴⁶⁷ ( cf. Ch. { -tsàɁt- }, { -tsʌ̃Ɂ̀ t- }, PNI * { -itsjõɁt- } ). PI * / nst /
PNI * / nst /, as in PI * { -nsteːɹ- } 'help' ³⁹¹ ( cf. Ch. { -hstèːl- }, PNI * { -ansteɹihst- } ).
and PNI * / nh /, as in PI * { -õːnh- } 'be alive' ⁴⁰⁶ ( cf. Ch. { -ʌ̃ː̀ hn- }, PNI * { -õnh- } ).
37
PI * { -htsuːhw- } 'colour' ²⁶⁸ ( cf. Ch. { -hsùːhw- }, PNI * { -ahtsohw- } ).
/ hj / and PNI * / hnj /, as in PI * { -hnj- } 'neck' ²³⁹ ( cf. Ch. { -hj- }, PNI * { -hnj- } ),
( cf. Ch. { -òɁj- }, PNI * { -aɁnj- } ) and PI * { -Ɂnjõːhs- } 'nose' ⁴⁹⁴ ( cf. Ch. { -Ɂjʌ̃ː̀ hs- },
PNI * { -Ɂnjõhs- } ).
developed an epenthetic / ì / in Cherokee, and in these cases PNI again seems to have
maintained the original cluster because a contrast exists in PNI that is not found in
PNI * / kn /, since PNI has both * / ken / and * / kn /, whereas Cherokee has the
38
basis of a correspondence between Cherokee / sìn / and PNI * / sn /, as in
PI * { -hsnuːɹiɁ } 'be fast' ²⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. { -hsìnűːlîː }, PNI * { -hsnoɹeɁ } ). PI * / kn / can
PNI * { kni- } ) and PI * { hskniː- } '2:1.DU' ⁰¹⁷ ( cf. Ch. { hskìnìː- }, PNI * { hskni- } ).
* { wantiː- } '3NS:3' ⁰⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. { ȁːnì- }, PNI * { wati- } ), * { juːntiː- } '3:3NS' ⁰⁶¹
( cf. Ch. { kʌ̃ː̀ wànì- }, PNI * { kõwati- } ), and on the basis of a correspondence between
39
pronominal prefixes, * { want- } '3NS:3' ⁰⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. { ȁːn- }, PNI * { wẽn- } ),
* { juːnt- } '3:3NS' ⁰⁶¹ ( cf. Ch. { ȕːn- }, PNI * { jon- } ), and * { kõːwant- } '3A:3NSA' ⁰⁶⁸
small amount of cognates shared by Cherokee and PNI. Consonant clusters that are
* / w / or * / j /, R is * / n / or * / ɹ /, K is * / k / or * / kʷ / or * / t /, S is * / s / or
* / ts /, and H is * / h / or * / Ɂ /.
G RG HRG
SKG HSKG
SG
HG KHG HKHG
R KR SKR HSKR
SR HSR
HR
K RK
KK
SK RSK, HSK
HK
S HS
H RH HRH
KH
SH
40
Since nearly all of the cluster correspondences established for PI show retention of the
cluster in PNI and reduction or epenthesis of the cluster in Cherokee, it is possible that
the phonotactic constraints of PI were relatively close to those still found in the
Finally note that PI verbs, like Cherokee verbs, may have had independent and
independent and dependent verb forms can be illustrated through comparison of the
independent Cherokee verb form / ȕːjòːhlʌ̃ː̌ Ɂi ̋ː / 'he has shot it' with its corresponding
dependent form / ùːjòːhlʌ̃ː̋ Ɂîː / 'he who has shot it, him who shot it, his having shot it'
(cf. forms in Feeling & Pulte 1975:64, 354). The independent form, / ȕːjòːhlʌ̃ː̌ Ɂi ̋ː /,
occurs only as the verb in a matrix clause; it is characterized by low falling tone on the
initial syllable ( the reflex of a word-initial glide that has been lost – in this case
PI * / j / ) and by high rising tone ( the reflex of PI word accent – see 3.3 ) on the final
syllable. In contrast, the dependent form, / ùːjòːhlʌ̃ː̋ Ɂîː /, occurs only as a verb in a
subordinate clause ( e.g. "she saw him shoot it" ) or as a deverbalized noun
( e.g. "the shooter", "him who shot it" ); it is characterized by low level tone on the
initial syllable ( owing to the absence of an initial glide in PI ) and by high rising tone
on the penultimate syllable rather than the final syllable. Like Cherokee, PNI appears to
41
have maintained word-initial glide loss as an indicator of deverbalized nouns and of
nouns in general (cf. Michelson 1988:43 and 2.10 below), but has generalized PI
PI had at least three lexical categories: verbs, nouns, and particles. These lexical
categories were continued in PNI, while Cherokee has added a fourth category,
adjectives, which are essentially stative verbs lacking the full range of inflection shown
by other verbs, and requiring the reflex of an existential auxiliary verb * { -iː- }
'be, be the one, exist' ²⁷⁵ in order to convey the full range of inflection associated with
The verb was the most complex of the three lexical categories of PI and appears to
42
Prepronominal prefixes, an optional component of the verb, conveyed a number of
different ideas ranging from temporal co-ordination ( e.g. the coincident prefix ) to
direction ( e.g. the translocative and cislocative prefixes ). Eight prepronominal prefixes
are found in Cherokee and ten can be reconstructed for PNI; the exact number of
described in 2.4.
The non-singular animate prefix, likewise an optional component of the verb, could
precede any pronominal prefix that had '3A' ( animate third person ) as one of its
arguments and make this argument non-singular. The non-singular animate prefix is
described in 2.5.
the agent and the patient involved in the action or state of the verb
( e.g. 'it acts on me', 'you act on it', 'it acts on them', etc ). PI had an inventory of at least
components of the verb, directed the effect or benefit of an action back on a pronominal
43
agent, somewhat like the reflexive and 'middle voice' of Indo-European languages. The
The final component of the PI verb was the verb base, which had four lexically-
determined aspectual forms ( present, punctual, imperfective, and perfect ) and could
also contain one or more lexically-determined derivational elements. The verb base is
described in 2.9.
allomorph and a general prevocalic allomorph, and at least four of the prefixes
( the partitive, distributive, cislocative, and repetitive ) seem to have had two sets of
allomorphs, one of which was associated with independent verb forms, the other with
dependent forms. Reflexes of the prepronominal prefixes that can be reconstructed for
PI occur in a set order in both Cherokee and PNI, and this order differs only with regard
to the respective position of the translocative and partitive prefixes: in Cherokee the
44
translocative precedes the partitive, while in PNI the partitive preceded the
translocative. The order of the prefixes in Cherokee is here interpreted as the archaic
pattern, if only because the allomorphy of the partitive includes a separate set of
dependent allomorphs ( typical of those prepronominal prefixes nearer the verb base ),
whereas the allomorphy of the translocative does not seem to have differentiated
coincident * / ts / ⁰⁰²
| translocative * / wi / ⁰⁰⁵
| | partitive * / ijiː / ⁰⁰⁴
| | | duplicative * / teː / ⁰⁰⁷
| | | | future * / ẽː / ⁰⁰⁸
| | | | | cislocative * / t / ⁰¹⁰
| | | | | | repetitive * / tsiː / ⁰¹¹
| | | | | | |
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
The PI coincident prefix appears to have had a prevocalic allomorph * / ts / ⁰⁰², and
at least one of its grammatical functions can be reconstructed for PI, which was the
marking of temporal coordination; compare the examples from Cherokee and Mohawk
45
(5) Ch. / tsìtsìːkòːɁʌ̃ː̋ kîː tsìtsìːjɁkhěːhʌ̃Ɂ́ sʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː /
{ tsì-tsìː-kòːɁ-ʌ̃ː̋ kîː tsì-tsìːj-àhkhěːhʌ̃Ɂ́ s-ʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː }
COIN-1SG:3A-see.PERF-ASRT.DEP COIN-1SG:3A-chase.PERF-ASRT.INDP
'when I saw him, I chased him' (cf. Cook 1979:62)
Etymologically the coincident prefix was probably also the source of various relativizer
particles of Northern Iroquoian languages ( e.g. Mo. / tsi /, Oe. / tsiɁ /, Oo. / tshaɁ /,
Ca. / tshẽh /, Hu. / stiɁ / ), all of which precede relative clauses and carry a meaning
'who, which, when, where, that'. The coincident prefix shows a comparable relativizer
46
an action was taking place at a distance from, or in a direction leading away from, the
speaker or the location of a narrative; compare the examples from Cherokee and
Tu. / wɛhraɁnə̃ˈkohthaɁ /
{ wɛ-hr-aɁnə̃kohthaɁ }
TLOC-3SG:3-go.beyond.IMPF
'he goes beyond it' (cf. Rudes 1999:490)
It is possible that the translocative prefix was also used to indicate a superlative
meaning in connection with certain stative verbs ( i.e. 'the most X' ). Compare the
Tu. / wɛhˈrahsthə̃ /
{ wɛ-hr-ahsthə̃ }
TLOC-3SGM:3-be.small.PERF
'he is the smallest' (cf. Rudes 1999:489)
47
In independent verb forms the PI partitive prefix had at least a prevocalic allomorph
allomorph * / iːji /. The primary grammatical function of the partitive appears to have
occurs when a number greater than two precedes a noun enumerating living things, in
which case it appears on an intermediary stative verb meaning 'be that many'; compare
the forms in (9) from Cherokee and Mohawk, where reflexes of the PI partitive are
underlined.
A comparable pattern may thus be posited for PI. The partitive prefix probably also
reconstructed; compare the sets in (10) from Cherokee and Mohawk where reflexes of
48
(10) Ch. / hìla̋ː ǐːka̋ːɁîː / 'how much? how many?'
/ hìla̋ː ǐːkóːhi ̋ːtâː / 'how long?' (time)
/ hi ̀la̋ː ìkánʌ̃ː̀ hi ̋ːtâː / 'how long?' (distance)
/ hi ̀la̋ː ìjátànʌ̃ː̀ hi ̋ːtâː / 'how far'
/ kàtȍː ìjűːstîː / 'what kind?' (cf. forms in Feeling & Pulte 1975)
Answers to the questions in (10), which will likewise specify quality or quantity, also
employ the partitive prefix ( e.g. Ch. / sǔːtáli ̋ː nìkáːɁa̋ː / 'there are six of them',
appeared before the future prefix; it may also have had a pre-consonantal allomorph
independent verb forms. Although reflexes of the duplicative are among the most
frequently employed prefixes in both Cherokee and PNI, the precise grammatical
pluralize adjectives referring to inanimate objects ( e.g. Ch. / ùwe̋ːthîː / 'old thing' Æ
/ tsùwe̋ːthîː / 'old things', cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:184 ), as well as to signal the
49
plurality of an object in transitive verbs that have a singular patient pronominal prefix
( e.g. Ch. / tsìkȍːwthi ́ha̋ː / 'I see it (one object)' Æ / tèːtsíkȍːwthi ́ha̋ː / 'I see them
(several objects)', cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:247 ), and to signal plural, individual acts in
( e.g. Ch. / kàtsìːjàɁlʌ̃ː́ Ɂi ̀ha̋ː / 'I am tying them up [one act]' Æ / tèːkátsìːjàɁlʌ̃ː́ Ɂìha̋ː /
'I am tying them up [several acts]', cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:257 ). The Cherokee
duplicative also occurs obligatorily in certain verbs that share no easily definable
( e.g. Ch. / tȁːhlìhkéha̋ː / 'it is rocking [here and there, back and forth]',
cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:68; / tèːkálìɁthàtéːka̋ː / 'I am jumping [up off the ground]',
cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:69 ). In PNI, the duplicative is used to signal the presence or
involvement of two similar objects ( e.g. PNI * / tekaˈnõhsakeːh / 'two houses' ¹⁵² ³⁸²,
PNI * / teˈwatjaɁks / 'it breaks in two' ²⁹⁷ ), and occurs obligatorily in a number of verbs
that share no easily definable characteristic apart, perhaps, from a subjective sense of
duality or a change of state ( e.g. PNI * / tekwẽˈninkẽɁts / 'I speak [out against
the silence]' ²⁸⁰ ⁴⁷⁶, PNI * / teˈkehkhwaɁ / 'I pick it up [off the ground]' ³⁵⁰ ).
terms it would seem to have expressed the presence or involvement of more than one
50
entity, more than one physical state, or more than one location.
The future prefix, which is present in all Northern Iroquoian languages but absent
from synchronic Cherokee grammar, is believed to have been present in PI on the basis
prefixes in Cherokee future tense verb forms, as previously observed by Cook (1979:68).
cislocative prefix ( which is used to form a periphrastic future construction ) and the
duplicative prefix; this is believed to be the reflex of a PI future prefix * / ẽː / ⁰⁰⁸, which
has a reflex in the same position in PNI. Cherokee and Oneida forms are presented in
sequence underlined.
Oe. / tʌ̃ˈtjôːkalaweɁ /
{ t-ʌ̃-t-jo-aɁkalaɁweɁ }
DUPL-FUT-CLOC-3:3SG-become.night.PUNC
'it will get dark again' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:315)
51
Presumably the PI future prefix had a meaning and function similar to that found in
PNI, where co-occurrence with the cislocative and duplicative prefixes is not obligatory,
and the prefix places events in the future tense ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkheːkẽh /
'I see someone' ³³² Æ * / ẽˈkheːkẽɁ / 'I will see someone' ). Whether a punctual aspect
base was also required when the future occurred, as is the case in Northern Iroquoian, is
( cf. e.g. PNI * / ˈkaːtsih / 'come here!' ³²⁹, Ch. / khěːna̋ː / 'come here!' (King 1975:67) ).
The primary grammatical function of the cislocative was to indicate that an action was
taking place at a distance from, or in a direction leading toward, the speaker or the
location of a narrative, and it was thus the functional inverse of the translocative
morpheme; compare the examples from Cherokee and Mohawk in (12), where the reflex
52
(12) Ch. / tàjáːɁi ̋ː /
{ tàj-àːɁìː-a̋ː }
CLOC-3SG:3-walk.PRES-INDC.INDP
'she's coming this way' (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:251)
Mo. / ˈtâːjʌ̃Ɂ /
{ t-aɁ-jʌ̃-eɁ }
CLOC-3I:3-walk.PUNC
'she's coming this way' (cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:351)
The PI repetitive prefix seems to have had a basic form * / tsiː / ⁰¹¹ before certain
pronominal prefixes that involve the second person in imperative verbs; PNI maintains
this allomorph only before the 1+2DU:3 and 1+2PL:3 prefixes, while Cherokee
maintains it before the 3:2SG prefix as well. In every other environment, however,
Cherokee has reshaped the repetitive prefix, making secure reconstruction of other
allomorphs difficult. The primary grammatical function of the repetitive was to indicate
that an action was repeated ( 'again' ) or that an action restored the original position of
an object ( 'back' ); compare the Cherokee and Cayuga forms in (13), where reflexes of
53
Ca. / tsiˈtwahsheːt /
{ tsi-tw-ahsheːt }
REP-1+2PL:3-count.PUNC
'let's count it again!' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:681)
The PI non-singular animate prefix * / ka / ⁰¹² could precede any pronominal prefix
( 2.6 ) having '3A' ( 'animate third person' ) as one of its arguments. The effect of the
non-singular animate prefix was to change the meaning of the '3A' argument to that of
non-singular animate (NSA). Compare the sets in (14) and (15), taken from Cherokee
(Feeling & Pulte 1975:268) and Cayuga (Froman et al. 2002:731), where a reflex of the
non-singular animate prefix, underlined, is added before the pronominal prefix in order
to alter the meaning; note that the '3I' or 'indefinite third person' pronominal prefix
series of Tuscarora is cognate with the '3A' pronominal prefix series of PI and Cherokee.
54
(15) Ca. / ɁaˈkheːkẽɁ / / ɁakaːˈkheːkẽɁ /
{ aɁ-khe-kẽɁ } { aɁ-ka-khe-kẽɁ }
FACT-1SG:3I-see.PUNC FACT-NSA-1SG:3I-see.PUNC
'I saw someone' 'I saw them'
PNI preserved the original range of the non-singular animate prefix intact, although
subsequent changes in many Northern Iroquoian languages have either altered its
referencing both a grammatical agent and a grammatical patient. In the present work
these core arguments are separated by a colon, with the grammatical agent preceding
the grammatical patient, e.g. * / ki / '1SG:3' ( 'I act on it' ), * / wakʷi / '3:1SG'
that the verbs meaning 'I walk' and 'I am cold' might literally be translated as 'I walk it'
55
(1SG:3) or 'it colds me' (3:1SG). Note that while prefixes employed with semantically
prefixes ( cf. e.g. Lounsbury 1978:337, Chafe 1967:13-19 ), all pronominal prefixes are
capable of bearing transitive meaning when paired with semantically transitive verbs,
and for purposes of diachronic description, particularly with regard to etymology and
person argument ( '3' ) could not occur as grammatical patient of a perfect aspect verb
base ( see 2.9 ), and instead occurred as grammatical agent; as a result, while e.g.
'I see it' (imperfective) was expressed with the prefix 1SG:3, the equivalent perfect form,
'I have seen it', was expressed with a prefix '3:1SG', yielding a form that also meant
'it has seen me'; this peculiarity is preserved in both branches of Iroquoian, and is
illustrated with examples in (16) and (17) from Cherokee (Feeling & Pulte 1975:268)
and Mohawk (Michelson 1973:69), where the relevant pronominal prefixes are
underlined.
56
(16) Ch. / tsìkȍːwhthìhskóɁi ̋ː /
{ tsì-kòHwàhthìhsk-óɁi ̋ː }
1SG:3-see.IMPF-HAB.INDP
'I see it'
Ch. / ȁːkìkòːhʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː /
{ ȁːkì-kòːh-ʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː }
3:1SG-see.PERF-ASRT.INDP
'I have seen it' ( also: 'it has seen me' )
Mo. / ˈwakkʌ̃ /
{ wak-kʌ̃ }
3:1SG-see.PERF
'I have seen it' ( also: 'it has seen me' )
various semantic distinctions among grammatical agents and patients. For descriptive
purposes the prefixes can be divided into 5 pronominal prefix groups, which are
described below. The following conventions of parsing are used in the descriptions that
follow: numbers (1, 2, 3) in all cases designate person; an ampersand (&) designates
ambiguity of agent and patient ( the 2PL&3 prefix, for example, could mean '2PL:3' or
'3:2PL' ); 3A designates animate third person; the small capital letter sequences SG, DU,
57
PL, and NS in all cases designate singular, dual, plural, and non-singular number; where
the number categories SG, DU, PL, or NS are attached with a period to the prefix, they
designate the maximum number of people present in either group ( e.g. 1:2.DU signified
'first person acts on second person, where at least one group has two people, and
PI pronominal prefixes appear to have had two principal allomorphs, one of which
occurred before at least some consonants and clusters ( e.g. * / ki / '1SG:3' ), the other
of which occurred before vowels and possibly before certain other consonants and
second persons; agents were distinguished from patients, and number was distinguished
as singular when both agent and patient were singular, dual when one or both were
dual and neither was plural, and plural when one or both were plural. The six
58
(18) PI Pronominal Prefix Group 1
Morphemes that can be identified in PI pronominal prefix group 1 are { k(i) } 'first
person singular agent', { hs(i) } 'second person agent', { kʷ(i) } 'first person patient',
{ õː } 'second person patient', and { niː } 'dual number'. The 1:2.SG prefix had an
allomorph * / kõːj / before vowels ( cf. Ch. / kʌ̃ː̀ j /; PNI * / kõj / ), the 2:1.SG prefix
had an allomorph * / hsk / before vowels ( cf. Ch. / hsk /; PNI * / hsk / ), and the
2:1.DU prefix had an allomorph * / hskn / before vowels ( cf. Ch. / hskìn /;
PNI * / hskn / ); the initial / h / of the 2:1 prefixes may have been lost word-initially
and when following a consonant. The forms of the 1:2.DU and 1:2PL prefixes differ in
PNI and Cherokee, but since the Cherokee forms are morphologically opaque, they may
or second person and third persons. Agents were distinguished from patients, and
animate third persons were distinguished from inanimate or unmarked third persons. PI
59
pronominal prefix group 2 is presented in (19).
Morphemes that can be identified in pronominal prefix group 2 are { k(i) } 'first person
singular agent', { hs(i) } 'second person agent', { wa } 'third person agent', { kʷ(i) } 'first
person patient', and { tsa } 'third person agent acting on second person singular patient'.
A morpheme { ji } was added initially to signal an animate third person agent and
respectively. The 1SG:3 prefix had an allomorph * / k / before vowels ( cf. Ch. / k /;
PNI * / k / ), and the 1SG:3A prefix had an allomorph * / kijij / before vowels
( cf. Ch. / tsìːj /; PNI * / khej / ). The 2SG:3 prefix had an allomorph * / hs / before
vowels ( cf. Ch. / h /; PNI * / hs / ), and the 2SG:3A prefix had an allomorph * / hsijij /
before vowels ( cf. Ch. / hìːj /; PNI * / hshej / ). The 3:1SG prefix had an allomorph
60
* / wakʷ / before at least some vowels ( cf. Ch. / ȁːkʷ / before all vowels;
PNI * / wakʷ / before bases beginning in * / a / ), and the 3A:1SG prefix had an
allomorph * / jõːkʷ / before at least some vowels ( cf. Ch. / ʌ̃ː̏ kʷ / before all vowels;
PNI * / jõkʷ / before bases beginning in * / a / ). The 3:2SG prefix had an allomorph
* / ts / before vowels ( cf. Ch. / ts /; PNI * / ts / ), and the 3A:2SG prefix had an
second persons and a third person. Agents and patients were not distinguished, but
second person dual and plural number were distinguished. PI pronominal prefix group 3
is presented in (20).
second person' and { niː } 'dual number'. The final element of the 2PL&3 prefix is
unclear. The 2DU&3 prefix had an allomorph * / tsn / before at least some vowels
( cf. Ch. / hst / before all vowels; PNI * / tsn / before vowels other than * / a / ).
61
first persons and a third person. Dual and plural number were distinguished in first
persons, agents were distinguished from patients, and inclusive and exclusive first
person agents were distinguished. The prefixes of group 4 are presented in (21).
Morphemes that can be identified in pronominal prefix group 4 are { t } 'inclusive first
person agent', { kʷ(i) } 'first person patient', and { niː } 'dual number'. The reconstructed
forms of the 1+2PL:3 and 3:1PL prefixes are unclear, as are the forms of the exclusive
first person agent prefixes. The 1DU:3 prefix had an allomorph * / tn / before at least
some vowels ( cf. Ch. / ȉːn /; PNI * / tn / before vowels other than * / a / ) and the
3:1DU prefix had an allomorph * / kn / before at least some vowels ( cf. Ch. / kìn /;
persons. Singular and non-singular number were distinguished in either the agent or
patient prefix, and unique 3A:3SGA and 3A:3NSA prefixes encoded interaction between
62
two animate third persons. PI pronominal prefix group 5 is presented in (22).
Morphemes that can be identified in pronominal prefix group 5 are { ka } 'third person',
{ wa } 'third person', { juː } 'third person patient', and { ntiː } 'non-singular number'.
the non-singular animate prefix. The functional or semantic distinction between the
prefixes * / ka / ⁰⁵¹ and * / wa / ⁰⁵² is not clear (cf. Cook 1979:16) and they appear to
associated with certain verbs on a lexically-conditioned basis, while in PNI they merged
Iroquoian third person pronominal prefixes does not project the last four prefixes in
(22) back to PI, I regard the Northern and Southern Iroquoian forms as probable
63
cognates in light of their preconsonantal allomorphs in * / i(ː) / and prevocalic
allomorphs in * / n / ( cf. e.g. Ch. / ȕːnìː- /, / ȕːn- / and PNI * / joti- /, * / jon- /
'3NS:3' ).
PNI * / k / before certain verb bases beginning in a vowel ). The 3SG:3 prefix * / wa /
had an allomorph * / w / before vowels ( cf. Ch. / Ø / with low falling tone on a
following vowel; PNI * / w / before certain verb bases beginning in a vowel ), and the
( cf. Ch. / kʌ̃ː̀ w /; PNI * / kõw / ). The 3:3SG prefix * / juː / replaced at least the vowel
* / aː / of a following base, as do its reflexes in PNI and Cherokee. The 3NS:3 prefix had
an allomorph * / want / or * / wan / before vowels ( cf. Ch. / ȁːn /; PNI * / wan / or
* / kõːwan / before vowels ( cf. Ch. / kʌ̃ː̀ wan /; PNI * / kõwan / or * / kõwẽn / ). The
3:3NS prefix had an allomorph * / juːnt / or * / juːn / before vowels ( cf. Ch. / ȕːn /;
PNI * / jon / ).
Four pronominal prefixes whose forms in Cherokee and PNI are at least partially
64
(23) PI Pronominal Prefixes Ending in a Plural Morpheme
the case of * / hskʷ… / ⁰¹⁸ where it corresponds to / ìːj / before a vowel. A plural
morpheme with the form * / iwa / before consonants seems plausible, with reduction of
* / waˈɁakweɁ / 'we go' ²⁰⁹ ( rather than ˣ / waˈɁaːkʷeɁ / ) could then be attributed to
( e.g. pre-PNI * / waˈɁakʷweɁ / 'we go' ). Evidence from other reflexes that would
Another subset of pronominal prefixes is also worthy of note, due to the odd effect
that these prefixes have on allomorphy both in Cherokee and PNI. In Cherokee, reflexes
65
prepronominal prefix allomorphs that show a vowel / e / rather than / a /
(cf. Cook 1979:83). Similarly in PNI, reflexes of the prefixes * { tsa } '3:2SG', * { tsniː }
as well as certain allomorphs that show a vowel * / i / rather than * / e /. The cause of
this variation is unclear, but it is conceivable that at least some of the prefixes, all of
which involve the second person, originally began in a vowel * / e / or similar segment
that has subsequently been lost ( i.e. * / (e)tsa /, * / (e)tsniː /, * / (e)tniː /, etc ); this
* / jeːtsa / '3A:2SG' in PI. Although the vowel alternants preceding these prefixes almost
The PI reflexive prefix had the basic form * / ataːt / ⁰⁷⁶ ( cf. Ch. / àtàːt /,
PNI * / atat / ), while the PI semireflexive prefix had the basic forms * / at / ⁰⁷⁷ and
66
* / aɹ / ⁰⁷⁸, apparently on a lexically-determined basis ( cf. Ch. / àt /, / àl /;
rendered a meaning 'self'; compare the forms in (24) and (25) from Cherokee
(Feeling & Pulte 1975:296) and Old Mohawk (cf. Bruyas 1863:47).
Ch. / ȁːkʷàtàːkòːhʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː /
{ ȁːkʷ-àtàː-kòːh-ʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː }
3:1SG-REFL-see.PERF-ASRT.INDP
'I have seen myself'
OMo. / wakaˈtatkʌ̃ /
{ wak-atat-kʌ̃ }
3:1SG-REFL-see.PERF
'I have seen myself'
The semireflexive prefix did not always render a consistent meaning, though often
verbs containing it involved a part of oneself rather than the whole; in such cases it
might be translated as 'own', e.g. 'I wash my (own) hands', 'I cut my (own) hair', etc.
67
Just as often, however, the semireflexive had become an integral part of the verb base,
consisting apparently of semireflexive * { at } ⁰⁷⁷ and a verb base related to the noun
root * { awẽɁ } ¹⁹⁶ 'water' ( cf. PNI * / joˈtaːwẽːh / 'she has swum'; Ch. / ȕːtàwóːɁʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː /
'she has swum, cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:8 ). Reflexes of semireflexive * { at } and
* { aɹ } are found in both branches of Iroquoian, although PNI has greatly generalized
PI verbs could optionally incorporate a noun stem if the noun was the grammatical
( e.g. 'the house is strong' Æ 'it house-strongs'; 'I build houses' Æ 'I house-build' ). Noun
found in Woodbury (1975); with only minor semantic differences, it may be extended to
all of the Northern Iroquoian languages, and probably to PI as well. For purposes of
68
(26) Mo. / kanũhsoˈwaːnʌ̃ / ( < PNI * / kanõhsoˈwaːnẽh / ²³⁷ ³⁸² )
{ ka-nũhs-owanʌ̃ }
3:3SG-house-JOIN-be.big.PERF
'the house is strong'
As can be seen in the examples in (26), the incorporated noun stem is preposed to the
verb base, and a 'joiner vowel' * / a / ⁰⁷⁹ is inserted between the two if the noun stem
ends in a consonant and the verb root begins in a consonant (cf. Michelson 1988:157).
Residual traces of this process survive in Cherokee, although the number of verbs that
Among the few Cherokee verbs that do permit some degree of productive noun
incorporation is { -t- } 'lay down, put down', some reflexes of which are presented in
69
(27) Ch. / ȁːtíɁa̋ː /
{ ȁː-tìɁ-a̋ː }
3SG:3-lay.down.PRES-INDC.INDP
'she is laying it down' (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:10)
More numerous in Cherokee than the examples in (27), however, are bound or
clothing, the noun stem exists independently in the language, but the accompanying
verb root is bound and does not occur elsewhere ( e.g. Ch. { -àlìhstùːlʌ̃Ɂ̀ k- }
Ch. { -àlìhstùːlàkìː- } 'take off a cap', { -àhsùːlàkìː- } 'take off pants', { -àlìjèɁsùːlàkìː- },
'take off shoes', cf. bases in Feeling & Pulte 1975 ). In other cases, involving what are
most often described as classificatory verbs (cf. King 1975:97, Cook 1979:185), a
70
recurring incorporated noun stem no longer exists independently in the language, but
precedes a verb root that does occur independently ( e.g. Ch. { -nèːhwàhth- }
'find a liquid', { -jʌ̃ː̀ hwàhth- } 'find a long object', { -nàhwàhth- } 'find a flexible object';
Ch. { -nèːhwìːt- } 'take a liquid', { -jʌ̃ː̀ hwìːt- } 'take a long object', { -nàhwìːt- } 'take a
flexible object', cf. bases in Feeling & Pulte 1975 ). In still other cases, perhaps most
numerous, semantically related verbs clearly contain the same verbal element, but
neither the noun stem nor the verb base occurs independently in the language
( cf. Ch. { -àhskòːl- } 'dig', { -hnùːkòːl- } 'claw', { -àtlàkòːl- } 'scratch', { -hstʌ̃ː̀ kòːl- }
'grind corn', { -àtànhòːkòːl- } 'undergo scratch ritual'; Ch. { -ʌ̃ː̀ hskʷàlòɁts- } 'crack',
'stub one's toe', { -àtàːhskʷàlòɁts- } 'bump one's head', cf. bases in King 1975 ). Forms
such as these suggest that PI did indeed have productive noun incorporation, on a level
comparable to that found in PNI, and support previous studies asserting that productive
71
2.9 VERB BASE
Under the heading of the PI verb base I include everything that Lounsbury
unpredictability ( the form that derivational and aspectual suffixes have is determined
lexically by the verb base ), and in part due to a subsequent development in Cherokee,
whereby productive and predictable modal suffixes were affixed to the PI 'verb base' as
a whole.
The PI verb base appears to have had four principal forms, each of which reflected a
different tense or aspect. The present base, which has reflexes in the present base of
Cherokee and in the present base of certain verbs in Mohawk and Cayuga, signaled that
an event was taking place at the present time. The punctual verb base, which has
reflexes in imperative verbs in both Cherokee and Northern Iroquoian, signaled that an
event was taking place at any single point in time except the present. The imperfective
base, which has reflexes in the imperfective base of Cherokee and in the imperfective
base of certain verbs in Mohawk and Cayuga, signaled that the action of a verb was
incomplete and ongoing. The perfect verb base, which has reflexes in the perfect base of
Cherokee, and in the perfect base ( sometimes referred to as the stative base ) of
72
modern Northern Iroquoian languages, signaled that the action of a verb was complete
Because relatively few verb bases remain cognate between Cherokee and PNI
( and the few that do have often been reshaped in Cherokee through addition of
derivational elements ), cognate status of the verb bases themselves is often difficult to
demonstrate, though their meanings remain essentially the same. Cherokee and PNI
reflexes for the verbs 'climb over' ( PI * { -atawẽːhɹat- } ¹⁶² ) and 'see'
( PI * { -kẽ(ː)- } ³³² ) are presented in (28) and (29) in their four basic forms.
Although the shapes of verb bases like those in (28) and (29) were lexically
determined and formally unpredictable, they nonetheless did often contain derivational
73
and aspectual morphemes with recurring forms that followed the constant form of the
verb root, occasionally with vowels and glottal consonants of uncertain meaning
a causative-instrumental morpheme that had the basic form * { -t- } ⁰⁸² or * { -hst- } ⁰⁸³
and the meaning 'make X, cause to X, use to X'; a reversive morpheme that had the basic
form * { -kʷ- } ⁰⁸⁵ and carried the meaning 'un-X', 'de-X'; and a dative-benefactive
morpheme that had the basic form * { -ts- } ⁰⁸⁸ and carried the meaning 'X to, X for, X
morphemes, but their meanings and their placement at some point after the constant
Many PI nouns were probably dependent verb forms that functioned as noun
phrases – e.g. 'they hunt' (independent verb form) Æ 'they who hunt; hunters'
(dependent verb form/deverbalized noun); 'they are hunting again' (independent verb
form) Æ 'those who are hunting again; re-hunters' (dependent verb form/deverbalized
noun); 'one uses it to hunt' (independent verb form) Æ 'that which one uses to hunt;
hunting place' (dependent verb form/deverbalized noun). Deverbalized nouns had the
74
same form and morphological structure as verbs except that word-initial glides were
lost from pronominal prefixes so that, for example, the PI pronominal prefix
* { wakʷi- } '3:1SG' has become * { akʷi- } '3:1SG' in a deverbalized noun, and the
pronominal prefix * { juːntiː- } '3:3NS' has become * { uːntiː- } '3:3NS'. This distinction
has been retained in nouns both in Cherokee and in PNI, so that one finds
e.g. Ch. / àkèːhjűːtsâː / '(she is a) girl' rather than / ȁːkèːhjùːtsa̋ː / and Mo. / aˈkũːkʷeh /
stem optionally preceded by a noun prefix and optionally followed by a noun suffix.
The two basic forms of the noun prefix were based on the 3SG:3 pronominal prefix
( both * / ka / ⁰⁵¹ and * / wa / ⁰⁵² ) and the 3:3SG pronominal prefix ( * / juː / ⁰⁵⁴ ).
These were lexically conditioned: some nouns took the former while others took the
latter. As in deverbalized nouns, word-initial glides were lost in simplex noun prefixes,
has become * / a /, and the allomorph * / juː / has become * / uː / ( cf. Ch. / Ø- /,
form of the noun suffix may have been a glottal stop preceded by * / a / when a root
ended in a consonant ( cf. PNI * / -aɁ / ¹²³ ). Some examples of simple nouns in PI
75
include * { kanõːnowẽɁ } 'pipe' ³²³ ( cf. Ch. / kànʌ̃ː̀ nòwa̋ː /; PNI * / kaˈnõnawẽɁ / ) and
noun with a prefix from one of two sets of possessive pronominal prefixes. One of the
sets seems to have been used to mark inalienable possession ( the soul and body parts ),
and was cognate with pronominal prefixes where the possessor was agent and '3' was
( e.g. 'my house' ), and was cognate with pronominal prefixes where '3' was agent and
the possessor was patient ( e.g. * / wakʷi / '3:1SG' Æ * / akʷi / '1SG.ALPOSS' ). The
only formal difference between pronominal prefixes and their possessive counterparts
again seems to have been loss of initial glides: cf. Mo. / wakeˈnũhsajʌ̃Ɂ / 'I have a house'
(verb), but / akeˈnũhsaɁ / 'my house' (noun); Mo. / jũkwaˈnũhsajʌ̃Ɂ / 'we have a house',
but / ũkwaˈnũhsaɁ / 'our house' (cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:198). There appears also to
have been a tendency, if not a rule, whereby inalienably possessed body parts occurred
with a locative suffix ( cf. PNI * / kjeˈɹõɁkeh / '(on/at) my body' ³⁰⁴ and
76
dependent stative verbs, e.g. * { akʷi-ɹhok } 'my paternal aunt' ⁴²⁵
( lit. 'it is a paternal aunt to you' ), * { kõː-ɹhok } 'I am your paternal aunt'
( lit. I am a paternal aunt to you )', etc. As in the first two examples, pronominal
prefixes with an argument '3' appear to have been used in place of prefixes that
( cf. PNI * / aˈkeɹhok / 'my paternal aunt' ⁴²⁵, rather than ˣ / jõˈkeɹhok /;
The third lexical category of PI consisted of particles, which were small, generally
replacement, even among sub-branches of Northern Iroquoian, only a few particles have
cognates in both branches of Iroquoian; among those that can be projected back to PI
are a temporal particle * { nẽh } 'now, then, at this or that time' ³⁶⁷ ( cf. Ch. / nǒːkʷʌ̃ː̋ /;
PNI * / ˈoːnẽ / ), * { iːnõh } 'far, distant' ²⁸¹ ( cf. Ch. / i ̋ːnâː / 'far', / ìːnʌ̃ː̀ hi ̋ː / 'too far';
PNI * / ˈiːnõh / 'far' ), and * { aɹeɁ } 'about, approximately' ¹⁵⁸ ( cf. Ch. / àle̋ː /;
77
PNI * / ˈaːɹeɁ / ). PI seems also to have had a particle of the form * { nV(H) } ³⁵²
( cf. Ch. / náː /; PNI * / neɁ / ) that preceded nouns and may have served a deictic
function ( 'the', 'this', 'that' ) or been used to mark out deverbalized nouns. Iroquoian
morphologically, and are thus also effectively particles; due to lexical replacement,
however, only one number is reconstructable at the level of PI: * / hwihsk / 'five' ²⁷³.
PI may also have had clitics: small morphemes, perhaps originally derived from
verbs, which were suffixed to their referents rather than preceding them. A clitic of the
form * { -hnõɁ } may have signified 'and' ( cf. Ch. * / -hnʌ̃ː̋ / 'also, and, moreover';
PMO * / ˈtahnũɁ / 'and, besides' ⁴³⁴ ). PI probably also a locative clitic or suffix that
followed nouns, although its exact form is unclear; Cherokee has * { -îː } 'at, on, in'
78
CHAPTER 3: CHEROKEE
3.1 INTRODUCTION
uncertain etymology ), whose territory in the early seventeenth century appears to have
ranged north to the foot of the Great Smoky Mountains, bordering on the territory of
the Yuchi, west to the foot of the Appalachian Plateau, bordering on the territory of the
Koasati, south to the Georgian Piedmont, bordering on the territory of the Creek, and
east to the foothills of the Blue Ridge Mountains, bordering on the territory of the
Catawba (cf. village locations in Mooney 1900:507-548; see Map 2). Today speakers of
Cherokee are located primarily on the Eastern Cherokee Indian Reservation in western
North Carolina and on the lands of the Cherokee Nation in eastern Oklahoma.
recorded at some point between 1751 and 1771 by the early American surveyor
William Gerard De Brahm, entitled Compendium of the Cherokee Indian Tongue in English,
archaisms, but is essentially in keeping with the modern language. Another list of
modern language.
79
Within modern Cherokee there exists a wide array of dialects and idiolects that
have yet to be charted and described. My preliminary analysis of early Cherokee sources
has suggested there may have been as many as ten dialects, which differ primarily with
* / tsl / has become dialectal Cherokee / ts /, / tl /, / thl /, and so on. Because differing
locations, nor even generally to the two Cherokee settlements in North Carolina and
Oklahoma, Cherokee dialectal developments are not addressed in the present work, but
rather require further investigation. Cherokee forms presented in this chapter are
ascertained.
grammars are in King (1975), Feeling & Pulte (1975), Cook (1979), and Montgomery-
Anderson (2008). A teaching grammar is in Holmes and Smith (1977), and dictionaries
are in King (1975) and Feeling & Pulte (1975); unless otherwise stated, Cherokee forms
in this chapter are based on data in King (1975) and Feeling & Pulte (1975).
80
3.2 PHONEMIC INVENTORY
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal m n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant j w
Lateral Approximant l
VOWELS
close iː uː
i u
mid eː ʌ̃ː oː
e ʌ̃ o
open aː
a
81
Cherokee / m / appears to have developed from reflexes of PI * / w / in the vicinity of
a nasal vowel. PI * / ɹ / appears to have been preserved in the extinct Eastern dialect of
Cherokee (cf. Mooney 1900:188), but has undergone a shift to / l / in all surviving
in part to make the formal relationship between pre-dialectal Cherokee and modern
All Cherokee vowels carry one of six tones: low level ( e.g. / àː / ), low falling
( / a̋ː / ) (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:xi); falling and rising tones occur only on long vowels
The complete, and probably vast, inventory of sound changes separating Cherokee
from PI is imperfectly understood at present due to the scarcity of secure PNI and
Cherokee cognates. Sound changes that do appear to be secure are described below as
CH-1 through CH-29. Where morphological disparity between PNI and Cherokee forms
82
has made comparison of whole words impossible, only morphological roots have been
given and cognate segments underlined. Where ordering of two changes appears to be
necessary in order to produce a correct output in Cherokee, this has been noted.
Ch. { tsì- }; PI * { kiji- } '1SG:3A' ⁰²⁰ > Ch. { tsìː- } '1SG:3A' ). This change seems to
have been levelled out in the 3SG:3 prefix * { ka- } ⁰⁵¹, which has the allomorph / k /
before all vowels, including / i / ( e.g. PI * { k-ihɹjuɁkẽh } '(it is a) chipmunk' ³⁴⁰ >
Ch. / khìjûːka̋ː /; PI * { k-iːɹ } '(it is a) dog' ³⁴¹ > Ch. / kìːlhi ̋ː / ); it must precede CH-2
and CH-22.
> Ch. { ȁːkì- }; PI * { jõkʷi- } '3A:1SG' ⁰²⁶ > Ch. { ʌ̃ː̏ kì- }; PI * { -kʷ- } 'pick up' ³⁵⁰ Æ
'five' ²⁷³ > Ch. / hi ̀hski ̋ː / ). This change must precede CH-22.
'ice' ³⁶⁵ Æ Ch. / ùněːhstála̋ː /; PI * { -atkahthw- } 'look at' ¹⁷⁹ Æ Ch. / hàkhthǒːhsta̋ː /
By CH-5, the approximant * / w / has been lost word-initially and the following
83
vowel has been lengthened and has acquired low falling tone ( e.g. PI * { wakʷi- }
'3:1SG' ⁰²⁵ > Ch. { ȁːkì- }; PI * { wantiː- } '3NS:3' ⁰⁵⁸ > Ch. { ȁːnìː- } ). This change does
not appear to have affected the translocative morpheme ( cf. PI * { wi- } ⁰⁰⁵ >
> Ch. / wàɁkùːli ̋ː / ). Similarly, by CH-6, the approximant * / j / has been lost word-
initially and the following vowel has been lengthened and has acquired low falling tone
( e.g. PI * { jõːkʷi- } '3A:1SG' ⁰²⁶ > Ch. { ʌ̃ː̏ kì- }; PI * { jeːtsa- } '3A:2SG' ⁰²⁹ >
Ch. { ȅːtsà- }; PI * { juː- } '3:3SG' ⁰⁵⁴ > Ch. { ȕː- }; PI * { juːntiː- } '3:3NS' ⁰⁶¹ >
Ch. { ȕːnìː- } ).
By CH-7, vowels have been lost after a consonant when they preceded * / hC /
( e.g. PI * { -atkahthw- } 'look at' ¹⁷⁹ Æ Ch. / hàkhthǒːhsta̋ː / 'look at it!' ). The
synchronic effect of this change can be seen in Cherokee verbs bases containing / h /
basis ( cf. / hìːjàkȁːthǒːhsta̋ː / 'look at her!', in which vowel deletion has not applied,
2SG:3A pronominal prefix is present; this phenomenon is discussed further in 3.4 ). This
By CH-8, * / Ɂ / has been lost before a consonant and the preceding vowel has been
84
lengthened and acquired low falling tone ( e.g. PI * { -koɁtiɁ } 'be a lot' ³⁴⁴ Æ
Ch. / ǔːkȍːti ̋ː / 'a lot' ). Note that low rising tone, rather than low falling tone, has
typically appeared if another syllable has been added by CH-12 ( cf. PI * { -nõɁt- }
By CH-9, glottal consonants have been lost word-finally and a preceding vowel has
been lengthened ( e.g. PI * { kuːɹeh } 'acorn' ³⁴⁸ > Ch. / kùːle̋ː /; PI * { aɹeɁ } 'about' ¹⁵⁸
> Ch. / àle̋ː / 'and'; PI * { -hsnuːɹiɁ } 'be fast' ²⁵⁸ Æ Ch. / ùhsìnűːlîː / 'it is fast' ).
( e.g. PI * { awẽɁ } 'water' ¹⁹⁶ > Ch. / àma̋ː /; PI * { kahnaːwẽːh } 'butterfly' ³¹⁷ >
seems to have been confined to the final two syllables of nouns and has not taken place
'far' ²⁸¹ > { i ̋ːnâː }; PI * { kanõːnowẽɁ } 'pipe' ³²³ > Ch. / kànʌ̃ː̀ nòwa̋ː /;
PI * { kihɹjuɁkẽh } 'chipmunk' ³⁴⁰ > Ch. / khìjûːka̋ː / ). This change must follow CH-9
and CH-10 and precede CH-12 and CH-27. (cf. also Rudes 1995:20).
By CH-12, a long vowel with high rising tone has been added to the end of words
ending in a consonant; typically this vowel is / a̋ː /, / i ̋ː / or / ʌ̃ː̋ /, but with variation by
85
dialect ( e.g. PI * { tsiːɹ } 'fire' ⁴⁵⁸ Æ Ch. / àtsǐːla̋ː / [ also: / àtsǐːlʌ̃ː̋ / ]; PI * { tiɁɹ }
'skunk' ⁴⁴³ > Ch. / tíɁli ̋ː / [ also: / tiɁla̋ː / ] ). As in the examples given, a previously
accented vowel has typically acquired low rising tone after this change has applied. This
By CH-13, * / iji / has become * / iː / ( e.g. PI * { kiji- } '1SG:3A' ⁰²⁰ > Ch. { tsìː- };
By CH-14, * / tsn / has become * / hst / ( e.g. PI * { tsniː- } '2DU&3' ⁰³¹ >
( e.g. PI * { -oɁnj- } 'hand' ⁴⁰⁰ > Ch. / àkʷȍːje̋ːnîː / 'my hand'; PI * { -Ɂnjõːhs- } 'nose' ⁴⁹⁴
Æ Ch. / tsȉːjʌ̃ː̀ hsőːlîː / 'my nose' ), and similarly, by CH-17, the resonant consonant
By CH-18, * / t / has been lost before * / k / ( e.g. PI * { -atkahthw- } 'look at' ¹⁷⁹ Æ
Ch. / hàkhthǒːhsta̋ː / 'look at it!'; PI * / wakʷataːtkẽːh / 'I have seen myself' ⁰⁷⁶ ³³² >
Ch. / ȁːkʷàtàːkòːhʌ̃ː̌ Ɂi ̋ː / ).
86
By CH-19, * / t / has been lost before * / n / ( e.g. PI * { tniː- } '1+2DU:3' ⁰³⁶ >
By CH-20, * / t / has been lost after * / n / ( e.g. PI * { wantiː- } '3NS:3' ⁰⁵⁸ >
Ch. / tsòːla̋ː /; PI * { -tsjõɁt- } 'fish' ⁴⁶⁷ Æ Ch. / àtsàɁti ̋ː / ). This change must precede
CH-22.
'be fast' ²⁵⁸ Æ Ch. / ùhsìnűːlîː / 'it is fast'; PI * { -nakɹ- } 'dwell' ³⁵⁴ Æ Ch. / ùnàkìlʌ̃ː̏ ti ̋ː /
'whippoorwill' ⁴⁷⁴ > Ch. / wàɁkùːli ̋ː /, which is apparently unaffected. The Common
Cherokee cluster / hsl /, which has different dialectal outcomes, seems also to have
been an exception to epenthesis. This change must follow CH-1, CH-3, CH-4, CH-7,
87
person singular ( PI * { hsi- } '2SG:3' ⁰²² > Ch. { hì- }; PI * { hsiji- } '2SG:3A' ⁰²³ >
Ch. { hìː- } ).
cognates illustrating this sound change except perhaps for * { -ksin- } 'cedar, stump'
posited on the basis of internal reconstruction; cf. Ch. / hìhnéːka̋ː / 'you're speaking',
Ch. / kʌ̃ː̀ hna̋ː / 'he is alive' ). This change must follow CH-22.
By CH-27, * / ẽ(ː) / has become * / o(ː) / ( e.g. PI * { -atawẽ- } 'swim' ¹⁶¹ >
'be embarrassed!'; PI * { -nawẽːh- } 'be melted' ³⁵⁸ > Ch. / hʌ̃ː̀ nàwǒːhi ̋ː / 'melt!';
PI * { -nẽːhskʷ- } 'steal' ³⁶⁸ > Ch. / hìnǒːhski ̋ː / 'steal it!' ). This change must follow
88
CH-10 and CH-11. (cf. also Rudes 1995:20).
By CH-28, * / õ(ː) / has become * / ə̃(ː) / ( e.g. PI * { kanõːnowẽɁ } 'pipe' ³²³ >
Ch. / kànʌ̃ː̀ nòwa̋ː /; PI * { õːtariɁ } 'lake' ⁴¹² > Ch. / ʌ̃ː̀ tàli ̋ː /; PI * { -Ɂnjõːhs- } 'nose' ⁴⁹⁴
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PI to Cherokee
CH-1 k > ts / _i
CH-2 kʷ >k / _i
CH-3 w > Ø / h_i
CH-4 w > Ø / t, th_
CH-5 wV¹ > V̏¹ː / #_
CH-6 jV¹ > V̏¹ː / #_
CH-7 V > Ø / C_hC
CH-8 V¹ɁC² > V̏¹ːC²
CH-9 V¹H > V¹ː / _#
CH-10 a(ː)wẽ(ː) > a(ː)ma(ː)
CH-11 Ṽ > aː / _#
CH-12 Ø > V̋ː / C_#
CH-13 iji > iː
CH-14 tsn > hst
CH-15 n > h / _st
89
CH-16 n > Ø / _j
CH-17 ɹ > Ø / _j
CH-18 t > Ø / _k
CH-19 t > Ø / _n
CH-20 t > Ø / n_
CH-21 j > Ø / ts_
CH-22 Ø > i / C_R
CH-23 s > Ø / #h_V
CH-24 ts > s / h_
CH-25 ks > ts / _V
CH-26 nh > hn
CH-27 ẽ(ː) > o(ː)
CH-28 õ(ː) > ə̃(ː)
CH-29 ɹ>l
In addition to the sound changes in (2), additional sound changes seem to have
taken place in one or more Cherokee dialects affecting various consonant clusters,
whose reconstructed identity in Common Cherokee can often only be guessed at, and
whose identity in PI is in most cases uncertain. Some cognate examples from the
dialects recorded by King (1975), Feeling & Pulte (1975), and Holmes & Smith (1977),
are presented in (3) as Dialect 1, Dialect 2, and Dialect 3, respectively, with variant
segments underlined.
90
(3) Some Dialectal Developments in Cherokee
found at least twelve sets of reflexes like those in (3), and many modern dialects might
Before moving on it should be noted that the process by which tonogenesis has
Cherokee and PNI. Reflexes of pronominal prefixes suggest that Cherokee low level tone
corresponds to primary word accent in PI, as its occurrence is limited to either the
penultimate or final syllable, and it occurs at least once, and only once, in every
91
Cherokee word. Cherokee low falling tone ( e.g. * / ȁː / ) seems to result diachronically
from pre-vocalic loss of a glide ( e.g. PI * / ju / > Ch. * / ȕː / ) and from post-vocalic
loss of a glottal consonant following a vowel ( e.g. PI * / uːɁ / > Ch. / ȕː / ). The
origins of Cherokee high level tone ( / áː / ), high falling tone ( / âː / ), and low rising
tone ( / ǎː / ) are less clear. Some cases of low rising tone appear to correlate with
diachronic addition of syllables to a word, and perhaps reflect earlier primary word
accent that has since moved to another syllable ( e.g. PI * { tsiːɹ } 'fire' ⁴⁵⁸ >
Pre-Cherokee * / (à)tsi ̋ːl / > Ch. / àtsǐːla̋ː / [ also: / àtsǐːlʌ̃ː̋ / ] ). Level high tone is
often associated with short vowels in the same context, perhaps due to a restriction that
imperative verbs that do not contain prepronominal prefixes, all vowels except those in
the last two syllables have low level tone ( àː ) unless a glottal stop follows the vowel, in
which case there is etymologically predictable low falling tone ( ȁː ) with loss of the
glottal stop ( by CH-8 ). If the vowel of the penultimate syllable is followed by a glottal
stop it also has predictable low falling tone ( ȁː ) with loss of the glottal stop
( by CH-8 ); if not, it has neutral low level tone ( à ) if short, and low rising tone ( ǎː ) if
92
long. The vowel of the final syllable will then have high rising tone ( a̋ː ). The pattern
just described suggests that the language may earlier have had neutral tone ( àː ) on all
vowels except those in the penultimate and ultimate syllables, one of which
( in independent verb forms, the final syllable ) received primary word accent,
represented by high rising tone ( cf. / hìnèːhstàlàta̋ː / 'freeze! [lit. lay down the ice!]',
containing a reflex of the PI root * { -neːhstwaɹ- } 'ice' ³⁶⁵ with associated neutral tone:
{ -nèːhstàl- } ). Subsequently, with the addition of productive modal suffixes to all non-
punctual verbs bases, and with the loss of glottal stops in coda and compensatory low
falling tone ( by CH-8 ), more complex patterns of tone appear to have developed
( cf. / kàněːhstálàtìɁa̋ː / 'it's freezing [lit. it's putting down the ice]', containing the same
PI root, now with associated high rising and level high tones rather than neutral tone:
that of PI apart from the apparent addition of a new set of modal suffixes to the verb
93
The principal verb bases posited for PI appear no longer to occur in their radical
forms in Cherokee active verbs and are always equipped with one of eight regular, fully
productive modal suffixes, each with a distinct meaning; the PI punctual verb base
appears to continue to occur in its radical form in Cherokee imperative verbs, though
typically with addition of an overt word-final vowel / a̋ː / by CH-12, which is also
analyzed below as an indicative suffix. For example, the reflex of the PI perfect base
may have in PI and must now be followed by a modal suffix, such as the assertive suffix
{ -ʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː }: Ch. { ȁːkì+kòːh+ʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː } 'I have seen it [this is a fact]'. The independent and
dependent forms of the eight modal suffixes of Cherokee are listed in (4) along with the
94
The approximate meaning of the modal suffixes can be gathered from the following
examples, all but the first and last of which employ the perfect base { -kòːh- }
'have seen': { ȁː-kòwhthíh-a̋ː } 'she sees it'; { ȕː-kòːh-óːɁi ̋ː } 'she has seen it (habitually)';
(I was a witness)'; { ùː-kòːh-ʌ̃ː̋ Ɂîː } '(her) having seen it'; { nùː-kòːh-ʌ̃ː̋ nâː } '(her) not
see it'. The infinitive suffix, seen in the last example, appears to follow the PI causative-
instrumental morpheme * { t } ⁰⁸² / * { hst } ⁰⁸³, which forms a fifth version of the verb
The new modal bases in (4) do not appear to have been added to inherently perfect
bases designating states rather than actions ( e.g. 'be old', 'be good', etc ), and this may
have led to the creation of a formal class of adjectives in Cherokee. Cherokee adjectives,
like Cherokee nouns, use the prefixes associated with dependent verb forms to inflect
for person and number ( e.g. { h-őːstâː } 'you are good', { àn-őːstâː } 'they are good',
etc. ), but lack suffixes with which to indicate tense, mood, negation, and other
grammatical information. Consequently, a reflex of the PI verb * { -iː- } 'be, be the one,
exist' ²⁷¹ seems to have been adopted as an auxiliary in order to show such distinctions,
e.g. / őːstâː / 'it is good'; / tláː őːstâː jìki ̋ː / 'it is not good'; / őːstâː kȅːsʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː /
95
'it was good'; / őːstâː kȅːséːhsti ̋ː / 'it will be good'; / őːstâː ji ̀ke̋ːsâː / 'if it were good', etc.
( cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:305, Cook 1979:125 ). The auxiliary always bears the 3SG:3
prefix allomorph / k- / and does not otherwise inflect to show person or number.
consonants ), which precedes all other prefixes; in dependent verb forms it carries a
meaning 'if' ( e.g. { j-ȕː-kőːh-âː } 'if she has seen it' ), and when preceded by a negative
particle in independent verb forms it carries a meaning 'not' ( e.g. { tláː j-ȕː-kòːh-ʌ̃ː̌ ki ̋ː }
'she has not seen it' ) (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:241). Cherokee appears also to have
( e.g. / kʌ̃ː̀ wàkòːhʌ̃ː̋ kîː / 'she has not seen it since' ), among several others
(cf. Cook 1979: 82-87). The Cherokee negative prefix follows the cislocative and
repetitive prefixes (cf. Cook 1979:58), which suggests that the prefix may be archaic,
96
consonant and / hstʌ̃ː̀ j / before a vowel with loss of / h / word-initially or following a
presented in (5).
97
To the third pronominal prefix group Cherokee seems to have added a 3A:2DU
prefix { ȅːhstìː } and 3A:2PL prefix { ȅːtsìː }, evidently formed by affixing { ȅː } 'animate'
to the 2DU&3 and 2PL&3 prefixes { stìː } ⁰³¹ and { ȉːtsìː } ⁰³³. There are no equivalent
2DU:3A and 2PL:3A prefixes in group 3 and the animate argument occurs only in a role
inclusive (1+2) and exclusive (1+3) first person pronominal prefixes in both agent and
patient; inclusive first person prefixes additionally mark the third person argument as
98
(8) Cherokee Pronominal Prefix Group 4
The prefixes in (8) may be analyzed as consisting of { nìː } 'first person inclusive dual
agent', { tìː } 'first person inclusive plural agent', { hstìː } 'first person exclusive dual
agent' ( formally identical to the 2DU&3 prefix ), { tsìː } 'first person exclusive plural
agent' ( formally identical to the 2PL&3 prefix ), { kìnìː } 'first person dual patient',
In pronominal prefix group 5, reflexes of the PI 3A:3SG and 3A:3NS prefixes seem
99
/ ȁːtsì / before consonants, and / ȁːk / before remaining vowels ) appears to have been
added. Curiously the initial vowel / ȁː / of this prefix becomes / ȅː / following the
cislocative, negative, and non-singular animate prefix (Cook 1979:75); the origin of the
is presented in (9).
( e.g. 1SG:3 ) are present, and as / h / when other pronominal prefixes ( e.g. 3SG:3 ) are
present ( cf. { k-àlɁìkíːɁa̋ː } 'I have a nightmare', but { ȁː-lhìkíːɁa̋ː } 'she has a nightmare';
100
̀ ska̋ː } 'she grows' ). About a third of the pronominal prefixes condition / Ɂ /
but { ȁː-thʌ̃h
rather than / h / (cf. Cook 1979:40), but no unifying element, diachronic or synchronic,
semantic or phonological, has yet been found among the pronominal prefixes in
( Ch. { -kìː- } ⁰⁸⁵ ), and dative-benefactive ( Ch. { -Ɂsìː- } ⁰⁸⁷ ) derivational suffixes,
which have cognates in PNI, Cherokee seems to have added ambulative { -ìːt- },
suffixes, the last three of which occur only in punctual and perfect verb bases. The
The noun morphology of Cherokee seems to be the same as that posited for PI
except that alienable and inalienable possessive prefixes no longer apply to body parts
and possessions, respectively; both now apply to body parts, some of which appear to
take the historically inalienable possessive prefixes ( e.g. 1SG:3 ), while others take the
101
reason determining which set is associated with which body parts, cf. { tsȉː-jʌ̃ː̀ sőːlîː }
'my nose' and { tsȉː-nìːke̋ːnîː } 'my knee', but { àkì-nʌ̃ː̌ tsíːta̋ː } 'my brain' and
{ àkì-ne̋ːkàɁlʌ̃ː̋ Ɂîː } 'my skin' (cf. Cook 1979:193). Alienable possession in Cherokee is
expressed by adding reflexes of the set of alienable prefixes to a dependent form of the
verb { -àtsèːli ̋ːɁîː } 'be a possession to', which follows the possessed object, e.g.
{ kòːhwèːli ̋ː àkʷ-àtsèːli ̋ːɁîː } 'my book', { kòːhwèːli ̋ː ù-tsèːli ̋ːɁîː } 'her book', etc.
102
CHAPTER 4: PROTO-NORTHERN IROQUOIAN
4.1 INTRODUCTION
Mohawk-Oneida, Onondaga, Cayuga, Seneca, and Huron derive. PNI itself derives from
PI, and is reconstructed on the basis of cognate forms found among its daughter
branches. Because forms among the numerous daughter languages do not always agree
( or sometimes agree due to areal diffusion or borrowing rather than due to genetic
First, forms that are common to Cherokee and at least one Northern Iroquoian language
can be projected back to PI, and I thus assume them to have been present in PNI as well
( the habitual suffix { -hsk } ¹¹³ of Old Mohawk, which has a cognate in Cherokee
of areal diffusion and potential borrowing among the Northern Iroquoian languages, I
have divided the better-attested Northern Iroquoian languages into three geographic
regions: the Coastal Plain ( Tuscarora, with supplemental data from Nottoway ),
( Huron and Wyandot ). If a form is found in any two of these regions I have projected
103
it back to PNI, but if a form is confined to only one of them I have not. Exceptions to
the latter approach, which I note as they arise, include a handful of pronominal prefixes
and derivational suffixes that are limited to Iroquoia, and have been noted as such in
the appendices; in part I have made an exception in these cases so that the relevant
chapters.
Finally, note that my theoretical assumption that forms present in at least Iroquoia
and Huronia can be projected back to PNI without a Tuscarora or Nottoway cognate is
contingent upon my proposed subgrouping ( which does not recognize Lake Iroquoian
as formally distinct from PNI itself ), and that researchers who do consider Lake
Iroquoian to have been formally distinct from PNI will require a cognate in Tuscarora or
104
(1) Phonemic Inventory of PNI
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant ɹ j w
VOWELS
Close iː
i
Mid eː ẽː oː õː
e ẽ o õ
Open aː
a
The phonemic inventory of PNI seems to have been essentially the same as that of
PI * / o(ː) / with reflexes of PI * / a(ː) /. PNI appears to have had predictable vowel
(cf. Michelson 1988:52, Rudes 1995:23); underlying short vowels were lengthened in
105
accented open penultimate syllables before single non-glottal consonants
( e.g. PNI * { tsjatahk } / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴, PNI * { õkʷeh } / ˈõːkʷeh / 'person' ⁴⁰⁵,
PNI * { o-ɹẽn-aɁ } / oˈɹẽːnaɁ / 'incantation' ⁴²² ), and contrastive vowel length was
retained in final syllables ( e.g. PNI * { wak-kẽh } / ˈwakkẽh / 'it sees me' ³³² vs.
PNI * { wak-kẽːh } / ˈwakkẽːh / 'it has seen me' ³³² ; cf. also Rudes 1995:22 ). Note that
indicated in both phonological forms ( e.g. PNI * / ˈwakkẽːh / 'it has seen me' ³³²,
* / weˈtninjaːk / 'we got married' ³⁷², * / kaˈnõhsoːt / 'a house stands' ³⁸² ³⁹⁷ ) and
stage, though the affricate * / ts / may have had a unique pronunciation before
106
so conceivably PNI * / n / may also have had an allophone [ nd ] before non-nasal
vowels.
4.2.1 VOWELS
Vowel reconstructions in this chapter are from Rudes (1995:18, 28), unless
otherwise stated. Reflexes of the 12 vowels reconstructed for PNI are relatively
(2) The Most Common Reflexes of PNI Vowels in Northern Iroquoian Languages
*/a/ a a a a a a a a
*/e/ ɛ e e e e e e e
*/ẽ/ ə̃ ʌ̃ ʌ̃ ẽ ẽ ɛ̃ ẽ ɛ̃
*/i/ i i i i i i i i
*/o/ o o o o o o o u
*/õ/ ə̃ ũ ũ ũ õ ɔ̃ õ ɔ̃
* / aː / aː a a ˈa aː aː aː ˈa
* / eː / ɛː e e ˈe eː eː eː ˈe
* / ẽː / ə̃ː ʌ̃ ʌ̃ ˈẽ ẽː ɛ̃ː ẽː ˈɛ̃
* / iː / iː i i ˈi iː iː iː ˈi
* / oː / oː o o ˈo oː oː oː ˈu
* / õː / ə̃ː ũ ũ ˈũ õː ɔ̃ː õː ˈɔ̃
Exceptions to the correspondence sets presented in (2) are few but, as noted by
107
Onondaga and Seneca when * / r / preceded, fronting of PNI * / o(ː) / and * / õ(ː) / to
PNI * / a(ː) / to / ɔ̃(ː) / in Seneca where * / n / preceded, and change of PNI * / a(ː) /
to / ɛ̃(ː) / in Seneca when * / nɹ / preceded. The PI nasal vowels * / ẽ(ː) / and * / õ(ː) /
have merged as / ə̃(ː) / in Tuscarora, while reflexes of PNI long vowels have merged
with reflexes of short vowels in Mohawk, Oneida, Onondaga and Wyandot, and have
acquired primary word accent in the latter two languages (cf. Rudes 1995:23).
PNI * / e(ː) / appears to have undergone a phonetic shift to / ɛ(ː) / in Tuscarora, while
PNI * / ẽ(ː) / seems to have undergone a phonetic shift to * / ə̃(ː) / in Mohawk and
Oneida, and to / ɛ̃(ː) / in Seneca and Wyandot; PNI * / õ(ː) / appears to have undergone
a phonetic shift to * / ũ(ː) / in Mohawk, Oneida and Onondaga, and to / ɔ̃(ː) / in Seneca
and Wyandot. In addition Mohawk and Oneida have developed epenthetic instances of
resonants.
PNI * / aː / is found in * { -aːt } 'be inside' ¹⁵⁹ ( cf. Tu. { -aːt }, Ca. { -aːt },
( cf. Tu. { tʃaɁnahk }, Ca. { tsatahk }, Se. { tsatak } ). PNI * / eː / is found in * { -akeːh }
108
'be a number' ¹⁵² ( cf. Tu. { -akjɛː }, Ca. { -akeː }, Se. { -akeːh } ) and PNI * / e / is found
in * { -õkʷeh } 'be a person' ⁴⁰⁵ ( cf. Tu. { -ə̃kʷɛh }, Ca. { -õkʷeh }, Se. { -ɔ̃kʷeh } ).
PNI * / ẽː / is present in * { -ahnshẽːh } 'be ten' ¹³⁵ ( cf. Tu. { -ahθhə̃ː }, Ca. { -ahshẽː },
Se. { -ashɛ̃ːh } ) and PNI * / ẽ / is found in * { ahsẽh } 'three' ¹⁴⁰ ( cf. Tu. { ahsə̃ },
( cf. Tu. { nɛktiː }, Ca. { tekniː }, Se. { tekniːh } ) and PNI * / i / is exemplified in
* { hwihsk } 'five' ²⁷³ ( cf. Tu. { wisk }, Ca. { hwihs }, Se. { wis } ). PNI * / oː / is found
in * { -ijoːh } 'be good' ²⁷⁹ ( cf. Tu. { -ijoː }, Ca. { -ijoː }, Se. { -ijoːh } ) and PNI * / o / is
present in * { -ɁtskoɁ- } 'drown' ⁴⁹⁷ ( cf. Tu. { -ɁθkoɁ- }, Ca. { -ɁskoɁ- }, Se. { -ɁskoɁ- } ).
PNI * / õː / is found in * { -õːt } 'be attached' ⁴¹⁰ ( cf. Tu. { -ə̃ːt }, Ca. { -õːt },
Se. { -ɔ̃ːt } ) and PNI * / õ / is found in * { inõh } 'far' ²⁸¹ ( cf. Tu. { inə̃ }, Ca. { inõh } ).
109
( cf. Tu. { -ahkar- }, Mo. { -ahkaɹ- }, Hu. { -ahkaɹ- } ), and has lost * / k / where / n /
follows, as seen in PNI * { tekniːh } 'two' ⁴³⁸ ( cf. Tu. { nɛktiː }, Mo. { tekeni },
PNI * / t / and * / s /, as in PNI * { -nõhwakt- } 'be sore' ³⁸³ ( cf. Tu. { -nə̃hwakn- },
Mo. { -nũhwakt- }, Hu. { -nõhwaht- } ) and PNI * { -aksẽːh } 'be bad' ¹⁵³
PNI * { -õkʷeh } 'be a person' ⁴⁰⁵ ( cf. Tu. { -ə̃kʷɛh }, Mo. { -ũkʷe }, Hu. { -õxʷeh } ).
Rudes 1995:56), as in PNI * { atokẽɁ } 'axe' ¹⁸² ( cf. Tu. { aɁnokə̃Ɂ }, Mo. { atokʌ̃Ɂ },
another consonant precedes, as in PNI * { -akanst- } 'be durable' ¹⁵¹ ( cf. Tu. { -akaθn- },
110
PNI * { -atɹe- } 'grandchild' ¹⁸⁸ ( cf. Tu. { -aɁrɛ- }, Mo. { -ateɹe- }, Hu. { -atɹe- } ).
and * / hw /, as in PNI * { twa- } '1+2PL:3' ⁰³⁸ ( cf. Tu. { Ɂnwa- }, Mo. { tewa- },
Hu. { kwa- } ).
'house' ³⁸² ( cf. Tu. { -nə̃hs- }, Mo. { -nũhs- }, Hu. { -nõhʃ- } ). Huron has also retained
( cf. Tu. { -nə̃hskʷ- }, Mo. { -nʌ̃hskʷ- }, Hu. { -nẽhskʷ- }, while Tuscarora has inserted
( cf. Tu. { -ohstr- }, Mo. { -ohseɹ- }, Hu. { -ohʃɹ- } ). Reflexes of PNI * / s / and * / ts /
111
intervocalic reflexes of PNI * / hs /, * / hts / and * / ts / before high and non-high
(3) Reflexes of PNI * / hs /, * / hts / and * / ts / Before High and Non-High Vowels
'family' ²⁶⁹ ( cf. Tu. { -hwatʃir- }, Mo. { -hwatsiɹ- }, Hu. { -hwatsiɹ- } ), and where
remaining vowels, as in PNI * { -atseːɁ } 'be new' ¹⁸⁹ ( cf. Tu. { -aθɛːɁ }, Mo. { -aseɁ },
112
/ hs / ( where a vowel precedes ) or / s / ( where a consonant precedes ) and Huron
Rudes 1995:56), as in PNI * { -nat- } 'town' ³⁵⁶ ( cf. Tu. { -taɁn- }, Mo. { -nat- },
Hu. { -nat- } ), although Tuscarora has retained * / n / before reflexes of nasal vowels
Mo. { -hsnoɹeɁ }, Hu. { -hstoɹeɁ } ), and is absent in both Tuscarora and Huron
PNI * { -Ɂnonsh- } 'be envious' ⁴⁹⁵ ( cf. Tu. { -Ɂtoθh- }, Mo. { -Ɂnosh- }, Hu. { -Ɂnosh- } ),
reflexes
113
( cf. Tu. { -Ɂθhaθn- }, Mo. { -Ɂshatst- }, Hu. { -Ɂshast- } ). Reflexes of PNI clusters
many of the Northern Iroquoian languages, including Tuscarora, where in most cases
the former seem to be differentiated from the latter only by penultimate lengthening of
a preceding vowel; for clarity, intervocalic reflexes of some PNI clusters beginning in
* / tsh / tʃh tsh tsh tsh tsh sh sh e.g. * { -nẽtsh- } 'arm' ³⁷⁰
* / tsk / θk hsk sk sk hsk sk sk e.g. * { -Ɂnitsko- } 'be late' ⁴⁹²
* / tskʷ / θkʷ hskʷ skʷ skʷ hskʷ skʷ skʷ e.g. * { -Ɂnitskʷ- } 'delay' ⁴⁹²
* / tst / θn hst st st hst st st e.g. * { -atst- } 'dry up' ¹⁹¹
PNI * / ɹ / is reconstructed where Tuscarora has / r / and Mohawk and Huron have
'incantation' ⁴²² ( cf. Tu. { -rə̃t- }, Mo. { -ɹʌ̃n- }, Hu. { -ɹẽn- } ). Reflexes of PNI * / ɹ /
114
( cf. Tu. { -rjoːɁ }, Mo. { -ɹjoɁ }, Hu. { -joːɁ } ).
(cf. Mithun 1979:180, Rudes 1995:56), as in PNI * { -wẽn- } 'word' ⁴⁷⁶ ( cf. Tu. { -wə̃t- },
Mo. { -wʌ̃n- }, Hu. { -wẽn- } ). Huron has inserted / k / before reflexes of * / w / when
a fricative or affricate precedes, as in PNI * { tswa- } '2PL&3' ⁰³³ ( cf. Tu. { θwa- },
in PNI * { -wɹ- } 'wind' ⁴⁸⁷ ( cf. Tu. { -wr- }, Mo. { -weɹ- }, Hu. { -ɹ- } ) and
PNI * { -wj- } 'arm, wing' ⁴⁸⁰ ( cf. Tu. { -wj- }, Mo. { -wej- }, Hu. { -j- } ). A cluster
Oneida shows / wjh /, as in PNI * { -wjhõh- } 'river' ²⁷⁸ ( cf. Tu. { -jhə̃h- },
Mo. { -hjũh- }, and Oe. { -wjhũh- } ), and a cluster * / whj / can be reconstructed where
Se. { -jɔ̃hkaR- } ).
PNI * { -ihej- } 'be dead' ²⁷⁶ ( cf. Tu. { -ihɛj- }, Mo. { -ihej- }, Hu. { -ihe- } ). Huron has
115
'sky' ⁴³⁰ ( cf. Tu. { -rə̃hj- }, Mo. { -ɹũhj- }, Hu. { -ɹõhj- } ). Both Tuscarora and Huron
PNI * / h / can be reconstructed where Tuscarora, Mohawk, and Huron all show
/ h / (cf. Mithun 1979:180, Rudes 1995:56), as in PNI * { -aɁtaɹihẽːh } 'be hot' ²⁰⁴
( cf. Tu. { -aɁnarihə̃ː }, Mo. { -aɁtaɹihʌ̃ }, Hu. { -aɁtaɹihẽːh } ). As in the example just
given, * / h / has been lost word-finally in Mohawk and word-finally after long vowels
Se. { -aɁtaiɛ̃ːh } ). Tuscarora and Mohawk preserve virtually all reflexes of non-final
'be used up' ¹⁷³ ( cf. Tu. { -atʃɁahn- }, Mo. { -athsaɁaht- } ), and PNI * / hnh / can be
PNI * { -hnhaɁ- } 'command' ²³² ( cf. Tu. { -nhaɁ- }, Mo. { -hnhaɁ- } ). PNI * / h / has
reconstructed where Onondaga and Cayuga show / h /, as in PNI * / ˈhwihsk / 'five' ²⁷³
PNI * / Ɂ / can be reconstructed where Tuscarora, Mohawk, and Huron all show
116
/ Ɂ / (cf. Mithun 1979:180, Rudes 1995:56), as in PNI * { -aɁawj- } 'dew' ¹⁹⁹
( cf. Tu. { -aɁawj- }, Mo. { -aɁawej- }, Hu. { -aɁaj- } ). Reflexes of PNI * / Ɂ / are among
The precise phonotactic constraints of PNI are difficult to determine due to the large
number of consonant clusters that occur and limitation of certain clusters to single
vocabulary items that lack cognates outside of the Coastal Plain, Iroquoia, or Huronia.
Clusters with the following shapes are attested in the six living Northern Iroquoian
languages, where G is / w / or / j /, H is / h / or / Ɂ /, K is / k / or / kʷ / or / t /, R is
GK, GKH, GKG, GR, GRG, GS, GSK, GSKH, GSR, H, K, KG, KH, KHG, KHGG, KHK, KHR,
KHRG, KHS, KHSG, KHSK, KK, KKG, KKH, KKHG, KKHR, KKK, KKR, KR, KRG, KS, KSG,
KSH, KSK, KSKG, KSKH, KSKR, KSR, KSRG, R, RG, RGH, RH, RHG, RHR, RK, RKG, RR,
RSR, S, SG, SH, SHG, SHR, SHRG, SK, SKG, SKH, SKHG, SKHRG, SKK, SKR, SKRG, SR,
SRG, SS, SSH, SSR; these shapes can normally also be preceded by H. (cf. also clusters
PNI appears to have undergone fewer sound changes relative to PI than Cherokee,
117
and most of the changes seem to involve vowels rather than consonants; note that if
further changes to consonants did occur in PNI, they are unlikely to be recoverable from
data in Cherokee, which in turn already shows significant changes to consonants and
clusters relative to forms in PI. Phonological changes that have occurred during the
transition from PI to PNI are described below as PNI-1 through PNI-12. Where
reconstructed words impossible, only roots have been given with cognate segments
By PNI-1, * / o(ː) / has become * / a(ː) / ( e.g. PI * { kanõːnowẽɁ } 'pipe' ³²³ >
PNI * / kaˈtsjaːɹaɁ / ). This change, which has merged all reflexes of PI * / a(ː) / and
* / o(ː) / in PNI, must precede PNI-2 and PNI-6. ( cf. also Rudes 1995:35)
By PNI-2, * / u(ː) / has become * / o(ː) / ( e.g. PI * { juhsnuːɹiɁ } 'it is fast' ²⁵⁸ >
PI * { kuːɹeh } 'oak' ³⁴⁸ > PNI * / ˈkoːɹeh /; PI * { -nuːɹ- } 'corn husk' ³⁹² Æ
PNI * / oˈnoːɹaɁ / ), and this change must follow PNI-1. Marianne Mithun (p.c.) notes
118
that change of PI * / u / to * / o / might instead distinguish languages of the traditional
Five Nations or Iroquois Proper subgrouping from other Northern Iroquoian languages,
and may thus potentially constitute a phonological innovation sufficient to group the
languages as a separate branch, rather than a change that should be projected back to
<u> in e.g. No. <Otkum> 'Devil' ¹⁸¹, <Coree> 'A red oak' ³⁴⁸, and <Goshera>
'Winter' ³⁹⁴, as well as retention of a pronunciation [ o ] for the reflex of this vowel in
Western Tuscarora when short and unaccented, cf. Rudes 1999:xxxvii ) suggests the
change was already present in PNI. (cf. also Mithun 1979:180, Rudes 1995:35).
perhaps after all non-glottal, non-resonant consonants ( e.g. PI * { kiji- } '1SG:3A' ⁰²⁰ >
PNI * { khe- } '1SG:3I'; PI * { hsiji- } '2SG:3A' ⁰²³ > PNI * { hshe- } '2SG:3I' ).
Interaction between PNI-3 and PNI-4 may be responsible for the discrepancy in form
between Proto-Mohawk-Oneida { ɹi- } '1SG:3M' ⁰²¹ and the form { he- } '1SG:3M', found
antecedent * { hɹiji- } or * { hɹeje- }. This change must precede PNI-4, PNI-7, and
PNI-9.
119
By PNI-4, short * / i / has become * / e / ( e.g. PI * { ki- } '1SG:3' ⁰¹⁹ >
PNI * { ke- }; PI * { hsi- } '2SG:3' ⁰²² > PNI * { hse- }; PI * { juhsnuːɹiɁ } 'it is fast' ²⁵⁸ >
PI * { õːtaɹiɁ } '(there is a) lake' ⁴¹² > PNI * / ˈõtaɹeɁ / ). Rudes (1995:32) notes some
exceptions in which this change appears not to have occurred before the cluster
* { hwihsk } 'five' ²⁷³ likewise seem not to have been subject to the change, which must
follow PNI-3, and precede PNI-5 and PNI-8. (cf. also Rudes 1995:35).
verbs containing only one syllable ( e.g. PI * { ki-k- } 'I eat' ³¹⁶ Æ PNI * / ˈiːkeks /;
monosyllabic verb stems demonstrably deriving from PI are limited, but other examples
from PNI include * / ˈiːkeɁts / 'I go' ²⁰⁹, * / ˈikhsɁahs / 'I finish it' ²⁶⁶, and * / ˈiːwaːt /
'it is inside' ¹⁵⁹ ). As rendered here, this change must follow PNI-4 and precede PNI-7.
By PNI-6, antepenultimate accent has been applied to words in which the vowel of
lake' ⁴¹² > PNI * / ˈõtaɹeɁ /; PI * { tawiːhskaɹaɁ } 'flint' ⁴³⁷ > PNI * / taˈwihskaɹaɁ / ).
120
This development has created the PNI antepenultimate accent as described in Michelson
(1988:52) and Rudes (1995:31); it must follow PNI-1 and PNI-3, and precede PNI-7 and
PNI-8. (cf. also Chafe 1977a:170). Subsequently, by PNI-7, penultimate accent has been
applied to all words that did not receive antepenultimate accent by PNI-6
( e.g. PI * { iːnõh } 'far' ²⁸¹ > PNI * / ˈiːnõh /; PI * { kihɹjuɁkẽh } 'chipmunk' ³⁴⁰ >
PNI * / tsihˈɹjoɁkẽh /; PI * { jukoɁtiɁ } 'there is a lot' ³⁴⁴ > PNI * / joˈkaɁteɁ / ), and this
change must follow PNI-6 and precede PNI-9. Marianne Mithun (p.c.) suggests an
had penultimate stress, and when a noun stem was incorporated into a verb before a
stem beginning in a consonant, a short epenthetic -a- was added to break the consonant
cluster. This epenthetic vowel did not affect the already well-established stress pattern.
As the vowel became stronger and took on the status of a regular vowel, in just these
outlined by Julian above will not actually yield the correct modern forms. A regular,
inherited, penultimate short a (not the linking vowel) did receive stress, and such
vowels continue to do so in the languages that have preserved the original pattern."
121
PI * { tawiːhskaɹaɁ } 'flint' ⁴³⁷ > PNI * / taˈwihskaɹaɁ / ), penultimate short * / a /,
even in cases like these where it is not a reflex of the joiner vowel, does not appear to
have received accent when a single non-glottal consonant followed, and nor does it
appear to have received accent in this environment in languages such as Mohawk and
cf. Mo. / shaˈwihskaɹaɁ / 'flint', Oe. / taˈwiskalaɁ / 'devil or the Evil twin' ⁴³⁷
( cf. e.g. Oe. / kajaɁˈtalhaɁ / { ka-jaɁt-(a)-l-haɁ } 'it is pictured' ⁴¹³, where the joiner
By PNI-8, inherited vowel length has been lost in all non-final syllables
( e.g. PI * { wakʷataːtkẽːh } 'I saw myself' ⁰⁷⁶ ³³² > PNI * / wakʷaˈtatkẽːh /;
PI * { õːtaɹiɁ } '(there is a) lake' ⁴¹² > PNI * / ˈõtaɹeɁ /; PI * { tawiːhskaɹaɁ } 'flint' ⁴³⁷ >
when they preceded a single non-glottal consonant ( e.g. PI * { atseːɁ } 'new' ¹⁸⁹ >
PNI * / ˈaːtseːɁ /; PI * { kijikẽːh } 'I saw someone' ⁰²⁰ ³³² > PNI * / ˈkheːkẽːh /;
122
PI * { kahnaːwẽːh } 'butterfly' ³¹⁷ > PNI * / kahˈnaːwẽːh / ). This change must follow
'2SG:3' ⁰²² > PNI * / se- / word-initially; PI * { hsiji- } '2SG:3A' ⁰²³ > PNI * / she- /
'2SG:3I' word-initially ). Though reflexes cannot be projected back to PI, the same loss
sequence * / hr /.
( e.g. PI * { wantiː- } '3NS:3' ⁰⁵⁸ > PNI * { wati- }; PI * { juːntiː- } '3:3NS' ⁰⁶¹ >
By PNI-12, * / t / has been lost after * / n / ( e.g. PI * { want- } '3NS:3' ⁰⁵⁸ >
PNI * { wan- } '3NS:3' ⁰⁵⁸; PI * { juːnt- } '3:3NS' ⁰⁶¹ > PNI * { jon- } ). This change must
follow PNI-11.
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PI to PNI are
recapitulated in (5).
123
(5) Phonological Changes in the Transition from PI to PNI
pronominal prefixes suggest that additional sound changes may have occurred relative
to PI, although conceivably such patterns may actually have been inherited from PI,
pronominal prefixes are presented in (6); the initial vowel of a following base is lost
124
(6) Morphophonemic Alternations in Allomorphs of PNI Pronominal Prefixes
_C _a _e, ẽ _i _o, õ
ii. * …e- * …Ø * …Ø * …Ø * …Ø
* …ke- * …kʷ- * …k- * …k- * …k-
v. * …a- * …Ø * …Ø * …ẽ- * …Ø
* …wa- * …w- * …w- * …wẽ- * …j-
* …wa- * …w- * …w- * …wẽ- * …waj-
Pronominal prefixes belonging to the first group in (6), such as PNI * { wati- }
'3PL:3' ⁰⁵⁸ and * { joti- } '3:3NS' ⁰⁶¹, have alternants that show only the effects of regular
to the second group, which include PNI * { ke- } '1SG:3' ⁰¹⁹ and { jõke- } '3I:1SG' ⁰²⁶,
descend from PI prefixes that appear to have had a preconsonantal allomorph ending in
125
* / i /, and have alternants that likewise show the effects of regular sound change;
however, prefixes having a final element * { kʷ(i) } 'first person patient' seem to have
lost the rounding of * / k /, both in their preconsonantal allomorph and before all
allomorphy in Mohawk and Oneida suggests ). Loss of rounding in this case was
probably due to analogy with prefixes containing reflexes of PI * { k(i) } 'first person
agent', and perhaps also to the presence of PNI final * / w / in allomorphs of plural
'1:2.SG' ⁰¹³, * { khe- } '1SG:3A' ⁰²⁰, and * { jetshi- } '2NS&3I' ⁰³⁵, have alternants that
possible sound change whereby reflexes of PI * / jiː / were lost in PNI following certain
vowels ( although apparently not all vowels – cf. alternants occurring before * / i /
in group v ).
Pronominal prefixes belonging to the fourth group in (6), such as PNI * { jo- }
'3:3SG' ⁰⁵⁴, are cognate with PI forms that ended in * / uː /, after which bases in * / aː /
appear to have lost their initial vowel, and this pattern seems also to have occurred in
126
PNI. Certain PNI bases in * / ẽ /, and all following bases in * / i /, likewise seem to
have lost their vowel after these prefixes in PNI, which suggests possible sound changes
Pronominal prefixes belonging to the fifth group in (6), which include * { tsa- }
'3:2SG' ⁰²⁸, * { tswa- } '2PL&3' ⁰³³, and * { kõwa- } '3I:3SG' ⁰⁶⁶, regularly lost their final
vowel * / ẽ / appears to have occurred instead and the * / i / of the following base is
lost. The latter pattern suggests a possible sound change whereby PI * / ajiː / became
before rounded vowels. The exceptional alternant * / waj / that occurs before bases
beginning in * / o / and * / õ / has perhaps been preserved so that the PNI prefix
127
* { kõwa- } '3I:3SG' ⁰⁶⁶ does not have an allomorph * / kõj / before rounded vowels,
since PNI * { kõ- } '1:2.SG' ⁰¹³ appears to have had an allomorph * / kõj / in the same
environment.
'2DU&3' ⁰³¹, end in reflexes of the PI dual number morpheme * { niː }, which seems to
have had a prevocalic allomorph * / n / in PI. The alternant * / j / that occurs in PNI
where cognate prefixes end in / niː / before bases beginning in long / aː / and the
vowel of the base is subsequently lost. PI may have had * / niːj / before bases in * / aː
/, with loss of the resulting syllable * / jaː / in Cherokee and loss of the syllable * / ni /
The seventh set of alternants in (6) pertains only to the pronominal prefix * { je- }
'3I:3' ⁰⁶³, which appears to be cognate with the element * { ji } that appears to have
128
* / ije(ː) /, and * / ijiː / became * / õ /, * / ẽ /, and * / e / respectively in PNI, the third
no obvious parallels in PI but may have been influenced formally by the related prefix
* / jako / '3:3I' ⁰⁶⁴. The alternant in * / a / that occurs before some bases beginning in
'factual' ⁰⁰⁶ + { wak } '3:1SG' ⁰²⁵ + { kẽɁ } 'see (punctual base)' ³³² = PNI * / ˈõkkẽɁ /
'it saw me' ( cf. Chafe 1996:557 for discussion of this pattern in Seneca ). If indeed the
morphophonemic rule may already have been present in PI ( but cf. Michelson 1988:40
129
4.4 MORPHOLOGICAL CHANGES
The morphological structures of the PNI noun and verb seem to have been
essentially the same as those posited for PI, although no phonological distinction was
retained between independent and dependent forms ( assuming, as I have here, that
such a distinction was present in PI ). Whereas Cherokee and PI seem to have had a
strictly linear order in which prepronominal prefixes occurred, PNI and all of its
daughter languages seem to have position classes, which likewise occur in a set order,
but can include more than one prefix, only one of which may occur in the same verb;
for example, the PNI negative, coincident, contrastive, and partitive prefixes all seem to
have belonged to position class 1, and only one of them seems to have been able to
occur in a given word. The reconstructed position class template for PNI prepronominal
130
| | | | | cislocative * / t / ⁰¹⁰
| | | | | repetitive * / ts / ⁰¹¹
| | | | | |
1 2 3 4 5 6
contrastive, factual, and optative prefixes. The negative prefix appears simply to have
negated a verb ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkheːkẽh / 'I see someone' ³³² Æ * / teɁˈkheːkẽh / 'I don't
see someone' ). The contrastive prefix seems to have implied that the verb reflects
something that is unusual or not to be expected ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkheːkẽh / 'I see someone'
Æ * / thiˈkheːkẽh / 'I see someone, unexpectedly or unusually' ). Like the future prefix,
the factual and optative prefixes seem to have indicated grammatical moods, and
because they always occur with a punctual verb base, are often referred to as modal
prefixes ( among other accounts, cf. Chafe 1967:15 ); their approximate respective
meanings may be gathered from the following examples: PNI * / ẽˈkheːkẽɁ / 'I will see
someone (in the future)'; * / waɁˈkheːkẽɁ / 'I saw someone (at a single point in time,
and that is a fact)'; * / aɹaˈkheːkẽɁ / 'I would, should, could, might or may see someone
(at some point, now or in the future, but I am not entirely sure)'. Like the translocative
and cislocative prefixes, the factual, future, and optative prefixes seem to have had
mutually exclusive meanings and for semantic reasons did not co-occur.
131
Reflexes of the negative and coincident prefixes have probably been lost in
Tuscarora but are present throughout Iroquoia and Huronia. In all of the remaining
Northern languages except Seneca, Huron, and Wyandot, the negative prefix can co-
occur only with the cislocative and repetitive prefixes; elsewhere the contrastive prefix
must be substituted ( which suggests e.g. PNI * / ˈthaɁkkẽɁ / 'I did not see it' and
* / ˈthẽkkẽɁ / 'I will not see it', with the contrastive ). Note, however, that Onondaga,
like Seneca, Huron and Wyandot, allows a negative prefix allomorph / taɁ / ⁰⁰¹ before
the duplicative prefix (cf. Woodbury 2003:41), and perhaps maintains the pattern that
The coincident, contrastive, and partitive prefixes of PNI position class 1 appear to
have shared a common allomorphy, with basic forms * / tsh / ⁰⁰², * / th / ⁰⁰³ and
* / n / ⁰⁰⁴ respectively before vowels and glides, and basic forms * / tshi /, * / thi / and
pronominal prefixes ), all three prefixes appear to have had allomorphs ending in a
132
with the factual prefix ); * / tshaɁ /, * / thaɁ / and * / nwaɁ / before * / j / with loss of
in the coincident and the membership of both prefixes in the same position class.
Possibly the three prefixes are etymologically comprised of a reflex of the dependent
form of the PI partitive ( * / ijiː / ⁰⁰⁴ ) preceded by coincident * / ts- / ⁰⁰², partitive
speculative proto-forms * / tsijiː /, * / tijiː /, and * / nijiː / would then have yielded PNI
* / tshi /, * / thi /, and * / ni / ( cf. PNI-3 ), with resulting prefixes sharing both their
position class and their pattern of allomorphy. A reflex of the PNI partitive without
The translocative prefix, which appears to have followed the coincident and
preceded the partitive in PI, had the form * / j(a)hwe / ⁰⁰⁵ in PNI, perhaps through
( PNI * / waɁ /, * / we / ). The PNI translocative had a basic form * / j(a)hw / before
vowels and a basic form * / j(a)hwe / before consonants. Like the coincident,
133
contrastive, and partitive prefixes of position class 1, the PNI translocative appears to
connection with the factual prefix ); and * / j(a)hwaɁ / before * / j / with loss of * / j /,
also to have had an allomorph * / jah / before the factual prefix and an allomorph
The PNI factual prefix seems to have had a zero allomorph word-initially before the
occurred both before * / wa /, with loss of * / wa /, and before the cislocative and
person pronominal prefixes * { tsa } '3:2SG' ⁰²⁸, * { tsni } '2DU&3' ⁰³¹, * { tswa }
'2PL&3' ⁰³³, * { tni } '1+2DU:3' ⁰³⁶, and * { twa } '1+2PL:3' ⁰³⁸, and an allomorph
* / waɁ / seems to have preceded the duplicative prefix and all remaining pronominal
the five pronominal prefixes listed should condition a unique allomorph of the factual
relative to other pronominal prefixes is unclear, but they also condition unique
134
allomorphs of the duplicative, optative, cislocative, repetitive, and non-singular animate
The PNI duplicative prefix seems to have had an allomorph * / tõ / ⁰⁰⁷ following the
factual before the cislocative or repetitive prefixes, and before the cislocative or
repetitive prefix and the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes ), and before the
2:1 imperative pronominal prefixes ( PNI * / take / '2:1.SG.IMP' ⁰⁷² etc. ), whose initial
* / ti / seems to have occurred following the factual prefix before five second person
The PNI future prefix was * / ẽ / ⁰⁰⁸ in all environments. The PNI optative prefix
appears to have had an allomorph * / aɹe / ⁰⁰⁹ before five second person pronominal
have been lost entirely in most of the daughter languages, and has been preserved only
135
in certain environments in Old Mohawk and Tuscarora; its earlier presence in Huron
in intervocalic position ( PNI * / V¹ɹjV² / > Hu. / V¹jV² /; cf. HU-19 in 16.3 ).
The PNI cislocative prefix seems to have had an allomorph * / ka / ⁰¹⁰ in at least
some imperative verbs ( cf. PNI * / ˈkaːtsih / 'come here!' ³²⁹ ), and an allomorph * / ti /
that regularly preceded the pronominal prefixes * { tni } '1+2DU:3' ⁰³⁶ and * { twa }
following the factual or optative prefix before five second person pronominal prefixes
to have occurred following the factual or optative prefix before * / wa / with loss of
before the five second person pronominal prefixes, an allomorph * / ti / seems to have
environments.
The allomorphy of the PNI repetitive prefix appears to have mirrored that of the
repetitive prefix: an allomorph * / tsi / ⁰¹¹ seems to have preceded the pronominal
prefixes * { tni } '1+2DU:3' ⁰³⁶ and * { twa } '1+2PL:3' ⁰³⁸ in imperative verbs, an
136
allomorph * / tse / seems to have occurred following the factual or optative prefix
before five second person pronominal prefixes ( enumerated above in connection with
the factual prefix ), an allomorph * / tsõ / seems to have occurred following the factual
seems to have occurred in imperative verbs elsewhere, as well as following the factual
or optative prefixes elsewhere. Elsewhere, before the five second person pronominal
The non-singular animate prefix of PNI appears to have had a single allomorph
and * / kõ / have developed in Huron before five second person pronominal prefixes
( see details of the factual prefix above ) and before * / wa / with loss of * / wa /,
pronominal prefixes with an argument '3' rather than '3I' ( see 16.4 ).
PNI seems to have had a 1:2.DU pronominal prefix * { kni } ⁰¹⁴ and a 1:2.PL
pronominal prefix * { kwa } ⁰¹⁵ in pronominal prefix group 1, which mirror the dual
and plural number 2:1 forms * { hskni } ⁰¹⁷ and * { hskwa } ⁰¹⁸. PNI pronominal prefix
137
(8) PNI Pronominal Prefix Group 1
In other pronominal prefix groups, PNI appears to have expanded considerably the
which signals that a third person was male, or that a group of third persons ( 'they' )
contains at least one male. The meaning of the third person animate ( '3A' ) argument in
prefixes also appears to have shifted to third person feminine-indefinite ( '3I' ), which
unknown individuals ( 'someone' ). The meaning of the third person inanimate appears
to have become in PNI a neuter-zoic ( '3' ), which equally refers to animals, inanimate
To pronominal prefix group 2, PNI appears to have added a third person masculine
gender ( '3M' ) prefix variant to all four pre-existing pairs of 3/3I prefixes, as seen in
(9).
138
(9) PNI Pronominal Prefix Group 2
The composition of the 1SG:3M and 3M:2SG prefixes of group 2 is unclear, although
they share patterns of allomorphy with their 3I counterparts. The 2SG:3M prefix
2SG:3 prefix, while the 3M:1SG prefix has replaced initial * / w / of the 3:1SG prefix
To pronominal prefix group 3 PNI again seems to have added third person
masculine gender (3M) variants, as well as a third person indefinite (3I) variant, as can
seen in (10).
139
(10) PNI Pronominal Prefix Group 3
( PI animate / PNI indefinite ) and an element * { tshi }, which carries a meaning '2NS'
( cf. Tu. / tʃhi / '2NS:3.IMP' ). The masculine gender prefixes seem to have had an
initial masculine gender element * { sh- } which occurred in the fourth and fifth
pronominal prefix groups as well; the vowel * { -e- } that followed appears to result
from the fact that * { tsni } and * { tswa } were among five second person pronominal
prefixes that seem to have taken a preceding vowel * { -e- } where other prefixes took
variants in * { -a(Ɂ)- } ( see discussion of the factual prefix above ), as also were
* { tni } and * { twa } of PNI pronominal prefix group 4, which had third person
masculine gender variants * { shetni } and * { shetwa } ( rather than ˣ { shatni } and
ˣ { shatwa } ).
between inclusive and exclusive first person agents ( if such a distinction was not
already present in PI ), as well as adding a third person masculine gender equivalent for
140
each existent prefix and a set of three indefinite third person prefix variants in which
inclusive and exclusive agents were not distinguished, as can be seen in (11).
( PI animate / PNI indefinite ), but its remaining composition is unclear, as are those of
the other indefinite prefix variants. Remaining novel morphemes may be analyzed as
person masculine', the latter of which replaced initial * / j / of the former elements
when it preceded them. Note that reflexes of * { jethi } are limited to Iroquoia; the
141
function of the latter prefix is subsumed under * { jakhi } in Huron, and the function of
all three indefinite prefixes is subsumed under the reflex of * { jõkhi } in Tuscarora.
additional distinctions of dual and plural number, masculine and feminine gender, and
142
The 3SG:3 prefixes * { wa } and * { ka } of PI appear to have merged in PNI as
* { kõti } is found in any Northern Iroquoian language with a meaning '3NS:3', the two
forms appear to have been in competition or at some point to have been independent
prefixes, perhaps signifying feminine versus neuter-zoic gender ( Huron has merged
allomorphs of the two prefix as a single 3NS:3 prefix * { wati } ). The elements * { ni }
'dual number', * { je } 'third person indefinite' ( the third person animate morpheme in
groups. The elements * { k } and * { h } preceding the dual number forms appear to be
* { jakoti } '3NS:3I' and * { shakoti } '3NSM:3I' are limited to Iroquoia, and that Huron
and Tuscarora express the same agent-patient interaction with constructions employing
Marianne Mithun (p.c.) notes that Tuscarora's lack of distinct masculine gender
forms in non-singular pronominal prefixes involving the third person, its lack of reflexes
for the third person dual number prefixes * { kni } '2DU:3' and * { hni } '2DUM:3', and
143
its lack of reflexes of third person plural number prefixes containing a plural marker
* { ti } ( e.g. * { wati } '3PL:3', * { hroti } '3:3NSM', etc ) suggest that these elements
may constitute an innovation shared by Huron and the languages of the Iroquois
having taken place among the Lake Iroquoian languages during their hypothetical
distinctions in question may equally have been present in PNI only to be levelled in
Tuscarora. Evidence that may support such a hypothesis in fact includes absence in
cognates in Cherokee and in other Northern languages ( e.g. PI * { wantiː } '3NS:3' ⁰⁵⁸
'3A:3SG' ⁰⁶⁶ and PI * { kõːwantiː } '3A:3NS' ⁰⁶⁸, which have cognates in Cherokee and all
of the other Northern Iroquoian languages except Cayuga, as well as the consideration
that pronominal relationships that traditionally appear to have been expressed using the
strikingly simpler than that found in any other Northern Iroquoian language; Tuscarora
144
equivalents for pronominal prefix forms whose reflexes occur in other Northern
The Tuscarora pronominal forms in (13) are uniformly composed of a third person
indefinite pronominal prefix ( PNI * { je- } '3I:3' ⁰⁶³ or * { jako- } '3:3I' ⁰⁶⁴ ) combined
with either the duplicative prefix / nɛ- / ( PNI * { te- } ⁰⁰⁷ ) to mark dual number or the
non-singular animate prefix / ka- / ( PNI * { ka- } ⁰¹² ) to mark plural number, and/or
the reflexive morpheme / -aɁnaɁn- / ( PNI * { -atat- } ⁰⁷⁶ ) to signal interaction between
at least two animate/indefinite third persons. While the Tuscarora system is certainly
simpler, its lack of four out of six Group 5 pronominal prefixes that can be
145
* { kõːwantiː } ⁰⁶⁸ ) raises questions about whether it is necessarily archaic, or whether
Tuscarora has simply replaced all non-singular third person gender distinctions
( both masculine and feminine-neuter-zoic forms, the latter forms having cognates in
Cherokee ) with forms involving the third person indefinite ( 'someone, people' ). The
case for the simpler system of Tuscarora being archaic would be more compelling if a
similar system were employed in Southern Iroquoian, but in fact the non-singular third
person pronominal prefixes of Cherokee are cognate with forms found in other Northern
Iroquoian languages rather than with forms found in Tuscarora. Although this
consideration does not in itself prove that other Northern Iroquoian languages have
maintained the archaic PNI pronominal prefix system while Tuscarora has innovated a
system employing gender third person indefinite forms throughout the non-singular,
In addition to the five groups of pronominal prefixes just discussed, PNI also seems
to have had a sixth group of prefixes consisting of imperative forms that differed from
146
(14) PNI Pronominal Prefix Group 6
( PNI * { hske } ⁰¹⁶, * { hskni } ⁰¹⁷, * { hskwa } ⁰¹⁸ ); initial * / ta / had its etymological
origin in the cislocative prefix, judging from the duplicative prefix allomorph * / tõ / ⁰⁰⁷
that preceded it ( cf. OMo. / tũtaˈkekhaɁs / 'join these for me', / tũtakʷahshaˈɹũthaɁs /
'put the handle back on for me' ). The prefix * { tse } '2SG:3.IMP' seems to have had the
allomorph * / t /.
* / i / ), and the consonants and clusters before which a reflex of * / e / occurs vary in
each of the daughter languages. The consonants and clusters before which the 1SG:3
147
(15) Distribution of Prefix Allomorphs with Epenthetic / e / in PNI (1SG:3)
/k/ _hV, hjV, hnV, hnjV, hɹV, hɹjV, hsV, hsCV, htsV, hwV, jV, kV, kʷV,
nV, ɹV, tV, tsV, wV, ɁV, ɁnV¹ ( Æ nV¹ ), ɁrV¹ ( Æ rV¹ ),
ɁtV¹ ( Ɂ Æ tV¹ ), ɁwV¹ ( Æ wV¹ ).
Note that the environments in (15), and those presented in subsequent chapters, reflect
environments associated with other prefixes that have allomorphs in * / e /, such as the
2SG:3 prefix ( PNI * / hs / and * / hse / ⁰²² ), may differ slightly from those associated
with the 1SG:3 prefix, but conclusions about these differences are difficult to draw
owing to attestation of these prefixes before far fewer bases than the 1SG:3 prefix.
Noun incorporation was continued in PNI, and the language seems additionally to
* { -Ɂtshɹ- } ⁰⁸¹ that were used to from abstract nouns from verbs, and to incorporate
nominalized verbs into other verbs ( cf. Mo. / joɁtaˈɹihʌ̃ / 'it is hot', / joɁtaɹiˈhʌ̃hseɹaɁ /
'it-is-hot-ness; heat', / wakaɁtaɹihʌ̃hseɹaˈnohsthaɁ / 'I have a cold', lit. 'my heat is made
The derivational morphemes of PNI seem frequently to have been connected to their
148
* { -h- }, * { -ah }, * { -Ɂ- }, * { -aɁ- }, or * { -a- }, and consequently only the uniform or
* { hst } ⁰⁸³ were continued in PNI, and an instrumental morpheme * { hkw } ⁰⁸⁴
appears to have been added ( cf. PNI * / jõteˈkaɁthaɁ / 'one makes it burn' ¹⁶⁵,
before vowels, and a second, lexically determined reversive morpheme * { hsi } ⁰⁸⁶,
with an allomorph * / hsj / before vowels, has been added ( cf. PNI * / waɁˈehnɹẽːɁ /
'someone tied it' ²⁴⁰, * / waɁehˈnɹẽhsiɁ / 'someone untied it' ). PNI seems to have had an
inchoative morpheme with the form * { Ɂ } ⁰⁸⁷ ( cf. PNI * / waˈtjeːtsẽh / 'it is easy' ¹⁷⁸,
morpheme * { ts } ⁰⁸⁸ ( cf. PNI * / katũnˈhoɁkthaɁ / 'I end my life' ³⁹⁸ ⁴⁰⁶,
imperative verbs ( cf. PNI * / waɁˈkõːniɁ / 'I made it' ²⁶⁰, PNI * / kheˈjõnjẽɁ / 'I made it
149
for someone' ), and a third, lexically-determined dative-benefactive morpheme
( cf. * / ˈkõːnihsk / 'I make it' ²⁶⁰, PNI * / khejõˈnjẽːnihsk / 'I make it for someone' ).
which carried the meaning 'go, go in order to' ( cf. Mo. / kaˈtoːɹaths / 'I hunt',
projected back to PNI are * { -n- } ⁰⁹¹, * { -h- } ⁰⁹², * { -ts- } ⁰⁹³, and * { -tsɹ- } ⁰⁹⁴;
reflexes of a fifth variant, * { -ɹ- } ⁰⁹⁵, seems to be limited to Iroquoia. PNI seems also to
meaning 'several', 'on several occasions', or 'all over the place' ( cf. Mo. / kaˈtoːɹaths /
'I hunt', / katoˈɹathũhs / 'I hunt several animals' or 'I hunt on several occasions' or 'I hunt
all over the place' ). Distributive morpheme variants that can probably be projected
back to PNI are * { -njõː- } ⁰⁹⁶, * { -nõː- } ⁰⁹⁷, * { -hõː- } ⁰⁹⁸, * { -õː- } ⁰⁹⁹, * { -ɹõː- } ¹⁰⁰,
* { -ɹjõː- } ¹⁰¹, * { -tsõː- } ¹⁰², and * { -tsɹõː- } ¹⁰³. Finally, PNI seems to have had a
progressive morpheme * { -tjeɁ } ¹⁰⁴, which carried the meaning 'along' or 'go along
doing' ( cf. Mo. / kaˈtoːɹaths / 'I hunt', / wakatoɹatũˈhatjeɁ / 'I go along hunting' ).
The four principal bases of the PI verb ( present, punctual, imperfective, and
perfect ) appear to have been continued in PNI; because verb bases show only a handful
150
of endings in Northern Iroquoian languages, however, they are more often analyzed as
consisting of a verb base plus a lexically-determined aspect suffix ( among other works
cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:15, Woodbury 2003:23 ), and such an analysis here will
permit useful generalizations to be made regarding the most common endings of PNI
verb bases.
probably be projected back to PNI include * { -hs } ¹⁰⁵, * { -ts } ¹⁰⁶, * { -haɁ } ¹⁰⁷, and
* { -h } ¹⁰⁸, while reflexes of the imperfective base appear mainly to have ended in
* { -hsk } ¹⁰⁹ ( 'imperfective suffix' ) in PNI. Reflexes of the present and imperfective
bases have since fallen together as a generic habitual base ( 'habitual suffix' or
'serial suffix' ) in all of the Northern Iroquoian languages except Mohawk and Cayuga,
however, and even these preserve a present vs. imperfective distinction in only a
handful of verbs ( e.g. Mo. / ˈkũːni / 'I am making it right now' vs. / ˈkũːnihs / 'I make it
habitually' ). Reflexes of both the present and imperfective bases could evidently be
followed by a past habitual suffix that had the form * { -kʷ } or * { -kʷVɁ } ¹¹⁰ and
carried the meaning 'used to' ( cf. Mo. / kaˈtoːɹaths / 'I hunt', / kaˈtoɹathskʷeɁ /
The PNI punctual base seems to have occurred in both modal and imperative verbs,
151
and to have had the same ending in both except that word-final * / Ɂ / of a modal
corresponding imperative ( cf. Oo. / waɁkéˈktũɁ / 'I examined it', but / séˈktũh /
'examine it!'; Oo. / ẽhahsoˈtsjoːtẽɁ / 'he will pile it up', but / soːˈtsjoːtẽh / 'pile it up!' ).
base, with * / Ɂ / having been added consequently to mark modal verbs. Lexically-
determined endings of the punctual base ( 'punctual suffix' ) which can be projected
that can probably be projected back to PNI include * { -õh } ¹¹⁵, * { -ẽh } ¹¹⁶,
* { -Ø } ¹¹⁷, * { -eɁ } ¹¹⁸, and * { -ih } ¹¹⁹. To the perfect base PNI could apparently add
a past perfect suffix, which had the form * { -n } or * { -nVɁ } ¹²⁰ and carried the
meaning 'had' rather than 'have' ( cf. Mo. / wakatoˈɹaːtũ / 'I have hunted',
/ wakatoɹaˈtũhneɁ / 'I had hunted' ). PNI also seems to have had a facilitative suffix
* { -nskõːh } ¹²¹ that was typically added to the perfect base and carried the meaning
'often', 'easily' or 'frequently' ( cf. Se. / oˈnɔ̃Ɂnɔ̃wɛ̃ːh / 'it has melted', / onɔ̃Ɂnɔ̃ˈwɛ̃skɔ̃ːh /
'it melts easily' ); etymologically the facilitative suffix appears to consist of past perfect
* { -n }, habitual * { -hsk }, and perfect * { -õːh }. PNI also seems to have had a
152
modalizer suffix * { -k } ¹²² that allowed perfect bases to occur with modal prefixes such
as the future and factual ( cf. OMo. / johˈsnoːɹeɁ / 'it is fast', / ʌ̃johˈsnoːɹek /
Unincorporated PNI nouns seem most often to have ended in a noun suffix
* { -aɁ } ¹²³ ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈnõhsaɁ / 'house' ³⁸² ), although some nouns seem to have
been exceptions in having freestanding forms that were shorter than their incorporating
Several suffixes could evidently follow the noun suffix; those that can probably be
projected back to PNI include an authenticative suffix * { -õweh } ¹²⁴ meaning 'real,
genuine, original'; a characterizer suffix * { -haːɁ } ¹²⁵, meaning 'that which is associated
with'; a decessive suffix of the form * { -kẽh } ¹²⁶ or perhaps * { -kẽhVH }, meaning 'the
late, the former, the deceased' ( perhaps derived from the PNI verb * / kẽˈheːjõh / 'it is
dead' ²⁷⁶ ); an intensifier suffix * { -tsih } ¹²⁷, meaning 'very much, really'; a locative
suffix * { -keh } ¹²⁸, meaning 'at, in, on'; a populative suffix * { -ɹonõɁ } ¹²⁹, meaning
'person of, people of, nation of', and a second, lexically-determined populative suffix
153
compounded, e.g. * { X-kehaːɁ } 'that which is associated with (what is done) at
'it is big, large, great' ³⁴⁹ ); a second, lexically-determined locative suffix of the shape
* { -neh }, also with a meaning 'at, in, on'; and a pluralizer suffix, generally of the shape
154
CHAPTER 5: PROTO-TUSCARORA NOTTOWAY
5.1 INTRODUCTION
reconstructed language from which Tuscarora and Nottoway derive. PTN itself derives
from PNI, and is reconstructed on the basis of cognate forms shared by Tuscarora and
Nottoway. Where forms in the two languages disagree, but cognate forms in other
Iroquoian languages agree with one of the languages, the form that agrees has been
interpreted as archaic, and is projected back to PTN. Where forms in the two languages
disagree, and where cognate forms in no other Iroquoian language agree with either
independent developments in Tuscarora and in Nottoway, and are not projected back to
PTN.
Rudes (1976, 1981a) on the basis of shared phonological changes and the large number
suggests that Nottoway shared the greatest number of cognates with Tuscarora (138),
155
followed, at considerable distance, by Onondaga (75), Mohawk (70), Cayuga (68),
Seneca (67), Oneida (65), and Huron (60). The lexical similarity, in conjunction with a
significant number of shared sound changes ( see 5.3 ), supports the status of PTN as a
CONSONANTS
Plosive t ˀt k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts tʃ
Approximant ɹ j w
156
VOWELS
Close iː
i
Mid eː ẽː oː õː
e ẽ o õ
Open aː
a
from PNI * / t /, and contrasted phonemically with PTN * / t /, which in turn appears
cluster * / ths / to * / tʃ /. Note that although Rudes (1976:73, 1981a:32) has argued
that the PNI nasal vowels * / ẽ(ː) / and * / õ(ː) / had already merged as a mid-central
nasal vowel * / ə̃(ː) / in PTN, the vowels appear still to be distinct in Nottoway
vocabulary, where reflexes of the former are most often rendered using orthographic
<e>, <en>, <ain> or <a>, and reflexes of the latter are most often rendered using
157
orthographic <um>, <un>, <oon> or <oo> (Marianne Mithun, p.c.).
Phonological changes that appear to have occurred during the transition from PNI
to PTN are described below as PTN-1 through PTN-6. Where ordering of two changes
appears to be necessary to produce a correct output in PTN, this has been noted.
( e.g. PNI * / ohˈnitshaɁ / 'thigh' ²³⁸ > PTN * / ohˈtitʃhaɁ /; PNI * / ˈtsiɁtẽɁ / 'bird' ⁴⁶² >
PTN * / ˈtʃiɁˀtẽɁ /; PNI * / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ > PTN * / ˈtʃaːˀtahk / ). This change
By PTN-2, * / ths / has become * / tʃ / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈjõːnoːths / 'it is raining' ⁴⁰⁷ >
PTN * / ˈjõːtoːtʃ /; PNI * / wathsɁˈahtõh / 'it is used up' ¹⁷³ > PTN * / watʃˈɁahˀtõh / ).
By PTN-3, * / j / has been lost after * / tʃ / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ >
PTN * / ˈtʃaːˀtahk /; PNI * / ˈkẽtsjõh / 'fish' ⁴⁶⁷ > PTN * / ˈkẽtʃõh / ). This change must
* / ˀt / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈtsiɁtẽɁ / 'bird' ⁴⁶² > PTN * / ˈtʃiɁˀtẽɁ /; PNI * / ahˈtsõːtaɁ /
158
'night' ¹⁴⁹ > PTN * / ahˈtsõːˀtaɁ /; PNI * / ˈkõːteh / 'eel' ³⁴⁶ > PTN * / ˈkõːˀteh /;
cf. 'bird' and 'night' for non-application of the change to * / t / in a unit affricate ). This
* / n / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkankõɁ / 'blood' ³⁷⁶ > PTN * / ˈkaːtkõɁ /; PNI * / waɁˈkkenskoɁ /
'I raised it' ³³¹ > PTN * / waɁˈkkeːtskoɁ /; PNI * / rawẽˈninkẽɁts / 'he speaks up' ²⁸⁰ ⁴⁷⁶
> PTN * / rawẽˈtiːtkẽɁts / ). PTN may have lengthened penultimate vowels before
certain other clusters, as in Tuscarora, but the latter cannot be inferred implicationally
and before * / h / ( e.g. PNI * / oˈnaːtaɁ / 'village' ³⁵⁶ > PTN * / oˈtaːˀtaɁ /;
PNI * / kaˈnaɁtaɹaɁ / 'bread' ³⁶¹ > PTN * / kaˈtaˀtaɹaɁ /; PNI * / oɁˈnjõhsaɁ / 'nose' ⁴⁹⁴
> PTN * / oɁˈtjõhsaɁ /; PNI * / ˈkankõɁ / 'blood' ³⁷⁶ > PTN * / ˈkaːtkõɁ /; but
'it began to snow' ³⁷⁸ > PTN * / waɁˈkaːnkʷẽɁt /; PNI * / kẽˈnhaɁkeh / 'summer' ³³⁴ >
PTN * / kẽˈnhaɁkeh /; cf. also Rudes 1981a:33 ). As a result of this change, the PI
cluster * / ns / has become the unit affricate * / ts / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈwahnshẽːh / 'ten' ¹³⁵
> PTN * / ˈwahtshẽːh /; PNI * / waˈkansteɁ / 'it is hard' ¹⁵¹ > PTN * / waˈkaːtsˀteɁ /;
159
PNI * / keɁˈnonshahs / 'I envy it' ⁴⁹⁵ > PTN * / keɁˈtoːtshahs / ). PTN-6 must follow
However, note that in at least two cases in Nottoway, PNI * / n / has failed to
change to * / t / before * / i(ː) / ( PNI * / ˈtekniːh / 'two' ⁴³⁸ > No. / ˈˀtekniːh /
<Dekanee>; PNI * / niˈwahnshẽːh / 'that many tens' ⁰⁰⁴ > No. / niˈwahshə̃ːh /
<neewarsa> ). The change does take place in other Nottoway vocabulary items,
however ( cf. PNI * { -hweɁnõniːh } 'be round' ²⁷⁰ Æ No. <Tatowenente> 'round';
PNI * / ohˈnitshaɁ / 'thigh' ²³⁸ > No. / ohˈtitʃhaɁ / <Otitchag> ). Rudes (1981a:34)
notes several instances where PNI * / n / has failed to become / t / before * / o(ː) / in
Tuscarora ( e.g. PNI * / teˈtsnoːɹẽh / 'you split it' ³⁹⁶ > Tu. / nɛˈθnoːrə̃h / ), but the
change is attested in other vocabulary items ( e.g. PNI * / oˈnoɁtsaɁ / 'tooth' ³⁸⁰ >
Tu. / oˈtoɁθɛh /; PNI * / keɁˈnonshahs / 'I envy it' ⁴⁹⁵ > Tu. * / kjɛɁˈtoːθahs / ).
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to PTN are
recapitulated in (2).
160
(2) Phonological Changes in the Transition from PNI to PTN
morphological data in surviving Nottoway word lists; what can be discerned from these
( see 7.4 ) suggests that Nottoway was somewhat more conservative than Tuscarora, so
that PTN must have been as well. Nottoway morphology does not demonstrably share
any innovative traits with Tuscarora to the exclusion of other Northern Iroquoian
161
CHAPTER 6: TUSCARORA
6.1 INTRODUCTION
uncertain etymology ), whose territory in the early seventeenth century appears to have
ranged from the Blue Ridge Mountains in the west, bordering on the territory of the
Catawba, to the mouth of the Tar River in the east, bordering on the territory of three
Carolina Algonquian tribes ( Chowan, Pamlico, and Neuse ), north to the Roanoke
River, bordering on the territory of the Meherrin, and south to the Neuse River Basin,
bordering on the territory of the Woccon (cf. Binford 1967:116, Boyce 1978:282;
see Map 3). Today Tuscarora speakers are located on the Six Nations of the Grand River
by John Lawson (1674-1711) during a 1701 exploration of the Carolinas and published
in London a few years later (Lawson 1709). This earliest record of the language differs
little from modern Tuscarora, but appears to lack sound changes TU-15 and TU-16,
which are related in 6.3. Tuscarora has developed an eastern dialect, spoken at Six
Nations of the Grand River Reserve, and a western dialect, spoken on the Tuscarora
162
Indian Nation Reservation (Rudes 1999: xix-xxi). Phonological developments that
differentiate the two dialects are presented in 6.3. Unless otherwise stated, forms
the modern language that existed prior to the separation of the two dialects.
texts are in Crouse & Rudes (1987) and a grammar is in Williams (1976). Dictionaries
are in Rudes (1987) and Rudes (1999). Tuscarora forms in this chapter are based on
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Trill r
Fricative θ s h
Affricate tʃ
Approximant j w
163
VOWELS
Close iː
i
Mid ɛː ə̃ː oː
ɛ ə̃ o
Open aː
a
shifted to / θ /, and PTN * / e(ː) / has shifted to / ɛ(ː) /. Reflexes of PTN * / ẽ(ː) / and
* / õ(ː) / have merged as / ə̃(ː) / in Tuscarora, which is realized as [ ɨ ̃(ː) ] except where
the short variant occurs in stressed syllables; the vowels * / a(ː) / and * / o(ː) / have
shifted phonetically to [ ɔ(ː) ] and [ u(ː) ], respectively, except where the short variants
occur in unstressed syllables, and in this case the pronunciations [ a ] and [ o ] have
been retained (cf. Rudes 1995:xxxvii); for the sake of etymological continuity I continue
to use / a(ː) / and / o(ː) / to render these respective phonemes in Common Tuscarora,
164
voiced allophones [ d ], [ g ] and [ gʷ ] in Tuscarora before vowels and resonants, and
for at least some speakers are preglottalized in all environments (cf. Rudes 1976:84);
[ j ̥ ] that occur word-finally and before non-resonant consonants. Accented long vowels
have developed falling tone when they occur before a resonant consonant
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PTN to
Tuscarora are described below as TU-1 through TU-19. Where ordering of two changes
appears necessary in order to produce a correct output in Tuscarora, this has been
noted.
cluster having the shape Ks, KR, KH, or RR, where H is a glottal consonant, R is a
'it is bad' ¹⁵³ > Tu. / ˈwaːksə̃ː /; PTN * / jeˈtakɹeɁ / 'one resides' ³⁵⁴ > Tu. / jɛˈtaːkrɛɁ /;
PTN * / ɹaˈjẽthohs / 'he plants it' ³¹¹ > Tu. / raˈjə̃ːthohs /; PTN * / ˈowjaɁ / 'wing' ⁴⁸⁰ >
165
By TU-2, the nasal vowels * / ẽ(ː) / and * / õ(ː) / have merged as * / ə̃(ː) /
( e.g. PTN * / oˈnẽtʃhaɁ / 'arm' ³⁷⁰ > Tu. / oˈnə̃tʃhɛh /; PTN * / joɁˀtaˈɹihẽːh /
'it is hot' ²⁰⁴ > Tu. / joɁnaˈrihə̃ː /; PTN * / oˈnõhsaɁ / 'house' ³⁸² > Tu. / oˈnə̃hsɛh /;
PTN * / ˈõːkʷeh / 'person' ⁴⁰⁵ > Tu. * / ˈə̃ːkʷɛh / ). (cf. also Mithun 1979:164).
By TU-3, * / ts / has become * / θ / ( e.g. PTN * / ˈiːts / ²⁸⁹ > Tu. / ˈiːθ /;
PTN * / waˈkaːtsˀteɁ / 'it is hard' ¹⁵¹ > Tu. / waˈkaːθnɛɁ /; PTN * / ˈwahtshẽːh / 'ten' ¹³⁵
'the two of them differ' ⁴⁴² > Tu. / nɛjotɁˈnihə̃ / ), and by TU-5, * / ˀt / has been lost
before * / t / ( e.g. PTN * / ˈˀttiːkẽh / 'we see it' ⁰³⁶ > Tu. / ˈtiːkə̃h / ). TU-5 may have
Nottoway data relating to reflexes of this cluster are lacking. TU-4 and TU-5 must
'my grandchild' ¹⁸⁸ > Tu. / ˈkʷaːɁrɛh /; PTN * / ˈɹẽɁˀtɹõɁ / 'he dwells' ²⁸² >
Tu. / ˈrə̃Ɂrə̃Ɂ /; PTN * / waɁkaˈˀtɹiːjoɁ / 'I fought' ⁰⁷⁷ ⁴²⁸ > Tu. / waɁkaɁˈriːjoɁ / ), and as
with the first example, accented vowels in penultimate syllables have remained long in
Tuscarora where the cluster / Ɂr / derives from PTN * / ˀtɹ / ( < PNI * / tɹ / ). This
166
change must follow TU-1 and precede TU-9. (cf. also Mithun 1979:164).
vowel or a glide ( e.g. PTN * / ˈtʃaːˀtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ > Tu. / ˈtʃaːɁnahk /;
PTN * / ˈkõːˀteh / 'eel' ³⁴⁶ > Tu. / ˈkə̃ːɁnɛh /; PTN * / ahˈtsõːˀtaɁ / 'night' ¹⁴⁹ >
Tu. / ohˈθə̃ːɁnɛh /; PTN * / ə̃hˈɹaˀtjẽɁ / 'he will sit' ⁰⁷⁷ ³⁰⁶ > Tu. / ə̃hˈraːɁnjə̃Ɂ / ).
As in all of the examples, accented vowels in penultimate syllables have remained long
follow TU-1 and precede TU-9. Similarly, by TU-8, * / ˀt / has become * / n / when
( e.g. PTN * / ˈˀtekɹõɁ / 'eight' ⁴³⁹ > Tu. / ˈnɛːkrə̃Ɂ /; PTN * / ohˈˀtahkʷaɁ / 'shoe' ¹⁴² >
PTN * / waˈkaːtsˀteɁ / 'it is hard' ¹⁵¹ > Tu. / waˈkaːθnɛɁ / ). TU-8 must follow TU-4 and
Tu. / ˈotkə̃Ɂ /; PTN * / ˈaˀthoɁ / 'cold' ¹⁷⁰ > Tu. / ˈaːthoɁ / ). TU-9 must follow TU-4,
167
'he feeds it' ²³⁶ > Tu. / raˈnhə̃ːthohs /; PTN * / waɁˈkhehnhaɁn / 'I hired someone' ²³² >
Tu. / waɁˈkhɛnhaɁ /; PTN * / ɹahˈtskehnhaɁ / 'he is competing for it' ¹⁴⁷ >
beginning of a word ( e.g. PTN * / ˈhwihsk / 'five' ²⁷³ > Tu. / ˈwisk / ). By TU-12,
* / tʃ / ( e.g. PTN * / ˈakhsoːt / 'my female ancestor' ²⁵⁹ > Tu. / ˈaksoːt /;
PTN * / ˈakhtʃiɁ / 'my older maternal female cousin' ²⁶⁷ > Tu. / ˈaktʃiɁ /;
PTN * / koˈɹokhsjõhs / 'I uncover it' ⁰⁸⁶ ⁴⁷² > Tu. / koˈroksjə̃hs / ). By TU-13, * / h / has
been lost word-finally after long vowels ( e.g. PTN * / joɁˀtaˈɹihẽːh / 'it is hot' ²⁰⁴ >
PTN * / waknõhˈsõːtiːh / 'I have built the house' ²⁶⁰ ³⁸² > Tu. / waknə̃hˈsə̃ːtiː / ), but has
been retained after short vowels ( cf. PTN * / knõhˈsõːtih / 'I build the house' ²⁶⁰ ³⁸² >
Tu. / knə̃hˈsə̃ːtih / ).
'it began to snow' ³⁷⁸ > Tu. / waɁˈkaːtkwə̃Ɂ / ). Rudes (2002:191) notes that this change
168
By TU-16, * / t / has been inserted between * / s / and * / ɹ / in the cluster
* / hsɹ / ( e.g. PTN * / ˈohsɹaɁ / 'year' ³⁹⁴ > Tu. / aˈwohstrɛh /; PTN * / oˈhohsɹaɁ /
'basswood' ²⁴¹ > Tu. / oˈhohstrɛh /; PTN * / oɁˀtaɹiˈhẽhsɹaɁ / 'heat' ²⁰⁵ >
Tu. / oɁnariˈhə̃hstrɛh / ). Rudes (2002:191) notes that this change also had not yet
( e.g. PTN * / ˈiːkeɁ / 'I am walking' ²⁰⁹ > Tu. / ˈiːkjɛɁ /; PTN * / ˈkeɹhiɁ / 'tree' ³²⁸ >
Tu. / ˈkjɛrhiɁ / ). This change must precede TU-18. (cf. also Mithun 1979:164).
By TU-18, * / e(ː) / has shifted to * / ɛ(ː) / ( e.g. PTN * / ˈkeɹhiɁ / 'tree' ³²⁸ >
Tu. / ˈkjɛrhiɁ /; PTN * / ˈkõːˀteh / 'eel' ³⁴⁶ > Tu. / ˈkə̃ːɁnɛh /; PTN * / ˈˀtekɹõɁ /
'eight' ⁴³⁹ > Tu. / ˈnɛːkrə̃Ɂ / ). This change must follow TU-17.
Tu. / ˈnɛːkrə̃Ɂ /; PTN * / ˈtʃiɹ / 'dog' ³⁴¹ > Tu. / ˈtʃir /; PTN * / ˈkoːɹeh / 'red oak' ³⁴⁸ >
Tu. / ˈkoːrɛh / ).
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PTN to
169
(2) Phonological Changes in the Transition from PTN to Tuscarora
TU-1 ˈV¹ > ˈV¹ː / _Ks, KR, KH, RR ( penultimate syllables only )
TU-2 Ṽ(ː) > ə̃(ː)
TU-3 ts > θ
TU-4 ˀt > tɁ / _ˀt
TU-5 ˀt > Ø / _t
TU-6 ˀt > Ɂ / _ɹ
TU-7 ˀt > Ɂn / V_V, w, j
TU-8 ˀt > n / #, C_V, w, j
TU-9 ˀt > t
TU-10 h > Ø / _nh
TU-11 h > Ø / #_w
TU-12 h > Ø / k_s, ts, tʃ
TU-13 h > Ø / Vː_#
TU-14 w > Ø / _jh
TU-15 n > t / _kʷ
TU-16 Ø > t / hs_ɹ
TU-17 Ø > j / k_e(ː)
TU-18 e(ː) > ɛ(ː)
TU-19 ɹ>r
In addition to the sound changes in (2), additional sound changes have taken place
in the modern dialects of Tuscarora (Rudes 1999: xix-xxi). Seven such changes are
Western Tu. / ˈoɁsrɛh /; Tu. / ˈθahɛɁ / 'beans' Æ Western Tu. / ˈsahɛɁ /; Tu. / ˈθriht /
'boil it!' Æ Western Tu. / ˈsriht / ). This change occurred in the western dialect.
170
Western Tu. / ˈoːjɛh /; Tu. / oˈwjahsɛh / 'cross' Æ Western Tu. / oˈjahsɛh /;
Tu. / oˈwjə̃ːnɛh / 'manner, skill' Æ Western Tu. / oˈjə̃ːnɛh / ). This change occurred in
Western Tu. [ wahˈrə̃hɛɁʃ ] ). This change occurred in the western dialect, although it is
also attested among some speakers of the eastern dialect (cf. Rudes 1999:xxii).
Eastern Tu. / uˈhuhstɛh / 'basswood' ). This change has occurred in the eastern dialect.
Eastern Tu. / uˈnɨ ̃hsɛh /; Tu. / oˈrə̃ːtɛh / 'magic' Æ Eastern Tu. / uˈrɨ ̃ːtɛh /;
Tu. / ˈə̃ːkʷɛh / 'person' Æ Eastern Tu. / ˈɨ ̃ːkʷɛh / ). This change has occurred in the
Eastern Tu. / ˈɔːthuɁ /; Tu. / ˈahsə̃ / 'three' Æ Eastern Tu. / ˈɔhsɨ ̃ /; Tu. / ˈwahθhə̃ː / 'ten'
Æ Eastern Tu. / ˈwɔhθhɨ ̃ː / ). This change has occurred in the eastern dialect
171
(cf. Rudes 1999:xxxvi-xxxvii).
Eastern Tu. / ɔˈwuhstɛh /; Tu. / oˈtoːrɛh / 'corn husk' Æ Eastern Tu. / uˈtuːrɛh /;
Tu. / kaˈnoːrə̃Ɂ / 'it is dear' Æ Eastern Tu. / kɔˈnuːrɨ ̃Ɂ / ). This change has occurred in
Sound changes that have occurred in Tuscarora dialects are recapitulated in (3).
In addition to the phonemic changes in (3), the western dialect of Tuscarora has
short unaccented / i / is realized as [ ɪ ]. Other phonetic shifts have also occurred, many
of which appear to be associated with individuals and idiolects rather than the two
main dialects (Rudes 1999: xxxvii-xxxix). Three changes that appear in the speech of all
speakers of the western dialect, along with some speakers of the eastern dialect, can be
172
noted here. First, word-final / r / is realized phonetically as long [ sː ]
( e.g. Tu. / ˈtʃir / 'dog' Æ Western Tu. [ ˈtʃisː ] ). Second, / j / is realized phonetically as
[ ʃ ] when it occurs in syllable coda ( e.g. Tu. / oˈjhə̃haːkt / 'Lewiston, New York' Æ
coda ( e.g. Tu. / ˈwaɁkhaɁw / 'I brought it' Æ Western Tu. [ ˈwɔɁkhaɁf ] ).
Tuscarora appears to have lost the negative and coincident prepronominal prefixes
of PNI but has added a 'generic' prefix / tʃwɛ / which carries the meaning 'all kinds'
( e.g. / tʃwɛkatʃiɁnə̃Ɂˈtʃraːkɛː / 'all kinds of birds' ); it does not occur with other
has become / j / in Tuscarora before vowels and / wɛ / before consonants, with the full
form / jah / being preserved only before the factual prefix. The Tuscarora duplicative
prefix * { nɛ } ⁰⁰⁷ is used to make the '3I' argument of a pronominal prefix dual
( e.g. Tu. { shɛ- } 'you act on her'; { nɛ-hshɛ- } 'you act on them two'; { jɛθa- }
'she acts on you'; { nɛ-jɛθa- } 'they two act on you' ). The optative prepronominal prefix
( PNI * { aɹa } ⁰⁰⁹ ) has lost * / ɹ / in Tuscarora except when it occurred before a glide
173
To the 1:2.DU and 1:2.PL pronominal prefixes Tuscarora appears to have added an
element { kjɛ } to yield { kjɛkti } ⁰¹⁴ and { kjɛkwa } ⁰¹⁵. The language lacks reflexes of
the PNI third person masculine prefixes * { ri } '1SG:3M', * { hehse } '2SG:3M', and
* { hja }, in place of which prefixes with the third person indefinite occur instead
( e.g. Tu. / ˈkhɛːkə̃h / 'I see him, her, someone' ), and all non-singular prefixes involving
third person indefinite prefixes with the duaplicative ( Tu. { nɛ- } ), non-singular
animate ( Tu. { ka- } ), and reflexive ( Tu. { -aɁnaɁn- } ) prefixes ( see 4.4 for
discussion ). The PNI prefix * { jetshi } '2NS&3I' ⁰³⁵ has become { jə̃tʃhi } in Tuscarora,
while the prefix * { jakhi } '1+3NS:3I' ⁰⁴⁹ has merged with the reflex of PNI * { jõkhi }
'3I:1NS' ⁰⁵⁰ to yield Tuscarora { jə̃khi } '1NS:3I'. In the imperative Tuscarora has a 2NS:3
pronominal prefix { tʃhi } ( with an allomorph { tʃhij } before vowels ) which is perhaps
archaic ( cf. PNI * { jetshi } '2NS&3I' ⁰⁰⁰, in which * { tshi } carries the meaning '2NS' ).
Pronominal prefixes with allomorphs in / w / before certain vowels seem in all cases to
( e.g. PNI * { j-õnoːths } 'it is raining' ⁴⁰⁷ > PTN * { j-õtoːtʃ } Æ Tu. { w-ə̃toːtʃ } ).
Tuscarora seem to be unaltered relative to PNI except for the effects of regular sound
174
change. Consonants and clusters before which the 1SG:3 pronominal prefix allomorphs
/k/ _hV, hjV, hrV, hnV, hrjV, hsV¹ ( Æ sV¹ ), hsC¹V¹ ( Æ sC¹V¹ ),
hθV¹ ( Æ θV¹ ), htʃV¹ ( Æ tʃV¹ ), hwV, hwjV, jV, kV, kʷV,
nV, rV, θV, tV, tjV¹, tʃV, wV, ɁV, ɁnV¹ ( Æ nV¹ ), ɁrV,
ɁtV¹ ( Æ tV¹ ), ɁtjV¹ ( Æ tjV¹ ), ɁwV.
conservative, though the language no longer makes a distinction between present and
imperfective forms and instead has habitual forms. Tuscarora seems to have replaced
the PNI punctual suffix * { -n } ¹¹⁴ with * { -Ø } ¹¹³, probably because the former
( e.g. PTN * / waɁˈkhehnhaɁt / 'I hired someone' ²³² > Tu. / waɁˈkhɛnhaɁ /;
PTN * / waɁˈkaːnkʷẽɁt / 'it began to snow' ³⁷⁸ > Tu. / waɁˈkaːtkwə̃Ɂ / ), and likewise
seems to have replaced the PNI past perfect suffix * { -n } ¹²⁰ with { -k }
( cf. Tu. / raˈhə̃stʃi / 'he is black', / raˈhə̃stʃihk / 'he was black' ), probably for the same
reason. Etymologically the past perfect is identical in form to the modalizer { -k } ¹²²,
175
'that it be cooked', / naɁkahˈsɁahnahk / 'it used it up' ).
The morphology of the Tuscarora noun seems to be the same as that of PNI and
PTN except that inalienably possessed body parts no longer require a locative suffix,
and the noun suffix { -aɁ } ¹²³ has been replaced by an innovative form { -ɛh }
( e.g. PTN * / oˈnõhsaɁ / 'house' ⁰⁰⁰ > Tu. / oˈnə̃hsɛh / ), a change which may still have
been underway in the twentieth century (cf. Williams 1976:202, Rudes 1976:87).
Tuscarora has maintained the noun suffixes of PNI and has added an augmentative
suffix { -ojɁ }.
176
CHAPTER 7: NOTTOWAY
7.1 INTRODUCTION
seventeenth century appears to have ranged from the Blue Ridge Mountains in the west,
bordering on the territory of the Catawba, to the Blackwater River in the east, bordering
on the territory of the Nansemond, north to the Appomattox River, bordering on the
territory of the Powhatan Confederacy, and south to the Nottoway River, bordering on
the territory of the Meherrin (cf. Binford 1967:116, Boyce 1978:282; see Map 3). The
Nottoway language is extinct, but is known from a list of over 200 items of vocabulary
recorded by John Wood in 1820 from Edie Turner. A small amount of additional
vocabulary occurs in the notes of ethnologist J. N. B. Hewitt (n.d) along with the
contents of Wood's (1820) original list. These additional items have probably been
collated from a list of vocabulary gathered by James Tresevant, who was mentioned by
177
7.2 PHONEMIC INVENTORY
CONSONANTS
Plosive t ˀt k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate tʃ
Approximant ɹ j w
VOWELS
Close iː
i
Mid eː ẽː oː õː
e ẽ o õ
Open aː
a
Tuscarora, but this cannot be determined from orthography; in accordance with the
178
principle of conservative reconstruction I have assumed that vowel lengths did not
change. In accordance with the same principle, reflexes of PTN * / ˀt / are assumed to
similar to that of other Northern Iroquoian languages ( except Huron and Wyandot ),
vowels or resonants ( cf. No. <Geree> 'tree' from PTN * / ˈkeɹhiɁ / ³²⁸;
No. <Gotatera> 'bread' from PTN * / kaˈtaɁˀtaɹaɁ / ³⁶¹; No. <Gatkum> 'blood' from
PTN * / ˈkaːtkõɁ / ³⁷⁶; No. <Dekra> 'eight' from PTN * / ˈˀteːkɹõɁ / ⁴³⁹ ). Virtually all
examples of such voicing occur in word-initial position, however, and even here there
are some exceptions ( cf. No. <Kunte> 'eel' from PTN * / ˈkõːˀteh / ³⁴⁶; No. <Coree>
'red oak' from PTN * / ˈkoːɹih / ³⁴⁸; and No. <Tawrettig> 'chicken', which has a
The only clearly phonological change that Nottoway has undergone ( NO-1 ) is
179
'it is hard' ¹⁵¹ > No. / waˈkaːsˀteɁ / <Wokoste> and <Wakaste>; PTN * / oˈtoɁtsaɁ /
'tooth' ³⁸⁰ > No. / oˈtoɁsaɁ / <Otosag>; PTN * / ahˈtsõːˀtaɁ / 'night time' ¹⁴⁹ >
From what little can be discerned from surviving vocabulary, Nottoway appears to
have inherited the morphology of PTN and PNI intact, and differed from its sister
language Tuscarora mainly in exhibiting greater retention of archaic traits from PNI and
PTN, such as continued use of the noun suffix * { -aɁ } ¹²³. Another archaic trait that
Nottoway appears to share with other Northern Iroquoian languages except Tuscarora,
is obligatory use of the locative suffix * { -keh } ¹²⁸ with inalienably possessed body
parts, cf. No. / shõhˈˀtaɁkeh / <Suntunke> 'your ear' ( cf. Tu. / ˈshə̃hnɛh / );
No. / sahˈsiɁkeh / <Saseeke> 'your foot' ( cf. Tu. / ˈsahsɛh / ); No. / s(e)taɁˈɹaɁkeh /
<Setarakē> 'your head' ( cf. Tu. / ˈstaɁrɛh / ). (cf. Rudes 1981a). Nottoway also
exhibits a 3SG:3 pronominal prefix allomorph / j / that occurs before bases beginning in
from PNI * / ˈjõːtoːths / 'it is raining' ⁴⁰⁷ ), whereas Tuscarora appears to have levelled
180
this allomorph to / w /.
181
CHAPTER 8: SUSQUEHANNOCK
8.1 INTRODUCTION
( Hu. / xanahstoɁxehˈɹoːnõɁ / 'People at the post in the water' ), whose territory in the
early seventeenth century may have ranged south to the mouth of the Susquehanna
River, bordering on the territory of the Piscataway, east to the Schuylkill and Lehigh
Rivers, bordering on the territory of the Delaware, north to the Wyoming Flats, at that
time uninhabited, and west to the Appalachian Plateau, bordering on the territory of
see Map 4). The Susquehannock language is extinct, but is known from a list of 80
vocabulary items recorded in the 1640s by the Swedish missionary Johann Campanius
1696). The list was reprinted by his grandson Thomas Campanius Holm (1702) with 9
additional vocabulary items, which presumably were in the original notes of Campanius
but not included in the previous publication. To these items of vocabulary one can also
add <Connadago> 'city', which occurs in the narrative of George Alsop (1666).
Laurentian forms in this chapter are based on data from these three sources.
Mithun (1981a:23) finds that Susquehannock cannot be subgrouped with any of the
182
Five Nations Iroquoian languages since it does not share joint innovations with any of
them, and her comparison of Susquehannock vocabulary against the vocabulary of other
Iroquoian languages (Mithun 1981a:5) suggests that the language shared the greatest
number of cognates with its closest geographic neighbours: Onondaga (59), Mohawk
(57), Oneida (57), Cayuga (50), Seneca (49), Tuscarora (48), Huron-Wyandot (44), and
from seven well-attested Northern languages yields lower numbers, but the order of
languages is broadly similar: Mohawk (48), Oneida (45), Onondaga (43), Seneca (38),
Cayuga (37), Tuscarora (36), and Huron (34). In general, Susquehannock phonology
and morphology appear to have been fairly conservative, and only a few changes
183
(1) Reconstituted Phonemic Inventory of Susquehannock
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant ɹ j w
VOWELS
Close iː
i
Mid eː ẽː oː õː
e ẽ o õ
Open aː
a
Contrastive vowel length may have been preserved in final syllables, but this cannot
( rather than <Hijs> ) for the reflex of PNI * / iːts / 'you' ²⁸⁹ suggests that it may have
been lost, but in accordance with the principle of conservative reconstruction I have left
184
contrastive vowel length intact.
system of phonetic voicing similar to those found in other Northern Iroquoian languages
( except Huron and Wyandot ), whereby non-glottal plosives were phonetically voiced
when preceded and followed by vowels or resonants, cf. Su. <Adwgen> 'axe'
( PNI * / aˈtoːkẽɁ / ¹⁸² ), Su. <Oneegha> 'water' ( cf. PNI * / ohˈneːkaɁ / 'water' ³⁶⁴ ),
Su. <Cannadago> ( cf. PNI * / kaˈnatakõːh / 'in the village' ³⁵⁶ ); the appearance of
orthographic <l> in a few words containing * / r / suggests that the realization of this
phoneme was closer to [ l ], as is also the case in Akwesasne Mohawk and Oneida
PNI. Like Old Mohawk and Old Onondaga, Susquehannock shows an epenthetic vowel,
<Tiggene> 'two ( PNI * / ˈtekniːh / ⁴³⁸ ), <Tickerom> 'eight' ( PNI * / ˈtekɹõɁ / ⁴³⁹ ),
Old Mohawk, and Proto-Mohawk-Oneida, this vowel was likely phonetic rather than
185
phonemic ( see 10.2 and 13.2 ).
'five' ²⁷³ > Su. <Wisck> ). This development is also found in Tuscarora, Mohawk,
By SU-2, * / ts / has become * / s / word-finally ( e.g. PNI * / iːts / 'you' ²⁸⁹ >
'turkey' begin with the same characterizer construction involving the repetitive prefix
( PNI * / tska- / ) found in Oo. / skáːˈjanis / 'fisher (lit. the long-tracked one)' and
Mo. / skaweˈɹowaneɁ / 'turkey (lit. the big-winded one)'. PNI * / ts / has failed to
( cf. PNI * / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ > Su. <Tzadack>; PNI * / ˈkatsheɁ / 'gourd, jar' ⁴⁵²
> Su. <Kaatzie>; PNI * / ˈkaːtsih / 'come here!' ³²⁹ > Su. <Kaatzie>;
PNI * / ˈtsitshoɁ / 'fox, lynx' ⁴⁶⁰ > Su. <Tzídtze> 'cat'; PNI * / ˈɹaːtsin / 'he is male' ⁴⁵⁷
> Su. <Ræætsin>; cf. also Su. <Katzha gaije?> 'what do you have?' and
Oe. / ˈkatshaɁ kaːˈjʌ̃ː̂ / 'which place?' from Michelson & Doxtator 2002:463 ).
Northern Iroquoian languages except Tuscarora ( e.g. PNI * / ˈwahnshẽːh / 'ten' ¹³⁵ >
Su. <Wásha> ).
186
The partial description of deaffrication presented by SU-2 and SU-3 is true also of
Mohawk, Oneida, Onondaga, and Cayuga, but not of Tuscarora, Seneca, Huron, or
Wyandot, which again suggests that Susquehannock most resembled its closest
geographic neighbours.
from PNI except perhaps, as noted by Mithun (1981a:19), for a single perfect verb form
( cf. OMo. / saɹihˈwaksʌ̃ / 'you are malicious' ). While this example might suggest the
language was closer to Onondaga or Huron, which also tend to show perfect bases in
* { -ih } ¹¹⁹, it is worth noting that both Onondaga and Huron in this case show reflexes
of * / -aksẽh / 'be bad' ¹⁵³, rather than ˣ / -aksih / ( attested in no language ); the
<e>, [ ẽ ].
187
CHAPTER 9: LAURENTIAN
9.1 INTRODUCTION
peoples whom Jacques Cartier found inhabiting a number of villages along the north
shore of the Saint Lawrence River between Ajoaste ( perhaps near modern Saint-Tite-
des-Caps, Quebec ) and Hochelaga ( now the Island of Montreal ) during three voyages
in the years 1534, 1535, and 1541 (see Map 5). These villages and their inhabitants had
vanished by the time Samuel de Champlain reached the Saint Lawrence River in 1603,
most probably from speakers from the village of Stadacona ( now part of Quebec City )
during his first and second voyages. In all over 200 items of vocabulary are represented
in the two lists, both of which are presented in Biggar (1924) and analyzed in Barbeau
(1961), though only about a third of the items have identifiable cognates in other
Mohawk, and Onondaga shared the greatest number of cognates with Laurentian
( all three having 47 ), followed by Oneida (41), Seneca (36), Tuscarora (34), and
188
Cayuga (32). Impressionistically, Laurentian phonology does not seem to have been
nearly as conservative as that of, for example, Susquehannock, though only a handful of
changes relative to PNI can be discerned from the forms that are recorded. Note that
Lounsbury (1978:335) and Mithun (1981b:241) posit that the Cartier vocabularies
contain words taken from several Iroquoian languages, owing to variant phonological
features like loss or retention of PNI * / ɹ /, but I regard the vocabularies as capable of
reflecting a single language exhibiting regular sound changes. Unless otherwise stated,
Laurentian forms in this chapter are based on data from the vocabulary lists in Biggar
(1924).
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s ʃ h
Affricate ts
Approximant ɹ j w
189
VOWELS
Close iː
i
Mid eː ẽː oː õː
e ẽ o õ
Open aː
a
Contrastive vowel length may have been preserved in final syllables, but this cannot
( orthographic <ch> ) appears to have developed from PNI * / s / and likely became
Like other Northern Iroquoian languages ( apart from Huron and Wyandot )
( cf. La. <Addogué> 'hatchet' from PNI * / aˈtoːkẽɁ / 'axe' ¹⁸²; La. <Asquenondo> 'doe'
190
and may have had a pronunciation [ dz ] or [ dʒ ] before voiced segments
( cf. La. <Azista> 'fire', from PNI * / oˈtsistaɁ / ⁴⁵⁹; La. <Quejon> 'fish', from
been realized as [ m ] when followed by a nasal vowel, cf. La. <Ame> 'water'
or [ l ] in some cases, cf. La. <Aiagla> 'night' ( PNI * { -aɁkɹaɁ- } 'become night' ²⁰¹ ).
have had an epenthetic phonetic vowel, orthographic <e>, that was in some instances
used to break up clusters involving a consonant and resonant ( cf. La. <Tigueny> 'two',
from PNI * / ˈtekniːh / ⁴³⁸ ; La. <Wadellon> 'nine', from PNI * / ˈwaɁtɹõh / ²²² ;
A list of seven phonological changes that separate Laurentian from PNI are
( e.g. PNI * / ˈahsẽh / ¹⁴⁰ 'three' > La. / ˈahʃẽh / <Asche>; PNI * / kaˈnõhsaɁ /
191
'house' ³⁸² > La. / kaˈnõhʃaɁ / <Canocha>; PNI * / ohˈsiɁtaɁ / 'foot' ¹³⁹ Æ
La. / ohʃiɁˈtahkõ / <Ochedasco> '[on the] foot' ). This change has not applied to the
unit affricate * / ts /.
non-high vowels as well ( cf. PNI * / ˈtsaheɁ / 'beans' ⁴⁵¹ > La. / ˈsaheɁ / <Sahé> ).
Note that, as in other Northern Iroquoian languages, reflexes of the unit affricate * / ts /
appear to remain before * / i / and * / j / in Laurentian ( e.g. PNI * / oˈtsistaɁ / 'fire' ⁴⁵⁹
> La. / aˈtsistaɁ / <Azista>; PNI * / ˈkẽtsjõh / 'fish' ⁴⁶⁷ > La. / ˈkẽtsjõh /
<Quejon> ).
By LA-3, * / n / has been lost before * / s / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈwahnshẽːh / 'ten' ¹³⁵ >
La. / ˈahshẽːh / <Assem>; PNI * / aõˈnonskõːh / 'it rains often' ¹²¹ ⁴⁰⁷
( cf. Huron / aõˈnoskõːh / 'il pleut toujours' ) > La. / (a)õˈnoskõːh / <Onnoscon> ).
'it is hot' ²⁰⁴ > La. / oɁtaˈihẽːh / <Odaian>; PNI * / tsjoˈtaːɹeɁ / 'six, seven' ⁴⁶⁶ >
La. / tsjoɁˈtaːeɁ / <Judayé> 'six'; PNI * / kaˈɹõhjaɁ / 'sky' ⁴³⁰ > La. / kaˈõhjaɁ /
<Quemhya> and <Quenhia>; PNI * / kaˈɹõːtaɁ / 'log, tree' ⁴³¹ > La. / kaˈõːtaɁ /
<Conda> 'wood' ). Note that in other environments ( except before * / j /, cf. LA-5 )
the phoneme has apparently been retained ( cf. e.g. La. <Carraconny> 'bread', from
192
PNI * / kahɹahˈkõːniːh / 'the bread has been made' ²⁴² ²⁶⁰, La. <Aiagla> 'night', from
( e.g. PNI * / kahˈɹjoɁkẽh / 'chipmunk' ³⁴⁰ > La. / kahˈjoɁkẽh / <Caioguen> 'squirrel';
PNI * / (aɁ)ˈkaɹjoːɁ / 'animal' ⁴²⁹ > La. / aɁˈkajoːɁ / <Aggayo> 'dog' ), and similarly,
by LA-6, the approximant * / w / has been lost before * / j / ( e.g. PNI * / aˈkewjaɁ /
By LA-7, * / j / has been lost word-initially ( e.g. PNI * / joɁtaˈɹihẽːh / 'it is hot' ²⁰⁴
vocabulary items as being potentially the result of the influence of French orthography
and not an actual sound change ( e.g. PNI * / joɁtaˈɹihẽːh / 'it is hot' ²⁰⁴ >
La. / oɁtaˈihẽːh / <Odaian>, but PNI * / ˈtsaheɁ / 'beans' ⁴⁵¹ > La. / ˈsaheɁ /
being cognate with PNI * { -hɹaɁt- } 'feather' ²⁴³ (cf. Mithun 1981b:240); the Laurentian
word <Angau> 'evening' may be cognate with PNI * / ẽˈkaːɹaːk / ˈthe moon will be
out' ⁴¹⁴ rather than with PNI * / ẽˈjoɁkɹaɁn / 'it will become night' ²⁰¹
( cf. La. <Aiagla> ), and as such the form may reflect loss of * / ɹ / intervocalically, or
193
perhaps word-finally, rather than post-consonantally (cf. Mithun 1981b:241). Several
other changes may potentially have occurred in Laurentian, such as loss of * / j / after
Secure phonological changes that occurred during the transition from PNI to Laurentian
All of the sound changes in (2) except LA-4 are reflected in Huron, though all
except LA-5 and LA-6 apply to other Northern Iroquoian languages as well. Sound
changes LA-5 and LA-6 ( which reduce PNI * / wj / and * / ɹj / to / j / ) are the most
diffusion of these changes from one language into the other cannot be ruled out. Other
linguistic hallmarks of Huron that are not found in Laurentian, such as apparent
194
maintenance of PNI * / k / in all environments in Laurentian ( without a shift to / x / ),
Cayuga, and Seneca, but absent from Huron ), presence of voiced allophones of non-
glottal plosives in Laurentian between vowels and resonants ( present also in all of the
Northern Iroquoian languages except for Huron and Wyandot ), and presence of an
consisting of a stop plus resonant ( present also in Susquehannock, Old Mohawk and
Old Onondaga as orthographic <e>, but not in Huron or Wyandot ). Due to these
Huron, Mohawk, and Onondaga with Laurentian, the language is here considered to
195
novel development involves a locative suffix with the form <sco> or <scon>,
196
CHAPTER 10: PROTO-MOHAWK-ONEIDA
10.1 INTRODUCTION
language from which Mohawk and Oneida derive. PMO itself derives from PNI, and is
reconstructed on the basis of cognate forms shared by Mohawk and Oneida. Where
forms in the two languages disagree, but cognate forms in other Iroquoian languages
agree with one of the languages, to the exclusion of the other, the form that agrees has
been interpreted as archaic, and is projected back to PMO. Where forms in the two
languages disagree, and where cognate forms in no other Iroquoian language agree with
either Mohawk or Oneida, the forms in question have been interpreted as subsequent
independent developments in Mohawk and in Oneida, and are not projected back to
PMO.
197
(1) Phonemic Inventory of Proto-Mohawk-Oneida
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant ɹ j w
VOWELS
Close iː ũː
i ũ
Mid eː ʌ̃ː oː
e ʌ̃ o
Open aː
a
PNI * / ẽ(ː) / and * / õ(ː) / have undergone a shift to PMO * / ʌ̃(ː) / and * / ũ(ː) /
respectively. Although vowel length seems to have been almost entirely predictable in
nonetheless appear to have been present ( e.g. PMO * { aː- } 'optative prefix' ⁰⁰⁹,
* { toːh } 'how many?' ⁴⁴⁷ ), and consequently distinctive long and short vowels have
198
been retained in the phonemic inventory.
and PMO * / s / may have had an allophone [ z ] that occurred word-initially before
( e.g. PMO * / swaˈnũhsaɁ / 'your house' Æ [ zwɐˈnũhsɐɁ ]; PMO * / ˈisiɁ / 'over there'
Æ [ ˈiziɁ ]; PMO * / ˈsêːsɹeɁ / 'you drag it' Æ [ ˈzêːzɪɹɛɁ ] ). The affricate * / ts / may
PMO appears to have had an epenthetic phonetic vowel * [ ɪ ] that was used to
Æ [ ˈdegɪni ] 'two'; PMO * / ˈknakɹeɁ / 'I dwell' Æ [ gɪˈnagɪɹɛɁ ]; this vowel later seems
199
word forms in Oneida. Epenthetic * [ ɪ ] likewise appears to have been used to separate
also appears to have developed falling tone on penultimate vowels that were lengthened
when they preceded a glottal consonant ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈnaɁtsjaɁ / 'pot, kettle' ³⁶³ >
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to PMO are
described below as PMO-1 through PMO-13. Where ordering of two changes seems to
be necessary in order to produce a correct output in PMO, this has been noted.
By PMO-1, the nasal vowel * / ẽ(ː) / has shifted to * / ʌ̃(ː) / ( e.g. PNI * / oˈɹẽːnaɁ /
'song' ⁴²² > PMO * / oˈɹʌ̃ːnaɁ /; PNI * / aˈkʷaːwẽh / 'my possession' ¹⁹⁴ >
PMO * / aˈkʷaːwʌ̃ /; PNI * / jeˈjẽthohs / 'someone plants it' ³¹¹ > PMO * / jeˈjʌ̃thohs / ),
and similarly, by PMO-2, the nasal vowel * / õ(ː) / has shifted to * / ũ(ː) /
( e.g. PNI * / kaˈɹõːtaɁ / 'log, tree' ⁴³¹ > PMO * / kaˈɹũːtaɁ /; PNI * / ˈkiɁtɹõɁ /
200
'I reside' ²⁹² > PMO * / ˈkîːtɹũɁ /; PNI * / kaˈnõhsaɁ / 'house' ³⁸² >
PMO * / kaˈnũhsaɁ / ).
PMO * / ˈwahstʌ̃hs /; PNI * / ɹoɁˈnitskʷahs / 'he is late' ⁴⁹² > PMO * / ɹoɁˈnihskʷahs / ).
PNI * / ˈɹaɁtskoɁts / 'he drowns' ⁴⁹⁷ > PMO * / ˈɹaɁskoɁs /; but PNI * / ohˈnitshaɁ /
'thigh' ²³⁸ > PMO * / ohˈnitshaɁ /; PNI * / oˈtsiɁnkʷaɹ / 'yellow, bile' ⁴⁶¹ >
PMO * / oˈtsîːnkʷaɹ /; PNI * / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ > PMO * / ˈtsjaːtahk / ). This
( e.g. PNI * / joɁˈnshansteɁ / 'it is strong' ⁴⁹⁶ > PMO * / joɁˈshatsteɁ /; PNI * / ˈansteh /
'outside' ¹⁵⁵ > PMO * / ˈatste /; PNI * / ɹaˈkenskʷahs / 'he lifts it' ³³¹ >
PMO * / ɹaˈketskʷahs / ). This change must follow PMO-3 and precede PMO-6.
201
'I am envious' ⁴⁹⁵ > PMO * / keɁˈnoshahs /; PNI * / joɁˈnshansteɁ / 'it is strong' ⁴⁹⁶ >
By PMO-7, an accented vowel has been lengthened and acquired falling tone when
( e.g. PNI * / niˈwaɁah / 'it is small' ¹⁹⁷ > PMO * / niˈwâːɁa /; PNI * / ohˈnjaɁsaɁ /
'neck' ²³⁹ > PMO * / ohˈnjâːsaɁ /; PNI * / oˈkahɹaɁ / 'eye' ³¹⁹ > PMO * / oˈkâːɹaɁ / ).
By PMO-8, * / Ɂ / has been lost after an accented long vowel whenever a consonant
followed ( e.g. PNI * / oˈjẽɁkʷaɁ / 'tobacco' ³¹⁴ > PMO * / oˈjʌ̃ː̂ kʷaɁ /; PNI * / oˈjaɁtaɁ /
'body' ²⁹⁸ > PMO * / oˈjâːtaɁ /; PNI * / kaˈnaɁtsjaɁ / 'pot, kettle' ³⁶³ >
PMO * / kaˈnâːtsjaɁ / ), and similarly, by PMO-9, * / h / has been lost after an accented
long vowel when a resonant consonant followed ( e.g. PNI * / oˈkahɹaɁ / 'eye' ³¹⁹ >
PNI * / oˈɹihwaɁ / 'matter' ⁴²⁷ > PMO * / oˈɹîːwaɁ / ). Both changes must follow PMO-7.
202
( e.g. PNI * / ˈɹahɹaːths / 'he counts it' ¹³⁷ > PMO * / ˈɹâːɹaths /; PNI * / ˈtekniːh /
'two' ⁴³⁸ > PMO * / ˈtekni /; PNI * / teˈwahnshẽːh / 'twenty' ¹³⁵ >
PMO * / teˈwahshʌ̃ / ). Some exceptions to this change include the particle * / toː /
'how much' and the imperative verb * / ˈkaːts / 'come here!' ( < PNI * / ˈkaːtsih / ³²⁹ ).
separated by inserting an epenthetic copy of the vowel that followed the cluster
( e.g. PNI * / ˈɹaɹɁokhs / 'he chops' ⁴³² > PMO * / ˈɹaɹoɁokhs /; PNI * / atˈɁẽhraɁ /
'palisade' ¹⁹³ > PMO * / atʌ̃ˈɁʌ̃ː̂ ɹaɁ /; PNI * / knõhˈsihsɁahs / 'I finish the house' ²⁶⁶ ³⁸²
possession' ¹⁹⁴ > PMO * / aˈkʷaːwʌ̃ /; PNI * / joɁtaˈrihẽːh / 'it is hot' ²⁰⁴ >
PMO * / joɁtaˈɹihʌ̃ /; PNI * / knõhˈsõːnih / 'I am building a house' ²⁶⁰ ³⁸² >
beginning of a word ( e.g. PNI * / ˈhwihsk / 'five' ²⁷³ > PMO * / ˈwihsk / ).
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to PMO are
recapitulated in (2).
203
(2) Phonological Changes in the Transition from PNI to PMO
PMO has lost the non-singular animate prefix of PNI, and pronominal prefixes with
forms ( e.g. PMO * { khe } '1SG:3I, I act on someone or them' ), as has occurred also in
Onondaga and Seneca. As in Cayuga and Onondaga, the negative prefix could not occur
with modal prefixes in PMO and the contrastive prefix was substituted. The initial
sequence * / j(a)h / of the PNI translocative prefix * / j(a)hwe / ⁰⁰⁵ appears to have
allomorphs ( with epenthetic vowels copied from a following syllable ) also seems to
204
have developed, and occurred in environments where the prefix would originally have
occupied the onset of a stressed syllable ( e.g. PNI * / j(a)hˈwâːkeɁ / 'I went' >
PMO * / jaˈhâːkeɁ /; PNI * / j(a)ˈhwẽːkeɁ / 'I will go' > PMO * / jʌ̃ˈhʌ̃ːkeɁ / ); Old
beginning in / j /, and which perhaps was also present in PMO with subsequent
'one has gone there' and / hejothaˈhiːnũ / <heioθahinnon> 'the road leads there', both
from MS58:22 ). The PNI factual prefix allomorph * / waɁ / ⁰⁰⁶ has become * / aɁ / in
all but word-initial position in PMO, and the glottal stop has been lost before * / h /, as
is also the case in Tuscarora, Seneca, and Cayuga. The distinction between the PNI
following the factual prefix, was lost in PMO and appears to have been replaced by a
occurring elsewhere. The * / ɹ / of the optative prefix ( PNI * { aɹa } ⁰⁰⁹ ) has been lost
The inventory of pronominal prefixes in PMO seems to have been essentially the
same as that of PNI except that the prefix * { wati } '3PL:3' ⁰⁵⁸ was apparently absent,
205
having been replaced by reflexes of PNI * { kũti } '3PL:3' ⁰⁵⁹. The PNI 2SG:3M prefix
* { hehse } ⁰²⁴ has become PMO * { hetshe }, and compound PNI pronominal prefixes
beginning in the element * { she } have also added an element * { het } to the
and so forth, which appear to have become Mo. { hetshiseni }, { hetshitewa } and
1981:109). The PNI prefix * { hni } '3DUM:3' ⁰⁵⁷ has lost * / h / word-initially in PMO,
and the '3:3I' and '3NS:3I' pronominal prefixes * { jako } ⁰⁶⁴ and * { jakoti } ⁰⁷⁰ have
* / e / and * / ə̃ /, although the reflex of 3:1SG prefix allomorph * / wakʷ / ⁰²⁵ was
apparently replaced by * / wak / in PMO before bases beginning in all vowels unless
pronominal prefix allomorphs in PMO were apparently unchanged relative to PNI apart
from the effects of PMO-11. Consonants and clusters before which the 1SG:3
206
presented in (3).
/k/ _hV, hjV, hnV, hnjV, hɹV, hɹjV, hsV, hsCV, htsV, hwV, jV ( < PNI * jV ),
kV, kʷV, nV, ɹV ( < PNI * ɹV ), tV ( < PNI * tV ), tsV, wV.
The aspectual and derivational morphology of PMO seems to have been relatively
handful of verbs ( cf. PMO * / ˈkũːnihsk / 'I make it' vs. * / ˈkũːni / 'I'm making it' ²⁶⁰,
PMO * / ˈkheːkʌ̃hsk / 'I see someone (habitually)' vs. * / ˈkheːkʌ̃ / 'I see someone
(right now)' ³³² ), but generally these bases appear to have merged as general habitual
resonant consonant ( e.g. PNI * / õˈkiːtaɁw / 'I fell asleep' ²⁸⁷ > PMO * / ũˈkiːtaɁweɁ / ).
Note that while this suffix was later extended to all punctual verbs formerly ending in a
consonant in both Mohawk and Oneida, forms in Old Mohawk lack the suffix after non-
(Bruyas 1863:93).
207
differed formally from their verbal counterparts, both in their apparent lack of initial
'3I.ALPOSS' ( e.g. PNI / ɹoˈnũhsaɁ / 'his house' ³⁸² Æ PMO * / ɹaoˈnũhsaɁ / >
Mo. / ɹaoˈnũhsaɁ /, Oe. / laoˈnũhsaɁ / ). PMO seems to have retained the noun suffixes
208
CHAPTER 11: MOHAWK
11.1 INTRODUCTION
of the flint' ), whose territory in the early seventeenth century seems to have ranged
roughly from Trenton Falls in the west, bordering on the territory of the Oneida, to the
Hudson River Valley in the east, bordering on the territory of the Mahican, north to the
Adirondack Mountains, bordering on the territory of the Algonquin, and south to the
2003:124, village locations in Swanton 1968:34; see Map 6). Today speakers of
Mohawk reside at Tyendinaga Mohawk Territory and on Six Nations of the Grand River
Quebec, and on the Akwesasne Reserve or Saint Regis Mohawk Reservation, located on
the Saint Lawrence River at the juncture of Quebec, Ontario, and New York State.
Records of the Mohawk language date back to the early seventeenth century. The
earliest such record appears to be a short list of numbers and months recorded by
Nicolaes Janszoon Van Wassenaer (1571-1630) and published in the February 1624
edition of his newspaper (Van Wassenaer 1624). The earliest work of any length in the
209
language is the late seventeenth century grammar and dictionary of Jesuit father
Three stages of Mohawk can be recognized. The first stage, Old Mohawk, represents
the earliest attested form of the language and is preserved, for example, in the
dictionary by Bruyas and MS 58. Old Mohawk differs from later forms of the language
in that it exhibits only six of the thirteen sound changes that lead from PMO to
Common Mohawk ( e.g. the consonant clusters / kn / and / tn / are still present in Old
Mohawk, but become / ken / and / ten / respectively in Common Mohawk ), and it
retains a handful of archaic reflexes that are not present in Common or Modern
all modern Mohawk dialects can be derived. Phonological and morphological changes
which are present in all modern dialects of Mohawk ( e.g. the shift of the consonant
clusters / kn / and / tn / to / ken / and / ten / ) are assumed to have been present in
Common Mohawk, while phonological changes which are evident in some modern
dialects of Mohawk but not others ( e.g. the shift of the consonant cluster / tj / to / kj /
in Akwesasne Mohawk ) are assumed not to have been present in Common Mohawk,
210
and are interpreted as dialectal developments which have occurred at some point after
the six Modern Mohawk speech communities separated. Note that, unless otherwise
Modern Mohawk refers to the six modern dialects of the Mohawk language:
Tyendinaga Mohawk Territory; and Six Nations Mohawk (SNMo.), spoken at the Six
Nations of the Grand River Reserve. The six dialects differ only slightly on points of
phonology and vocabulary; all are mutually intelligible. Sound changes that separate
the six modern dialects from Common Mohawk will be presented in 11.3.
Dictionaries of Old Mohawk are in Bruyas (1863) and MS 58, and dictionaries of
Modern Mohawk are in Cuoq (1882), Michelson (1973), and Maracle (1990). Common
211
Mohawk forms in this chapter are based on data from Michelson (1973), Deering &
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant ɹ j w
VOWELS
Close iː ũː
i ũ
Mid eː ʌ̃ː oː
e ʌ̃ o
Open aː
a
212
The phonemes / t /, / k /, and / kʷ / have voiced allophones [ d ], [ g ], and [ gʷ ]
between vowels and resonants other than / j /. In Common Mohawk the affricate / ts /
Penultimate vowel lengthening does not occur in a handful of Mohawk forms due to
etymological origin, e.g. / ˈkeɹoɁkhs / 'I chop' ⁴³², from PMO * / ˈkeɹoɁokhs /
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PMO to
Mohawk are described below as MO-1 through MO-13. Where ordering of two or more
noted.
By MO-1, the vowel in a final syllable has been lost if the vowel of the penultimate
213
syllable was identical, the two vowels were separated by a glottal stop, and the word
had antepenultimate stress ( e.g. PMO * / ˈkeɹoɁokhs / 'I chop it' ⁴³² >
Mo. / ˈkeɹoɁkhs /; PMO * / ˈikhsaɁas / 'I finish it' ²⁶⁶ > Mo. / ˈikhsaɁs /;
PMO * / jaɁteˈkojaɁakhs / 'I throw it' ³⁹⁵ > Mo. / jaɁteˈkojaɁkhs / ). This change was
already present in Old Mohawk, cf. <Garógon>, <roks, rok, roχe> 'bûcher, faire du
cluster * / tshɹ / ( e.g. PMO * / atə̃ˈnâːtshɹaɁ / 'provisions' ¹⁶⁸ > Mo. / atʌ̃ˈnâːtsheɹaɁ /;
This change was already present in Old Mohawk, cf. <Atennatsera> 'provision'
PMO * / ˈohsɹaɁ / 'winter, year' ³⁹⁴ > Mo. / ˈohseɹaɁ /; PMO * / ahˈsɹiːjeɁ / 'string' ¹⁴¹ >
Mo. / ahseˈɹiːjeɁ / ), and this change was also already present in Old Mohawk,
cf. <Ennisera> 'jour' (Bruyas 1863:109). (cf. also Michelson 1981a:99). By MO-4,
( e.g. PMO * / oˈnɹahsaɁ / 'mushroom' ³⁸⁹ > Mo. / oneˈɹahsaɁ /; PMO * / teˈkkâːnɹeɁ /
214
'I look at it' ³¹⁸ > Mo. / teˈkkâːneɹeɁ /; PMO * / kajaˈnɹʌ̃hsɹaɁ / 'law' ²⁹⁶ >
Mo. / kajaneˈɹʌ̃hseɹaɁ / ), and this change was likewise already present in Old Mohawk,
By MO-5, * / w / has been lost before * / jh / ( e.g. PMO * / kaˈwjhũhaɁ / 'river' ²⁷⁸
> OMo. / kaˈjhũhaɁ / ). This change seems already to have been present in Old
Mohawk, cf. <Gaihonha> 'rivière, ruisseau' (Bruyas 1863:63), and must precede MO-7.
( e.g. PMO * / oˈwhjũhkaɹ / 'thumb' ⁴⁷⁷ > Mo. / oweˈjũhkaɹaɁ / ). This change appears
to have been present in Old Mohawk ( cf. <oionkara> 'Pouce' from MS 58:968 ) and
Mo. / kahˈjũhaɁ / ). This change was not present in Old Mohawk, cf. <Gaihonha>
PMO * / wakewjʌ̃nʌ̃ˈtâːɁũ / 'I am ready' ²²⁰ ⁴⁸⁴ > Mo. / wakewejʌ̃nʌ̃ˈtâːɁũ / ). This
change was not present in Old Mohawk, cf. <Ga8iahontsa> 'Aisle' (MS 58:21). It must
215
follow MO-5 and MO-6.
* / kʷ / ( e.g. PMO * / oˈtsîːnkʷaɹ / 'bile, green, yellow' ⁴⁶¹ > Mo. / oˈtsîːnekʷaɹ /;
'ankle' ²⁵¹ > Mo. / ohsineˈkôːtaɁ / ). This change phonemicized the phonetic vowel
* [ ɪ ] that was used to break up the clusters * / nk / and * / nkʷ / in PMO. The change
does not appear to have been present in Old Mohawk, since the vowel between * / n /
(Bruyas 1863:97).
* / r / ( e.g. PMO * / ˈknakɹeɁ / 'I dwell' ³⁵⁴ > Mo. / keˈnakeɹeɁ /; PMO * / ˈôːkɹaɁ /
'snowflake' ²⁰² > Mo. / ˈôːkeɹaɁ /; PMO * / ˈkʷatɹe / 'grandchild!' ¹⁸⁸ > Mo. / ˈkʷateɹe /;
PMO * / ˈtɹîːtɹiɁ / 'bluejay' ⁴⁵⁰ > Mo. / teˈɹîːteɹiɁ / ). This change was not present in Old
'mon petit fils ou p. fille' (Bruyas 1863:88). Similarly, by MO-11, epenthetic * / e / has
216
been inserted between a non-glottal plosive and * / n / ( e.g. PMO * / ˈtekni / 'two' ⁴³⁸
> Mo. / ˈtekeni /; PMO * / aˈknũhsaɁ / 'my house' ³⁸² > Mo. / akeˈnũhsaɁ /;
PMO * / tshaɁˈtetnaɁ / 'we are the same size' ¹⁹⁷ > Mo. / shaɁˈtetenaɁ /;
PMO * / ˈwetneɁ / 'we went, we are going' ²⁰⁹ > Mo. / ˈweteneɁ / ), and this change
was not present in Old Mohawk, cf. <Tegni> 'deux' (Bruyas 1863:39), <satetna>
( e.g. PMO * / twaˈɹâːseɁ / 'we are cousins' ¹⁵⁷ > Mo. / tewaˈɹâːseɁ /; PMO * / ˈwetweɁ /
'we went, we are going' ²⁰⁹ > Mo. / ˈweteweɁ /; PMO * / katwʌ̃ˈtehthaɁ /
'I abandon it' ¹⁹² > Mo. / katewʌ̃ˈtehthaɁ / ). This change was not present in Old
* / w / when a word boundary or vowel preceded ( e.g. PMO * / tshaɁˈtesnaɁ / 'you are
the same size' ¹⁹⁷ > Mo. / shaɁˈtesenaɁ /; PMO * / ˈsnakɹeɁ / 'you reside' ³⁵⁴ >
Mo. / seˈnakeɹeɁ /; PMO * / swatɹakeˈwâːthaɁ / 'that which is used to erase it' ⁴¹⁶ >
Mo. / sewateɹakeˈwâːthaɁ /; PMO * / ˈwesweɁ / 'you went, you are going' ²⁰⁹ >
Mo. / ˈweseweɁ / ). This change was not present in Old Mohawk, cf. <satesna>
'[you are the same size]' (Bruyas 1863:21), <S8atrage8aθa> 'Ce qui fait effacer'
217
(Bruyas 1863:33).
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PMO to
Several changes additional to those in (2) have since taken place in various Modern
Mohawk dialects. Seven such changes are described below as MO-D1 through MO-D7,
Mo. / tjotjeˈɹʌ̃htũ / 'first' > AkMo. / kjokjeˈlʌ̃htũ / ). This change occurs in the
218
Akwesasne dialect (cf. Mithun 1979:178, Bonvillain 1984:315).
AkMo. / ˈkjohtũ /; Mo. / ˈohstjʌ̃Ɂ / 'bone' > AkMo. / ˈoskjʌ̃Ɂ /; Mo. / wakahˈtʌ̃tjũ /
'I've left' > AkMo. / wakahˈtʌ̃kjũ / ). This change occurs in the Akwesasne dialect
Mo. / oˈɹîːwaɁ / 'business' > AkMo. / oˈɭîːwaɁ / ). This change occurs in the
Mo. / waˈkjoɁteɁ / 'I work' > CaMo. / waˈtjoɁteɁ / ). This change occurs in the
Caughnawaga, Oka, and Wahta dialects (cf. Mithun 1979:178, Bonvillain 1984:315).
precedes ( e.g. Mo. / ˈikhswʌ̃hs / 'I hate it' > CaMo. / ˈikhsũhs /; Mo. / ũhˈwʌ̃tsjaɁ /
'earth' > CaMo. / oˈhũtsaɁ /; Mo. / ohˈswʌ̃ː̂ kaɹeɁ / 'board' > CaMo. / ohˈsũ̂ːkaɭeɁ / ).
This change occurs in the Akwesasne, Caughnawaga, Oka, and Wahta dialects.
By MO-D6, * / j / has been lost after * / ts / ( e.g. Mo. / ˈkʌ̃tsjũɁ / 'fish' >
219
CaMo. / ˈkʌ̃tsũɁ /; Mo. / oˈtsîːtsjaɁ / 'flower' > CaMo. / oˈtsîːtsaɁ /; Mo. / kaˈnâːtsjũɁ /
'kettle' > CaMo. / kaˈnâːtsũɁ / ). This change occurs in the Akwesasne, Caughnawaga,
Oka, and Wahta dialects (cf. Mithun 1979:178, Bonvillain 1984:316, Mithun 1985:506).
> TyMo. / oˈtsiːseɹaɁ /; Mo. / ˈkʷateɹe / 'grandchild!' > / ˈkʷaːteɹe /; Mo. / joˈjaneɹeɁ /
'it is good' > TyMo. / joˈjaːneɹeɁ /; Mo. / kewʌ̃ˈninekʌ̃Ɂs / 'I speak out' >
and may reflect the retention of an older, dialectal sound change whereby vowels in
( i.e. before / e / was inserted into these clusters by MO-3, MO-4, MO-9, and MO-10 ).
(3).
220
(3) Phonological Changes in Mohawk Dialects
In addition to the phonemic changes in (3), certain phonetic changes have occurred
CaMo. [ ˈzadʒjʌ̃ ]; Mo. / ˈthjʌ̃ː̂ Ɂa / 'your son' Æ CaMo. [ tʃhjʌ̃ː̂ Ɂɐ ] ). In the Six Nations
'foot' Æ SNMo [ ʊhˈʃîːdɐɁ ] 'foot' ). In the Akwesasne, Caughnawaga, Oka, and Wahta
AkMo. [ ʊɁˈfahsɐɁ ] ).
221
11.4 MORPHOLOGICAL CHANGES
same as that of PMO, though the partitive and coincident prefixes ( both belonging to
position class 1 ) are attested together in Old Mohawk in at least one instance as
ok8ari kennasate8a> 'il a tué deux ours d’égale grandeur' (Bruyas 1863:21). The initial
( cf. OMo. <Sate8a> 'aussi grand l’un que l’autre' (Bruyas 1863:21), modern
Mo. / shaɁˈteːwaɁ / 'they are the same size' ). An overt factual prefix allomorph / ũ /
occurred word-initially before the cislocative and repetitive prefixes in at least some
dialects of Old Mohawk, cf. / ũtahaˈjâːtʌ̃ɁneɁ / <Ontahajatenne> 'il est tombé d’en
il est guéri' (Bruyas 1863:75); curiously most dialects of Huron had an allomorph / õ /
that occurred in the same environment, while Modern Mohawk has an allomorph / Ø /,
in keeping with PNI and PMO. Also in common with dialectal Huron, at least some
222
d’en haut' (Bruyas 1863:110); / eˈtjôːkaɹaɁs / <Etiogaras> 'il fait nuit'
(Bruyas 1863:21). Modern Mohawk has / t / in this environment, again in keeping with
Mohawk relative to PMO. The first of these is the change of the 3PLM:3 allomorphs
* / ɹʌ̃n / and * / hʌ̃n / ⁰⁶⁰ to / ɹũn / and / hũn / respectively, likely by analogy with
the 3PL:3 allomorph / kũn /, which occurs in the same environment. Second is the
{ hetshiteni } '1+2DU:3M' ⁰³⁷, and { hetshitewa } '1+2PL:3M' ⁰³⁹. Change of the vowel
prefixes { seni } '2DU&3', { sewa } '2PL&3', { teni } '1+2DU:3', and { tewa } '1+2PL:3',
which constitute four of the five pronominal prefixes normally preceded by cislocative
and repetitive prefix allomorphs / ti / ⁰¹⁰ and / tsi / ⁰¹¹ rather than / te / and / se /.
In the Caughnawaga dialect of Mohawk the initial element / he / has been lost in
223
and Wahta dialects, imperative pronominal prefix allomorphs are now regularly used
even in non-imperative verbs, e.g. Mo. / skwaˈjʌ̃ː̂ Ɂa / 'you are our father' Æ
OkMo. / takwaˈjʌ̃ː̂ Ɂa / (Cuoq 1873:15); Mo. / ˈsiːtʌ̃hɹeɁ / 'you (singular) feel pity' Æ
Old Mohawk prefix allomorphs appear to have been unchanged relative to PMO
( see 10.4 ). Consonants and clusters before which the 1SG:3 pronominal prefix
/k/ _hV, hjV ( < PMO * hjV ), hnV, hnjV, hɹV, hɹjV, hsV, hsCV, htsV, hwV,
jV ( < PNI * jV ), kV, kʷV, ɹV, tV ( < PNI * tV ), tsV.
/ ke / _hj ( < PMO * wjh ), jVɁ ( < PNI * jɁ ), tVɁ ( < PNI * tɁ ),
and all remaining consonants and clusters.
The imperfective suffix / hsk / ¹⁰⁹, still present in Old Mohawk (cf. Bruyas 1863),
has been reduced to / hs / in Common Mohawk ( e.g. PMO * / ˈkũːnihsk / 'I make it' ²⁶⁰
Æ Mo. / ˈkũːnihs /; PMO / kkaˈɹhathohsk / 'I turn it over' ³²⁷ Æ Mo. / kkaˈɹhathohs / ).
An aspectual suffix / eɁ / has been added to virtually all perfect and punctual verb
224
'she would light a fire' ¹⁶⁵ Æ Mo. / aːjũˈteːkaɁteɁ /; PMO * / ʌ̃ˈjoːtek /
'it will be burning' ¹⁶⁴ Æ Mo. / ʌ̃ˈjoːtekeɁ /; PMO * / kaˈnũhsot / 'there is a house' ³⁸² ³⁹⁷
Æ Mo. / kaˈnũhsoteɁ / ).
225
CHAPTER 12: ONEIDA
12.1 INTRODUCTION
the standing stone' ), whose territory in the early seventeenth century appears to have
ranged from Oneida Lake in the west, bordering on the territory of the Onondaga, to
Trenton Falls in the east, bordering on the territory of the Mohawk, north to the Black
River, and south into what is now northern Pennsylvania (cf. Engelbrecht 2003:122,
village locations in Swanton 1968:35; see Map 6). Today speakers of Oneida are located
primarily on the Oneida Nation of the Thames Reserve in southwestern Ontario and on
The Oneida were in contact with the Jesuits and other Europeans beginning in the
mid-seventeenth century, but the names of Oneida chiefs and settlements are invariably
given in Huron or Mohawk in early sources; the earliest missionary publications written
for the Oneida, which date from 1810, are actually written in Mohawk, owing to the
fact that the Anglican missionary who wrote them, Eleazar Williams (1787-1858), was a
A Collection of Hymns in the Oneida Language (Sickles 1855), written by the Wesleyan
missionary Abraham W. Sickles (1810-1884), an Oneida from New York State who had
226
relocated to the Thames community in Upper Canada in 1840.
Because virtually all of the 28 sound changes that separate Oneida from PMO
involve accent, glottal consonants, or devoiced vowels ( which are not rendered
PMO * / ɹ / to Oneida / l / does not appear to have been complete until relatively
recently, as documents from Ontario and Wisconsin continue to show both <r> and
<l> into the late nineteenth century. The modern Ontario and Wisconsin dialects of
Oneida are differentiated to some extent by vocabulary, and by a single sound change
Oneida texts are in Michelson (1981b), and grammars are in Lounsbury (1953) and
Abbott (2000). Dictionaries are in Abbott (1996) and Michelson and Doxtator (2002).
Unless otherwise stated, Oneida Forms in this chapter are based on data from Michelson
227
(1) Phonemic Inventory of Oneida
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant j w
Lateral Approximant l
VOWELS
Close iː ũː
i ũ
Mid eː ʌ̃ː oː
e ʌ̃ o
Open aː
a
228
other than / j /. The affricate / ts / is realized as [ dʒ ] before / i / and / j / and as
unstressed syllables.
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PMO to
Common Oneida are described below as OE-1 through OE-28. Unique to Oneida are
twelve phonological changes that have applied to words only in pre-pausal position,
and which are characterized primarily by the devoicing of one or more segments at the
detail by Michelson (1988:80-89), and at least some of the changes are lexically
conditioned; twelve pre-pausal (PP) sound changes which can account for remaining
forms in Michelson & Doxtator (2002) are presented as OE-17 through OE-28 below.
By OE-1, accent has shifted from a long vowel without falling tone to the vowel of
the following syllable ( e.g. PMO * / othahˈjũːni / 'wolf' ¹⁶⁹ > Oe. / othahjũːˈni /;
229
PMO * / aɁˈnoːwaɹ / 'turtle' ²⁰³ > Oe. / aɁnoːˈwal /; PMO * / ohˈneːkaɁ /
'water, liquid' ³⁶⁴ > Oe. / ohneːˈkâː / ). This change must precede OE-2, OE-3, OE-12,
OE-13, OE-14, and OE-16. (cf. also Chafe 1977a:172, Michelson 1988:68).
lengthened and acquired falling tone ( e.g. PMO * / oˈkaːɹaɁ / 'story' ³²⁵ >
Oe. / okaːˈlâː /; PMO * / oˈɹaːtaɁ / 'heel' ⁴¹⁹ > Oe. / olaːˈtâː /; PMO * / ohˈneːkaɁ /
'water, liquid' ³⁶⁴ > Oe. / ohneːˈkâː / ). This change must follow OE-1, and precede
By OE-3, * / Ɂ / has been lost after an accented long vowel ( e.g. PMO * / ˈôːɁaɹeɁ /
'net' ¹⁹⁸ > Oe. / ˈôːaleɁ /; PMO * / johˈtũ̂ːɁũ / 'it has vanished' ¹⁴⁵ > Oe. / johˈtũ̂ːũ /;
PMO * / waˈknîːɁũ / 'I am stingy' ³⁷¹ > Oe. / waˈknîːũ / ). This change must follow OE-1
PMO * / jũtnkʷʌ̃htaˈɹaɹhohs / 'she coats herself with red' ³⁷⁷ ⁴²⁴ >
230
environments of the phonetic vowel * [ ɪ ] that was apparently used to break up
By OE-5, * / ths / has become * / tsh / ( e.g. PMO * / aˈthsinhaɁ / 'garter' ¹⁷² >
Oe. / aˈtshinhaɁ /; PMO * / kathsaˈɁahthaɁ / 'I burn it' ¹⁷³ > Oe. / katshaˈɁathaɁ / ).
( e.g. PMO * / johˈsnoːɹeɁ / 'it is fast' ²⁵⁸ > Oe. / joshnoːˈlêː /; PMO * / ohˈsnũhsaɁ /
'hand' ²⁵⁷ > Oe. / oˈshnũhsaɁ /; PMO * / ˈohswaɁ / 'back' ²⁶⁴ > Oe. / ˈoshwaɁ / ).
By OE-7, * / h / has been lost when it occurred before another consonant plus
* / h / ( e.g. PMO * / kahˈsehthaɁ / 'I hide it' ¹⁴⁶ > Oe. / kahˈsethaɁ /;
PMO * / teˈwahshʌ̃ / 'twenty' ¹³⁵ > Oe. / teˈwashʌ̃ / ) (cf. also Michelson 1988:18), and
* / ts / ( e.g. PMO * / ˈikhsaɁas / 'I finish it' ²⁶⁶ > Oe. / ˈiksaɁas /; PMO * / aˈkhtsîːɁa /
or / aˈkhtsiha / 'my older sister' ²⁶⁷ > Oe. / aˈktsiha / ). (cf. also Michelson 1988:19).
By OE-9, * / h / has been lost when it occurred before the clusters * / sk / and
* / st / ( e.g. PMO * / ˈwihsk / 'five' ²⁷³ > Oe. / ˈwisk /; PMO * / teˈkjehsthaɁ /
231
'I mix it' ³⁰⁰ > Oe. / teˈkjestaɁ /; PMO * / ˈknʌ̃hskʷahs / 'I steal it' ³⁶⁸ >
Oe. / ˈknʌ̃skʷas / ), and by OE-10, * / h / has been lost when it occurred after * / st / or
* / tst / ( e.g. PMO * / katjenaˈwahsthaɁ / 'I keep it' ¹⁷⁷ > Oe. / katjenaˈwastaɁ /;
though note that this change has not occurred in pre-pausal forms of words.
resonant consonant ( e.g. PMO * / ˈkêːjahɹeɁ / 'I remember' ²¹⁰ > Oe. / ˈkêːjaleɁ /;
PMO * / waɁˈkahsohweɁ / 'I painted it' ²⁶⁸ > Oe. / waˈkahsoweɁ / ). Similarly, by
OE-13, * / Ɂ / has been lost in post-tonic syllables when it occurred before a resonant
consonant ( e.g. PMO * / waˈkêːskoɁneɁ / 'I'm drowning' ⁴⁹⁷ > Oe. / waˈkêːskoneɁ /;
PMO * / waɁˈkêːskoɁweɁ / 'I fell in' ⁴⁹⁷ > Oe. / waɁˈkêːskoweɁ / ). Both changes must
follow OE-1, and OE-13 must precede OE-14. (cf. also Michelson 1988:76).
232
a consonant ( e.g. PMO * / ˈwâːsʌ̃Ɂs / 'it fell' ²²¹ > Oe. / ˈwâːsʌ̃hseɁ /; PMO * / ˈʌ̃kjaɁk /
'I will cut it' ²⁹⁷ > Oe / ˈʌ̃kjahkeɁ /; PMO * / waɁkhejaˈtetsjʌ̃Ɂt / 'I healed her' ¹⁶⁷ >
Oe. / aɁnoːˈwal /; PMO * / oˈkaːɹaɁ / 'story' ³²⁵ > Oe. / okaːˈlâː /; PMO * / oˈɹaːtaɁ /
By OE-16, accented long vowels with falling tone have been shortened and have lost
( e.g. PMO * / waɁˈkjaːkʌ̃ɁneɁ / 'I went out' ⁰⁰⁰ > / waɁkjaːˈkʌ̃ː̂ neɁ / >
Oe. / waɁkjaːˈkʌ̃neɁ /; PMO * / ũˈkiːtaɁw / 'I fell asleep' ⁰⁰⁰ > / ũkiːˈtâːweɁ / >
Oe. / ũkiːˈtaweɁ / ). This change must follow OE-1, OE-2, and OE-3. (cf. also Michelson
1988:79-80).
233
other' Æ PP / tehũtaˈtkâːnel ̥e̥Ɂ ̥ /; / tehsahˈsũtlə̃ / 'plug it in!' Æ PP / tehsahˈsutelə̥̃ / ).
Both changes must precede OE-21, OE-23, and OE-24. (cf. also Michelson 1988:85).
* / o / before * / w / ( e.g. / ˈitweɁ / 'we are going' Æ PP / ˈitow̥ e̥Ɂ ̥ / ), and by OE-20,
subsequently / waˈtaliɁ ̥ / by OE-26 ). Both changes must precede OE-21, OE-23, and
devoiced all segments from the falling tone of the long vowel to the end of the word
( e.g. / skôːna / 'go get it!' Æ PP / skô̥ːn̥ḁ /; / shneˈkîːla / 'drink!' Æ PP / shneˈkiːl̥̂ ̥ḁ /;
PP / ʌ̃wakiˈtâː̥ w̥ h̥aɁ̥ ̥ / ). This change must follow OE-17, OE-18, OE-19, and OE-20, and
By OE-22, pre-pausal word forms ending in a sequence * / CV̂ː / have devoiced all
234
segments from the consonant to the end of the word ( e.g. / johjoɁthiːˈjêː / 'it is sharp'
By OE-23, pre-pausal word forms ending in a sequence * / CVɁ / have devoiced all
segments from the consonant to the end of the word ( e.g. / joˈtekhaɁ / 'it burns' Æ
conditioned exceptions to OE-23 include the particles / ˈkiɁ / 'though' and / ˈjahtʌ̃Ɂ /
'not'. This change must follow OE-17, OE-18, OE-19, OE-20, and OE-21.
By OE-24, pre-pausal word forms ending in a resonant consonant plus a short vowel
have devoiced the final vowel ( e.g. / akaːˈjũ / 'it is old' Æ PP / akaːˈjũ̥ /; / jothahiːˈnũ /
'the road goes' Æ PP / jothahiːˈnũ̥ /; / jũkjatatʌ̃ːˈlʌ̃ / 'we have some left over' Æ
PP / jũkjatatʌ̃ːˈlʌ̥̃ / ). This change must follow OE-17, OE-18, OE-19, OE-20, and OE-21,
vowel have devoiced both the consonant and the vowel ( e.g. / saˈtketsko / 'get up!' Æ
235
'put the bread in the oven!' Æ PP / satnaɁtalũːˈt ̥ʌ̥̃ / ). This change must follow OE-21
/ waˈtaliɁ ̥ / ). This change must follow OE-20, OE-21, OE-23, OE-24, and OE-25.
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PMO to Oneida
236
(2) Phonological Changes in the Transition from PMO to Oneida
237
automatically been shortened before a syllable containing an accented short vowel
( e.g. Oe. / akʷeːˈkũ / > Ontario Oe. / akʷeˈkũ / 'all'; Oe. / kwaːˈnʌ̃ / >
Ontario Oe. / kwaˈnʌ̃ / 'it is big'; Oe. / wakatolaːˈtũ / > Ontario Oe. / wakatolaˈtũ /
'I have hunted' ). The effect of OE-D1 has been that it eliminates the occurrence of long
vowels in words whenever the accented vowel itself is not long. OE-D1 does not apply
to pre-pausal forms.
inventory of Oneida relative to PMO. First, the 2:1PL prefix allomorph * / kwʌ̃ / ⁰¹⁵ has
added a syllable / ja / to yield / jakwʌ̃ / in Oneida; the analogical source of the added
syllable is unclear and it does not appear in other allomorphs of the prefix. The PMO
'3:3I' ⁰⁶⁴, and * { shako } '3SGM:3I' ⁰⁶⁵ appear to have developed allomorphs / joj /,
/ hoj /, / jakoj /, and / shakoj /, which occur before bases in / o / and / ũ / when both
238
the agent and the patient of a verb are animate ( e.g. / waɁoːˈjũ̂ː / 'she gave it to her',
Reflexes of PMO pronominal prefixes that begin with * / h / or * / j / have lost this
consonant word-initially in Oneida ( cf. PMO * / jesa / '3I:2SG' ⁰²⁹ Æ Oe. / esa /
Onondaga, Cayuga, Seneca, Huron, and Wyandot, and has likely spread from one or
The PMO reflexive prefix allomorph * / atʌ̃n / ⁰⁷⁶ and semireflexive prefix allomorph
Oneida prefix allomorphs seem to be essentially unchanged from PMO apart from the
effects of regular sound change, although bases beginning in / sC / ( < PMO * / hsC / )
have apparently been added (cf. Michelson 1988:144). Consonants and clusters before
which the 1SG:3 pronominal prefix allomorphs / k / and / ke / occur in Oneida are
depicted in (3).
239
(3) Distribution of Prefix Allomorphs with Epenthetic / e / in Oneida (1SG:3)
/k/ _hV, hjV, hlV, hljV, hnV, hnjV, hsV¹ ( Æ sV¹ ), htsV¹ ( Æ tsV¹ ),
hwV, jV ( < PNI * jV ), kV, kʷV, lV ( < PNI * ɹV ), nV,
tV ( < PNI * tV ), tsV, wV.
No distinction is made in Oneida between present and imperfective bases and the
language instead has habitual bases. The PMO present suffix * / hs / ¹⁰⁵ and
consonant and have become * / heɁ / elsewhere ( e.g. PMO * / ˈkhɹeːnahs / 'I cut it' ²⁹⁷
Æ Oe. / khleːˈnas /; PMO * / kkaˈɹhathohsk / 'I turn it over' ³²⁷ Æ Oe. / kkaˈlhathos /
PMO * / kaˈtaːtihsk / 'I speak' ¹⁶⁰ Æ Oe. / kataːˈtiheɁ /; PMO * / ˈkatjʌ̃hsk / 'I sit' ³⁰⁶ Æ
non-imperative verb forms ending in a non-glottal consonant, including forms with final
'she would light a fire' ¹⁶⁵ Æ Oe. / ajũteːˈkâːteɁ /; PMO * / ʌ̃ˈjoːtek / 'it will be
burning' ¹⁶⁴ Æ Oe. / ʌ̃joːˈtekeɁ /; PMO * / waˈkjʌ̃ːtaɁs / 'I obtain it' ³⁰⁹ Æ
240
The augmentative suffix of Oneida has the form { -kôː } rather than { -koːwaɁ }, the
decessive suffix has the form { -kʌ̃ }, and the intensifier suffix has a form { -tsiɁ } rather
than { -tsi }.
241
CHAPTER 13: ONONDAGA
13.1 INTRODUCTION
of the hill' ), whose territory in the early seventeenth century appears to have ranged
from Skaneateles Lake in the west, bordering on the territory of the Cayuga, to Oneida
Lake in the east, bordering on the territory of the Oneida, north to Lake Ontario, and
perhaps south to the Chenango Forks (cf. Engelbrecht 2003:121, village locations in
Swanton 1968:35; see Map 6). Today speakers of Onondaga are located on the Six
Nations of the Grand River Reserve in southwestern Ontario and on the Onondaga
century Jesuit dictionary, reproduced in Shea (1860). The Shea dictionary contains an
242
published in 1887, and a grammar by Zeisberger, published in 1888. The Zeisberger
material contains a stage of the language here referred to as Old Onondaga, in which
the phoneme / ɹ / is still present, but the earlier contrast between / ʃ / and / s / has
been lost, both reflexes having merged as a single phoneme / s /. Modern Onondaga,
which dates from the late eighteenth or early nineteenth century, is in turn
Two dialects of Modern Onondaga exist, one at Six Nations of the Grand River
Reserve in Ontario and the other at Onondaga Nation Reservation in New York State.
The Ontario and New York dialects differ mainly with regard to vocabulary
(cf. Woodbury 2003:1) rather than general phonology; forms presented in this chapter
reflect Common Modern Onondaga, a reconstructed form of the language that predates
243
13.2 PHONEMIC INVENTORY
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant j w
VOWELS
Close iː ũː
i ũ
Mid eː ẽː oː
e ẽ o
Open æː aː
æ a
The phonemic inventory of Archaic Onondaga appears to have differed from that of
Modern Onondaga in having the additional phonemes / ʃ / and / ɹ /, and lacking the
244
phonemes / æː / and / æ /. Jesuit orthography suggests that the phoneme / ts /
contrast of Archaic Onondaga but retained the phoneme / ɹ /; Modern Onondaga has
lost / ɹ /, and the phonemes / æː / and / æ / have developed from other vowels as a
result of r-loss.
respectively in Modern Onondaga before vowels and resonant consonants. The affricate
Forms in Archaic and Old Onondaga suggest that the language formerly employed
up / Cr / clusters, cf. PNI * / ˈtekɹõɁ / 'eight' ⁴³⁹, AOo. <tegueron> (Shea 1860:6),
'nine' ²²², AOo. <8aderon> (Shea 1860:6), OOo. <Watiro> (Zeisberger 1888:8),
245
the presence of the epenthetic vowel likely accounts for phonemic vowel length in
resultant reflexes, via a developmental sequence of the type * [ dɹẽ ] > [ dɪɹẽ ] >
[ dɪẽ ] > [ dẽː ], * [ sɹi ] > [ sɪɹi ] > [ sɪi ] > [ siː ], (cf. OO-26 below). The former
presence of an epenthetic phonetic vowel between consonants and resonants may also
account for the fact that a sequence / CRV / at the beginning of a word counts as two
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to Modern
Onondaga are described below as OO-1 through OO-32. Where ordering of any two
changes appears necessary in order to produce a correct output in Onondaga, this has
been noted.
part of the unit affricate * / ts /. This change cannot be illustrated in the modern
language due to subsequent merger of the phoneme / ʃ / with / s /, but was present in
Archaic Onondaga ( cf. PNI * / ˈahsẽh / 'three' ¹⁴⁰ > AOo. / ˈahʃẽh / <achen>
(Shea 1860:7); PNI * / ˈsɹõtjaɁks / 'you fell a tree' ²⁹⁷ ⁴³¹ > AOo. / ˈʃɹõtjaɁkʃ /
246
<chrontiakch> (Shea 1860:10); PNI * / ˈsnẽhskʷahs / 'you steal it' ³⁶⁸ >
'he lifts it' ³³¹ > Oo. / háˈketskʷas /, rather than ˣ / haˈkeʃkʷahʃ / and eventually
ˣ / háˈkeskʷas / by OO-21; PNI * / otsaˈheɁtaɁ / 'beans' ⁴⁵¹ > Oo. / ohsáˈheɁtaɁ /, rather
( cf. PNI * / ˈatsõh / 'not yet, still' ¹⁴⁸ > Oo. / ˈáhsũh /; PNI * / ˈatseːɁ / 'new' ¹⁸⁹ >
Oo. / áhˈseɁ /; PNI * / otsaˈheɁtaɁ / 'beans' ⁴⁵¹ > Oo. / ohsáˈheɁtaɁ /; but cf.
PNI * / otsiˈkheɁtaɁ / 'salt' ⁴⁵⁶ > Oo. / otsíˈkheɁtaɁ / ). This change was already present
in Archaic Onondaga, cf. <àson> 'pas encore' (Shea 1860:8); it must follow OO-1 and
PNI * / ɹaˈjẽtsɹõhs / 'he skins it' ³¹² > Oo. / háˈjẽshæːs /; PNI * / ɹoɁˈtskoɁõh /
'he has drowned' ⁴⁹⁷ > Oo. / hóɁˈskoɁih /; but cf. PNI / ohˈnitshaɁ / 'thigh' ²³⁸ >
PNI * / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ > Oo. / ˈtsjáːtak / ). This change seems already to have
247
been present in Archaic Onondaga, cf. / skeɁˈnjaːkẽɁs / <skeniaguens> 'je me sauve'
(Shea 1860:93), from PNI * / tskeɁˈnjaːkẽɁts / ⁴⁹³ ; it must follow OO-1 and OO-2.
( e.g. PNI * / ɹẽniˈnskɹõtjeɁts / 'he spits' ¹⁷⁴ ²⁸⁴ > Oo. / hẽníˈtskẽːtjeɁs /;
PNI * / ɹaˈkenskʷahs / 'he lifts it' ³³¹ > Oo. / háˈketskʷas /; PNI * / -nskõːh /
'facilitative' ¹²¹ > Oo. / -tskũh / ). This change seems already to have been present in
By OO-5, * / n / has been lost before * / s / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈansteh / 'outside' ¹⁵⁵ >
Oo. / ˈásteh /; PNI * / ˈwahnshẽːh / 'ten' ¹³⁵ > Oo. / wáˈshẽh /; PNI * / joɁˈnshansteɁ /
'it is strong' ⁴⁹⁶ > Oo. / jóɁˈshatsteɁ / ). This change was already present in Archaic
Onondaga, cf. <asté> 'Dehors' (Shea 1860:41). It must follow OO-1 and OO-4.
By OO-6, word accent has shifted from the penult to a long vowel in the final
( e.g. PNI * / ˈwahnshẽːh / 'ten' ¹³⁵ > Oo. / wáˈshẽh /; PNI * / ohaˈhiːjoh /
'good road' ¹³¹ ²⁷⁹ > Oo. / ohahíˈjoh /; PNI * / jõˈkjõːniːh / 'we have made it' ²⁶⁰ >
Oo. / ũkjṹːˈnih / ). There are some exceptions to this change that remain unexplained
248
( e.g. PNI * / ɹaˈtoːɹaːths / 'he hunts' ¹⁸⁴ > Oo. / háˈtoːwæts /; PNI * / ˈkaɹjoːɁ /
'animal' ⁰⁰⁰ > Oo. / ˈkáːjoɁ / ). This change must precede OO-7 and OO-19, and would
thus appear already to have been present in Archaic Onondaga; its presence in Old
( e.g. PNI * / oˈnõtaɹaɁ / 'corn soup' ³⁸⁶ > Oo. / óˈnũːtaːɁ /; PNI * / oˈtsiɁnkʷaɹaɁ /
'green, yellow, bile' ⁴⁶¹ > Oo. / óˈtsiɁtkʷaːɁ /; PNI * / oˈnaɁkaɹaɁ / 'horn, antler' ³⁵⁹ >
Oo. / óˈnaɁkaːɁ /; but cf. PNI * / ohˈkaːɹaɁ / 'wood chips' ¹³⁴ > Oo. / óhˈkaːæɁ / ). This
(Shea 1860:95). It must follow OO-6, and precede OO-11 and OO-29.
249
'I put someone out' ²⁸⁰ ²⁹⁸ > Oo. / khejaɁˈtitkẽk /; but cf. PNI * / ˈheɁnkẽh / 'above' ²¹⁴
> AOo. / ˈheɁnkẽh / <henkuen> (Shea 1860:8) ); this change was already present in
Archaic Onondaga, cf. / oˈtkʷẽhsaɁ / <hotk8ensa> 'Sang' (Shea 1860:92), and must
when * / Ɂ / preceded ( e.g. PNI * / ˈheɁnkẽh / 'above' ²¹⁴ > Oo. / ˈhéɁtkẽh /;
PNI * / oˈtsiɁnkʷaɹaɁ / 'green, yellow, bile' ⁴⁶¹ > Oo. / óˈtsitkʷaːɁ / ). This change was
(Shea 1860:8) and / oˈtsiɁnkʷaːɁ / <hotging8aa> 'Verd' (Shea 1860:101), but it was
( e.g. PNI * / ˈwẽtateɁ / 'there is a day' ²¹⁹ ⁴³³ > Oo. / wẽˈ́ taːteɁ / 'day';
́ ˈjaːteɁ / 'sky';
PNI * / kaˈɹõhjateɁ / 'there is a sky' ⁴³⁰ ⁴³³ > Oo. / kaẽh
PNI * / ɹoˈnahskʷajẽɁ / 'he has a slave' ³⁰⁶ ³⁵³ > Oo. / honáˈskʷaːjẽɁ / ). This change
may or may not have been present in Archaic Onondaga, but is clearly present in Old
Onondaga, cf. PNI * / kaˈnjataɹeɁ / 'there is a lake' ³⁷⁴ ⁴¹³ > OOo. / kanjaˈtaːɹeɁ /
<Ganiatáre> 'the Sea' (Zeisberger 1888:4); PNI * / ˈjohateɁ / 'there is a path' ¹³¹ ⁴³³ >
OOo. / joˈhaːteɁ / <Joháte> 'the Way' (Zeisberger 1888:4); PNI * / kaˈnatajẽɁ / 'there
250
is a village' ³⁰⁶ ³⁵⁶ Æ OOo. / kanaˈtaːjẽɁ / <ganatáje> 'a Town' (Zeisberger 1888:4). It
( cf. AOo. / ˈahʃẽh / 'three' ¹⁴⁰ > Oo. / ˈahsẽh /; AOo. / ˈʃɹõtjaɁkʃ / 'you fell a tree' ²⁹⁷ ⁴³¹
> Oo. / ˈsẽː́ tjaɁks /; AOo. / ˈʃnẽhʃkʷahʃ / 'you steal it' ³⁶⁸ > Oo. / ˈsnẽskʷas / ). This
change appears to have been present in Old Onondaga, cf. <àchso> 'three'
'he has tied it' ²⁴⁰ > Oo. / hóˈhẽːh /; PNI * / ˈtshnɹẽk / 'tie it!' ²⁴⁰ > Oo. / ˈshẽː́ kah / ).
This change does not appear to have been present in Archaic Onondaga,
cf. / ehˈnɹẽhʃthaɁ / <henerinchta> 'Lien' (Shea 1860:67), but was present in Old
Onondaga, cf. / ˈshɹẽːkah / <srœ̃ nge> 'tie it' (Zeisberger 1887:198). OO-13 must
precede OO-26.
( e.g. PNI * / oˈwhjõhkaɹaɁ / 'thumb' ⁴⁷⁷ > Oo. / óːˈjũhkaːɁ / ). This change does not
(Shea 1860:82), but does appear to have been present in Old Onondaga,
251
cf. <ojúnchkara> 'Thumb' with <oj-> rather than <ochj-> (Zeisberger 1887:198).
lost ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈwjẽːnaɁ / 'manner' ⁴⁸⁴ > Oo. / káːˈjẽːnaɁ /; PNI * / hoˈwjẽhõh /
'he has learned how' ⁴⁸³ Æ Oo. / hóːˈjẽɁih / ). This change must follow OO-14.
By OO-16, accented vowels have been lengthened when they preceded either a
( e.g. PNI * / ˈknakɹeɁ / 'I reside' ³⁵⁴ > Oo. / ˈknáːkeːɁ /; PNI * / kheˈjatɹeɁ /
'my granddaughter' ¹⁸⁸ > Oo. / khéˈjaːteːɁ /; PNI * / waˈkatjõh / 'I've thrown it out' ¹⁷⁴
> Oo. / áˈkaːtjũh / ). This change may or may not have been present in Archaic and
Old Onondaga; it must follow OO-11 and precede OO-26. (cf. also Michelson 1988:93).
'my name' ²⁴⁷ > Oo. / áˈkshẽːnaɁ /; PNI * / ohˈswẽɁkaɹaɁ / 'board' ²⁶⁵ >
Oo. / óˈshwẽɁkaːɁ / ), and similarly, by OO-18, * / hts / has become * / tsh / after a
consonant and before a vowel ( e.g. PNI * / aˈkhtsiɁaːh / 'my older sibling' ²⁶⁷ >
Oo. / aktshíˈɁah / ). Both changes, which must follow OO-2, may or may not have been
present in Archaic or Old Onondaga; if OO-17 was present in Archaic Onondaga then it
252
By OO-19, vowels in even-numbered syllables have been lengthened before a single
non-glottal consonant or a single non-glottal consonant plus resonant if the vowel of the
following syllable was accented and long or accented and followed by a glottal
consonant ( e.g. PNI * / onõˈtaɁkeh / 'on the hill' ³⁸⁵ > Oo. / onṹːˈtaɁkeh /;
PNI * / jekoˈwaːnẽh / 'she is big' ³⁴⁹ > Oo. / ekóːˈwaːnẽh /; PNI * / kaˈnatajẽɁ /
'there is a town' ³⁰⁶ ³⁵⁶ > Oo. / kanáːˈtaːjẽɁ / 'it is a town' ). The examples
Oo. / ũnṹːˈnjaːjẽh / 'it is frozen' vs. / ẽkanṹˈnjaːjẽɁ / 'it will freeze', and / akjṹːˈtjahaɁ /
'I'm laughing' vs. / ũkwajṹˈtjahaɁ / 'we are laughing' from Michelson (1988:94) illustrate
odd-numbered syllables. Note that the joiner vowel * / a / does not appear to have been
subject to this pre-tonic lengthening (Chafe 1977a:174, Michelson 1988:95), and that a
sequence * / CRV / at the beginning of a word seems to have counted as two syllables
This change may or may not have been present in Archaic and Old Onondaga; it must
By OO-20, the vowel in the second syllable of a word of five or more syllables has
resonant ( e.g. PNI * / tewakɹihˈwahkʷẽh / 'I have sung' ³⁵⁰ ⁴²⁷ >
253
Oo. / tewaːkíːhˈwahkʷẽh /; cf. Mithun 1979:175, Michelson 1988:97, Woodbury
2003:62 ). This change, which may or may not have been present in Archaic and Old
By OO-21, high pitch has separated from accent and moved to the vowel of the
preceding syllable; if the vowel of the preceding syllable was the joiner vowel * / a /,
high pitch instead occurred on the vowel of the syllable preceding the joiner vowel
PNI * / kaˈtsistajẽɁ / 'there is a fire' ³⁰⁶ ⁴⁵⁹ > Oo. / katsíˈstaːjẽɁ / 'there is a fireplace';
PNI * / waˈhetkẽɁ / 'it is bad' ¹³² > Oo. / wáˈhetkẽɁ /; cf. also Michelson 1988:92 and
Woodbury 2003:60 ). This change may or may not have been present in Archaic and
( e.g. PNI * / ˈkɹakwahs / 'I choose' ⁴¹⁷ > Oo. / ˈkǽːkwas /; PNI * / ɹaˈɹaɁthẽhs /
'he climbs' ⁴²⁰ > Oo. / haǽɁˈthẽs /; PNI * / ˈjowɹateɁ / 'there is a wind' ⁴³³ ⁴⁸⁷ >
Oo. / óˈwæːteɁ / 'it is windy' ). This change had not yet occurred in Old Onondaga,
where [ æ(ː) ], if present, was still an allophone of / a(ː) / after / ɹ /, and did not apply
254
Onondaga, cf. AOo. <haratinha> '[il monte dans un chambre]' (Shea 1860:71). In a
( e.g. PNI * / kawɹoˈwaːnẽh / 'a big wind' ³⁴⁹ ⁴⁸⁷ > Oo. / kaːéːˈwaːnẽh /;
PNI * / nikakaˈɹoɁtẽːh / 'that kind of story' ³²⁵ ⁴⁰¹ > Oo. / nikakaéɁˈtẽh / ). OO-23 had
likewise not yet occurred in Old Onondaga and did not apply to reflexes of pronominal
prefixes containing a syllable * / ɹo / since these sequences had already been levelled to
/ ho / in Archaic Onondaga, cf. AOo. / ˈhoːjẽɁ / <hoyen> '[il a]' ³⁰⁶ (Shea 1860:25).
( e.g. PNI * / kaˈɹõːtaɁ / 'log, tree' ⁴³¹ > Oo. / káˈẽːtaɁ /; PNI * / oˈɹõhjaɁ / 'sky, blue' ⁴³⁰
> Oo. / óˈwẽhjaɁ /; PNI * / ɹahˈɹõkhaɁ / 'he hears, he understands' ¹³⁸ >
Oo. / háˈhẽːkhaɁ / ). This change was likewise not present in Old Onondaga,
cf. <garōnta> 'Log' (Zeisberger 1887:115). OO-22, OO-23, and OO-24 must precede
OO-26, OO-27, OO-28 and OO-29. (cf. also Mithun 1979:174, Woodbury 1981:116).
By OO-25, * / ɹ / has been lost before a consonant and a preceding vowel has been
compensatorily lengthened ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkaɹjoːɁ / 'animal' ⁴²⁹ > Oo. / ˈkáːjoɁ /;
PNI * / tẽkkaɹhaˈteːniɁ / 'I will turn it' ³²⁶ > Oo. / tẽhkaːháˈteːniɁ /;
PNI * / kajaɁˈtaɹhaɁ / 'there are beings on it' ²⁹⁸ ⁴¹³ > Oo. / kajáɁˈtaːhaɁ /
'screen, television' ). This change was not present in Old Onondaga, cf. <gárrio>
255
'Animal' (Zeisberger 1887:11); it must precede OO-29. (cf. also Woodbury 1981:112).
By OO-26, * / r / has been lost after a consonant and before a vowel, with
compensatory lengthening of the following vowel ( e.g. PNI * / ˈtekɹõɁ / 'eight' ⁴³⁹ >
Oo. / ˈtéːkẽːɁ /; PNI * / ˈwaɁtɹõh / 'nine' ²²² > Oo. / ˈwáɁtẽːh /; PNI * / joˈjanɹeɁ /
'it is good' ²⁹⁵ > Oo. / óˈjaːneːɁ /; PNI * / tẽkahˈsõtɹẽɁ / 'I will join it' ¹⁵⁰ >
Oo. / tẽkáhˈsũːtẽːɁ /; PNI * / kaɁˈtsɹehtaɁ / 'it is used to pull, drag' ⁴⁹⁸ >
involves the sequence * { -hɹæɁ- }, which sometimes yields the expected outcome
{ -hæːɁ- } ( e.g. PNI * / kaɁniˈkõhɹaɁ / 'mind' ⁴⁹¹ > Oo. / kaɁníˈkũhæːɁ / ), and
sometimes yields short { -hæɁ- } ( e.g. PNI * / ohˈɹaɁtaɁ / 'feather' ²⁴³ >
Oo. / óˈhæɁtaɁ /; PNI * / oˈkahɹaɁ / 'eye' ³¹⁹ > Oo. / óˈkahæɁ / ). OO-26 must follow
OO-13, OO-16, OO-19, OO-20, OO-22, OO-23, and OO-24, and precede OO-29 and
* / i(ː) / ( e.g. PNI * / oˈwiːɹaɁ / 'baby' ⁴⁷⁹ > Oo. / óˈwiːjæɁ /; PNI * / oˈheːɹaɁ /
'corn stalk' ²²⁴ > Oo. / óˈheːjæɁ /; PNI * / akhwaˈtsiːɹaɁ / 'my family' ²⁶⁹ >
Oo. / akhwáˈtsiːjæɁ / ), and this change was not present in Old Onondaga,
256
* / w / intervocalically after * / o / or * / õ / ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈnoːɹõɁ / 'it is valuable' ³⁹³
> Oo. / káˈnoːwẽɁ /; PNI * / johˈsnoːɹeɁ / 'it is fast' ²⁵⁸ > Oo. / óˈsnoːweɁ /;
PNI * / jakoˈthɹoːɹih / 'someone is telling it' ²⁴⁶ > Oo. / kóˈthoːwih / ). This change was
(Zeisberger 1887:215). OO-27 and OO-28 must follow OO-22, OO-23, and OO-24, and
precede OO-29. (cf. also Mithun 1979:174, Woodbury 1981:111, Michelson 1988:171).
By OO-29, * / ɹ / has been lost ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈɹõːtaɁ / 'log, tree' ⁴³¹ >
Oo. / káˈẽːtaɁ /; PNI * / kaˈɹẽːnaɁ / 'song' ⁴²² > Oo. / káˈẽːnaɁ /; PNI * / kaˈjeːɹih /
'four' ³²⁰ > Oo. / kaˈjeːih /; PNI * / kaɹõhˈjaɁkeh / 'in the sky' ⁴³⁰ >
́ ˈjaɁkeh / ). This change, which must follow OO-7, OO-22, OO-23, OO-24,
Oo. / kaẽh
OO-25, OO-26, OO-27 and OO-28, was not present in Old Onondaga, cf. <garōnta>
(cf. also Mithun 1979:174, Woodbury 1981, Chafe & Foster 1981:139).
and before the cluster * / sn / ( e.g. PNI * / wathsˈɁahthaɁ / 'it gets used up' ¹⁷³ >
Oo. / watsˈɁathaɁ /; PNI * / ˈknẽhskʷahs / 'I steal it' ³⁶⁸ > Oo. / ˈknẽś kʷas /;
PNI * / ẽkehˈsɹõːniːhk / 'I will be making it' ²⁶⁰ > Oo. / ẽkehsẽː́ ˈnik /;
257
PNI * / johˈsnoːɹeɁ / 'it is fast' ²⁵⁸ > Oo. / óˈsnoːweɁ / ). This change must follow OO-2,
OO-11, and OO-26, and by implication appears not to have been present in Archaic or
Old Onondaga.
'I have seen it' ³³² > Oo. / áhˈkẽh /; PNI * / ˈwaɁkkẽɁ / 'I saw it' ³³² >
Oo. / ˈwáɁhkẽɁ / ). This change was not present in Old Onondaga, cf. <wakgéhha>
By OO-32, * / õ(ː) / has shifted to * / ũ(ː) / ( e.g. PNI * / õhˈwẽtsjaɁ / 'earth' ⁴⁰² >
Oo. / ṹhˈwẽːtsjaɁ /; PNI * / ˈõːkʷeh / 'person' ⁴⁰⁵ > Oo. / ˈṹːkʷeh /; PNI * / ˈkõnheɁ /
'I am alive' ⁴⁰⁶ > Oo. / ˈkṹnheɁ / ). It is not clear at what stage of the language this shift
took place.
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to Modern
258
OO-8 jh > hj
OO-9 n > t / _k, kʷ ( except when a glottal stop precedes )
OO-10 n > t / _k, kʷ
OO-11 ˈV¹(C²)(C³)C⁴aC⁵V⁶ > V¹(C²)ˈ(C³)C⁴aC⁵V⁶
OO-12 ʃ>s
OO-13 n > Ø / h_ɹ
OO-14 h > Ø / w_j
OO-15 V¹w > V¹ː / _ɹ, j
OO-16 ˈV¹ > ˈV¹ː / _C(R)V
OO-17 hs > sh / C_
OO-18 hts > tsh / C_V
OO-19 pre-tonic lengthening ( see description )
OO-20 second syllable lengthening ( see description )
OO-21 high pitch separating from accent ( see description )
OO-22 a(ː) > æ(ː) / ɹ_
OO-23 o(ː) > e(ː) / ɹ_
OO-24 õ(ː) > ẽ(ː) / ɹ_
OO-25 V¹ɹ > V¹ː / _C
OO-26 ɹV¹ > V¹ː / C_
OO-27 ɹ > j / e(ː), ẽ(ː), i(ː)_V
OO-28 ɹ > w / o(ː), õ(ː)_V
OO-29 ɹ>Ø
OO-30 h > Ø / _CC, _C# ( where C is k, t, ts, or s, or where CC is sn )
OO-31 k > h / _k
OO-32 õ(ː) > ũ(ː)
Note that the changes in (2) that involve accent are realized only in Onondaga
utterance-final forms; in contrast, Onondaga utterance-medial forms bear high pitch and
accent on the final syllable, and instances of final / h / have been dropped except when
a word already has final accent ( e.g. Oo. / ˈáhsẽh / 'three', / ahˈsẽ́ niwáˈshẽh / 'thirty',
/ ahˈsẽ́ niwaˈshẽh
́ ˈáhsẽh / 'thirty three' ). (cf. also Woodbury 2003:59). This pattern of
259
accenting the final syllable in utterance-medial forms is also found in Cayuga, albeit
Cayuga and Proto-Mohawk-Oneida, the negative prefix cannot occur with modal
prefixes in Onondaga, and the contrastive prefix is substituted; the glottal stop of the
negative prefix induces loss of a following glide, as in Cayuga. The non-singular animate
prefix has been lost, with '3I' pronominal prefixes subsuming the scope of earlier non-
singular forms ( e.g. Oo. { khe- } '1SG:3I, I act on someone or them' ), as is also the case
Archaic and Old Onondaga retained * / ɹ / in allomorphs of the 3SGM:3 prefix that
occurred before bases beginning in vowels other than / a(ː) / or / i(ː) /, but all reflexes
prefixes appears to have been lost in word-initial position ( e.g. PNI * / jetshi- /
260
'2NS&3I' ⁰³⁵ Æ Oo. / etshi- / ). As in Proto-Mohawk-Oneida, the reflex of the Proto-
Northern Iroquoian 3PL:3 prefix * { kõti } ⁰⁵⁹ has replaced * { wati } ⁰⁵⁸. Onondaga has
a prefix { hũk } 'they (masculine) act on me' (3NSM:1SG) that appears to have been
created by analogy with the prefix { jũk } '3I:1SG' ⁰²⁶, which has a meaning 'they
animate prefix. As in Cayuga, Seneca, Huron, and Wyandot, the Onondaga prefixes
/ sk / '2:1.SG' ⁰¹⁶, / wak / '3:1SG' ⁰²⁵, / jũk / '3I:1SG' ⁰²⁶, / hak / '3M:1SG' ⁰²⁷, / tak /
'2:1.SG.IMP' ⁰⁷² and / hũk / '3NSM:1SG' lack allomorphs ending in / kʷ / before bases
trait shared by Seneca. The modern New York dialect of Onondaga, like Cayuga, has 2:1
'2:1.DU.IMP' ⁰⁷³, and { taskwa } '2:1.PL.IMP' ⁰⁷⁴, which have apparently been created by
Onondaga pronominal prefixes seem to have added / hnjV / and / hsCV / to the
consonants and clusters before which a reflex of epenthetic * / e / appears, but these
seem otherwise to be unaltered apart from the effects of regular sound change.
Consonants and clusters before which the 1SG:3 pronominal prefix allomorphs / h /
261
( < PNI * k ), / k /, / keː / ( < PNI * ke ), and / ke / occur in Onondaga are presented
in (3).
/k/ _hV, hjV, hnV, hsV¹ ( Æ shV¹ ), htsV¹ ( Æ tshV¹ ), hwV, jV ( < PNI * jV ),
nV, tV, tsV, wV, ɁnV, ɁwV.
As in Huron and Wyandot, the vast majority of perfect verbs in Onondaga end in a
suffix / -ih / rather than in / -ũh / or / -ẽh / ( cf. Oo. / áˈkjẽthwih / 'I have
planted it' ³¹¹ ). Imperfective verbs ending in / -was / regularly have punctual
'I will plant it' ³¹¹ ), and reflexes of PNI punctual bases that seem to have ended in
* / -Ɂn / ¹¹⁴ show / -ɁnhaɁ / in Onondaga ( e.g. PNI * / waɁˈkhehnhaɁn / 'I hired
and imperfective forms in Onondaga, and, as in Huron, the PNI present suffix
differentiate habitual forms from perfect forms ending in * / -õh / ¹¹⁵, * / -ẽh / ¹¹⁶,
262
and * / -ih / ¹¹⁹.
Like the other Five Nations languages Onondaga has a second locative suffix,
{ -neh }, a pluralizer suffix, { -shũɁ }, and an augmentative suffix, which has the shape
{ -koːnah } rather than the more usual shape { -koːwaɁ }. The intensifier suffix has the
form { -tsihwẽh } in Onondaga, and the decessive suffix has the form { -kẽhæɁ }.
263
CHAPTER 14: CAYUGA
14.1 INTRODUCTION
uncertain etymology ), whose territory in the early seventeenth century appears to have
ranged from Cayuga Lake in the west, bordering on the territory of the Seneca, to
Skaneateles Lake in the east, bordering on the territory of the Onondaga, north to Lake
Ontario, and perhaps south into what is now Pennsylvania (cf. Engelbrecht 2003:121,
village locations in Swanton 1968:34; see Map 6). Today Cayuga speakers are located
primarily on the Six Nations of the Grand River Reserve in southwestern Ontario.
The Cayuga were in contact with the Jesuits and other Europeans beginning in the
mid-seventeenth century, but the names of Cayuga chiefs and settlements are invariably
given in Huron or Mohawk in early sources, so that the language itself is of relatively
what can be discerned from this vocabulary the language seems at this point to have
already been in its modern stage. The earliest considerable record of the Cayuga
(1850-1912), as dictated by his widow Mary Anne Skye Gibson in 1914, and transcribed
264
by ethnologist Alexander Goldenweiser (Canadian Museum of Civilization, Ethnological
Documents, MCC107756-2).
Cayuga has both an Upper dialect, spoken in the southwestern part of Six Nations,
and a Lower dialect, spoken in the eastern part of Six Nations (Froman et al. 2002: xii);
a third dialect, whose youngest speaker was born in 1918 (Sturtevant 1978:543), was
(cf. Mithun 1989:243). The language described in this chapter, unless otherwise stated,
is Common Cayuga, a pre-dialectal form of the language that predates the breakup of
Upper and Lower Cayuga. Dialectal innovations that have subsequently given rise to
Notes on Oklahoma Cayuga are in Mithun (1989) and a teaching grammar based on
the Lower Cayuga dialect is in Mithun and Henry (1984). A dictionary is in Froman et
al. (2002). Cayuga forms in this chapter are based on data in Froman et al. (2002)
265
(1) Phonemic Inventory of Cayuga
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant ɹ j w
VOWELS
Close iː uː
i u
Mid eː ẽː oː õː
e ẽ o õ
Open aː
a
possibly add diminutive meaning to lexical roots ( e.g. 'small', 'narrow', 'dull',
266
'cold', etc ). They occur obligatorily in the verb / niˈwuːɁuh / 'it is small' for all speakers
( cf. forms in Mithun & Henry 1984 and Froman et al. 2002 ) and optionally elsewhere
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to Common
Cayuga are described below as CA-1 through CA-35. Where ordering of any two
noted.
'wing, fin' ⁴⁸⁰ > Ca. / oˈwaːjaɁ /; PNI * / joˈɁawjeɁ / 'there is dew' ¹⁹⁹ >
Ca. / oɁˈawajeɁ /; PNI * / ẽhsaˈtewjẽhst / 'you will learn' ⁴⁸² > Ca. / ẽhsateˈwaːjẽːs / ).
in a penultimate syllable ( e.g. PNI * / nikaˈnaɁtsjakeːh / 'that many pails, kettles' ¹⁵² ³⁶³
> Ca. / nikanaɁˈtsaːkeː /; PNI * / jotõhˈwẽtsjateɁ / 'land exists' ⁴⁰² ⁴³³ >
267
Ca. / otõhwẽˈtsaːteɁ / 'country'; PNI * / ˈjowɹanoːh / 'it is a cold wind' ³⁷⁹ ⁴⁸⁷ >
Ca. / oˈwaːnoː / ). This change must follow CA-1 and precede CA-10 and CA-11.
By CA-3, * / h / has been lost before * / nh / ( e.g. PNI * / kahˈnhohaɁ / 'door' ²³³
> Ca. / kaˈnhohaɁ /; PNI * / tjohˈnhohskʷaɹõːt / 'it has jowls protruding' ²³⁵ >
Ca. / tjoˈnhohskʷaõːt / 'cow'; PNI * / ẽhˈshehnhaɁn / 'you will hire someone' ²³² >
( e.g. PNI * / oˈtsiɁnkʷaɹaɁ / 'bile, green, yellow' ⁴⁶¹ > Ca. / oˈtsiːtkʷaːɁ /;
PNI * / ˈheɁnkẽh / 'above' ²¹⁴ > Ca. / heːtkẽh / ). This change must precede CA-5 and
CA-10.
( e.g. PNI * / oˈnkʷẽhsaɁ / 'blood' ³⁷⁶ > Ca. / oˈtkʷẽhsaɁ /; PNI * / ɹawẽˈninkẽɁs /
'he speaks up' ²⁸⁰ ⁴⁷⁶ > Ca. / haˈwẽnitkẽɁs /; PNI * / ˈheɁnkẽh / 'above' ²¹⁴ >
( e.g. PNI * / ˈansteh / 'outside' ¹⁵⁵ > Ca. / ahsteh /; PNI * / kaˈnstaɹhaɁ /
'I am crying' ¹⁵⁴ > Ca. / kahˈstaːhaɁ /; PNI * / ohˈwẽɁnstaɁ / 'foam' ²⁷¹ >
268
Ca. / ohˈwẽɁstaɁ /; PNI * / teˈwahnshẽːh / 'twenty' ¹³⁵ > Ca. / teˈwahshẽː /;
PNI * / nikaɁˈnshansteɁ / 'it is that strong' ⁴⁹⁶ > Ca. / niˈkaɁshahsteɁ /; but cf.
PNI * / ˈkinskoːt / 'I am sitting' ²⁸³ > Ca. / kitskoːt / rather than ˣ / kihskoːt / by CA-7;
PNI * / ˈskenskʷahs / 'you lift it' ³³¹ > Ca. / seˈkeːtskʷahs / rather than
be late' ⁴⁹² > Ca. / ẽˈwakhnihskoɁ /; PNI * / ẽˈtsniːkẽɁ / 'you will see it' ³³² >
Ca. / ẽhˈsniːkẽɁ /; PNI * / ẽtswaˈnẽhskoɁ / 'you will steal it' ³⁶⁸ >
Ca. / ẽhˈswanẽhskoɁ /; but cf. PNI * / oˈnẽtshaɁ / 'arm' ³⁷⁰ > Ca. / oˈnẽːtshaɁ /;
PNI * / otsiˈkheɁtaɁ 'salt' ⁴⁵⁶ > Ca. / oˈtsikheɁtaɁ /; PNI * / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ >
This change must follow CA-6 and precede CA-8, CA-14, and CA-15.
PNI * / ˈaːtseːɁ / 'it is new' ¹⁸⁹ > Ca. / ˈaːseːɁ /; PNI * / kahˈtsehthaɁ / 'I hide it' ¹⁴⁶ >
Ca. / kahˈsehthaɁ /; but cf. PNI * / atẽˈnaɁtshɹaɁ / 'food, provisions' ¹⁶⁸ >
Ca. / aˈtẽnɁatshɹaɁ /; PNI * / jeˈkẽhtsih / 'she is old' ³³³ > Ca. / eˈkẽhtsih /;
269
PNI * / kaˈnaɁtsjaɁ / 'pail' ³⁶³ > Ca. / kaˈnaɁtsaɁ /; PNI * / waɁˈkeɁtsɹeːɁ /
'I dragged it' ⁴⁹⁸ > Ca. / ɁaˈkeɁtsɹeːɁ / ). This change must follow CA-6 and CA-7, and
( e.g. PNI * / seˈkenskʷahs / 'you lift it' ³³¹ > Ca. / seˈkeːtskʷahs /;
PNI * / tsẽniˈnskɹõːtih / 'spit!' ¹⁷⁴ ²⁸⁴ > Ca. / sẽniːˈtskɹõːtih /; PNI * / ˈkinskoːt /
'I am sitting' ²⁸³ > Ca. / kitskoːt / ). This change must follow CA-8.
it occurred before a non-glottal consonant ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈnaktaɁ / 'bed' ³⁵⁵ >
PNI * / seɁˈnonshahs / 'you are envious' ⁴⁹⁵ > Ca. / seɁˈnoːshahs / ). This change must
follow CA-1, CA-2, CA-3, and CA-4, and precede CA-14, CA-21 and CA-23.
(cf. also Chafe 1977a:176, Chafe & Foster 1981:131, Michelson 1988:99).
glottal consonant if the following syllable was accented ( e.g. PNI * / kahwaˈtsiːɹaɁ /
'family' ²⁶⁹ > Ca. / kahwaːˈtsiːjaɁ /; PNI * / ohneˈkiːjoːh / 'good water' ²⁷⁹ ³⁶⁴ >
Ca. / ohneːˈkiːjoː /; PNI * / ẽhaˈkʷeːniɁ / 'he will be able' ³⁵¹ > Ca. / ẽhaːˈkʷeːniɁ / ).
This change must follow CA-1 and CA-2, and precede CA-14, CA-21 and CA-23.
270
(cf. also Chafe 1977a:176, Michelson 1988:100).
By CA-12, accented short vowels in odd-numbered penults have lost their accent
( e.g. PNI * / ˈtekɹõɁ / 'eight' ⁴³⁹ > Ca. / tekɹõɁ /; PNI * / ẽkaˈtahseht / 'I will hide' ¹⁴⁶
> Ca. / ẽˈkatahseht /; PNI * / ẽhseˈkenskoɁ / 'you will lift it' ³³¹ >
Ca. / ẽhˈseketskoɁ / ). Two-syllable words that have lost accent by this change remain
without accent in Cayuga, while words of three or more syllables have acquired new
By CA-13, words with no accent have accented the vowel of the last non-final even
syllable of the word ( e.g. PNI * / ẽkaˈtahseht / 'I will hide' ¹⁴⁶ > Ca. / ẽˈkatahseht /;
PNI * / ẽhseˈkenskoɁ / 'you will lift it' ³³¹ > Ca. / ẽhˈseketskoɁ /;
PNI * / tewakeˈkhahsjõːh / 'I have separated it' ³³⁷ > Ca. / tewakeˈkhahsõː / ). This
change must follow CA-12 and precede CA-14. (cf. also Chafe 1977a:176,
Michelson 1988:99).
'mind' ⁴⁹¹ > Ca. / kɁaˈnikõhaɁ /; PNI * / ẽhsatjaɁˈtaːwiɁt / 'you will get dressed' ¹⁷⁶ >
Ca. / ẽhsatjɁaˈtaːwiɁt /; PNI * / kahwihˈstaɁeks / 'it strikes metal' ²⁰⁰ ²⁷⁴ >
271
Ca. / kahˈwihstɁaehs / 'clock'; cf. also Michelson 1988:104 ). Although this change was
(cf. Mithun 1989:253), perhaps owing to analogy of metathesized forms with non-
must follow CA-7, CA-12, and CA-13. (cf. Chafe 1977a:177, Chafe & Foster 1981:137,
Michelson 1988:104).
By CA-15, * / j / has been lost after * / ts / ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈnaɁtsjaɁ / 'pail' ³⁶³ >
PNI * / ohwẽˈtsjaɁke / 'on the land' ⁴⁰² > Ca. / ohˈwẽtsɁakeh / ). This change must
( e.g. PNI * / kõhˈshwahẽhs / 'I hate you' ²⁵⁰ > Ca. / kõhˈswahẽhs / ).
( e.g. PNI * / tekaˈwẽɹjeh / 'I stir it' ¹⁹⁵ > Ca. / teˈkawẽnjeh /; PNI * / saˈtõɹjeɁts /
'you breathe' ¹⁸⁵ > Ca. / saˈtõːnjeɁs /; PNI * / waɁˈhɹaɹjoɁ / 'he killed it' ⁴²⁸ >
272
'it is cold' ¹⁷⁰ > Ca. / oˈthoːweɁ /; PNI * / shehˈɹoːɹi / 'tell her!' ²⁴⁶ > Ca. / sheˈhoːwih /;
PNI * / johˈsnoːɹeɁ / 'it is fast' ²⁵⁸ > Ca. / ohˈsnoːweɁ / ). Similarly, by CA-19, * / ɹ /
and before a non-front vowel ( e.g. PNI * / oˈheːɹaɁ / 'cornstalk' ²²⁴ > Ca. / oˈheːjaɁ /;
PNI * / johˈniːɹõh / 'it is hard' ²³⁷ > Ca. / ohˈniːjõh /; PNI * / tewatiˈɹõthaɁ /
'it stretches' ¹⁷⁵ > Ca. / tewatiˈjõːthaɁ / ). Both changes must precede CA-23.
By CA-20, * / ɹ / has been lost when preceded by a vowel and a glottal consonant
( e.g. PNI * / oˈkahɹaɁ / 'eye' ³¹⁹ > Ca. / oˈkahaɁ /; PNI * / ohˈsheɁɹaɁ / 'dough' ²⁴⁸ >
(cf. also Chafe & Foster 1981:131). By CA-21, * / ɹ / has been lost when followed by a
glottal consonant ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkeɹɁoks / 'I chop' ⁴³² > Ca. / keɁohs /;
PNI * / aˈkeɹhak / 'my aunt' ⁴²⁵ > Ca. / aˈkeːhak /; PNI * / kaˈɹhaːtaɁ / 'bush, forest' ⁴²³
> Ca. / kaˈhaːtaɁ / ), and this change must follow CA-10 and CA-11.
(cf. also Chafe & Foster 1981:135). By CA-22, * / ɹ / has been lost after * / w /
( e.g. PNI * / ˈjowɹateɁ / 'there is a wind' ⁴³³ ⁴⁸⁷ > Ca. / oˈwaːteɁ / 'it is windy';
PNI * / ˈjowɹanoːh / 'it is a cold wind' ³⁷⁹ ⁴⁸⁷ > Ca. / oˈwaːnoː / ), and by CA-23, * / r /
has been lost between vowels ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈɹõːtaɁ / 'log' ⁴³¹ > Ca. / ˈkaõːtaɁ /;
273
PNI * / joɁtaˈɹihẽːh / 'it is hot' ²⁰⁴ > Ca. / oɁˈtaihẽː /; PNI * / ẽtsaˈkaːɹiːɁ /
'it will bite you' ³²⁴ > Ca. / ẽsaːˈkaiːɁ / 'it will bite you' ). CA-23 must follow CA-10,
CA-11, CA-18 and CA-19, and precede CA-24, CA-25, CA-26, CA-27 and CA-28.
By CA-24, when accent occurred on the second of two adjacent vowels, it has
shifted to the first vowel ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈɹẽːnaɁ / 'song, incantation' ⁴²² >
Ca. / ˈkaẽːnaɁ /; PNI * / kaˈɹõːtaɁ / 'log' ⁴³¹ > Ca. / ˈkaõːtaɁ /; PNI * / kaɹõhˈjaɁke /
'in the sky' ⁴³⁰ > Ca. / ˈkaõhjɁakeh / 'sky' ). Similarly, by CA-25, accented long vowels
have been shortened when followed by a vowel ( e.g. PNI * / tkaˈjeːɹiːh / 'it is right' ³⁰³
> Ca. / tkaːˈjeiː / 'it is right'; PNI * / tejonaɁˈkaːɹõːt / 'it has horns' ³⁵⁹ ⁴¹⁰ >
Ca. / tejonɁaˈkaõːt / 'sheep' ). Both changes must follow CA-23, and precede CA-26,
By CA-26, * / e(ː) / has nasalized before * / ẽ(ː) / ( e.g. PNI * / nijoˈjeːɹẽh / '(what)
it is doing' ³⁰² > Ca. / niˈjoːjẽː /; PNI * / ẽtitsanaɁˈkeːɹẽɁ / 'it will imitate you' ³⁶⁰ >
Ca. / ẽtisaˈnaɁkjẽːɁ / ) (cf. Chafe & Foster 1981:137), and similarly, by CA-27, * / o(ː) /
has nasalized before * / õ(ː) / ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈnoːɹõɁ / 'it is expensive' ³⁹³ >
Ca. / kanõːɁ / ). Both changes must follow CA-23, CA-24, and CA-25, and precede
274
By CA-28, an accented vowel followed by an identical vowel has become a single
unaccented long vowel ( e.g. PNI * / oɁˈtaːɹaɁ / 'clay' ²⁹¹ > Ca. / oɁtaːɁ /;
PNI * / ˈkẽnskaɹaɁ / 'mattress' ²⁸² > Ca. / kẽtskaːɁ / 'mattress' ). This change must
By CA-29, a resonant consonant has been lost word-finally after a glottal stop
( e.g. PNI * / tsõˈkiːtaɁw / 'I went back to sleep' ²⁸⁷ > Ca. / sõˈkiːtaɁ /;
PNI * / ˈtetstaɁn / 'stand up!' ⁴⁴⁹ > Ca. / tehstaɁ /; PNI * / ẽˈkõnhaɁn /
'you will hire me' ²³² > Ca. / ẽˈkõːnhaɁ / ). (cf. also Chafe & Foster 1981:135).
( e.g. PNI * / j(a)hejoˈthahoɁkt / 'end of the trail' ¹³¹ ³⁹⁸ > Ca. / heˈjothahoɁk /;
PNI * / ˈhwihsk / 'five' ²⁷³ > Ca. / hwihs /; PNI * / ˈtẽhsjehst / 'you will mix' ³⁰⁰ >
By CA-31, a consonant plus * / h / has been lost before * / s / at the end of a word
( e.g. PNI * / kaˈthõːtaːths / 'I listen' ¹⁷¹ > Ca. / kaˈthõːtaːs /; PNI * / ˈjoɁkʷaːths /
'it digs' ³⁹⁹ > Ca. / oɁkʷaːs /; PNI * / ɹaˈtoːɹaːths / 'he hunts' ¹⁸⁴ > Ca. / haˈtoːwaːs /;
PNI * / ˈiːkeːkhs / 'I eat' ³¹⁶ > Ca. / ˈiːkeːs /; PNI * / joɹaɁˈnẽːtaːkhs / 'it sticks' ⁴²¹ >
Ca. / owaɁˈnẽːtaːs /; PNI * / ɹaˈtijɁaːkhs / 'they shoot' ²⁹⁰ > Ca. / haˈtijɁaːs / ).
This change must precede CA-34. (cf. also Chafe & Foster 1981:137).
275
By CA-32, * / h / has been lost word-finally after long vowels
( e.g. PNI * / kahẽˈtiːjoːh / 'good field' ²²⁶ ²⁷⁹ > Ca. / kahẽːˈtiːjoː /; PNI * / ˈwakkẽːh / or
* / waˈkeːkẽːh / 'I have seen it' ³³² > Ca. / aˈkeːkẽː /; PNI * / ˈtshoːjõːh /
By CA-34, * / ths / has become * / tsh / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈithseɁts / 'you come here' ²⁰⁹
> Ca. / ˈitsheɁs /; PNI * / athsiˈnhahsɹaɁ / 'garters' ¹⁷² Æ Ca. / oˈtshinhahsthaɁ / ). This
( e.g. PNI * / waɁsheˈtniːkẽɁ / 'we saw him' ³³² > Ca. / ɁasheːˈthniːkẽɁ /;
PNI * / ˈtekniːh / 'two' ⁴³⁸ > Ca. / teˈkhniː /; PNI * / ẽwaˈknitskoɁ / 'I will be late' ⁴⁹² >
Ca. / ẽˈwaknihskoɁ / ). (cf. also Chafe & Foster 1981:136, Michelson 1988:24).
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to Common
276
CA-4 Ɂ > Ø / _nk, nkʷ
CA-5 n > t / _k, kʷ
CA-6 ns > ts ( except before k or kʷ )
CA-7 ts > hs / V_ ( except before h, i, j, ɹ )
CA-8 ts > s ( except before h, i, j, ɹ )
CA-9 ns > ts / _k, kʷ
CA-10 lengthening in accented penults ( see description )
CA-11 pre-tonic lengthening ( see description )
CA-12 loss of accent on short odd penults ( see description )
CA-13 new accent rule for words without accent ( see description )
CA-14 glottal metathesis ( see description )
CA-15 j > Ø / ts_
CA-16 h > Ø / s_w
CA-17 ɹ > n / _(h)j
CA-18 ɹ > w / o(ː), õ(ː)_a(ː), e(ː), ẽ(ː), i(ː)
CA-19 ɹ > j / e(ː), ẽ(ː), i(ː)_a(ː), o(ː), õ(ː), u(ː)
CA-20 ɹ > Ø / VH_
CA-21 ɹ > Ø / _H
CA-22 ɹ > Ø / w_
CA-23 ɹ > Ø / V_V
CA-24 V¹ˈV² > ˈV¹V²
CA-25 ˈV¹ːV² > ˈV¹V²
CA-26 e(ː) > ẽ / _ẽ(ː)
CA-27 o(ː) > õ / _õ(ː)
CA-28 ˈV¹V¹ > V¹ː
CA-29 R > Ø / Ɂ_#
CA-30 C > Ø / s, k_#
CA-31 Ch > Ø / _s#
CA-32 h > Ø / Vː_#
CA-33 t > h / _t
CA-34 ths > tsh
CA-35 Ø > h / t, k_n
277
The forms and examples above consist of Cayuga utterance-final forms; in contrast,
Cayuga utterance-medial forms are accented on the final syllable, overriding all other
phonological changes involving accent given above ( e.g. Ca. / ˈtsaːtahk / 'seven',
( cf. Froman et al. 2002:xxxiii ). This pattern of automatically accenting the final
sound changes exist at Six Nations of the Grand River which differentiate Upper Cayuga
from Lower Cayuga ( cf. forms in Mithun & Henry 1984:13 and notes in Froman et al.
2002: xii ). These phonemic changes are described below as CA-D1 through CA-D4,
'nice day' > Upper Ca. / wẽhnihˈfɹiːjoː / ). This change occurs in the Upper Cayuga
'cut it!' > Upper Ca. / ishɹeː /; Ca. / oˈwiːtsɹaɁ / 'ice' > Upper Ca. / oˈwiːsɹaɁ /;
278
Ca. / oˈhoːtsɹaɁ / 'basswood' > Upper Ca. / oˈhoːsɹaɁ / ). This change occurs in the
'cut it!' > Lower Ca. / ithɹeː /; Ca. / oˈwiːtsɹaɁ / 'ice' > Lower Ca. / oˈwiːtɹaɁ / 'ice';
Ca. / oˈhoːtsɹaɁ / 'basswood' > Lower Ca. / oˈhoːtɹaɁ / ). This change occurs in the
Lower Cayuga dialect and was also present in the Oklahoma dialect; cf.
Ca. / saˈtshɹõnɁitaɁ /.
Lower Ca. / ohˈskjẽɁtaɁ /; Ca. / waˈtjeːsẽh / 'it is easy' > Lower Ca. / waˈkjeːsẽh /;
Ca. / ˈiːtjeːt / 'she is standing there' > Lower Ca. / ˈiːkjeːt / ). This change occurs in the
Phonological changes that have occurred in Cayuga dialects are recapitulated in (3).
279
In addition to the phonemic sound changes in (3) some phonetic shifts have
occurred as well. In the Lower Cayuga dialect, vowels in odd syllables are phonetically
Lower Ca. [ saˈgjẽni ̥hdõh ] ), and pronounced with creaky voice if preceded by a
post-consonantal glottal stop, which in turn may manifest itself as an ejective feature on
the preceding consonant ( e.g. Ca. / kɁaˈnikõhaɁ / 'mind' > Lower Ca. [ k’a̰ˈnikõ̥haɁ ] ),
Lower Ca. [ kḁhˈnawa̰geh ] ) (cf. Froman et al. 2002:xii), and devoiced vowels fail to
varieties of Lower Cayuga ( see Mithun & Henry 1984:13 for an exception ).
modal prefixes in Cayuga, and the contrastive prefix is substituted; the glottal stop of
the negative prefix ( PNI * / teɁ / ⁰⁰¹ ) induces loss of a following glide, as in Onondaga.
Cayuga appear to have levelled initial * / jah / to / h /. Allomorphs of the factual prefix
280
( PNI * / waɁ /, * / we / ⁰⁰⁶ ) seem to have lost * / w / in all environments and the
Mohawk-Oneida, and Seneca. The non-singular animate prefix of PNI has been
preserved in Cayuga and can be added to any pronominal prefix with the argument 3I
to make that argument non-singular and animate, but only if the other argument is
combination with the prefix ( cf. PNI * / waɁeˈtsaːkẽɁ / 'someone saw you' ³³² Æ
pronominal prefixes has been lost in word initial position ( e.g. PNI * / jetshi /
'2NS&3I' ⁰³⁵ Æ Ca. / etshi / ). All reflexes of PNI masculine gender pronominal prefixes
( e.g. PNI * / ɹa / '3SGM:3' ⁰⁵³ Æ Ca. / ha / ). The third person dual number pronominal
prefixes * { kni } '3DU:3' and * { hni } '3DUM:3' seem to have been lost in Cayuga, as
does the reflex of the prefix * { kõwa } '3I:3SG', which is replaced in Cayuga by a reflex
of PNI * { jõtate- } 'one acts on oneself or another' ⁰⁶³ ⁰⁷⁶, as in Tuscarora. Reflexes of
the 3PL:3 prefixes * { wati } ⁰⁵⁸ and * { kõti } ⁰⁵⁹ appear to have merged in Cayuga as a
281
( < PNI * { kaje } '3NSA+3I:3' ) and { koti } '3:3NS', function as non-singular feminine
indefinite forms. As in Onondaga, Seneca, Huron, and Wyandot, the Cayuga prefixes
/ hsk / '2:1.SG' ⁰¹⁶, / wak / '3:1SG' ⁰²⁵, / jõk / '3I:1SG' ⁰²⁶, / hak / '3M:1SG' ⁰²⁷, and
/ tahsk / '2:1.SG.IMP' ⁰⁷² appear to have levelled out allomorphs ending in / kʷ / before
bases beginning in / a /. Like the modern New York dialect of Onondaga, Cayuga has
indicative counterparts. The 2SG:3.IMP prefix ( PNI * { tse } ⁰⁷⁵ ) seems to have been
Cayuga.
Cayuga have apparently been added / hnjV /, / hsV /, / hsCV /, / htsV /, / kV / and
/ kʷV /, along with others added owing to regular sound change. Consonants and
clusters before which the 1SG:3 pronominal prefix allomorphs / kh / ( < PNI * / k / ),
282
(4) Distribution of Prefix Allomorphs with Epenthetic / e / in Cayuga (1SG:3)
/k/ _hV, hjV, hnV, hrV, hwV, jV, ɹV, tV, wV.
The aspectual and derivational morphology of Cayuga is essentially the same as that
of PNI, although the past habitual suffix * { -kʷ } ¹¹⁰ appears to have been replaced by
{ -kẽheːɁ }, a derivative of the decessive suffix. Like Seneca, Cayuga has an eventuative
suffix { -hsɁõh }, which is added to perfective forms to signify that an event is finally
complete ( e.g. Ca. / satehsɹõˈnihsɁõh / 'you are ready' ). Like the other Five Nations
languages, Cayuga has a second locative suffix, { -neh }, a pluralizer suffix, { -shõɁ },
283
CHAPTER 15: SENECA
15.1 INTRODUCTION
of the great hill' ), whose territory in the early seventeenth century appears to have
ranged from the Genesee River in the west, bordering on the territory of the Wenro and
the Erie, to Cayuga Lake in the east, bordering on the territory of the Cayuga, north to
(cf. Engelbrecht 2003:114, village locations in Swanton 1968:35-37; see Map 6). Today
speakers of Seneca are located primarily on the Cattaraugus, Alleghany, and Tonawanda
Although the Seneca were in contact with Europeans beginning in the mid-
seventeenth century, the names of Seneca chiefs and Seneca settlements are invariably
given in Huron or Mohawk in early sources and the Seneca language itself is of
relatively late attestation. The first publication of vocabulary in the language appears to
the visit of seven Senecas to England accompanied by members of the Religious Society
of Friends; it was followed in the same year by a book of hymns in the language,
written and translated by the Presbyterian school teacher Jabez Backus Hyde (1818).
284
The language of these texts, and of numerous other early missionary publications that
followed in the 1820s and 1830s, appears to be the same as Modern Seneca.
Chafe (1964b), and phonology in Dudley (1974). Texts of the language are in Chafe
(1961), and a sketch of the language in Chafe (1996). A dictionary is in Chafe (1967).
Unless otherwise stated, Seneca forms in this chapter are based on data from Chafe
(1967).
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s h
Affricate ts
Approximant j w
285
VOWELS
Close iː
i
Open æː aː
æ a
PNI * / ɹ / has been lost in Seneca and the vowels / æː / and / æ / have arisen in
certain environments as a result of r-loss. PNI * / ẽ(ː) / and * / õ(ː) / have shifted to
different speakers (Mithun 1985:505), and some speakers realize / o(ː) / as [ u(ː) ] in
286
15.3 PHONOLOGICAL CHANGES
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to Seneca
are described below as SE-1 through SE-46. Where ordering of two changes appears to
( e.g. PNI * / ˈaːtseːɁ / 'new' ¹⁸⁹ > Se. / ˈwaːseːɁ / 'it is new'; PNI * / ɹaɁˈtswahthaɁ /
'he extinguishes it' ²⁰⁸ > Se. / haɁˈswathaɁ /; PNI * / oˈnẽtshaɁ / 'arm' ³⁷⁰ >
Se. / onɛ̃ːshaɁ /; but cf. PNI * / ˈkaːtsih / 'come here!' ³²⁹ > Se. / kaːtsih /;
PNI * / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ > Se. / tsaːtak / ). This change must precede SE-2, SE-20,
SE-21, SE-24, SE-39, SE-40, and SE-41. (cf. also Rudes 1976:32, Mithun 1985:506).
By SE-2, * / j / has been lost after * / ts / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkẽtsjõh / 'fish' ⁴⁶⁷ >
Se. / kɛ̃tsɔ̃h /; PNI * / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ > Se. / tsaːtak /; PNI * / oˈnatsjaɁ /
'wheat' ³⁵⁷ > Se. / onɔ̃ːtsaɁ /; PNI * / kaˈnaɁtsjaɁ / 'pot, kettle' ³⁶³ > Se. / kaˈnɔ̃ɁtsaɁ / ).
'I am envious' ⁴⁹⁵ > Se. / keɁnoːshas /; PNI * / teˈwahnshẽːh / 'twenty' ¹³⁵ >
Se. / teˈwashɛ̃ːh /; PNI * / ɹaˈkenskʷahs / 'he raises it' ³³¹ > Se. / hakeːskʷas / ). This
287
By SE-4, vowels have been lengthened in even-numbered penultimate syllables
'I have changed it' ⁴⁴⁰ > Se. / akteːnjɔ̃ːh /; PNI * / ɹoˈnatkõɁ / 'they are sorcerors' ¹⁸¹ >
Se. / honɔ̃ːtkɔ̃Ɂ /; PNI * / tekattsiˈstokwas / 'I scatter sparks' ⁴⁵⁹ ⁴⁷¹ >
Se. / teˈkatshistoːkwas / ). This change must precede SE-5, SE-6, SE-11, SE-13, SE-18,
By SE-5, the original accent of PNI has been lost ( e.g. PNI * / oɁˈnẽːjaɁ / 'bone' ²⁵⁵
> Se. / oɁnɛ̃ːjaɁ /; PNI * / ˈõːkʷeh / 'person' ⁴⁰⁵ > Se. / ɔ̃ːkʷeh /; PNI * / kaˈtoːkẽːh / 'it
is true, exact' ⁴⁴⁸ > Se. / katoːkɛ̃ːh / ). This change must follow SE-4 and precede SE-6.
By SE-6, a new accentual system has been introduced whereby accent fell on the
vowel in the last nonfinal even short syllable of a word if it was followed directly by a
'I am dead' ²⁷⁶ > Se. / aˈkiːejɔ̃ːh /; PNI * / waɁkɹaɁˈnẽːtaːk / 'I stuck it on' ⁴²¹ >
Se. / oɁˈkæɁnɛ̃ːtaːk /; PNI * / ahˈtahkʷaɁ / 'shoe' ¹⁴² > Se. / ahˈtahkʷaɁ / ); failing that,
accent has been assigned to the last non-final even short syllable that was followed by a
288
'I've lifted it' ³⁵⁰ > Se. / teˈwakehkʷɛ̃h /; PNI * / onõˈtaɁkeh / 'on the hill' ³⁸⁵ >
Se. / oˈnɔ̃taɁkeh / 'on the hill'; PNI * / ẽjeˈtakheɁ / 'someone will run' ⁴³⁵ >
Se. / ɛ̃ˈjetakheɁ / ); failing that, a word has no accent ( e.g. PNI * / kahˈnhoːtõɁ /
'the door is closed' ²³⁴ > Se. / kahoːtɔ̃Ɂ /; PNI * / wak(e)ˈkaɹjahs / 'it bites me' ³²⁴ >
Se. / akekanjas /; PNI * / waɁkaˈtwẽːteht / 'I lent it' ¹⁹² > Se. / oɁkatwɛ̃ːtet / ). For
further discussion and illustration of Seneca accent, see Chafe (1977a:178) and
Michelson (1988:110-111). This change must follow SE-2, SE-4 and SE-5, and precede
( e.g. PNI * / oˈnaɁkaɹaɁ / 'horn' ³⁵⁹ > Se. / oˈnɔ̃ɁkææɁ /; PNI * / oˈnõtaɹaɁ /
'hominy' ³⁸⁶ > Se. / onɔ̃tææɁ /; PNI * / jakjaˈɹaɁtseːɁ / 'we are cousins' ¹⁵⁷ >
( e.g. PNI * / ts(e)kaˈɹoːtẽh / 'tell a story!' ³²⁵ ³⁹⁷ > Se. / sekeotɛ̃h /;
PNI * / kanaɁkaɹoˈtahsɹaɁ / 'thing with a horn standing up' ³⁵⁹ ³⁹⁷ >
Se. / kanɔ̃ɁˈkeotashæɁ / 'horn war club' ). Similarly, by SE-9, short * / a / has become
289
Se. / teˈwahkɛ̃ɔt̃ / 'eaves' ). Both changes must precede SE-24. (cf. also Chafe 1959:490).
'story' ³²⁵ > Se. / kakaːæɁ /; PNI * / oˈɹaːnaɁ / 'syrup' ⁴¹⁸ > Se. / owæːnɔ̃Ɂ /;
PNI * / tjoɹaɁˈnẽːtaːks / 'it's sticky' ⁴²¹ > Se. / ˈtjoæɁnɛ̃ːtaːs / ). This change must
precede SE-17, SE-21, SE-24, and SE-43. (cf. also Chafe 1959:490).
By SE-11, * / h / has been lost before the cluster * / tk /, before the cluster * / nh /,
PNI * / kahˈnhohaɁ / 'door' ²³³ > Se. / kaˈhoaɁ /; PNI * / ohˈwihstaɁ / 'money' ²⁷⁴ >
Se. / oːˈwistaɁ /; PNI * / johˈsnoːɹeɁ / 'it is fast' ²⁵⁸ > Se. / osnoːweɁ /;
PNI * / ˈtsjaːtahk / 'seven' ⁴⁶⁴ > Se. / tsaːtak /; PNI * / ɹaˈtoːɹaːths / 'he hunts' ¹⁸⁴ >
Se. / hatoːwæːs /; PNI * / tsaˈtwẽːteht / 'lend it!' ¹⁹² > Se. / satwɛ̃ːtet /;
PNI * / ˈtsaɁswaht / 'put out the fire!' ²⁰⁸ > Se. / saɁswat / ). This change must follow
SE-3, SE-4 and SE-6, and precede SE-18, SE-33 and SE-35.
290
( e.g. PNI * / kewhjõhkaˈɹaɁke / 'on my thumb' ⁴⁷⁷ > Se. / kjɔ̃hˈkæːɁkeh / ). This change
'the earth (is present)' ⁴⁰² ⁴³³ > Se. / jɔ̃ɛt̃ sateɁ /; PNI * / ɹaˈnõhweɁts / 'he likes it' ³⁸⁴ >
Se. / haˈnɔ̃eɁs /; PNI * / jonõhˈwakteɁ / 'it is sore' ³⁸³ > Se. / oˈnɔ̃ɔk̃ teɁ / ). This change
'there is dew' ¹⁹⁹ > Se. / oɁaːjeɁ /; PNI * / keˈwjẽhõɁts / 'I know how' ⁴⁸³ >
Se. / keˈjɛ̃ɔɁ̃ s /; PNI * / tejowɹaˈwẽɹjõːh / 'the wind has stirred' ¹⁹⁵ ⁴⁸⁷ >
Se. / tejoæwɛ̃ːnjɔ̃ːh / 'the breeze is stirring' ). This change must follow SE-13, and
'animal' ⁴²⁹ > Se. / kanjoːɁ /; PNI * / wak(e)ˈkaɹjahs / 'it bites me' ³²⁴ >
This change must precede SE-17, SE-18, SE-24 and SE-33. (cf. also Chafe 1959:489).
( e.g. PNI * / ohˈɹaɁtaɁ / 'feather' ²⁴³ > Se. / ˈoæɁtaɁ /; PNI * / kaˈkahɹaɁ / 'eye' ³¹⁹ >
Se. / kaˈkaaɁ /; PNI * / roɁniˈkõhɹaɁ / 'his mind' ⁴⁹¹ > / hoɁˈnikɔ̃ɛɁ̃ / ). This change
291
must follow SE-6, SE-10, and SE-16, and precede SE-19, SE-22, SE-23, and SE-24.
By SE-18, * / h / has been lost when it occurred after a vowel and before * / n /,
'water, liquid' ³⁶⁴ > Se. / oːneːkaɁ / 'whiskey'; PNI * / ohˈwihstaɁ / 'metal, money' ²⁷⁴ >
Se. / oːˈwistaɁ /; PNI * / tejohˈjoɁtsihs / 'it is sour' ¹³³ > Se. / teˈjoːjoɁtsis / ). This
change must follow SE-4, SE-6, SE-11, SE-13, and SE-16, and precede SE-19, SE-20,
grain' ³⁶⁶ > Se. / oˈnɛ̃ɔɁ̃ /; PNI * / wakiˈheːjõːh / 'I am dead' ²⁷⁶ > Se. / aˈkiːejɔ̃ːh /;
PNI * / ˈjohateɁ / 'there is a path' ¹³¹ ⁴³³ > Se. / oːateɁ / 'road, trail, path' ). This change
must follow SE-6, SE-17, and SE-18, and precede SE-25, SE-26, SE-27, SE-28, SE-29, and
( e.g. PNI * / ˈtekɹõɁ / 'eight' ⁴³⁹ > Se. / tekjɔ̃Ɂ /; PNI * / tewak(e)ˈkẽtsɹõːh /
'I have told of it' ²⁴⁶ > Se. / aˈkathjoːwiːh / ). This change must follow SE-1 and SE-18,
292
By SE-21, * / ɹ / has become * / h / after * / s / ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈhahsɹaɁ /
'flame, council fire, torch' ²²³ > Se. / ˈkaːshæɁ /; PNI * / ẽhˈnitsɹaɁ / 'day' ²¹⁶ >
Se. / kaːˈjatɔ̃shæɁ / ). This change must follow SE-1 and SE-10, and must precede SE-22
and SE-24.
( e.g. PNI * / waɁtkatiˈɹõːtẽɁ / 'I stretched it' ¹⁷⁵ > Se. / oɁtkatijɔ̃ːtɛ̃Ɂ / ), and this change
must follow SE-17, SE-21 and precede SE-24. Similarly, by SE-23, * / ɹ / has become
* / w / intervocalically after a rounded vowel ( e.g. PNI * / johˈsnoːɹeɁ / 'it is fast' ²⁵⁸ >
Se. / osnoːweɁ /; PNI * / tewaˈkoːɹẽɁ / 'I have split it' ³⁹⁶ > Se. / tewakoːwɛ̃Ɂ /;
PNI * / wakʷaˈthɹoːɹiːh / 'I have told of it' ²⁴⁶ > Se. / aˈkathjoːwiːh / ), and this change
must follow SE-15 and SE-17 and precede SE-24. (cf. also Chafe 1959:489).
By SE-24, * / ɹ / has been lost ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈɹhaːtaɁ / 'forest' ⁴²³ >
Se. / kahaːtaɁ /; * / ohˈkaːɹaɁ / 'wood chip' ¹³⁴ > Se. / ohkaːaɁ /; PNI * / oˈnjaːɹaɁ /
'neck' ²³⁹ > Se. / onjaːaɁ /; PNI * / kaˈɹẽːnaɁ / 'song, incantation' ⁴²² > Se. / kaɛ̃nɔ̃Ɂ /;
PNI * / kaˈnoːɹõɁ / 'it is dear' ³⁹³ > Se. / kanoːɔ̃Ɂ /; PNI * / niwaˈkjeːɹẽh /
293
SE-6, SE-8, SE-9, SE-10, SE-15, SE-16, SE-17, SE-20, SE-21, and SE-22, and must
precede SE-25, SE-26, SE-27, SE-28, and SE-29. (cf. Chafe 1959:489).
( e.g. PNI * / ˈjohateɁ / 'there is a road' ¹³¹ > Se. / oːateɁ /; PNI * / ˈjowɹateɁ / 'there is
a wind' ⁴⁸⁷ > Se. / oːwæteɁ / ). This change must follow SE-19 and SE-24, and precede
SE-26.
( e.g. PNI * / kaˈɹõːtaɁ / 'log, tree' ⁴³¹ > Se. / kɛɔ̃ t̃ aɁ /; PNI * / kaˈɹẽːnaɁ / 'song' ⁴²² >
Se. / kaɛ̃nɔ̃Ɂ /; PNI * / oˈheːɹaɁ / 'cornstalk' ²²⁴ > Se. / oeæɁ / 'cornstalk' ). This change
must follow SE-18, SE-19, SE-24, and SE-25. (cf. also Chafe 1959:492).
By SE-27, when accent occurred on the second of two adjacent vowels, it has shifted
to the first vowel ( e.g. PNI * / oˈhõhtaɁ / 'ear' ¹³⁶ > Se. / ˈoɔ̃htaɁ /; PNI * / ohˈɹaɁtaɁ /
'feather' ²⁴³ > Se. / ˈoæɁtaɁ /; PNI * / joɹaɁnẽˈtaːkõh / 'it is stuck on' ⁴²¹ >
Se. / ˈoæɁnẽtaːkɔ̃h / ). This change must follow SE-6, SE-19 and SE-24.
* / ẽ(ː) / or * / õ(ː) / ( e.g. PNI * / kaˈɹõhjateɁ / 'the sky (is present)' ⁴³⁰ ⁴³³ >
Se. / ˈkɛ̃ɔj̃ ateɁ /; * / kaˈkẽːɹat / 'it is white' ³³⁵ > Se. / kakɛː̃ ɛ̃t /; PNI * / roɁniˈkõhɹaɁ /
'his mind' ⁴⁹¹ > Se. / hoɁˈnikɔ̃ɛɁ̃ /; PNI * / oˈnstẽhɹaɁ / 'cliff, rock bank' ²⁸⁶ >
294
Se. / oˈstɛ̃ːɛ̃Ɂ /, with anomalous vowel length ). This change must follow SE-19 and
* / o(ː) / ( e.g. PNI * / wahtjaˈwẽːɹeɁ / 'it is taboo' ¹⁴³ > Se. / wahtjaweːeɁ /;
PNI * / nijawẽˈhoːtẽːh / 'that kind of flower' ⁴⁰¹ ⁴¹⁰ > Se. / nijaˈweoɁtɛ̃ːh / ).
'above, high, over' ²¹⁴ > Se. / hetkɛ̃h /; PNI * / oˈtsiɁnkʷaɹaɁ / 'bile' ⁴⁶¹ >
'wampum' ³⁷⁵ > Se. / oˈtkoɁæɁ /; PNI * / tjorahkʷiˈnkẽɁõh / 'the sun has come
out' ²⁸⁰ ⁴¹⁵ > Se. / tjoæhkʷiˈtkɛ̃Ɂɔ̃h / ), and this change must follow SE-30. Similarly, by
'I stood up' ⁴⁴⁹ > Se. / ˈoɁtktaɁt /; PNI * / waɁõˈkehnhaɁn / 'someone hired me' ²³² >
Se. / waˈɁɔ̃kehaɁt /; PNI * / õˈkjẽːtaɁn / 'I obtained it' ³⁰⁹ > Se. / ɔ̃kjɛ̃ːtaɁt / ).
By SE-33, a resonant consonant has been lost before * / h / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkõnheɁ /
295
'I am alive' ⁴⁰⁶ > Se. / kɔ̃heɁ /; PNI * / oɁˈnhõhsaɁ / 'egg' ⁴⁹⁰ > Se. / oɁˈhɔ̃hsaɁ /;
PNI * / kẽˈjhõhateɁ / 'there is a creek' ²⁷⁸ ⁴³³ > Se. / kẽhɔ̃ːteɁ / ). This change must
word boundary ( e.g. PNI * / ˈhwihsk / 'five' ²⁷³ > Se. / wis /; PNI * / ˈtakjehst /
'I added, I contributed' ³⁰⁰ > Se. / takjes / ). (cf. also Chafe & Foster 1981:135).
By SE-35, a non-glottal consonant has been lost before * / s / at the end of a word
( e.g. PNI * / ˈɹaɁjaːkhs / 'he shoots' ²⁹⁰ > Se. / haɁjaːs /; PNI * / ɹaˈtoːɹaːths /
'he hunts' ¹⁸⁴ > Se. / hatoːwæːs /; PNI * / ˈjohskaːths / 'it is good' ²⁵³ > Se. / oskaːs / ).
'my older brother' ²⁶⁷ > Se. / hahtsiɁ /; PNI * / ˈakhtsiɁ / 'my older sister' ²⁶⁷ >
Se. / ahtsiɁ / ).
'they see it' ⁰³⁶ > Se. / hniːkɛ̃h /; PNI * / oˈttenjõːh / 'it has changed' ⁴⁴⁰ >
Se. / tewahˈtiaɁthaɁ / ). This change must follow SE-18. (cf. also Chafe 1959:484).
296
( e.g. PNI * / ˈjoːtoɁkt / 'it is at the end' ³⁹⁸ > Se. / oːtoɁk /; PNI * / waɁthatõˈtaːɹikt /
'he laughed' ¹⁸⁶ > Se. / oɁthatɔ̃taːik / ). (cf. also Chafe 1959:486).
By SE-39, * / ths / has become * / tsh / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈẽthseɁ / 'you will come
here' ²⁰⁹ > Se. / ɛ̃tsheɁ /; PNI * / athsiˈnhahsɹaɁ / 'garters' ¹⁷² > Se. / aˈtshihashæɁ / ).
'I scatter sparks' ⁴⁵⁹ ⁴⁷¹ > Se. / teˈkatshistoːkwas / ). This change must follow SE-1.
By SE-41, * / a(ː) / has become * / õ(ː) / if the vowel of the preceding syllable was
( e.g. PNI * / aˈwẽhaɁ / 'flower' ²¹⁵ > Se. / aˈwɛɔ̃ Ɂ̃ /; PNI * / kaˈkẽnhateɁ / '(it is)
summer' ³³⁴ ⁴³³ > Se. / kaˈkɛ̃hɔ̃teɁ /; PNI * / ohstahˈɹõːwaɁ / 'marrow' ²⁶² >
Se. / oˈstaːɔ̃wɔ̃Ɂ /; PNI * / kahnaˈwaɁke / 'at the rapids' ²³⁰ > Se. / kaːˈnɔ̃wɔ̃Ɂkeh /;
cf. also Chafe 1959:491 and 1967:9 ). This change must follow SE-1.
'footprint' ²⁹⁴ > Se. / oˈjaːnɔ̃Ɂ /; PNI * / kaˈnatajẽɁ / 'there is a village' ³⁰⁶ ³⁵⁶ >
Se. / kanɔ̃tajɛ̃Ɂ /; PNI * / kaˈnakɹeɁ / 'there is a lot' ³⁵⁴ > Se. / kanɔ̃ːkeɁ / ).
(cf. also Chafe 1959:491). Similarly, by SE-43, * / æ(ː) / has become * / ẽ(ː) / after
297
* / n / ( e.g. PNI * / oˈnɹahsaɁ / 'fungus' ³⁸⁹ > Se. / oˈnɛ̃hsaɁ /; PNI * / oˈnɹahtaɁ / ³⁹⁰
> Se. / oˈnɛ̃htaɁ / 'leaf' ), and this change must follow SE-11.
By SE-44, * / ẽ(ː) / has shifted to / ɛ(̃ ː) / ( e.g. PNI * / oˈnẽtshaɁ / 'arm' ³⁷⁰ >
Se. / onɛ̃ːshaɁ /; PNI * / ˈkẽtsjõh / 'fish' ⁴⁶⁷ > Se. / kɛ̃tsɔ̃h /; PNI * / oɁˈnẽːjaɁ / 'bone' ⁴⁸⁹
( e.g. PNI * / ˈõːkʷeh / 'person' ⁴⁰⁵ > Se. / ɔ̃ːkʷeh /; PNI * / oˈhõhtaɁ / 'ear' ¹³⁶ >
Se. / ˈoɔ̃htaɁ /; PNI * / wakiˈheːjõːh / 'I am dead' ²⁷⁶ > Se. / aˈkiːejɔ̃ːh / ).
( e.g. PNI * / ˈtekniːh / 'two' ⁴³⁸ > Se. / tekhniː /; PNI * / waknõhˈweɁõh / 'I like it' ³⁸⁴
> / aˈkhnɔ̃eɁɔ̃h /; PNI * / waɁaˈkniːkẽɁ / 'we saw it' ³³² > Se. / waˈɁakhniːkɛ̃Ɂ / ).
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to Seneca
298
SE-6 new accentual system ( see description )
SE-7 a > æ / _ɹa(ː)
SE-8 a > e / _ɹo(ː)
SE-9 a > ẽ / _ɹõ(ː)
SE-10 a(ː) > æ(ː) / ɹ_
SE-11 h > Ø / _tk, nh, sC, C#
SE-12 h > Ø / #_w
SE-13 h > Ø / w_j
SE-14 hw > Ø / õ_
SE-15 w > Ø / _ɹ, j
SE-16 ɹ > n / _(h)j
SE-17 ɹ > Ø / Vh_
SE-18 V¹h > V¹ː / _n, w, j
SE-19 h > Ø / V_V
SE-20 ɹ > j / K(h)_o(ː), õ(ː)
SE-21 ɹ > h / s_
SE-22 ɹ > j / i(ː)_V
SE-23 ɹ > w / o(ː), õ(ː)_V
SE-24 ɹ>Ø
SE-25 o > oː / _aː, æː
SE-26 V¹ː > V¹ / V_
SE-27 V¹ˈV² > ˈV¹V²
SE-28 a(ː) > ẽ(ː) / _ẽ(ː), õ(ː); ẽ(ː), õ(ː)_
SE-29 ẽ(ː) > e(ː) / _e(ː), o(ː)
SE-30 Ɂ > Ø / _nk, nkʷ
SE-31 n > t / _k, kʷ
SE-32 n > t / Ɂ_#
SE-33 R > Ø / _h
SE-34 C > Ø / s_#
SE-35 K > Ø / _s#
SE-36 k > Ø / _hts
SE-37 t > h / _n, t
SE-38 t > Ø / k_#
SE-39 ths > tsh
SE-40 tts > tsh
SE-41 a(ː) > õ(ː) / ṼH, Ṽ(Ɂ)w, Ṽsw_
299
SE-42 a(ː) > õ(ː) / n_
SE-43 æ(ː) > ẽ(ː) / n_
SE-44 ẽ(ː) > ɛ̃(ː)
SE-45 õ(ː) > ɔ̃(ː)
SE-46 Ø > h / k_n
Seneca has lost the non-singular animate prefix and pronominal prefixes with an
argument '3I' take the place of earlier non-singular forms ( e.g. Se. { khe } '1SG:3I, I act
before all pronominal prefixes except for those beginning in * / h / and * / j /, and the
that begin with * / j / lose this consonant word-initially in Seneca ( e.g. PNI * / jetsa /
'3I:2SG' ⁰²⁹ Æ Se. / esa / ), though it is preserved in the prefixes { je } '3I:3' ⁰⁶³ and
{ jako } '3:3I' ⁰⁶⁴ ; initial * / hj / has been reduced to / j / in the same environment
( e.g. PNI * / hja / '3M:2SG' ⁰³⁰ Æ Se. / ja / ). As in Onondaga and Cayuga, all reflexes
300
of PNI masculine gender pronominal prefixes with allomorphs in * / hɹ / and * / ɹ /
have levelled these sequences to / h / ( e.g. PNI * { hɹa } '3SGM:3' ⁰⁵³ Æ Se. { ha } ).
The language has a reflex of PNI * { wati } '3PL:3' ⁰⁵⁸ rather than * { kõti } ⁰⁵⁹, as well
( cf. Onondaga { hũk } '3NSM:3' ); the PNI prefix * { jakoti } '3NS:3I' ⁰⁷⁰ does not have a
reflex in Seneca.
'2:1.SG' ⁰¹⁶, / wak / '3:1SG' ⁰²⁵, / jɔ̃k / '3I:1SG' ⁰²⁶, / hak / '3M:1SG' ⁰²⁷, and / tak /
'Southern Seneca' forms in Dudley (1974:49) show that certain varieties of the
languages have not only the predicted allomorph / ts / '2DU&3' ⁰³¹ in place of / snj /
before bases beginning in / a /, but also an allomorph / tsi / that occurs before
consonants ( cf. Huron and Wyandot / tsi / '2DU&3' in the same environment ). Other
forms in Dudley (1974:59) show that the same varieties replace allomorphs of
PNI * / hse / '2SG:3' ⁰²² with allomorphs of PNI * / tse / '2SG:3.IMP' ⁰⁷⁵ and eliminate
301
the earlier distinction between imperative and non-imperative second person
pronominal prefixes.
clusters beginning with these sequences. Consonants and clusters before which the
/ Ø / _htsV
/ kh / _nV
/k/ _hV ( < PNI * hV, hɹV ), hjV, hnV, hwV, jV ( < PNI * jV ), tV, tsV, wV.
/ ke / _hV ( < PNI * wjhV ), jV ( < PNI * jɁV, wjV ), and all remaining
consonants and clusters.
Seneca, like Cayuga, has an eventuative suffix { -sɁɔ̃h } which is added to perfect
forms to signify that an event is finally complete ( e.g. Se. / akahsehˈtasɁɔ̃h / 'I've finally
hidden it' ). No distinction is made between present and imperfective verb bases, and
the language instead has general habitual aspect bases. The intensifier suffix has the
form { -tsiːwɛ̃h }. Like the other Five Nations languages, Seneca has a second locative
302
suffix, { -neh }, a pluralizer suffix, { -shɔ̃Ɂ }, and an augmentative suffix, { -koːwaːh }.
303
CHAPTER 16: HURON
16.1 INTRODUCTION
This chapter describes the common language of the Huron and Petun Confederacies,
language'. The territory of the Huron in the early seventeenth century appears to have
ranged from the Nottawasaga and Credit Rivers in the west, bordering on the territory
of the Petun and the Neutral, to the Saint Lawrence River in the east, bordering on the
territory of the Algonquin, north to the Canadian Shield, bordering on the territory of
the Nipissing and the Algonquin, and south to Lake Ontario (cf. Trigger 1987:27-30,
Warrick 2008:9-11; see Map 7). The territory of the Petun in the early seventeenth
century appears to have ranged from Owen Sound in the west, bordering on the
territory of the Ottawa, to the Nottawasaga River in the east, bordering on the territory
of the Huron, north to Georgian Bay, and south to the Grand River Valley, bordering on
the territory of the Neutral (cf. Trigger 1987:91-94, Warrick 2008:11-12; see Map 7).
All dialects and derivatives of Huron ( including Wyandot, the subject of the next
chapter, which descends from the Petun dialect of Huron ) are extinct, but the language
304
points of origin, Steckley (1997, 2007a) has established the former existence of at least
five Huron dialects: Rock Huron (RoHu), associated with Huron Rock villages situated
near Lake Couchiching; Cord Huron (CoHu), associated with Huron Cord villages
situated near Kempenfelt Bay; Northern Bear Huron (NBeHu), associated with Huron
Bear villages situated in the Penetanguishine peninsula; Southern Bear Huron (SBeHu),
associated with Huron Bear villages situated directly south of the Penetanguishine
Peninsula; and Petun (PeHu), associated with Petun villages near the Blue Mountain.
To these five can be added Lorette Huron (LoHu), the dialect spoken by the Huron
Huron forms presented in this chapter reflect Common Huron, a reconstructed form
of the language that predates dialect divisions. Subsequent sound changes that separate
the six known Huron dialects from Common Huron are presented in 16.3.
religious texts in Steckley (2004), and a grammar based on early sources in Lagarde
(1980). An early dictionary is in Sagard (1632), and slightly later and more reliable
Jesuit dictionaries are in MS 59, MS 62, MS 65, MS 67, Fraser (1920), and Steckley
(2007b). Unless otherwise stated, Huron forms in this chapter are based on data from
305
MS 59.
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s ʃ ( x xʷ ) h
Affricate ts
Approximant ɹ j w
VOWELS
Close iː
i
Mid eː ẽː oː õː
e ẽ o õ
Open aː
a
306
The phonemes / ʃ /, / x /, and / xʷ / appear to have developed from reflexes of PNI
sounds represented by / x / and / xʷ / ( <˛> and <˛8> in the Jesuit orthography, with
<g> and <g8> used in dictionaries for purposes of alphabetization ) is uncertain, and
although unrounded and rounded voiceless velar fricatives are used to represent them
here (cf. Rudes 1976:23), other possibilities that have been suggested include voiceless
(cf. Lagarde 1980:34). Whether contrastive vowel length was maintained in final
Unlike other Northern Iroquoian languages, Huron does not appear to have had
307
have approached [ u(ː) ] in certain environments, and Mithun (1985:505) notes that
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to Common
Huron are described below as HU-1 through HU-25. Where ordering of any two changes
'depth, hollow' ²⁴⁹ > Hu. / ohˈʃhõːwaɁ / <ochon8a>; PNI * / ˈohsɹaɁ / 'winter, year' ³⁹⁴
PNI * / kahˈsẽːnaɁ / 'name' ²⁴⁷ > Hu. / xahˈʃẽːnaɁ / <gachienda>; but cf. e.g.
PNI * / kaˈnahskʷaɁ / 'slave' ³⁵³ > Hu. / xaˈnahskʷaɁ / <gandask8a>; PNI * / ˈansteh /
'outside' ¹⁵⁵ > Hu. / ˈasteh / <Aste>; PNI * / ˈaːtseːɁ / 'it is new' ¹⁸⁹ > Hu. / ˈaːseːɁ /
308
<Ase> ). This change must precede HU-3 and HU-5. (cf. also Mithun 1979:167).
By HU-2, * / ts / has become * / ʃ / before * / ɹ / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈaɁtsɹaɁ / 'axe' ²⁰⁶ >
PNI * / ˈtsɹaɁtẽh / 'climb!' ⁴²⁰ > Hu. / ˈʃɹaɁtẽh / <chraten> ). This change must
precede HU-3.
( e.g. PNI * / ˈaːtseːɁ / 'it is new' ¹⁸⁹ > Hu. / ˈaːseːɁ / <Ase>; PNI * / kaˈnẽtshaɁ /
'arm' ³⁷⁰ > Hu. / xaˈnẽshaɁ / <gannensa>; but cf. PNI * / oˈtsihkʷaɁ / 'knot, fist,
club' ⁴⁵³ > Hu. / oˈtsihkʷaɁ / <otsik8a>; PNI * / ˈkẽtsjõh / 'fish' ⁴⁶⁷ > Hu. / ˈxẽtsõh /
<gentson> ). This change must follow HU-1 and HU-2 and precede HU-9, HU-10,
'we reside' ³⁵⁴ > Hu. / tiˈnaxɹeɁ / <tinda˛re>; PNI * / ˈẽthneɁ / 'they will come' ²⁰⁹ >
By HU-5, * / n / has been lost before * / s / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈwahnshẽːh / 'ten' ¹³⁵ >
Hu. / ˈahshẽːh / <A’sen>; PNI * / ˈansteh / 'outside' ¹⁵⁵ > Hu. / ˈasteh / <Aste>;
PNI * / kaɁˈnshansteɁ / 'it is powerful' ⁴⁹⁶ > Hu. / xaɁˈshasteɁ / <gasaste> ). This
309
By HU-6, * / n / has been lost in Huron prefixes before * / i /
( e.g. PNI * / waɁˈhniːkẽɁ / 'they saw it' ³³² > Hu. / aɁˈhiːxẽɁ / <ahi˛en>;
PNI * / waɁˈtsniːkẽɁ / 'you saw it' ³³² > Hu. / aɁˈtsiːxẽɁ / <atsi˛en>;
PNI * / jakniˈnakɹeɁ / 'we reside' ³⁵⁴ > Hu. / axiˈnaxɹeɁ / <a˛inda˛re> ). This change
occurred only when * / n / and * / i / were both part of the prefix, and did not occur
outside of prefixes ( cf. PNI * / ˈtekniːh / 'two' ⁴³⁸ > Hu. / ˈteniːh / <tendi>;
PNI * / johˈniːɹõh / 'it is durable' ²³⁷ > Hu. / ohˈniːɹih / <ondiri> ). This change must
( e.g. PNI * / ˈknõːkʷeh / 'they are people' ⁴⁰⁵ > Hu. / iˈnõːxʷeh / <innon˛8e>;
PNI * / ˈknaɁtaɹaks / 'I eat bread' ³¹⁶ ³⁶¹ > Hu. / iˈnaɁtaɹahʃ / <indatarach> ). This
'we are going' ²⁰⁹ > Hu. / aˈɁaneɁ / <aande>; PNI * / waɁˈknaɁtaɹak /
'I ate bread' ³¹⁶ ³⁶¹ > Hu. / aɁˈnaɁtaɹax / <andatara˛>; PNI * / waɁknõhˈsõːniɁ /
'I built the house' ²⁶⁰ ³⁸² > Hu. / aɁnõhˈʃõːniɁ / <annonchiondi> ). This change must
follow HU-6 and HU-7 and precede HU-13. (cf. also Lagarde 1980:66).
310
'it is fast' ²⁵⁸ > Hu. / ohˈstoːɹeɁ / <ostore>; PNI * / tsniˈheːjõːh / 'you are dead' ²⁷⁶ >
Hu. / stiˈheːõːh / <stiheon>; PNI * / ˈẽtskneɁ / 'they will go back' ²⁰⁹ > Hu. / ˈesteɁ /
<este> ). This change must follow HU-3, HU-6, and HU-7. (cf. also Lagarde 1980:62).
'they will go again' ²⁰⁹ > Hu. / ˈestheɁ / <esθe> ). This change must follow HU-3.
( e.g. PNI * / ˈtsskwaːkẽh / 'you still see us' ³³² > Hu. / ˈskhwaːxẽhk / <sχ8a˛enk> ).
( e.g. PNI * / kaˈnaktaɁ / 'bed' ³⁵⁵ > Hu. / xaˈnahtaɁ / <ganda‛ta>; PNI * / ˈkaktsaɁ /
'dish' ³⁴⁷ > Hu. / ˈxahsaɁ / <gasa>; PNI * / ˈwaksẽːh / 'it is bad' ¹⁵³ > Hu. / ˈahʃẽːh /
<Achen>; PNI * / ˈihɹɁoks / 'he chops' ⁴³² > Hu. / ˈihaɁohʃ / <iharoch> ).
( e.g. PNI * / waɁkaːkẽɁ / 'it saw it' ³³² > Hu. / aɁˈxaːxẽɁ / <a˛a˛en>; PNI * / ˈkankõɁ /
'blood' ³⁷⁶ > Hu. / ˈxanxõɁ / <˛angon>; PNI * / ẽˈkɹõtjaɁk / 'I will cut down
the tree' ²⁹⁷ ⁴³¹ > Hu. / eˈxɹõtjaɁx / <e˛rontiaj> ). This change must follow HU-6, HU-7
311
and HU-8, and precede HU-15, and HU-16.
'person' ⁴⁰⁵ > Hu. / ˈõːxʷeh / <on˛8e> ). This change must precede HU-16.
( e.g. PNI * / twaˈnakɹeɁ / 'we reside' ³⁵⁴ > Hu. / kwaˈnaxɹeɁ / <k8anda˛re>;
PNI * / wakʷaˈtwiɹajẽɁ / 'I have children' ⁴⁷⁹ ³⁰⁶ > Hu. / axaˈkwiɹaẽɁ / <a˛ak8iraen>;
PNI * / wakʷaˈtkahthwẽh / 'I have looked at it' ¹⁷⁹ > Hu. / axaˈkahkhwih /
'it is there' ³⁰⁶ > Hu. / ˈkaːẽɁ / <Kaen>; PNI * / oˈtkẽhtsɹaɁ / 'purulence' ¹⁸⁰ >
PNI * / kaˈtkahthwahs / 'I look at it' ¹⁷⁹ > Hu. / xaˈkahkhwahʃ / <˛aka‛k8ach> ). This
change must follow HU-13, HU-14 and HU-15. (cf. also Lagarde 1980:65).
By HU-17, * / j / has been lost after * / ts / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkẽtsjõh / 'fish' ⁴⁶⁷ >
Hu. / ˈxẽtsõh / <gentson>; PNI * / kaˈtsjaːɹaɁ / 'tobacco' ⁴⁶⁵ > Hu. / xaˈtsaːɹaɁ /
312
(cf. also Mithun 1979:167).
( e.g. PNI * / jeˈnakɹeɁ / 'one resides' ³⁵⁴ > Hu. / eˈnaxɹeɁ / <enda˛re>;
PNI * / waɁˈkẽhej / 'it died' ²⁷⁶ > Hu. / aɁˈxẽhe / <a˛enhe>; PNI * / kaˈjeːɹiːh /
'it is complete' ³⁰³ > Hu. / xaˈeːɹiːh / <gaeri> ), and this change must precede HU-19
By HU-19, * / ɹ / has been lost before * / j / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkaɹjoːɁ / 'animal' ⁴²⁹ >
Hu. / ˈxajoːɁ / <gaïo>; PNI * / eˈɹjahsaɁ / 'heart' ²¹² > Hu. / eˈjahʃaɁ / <Eiachia>;
PNI * / aˈɹjaːwẽhk / 'it would have happened' ²²¹ > Hu. / aˈjaːwẽhk / <aia8enk> ),
and similarly, by HU-20, * / w / has been lost before * / j / ( e.g. PNI * / ˈkawjaɁ /
'wing' ⁴⁸⁰ > Hu. / ˈxajaɁ / <gaïa>; PNI * / kaˈwjẽːnaɁ / 'manner' ⁴⁸⁴ >
Hu. / xaˈjẽːnaɁ / <gaïenda>; PNI * / keˈwjẽhwẽh / 'I know how' ⁴⁸³ >
( e.g. PNI * / ˈwahnshẽːh / 'ten' ¹³⁵ > Hu. / ˈahshẽːh / <A‛sen>; PNI * / wakiˈheːjõːh /
'I am dead' ²⁷⁶ > Hu. / axiˈheːõːh / <a˛iheon>; PNI * / ẽwaˈkiːtaɁw / 'I will sleep' ²⁸⁷ >
313
Hu. / ewaˈxiːtaɁ / <e8a˛ita> ), and this change must precede HU-23.
'five' ²⁷³ > Hu. / ˈwihʃ / <8ich> ), and this change must follow HU-22.
( e.g. PNI * / ˈtswẽːtaːt / 'one day' ¹⁵⁹ ²¹⁹ > Hu. / ˈskwẽːtaːt / <sk8entat>;
* / kehˈshwahẽɁts / 'I hate it' ²⁵⁰ > Hu. / xehˈskhwahẽɁs / <˛esk8ahens> ). This
( e.g. PNI * / waˈkehsɁõh / 'I have finished it' ²⁶⁶ > Hu. / aˈxehʃaɁih / <a˛echiai>;
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from PNI to Common
314
(2) Phonological Changes in the Transition from PNI to Common Huron
Several changes additional to those in (2) have taken place in various Huron
dialects (cf. Steckley 1997 and 2007a:35-45). Six such changes are described below as
315
RoHu. / ˈaɁʃhaɁ / <Acha>; Hu. / ˈohʃɹaɁ / 'winter, year' > RoHu. / ˈohʃhaɁ /
<chraten> 'climb!' ). This change occurred in the Northern Bear, Lorette, and Rock
virtually all Huron dialects but appears to have occurred systematically in Northern
Bear and Lorette Huron, effectively removing * / xʷ / from the phonemic inventory of
those dialects.
By HU-D3, * / x / has been lost before resonant consonants ( e.g. Hu. / aɁˈtexɹeɁ /
'eight' > LoHu. / aɁˈteɹeɁ / <atere> (MS 66:133); Hu. / taˈxwẽːtẽɹ / 'pity us!' >
virtually all Huron dialects, but seems to have occurred systematically in the Northern
316
> NBeHu. / taeɁˈnjaːẽɁs / <taenguiaens> (Thwaites 1896-1901, 10:265);
dialect and appears to have removed / x / from the phonemic inventory of that dialect.
occurred in the Northern Bear, Cord, Southern Bear, and Petun dialects of Huron, but
not in the Rock and Lorette dialects. (cf. also Mithun 1985:505, Steckley 2007a:39).
Phonological changes that have occurred in Huron dialects are recapitulated in (3).
317
(3) Phonological Changes in Huron Dialects
In addition to the phonemic changes presented above, several phonetic shifts have
occurred in Huron dialects (cf. Steckley 1997, 2007a:35-45). In the Southern Bear,
Cord, and Petun dialects of Huron the sequence / nɹ / appears to have been realized as
[ ndɹ ] ( e.g. Hu. / oˈnɹrahtaɁ / 'leaf' Æ PeHu. [ oˈndɹahtaɁ ] ). In the Southern Bear and
Cord dialects of Huron the phoneme / w / appears to have been realized as [ m ] when
SBeHu. [ kjothõmaˈihʃaɁ ] ). In the Northern Bear, Southern Bear, and Petun dialects of
Huron the cluster / nj / seems to have been realized as [ ŋj ] ( rather than [ ndj ] )
when it preceded a non-nasal vowel ( e.g. Hu. / ˈnjaːɹeɁ / 'first' Æ PeHu. [ ˈŋjaːɹeɁ ] ).
318
16.4 MORPHOLOGICAL CHANGES
To prepronominal prefix position class 1 Huron has added an intensive prefix / its /,
which is perhaps a reflex of the PNI particle * / ˈitswaɁ / 'much, many, a lot' ²¹³
<iskatsi8a˛en> 'it smells very bad' (Steckley 2004:6) ). Initial * / n / of the partitive
preserved in Huron; see 2.4 ), and initial * / tsh / of the contrastive prepronominal
prefix has become / ʃ / in Huron, rather than the expected / sh /. Allomorphs of the
factual prefix ( PNI * / waɁ / ⁰⁰⁶ ) have lost / w / in all environments in Huron, and an
most dialects, a development also common to at least some dialects of Old Mohawk
'il retourne' from MS 59:155 ). Also in common with some dialects of Old Mohawk,
Huron had a cislocative allomorph / et / ⁰¹⁰ that seems to have occurred word-initially
before a following consonant, e.g. / etjoɁˈsẽɁnih / 'it has fallen from above'. The vowel
of the future prepronominal prefix ( PNI * / ẽ / ⁰⁰⁸ ) appears to have lost nasality and
became / e /, while the optative prefix ( PNI * / aɹa / ⁰⁰⁹ ) lost * / ɹ / in all allomorphs,
319
leaving lexicalized long / aː / before consonants other than * / j / as in the Five Nations
languages. From the nominalizer particle / neɁ / ³⁵², the temporal particle / ˈoːnẽh / ³⁶⁷,
and the relativizer particle / stiɁ /, Huron developed clitics / n- /, / on- /, and / st- /
'il ē tard' (MS 59:129); / steohˈʃɹatheɁ / <st’eochaθe> 'l’hyver prochain' (MS 59:181),
The non-singular animate prefix has been preserved in Huron and acquired a gender
distinction whereby the reflex of PNI * { ka } ⁰¹² signals neuter or feminine gender
antecedent, the Huron non-singular animate prefixes precede pronominal prefixes with
an argument '3' rather than '3I', so that { xa } 'NSA' was added not to { khe } '1SG:3I'
but to { xe } '1SG:3' ( i.e. PNI * / kaˈkheːkẽh / 'I see them' ³³² Æ Hu. * / xaˈxeːxẽh /
'I see them (females)', and * / haˈxeːxẽh / 'I see them (males)', rather than
( before the group of second person pronominal prefixes described in 4.4 ). Huron
320
/ ha / 'NSAM' had allomorphs / hõ / and / he / in the same environments.
'3SGM:3' ⁰⁵³ Æ Hu. / ha / ) except in allomorphs of the 3SGM:3 prefix that occurred
before bases beginning in vowels other than / a(ː) / or / i(ː) / (cf. Woodbury 1981:108).
Reflexes of the 3PL:3 prefixes * { wati } ⁰⁵⁸ and * { kõti } ⁰⁵⁹ have merged in Huron as a
environments. The role of the 1+2NS:3I prefix * { jethi } ⁰⁴⁸ was subsumed by the
reflex of the PNI prefix * { jakhi } '1+3NS:3I' ⁰⁴⁹ in Huron to yield { akhi } '1NS:3I'.
From the agent pronominal prefix { je } '3I:3' ⁰⁶³ Huron appears to have innovated a
prefix { she } '3I:3SGM', probably by analogy with the patient pronominal prefix
pairing, { jaxo } '3:3I' ⁰⁶⁴ and { shaxo } '3SGM:3I' ⁰⁶⁵. The roles of the prefixes
* { jakoti } '3NS:3I' ⁰⁷⁰ and * { shakoti } '3NSM:3I' ⁰⁷¹, confined to Iroquoia, were in
seem to have increased in Huron relative to PNI. Consonants and clusters before which
321
(4) Distribution of Prefix Allomorphs with Epenthetic / e / in Huron (1SG:3)
/i/ _nV.
/ xe / _jV ( < PNI * wjV, ɹjV ), ɹV ( < PNI * wɹV ), and all remaining
consonants and clusters.
PNI * / (a)kw / ⁰⁸⁴ with unexplained loss of * / k /. Huron derivational and aspectual
perfect aspect verb bases ended in / -ih / ¹¹⁹ rather than / -õh / ¹¹⁵ or / -ẽh / ¹¹⁶; no
distinction appears to have been made between habitual and present tense forms and,
as in Onondaga, the PNI present suffix * { -h } ¹⁰⁸ seems to have become { -k } in order
to better differentiate habitual forms from perfect forms in * { -õh } ¹¹⁵, * { -ẽh } ¹¹⁶,
and * { -ih } ¹¹⁹. Likewise, habitual bases in / -was / regularly have punctual
counterparts in / -waɁ / rather than / -oɁ /, and reflexes of PNI punctual bases that
from Wyandot ) in Huron ( e.g. PNI * / tõtsahˈɹaːtaɁn / 'he took his place again' ⁴⁴⁹ >
322
Northern Iroquoian languages and no attested innovations. Among the few suffixes that
are attested are locative { -xeh } ¹²⁸ and populative { -ɹoːnõɁ } ¹²⁹.
323
CHAPTER 17: WYANDOT
17.1 INTRODUCTION
of one language' ), descendants of Huron and Petun refugees who were displaced by war
with the Iroquois after 1650 (cf. Tooker 1978:398). Wyandot most likely descends from
the Petun dialect of Huron (cf. Kopris 2001:361), but has diverged enough from
assumed a form distinct from Huron at some point between the mid-eighteenth century,
when a grammar based on the Petun dialect of Huron was composed by the Jesuit
missionary Pierre Potier (1708-1781), and the mid-nineteenth century, when the
Oklahoma and by expatriate communities in Kansas, Ohio, and Essex County, Ontario,
is now extinct, but it is preserved in forty narratives that were recorded in the early
twentieth century and published in Barbeau (1960). Kopris (2001) compiled a grammar
and dictionary based on the Barbeau narratives. Unless otherwise stated, Wyandot
forms in this chapter are based on data from the narratives in Barbeau (1960).
324
17.2 PHONEMIC INVENTORY
CONSONANTS
Plosive t k kʷ Ɂ
Nasal n
Fricative s ʃ ʒ h
Affricate ts
Approximant ɹ j w
VOWELS
Close iː uː
i u
Open aː
a
325
/ o(ː) / has shifted to / u(ː) /. The nasal vowels / ẽ(ː) / and / õ(ː) / have shifted to
/ ɛ̃(ː) / and / ɔ̃(ː) / (cf. Barbeau 1960:57). Contrastive vowel length seems to have been
lost in final syllables but was introduced elsewhere in the language following loss of
when preceded or followed by a nasal vowel ( e.g. Wy. / ɔ̃watɔ̃ːˈwɛ̃tsaɁ / 'our land' Æ
[ ɔ̃matɔ̃ːˈmɛ̃tsaɁ ] ), although an argument can also be made for the marginal phonemic
vowel has been dropped ( e.g. Wy. [ ˈmɛ̃Ɂjeh ] 'in the water', from Hu. / aˈwẽɁxeh / ¹⁹⁶ )
(cf. Kopris 2001:53). The phoneme / ʒ / had an unusual ( but regular ) allophone [ ɲ ]
( e.g. Wy. / haˈʒɛ̃ːwih / 'he knows how' Æ [ haˈɲɛ̃ːmih ]; / eːʒaːˈʒuɁ / 'he will kill you' Æ
326
[ hahˈʃɹɔ̃ŋjah ] ). Note, however, that an argument can also be made that / n / and
(cf. Kopris 2001:73). Pronunciation of the vowel / ɛ̃(ː) / approached [ ã(ː) ] when the
Curiously many, but not all, reflexes of words that had long vowels in final syllables
( e.g. Hu. / ˈteniːh / 'two' ⁴³⁸ > Wy. / teˈnih /; Hu. / ˈahshẽːh / 'ten' ¹³⁵ >
Wy. / ahˈsɛ̃h /; Hu. / ˈxajoːɁ / 'animal' ⁴²⁹ > Wy. / jaˈʒuɁ / or / jaˈʃuɁ /;
Hu. / xaˈtoːkẽːh / 'it is certain' ⁴⁴⁸ > Wy. / jatuːˈjɛ h̃ /; Hu. / ˈwẽːnaːt / 'Huron' ¹⁵⁹ ⁴⁷⁶ >
PNI word accent may already have been present in Huron, but cannot be discerned
Phonological changes that have occurred during the transition from Huron to
327
Wyandot are described below as WY-1 through WY-18. Where ordering of any two
noted.
( e.g. Hu. * / tjohˈnhõhskʷeɹõːt / 'it has jowls protruding' ²³⁵ > Wy. / kjuˈtɔ̃hskʷeɹɔ̃t /
'snake' ⁴⁴⁵ > Wy. / kjuɁˈʒɛ̃tsih /; Hu. / tjohˈskẽːẽɁ / 'it is nearby' ⁴⁴⁴ >
Wy. / kjuhˈskɛ̃ːʒɛ̃Ɂ /; Hu. / oˈtjohkʷaɁ / 'crowd' ²⁸⁸ > Wy. / uˈkjuhkʷaɁ / ). This change
was already present in the Petun dialect of Huron (cf. HU-D6 in 16.3) from which
Wyandot descends.
By WY-3, * / xʷ / has become * / w / ( e.g. Hu. / aˈxõːxʷeh / 'she is a person' ⁴⁰⁵ >
Wy. / ajɔ̃ːˈweh / ). This change was earlier common to at least the Lorette and Northern
Bear dialects of Huron (cf. HU-D2 in 16.3), with sporadic attestation in other dialects.
( e.g. Hu. * / tjohˈnhõhskʷeɹõːt / 'it has jowls protruding' ²³⁵ > Wy. / kjuˈtɔ̃hskʷeɹɔ̃t /
'cow'; Hu. / oɁˈnhõhʃaɁ / 'egg' ⁴⁹⁰ > Wy. / uˈtɔ̃hʃaɁ /; Hu. / xahˈnhohwaɁ / 'door' ²³³ >
328
Wy. / jatuːˈwaɁ / ). This change must precede WY-5 and WY-7.
'that kind of life' ⁴⁰¹ ⁴⁰⁶ > Wy. / iˈʒɔ̃tuɁtɛ̃Ɂ /; Hu. / oɁˈnhõhʃaɁ / 'egg' ⁴⁹⁰ >
Wy. / uˈtɔ̃hʃaɁ /; Hu. / xahˈnhohwaɁ / 'door' ²³³ > Wy. / jatuːˈwaɁ / ). This change must
( e.g. Hu. / otsiˈkheɁtaɁ / 'sugar' ⁴⁵⁶ > Wy. / uˈtsikeɁtaɁ /; Hu. / thatiˈnaxɹeɁ /
'they live there' ³⁵⁴ > Wy. / tatiˈnaːɹeɁ /; Hu. / shaxoˈẽhtih / 'he has hit someone' ³⁰⁷ >
By WY-7, * / h / has been lost between a vowel and a resonant consonant, and the
vowel has been lengthened ( e.g. Hu. / aɁˈxehnɹẽx / 'I tied it' ²⁴⁰ > Wy. / aɁˈjeːnɹɛ̃ /;
Hu. * / nõsahjatɹihwahˈʃɹõnjaɁ / 'now again they made an agreement' ²⁶⁰ ⁴²⁷ >
Wy. / nusaːʒaˈtɹiːwahʃɹɔ̃njaɁ / ). This change must follow WY-4 and precede WY-12.
By WY-8, * / w / has been inserted between a rounded vowel and a following vowel
( e.g. Hu. / axoˈẽɁah / 'her child' ³¹³ > Wy. / ajuˈwɛ̃Ɂah /; Hu. / aɁhoeˈnaːõɁ /
'he threw them out' ¹⁷⁴ > Wy. / aɁsajuwaɁˈtɔ̃ːtiɁ / ), and this change must precede
329
WY-10. Similarly, by WY-9, * / j / has been inserted between a front vowel and a
following vowel ( e.g. Hu. / tehstiˈahjaɁx / 'you cross over' ²⁷⁷ > Wy. / tehstiˈʒaːʒaɁ /;
Hu. / õxiˈẽkhwih / 'we have planted it' ³¹¹ > Wy. / ɔ̃iˈʒɛ̃kwih /; Hu. * / iõhwẽˈtsoɁtẽh /
'that kind of land' ⁴⁰¹ ⁴⁰² > Wy. / iʒɔ̃ːwɛ̃ˈtsuɁtɛ̃h / ). Since phonetically this change is
'we have planted it' ³¹¹ > Wy. / ɔ̃iˈʒɛ̃kwih /; Hu. / axeˈnjaːkih / 'I am married' ³⁷² >
Wy. / ajeˈnjaːih /; Hu. / teɁxjẽˈteːɹih / 'I don't know' ³¹⁰ > Wy. / teɁʒɛ̃ˈteːɹih / ). This
( e.g. Hu. / ˈxɹiːjohʃ / 'I kill it' ⁴²⁸ > Wy. / eɹiːˈʒuhʃ /; Hu. * / xɹihwahˈɹõkhaɁ / 'I hear a
story' ¹³⁸ ⁴²⁷ > Wy. / eɹiːwaːˈɹɔ̃kaɁ / ). This change must precede WY-14.
stop, or vowel, and preceded a vowel ( e.g. Hu. / tehstiˈahjaɁx / 'you cross over' ²⁷⁷ >
Wy. / tehstiˈʒaːʒaɁ /; Hu. / ˈxajoːɁ / 'animal' ⁴²⁹ > Wy. / jaˈʒuɁ /; Hu. / ehˈjajoɁ /
'he will kill you' ⁴²⁸ > Wy. / eːʒaːˈʒuɁ / ). This change must follow WY-7 and WY-9, and
precede WY-13. However, note that in at least one instance WY-13 appears to feed
330
WY-12: from Hu. * / aɁxõxõhʃoˈɹẽhaɁ /, literally 'I found your face', one finds both a
(Barbeau 1960:177). This suggests that WY-12 and WY-13 may have overlapped to
By WY-13, * / x / has become * / j / before a vowel ( e.g. Hu. / ˈxanxõh / 'blood' ³⁷⁶
> Wy. / jaˈnjɔ̃h /; Hu. / aˈtoːxẽɁ / 'axe' ¹⁸² > Wy. / aˈtuːjɛɁ̃ /; Hu. / xaaɁtaˈxẽhtsihk /
'old woman' ²⁹⁸ ³³³ > Wy. / jaˈaɁtajɛ̃htsih /; Hu. / xõtaˈɹaɁxeh / 'at the lake' ⁴¹² >
Wy. / jɔ̃taˈɹaɁjeh / ). This change must follow WY-12 and must precede WY-14.
By WY-14, * / x / has been lost elsewhere ( e.g. Hu. / aɁˈhahɹõx / 'he heard' ¹³⁸ >
Wy. / aɁˈhaːɹɔ̃ /; Hu. / tehstihˈtiːaɁx / 'you cross a field' ²⁷⁷ > Wy. / tehstihˈtiːʒaɁ /;
Hu. / thatiˈnaxɹeɁ / 'they live there' ³⁵⁴ > Wy. / tatiˈnaːɹeɁ /; Hu. / õsaˈxɹiːjoɁ /
'I killed again' ⁴²⁸ > Wy. / usaɹiːˈʒuɁ / ). This change must follow WY-10, WY-11, and
WY-13.
By WY-15, * / k / has been lost word-finally ( e.g. Hu. / tjoɁˈxjẽtsihk / 'snake' ⁴⁴⁵ >
Wy. / kjuɁˈʒɛ̃tsih /; Hu. / xaaɁtaˈxẽhtsihk / 'old woman' ²⁹⁸ ³³³ > Wy. / jaaɁtaˈjɛ̃htsih /;
Hu. * / hateɁˈnõnjahk / 'he makes arrows' ²⁶⁰ ⁴⁸⁸ > Wy. / hateɁˈnɔ̃njah / ).
331
By WY-16, * / ẽ(ː) / has shifted to * / ɛ (̃ ː) / ( e.g. Hu. / ohskẽˈnõːtõɁ / 'deer' ²⁵⁴ >
Wy. / uhskɛ̃ˈnɔ̃ːtɔ̃Ɂ /; Hu. / ˈskwẽːtaːt / 'one day' ¹⁵⁹ ²¹⁹ > Wy. / ˈskwẽːtat /;
Hu. / teɁxjẽˈteːɹih / 'I don't know' ³¹⁰ > Wy. / teɁʒɛ̃ˈteːɹih / ), and by WY-17, * / õ(ː) /
has shifted to * / ɔ̃(ː) / ( e.g. Hu. / xaˈnõhʃaɁ / 'house' ³⁸² > Wy. / jaˈnɔ̃hʃaɁ /;
Hu. / aˈxõːxʷeh / 'she is a person' ⁴⁰⁵ > Wy. / ajɔ̃ːˈweh /; Hu. / tahatiˈɹõːtẽɁ /
By WY-18, * / o(ː) / has shifted to * / u(ː) / ( e.g. Hu. / oˈtjohkʷaɁ / 'crowd' ²⁸⁸ >
Wy. / uˈkjuhkʷaɁ /; Hu. / aˈtoːxẽɁ / 'axe' ¹⁸² > Wy. / aˈtuːjɛ̃Ɂ /; Hu. / tjoˈnõːtoːt /
Phonological changes that have occurred in the transition from Common Huron to
332
WY-12 j > ʒ / #, Ɂ, V_V
WY-13 x > j / _V
WY-14 x>Ø
WY-15 k > Ø / _#
WY-16 ẽ(ː) > ɛ̃(ː)
WY-17 õ(ː) > ɔ̃(ː)
WY-18 o(ː) > u(ː)
morphology appears to have been essentially the same as that of Huron, though the
Wyandot ( e.g. Wy. / utaˈʒaɁkjɔ̃Ɂ / 'I am bringing [the dog] in' ³¹⁵ ; / usahɔ̃tɹiːˈʒuɁ /
333
CHAPTER 18: FRAGMENTARY LANGUAGES
18.1 INTRODUCTION
In all, about seventeen Iroquoian languages are thought to have been spoken at the
time of first European contact ( at least sixteen Northern Iroquoian languages plus
Cherokee ), but records for four of these languages are fragmentary or non-existent, and
two nations that were identified as speakers of Iroquoian languages in the seventeenth
century, called in Northern Bear Huron <Conkhandeenrhonon> 'Nation of the ? ' and
conclusively.
seventeenth century, excluding those spoken in Virginia and North Carolina, is based
(Thwaites 1896-1901, 8:114), in which Father Jean de Brébeuf indicates that Huron
There are twenty [ Huron ] Towns, which indicate about 30,000 souls
speaking the same tongue, which is not difficult to one who has a master.
It has distinction of genders, number, tense, person, moods; and, in short,
it is very complete and very regular, contrary to the opinion of many. I am
rejoiced to find that this language is common to some twelve other Nations,
all settled and numerous; these are, the Conkhandeenrhonons [ ? ],
334
Khionontaterrhonons [ Petun ], Atiouandaronks [ Neutral ], Sonontoerrhonons
[ Seneca ], Onontaerrhonons [ Onondaga ], Oüioenrhonons [ Cayuga ],
Onoiochrhonons [ Oneida ], Agnierrhonons [ Mohawk ], Andastoerrhonons
[ Susquehannock ], Scahentoarrhonons [ ? ], Rhiierrhonons [ Erie ], and
Ahouenrochrhonons [ Wenro ]. The Hurons are friends of all these people,
except the Sonontoerrhonons, Onontaerrhonons, Oüioenrhonons,
Onoiochrhonons and Agnierrhonons, all of whom we comprise under the
name Hiroquois.
To the languages mentioned may be added Laurentian, which was already extinct
all spoken further to the south. In analyzing Petun and Huron as dialects of a single
18.2 MEHERRIN
The territory of the Meherrin in the early seventeenth century seems to have ranged
from the Blue Ridge Mountains in the west, bordering on the territory of the Catawba,
to the mouth of the Blackwater River in the east, bordering on the territory of the
Chowan, north to the Nottoway River, bordering on the territory of the Nottoway, and
335
(cf. Binford 1967:116, Boyce 1978:282; see Map 3). The Meherrin appear to have
The names of two Meherrin settlements, given during legal depositions in Virginia
in 1707, represent all that is known to survive of the Meherrin language. The first of
(Binford 1967:153). Both names appear to begin with reflexes of third person
word-initially in nouns ). The first name is doubtless the source of the Tuscarora word
Meherrin between the territories of the Tuscarora and Nottoway, it seems likely that the
18.3 WENRO
The territory of the Wenro in the early seventeenth century seems to have ranged
from the Niagara River in the west, bordering on the territory of the Neutral, to the
Genesee River in the east, bordering on the territory of the Seneca, north to Lake
Ontario, and south to Buffalo Creek, bordering on the territory of the Erie
336
(cf. White 1978a:407, Trigger 1987:96-97; see Map 8). The Wenro were known to the
translate as 'Nation of the moss in the water' or 'Nation of the floating scum', and have
led to a belief, as yet unsubstantiated, that the primary Wenro village was once located
at an oil springs near modern Cuba, New York (Hewitt 1912:933). Beginning in the late
1630s, their traditional protective alliance with the Neutral and Erie having lapsed, the
Wenro were driven from their territory by the Iroquois Confederacy and survivors
Roche Dallion in 1627 (Le Clerq 1882:268), although this was more likely the
ethnonym for informants who had come from there ( Hu. <8enro‛ronon> 'Wenro' ).
18.4 ERIE
The territory of the Erie in the early seventeenth century seems to have ranged at
least from Presque Isle Bay in the west, bordering on the territory of unknown tribes, to
the Genesee River in the east, bordering on the territory of the Seneca, north to Buffalo
Creek, bordering on the territory of the Wenro, and south into what is now
337
Trigger 1987:96; see Map 8). The Erie were known to the Huron as / eɹiɁxehˈɹoːnõɁ /
'Nation at the Cherry Trees' (cf. Wright 1972:63). The Erie survived as a political entity
until being defeated by the Iroquois Confederacy in 1656, after which survivors, among
them many Wenro and Neutral refugees, were assimilated into the ranks of their
conquerors.
No words of the Erie language are known to have been recorded, although the
personal name <Gandeaktena> (Thwaites 1896-1901, 61:193) and the place names
61:193), all of which were recorded by Jesuits living among the Iroquois Proper after
the conquest of the Erie, could conceivably represent authentic Erie names.
18.5 NEUTRAL
The territory of the Neutral in the early seventeenth century seems to have ranged
from Lake Saint Clair in the west, bordering on the territories of the Fox and Kickapoo,
to the Credit and Niagara Rivers in the east, bordering on the territories of the Huron
338
and Wenro, north to the Grand River Valley, bordering on the territories of the
Ottawa and Petun, and south to Lake Erie (cf. White 1978a:407, Trigger 1987:94-95;
see Map 8). The Neutral were known to the Huron as <Atiouandaronk> meaning
cf. Hu. / atiwẽnahˈɹõkhaɁ / 'they understand the language' ). The Neutral continued to
exist as a nation until the early 1650s when they were defeated by the Iroquois
Confederacy and survivors, for the most part, were assimilated into the ranks of their
conquerors.
Two words of what may represent authentic Neutral are preserved in a letter sent to
France in 1627 by Jesuit Father Joseph de la Roche Dallion, who had spent several
months residing in Neutral towns the previous year: the name of a town,
<Ounontisaston>, and its chief, <Souharissen> (Le Clercq 1882: 265, 269). From
these words, the first perhaps meaning 'Mountain Corner' ( Hu. / onõtiˈsahtih / ),
one can discern that Neutral was indeed an Iroquoian language, was rhotic
( i.e. retained PNI * / ɹ /, unlike Seneca and Modern Onondaga ), had causative perfects
in * { -õh } ( in contrast to the { -ih } of Huron and Onondaga ), and realized the reflex
339
APPENDIX A: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY
340
/ j /, and before remaining consonants in an imperative verb, and
before the duplicative prefix; / shi / elsewhere.
Oe. / tsh / before the translocative prefix, and before vowels; / tshũ / before
/ wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before the
cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / tshe / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / tsha / before / h / in an
imperative verb; / tshaɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ j /, and before remaining consonants in an imperative verb, and before
the duplicative prefix; / tshi / elsewhere.
Oo. / ts / before the translocative prefix; / tsh / before the factual prefix, and
before vowels; / tshũ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ wa /, and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs
not containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / tsheɁ / after the
negative prefix, and before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an
imperative verb; / tshaɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ j /, and before the duplicative prefix; / tshi / before consonants
elsewhere.
Ca. / ts / before the translocative prefix; / tsh / before vowels; / tshõ / before
/ wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before the
cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / tsheɁ / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / tsha / before / h / in an
imperative verb; / tshaɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ j /, and before remaining consonants in an imperative verb, and before
the duplicative prefix; / tshi / before consonants elsewhere.
Se. / ts / or / s / before the translocative prefix; / tsh / or / sh / before vowels;
/ tshɔ̃ / or / shɔ̃ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ wa /, and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs
not containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / tsheɁ / or / sheɁ /
before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb;
/ tshaː / or / shaː / before / hC / in an imperative verb with loss of / h /;
/ tsha / or / sha / before / hV / in an imperative verb with loss of / h /;
/ tshaɁ / or / shaɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of / j /,
and before the duplicative prefix; / tshaɁ / or / shaɁ / before remaining
consonants in an imperative verb; / tshiː / or / shiː / before / hC / with
341
loss of / h /; / tshi / or / shi / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before
remaining consonants elsewhere.
Hu. / ʃ / before vowels; / ʃõ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ wa /, and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs
not containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / ʃe / before certain
pronominal prefixes; / ʃaɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb with loss
of / j /, and before the duplicative prefix; / ʃi / before consonants
elsewhere.
342
Oo. / t / before the translocative prefix; / th / before the factual prefix, and
before vowels; / thũ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ wa /, and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs
not containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / theɁ / before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / thaɁ / before / j /
in an imperative verb with loss of / j /, and before the duplicative prefix;
/ thi / before consonants elsewhere.
Ca. / t / before the translocative prefix; / th / before vowels; / thõ / before
/ wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before the
cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / theɁ / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / tha / before / h / in an
imperative verb; / thaɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ j /, and before remaining consonants in an imperative verb, and before
the duplicative prefix; / thi / before consonants elsewhere.
Se. / th / before vowels; / thɔ̃ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ wa /, and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs
not containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / theɁ / before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / thaː / before
/ hC / in an imperative verb with loss of / h /; / tha / before / hV / in an
imperative verb with loss of / h /; / thaɁ / before / j / in an imperative
verb with loss of / j /, and before remaining consonants in an imperative
verb, and before the duplicative prefix; / thiː / before / hC / with loss of
/ h /; / thi / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before remaining
consonants elsewhere.
Hu. / th / before vowels; / thõ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ wa /, and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs
not containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / the / before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / thaɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb
with loss of / j /, and before the duplicative prefix; / thi / before
consonants elsewhere.
343
( probably ) before consonants elsewhere.
Ch. / ìj / before vowels in deverbalized nouns and dependent verb forms; / ǐː /
before consonants in deverbalized nouns and dependent verb forms; / n /
before vowels elsewhere, and before / h / elsewhere; / nì / before
remaining consonants elsewhere.
PNI ( here hypothesized to derive from PI * / n + ijiː / 'partitive
(independent form)+partitive (dependent form)', with Huron perhaps
retaining reflexes of * / ijiː / 'partitive (dependent form) without initial
* / n / ): * / nõ / before * / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of
* / wa /, and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative
verbs not containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; * / nwe / before
certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; * / nwaɁ /
before * / j / in an imperative verb with loss of * / j /, and before
remaining consonants in an imperative verb; * / naɁ / before the
duplicative prefix elsewhere; * / n / before vowels and glides elsewhere
( cf. Tu. / tj / 'partitive+translocative'; Oo. / nwaɁ /
'partitive+factual' ); * / ni / before consonants elsewhere.
Tu. / nə̃ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before the
cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / thwɛ / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / thwa / before / h / in an
imperative verb; / thwaɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ j /, and before remaining consonants in an imperative verb; / taɁ /
before the duplicative prefix elsewhere; / th / before / w / elsewhere;
/ n / before / jə̃ / elsewhere, and before vowels; / t / before / j /
elsewhere; / ti / before remaining consonants elsewhere.
Mo. / n / before the translocative prefix, and before vowels; / nũ / before / wa /
in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before the cislocative or
repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the 1+2DU:3 or
1+2PL:3 prefixes; / ne / before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in
an imperative verb; / na / before / h / in an imperative verb; / naɁ /
before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of / j /, and before remaining
consonants in an imperative verb, and before the duplicative prefix;
/ ni / before consonants elsewhere.
Oe. [ as in Mohawk ].
Oo. / n / before the translocative prefix, and before the factual prefix, and
344
before vowels; / nũ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ wa /, and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs
not containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / nweɁ / before
certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / nwaɁ /
before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of / j /, and before remaining
consonants in an imperative verb; / naɁ / before the duplicative prefix
elsewhere; / ni / before consonants elsewhere.
Ca. / n / before the translocative prefix, and before vowels; / nõ / before / wa /
in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before the cislocative or
repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the 1+2DU:3 or
1+2PL:3 prefixes; / neɁ / before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 )
in an imperative verb; / na / before / h / in an imperative verb; / naɁ /
before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of / j /, and before remaining
consonants in an imperative verb, and before the duplicative prefix;
/ ni / before consonants elsewhere.
Se. / n / before vowels; / nɔ̃ː / before / hC / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ h /; / nɔ̃ / before / hV / in an imperative verb with loss of / h /, and
before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before the
cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / neɁ / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / nɔ̃Ɂ / before / j / in an
imperative verb with loss of / j /, and before the duplicative prefix;
/ nɔ̃Ɂ / before remaining consonants in an imperative verb; / niː / before
/ hC / with loss of / h /; / ni / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and
before remaining consonants elsewhere.
Hu. / Ø / before vowels; / õ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ wa /, and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs
not containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / e / before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / aɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb
with loss of / j /, and before the duplicative prefix; / i / before
consonants elsewhere.
⁰⁰⁵ PI * { wi } 'translocative'
PI * / w / before vowels; * / wi / before at least some consonants;
( elsewhere = ? )
Ch. / w / before vowels, and before / h /; / wì / before remaining consonants.
345
PNI ( here hypothesized to derive from PI * / wi / preceded by a cliticized
particle with the form * / jah / ): * / j(a)hõ / before * / wa / in an
imperative verb with loss of * / wa /, and before the cislocative or
repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the 1+2DU:3 or
1+2PL:3 prefixes; * / j(a)hwe / before certain pronominal prefixes
( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; * / j(a)hwaɁ / before * / j / in an
imperative verb with loss of * / j /, and before remaining consonants in
an imperative verb; * / jaɁ / before the duplicative prefix elsewhere;
* / jah / before the factual prefix; * / j(a)hw / before vowels;
* / j(a)hwe / before consonants elsewhere.
Tu. / ə̃ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before the
cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / wɛ / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / wa / before / h / in an
imperative verb; / waɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of
/ j /, and before remaining consonants in an imperative verb; / jaɁ /
before the duplicative prefix elsewhere; / jah / before the factual prefix;
/ j / before vowels; / wɛ / before consonants elsewhere.
Mo. / jũ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before
the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / jaˈha / before / h / in an imperative
verb provided word accent falls on / ˈha / as indicated; / jaˈhe / before
certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb, provided
word accent falls on / ˈhe / as indicated; / jaˈhaɁ / before / j / in an
imperative verb with loss of / j /, provided word accent falls on / ˈhaɁ /
as indicated; / ja / before / h / in an imperative verb elsewhere; / je /
before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) in an imperative verb
elsewhere; / jaɁ / before / j / in an imperative verb elsewhere with loss
of / j /, and before remaining consonants in an imperative verb
elsewhere, and before the duplicative prefix; / jaˈh / before an accented
vowel / a / or / e / elsewhere; / jʌ̃ˈh / before an accented vowel / ʌ̃ /
elsewhere; / j / before vowels elsewhere; / je / before consonants
elsewhere.
Oe. [ as in Mohawk ].
Oo. / hũ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before
the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
346
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / hweɁ / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / hwaɁ / before / j / in an
imperative verb with loss of / j /, and before remaining consonants in an
imperative verb; / haɁ / before the duplicative prefix elsewhere; / h /
before the factual prefix, and before vowels; / he / before consonants
elsewhere.
Ca. / hõ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before
the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / heɁ / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / haɁ / before the duplicative
prefix, and before / j / in an imperative verb with loss of / j /, and before
remaining consonants in an imperative verb, and before the duplicative
prefix; / h / before vowels; / he / before consonants elsewhere.
Se. / hɔ̃ / before / wa / in an imperative verb with loss of / wa /, and before the
cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not containing the
1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / heɁ / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) in an imperative verb; / haː / before / hC / in an
imperative verb with loss of / h /; / ha / before / hV / in an imperative
verb with loss of / h /; / haɁ / before the duplicative prefix, and before
/ j / in an imperative verb with loss of / j /, and before remaining
consonants in an imperative verb; / h / before vowels and glides
elsewhere; / heː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / he / before / hV /
with loss of / h /, and before remaining consonants elsewhere.
Hu. / Ø / in imperative verbs; / haɁ / before the duplicative prefix; / h / before
vowels; / he / before consonants elsewhere.
347
consonants elsewhere.
Mo. / Ø / word-initially before the cislocative or repetitive prefix; / ũ / before
/ wa / with loss of / wa /, and before the repetitive or cislocative prefix
elsewhere; / we / word-initially before certain pronominal prefixes
( see 4.4 ); / e / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere;
/ wa / word-initially before / h /; / a / before / h / elsewhere; / waɁ /
word-initially before / j / with loss of / j /, and word-initially before
remaining consonants; / aɁ / before / j / elsewhere with loss of / j /, and
before remaining consonants elsewhere.
Oe. [ as in Mohawk ].
Oo. / Ø / word-initially before the cislocative or repetitive prefix; / ũ / before
/ wa / with loss of / wa /, and before the repetitive or cislocative prefix
elsewhere; / eɁ / following the coincident or contrastive prefix before
certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / weɁ / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ) elsewhere; / aɁ / following the coincident or
contrastive prefix before / j / with loss of / j /, and following the
coincident or contrastive prefix before remaining consonants; / waɁ /
before / j / elsewhere with loss of / j /, and before remaining consonants
elsewhere.
Ca. / Ø / word-initially before the cislocative or repetitive prefix; / õ / before
/ wa / with loss of / wa /, and before the repetitive or cislocative prefix
elsewhere; / eɁ / before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / a /
before / h / elsewhere; / aɁ / before / j / elsewhere with loss of / j /,
and before remaining consonants elsewhere.
Se. / Ø / word-initially before the cislocative or repetitive prefix; / ɔ̃ / before the
cislocative or repetitive prefix elsewhere; / eɁ / before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / waː / word-initially before / hC / with
loss of / h /; / ɔ̃ː / following the partitive prefix before / hC / with loss
of / h /; / aː / before / hC / elsewhere with loss of / h /; / wa /
word-initially before / hV / with loss of / h /; / ɔ̃ / following the
partitive prefix before / hV / with loss of / h /; / a / before / hV /
elsewhere with loss of / h /; / waɁ / word-initially before / j / with loss
of / j /; / ɔ̃Ɂ / following the partitive prefix before / j / with loss of / j /;
/ aɁ / before / j / elsewhere with loss of / j /; / oɁ / elsewhere.
Hu. / Ø / word-initially before the cislocative or repetitive prefix ( but more
usually / õ / in this environment ); / õ / before / wa / with loss of
348
/ wa /, and before the repetitive or cislocative prefix elsewhere; / e /
before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / aɁ / before / j / with loss
of / j /, and before remaining consonants elsewhere.
349
imperative pronominal prefixes; / t / before vowels, and following the
factual prefix before / hV /, / jV /, or the '1SG:3' pronominal prefix
allomorph / k /; / te / elsewhere.
Oe. [ as in Mohawk ].
Oo. / tũ / following the factual prefix before the cislocative or repetitive prefix,
and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not
containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes, and before the 2:1
imperative pronominal prefixes; / ti / following the factual prefix before
certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / t / before vowels, and following
the factual prefix elsewhere; / te / elsewhere.
Ca. / tõ / following the factual prefix before the cislocative or repetitive prefix,
and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not
containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes, and before the 2:1
imperative pronominal prefixes; / ti / following the factual prefix before
certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / t / before vowels, and following
the factual prefix elsewhere; / te / elsewhere.
Se. / tɔ̃ / following the factual prefix before the cislocative or repetitive prefix,
and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not
containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes, and before the 2:1
imperative pronominal prefixes; / ti / following the factual prefix before
certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ), / t / before vowels, and following
the factual prefix elsewhere; / teː / before / hC / elsewhere with loss of
/ h /; / te / before / hV / elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before
remaining consonants elsewhere.
Hu. / tõ / following the factual prefix before the cislocative or repetitive prefix,
and before the cislocative or repetitive prefix in imperative verbs not
containing the 1+2DU:3 or 1+2PL:3 prefixes; / ti / following the factual
prefix before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ), and in imperative
verbs elsewhere; / k / following the factual prefix before / w /; / Ø /
following the factual prefix before / k / or / s /; / t / before vowels, and
following the factual prefix elsewhere; / te / elsewhere.
⁰⁰⁸ PI * { ẽː } 'future'
PI * / ẽː /;
Ch. / òː / ( synchronically absent; preserved in the combination / tòːt /
'duplicative+cislocative ).
350
PNI * / ẽ /.
Tu. / ə̃ /.
Mo. / ʌ̃ /.
Oe. / ʌ̃ /.
Oo. / ẽ /.
Ca. / ẽ /.
Se. / ẽː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / ẽ / before / hV / with loss of / h /,
and before remaining consonants elsewhere.
Hu. / e /.
351
with loss of / wa /, and before the repetitive or cislocative prefix; / a /
before / j /; / aː / before remaining consonants elsewhere.
⁰¹⁰ PI * { t } 'cislocative'
PI * / k / or / ka / word-initially in at least some imperative verbs; * / ta /
before at least some consonants in some ( dependent? ) verb forms;
* / t / before at least some vowels and consonants elsewhere;
( elsewhere = ? )
Ch. / k / word-initially in some imperative verbs ( cf. / khěːna̋ː / 'come here!' );
/ t / before the repetitive prefix and before / h /; / tʌ̃ː̀ / in deverbalized
nouns and dependent verb forms before / à / with loss of / à /; / tȁːɁ / in
deverbalized nouns and dependent verb forms before / ì / with loss of
/ ì /; / tàj / in deverbalized nouns and dependent verb forms before
remaining vowels; / tà / in deverbalized nouns and dependent verb
forms before remaining consonants; / tìː / before / ȁː / and / ȉː /
elsewhere with loss of / ȁː / and / ȉː /; / t / before vowels elsewhere;
/ tì / before consonants elsewhere.
PNI * / ka / word-initially in at least some imperative verbs; * / ti / before the
1+2DU:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in imperative verbs; * / te /
following the factual prefix or optative prefix before certain pronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ); * / tõ / following the factual prefix or optative prefix
before * / wa / with loss of * / wa /; * / ta / in imperative verbs
elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or optative prefix elsewhere;
* / ti / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere; * / t /
elsewhere.
Tu. / ka / word-initially in some imperative verbs ( cf. / ˈkaːtʃi / 'come!' ); / ni /
word-initially before the 1+2SG:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in
imperative verbs; / Ɂni / before the 1+2SG:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal
prefixes in imperative verbs elsewhere; / Ɂnɛ / following the factual
prefix or optative prefix before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 );
/ Ɂnə̃ / following the factual prefix or optative prefix before / wa / with
loss of / wa /; / na / word-initially in imperative verbs elsewhere;
/ Ɂna / in imperative verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or
optative prefix elsewhere; / ni / word-initially before certain pronominal
prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere; / Ɂni / before certain pronominal prefixes
( 4.4 ) elsewhere; / n / word-initially before / w / or / j /; / Ɂn / before
352
/ w / or / j / elsewhere; / tʃ / before / hs / or / hsj / elsewhere with loss
of / hs / or / hsj /; / t / elsewhere.
Mo. / ka / word-initially in some imperative verbs ( cf. / kaːts / 'come here!' );
/ ti / before the 1+2DU:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in
imperative verbs; / te / following the factual prefix or optative prefix
before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / tũ / following the factual
prefix or optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / ta / in
imperative verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or optative
prefix elsewhere; / ti / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 )
elsewhere; / te / before consonant clusters elsewhere; / t / elsewhere.
Oe. / ka / word-initially in some imperative verbs ( cf. / kaːˈats / 'come here!' );
/ ti / before the 1+2DU:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in
imperative verbs; / te / following the factual prefix or optative prefix
before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / tũ / following the factual
prefix or optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / ta / in
imperative verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or optative
prefix elsewhere; / ti / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 )
elsewhere; / te / before consonant clusters elsewhere; / t / elsewhere.
Oo. / ka / word-initially in some imperative verbs ( cf. / ˈkáːtsih / 'come here!' );
/ ti / before the 1+2DU:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in
imperative verbs; / te / following the factual prefix or optative prefix
before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / tũ / following the factual
prefix or optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / ta / in
imperative verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or optative
prefix elsewhere; / ti / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 )
elsewhere; / t / elsewhere.
Ca. / ka / word-initially in some imperative verbs ( cf. / ˈkaːtsih / 'come here!' );
/ ti / before the 1+2DU:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in
imperative verbs; / te / following the factual prefix or optative prefix
before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / tõ / following the factual
prefix or optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / ta / in
imperative verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or optative
prefix elsewhere; / ti / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 )
elsewhere; / t / elsewhere.
Se. / ka / word-initially in some imperative verbs ( cf. / kaːtsih / 'come here!' );
/ ti / before the 1+2DU:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in
353
imperative verbs; / te / following the factual prefix or optative prefix
before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / tɔ̃ / following the factual
prefix or optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / taː / in
imperative verbs before / hC / with loss of / h /, and following the
factual prefix or optative prefix before / hC / with loss of / h /; / ta / in
imperative verbs before / hV / with loss of / h /, and following the
factual prefix or optative prefix before / hV / with loss of / h /, and
following the factual prefix or optative prefix before remaining
consonants elsewhere, and before remaining consonants in imperative
verbs; / ti / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere; / t /
elsewhere.
Hu. / a / word-initially in imperative verbs; / ti / before the 1+2DU:3 and
1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in imperative verbs; / te / following the
factual prefix or optative prefix before certain pronominal prefixes
( see 4.4 ); / tõ / following the factual prefix or optative prefix before
/ wa / with loss of / wa /; / ta / in imperative verbs, and following the
factual prefix or optative prefix elsewhere; / k / before / wV / elsewhere;
/ Ø / before / k / or / s / elsewhere; / ti / before certain pronominal
prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere; / et / word-initially before a consonant
elsewhere; / t / elsewhere.
⁰¹¹ PI * { ts } 'repetitive'
PI * / tsiː / in certain verb forms before the 1+2DU:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal
prefixes and the 3:2SG pronominal prefix; elsewhere = ?
Ch. / ěːɁ / following the duplicative prefix before / ȅː / or / ȉː / with loss of
/ ȅː / or / ȉː /, and following the duplicative prefix before remaining
vowels; / ěː / following the duplicative prefix before consonants; / tsǐːɁ /
in deverbalized nouns and dependent verb forms before the 1+2DU:3
and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes; / tsǐː / in deverbalized nouns and
dependent verb forms before the 3:2SG pronominal prefix'; / ʌ̃ː́ /
word-initially in deverbalized nouns and dependent verb forms before
/ ì / with loss of / ì /; / hʌ̃ː́ / in deverbalized nouns and dependent verb
forms before / ì / elsewhere with loss of / ì /; / ʌ̃ː̌ Ɂ / word-initially in
deverbalized nouns and dependent verb forms before remaining vowels;
/ hʌ̃ː̌ Ɂ / in deverbalized nouns and dependent verb forms before
remaining vowels elsewhere; / ʌ̃ː̌ / word-initially in deverbalized nouns
354
and dependent verb forms before consonants; / hʌ̃ː̌ / in deverbalized
nouns and dependent verb forms before consonants elsewhere; / íː /
word-initially before / ȉː / elsewhere with loss of / ȉː /; / híː / before
/ ȉː / elsewhere with loss of / ȉː /; / ǐːɁ / word-initially before vowels
elsewhere; / hǐːɁ / before vowels elsewhere; / ǐː / word-initially before
consonants elsewhere; / hǐː / before consonants elsewhere.
PNI * / tsi / before the 1+2SG:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in
imperative verbs; * / tse / following the factual prefix or optative prefix
before certain pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); * / tsõ / following the
factual prefix or optative prefix before * / wa / with loss of * / wa /;
* / tsa / in imperative verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix
or optative prefix elsewhere; * / tsi / before certain pronominal prefixes
( 4.4 ) elsewhere; * / ts / elsewhere.
Tu. / tʃi / before the 1+2SG:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in imperative
verbs; / θɛ / following the factual prefix or optative prefix before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / θə̃ / following the factual prefix or
optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / θa / in imperative
verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or optative prefix
elsewhere; / tʃi / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere;
/ tʃ / before / j / elsewhere; / θ / elsewhere.
Mo. / tsi / before the 1+2SG:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in imperative
verbs; / se / following the factual prefix or optative prefix before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / sũ / following the factual prefix or
optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / sa / in imperative
verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or optative prefix
elsewhere; / tsi / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere;
/ ts / before / j / elsewhere; / s / before / hV / or / kV / elsewhere;
/ se / elsewhere.
Oe. [ as in Mohawk ].
Oo. / tsi / before the 1+2SG:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in imperative
verbs; / se / following the factual prefix or optative prefix before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / sũ / following the factual prefix or
optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / sa / in imperative
verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or optative prefix
elsewhere; / tsi / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere;
/ ts / before / j / elsewhere; / t / before / s / elsewhere; / s / elsewhere.
355
Ca. / tsi / before the 1+2SG:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in imperative
verbs; / se / following the factual prefix or optative prefix before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / sõ / following the factual prefix or
optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / sa / in imperative
verbs elsewhere, and following the factual prefix or optative prefix
elsewhere; / tsi / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere;
/ ts / before / j / elsewhere with loss of / j /; / t / before / s / elsewhere;
/ s / elsewhere.
Se. / tsi / before the 1+2SG:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in imperative
verbs; / se / following the factual prefix or optative prefix before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / sɔ̃ / following the factual prefix or
optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / saː / in imperative
verbs before / hC / with loss of / h /, and following the factual prefix or
optative prefix before / hC / with loss of / h /; / sa / in imperative verbs
before / hV / with loss of / h /, and following the factual prefix or
optative prefix before / hV / with loss of / h /, and following the factual
prefix or optative prefix elsewhere, and in imperative verbs elsewhere;
/ tsi / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere; / ts / before
/ j / elsewhere with loss of / j /; / t / before / s / elsewhere; / s /
elsewhere.
Hu. / tsi / before the 1+2DU:3 and 1+2PL:3 pronominal prefixes in imperative
verbs; / se / following the factual prefix or optative prefix before certain
pronominal prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / sõ / following the factual prefix or
optative prefix before / wa / with loss of / wa /; / sa / in imperative
verbs, and following the factual prefix or optative prefix elsewhere;
/ tsi / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 ) elsewhere; / Ø / before
/ sC / with / h / added after / C /; / sk / before / w /; / ts / before / j /
elsewhere with loss of / j /; / s / before / ʃ / elsewhere; / s / elsewhere.
356
/ wa /; / xõ / before / wa / elsewhere with loss of / wa /; / kae /
following any consonant except / Ɂ / before certain prepronominal
prefixes ( see 4.4 ); / xae / before certain pronominal prefixes ( 4.4 )
elsewhere; / ka / following any consonant except / Ɂ / elsewhere; / xa /
elsewhere.
357
vowels.
Oo. / kni / before consonants; / kj / before / a / and / ẽ /; / kn / before
remaining vowels.
Ca. / khni / before consonants; / kj / before / a /; / khn / before remaining
vowels.
Se. / khniː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / khni / before / hV / with loss of
/ h /, and before remaining consonants; / khnj / before / a / and / ɛ̃ /
( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy ); / khn / before remaining
vowels.
Hu. / ki / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before a consonant; / xi / before
a consonant elsewhere; / kj / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before
/ a /; / xj / before / a / elsewhere; / in / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / n / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
358
loss of / i /; / kj / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before / o / or
/ õ /; / xj / before / o / and / õ / elsewhere; / kw / following any
consonant except / Ɂ / before remaining vowels; / xw / before remaining
vowels elsewhere.
359
/ hske / before certain consonants and clusters ( 11.4 ) elsewhere; / sk /
word-initially or following a consonant before certain other consonants
and clusters ( 11.4 ), and word-initially or following a consonant before
vowels; / hsk / before certain other consonants and clusters ( 11.4 )
elsewhere, and before vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / skʷ / before / a /, / e /, and / ʌ̃ /; / ske / before certain consonants and
clusters ( cf. 12.4 ); / sk / before certain other consonants and clusters
( 12.4 ), and before vowels.
Oo. / ske / before certain consonants and clusters ( cf. 13.4 ); / skeː / before
certain other consonants and certain vowels ( 13.4 ); / sk / before certain
other consonants and clusters ( 13.4 ), and before remaining vowels.
Ca. / ske / word-initially or following a consonant before certain consonants and
clusters ( cf. 14.4 ); / hske / before certain consonants and clusters
( 14.4 ) elsewhere; / sk / word-initially or following a consonant before
certain other consonants and clusters ( 14.4 ), and word-initially before
vowels; / hsk / or following a consonant before certain other consonants
and clusters ( 14.4 ) elsewhere, and before vowels elsewhere.
Se. / ske / before certain consonants and clusters ( cf. 15.4 ); / sk / before
certain other consonants and clusters ( 15.4 ), and before vowels.
Hu. / s / word-initially or following a consonant before / n / with change of
/ n / to / t /; / hs / before / n / elsewhere with change of / n / to / t /;
/ sk / word-initially or following a consonant before vowels, and
word-initially before / ɹ /, and word-initially or following a consonant
before some bases beginning in / j / ( < PNI * / j / ); / hsk / before
vowels elsewhere, and before / ɹ / elsewhere, and before some bases
beginning in / j / ( < PNI * / j / ) elsewhere; / ske / word-initially or
following a consonant before certain remaining consonants and clusters
( see 16.4 ); / hske / before certain remaining consonants and clusters
( 16.4 ) elsewhere.
360
/ àː /, and word-initially or following a consonant before a consonant;
/ hskìnìː / before / àː / with loss of / àː / elsewhere, and before a
consonant elsewhere; / skìn / word-initially or following a consonant
before vowels; / hskìn / before vowels elsewhere.
PNI * / skni / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
* / hskni / before consonants elsewhere; * / skj / word-initially or
following a consonant before / a /; * / hskj / before / a / elsewhere;
* / skn / word-initially or following a consonant before remaining
vowels; * / hskn / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Tu. / skti / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants; / hskti /
before consonants elsewhere; / skj / word-initially or following a
consonant before / a /; / hskj / before / a / elsewhere; / skn /
word-initially or following a consonant before / ə̃ / and word-initially or
following a consonant before some bases beginning in / o /; / hskn /
before / ə̃ / elsewhere and before some bases beginning in / o /
elsewhere; / skt / word-initially or following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / hskt / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Mo. / skeni / word-initially before consonants; / hskeni / before consonants
elsewhere; / skj / word-initially before / a /; / hskj / before / a /
elsewhere; / sken / word-initially before remaining vowels; / hsken /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / skni / before consonants; / skj / before / a /; / skn / before remaining
vowels.
Oo. / skni / before consonants; / skj / before / a / and / ẽ /; / skn / before
remaining vowels.
Ca. / skhni / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
/ hskhni / before consonants elsewhere; / skj / word-initially or
following a consonant before / a /; / hskj / before / a / elsewhere;
/ skhn / word-initially or following a consonant before remaining
vowels; / hskhn / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / skhniː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / skhni / before / hV / with loss of
/ h /, and before remaining consonants; / skhnj / before / a / and / ɛ̃ /
( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy ); / skhn / before remaining
vowels.
Hu. / ski / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants; / hski /
before consonants elsewhere; / skj / word-initially or following a
361
consonant before / a /; / hskj / before / a / elsewhere; / st /
word-initially or following a consonant before remaining vowels; / hst /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
362
/ hskwa / before consonants elsewhere; / skwẽ / word-initially or
following a consonant before / i / with loss of / i /; / hskwẽ / before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / skj / word-initially or following a
consonant before / o /, / õ / and / u /; / hskj / before / o /, / õ / and
/ u / elsewhere; / skw / word-initially or following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / hskw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / skwæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / skwaː / before / hC / with loss
of / h /; / skwa / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before remaining
consonants; / skwɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i / with loss of
/ i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / skj / before / o / and / ɔ̃ /;
/ skw / before some bases beginning in / i /, and before remaining
vowels.
Hu. / skwa / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
/ hskwa / before consonants elsewhere; / skwẽ / word-initially or
following a consonant before / i / with loss of / i /; / hskwẽ / before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / skj / word-initially or following a
consonant before / o / and / õ / elsewhere; / hskj / before / o / and
/ õ / elsewhere; / skw / word-initially or following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / hskw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
⁰¹⁹ PI * { ki } '1SG:3'
PI * / ki / before certain consonants, and perhaps before all consonants; * / k /
before vowels, and perhaps also before certain other consonants.
Ch. / tsì / before consonants; / tsìː / before / àː / with loss of / àː /; / k / before
remaining vowels.
PNI * / ke / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 4.4 ); * / k / before
certain other consonants and clusters ( 4.4 ), and before vowels.
Tu. / kjɛ / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 6.4 ); / kj / before / ɛ /;
/ k / before certain other consonants and clusters ( 6.4 ), and before
remaining vowels.
Mo. / ke / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 11.4 ); / k / before
certain other consonants and clusters ( 11.4 ), and before vowels.
Oe. / ke / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 12.4 ); / k / before certain
other consonants and clusters ( 12.4 ), and before vowels.
Oo. / h / before / k / and / kʷ /; / ke / before certain remaining consonants and
clusters ( see 13.4 ); / keː / before certain other remaining consonants
363
( 13.4 ) and certain vowels; / sk / before certain other remaining
consonants and clusters ( 13.4 ), and before remaining vowels.
Ca. / ke / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 14.4 ); / k / before certain
other consonants and clusters ( 14.4 ), and before vowels.
Se. / Ø / before / hts /; / kh / before / nV /; / ke / before certain consonants
and clusters ( see 15.4 ); / k / before certain other consonants and
clusters ( 15.4 ), and before vowels.
Hu. / ke / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before certain consonants and
clusters ( see 16.4 ); / xe / before certain consonants and clusters ( 16.4 )
elsewhere; / k / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before certain
other consonants and clusters ( 16.4 ), and following any consonant
except / Ɂ / before vowels; / i / before / n / word-initially; / Ø / before
/ n / elsewhere; / x / before certain other remaining consonants and
clusters ( 16.4 ) elsewhere, and before vowels elsewhere.
364
before * / i / with loss of * / i /; * / he / or * / hi / ( or possibly
* / hɹi / ) elsewhere before consonants, and before * / i / elsewhere
with loss of * / i /; * / hej / or * / ɹij / word-initially before remaining
vowels; * / hej / or * / hij / ( or possibly * / hɹij / ) before remaining
vowels elsewhere.
Mo. / ɹi / word-initially before consonants, and word-initially before / i / with
loss of / i /; / hi / before consonants elsewhere, and before / i /
elsewhere with loss of / i /; / ɹij / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / hij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / li / word-initially before consonants, and word-initially before / i / with
loss of / i /; / hi / before consonants elsewhere, and before / i /
elsewhere with loss of / i /; / lij / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / hij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / he / before consonants, and before / i / with loss of / i /; / hej / before
remaining vowels.
Ca. / hi / or / he / before consonants; / he / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ hej / before remaining vowels.
Se. / heː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / he / before / hV / with loss of / h /,
before remaining consonants, and before / i / with loss of / i /; / hej /
before remaining vowels.
Hu. / he / before consonants, and before / i / with loss of / i /, and before
remaining vowels.
365
other consonants and clusters ( 6.4 ), and word-initially or following a
366
Hu. / se / following a cislocative prepronominal prefix allomorph / t /, which is
lost, before certain consonants and clusters ( cf. 16.4 ); / hʃe / following
a vowel before certain consonants and clusters ( 16.4 ) elsewhere; / ʃe /
word-initially before certain consonants and clusters ( 16.4 ); / s /
following a cislocative prepronominal prefix allomorph / t /, which is
lost, before certain other consonants and clusters ( 16.4 ) and before
vowels; / ʃ / word-initially before certain other consonants and clusters
( 16.4 ), and word-initially before vowels.
367
remaining vowels; / hshej / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / sheː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / she / before / hV / with loss of
/ h /, and before remaining consonants, and before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ shej / before remaining vowels.
Hu. / she / following a cislocative prepronominal prefix allomorph / t /, which
is lost; / ʃhe / word-initially or following a repetitive prepronominal
prefix allomorph / s / before consonants, and before / i / with loss of
/ i /, and before remaining vowels; / hʃhe / following a vowel before
consonants elsewhere, and before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /, and
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
368
⁰²⁵ PI * { wakʷi } '3:1SG'
PI * / wakʷi / before certain consonants, and perhaps before all consonants;
* / wakʷ / before * / a(ː) /, and perhaps before all vowels.
Ch. / ȁːkì / before consonants; / ȁːkìː / before / àː / with loss of / àː /; / ȁːkʷ /
before remaining vowels.
PNI * / wakʷ / before * / a /, and perhaps also before * / e / and * / ẽ /;
* / wake / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 4.4 ); * / wak /
before certain other consonants and clusters ( 4.4 ), and before remaining
vowels.
Tu. / wakʷ / before / a /; / wakjɛ / before certain consonants and clusters
( see 6.4 ); / wakj / before / ɛ /; / wak / before certain other consonants
and clusters ( 6.4 ), and before remaining vowels.
Mo. / wakʷ / following the factual or optative prefix before / a /, / e /, or / ʌ̃ /;
/ wake / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 11.4 ); / wak /
before certain other consonants and clusters ( 11.4 ), and before
remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / wakʷ / following the factual or optative prefix before / a /, / e /, or / ʌ̃ /;
/ wake / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 12.4 ); / wak /
before certain other consonants and clusters ( 12.4 ), and before
remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / ah / word-initially before / k / and / kʷ /; / wah / before / k / and / kʷ /
elsewhere; / ake / word-initially before certain consonants and clusters
( see 13.4 ); / wake / before certain consonants and clusters ( 13.4 )
elsewhere; / akeː / word-initially before certain other consonants and
certain other vowels ( 13.4 ); / wakeː / before certain other consonants
and certain other vowels ( 13.4 ) elsewhere; / ak / word-initially before
certain other consonants and clusters ( 13.4 ), and word-initially before
remaining vowels; / wak / before certain other consonants and clusters
( 13.4 ) elsewhere, and before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Ca. / ake / word-initially before certain consonants and clusters ( see 14.4 );
/ wake / before certain consonants and clusters ( 14.4 ) elsewhere; / ak /
word-initially before certain other consonants and clusters ( 14.4 ), and
word-initially before vowels; / wak / before certain other consonants and
clusters ( 14.4 ) elsewhere, and before vowels elsewhere.
Se. / ake / word-initially before certain consonants and clusters ( see 15.4 );
/ wɔ̃ke / following the future prefix before certain consonants and
369
clusters ( see 15.4 ); / wake / before certain consonants and clusters
( 15.4 ) elsewhere; / a / word-initially before / hts /; / wɔ̃ / following
the future prefix before / hts /; / wa / before / hts / elsewhere; / akh /
word-initially before / n /; / wɔ̃kh / following the future prefix before
/ n /; / wakh / before / n / elsewhere; / ak / word-initially before
certain other consonants and clusters ( 15.4 ), and word-initially before
vowels; / wɔ̃k / following the future prefix before certain other
consonants and clusters ( 15.4 ), and following the future prefix before
vowels; / wak / before certain consonants and clusters ( 15.4 )
elsewhere, and before vowels elsewhere.
Hu. / a / word-initial before / n /; / wa / before / n / elsewhere; / axe /
word-initially before certain remaining consonants and clusters
( see 16.4 ); / waxe / before certain remaining consonants and clusters
( 16.4 ) elsewhere; / ax / word-initially before certain remaining
consonants and clusters ( 16.4 ) and word-initially before vowels; / wax /
before certain remaining consonants and clusters ( 16.4 ) elsewhere, and
before vowels elsewhere.
370
Oo. / ũh / word-initially before / k / and / kʷ /; / jũh / before / k / and / kʷ /
elsewhere; / ũke / word-initially before certain consonants and clusters
( see 13.4 ); / jũke / before certain consonants and clusters ( 13.4 )
elsewhere; / ũkeː / word-initially before certain other consonants and
certain vowels ( 13.4 ); / jũkeː / before certain other consonants and
certain vowels ( 13.4 ) elsewhere; / ũk / word-initially before certain
other consonants and clusters ( 13.4 ), and word-initially before
remaining vowels; / jũk / before certain other consonants and clusters
( 13.4 ) elsewhere, and before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Ca. / jõke / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 14.4 ); / jõk / before
certain other consonants and clusters ( 14.4 ), and before vowels.
Se. / ɔ̃ke / word-initially before certain consonants and clusters ( see 15.4 );
/ jɔ̃ke / before certain consonants and clusters ( 15.4 ) elsewhere;
/ ɔ̃kh / word-initially before / n /; / jɔ̃kh / before / n / elsewhere;
/ ɔ̃ / word-initially before / hts /; / jɔ̃ / before / hts / elsewhere; / ɔ̃k /
word-initially before certain other consonants and clusters ( 15.4 ), and
word-initially before vowels; / jɔ̃k / before certain other consonants and
clusters ( 15.4 ) elsewhere, and before vowels elsewhere.
Hu. / jõ / following a consonant before / n /; / õ / before / n / elsewhere;
/ jõxe / following a consonant before certain remaining consonants and
clusters ( see 16.4 ); / õxe / before certain remaining consonants and
clusters ( 16.4 ) elsewhere; / jõx / following a consonant before certain
remaining consonants and clusters ( 16.4 ), and following a consonant
before a vowel; / õx / before certain remaining consonants and clusters
( 16.4 ) elsewhere, and before vowels elsewhere.
371
word-initially before / ɛ /; / hrakj / before / ɛ / elsewhere; / rakjɛ /
word-initially before certain consonants and clusters ( see 6.4 );
/ hrakjɛ / before certain consonants and clusters ( 6.4 ) elsewhere;
/ rak / word-initially before certain other consonants and clusters ( 6.4 ),
and before remaining vowels; / hrak / before certain other consonants
and clusters ( 6.4 ) elsewhere, and before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Mo. / ɹakʷ / word-initially before / a /, / e /, and / ʌ̃ /; / hakʷ / before / a /,
/ e /, and / ʌ̃ / elsewhere; / ɹake / word-initially before certain
consonants and clusters ( see 11.4 ); / hake / before certain consonants
and clusters ( 11.4 )elsewhere; / ɹak / word-initially before certain other
consonants and clusters ( 11.4 ), and word-initially before remaining
vowels; / hak / before certain other consonants and clusters ( 11.4 )
elsewhere, and before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / lakʷ / word-initially before / a /, / e /, and / ʌ̃ /; / hakʷ / before / a /,
/ e /, and / ʌ̃ / elsewhere; / lake / word-initially before certain
consonants and clusters ( see 12.4 ); / hake / before certain consonants
and clusters ( 12.4 ) elsewhere; / lak / word-initially before certain other
consonants and clusters ( 12.4 ), and word-initially before remaining
vowels; / hak / before certain other consonants and clusters ( 12.4 )
elsewhere, and before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / hah / before / k / and / kʷ /; / hake / before certain consonants and
clusters ( see 13.4 ); / hakeː / before certain other consonants and certain
vowels ( 13.4 ); / hak / before certain other consonants and clusters
( 13.4 ), and before remaining vowels.
Ca. / hake / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 14.4 ); / hak / before
certain other consonants and clusters ( 14.4 ), and before vowels.
Se. / hake / before certain consonants and clusters ( see 15.4 ); / hakh / before
/ n /; / ha / before / hts /; / hak / before certain other consonants and
clusters ( 15.4 ), and before vowels.
Hu. / ha / before / n /; / haxe / before certain remaining consonants and
clusters ( see 16.4 ); / hax / before certain other remaining consonants
and clusters ( 16.4 ), and before vowels.
372
Ch. / tsà / before consonants; / ts / before vowels.
PNI * / tsa / before consonants; * / tsẽ / before * / i / with loss of * / i /;
* / ts / before remaining vowels.
Tu. / θa / before consonants; / θə̃ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / θ / before
remaining vowels.
Mo. / sa / before consonants; / sʌ̃ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / s / before
remaining vowels.
Oe. [ as in Mohawk ].
Oo. / sa / before consonants; / sẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / s / before
remaining vowels.
Ca. / sa / before consonants; / sẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / s / before
remaining vowels.
Se. / sæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / saː / before / hC / with loss of
/ h /; / sa / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before / o / and / ɔ̃ /,
and before remaining consonants; / sɛ ̃ / before some bases beginning in
/ i / with loss of / i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / s / before
some bases beginning in / i /, and before remaining vowels.
Hu. / sa / before consonants; / sẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / s / before
remaining vowels.
373
Oo. / esa / word-initially before consonants; / jesa / before consonants
elsewhere; / esẽ / word-initially before / i / with loss of / i /; / jesẽ /
before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / esaj / word-initially before
/ o / and / ũ /; / jesaj / before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere; / es /
word-initially before remaining vowels; / jes / before remaining vowels
elsewhere.
Ca. / jesa / before consonants; / jesẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / jesaj /
before / o /, / õ / and / u /; / jes / before remaining vowels.
Se. / esæː / before / Ræ / word-initially with loss of / Ræ /; / jesæː / before
/ Ræ / elsewhere with loss of / Ræ /; / esaː / before / hC / word-initially
with loss of / h /; / jesaː / before / hC / elsewhere with loss of / h /;
/ esa / before / hV / word-initially with loss of / h /, and before / o /
and / ɔ̃ / word-initially, and before remaining consonants word-initially;
/ jesa / before / hV / elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before / o / and
/ ɔ̃ / elsewhere, and before remaining consonants elsewhere; / esɛ ̃ /
before some bases beginning in / i / word-initially with loss of / i /, and
before / jɔ̃ / word-initially with loss of / j /; / jesɛ ̃ / before some bases
beginning in / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /, and before / jɔ̃ / elsewhere
with loss of / j /; / es / before some bases beginning in / i /
word-initially, and before remaining vowels word-initially; / jes / before
some bases beginning in / i / elsewhere, and before remaining vowels
elsewhere.
Hu. / esa / following a consonant before a consonant; / esa / before consonants
elsewhere; / jesɛ̃ / following a consonant before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ esɛ̃ / before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / jes / following a
consonant before remaining vowels; / es / before remaining vowels
elsewhere.
374
/ hjaj / before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere; / j / word-initially before
remaining vowels; / hj / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / hja / before consonants; / hjẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / hjaj /
before / o / and / ũ /; / hj / before remaining vowels.
Ca. / hja / before consonants; / hjẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / hjaj /
before / o /, / õ / and / u /; / hj / before remaining vowels.
Se. / jæː / before / Ræ / word-initially with loss of / Ræ /; / hjæː / before / Ræ /
elsewhere with loss of / Ræ /; / jaː / before / hC / word-initially with
loss of / h /; / hjaː / before / hC / elsewhere with loss of / h /; / ja /
before / hV / word-initially with loss of / h /, and before / o / and / ɔ̃ /
word-initially, and before remaining consonants word-initially; / hja /
before / hV / elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before / o / and / õ /
elsewhere, and before remaining consonants elsewhere; / jɛ̃ / before
some bases beginning in / i / word-initially with loss of / i /, and before
/ jɔ̃ / word-initially with loss of / j /; / hjɛ̃ / before some bases beginning
in / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /, and before / jɔ̃ / elsewhere with loss
of / j /; / j / before some bases beginning in / i / word-initially, and
before remaining vowels word-initially; / hj / before some bases
beginning in / i / elsewhere, and before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Hu. / hja / before consonants; / hjẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / hj / before
remaining vowels.
375
Oe. / sni / before consonants; / tsj / before / a /; / sn / before remaining
vowels.
Oo. / sni / before consonants; / tsj / before / a / and / ẽ /; / sn / before
remaining vowels.
Ca. / hsni / following a vowel before a consonant; / sni / before a consonant
elsewhere; / ts / before / a /; / hsn / following a vowel before remaining
vowels; / sn / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / sniː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / sni / before / hV / with loss of
/ h /, and before remaining consonants; / snj / before / a / and / ɛ̃ /
( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy ); / sn / before remaining
vowels.
Hu. / tsi / before consonants; / ts / before / a /; / st / before remaining vowels.
376
Hu. / hetsi / before consonants; / hets / before / a /; / hest / before remaining
vowels.
377
following a consonant before * / o / and * / õ /; * / hshetsj / before
* / o / and * / õ / elsewhere; * / shetsw / word-initially or following a
consonant before remaining vowels; * / hshetsw / before remaining
vowels elsewhere.
Mo. / hetshisewa / before consonants; / hetshisewʌ̃ / before / i / with loss of
/ i /; / hetshitsj / before / o / and / ũ /; / hetshisew / before remaining
vowels.
Oe. / etswa / word-initially before consonants; / hetswa / before consonants
elsewhere; / etswʌ̃ / word-initially before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ hetswʌ̃ / before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / etsj / word-initially
before / o / and / ũ /; / hetsj / before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere; / etsw /
word-initially before remaining vowels; / etsw / before remaining vowels
elsewhere.
Oo. / sheswa / before consonants; / sheswẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ shetsj / before / o / and / ũ /; / shesw / before remaining vowels.
Ca. / shehswa / word-initially or following a consonant before a consonant;
/ hshehswa / before a consonant elsewhere; / shehswẽ / word-initially or
following a consonant before / i / with loss of / i /; / hshehswẽ / before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / shetsj / word-initially or following a
consonant before / o /, / õ / or / u /; / hshetsj / before / o /, / õ / and
/ u / elsewhere; / shehsw / word-initially or following a consonant
before remaining vowels; / hshehsw / before remaining vowels
elsewhere.
Se. / sheswæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / sheswaː / before / hC / with
loss of / h /; / sheswa / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before
remaining consonants; / sheswɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i /
with loss of / i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / shets / before / o /
and / ɔ̃ /; / sw / before some bases beginning in / i /, and before
remaining vowels.
Hu. / heskwa / before consonants; / heskwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ hets / before / o / and / ũ /; / heskw / before remaining vowels.
378
before remaining vowels.
Mo. / jetshi / before consonants, and before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /;
/ jetshij / before remaining vowels.
Oe. / etshi / word-initially before consonants, and word-initially before / i /
with loss of / i /; / jetshi / before consonants elsewhere, and before / i /
elsewhere with loss of / i /; / etshij / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / jetshij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / etshi / word-initially before consonants, and word-initially before / i /
with loss of / i /; / jetshi / before consonants elsewhere, and before / i /
elsewhere with loss of / i /; / etshij / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / jetshij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Ca. / etshi / word-initially before consonants, and word-initially before / i /
with loss of / i /; / jetshi / before consonants elsewhere, and before / i /
elsewhere with loss of / i /; / etshij / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / jetshij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / etshiː / word-initially before / hC / with loss of / h /; / jetshiː / before
/ hC / elsewhere with loss of / h /; / etshi / word-initially before / hV /
with loss of / h /, and word-initially before remaining consonants, and
word-initially before / i / with loss of / i /; / jetshi / before / hV /
elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before remaining consonants elsewhere,
and before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / etshij / word-initially
before remaining vowels; / jetshij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Hu. / jetshi / following a consonant before a consonant, and following a
consonant before / i / with loss of / i /, and following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / etshi / before a consonant elsewhere, and before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /, and before remaining vowels elsewhere.
379
Mo. / teni / before consonants; / tj / before / a /; / ten / before remaining
vowels.
Oe. / tni / before consonants; / tj / before / a /; / tn / before remaining vowels.
Oo. / tni / before consonants; / tj / before / a / and / ẽ /; / tn / before
remaining vowels.
Ca. / thni / before consonants; / tj / before / a /; / thn / before remaining
vowels.
Se. / hniː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / hni / before / hV / with loss of
/ h /, and before remaining consonants; / hnj / before / a / and / ɛ̃ /
( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy ); / hn / before remaining
vowels.
Hu. / ti / before consonants; / tj / before / a /; / t / before remaining vowels.
380
Hu. / heti / before consonants; / hetj / before / a /; / het / before remaining
vowels.
⁰³⁸ PI * { t… } '1+2PL:3'
Ch. / ȉːtìː / before / àː / with loss of / àː / and before a consonant; / ȉːt / before
remaining vowels.
PNI * / twa / before consonants; * / twẽ / before * / i / with loss of * / i /;
* / tj / before * / o / and * / õ /; * / tw / before remaining vowels.
Tu. / Ɂnwa / after a vowel and before a consonant; / nwa / before a consonant
elsewhere; / Ɂnwə̃ / after a vowel and before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ nwə̃ / before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / Ɂnw / after a vowel
before remaining vowels / nw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Mo. / tewa / before consonants; / tewʌ̃ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / tj /
before / o / and / ũ /; / tew / before remaining vowels.
Oe. / twa / before consonants; / twʌ̃ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / tj / before
/ o / and / ũ /; / tw / before remaining vowels.
Oo. / twa / before consonants; / twẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / tj / before
/ o / and / ũ /; / tw / before remaining vowels.
Ca. / twa / before consonants; / twẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / tj / before
/ o /, / õ / and / u /; / tw / before remaining vowels.
Se. / twæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / twaː / before / hC / with loss of
/ h /; / twa / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before remaining
consonants; / twɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i / with loss of
/ i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / tj / before / o / and / ɔ̃ /;
/ tw / before some bases beginning in / i /, and before remaining
vowels.
Hu. / kwa / before consonants; / kwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / tj /
before / o / and / õ /; / kw / before remaining vowels.
381
consonant before remaining vowels; * / hshetw / before remaining
vowels elsewhere.
Mo. / hetshitewa / before consonants; / hetshitewʌ̃ / before / i / with loss of
/ i /; / hetshitj / before / o / and / ũ /; / hetshitew / before remaining
vowels.
Oe. / ethwa / word-initially before consonants; / hethwa / before consonants
elsewhere; / ethwʌ̃ / word-initially before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ hethwʌ̃ / before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / ethj / word-initially
before / o / and / ũ /; / hethj / before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere;
/ ethw / word-initially before remaining vowels; / hethw / before
remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / shetwa / before consonants; / shetwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ shetj / before / o / and / ũ /; / shetw / before remaining vowels.
Ca. / shetwa / word-initially or following a consonant before a consonant;
/ hshetwa / before a consonant elsewhere; / shetwẽ / word-initially or
following a consonant before / i / with loss of / i /; / hshetwẽ / before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / shetj / word-initially or following a
consonant before / o /, / õ / or / u /; / hshetj / before / o /, / õ / or
/ u / elsewhere; / shetw / word-initially or following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / hshetw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / shetwæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / shetwaː / before / hC / with
loss of / h /; / shetwa / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before
remaining consonants; / shetwɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i /
with loss of / i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / shetj / before / o /
and / ɔ̃ /; / shetw / before some bases beginning in / i /, and before
remaining vowels.
Hu. / hekwa / before consonants; / hekwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ hetj / before / o / and / õ /; / hekw / before remaining vowels.
382
Oe. / akni / word-initially before consonants; / jakni / before consonants
elsewhere; / akj / word-initially before / a /; / jakj / before / a /
elsewhere; / akn / word-initially before remaining vowels; / jakn /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / akni / word-initially before consonants; / jakni / before consonants
elsewhere; / akj / word-initially before / a / and / ẽ /; / jakj / before
/ a / and / ẽ / elsewhere; / akn / word-initially before remaining vowels;
/ jakn / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Ca. / akhni / word-initially before consonants; / jakhni / before consonants
elsewhere; / akj / word-initially before / a /; / jakj / before / a /
elsewhere; / akhn / word-initially before remaining vowels; / jakhn /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / akhniː / word-initially before / hC / with loss of / h /; / jakhniː / before
/ hC / elsewhere with loss of / h /; / akhni / word-initially before / hV /
with loss of / h /, and word-initially before remaining consonants;
/ jakhni / before / hV / elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before
remaining consonants elsewhere; / akhnj / word-initially before / a /
and / ɛ̃ / ( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy ); / jakhnj / before
/ a / and / ɛ̃ / elsewhere ( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy );
/ akhn / word-initially before remaining vowels; / jakn / elsewhere
before remaining vowels.
Hu. / jaxi / following a consonant before a consonant; / axi / before a consonant
elsewhere; / jaxj / following a consonant before / a /; / axj / before / a /
elsewhere; / jan / following a consonant before remaining vowels; / an /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
383
vowels; / hshaken / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / shakni / before consonants; / shakj / before / a /; / shakn / before
remaining vowels.
Oo. / shakni / before consonants; / shakj / before / a / and / ẽ /; / shakn /
before remaining vowels.
Ca. / shakhni / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
/ hshakhni / before consonants elsewhere; / shakj / word-initially or
following a consonant before / a /; / hshakj / before / a / elsewhere;
/ shakhn / word-initially or following a consonant before remaining
vowels; / hshakhn / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / shakhniː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / shakhni / before / hV / with
loss of / h /, and before remaining consonants; / shakhnj / before / a /
and / ɛ̃ / ( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy ); / shakhn /
before remaining vowels.
Hu. / shaxi / before a consonant; / shaxj / before / a /; / shan / before
remaining vowels.
384
Ca. / akwa / word-initially before consonants; / jakwa / before consonants
elsewhere; / akwẽ / word-initially before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ jakwẽ / before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / akj / word-initially
before / o /, / õ /, and / u /; / jakj / before / o /, / õ /, and / u /
elsewhere; / akw / word-initially before remaining vowels; / jakw /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / akwæː / word-initially before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / jakwæː / before
/ Ræ / elsewhere with loss of / Ræ /; / akwaː / word-initially before
/ hC / with loss of / h /; / jakwaː / before / hC / elsewhere with loss of
/ h /; / akwa / word-initially before / hV / with loss of / h /, and
word-initially before remaining consonants; / jakwa / before / hV /
elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before remaining consonants elsewhere;
/ akwɛ̃ / word-initially before some bases beginning in / i / with loss of
/ i /, and word-initially before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / jakwɛ̃ / before
some bases beginning in / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /, and before
/ jɔ̃ / elsewhere with loss of / j /; / akj / word-initially before / o / and
/ ɔ̃ /; / jakj / before / o / and / ɔ̃ / elsewhere; / akw / word-initially
before some bases beginning in / i /, and word-initially before remaining
vowels; / jakw / before some bases beginning in / i / elsewhere, and
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Hu. / jaxwa / following a consonant before a consonant; / axwa / before a
consonant elsewhere; / jaxwẽ / following a consonant before / i / with
loss of / i /; / axwẽ / before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / jaxj /
following a consonant before / o / or / õ /; / axj / before / o / and / õ /
elsewhere; / jaxw / following a consonant before remaining vowels;
/ axw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
385
Mo. / shakwa / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
/ hshakwa / before consonants elsewhere; / shakwʌ̃ / word-initially or
following a consonant before / i / with loss of / i /; / hshakwʌ̃ / before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / shakj / word-initially or following a
consonant before / o / and / ũ /; / hshakj / before / o / and / ũ /
elsewhere; / shakw / word-initially or following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / hshakw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / shakwa / before consonants; / shakwʌ̃ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ shakj / before / o / and / ũ /; / shakw / before remaining vowels.
Oo. / shakwa / before consonants; / shakwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ shakj / before / o / and / ũ /; / shakw / before remaining vowels.
Ca. / shakwa / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
/ hshakwa / before consonants elsewhere; / shakwẽ / word-initially or
following a consonant before / i / with loss of / i /; / hshakwẽ / before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / shakj / word-initially or following a
consonant before / o /, / õ / and / u /; / hshakj / before / o /, / õ / and
/ u / elsewhere; / shakw / word-initially or following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / hshakw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / shakwæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / shakwaː / before / hC /
with loss of / h /; / shakwa / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before
remaining consonants; / shakwɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i /
with loss of / i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / shakj / before / o /
and / ɔ̃ /; / shakw / before some bases beginning in / i /, and before
remaining vowels.
Hu. / shaxwa / before a consonant; / shaxwɛ ̃ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ shaxj / before / o / and / õ /; / shaxw / before remaining vowels.
386
Mo. / jũkeni / before consonants; / jũkj / before / a /; / jũken / before
remaining vowels.
Oe. / ũkni / word-initially before consonants; / jũkni / before consonants
elsewhere; / ũkj / word-initially before / a /; / jũkj / before / a /
elsewhere; / ũkn / word-initially before remaining consonants; / jũkn /
before remaining consonants elsewhere.
Oo. / ũkni / word-initially before consonants; / jũkni / before consonants
elsewhere; / ũkj / word-initially before / a / and / ẽ /; / jũkj / before
/ a / and / ẽ / elsewhere; / ũkn / word-initially before remaining vowels;
/ jũkn / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Ca. / õkhni / word-initially before consonants; / jõkhni / before consonants
elsewhere; / õkj / word-initially before / a /; / jõkj / before / a /
elsewhere; / õkhn / word-initially before remaining vowels; / jõkhn /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / ɔ̃khniː / word-initially before / hC / with loss of / h /; / jɔ̃khniː / before
/ hC / elsewhere with loss of / h /; / ɔ̃khni / word-initially before / hV /
with loss of / h /, and word-initially before remaining consonants;
/ jɔ̃khni / before / hV / elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before
remaining consonants elsewhere; / ɔ̃khnj / word-initially before / a / and
/ ẽ / ( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy ); / jɔ̃khnj / before
/ a / and / ẽ / elsewhere; / ɔ̃khn / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / jɔ̃khn / elsewhere before remaining vowels.
Hu. / jõxi / following a consonant before a consonant; / õxi / before a
consonant elsewhere; / jõxj / following a consonant before / a /; / õxj /
before / a / elsewhere; / jõn / following a consonant before remaining
vowels; / õn / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
387
/ shũken / word-initially or following a consonant before remaining
vowels; / hshũken / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / shũkni / before consonants; / shũkj / before / a /; / shũkn / before
remaining vowels.
Oo. / shũkni / before consonants; / shũkj / before / a / and / ẽ /; / shũkn /
before remaining vowels.
Ca. / shõkhni / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
/ hshõkhni / before consonants elsewhere; / shõkj / word-initially or
following a consonant before / a /; / hshõkj / before / a / elsewhere;
/ shõkhn / word-initially or following a consonant before remaining
vowels; / hshõkhn / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / shɔ̃khniː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / shɔ̃khni / before / hV / with
loss of / h /, and before remaining consonants; / shɔ̃khnj / before / a /
and / ɛ̃ / ( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy ); / shɔ̃khn /
before remaining vowels.
Hu. / shõxi / before a consonant; / shõxj / before / a /; / shõn / before
remaining vowels.
388
before / o / and / ũ /; / jũkj / before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere; / ũkw /
word-initially before remaining vowels; / jũkw / before remaining
vowels elsewhere.
Ca. / õkwa / word-initially before consonants; / jõkwa / before consonants
elsewhere; / õkwẽ / word-initially before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ jõkwẽ / before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / õkj / word-initially
before / o /, / õ /, and / u /; / jõkj / before / o /, / õ /, and / u /
elsewhere; / õkw / word-initially before remaining vowels; / jõkw /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / ɔ̃kwæː / word-initially before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / jɔ̃kwæː / before
/ Ræ / elsewhere with loss of / Ræ /; / ɔ̃kwaː / word-initially before
/ hC / with loss of / h /; / jɔ̃kwaː / before / hC / elsewhere with loss of
/ h /; / ɔ̃kwa / word-initially before / hV / with loss of / h /, and
word-initially before remaining consonants; / jɔ̃kwa / before / hV /
elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before remaining consonants elsewhere;
/ ɔ̃kwɛ̃ / word-initially before some bases beginning in / i / with loss of
/ i /, and word-initially before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / jɔ̃kwɛ̃ / before
some bases beginning in / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /, and before
/ jɔ̃ / elsewhere with loss of / j /; / ɔ̃kj / word-initially before / o / and
/ ɔ̃ /; / jɔ̃kj / before / o / and / ɔ̃ / elsewhere; / ɔ̃kw / word-initially
before some bases beginning in / i /, and word-initially before remaining
vowels; / jɔ̃kw / before some bases beginning in / i / elsewhere, and
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Hu. / jõxwa / following a consonant before a consonant; / õxwa / before a
consonant elsewhere; / jõxwẽ / following a consonant before / i / with
loss of / i /; / õxwẽ / before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / jõxj /
following a consonant before / o / or / õ /; / õxj / before / o / and / õ /
elsewhere; / jõxw / following a consonant before remaining vowels;
/ õxw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
389
* / o / and * / õ /; * / shõkw / word-initially or following a consonant
before remaining vowels; * / hshõkw / before remaining vowels
elsewhere.
Mo. / shũkwa / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
/ hshũkwa / before consonants elsewhere; / shũkwʌ̃ / word-initially or
following a consonant before / i / with loss of / i /; / hshũkwʌ̃ / before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / shũkj / word-initially or following a
consonant before / o / and / ũ /; / hshũkj / before / o / and / ũ /
elsewhere; / shũkw / word-initially or following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / hshũkw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / shũkwa / before consonants; / shũkwʌ̃ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ shũkj / before / o / and / ũ /; / shũkw / before remaining vowels.
Oo. / shũkwa / before consonants; / shũkwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ shũkj / before / o / and / ũ /; / shũkw / before remaining vowels.
Ca. / shõkwa / word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
/ hshõkwa / before consonants elsewhere; / shõkwẽ / word-initially or
following a consonant before / i / with loss of / i /; / hshõkwẽ / before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / shõkj / word-initially or following a
consonant before / o /, / õ / and / u /; / hshõkj / before / o /, / õ / and
/ u / elsewhere; / shõkw / word-initially or following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / hshõkw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / shɔ̃kwæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / shɔ̃kwaː / before / hC / with
loss of / h /; / shɔ̃kwa / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before
remaining consonants; / shɔ̃kwɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i /
with loss of / i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / shɔ̃kj / before / o /
and / ɔ̃ /; / shɔ̃kw / before some bases beginning in / i /, and before
remaining vowels.
Hu. / shõxwa / before a consonant; / shõxwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ shõxj / before / o / and / õ /; / shõxw / before remaining vowels.
390
loss of / i /; / jethi / before consonants elsewhere, and before / i /
elsewhere with loss of / i /; / ethij / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / jethij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / ethi / word-initially before consonants, and word-initially before / i / with
loss of / i /; / jethi / before consonants elsewhere, and before / i /
elsewhere with loss of / i /; / ethij / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / jethij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Ca. / ethi / word-initially before consonants, and word-initially before / i / with
loss of / i /; / jethi / before consonants elsewhere, and before / i /
elsewhere with loss of / i /; / ethij / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / jethij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Se. / ethiː / word-initially before / hC / with loss of / h /; / jethiː / before / hC /
elsewhere with loss of / h /; / ethi / word-initially before / hV / with
loss of / h /, and word-initially before remaining consonants, and
word-initially before / i / with loss of / i /; / jethi / before / hV /
elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before remaining consonants elsewhere,
and before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / ethij / word-initially before
remaining vowels; / jethij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
391
Se. / akhiː / word-initially before / hC / with loss of / h /; / jakhiː / before
/ hC / elsewhere with loss of / h /; / akhi / word-initially before / hV /
with loss of / h /, and word-initially before remaining consonants, and
word-initially before / i / with loss of / i /; / jakhi / before / hV /
elsewhere with loss of / h /, and before remaining consonants elsewhere,
and before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / akhij / word-initially
before remaining vowels; / jakhij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Hu. / jakhi / following a consonant before a consonant, and following a
consonant before / i / with loss of / i /, and following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / akhi / before a consonant elsewhere, and before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /, and before remaining vowels elsewhere.
392
and before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / ɔ̃khij / word-initially
before remaining vowels; / jɔ̃khij / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Hu. / jõkhi / following a consonant before a consonant, and following a
consonant before / i / with loss of / i /, and following a consonant before
remaining vowels; / õkhi / before a consonant elsewhere, and before
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / i /, and before remaining vowels elsewhere.
393
before some bases beginning in / i /, and before remaining vowels.
Hu. / ka / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before a consonant; / xa /
before a consonant elsewhere; / kẽ / following any consonant except
/ Ɂ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / xẽ / before / i / elsewhere with loss
of / i /; / k / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before some bases
beginning in / o /; / x / before some bases beginning in / o / elsewhere;
/ j / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before some bases beginning in
/ o / and before / õ /; / Ø / before some bases beginning in / o / and
before / õ / elsewhere, as well as word-initially following / a /, / e /,
and / ẽ /; / w / before / a /, / e /, and / ẽ / elsewhere.
394
Ca. / ha / before consonants; / hẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / h / before
remaining vowels.
Se. / hæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / haː / before / hC / with loss of
/ h /; / ha / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before remaining
consonants; / hɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i / with loss of / i /,
and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / h / before remaining vowels.
Hu. / ha / before consonants; / hẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / h / before
/ a /; / hɹ / before remaining vowels.
395
/ jaw / before / e / elsewhere and before some bases beginning in / ẽ /
elsewhere; / a / word-initially before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere; / ja /
before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere; / o / word-initially before some bases
beginning in / ẽ / with loss of / ẽ /, and word-initially before / a / and
/ i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and word-initially before consonants;
/ jo / before some bases beginning in / ẽ / elsewhere with loss of / ẽ /,
and before / a / and / i / elsewhere with loss of / a / and / i /, and
before consonants elsewhere.
Ca. / aw / word-initially before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ẽ /;
/ jaw / before / e / elsewhere and before some bases beginning in / ẽ /
elsewhere; / a / word-initially before / o /, / õ /, and / u / elsewhere;
/ ja / before / o /, / õ /, and / u / elsewhere; / o / word-initially before
some bases beginning in / ẽ / with loss of / ẽ /, and word-initially before
/ a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and word-initially before
consonants; / jo / before some bases beginning in / ẽ / elsewhere with
loss of / ẽ /, and before / a / and / i / elsewhere with loss of / a / and
/ i /, and before consonants elsewhere.
Se. / aw / word-initially before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ɛ̃ /;
/ jaw / before / e / elsewhere and before some bases beginning in / ẽ /
elsewhere; / a / word-initially before / o / and / ɔ̃ /; / ja / before / o /
and / ɔ̃ / elsewhere; / o / word-initially before some bases beginning in
/ ɛ̃ / with loss of / ɛ̃ /, and word-initially before / a / and / i / with loss
of / a / and / i /; / jo / before some bases beginning in / ɛ ̃ / elsewhere
with loss of / ɛ̃ /, and before / a / and / i / elsewhere with loss of / a /
and / i /, and before consonants.
Hu. / jaw / following a consonant before / e /, and following a consonant before
some bases beginning in / ẽ /; / aw / before / e / elsewhere, and before
some bases beginning in / ẽ / elsewhere; / ja / following a consonant
before / o / or / õ /; / a / before / o / and / õ / elsewhere; / jo /
following a consonant before some bases beginning in / ẽ / with loss of
/ ẽ /, and following a consonant before / a / and / i / with loss of / a /
and / i /, and following a consonant before consonants; / o / before some
bases beginning in / ẽ / elsewhere with loss of / ẽ /, and before / a / and
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / a / and / i /, and before consonants
elsewhere.
396
⁰⁵⁵ PNI * { hɹo } '3:3SGM'
PNI * / ɹaw / word-initially before * / e / and word-initially before some bases
beginning in * / ẽ /; * / hɹaw / before * / e / elsewhere and before some
bases beginning in * / ẽ / elsewhere; * / ɹa / word-initially before * / o /
and * / õ /; * / hɹa / before * / o / and * / õ / elsewhere; * / ɹo /
word-initially before some bases beginning in * / ẽ / with loss of * / ẽ /,
and word-initially before * / a / and * / i / with loss of * / a / and
* / i /, and word-initially before consonants; * / hɹo / before some bases
beginning in * / ẽ / elsewhere with loss of * / ẽ /, and before * / a / and
* / i / elsewhere with loss of * / a / and * / i /, and before consonants
elsewhere.
Tu. / ro / word-initially before consonants, and word-initially before / a / with
loss of / a /; / hro / before consonants elsewhere, and before / a /
elsewhere with loss of / a /; / raw / word-initially before remaining
vowels; / hraw / before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Mo. / ɹaw / word-initially before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ʌ̃ /;
/ haw / before / e / elsewhere and before some bases beginning in / ʌ̃ /
elsewhere; / ɹa / word-initially before / o / and / ũ /; / ha / before / o /
and / ũ / elsewhere; / ɹo / word-initially before some bases beginning in
/ ʌ̃ / with loss of / ʌ̃ /, and word-initially before / a / and / i / with loss
of / a / and / i /, and word-initially before consonants; / ho / before
some bases beginning in / ʌ̃ / elsewhere with loss of / ʌ̃ /, and before
/ a / and / i / elsewhere with loss of / a / and / i /, and before
consonants elsewhere.
Oe. / law / word-initially before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ʌ̃ /;
/ haw / before / e / elsewhere and before some bases beginning in / ʌ̃ /
elsewhere; / loj / word-initially before / o / and / ũ / in semantically
transitive verbs; / hoj / before / o / and / ũ / in semantically transitive
verbs elsewhere; / la / word-initially before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere;
/ ha / before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere; / lo / word-initially before some
bases beginning in / ʌ̃ / with loss of / ʌ̃ /, and word-initially before / a /
and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and word-initially before
consonants; / ho / before some bases beginning in / ʌ̃ / elsewhere with
loss of / ʌ̃ /, and before / a / and / i / elsewhere with loss of / a / and
/ i /, and before consonants elsewhere.
Oo. / haw / before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ẽ /; / ha / before
397
/ o / and / ũ /; / ho / before some bases beginning in / ẽ / with loss of
/ ẽ /, and before / a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and before
consonants.
Ca. / haw / before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ẽ /; / ha / before
/ o /, / õ / and / u /; / ho / before some bases beginning in / ẽ / with
loss of / ẽ /, and before / a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and
before consonants.
Se. / haw / before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ɛ̃ /; / ha / before
/ o / and / ɔ̃ /; / ho / before some bases beginning in / ɛ̃ / with loss of
/ ɛ̃ /, and before / a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and before
consonants.
Hu. / haw / before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ẽ /; / ha / before
/ o / and / õ /; / ho / before some bases beginning in / ẽ / with loss of
/ ẽ /, and before / a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and before a
consonant.
398
vowels.
Mo. / ni / word-initially before consonants; / hni / before consonants elsewhere;
/ j / word-initially before / a /; / hj / before / a / elsewhere; / n /
word-initially before remaining vowels; / hn / before remaining vowels
elsewhere.
Oe. / ni / word-initially before consonants; / hni / before consonants elsewhere;
/ j / word-initially before / a /; / hj / before / a / elsewhere; / n /
word-initially before remaining vowels; / hn / before remaining vowels
elsewhere.
Oo. / hni / before consonants; / hj / before / a / and / ẽ /; / hn / before
remaining vowels.
Se. / hniː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / hni / before / hV / with loss of
/ h /, and before remaining consonants; / hnj / before / a / and / ɛ̃ /
( appearance of / n / interpreted as analogy ); / hn / before remaining
vowels.
Hu. / thi / following / s / before a consonant; / hi / before a consonant
otherwise; / hj / before / a /; / th / following / s / before remaining
vowels; / h / following / t / before remaining vowels; / ihn /
word-initially before remaining vowels; and / hn / before remaining
vowels elsewhere.
399
word-initially before / i /; / wat / before / i / otherwise; / ẽn /
word-initially before a vowel; / wẽn / before a vowel elsewhere.
⁰⁵⁹ PNI * { kõti } '3PL:3' ( second variant, in competition with * { wati } ⁰⁵⁸ )
PNI * / kõti / before consonants; * / kõ / before * / a / with loss of * / a /;
* / kũt / before * / i /; * / kõn / before remaining vowels.
Mo. / kũti / before consonants; / kũ / before / a / with loss of / a /; / kũt /
before / i /; / kũn / before remaining vowels.
Oe. / kũti / before consonants; / kũ / before / a / with loss of / a /; / kũt /
before / i /; / kũn / before remaining vowels.
Oo. / kũti / before consonants; / kũ / before / a / with loss of / a /; / kũn /
before remaining vowels.
Hu. / kõ / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before / a / with loss of / a /;
/ xõ / before / a / otherwise with loss of / a / ( partial paradigm
preserved following merger with reflexes of PNI * { wati } ⁰⁵⁹ ).
400
Se. / hatiː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / hati / before / hV / with loss of
/ h /, and before remaining consonants; / hɛñ / before vowels.
Hu. / hati / before consonants; / hat / before / i /; / hõ / before / a / with loss
of / a /; / hẽn / before remaining vowels.
401
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Mo. / ɹoti / word-initially before consonants; / hoti / before consonants
elsewhere; / ɹot / word-initially before / i /; / hot / before / i /
elsewhere; / ɹon / word-initially before remaining vowels; / hon /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / loti / word-initially before consonants; / hoti / before consonants
elsewhere; / lot / word-initially before / i /; / hot / before / i /
elsewhere; / lon / word-initially before remaining vowels; / hon /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / hoti / before consonants; / hon / before vowels.
Ca. / hoti / before consonants; / hot / before / i /; / hon / before remaining
vowels.
Se. / hotiː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / hoti / before / hV / with loss of
/ h /, and before remaining consonants; / hon / before vowels.
Hu. / hoti / before consonants; / hot / before / i /; / hon / before remaining
vowels.
402
/ ak / word-initially before some bases beginning in / e /, and
word-initially before / ʌ̃ /, / o / and / ũ /; / jak / before some bases
beginning in / e / elsewhere, and before / ʌ̃ /, / o / and / ũ / elsewhere;
/ ʌ̃ / word-initially before / e / with loss of / e /; / jʌ̃ / before / e /
elsewhere with loss of / e /; / e / word-initially before some bases
beginning in / i / with loss of / i /, and word-initially before consonants;
/ je / before some bases beginning in / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /,
and before consonants elsewhere.
Oo. / ũ / word-initially before / a / with loss of / a /; / jũ / before / a /
elsewhere with loss of / a /; / a / word-initially before some bases
beginning in / i /; / ja / before some bases beginning in / i / elsewhere;
/ ak / word-initially before some bases beginning in / e /, and
word-initially before / ẽ /, / o / and / ũ /; / jak / before some bases
beginning in / e / elsewhere, and before / ẽ /, / o / and / ũ / elsewhere;
/ ẽ / word-initially before / e / with loss of / e /; / jẽ / before / e /
elsewhere with loss of / e /; / e / word-initially before some bases
beginning in / i / with loss of / i /, and word-initially before consonants;
/ je / before some bases beginning in / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /,
and before consonants elsewhere.
Ca. / õ / word-initially before / a / with loss of / a /; / jõ / before / a /
elsewhere with loss of / a /; / a / word-initially before some bases
beginning in / i /; / ja / before some bases beginning in / i /
elsewhere; / ak / word-initially before some bases beginning in / e /, and
word-initially before / ẽ /, / o /, / õ /, and / u /; / jak / before some
bases beginning in / e / elsewhere, and before / ẽ /, / o /, / õ /, and
/ ũ / elsewhere; / ẽ / word-initially before / e / with loss of / e /; / jẽ /
before / e / elsewhere with loss of / e /; / e / word-initially before some
bases beginning in / i / with loss of / i /, and word-initially before
consonants; / je / before some bases beginning in / i / elsewhere with
loss of / i /, and before consonants elsewhere.
Se. / jɔ̃ / before / a / with loss of / a /; / ja / before some bases beginning in
/ i /; / jak / before some bases beginning in / a /, and before / o / and
/ ɔ̃ /; / jɛ̃ / before / e / and / ɛ̃ / with loss of / e / and / ɛ̃ /; / je / before
some bases beginning in / i / with loss of / i /, and before consonants.
Hu. / jõ / following a consonant before / a / with loss of / a /; / õ / before / a /
elsewhere with loss of / a /; / jax / following a consonant before some
403
bases beginning in / e /, and before / ẽ /, / o /, and / õ /; / ax / before
some bases beginning in / e / elsewhere, and before / ẽ /, / o /, and
/ õ / elsewhere; / jaxẽ / following a consonant before some bases
beginning in / i / with loss of / i /; / axẽ / before some bases beginning
in / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / jẽ / following a consonant before
some bases beginning in / e / with loss of / e /; / ẽ / before some bases
beginning in / e / elsewhere with loss of / e /; / je / following a
consonant before some bases beginning in / i / with loss of / i /, and
following a consonant before a consonant; / e / before some bases
beginning in / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /, and before a consonant
elsewhere.
404
/ ako / word-initially before some bases beginning in / ʌ̃ / with loss of
/ ʌ̃ /, and word-initially before / a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /,
and word-initially before consonants; / jako / before some bases
beginning in / ʌ̃ / elsewhere with loss of / ʌ̃ /, and before / a / and / i /
elsewhere with loss of / a / and / i /, and before consonants elsewhere.
Oo. / kaw / word-initially before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ẽ /;
/ jakaw / before / e / elsewhere and before some bases beginning in
/ ẽ / elsewhere; / ka / word-initially before / o / and / ũ /; / jaka /
before / o / and / ũ / elsewhere; / ko / word-initially before some bases
beginning in / ẽ / with loss of / ẽ /, and word-initially before / a / and
/ i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and word-initially before consonants;
/ jako / before some bases beginning in / ẽ / elsewhere with loss of / ẽ /,
and before / a / and / i / elsewhere with loss of / a / and / i /, and
before consonants elsewhere.
Ca. / kaw / word-initially or following / ka / before / e / and some bases
beginning in / ẽ /; / jakaw / before / e / and some bases beginning in
/ ẽ / elsewhere; / ka / word-initially or following / ka / before / o /,
/ õ / or / u /; / jaka / before / o /, / õ / or / u / elsewhere; / ko /
word-initially or following / ka / before some bases beginning in / ẽ /
with loss of / ẽ /, and word-initially following / a / and / i / with loss of
/ a / and / i /, and word-initially before consonants; / jako / before some
bases beginning in / ẽ / elsewhere with loss of / ẽ /, and before / a / and
/ i / elsewhere with loss of / a / and / i /, and before consonants
elsewhere.
Se. / kaw / word-initially before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ɛ̃ /;
/ jakaw / before / e / elsewhere and before some bases beginning in
/ ɛ̃ / elsewhere; / ka / word-initially before / o / and / ɔ̃ /; / jaka /
before / o / and / ɔ̃ / elsewhere; / ko / word-initially before some bases
beginning in / ɛ̃ / with loss of / ɛ̃ /, and word-initially before / a / and
/ i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and word-initially before consonants;
/ jako / before some bases beginning in / ɛ̃ / elsewhere with loss of / ɛ̃ /,
and before / a / and / i / elsewhere with loss of / a / and / i /, and
before consonants elsewhere.
Hu. / jaxaw / following a consonant before / e / and some bases beginning in
/ ẽ /; / axaw / before / e / and some bases beginning in / ẽ / elsewhere;
/ jaxa / following a consonant before / o / or / õ /; / axa / before / o /
405
and / õ / elsewhere; / jaxo / following a consonant before some bases
beginning in / ẽ / with loss of / ẽ /, and following a consonant before
/ a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /, and following a consonant
before a consonant; / axo / before some bases beginning in / ẽ /
elsewhere with loss of / ẽ /, and before / a / and / i / elsewhere with
loss of / a / and / i /, and before consonants elsewhere.
406
with loss of / ẽ /, and before / a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /,
and before consonants.
Ca. / shakaw / word-initially or following a consonant before / e / and some
bases beginning in / ẽ /; / hshakaw / before / e / and some bases
beginning in / ẽ / elsewhere; / shaka / word-initially or following a
consonant before / o /, / õ / or / u /; / hshaka / before / o /, / õ / or
/ u / elsewhere; / shako / word-initially or following a consonant before
some bases beginning in / ẽ / with loss of / ẽ /, and word-initially or
following a consonant before / a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /,
and word-initially or following a consonant before consonants;
/ hshako / before some bases beginning in / ẽ / elsewhere with loss of
/ ẽ /, and before / a / and / i / elsewhere with loss of / a / and / i /, and
before consonants elsewhere.
Se. / shakaw / before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ɛ̃ /; / shaka /
before / o / and / õ /; / shako / before some bases beginning in / ɛ̃ /
with loss of / ɛ̃ /, and before / a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /,
and before consonants.
Hu. / shaxaw / before / e / and before some bases beginning in / ẽ /; / shaxa /
before / o / and / õ /; / shaxo / before some bases beginning in / ẽ /
with loss of / ẽ /, and before / a / and / i / with loss of / a / and / i /,
and before consonants.
407
remaining consonants; / kɔ̃wɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i / with
loss of / i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / kɔ̃wɔ̃j / before / o / and
/ ɔ̃ /; / kɔ̃w / before some bases beginning in / i /, and before remaining
vowels.
Hu. / kõwa / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before a consonant, and
following any consonant except / Ɂ / before / o / or / õ /; / xõwa /
before consonants elsewhere, and before / o / and / õ / elsewhere;
/ kõwẽ / following any consonant except / Ɂ / before / i / with loss of
/ i /; / xõwẽ / before / i / elsewhere with loss of / i /; / kõw / following
any consonant except / Ɂ / and before remaining vowels; / xõw / before
remaining vowels elsewhere.
408
/ hõwaj / before / o /, / õ /, and / ũ /; / hõw / before remaining vowels
elsewhere.
Se. / hɔ̃wæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / hɔ̃waː / before / hC / with loss
of / h /; / hõwa / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before remaining
consonants; / hɔ̃wɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i / with loss of
/ i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / hɔ̃wɔ̃j / before / o / and / ɔ̃ /;
/ hɔ̃w / before some bases beginning in / i /, and before remaining
vowels.
Hu. / hõwa / before consonants, and before / o / and / õ /; / hõwẽ / before / i /
with loss of / i /; / hõw / before remaining vowels.
409
elsewhere; / ɹũwʌ̃n / word-initially before remaining vowels; / hũwʌ̃n /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / lũwati / word-initially before consonants; / hũwati / before consonants
elsewhere; / lũwat / word-initially before / i /; / hũwat / before / i /
elsewhere; / lũwʌ̃n / word-initially before remaining vowels; / hũwʌ̃n /
before remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oo. / hũwati / before consonants; / hũwan / before vowels.
Ca. / hõwati / before consonants; / hõwat / before / i /; / hõwẽn / before
remaining vowels.
Se. / hɔ̃wɔ̃tiː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / hɔ̃wɔ̃ti / before / hV / with loss
of / h /, and before remaining consonants; / hɔ̃wɛ̃n / before vowels.
Hu. / hõwati / before consonants; / hõwat / before / i /; / hõwẽn / before
remaining vowels.
410
or following a consonant before remaining vowels; / hshakon / before
remaining vowels elsewhere.
Oe. / shakoti / before consonants; / shakot / before / i /; / shakon / before
remaining vowels.
Oo. / shakoti / before consonants; / shakon / before vowels.
Se. / shakotiː / before / hC / with loss of / h /; / shakoti / before / hV / with
loss of / h /, and before remaining consonants; / shakon / before vowels.
411
clusters ( 16.4 ), and before vowels elsewhere.
412
Oe. / takwa / before consonants; / takwʌ̃ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / takj /
before / o / and / ũ /; / takw / before remaining vowels.
Oo. / takwa / before consonants; / takwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / takj /
before / o / and / ũ /; / takw / before remaining vowels.
Ca. / tahskwa / before consonants; / tahskwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /;
/ tahskj / before / o /, / õ / and / u /; / tahskw / before remaining
vowels.
Se. / takwæː / before / Ræ / with loss of / Ræ /; / takwaː / before / hC / with
loss of / h /; / takwa / before / hV / with loss of / h /, and before
remaining consonants; / takwɛ̃ / before some bases beginning in / i /
with loss of / i /, and before / jɔ̃ / with loss of / j /; / takj / before / o /
and / ɔ̃ /; / takw / before some bases beginning in / i /, and before
remaining vowels.
Hu. / taxwa / before consonants; / taxwẽ / before / i / with loss of / i /; / taxj /
before / o / and / õ /; / taxw / before remaining vowels.
413
/ hse / before certain consonants and clusters ( 14.4 ) elsewhere; / s /
word-initially before certain other consonants and clusters ( 14.4 ), and
word-initially before vowels; / hs / before certain other consonants and
clusters ( 14.4 ) elsewhere, and before vowels elsewhere ( recast by
analogy with '2SG:3' ).
Se. / ts / before / i /, and before / j / with loss of / j /; / se / before certain
remaining consonants and clusters ( cf. 15.4 ); / s / before certain other
remaining consonants and clusters ( 15.4 ), and before remaining vowels.
Hu. / ʃ / before / ɹV /; / ts / before / i / and before some bases beginning in
/ j / ( < PNI * / j / ) with loss of / j /; / s / before / nV /, and before
remaining vowels; / se / before certain remaining consonants and
clusters ( see 16.4 ).
414
consonants and clusters ( cf. 14.4 ); / atat / before certain other
consonants and clusters ( 14.4 ), and before remaining vowels.
Se. / atɛ̃ / before / Hn /; / atɛ̃n / before / i /; / atatsh / before / ts / and / hs /
with loss of / ts / and / hs /; / atah / before / t / and / n /; / atate /
before certain remaining consonants and clusters ( cf. 15.4 ); / atat /
before certain other remaining consonants and clusters ( 15.4 ), and
before remaining vowels.
Hu. / atẽ / before / Hn /; / atẽn / before / i /; / ata / before / kV /; / atate /
before certain remaining consonants and clusters ( cf. 16.4 ); / atat /
before certain other remaining consonants and clusters ( 16.4 ), and
before remaining vowels.
415
Se. / ɛ̃ / before / Hn /; / ɛ̃n / before / i /; / atsh / before / ts / and / hs / with
loss of / ts / and / hs /; / ah / before / t / and / n /; / ate / before
certain remaining consonants and clusters ( cf. 15.4 ); / at / before
certain other remaining consonants and clusters ( 15.4 ), and before
remaining vowels.
Hu. / ẽ / before / Hn /; / ẽn / before / i /; / a / before / kV /; / ate / before
certain remaining consonants and clusters ( cf. 16.4 ); / at / before
certain other remaining consonants and clusters ( 16.4 ), and before
remaining vowels.
416
(MS 59:78).
Wy. / a / in / hutiɁtaˈɹaːẽɁ / 'they have the (same) clan' (cf. Barbeau 1960:100).
417
⁰⁸² PI * { t } 'causative-instrumental' ( lexically determined variant 1 )
PI * / t /.
Ch. / t / in / ȕːlhàwìːttánə̃ː́ Ɂi ̋ː / 'he made it fly' (Feeling & Pulte 1975:21).
PNI * / t /, * / (h)t /, * / (ah)t /, * / (Ɂ)t /, * / (aɁ)t /.
Tu. / (h)t / in / raˈrihthaɁ / 'he cooks it [lit. makes it cooked]'
(cf. Rudes 1999:567).
Mo. / (h)t / in / kheˈtkʌ̃hthaɁ / 'I make, cause something to be ugly
[lit. make it ugly]' (cf. Maracle 1990:150).
Oe. / (aɁ)t / in / jũkʷahlũˈkâːthaɁ / 'she sends word to me [lit. makes me know]'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:66).
Oo. / (aɁ)t / in / waɁhatéˈkaɁtaɁ / 'he kindled a fire [lit. made it burn]'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:112).
Ca. / (ih)t / in / ẽhˈseːtkiht / 'you will dirty it up [lit. make it dirty]'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:737).
Se. / (Ɂ)t / in / kahˈtɔ̃ɁthaɁ / 'I make it disappear [lit. make it lost]'
(cf. Chafe 1967:24).
Hu. / (ah)t / in / ʃɹihˈwaːwaht / <chrih8awat> 'abbrége ton discours'
(MS 59:1), preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
Wy. / (ah)t / in / aɁˈʃakjesaht / 'you spoiled it [lit. made it wasted]'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:59).
418
Se. / st / in / ɛ̃ˈkanɔ̃Ɂnos / 'it will get cold [lit. will be made cold]'
(cf. Chafe 1967:24).
Hu. / hst / in / ʃjaɁtaˈnohsthaɁ / <chiaatand8sθa?> 'as tu froid?
[lit. your body is made cold?]' (MS 59:106), preconsonantal / h /
assumed present.
Wy. / hst / in / nusahaˈaɁtanuhst / 'when he got cold again
[lit. was made cold again]' (cf. Barbeau 1960:173).
419
Oe. / (a)kw / in / lonũwaloˈtakwʌ̃ / 'he has pulled the nails out
[lit. un-stood-up the nails]' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:617).
Oo. / (a)kw / in / hateɁskṹˈtaːkwas / 'he is taking it out of the oven
[lit. un-putting it on the fire]' (cf. Woodbury 2003:128).
Ca. / ko / in / ẽtshatshaˈtõːkoɁ / 'you will unearth it [lit. un-bury it]'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:353).
Se. / ko / in / sehotɔ̃ːkoh / 'open the door! [lit. un-shut the door!]'
(cf. Chafe 1967:24).
Hu. / (a)w / in / taxitõnjoˈtawaɁs / <Ta˛itonni8ta8as> 'arrache moy ce clou
[lit. un-stand-up the nail for me!]' (MS 59:201), loss of * / k /
anomalous.
Wy. / (a)w / in / sahɔ̃kjaɁˈtɔ̃tawaɁ / 'they unfastened the body
[lit. un-attached the body]' (cf. Barbeau 1960:293).
420
Se. / Ɂ / in / aˈkjɛ̃ːɁɔ̃h / 'I've gotten hurt' (cf. Chafe 1967:23).
Hu. / (a)Ɂ / in / ehʃihnaxẽɹaˈtaɁhaɁ / <echinda˛enrataha>
'ta peau deviendra blanche' (MS 59:68), / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. / (a)Ɂ / in / jaˈkjeːsaɁs / 'it gets easy' (cf. Barbeau 1960:284).
421
Hu. / ẽ / in / eshõˈxjõnjẽɁ / <eson˛onnien> 'il nous mettra' (MS 59:184).
Wy. / ɛ̃ / in / eskejaˈʒaɁsɛ̃Ɂ / 'you will pay me' (cf. Barbeau 1960:105).
422
⁰⁹² PNI * { h } 'dislocative' ( lexically determined variant 2 )
PNI * / h /.
Tu. / h / in / wɛwahˈknə̃hɛɁ / 'it was going and coming' (cf. Rudes 1999:19).
Mo. / h / in / ɹotoˈɹathũ / 'he has gone to hunt' (cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:447).
Oe. / h / in / wakatlohˈlokhũ / 'I have gone there to watch'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:240).
Oo. / h / in / tewakaǽhˈtathũh / 'I have gone somewhere to run'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:75).
Ca. / h / in / sihˈsaːkhah / 'go and look for it' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:738).
Se. / h / in / ˈhoihsakhɔ̃ːh / 'he's gone looking for it' (cf. Chafe 1967:25).
Hu. / h / in / taxeɁnhataɁeɁsẽˈniha / <ta˛ennhataesenniha>
'vas bucher pr. moy' (MS 59:3).
Wy. / h / in / ahatiɁˈʒaɁkaɁ / 'they went out hunting' (cf. Barbeau 1960:112).
423
Se. / sR / in / ɛ̃katɛ̃ˈnɔ̃Ɂɔ̃shæɁ / 'I'll go and watch over it' (cf. Chafe 1967:25).
Hu. / (h)ʃr / in / satsihstahˈɹehʃɹah / <Satsistarecha> 'vas attiser le feu'
(MS 59:138), preconsonantal / h / assumed present, / ɹ / has become
/ h / in Rock dialect.
Wy. / (Ɂ)ʃɹ / in / hehˈsehskjɔ̃Ɂʃɹah / 'you go to make love to him'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:78).
424
(cf. Rudes 1999:21).
Ca. / (h)nõ / in / kaˈhaɁtõhnõɁ / 'a clothesline [lit. several things drying]'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:738).
Se. / (h)nɔ̃ / in / haːwiːnɔ̃h / 'he's carrying things' (cf. Chafe 1967:24).
Hu. / (aɁ)nõ / in / hajẽhwiˈtaɁnõhk / <haïenh8itannonk> 'il ē a toute main'
(MS 59:80), / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. / (aɁ)nɔ̃ / in / hɔ̃waɛ̃hˈtaɁnɔ̃ / 'they hit him all over' (cf. Barbeau 1960:168).
425
¹⁰⁰ PNI * { ɹõː } 'distributive' ( lexically determined variant 5 )
PNI * / (a)ɹõː /.
Tu. / rə̃ː / in / wahratiˈjə̃Ɂkhrə̃ːɁ / 'he shot several (arrows)'
(cf. Rudes 1999:212).
Oo. / (a)hẽː / in / ẽkaháɁˈtahẽːk / '[several things] will be dry'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:495).
Se. / (a)ɔ̃ / in / hatijẽˈthwakwaɔ̃h / 'they (masc. pl.) are harvesting things'
(cf. Chafe 1967:24).
426
Oo. / (h)sẽː / in / tejotóˈwẽhsẽːɁ / 'it is split into several pieces, it is splintered'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:168).
Se. / sjɔ̃ / in / ˈoetkɛ̃Ɂsjɔ̃Ɂ / 'things have spoiled' (cf. Chafe 1967:24).
Hu. / (h)ʃɹõː / in { -atathɹehʃɹõː- } 'several to push against each other',
cf. <K8atatrechon> 'Multip. Recip. plus.rs. S'entrepousser' (MS 59:138),
preconsonantal / h / and long vowel assumed present, / ɹ / has become
/ h / in Rock dialect.
427
Ca. / hs / in / hanjohs / 'he kills s.t.' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:173).
Se. / as / in / kekwas / 'I pick it' (cf. Chafe 1967:12).
Hu. / ʃ / in / ʃehˈskẽːnahʃ / <chieskendach> 'tu es un Mangeur de Morts'
(MS 59:148).
Wy. / s / in / keaɁˈtihʃahs / 'I look for one' (cf. Barbeau 1960:225).
428
Mo. / Ø / word-finally in / ɹatheˈɹũːni / 'he is making a basket'
(cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:443).
Oe. / heɁ / in / kũːˈniheɁ / 'I'm making it' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:756).
Oo. / k / in / ˈkhníːnũk / 'I am a buyer, I buy' (cf. Woodbury 2003:538).
Ca. / h / in / ˈkõːnih / 'I make, earn' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:191).
Se. / h / in / jeˈsheɁɔ̃ːnih / 'she makes the dough' (cf. Chafe 1967:12).
Hu. / hk / in / xõatehaɁˈsẽːnihk / <˛onatehasennik> 'jay honte pr. toy'
(MS 59:22), / h / assumed present.
¹⁰⁹ PNI * { hsk } 'imperfective suffix' ( cf. Cherokee imperfective verbs in { hsk } )
PNI * / hsk /.
OMo. / hsk / in / ˈjahtə̃Ɂ shakohseˈɹũːnihsk / <Ja‛ten Sagoseronnisk>
'il ne les accode point d’habits' (MS 58:10).
Mo. / hs / in / katheˈɹũːnihs / 'I make baskets' (cf. Maracle 1990:123).
Ca. / hs / in / ˈkõːnihs / 'I make, earn' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:191).
429
/ Ø / word-finally in / seˈnohare / 'wash!'
(cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:447).
Oe. / Ɂ / in / waɁkahˈtsjohaleɁ / 'I washed my hands'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:84); / Ø / word-finally in / sahˈtsjohale /
'Wash your hands!' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:84).
Oo. / Ɂ / in / waɁkéˈktũɁ / 'I examined it' (cf. Woodbury 2003:663); / h / in
/ seˈktũh / 'Check it out!' (cf. Woodbury 2003:663).
Ca. / Ɂ / in / ẽhˈsheːtẽːɁ / 'you will pity her' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:487); / Ø /
word-finally in / tahˈskiːtẽː / 'pity me' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:487).
Se. / Ɂ / in / ɛ̃sniːkɛ̃Ɂ / 'you'll see it' (cf. Chafe 1967:17); / h / in / haːkɛ̃h /
'let him see it!' (cf. Chafe 1967:12).
Hu. / Ɂ / in / aɁthaɹihnaɁˈneːtẽɁ / <aθaɹindandeten> 'il a redit deux fois
la m. ch.' (MS 59:86), / Ɂ / assumed present; / h / in / taxɹihwaˈwahtẽh /
<Ta˛rih8awa‛ten> 'abbrége moy cette affaire' (MS 59:1), / h / assumed
present.
Wy. / Ɂ / in / eːʒaːˈʒuɁ / 'he will kill you' (cf. Barbeau 1960:82).
430
¹¹³ PNI * { Ø } 'punctual suffix' ( lexically determined variant 3 )
PNI * / Ø / in both modal and imperative verbs.
Tu. / Ø / word-finally in / ə̃Ɂˈnjɛtʃnə̃ht / 'one will get down there'
(cf. Rudes 1999:6).
OMo. / Ø / word-finally in / wahsɹihˈwahstoht / <8asrih8asθot>
'tu amoindris l'affaire' (Bruyas 1863:102).
Mo. / eɁ / in / ʌ̃haˈtoːɹateɁ / 'he will hunt' (cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:447);
/ Ø / word-finally in / saˈtoːɹat / 'hunt!' (cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:447).
Oe. / eɁ / in / waɁkeɁˈnetskahteɁ / 'I loosened it'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:852); / Ø / word-finally in / seɁˈnetskat /
'loosen it!' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:852).
Oo. / Ø / word-finally in / aːháːˈkaːjaɁk / 'he would pay'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:444).
Ca. / Ø / word-finally in / ẽhˈsehsthoɁtɹẽht / 'you will pluck feathers'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:481).
Se. / Ø / word-finally in / sɔ̃Ɂneːk / 'move out of the way!'
(cf. Chafe 1967:13).
Hu. / Ø / word-finally in / tiˈsaɁehst / <ti saest> 'perce cela' (MS 59:3).
Wy. / Ø / word-finally in / naɁˈjɔ̃ːnɔ̃t / 'that I gave to you'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:124).
431
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:389).
Se. / t / in / oɁjɔ̃ːsheɁt / 'it became long' (cf. Chafe 1967:24).
Hu. / haɁ / in / ewatjeˈsaɁhaɁ / <e8atiesaha> 'cela deviendra facile'
(MS 59:27), / Ɂ / assumed present; / hah / in / satsaˈxẽɁhah / 'go out!',
cf. <Satsa˛enha> 'Sors' (MS 59:39), final / h / assumed present.
432
OMo. / Ø / word-finally in / joˈɹahkot / <jorak8t> 'il fait chaud'
(Bruyas 1863:86).
Mo. / (eɁ) / in / joˈɹahkoteɁ / 'it is sunny' (cf. Maracle 1990:20).
Oe. / (eɁ) / in / lanawiːˈloteɁ / 'he has a tooth, he has teeth'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:557).
Oo. / (aɁ) / in / óhˈnjoːtaɁ / 'it is protruding from the ground,
it is growing out of the ground' (cf. Woodbury 2003:543).
Ca. / Ø / word-finally in / oˈkwaõt / 'it has a lump; a bulge'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:190).
Se. / Ø / word-finally in / kaniːjɔ̃ːt / 'it's hanging, it's been hung'
(cf. Chafe 1967:12).
Hu. / Ø / word-finally in / ˈiːwaːt / <i8at> 'il y a q.c. dedans' (MS 59:19).
Wy. / Ø / word-finally in / tsuˈnɛ̃hstat / '[it is] one grain' (cf. Barbeau 1960:90).
433
¹²⁰ PNI * { n } 'past perfect suffix'
PNI * / n / or * / nVɁ /.
Mo. / neɁ / in / ɹotoɹaˈthũhneɁ / 'he had gone to hunt'
(cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:447).
Oe. / neɁ / in / loleɁsʌ̃ː̂ ˈhnêː / 'he used to be fat'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:99).
Oo. / naɁ / in / hatowǽˈtihnaɁ / 'he had been hunting'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:167).
Ca. / neːɁ / in / akathɹoˈwihneːɁ / 'I'd told' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:741).
Se. / nɔ̃Ɂ / in / haˈjasɔ̃ːnɔ̃Ɂ / 'it was his name, he was called' (cf. Chafe 1967:28).
Hu. / nẽɁ / in / naõxaɹahˈskʷẽhnẽɁ / <d'aon˛arask8annen>
'le dessein que javois de partir' (MS 59:5), / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. / nɛ̃Ɂ / in / hunatɹiˈʒuːnɛ̃Ɂ / 'they had fought' (cf. Barbeau 1960:292).
434
Mo. / (a)k / in / ʌ̃hoˈjə̃ːtakeɁ / 'he will have it' (cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:444).
Oe. / (a)k / in / ʌ̃kaɁslehtiˈjohakeɁ / 'it will be a good car'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:441).
Oo. / k / in / ẽhohkáˈstek / 'he will be tough' (cf. Woodbury 2003:228).
Ca. / (aː)k / in / ẽkathɹoˈwihaːk / 'I will be telling' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:741).
Se. / (eː)k / in / ɛ̃ˈwɔ̃kathɔ̃ːteːk / 'I'll continue to be listening'
(cf. Chafe 1967:28).
Hu. / k / in / etsikwahwatsiɹaˈweːxihk / <etsik8ah8atsira8e˛ik>
'nous nous retrouverons avec nos Parents' (MS 59:78).
Wy. / (a)h / in / ewateˈjahstah / 'it will be boiled down' (cf. Barbeau 1960:124).
435
¹²⁵ PNI * { haːɁ } 'characterizer suffix'
PNI * / haːɁ /.
Tu. / haːɁ / in / ohahaɁˈkjɛhaːɁ / 'plantain [lit. 'the one that is on the path]'
(cf. Rudes 1999:11).
Mo. / ha / in / ahsũˈthʌ̃kha / 'the night time star, the moon
[lit. the midnight one]' (cf. Maracle 1990:2).
Oe. / ha / in / kalhaˈkũha / 'hawk [lit. the one in the forest]'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:457).
Oo. / haɁ / in / onũtaɁkeˈhaɁ / 'Onondaga [lit. the one at the hill]'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:761).
Ca. / haːɁ / in / nehaːɁ /, / kehaːɁ /, / khaːɁ / 'customary suffixes'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:714).
Se. / haːɁ / in / okeoɁˈtsaɁkeːaɁ / 'green smake [lit. the one on the grass]'
(cf. Chafe 1967:29).
436
Se. / tsiːwẽh / in / saˈthɔ̃tehtsiːwɛ̃h / 'you are listening carefully'
(cf. Chafe 1967:25).
437
(cf. Barbeau 1960:301).
438
APPENDIX B: PARTICLES AND LEXICAL ROOTS
439
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:306).
Se. { -ahjoɁtsis } in / teˈjoːjoɁtsis / 'it's sour' (cf. Chafe 1967:58).
Hu. { -oɁtsihst- } in / aoɁˈtsihsteɁ / <aotsiste> 'cela ē aigre' (MS 59:199), / Ɂ /
and / h / assumed present.
440
¹³⁶ PNI * { -(a)hõht- } 'ear'
Tu. { -hə̃hn- } in / oˈhə̃hnɛh / 'ear' (cf. Rudes 1999:202).
No. * { -hõhˀt- } in <Suntunke> 'The ears' (cf. Rudes 1981a:46).
La. * { -ahõht- } in <Ahontascon> 'Les oreilles' (Biggar 1924:241).
Mo. { -ahũht- } in / oˈhũhtaɁ / 'an ear, the outer ear' (cf. Maracle 1990:243).
Oe. { -ahũht- } in / oˈhũhtaɁ / 'ear' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:86).
Oo. { -ahũht- } in / óˈhũhtaɁ / 'ear' (cf. Woodbury 2003:565).
Ca. { -ahõht- } in / oˈhõhtaɁ / 'ear' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:99).
Se. { -aɔ̃ht- } in / ˈoɔ̃htaɁ / 'ear' (cf. Chafe 1967:39).
Hu. { -ahõht- } in / aˈhõhtaɁ / <Ahonta> 'oreille' (MS 59:4), preconsonantal
/ h / assumed present.
Wy. { -ahɔ̃ht- } in / tewahɔ̃hˈteːtsih / 'it has long ears' (cf. Barbeau 1960:273).
[ cf. Mithun 1981b:239 ]
441
¹³⁹ PI * { -aːhs- }, * { -aːhsiɁt- } 'foot'
Ch. { -àːhs- } in { -áːhsùːhkàl- } 'claw' (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:179), { -àːhsìht- }
in / ùːlȁːsihte̋ːnîː / 'his foot' (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:172), with apparent
exchange of / h / and / Ɂ /.
PNI * { -ahs- }, * { -ahsiɁt- } 'foot'.
Tu. { -ahs- } in / ˈkahsɛh / 'my foot' (cf. Rudes 1999:29).
No. * { -ahs- } in <Saseeke> 'The foot' (cf. Rudes 1981a:46).
La. * { -ahʃiɁt- } in <Ouchidascon> 'Les piedz' (Biggar 1924:242).
Mo. { -ahsiɁt- } in / ohˈsîːtaɁ / 'foot' (cf. Maracle 1990:64).
Oe. { -ahsiɁt- } in / ohˈsîːtaɁ / 'foot' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:71).
Oo. { -ahsiɁt- } in / ohˈsiɁtaɁ / 'foot, hoof' (cf. Woodbury 2003:238).
Ca. { -ahsiɁt- } in / ohˈsiɁtaɁ / 'feet' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:125).
Se. { -ahsiɁt- } in / kahˈsiɁtaɁ / 'my foot' (cf. Chafe 1967:38).
Hu. { -ahʃiɁt- } in / ahˈʃiɁtaɁ / <Achita> 'Pied' (MS 59:3),
/ h / and / Ɂ / assumed present.
[ cf. Lounsbury 1978:342, Mithun 1979:183, Rudes 1995:45 ]
442
[ cf. Mithun 1981:276, Rudes 1995:55 ]
¹⁴⁴ PNI * { -ahtkaɁw- } 'abandon, give up, leave behind, let go, quit, release'
Mo. { -ahtkaɁw- } in / kahˈtkâːwahs / 'I let go, release, quit, desist, I resign,
abdicate, abandon' (cf. Maracle 1990:35).
Oo. { -atkaɁw- } in / hóˈtkaɁwih / 'he has left it behind, he has let it go'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:269).
Ca. { -ahtkaɁw- } in / sahtkaɁs / 'you forfeit, you let go of things all the time'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:415).
Se. { -atkaɁw- } in / aˈkatkaɁwɛ̃h / 'I've provided it' (cf. Chafe 1967:43).
Hu. { -ahkaɁw- }, cf. <A‛ka8an> 'cesser, laisser, abandonner q.c. or qlq.'
(MS 59:5), / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -ahkaɁw- } in / eˈhɔ̃hkaɁ / 'they will give up' (cf. Barbeau 1960:292).
443
¹⁴⁵ PNI * { -ahtõ- }, * { -ahtõɁ- } 'disappear, vanish'
Tu. { -ahnə̃- } in / ˈwahnə̃h / 'it disappears' (cf. Rudes 1999:20).
Mo. { -ahtũɁ- } in / johˈtũ̂ːɁũ / 'it has disappeared, vanished, is lost'
(cf. Maracle 1990:292), with inchoative.
Oe. { -ahtũɁ- } in / johˈtũ̂ːũ / 'it has vanished'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:84), with inchoative.
Oo. { -ahtũ- } in / ˈwáhtũk / 'it disappears, it is easy to lose'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:224).
Ca. { -ahtõ- } in / wahtõhs / 'it disappears' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:89).
Se. { -ahtɔ̃Ɂ- } in / ohˈtɔ̃Ɂɔ̃h / 'it's lost' (cf. Chafe 1967:38), with inchoative.
Hu. { -ahtõ- }, cf. <A‛ton> 'etre perdu, égarer, oublié, effacé' (MS 59:31).
¹⁴⁷ PNI * { -ahtskehnh- } 'argue for, compete for, contend for, fight for'
Tu. { -ahθkjɛnh- } in / rahˈθkjɛnhaɁ / 'he is fighting for it, he is competing for it'
(cf. Rudes 1999:27).
Mo. { -ahskehnh- } in / kahˈskehnhahs / 'I argue for, strive for, contend;
I am a rival for' (cf. Maracle 1990:27).
Oe. { -askenh- } in / kaˈskenhas / 'I try to take something away from someone,
I'm greedy' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:121).
Oo. { -askehnh- } in / hṹˈskehnhas / 'they contend for it'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:249).
444
¹⁴⁸ PNI * { ahtsõh }, * { atsõh } 'not yet, still'
Tu. / ˈaθə̃ theɁ / 'not yet' (cf. Rudes 1999:50).
Oe. / ˈahsũ / 'not yet' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:75).
Oo. / ˈáhsũh / 'again, more, yet, still' (cf. Woodbury 2003:241).
Ca. / ahsõh / 'still, yet, some more' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:674).
Hu. / ˈahsõh / <a‛son> 'encore' (MS 59:111),
final / h / and penultimate accent assumed present.
Wy. / sɔ̃h / 'still' (cf. Barbeau 1960:288).
445
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:171).
446
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:92).
́ ˈnaksẽh / 'fox' (cf. Woodbury 2003:782).
Oo. { -aksẽh } in / skẽh
Hu. { -ahʃẽːh }, cf. <gachen> 'laid, mauv. meprisable' (MS 59:44),
long vowel and / h / assumed present.
447
with unexpected loss of vowel length in final syllable.
Mo. { -aɹaɁseɁ } in / ũkjaɹaɁˈsêːhaɁ / 'my cousin' (Deering & Delisle 1976:32).
Oe. { -alaɁseɁ } in / ũkjaˈlâːseɁ / 'my cousin' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:98).
Oo. { -aæɁseɁ } in / ṹˈkjaæɁseɁ / 'we two are cousins, my cousin'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:77).
Ca. { -aɁseːɁ } in / õˈkjaɁseːɁ / 'my cousin' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:610).
Se. { -æːɁseːɁ } in / aˈkjææɁseːɁ / 'we are cousins' (cf. Chafe 1967:37).
Hu. { -aɹaɁseːɁ }, cf. <Arase> 'Cousin' (MS 59:11),
long vowel and reflexes of * / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -aɹaɁseɁ } in / hɔ̃ˈɹaɁseɁ / 'they are cousins' (cf. Barbeau 1960:100).
448
¹⁶⁰ PNI * { -atati- }, * { -atatj- } 'speak, talk'
Mo. { -atati- } in / kaˈtaːtihs / 'I talk, I speak' (cf. Maracle 1990:31).
Oe. { -atati- } in / lataːˈtiheɁ / 'he speaks' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:139).
Oo. { -atatj- } in / hatáːˈtjahaɁ / 'he talks, he speaks, he is the speaker'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:85).
Hu. { -atati- }, { -atatj- }, cf. <Atati> 'parler' (MS 59:22).
Wy. { -atakj- } in / tehaˈtakjah / 'he talks' (cf. Barbeau 1960:145).
449
(cf. Woodbury 2003:98).
Se. { -atawɛ̃ɛt̃ - } in / oɁwataˈwɛ̃ːɛ̃t / 'it went up, climbed over'
(cf. Chafe 1967:45).
Hu. { -awẽhɹat- }, cf. <A8en‛rati> 'passer par dessus q.c. ou qlq.' (MS 59:10).
[ cf. Lounsbury 1978:342 ]
450
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:156).
Oo. { -atekaɁt- } in / káˈtekaɁthaɁ / 'I light fires' (cf. Woodbury 2003:112).
Ca. { -atekaɁt- } in / õˈtekɁathaɁ / 's.o. burns up s.t.' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:42).
Se. { -atekaɁt- } in / akateˈkaɁtɔ̃h / 'I've made a fire' (cf. Chafe 1967:41).
Hu. { -atexaɁt- }, cf. <Ate˛ati> 'fe. du feu, brusler q.c.' (MS 59:22),
/ Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -atejaɁt- } in / aɁjɔ̃ˈwaaɁtateːjaɁt / 'they burned her body'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:157).
451
¹⁶⁸ PNI * { -atẽnaɁtshɹ- } 'food taken along, provisions'
Tu. { -aɁnə̃tahtʃr- } in / oɁnə̃ˈtahtʃrɛh / 'provisions' (cf. Rudes 1999:92),
final cluster anomalous.
Mo. { -atʌ̃naɁtsheɹ- } in / atʌ̃ˈnâːtsheɹaɁ / 'food, groceries, provisions'
(cf. Maracle 1990:5).
Oe. { -atʌ̃naɁtsl- } in / atʌ̃ˈnâːtsliɁ / 'groceries, lunch'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:182).
Oo. { -atẽnaɁtshR- } in / atẽː́ ˈnaɁtshæɁ / 'groceries' (cf. Woodbury 2003:139).
Ca. { -atẽnaɁtshɹ- } in / aˈtẽnɁatshɹaɁ / 'lunch; groceries'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:142).
Se. { -atɛ̃nɔ̃ɁshR- } in / aˈtɛ̃nɔɁshæɁ / 'food, lunch' (cf. Chafe 1967:42).
Hu. { -atẽnaɁʃhɹ- } in / atẽˈnaɁʃhɹaɁ / <Atendacha> 'Provisions, vivres,
viatique' (MS 59:23), / Ɂ / assumed present, / ɹ / lost in Rock dialect.
Wy. { -atɛ̃naɁʃɹ- } in / tahɔ̃tɛ̃ˈnaɁʃɹahʃaht / 'their food ran out'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:295).
¹⁶⁹ PNI * { -athahjõniːh } 'be a member of the Wolf Clan' ( limited to Iroquoia )
Oe. { -athahjũni } in / othahjũːˈni / 'wolf' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:697).
Oo. { -athahjũnih } in / akathahjṹːˈnih / 'I am a member of the Wolf Clan'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:283).
Ca. { -athahjõniː } in / hothahˈjõ·niː / '(he is) Wolf clan'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:609).
452
[ cf. Mithun 1979:182, Michelson 1988:173 ]
¹⁷³ PNI * { -athsɁaht- } 'be burnt, be consumed, be used up, burn, use oneself up'
Tu. { -atʃɁahn- } in / watʃˈɁahnə̃ / 'it drips' (cf. Rudes 1999:7).
Mo. { -athsaɁaht- } in / wathsaˈɁahtũ / 'it is burnt' (cf. Maracle 1990:110).
Oe. { -atshaɁaht- } in / katshaˈɁathaɁ / 'I'm burning it'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:262).
Oo. { -atsɁaht- } in / watsˈɁathaɁ / 'it gets used up' (cf. Woodbury 2003:305).
Ca. { -atsɁaht- } in / hawathneˈkatsɁaht / 'water is all gone'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:356).
¹⁷⁴ PNI * { -ati- }, * { -atj- }, * { -õti- }, * { -õtj- } 'abandon, throw, throw out'
Tu. { -aɁni- } in / wahˈroːɁniɁ / 'he threw' (cf. Rudes 1999:420);
{ -ə̃Ɂnj- } in / roɁˈtə̃ːɁnjə̃hs / 'he shoots arrows' (cf. Rudes 1999:94).
Mo. { -atj- } in / jewaˈkatjehs / 'I throw away, throw out, get rid of'
453
(cf. Maracle 1990:15); { -ũtj- } in / jekhejaɁˈtũtjes /
'I throw, put someone out' (cf. Maracle 1990:14).
Oe. { -atj- } in / jewaˈkatjehseɁ / 'I'm throwing it'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:295); { -ũti- } in / ʌ̃tisaɁkʌ̃hlũːˈtîː /
'you will throw dirt' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:295).
Oo. { -atj- } in / áˈkaːtjũh / 'I've thrown it out' (cf. Woodbury 2003:188); { -ũti- }
in / waɁakohnéːˈkũːtiɁ / 'she threw out the water'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:188).
Ca. { -ati- } in / heɁˈsaːtih / 'throw it away from me'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:338); { -õti- } in / sẽniːˈtskɹõːtih / 'spit'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:309).
Se. { -atj- } in / hewakatjɔ̃ːh / 'I've thrown it' (cf. Chafe 1967:44); { -ɔ̃ti- } in
/ waotwijɔ̃ːtiɁ / 'he abandoned his children' (cf. Chafe 1967:78).
Hu. { -ati- }, { -atj- }, cf. <Ation> 'jetter, laisser, abandonner q.c.' (MS 59:28);
{ -õti- }, { -õtj- }, cf. <Ontion> 'laisser, abandonner, rejetter qlq. ou q.c.'
(MS 59:194).
Wy. { -ati- } in / haˈhuːtiɁ / 'he threw it' (cf. Barbeau 1960:250); { -ɔ̃ti- } in
/ aɁsajuwaɁˈtɔ̃tiɁ / 'he threw them out' (cf. Barbeau 1960:244).
454
Oo. { -atjaɁtawiɁt- } in / ẽkaːtjáɁtaˈwiɁtaɁ / 'I will put on my clothes'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:194).
Ca. { -atjaɁtawiɁt- } in / satjaɁtaˈwiɁthaɁ / 'you are always putting it on'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:589).
Se. { -atjaɁtawiɁt- } in / atjaɁtaˈwiɁshæɁ / 'smock' (cf. Chafe 1967:45).
455
Hu. { -akahkw- }, cf. <Aka‛k8i> 'Regarder' (MS 59:4).
Wy. { -akahkw- } in / ɔ̃ˈkahkwaɁ / 'someone looked' (cf. Barbeau 1960:233).
[ cf. Mithun 1981b:237 ]
456
¹⁸³ PNI * { -atokw- }, * { -atokwaɁtshɹ- } 'spoon' ( limited to Iroquoia )
Mo. { -atokw- } in / aˈtokwaɁ / 'a spoon, ladle' (cf. Maracle 1990:7);
{ -atokwaɁtsheɹ- } in / watokwaɁtsheˈɹiːjo / 'a good, useful spoon'
(cf. Maracle 1990:123).
Oe. / aˈtokwaɁt / 'spoon' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:255); { -atokwaɁtsl- }
in / swatoˈkwâːtslat / 'one spoon, one spoonful'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:255).
Oo. { -atokwaɁtshR- } in / akatóˈkwaɁtshæɁ / 'my spoon'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:165).
Se. { -atokwaɁshR- } in / aˈtokwaɁshæɁ / 'spoon, ladle' (cf. Chafe 1967:75).
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:277 ]
457
Hu. { -atõj- }, cf. <Atonïon> 'Respirer' (MS 59:33).
[ cf. Michelson 1988:181 ]
458
(cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:132).
PNI * / ˈaːtseːɁ / 'green, new'.
Tu. / ˈaːθɛːɁ / 'new, green, raw, unripe' (cf. Rudes 1999:50).
No. <Osae> 'Young' (cf. Rudes 1981a:47).
Mo. / ˈaːseɁ / 'new, fresh' (cf. Maracle 1990:1).
Oe. / aːˈsêː / 'it's new' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:117).
Oo. / ahˈseɁ / 'it is new' (cf. Woodbury 2003:233).
Ca. / ˈaːseːɁ / 'it is fresh, new' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:410).
Se. { -aseːɁ } in / ˈwaːseːɁ / 'it's new' (cf. Chafe 1967:45).
Hu. / ˈaːseːɁ / cf. <Ase> 'neuf, recent, frais' (MS 59:14), long vowel and / Ɂ /
assumed present.
[ cf. Mithun 1979:184, Rudes 1995:50 ]
¹⁹¹ PNI * { -atst- }, * { -atstathẽ- } 'dry out, dry up, evaporate, run dry'
Tu. { -aθn- } in / ˈwaθnə̃hs / 'it runs dry, it evaporates' (cf. Rudes 1999:52).
Mo. { -ahst- } in / ˈwahstehs / 'it dries up' (cf. Maracle 1990:342).
Oe. { -astathʌ̃ } in / joˈstathʌ̃ / 'it's dry' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:122).
Oo. { -ast- } in / óˈsteh / 'it is dried up, it is evaporated'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:248).
Ca. { -ahst- } in / ohˈstehstõh / 'it has evaporated; it is all dried up'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:108).
Se. { -ast- } in / osteːh / 'it's evaporated' (cf. Chafe 1967:40).
Hu. { -stathẽ- }, cf. <gastaθen> 'etre Sec' (MS 59:152),
lack of initial / a / anomalous.
Wy. { -statɛ̃- } in / naˈɁustatɛ̃Ɂ / 'when it was dry' (cf. Barbeau 1960:284).
459
¹⁹² PNI * { -atwẽteht- } 'abandon, lend, let go, spare' ( limited to Iroquoia )
Mo. { -atewʌ̃teht- } in / katewʌ̃ˈtehthaɁ / 'I abandon, reject, denounce,
I leave behind, speak poorly of' (cf. Maracle 1990:33).
Oe. { -atwʌ̃teht- } in / wakatwʌ̃ˈtehtũ / 'I have spared some'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:292).
Oo. { -atwẽteht- } in / katwẽˈ́ tethaɁ / 'I share it, I let go of it'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:184).
Ca. { -atwẽteht- } in / saˈtwẽtehthaɁ / 'you forfeit things all the time'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:126).
Se. { -atwɛ̃teht- } in / oɁkatwɛ̃ːtet / 'I lent it' (cf. Chafe 1967:44).
460
Ca. { -awẽnj- } in / teˈkawẽnjeh / 'I stir all the time'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:422).
Se. { -awɛ̃nj- } in / tejɔ̃wɛ̃njeh / 'she stirs it' (cf. Chafe 1967:45).
[ cf. Michelson 1988:181 ]
461
Se. { -aɁaR- } in / oˈtaɁææɁ / 'veil, netting' (cf. Chafe 1967:46).
462
²⁰² PNI * { -aɁkɹ- } 'snow, snowflake' ( limited to Iroquoia )
Mo. { -aɁkeɹ- } in / ˈôːkeɹaɁ / 'a snowflake, snowflakes' (cf. Maracle 1990:59).
Oe. { -aɁkl- } in / ˈôːklaɁ / 'snowflake' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:317).
Oo. { -aɁkR- } in / ˈóɁkæːɁ / 'snow' (cf. Woodbury 2003:316).
Ca. { -aɁkɹ- } in / oɁkɹaɁ / 'snow; a snowflake' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:303).
Se. { -aɁkR- } in / oɁkæɁ / 'snow, snowflake' (cf. Chafe 1967:90).
[ cf. Michelson 1988:180 ]
463
(cf. Maracle 1990:143).
Se. { -aɁtaiɛ̃shR- } in / oɁtaiˈɛ̃shæɁ / 'sweat' (cf. Chafe 1967:46).
Hu. { -aɁtaɹihẽhʃɹ- } in / oɁtaɹiˈhẽhʃɹaɁ / <Otarihencha> 'sueur' (MS 59:21),
/ Ɂ / and preconsonantal / h / assumed present, / ɹ / has become / h / in
Rock dialect.
464
Tu. { -ɛ- } in / ˈiːkjɛɁ / 'I am walking' (cf. Rudes 1999:139); / ˈiθhrɛɁθ /
'he returned' (cf. Rudes 1999:139).
La. { -e- } in <Canada undagueny> 'D'où venez vous?' (Biggar 1924:245).
Mo. { -e- } in / ˈiːkeɁ / 'I walk, I am in motion' (cf. Maracle 1990:9).
Oe. { -e- } in / iːˈkêː / 'I'm walking' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:327).
Oo. { -e- } in / ˈiheɁ / 'he is walking (cf. Woodbury 2003:372).
Ca. { -e- } in / ˈitsheɁs / 'you wander (all the time), you are over there'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:431).
Se. { -e- } in / iːkeɁ / 'I'm walking' (cf. Chafe 1967:48).
Hu. { -e- }, cf. <Ennon> 'verba Motūs' (MS 59:170).
Wy. { -e- } in / ˈhesteh / 'you two go!' (cf. Barbeau 1960:84).
[ cf. Mithun 1981b:233, Rudes 1995:46 ]
465
Wy. { -eR- } in / iˈweːheɁ / 'she thinks' (cf. Barbeau 1960:67).
[ cf. Rudes 1995:37 ]
466
²¹⁶ PNI * { -ẽhnitsɹ- } 'day' ( limited to Iroquoia )
Mo. { -ʌ̃hniseɹ- } in / ʌ̃hˈniseɹaɁ / 'day, time of daylight' (cf. Maracle 1990:127).
Oe. { -ʌ̃hnisl- } in / awʌ̃hˈnislaɁ / 'day' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:346).
Oo. { -ẽhnihsR- } in / akẽh ́ ˈnihsæːɁ / 'my season, my day'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:397).
Ca. { -ẽhnihsɹ- } in / wẽhnihˈsɹiːjoː / 'nice day' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:80),
probably a borrowed root.
Se. { -ɛ̃ːnishR- } in / ɛ̃ːˈnishæɁ / 'day' (cf. Chafe 1967:49).
²¹⁷ PNI * { -ẽhwatẽɁ }, * { -jẽhwatẽɁ } 'be uncle to, have as nephew or niece'
Tu. { -jə̃hwaɁnə̃Ɂ } in / kə̃jə̃hˈwaɁnə̃Ɂ / 'my nephew' (cf. Rudes 1999:155).
La. <Yuadin> 'Mon nepveu' (Biggar 1924:246).
Mo. { -ũhwatʌ̃Ɂ- } in / ɹijũhwaˈtʌ̃ː̂ Ɂa / 'my nephew' (cf. Maracle 1990:78),
PMO change of * / ẽ / to / ũ / anomalous.
Oe. { -ũhwatʌ̃Ɂ- } in / lijũhwaˈtʌ̃ha / 'my nephew'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:748).
́ ˈwaːtẽɁ / 'my nephew' (cf. Woodbury 2003:400).
Oo. { -ẽhwatẽɁ- } in / hejẽh
Ca. { -ẽhwatẽɁ } in / kakhejõhˈwaːtẽɁ / 'my brother's children;
my nieces and nephews' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:616).
Se. { -jɛ̃ːwɔ̃tɛ̃Ɂ } in / hɔ̃ˈwɔ̃jɛ̃ːwɔ̃ːtɛ̃Ɂ / 'their nephew' (cf. Chafe 1967:51).
Hu. { -ẽhwatẽɁ- } in / shõxwẽhˈwaːtẽɁ / <Son˛8enh8aten>
'Cest ne. oncle Maternel' (MS 59:179), / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -ɛ̃ːwatɛ̃Ɂ- } in / tawɛ̃ːwaˈtɛ̃Ɂah / 'his nephew' (cf. Barbeau 1960:205).
467
²¹⁹ PNI * { -ẽt- } 'day'
Tu. { -ə̃Ɂn- } in / aˈwə̃ːɁnɛh / 'day, daytime' (cf. Rudes 1999:167).
No. * { -ẽt- } in <Antyeke> 'Day-time' (cf. Rudes 1981a:47).
Mo. { -ʌ̃t- } in / ˈʌ̃ːtaɁ / 'day, festival, occasion, holiday' (cf. Maracle 1990:8).
Oe. { -ʌ̃t- } in / swʌ̃ːˈtat / 'one week' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:349).
Oo. { -ẽt- } in / wẽˈ́ taːteɁ / '(it is a) day' (cf. Woodbury 2003:390).
Ca. { -ẽt- } in / ẽˈtehkhaːɁ / 'day kind (things used during the day)'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:80).
Se. { -ɛ̃t- } in / tawɛ̃tɔ̃ːtiɁ / 'daybreak' (cf. Chafe 1967:50).
Hu. { -ẽt- } in / ˈskwẽːtaːt / <Sk8entat> 'un seul jour' (MS 59:171).
Wy. { -ɛ̃t- } in / ˈskwɛ̃ːtat / 'one day' (cf. Barbeau 1960:310).
²²⁰ PNI * { -ẽtaɁ- } 'be finished, be over, be prepared, be ready, come to an end'
Mo. { -ʌ̃taɁ- } in / wə̃ˈtʌ̃ːtaɁs / 'the holiday, festival ends, comes to an end'
(cf. Maracle 1990:125).
Oe. { -ʌ̃taɁ- } in / kewjʌ̃nʌ̃ːˈtâːseɁ / 'I'm getting something ready'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:777).
Oo. { -ẽtaɁ- } in / gaíhˈwẽːtaɁs / 'the matter is ending' (cf. Woodbury 2003:627).
Ca. { -ẽtaɁ- } in / ẽkaːˈkhwẽːtaɁ / 'it is going to finish eating'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:504).
Se. { -ɛ̃taɁ- } in / akekhwɛ̃ˈtaɁɔ̃h / 'I've finished eating' (cf. Chafe 1967:50).
Hu. { -ẽtaɁ- }, cf. <Entandi> 'finir, Suzer, se terminer' (MS 59:172).
468
{ -aɁsẽ- }, cf. <Asendi> 'tomber' (MS 59:15), / Ɂ / assumed present.
[ cf. Lounsbury 1978:342 ]
469
²²⁵ PNI * { -hẽɹeht- } 'shout, yell'
Tu. { -hə̃rɛhθ } in / raˈhə̃ːrɛhθ / 'he shouts, he is shouting' (cf. Rudes 1999:203).
Mo. { -hʌ̃ɹeht- } in / tewakhʌ̃ˈɹehthaɁ / 'I shout' (cf. Maracle 1990:391).
Oe. { -hʌ̃leht- } in / tewakhʌ̃ˈlethaɁ / 'I'm yelling'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:369).
Oo. { -hẽeht- } in / tewáˈkhẽethaɁ / 'I am yelling' (cf. Woodbury 2003:514).
Se. { -heeht- } in / tewaˈkheehtɔ̃h / 'I've shouted' (cf. Chafe 1967:52).
Hu. { -hẽɹeht- }, cf. <Kahenre‛ti> 'fe. un gr. cry' (MS 59:74).
Wy. { -hɛ̃ɹeht- } in / aɁˈtuhɛ̃ɹeht / 'he gave a yell' (cf. Barbeau 1960:233).
[ cf. Michelson 1988:175 ]
470
²²⁸ PNI * { -hjatõhsɹ- } 'book, something marked, something written'
Tu. { -hjatə̃hstr- } in / ohjaˈtə̃hstrɛh / 'book, card, newspaper, paper'
(cf. Rudes 1999:244), a borrowed word.
Mo. { -hjatũhseɹ- } in / kahjaˈtũhseɹaɁ / 'book, paper, written material, writing,
script' (cf. Maracle 1990:25).
Oe. { -hjatũhsl- } in / kahjaˈtũhslaɁ / 'paper, book, letter'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:419).
Oo. { -hjatũhsR- } in / kahjáˈtũhsæːɁ / 'book, paper, magazine'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:584).
Ca. { -hjatõhsɹ- } in / ohjatõhˈsɹõːtõɁ / 'book' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:35),
perhaps a borrowed word.
Se. { -Hjatɔ̃shR- } in / kaːˈjatɔ̃shæɁ / 'paper, book, curtain' (cf. Chafe 1967:58).
Hu. { -hjatõhʃɹ- } in / xahjaˈtõhʃɹaɁ / <gahiatoncha> 'Ecriture' (MS 59:75),
preconsonantal / h / assumed present, / ɹ / has become / h / in
Rock dialect.
471
²³¹ PI * { -hnẽː- } 'arm, shoulder'
Ch. { -hnòː- } in / khànòːke̋ː / 'arm, wing' (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:141).
PNI * { -hnẽ- }, * { -hnẽhts- } 'arm, shoulder'.
Tu. { -hnə̃- } in / ohˈnə̃hstɛh / 'armful' (cf. Rudes 1999:548),
where { -hst- } is a reflex of a PNI nominalizer sequence * { -hsr- };
Tu. / ohˈnə̃hwɛh / 'upper shoulder' (cf. Rudes 1999:208).
Mo. { -hnʌ̃hs- } in / ohˈnʌ̃hsaɁ / 'shoulder' (cf. Maracle 1990:62).
Oe. { -hnʌ̃hs- } in / ohˈnʌ̃hsaɁ / 'shoulder' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:389).
Oo. { -hnẽhs- } in / óhˈnẽhsaɁ / 'shoulder' (cf. Woodbury 2003:532).
Ca. { -hnẽhs- } in / ohˈnẽhsaɁ / 'shoulder' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:290).
Se. { -hnɛ̃hs- } in / khnɛ̃hˈsaɁkeh / '(on) my shoulder' (cf. Chafe 1967:54).
Hu. { -hnẽhs- } in / xahˈnẽhsaɁ / <gannensa> 'Bras' (MS 59:110),
preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
[ cf. Rudes 1995:36 ]
472
²³⁴ PNI * { -hnhotõ- } 'close a door, door to be closed'
La. * { -hnhotõ- } in <Asnodyan> 'Fermez la porte' (Biggar 1924:245).
Mo. { -hnhotũ- } in / kehˈnhoːtũhs / 'I close the door, I close, imprison,
shut something in' (cf. Maracle 1990:39).
Oe. { -nhotũ- } in / kenhoːˈtũheɁ / 'I'm closing the door'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:587).
Oo. { -hnhotũ- } in / éhˈnhoːtũk / 'she locks the door, she closes the door'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:535).
Se. { -hotɔ̃- } in / kahoːtɔ̃Ɂ / 'the door is closed' (cf. Chafe 1967:84).
Hu. { -hnhotõ- }, cf. <gandhoton> 'fermer la P[orte].' (MS 59:96).
473
Oe. { -hnil- } in / johniːˈlũ / 'it's hard, it's solid'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:390).
Oo. { -hniR- } in / ohˈniːh / 'it is hard, sturdy, solid' (cf. Woodbury 2003:538).
Ca. { -hniR- } in / ohˈniːjõh / 'it is hard' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:148).
Se. { -HniR- } in / oːniːjɔ̃h / 'it's solid' (cf. Chafe 1967:54).
Hu. { -hniɹ- }, cf. <ga‛ndiri> 'etre dur, fort, stable' (MS 59:102).
474
Wy. { -Hnɹ- } in / aɁˈjeːnɹɛ̃ / 'I tied it' (cf. Barbeau 1960:159).
[ cf. Rudes 1995:38 ]
475
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:375).
Oo. { -hen- } in / ˈkheːnas / 'I cut' (cf. Woodbury 2003:507).
Ca. { -hɹeːn- } in / ˈẽkhreːɁ / 'I will cut it' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:78).
Se. { -Heːn- } in / akheːnɛ̃h / 'I'm cutting it' (cf. Chafe 1967:53), / oɁˈkatatheːɁ /
'I cut myself' (cf. Chafe 1967:53); with reflexive.
Hu. { -hɹeːn- }, cf. <ga‛rennen> 'couper, incizer, Scarifier' (MS 59:140),
long vowel assumed present.
476
Se. { -hsɛ̃n- } in / aˈkehsɛ̃ːnɔ̃Ɂ / 'my name' (cf. Chafe 1967:56).
Hu. { -hʃẽn- } in / xahˈʃẽːnaɁ / <gachienda> 'nom, Personne Csiderab'
(MS 59:47), preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
477
²⁵¹ PNI * { -hsinkoɁt- } 'ankle, knee'
La. * { -hʃinkoɁt- } in <Agochinegodascon> 'Les genoilz' (Biggar 1924:242).
Mo. { -hsinekoɁt- } in / ohsineˈkôːtaɁ / 'the ankle' (cf. Maracle 1990:65).
Oe. { -hsinikoɁt- } in / ohsiniˈkôːtaɁ / 'ankle'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:404).
Hu. { -hʃinxoɁt- } in / ohʃiˈnxoɁtaɁ / <ochingota> 'genouil' (MS 59:48),
/ Ɂ / assumed present.
[ cf. Lounsbury 1978:340 ]
478
Oe. { -skʌ̃nũtũɁ } in / oskʌ̃nũˈtũ̂ː / 'deer' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:642).
Oo. / skẽnũˈtũɁ / 'deer' (cf. Woodbury 2003:783).
Hu. { -hskẽnõtõɁ } in / ohskẽˈnõːtõɁ / <oskennonton> 'cerf, chevreuil'
(MS 59:148), / h / and / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -hskɛ̃nɔ̃tɔ̃Ɂ } in / uhskɛ̃ˈnɔ̃ːtɔ̃Ɂ / 'deer' (cf. Barbeau 1960:108).
²⁵⁵ PNI * { -hskẽɁɹ- } 'bone'; * { -hskẽɁɹakeht- } 'be a warrior', lit. 'carry the bone'
Tu. { -hskə̃Ɂr- } in / ohˈskə̃Ɂrɛh / 'bone, skeleton' (cf. Rudes 1999:225),
/ rohskə̃ɁraˈkjɛhnɛɁ / 'warrior' (cf. Rudes 1999:226).
Mo. { -hskʌ̃Ɂɹ- } in / rohskʌ̃ɁɹaˈkehteɁ / 'warrior, male' (cf. Maracle 1990:78).
Oo. { -skẽɁR- } in / hoskẽɁ́ æˈkehtah / 'young man' (cf. Woodbury 2003:783).
Se. { -skɛ̃ɁR- } in / hoskɛ̃Ɂɛ̃ˈkehtaɁ / 'warrior' (cf. Chafe 1967:80).
Hu. { -hskẽɁɹ- } in / sahskẽɁɹaˈxehteɁ / <Saskenra˛e‛te> 'tu es Soldat'
(MS 59:149), preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
Wy. { -hskɛ̃Ɂɹ- } in / tihskɛ̃ɁɹaˈjehtaɁ / 'warriors' (cf. Barbeau 1960:300).
[ cf. Rudes 1995:38 ]
479
PNI * { -hsnoɹeɁ } 'be fast, be quick'.
Tu. { -hstorɛɁ } in / juhˈstoːrɛɁ / 'it is fast' (cf. Rudes 1999:583).
Su. * { -hsnoɹeɁ } in <Zatznuri> 'War snaar' (cf. Mithun 1981a:23).
Mo. { -hsnoɹeɁ } in / johˈsnoːɹeɁ / 'it is fast, quick, quickly'
(cf. Maracle 1990:19).
Oe. { -shnoleɁ } in / joshnoːˈlêː / 'it is fast' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:658).
Oo. { -snoweɁ } in / óˈsnoːweɁ / 'it is fast, it is swift' (cf. Woodbury 2003:797).
Ca. { -hsnoweɁ } in / ohˈsnoːweɁ / 'it is fast, quick' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:114).
Se. { -snoweɁ } in / osnoːweɁ / 'it's fast' (cf. Chafe 1967:40).
Hu. { -hstoɹeɁ }, cf. <ga‛store> 'etre prompt, vit, expeditif' (MS 59:153),
/ Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -hstuɹeɁ } in / uhstuːˈɹeɁ / 'quickly' (cf. Barbeau 1960:285).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:182, Rudes 1995:44 ]
480
accommodate' (cf. Maracle 1990:16); { -ũni- } in / ˈkũːnis / 'I make,
construct, fabricate, produce' (cf. Maracle 1990:55).
Oe. { -hslũni- } in / khehslũːˈniheɁ / 'I'm dressing her or them'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:406); { -ũni- } in / kũːˈniheɁ /
'I'm making it' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:756).
Oo. { -hsẽːnj- } in / waɁkéhˈsẽːnjaɁ / 'I made it' (cf. Woodbury 2003:554);
{ -ũnj- } in / waɁkhǽːhˈkũːnjaɁ / 'I baked bread'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:851).
Ca. { -hsɹõni- } in / ẽhsehˈsɹõːniɁ / 'you will create, make s.t.'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:121); { -õni- } in / ˈkõːnihs / 'I make, earn'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:191).
Se. { -sjɔ̃ni- } in / aˈkesjɔ̃ːniːh / 'I've made it, prepared it' (cf. Chafe 1967:81);
{ -õni- } in / jeˈsheɁõːnih / 'she makes dough' (cf. Chafe 1967:77).
Hu. { -hʃɹõni- }, { -hʃɹõnj- }, cf. <gachondi> 'accomoder, ajuster, orner q.c.'
(MS 59:49), preconsonantal / h / assumed present, / ɹ / has become
/ h / in Rock dialect; { -õni- }, { -õnj- }, cf. <Ondi> 'fe. q.c.'
(MS 59:184).
Wy. { -hʃɹɔ̃nj- } in / aɁˈhatehʃɹɔ̃ːnjaɁ / 'he got ready' (cf. Barbeau 1960:180);
{ -ɔ̃nj- } in / hateɁˈnɔ̃njah / 'he makes arrows' (cf. Barbeau 1960:201),
{ -ɔ̃ni- } in / utɔ̃taˈɹɔ̃ːnih / 'she has made a lake' (cf. Barbeau 1960:103).
481
Oe. { -stahlũw- } in / otstahrũːˈwâː / 'marrow'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:645).
Se. { -staːɔ̃w- } in / oˈstaːɔ̃wɔ̃Ɂ / 'marrow' (cf. Chafe 1967:80).
482
/ ranə̃hˈsihsɁahs / 'he finishes the house' (cf. Rudes 1999:256).
Mo. { -hsaɁ- } in / ˈikhsaɁs / 'I create, achieve, arrive at, perfect, finish,
complete' (cf. Maracle 1990:15); { -ihsaɁ- } in / kerihˈwihsaɁs /
'I decree, order, decide' (cf. Maracle 1990:134).
Oe. { -hsaɁ- } in / ˈiksaɁas / 'I'm finishing it'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:400); { -isaɁ- } in / ʌ̃hanũhˈsisaneɁ /
'he will finish building the house' (604), loss of / h / in incorporating
form anomalous.
Oo. { -hsɁ- } in / kéhsˈɁas / 'I finish, I'm eating it all up'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:561); { -ihsɁ- } in / waknṹhˈsihsɁaɁ /
'I finished a house' (cf. Woodbury 2003:561).
Ca. { -ihsɁ- } in / õkwathẽˈtihsɁõh / 'we finished the field'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:119).
Se. { -sɁ- } in / heˈjosɁahtɔ̃h / 'it's all gone' (cf. Chafe 1967:56).
Hu. { -ihʃaɁ- }, cf. <ichai> 'achever, finir, conclurre' (MS 59:203),
/ h / and / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -ihʃɛ̃Ɂ } in / naˈhatekwihʃɛ̃Ɂ / 'when he has eaten (cf. Barbeau 1960:116).
²⁶⁷ PNI * { -htsiɁ } 'have as older sibling, have as older maternal female cousin'
Tu. { -htʃiɁ } in / ˈaktʃiɁ / 'my older sister, my older maternal female cousin'
(cf. Rudes 1999:195).
No. * { -htʃiɁ } in <ahkahchee> 'Sister' (cf. Rudes 1981a:48).
Mo. { -htsiɁ } in / aˈkhtsîːɁa / 'my older sister' (Deering & Delisle 1976:31).
Oe. { -htsi- } in / aˈktsiha / 'my older sister, she's my older sister'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:729).
Oo. { -htsiɁ- } in / aktshíˈɁah / 'my older sibling, my older brother or sister'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:562).
Ca. { -htsiɁ- } in / khehˈtsiɁah / 'my older sister' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:612).
Se. { -htsiɁ } in / hahtsiɁ / 'my older brother' (cf. Chafe 1967:54), / ahtsiɁ /
'my older sister' (cf. Chafe 1967:54).
483
Mo. { -ahsoh- } in / ˈkahsohs / 'I dye something, I colour something'
(cf. Maracle 1990:27).
Oe. { -ahsohw- } in / waˈhahsoweɁ / 'he coloured'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:74).
Oo. { -ahsohw- } in / akáhˈsohwih / 'I have painted it' (cf. Woodbury 2003:239).
Ca. { -ahsoh- } in / wahˈsohõːt / 'it is coloured, dyed' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:99).
Se. { -ahsoh- } in / aˈkahsoɔ̃h / 'I've painted it' (cf. Chafe 1967:38).
Hu. { -ahsohw- }, cf. <Asoh8i> 'Teindre' (MS 59:17),
preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:260, Rudes 1995:51 ]
484
Se. { -HweɁnɔniːh } in / teˈjothweɁnɔːniːh / 'it's round' (cf. Chafe 1967:57),
with semireflexive.
Hu. { -hweɁnõniːh }, cf. <gah8ennondi> 'plier en rond. en rouleau' (MS 59:78),
long vowel and final / h / assumed present.
485
Mo. / ˈwihsk / 'five' (cf. Maracle 1990:111).
Oe. / ˈwisk / 'five' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:774).
Oo. / ˈhwíks / 'five' (cf. Woodbury 2003:578).
Ca. / hwihs / 'five' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:605).
Se. / wis / 'five' (cf. Chafe 1967:85).
Hu. / ˈwihʃ / <8ich> 'Cinq' (MS 59:138), preconsonantal / h / assumed
present.
Wy. / uˈwihʃ / 'five' (cf. Barbeau 1960:185).
[ cf. Lounsbury 1978:339, Mithun 1979:183, Mithun 1981a:3, Rudes 1995:45 ]
486
²⁷⁶ PNI * { -ihej- } 'be dead, die'
Tu. { -ihej- } in / ə̃ˈkihejɁ / 'I will die' (cf. Rudes 1999:249).
No. * { -ihej- } in <Anseehe> 'Death' (cf. Rudes 1981a:46).
Mo. { -ihej- } in / kiˈheːjũhs / 'I die, I pass away, I become dead'
(cf. Maracle 1990:53).
Oe. { -ihej- } in / jaknʌ̃heːˈjũ / 'we two have died'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:423).
Oo. { -ihej- } in / ẽkíˈheːjaɁ / 'I will die' (cf. Woodbury 2003:592).
Ca. { -ihej- } in / kiˈheːjõhs / 'I am dying' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:86).
Se. { -iej- } in / aˈkiːejɔ̃ːh / 'I'm dead' (cf. Chafe 1967:59).
Hu. { -iheJ- }, cf. <genheon> 'Mourir' (MS 59:174).
Wy. { -iheJ- } in / aɁˈjɛ̃he / 'she died' (cf. Barbeau 1960:311).
487
Mo. { -ijo } in / kanũhkʷaɁtsheˈɹiːjo / 'good medicine, a good remedy'
(cf. Maracle 1990:273).
Oe. { -ijo } in / kakhwiːˈjo / 'good food' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:441).
Oo. { -ijoh } in / ohahíˈjoh / 'good road' (cf. Woodbury 2003:635).
Ca. { -ijoː } in / ohneːˈkiːjoː / 'good water' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:363).
Se. { -ijoːh } in / wiːjoːh / 'it's good' (cf. Chafe 1967:46).
Hu. { -ioːh }, cf. <ïo> 'beau, bon, grand' (MS 59:204),
long vowel and / h / assumed present.
488
Mo. { -itskaɹ- } in / ˈkʌ̃tskaɹeɁ / 'a mattress, cover, sheet, mat, carpet, flooring'
(cf. Maracle 1990:43).
Oe. { -itskal- } in / waˈkitskalũɁ / 'I have put the sheet down'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:434).
Ca. { -itskaR- } in / kẽtskaːɁ / 'mattress, sleeping mat'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:195).
Se. { -iskaR- } in / kɛ̃skaːaɁ / 'mat, something to sleep on' (cf. Chafe 1967:60).
489
²⁸⁶ PNI * { -instẽhɹ- }, * { -nstẽhɹ- } 'cliff, rock, rock bank'
Tu. { -itʃtə̃hr- } in / oˈtʃtə̃hrɛh / 'stone', / joˈtʃtə̃hrɛh / 'cliff on land or in water'
(cf. Rudes 1999:136), probably a borrowed root.
Mo. { -itstʌ̃hɹ- } in / oˈtstʌ̃ː̂ ɹaɁ / 'a rock, boulder, stone' (cf. Maracle 1990:76).
Oe. { -itstʌ̃hl- } in / oˈtstʌ̃ː̂ laɁ / 'rock, mountain, outcropping'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:437).
Oo. { -stẽhR- } in / óˈstẽhæɁ / 'rock' (cf. Woodbury 2003:777).
Se. { -stɛ̃ːR- } in / oˈstɛ̃ːɛ̃Ɂ / 'cliff' (cf. Chafe 1967:81).
490
Wy. { -ikjuhkʷ- } in / huˈkjuhkʷaɁ / 'his crowd' (cf. Barbeau 1960:294).
491
²⁹² PNI * { -iɁtɹõ- } 'abide, dwell, reside, sit'
Tu. { -iɁrə̃- } in / ˈrə̃Ɂrə̃Ɂ / 'he abides, he is placed, he is seated, he remains'
(cf. Rudes 1999:270).
Mo. { -iɁteɹũ- } in / ˈkîːteɹũɁ / 'I reside, live, stay somewhere, I am present'
(cf. Maracle 1990:52).
Oe. { -iɁtlũ- } in / ˈkîːtlũɁ / 'I'm sitting, I'm at home'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:444).
Oo. { -iɁtẽː- } in / ˈkẽɁ́ tẽːɁ / 'it is sitting [there], it is in place'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:640).
Ca. { -iɁtɹõ- } in / haˈtiɁtɹõɁ / 'they (m) are at home'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:487).
Se. { -iɁtjɔ̃- } in / kiɁtjɔ̃Ɂ / 'I'm settled, I dwell' (cf. Chafe 1967:61).
Hu. { -iɁtɹõ- }, cf. <gentron> 'etre en q. lieu' (MS 59:178),
/ Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -iɁtɹɔ̃- } in / tiˈhɛ̃Ɂtɹɔ̃Ɂ / 'there he sits' (cf. Barbeau 1960:275).
492
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:591).
Se. { -jan- } in / oˈjaːnɔ̃Ɂ / 'footprint, track, tire' (cf. Chafe 1967:86).
²⁹⁵ PNI * { -janɹ- } 'be a chief, be good, be lawful, be proper' ( limited to Iroquoia )
Mo. { -janeɹ- } in / joˈjaneɹeɁ / 'it is good, righteous, proper'
(cf. Maracle 1990:19).
Oe. { -janl- } in / joˈjanleɁ / 'it's good' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:7687).
Oo. { -janR- } in / óˈjaːneːɁ / 'it is good' (cf. Woodbury 2003:879).
Ca. { -janɹ- } in / oˈjaːnɹeɁ / 'it is nice, good, beautiful'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:592).
Se. { -janR- } in / hojaːneːh / '(he's a) chief' (cf. Chafe 1967:86).
493
/ Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -JaɁK- } in / ehˈʃɹɔ̃ːkjaɁ / 'you will cut down the tree'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:204).
494
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:201).
Se. { -jest- } in / twaˈkjestɔ̃h / 'I've given, contributed' (cf. Chafe 1967:87).
Hu. { -Jehst- }, cf. <gaesti> 'amasser, assembler' (MS 59:59),
preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
495
Oe. { -jeri- } in / tkajeːˈlîː / 'it's right' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:812).
Oo. { -jei- } in / tkáˈjeːiɁ / 'it is right' (cf. Woodbury 2003:909).
Ca. { -jei- } in / tkaːˈjeiː / 'it is right, correct' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:268).
Se. { -jei- } in / tkajeːiɁ / 'it's right, in fact, really' (cf. Chafe 1967:87).
Hu. { -Jeɹi- }, cf. <gaerihen> 'etre Droit, egal, uni' (MS 59:56).
³⁰⁶ PNI * { -jẽ- } 'be situated, exist, have, put down, set down, sit down'
Tu. { -jə̃- } in / ˈroːjə̃Ɂ / 'he has' (cf. Rudes 1999:512).
Su. * { -jẽ- } in <Gáije> 'Har tu' (cf. Mithun 1981a:23).
Mo. { -jʌ̃- } in / ˈikjʌ̃hs / 'I set, lay, put something down' (cf. Maracle 1990:16).
Oe. { -jʌ̃- } in / ˈlotjʌ̃Ɂ / 'he has sat down' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:308),
496
with semireflexive.
Oo. { -jẽ- } in / háˈjẽhaɁ / 'he sets it down' (cf. Woodbury 2003:915).
Ca. { -jẽ- } in / ˈkaːjẽɁ / 'it is lying on the ground' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:595).
Se. { -jɛ̃- } in / akjɛ̃Ɂ / 'I have it, I've put it down' (cf. Chafe 1967:87).
Hu. { -Jẽ- }, cf. <gaen> 'y avoir q.c. en q.l; mettre q.c. en q.l.' (MS 59:51).
Wy. { -Jẽ- } in / ajekwihˈstaːɛ̃Ɂ / 'I have money' (cf. Barbeau 1960:280).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:185, Mithun 1981a:3 ]
497
³¹⁰ PNI * { -jẽteɹ- } 'know'
Tu. { -jə̃Ɂnɛr- } in / kjə̃Ɂˈnɛːrih / 'I know' (cf. Rudes 1999:518).
Mo. { -jʌ̃teɹ- } in / khejʌ̃ˈteːɹi / 'I know someone' (cf. Maracle 1990:207).
Oe. { -jʌ̃tel- } in / jũkhijʌ̃teːˈli / 'they know of us'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:830).
Oo. { -jẽteR- } in / etshijẽˈteːih / 'you (cf. Rudes 1981a:n-singular) know them'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:921).
Se. { -jɛ̃teR- } in / kjɛ̃teːih / 'I know it' (cf. Chafe 1967:88).
Hu. { -Jẽteɹ- }, cf. <gaenteri> 'connoitre q.c. ou qlq. y etre accoutumé par
usage, experience' (MS 59:54).
Wy. { -Jɛ̃teɹ- } in / teɁʒɛ̃ˈteːɹih / 'I don't know' (cf. Barbeau 1960:127).
498
Hu. { -Jẽʃɹ- }, cf. <gaenchon> 'écorcher' (MS 59:52),
/ ɹ / has become / h / in Rock dialect.
Wy. { -Jɛ̃ʃɹ- } in / ehˈskjɛ̃ːʃɹaɁ / 'you will skin me' (cf. Barbeau 1960:215).
499
³¹⁶ PI * { -k- } 'eat'
Ch. { -k- } in / tsìkíɁa̋ː / 'I'm eating it' (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:16).
PNI * { -k- }, * { -aːk- }, * { -eːk- } 'eat'.
Tu. { -k- } in / joˈtʃiɁraːks / 'one eats embers' (cf. Rudes 1999:275).
Mo. { -k- } in / ˈiːkekhs / 'I eat something' (cf. Maracle 1990:15).
Oe. { -k- } in / iːˈkeks / 'I'm eating' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:447).
Oo. { -k- } in / ˈẽː́ jek / 'she will eat' (cf. Woodbury 2003:381).
Ca. { -k- } in / ˈẽːkeːk / 'I will eat' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:441).
Se. { -k- } in / akeːkɔ̃h / 'I've eaten it' (cf. Chafe 1967:63).
Hu. { -K- }, cf. <ga˛i> 'manger' (MS 59:70).
500
Mo. { -kahɹ- } in / oˈkâːɹaɁ / 'eye, view, look' (cf. Maracle 1990:66).
Oe. { -kahl- } in / oˈkâːlaɁ / 'eye' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:449).
Oo. { -kahR- } in / óˈkahæɁ / 'eye' (cf. Woodbury 2003:428).
Ca. { -kahR- } in / oˈkahaɁ / 'eye' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:111).
Se. { -kaHR- } in / kaˈkaaɁ / 'its eye' (cf. Chafe 1967:63).
Hu. { -Kahɹ- } in / oˈxahɹaɁ / <o˛ara> 'oeil' (MS 59:63), / h / assumed present.
Wy. { -Kaːɹ- } in / haɁkwakaːˈɹiːʒɛ̃Ɂ / 'she opened her eyes'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:167).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:182 ]
501
(cf. Maracle 1990:373).
Oe. / ˈkanikeɁ / 'wherever' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:461),
change of / a / to / e / anomalous.
Se. { katkaɁ } in / kaˈtkaɁhɔ̃h / 'somewhere, anywhere' (cf. Chafe 1967:64).
502
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:318), glottal stop anomalous.
Se. { -kaR- } in / kakaːæɁ / 'story' (cf. Chafe 1967:63).
503
³²⁹ PNI * { katsih } 'come here!'
Tu. / ˈkaːtʃi / 'come!' (cf. Rudes 1999:275).
Su. <Kaatzie> 'Kom hijt' (cf. Mithun 1981a:23).
Mo. / ˈkaːts / 'come here!' (Deering & Delisle 1976:43).
Oe. / kaːˈats / 'Come here!' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:448),
doubled vowel anomalous.
Ca. / ˈkaːtsih / 'come here' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:62).
Se. / kaːtsih / 'come here!' (cf. Chafe 1967:64).
[ cf. Mithun 1981a:3 ]
504
perfective { -kòː- } in / ȕːkòːhʌ̃ː̌ Ɂîː / 'he saw it'
(cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:18).
PNI * { -kẽ- } 'see'; perfect * { -kẽːh } 'have seen'.
Tu. { -kə̃- } in / ˈiːkkə̃h / 'I see it' (cf. Rudes 1999:293); / ˈroːkə̃ː / 'he has seen it'
(cf. Rudes 1999:293).
Mo. { -kʌ̃- } in / ˈikkʌ̃hs / 'I see, perceive, have the ability to see'
(cf. Maracle 1990:16).
Oe. { -kʌ̃- } in / ˈikkʌ̃heɁ / 'I can see' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:473).
Oo. { -kẽ- } in / wáˈɁeːkẽɁ / 'she saw it' (cf. Woodbury 2003:456).
Ca. { -kẽ- } in / aˈhaːkẽɁ / 'he saw' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:283).
Se. { -kɛ̃- } in / akeːkɛ̃ːh / 'I've seen it' (cf. Chafe 1967:65), / akeːkɛ ː̃ h /
'I've seen it' (cf. Chafe 1967:65).
Hu. { -Kẽ- }, cf. <ga˛en> 'voir' (MS 59:65).
Wy. { -Kɛ̃- } in / aɁhaːˈjɛ̃Ɂ / 'he saw' (cf. Barbeau 1960:53).
[ cf. Lounsbury 1978:343, Rudes 1995:53 ]
505
Hu. { -xẽnh- } in / xẽˈnheɁxeh / <˛ennhe˛e> 'été' (MS 59:32).
Wy. { -Kɛ̃t- } in / uˈjɛ̃tɛ̃haɔ̃Ɂ / 'in the summer' (cf. Barbeau 1960:253).
506
Hu. { -khahsk- }, cf. <gaχaska8an> 'Séparer q.c. d'une autre' (MS 59:83),
preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
Wy. { -kahsk- } in / aˈhatikahskaɁ / 'they are divided' (cf. Barbeau 1960:98).
507
Oe. / tsjoˈlhjohkʷaɁ / or / tsihˈljô·kʷə̃ / 'chipmunk'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:742), partially reshaped, cf. Seneca form.
Ca. / tsihˈnjoɁkẽːɁ / 'chipmunk' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:470).
Se. / tsiˈhoɁkʷaes / 'chipmunk' (cf. Chafe 1967:55), reshaped.
Hu. / ohˈjoɁxẽh / <ohio˛en> 'Escurieux' (MS 62:7),
/ Ɂ / and / h / assumed present.
Wy. / uːˈʒuɁjɛ̃h / 'ground squirrel' (cf. Barbeau 1960:148).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:181, Mithun 1984b:265, Rudes 1995:40 ]
508
Wy. / jahˈkaɁ / 'crow' (cf. Barbeau 1960:198).
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:261, Rudes 1995:41 ]
509
preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
Wy. { -Kɔ̃hʃ- } in / aɁjɔ̃jɔ̃hʃuˈɹɛ̃haɁ / 'I found your face' (cf. Barbeau 1960:178).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:182 ]
510
³⁴⁹ PI * { -kuwa- }, * { -kʷa- } 'be big, be large'
Ch. { -kʷà- } in / e̋ːkʷâː / 'huge, large' (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:89).
PNI * { -kowa- } 'big', * { -kowanẽh } 'be big',
* { -owanẽh } with incorporated nouns.
Tu. / ˈkʷaːnə̃ / 'a lot, great, many, much' (cf. Rudes 1999:305),
loss of * / o / anomalous, possibly a borrowed word.
Su. <Koonæ> 'Stoor' (cf. Mithun 1981a:23).
La. * { -kowanẽh } in <Agouhanna> 'quant ilz le veullent appeller seigneur ilz
l'appellent' (Biggar 1924:245).
Mo. { -kowanʌ̃ } in / kkoˈwaːnʌ̃ / 'I am big, large' (cf. Maracle 1990:54).
Oe. { -kʷanʌ̃ } in / lakʷaːˈnʌ̃ / 'he's big' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:498),
loss of * / o / anomalous.
Oo. { -kowanẽh } in / hkóˈwaːnẽh / 'I am big' (cf. Woodbury 2003:466);
/ hakóːˈwaːnẽh / 'he is a chief' (cf. Woodbury 2003:466).
Ca. { -kowanẽh } in / kaˈkowanẽh / 'it is big' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:30).
Se. { -kowanɛ̃h } in / kakowanɛ̃h / 'it's big, important' (cf. Chafe 1967:66).
Hu. { -Kowanẽh }, cf. <ga˛8annen> 'etre aagé' (MS 59:72),
/ h / assumed present.
Wy. { -Kuwanɛ̃h } in / tajuˈwaːnɛ̃h / 'he is the eldest' (cf. Barbeau 1960:169).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:183, Mithun 1981a:3, Rudes 1995:37 ]
511
Hu. { -hkw- }, cf. <ka‛k8an> 'prendre, Saissir q.c, la ramasser, lenlever,
la Retirer chez soy, la Retenir' (MS 59:83).
Wy. { -hkw- } in / tewaˈjehkwɛ̃h / 'I have picked it up' (cf. Barbeau 1960:288);
with duplicative.
512
Ca. { -nahskʷ- } in / kaˈnahskʷaɁ / 'tame animal; pet; domestic animal'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:94).
Se. { -nɔ̃skʷ- } in / kanɔ̃ːskʷaɁ / 'domestic animal' (cf. Chafe 1967:72),
penultimate lengthening anomalous.
Hu. { -nahskʷ- } in / xaˈnahskʷaɁ / <gandask8a> 'Esclave, Captif,
animal domestique' (MS 59:89), preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
Wy. { -nahskʷ- } in / hunahskʷaɛ̃Ɂ / 'he has a slave' (cf. Barbeau 1960:210).
513
Hu. { -naht- } in / xaˈnahtaɁ / <ganda‛ta> 'chambre, place a coucher, natte'
(MS 59:90).
514
Mo. { -naɁnawʌ̃ } in / joˈnâːnawʌ̃ / 'it is wet, soaked' (cf. Maracle 1990:20).
Oe. { -naɁnawʌ̃ } in / joˈnâːnawʌ̃ / 'it's wet'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:562).
Oo. { -naɁnawẽh } in / onáɁnaˈwẽh / 'it is damp, it is wet'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:690).
Ca. { -naɁnawẽh- } in / onaɁnaˈwẽhtõh / 'it has been melted'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:367), with causative.
Se. { -nɔ̃Ɂnɔ̃wɛ̃h } in / oˈnɔ̃Ɂnɔ̃wɛ̃ːh / 'it has melted, soaked' (cf. Chafe 1967:73).
Hu. { -nawẽːh }, cf. <ganda8an> 'etre chaud, temperé' (MS 59:87),
long vowel and / h / assumed present.
Wy. { -nawɛ̃- } in / usahuaɁtaˈnaːwɛ̃Ɂ / 'he became warm again'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:249), with inchoative.
[ cf. Rudes 1995:49 ]
515
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:163), presence of / j / anomalous.
Se. { -nɔ̃ɁkeR- } in / waɔ̃wɔ̃ˈnɔ̃Ɂkeːɛ̃Ɂ / 'they imitated him' (cf. Chafe 1967:73),
with dative.
Hu. { -naɁxeɹ- }, cf. <gandagerandi> 'imiter qlq.' (MS 59:85), with dative,
/ Ɂ / assumed present.
[ cf. Michelson 1988:174 ]
516
/ Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -naɁts- } in / jaˈnaɁtsaɁ / 'kettle' (cf. Barbeau 1960:170).
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:277 ]
517
³⁶⁶ PI * { -nẽh- } 'corn, grain, kernel'
Ch. { -nòh- } in / kànòhe̋ːnâː / 'rice' (King 1975:192), / kànòhe̋ːnîː /
'hominy grits' (King 1975:193), both meaning literally 'long grain'.
PNI * { -nẽh- }, * { -nẽhst- } 'corn, grain, kernel, seed'.
Tu. { -nə̃h- } in / oˈnə̃hɛh / 'corn (Zea mays)' (cf. Rudes 1999:351).
No. * { -nẽh- } in <ohnehahk> 'Corn, maize' (cf. Rudes 1981a:48).
Su. * { -nẽhst- } in <Onaesta> 'Sod eller Magiiz' (cf. Mithun 1981a:23).
Mo. { -nʌ̃h- } in / ˈkaːnʌ̃h / 'seed, seeds (for planting, sowing)
(cf. Maracle 1990:24).
Oe. { -nʌ̃h- } in / kaːˈnʌ̃heɁ / 'seed, pit (of a fruit), grain, oats'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:576).
Oo. { -nẽh- } in / óˈnẽhaɁ / 'corn' (cf. Woodbury 2003:698).
Ca. { -nẽh- } in / oˈnẽhẽːɁ / 'corn' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:70).
Se. { -nɛ̃h- } in / oˈnɛ̃ɔɁ̃ / 'corn, corn kernel' (cf. Chafe 1967:69).
Hu. { -nẽh- } in / oˈnẽhaɁ / <onnenha> 'bled' (MS 59:32).
Wy. { -nɛ̃h- } in / uˈnɛ̃haɁ / 'corn' (cf. Barbeau 1960:59).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:184, Mithun 1984b:272 ]
518
PNI * { -nẽhsko- }, * { -nẽhskʷ- } 'steal'.
Tu. { -nə̃hskʷ- } in / raˈnə̃hskʷahs / 'he steals' (cf. Rudes 1999:362).
Mo. { -nʌ̃hskʷ- } in / keˈnʌ̃hskʷahs / 'I steal, rob, I am a thief, robber'
(cf. Maracle 1990:40).
Oe. { -nʌ̃skʷ- } in / ˈknʌ̃skʷas / 'I steal' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:578).
Oo. { -nẽskʷ- } in / hatíˈnẽskʷas / 'they steal' (cf. Woodbury 2003:701).
Ca. { -nẽhsko- } in / ẽhˈsnẽhskoɁ / 'you will steal s.t.'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:315).
Se. { -nɔ̃skʷ- } in / haˈnɔ̃skʷas / 'he steals, thief' (cf. Chafe 1967:72),
change of vowel anomalous.
Wy. { -nɛ̃hskʷ- } in / aɁhaˈnɛ̃hskʷaɁ / 'he stole' (cf. Barbeau 1960:271).
[ cf. Rudes 1995:54 ]
519
³⁷² PNI * { -njaːk- } 'marry'
Tu. { -tjaːk- } in / kaˈtjaːkə̃ / 'husband, spouse, wife' (cf. Rudes 1999:457),
/ ə̃jə̃ˈktiːtjaːk / 'the two of us will marry one another'
(cf. Rudes 1999:457).
Mo. { -njak- } in / wakeˈnjaːkũ / 'I am married' (cf. Maracle 1990:105).
Oe. { -njak- } in / ˈlonjaks / 'he's getting married'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:621).
Oo. { -njak- } in / hóˈtiːnjaks / 'they are marrying' (cf. Woodbury 2003:740).
Ca. { -njaːk- } in / aˈkeːnjaːs / 'I am getting married right now'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:531).
Se. { -njaːk- } in / waotinjaːk / 'they got married' (cf. Chafe 1967:74).
Hu. { -njaːK- }, cf. <gandia˛i> 'Se Marier' (MS 59:96),
long vowel assumed present.
Wy. { -njaːK- } in / ajeˈnjaːih / 'I am married' (cf. Barbeau 1960:67),
/ sahuˈnjakaɁ / 'he married again' (cf. Barbeau 1960:135).
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:278 ]
520
change of vowel anomalous.
[ cf. Lounsbury 1961:13, Mithun 1984b:274 ]
521
[ cf. Rudes 2002:191 ]
522
³⁸² PI * { -nõhs- } 'house'
̀ s- } in / kànhsùːkàhlʌ̃ː̋ Ɂîː / 'wall' (King 1975:193), / khànʌ̃ː́ sùːlʌ̃ː̋ Ɂîː /
Ch. { -nə̃h
'room' (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:142), / h / in second form likely
metathesized, literal meaning of word perhaps 'the house is divided',
cf. / ȁːjâːtòhlʌ̃ː̋ Ɂîː / 'portion, chapter, share' (cf. King 1975:182,
Feeling & Pulte 1975:63).
PNI * { -nõhs- } 'house'.
Tu. { -nə̃hs- } in / oˈnə̃hsɛh / 'cage, cottage, house, hut, umbrella'
(cf. Rudes 1999:357), / nɛkanə̃hˈsaːkjɛː / 'there are two houses'
(cf. Rudes 1999:37), / kaˈnə̃hsoːt / 'the house stands'
(cf. Rudes 1999:472).
No. * { -nõhs- } in <Onushag> 'A House' (cf. Rudes 1981a:47).
La. * { -nõhʃ- } in <Canocha> 'Une maison' (Biggar 1924:245).
Mo. { -nũhs- } in / kaˈnũhsaɁ / 'house, dwelling, residence, home, hut, cabin,
lair' (cf. Maracle 1990:30), / tekaˈnũhsake / 'two houses'
(cf. Maracle 1990:174), / raoˈnũhsaɁ / 'his house'
(cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:198), / kaˈnũhsoteɁ /
'a house is standing there' (cf. Deering & Delisle 1976:500).
Oe. { -nũhs- } in / kaˈnũhsaɁ / 'house, building'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:602), / laoˈnũhsaɁ / 'his house'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:602).
Oo. { -nũhs- } in / kanṹhˈsaɁkeh / 'at the house' (cf. Woodbury 2003:726).
Ca. { -nõhs- } in / kaˈnõhsaɁ / 'house' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:159).
Se. { -nɔ̃hs- } in / kaˈnɔ̃hsoːt / 'house' (cf. Chafe 1967:71).
Hu. { -nõhʃ- } in / xaˈnõhʃaɁ / <gannoncha> 'Cabane' (MS 59:116),
preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
Wy. { -nɔ̃hʃ- } in / jaˈnɔ̃hʃaɁ / 'house' (cf. Barbeau 1960:171).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:183, Mithun 1981a:3, Rudes 1995:52 ]
523
Ca. { -nõhõkt- } in / oˈnohõkteɁ / 'it is painful' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:223),
borrowed from Seneca.
Se. { -nɔ̃ɔk̃ t- } in / oˈnɔ̃ɔk̃ teɁ / 'it's sore' (cf. Chafe 1967:72).
Hu. { -nõhwaht- }, cf. <gannonh8a‛ti> 'etre pris de mal, sentir du mal'
(MS 59:118).
524
(cf. Woodbury 2003:720).
Se. { -nɔ̃taR- } in / onɔ̃tææɁ / 'hominy' (cf. Chafe 1967:72).
525
Mo. { -neɹaht- } in / ˈoneɹahteɁ / 'a leaf, leaves, foliage' (cf. Maracle 1990:231).
Oe. { -nlaht- } in / ˈonlahteɁ / 'leaf' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:594).
Oo. { -næːht- } in / óˈnæːhtaɁ / 'leaf, leafy vegetable' (cf. Woodbury 2003:693).
Ca. { -nɹaht- } in / oˈnɹahtaɁ / 'leaf' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:179).
Se. { -nɛ̃ht- } in / oˈnɛ̃htaɁ / 'leaf' (cf. Chafe 1967:69).
Hu. { -nɹaht- } in / oˈnɹahtaɁ / <Onnrata> 'feuille' (MS 59:121),
/ h / assumed present.
Wy. { -nɹaht- } in / nɹahˈtaɁjeh / 'on the leaves' (cf. Barbeau 1960:112).
[ cf. Michelson 1988:180 ]
526
Tu. { -torə̃- } in / raˈtoːrə̃h / 'he fails to do it' (cf. Rudes 1999:582),
/ kaˈnoːrə̃Ɂ / 'it is expensive' (cf. Rudes 1999:581).
Mo. { -noɹũ- } in / kaˈnoːɹũɁ / 'it is dear, expensive, costly, precious, valuable,
hard to come by' (cf. Maracle 1990:29).
Oe. { -nolũ- } in / ũknoːˈlũ̂ː / 'I got stuck, I couldn't quite make it,
it got me down' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:598), / kanoːˈlũ̂ː /
'it's expensive' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:598).
Oo. { -nowẽ- } in / hóˈnoːwẽk / 'he fails' (cf. Woodbury 2003:716),
/ káˈnoːwẽɁ / 'it is valuable' (cf. Woodbury 2003:716).
Ca. { -nõːɁ } in / ẽˈwaːkhnõːɁ / 'I will fail' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:525),
/ kanõːɁ / 'it is expensive, dear, precious' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:525).
Se. { -noɔ̃- } in / kanoːɔ̃Ɂ / 'it's difficult, expensive' (cf. Chafe 1967:70).
Hu. { -noɹõ- }, cf. <gandóron> 'trouver q.c. ou qlq. difficile' (MS 59:104).
Wy. { -nuɹɔ̃- } in / teɁjaˈnuːɹɔ̃h / 'it is not difficult' (cf. Barbeau 1960:284).
[ cf. Michelson 1988:173 ]
527
(cf. Chafe 1967:76).
528
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:539).
Se. { -oɁkt- } in / oːtoɁk / 'it's at the end' (cf. Chafe 1967:76), with semireflexive.
529
(MS 59:202), / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -uɁtɛ̃h } in / hutiɁtaɹuɁtɛ̃h / 'they are that kind of clan'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:243).
⁴⁰³ PI * { -õːkuːh- } 'go beyond, go through, leak, pass through, penetrate, reach'
Ch. { -ʌ̃ː̀ kùːh- } in / kʌ̃ː̀ kǔːhska̋ː / 'it's leaking' (cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:126).
PNI * { -õkoh- } 'go through, pass through, reach',
more often * { -õkoht- }, with causative.
Tu. { -ə̃koht- } in / rə̃ˈkohthaɁ / 'he sifts it' (cf. Rudes 1999:156),
/ wɛhraɁnə̃ˈkohthaɁ / 'he goes beyond' (Rudes 1999:490),
with semireflexive.
Mo. { -ũkoh- } in / teˈkũːkohs / 'I touch, reach something by throwing'
(cf. Maracle 1990:91).
Oe. { -ũkoht- } in / tejũˈkothaɁ / 'it's coming through (e.g. water is coming
through a hole in the roof)' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:751).
Oo. { -ũkoht- } in / aṹˈkohtih / 'it is too much, it has gone beyond it'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:839).
Ca. { -õkoht- } in / tehˈsõːkoht / 'filter it, strain it' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:544).
Se. { -ɔ̃koht- } in / aˈkɔ̃kohtɔ̃h / 'I've put it through an opening'
(cf. Chafe 1967:77).
Hu. { -õxoh- }, cf. <On˛o> 'penetrer, passer au Travers' (MS 59:188),
/ h / assumed present.
530
[ cf. Rudes 1995:46 ]
531
Ca. { -õnh- } in / aˈkõːnheɁ / 'she is alive' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:546).
Se. { -ɔ̃h- } in / kɔ̃heɁ / 'I'm alive' (cf. Chafe 1967:77).
Hu. { -õnh- }, cf. <Onnhe> 'vivre' (MS 59:189).
Wy. { -ɔ̃t- } in / ihˈʃɔ̃ːteɁ / 'your life' (cf. Barbeau 1960:178).
[ cf. Rudes 1995:36 ]
532
Mo. { -ũt- } in / ˈkjâːtũteɁ / 'I have a body, I am mortal' (cf. Maracle 1990:52).
Oe. { -ũt- } in / johnaɁˈtatslũteɁ / 'it has a pocket'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:386).
Oo. { -ũt- } in / áˈũːtaɁ / it is attached, it has something attached to it'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:855).
Ca. { -õːt } in / tejonɁaˈkaõːt / 'sheep; lams; elk [lit. it has horns]'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:505).
Se. { -ɔ̃ːt } in / aˈwɛ̃ːɔ̃ːt / 'there's a flower on it' (cf. Chafe 1967:78).
Hu. { -õːt }, cf. <Ont> 'etre, y avoir' (MS 59:192), long vowel assumed present.
Wy. { -ɔ̃t } in / teɁhaˈnɔ̃ːtɔ̃t / 'he has no legs' (cf. Barbeau 1960:199).
533
Oo. { -R- } in / kajáɁˈtaːhaɁ / 'movie, camera [lit. there are beings in it]'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:443).
Ca. { -R- } in / kaˈjaɁtaːɁ / 'a picture [lit. there is a being in it]'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:587).
Se. { -R- } in / haːnekahaɁ / 'he puts water in it' (cf. Chafe 1967:47).
Hu. { -ɹ- }, cf. <gar> 'q.c. etre dedans, etre avec les autres' (MS 59:130).
Wy. { -ɹ- } in / iˈjaɹ / 'it is in it' (cf. Barbeau 1960:96).
534
Oe. { -lakew- } in / waklakeˈwâːthũ / 'I have used it to wipe something'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:508), with causative.
Oo. { -Rækew- } in / kaǽːˈkeːwẽh / 'it is wiped' (cf. Woodbury 2003:326).
Ca. { -Rakew- } in / kɹaˈkeːwahs / 'I am erasing it, wiping it'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:370).
[ cf. Michelson 1988:180 ]
535
Hu. { -ɹat- } in / ʃɹaˈtaɁxeh / <chrata˛e> 'a ton Talon' (MS 59:137).
536
(MS 59:139).
Wy. { -ɹɛ̃n- } in / tehutiˈɹɛ̃nuɁtɛ̃hk / 'their song will be of that sort'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:104).
[ cf. Michelson 1988:175 ]
537
No. * { -ɹihsɹ- } in <Orisrāg> 'Stockings' (cf. Rudes 1981a:47).
Su. * { -ɹis } in <Khaalis> 'Strumpor' (cf. Mithun 1981a:23).
Mo. { -ɹihs } in / ˈkaːɹihs / 'stocking' (cf. Maracle 1990:398).
Oe. { -lisl- } in / oˈlislaɁ / 'pant leg' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:536).
Oo. { -Ris- } in / ˈkáːis / 'leggings' (cf. Woodbury 2003:437).
Ca. { -Risɹ- } in / kaˈiːsɹaɁ / 'leggings' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:180),
lack of / h / anomalous.
Se. { -RishR- } in / ˈkaishæɁ / 'leggings' (cf. Chafe 1967:54).
Hu. { -ɹihʃ } in / ˈxaːɹihʃ / <garich> 'Chausses' (MS 59:143),
/ h / assumed present.
Wy. { -ɹihʃɹ- } in / jaɹihˈʃɹuɁtɛ̃h / 'kind of leggings' (cf. Barbeau 1960:89).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:184, Mithun 1984b:275 ]
538
No. * { -ɹijo- } in <Wauntrehu> 'To fight', <Untatreeyou> 'To kill'
(cf. Rudes 1981a:48).
Mo. { -ɹijo- } in / kateˈɹiːjohs / 'I fight, battle' (cf. Maracle 1990:33),
with semireflexive; { -ɹjo- } in / ˈkeɹjohs / 'I kill, vaniquish'
(cf. Maracle 1990:44).
Oe. { -lijo- } in / katliːˈjos / 'I fight, I'm a fighter'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:240), with semireflexive; { -ljo- } in
/ kliːˈjos / 'I'm beating it, I'm killing it'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:545).
Oo. { -Rjo- } in / ˈkéːjos / 'I am killing it' (cf. Woodbury 2003:932).
Ca. { -Rijo- } in / ẽhˈsɹiːjoɁ / 'you will kill s.t.' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:173);
{ -njo- } in / aˈhaːnjoɁ / 'he killed it' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:173).
Se. { -Rijo- } in / oɁkatiːjoɁ / 'I fought' (cf. Chafe 1967:74), with semireflexive;
{ -njo- } in / hanjos / 'he's killing it' (cf. Chafe 1967:74).
Hu. { -ɹio- }, { -jo- }, cf. <gario>, <gaïo> 'frapper, battre, prendre'
(MS 59:82).
Wy. { -ɹiʒu- } in / eɹiːˈʒuhʃ / 'I kill it' (cf. Barbeau 1960:284), / usahɔ̃tɹiːˈʒuɁ /
'they fought again' (cf. Barbeau 1960:309), with semireflexive; { -ʒu- }
in / eːʒaːˈʒuɁ / 'he will kill you' (cf. Barbeau 1960:82).
539
⁴³⁰ PNI * { -ɹõhj- } 'blue, sky'
Tu. { -rə̃hj- } in / oˈrə̃hjɛh / 'sky, blue' (cf. Rudes 1999:384).
La. * { -Rõhj- } in <Quemhya> 'Le ciel' (Biggar 1924:244).
Mo. { -ɹũhj- } in / oˈɹũ̂ːjaɁ / 'blue' (cf. Maracle 1990:73);
/ kaˈɹũ̂ːjaɁ / 'sky, heavens, atmosphere' (cf. Maracle 1990:30).
Oo. { -Rẽhj- } in / óˈwẽhjaɁ / 'blue' (cf. Woodbury 2003:400);
́ ˈjaɁkeh / 'in the sky' (cf. Woodbury 2003:400).
/ kaẽh
Ca. { -Rõhj- } in / ˈaõhjaẽːɁ / 'blue' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:33);
/ ˈkaõhjɁakeh / 'in the heavens; in the sky' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:297).
Se. { -Rɔ̃ːj- } in / ˈkɛ̃ɔj̃ ateɁ / 'the sky (is present)' (cf. Chafe 1967:89).
Hu. { -ɹõhj- } in / xaˈɹõhjaɁ / <garonhia> 'le Ciel' (MS 59:146).
Wy. { -ɹɔ̃ːʒ- } in / hatiɹɔ̃ːʒɔ̃Ɂ / 'they are many in the sky' (cf. Barbeau 1960:77).
[ cf. Lounsbury 1978:340, Mithun 1979:185 ]
540
Wy. { -ɹɁu- } in / aɁˈjeɹɁu / 'I chopped' (cf. Barbeau 1960:227).
541
repetitive, refers to evil twin of / thaɹũhjaˈwaːkũ /, a creator god.
Oe. / taˈwiskalaɁ / 'devil or the Evil twin' (cf. Rudes 1995:45).
Hu. / ataˈwihskaɹaɁ / <ata8iskara> 'Pierre a fusil' (MS 67:155),
/ h / assumed present, word-initial / t / recast as reflexive { at }.
Wy. / taˈwihskaːɹaɁ / 'flint' (cf. Barbeau 1960:206).
[ cf. Rudes 1995:45 ]
542
(cf. Maracle 1990:92).
Oe. { -tenj- } in / teˈktenjehseɁ / 'I'm changing it'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:687).
Oo. { -tenj- } in / téˈkteːnjũs / 'I change things, I make changes'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:349).
Ca. { -teni- } in / ẽhsatkʷẽnjaːˈteːniɁ / 'you will change your clothes'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:426).
Se. { -tenj- } in / akteːnjɔ̃ːh / 'I've changed it' (cf. Chafe 1967:82).
Hu. { -teni- }, { -tenj- }, cf. <gatendi> 'changer' (MS 59:158).
Wy. { -tenj- } / tuteˈteːnjɔ̃h / 'it has changed there' (cf. Barbeau 1960:288),
with semireflexive.
543
Mo. / aˈtîːɹũ / 'raccoon' (cf. Maracle 1990:6).
Oe. / ʌ̃tiːˈlũ / 'raccoon' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:351), unexpected loss of
glottal stop.
Hu. / ẽˈtiɁɹõ / <Entiron> 'Chat Sauvage' (MS 62:7), / Ɂ / assumed present.
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:265, Rudes 1995:53 ]
⁴⁴⁷ PNI * { toːh } 'how, how many, how much' ( limited to Iroquoia )
Mo. / ˈtoː / 'how' (cf. Maracle 1990:98).
Oe. / ˈtoː / 'how many, how much' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:712).
544
Oo. / ˈtoː / 'how, how much' (cf. Woodbury 2003:362).
Ca. / toː / 'how (or other terms involving measurement)'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:676).
Se. / toːh / 'how many?' (cf. Chafe 1967:84).
545
Se. / ˈtiɁtiːɁ / 'bluejay (Cyanocitta cristata)' (cf. Chafe 1967:83).
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:268 ]
546
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:730).
Oo. { -tsihkʷ- } in / óˈtsihkʷaɁ / 'fist, knot, button' (cf. Woodbury 2003:645).
Hu. { -tsihkʷ- } in / oˈtsihkʷaɁ / <otsik8a> 'casse-tete' (Fraser 1920:454),
preconsonantal / h / assumed present.
Wy. { -tsihkʷ- } in / haɁˈutsihkuːtɛ̃Ɂ / 'it became a knot on a tree'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:183), loss of rounding before / u /.
547
⁴⁵⁷ PNI * { -tsin } 'be male'
Su. * { -tsin } in <Ræætsin> 'En Mann' (cf. Mithun 1981a:23).
Mo. { -tsin } in / ˈɹaːtsin / 'a male, it is male' (cf. Maracle 1990:77).
Oe. { -tsin } in / keːˈtsin / 'I'm industrious, determined, aggressive'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:731).
Oo. { -tsin- } in / hatsíːˈnah / 'he is a male, he is a man'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:647).
Ca. { -tsin- } in / haˈtsiːnah / 'he is masculine, brave'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:192).
Se. { -tsin- } in / hatsiːnɔ̃h / 'he's a male' (cf. Chafe 1967:62).
548
⁴⁶⁰ PNI * { tsitshoɁ } 'fox, lynx'
Tu. / ˈtʃitʃho / 'small fox, lynx' (cf. Rudes 1999:127).
Su. <Tzídtze> 'Katta' (cf. Mithun 1981a:23).
Mo. / ˈtsitshoɁ / 'a fox' (cf. Maracle 1990:100).
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:265, Rudes 1995:46 ]
549
Hu. { -tsiɁts- } in / oˈtsiɁtsaɁ / <Otsitsa> 'Fleur' (MS 67:99),
/ Ɂ / assumed present.
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:263, Rudes 1995:41 ]
550
'minnow', lit. 'real fish'.
PNI * { -itsjõh }, incorporating form * { -itsj- }, * { -itsjõɁt- } 'fish';
origin of * / i / unclear.
Tu. { -itʃə̃h } in / ˈkə̃ːtʃə̃h / 'fish' (cf. Rudes 1999:246).
No. * { -itʃõh } in <Kaintu> 'A fish' (cf. Rudes 1981a:46).
La. * { -itsõh } in <Quejon> 'Poisson' (Biggar 1924:242).
Mo. { -itsjũɁ } in / ˈkə̃tsjũɁ / 'fish' (cf. Maracle 1990:43).
Oe. { -itsj- } in / ˈkə̃tsiɁ / 'fish' (cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:438).
Oo. { -itsjũɁt- } in / óˈtsjũɁtaɁ / 'fish' (cf. Woodbury 2003:632).
Ca. { -itsõɁt- } in / oˈtsõɁtaɁ / 'fish' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:121).
Se. { -itsɔ̃h } in / kɛ̃tsɔ̃h / 'fish' (cf. Chafe 1967:59).
Hu. { -itsõh } in / ˈxẽtsõh / <gentson> 'Poisson' (MS 59:179),
final / h / assumed present.
[ cf. Lounsbury 1978:342, Mithun 1979:182, Mithun 1981b:238 and 1984b:274,
Rudes 1995:45 ]
551
(cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:125).
PNI * { -oː- } 'put into water'.
Tu. { -oː- } in / waɁɛhˈsɛharoːɁ / 'one put lye in water'
(cf. Feeling & Pulte 1975:462).
Mo. { -o- } in / ˈjeːkohs / 'I put, immerse, throw something into a liquid'
(cf. Maracle 1990:12).
Oe. { -o- } in / waˈkitsjoɁ / 'I'm boiling fish'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:630).
Oo. { -o- } in / ˈwáɁkok / 'I immersed it in water, I dipped it into water'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:742).
Ca. { -oː- } in / shõˈnaɁtoː / 'boil the potatoes' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:535).
Se. { -o- } in / aˈkoɔ̃h / 'I've put it in the water' (cf. Chafe 1967:74).
Hu. { -oː- }, cf. <O> 'dans leau' (MS 59:180), long vowel assumed present.
Wy. { -u- } in / tawaˈteɁskuh / 'she got into the water' (cf. Barbeau 1960:89).
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:274 ]
552
Tu. { -orok- } in / ˈroːroks / 'he covers it' (cf. Rudes 1999:569).
Mo. { -Ɂɹhoɹok- } in / keɁˈɹhoːɹokhs / 'I cover up, cover over'
(cf. Maracle 1990:44).
Oe. { -Ɂlholok- } in / keɁlhoːˈloks / 'I'm covering it up'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:849).
Oo. { -Ɂhow- } in / óɁˈhoːwih / 'it is covering it, it has covered it'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:1063).
Ca. { -Ɂhowek- } in / keɁˈhoːweːs / 'I am covering s.t.'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:73).
Se. { -Ɂhowek- } in / waɁˈeɁhoːweːk / 'she covered it' (cf. Chafe 1967:90).
Hu. { -Ɂɹhor- }, cf. <garhori> 'empaqueter, envelopper' (MS 59:142),
/ Ɂ / assumed present; { -oɹ- }, cf. <Ori> 'couvrir, se couvrir,
etre couvert' (MS 59:197).
Wy. { -uɹ- } in / aɁhakjaɁˈtuːɹiɁ / 'he covered his body' (cf. Barbeau 1960:258).
553
Mo. / waɁˈkôːɹjaɁ / 'whippoorwill' (cf. Maracle 1990:411).
Oe. / kwaɁˈkôːli / 'whippoorwill' (cf. Mithun 1981:268).
Ca. / kwẽɁˈkohnjẽɁ / 'whippoorwill' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:368),
partially reshaped, perhaps borrowed from Seneca.
Se. / ˈkwɛ̃Ɂkoːnjɛ̃Ɂ / 'whippoorwill (Caprimulgus vociferus)' (cf. Chafe 1967:68),
partially reshaped.
[ cf. Mithun 1984b:268 ]
554
Oe. { -whjũhkar- } in / kewhjũhkaˈlâːke / '(on) my thumb'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:771).
Oo. { -WjũhkaR- } in / keːjṹhˈkaːɁkeh / 'my thumb' (cf. Woodbury 2003:939).
Ca. { -wẽɁjõhkaR- } in / kwɁẽjõhˈkaːɁkeh / 'on my thumb'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:338), probably recast.
Se. { -jɔ̃hkaR- } in / kjɔ̃hˈkææɁkeh / 'on my thumb' (cf. Chafe 1967:89).
[ cf. Michelson 1988:177 ]
555
Hu. { -j- } in / ˈxajaɁ / <gaïa> 'aile' (MS 59:79); { -jahʃ- } in / xaˈjahʃaɁ /
<gaïachia> 'bras' (MS 59:79), / h / assumed present.
Wy. { -ʒahʃ- } in / haˈʒahʃaɁ / 'his arm' (cf. Barbeau 1960:125).
556
Oe. { -wjʌ̃n- } in / kewjʌ̃nʌ̃ːˈtâːseɁ / 'I'm getting something ready'
(cf. Michelson & Doxtator 2002:777).
Oo. { -Wjẽn- } in / káːˈjẽːnaɁ / 'task, manner, way' (cf. Woodbury 2003:924).
Ca. { -wajẽn- } in / aɁewajẽːˈnẽːtaɁ / 'she finished' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:581).
Hu. { -jẽn- } in / xaˈjẽːnaɁ / <gaïenda> 'maniere dagir b.' (MS 59:80).
557
PNI * { -Ɂn- } 'arrow'.
Tu. { -Ɂt- } in / ˈaɁtɛh / 'arrow, bullet, dart, projectile' (cf. Rudes 1999:112).
Ca. { -Ɂn- } in / kɁanõh / 'arrow' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:15),
borrowed from Seneca.
Se. { -Ɂn- } in / kaɁnɔ̃Ɂ / 'arrow' (cf. Chafe 1967:91).
Hu. { -Ɂn- } in / ˈxaɁnaɁ / <ganda> 'fléche' (MS 59:84), / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -Ɂn- } in / ˈuɁnaɁ / 'arrow' (cf. Barbeau 1960:154).
[ cf. Mithun 1979:181, Mithun 1984b:269 ]
558
Se. { -Ɂnikɔ̃HR- } in / hoɁˈnikɔ̃ɛɁ̃ / 'his mind' (cf. Chafe 1967:91).
Hu. { -Ɂnixõhɹ- } in / xaɁniˈxõhɹaɁ / <gandigon‛ra> 'esprit, ame, pensée'
(MS 59:100), / Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -Ɂnijɔ̃ːɹ- } in / huɁnijɔ̃ːɹuˈwaːnɛ̃h / 'his mind is great'
(cf. Barbeau 1960:266).
559
Oo. { -Ɂnjũhs- } in / keɁnjṹhˈsaɁkeh / 'my nose' (cf. Woodbury 2003:970).
Ca. { -Ɂnjõhs- } in / oɁˈnjõhsaɁ / 'nose' (cf. Froman et al. 2002:213).
Se. { -Ɂnjɔ̃hs- } in / oɁˈnjɔ̃hsaɁ / 'nose' (cf. Chafe 1967:92).
[ cf. Rudes 1995:50 ]
560
Oo. { -ɁskoɁ- } in / hóɁˈskoɁih / 'he has drowned, he has fallen into the water'
(cf. Woodbury 2003:975).
Ca. { -ɁskoɁ- } in / ahõwaˈtiɁskoɁ / 'they (m) have baptized him, them'
(cf. Froman et al. 2002:387).
Se. { -ɁskoɁ- } in / oɁˈkeɁskoɁt / 'I fell in the water' (cf. Chafe 1967:93).
Hu. { -ɁskoɁ- }, cf. <gasko> 'tomb. a leau, enfoucer, fe. naufrage' (MS 59:149),
/ Ɂ / assumed present.
Wy. { -ɁskuɁ- } in / eˈheɁskuhk / 'I will drown him' (cf. Barbeau 1960:249).
561
REFERENCES
Abbott, Clifford. (2000). Oneida. Languages of the World, Materials, 301. Munich:
Lincom Europa.
Alsop, George. (1666). A Character of the Province of Maryland. London: Peter Dring.
Barbeau, Marius. (1961). The Language of Canada in the Voyages of Jacques Cartier
(1534-1538). Contributions to Anthropology 1959, 108-229. Ottawa: National
Museum of Canada, Bulletin 173.
Beatty, John. (1972). Mohawk Morphology. Ph.D. dissertation, City University of New
York.
Biggar, Henry Percival. (1924). The Voyages of Jacques Cartier: Published from the
Originals with Translations, Notes and Appendices. Ottawa: F. A. Acland.
562
Boyce, Douglas. (1978). Iroquoian Tribes of the Virginia-North-Carolina Coastal Plain.
In William Sturtevant, ed. Handbook of North American Indians. Volume 15,
282-289.
Campanius Holm, Thomas. (1702). Kort beskrifning om provincien Nya Swerige uti
America: som nu förtjden af the Engelske kallas Pensylvania. Stockholm:
J. H. Werner.
Castiglioni, Luigi. (1790). Viaggio negli Stati Uniti dell' America Settentrionale, fatto negli
anni 1785, 1786, e 1787. Milan: Giuseppe Marelli.
Chafe, Wallace. (1964a). Another Look at Siouan and Iroquoian. American Anthropologist
66: 852-862.
Chafe, Wallace. (1964b). Linguistic Evidence for the Relative Age of Iroquois Religious
Practices. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 20: 278-285.
563
Chafe, Wallace. (1970b). Review of Paul Postal, Aspects of Phonological Theory.
Language 46: 116-125.
Chafe, Wallace. (1973). Siouan, Iroquoian, and Caddoan. In Thomas Sebeok, ed. Current
Trends in Linguistics, Volume 10, 1164-1209.
Chafe, Wallace. (1976). The Caddoan, Iroquoian, and Siouan Languages. The Hague:
Mouton.
Chafe, Wallace. (1977a). Accent and Related Phenomena in the Five Nations Iroquois
Languages. In Larry Hyman, ed. Studies in Stress and Accent, 169-181. Southern
California Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4.
Chafe, Wallace. (1977b). The Evolution of Third Person Verb Agreement in the
Iroquoian Languages. In Charles Li, ed. Mechanisms of Syntactic Change. Austin:
University of Texas Press. 493-524.
Chafe, Wallace. (1984). How To Say They Drank in Iroquois. In Michael Foster, Jack
Campisi, and Marianne Mithun, eds. Extending the Rafters: Interdisciplinary
Approaches to Iroquoian Studies, 301-311. Albany: State University of New York
Press.
Chafe, Wallace, and Michael Foster. (1981). Prehistoric Divergences and Recontacts
Between Cayuga, Seneca, and the Other Northern Iroquoian Languages.
International Journal of American Linguistics 47: 121-142.
Crouse, Dorothy, and Blair Rudes. (1987). The Tuscarora Legacy of J. N. B. Hewitt:
Materials for the Study of the Tuscarora Language and Culture. Mercury Series,
Canadian Ethnology Service Paper no. 108. Ottawa: Canadian Museum of
Civilization.
564
Cuoq, Jean-André. (1873). Kaiatonsera ionteweienstakwa kaiatonserase: nouveau syllabaire
iroquois. Montreal: John Lovell.
De Vorsey, Louis, ed. (1971). De Brahm's Report of the General Survey in the Southern
District of North America. Columbia, South Carolina: University of South Carolina
Press.
Deering, Nora, and Helga Harries Delisle. (1976). Mohawk Teaching Grammar
(Preliminary Version). Kahnawake: Kanien'kehaka Raotitiohkwa Cultural Center.
Dudley, Ruth Holmes. (1974). Seneca Stridents. Ph.D. dissertation, State University of
New York at Buffalo.
Foster, Michael. (1982). Alternating Weak and Strong Syllables in Cayuga Words.
International Journal of American Linguistics 48: 59-72.
Fraser, Alexander, ed. (1920). Huron Manuscripts From Rev. Pierre Potier's Collection.
15th Report of the Bureau of Archives for the Province of Ontario. Toronto:
Clarkson W. James.
Gallatin, Albert. (1836). A Synopsis of the Indian Tribes of North America. In Archaeologia
Americana, Transactions and Collections of the American Antiquarian Society 2:
1-422.
565
Hewitt, John Napoleon Brinton, ed. (n.d). Nottoway-Anonymous. National
Anthropological Archives, manuscript no. 3603, Washington, D.C.
Hewitt, John Napoleon Brinton. (1912). Wenrohronon. In Frederick Webb Hodge, ed.
Handbook of American Indians North of Mexico. Bureau of American Ethnology
Bulletin 30, 2: 932-934.
Holmes, Ruth Bradley, and Betty Sharp Smith. (1977). Beginning Cherokee. Second
edition. Norman, Oklahoma: University of Oklahoma Press.
Hyde, Jabez Backus. (1818). Kianasa Nana Nonedowaga Neuwenuda / Hymns in the
Seneca Language. Buffalo, New York: H. A. Salisbury.
Kilham, Hannah Spurr. (1818). A Short Vocabulary in the Language of the Seneca Nation,
and in English / Ung-eish-neut Teu-au-geh Neh-huh Yoh-weh-neut-dah Eng-lish.
London: W. & S. Graves.
King, Duane. (1975). A Grammar and Dictionary of the Cherokee Language. Ph.D.
dissertation, University of Georgia.
Kopris, Craig. (2001). A Grammar and Dictionary of Wyandot. Ph.D. dissertation, State
University of New York, Buffalo.
Lagarde, Pierrette. (1980). Le verbe huron: étude morphologique d'après une description
grammaticale de la seconde moitié du XVIIe siècle. Paris: L'Harmattan.
566
Lawson, John. (1709). A New Voyage to Carolina. A New Collection of Voyages and
Travels, With Historical Accounts of Discoveries and Conquest in all Parts of the
World. London: John Stevens.
Le Clercq, Crestien. (1882). First Establishment of the Faith in New France. John Gilmary
Shea, translator. New York, New York: Privately Published.
Michelson, Karin. (1981b). Three Stories in Oneida, Told and Translated by Georgina
Nicholas. National Museum of Man, Mercury Series, Ethnology Division,
Paper No. 73. Ottawa: National Museums of Canada.
Michelson, Karin. (1986). Ghost R's in Onondaga. In Leo Wetzels and Engin Sezer, eds.
Studies in Compensatory Lengthening, 147-166. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.
567
Michelson, Karin, and Mercy Doxtator. (2002). Oneida-English / English-Oneida
Dictionary. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Mithun, Marianne. (1979). Iroquoian. In Lyle Campbell & Marianne Mithun, eds.
The Languages of Native America: A Historical and Comparative Assessment,
133-212. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Mithun, Marianne. (1982). The Synchronic and Diachronic Behavior of Plops, Squeaks,
Croaks, and Moans. International Journal of American Linguistics 48: 49-58.
Mithun, Marianne. (1985). Untangling the Huron and the Iroquois. International Journal
of American Linguistics 51: 504-507.
568
Mithun, Marianne. (1989). The Incipient Obsolescence of Polysynthesis: Cayuga in
Ontario and Oklahoma. In Nancy Dorian, ed. Investigating Obsolescence: Studies in
Language Contraction and Death, 243-257. New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Mithun, Marianne. (1999). The Languages of Native North America. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Mooney, James. (1900). Myths of the Cherokee. 19th Annual Report of the Bureau of
American Ethnology, Part 1, 3-548. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing
Office.
MS 66. (n.d.). Recueil de Chants Hurons. [by Paul Picard Tsawenhohi]. Collection de
manuscrits, Archives du Séminaire de Québec.
569
Pendergast, James. (1991). The Massawomeck: Raiders and Traders into the Chesapeake
Bay in the Seventeenth Century. Transactions of the American Philosophical
Society, Vol. 81, Part 2. Philadelphia: The American Philosophical Society.
Postal, Paul. (1968). Aspects of Phonological Theory. New York: Harper & Row.
Postal, Paul. (1979). Some Syntactic Rules of Mohawk. Ph.D. dissertation, Yale
University.
Rudes, Blair. (1976). Historical Phonology and the Development of the Tuscarora Sound
System. Ph.D. dissertation, State University of New York at Buffalo.
Rudes, Blair. (1987). Tuscarora Roots, Stems, and Particles: Towards a Dictionary of
Tuscarora. Algonquian and Iroquoian Linguistics Memoir no. 3. Winnipeg:
Algonquian and Iroquoian Linguistics.
Rudes, Blair. (2002). Early Vocabularies and Dictionary Development. In Barbara Burnaby
and Jon Reyhner, eds. Indigenous Languages Across the Community. Flagstaff,
Arizona: Northern Arizona University Press.
570
Sagard, Gabriel. (1632). Dictionaire de la langue huronne necessaire à ceux qui n'ont
l'intelligence d'icelle & ont a traiter avec les savages du pays. Paris: Chez Denys
Moreau.
Sickles, Abraham W. (1855). A Collection of Hymns in the Oneida Language, for the Use of
Native Christians. Toronto: Wesleyan Missionary Society.
Steckley, John. (1997). Wendat Dialects and the Development of the Huron Alliance.
Northeast Anthropology 54: 23-36.
Steckley, John. (2004). De Religione: Telling the Seventeenth-Century Jesuit Story in Huron
to the Iroquois. Norman, Oklahoma: University of Oklahoma Press.
Steckley, John. (2007a). Words of the Huron. Waterloo, Ontario: Wilfrid Laurier
University Press.
Swanton, John Reed. (1968). The Indian Tribes of North America. Bureau of American
Ethnology Bulletin 145.
Thwaites, Reuben Gold, ed. (1896-1901). The Jesuit Relations and Allied Documents:
Travels and Explorations of the Jesuit Missionaries in New France 1610-1791.
Cleveland, Ohio: Burrows Brothers.
571
Trigger, Bruce. (1987). The Children of Aataentsic: A History of the Huron People to 1660.
Montreal: McGill-Queen's University Press.
White, Marian. (1978a). Neutral and Wenro. In William Sturtevant, ed. Handbook of
North American Indians. Volume 15, 407-411.
White, Marian. (1978b). Erie. In William Sturtevant, ed. Handbook of North American
Indians. Volume 15, 412-417.
Wood, John. (1820). Vocabulary of the Language of the Nottoway Tribe of Indians.
American Philosophical Society, manuscript no. 2478, Philadephia.
Woodbury, Hanni, ed. (1992). Concerning the League: The Iroquois League Tradition as
Dictated in Onondaga by John Arthur Gibson. Winnipeg: Algonquian and Iroquoian
Linguistics.
572
Wright, Richard. (1996). Tone and Accent in Oklahoma Cherokee. In Pamela Munro, ed.
Cherokee Papers from UCLA, 11-22. Los Angeles: Department of Linguistics,
UCLA.
Wright, Roy. (1974). People of the Panther: A Long Erie Tale. In Michael Foster, ed.
Papers in Linguistics from the 1972 Conference on Iroquoian Research, 47-118.
National Museum of Man Mercury Series, Ethnology Division Paper no. 10.
Ottawa: National Museums of Canada.
573