The Study of Sentence Structure Clause Patterns: You Can Borrow My Car If You Need It
The Study of Sentence Structure Clause Patterns: You Can Borrow My Car If You Need It
The Study of Sentence Structure Clause Patterns: You Can Borrow My Car If You Need It
Sentences are either simple or multiple. A simple sentence consists of a single independent
clause. A multiple sentence contains one or more clauses as its immediate constituents.
clauses: I was doing the washing up and the children were reading tales.
- A complex sentence with one subordinate clause can be analyzed once for the sentence
as a whole and once for the subclause included within the sentence-
S V Od A
conj. S V Od
Distinctions between the elements and between types within the elements are based on –
a/ form
b/ position
c/ syntactic function
d/ semantic role
SUBJECT
FORM: it is normally a NP or a nominal clause
POSITION: it occurs before the V in declarative clauses and after the opetaror in interrogative
clauses
SYNTACTIC FUNCTION: obligatory (imperative!)
1
- It determines the number and person of the V:
Nancy knows my parents.
My parents know Nancy.
-It determines the number of Cs when it is a NP:
Mary is my sister.
Mary and Jane are my sisters.
-It determines the number, person and gender of the reflexive pronoun as Od, Oi, Cs or
prepositional complement:
He shaved himself with his own razor.
DIRECT OBJECT
FORM: it is normally a NP or a nominal clause.
POSITION: It normally follows the S and the V.
FUNCTION: it requires the objective form for pronouns with distinctive case forms.
They amuse me. They gave me ..
If it is coreferential with the S it normally requires a reflexive pronoun:
I have made myself a cake.
Omission of objects is possible:
Shake well before use.
She is washing.
Do you drink? (restricted meaning)
INDIRECT OBJECT
FORM: it is normally a NP or a nominal clause
POSITION: it generally corresponds to a Prep-al Phrase:
Pour me a drink. Pour a drink for me.
COMPLEMENTS
Cs relates to the S and the verb is copular or linking:
My plate is empty.
He’ll become a doctor.
Co relates to Od:
We find them very pleasant.
FORM: normally a NP or an Adj P, may also be a nominal clause:
My hobby is listening to music.
POSITION: Cs follows the S and the V, Co follows the S, V, and Od.
2
FUNCTION: if it is a NP, Cs has concord of number with S, Co with Od.
If it is a reflexive pronoun, Cs has concord of number, person, gender with the S:
She is not herself today.
ADVERBIAL
The most diverse of the clause elements.
FORM: NP, AdverbP, PrepP, Adverbial clause.
POSITION: It is capable of occurring in more than one position. Constraints on its mobility
depend on the type and form of the A.
FUNCTION: except for the obligatory A in SVA, SVOA types, adverbials are optional.
Subject-related A-s may be space adjuncts:
We are in the lecture hall.
Object-related A-s can also function as space or time adjuncts:
He directed his speech at his colleagues.
The order of clause elements is relatively fixed, in general following the sequence in the
designation of the 7 clause types in English.
Yesterday I was excited.
I was excited yesterday.
Apart from its agentive function, the S frequently has an Instrumental role: a generally
inanimate, unwitting cause of an event.
3
There can be seen a regular relation in terms of clause function between
intransitive Verbs or adjectives and the corresponding transitive Verbs
expressing causative meaning:
S affected V S agent/instr V O affected
The door opened. John/the key opened the door.
I got angry. Something angered me.
My dog was barking. I was walking my dog.
The S may also have a Recipient role with Verbs like i/ have/possess, etc.:
Mr Smith has bought his son a car. Mr Smith’s son has a car.
ii/ The perceptual Verbs see/hear also require a Recipient S, look at/listen to- are agentive.
I can see a boat in the distance.
A Subject which lacks semantic content altogether and consists only of the meaningless ’prop’
word it, is the Empty S, occurring mostly in clauses concerning time or weather.
It’s getting cold.
It’s Saturday tomorrow.
2. The most typical function of Od is that of the Affected participant. An animate or
inanimate participant does not cause the happening denoted by the Verb but is directly
involved in some other way.
They criticized the Prime Minister.
I’ve broken a plate.
A Locative object superficially may look like an adverbial with an omitted preposition but the
status as O can be proved by the passive.
He swam the river. – across
4
The horse jumped the fence. – over /The fence was jumped by the horse.
Effected objects can be of three kinds:
i/ It is resultant if it refers to something which exists only by virtue of the activity.
Baird invented TV. I’m writing a letter.
ii/ Cognate objects that repeat wholly or partially the meaning of the Verb.
I dreamt a terrible dream. He died a miserable death.
(sing a song, fight a good fight, etc.)
iii/ The third type of effected O, the eventive, takes the form of a verbal N preceded by a
common V of general meaning, e.g. do/give/have/make/take
He did little work. (He worked little.) He’s having a bath. He took a rest.
3. The most typical function of Oi is that of Recipient or dative participant, of animate being
passively implicated by the happening or state.
I’ve found you a place.
An exceptional type is the Affected Oi with verbs like give/pay, which have an effected Od.
I gave the door three kicks. I paid her a visit.
SUBJECT-VERB CONCORD
Concord is a kind of agreement between two grammatical units. The most important type of
concord in English is concord of 3rd person number between Subject and Verb.
-When the S is realized by a NP, it counts as singular if its head is singular:
The change in male attitudes is obvious.
-Finite and non-finite clauses generally count as singular:
How you did it doesn’t concern me.
-Nominal relative clauses depend on the interpretation of the number of the wh-element:
What were new proposals were, in fact, already known.
5
3. The principle of proximity, ‘attraction’ denotes agreement of the V with a closely
preceding NP in preference to agreement with the head of the NP that functions as S:
Nobody except his own supporters agree with him.
The three principles and their interaction can be illustrated in three problematic areas:
i/ Collective nouns
ii/ Coordination
iii/ Indefinite expressions
i/ Collective nouns
Singular collective nouns may be notionally plural:
The audience were enjoying the play.
The choice depends on whether the group is being considered as a single undivided body,
or as a collection of individuals. The plural is more popular in speech, whereas in writing
the singular is preferred. AmE treats collective nouns as singular.
ii/ Coordination- syndetic vs. asyndetic
-When a S consists of two or more NPs:
Tom and Jerry are ready to begin the fight.
6
-In the case of quasi-coordination, when NPs are linked by prepositions, grammatical concord
will decide:
The captain as well as the other players, was tired.
2. Distributive number
It is used in a plural NP to refer to a set of entities matched individually with individual
entities in another set:
Have you all brought your cameras? /Each has a camera/
7
With idioms and metaphors the singular is obligatory or preferable:
We’re keeping an open mind.
3. Pronoun reference
Concord of number/person/gender is necessary between S and O or C if the second element is
a reflexive pronoun:
She bought herself a raincoat.
Relative pronouns agree with their antecedent in the main clause in gender:
That’s the man whom I want to talk to.
Here is the hammer which I want to use.
2.
SENTENCE TYPES AND DISCOURSE FUNCTIONS
FORMAL CLASSIFICATION
Simple sentences may be divided into 4 major syntactic types differentiated by their form.
Their use correlates largely with different discourse functions.
1. DECLARATIVES are sentences in which the Subject is present and generally precedes the
Verb.
3. IMPERATIVES are sentences which normally have no overt grammatical Subject and
SEMANTIC CLASSIFICATION
Associated with these four sentence types are four classes of discourse functions.
1. STATEMENTS are primarily used to convey information,
2. QUESTIONS are primarily used to seek information on a specific point,
8
3. DIRECTIVES are used to instruct somebody to do something,
4. EXCLAMATIONS are used for expressing the extent to which the speaker is impressed by
something.
Direct association between syntactic class and semantic class is the norm, but the two do not
always match:
Isn’t she clever! – interrogative exclamation
You gave him a WATCH? – declarative question
PRAGMATIC CATEGORIES
If we make more refined distinctions, then a statement can be used to make an
They’re pragmatic categories that indicate how the semantic classes of sentences are used in
actual utterances. Utterances of sentences are SPEECH ACTS, that is acts of verbal behaviour
(spoken or written).
When a person performs a speech act, he utters a locutionary act. We use ‘illocutionary act’ to
refer to a speech act identified with reference to the communication intention of the hearer.
Occasionally the speaker explicitly refers to the illocutionary act being performed by using a
performative verb.
Illocutionary acts are typically associated with particular semantic classes of sentences: e.g.
9
Statements are related to a very large range of illocutionary acts.
Semantic and pragmatic classes are not always directly associated. Sentences from one
semantic class are very often used to express an illocutionary act typically associated with
The illocutionary force of an utterance is dependent on the context and a particular utterance
promise
warning
10
NEGATION
We distinguish 3 types of negation:
A/ CLAUSE N: through which the whole clause is syntactically treated as Negation.
B/ LOCAL N: in which one constituent is negated
C/ PREDICATION N: a minor type applying only after certain aux-ies, in which the
predication is negated
A/ Clause N through verb N
i/ A simple positive sentence or a positive finite clause within a complex sent. is negated by
inserting the clause negator not between the operator used and the predication.
I’ve not finished. /They are not playing. /He may not be working.
The operator is the 1st aux. V. of a complex VP or either BE, or stative HAVE as the V in a
simple VP.
They are not noisy./He has not enough money. /BrE
ii/ If an operator is not present in the positive sentence the substitute aux. DO is introduced.
She doesn’t sleep well. /I did not pay for it.
If the operator can be contracted to a form enclitic to the subject, there are 2 possibilities for
contraction in negative clause
Negator contraction Aux. contraction
We aren’t ready. We’re not ready.
A/ Syntactic features of CLAUSE N
11
Clausal N may be accomplished by negating a clause element other than the V or by using a
negative word such as none or never
Verb N N of other element
He is not a friend of yours. He is no friend of yours.
An honest man would not lie. No honest man would lie.
I won’t make that mistake. I’ll never make that mistake.
Many people didn’t come to the party. – implies the absence of many people.
Not many people came to the party. – implies the presence of few people
In formal style, the negative element may be moved out of its usual position to the initial (!)
position, in which case Subject-Operator inversion is often required.
Not a word did he say. clausal N as shown by their
Not one bottle did we leave behind. required positive tags.
No longer are they staying.
Under no circumstances will she return here.
They also require non-assertives.
Neither of us has ever had a university education.
Negation with no may have different implications than Verb N with not .
He is not a teacher – denotes his occupation is not teaching
He is no teacher – indicates that he lacks the skills needed for teaching
12
Syntactic class Assertive Non-assertive Negation
det some any no
pronoun some any none
process adverb some how in any way in no way
place adverb Somewhere Anywhere nowhere
time adverb Sometime(s) ever never
The combination of not with a nonassertive form can be replaced by the negative, there are
consequently 2 negative equivalents of each positive sentence:
We’ve had some lunch. (specific) ~We haven’t had any lunch. (more colloquial
and idiomatic)
We have had no lunch. (non-specific)
Nonassertive contexts
Non-assertive items appear in a number of other contexts.
i/ Yes-no questions that expect a negative response or are neutral in expectation:
Do you know any of the teachers here?
ii/ wh-questions:
Who has ever read the play?
iii/ putative should-clauses:
It’s odd that he should ever notice it.
iv/ conditional clause:
If anyone ever says that, pretend not to hear.
v/ comparative clauses:
I’ve more stamps than I’ve yet shown to you.
vi/ restrictive relative clauses modifying generic NP-s, where the clauses have conditional
meaning:
Students who have any complaints should raise their hands.
vii/ after words that are morphologically negative or that have negative import:
It’s unlikely that she has ever been to Scotland.
They can prevent any demonstration. /It’s hard to do any work under these conditions.
Negative intensification
There are various ways of giving emotive intensification to a negative:
by any means / in the slightest / in the least / in any way/ a bit (¬informal)
Negative determiners and pronouns are given emphasis by: at all/ whatever/whatsoever:
13
You have no excuse whatever.
I don’t care a damn whether … (¬familiar)
No way will I accept such an offer (¬familiar)
Some expressions are formed in combination with specific V-s:
I didn’t sleep a wink. He won’t lift a finger to help you.
I won’t drink a drop. They won’t budge an inch.
He didn’t move a muscle. We didn’t see a soul.
Scope of negation
the stretch of language over which the negative item has a semantic influence.
A negative item may be said to govern /or determine the occurrence of/ a non-assertive only if
the latter is within the scope of the negative.
It normally extends from the negative item itself to the end of the clause:
She definitely didn’t speak to him. ~ it’s def. that she didn’t
She didn’t definitely speak to him. ~ it’s not definite that she did.
Focus of negation information focus
a special or nuclear stress falling on a particular part of the clause:
B/ Local N
it negates a word or phrase, without making the clause negative
He’s not a too sympathetic doctor. = he’s a rather unsympathetic doctor
The double negative phrases require a gradable adj. or adv as head, the negation indicating a
point between the two extremes of the gradable scale.
In local negation, an initial negative adverbial does not cause S –op inversion:
In no time we cleared the table.
within a short time
Negation of modals
The scope of negation may or may not include the meaning of the modal aux-s. We therefore
distinguish between AUX. N& MAIN VERB N
14
Aux ® You may not smoke in here. ~ you’re not allowed
Main V ® They may not like the party! it’s possible that they don’t
Aux Main
You can’t be serious. (not possible) You shan’t lose your reward.
You can’t go. (not allowed)
He can’t ride. (not able to)
- the aux. negation of must in the logical necessity sense
a/ is usually achieved through can’t:
They must be telling lies. ® can’t be
b/ A less emphatic negation in the same sense may be achieved with needn’t and don’t have
to.
It must be hot there. It needn’t/doesn’t have to be hot.
= The aux. negation of must in the obligation sense is needn’t / don’t have to
- Must we pack?
- No, we don’t have to / needn’t pack till tomorrow.
15
They won’t have arrived yet. it’s not probable
I predict they haven’t
C/ Predication N
It occurs in the context of denials and permission very rarely. In pred. N a modal aux. is used
with a different scope of negation than is normal for that aux. Special emphatic pause before
not.
They may ‘not go swimming. ~ they’re allowed not to go.
Pred. N differs from local N in that it can extend over several clause elements beginning with
the main verb:
You could ‘not attend any of the meetings. ~ it’s possible for you not to attend.
Double negation
Two negatives occasionally occur in the same clause:
Not many people have nowhere to live. ~ most people have somewhere to live.
Not all imperatives have no subject. ~ some have
None of us have never told lies. ~ all of us have sometime
QUESTIONS
Questions can be divided into 3 major classes according to the type of reply they expect.
-Those that expect affirmation or negation: Have you finished the book?
-Those that expect a reply from an open range of replies: How old are you?
-Those that expect as the reply one of two or more options: Would you like to go or stay?
It is possible for a question to be answered by another question:
Is that your baby? - What do you think?
Tag questions are a further type of yes/no questions which convey positive or negative
orientation. The general rules for forming the most common types of tags are:
a/ The tag question consists of an operator and the subject.
b/ The operator is generally the same as the operator of the preceding statement:
I haven’t seen you, have I?
c/ The subject of the tag must be a pronoun which either repeats or is in co-reference with the
Subject of the statement agreeing with it in number /person/gender.
d/ If the statement is positive, the tag is generally negative and vice versa.
16
e/ The nuclear tone of the tag occurs on the auxiliary and is either rising or falling.
The meaning of such sentences involve a statement and a question. Each of them asserts
something, then invites the listener’s response to it.
Assumption is expressed by the statement/ Expectation is expressed by the tag.
i/ positive A plus neutral E – rising tone
ii/ negative A plus neutral E – rising tone
iii/ positive A plus positive E – falling tone
iv/ negative A plus negative E – falling tone
The tag with rising tone invites verification expecting the hearer to decide the truth of the
proposition in the statement. The falling tone invites conformation of the statement, has the
force of an exclamation rather than a question.
- There is a further type of tag question in which both statement and question are positive:
Your car is outside, is it?
The tag always has a rising nucleus and the situation is characteristically preceded, indicating
the speaker’s arrival at a conclusion by inference or by recalling what has already been said.
Declarative questions
Not all yes-no questions have Subject – Operator inversion. The declarative question is a type
of question which is identical in form to a declarative except for the final rising intonation:
You realize what the RISKS are?
They’ve spoken to the amBASSADOR, of course?
Declarative questions are conducive and resemble tag questions with a rising tone in that they
invite the hearer’s verification.
Tag questions with imperatives and exclamatives
i/ invite the listener’s consent. For positive imperatives 3 types are available:
Open the DOOR, WON’T you? – least insistent
Open the DOOR, WÔN’T you?
Open the DOOR, WILL you? – most insistent
Other auxiliaries and subjects also occur:
Open the door, can’t you? Hand me a knife, won’t somebody?
Negative imperatives are less commonly followed by tag questions. The only type that seems
possible is ‘will you’ with a falling tone.
Don’t make a NOISE, W Ì LL you?
The tag is a persuasive softener of the imperative:
Let’s play another game, shall we?
17
ii/With exclamatives the tag questions invite the hearer’s agreement:
How THIN she is, isn’t she?
-YES/NO QUESTIONS with modal auxiliaries are subject to certain limitations and shifts of
meaning.
The modals of permission MAY (BrE)
CAN
~ of obligation MUST (BrE) generally involve the
speaker’s authority in statements and
the hearer’s authority in questions:
May I leave now? - Will you permit me?
Yes, you may. - I will permit you.
A similar switch from hearer to speaker takes place with SHALL (volition) which involves the
speaker’s will in statements, but the hearer’s will in questions:
You shall suffer for this! – I intend to make you suffer.
Shall I switch off the tv? - Do you want me to…?
MAY in the possibility sense is not often used in questions, CAN/COULD replace it:
Can they have missed the bus? Yes, they may have.
In yes/no questions the past tense forms are regularly used for politeness. If modal auxiliaries
are retained the present forms are generally submitted in responses:
Might I call you by your fist name? Yes, you may.
Would you pay for me? Yes, I will.
NEED is used as a nonassertive modal auxiliary but the corresponding positive forms are
MUST / HAVE TO / main verb NEED:
Need they leave now?
Do they need to leave now? must.
have to Yes, they have to.
need to.
MUST in the necessity sense has positive orientation:
Must it always happen this way?
-Wh-QUESTIONS are formed with the aid of one of the following single interrogative
words. Who/whom/whose/what/which/when/where/how/why generally have falling intonation.
As a result:
i/ the wh-element comes 1st in the sentence apart from some conjuncts: on the other hand
18
ii/ the wh-word itself takes 1st position
(!) The only exception to the 2nd principle occurs when the wh-word is within a prepositional
complement in formal style:
On what did you base your prediction?
Functions of wh-element
The wh-element operates in various clause functions:
Who ever opened my letter? – S
Which books have you lent him? - Od
Whose beautiful photos are these? - Cs
How wide did they make the bookcase? - Co
When will you be promoted? – Adverbial of time
Why are they always complaining? – Adverbial of manner
Subject – operator inversion is the same in its application to wh-questions as in its application
to yes-no questions. DO / BE / HAVE can act as operator.
Positive wh –questions
A wh-question may generally be matched with a statement called its presupposition. This is a
statement which, in place of the wh-element contains an indefinite expression and is
somebody.
PRESUPPOSITION
Someone opened my letter.
You visit NY sometimes.
Modifying WHAT/WHICH have different presuppositions:
What composers do you like best? You like some … best.
Which composers do you like best? You like some of the composers best.
In some cases there is no sensible presupposition:
What time is it? – It’s some time or other.
How should I know?
Where were you born? – somewhere
Negative wh-questions Presupposition
Who hasn’t had any coffee? Somebody hasn’t had.
Why didn’t you tell me? You didn’t tell me for some reason.
Questions beginning with WHY DON’T YOU/ WHY NOT are commonly used as directives,
invitations, suggestions, instructions:
Why don’t you come for a meal?
19
Why not go by car? Why don’t you take sleeping tablets?
Anyone else would. It conveys advice, frequently has a critical and irritable tone. It can be
used as an inquiry: Why don’t you ever write?
More than one wh-element
If one of the elements is Subject, it must be initial:
Who said what to whom?
How and why did it happen? – coordinated
If one element is an adverbial and the other is Od only appended coordination is fully
acceptable: * What and where does she teach?
What does she teach and where?
-ALTERNATIVE QUESTIONS
One type resembles a yes-no, the other a wh-question:
Would you like chocolate or lemon ice cream?
Which ice cream would you like? Chocolate or lemon?
In the 1st case the difference is only in intonation. A rise occurs on each item in the list, except
the last on which there is a fall indicating that the list is complete.
Alternative ® Shall we go by BUS or TRAIN?
Yes-no ® Shall we go by car or train? – Let’s take the car.
An alternative question presupposes the truth of only one of the propositions:
Are you a democrat or a republican?
MINOR TYPES
-Exclamatory questions are interrogative in structure but have the illocutionary force of an
exclamatory assertion: Hasn’t she grown!
-Rhetorical questions are interrogative in structure but have the force of a strong assertion
generally not expecting an answer.
POSITIVE: Is that a reason for despair? - Surely not.
NEGATIVE: Isn’t the answer obvious? - Surely it is.
There are rhetorical wh-questions, the positive question is equivalent to a statement in which
the wh-element is replaced by a negative element.
Who knows? - Nobody knows.
What should I say? - There’s nothing that I should say.
20
DIRECTIVES
Directives typically take the form of an imperative sentence,
i/ it generally has no subject
ii/ it has either a main verb in the base form or an aux. followed by the appropriate form of the
main verb. Otherwise, the clause patterns of imperative sentences show the same range and
ordering of elements as declaratives:
Jump. (S)V
Open the door. (S)VO
Be reasonable. (S)VC
Get inside. (S)VA
Tell me the truth. (S)VOO
Consider yourself lucky. (S)VOC
Put the flowers on the table. (S)VOA
The imperative V lacks tense distinction and does not allow modal aux-ies. Progressive form
is rare; perfective even rarer.
Passives with be occur chiefly in negative directives where they generally have the meaning.
Don’t allow yourself to:
Don’t be deceived by his looks.
Less common in positive directives:
Be guided by what I say.
What might be treated as passives occur with get.
Get dressed. /Get lost. – informal ~ go away.
Directives with Subjects
It’s intuitively clear that the meaning of a directive implies that the omitted S is the 2 nd person
pronoun ‘you’. Be quiet, will you?
There is, however a type of directive in which the stressed S you is added, expressing strong
irritation:
You be quiet!/ You take the book!
Contrastive – I know you can do it. You show me what you can do.
Vocative – You come here. – very impolite
Directives with let
1st person imperatives can be formed by preposing the V let followed by a S in the objective
case.
Let me think. Let’s go.
21
But – Let no one think that a teacher’s life is easy. ® archaic
Summary: i/ Open the door. -without S 2nd person (*by far the most common
type)
ii/ You open the door. -with S without let
iii/ Someone open the door. -with S without 3rd
iv/ Let me open. -with S with let
v/ Let someone open. -with S with let 3rd
Negative imperatives
To negate the first 3 classes, one simply adds an initial Don’t or Do not replacing assertive by
nonassertive items.
Don’t open. Don’t you open – you don’t open/ less common
1st person imperatives are generally negated by the insertion of not.
Let’s not say anything about it.
Informally the negation with Don’t is frequently heard.
Don’t let’s say anything. /BrE
Let’s don’t say anything. /AmE
3rd person imperatives are by not after let or by initial don’t more informally.
Let not anyone fool himself. / Don’t let anyone fool himself.
3.
PRO- FORMS & ELLIPSIS
23
Pro-forms such as personal pronouns have very unspecific meanings. Therefore, for the
interpretation of these words it’s necessary to have info from which we can uniquely predict
their intended referents.
Essentially the same distinctions apply to ellipsis. Words are omitted where understanding can
be achieved without word repetition.
If you want [me to (buy the tickets)], I’ll buy the tickets.
A host may invite a guest to eat a sandwich:
‘Like one?’ elliptical variant of ‘Would you like one?’
Pro-form substitution
The bond between a pro-form and its antecedent may be of two different kinds.
COREFERENCE SUBSTITUTION
Means the bond of cross-reference is a relation between pro-form and
between two items which refer to the antecedent whereby the pro-form
same thing. ‘replaces’ a repeated occurrence of the
Two players injured themselves during antecedent.
the match. Bill got a first prize this year, and I got
one last year.
24
The indefinite or quantifier pronouns /some, any, few, enough, each/ can all act as substitutes
for NP-s introduced by the corresponding determiners.
When the children entered, each was given a present.
ii/ None is the only quantifier pronoun whose form is different from that of its corresponding
determiner no.
iii/ Same The soup smells delicious and the turkey smells the same.
TYPES OF ELLIPSIS
COORDINATION
Coordination and subordination involve the linking of units of the same rank. However,
coordination involves constituents at the same level of constituent structure, whereas in
subordination they form a hierarchy.
There are special cases of 2 types of syntactic arrangement:
PARATAXIS: equal arrangement (can be an appended clause)
HYPOTAXIS: underneath arrangement or tag.
26
* his first and best novel: coordinate Adj. Phrase functions as a premodifier of novel.
* his first successful novel
doesn’t modify novel directly thus there is a hierarchy
Syndetic & asyndetic coordination
S= is marked by overt signals of coordinating: and / or / but and linking words /coordinating
conjunctions/ coordinators
A= is not overtly marked; used for dramatic intensification
The uses of or
It introduces an alternative. It conveys the meaning that if one of the individual conjoins is
true then the whole sentence is true. But there is the pragmatic requirement that the contents
of the 2 clauses should have sufficient in common to justify their juxtaposition as alternatives.
1. Typically, or is EXCLUSIVE:
You can sleep on the floor or you can go to a hotel.
2. There can occur an INCLUSIVE interpretation of or, where it is implied that both conjoins
may be true:
You can boil an egg, (or) you can make sandwiches, or you can do both.
3. The alternative expressed by or may also be a restatement or CORRECTIVE to what is
said in the 1st conjoin:
They are happy, or at least they appear to be so.
4. It may imply NEGATIVE CONDITION:
Give me some money or (else) I’ll shoot you. ~ If you don’t…
28
The use of but
It expresses a contrast which could usually be alternatively expressed by and followed by yet:
John is poor, but he is happy. ~ … and yet
The sentence implies that his happiness is unexpected in view of his poverty.
Correlatives
To reinforce or clarify the conjoining function of and it’s possible to place the word both in
front of the 1st conjoin:
He’s met (both) her mother and her father.
Similarly, either can be placed in front of the 1 st conjoin to reinforce or, and there’s a further
correlative pair neither …nor.
He’s met (either) her mother or her father.
C. of clauses
C. of complete independent clauses: [Who are you] and [where do you live]?
Dependent clauses may also be coordinated, so long as they belong to the same function class.
a/ Coordinate adverbial clause:
If you pass the exam and (if) no one else supplies, you’ll get the job.
b/ Coordinate nominal THAT-clause:
I believe that you’re right and (that) you’ll always be.
29
c/ Coordinate nominal WH-clause:
I didn’t know who she was or what she wanted.
d/ Coordinate relative clause:
Someone who knows the area, but whose home is outside,
is more likely to be a successful representative.
e/ Coordinate TO-INF clause:
I’ve asked her to phone on Monday or come on Thursday.
f/ Coordinate –ING-PART clause:
She’s fond of working at night and getting up late in the morning.
g/ Coordinate –ED PART clause:
They helped to rebuild the houses damaged by the storm or washed away by the flood.
Coordinated N heads
When coordinated heads are preceded by a determiner, the normal interpretation is that the
determiner applies to each of the conjoins:
a knife and fork ~ a knife and a fork
Coordinated premodifiers
He sells old and valuable books. – Adj-s
combinatory meaning segregatory meaning (possible)
But: red, white, and green flags
Coordinated postmodifiers afford the possibility of either a segregatory or a combinatory
interpretation of and:
The bus for the Houses of P. and (for) the British M. - combinatory
Buses for the Houses of P. and (for) the British M. - ambiguity
If a phrase containing modifiers coordinated by and has a singular count noun head, only one
interpretation, the combinatory one is possible. But if it has a plural head, there are 4 possible
segregatory interpretations for a NP containing 2 conjoins:
the meetings on Monday and Tuesday: 1M+1T
2M+1T
1M+2T
2M+2T
4.
THE COMPLEX SENTENCE
One of the two major devices for linking clauses within the same sentence is coordination.
The other is subordination to make complex sentences. A complex sentence has one or more
SUBORDINATE/ DEPENDENT clauses functioning as an element of the sentences.
Subordination is an asymmetrical relation. The device of subordination enables us to construct
a multiple hierarchy of clauses, one within the other:
31
S.– Although I admire her reasoning, I reject her conclusions.
prep– Despite my admiration for… – nominalization
both function as Adverbials within their sentence.
A clause that is not subordinate to another one is INDEPENDENT.
32
Verbless clauses take syntactic compression one stage further than nonfinite clauses. (it’s
often possible to postulate a missing form of BE and to recover the S.):
Whether (he is) right or wrong, he always shouts.
Sometimes they are reductions of nonfinite clauses:
(Being) Too nervous to reply, he stared.
The verbless clause is limited to the clause-types: SVC and SVA.
When ripe, these apples will be delicious.
While at College, he was a member of …
33
3. Subject-operator inversion is a marker of Subordination in conditional and comparative
clauses –had/ should/ were:
Were she here, she’d support the motion.
Unusual syntactic ordering *temporal, conditional, concessive
4. VP in subordinate clauses
Simple present in Adverbial and Nominal cl-s to express future meaning:
When/ after / if / as soon as he arrives, the band will play.
Clauses of similarity and proportion may also have simple present:
Next time I’ll do as he says.
After hope / bet / see to it/ take care
Suppose he loses his way.
5.
ADVERBIAL CLAUSES
function mainly as adjuncts or disjuncts.
Conjuncts - stereotyped: What is more…/What is most worrying…
to begin with / to cap it all / to continue / to sum up
Subjuncts - viewpoint
As far as the economy is concerned the next month is critical.
Looking at it objectively
Viewed objectively he is at fault.
Adjuncts & disjuncts; semantic differences
A-s denote circumstances of the situation in the main clause.
D-s comment on the style or form of what is said or on its content
ADJUNCT Cl. CONTENT DISJUNCT Cl.
Temporal Since Reason Since
I’ve read since they left. He took his coat, since it was cold.
Temporal While Concessive While
37
He looked after my dog while I was out. He lives in Leeds, while his wife in
Durham.
Purpose So that Result So that
They took a plane so that they could get We know her well, so that we can speak to
there on time. her.
Conditional If/unless
If you ask them politely, they’ll help you. I’ll get lost unless I find my compass.
Temporal As Reason As
They were stopped as they were about to I went home as I wanted to rest.
enter.
38
The meaning of several subordinators may be neutralized in certain contexts to convey a more
abstract notion of recurrent or habitual contingency - when (ever) / if
They may then be paraphrased by such prep-al phrases as ‘in cases when’ or ‘in circumstances
where’
Whenever necessary, send me…
Whenever in doubt, see me.
If possible, you should…
3. Clauses of place
introduced by where(ever)
specific non-specific
Clause may indicate position or direction.
They went wherever they could find work.
Several temporal subordinators may have primarily a place meaning.
in descriptions of scenes, when those are described dynamically in terms of movement from
one place to another
The building becomes narrower as it rises higher.
The river continues winding until it reaches a large lake.
39
5. Clauses of exception
but that / except that / save that /
I’d pay you now, except I don’t have any money on me.
6. Reason clauses
- cause & effect
He’s thin because he doesn’t eat enough.
- reason & consequence
Since she’s my friend, she must have agreed with me.
- motivation & result
You’ll help me because you’re my friend.
- circumstances & consequence
Since the weather has improved, the game will be held.
Since you seem to know them, why don’t you introduce me to them? indirect reason
7. Clauses of purpose -inf/in order to/ so as to:
I left early to catch the train.
They left the door open in order for me to hear the baby.
Students should take notes so as to make revision easier.
8. Clauses of result - So that ~ formal / So
We paid him so that he left contented. /reason
We paid him so that he would leave contented./ purpose
9. Clauses of similarity and comparison -as/like introduce them
He looks as if he’s getting better.
10. Clauses of proportion involve a hint of comparison as... so / the … the
As he grew disheartened, so his work deteriorated.
The harder he worked, the happier he felt.
11. Clauses of preference - rather than / sooner than
Rather than go there by air, I’d take a train.
12. Comment clauses are parenthetical disjuncts: as you probably know/I believe/ to be
honest
The comment cl-s may have various semantic functions:
- Speaker’s tentativeness: I guess / presume / suppose
- Speaker’s certainty: I’m convinced/ I admit/ there’s no doubt
- Speaker’s emotional attitude towards the content: I’m pleased to say / it grieves me to..
To claim the hearer’s attention: you know / mind you / can’t you see?
40
13. Sentential relative clauses refer back to a whole clause or sent
After that things improved, which surprised me.
the antecedent is the whole of the event described in the main cl. They are introduced by the
rel. word ‘which’ and are closely parallel to non-restrictive postmodifying clauses in NP-s.
Frequently it is not a pronoun but a determiner preceding an abstract noun such as fact/ case,
etc. which could function in apposition to a clause
It may also occur is prep-al compl. ® as a result of which/ instead of which
14. Comparative clauses
Two propositions are compared one in the main is compared with proposition in the sub with
respect to something they have in common/ some standard of comparison.
His name is the same as his father’s (is).
Comparison concerns a property measurable in terms of degree and by means of comparative
items -er/ none / less / more together with the correlative clause introduces than.
ambiguity
He loves the dog more than his wife.
her ® S, O informal
she ® S formal
Single-, two-, and three-variable comparisons
More than one cl. element can also contrast
C1 C2 C1 C2
James | knows | more | about films | than | Sue | does | about music.
You’re getting slimmer (than you were).
Six functions of the comparative item more
(i) As quantifier: Jack has more girlfriends than his brother (has).
(ii) As head of NP: More were at home than abroad.
(iii) As adjunct: I agree with you more than (I agree with) Robert.
(iv) As modifier of adj.:
It was a more lively discussion than I expected.
(v) As modifier of adj. head:
His speech was more interesting than I expected.
(vi) As modifier of adverb:
The time passed more quickly than last year.
As … as
Comparison based on equivalence / excess / sufficiency
as … as such … that enough so … that too
Jack has as many girl-friends as his brother.
42
I agree with you as much as (with) Robert.
Too has negative force: She’s too old to do any work.
He’s too good a man to kill.
43