VickeryR PDF
VickeryR PDF
VickeryR PDF
Retraining on Performance in
Competitive Cyclists
Rachel Vickery
October 2007
i
3.3. Experimental Procedures ........................................................................................... 66
3.3.1. General Protocol ............................................................................................ 66
3.3.2. Spirometry...................................................................................................... 68
3.3.3. Incremental Power Step Test ......................................................................... 70
3.3.4. Nijmegen Questionnaire................................................................................. 71
3.3.5. 20km Time Trial ............................................................................................. 72
3.3.6. Intervention Sessions - Randomisation .......................................................... 73
3.3.7. Intervention Sessions - Intervention Group .................................................... 73
3.3.8. Intervention Sessions - Control Group ........................................................... 74
3.4. Data Analysis ............................................................................................................. 75
3.4.1. Pmax determination.......................................................................................... 75
3.4.2. Lactate ........................................................................................................... 75
3.4.3. Sub-maximal and maximal VO2...................................................................... 75
3.4.4. Nijmegen Questionnaire................................................................................. 76
3.4.5. Statistics......................................................................................................... 77
Chapter Four: Results.................................................................................................................. 78
4.1. Participant Characteristics.......................................................................................... 78
4.2. Effects on cycling performance (Incremental power and 20km-TT performance)....... 79
4.3. Effect on Physiological Measures............................................................................... 79
4.4. Effect on Respiratory Measures ................................................................................. 80
4.4.1. Comparison of measures at absolute workloads pre and post-BPR .............. 81
4.5. Changes in RPE measures ........................................................................................ 84
4.5.1. RPE................................................................................................................ 84
4.5.2. Reason for terminating exercise..................................................................... 85
4.5.3. Correlations between RPEbreathing, RPElegs, fb and performance changes....... 85
Chapter Five: Discussion............................................................................................................. 86
5.1. Effect of BPR on cycling performance ........................................................................ 87
5.1.1. Effects on 20km-TT performance................................................................... 87
5.1.2. Effects on Incremental Peak Power (Pmax) ..................................................... 90
5.2. Effects of BPR on Respiratory Measures ................................................................... 91
5.2.1. Breathing frequency (fb) and Tidal Volume (VT) ............................................. 92
5.2.2. Inspiratory and Expiratory Time ..................................................................... 93
5.2.3. Minute Ventilation........................................................................................... 94
5.2.4. Inspiratory Drive and Timing ........................................................................ 100
5.3. Effects of BPR on Perceived Effort........................................................................... 101
5.3.1. Perceived Breathing Effort ........................................................................... 101
5.3.2. Perceived Leg Effort..................................................................................... 104
5.4. Effects of BPR on Metabolic Measures .................................................................... 105
5.4.1. Lactate, Power and Heart Rate – LT and LTP ............................................. 105
5.4.2. Measures of Oxygen Uptake - sub-maximal (economy) and VO2peak ........... 106
5.5. Limitations ................................................................................................................ 108
5.6. Future Directions ...................................................................................................... 110
5.7. Conclusion............................................................................................................... 111
Appendices................................................................................................................................. 112
References ................................................................................................................................. 123
ii
List of Tables
Table 1. Muscles and Structures of Respiration (From Chaitow et al., 2002) ................................ 9
Table 2. Effect of Breathing Rate and Depth on Alveolar Ventilation in Three Scenarios (from
Marieb, 1992) ................................................................................................................ 15
Table 4. A summary of Exercise Performance Studies using Inspiratory Muscle Training (IMT). 46
Table 7. Base Line Performance and Physiological Characteristics for Intervention and Control
Groups*......................................................................................................................... 78
Table 8. Mean Changes in Performance Measures post Breathing Pattern Retraining and Control
and Chances That the True Difference in the Changes Is Substantial .......................... 79
Table 9. Mean changes in Physiological Measures post Breathing Pattern Retraining and Control
and Chances That the True Difference in the Changes is Substantial .......................... 80
Table 10. Mean Changes in Respiratory Measures (non-exercise and exercise) post Breathing
Pattern Retraining and Control and Chances That the True Difference in the Changes Is
Substantial .................................................................................................................... 81
Table 11. Mean Changes in Rate of Perceived Exertion Post Breathing Pattern Retraining and
Control and Chances That the True Difference in the Change is Substantial ............... 85
iii
List of Figures
Figure 1. Ventilatory response of the elite (E) and professional (P) cyclists during the tests:
pulmonary ventilation (VE), breathing frequency (fb) and) tidal volume (VT). Max Maximal
power output. * P < 0.05 ** P < 0.01, § significant (P < 0.05) interactive effect (group x
exercise intensity) (From Lucia et al., 1999).................................................................. 17
Figure 2. Inspiratory (tI) and expiratory (tE) times. Definitions as for Figure 2.1. * P < 0.05, ** P <
0.01, § significant (P < 0.05) interactive effect (group x exercise intensity. (From Lucia et
al., 1999). ...................................................................................................................... 17
Figure 3. A typical athlete’s cycling position during a road time-trial (reprinted with the permission
of Stephen Sheldrake, NZ Triathlete) ............................................................................ 22
Figure 5. Time line (weeks) of familiarisation trials (F), formal incremental step test (ST) and
20km time trial (TT) and intervention period (I). ............................................................ 67
Figure 6. Effect of breathing pattern retraining on breathing frequency, tidal volume and minute
ventilation in the intervention (left panel) and control groups (right panel). ................... 82
Figure 7. Effect of Breathing pattern retraining on inspiratory and expiratory time in the
intervention (left panel) and control (right panel) groups. .............................................. 83
Figure 8. Effect of breathing pattern retraining on inspiratory drive (VT/tI) and inspiratory timing
(tI/tTOT ) in the intervention (left panel) and control (right panel) groups. ........................ 82
Figure 9. Effect of breathing pattern retraining on blood lactate in the intervention (left) and
control (right) groups. .................................................................................................... 82
Figure 10. Effect of breathing pattern retraining on VO2 and VCO2 in the intervention (left panel)
and control (right panel) groups. ................................................................................... 84
iv
List of Appendices
v
Attestation of Authorship
I hereby declare that this submission is my own work and that, to the best of my knowledge and
belief, it contains no material previously published or written by another person nor material which
to a substantial extent has been accepted for the qualification of any degree or diploma of a
university or other institution of higher learning, except where due acknowledgement is made in
the acknowledgements.
Signed: _______________________________
Date: __________/__________/_________
vi
Dedication
This thesis is dedicated in the first instance to Neil Dearberg, one of life’s true gentlemen,
I also dedicate this thesis to my parents, Evan and Heather Vickery, who are not only
incredible parents, but are amazing, inspirational people. I am proud to be your daughter.
vii
Acknowledgements
My primary supervisor, Dr Andrew Kilding. For taking on a physio with no knowledge of exercise
physiology – what were you thinking! For your guidance, teaching, critiquing and encouragement
to always find that next level – thank you.
My secondary supervisor, (soon to be Dr) David Nicholls. For never being afraid to say “I told you
so”. For always being there to help, advise, inspire. For encouraging me to soar, or to reign me
in when I needed it. For your years of friendship – thank you.
To Judith Paterson. Thank you for your friendship and incredible generosity in providing me a
home away from home where I have always been welcome. This past 10 months would not have
been possible without you.
To Quinton and Vicky Swanson, and Jamie Singe at Contact Self Defence. For providing the
physical outlet to balance this mental challenge! For making me part of the family and for your
friendship. You will never truly know what a difference you have made to my year.
To the post-grad students, especially Joe McQuillan, for taking me under their wing and showing
me the physiology ropes.
To all the participants in the study. What an amazing bunch of guys and such commitment! It
wouldn’t have happened without you all.
To the Auckland University of Technology Ethics Committee for granting ethical approval – 21st
December 2006; 06/207.
To Zephyrtech (Auckland, NZ) for providing the breathing harness used with the control group.
viii
Intellectual Property Rights
This thesis may be utilised for the purpose of research or private study provided that due
acknowledgement is made where appropriate and that the author’s written permission is obtained
I agree that the AUT University library may copy this thesis, in electronic or photocopied form, to
supply the collection of other libraries on request from those libraries. I also agree that this thesis
may be photocopied for supply to any person in accordance with the provision of Section 56 of the
ix
Abstract
Background: The increased work of breathing associated with intense cycling has been identified
as a factor that may negatively affect cycling performance. The aerodynamic position, abnormal
respiratory mechanics either at rest or during exercise, and the development of a tachypnoeic
breathing pattern are factors known to increase the work of breathing. Breathing pattern
retraining aims to decrease the work of breathing by delaying the onset of dynamic hyperinflation
and the recruitment of accessory breathing muscles. To date no studies have investigated the
performance, physiological and perceptual consequences of manipulating breathing pattern in
trained cyclists. Purpose: The aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of breathing
pattern retraining on 20-km time trial performance and respiratory and metabolic measures in
competitive cyclists. Method: Twenty-four competitive male cyclists (age 37.7 ± 8.6 years, mean
± SD; V&O 2 peak 4.34 ± 0.47 Lmin-1) were match paired on 20-km time trial performance and
assigned at random to either an intervention group (breathing pattern retraining; N = 12) or control
group (N = 12). 20-km time trial performance, pulmonary function and the physiological and
perceptual response during a maximal incremental cycle step test were assessed pre- and post-
intervention. The intervention group underwent four weeks of specific breathing pattern retraining
using exercises designed to reduce dynamic hyperinflation and optimise respiratory mechanics.
The control group attended the laboratory once a week during this period and performed a 10
minute sub-maximal ride wearing a biofeedback breathing harness. The control group was led to
believe the purpose for their participation was to investigate the effect that maximal exercise had
on breathing pattern, and to test the reliability of the breathing harness. There was no attempt to
modify the breathing pattern of the control group. Data were analysed using an MS Excel
spreadsheet designed for statistical analysis. The uncertainty in the effect was expressed as 90%
confidence limits and a smallest worthwhile effect of 1.0% was assumed. Results: The
intervention group showed substantial improvements in 20-km time trial performance (-1.5 ±
1.1%) and incremental power (3.2 ± 3%). Additionally, breathing frequency (-13.2 ± 8.9%; -9.5 ±
8.4%), tidal volume (10.6 ± 8.5%; 9.4 ± 7.6%), inspiratory time (10.1 ± 8%; 9.4 ± 7.7%), breathing
RPE (-30 ± 33.9%; -24.7 ± 28.1%) and leg RPE (-27.9 ± 38.5%; -24.7 ± 28.2%) were all
positively affected at lactate threshold and lactate turn point. No positive changes were observed
in the control group for 20-km time trial performance (0.0 ± 1.0%), incremental power (-1.4 ±
3.5%), breathing frequency (-1.6 ± 8.0%; -2.0 ± 7.9%), tidal volume (0.9 ± 7.2%; 2.9 ± 9.4%),
breathing RPE (16.1 ± 50.2%, 24.8 ± 43%) or leg RPE (13.4 ± 39.6%; 19.9 ± 43.2%) .
Conclusion: These results provide evidence of the performance enhancing effect of four weeks
of breathing pattern retraining in cyclists. Furthermore, they suggest breathing pattern can be
retrained to exhibit a controlled pattern, without a tachypnoeic shift, during high intensity cycling.
Additionally, these results indicate breathing pattern retraining attenuates the respiratory and
peripheral perceived effort during incremental exercise.
Key words: Breathing pattern disorders, retraining, blood stealing, cycling, performance, power
output, respiratory mechanics, perceived exertion, 20km-TT
x
Chapter One: Introduction
In the 2004 Olympic men’s road cycling time trial (48km), the time difference between winning the
gold medal and second place was 18.84 seconds (0.5%). Because small improvements in
performance at the top level of cycling can make a significant difference to overall results, cyclists,
coaches and sports scientists are continually looking for ways to facilitate these performance
gains such as employing new physical training methods (Paton & Hopkins, 2005), consuming
nutritional supplements (Wiles, Coleman, Tegerdine, & Swaine, 2006) and manipulating
equipment (Belen, Habrard, Micallef, & Le Gallais, 2007). An area that appears to be overlooked
is the effect that the mechanics of breathing may have on a cyclist’s performance.
Abnormal breathing patterns, or breathing pattern disorders, may be the long term consequence
due to:
iii) biomechanical factors for example the postures adopted by athletes in their
sport and/or;
iv) physiological factors, such as the changes that occur to breathing frequency
Either alone or in combination, these factors may cause the development of a chronic breathing
pattern disorder. Of particular interest to this study is the development, and potential
1
consequences, of a chronically abnormal breathing pattern in cyclists whose breathing pattern
becomes maladaptive.
The consequences of a breathing pattern disorder may have a detrimental effect on a cyclist’s
performance for several reasons. Altered respiratory mechanics during exercise may lead to a
tachypnoeic shift (increased breathing frequency at the expense of maintaining tidal volume; Lucia,
Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999), increased dynamic hyperinflation (Johnson,
Saupe, & Dempsey, 1992; McConnell, 2005) and a subsequent increase in the work of breathing
(Harms et al., 1998). Consequently, an increased work of breathing may cause blood stealing
(Sheel et al., 2001), a phenomenon where blood is diverted from working peripheral muscles to
the respiratory muscles to meet increased demand. Such decreased blood flow may negatively
affect the ability of the peripheral muscles to generate power (Romer & Dempsey, 2006).
Additionally, an increased work of breathing can result in respiratory muscle fatigue (Johnson,
Aaron, Babcock, & Dempsey, 1996) and increased dyspnoea (Grazzini, Stendardi, Gigliotti, &
Traditionally, the idea that breathing may be a limiting factor in high intensity exercise has been
disregarded due to the apparent breathing reserve observed in athletes even at the highest level
of exertion (Dempsey, 1986). However, with evidence suggesting expiratory flow limitation may
occur in elite athletes (Johnson, Saupe, & Dempsey, 1992) and that the work of breathing during
intense exercise may cause blood stealing (Harms, Wetter, St Croix, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2000;
Sheel et al., 2001), attention has been brought to the possibility that breathing may be a limitation
inspiratory muscle training (IMT) (Dellaca et al., 2001; Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002a) and
2
voluntary isocapnic hyperpnoea (VIH) (Holm, Sattler, & Fregosi, 2004; Leddy et al., 2007;
McMahon, Boutellier, Smith, & Spengler, 2002) in improving performance in endurance sport
suggests that specific intervention strategies can be employed to overcome this limitation.
Although strong evidence exists to suggest that IMT is effective in increasing performance by
increasing the strength and endurance of the respiratory muscles and reducing inspiratory muscle
fatigue (Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002a, , 2002b; Volianitis et al., 2001), IMT in itself does not
address the pattern of breathing. This could be likened to increasing the leg strength and power
The ability to maintain an even, controlled breathing pattern during high exercise intensities,
professional cyclists (Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999). It is not known,
however, whether this breathing pattern is a physiological adaptation (e.g. to years of high
intensity training,) or whether it could be likened to a learned skill and could therefore be
manipulated.
respiratory mechanics and breathing pattern disorders (Blager, 2000; Chaitow, Bradley, & Gilbert,
2002; Jones, Dean, & Chow, 2003). Although used typically in the management of patients with
respiratory pathology, the concept and application is similar for athletes. Breathing pattern
retraining may benefit cyclists in two ways - firstly by correcting any abnormal respiratory
mechanics or breathing pattern disorders that are present at rest, and secondly by attenuating the
consequences of poor respiratory mechanics that often occur during intense exercise (Fallon,
2004; 2007).
3
Whilst breathing pattern retraining techniques are used clinically by physiotherapists to address
breathing pattern disorders in athletes, there are no known published studies documenting the
ergogenic potential of this technique in this population. Further, the effect that optimising
the negative consequences of an increased work of breathing, has not been reported. The aim of
Chapter Two (Review of Literature) is to introduce the reader to the concept and consequences of
abnormal breathing patterns in athletes and to discuss how breathing pattern retraining may not
only improve the efficiency of breathing in cyclists, but also facilitate increased cycling
performance. In consideration of how or why breathing pattern retraining may potentially enhance
cycling performance, a brief description of normal and abnormal breathing patterns is provided
and the prevalence of breathing disorders in athletes will be discussed. In addition, the theories
and evidence that underpins the ventilatory response to exercise, subsequent changes to the
work of breathing and consequences of this, will be presented. Finally, strategies that may
decrease the effect of altered respiratory mechanics during exercise, including respiratory muscle
Using breathing pattern retraining to manipulate breathing in athletes is a novel area of research.
Indeed there is a paucity of literature that objectively defines breathing pattern retraining and its
The aim of this thesis is to address these gaps by investigating the use of breathing pattern
4
1.1. Aim of The Study
The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of four weeks of breathing pattern retraining on
20-km time-trial performance, and on respiratory and metabolic measures during an incremental
5
Chapter Two: Literature Review. The Development, Consequence
and Management of Abnormal Respiratory Mechanics in Athletes
The difference between winning an Olympic gold medal and second place in the men’s 48km time
trial in 2004 was 18.84 seconds (0.5%) (www.cyclingnews.com). Even the smallest improvement
in performance at the top level can make a significant difference to the overall result (Jeukendrup
& Martin, 2001). Cycling performance is dependent on physiological factors which influence
mechanical power production (for example training modification, nutrition and possibly breathing
pattern) (Lucia et al., 1999), in addition to mechanical and environmental factors that affect power
demand (for example body mass, body position, bicycle) (Jeukendrup & Martin, 2001). These
factors are even more important in an individual time trial (TT) where race tactics play a lesser
part (Jeukendrup & Martin, 2001) and physical performance factors come to the fore. For
example, the effects of modified training on a 40km-TT in already well-trained individuals has
been reported to be 2-4% (Jeukendrup & Martin, 2001) which could make a difference of 70-140
seconds over 40km. This time difference applied to the 2004 Olympic results described above
Competitive road cyclists may take part in a one day race, or multistage events which are
characterised by back-to-back days of racing including mass start races, individual and team time
trials. Of all the road race formats, the individual time trial is considered the most physically
demanding (Mujika & Padilla, 2001) and is a component of major tour races (e.g. the Tour de
France), or in some cases is a stand-alone event (e.g. World Championships and Olympic
Games). An individual time trial demands each cyclist cover the required distance in the shortest
6
possible time by sustaining the highest average power output that will allow them to gain a time
Mass start races allow drafting, which means a cyclist can ride in the slipstream of the rider(s)
ahead to decrease their resistance (Faria, Parker, & Faria, 2005b). Drafting can decrease energy
utilization by up to 40% (Faria, Parker, & Faria, 2005b), however drafting is not allowed in time
trial events, meaning cyclists must be able to sustain high energy outputs for prolonged periods.
Although many factors influence cycling performance, including years of training (Coyle et al.,
1991), cycling economy (Lucia, Hoyos, & Perez, 2002) and V&O 2 max (Lucia et al., 2006), power
output at lactate threshold and peak power output indicating a power-to-weight ratio of greater or
equal to 5.5W/kg have been suggested to be the physiological markers most predictive of cycling
ways in which power output can be maximised in cyclists. These include hypoxic training (Clark
et al., 2007), recovery strategies (Maxwell, Castle, & Spencer, 2007), aerodynamics (Grappe,
Candau, & Belli, 1997) and inspiratory muscle training (Johnson, Sharpe, & Brown, 2007). Whilst
attention has been paid to ventilatory aspects, including the changes that occur in breathing
pattern during exercise (Dempsey, 2006a; Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999;
Lucia, Hoyos, & Perez, 2002; Lucia et al., 2006), no attention has been paid to manipulating
breathing pattern on a long term basis. Given the reported high prevalence of breathing disorders
in athletes (Fallon, 2004; Mendelli, Lounana, Messan, Menuet, & Petitjean, 2006; Rundell &
Spiering, 2003) and the changes to respiratory mechanics that occur during exercise (Johnson,
7
Saupe, & Dempsey, 1992; Verges, Notter, & Spengler, 2006; Ward, 2007), it is surprising that no
study has focused on the potential improvements in cycling work output and performance that can
A breathing pattern disorder (BPD) is a general term used to describe the consequences of long-
standing abnormal respiratory mechanics. Little is published in the medical and scientific
literature describing BPDs; what they are, how they are established and how they affect otherwise
healthy individuals. Even less is known about how BPDs and abnormal respiratory mechanics
may affect athletes and, specifically to this review, cyclists. Breathing in cyclists may be
compromised in a number of ways (e.g. altered mechanics of breathing caused by the cyclists
unique exercising posture, changes in respiratory timing and/or muscle recruitment pattern due to
limitations and increased intra- and post-pulmonary shunt) (Dempsey & Wagner, 1999; Stickland
& Lovering, 2006), and while most of these are beyond the scope of this review, it is possible that
with regard to respiratory mechanics, a cyclist’s breathing may be compromised by the presence
of a breathing pattern disorder at rest, or the changes to respiratory timing and respiratory muscle
recruitment pattern that occurs with the normal respiratory response to exercise. To gain further
understanding of the aetiology of BPDs, this section will focus on the mechanics of normal and
abnormal breathing at rest and during exercise. The prevalence of abnormal breathing patterns in
8
2.2.1. Normal Breathing at Rest
Breathing at rest, in people without respiratory illness, requires less than five percent of whole-
body oxygen consumption ( V&O 2 ) (Jones, Dean, & Chow, 2003). This proportion of V&O 2 enables
the respiratory muscles to generate appropriate pressure changes within the thoracic cavity to
overcome a combination of airway resistance and lung compliance, resulting in inspiration. As the
primary muscle of inspiration, the diaphragm is normally responsible for 95% of the work of quiet,
tidal breathing, with minimal contribution from the accessory muscles (Faithfull, Jones, & Jordan,
Accessory Accessory
Sternocleidomastoid Interosseous internal intercostals
Upper trapezius Abdominal muscles (rectus abdominus, internal
Serratus anterior (arms elevated) and external obliques, transverse abdominus)
Latissimus dorsi (arms elevated) Transversus thoracis
Serratus posterior superior Subcostales
Iliocostalis Iliocostalis lumborum
Subclavius Quadratus lumborum
Omohyoid Serratus posterior inferior
Latissimus dorsi
Assuming optimal respiratory mechanics, the diaphragm descends into the abdominal cavity
during inspiration, pushing the abdominal contents downwards and forwards, increasing the
vertical dimensions of the chest cavity, as the ribs are lifted and move out to increase the
transverse dimensions (Chaitow & Bradley, 2002). Expiration during quiet breathing is normally
passive and as the diaphragm relaxes the abdomen and chest wall return to their starting position.
9
Minute ventilation (VE) is the volume of gas ventilated in one minute and is the product of tidal
volume and respiratory rate. The primary aim of VE is to meet the metabolic demands of the body
by moving oxygen (O2) into and carbon dioxide (CO2) out of the lungs in the most efficient way
(Abernethy, Mackinnon, Neal, Kippers, & Hanrahan, 1996). It is suggested that meeting the
metabolic demands of the body at rest (which requires approximately 6 L•min-1; Webber & Pryor,
1994) in the most efficient way, is achieved using a steady, rhythmical pattern of the diaphragm; a
respiratory rate of 10-14 breathsmin-1; and an inspiratory to expiratory ratio of 1:1.5-2 (Chaitow &
Bradley, 2002). Ideally, a person naturally adopts an optimal breathing pattern that requires the
least amount of mechanical stress and work from the respiratory musculature (Jones, Dean, &
Chow, 2003). However in some instances this optimal pattern can be disrupted such that
abnormal, and potentially inefficient, respiratory mechanics become the default pattern.
According to Chaitow et al. (2002) abnormal respiratory mechanics in a healthy human at rest
Abnormal respiratory mechanics may occur as the result of, but not limited to, an acute or chronic
illness, prolonged exposure to stressful situations, occupational factors (for example occupations
10
that require large amounts of talking), or poor posture (Blager, 2000; Chaitow, Bradley, & Gilbert,
2002).
increased fb, is an acute decrease in the level of CO2 in the bloodstream (Garssen, de Ruiter, &
van Dyke, 1992). The physiology underpinning how breathing pattern may be altered chronically
is not known. One theory that has been presented however, is related to the body’s sensitivity to
CO2 (Garssen, de Ruiter, & van Dyke, 1992; Gilbert, 2005). Whilst the body has compensatory
strategies to buffer the decreased CO2 in the short term, it is thought that if abnormal respiratory
mechanics persist chronically, the brainstem respiratory centres are ‘reset’ to respond to the new,
decreased level (Garssen, de Ruiter, & van Dyke, 1992). Accordingly, the centrally mediated
automatic breathing pattern is ‘reprogrammed’ such that an apical breathing pattern with an
elevated fb is accepted as ‘normal’ and a decreased level of circulating blood CO2 is maintained.
Consequently, a chronic BPD is established. Symptoms and signs of a BPD can include
shortness of breath inappropriate to the level of activity, difficulty inhaling during exercise,
inspiratory wheeze or stridor, air hunger, chest wall pain and heart palpitations (Chaitow & Bradley,
2002; Fallon, 2004). It is not known why some people develop BPDs whilst others exposed to the
pattern with ‘normal’ respiratory mechanics (Chaitow, Bradley, & Gilbert, 2002). Attention is paid
to a patient’s breathing frequency, whether they are breathing apically or diaphragmatically, via
their mouth or nose, the degree to which their breathing pattern is rhythmical and the presence of
frequent sighs, yawns and/or abdominal splinting (Chaitow & Bradley, 2002). There is currently
11
no formal, objective, reliable and valid single test to clinically diagnose abnormal respiratory
mechanics and a BPD. Spirometry can give an indication of compromised respiratory mechanics,
for example airflow limitations, as can a bronchoscopy, however both of these tests require a
patient to be symptomatic at the time of testing (Blager, 2000). The Nijmegen questionnaire
(Appendix 1) has been used as a supportive diagnostic tool for identifying breathing pattern
disorders in research studies (Han, Stegen, De Valck, Clement, & Van de Woestijne, 1996;
Thomas et al., 2003), and is used by some clinicians to confirm diagnosis of hyperventilation
syndrome (HVS) (Chaitow, Bradley, & Gilbert, 2002). This noninvasive questionnaire identifies
anxiety related breathing pattern disorders. It has a sensitivity of 91% and a specificity of 95% in
patients with a clinical diagnosis of HVS, and enables patients to see the widespread nature of
symptoms and progress with treatment (Van Dixhoorn & Duivenvoorden, 1984).
The prevalence of abnormal respiratory mechanics and BPDs in athletes, in the absence of
respiratory pathology, has not been reported in the literature. It would be reasonable to speculate
that if abnormal respiratory mechanics and/or BPDs are present in athletes at rest, the effect and
consequences of such might be exaggerated during exercise due to the increased physiological
demands upon the respiratory, cardiovascular and musculoskeletal systems during exercise.
Before addressing these factors in more detail, a brief review of the normal respiratory response
to exercise is provided.
The increased metabolic demands experienced by the body during exercise must be met by
individuals who elicit a VE of 112 ± 19.9 Lmin-1 (Sheel et al., 2006) during maximal intensity
12
exercise (VEmax), studies of top level athletes have demonstrated that VE can increase to 190
Lmin-1 in cycling (Harms, Wetter, St Croix, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2000), and often above 200
Lmin-1 in rowers (Sheel, Lama, Potvin, Coutts, & McKenzie, 1996), sometimes peaking as high as
270 Lmin-1 (Shephard, 1998). To achieve a breathing pattern that will enable the ventilatory
demand of high intensity exercise to be met usually causes an athlete’s respiratory pattern to shift
from diaphragmatic via the nose, to apical via the mouth in an attempt to shift larger volumes of air
faster (Bradley, 2002). The workload at which this occurs has not been documented in the
literature. It is possible that this shift in pattern corresponds with the respiratory compensation
point – the onset of hyperventilation during incremental exercise that is thought to represent the
body’s attempt to compensate for metabolic acidosis (Meyer, Faude, Urhausen, & Kindermann,
2004). During exercise VE can be met by increasing fb and tidal volume (VT) and it is not known
whether the shift from diaphragmatic to apical breathing is a physiological necessity, or a ‘learned
habit’. Further, an optimal breathing pattern during exercise (as opposed to the ‘normal
To achieve such large increases in VE, the normal respiratory response to exercise is to increase
the depth of breathing (increased VT) and as the workload further increases, by increasing fb
(Gallagher, Brown, & Younes, 1987). The initial increase in VT may cause the end-expiratory lung
volume (EELV) to decrease below tidal EELV (Johnson, Saupe, & Dempsey, 1992). This
lengthens the diaphragm and other inspiratory muscles at end-expiration, allowing them to
operate near their optimal length for force generation as exercise intensity increases (Ward,
Eidelman, & Stubbing, 1988). In addition, a decreased EELV (below functional residual volume)
may increase the proportion of “passive” inspiration due to the elastic recoil of the chest wall,
13
further enhancing the attainment of a high VE whilst preserving respiratory muscle length (Verges,
As ventilatory demands increase further, fb continues to rise, and VT will either plateau at
approximately 50-60% of vital capacity (Clark, Hagerman, & Gelfand, 1983; Hey, Lloyd,
Cunningham, Jukes, & Bolton, 1966) or start to decrease (Spengler, Knopfli-Lenzin, Birchler,
Trapletti & Boutellier, 2000; Gallagher, Brown & Younes, 1987). Potentially EELV may increase
above resting levels resulting in dynamic hyperinflation of the lungs (Johnson, Saupe, & Dempsey,
1992). Although dynamic hyperinflation permits increases in expiratory flow rates (Miller et al.,
2005), which may assist the attainment of the high VE necessary during maximal exercise, it
comes at the expense of an increased work of breathing (the effect of which will be discussed
later in this review) (Witt, Guenette, Rupert, McKenzie, & Sheel, 2007).
It has been observed that as exercise intensity rises, fb increases at the expense of maintaining VT
inefficient pattern develops (Gallagher, Brown, & Younes, 1987; Scheuermann & Kowalchuk,
metabolically (Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999). The likely consequence of
a tachypnoeic shift is dynamic hyperinflation due to the decreased expiratory time and subsequent
breath stacking which ultimately increases the work of breathing caused by breathing at increased
lung volumes. Further, VE may remain unchanged despite further changes to fb and VT, whilst the
amount of O2 actually reaching the alveoli for gas exchange decreases due to increased dead
space volume (Hanson, Claremont, Dempsey, & Reddan, 1982) (Table 2).
14
Table 2. Effect of Breathing Rate and Depth on Alveolar Ventilation in Three Scenarios
(from Marieb, 1992)
Breathing Dead Tidal Respiratory Minute Alveolar % of TV =
pattern space volume rate respiratory ventilation dead space
volume (TV) volume rate volume
I - Normal rate 150ml 500ml 20/min 10,000 ml/min 7000 ml/min 30%
and depth
II - Slow, deep 150ml 1000ml 10/min 10,000 ml/min 8500 ml/min 15%
breathing
III - Rapid, 150ml 250ml 40/min 10,000 ml/min 4000 ml/min 60%
shallow breathing
It could be suggested that the ability to maintain an even, controlled breathing pattern, rather than
allowing a tachypnoeic shift to develop, would be advantageous to athletes during high exercise
intensities. Given the significant increase in VE (often 10-15 times resting; Johnson, Aaron,
Babcock, & Dempsey, 1996) that must be achieved during maximal exercise, it would seem
advantageous to achieve this in the most efficient way. Certainly, it appears that some athletes
can maintain an effective respiratory pattern (as defined by lack of tachypnoeic shift; Lucia,
Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999) whilst others do not. In the few studies that have
compared the respiratory pattern of different levels of athletes, it appears that only the very highly
trained or experienced athletes demonstrate this desired characteristic (Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon,
Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999; Mahler, Shuhart, Brew, & Stukel, 1991; Vaughan, 1984). More
specifically, a clear difference in respiratory pattern has been observed between professional and
non professional cyclists (Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999) at high exercise
intensities. Lucia et al. (1999) compared fb, VT, VE, inspiratory and expiratory time in 14
professional Tour de France level male road cyclists ( V&O 2 peak: 73.2 ± 6.6 mlkgmin-1, Pmax: 471
± 30.9W) and 11 amateur, elite male road cyclists ( V&O 2 peak: 73.8 ± 5 mlkgmin-1, Pmax: 437 ±
25.2W) during incremental exercise from sub-maximal to maximal intensities (exhaustion). The
15
VEmax did not differ between the two groups (170 vs. 175 Lmin-1, Figure 1), although at intensities
between 300 and 400W professional cyclists exhibited a 15% (P < 0.01) lower VE than elites. At
power outputs less than 200W fb tended to be higher in professionals than elite cyclists (for
example 25 versus 20 breathsmin-1; P < 0.05 at 100W, Figure 1) but from 200W to maximum
intensities the elite group had a significantly higher fb than professionals (58 vs. 51 breathsmin-1
at maximal; P < 0.05, Figure 1). Inspiratory time reached a plateau in professionals at low
intensities (300W; 63.7% Pmax) and then decreased with increasing exercise intensity, whereas it
consistently decreased in elite from the lowest to the highest intensities (50 to 437.5W; Figure 2).
Expiratory time was significantly higher in professionals than elites at most moderate-to-high
power outputs (P < 0.05 for 250 and 300W, and P < 0.01 for 350 and 400W, Figure 2). These
findings indicate that although the maximum volume of air delivered to the lungs was not
statistically different between the two groups, professional cyclists did not adopt a tachypnoeic
16
Figure 2. Inspiratory (tI) and expiratory (tE) times.
Definitions as for Figure .1. * P < 0.05, ** P < 0.01, §
significant (P < 0.05) interactive effect (group x
exercise intensity. (From Lucia et al., 1999).
environment. The subjects breathing was measured using a mouth piece and nose clip which
have been shown to influence the breathing pattern in normal subjects by causing an increase in
VT while fb is unchanged or decreased (Askanazi et al., 1980). Lucia et al. (1999) believed their
17
subjects, having participated in previous studies using the same respiratory apparatus, were
familiar with the equipment and it would have been unlikely to have affected their breathing.
Additionally, both groups of cyclists were tested using the same equipment meaning both groups
would have been affected in the same way. Lucia et al. (1999) set the cadence for their subjects,
meaning exercise intensity and power could only be generated by increasing the force applied to
each pedal stroke, whereas in field conditions cyclists increase power by increasing the cadence
in addition to the force applied to each stroke (Conconi et al., 1996). Lucia et al. (1999)
acknowledge that in real cycling conditions at high intensities, increasing cadences might alter
breathing patterns through the sensory input from muscle mechanoreceptors to the respiratory
In sports with a rhythmic component such as rowing, respiration is coordinated with the action of
the locomotor system (entrainment) to maximise respiratory efficiency, however entrainment has
only been observed in high level rowers (Mahler, Shuhart, Brew, & Stukel, 1991; Vaughan, 1984).
Mahler et al. (1991) compared the respiratory pattern of 16 novice and 18 experienced rowers and
found entrainment at maximal effort in 78% of elite female rowers, but only in 30% of novices.
Similar to the professional cyclists in Lucia et al.’s (1999) study, the elite rowers maintained a
respiratory pattern that was void of a tachypnoeic shift, whilst the novice rowers increased
It is possible that maintaining optimal levels of effective ventilation is one factor that enables
athletes to achieve levels of performance necessary to excel in their own sport. Lucia et al.
(1999) speculated that a breathing pattern devoid of a tachypnoeic shift, and with a prolonged tE,
is required to perform at the highest level of cycling. Currently, there is insufficient research to
18
identify which physiological characteristics enable professional cyclists to breathe more efficiently,
independent factor that could be likened to a learned technique or skill that optimises performance.
In an attempt to explain the observed differences in breathing patterns between professional and
elite athletes, Lucia (1999) initially suggested that physiological factors related to the more
demanding training carried out by the professional, as compared with the elite, cyclists was
responsible. However, in a subsequent longitudinal study, Lucia et al. (2001) later demonstrated
that a professional cyclist’s breathing pattern did not vary during the three periods (rest, pre-
competition and competition) of a competitive cycling season, despite the fact that a significant
overall training effect on power output and V&O 2 was shown throughout the season. These
findings suggest that training volume was not the causative factor, or if it was, it possibly occurred
over years of training, and was not influenced by the differences occurring throughout a
professional cyclist’s season. Another suggestion is that the high fb and decreased VT often
observed at high intensity exercise (Clark, Hagerman, & Gelfand, 1983; Folinsbee, Wallace, Bedi,
& Horvath, 1983; Gallagher, Brown, & Younes, 1987) could simply be attributed to respiratory
muscle fatigue, and that high level athletes develop a superior level of resistance to such fatigue
due to their greater years of experience and volume of training (Syabbalo, Krishnan, Zintel, &
Gallagher, 1994). In a group of competitive, but not professional, cyclists, respiratory muscle
fatigue (measured by inspiratory pressure at zero flow) has been recorded at 17 ± 4% and13 ±
19
In contrast, one theory that has been supported across decades (Hey, Lloyd, Cunningham, Jukes,
& Bolton, 1966; Scheuermann & Kowalchuk, 1999) is that the integration of the depth and
of known respiratory stimuli, and may simply reflect a pattern of breathing that results in a minimal
amount of work by the respiratory muscles. This theory may support the suggestion that
breathing pattern may be a learned skill and could therefore be retrained. Clearly, experimental
work is needed to determine whether an efficient breathing pattern can be taught, or whether it is
Ventilation during exercise may be compromised in athletes for a number of reasons including,
increased intra- and post-pulmonary shunt (Dempsey & Wagner, 1999; Stickland & Lovering,
2006)), environmental and pathological (exercise-induced asthma, bronchospasm and vocal cord
postures). Whilst further details of the mechanistic factors are beyond the scope of this review,
The prevalence of disordered breathing in athletes is unknown; however the literature suggests
that athletes are just as much at risk as the general population, if not more, of developing
breathing related problems, which will subsequently affect their breathing pattern during exercise
(Rundell & Spiering, 2003). The incidence of asthma in athletes (11-50%; (Larsson,
Hemmingsson, & Boethius, 1994; Rundell & Spiering, 2003)), is known to be greater than in the
20
general population (10-12%; (AIHW, 2005)) which could be due to increased exposure to cold, dry
environments as well as pollen and pollution whilst training (Mendelli, Lounana, Messan, Menuet,
& Petitjean, 2006). For example, Rundell and Spiering (2003) reported a 30% incidence of EIB in
370 developmental and elite athletes. In Olympic athletes, Fallon (2003) suggested that the
production (14.9%), cough (7.3%), wheezing (6.2%) and chest tightness (4.9%) were amongst
respiratory symptoms reported in a sample of 698 athletes despite only 15.3% having diagnosed
asthma (Turcotte, Langdeau, Thibault & Boulet, 2003) suggesting that EIA is not the only cause of
respiratory symptoms in athletes. Inspiratory stridor is another respiratory sign seen in athletes
and is often associated with Vocal Cord Dysfunction (VCD) - the paradoxical closure of the vocal
cords that occurs during inspiration (Rundell & Spiering, 2003). Mouth breathing, increased
activation of the scalene muscles (accessory respiratory muscles), anxiety and air pollutants are
all triggers of VCD. Symptoms usually present at a high VE during both intense training and/or
competition (Fallon, 2003), causing stridor, air hunger and chest tightness – all symptoms that
closely mimic EIA (Brugman & Simons, 1998). Not surprisingly, VCD is often misdiagnosed in
athletes as EIA and treated as such (Brugman & Simons, 1998; Fallon, 2004; Rundell & Spiering,
2003). VCD is unresponsive to traditional asthma medications, potentially meaning that athletes
never receive the appropriate treatment and subsequent resolution of their symptoms (Fallon,
2004).
In addition to the environmental and pathological factors presented above, psychological factors
may also affect an athlete’s breathing pattern (Taylor, Rehder, Hyatt, & Parker, 1989). Anxiety is
known to increase respiratory rate through stimulation of the sympathetic nervous system (Ley &
21
Yelich, 1998; Bradley, 2002). Athletes competing in a sporting environment are subjected to
stressful, anxiety-inducing environments on a regular basis (Baker, Cote, & Hawes, 2000), which
may predispose them to BPDs. It is possible that the incidence of BPDs is higher in athletes than
the general population due to the high levels of performance anxiety to which they are exposed.
Breathing may also be compromised in cyclists through biomechanical factors. More specifically,
it is possible that the posture adopted by a cyclist when riding may compromise breathing
efficiency. For example, time trial riders adopt an aerodynamic position (Figure 3) to minimize
frontal area in order to maximize speed (Capelli, Rosa, & Butti, 1993; Faria, Parker, & Faria,
2005b).
Figure 3. A typical athlete’s cycling position during a road time-trial (reprinted with the permission of Stephen
Sheldrake, NZ Triathlete)
22
When cyclists adopt this position, the elbows are brought in close to the body and the trunk close
to the thighs – a position that compromises the ability of the thoracic cavity to expand fully to
enable breathing to maximal capacity (Hogg, 2005). This aerodynamic position may induce an
increase in abdominal impedance, and therefore in diaphragmatic work to overcome it, leading to
premature recruitment of the accessory respiratory muscles accelerating the onset of respiratory
muscle fatigue (Boussana et al., 2007; Johnson, Babcock, Suman, & Dempsey, 1993).
While gains are to be made by adopting an aerodynamic position, some evidence exists to
suggest that the aerodynamic position may not be metabolically efficient. Gnehn, Reichenbach,
Altpeter, Widmer & Hoppeler (1997) observed a significant increase in V&O 2 (3%; P = 0.02), heart
rate (3.3%; P = 0.02), and respiratory exchange ratio (1.3%; P = 0.02) when a cyclist’s upper body
configuration was changed from the upright to the time trial position at 70% V&O 2 max. Although
these are small percentage changes, they may be important if they carry over to the cyclist’s
performance. Conversely, in two sub-maximal conditions (70 and 80% V&O 2 max), Johnson and
Shultz (1990) found no evidence to suggest that riding in an aerodynamic position interfered with
breathing mechanics in any way (70% VO2max), nor did they show a significant difference in V&O 2
when cyclists rode at 80% of V&O 2 max during a 10 minute steady state ride.
Clearly, the apparent increased metabolic cost that may be associated with the time trial position,
is offset by the gains made by decreasing the aerodynamic resistance (Gnehm, Reichenbach,
Altpeter, Widmer, & Hoppeler, 1997). At speeds greater than 50kph (often achieved during time
trials in elite competition) some authors argue that aerodynamic resistance is the most
23
performance-determining variable (Faria, Parker, & Faria, 2005). The effect of the time trial
position on breathing has primarily been investigated at sub-maximal intensities (Gnehm et al.,
1990; Johnson & Shultz, 1997). However, time trial competitions are raced at high intensities and
it is unclear from the literature what effect the aerodynamic time trial position would have on
breathing pattern at higher intensity domains (i.e. closer to V&O 2 max). It is possible that adopting
an efficient breathing pattern whilst riding in the time trial position may reduce the elevated
metabolic cost previously reported in this position and, added to the gains of optimising
to the potential contribution that posture makes to altered respiratory mechanics, may affect an
athlete’s breathing. This may occur acutely, for example increased fb immediately prior to a race
or during training, but more detrimentally may affect it chronically through alteration of the
subconscious breathing pattern and altered respiratory mechanics. The consequences of altered
respiratory mechanics are multifactorial and, in addition to compromising breathing efficiency, may
ultimately have a negative effect on performance. These consequences will be further discussed
Respiratory mechanics during exercise may be altered by the presence of BPDs at rest, the
adopted when riding. The consequences of altered respiratory mechanics (changes to respiratory
timing and muscle recruitment patterns) may be detrimental to breathing efficiency and ultimately
24
exercise performance by initiating dynamic hyperinflation of the lungs and increasing the work of
breathing. Dynamic hyperinflation and increased work of breathing may contribute to respiratory
muscle fatigue, blood stealing and dyspnoea – all factors that have been associated with
decreased exercise performance. This section will explore how altered respiratory mechanics
increase the work of breathing and lead to such consequences, and in turn how exercise
The “normal” timing of the inspiratory to expiratory ratio at rest is 1:1.5 - 2 (West, 1995). To
achieve the increased fb and decreased VT associated with exercise, the ratio and length of
inspiratory (tI) and expiratory (tE) time has to change. In normal breathing, the lengthened
expiratory phase ensures the inspiratory muscles are returned to their optimal length prior to the
next inspiration. Previous research with sedentary individuals and elite cyclists (Folinsbee,
Wallace, Bedi, & Horvath, 1983) indicates that at high respiratory rates the reduction of expiratory
time makes a significantly greater contribution to the rise in fb, than the decrease in tI, effectively
resulting in a reverse inspiratory to expiratory ratio of 2:1. This breathing ratio leads to “breath
stacking” – a phenomenon where a subsequent inspiration is taken prior to the full exhalation of
the previous breath, resulting in tidal breathing occurring closer to total lung capacity (the volume
of air in the lungs at the height of a maximal inspiration) (Chaitow & Bradley, 2002; McConnell,
2005). Increased end-inspiratory (Grazzini, Stendardi, Gigliotti, & Scano, 2005; Johnson, Aaron,
Babcock, & Dempsey, 1996) and increased end-expiratory lung volumes have been observed in
athletes (Sutton, 1992), suggesting that breathing at higher lung volumes or “dynamic
25
2.3.2. Dynamic Hyperinflation
Whilst the initial increase in VT at the onset of exercise may cause end-expiratory lung volume
(EELV) to decrease, increases to fb and the subsequent changes to respiratory timing discussed
above may increase EELV, resulting in dynamic hyperinflation (Johnson, Saupe, & Dempsey,
1992). Dynamic hyperinflation affects the mechanics of the respiratory muscles in three ways - by
altering the length-tension, the pressure-volume and the force-velocity relationships. Muscles
have an optimal length at which they can generate their maximal force. This is referred to as the
length-tension relationship of muscles (Rack & Westbury, 1969). When a muscle is required to
contract in a position that is shorter or longer than its optimal length, its force generating potential
shortening the operating lengths of the diaphragm and accessory muscles. The inspiratory
muscles therefore move to a weaker portion of the length tension relationship and their ability to
generate a forceful contraction is compromised (Grazzini, Stendardi, Gigliotti, & Scano, 2005).
Additionally, because the operating length of the muscles is shortened, the time available for
contraction through range is less. The shorter time through which a muscle must contract, the
lower its force generating potential (Grazzini, Stendardi, Gigliotti, & Scano, 2005) therefore the
Lung compliance refers to the capability of the lungs to distend under pressure as measured by
pulmonary volume change per unit pressure change (West, 1995). As inspiration approaches
total lung capacity the lungs are almost at maximal distension and the lungs become ‘stiffer’.
Therefore, for a given inspiratory pressure (effort), there is a smaller increase in lung volume
during dynamic hyperinflation, than at normal lung volumes, resulting in decreased pulmonary
ventilation. Additionally, the inward elastic recoil of the respiratory system is greater at higher
26
volumes, imposing an added load on the inspiratory muscles, which are already contracting
The alteration in the length-tension, force-velocity and pressure-volume relationships means that if
dynamic hyperinflation occurs (through an inefficient breathing pattern) at a time when the
demands on the inspiratory muscles are greatest (i.e. during intense exercise), then the muscle’s
force generation capacity will be significantly compromised (Johnson, Aaron, Babcock, &
Dempsey, 1996). The capacity of the inspiratory muscles to generate dynamic pressure has been
predicted to decrease by 17% for each 10% of the total lung capacity that is accounted for by an
increased tidal volume above functional residual capacity (El-Manshawi, Killian, & Summers,
1986). Thus, even a small increase in end-expiratory lung volume will reduce the contractile
properties of the inspiratory muscles, and add to the load presented to them such that the work
significantly increased.
Although traditionally the mechanics of inspiration have been the main subject of investigation in
ventilation during exercise (Grazzini, Stendardi, Gigliotti, & Scano, 2005; Johnson, Saupe, &
Dempsey, 1992), the mechanics of expiration may also be negatively affected in some athletes.
At high levels of ventilation, an expiratory flow limitation has been identified in some athletes,
specifically elite, endurance athletes (Johnson, Saupe, & Dempsey, 1992). As expiration
becomes active to meet the increasing ventilatory demands experienced with high intensity
exercise, the abdominal muscles contract to force the diaphragm up and the resulting pleural
pressure can become positive. Positive pleural pressure may temporarily collapse the bronchi
27
and cause limitation of expiratory airflow. Thus, further increases in expiratory pressure limits
expiratory flow, and requires additional metabolic work in an attempt to overcome the flow
limitation (Kayser, Sliwinski, Yan, Tobiasz, & Macklem, 1997). It is suggested that the presence
of expiratory flow limitation promotes dynamic pulmonary hyperinflation with the concomitant
increase of work of breathing and impairment of inspiratory muscle function discussed above,
which may contribute to dyspnoea and limitation to exercise (Koulouris & Kosmas, 2002).
Breathing at rest in healthy individuals requires less than 5% of the body’s total V&O 2 (Jones,
Dean, & Chow, 2003). However, in highly trained individuals, the work of breathing during intense
exercise (92-100% V&O 2 max) may require up to 15% of the body’s total V&O 2 (Aaron, Seow,
Johnson, & Dempsey, 1992B), to enable the respiratory muscles to meet the metabolic demands
of this level of exercise. It would be plausible to expect that to meet such an increase in metabolic
demand, the respiratory muscles would also require a significant increase in the percentage of
cardiac output (Q). In an attempt to demonstrate this, Harms et al (1998) manipulated the work of
breathing using a proportional assist ventilator (PAV) to either decrease or increase the work of
breathing. Using this technique during maximal intensity exercise, Harms et al. (1998) showed
that a substantial portion of Q (14-16%) was indeed directed to the respiratory muscles to meet
their metabolic requirements. Given the importance of a high VE during maximal intensity
exercise, and the consequent increase in the work of breathing, changes to breathing pattern,
respiratory mechanics and work of breathing at maximal exercise could potentially play a part in
28
2.3.5. Respiratory Muscle Fatigue
The diaphragm is the most fatigue resistant of all the skeletal muscles (Dempsey, 2006b). A
healthy diaphragm is composed of approximately 45% type I and 55% type II muscle fibres
(McConnell, 2005) and unsurprisingly, has a very high aerobic enzymatic capacity and blood
supply (Dempsey, Romer, Rodman, Miller, & Smith, 2006). Despite these fatigue-resistant
qualities, the diaphragm has been shown to fatigue during exercise to exhaustion at intensities
greater than 85% of V&O 2 max. Using bilateral phrenic nerve stimulation (BPNS) (1-20Hz) to
measure transdiaphragmatic pressure before and after exercise to exhaustion at 80% V&O 2 max in
methodology in active individuals exercising at 85 and 95% of V&O 2 max, Johnson et al. (1996)
demonstrated that transdiaphragmatic pressure immediately after exercise was reduced 25-50%
below pre-exercise measures and took two hours to recover. Johnson et al. (1996) concluded
that heavy, whole-body exercise to exhaustion in relatively fit, normal subjects resulted in
significant diaphragmatic fatigue. Comparing these two studies, it appears that the degree to
which the diaphragm fatigues, and the speed at which it recovers, is significantly affected by the
intensity of exercise. Conversely, diaphragm fatigue has not been shown to occur below 80%
V&O 2 max (Gallagher & Younes, 1989) or when the diaphragm is fatiguable with short term (less
than eight minutes) progressive exercise to exhaustion unless a resistive load is added (Levine &
Henson, 1988). This would suggest that diaphragm fatigue is determined by a combination of the
29
The actual work performed by the inspiratory muscles, specifically the diaphragm, per se is,
however, not enough to explain the diaphragm fatigue observed by Mador et al. (1993) or
Johnson et al. (1996). When resting subjects were asked to mimic (using auditory and visual
feedback) the duration and magnitude of diaphragm work achieved during maximal intensity,
endurance exercise, fatigue did not occur. Only by increasing diaphragm work voluntarily to twice
that required during maximal exercise did diaphragm fatigue occur (Babcock, Pegelow, McClaran,
Suman, & Dempsey, 1995). This would suggest that other changes that occur, perhaps
elsewhere in the body, during intense exercise contribute to respiratory muscle fatigue, rather
than just an inability of the respiratory muscles to meet the work demand.
Global inspiratory muscle fatigue has also been demonstrated to occur immediately following
exercise both in untrained (Boutellier & Piwko, 1992) and trained individuals (Romer, McConnell,
& Jones, 2002c; Volianitis et al., 2001). For example, Romer et al. (2002) demonstrated
respiratory muscles at zero flow) of -17 ± 4% and -13 ± 3% following a simulated 20 and 40km-TT
minutes post-exercise. Similarly, but in competitive swimmers, Lomax and McConnell (2003)
reported a 29% deficit from baseline measures in maximal inspiratory pressure (MIP) following a
200m freestyle swim at 90-95% race pace. In competitive rowers, Volianitis et al. (2001)
demonstrated a 10% decline in MIP after six minutes of rowing ergometry at 100%. One
explanation for the large differences in deficits observed in these three studies (apart from the
obviously important postural differences) could be due to the nature of the sports and the
ventilatory requirements of each. For example, entrainment has been observed in high level
rowers and it is possible that the rowers used in the study subconsciously used entrainment to
30
optimise breathing and subsequently attenuate respiratory muscle fatigue. Conversely, in
freestyle swimming the mouth is out of water for a very short time, during which an inspiratory
effort must be taken. This short, fast inspiratory effort may predispose to dynamic hyperinflation
and exaggerate the pressure generating deficit from respiratory muscle fatigue, due to alterations
of the length – tension relationship (discussed earlier). Another factor that may exacerbate
respiratory muscle fatigue in swimming, and contribute to the different degrees of inspiratory
muscle fatigue observed by Romer et al. (2002) and Volianitis et al. (2001), is the hydrostatic
compression around the chest wall when the inspiratory muscles are relaxed (Withers & Hamdorf,
1989). Hydrostatic pressure counteracts inspiratory muscle force (Frangolias & Rhodes, 1996)
Exercise duration, exercise intensity and inefficient respiratory mechanics can increase the work
of breathing to the degree that respiratory muscle fatigue may occur, with negative consequences
on exercise performance (Harms, Wetter, St Croix, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2000). Harms et al.
(2000) demonstrated this concept by unloading the respiratory muscles during high intensity
exercise (90% V&O 2 max) using a PAV. Subjects were able to cycle longer (14.4 ± 4.9%; p<0.05)
than the control duration, whereas increasing respiratory muscle work significantly reduced (-15.1
± 3.3%; p<0.05) exercise time from control values. It is possible that there were other factors
contributing to the effect on exercise time in Harms et al.’s (2000) study in addition to respiratory
muscle fatigue (for example blood stealing, which will be discussed later) however certainly when
load was removed from the respiratory muscles their capability for sustained power generation
was increased.
31
Respiratory muscle fatigue may negatively affect exercise performance by altering the way the
respiratory muscles are recruited (Johnson, Aaron, Babcock, & Dempsey, 1996). Indirect
evidence from pressure recordings suggest that the fatiguing diaphragm actually reduces its
output as a force generator during the later stages of sustained endurance exercise and
accessory inspiratory and expiratory muscles become the dominant effecter of time dependant
hyperventilation (Dempsey, 2006b). Johnson et al. (1996) suggested that although total VE and
respiratory muscle pressure may continue to rise, the diaphragm is deactivated in an attempt to
reserve function. Diaphragm fatigue may indirectly influence performance by a reduction in the
relative contribution of the diaphragm; and a subsequent increase of accessory muscle use may
negatively affect performance. This change in muscle recruitment pattern may lead to dynamic
ultimately further respiratory muscle fatigue (Harms, Wetter, St Croix, Pegelow, & Dempsey,
2000) and blood stealing (Harms et al., 1997) – the latter being detrimental to limb function during
strenuous exercise.
The ability of professional cyclists, unlike elite amateur and lower level cyclists, to maintain the
diaphragmatic breathing pattern from low to high intensity (exhaustive) exercise (Lucia, Carvajal,
Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999) demonstrates that a mechanism may exist that enables
high intensity exercise to be maintained, without an increase in the work of breathing to the point
that respiratory muscle fatigue, and the subsequent negative effect on performance, occur. It has
been suggested that this controlled breathing pattern is one of the major factors that enables
professional cyclists to perform better than the tier of cyclists below them (Faria, Parker, & Faria,
2005b). Lucia et al.’s (1999) findings suggest that either respiratory muscle fatigue did not occur
in professional cyclists, or, if it did, it did not result in deactivation of the diaphragm with a
32
subsequent shift to an apical breathing pattern. This observation would suggest that the
decreased diaphragm recruitment speculated by Johnson et al. (1996) is more an inefficient but
common response during exercise, rather than an efficient physiological necessity. Johnson et al.
(1996) commented that although the diaphragm appeared to be deactivated, total VE and
respiratory muscle output continued to rise. However, it is not known what, if any, greater
increases in these variables would have occurred had the diaphragm continued to be the primary
pressure generator. It is therefore possible that the presence of an altered breathing pattern is the
instigator that then leads to respiratory muscle fatigue through the altered respiratory muscle
mechanics discussed above, rather than the result of respiratory muscle fatigue itself.
Regardless of whether altered respiratory recruitment pattern is the cause or effect of respiratory
muscle fatigue, the implications of it may not just be limited to the mechanical effects. Increased
work of breathing and respiratory muscle fatigue may have a further effect of causing reflex
vasoconstriction to exercising muscles, reducing blood flow to the muscles responsible for power
A further consequence of altered respiratory mechanics, and the subsequent increased work of
breathing, is the potential competition for blood supply between the peripheral locomotor and
respiratory muscles. During exercise, an adequate blood supply is vital to ensure required
nutrients are delivered to, and exercise metabolites are removed from muscle (Barclay, 1986;
Frisbee, Murrant, Wilson, & Barclay, 1999). At maximal intensity exercise, competition exists
between the respiratory and locomotor muscles for a supply of energy that is insufficient to meet
the needs of both (Aliverti & Macklem, 2001). Subsequently blood flow may be diverted from the
33
exercising (locomotor) muscles to the respiratory muscles under certain respiratory conditions
(blood stealing) to maintain the vital supply of respiratory muscle V&O 2 (Harms et al., 1997).
The concept of blood stealing was first investigated by Secher et al. (1977) who demonstrated
that recruiting additional muscle mass during sub-maximal leg cycling ( V&O 2 legs 67% V&O 2 max)
by simultaneously arm cranking ( V&O 2 arms 44%; total body exercise 78% V&O 2 max) decreased
blood flow to the already exercising leg muscle by approximately 15% (12.4 to 10.5 Lmin-1).
Since that pioneering work further studies have been undertaken that both refute (Kowalchuk,
Rossitter, Ward, & Whipp, 2002; Richter, Kiens, Hargreaves, & Kjaer, 1992) and support (Harms
et al., 1997; Harms et al., 1998; Sheel et al., 2001) the existence of the blood stealing
phenomenon. In contrast to Secher et al.’s (1977) findings, Richter et al. (1992) was unable to
demonstrate a decrease in Qlegs when arm cranking was added to cycling in untrained cyclists.
Further, Kowalchuk et al. (2002) demonstrated that increasing the work of breathing by adding a
load to both inspiratory and expiratory muscles during heavy intensity exercise (93 ± 3% V&O 2 max),
also in untrained cyclist, did not increase leg de-oxygenation (the variable these authors chose to
indicate decreased Qlegs), implying that Qlegs was not reduced consequent to the increased work of
breathing.
In contrast to the studies in untrained cyclists that refute blood stealing, two well designed studies
that support its existence were completed by Harms et al. (1997; 1998) involving competitive male
cyclists (N = 7; N = 8). These authors demonstrated that using a PAV to unload the respiratory
muscles and decrease the work of breathing (by 63%, compared to control) during maximal
incremental cycling to exhaustion, resulted in a decrease in Q (2.4 ± 0.7 l/min) and limb vascular
34
resistance (LVR) (0.5 ± 0.1mmHg1-1.min), whilst blood flow to the leg muscles (Qlegs) increased
(0.8 ± 0.3Lmin-1). Conversely, when the work of breathing was artificially increased (by 28%) by
and Qlegs (1.3 ± 0.2 l/min; p = 0.002) decreased (Harms et al., 1997). These results clearly show
that a relationship exists between increased work of breathing and Qlegs such that increasing the
work of breathing has a detrimental effect on locomotor muscle blood flow. This could suggest
that factors that increase the work of breathing in cyclists, for example posture and altered
respiratory mechanics, may compromise leg blood flow – the consequences of which are
discussed below. Conversely, factors that prevent the work of breathing from increasing may
preserve Qlegs.
(Harms et al., 1998). It is postulated that during maximal exercise, the work and / or aerobic
status of the respiratory muscles may stimulate type III-IV afferents in the diaphragm and other
respiratory muscles which then causes reflex vasoconstriction of limb vasculature during control
or loaded conditions and local vasodilation with unloading of the respiratory muscles (Wetter,
Harms, Nelson, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 1999). Increased sympathetic excitation of limb
vasculature is measured by norepinephrine (NE) spillover (Harms et al., 1997). Harms et al.
work of breathing) during maximal exercise, which was significantly related to LVR (r = 0.71, P <
0.05) suggesting a significant increase in limb vasoconstriction with increased work of breathing.
To identify the specific role that the respiratory muscles had in the aetiology of blood stealing,
Sheel et al. (2001) investigated the effect that respiratory muscle fatigue had on Qlegs at rest.
35
Using a combination of varying inspiratory resistance and respiratory patterns to fatigue the
respiratory muscles in different ways, (i.e. changes to fb and duty cycle) Sheel et al. (2001)
demonstrated decreased Qlegs (-30%) and increased LVR (50-60%), when compared with control
conditions, only at the point of diaphragm fatigue. Voluntary increases in inspiratory effort without
respiratory muscle fatigue had no effect on either Qlegs or LVR, indicating that the work of
breathing must be sufficient to cause respiratory muscle fatigue in order for blood stealing to occur.
The contrasting results above that support and refute the existence of blood stealing may be
explained, however, by methodological factors. Wetter et al. (1999) observed that the effect of
respiratory muscle unloading on Qlegs did not occur unless exercise intensity was greater than
75% V&O 2 max. During sub-maximal (50% and 75% of V&O 2 max) exercise, V&O 2 total increased with
increasing work of breathing and decreased with decreasing work of breathing but Qlegs and LVR
did not change with changing work of breathing. Even substantial increases in the work of
breathing (50-70%), although raising whole body V&O 2 , did not cause alterations in LVR or Qlegs
during sub maximal exercise (Wetter, Harms, Nelson, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 1999). Therefore, it
would appear that blood stealing only occurs during very high intensity exercise, and under
conditions that fatigue the respiratory muscles. Although Kowalchuk et al. (2002) used a much
greater inspiratory resistance than Harm et al. (1997; 1998) (7cmH2OL-1s-1 vs. 3-5cmH2OL-1s-1
respectively) at a work load that had previously been shown to induce decreased Qlegs, they
controlled both fb and VT during all experimental protocols. It is possible that by doing this the
respiratory muscles were not fatigued enough to invoke the metaboreflex and subsequent
changes to Qlegs and LVR. Further, Richardson et al. (1995) and Richter et al. (1992) increased
exercising muscle mass by adding another peripheral muscle group, rather than increasing the
36
work of breathing. The respiratory muscles were not specifically fatigued in these studies, and as
the exercise intensity (<75% V&O 2 max) was unlikely to be sufficient to induce respiratory muscle
When methodological conditions are taken into consideration, the evidence presented above
suggests that blood stealing does occur when the work of breathing is increased. The work of the
have two effects on the cardiovascular response – a) a substantial proportion of cardiac output is
directed to the respiratory muscles to support their metabolic requirements as discussed above,
and b) blood flow is reduced to (or ‘stolen from’) working locomotor muscles (Harms et al., 1997;
Harms et al., 1998). Limb muscle force output and fatigue are highly responsive to changes in
limb muscle blood flow under conditions of high intensities of muscle contraction (Romer &
Dempsey, 2006). The effects of changing limb blood flow have been attributed to changes in O2
transport and/or changes in the washout of local metabolites (Barclay, 1986). Given that blood
flow to the legs during maximal intensity exercise may be in the order of 15.4 ± 0.3 Lmin-1 (Harms
et al., 1997), a decrease of 1.3 Lmin-1 (8.4%) would likely have a large impact on performance.
To investigate this effect, Harms et al. (2000) again used a PAV to offload, (and inspiratory
resistance to load) the respiratory muscles during incremental tests to exhaustion at V&O 2 max in
competitive cyclists. When inspiratory muscle work was decreased by 63%, subjects were able to
exercise 1.3 ± 2 minutes longer (range -1.2 – 5.3 min; +14.4% ± 4.9%, p<0.05) than during
control conditions, whereas with loading, subjects exercised 1.0 ± 0.8 minutes less (range -0.4-
4.7 min; 15.1 ± 3.3%; p<0.05) (Harms, Wetter, St Croix, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2000). Clearly this
suggests that the decrease in Qlegs caused by increased work of breathing was sufficient to alter
37
cycling performance. To further investigate the effect of blood stealing on performance, and using
similar methodology, Romer and Dempsey (2006) recently measured quadriceps power, as a
measurement of peripheral fatigue in eight endurance trained male cyclists, before and after
incremental cycling in a randomised series of control, inspiratory muscle loading and unloading.
Exercise at 90% of maximum work rate (292W for 13.2 minutes) under control conditions reduced
force output of the quadriceps by 28%, indicating an expected presence of quadriceps fatigue
after exercise to exhaustion. This fatigue was gradually recovered with 70 minutes of rest
following exercise. After completion of an identical amount and duration of exercise as in the
control, but with unloading sufficient to reduce the work of breathing by 50-60%, limb fatigue was
8% less than the control, whereas respiratory muscle loading increased fatigue by 20% greater
than the control and quadricep strength had not recovered by 70 minutes. This suggests that
preventing the work of breathing from increasing to the point that the Qlegs is compromised, is an
important consideration for cyclists in order to optimise power output and exercise performance.
The findings by Harms et al. (1997, 2000), Sheel et al. (2001) and Romer et al. (2006) clearly give
strength to the suggestion that increased respiratory muscle work, particularly to the point of
respiratory muscle fatigue, will decrease blood flow to the exercising peripheral muscles to the
degree that physical exercise performance is impaired. If the altered respiratory muscle
exercise was such that respiratory muscle fatigue was invoked, theoretically performance at
employed to optimise respiratory mechanics and prevent unnecessary respiratory muscle fatigue,
38
Clearly, maintaining optimal respiratory mechanics may be a simple way to provide the small
improvement in performance that is so sought after by cyclists and sports scientists alike.
Maintaining optimal respiratory mechanics to prevent respiratory muscle fatigue may have a
second benefit with regards to optimising cycling performance – quite separate to preventing
2.3.7. Dyspnoea
Dyspnoea is one of the primary reasons given by healthy individuals for the termination of
exercise to exhaustion (Hamilton, Killian, Summers, & Jones, 1996; Killian et al., 1992) indicating
that some aspect of ventilation limits exercise. Dyspnoea is the subjective awareness of an
increased work of breathing (Webber & Pryor, 1994, p. 5). It is a general term used to
characterise a range of different descriptors that vary in intensity and are influenced by a wide
variety of factors, such as cultural expectations and previous experience (Grazzini, Stendardi,
Gigliotti, & Scano, 2005). In healthy humans, dyspnoea limits exercise when it reaches a level
that is deemed no longer tolerable (Grazzini, Stendardi, Gigliotti, & Scano, 2005).
The pathophysiology underlying dyspnoea is both complex and uncertain and a detailed
discussion of all potential contributing factors to dyspnoea during exercise (ATS, 1998; McConnell
& Romer, 2004a) is beyond the scope of this review. Given the complexity of changes to
respiratory mechanics during exercise it is difficult to be sure which alterations contribute most
strongly to the sensation of dyspnoea. However, with regards to limiting exercise in athletes, it
appears that the contractile properties of the respiratory muscles, combined with operational high
39
lung volumes and the conscious awareness of the outgoing respiratory motor command are likely
contributing factors (Grazzini, Stendardi, Gigliotti, & Scano, 2005). Factors that impair the
contractile properties of the respiratory muscles (for example functional weakening, pattern of
recruitment and fatigue) have the potential to increase the intensity of dyspnoea, whereas factors
that improve the contractile properties of these respiratory muscles have the potential to reduce
In the 1960’s Moran Campbell coined the term length-tension inappropriateness (Campbell, 1966)
and suggested that humans have a quantitative appreciation of the degree of effort associated
with breathing. Further, dissociation or a mismatch between central respiratory motor activity and
the mechanical response of the respiratory system may produce a sensation of respiratory
discomfort (dyspnoea) (McConnell & Romer, 2004a). As such, the intensity of dyspnoea is
increased when changes in respiratory muscle length (i.e. volume) or tension (i.e. pressure) are
inappropriate for the outgoing motor command. During exercise, the central outgoing drive for
ventilation is increased, however the decrease in respiratory muscle efficiency through altered
respiratory mechanics (i.e. breathing occurring on a less compliant portion of the pressure-volume
curve, increased elastic recoil of the respiratory system at end expiration, and respiratory muscle
shortening) (ATS, 1998) does not enable appropriate volume and pressure changes to occur. In
other words, although the drive to breathe has been increased in response to intensifying exercise,
that this inappropriate response is perceived in the brain as ‘not breathing enough’ resulting in the
mechanics could enable the respiratory muscles to respond more appropriately to the increased
central output and therefore decrease perceptions of dyspnoea (McConnell & Romer, 2004a).
40
The order in which the respiratory muscles are recruited in response to exercise may also
the diaphragm is recruited first, followed by increasing activation of the respiratory accessory
muscles as breathing frequency increases. Animal studies show that the diaphragm is less well
supplied with proprioceptors than the intercostal muscles (accessory muscles) (Corda, von Euler,
& Lennerstrand, 1965). If the same is assumed in humans, more sensory input as to the work of
the muscles would be received centrally from the accessory muscles, than from the diaphragm,
potentially giving the impression that more work was being done, with a subsequent increase in
The importance of the inspiratory accessory muscles in the aetiology of dyspnoea is further
supported by evidence that the increase in respiratory effort during fatiguing inspiratory resistive
loading correlated better with the level of activation of muscles of the rib cage and neck muscles
than with the level of activation of the diaphragm (Ward, Eidelman, & Stubbing, 1988). Further,
loaded breathing tasks preferentially fatigue the inspiratory accessory muscles rather than the
diaphragm (Hershenson, Kikuchi, & Tzelepis, 1989). As the force generating capacity of the
diaphragm exceeds that of the accessory muscles (Hershenson, Kikuchi, & Loring, 1988), a
recruitment strategy that brings in the accessory muscles, especially sooner than necessary
through a faulty breathing pattern, may necessitate a greater motor outflow since recruitment of
weaker muscles to achieve a given intra-thoracic pressures change would require a higher level of
motor outflow (McConnell & Romer, 2004a). Minimising, or at least delaying, the recruitment of
41
attenuate or delay the onset of dyspnoea by decreasing sensory input received from the
If dyspnoea was due solely to mechanical or physical changes to the respiratory pump, then one
would expect patients with similar FEV1 (forced expiratory volume in one second; a measure of
lung function) values to report similar levels of dyspnoea. However, Cooper (2006) reported there
was no correlation between these two variables, supporting the theory that dyspnoea may also be
influenced by cerebral factors (ATS, 1998). It is acknowledged that the conscious awareness of
the outgoing respiratory motor command has an important role in the perception of dyspnoea
(McConnell & Romer, 2004a), and that sensory adaptation to intensity of dyspnoea may also
occur (ATS, 1998). Whilst the magnitude of these topics puts them beyond the scope of this
likely affected by a raft of psychological and cerebral components, and any strategies used in an
attempt to attenuate dyspnoea must recognise the subjectivity of the sensation and manage this
Traditionally, the idea that breathing may be a limiting factor to exercise has been disregarded
due to the apparent breathing reserve observed in athletes even at the highest level of exertion
(Dempsey, 1986). This argument was based on the fact that even at exhaustion both trained and
sedentary subjects breathed below their maximal voluntary ventilation (MVV; maximal voluntary
42
ventilation, usually measured in 15 seconds) and therefore still had a significant breathing reserve
to increase ventilation further if needed (breathing reserve is calculated as MVV – VEmax, (Markov
et al., 2001)). For example, immediately after exercise to exhaustion at 85 and 95% of V&O 2 max,
Johnson et al. (1996) recorded values of 70 and 83% MVV respectively in 19 healthy individuals
( V&O 2 max range 40-80ml.kg-1.min-1) after exercise to exhaustion. Similarly, elite cyclists reached
86.9% MVV and professional cyclists 90% MVV (Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro,
1999) indicating that even highly trained athletes, performing at maximal levels of exhaustion,
However, in large studies (N= 578; N=417) comparing reasons for volitional termination in
maximal intensity exercise tests in healthy individuals and those with respiratory pathology, both
Hamilton et al. (1996) and Killian et al. (1992) identified that breathlessness (either alone or in
combination with leg fatigue) contributed equally in both groups. Despite the healthy individuals
having no underlying respiratory pathology and, based on the above studies, presumably ample
breathing reserve (Johnson, Saupe, & Dempsey, 1992; Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, &
Chicharro, 1999), breathlessness still caused some healthy individuals to terminate exercise.
Taking into consideration the apparent breathing reserve, this would suggest that although the
physical capability exists to increase ventilation to meet the metabolic demands of maximal
exercise, factors exist that prevent the person from maximizing their ventilatory effort.
The literature presented in the previous section indicates that breathing may be a factor that
compromises maximal exercise performance if the work of breathing is such that respiratory
muscle fatigue occurs. Increased work of breathing has been shown to increase respiratory
43
muscle fatigue (Johnson, Babcock, Suman, & Dempsey, 1993), decrease blood flow to the
locomotor muscles (Harms et al., 1997) and increase dyspnoea (Grazzini, Stendardi, Gigliotti, &
Scano, 2005) – a combination of which negatively affect exercise performance (Harms, Wetter, St
Croix, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2000). It would therefore be plausible to suggest that strategies that
decrease the work of breathing would attenuate these negative consequences and have a
positive effect on exercise tolerance. Certainly, Harms et al. (2000) demonstrated that decreased
work of breathing increased exercise performance. More specifically, these authors demonstrated
that decreasing the work of breathing using a PAV significantly decreased dyspnoea (0-10 Borg)
at both isotonic (5 minutes, 8.5 ± 0.3 unloaded; 9.6 ± 0.2 control) and at end-exercise
(approximately 9 minutes, 9.6 ± 0.1; 9.9 ± 0.1). Of particular relevance to athletes and sports
performance was that the significant increase in time to exhaustion was most strongly correlated
with the change in dyspnoea (r=-0.54, P ≤ 0.05) rather than changes to RPEleg (r = -0.42, P ≤
One strategy that appears to decrease the symptoms and exercise limiting consequences
associated with poor respiratory mechanics and increased work of breathing is inspiratory muscle
training (Markov et al., 2001; Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002c; Witt, Guenette, Rupert,
McKenzie, & Sheel, 2007). Inspiratory muscle training (IMT) aims to increase the strength and
endurance of the respiratory muscles and requires a person to breathe against a variable
resistance, usually via a hand-held device. A variety of IMT methods exist including voluntary
isocapnic hyperpnoea (Leddy et al., 2007; McMahon, Boutellier, Smith, & Spengler, 2002), flow
resistive loading (Hanel & Secher, 1991) and pressure threshold loading (Romer, McConnell, &
44
Jones, 2002a; Volianitis et al., 2001); each with their own methodological strengths and
Effects on respiratory Only affects velocity axis of Only affects force axis of Affects both force and
mechanics force-velocity relationship force-velocity relationship velocity axis of force-
velocity relationship.
provided by McConnell and Romer (2004b) and therefore will not be covered in depth in this
review. In addition to different modes of IMT, a variety of training durations, intensities and
protocols are typically used in studies investigating the effect of IMT upon exercise performance
45
Table 4. A summary of Exercise Performance Studies using Inspiratory Muscle Training (IMT)
Study Age (years) Subject Data Study Design IMT Intervention Measures Collected Main findings of the effect
of RMT intervention
Morgan, DW Trained male RCT 3/52 VIH VO2max No change in Endurance time
Kohrt, W M cyclists IMT N = 4 Cycling end. 95% VO2max No change VO2max
Bates, B J Control N = 5 RME
Skinner, J S MVV
(1987)
Fairbarn, M S Experienced RCT 4/52 VIH VO2max RME 12%
Coutts, K C male cyclists IMT N = 5 (16 sessions) RME No change VO2max
Pardy, R L Control N = 5 Cycling end. @ 90% Pmax No change cycling endurance
McKenzie, D C time
(1991)
Hanel, B Healthy Humans RCT 27.5 days, 12 min run ↑ MIP 18%
Secher, NH IMT N = 10 30mins, 2 x daily IFRL PImax ↑12 min run 8% (control ↑ 6%)
1991 Control N = 10 VEmax No change VEmax
VO2max No change VO2max
fb ↓fb 5%
Spenger, C M 26.3 ± 3.3 Athletic males No control 4/52 VIH Cycling endurance ↑Cycling endurance 27%
Roos, B N = 20 30 mins, 5 x week Lung function No change lung function
Laube, S M VO2peak ↑VE 23%
Boutellier, U Pmax ↑fb 37.5%
(1999) Lactate ↓lactate after endurance 7%
MVV ↓lactate after incremental 15%
fb ↑MVV 19%
VE No change lung function
Chatham, K 26 ± 14 Healthy, active RCT 8/52 IFRL RRPE ↑ VO2max (Pred.) 3.78%
Baldwin, J 10 Males (5 IMT N = 11 80% MIP PRPE ↑ shuttles
Griffiths, H active) Control N = 11 3 x week VO2max Predicted ↑ RMS 31%
Sumers, L 12 females (6 Shuttle runs ↑ RME 42%
Enright, S active) RMS No change to RRPE
1999 RME No change to PRPE
No change in control
Inbar, O 28.9 ± 8.9 Well trained RCT 10/52 IPTL using threshold PmPeak ↑ PmPeak 10%
Weiner, P endurance track SIMT inspiratory muscle trainer, PImax ↑ PImax 24.9%
Azgad, Y athletes N = 10 30min, 6x / week VEmax No change to VEmax or VO2max
Rotstein, A Placebo N= 10 30-80% WImax VO2max
Weinstein, Y
2000
46
Study Age (years) Subject Data Study Design IMT Intervention Measures Collected Main findings of the effect
of RMT intervention
Markov, G RMT 43 ± 7 Healthy, RCT 40 training sessions over 14 Wpeak ↑ Breathing endurance
Spengler, C ET 40 ± 10 sedentary IMT N = 15 weeks of: VO2peak ↑ Cycling endurance
Knopfli-Lenzin, C Placebo 37 ± 9 subjects ET N = 10 1. VIH 30mins (60%MVV, Cycling Endurance SV unchanged
Steussi, C Males = 20 Placebo N = 15 continuous breathing) Stroke Volume
Boutellier, U Females = 18 2. ET 30 mins running or
2001 cycling
3. No training
Sonetti, D A Competitive male RCT 4/52 IPTL AND VIH MIP ↑ MIP 8%
Wetter, T J cyclists IMT N = 9 3-5 mins IPTL and 30 mins Fixed work rate cycling ↑ Fixed work rate 26% (control
Pegelow, D F Control N = 8 VIH day, 5 x week 8km TT 16%)
Dempsey, J A Pmax ↑ 1.8%
(2001) Lactate ↑Pmax 9% (control 6%)
HR No significant difference
VE between control and IMT in
above variables
No change lactate
No change HR
No change VE
Steussi, C 43 ± 7 Sedentary RCT 40 x 30 min sessions Static lung function ↑ Cycling end 23%
Spengler, C M Males = 16 IMT N = 13 VIH Cycling end @ 70% Pmax ↑ RME 632%
Knopfi-Lenzin, C Females = 12 Control N = 15 Pmax No change lung function
Markov, G HR No change HR
Boutellier, U VE No change VE
(2001) No change Pmax
Volianitis, S 23.8 ± 3.8 Competitive RCT 11/52 IMT 6 min all out 4/52:
McConnell, A K female rowers IMT N = 7 30 inspiratory efforts @ 50% Sub max incremental load ↑ MIP 40.9 %
Koutedakis, Y Control N = 7 MIP 5000m TT 6 min 3.4%
McNaughton, L 2 x daily RRPE 5000m 3.1%
Backx, K Lactate RRPE no change
Jones, D A VT 11/52
(2001) fb ↑ MIP 45.3 % (from baseline)
6 min 3.5% (from baseline
5000m N/A
↓Lactate 1.3% (not different
from control)
↑ VT
No change fb
47
Study Age (years) Subject Data Study Design IMT Intervention Measures Collected Main findings of the effect
of RMT intervention
Williams, J S 20.9 ± 1.2 Cross country No placebo 4/52 IMT with inspiratory PImax ↑31% PImax
Wongsathikun, J runners. resistive device. 25 mins breathing endurance time ↑128% breathing endurance
Boon, S M N=7 4-5 x week VO2peak time
Acevedo, E O Males = 5 50% MIP ↑ by 5% each run time @ 80% VO2peak VO2peak No change
2002 Females = 2 week Run time no change
McMahon, M E RMT = 26 ± 4 Experienced RCT 4-6/52 VIH Wpeak Wpeak no change
Boutellier, U Control 28 ± 6 male cyclists RMT N = 10 IMT = 30 min sessions x Cycling endurance time to ↑ Cycling endurance time
Smith, R M Control N = 10 20, VE = 60% MVV exhaustion @ 85% Wpeak 3.26%
Spengler, C Control = unclear Breathing endurance ↑ Breathing endurance
2002 Peripheral chemoreceptor ↓ Peripheral chemoreceptor
sensitivity sensitivity
Romer, L M IMT 29.5 +/- 3.3 Male DBRCT 6/52 IMT using Inspiratory muscle pressure pre ↓Po change after 20 &
McConnell, A K Placebo 30.3 +/- 2.6 competitive IMT N=8 POWERbreathe and post 20km and 40kmTT. 40kmTT. Intervention group
Jones, D A cyclists Placebo N=8 30 dynamic inspiratory returned to baseline at 30
2002C (5 triathletes) efforts, twice a day. mins, placebo >30mins
Pressure threshold 50% of
WImax
Romer, L M IMT 29.5 +/- 3.3 Male DBRCT 6/52 IMT using VT ↑VT= 0.16L (90%Wmax)
McConnell, A K Placebo 30.3 +/- 2.6 competitive IMT N=8 POWERbreathe Respiratory RPE ↑VT= 0.17L(100%Wmax)
Jones, D A cyclists Placebo N=8 30 dynamic inspiratory Peripheral RPE ↑20kmTT = 65 ± 30s
2002A (5 triathletes) efforts, twice a day. 20kmTT & 40kmTT time ↑40kmTT = 114 ± 38s
Pressure threshold 50% of VO2max ↓RRPE 16 ± 4%
WImax Power output ↓PRPE 18 ± 4%
No change to power or
VO2max
Romer, L M IMT 21.3 ± 1.1 Male sprint DBRCT 6/52 IMT using Recovery time ↓recovery time 6.9 ± 1.3%
McConnell, A K Placebo 20.2 ± 0.7 athletes IMT N=12 POWERbreathe RRPE ↓RRPE 7.9 ± 0.6%
Jones, D A Placebo N=12 30 dynamic inspiratory PRPE ↓PRPE 7.2 ± 0.6%
2002B efforts, twice a day.
Pressure threshold 50% of
WImax
Holm, P Experienced IMT N = 10 4/52 VIH RME ↑ RME 12%
Sutter, A cyclists or Placebo N = 4 20 sessions 30 mins VE ↑ VE 20%
Fregosi, R F triathletes Control N = 6 Gradually increasing fb r VT Fb ↑ Fb significantly” (P = 0.013)
(2004) Males = 4 to achieve 18-19 or RPE VT No change VT
Females = 16 scale HR No change HR
Approx 40km-TT ↑ TT 4.7%
48
Study Age (years) Subject Data Study Design IMT Intervention Measures Collected Main findings of the effect
of RMT intervention
Edwards, A M Healthy, IMT = 10 4/52 IMT using MIP ↑ significantly (P < 0.01)
Cooke, C B physically active Control = 8 POWERbreathe (IPTL) Run time to exhaustion (treadmill) ↑ Run time 4.3% (P < 0.05)
(2004) males 30 dynamic inspiratory Blood lactate No change lactate
efforts, twice a day. Lung function No change lung function
Pressure threshold 50% of VO2 No change VO2
MIP
McConnell, A K IMT = 23.2 ± 2.64 Recreationally RCT 6/52 IMT using MIP ↑ Increased 26%
Sharpe, G R Placebo = 23.8 ± active unclear N = 12 POWERbreathe Cycling Power at MLSS Cycling Power at MLSS no
2005 4.75 whether male vs IMT N = 6 30 dynamic inspiratory Lactate at MLSS change
female? Sham N = 6 efforts, twice a day. ↓ Lactate at MLSS
Pressure threshold 50% of
MIP
Leddy, J J RMT 29 ± 8 Experienced RCT 4/52 VIH PImax PImax no change
Limprasertkul, A Control 34 ± 6 male distance RMT N = 15 RMT = 30 mins, 5 x week PEmax PEmax no change
Patel, S runners Control N = 7 Control = similar protocol VO2peak ↑VO2peak
Modlich, F with no rebreathing bag. Run time @ 80% VO2peak ↑Run time @ 80% VO2peak
Buyea, C 4 mile time ↑4 mile time
Pendergast, D R
Lundgren, C E G
2007
DBRCT = double blind, randomised controlled study; RCT = randomised controlled trial; VIH = Voluntary Isocapnic Hyperpnoea; IFRL = Inspiratory Flow Resistive Loading; IPRL
= Inspiratory Pressure Resistive Loading; WImax = Maximum Inspiratory Work; IMT = Inspiratory Muscle Training; RMT = Respiratory Muscle Training; TT = Time Trial; Po =
Pressure generated by inspiratory muscles at zero flow; VO2max = Maximum oxygen uptake; Vmax = maximum flow; RRPE =respiratory rate of perceived exertion; PRPE =
peripheral rate of perceived exertion; Wmax = Maximum power; SIMT = specific inspiratory muscle training; VEmax = maximal minute ventilation; ET = endurance training; MVV =
maximum voluntary ventilation; VT = tidal volume; SV = stroke volume; Wpeak = Peak power; VO2peak = peak oxygen uptake; VIH = Voluntary Isocapnic Hyperpnea; MLSS =
maximum lactate steady state; RME = respiratory muscle endurance
49
Whilst it is generally accepted that IMT is effective in increasing the strength and endurance of the
respiratory muscles (8 - 45.3% and 12-632% respectively) regardless of mode (see Table 4), some
studies have failed to report positive ergogenic (performance enhancing) effects as a result of
these changes (Fairburn, Coutts, Pardy, & McKenzie, 1991; Hanel & Secher, 1991; Morgan, Kohrt,
Bates, & Skinner, 1987). The lack of performance changes reported by these authors may be due
to methodological factors, rather than inadequacy of the intervention itself. For example, Fairburn
et al. (1991) reported no change to time to exhaustion in a fixed rate (90% V&O 2 max) ride following
RMT despite changes to inspiratory muscle strength and endurance in highly trained cyclists
( V&O 2 max = 66.1± 4.7 mlkgmin-1). However on closer inspection McConnell & Romer (2005)
identified large performance changes were in fact made – for example, a mean 25% increase in
time compared with 4% in the control, but the study had insufficient power due to low subject
Conversely, there are a number of studies that demonstrate IMT produces a range of positive
performance benefits; for example, improved cycling endurance ((24%, P < 0.01) (Markov,
Spengler, Knopfli-Lenzin, Stuessi, & Boutellier, 2001), (3.26 ± 4.29%) (McMahon, Boutellier, Smith,
& Spengler, 2002) (23%; P < 0.05) (Steussi, Spengler, Knopfli-Lenzin, Markov, & Boutellier, 2001))
cycling time trial time (3.8 ± 1.7) (Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002a), recovery time following high
intensity sprints (6.9 ± 1.3%; Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002b), rowing (3.1 ± 0.8%; Volianitis et
al., 2001), running (4.3%; P < 0.05; Edwards & Cooke, 2004) (50%; P < 0.001; Leddy et al., 2007)
and swimming performance (66%; P < 0.001; Wyelegala, Pendergrast, Gosselin, Warkander, &
50
A further benefit of IMT appears to be the ability to decrease dyspnoea – another consequence of
altered respiratory mechanics that may compromise exercise performance. Just as factors that
impair the contractile properties of the respiratory muscles are thought to contribute to dyspnoea,
factors that improve the contractile properties are thought to attenuate it (McConnell & Romer,
inspiratory muscle strength and therefore enabling the inspiratory muscles to create greater length
and tension changes for each breath, in response to exercise (McConnell, 2005; McConnell &
It is not clearly understood how and/or why IMT improves performance. Inspiratory muscle training
has consistently been shown to have no effect on V&O 2 max or traditional parameters of respiratory
function i.e. PEF, FEV1 (Inbar, Weiner, Azgad, Rotstein, & Weinstein, 2000; Romer, McConnell, &
Jones, 2002c). One theory receiving increasing interest is that by delaying or decreasing
respiratory muscle fatigue by increasing inspiratory muscle strength and endurance, the reflex
activity from the type III/IV chemo-sensitive receptors in the respiratory muscles thought to trigger
sympathetic vasoconstriction in the limbs, may be attenuated (McConnell & Romer, 2004b). Witt et
mediated heart rate (83 ± 4 vs. 74 ± 2 beatsmin-1) and mean arterial pressure (99 ± 3 vs. 89 ± 2
mmHg) during a eucapnic resistive breathing task designed to invoke respiratory muscle fatigue
after a five week intervention of IMT. In providing an explanation for why whole-body exercise
improvements are seen following IMT, these authors suggested that the attenuated cardiovascular
response they observed indicates IMT causes blunted sympatho-excitation to resistive inspiratory
51
work, which may subsequently protect locomotor muscle blood flow. McConnell and Sharpe
(2005) demonstrated a significant decrease in blood lactate concentrations (B[La]) at MLSS power
(power at maximal lactate steady state) following six weeks of IMT and suggested that the
Whether IMT is effective in eliciting improved exercise performance and decreased dyspnoea by
decreasing the work of breathing (McConnell & Romer, 2004b; Witt, Guenette, Rupert, McKenzie,
& Sheel, 2007) or by some other means, it appears from studies to date that increasing the
strength and endurance of the respiratory muscles is a common and important finding (Inbar,
Weiner, Azgad, Rotstein, & Weinstein, 2000; Markov et al., 2001; Romer, McConnell, & Jones,
2002a, , 2002b, , 2002c). Interestingly, it does not appear from the methodology of any of the
studies that attention is paid to the pattern of breathing used by subjects when completing their IMT.
In some instances, it might also exacerbate (or reinforce) an already poor breathing pattern. A
recent case history presented by Dickinson, Whyte & McConnell (2007) is the only known paper
that mentions the breathing pattern that is used during IMT. These authors specifically instructed
the athlete to breathe to her diaphragm and to minimise cranial (upwards) shoulder movement.
With exception of the case history of Dickinson et al. (2007), IMT does not appear to specifically
address dynamic hyperinflation or the recruitment order of the inspiratory muscles – rather it looks
to increase inspiratory muscle strength and endurance within the positional constraints of altered
respiratory mechanics. However for athletes (who are presumably able to attain optimal
specifically address factors that are known to contribute to respiratory muscle fatigue and
dyspnoea, for example dynamic hyperinflation and the respiratory muscle recruitment order by
52
addressing the mechanics of breathing, to decrease the work of breathing and optimise exercise
performance.
A strategy that specifically addresses dynamic hyperinflation and respiratory muscle recruitment
pattern, and pays specific attention to breathing pattern both at rest and under load, is breathing
pattern retraining (Blager, 2000; Jones, Dean, & Chow, 2003). Physiotherapists have been using
breathing exercises as a treatment modality for respiratory conditions for many years, with
breathing pattern retraining being recommended for breathing pattern disorders as early as 1938
(Soley & Shock, 1938). Clinically, physiotherapists used diaphragmatic breathing pattern retraining
and/or pursed lip breathing 1 to relieve the symptoms and exercise limitations associated with
COPD (Jones, Dean, & Chow, 2003), VCD (Blager, 2000) and BPDs (Chaitow, Bradley, & Gilbert,
2002; Lum, 1977). Diaphragmatic breathing retraining involves educating the patient of its
importance, discussion of potential feelings of discomfort that may arise during the retraining
process, and demonstration and instruction of how to diaphragmatically breathe correctly (Chaitow,
Bradley, & Gilbert, 2002; Lum, 1977) (refer to Appendix 2 and 3 for a further description of clinical
breathing pattern retraining and BPR exercises). The beneficial effects of diaphragm breathing
may be due to decreased work of breathing through improved respiratory mechanics, decreased
dynamic hyperinflation and increased tolerance and normalisation of PaCO2 through a slower,
deeper respiratory pattern (Falling, 1995; Thoman, Stoker, & Ross, 1966; Tisp et al., 1986).
Pursed Lip Breathing (PLB) is taught clinically to assist airflow in patients with COPD (Jones, Dean,
& Chow, 2003) and VCD (Blager, 2000) and to help elongate the expiratory phase in athletes to
maintain ventilation at appropriate lung volumes (Fallon, 2004). The emphasis is on expiring
1 Normal inhalation with expiration through slightly parted lips, exhaling air over the bottom lip
53
longer than the inspiratory phase to enhance lung emptying (Jones, Dean, & Chow, 2003), avoid a
tachypnoeic shift as exercise intensity increases and to avoid breath stacking. The benefits of PLB
may be due to decreased respiratory rate, changes in pattern of respiratory muscle recruitment,
longer tE and larger VT (Ito, Kakizaki, Tsuzura, & Yamada, 1999; Sharp, Drutz, & Moisan, 1980).
These changes may also lead to a decrease in dyspnoea and work of breathing (ATS, 1998).
Whilst BPR studies have been conducted with success in individuals with respiratory related
pathologies for example asthma (Grossman, DeSwart, & Defares, 1985; Ram, Holloway, & Jones,
2003; Thomas et al., 2003), HVS (Grossman, DeSwart, & Defares, 1985) and VCD (Banez &
Culbert, 2005) no study has been published detailing the potential efficacy of such techniques in
athletic populations. This is surprising given the importance of optimal breathing in athletes, the
numerous factors that can compromise breathing in athletes and the consequence of these on
compromised by the presence of a BPD at rest, alterations to respiratory timing and pattern caused
by the normal respiratory response to exercise, psychological and postural factors, resulting in an
inefficient breathing pattern. It has been shown that an inefficient breathing pattern may increase
the work of breathing (Jones, Dean, & Chow, 2003). Increased work of breathing may cause blood
stealing (Harms, Wetter, St Croix, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2000), decreased power output (Romer &
Dempsey, 2006), increased dyspnoea (McConnell & Romer, 2004a) and decreased exercise time
(Harms, Wetter, St Croix, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2000). Optimising a cyclist’s breathing pattern
could potentially decrease the work of breathing such that blood stealing is delayed or prevented,
power output is maintained for longer, and a decrease or delayed perception of dyspnoea.
Dyspnoea has been identified as one of the limiting factors to exercise performance (Aliverti &
Macklem, 2001). If dyspnoea could be delayed or prevented, this could enable cyclists to both
54
train and compete at a greater intensity. In elite cyclists the margins for improvement through
training alone is thought to be 1-3% (Jeukendrup & Martin, 2001) and in a 40km-TT this small
alteration could decrease race time by up to 1.33 minutes. If dyspnoea could be minimized, higher
quality, more intense and longer training sessions might be possible and the physiological benefits
Although diaphragmatic breathing pattern retraining and PLB are used clinically, there are no
studies reported in the scientific literature that investigate their use in the healthy, athletic
population and the true effect of these breathing exercises in respiratory pathology is unclear. For
example, Gosselink et al (1995) demonstrated that diaphragmatic breathing reduced rather than
enhanced breathing efficiency in patients with severe COPD, concluding that diaphragmatic
breathing contributed to inappropriate chest wall motion and decreased mechanical efficiency while
increasing dyspnoea. Conversely, PLB when combined with diaphragmatic breathing has been
shown to enhance pulmonary mechanics and breathing efficiency, relieve dyspnoea, slow the
respiratory rate, increase tidal volume and help to restore diaphragmatic function (Falling, 1995;
Thoman, Stoker, & Ross, 1966; Tisp et al., 1986). These are all changes that would theoretically
assist athletes in containing the work of breathing, and the consequences associated with this, and
More recently, Jones, Dean and Chow (2003) investigated the differences in V&O 2 and respiratory
rate during three commonly described breathing exercises (PLB, diaphragmatic breathing and a
combination of the two) and spontaneous breathing at rest in patients with COPD. Subjects (N=30)
were ‘experienced’ in all three breathing exercises and could perform them on command. Over a
55
ten minute recording period, respiratory rate was decreased from 17.3 ± 4.23 breaths/min in
spontaneous breathing to 15 ± 4.32 in diaphragmatic breathing, 12.8 ± 3.53 during PLB and 11.2
± 2.7 during combination. V&O 2 decreased significantly from 174.5 ± 25.2 mLO2min-1 in
spontaneous breathing to 165.8 ± 22.3 in diaphragmatic breathing, 164.8 ± 20.9 in PLB and 167.7
± 20.7 in combination breathing indicating that people with COPD adopt a breathing pattern at rest
that is not associated with the least V&O 2 . Whilst significant, the differences observed by Jones et
al. (2003) were recorded at rest, and were only small changes. Theoretically the differences
between spontaneous breathing and the different breathing patterns could be larger at higher
workloads, when more efficient breathing patterns would be desirable. The findings of Jones et al.
(2003) may suggest that the components of respiratory mechanics and respiratory muscle
efficiency may play a greater role in supporting spontaneous breathing than simply minimising
V&O 2 . Interestingly, despite the fact that all patients were “experienced” in the techniques and
were able to adopt the required breathing patterns on command, none of the patterns were
adopted by subjects as their automatic breathing pattern, despite the apparent improved efficiency.
This could indicate that subjects had only learnt the patterns as a motor skill, not achieved the
An explanation for the variable results in studies investigating breathing retraining in COPD may be
due to lack of ‘reprogramming’ or ‘resetting’ of diaphragmatic breathing and PLB as the default or
automatic breathing pattern. No indication was given in either study regarding the length of the
intervention period. It is possible that subjects had ‘learnt’ the techniques and could voluntarily
reproduce them at will, but were unable to maintain these patterns indefinitely therefore reverted
back during spontaneous breathing. According to Grossman et al. (1985), unless the respiratory
56
centres are reprogrammed, patients can only tolerate small decreases in respiratory rate and
increased CO2 can be tolerated for approximately 10 minutes before the urge to revert to
spontaneous breathing becomes overwhelming2. It is possible that neither Gosselink et al. (1995)
or Jones, Dean and Chow (2003) incorporated a sufficient intervention period to allow central
adaptation to occur. Maclennan et al. (1994) and Fabre et al. (2007) attempted to investigate the
effect of entraining breathing with exercise during rowing and roller-ski skating respectively.
Neither author provided an intervention period for participants to adjust to the new breathing
pattern, testing time was greater than 10 minutes, and unsurprisingly neither author identified
It seems apparent that regardless of the mechanisms underpinning how breathing patterns are
‘retrained’, an unknown period of time is required for the new breathing pattern to become the
automatic pattern and therefore become beneficial. This time frame may be longer in athletes who
not only need to adjust to a new breathing pattern at rest, but also need to assimilate it to their
Another factor that needs to be considered when analysing studies of breathing pattern retraining
in COPD is that often the altered breathing pattern is the effect of an impairment in respiratory
mechanics due to the disease process, rather than the symptoms of COPD (for example dyspnoea
and hyperinflation) being caused in the first instance by altered respiratory mechanics. Healthy
humans who have no underlying respiratory pathology should theoretically have no need for
altered respiratory mechanics and should therefore have the ability to achieve an optimal breathing
pattern.
2 The concept of respiratory centres being ‘reprogrammed’ is only a theoretical notion. The precise mechanisms
underpinning how breathing pattern is altered in a BPD, and subsequently ‘retrained’ to demonstrate optimal
respiratory mechanics, is not known.
57
At this stage there is insufficient evidence, predominantly due to poor methodology of those studies
that have been undertaken, to strongly support or reject the use of breathing pattern retraining in
athletes or healthy humans. For example, Lum (1983) reported the effectiveness of breathing
breathing pattern disorder). In a group of more than 1000 patients, 80% became symptom free.
However, the value of this study is limited as no details were given on the exact nature of the
treatment or the instruments used for evaluating therapeutic outcome and no control was used.
Small subject numbers, lack of control, unclear separation of treatment modalities and poor
outcome measures are weaknesses of the six studies critiqued (Table 5) that have investigated the
58
Table 5. Overview of Breathing Pattern Retraining Studies
Study Treatment Group Intervention Control Group Intervention Outcome measure Findings
(N) (N)
Lum (1983) N = 1350 Clinical judgement 80% symptom free
Intervention unclear
Grossman et al. (1985) AR BI Self-report questionnaires e.g. AR > BI, on psychological
7 sessions 7 sessions STAI, EPI measures at 1 month post
N=5 N = 22 Respiratory measures treatment
Van Doorn et al. (1982) BF BI Self report HVS symptoms, BF > BI on HVS symptoms at
7 sessions 4 sessions PetCO2 post-treatment
N = 10 N = 10 BF = BI on HVS symptoms at
3 month follow up
BF = BI on PetCO2 at post
treatment and follow up
Kraft & Hoogduin (1984) 1. Voluntary hyperventilation No control Self report HVS symptoms, 1 = 2 = 3, on psychological
N = 6; 6 sessions HVS attacks, depression measures, at post-treatment
2. Relaxation breathing retraining and one month follow up
N = 6; 6 sessions
3. Stress management
N = 7; 6 sessions
Rapee (1985) B No control Self report panic attacks, Reduction in panic attacks,
3 sessions anxiety, physical symptoms symptoms and anxiety
N=1
De Ruiter et al. (1989) B+E 1. E Self report panic attacks, B + C = 1 = 2, on
8 sessions 8 sessions agoraphobia, PCO2, breathing psychological and respiratory
N = 13 N = 13 frequency measures at post treatment
2. B + C + E
8 sessions
N = 13
AR = auditory regulation; BI = breathing instruction; BF = biofeedback; R = relaxation; C = cognitive restructuring; B = breathing retraining; CT = cognitive therapy; E = exposure;
STAI = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; AR > BI = auditory regulation scored significantly better than breathing instruction, PetCO2 = End-tidal CO2 pressure
59
Clearly there is a paucity of well-designed studies that investigate the effect of breathing pattern
addition to the methodological issues discussed above, nothing can be found in the scientific literature
to indicate how long each individual training session needs to be, the optimal frequency of sessions per
day or per week, or total number of weeks an intervention should be followed in order to retrain a
persons automatic breathing pattern. Further, and more specifically, no studies have been identified
that investigate the effect of breathing pattern retraining on optimising respiratory mechanics in healthy
athletes. Given the high demands placed on the respiratory system during racing and intense training,
and the potential for maladaption in the person’s mechanics of ventilation highlighted above, it has been
previously suggested that optimising an athlete’s breathing pattern would have beneficial effects on
2.5. Summary
Competitive road cycling is a sport where very small improvements in physiology can have a significant
affect on overall performance. Much attention is given to find ways that may enhance a rider’s mean
power during time trials and peak aerobic power during incremental endurance exercise.
An area that has not been explored to date is the mechanisms of breathing and the effect that
optimising a cyclist’s breathing pattern may have on performance. This is surprising given the
significant increases in ventilatory demands observed during maximal intensity cycling. To meet these
ventilatory demands both breathing frequency and tidal volume are altered. In most athletes this is
achieved by shortening the expiratory time rather than the inspiratory time for each breath – a change
that is associated with a tachypnoeic shift, breath stacking, dynamic hyperinflation and a subsequent
increase in the work of breathing. Increased work of breathing, particularly to the point of respiratory
60
muscle fatigue, has been associated with decreased blood flow to the leg muscles during high intensity
If increased work of breathing has a negative effect on cycling performance, it would be reasonable to
argue that strategies that decrease the work of breathing may have a positive effect. Whilst respiratory
muscle training has been shown to decrease the work of breathing and increase exercise performance,
it fails to specifically address the pattern of breathing. Breathing pattern retraining is an intervention
hyperinflation and delaying the onset of respiratory muscle fatigue. Theoretically, optimising an efficient,
diaphragmatic breathing pattern in cyclists would serve to decrease the work of breathing, preserve
blood flow to the legs, decreases perceptions of effort which collectively would enable a cyclist to
Despite the apparent benefit to cycling performance, no studies have investigated, to date, the effect
that breathing pattern retraining could have on performance in cycling. Given the strength of theoretical
argument presented in this review and the lack of existing research into the area of breathing pattern
retraining in cyclists, an investigation into the effect of breathing pattern retraining on exercise
61
Chapter Three: Methods
3.1. Participants
Twenty-four competitive male cyclists and triathletes from various cycling and triathlon clubs within the
greater Auckland area were recruited for this study by flyer distribution and direct contact to clubs
(Intervention Group N = 12, Control Group N = 12; match paired on pre-intervention 20km-TT time ).
Two participants withdrew from the intervention group during the study – one due to an acute respiratory
illness, the other due to the inability to comply with standardising his training schedule. All participants
were informed of the risks associated with the testing and the requirements of participation both verbally
and in written form, and were given the opportunity to have any questions answered. Prior to
participation, all participants gave their written informed consent in accordance with Auckland University
Since it is not possible to teach a person “sham breathing” without affecting their breathing pattern in
some way, some deception as to the true aims of the study was applied. In order to have a true control
group, full disclosure of the real purpose of the study was not provided to participants at its onset.
Further written information about the true study purpose was provided and written consent was gained
from the participants randomised into the intervention group. All participants were aware they could
Participants’ characteristics are presented in Table 6. The majority of participants competed regularly in
club level races or higher, in either road cycling or triathlon. Participants were excluded from the study if
they were current smokers, had suffered an acute respiratory illness or injury that had affected training
within the previous four weeks, or had previously received any formal breathing retraining or advice.
62
The study took place in the competitive phase of the athlete’s season. Participants were training and
competing regularly at the time of the testing, with an average riding volume of 246 ± 121.8 km per
week (range 145-580 km). Training volume over the period of the study was not set by the investigator,
however a requirement of inclusion in the study was that participants were in a stable phase of training
and were requested to keep their training volume consistent over the duration of the study. To monitor
the actual training load throughout the study, participants were given a heart rate monitor (PolarS625x;
Polar Electro Oy, Kemplele, Finland) and a training diary (Appendix 4) to record training data.
Intervention* Control*
Age (yr) 38.6 ± 5.9 37 ± 10.6
Body Mass (kg) 78.3 ± 11.2 84 ± 10
Height (cm) 176.1 ± 6 182 ± 4.3
Years Competing 5.3 ± 2.8 3.9 ± 3
Hours per week training 8.8 ± 2.3 7.3 ± 4.1
3.2. Equipment
3.2.1. Ergometer
An electro-magnetically braked cycle ergometer (Velotron, Racemate Inc, Seattle, USA) was used for all
physiological assessments and sub-maximal rides during the intervention period. This model of
ergometer utilises an electro-magnetic braking system to increase the amount of force applied to the
rear wheel of the machine, where the assessor can predetermine the distance required to complete (as
with the 20km-TT), or the level of resistance (i.e. power output) on the flywheel, independent of pedaling
cadence (as with the incremental step test). This ergometer allowed for the design of different
‘protocols’ or ‘courses’, which in this study were the incremental step test and sub-maximal intervention
63
3.2.2. Pulmonary Gas Exchange and Spirometry
An automated breath-by-breath system (Metamax 3B, Cortex, Leipzig, Germany) was used to record
pulmonary gas exchange measures during the pre- and post-intervention incremental test. The same
system was used to perform spirometry assessment in the same sessions. This system has a proven
reliability and validity when compared to the Douglas bag as the traditional gold standard measurement
Calibration of the gas analysis system was performed prior to and immediately after all assessments,
using a two-point calibration procedure. This involved calibrating the apparatus to ambient air, which
was assumed to be 20.93% O2 and 0.03% CO2, and then to a known mixture of high-tolerance
calibration gas (BOC, Auckland, NZ), composed of 14.82% O2 and 4.80% CO2. The two-point
calibration and ambient air checking procedure was repeated until acceptable values (± 0.02%) were
reached. Following the gas calibration, the flow-volume transducer was calibrated using a three litre
syringe (Hans Rudolph, US). This calibration was verified with three different ventilation rates,
Blood lactate concentration was measured using the Lactate Pro lactate analyser (Akray, Tokyo, Japan).
Studies by Pyne, Boston, Martin, and Logan (2000) and Buckley, Bourdon and Woolford (2003) support
the accuracy, reliability and versatility of this device when compared to other devices such as the Abl
700 Series Acid-Base Analyser 2, the Accusport Lactate Meter, the YSI 2300 Stat lactate analyser and
Prior to each use, calibration of the lactate pro device was checked using the magnetic strip provided by
the manufacturer of the lactate testing strips. Blood samples were taken from the right ear lobe after the
64
lobe had been cleaned with an alcohol swab to ensure an adequate blood sample was returned, and
that this sample was not contaminated by sweat. The same Lactate Pro device was used for all
participants.
Rating of perceived effort for leg and breathing effort was recorded using the modified Borg Scale
(MBS) (Appendix 5). This rating scale (0-10) was used in the incremental power test and the 20km time
trial. Although the Borg scale is typically used for patients with respiratory disease, normal healthy
people with exercise induced dyspnoea are also able to distinguish between different levels of
breathlessness using the MBS (Wilson & Jones, 1989). The same scale, modified to reflect leg effort
rather than breathlessness, was used so as not to confuse participants with different scales.
The Nijmegen Questionnaire (Appendix 1) was used as a general screening tool to identify the presence
of an underlying breathing pattern disorder (BPD) in the participants. The Nijmegen Questionnaire is a
list of sixteen signs and symptoms that, grouped together, are used as a screening tool for anxiety-
based BPDs. There are no specific objective measures to identify abnormal respiratory mechanics or
BPDs in athletes. The sensitivity of the questionnaire in relation to the clinical diagnosis of anxiety-
based BPDs is reported to be 91% and the specificity 95% (Van Dixhoorn & Duivenvoorden, 1984).
A newly developed breathing harness (Zephyrtech, Auckland, New Zealand) was used with the control
group during the intervention period. The harness is worn as two straps, one around the upper chest
and the other around the abdomen (Figure 4). Each strap has a sensor that detects expansion and
65
contraction of the underlying thoracic cavity. The upper chest sensor is positioned on the lateral aspect
of the chest wall, and the lower sensor is positioned midline, on the upper abdomen.
The harness was used to differentiate between upper and lower chest breathing. Each of the sensors
capture the amplitude of the expansion and contraction of the ribcage during inspiration and expiration
respectively, and this data is presented in real time on a computer screen. The breathing harness is still
in the developmental stages therefore there is no reliability or validity data to support its use. However,
because the primary reason for using the harness was to create a “research purpose” for the control
group (see Intervention Sessions - Control Group, 3.3.8.), this lack of data was not deemed important.
All tests were performed at AUT University’s Human Performance Laboratory located at the Millennium
Institute of Sport and Health, Auckland, New Zealand. The laboratory was well ventilated and
environmental conditions were standardised. The laboratory was temperature-controlled within the
66
Participants were required to attend the laboratory ten times over a period of seven weeks (Figure 5).
During session one the medical prescreening questionnaires were analysed, height and body mass (in
their riding clothes, without shoes) were recorded and participants underwent pulmonary function testing
(spirometry), immediately followed by a familiarisation incremental power step test. During session two
participants performed a 20km time trial (TT) familiarisation test. Sessions three and four were the
formal baseline (pre-intervention) sessions for spirometry and the incremental power test, and the
20km-TT test respectively. Following baseline assessments, participants completed the intervention
(sessions 5-8), then post-intervention assessments (sessions 9 and 10) in the order above.
F F ST TT I ST TT
-2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 +1
Figure 5. Time line (weeks) of familiarisation trials (F), formal incremental step test (ST) and 20km
time trial (TT) and intervention period (I).
The incremental step test (including spirometry) and 20km-TT (for each of the familiarisation, pre- and
post-intervention testing sessions) were completed at least 48 hours apart. To minimise sources of
variation, the time between each of the tests, time of day and day of the week were kept consistent for
Prior to the first assessment the participant’s own bike dimensions were measured and recorded so that
the cycle ergometer resembled the riders preferred bike setup as closely as possible. This included the
height, reach and angle of the handle bars, as well as the height, fore and aft position, and angle of the
seat. Additionally, participant’s own pedals were attached to the cranks to allow them to ride in their
own shoes. Prior to the first assessment participants were able to request changes to any of the bike’s
dimensions during their warm-up. Once confirmed, this setup was recorded and held constant for all
67
To minimise the effect of nutrition on performance, food and fluid intake were kept consistent for pre-
and post-testing. Participants were instructed to submit a food diary of the food and fluid consumed and
at what time during the 24 hours preceding the formal incremental power and 20km-TT pre tests. This
food diary was returned to participants for replication prior to their post-intervention tests. Participants
were instructed not to consume any caffeinated beverages in the 12 hours preceding each test and to
abstain from exhaustive or prolonged exercise for at least 48 hours prior to each test. All participants
3.3.2. Spirometry
Each participant’s vital capacity (VC), forced vital capacity (FVC), forced expiratory volume in one
second (FEV1), peak expiratory flow (PEF), maximum voluntary ventilation (MVV), inspiratory and
expiratory lung volumes (IRV and ERV respectively) and tidal volume (VT) were determined in
accordance with American Thoracic Society guidelines (Miller et al., 2005). For all tests, participants sat
in an upright position and wore a nose-clip to eliminate nose breathing. Participants completed three
different procedures with a small rest (one to two minutes) between each test. A procedure was
stopped and repeated if a participant bent forward at the waist in an attempt to maximise the expiratory
phase, or if there were symptoms suggestive of upper airway occlusion during the maximal flow-volume
The first procedure required participants to be quietly seated with the mouth piece and nose clip in situ,
breathing normally. When they were ready a maximal inhalation was taken, followed by a controlled but
maximal exhalation. Normal tidal breathing with the mouth piece and nose clip still in situ was
commenced immediately after the maximal exhalation. After three or four tidal breaths the maneuver
68
was repeated. This procedure was repeated three times before the mouth piece was removed. The
highest values for VC, IRV, ERV and VT were recorded from this procedure.
The second procedure (maximal flow-volume loop) required a maximal inhalation to be taken with the
nose clip on but without the mouth piece in situ. The participant then inserted the mouth piece at the
peak of the inhalation, ensuring a firm seal was achieved, and exhaled as hard, fast and completely as
possible. When the participant felt they could not exhale any further (approximately 6s) a forceful, fast
and maximal inspiration was performed. When no further inhalation could be taken the participant
removed the mouth piece and breathed normally until they were ready to repeat the procedure. This
procedure was repeated until three consistently reproducible efforts were achieved (within 0.15L of each
other; Miller et al., 2005)), as determined by computer analysis. The highest values for FVC, FEV1 and
The final spirometry measure was the MVV test. In the same testing position used above, participants
were instructed to “breathe as hard, fast and deeply (in and out) as possible” for 12 seconds.
Participants were instructed to sit quietly after completion of the 12 seconds and recover their breathing.
They were advised prior to the test that they may feel dizzy or light headed during the last few seconds
or on completion of this test and that this feeling would soon pass. Lucia et al (1999) in their study with
competitive elite cyclists measured FVC and MVV in a standing position. Although the present study
was also done with cyclists it was decided to use a seated position for two reasons: a) cycling is
performed in a seated (albeit forward leaning) position so it seemed more appropriate to and b) since
the MVV test is a maximal hyperventilatory test it is not uncommon for participants to feel faint or light
headed during this test and it was simply deemed safer to have participants in a seated rather than
standing position.
69
3.3.3. Incremental Power Step Test
Each participant’s peak oxygen uptake ( V&O 2 peak), lactate threshold (LT), lactate turn point (LTP) and
maximum aerobic power (Pmax) were determined using an incremental step test to volitional exhaustion.
Prior to this test participants had completed spirometry procedures, as described above. The face mask
was then attached, ensuring a firm seal with no air leaks. The ergometer was calibrated in accordance
with the manufacturer’s instructions and when acceptable variations were achieved (<1%) participants
commenced their 6 minute self selected warm-up. Participants did not have to use the whole 6 minutes
and were able to perform any final stretches before commencing the test. The self selected warm up for
the pre-intervention tests was recorded for each participant, and repeated prior to their post-intervention
tests. Final calibration checks of the gas analysis system were performed during this warm-up period
The incremental test for most participants started at 150W and consisted of a continuous step test
protocol with three minute stages. However, to cater for a range of fitness levels, the starting power
was adjusted to ensure that each participant completed at least seven but not more than nine stages.
The starting power for each participant was the same for the pre- and post-test. Power output was
progressively increased by 30W at the end of each three minute stage until exhaustion. The test was
terminated when the participant stopped completely, or when the participant could no longer hold a
cadence above 60 rev.min-1. Participants were instructed to stay seated throughout the duration of the
test, and to try to maintain a consistent cadence. A designated set cadence was not used for either the
incremental test or the 20km-TT since each cyclist develops power at his own pedaling rate (Zavorsky
et al., 2007) and a fixed cadence may not have resulted in an accurate reflection of a participant’s
70
Prior to commencement of the test participants were shown the modified (0-10) Borg scale (Kendrick,
Baxi, & Smith, 2000), its use was explained to them and they had time to become familiar with it.
Twenty seconds before the end of each stage participants were asked to rate their perceived leg effort,
followed by their breathing effort. This order was maintained during all testing, to ensure participants
were clear whether they were describing their leg or breathing effort. In addition, a blood lactate sample
was taken from the participant’s right ear lobe in the last 15 seconds of each stage. Heart rate and
Consistent feedback was given to each participant at the end of each power stage. Maximal verbal
encouragement was given to each participant when it was obvious they were in the final stage of their
test. There were always two assessors (primary investigator and research assistant) present for each
test. To avoid bias in the feedback, and therefore to avoid the influence of externally motivating factors,
the assistant assessor provided the maximal encouragement in the last stage and was blinded as to
The Nijmegen questionnaire was completed prior to the pre-intervention 20km-TT. As no anxiety based
BPDs were identified using this tool in either group, the questionnaire was not repeated post-
intervention. The primary investigator read out each of the 16 signs and symptoms listed in Appendix 1
and participant was asked to rate them in accordance with the frequency chart provided (Appendix 1),
over the previous six month period. Participants were only told that it was a breathing related
71
3.3.5. 20km Time Trial
Laboratory based time trials (TT) of varying distances have previously been shown to be both reliable
and valid measures of cycling performance (Laursen, Shing, & Jenkins, 2003). More recently, Zavorsky
et al (2007) has demonstrated that a familiarisation session improves the reliability of the formal pre-
and post-testing measurements. A familiarisation TT followed by a second TT has a larger CV than that
observed between the second and a third TT. Given that the smallest worthwhile meaningful change
can be as little as 0.5-1.5% (Paton & Hopkins, 2001) it was considered important to include a
familiarisation trial so as not to confuse typical error with performance improvement. Therefore, over the
course of the study participants complete three 20km-TT’s – a familiarisation trial, and both a pre- and
post-intervention trial.
Participants presented to the laboratory at least 48 hours after their incremental power test, having
followed the same guidelines as the incremental test with respect to exhaustive exercise, food and
caffeinated beverages. The TT protocol allowed participants to change gears like they would on a road
bike. Prior to the familiarisation trial participants had the opportunity to become familiar with the gear
After calibration of the ergometer and a six minute self-selected warm-up (consistent for pre- and post-
testing), participants were asked to bring the ergometer to a stop, so a “standing start” (seated, with no
movement of the cranks) could be achieved. The ergometer had been programmed to run a 20km-TT
protocol, such that when the program was started (five second count down for participants) the time
clock ran until automatically stopping once the 20kms had been reached. Participants were able to
select their own gearing and cadence and could alter either of these as needed throughout the test.
Participants remained seated throughout the entire TT, with their hands on either the brake hoods or the
top bar. These riding positions were chosen, as opposed to the aerodynamic position, to enable a
72
better comparison to the breathing patterns observed by Lucia et al. (1999) who also used these testing
At five minute intervals HR, power, speed, leg and lung RPE were recorded. Standardised verbal
encouragement was given to participants at the 5, 10, 15, 19 and 19.5 km mark (for example “that’s 5km,
going well”). Standardised maximal verbal encouragement was given in the last 500m of the test.
In an attempt to have two even groups based on performance measures, participants were match
paired based on their pre-intervention 20km-TT time. Randomisation to either the intervention or control
group was done by the toss of a coin, such that the individual in the pair whose christian name was
alphabetically first was a “head”, the other “tails”. Whoever corresponded with the side of the coin that
During session five, participants in the intervention group were told the true purpose of the study (i.e.
that the investigators were interested in whether breathing pattern could be retrained in cyclists) and
given a concise but comprehensive overview of efficient versus inefficient breathing, the consequences
of each, the basic mechanics of breathing and how breathing can be retrained. In an attempt to
minimise the placebo effect on the outcome of the study participants were not told that the effect on
performance of changing respiratory pattern was of primary interest to the investigators. Participants
were taught the two breathing exercises (static exercises) (Appendix 3) to be completed at home. The
ways in which the static breathing exercises differed from the ideal breathing pattern for on the bike
were discussed and demonstrated (dynamic exercise) but the participant did not perform the dynamic
exercises during this session. Participants had the opportunity to have any questions answered and
73
were given the opportunity to withdraw from the study if they did not wish to continue with the new study
protocol. Participants were provided with a CD with spoken instructions and a counted rhythm for the
10 minute static breathing exercise, 10 gold star stickers to be used as visual triggers, and written hand
outs of the exercises (Appendix 3), potential symptoms they may experience during BPR (Appendix 6)
and main points that were discussed during this session (Appendix 7).
During session six, participants demonstrated their 10 minute static breathing exercise technique and
feedback was provided with appropriate changes made. Following this, participants completed a 10
minute sub-maximal ride (60% of Pmax) during which they were taught to apply the diaphragmatic
pattern with emphasis on the expiratory phase (dynamic breathing exercise). Sessions seven and eight
each involved a 10 minute sub maximal (60% of Pmax) ride with two 30 second maximal efforts. During
each maximal effort the emphasis was on breathing control and on recovering one’s respiratory rate
Participants were encouraged to continue using their dynamic breathing skills during training in addition
Participants who were randomised into the control group were led to believe the purpose of the study
was twofold:
Over the four weeks of the intervention period participants completed one 10 minute sub maximal (60%
of Pmax) ride each week wearing the breathing harness. Participants were asked to rate their leg and
74
When participants wore the breathing harness for the first time they were shown how it worked and
were able to view the computer screen to observe the different amplitudes of the apical and
diaphragmatic breaths in real time. However, during the sub-maximal rides the screen was turned away
from them. Participants were told that this was to prevent them from pacing or consciously altering their
breathing. No formal data was collected from the breathing harness, or from the participants, during this
intervention period. No feedback on adopted breathing pattern was given to the control group.
As the stages of the peak power incremental test were three minutes in duration, final maximum power
(Pmax) was measured as the participants’ last fully completed three minute stage, plus 0.16W for each
3.4.2. Lactate
The LT was defined as the first deflection point of 1 mmol.L-1 or greater above baseline (Yoshida, Chida,
Ichioka, & Suda, 1987) in the Lactate (B[La]) vs. power output curve. LTP was defined as the point in
the B[La] vs. power output curve before the observation of a second sudden and sustained increase in
B[La] that coincided with a B[La] of between approximately 2-5 mmol.L-1 (Davis, Bassett, Hughes, &
Gass, 1983). All LT and LTP analyses were performed by two independent examiners, and where
V&O 2 max represents the maximum rate of oxygen that can be inhaled and delivered to and used by the
working muscles during maximal exercise (Astrand & Rodahl, 1986; Saltin & Astrand, 1967). The
V&O 2 max is often identified by a plateau in V&O 2 despite continued increases in exercise intensity.
75
However, a plateau in V&O 2 is not always observed (Doherty, Nobbs, & Noakes, 2003; Lucia et al.,
2006) and along with the finding that different protocols (e.g. discontinuous step versus continuous
ramp) and different exercise modes (e.g. cycling versus running) may yield different maximal results,
(Lucia et al., 2006) it has been proposed that it may be more accurate to term this the peak, rather than
the maximum, V&O 2 (Day, Rossiter, Coats, Skasick, & Whipp, 2003); i.e. the V&O 2 peak. For the purpose of this study,
V&O 2 peak was defined as the peak value achieved during the incremental assessment, averaged over a
30 second period. To quantify the O2 cost of cycling (cycling economy) the mean V&O 2 in the last 30
All V&O 2 and V& C O2 data was recorded breath-by-breath, and to reduce the effect of breath-by-breath
variability, outliers were removed using in-built functions in the Cortex software, thereby reducing the
‘noise’. For determination of V&O 2 peak data during the incremental assessment was averaged on a 30
second basis. Each participant’s absolute V&O 2 peak was recorded as the highest 30-second V&O 2 value
The Nijmegen Questionnaire is scored based on the frequency that a participant experiences the
described signs and symptoms (Appendix 1). The maximum score is 64, and a score greater than or
76
3.4.5. Statistics
Once individual values had been obtained they were entered into an MS Excel statistics spreadsheet
designed by Hopkins (2003) and values were log-transformed. To make inferences about true
(population) values of the effect of BPR on cycling performance, the uncertainty in the effect was
expressed as 90% confidence limits and as likelihoods that the true value of the effect represents
substantial change (Batterham & Hopkins, 2005). An effect was deemed unclear if its confidence
interval overlapped the thresholds for substantiveness; that is, if the effect could be substantially positive
and negative. An estimate of the smallest substantial change in power output was required to make
these inferences. Paton and Hopkins (2001) estimated smallest effects of 0.5-1.5% in mean power,
based on variability in competitive performance of elite cyclists in various time trials where drafting and
group tactics did not contribute. A smallest worthwhile effect of 1.0% was assumed. Pearson
correlations were performed to identify the strength of relationship between changes in RPE, fb, Pmax,
77
Chapter Four: Results
The baseline performance and physiological characteristics of the 22 participants are presented in Table
7.
The Nijmegan Questionnaire did not identify any anxiety-based breathing pattern disorders in either
group, with the mean scores for both the intervention group (6.4 ± 5.06) and the control group (8.55 ±
3.75) being well below a score of 23 necessary to be indicative of an anxiety-based breathing pattern
disorder.
78
Attendance to each intervention session was high for both the intervention (100%) and control (98.3%)
groups. Compliance with the BPR was 89%, and all participants were able to accurately and
appropriately perform their static and dynamic exercises, indicating they had practiced regularly.
Table 8 shows the effect of BPR on cycling performance. After 4 weeks of BPR there were substantial
changes to Pmax (3.2 ± 3%) in the incremental step test and to Pave (3.1 ± 2.9%), average speed (1.3 ±
Table 8. Mean Changes in Performance Measures post Breathing Pattern Retraining and Control and
Chances That the True Difference in the Changes Is Substantial
Change in Measure*
Intervention Control Difference Cohen’s ES Practical Inference
(N=10) (N=12) ± 90% CL ± 90% CL† Descriptive
Incremental
Pmax 1.8 ± 4.5 -1.4 ± 3.5 3.2 ± 3.0 0.32 ± 0.3 Very likely +ve
20kmTT
PAve 3.1 ± 4.5 0.0 ± 2.9 3.1 ± 2.9 0.3 ± 0.27 Very likely +ve
SpeedAve 1.3 ± 2.0 0.0 ± 1.1 1.3 ± 1.2 0.34 ± 0.32 Very likely +ve
Time -1.5 ± 1.8 0.0 ± 1.0 -1.5 ± 1.1 -0.38 ± 0.28 Very likely +ve
A comparison of the physiological measures determined during the incremental test to exhaustion in the
two groups revealed no clear differences (Table 9). Although the effect of BPR on lactate and heart rate
at LT and LTP was unclear, there was a trend for both of these variables to be decreased in the
intervention group at LTP (-4.6 ± 16.6%; -2.3 ± 2.5% respectively). All other measures were unclear.
79
Table 9. Mean changes in Physiological Measures post Breathing Pattern Retraining and
Control and Chances That the True Difference in the Changes is Substantial
Change In Measure*
Intervention Control Difference Cohen’s ES Practical Inference
(N=10) (N=12) mean ± SD ± 90% CL† Descriptive‡
Power at LT -2.7 ± 6.0 -0.5 ± 13.4 -2.2 ± 7.3 -0.13 ± 0.41 unclear
Power at LTP 1.7 ± 6.6 -1.4 ± 10.0 3.1 ± 6.1 0.21 ± 0.40 unclear
HRmax 0.1 ± 2.2 -0.1 ± 2.0 0.0 ± 1.6 -0.01 ± 2.80 unclear
B[La] at LT 13.8 ± 21.7 10.8 ± 22.8 2.7 ± 16.0 0.08 ± 0.46 unclear
B[La] at LTP -0.9 ± 22.0 3.9 ± 24.2 -4.6 ± 16.6 -0.17 ± 0.54 unclear
HR at LT 0.9 ± 3.7 2.5 ± 3.9 -1.5 ± 2.8 -0.19 ± 0.34 unclear
HR at LTP -0.7 ± 3.0 1.7 ± 3.5 -2.3 ± 2.5 -0.3 ± 0.31 unclear
HRave 2.5 ± 2.3 0.2 ± 4.9 2.3 ± 2.9 0.32 ± 0.40 unclear
VO2 at LT (Lmin-1) -0.5 ± 7.1 0.0 ± 6.7 -0.4 ± 5.1 -0.03 ± 0.35 unclear
VO2 at LTP (Lmin-1) -0.7 ± 6.2 0.3 ± 7.6 1.0 ± 5.1 0.08 ± 0.38 unclear
VCO2 at LT (Lmin-1) 0.7 ± 6.9 -1.5 ± 7.2 2.3 ± 5.2 0.14 ± 0.31 unclear
VCO2 at LTP (Lmin-1) 0.7 ± 7.0 0.7 ± 6.4 1.4 ± 4.9 0.09 ± 0.31 unclear
VO2 at LT (% VO2peak) -1.0 ± 5.9 4.3 ± 11.1 -5.1 ± 6.3 0.58 ± 0.68 unclear
VO2 at LTP (% VO2peak) -0.2 ± 5.0 0.9 ± 6.9 -1.2 ± 4.3 -0.15 ± 0.57 unclear
VO2max (Lmin-1) 0.5 ± 6.9 -0.4 ± 6.4 1.0 ± 4.9 0.09 ± 0.45 unclear
VO2max (mlkgmin-1) 0.7 ± 6.6 -0.5 ± 6.2 1.2 ± 4.7 0.08 ± 0.31 unclear
The effect of BPR on static and dynamic lung function tests was unclear for all measures, except for
FVC where the effect was deemed unlikely to be beneficial. Whilst there was a trend of both IRV (18 ±
36.4%) and ERV (9.8 ± 44.3%) to increase, and for FEV1 and MVV to decrease, the 90% CI associated
The effect of BPR on tI (10.1 ± 8%; 9.4 ± 7.7%) and VT (11.6 ± 14%; 12.6 ± 11.2%) at both LT and LTP
respectively was substantial. There was a strong trend for tE to be increased at both LT and LTP (5.0 ±
9%; 5.9 ± 7.1%). There was a clear trend for VE at both LT (-5.6 ± 4%) and LTP (-5.2 ± 4%) to be lower
post-BPR. Similarly, breathing frequency (fb) was also substantially lower both at LT (-13.2 ± 8.9%) and
LTP (-9.5 ± 8.4%). The effect on respiratory drive (VT/tI) and respiratory timing (tI/tTOT) at LT and LTP
was unclear. Table 10 shows the mean changes in respiratory measures for the BPR relative to the
control condition and the statistics for the differences in the changes.
80
Table 10. Mean Changes in Respiratory Measures (non-exercise and exercise) post Breathing
Pattern Retraining and Control and Chances That the True Difference in the Changes Is
Substantial
Change in Measure*
Intervention Control Difference Cohen’s ES Practical Inference
(N=10) (N=12) ± 90% CL ± 90% CL Descriptive‡
Lung Function Tests
IRV 6.3 ± 35.3 -10 ± 62.7 18 ± 36.4 0.33 ± 0.61 unclear
ERV 8.7 ± 79.7 -1.1 ± 37.7 9.8 ± 44.3 0.2 ± 0.78 unclear
FVC -2.8 ± 5.9 2.7 ± 8.1 -5.4 ± 5.1 0.44 ± 0.39 probably -ve
FEV1 -1.7 ± 8.1 0.9 ± 6.5 -2.7 ± 5.4 -0.19 ± 0.37 unclear
PEF -0.6 ± 5.9 -2.5 ± 16.3 1.98 ± 8.7 0.09 ± 0.39 unclear
MVV -4.9 ± 16.7 5.5 ± 30.6 -9.9 ± 17.1 -0.71 ± 1.07 unclear
Incremental Test
tE at LT (sec-1) 12.1 ± 13.6 6.8 ± 10.3 5.0 ± 9 0.25 ± 0.43 unclear
tE at LTP (sec-1) 10 ± 10.7 3.9 ± 8.2 5.9 ± 7.1 0.29 ± 0.35 unclear
tI at LT (sec-1) 10.8 ± 13.4 0.7 ± 6.3 10.1 ± 8 0.42 ± 0.34 Very likely +ve
tI at LTP (sec-1) 11.8 ± 12.4 2.1 ± 7.2 9.4 ± 7.7 0.43 ± 0.35 Very likely +ve
VT at LT (L) 11.6 ± 14 0.9 ± 7.2 10.6 ± 8.5 0.53 ± 0.42 Very likely +ve
VT at LTP (L) 12.6 ± 11.2 2.9 ± 9.4 9.4 ± 7.6 0.47 ± 0.39 Very likely +ve
VE at LT (L·min-1) -5-5 ± 5.7 -0.1 ± 5 -5.6 ± 4 -0.33 ± 0.22 Very likely +ve
VE at LTP (L·min-1) -4.1 ± 4 1.2 ± 6.7 -5.2 ± 4 -0.32 ± 0.23 Very likely +ve
VEmax (L·min-1) -2.2 ± 11 -2.2 ± 11.5 0.0 ± 8.2 0.0 ± 0.52 unclear
VT/tI at LT (L·sec-1) 0.7 ± 16.8 0.2 ± 3.5 0.5 ± 9.6 0.03 ± 0.46 unclear
VT/tI at LTP (L·sec-1) -2.9 ± 9.8 0.8 ± 9.4 -3.6 ± 7 -0.2 ± 0.37 unclear
fb at LT (1·min-1) -14.5 ± 14.4 -1.6 ± 8 -13.2 ± 8.9 -0.71 ± 0.43 Very likely +ve
fb at LTP (1·min-1) -11.3 ± 13.5 -2.0 ± 7.9 -9.5 ± 8.4 -0.51 ± 0.41 Very likely +ve
tI/tTOT at LT -1.2 ± 4.1 -2.1 ± 4.1 0.9 ± 3.8 0.14 ± 0.57 unclear
tI/tTOT at LTP -0.5 ± 5 -0.8 ± 3.2 0.3 ± 3.2 0.05 ± 3.2 unclear
Several measures were compared over a range of absolute workloads pre- and post-intervention
(Figures 6 to 10).
At each workload, fb decreased (Figure 6) and VT increased following BPR (Figure 6). There was a
81
Breath in g freq u en cy (b reath s•m in 1 )
60.00 70.00
50.00
40.00
40.00
30.00
30.00
20.00
20.00 Pre BPR
Pre BPR Post BPR
10.00 10.00
Post BPR
0.00 0.00
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
3.50 4.00
3.50
3.00 3.00
T id al V o lu m e (L )
T id al V o lu m e (L )
2.50
2.50
2.00
2.00 1.50
Pre BPR Pre BPR
1.00
1.50 Post BPR Post BPR
0.50
1.00 0.00
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
160 160
M inu te V entilation (L •m in-1)
M in ute V entilation (L•m in -1)
140 140
120 120
100 100
80 80
Pre BPR
60 60
Pre BPR Post BPR
40 Post BPR 40
20 20
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
Figure 6. Effect of breathing pattern retraining on breathing frequency, tidal volume and minute ventilation in the
intervention (left panel) and control groups (right panel).
As anticipated, the tI and tE decreased with the progressive increase in workload. After BPR, both tI
82
1.80 1.80
Pre BPR 1.60
1.60 Pre BPR
Post BPR
1.40 Post BPR
In sp irato ry T im e (s)
In sp irato ry T im e (s)
1.40
1.20
1.20 1.00
1.00 0.80
0.60
0.80
0.40
0.60 0.20
0.40 0.00
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
Power (W) Power (W)
2.00 2.00
1.80 1.80
Pre BPR Pre BPR
1.60
E xp irato ry T im e (s)
E xp irato ry T im e (s)
Figure 7. Effect of Breathing pattern retraining on inspiratory and expiratory time in the intervention (left panel)
and control (right panel) groups.
Inspiratory drive (VT/tI) and inspiratory timing (tI/tTOT) did not change substantially throughout the
83
6.0 6.0
5.0 5.0
V T /tI (L •m in -1)
V T /tI (L •m in -1)
4.0 4.0
3.0 3.0
2.0 2.0
Pre BPR Pre BPR
1.0 Post BPR 1.0 Post BPR
0.0 0.0
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
Power (W) Power (W)
0.55
0.54
Pre BPR
0.52 Post BPR
0.50
0.50
T i/T to t
T i/T to t
0.48
0.45
0.46
0.35 0.40
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
Power (W) Power (W)
Figure 8. Effect of breathing pattern retraining on inspiratory drive (VT/tI) and inspiratory timing (tI/tTOT ) in the
intervention (left panel) and control (right panel) groups.
There was a slight trend for lactate levels to be decreased from 240 to 330W in the intervention group
(Figure 9).
12.00
10.00
Blo o d L actate (m m o l.L )
14.00
8.00
Blo o d L actate (m m o l.l)
2.00 4.00
2.00
0.00
0.00
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
Power (W)
Power (W)
82
End-tidal partial pressures for oxygen (PETO2) and carbon dioxide (PETCO2) (data not presented), V&O 2
and V& C O2 were not affected by BPR at any stage throughout the incremental test (Figure 10).
5.00 5.00
4.50 4.50
4.00 4.00
3.50 3.50
V O 2 (L .m in )
V O 2 (m in .L )
3.00 3.00
Pre BPR
2.50 2.50
Post BPR
2.00 2.00
Pre BPR
1.50 1.50
1.00 Post BPR 1.00
0.50 0.50
0.00 0.00
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
Power (W) Power (W)
6.0 6.0
5.0 5.0
V CO 2 (L .m in )
V CO 2 (L .m in )
4.0 4.0
3.0 3.0
Pre BPR
2.0 Pre BPR 2.0
Post BPR
Post BPR
1.0 1.0
0.0 0.0
90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 360
Power (W) Power (W)
Figure 10. Effect of breathing pattern retraining on VO2 and VCO2 in the intervention (left panel)
and control (right panel) groups.
4.5.1. RPE
Table 11 shows the effect of BPR on mean RPE (legs and the breathing) during both the
incremental test and 20km-TT. The RPE at LT for both the legs (-27.9 ± 38.5%) and breathing (-
30 ± 33.9%) were lower post-intervention. Similarly, leg and breathing RPE were also lowered at
LTP (-24.7 ± 28.2%; -24.7 ± 28.1%). It was unclear whether BPR had a substantial effect on leg
84
4.5.2. Reason for terminating exercise
Prior to BPR, 60% of the intervention group rated their lungs/breathing as the terminating factor in
the incremental test, 30% rated their legs, and 10% both legs and lungs. In the control group,
42% rated their lungs, 42% rated their legs and 10% both legs and lungs as the terminating factor.
Following BPR, only legs (90%) or both legs and lungs (10%) were rated as terminating factors in
the intervention group whilst the control group continued to rate lungs (58%) or legs (42%) as the
terminating factor.
Table 11. Mean Changes in Rate of Perceived Exertion Post Breathing Pattern Retraining
and Control and Chances That the True Difference in the Change is Substantial
Change in Measure*
Intervention Control Difference Cohen’s ES Practical Inference
(N=10) (N=12) ± 90% CL ± 90% CL† Descriptive‡
Incremental Test
LT LegRPE -18.2 ± 64 13.4 ± 39.6 -27.9 ± 38.5 -0.63 ± 0.62 Very likely +ve
LTP LegRPE -9.8 ± 37.1 19.9 ± 43.2 -24.7 ± 28.2 -0.69 ± 0.60 Likely +ve
LT BreathingRPE -18.8 ± 47 16.1 ± 50.2 -30.0 ± 33.90 -0.77 ± 0.63 Likely +ve
LTP BreathingRPE -6.1 ± 37.1 24.8 ± 43 -24.7 ± 28.1 -0.69 ± 0.60 Likely +ve
20km TT
15 min LegRPE 3.8 ± 22.2 1.4 ± 33.5 2.5 ± 19.9 0.08 ± 0.58 unclear
Final LegRPE -0.1 ± 10.8 5.2 ± 31.2 -5.1 ± 16.1 -0.18 ± 0.52 unclear
15 min BreathingRPE -4.0 ± 28.4 6.7 ± 40.0 -10.0 ± 24.1 -0.39 ± 0.80 unclear
Final BreathingRPE -6.0 ± 14.4 1.6 ± 38.0 -7.4 ± 19.6 -0.34 ± 0.78 unclear
There were no clear correlations between RPEbreathing, RPElegs, or fb and the performance
85
Chapter Five: Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of breathing pattern retraining (BPR) on cycling
performance, and on respiratory, metabolic and perceptual responses during an incremental test
in competitive cyclists. The main findings were that four weeks of specific BPR clearly enhanced
endurance performance and incremental peak power and positively affected breathing pattern
To the best of the author’s knowledge, this is the first study that has investigated the effect of
the results from the present study in the context of those that have been previously reported.
However, where appropriate, the findings of the present study have been compared to other
cycling studies, or studies that have attempted to influence or modify ventilation in athletes using
related methods such as IMT (Johnson, Sharpe, & Brown, 2007; Romer, McConnell, & Jones,
2002a) and VIH (Holm, Sattler, & Fregosi, 2004; Sonetti, Wetter, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2001).
Furthermore, in some instances reference has been made to studies that have acutely imposed a
change to breathing pattern (i.e. entrainment), during a one off test (Fabre, Perrey, Arbez, &
Rouillon, 2007; Maclennan, Silvestri, Ward, & Mahler, 1994) as opposed to specifically retraining
breathing pattern.
individual response (intervention and control groups) to the intervention period. Whilst these
responses are noted, the following discussion will focus on group changes and trends.
86
5.1. Effect of BPR on cycling performance
In the present study, cycling performance was measured in two-ways: i) a simulated (laboratory-
based) 20km-TT and ii) an incremental step test to exhaustion. Both tests were performed on the
The four week BPR intervention had a clear beneficial effect both on average 20km-TT power
(3.1 ± 2.9%), average speed (1.3 ± 1.2%) and 20km-TT performance time (-1.5 ± 1.1%). These
results meet the criteria for worthwhile effect size (0.5-1.5%) estimated by Paton and Hopkins
(2001) in mean power, based on variability in competitive performance of elite cyclists. In terms
of changes in performance time, the observed performance improvement is somewhat less than
the cycling performance changes reported by Romer et al. (2002) (-3.8 ± 1.7%; 20km-TT) and
Johnson et al. (2007) (-2.66 ± 2.5%; 25km-TT) after six weeks of IMT, or the improvement in
approximate 40km-TT (-4.7 ± 1.6%) after four weeks of VIH (Holm, Sattler, & Fregosi, 2004).
Conversely it is consistent with the findings of Sonnetti et al. (2001) who reported a 1.5%
improvement in 8km-TT after four weeks of combined VIH and IPTL (although this latter study did
not show a significant difference between the control and intervention groups). Similarly, but in
rowing, the magnitude of performance change has been greater for IMT than the present study (-
Whilst the substantial enhancement in cycling performance after BPR is encouraging, the smaller
several methodological reasons including: i) the differences in intervention period and number of
intervention sessions; ii) the time of competitive season that the intervention took place; and iii)
physiological reasons including initial (pre-intervention) aerobic fitness. In terms of duration, the
87
present study involved a four week (28 sessions) intervention period. In contrast, the cycling
studies that reported larger performance changes have typically used six week interventions both
involving 84 sessions (Johnson, Sharpe, & Brown, 2007; Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002a).
Holm et al. (1994) used a four week intervention which involved 40 sessions and recorded a
larger change than the present study. The cycling study that reported a similar performance
change (Sonetti, Wetter, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2001) also used a four week intervention period
with 20 sessions. The greater number of sessions could benefit athletes in two ways; first by
enhancing the benefits seen during exercise (acute response to intervention), and second, by
allowing athletes to train at a higher intensity, further enhancing performance (long term benefit).
In support of the latter, Dickinson, Whyte and McConnell (2007) recently commented that
improvements to breathing after IMT may enable athletes to train at higher intensities, with
Adopting a new automatic breathing pattern is not achieved immediately upon commencement of
BPR (Grossman, DeSwart, & Defares, 1985). Indeed, a period during which respiratory exertion
is perceived as being greater is often experienced during the adaptation phase (Ley & Yelich,
1998). Interestingly, participants in the BPR group of the present study reported it took
approximately three weeks (21 sessions) until they felt they were using their new breathing
pattern comfortably and automatically during training. It is possible therefore that the
performance improvements seen in the present study solely reflected a response to the
participants’ breathing pattern during exercise, but was probably not long enough for participants
to benefit in terms of increasing their training effort. Thus additional weeks and/or sessions of
BPR may have allowed participants to train at a higher intensity with their newly adopted
enhancement.
88
The time of the competitive season that the studies were performed may also have contributed to
the variation observed in performance. As highlighted by Paton and Hopkins (2004), most
the season and that smaller performance gains are to be made by cyclists during their
competition season due to the high level of intensity that they are training at. During this time of
the season, the magnitude of change of some interventions may be inflated since athletes are
further away from their peak condition. The present study was purposely undertaken during the
competition season so that the true benefit of BPR could be determined. In comparison, Romer
et al. (2002) conducted their study during the maintenance phase. In other studies, it is not
stated what part of the season studies were performed (Holm, Sattler, & Fregosi, 2004; Johnson,
It is unlikely that the difference (approximately 1-2%) in TT performance between the present
study and those previously reported are due to the experience or ability of the participants. It
would be reasonable to expect that higher level athletes may make smaller performance gains
due to the smaller margins for improvement they possess (Hamlin & Hellemans, 2006). This is
unlikely to be the case in this instance as the athletes recruited in the present study were of
similar fitness level ( V&O 2 peak 4.34 ± 0.47Lmin-1) to those recruited by Romer et al. (4.58 ± 0.17
L•min-1, 2002a) and should theoretically have had the same potential to improve their aerobic
performance. Further, despite recruiting cyclists with fitness levels lower than the present study
Johnson et al. (3.65 ± 0.86 L•min-1, 2007) and Holm et al. (3.78 Lmin-1, 2004) demonstrated
larger performance gains than the present study, again suggesting that pre-intervention fitness
level was not a contributing factor to the variation in performance change observed between the
89
Finally, it is probable that the difference between the results of the present study and those
previously reported may be due to different aspects of respiratory muscle function that IMT and
BPR attempt to have an effect on. For example, IMT primarily aims to increase both the strength
and endurance of the inspiratory muscles in an attempt to reduce inspiratory muscle fatigue (as
measured by changes to MIP) (Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002b; Volianitis et al., 2001).
Conversely, BPR does not specifically train the strength and endurance of the inspiratory muscles
but instead aims to optimise the mechanics of breathing thus decreasing the work of breathing
during exercise. Therefore BPR may affect TT performance by primarily altering the recruitment
pattern of the accessory muscles and enhancing the mechanical advantage of the respiratory
muscles (the benefits of which will be discussed later), with changes in B[La] being secondary. It
is possible that if IMT was combined with BPR such that an optimal breathing pattern (automatic)
was established prior to commencing and throughout the duration of IMT, there could be an
Table 8) was also observed following BPR. In contrast, neither IMT nor VIH have been shown to
induce an ergogenic effect on incremental power in rowing (Volianitis et al., 2001) or cycling
(Holm, Sattler, & Fregosi, 2004; McConnell & Sharpe, 2005; McMahon, Boutellier, Smith, &
Spengler, 2002; Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002a) and there are conflicting results that both
refute (Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002b) and support (Chatham, Baldwin, Griffiths, Summers,
& Enright, 1999) its effect on incremental running. For example, in a well controlled, double blind
study Romer et al. (2002b) were unable to demonstrate changes to incremental shuttle running
performance after four weeks of IMT. Conversely, Chatham et al. (1999) demonstrated a 3.78%
90
improvement in shuttle running. However this latter study has been criticised because the IMT
group was led to believe that IMT would enhance their performance, which inevitably may have
motivated them to perform better, compared to the control group who had no expectation.
It is possible that, as with Chatham et al. (1999), participants in the BPR group of the present
study had a higher expectation of improved performance and therefore their motivation to do
better was elevated. To address this potential confounding variable from the outset, participants
in the present study were at no stage led to believe that the aim of BPR was to improve
performance. Rather, they were informed that the purpose was solely to investigate whether
breathing pattern could indeed be altered during exercise. The combination of increased
performance, yet decreased perceived exertion (see Effects of BPR on Perceived Effort) supports
the assertion that increased motivation was not the primary influence on performance in the
present study.
An important finding in the present study was that BPR, as anticipated, resulted in a substantial
change in breathing pattern during exercise. Several respiratory measures reflecting breathing
pattern were determined during the incremental step test to exhaustion and for direct comparison
were compared at similar absolute workloads pre- and post-BPR. These included VE, fb, VT, tI, tE,
inspiratory drive (VT / tI) and inspiratory timing (tI/tTOT). Dynamic measures of lung function were
measured at rest.
As anticipated, no changes were observed in most dynamic lung function measures post-BPR
(benefit unlikely, Table 10) – a finding that coheres well with IMT studies (Romer, McConnell, &
91
Jones, 2002a) (McMahon, Boutellier, Smith, & Spengler, 2002). However, respiratory measures
during exercise were altered. BPR resulted in a substantial decrease in fb and VE, combined with
an increase in VT and tI and it is possible that these changes explain the observed performance
improvements.
Most healthy humans increase ventilation at high exercise intensities by initially increasing VT,
then fb. As exercise intensity increases, the continued increase in fb compromises VT such that
VT plateaus or even slightly decreases, resulting in a tachypnoeic shift (Eastwood, Hillman, &
Finucane, 2001; Gallagher, Brown, & Younes, 1987; Scheuermann & Kowalchuk, 1999). This
ventilatory response has been demonstrated in untrained subjects with a wide range of fitness
levels (Folinsbee, Wallace, Bedi, & Horvath, 1983; Gallagher, Brown, & Younes, 1987) as well as
elite (but not professional) cyclists (Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999). In the
present study the BPR and control groups demonstrated similar breathing patterns pre-
intervention, with similar fb (51 vs. 56 breathsmin-1 respectively) and VT (2.7 vs. 2.7 Lmin-1) being
achieved at termination of exercise (330 vs. 340W) however substantial differences in breathing
pattern were observed post-BPR. Whilst fb increased and VT decreased across workloads in the
incremental test, the BPR group showed a significant decrease in fb and an increase in VT
compared with the breathing pattern they adopted pre-intervention (Figure 6). Conversely, the
breathing pattern of the control group did not change except for a decreased tE post-intervention
(which is not thought to be a desired change). The biggest difference between the two groups
was the change in fb, with the intervention group reaching a maximal fb of 47 ± 7 breathsmin-1 at
breathsmin-1 (333 ± 30W). These fb values are comparable to those observed by Lucia et al.
92
cyclists respectively, though it should be acknowledged that a greater power output was reached
(>400W) by Lucia et al.’s (1999) subjects (Figure 1). It is likely that the emphasis during BPR of
maintaining a diaphragmatic breathing pattern, with forced exhalation, enabled the BPR group to
In an extensive investigation of the breathing pattern measures in elite and professional cyclists,
Lucia et al. (1999) identified that tI and tE were the two parameters that differentiated elite and
professional cyclists and that such difference may possibly contribute to the superior level of
low intensities (300W) and then decreased with increasing exercise intensity, whereas it
continuously decreased in elites from the lowest to the highest intensities (Figure 2). Expiratory
time was significantly higher in professionals than elites at most moderate-to-high power outputs
(Figure 2). Prior to the intervention of the present study, both BPR and control groups displayed
a decrease in tI from low to high intensities (Figure 7) consistent with the elite cyclists of Lucia et
al. (1999). After intervention the control group, as expected, retained the same breathing pattern,
however the BPR group’s breathing pattern changed. Specifically, the tI initially increased but
then plateaued between 150 and 240W (early to mid workloads), before gradually decreasing
(Figure 7) in a pattern similar to professional cyclists. It is likely that the BPR allowed subjects to
maintain control of the length of tI and to ultimately control their breathing pattern during
incremental exercise.
Although the difference in tE pre- versus post-intervention was unclear, there was a trend for tE to
be lengthened in the intervention group post-BPR at both LT and LTP (5.0 ± 9; 5.9 ± 7.1%
respectively; Table 10). An increased tE may also have been an important factor in preventing a
93
tachypnoeic breathing pattern developing, as evident by the increased VT and decreased fb
discussed above. Lucia et al. (1999) suggested that the ability to maintain a breathing pattern
during maximal intensity exercise void of a tachypnoeic shift is an important differential between
professional and amateur elite cyclists, with the prolongation of tE being an important adaptation
made by professional cyclists during high exercise intensities. Most healthy humans increase fb
by decreasing tE rather than tI (Folinsbee, Wallace, Bedi, & Horvath, 1983) – a breathing pattern
that reverses the normal inspiratory to expiratory ratio, and may lead to breath-stacking and
dynamic hyperinflation (Chaitow & Bradley, 2002), the consequence of which may ultimately
decrease exercise performance. Prolonging tE in the BPR group may have aided in preventing a
exercise performance.
The change in tI and tE observed in the present study is exciting as it suggests that it is possible
to change an athletes breathing pattern, and for them to maintain this during high intensity
been suggested that this difference was an acquired physiological response that perhaps
developed over years of training (Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999), rather
than something that could be learned in a relatively short period of time. It is believed that no
other study has reported changes in tI or tE after a respiratory related intervention in athletes.
The largest physiological change observed in the present study after BPR was the decrease in VE
at both LT and LTP (Table 10). Although VEmax was unchanged in the present study (Benefit
likely, Table 10), there was a trend for VE to be decreased at workloads above LTP (240 – 300W;
Figure 6). This finding is consistent with that of Lucia et al. (1999) who showed that VEmax did not
94
differ between elite and professional cyclists (Figure 1), although it remained lower in professional
than elite cyclists at higher workloads (300-400W). The change in fb and VT discussed above,
coupled with an unchanged VEmax suggest that cyclists in the present study were able to achieve
the same VE in a more ‘controlled’ way, which prevented the occurrence of a tachypnoeic shift.
Lack of tachypnoeic shift is one of the major differences in breathing pattern highlighted by Lucia
et al. (1999) and is thought to be beneficial to exercise performance by decreasing the work of
breathing (Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999), reducing respiratory muscle
fatigue (Johnson, Aaron, Babcock, & Dempsey, 1996) and at high workloads reducing blood
Considering VE responds more closely to demands for CO2 clearance than for O2 uptake (Ward,
2007) it would be reasonable to expect that a change in B[La] could serve to decrease VE.
However, whilst significant changes were observed in VE at LT and LTP, no clear change in B[La]
at these points was observed, although there was a trend for both VE and B[La] to be decreased
in the higher workloads (240-300W; Figures 6 and 9). This would suggest that the changes in VE
were not solely in response to metabolic changes but that they were likely due to the change in fb
The respiratory compensation point (RCP) marks the onset of hyperventilation during incremental
exercise and is thought to be mediated by metabolic acidosis (Meyer, Faude, Urhausen, &
Kindermann, 2004). It could be speculated that one of the mechanisms underlying the ergogenic
effect of BPR is the ability to control ones breathing pattern at the RCP such that the work of
breathing is decreased, rather than due to any real metabolic changes. This suggestion is
supported by the results of the present study demonstrating a decrease in fb and VE, and increase
in VT, post-BPR at LTP (275 ± 26W; Figure 6). In contrast, other breathing interventions such as
95
IMT do not appear to consistently influence VE, (Griffiths & McConnell, 2007; Romer, McConnell,
& Jones, 2002a; Spengler, Roos, Laube, & Boutellier, 1999; Steussi, Spengler, Knopfli-Lenzin,
Markov, & Boutellier, 2001; Volianitis et al., 2001; Williams, Wongsathikun, Boon, & Acevedo,
2002) despite an abatement in B[La] being a consistent finding (Griffiths & McConnell, 2007;
McConnell & Sharpe, 2005; Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002b; Volianitis et al., 2001). This is
surprising as theoretically decreased B[La] would result in decreased metabolites and as such a
Maintaining a low VE may be beneficial for at least two reasons, the first by potentially preventing
expiratory flow limitation. Expiratory flow limitation occurs when the breathing reserve (MVV - VE)
becomes small, or even negative, such that despite further increases in ventilatory drive in
response to increasing exercise, ventilation can not increase further unless lung operating
volumes increase. The ensuing CO2 retention and arterial hypoxaemia predispose to decreased
exercise tolerance (Ward, 2007). Although increasing lung operating volume in the presence of
expiratory flow limitation can enable the necessary increased ventilatory demands to be met, the
resulting increase in end-expiratory lung volume may result in dynamic hyperinflation which
initiates a cascade of events (i.e. respiratory muscle fatigue, increased work of breathing, blood
stealing; see section 2.3.6, pp33) that ultimately reduces exercise tolerance. In addition,
decreased VE may directly decrease the metabolic and therefore perfusion cost of the respiratory
muscles which may also positively benefit exercise performance through attenuating the
The clear decrease in VE that occurred during incremental exercise may reflect changes to the
dead space-VT ratio (see section 2.2.3, pp13). When a tachypnoeic shift occurs, the dead space-
VT ratio rises, and alveolar ventilation may be reduced, resulting in increased PCO2 (Hanson,
96
Claremont, Dempsey, & Reddan, 1982). To preserve alveolar ventilation VE may be increased by
10-30% (Hanson, Claremont, Dempsey, & Reddan, 1982). It has been demonstrated however,
that such an increase in VE is not required when VT is maintained during high exercise intensities
(Hanson, Claremont, Dempsey, & Reddan, 1982). It is possible that the intervention group did
not need to increase their VE to the extent they did pre-BPR as they were able to maintain a more
effective VT post-BPR.
The changes after BPR described above may have contributed to the improved 20km-TT
performance and incremental power observed in the present study by attenuating the
metaboreflex associated with increased work of breathing (Sheel et al., 2001), although no
correlation was identified between the breathing pattern changes and exercise performance.
Harms et al. (1997) demonstrated that increasing the work of breathing decreased blood flow to
the legs in cyclists. Such decreased leg blood flow has been shown to decrease exercise
tolerance and power output in cyclists (Harms, Wetter, St Croix, Pegelow, & Dempsey, 2000). A
tachypnoeic breathing pattern is associated with increased dynamic hyperinflation which alters
the mechanical contractile properties of the inspiratory muscles, increases the work of breathing
and causes greater respiratory muscle fatigue (Scheuermann & Kowalchuk, 1999). Therefore, it
is possible that BPR in the present study prevented the onset of a tachypnoeic breathing pattern
and dynamic hyperinflation (in comparison to pre-intervention breathing pattern) thus containing
the work of breathing and preserving leg muscle blood flow ultimately allowing subjects to
increase both their 20km-TT and incremental step test performance. Direct measures of
respiratory work and blood flow distribution during intense pre- and post-BPR would support (or
97
It is thought that the sympathetically mediated reflex vasoconstriction in the limbs may be induced
by reflex activity from chemo-sensitive type III / IV receptors of fatigueing respiratory muscle
(Sheel et al., 2001). It is speculated that respiratory muscle fatigue may be caused by the
high elastic loads and velocity of muscle shortening and working at large percentages of their
available capacity for muscle pressure generation (Johnson, Babcock, Suman, & Dempsey,
1993)), as occurs with dynamic hyperinflation, and that preventing or delaying dynamic
hyperinflation may decrease respiratory muscle fatigue. In cyclists this would be an important
change given the possibility of increased inspiratory muscle fatigue caused by the crouched
cycling position (Boussana et al., 2007; Johnson, Babcock, Suman, & Dempsey, 1993).
Unfortunately, respiratory muscle fatigue was not measured in the present study and therefore
The exciting importance of the breathing pattern changes observed in the present study is that
following BPR no tachypnoeic shift was observed, which reflects a breathing pattern usually
observed only in very high level athletes (professional cyclists), and supposedly only achievable
after years of training (Eastwood, Hillman, & Finucane, 2001; Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso,
& Chicharro, 1999; Shephard, 1998; Steinacker, Both, & Whipp, 1993). Lucia et al. (Lucia, Hoyos,
Pardo, & Chicharro, 2001) suggested that level of endurance training was not the causative factor
for the difference in breathing pattern they observed in their 1999 study, unless it was a change
that occurred over a number of years. Syabbalo et al. (1994) suggested that a tachypnoeic
breathing pattern was caused by respiratory muscle fatigue and that high level athletes simply
developed a superior level of resistance to such fatigue due to their greater years of training and
therefore did not demonstrate such a pattern. This suggestion is unsupported by Romer et al.
(2002c) or Griffiths and McConnell (2007) who did not identify changes to breathing pattern
98
despite identifying changes in maximal inspiratory effort of 28 ± 7% and 26% respectively – a
measure used by these authors to identify respiratory muscle fatigue. In contrast, Hey et al.
(1966) theorised that the integration of the depth and frequency of breathing required to achieve a
given ventilatory demand is independent of known respiratory stimuli, and may simply reflect a
pattern of breathing that results in a minimal amount of work by the respiratory muscles. The
findings of the present study may give strength to this theory, and it is possible that the
participants who underwent BPR were able to learn, or relearn, a breathing pattern that
decreased the work of the respiratory muscles and which led to a positive effect on their exercise
performance.
It is not known how breathing pattern is reprogrammed, and prior to this study whether in fact it is
possible to reprogramme breathing pattern in trained athletes. However the results of this study
may indicate that a change at some level of automatic ventilatory control did occur. Grossman et
al. (1985) suggested that ones automatic breathing pattern can only be overridden for
approximately ten minutes before one reverts back to a pattern that is comfortable and natural,
yet the average incremental test in the present study lasted 21 minutes. The changes in
breathing pattern discussed above were evident throughout the entire duration of each
participant’s test. This could suggest that the brainstem respiratory centres, or whatever factor(s)
control the maintenance of automatic breathing pattern (currently unknown), were in some way
altered. It could be argued that the participants in the BPR group simply altered their breathing
pattern in response to verbal cues provided by the primary investigator. To address this issue, an
assistant, blinded to which research group the participants belonged, was present for all post
testing and provided the verbal encouragement. The assistant had no way of knowing which
participants would benefit from verbal cueing. Further, studies that have altered breathing pattern,
without giving an intervention period during which an athlete could acclimatise to, or automatically
99
adopt the new pattern, have failed to demonstrate changes to exercise performance (Fabre,
Perrey, Arbez, & Rouillon, 2007; Maclennan, Silvestri, Ward, & Mahler, 1994) suggesting that a
period of time is necessary (but currently unknown) for a new breathing pattern to become
automatic. Based on the findings of this study it is suggested that if BPR is to be used in
combination with IMT to further enhance the benefits of IMT, that a period of time is allowed for
BPR and the (assumed) changes to automatic breathing pattern to occur prior to the introduction
of IMT.
Despite observing changes in tI and tE, Lucia et al. (1999) found no difference in inspiratory drive
or timing between the two groups. Similarly, no change was observed in the present study post-
intervention (Figure 8) for either variable despite the changes to fb and VT. It should be borne in
mind however that changes in the mechanical properties of the respiratory system can alter VT/tI
for a given neural output of the respiratory centres (Milic-Emili & Grunstein, 1976). The changes
in the present study to both components of inspiratory drive (i.e. VT and tI), combined with the
possibility that BPR altered the mechanical properties of the respiratory system by decreasing
hyperinflation may mask any changes to VT/tI that may have occurred. It is suggested that mouth
indicators of inspiratory drive than VT/tI (Lucia, Carvajal, Calderon, Alfonso, & Chicharro, 1999).
Mean values for inspiratory timing were consistently smaller than 0.5 in both groups pre and post-
100
5.3. Effects of BPR on Perceived Effort
The results from the present study show that four weeks of BPR is effective in decreasing the
Following BPR RPEbreathing during the incremental test was decreased (benefit likely, Table 11) at
both LT (-30 ± 33.9%) and LTP (-24.7 ± 28.1%;) when compared with the control group. The
changes to RPEbreathing in the present study are larger than those reported previously. For
example, RPEbreathing was decreased following IMT by -16 ± 4% in cycling (Romer, McConnell, &
Jones, 2002a), -7.9 ± 6% in repeated sprint activity (Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002b) and
approximately -22%, (precise data not provided) during an incremental rowing test, however no
changes were observed at the end of this test, or during an all-out six minute test (Volianitis et al.,
2001). The effects of IMT on perceived respiratory effort has been strongly correlated (r = 0.64; P
< 0.05) with improved performance (Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002b) however no correlation
was identified in the present study between these two variables. Unlike the incremental test,
RPEbreathing was not substantially different during the time trial at either 15 minutes (isotime) or at
completion of the time trial. This finding is not totally surprising as the incremental test is ridden
at prescribed workloads whereas the TT has no set workload, enabling participants to ride at their
highest tolerated intensity. It is more likely that changes in perceived exertion could be identified
during the incremental test where effort during equal absolute workloads is compared, than in the
TT where workloads are variable. It is interesting to note that RPEbreathing for the control group
was increased at LT and LTP (Table 11). This is likely due to a slight detraining effect as shown
101
Perceived respiratory exertion, or dyspnoea, is one of the primary reasons given for the
termination of exercise in healthy humans (Hamilton, Killian, Summers, & Jones, 1996; Killian et
al., 1992). Theoretically if dyspnoea could be decreased, exercise tolerance may be increased.
The degree to which RPEbreathing was decreased at LT and LTP in the present study may have
been a contributing factor in increased exercise performance. Just as factors that impair the
contractile properties of the respiratory muscles are thought to increase dyspnoea (for example
factors that improve the contractile properties have the potential to decrease the intensity of
diaphragmatic breathing pattern. It is speculated that BPR was effective in optimising respiratory
muscle function to the degree that RPEbreathing was significantly decreased, allowing participants
to exercise longer with less discomfort. Because pacing strategies are thought to be affected by
afferent (incoming central) information (Tucker et al., 2006) it is possible that the BPR group were
able to ride at a higher intensity due to changes in afferent information that was reflecting the
decreased work of breathing i.e. they rode harder because it felt easier. It is interesting to note
that post-BPR only one participant in the intervention group terminated the incremental test due
to his breathing (in combination with his legs) compared with seven pre-BPR, whilst seven in the
control group terminated exercise due to their breathing and this was unchanged pre and post-
intervention. This suggests that BPR was effective in decreasing dyspnoea to the point it was no
longer perceived as the limiting factor during exercise. This could potentially mean cyclists could
emotional influences (Grazzini, Stendardi, Gigliotti, & Scano, 2005). During the initial stages of
102
BPR participants were exposed to feelings of breathing discomfort as they attempted to alter
(reprogramme) their VT and fb at rest and during exercise. In the early stages of retraining
participants subjectively reported feelings of air-hunger and inability to inhale deeply enough.
During this time they were educated and re-assured that these feelings were normal and just a
perceived experience, not an indication of their body’s requirement for oxygen. It is possible that
BPR group became more familiar and comfortable with the feeling of dyspnoea, knew they were
in control of their breathing and no longer perceived respiratory exertion as such an intense
experience.
Changes to the automatic breathing pattern do not occur immediately once BPR is undertaken
(Grossman, DeSwart, & Defares, 1985). Indeed it is apparent from studies that have attempted
to acutely manipulate breathing pattern that such short term changes do not affect perceived
respiratory exertion. For example, paced breathing during roller-ski skating did not change
RPEbreathing (Fabre, Perrey, Arbez, & Rouillon, 2007), nor did entraining breathing on command
during a rowing trial in rowers who were unfamiliar with entrainment (Maclennan, Silvestri, Ward,
& Mahler, 1994), which is an interesting observation considering entrainment of breathing during
upper limb exercise is thought to be a more efficient pattern (Maclennan, Silvestri, Ward, &
Mahler, 1994; Steinacker, Both, & Whipp, 1993). As anticipated, the BPR group in the present
study reported an increase in RPEbreathing during the first 2-3 weeks of BPR, both in training and
when doing their BPR exercises. This is possibly due to increased awareness of their breathing
while they were consciously trying to change it (Ley & Yelich, 1998). The findings of Fabre et al.
(2007) and Maclennan et al. (1994) in context with the subjective increased, prior to decreased
RPEbreathing in the present study suggests that an adjustment period is needed in order for
conscious changes to breathing pattern to have a beneficial effect on RPEbreathing during high
103
intensity exercise. It is not known how long this adjustment period needs to be in order to be
effective although it would have been interesting to observe what changes may have occurred
had participants in Fabre et al.’s (2007) and Maclennan et al.’s (1994) studies had time to adjust
In addition to decreasing RPEbreathing, four weeks of BPR also decreased (benefit likely, Table 11)
perceptions of leg effort (RPElegs) in the incremental test. The percent change in RPElegs was
similar at LT (-27.9 ± 38.5%) and LTP (-24.7 ± 28.3%). As observed for RPEbreathing, the
decrease in RPElegs is larger than that observed after IMT in cyclists (-18.4%) (Romer, McConnell,
& Jones, 2002a) or repeated sprint activity (7.2 ± 0.6%) (Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002b).
Similar to breathing RPE, the control group in the present study rated their RPElegs as greater at
LT (13.4 ± 39.6%) and LTP (19.9 ± 43.2%) during the post-intervention test, demonstrating a
The decrease in RPElegs may have been simply due to the same mechanism that resulted in
which subsequently decreased the work of breathing as discussed above. Harms et al. (1997)
demonstrated that decreasing the work of breathing significantly increased blood flow to the legs
in cyclists, which may aid the clearance of metabolites usually associated with peripheral fatigue
intensities that equal or exceed the lactate threshold as in the present study (Kostka & Cafarelli,
1982). By preventing blood stealing, blood flow to the legs may have been preserved allowing
appropriate clearance of metabolites and affecting pH to a degree that peripheral exertion was
perceived as less.
104
5.4. Effects of BPR on Metabolic Measures
Breathing pattern did not clearly influence any of the physiological variables measured in the
present study, although trends suggest that BPR may positively influence B[La], power and heart
rate at LTP (Table 9). This may be significant given the nature of physiological events that occur
Ventilatory work during heavy endurance exercise may contribute to the accumulation of
metabolites (Johnson, Sharpe, & Brown, 2007) that exacerbate respiratory and locomotor muscle
fatigue. One of the physiological parameters that is most often associated with improved
exercise performance following IMT is an attenuated B[La] response (Boutellier, Buchel, Kundert,
& Spengler, 1992; Boutellier & Piwko, 1992; McConnell & Sharpe, 2005; Romer, McConnell, &
Jones, 2002b; Spengler, Roos, Laube, & Boutellier, 1999) which suggests that metabolic
responses can be modified by a breathing intervention. The present study failed to show a clear
reduction in B[La] at absolute workloads pre- and post-BPR, however there was a trend for B[La]
to be decreased at LTP (-4.8 ± 16.6%) but not LT (2.7 ± 16%) after BPR, though unclear
because of the sizeable 90% confidence intervals. It is difficult to understand why IMT affected
B[La] whilst the present study failed to show such a response. Spengler et al (1999) suggested
that IMT was effective in decreasing B[La] by increasing the ability of the respiratory muscles to
use more lactate as fuel to maintain work output. Because BPR does not specifically train the
lactate turnover, which may explain the difference. Rather, BPR served to decrease the work of
perception of dyspnoea and work of breathing and may lead to a reduction of blood stealing.
105
The present study measured B[La] during incremental exercise – a performance test that
McConnell and Romer (2005) suggested would overwhelm any alterations to B[La] turnover that
had been induced by IMT, which may explain why this parameter did not significantly change in
the present study. Volianitis et al. (2001) also measured B[La] during incremental exercise and,
like the present study, demonstrated a trend for B[La] to decrease, without reaching statistical
significance. In contrast, previous studies that reported large changes in B[La] were measured
during an endurance or sub maximal test – where it is thought the ability to limit the effect of
gradually increasing blood lactate may be enhanced (McConnell, 2005). It is possible that a
larger change, more consistent with previous findings, may have been observed if B[La] had been
measured during the 20km-TT. Of course, it is possible that because BPR does not specifically
increase the strength and endurance of the respiratory muscles, its ergogenic effect is not related
to minimising the B[La] response regardless of the nature of the test protocol.
Whilst power at LTP showed a trend to increase (Table 9), this was not a clear difference – a
finding consistent with McConnell and Sharpe (2005) who demonstrated no change to MLSS
power after four weeks of IMT. Further, heart rate was unchanged in the present study (despite
the trend to be decreased at LTP; Table 9) which is consistent with the change observed after
IMT (Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002a). Collectively, this data suggests that the ergogenic
The present study measured sub-maximal V&O 2 and V&O 2 peak in an attempt to determine whether
BPR could reduce or affect the O2 cost (work) of breathing associated with incremental exercise.
(comparison of absolute V&O 2 at same power output pre- and post-intervention), V&O 2 peak (or
106
V& C O2 ) in either the intervention or the control group, a finding that is consistent with the lack of
effect reported by other authors who have demonstrated performance improvements after IMT
(Griffiths & McConnell, 2007; Romer, McConnell, & Jones, 2002a), VIH (Holm, Sattler, & Fregosi,
2004; Leddy et al., 2007; McMahon, Boutellier, Smith, & Spengler, 2002) or acute breathing
Potentially, changes in sub-maximal V&O 2 during cycling could be achieved by reducing the O2
cost of breathing by optimizing the work of breathing. Wetter et al. (1999) demonstrated that
V&O 2 max significantly decreased V&O 2 from 3.2 ± 0.1 to 3.1 ± 0.1 Lmin-1. At V&O 2 max intensity,
Harms et al. (1997) demonstrated a significant decrease in whole-body V&O 2 (6.2%) when
respiratory muscles were offloaded by 63%, which resulted in an 8% increase in V&O 2 legs
showing that blood (and hence O2) stealing occurred at maximal intensity. When respiratory
muscle work at V&O 2 max intensity was increased by 28%, V&O 2 legs was significantly decreased
(9.4%) but no change in whole-body V&O 2 was evident. In the present study, however, no
changes in O2 cost of exercise were observed after BPR (Table 9). To expect BPR to decrease
the work of breathing by 63%, or even 40%, would be ambitious. It is likely that the effects of
BPR on the work of breathing were much smaller and this could explain why no changes were
detected in either V&O 2 or V&O 2 peak in the present study. Although no changes to V&O 2 peak were
observed, the distribution of V&O 2 between the legs and respiratory muscles may have changed
such that V&O 2 legs was enhanced even if only by a few percent (compared with the much greater
8% increase with PVA). To be able to detect such changes was beyond the scope of this study.
107
However, the ability to measure and differentiate Qlegs from V&O 2 TOT may enhance our
understanding of the influence of BPR on blood and oxygen distribution and utilisation.
In summary, the minimal physiological changes observed after BPR are generally consistent with
previous findings for other breathing interventions such as IMT and VIH. This could be the result
of actual negligible change or a meaningful change but a lack of sensitivity of measures to detect
such small changes in the physiological measures included as part of this study. The
mechanisms underpinning how BPR contributed to improved time trial and incremental
performance may be more related to changes in respiratory muscle work, than to any
5.5. Limitations
The findings presented in this thesis are considered unique insofar as this is the first study to
investigate the use of a physiotherapy intervention traditionally used with chronic respiratory
blueprints to guide the direction of this study. Perhaps understandably, there are a number of
To further understand the effect of BPR on performance it would have been useful to measure
respiratory muscle recruitment pattern, dynamic hyperinflation and locomotor muscle blood flow.
understanding of whether, and then how, BPR may affect exercise performance. Unfortunately,
the resources were not available to measures these parameters, therefore it is not possible to
objectively claim that BPR altered or delayed recruitment of the accessory muscles. Similarly, as
108
objective measures of dynamic hyperinflation during exercise were not utilised it is unclear
whether dynamic hyperinflation was i) present pre-BPR or ii) altered post-BPR. Advanced
physiological measures such as limb blood flow were not measured, adding to the difficulty
ascertaining how BPR may have influenced performance from a mechanistic perspective.
The extent to which the respiratory muscles fatigued following endurance and incremental cycling
was not established in the present study. In this thesis, it has been speculated that some of the
decrease in MIP observed by others after exhaustive exercise may not be solely due to
inspiratory muscle fatigue, but may be decreased due to the shift in mechanical advantage of the
respiratory muscles caused by a tachypnoeic breathing pattern. To this end, MIP could have
been measured pre- and post-BPR to determine whether BPR did indeed have an effect on this
variable. In addition, it is possible that the four week intervention period did not allow the full
effect of BPR to be realised (and observed/measured) for the reasons discussed in section 5.1.1.
However this time frame was chosen to ensure compliance of the participants with i) their BPR
exercises, and ii) was the maximum compliable time for standardisation of training during the
subjects competitive season. Although training diaries were provided to all participants,
compliance with accurately recorded heart rate and training volume was insufficient to accurately
quantify consistency of training volume during the intervention period. However, analysis of the
available data indicated there were no large variations in weekly training load in either group
Finally, it could be argued that failing to set the cadence in the present study may have
introduced a confounding variable that affected breathing pattern. However, Prabhu et al. (1992)
found no change in fb, VT or tI/tTOT between pedalling rates from 40-80rpm and participants in the
109
present study were required to replicate their self-selected cadence between pre- and post-
intervention assessments.
The mechanisms underpinning how BPR resulted in enhanced endurance and incremental
cycling performance remain unclear. It is speculated that BPR results in changes to the
automatic breathing pattern, and that collectively these changes to respiratory mechanics may
benefit an athlete in three ways. Firstly, prevention of a tachypnoeic shift after BPR may have a
direct effect on inspiratory muscle fatigue by enhancing the mechanical advantage of the
respiratory muscle during intense exercise. Secondly, improved breathing mechanics may have
an indirect effect of improving blood flow distribution to the leg muscles during intense exercise
thus optimising power output. Thirdly improved respiratory mechanics may have an effect on the
intensity with which respiratory and peripheral efforts are perceived allowing the athlete to exert
themselves more.
In light of the positive outcome of the present study the list of questions still deserving attention is
long. For example, does BPR actually influence the inspiratory muscle recruitment pattern,
dynamic hyperinflation and respiratory muscle fatigue? Does BPR actually affect locomotor
muscle blood flow? What mechanisms underlie how the automatic breathing pattern is retrained,
how long does it take and once changes are established are they lasting? What is the incidence
To address some of these questions a number of suggestions for future studies can be made.
Increasing the intervention period would allow further understanding of whether BPR had a
greater effect on exercise performance once the new pattern was assimilated into training.
110
Maximal inspiratory pressure or some measure of respiratory muscle fatigue needs to be
recorded to assess whether BPR influences respiratory muscle fatigue. Similarly, recording leg
blood flow during cycling pre- and post-BPR would help the understanding as to whether BPR
enhances exercise performance by preventing blood stealing. An objective tool to measure the
incidence of breathing pattern disorders in athletes needs to be developed or the current practice
5.7. Conclusion
The results of the present study suggest BPR has a positive influence on 20km-TT performance
and incremental power during cycling in trained athletes. In the absence of any strong
physiological changes, it appears the observed changes in performance were due to altered
decreased fb and VE, and increased VT and tI compared with pre-intervention values, resulting in a
more controlled breathing pattern at high exercise intensities. The mechanisms underpinning the
ergogenic effect of BPR remain unclear. Further research to enhance our understanding of the
111
Appendices
112
Appendix 1 Nijmegen Questionnaire
Nijmegen Questionnaire
Never Rare Sometimes Often Very Often
1 2 1 2 1 2 1 2 1 2
Chest Pain
Feeling Tense
Blurred Vision
Dizzy spells /
light headed
Confusion
Faster / deeper
breathing
Shortness of
Breath
Tight feelings in
the chest
Bloated feeling in
the stomach
Tingling Fingers
Unable to
breathe deeply
Stiff fingers or
arms
Tight feelings
around the
mouth, jaw or
throat
Cold hands or
feet
Heart racing
(palpitations)
Feelings of
Anxiety
Total 1 / 64 date……………………..
Total 2 / 64 date……………………..
113
Nijmegen Descriptors3
Never: Never.
3 Participants have a copy of these descriptors in front of them when they complete the Nijmegen Questionnaire
114
Appendix 2 Clinical Breathing Retraining
Despite the fact that breathing pattern retraining has been used by physiotherapists since the
1930’s (Soley & Shock, 1938) there are no “gold standard” treatment methods. Instead the basic
method described by Lum (1983) has been modified and integrated into clinical practice by
clinicians working in the area of breathing pattern disorders and retraining. The description
provided below is a general overview, summarizing current clinical practice. The reader is
reminded that, to date, no studies have investigated time frames, appropriate intervention
strategies or physiological reasons for how and/or why breathing pattern retraining may be an
effective tool for optimising respiratory mechanics. Therefore much of this description is provided
without scientific rational for its existence, instead it is based on the experience of the author and
other physiotherapists working in the field of BPDs (Bradley, 1992; Chaitow, Bradley, & Gilbert,
2002).
There are two components of breathing pattern retraining. The first involves learning a new
motor pattern, to train the inspiratory muscles to be recruited in the optimal way. Just like
learning a new golf swing or swimming technique, the initial phase of learning and retraining
requires intense concentration. With further practice the new muscle firing pattern will become
more efficient and eventually automatic (Wilmore & Costill, 1999) and based on studies
investigating neural plasticity and motor skill acquisition this may occur anytime in the first four to
six weeks of retraining (Enorka, 1997). There are two main techniques involved in breathing
pattern retraining – diaphragmatic breathing and pursed lip breathing. Diaphragmatic breathing
retraining requires a conscious appreciation of inspiring air to the lung bases with a slight forward
abdominal displacement and passive, relaxed expiration (Jones, Dean, & Chow, 2003) and is
used to delay recruitment of the accessory breathing muscles and to encourage breathing at
optimal lung volumes. Pursed lip breathing is taught to help elongate the expiratory phase of
115
breathing to maintain an optimal inspiratory to expiratory ration, prevent breath stacking and to
avoid a tachypnoeic shift in breathing pattern (Jones, Dean, & Chow, 2003).
A patient is often able to demonstrate a diaphragmatic breathing pattern within the first treatment
session, albeit not comfortably, and to continue to demonstrate it correctly when requested. At
this point the diaphragm pattern is a learned motor skill that can be used to voluntarily override
The second part of breathing pattern retraining is more difficult as it involves re-programming the
brainstem respiratory centres and based on Grossman et al.’s theory (1985) involves retraining
the body to respond to a higher level of CO2. The normal reaction of the body to a rising CO2
level is to increase the respiratory rate to bring the CO2 level back to appropriate parameters
(Wilmore & Costill, 1999). When a breathing pattern disorder is well established, the CO2 in the
blood stream may be lower than normal (Garssen, de Ruiter, & van Dyke, 1992). One aim of
breathing pattern retraining is to decrease the respiratory rate to 12-14 breaths per minute. As a
result, PaCO2 starts to rise relative to the brainstems incorrect perception of “normal” and the
appropriate but undesired response of trying to increase respiration is initiated. Clinicians find
that this part of breathing pattern retraining takes patients the most time, and is most difficult to
overcome as it feels uncomfortable and unnatural to the patient. It has been suggested that
decreased respiratory rate can only be tolerated for up to ten minutes at a time, and only small
increases in CO2 can be withstood before the urge to breathe is overwhelming (Grossman,
DeSwart, & Defares, 1985). A session of breathing pattern retraining is usually broken into small
groups of “correct” breathing, interspersed with the patient’s comfortable pattern, to build
tolerance to the new CO2 levels. The length of time required to adjust to the appropriate PaCO2
is unknown. However, it is likely that a breathing pattern disorder will not be overcome, despite
116
that ability to demonstrate a diaphragmatic breathing pattern on demand, until the PaCO2 level
It is common, and almost expected, that patients will feel significant discomfort when trying to
correct their breathing pattern (Innocenti, 1998). These symptoms are explained to patients at
the outset of retraining and further discussion can take place if they do arise. If a breathing
pattern disorder is well established it will feel comfortable and even normal to the patient, despite
the symptoms generated. Correction of breathing pattern will be opposed by the decreased
respiratory rate causing increased CO2 which may initiate feelings of ‘air hunger’. Further, a
patient may be used to breathing at high lung volumes secondary to dynamic hyperinflation. A
full, relaxed exhalation will alter operating lung volumes and give the impression that no enough
air is being taken into the lungs. Additionally, if mouth breathing is a habit, the increased (but
appropriate) resistance of nose breathing will feel difficult at first (Garssen, de Ruiter, & van Dyke,
1992). All of these symptoms will decrease with time and practice. If retraining is consistent and
possible that once automatic breathing has been reprogrammed, and causative factors have
been addressed, (for example, anxiety or postural factors) no ongoing breathing intervention
would be necessary. This is unlike strength training, including respiratory muscle training, which
requires an ongoing maintenance program to preserve strength gains (Wilmore & Costill, 1999).
117
Appendix 3 Breathing Retraining
10 minutes Daily
Gold Stars
• These are to remind you to correct your breathing through out the day.
• Put them in various places in your environment e.g. on your head stem, on your
computer monitor, dashboard, phone etc.
• When you see one do the following:
- Stop mentally what you are thinking about,
- Drop your shoulders, upper chest and breathe out,
- Take three normal size breaths into your stomach.
• It is very important that you do this to “reprogram” your breathing.
When Exercising
• Try to keep breathing into your stomach for as long as possible. At some point you may
revert to breathing to your upper chest but try to delay this as long as possible.
• When you breathe in, think of “sipping” the air in over your bottom lip. As you breathe out,
blow the air out over your bottom lip as if blowing out a straw. Allow your out breath to
become more forceful as exercise intensity increases.
• Try to keep your mouth opening as small as possible, for as long as possible.
118
Appendix 4 Training Diary
TRAINING DIARY
AUT University
Contact: Rachel Vickery, 0211779309, rachinaus@hotmail.com
• Fill in the date, the duration, the distance in kms, heart rate (average and max) and average speed.
• Then mark the intensity of the session, and how breathless you felt when you did it (using the 0-10 scale at the front of your diary).
• If you do sessions other than on the bike please indicate what type of session you did.
• Record training as soon as possible after each session.
• Bring this diary with you each week you come to the Millennium Institute
HR
(Average & Average Intensity Breathlessness: Any Comment?
Date Duration Distance Max) Speed 0-10 0-10 (Breathing Exercises)*
Monday
Tuesday
Wednesday
Thursday
Friday
Saturday
Sunday
* The comment on breathing exercises was only included in the diaries given to the Intervention Group participants
SCALE SEVERITY
0 No Breathlessness At All
0.5 Very, Very Slight (Just Noticeable)
1 Very Slight
2 Slight Breathlessness
3 Moderate
4 Some What Severe
5 Severe Breathlessness
6
7 Very Severe Breathlessness
8
9 Very, Very Severe (Almost Maximum)
10 Maximum
120
Appendix 6 Patient Handout – Potential Symptoms associated with
BPR
• Air hunger
• “not getting enough air in”
• “not breathing in long enough”
• Light headed
• Dizziness
• Needing to take a deep breath in after finishing the “correct” breaths.
• General discomfort
All these feelings are normal. When you breathe slower (correctly) your body is retaining more
carbon dioxide than what it is used to. Usually when our carbon dioxide level gets too high we
feel the need to breathe more (think about how you feel when you have held your breath as long
as possible). It takes time for your body to adjust to the new carbon dioxide level. Over time
these feelings will decrease and the new way of breathing will feel much easier. The only thing
that will speed the change over time is spending your 10 minutes daily breathing with the 1,2,3,4
pattern, using your gold stars and trying to breathe to your stomach when you are riding.
Initially you may feel more out of breath as you exercise, or you may feel that it is harder to
breathe when you ride. Again this is normal, as you are breathing through a smaller hole,
however this will also improve and the more you practice the quicker you will adjust.
121
Appendix 7 Participant Handout – Rationale for Exercises
There are two parts to changing your breathing pattern. The first is the easy part and is as simple
as learning to stride out more when you run. It is what we call a motor skill or technique. The
gold stars are used as a reminder for you to be aware of how you are breathing and to correct
your technique if necessary. It is important to break the habit of how you are breathing, and use
the correct technique. This will be achieved through awareness of how you are breathing, and
consciously changing to the new technique of breathing to your belly. It is important to use the
visual triggers (gold stars). From experience you will not correct your breathing technique as
often if you don’t use them, and it will then be harder to breathe properly when you ride.
The second part is the hardest part as you need to reprogram the part of your brain that controls
breathing. When you breathe incorrectly over a period of time your brain gets used to a certain
level of carbon dioxide in your blood stream (usually a lower level) and bases future breathing
rates and depths on this. Although you can override your brain’s program (for example, holding
your breath under water, or breathing really quickly on command), over-riding your brain’s
program can not be tolerated for longer than about 10 minutes. The only way you can achieve a
correct breathing pattern long term is to reset the level of carbon dioxide that your brain responds
to. This is why you do your 10 minutes of correct breathing! By consciously slowing your
breathing rate, your carbon dioxide level will slowly normalise and you will be able to tolerate the
correct breathing pattern longer.
Combining the awareness and correction of how you are breathing (gold stars), with the tolerance
to maintain this correct pattern, will help you breathe more efficiently on the bike.
122
References
Aaron, E. A., Seow, K. C., Johnson, B. D., & Dempsey, J. A. (1992B). Oxygen cost of
exercise hyperpnea: implications for performance. Journal of Applied
Physiology, 72(5), 1818-1825.
Abernethy, A. B., Mackinnon, L. T., Neal, R. J., Kippers, V., & Hanrahan, S. J. (1996).
The biophysical foundations of human movement. Melbourne: Human Kinetics
Publishers.
AIHW. (2005). Australian Centre for Asthma Monitoring. Asthma in Australia 2005.
AIHW Asthma series 2. Canberra.
Aliverti, A., & Macklem, P. T. (2001). How and why exercise is impaired in COPD.
Respiration, 68, 229-239.
Askanazi, J., Silverberg, P. A., Foster, R. J., Hyman, A. I., Milic-Emili, J., & Kinney, J.
M. (1980). Effects of respiratory apparatus on breathing pattern. Journal of
Applied Physiology, 48, 577-580.
Astrand, P. O., & Rodahl, K. (1986). Textbook of work physiology: physiological bases
of exercise (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
ATS. (1998). Dyspnea. Mechanisms, assessment and management: a consensus
statement. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, 159,
321-340.
Babcock, M. A., Pegelow, D., McClaran, S. R., Suman, O. E., & Dempsey, J. A. (1995).
Contribution of diaphragm power output to exercise-induced diaphragm fatigue.
Journal of Applied Physiology, 78(5), 1710-1719.
Baker, J., Cote, J., & Hawes. (2000). The relationship between coaching behaviours and
sport anxiety in athletes. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 3(2), 110-
119.
Banez, G. A., & Culbert, T. P. (2005). An integrative approach to vocal cord
dysfunction in young athletes. Biofeedback, 33(2), 54-57.
Barclay, J. K. (1986). A delivery-independent blood flow effect on skeletal muscle
fatigue. Journal of Applied Physiology, 61(3), 1084-1090.
Batterham, A. M., & Hopkins, W. G. (2005). Making meaningful inferences about
magnitudes. Sportscience, 9, 6-13.
Belen, L., Habrard, M., Micallef, J. P., & Le Gallais, D. (2007). The performance and
efficiency of cycling with a carbon fiber eccentric chainring during incremental
exercise. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 47(1), 40-45.
Blager, F. B. (2000). Paradoxical vocal fold movement: diagnosis and management.
Current Opinion in Otolaryngology & Head and Neck Surgery, 8, 180-183.
Boussana, A., Galy, O., Hue, O., Meatecki, S., Varray, A., Ramonatxo, M., et al. (2007).
The effects of prior cycling and a successive run on respiratory muscle
performance in triathletes. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 24(63-70).
Boutellier, U., Buchel, R., Kundert, A., & Spengler, C. (1992). The respiratory system
as an exercising limiting factor in normal trained subjects. European Journal of
Applied Physiology, 65, 347-353.
Boutellier, U., & Piwko, P. (1992). The respiratory system as an exercising limiting
factor in normal sedentary subjects. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 64,
145-152.
Bradley, D. (1992). Hyperventilation Syndrome. Auckland: Random House.
Brugman, S. M., & Simons, S. M. (1998). Vocal Cord Dysfunction. Don't mistake it for
asthma. The physician and sports medicine, 26(5), 63-85.
123
Buckley, J. D., Bourdon, P. C., & Woolford, S. M. (2003). Effect of measuring blood
lactate concentrations using different automated lactate analysers on blood
lactate transition thresholds. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 6(4),
408-421.
Cain, W. S. (1973). Nature of perceived effort and fatigue: roles of strength and blood
flow in muscle contractions. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 5, 33-47.
Campbell, E. J. M. (1966). The relationship of the sensation of breathlessness to the act
of breathing. London: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Capelli, C., Rosa, G., & Butti, F. (1993). Energy cost and efficiency of riding
aerodynamic bicycles. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 67, 144-149.
Chaitow, L., & Bradley, D. (2002). The structure and function of breathing. In L.
Chaitow, D. Bradley & C. Gilbert (Eds.), Multidisciplinary Approaches to
Breathing Pattern Disorders (pp. 1-41). New York: Churchill Livingston.
Chaitow, L., Bradley, D., & Gilbert, C. (2002). Multidisciplinary approaches to
breathing pattern disorders. London: Harcourt Publishers Limited.
Chatham, K., Baldwin, J., Griffiths, H., Summers, L., & Enright, S. (1999). Inspiratory
muscle training improves shuttle run performance in healthy subjects.
Physiotherapy, 85, 676-683.
Clark, J. M., Hagerman, F., & Gelfand, R. (1983). Breathing patterns during
submaximal and maximal exercise in elite oarsman. Journal of Applied
Physiology, 55(2), 440-446.
Clark, S. A., Bourdon, P. C., Schmidt, W., Singh, B., Cable, G., Onus, K. J., et al.
(2007). The effect of acute simulated moderate altitude on power, performance
and pacing strategies in well-trained cyclists. European Journal of Applied
Physiology, 20(Epub ahead of print).
Conconi, F., Grazzi, G., Casoni, I., Guglielmi, C., Borsetto, C., Ballarin, E., et al. (1996).
The conconi test; methodology after 12 years of application. International
Journal of Sports Medicine, 17, 509-519.
Corda, M., von Euler, C., & Lennerstrand, G. (1965). Proprioceptive innervation of the
diaphragm. Journal of Physiology, 178, 161-178.
Coyle, E. F., Feltner, M. E., Kautz, S. A., Hamilton, M. T., Montain, S. J., Baylor, A.
M., et al. (1991). Physiological and biomechanical factors associated with elite
endurance cycling performance. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 23(1),
93-107.
Davis, H. A., Bassett, J., Hughes, P., & Gass, G. C. (1983). Anaerobic threshold and
lactate turnpoint. European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational
Physiology, 50(3), 383-392.
Day, J. R., Rossiter, H. B., Coats, E. M., Skasick, A., & Whipp, B. J. (2003). The
maximally attainable VO2 during exercise in humans: the peak versus maximal
issue. Journal of Applied Physiology, 95(5), 1901-1907.
Dellaca, R. L., Aliverti, A., Pelosi, P., Carlesso, E., Chiumello, D., Pedotti, A., et al.
(2001). Estimation of end-expiratory lung volume variations by optolelectronic
plethysmography. Critcal Care Medicine, 29(9), 1807-1811.
Dempsey, J. A. (1986). Is the lung built for exercise? Medicine & Science in Sport &
Exercise, 18(143-155).
Dempsey, J. A. (2006a). Challenges for future research in exercise physiology as
applied to the respiratory system. Exercise and Sport Sciences Review, 34, 92-98.
Dempsey, J. A. (2006b). Is the healthy respiratory system (always) built for exercise.
The Journal of Physiology, 576, 339-340.
124
Dempsey, J. A., Romer, L. M., Rodman, J., Miller, J., & Smith, C. (2006).
Consequences of exercise-induced respiratory muscle work. Respiratory
Physiology & Neurobiology, 151, 242-250.
Dempsey, J. A., & Wagner, P. D. (1999). Exercise-enduced arterial hypoxaemia.
Journal of Applied Physiology, 87, 1997-2006.
Dickinson, J., Whyte, G., & McConnell, A. K. (2007). Inspiratory muscle training: a
simple cost -effective treatment for inspiratory stridor. British Journal of Sports
Medicine, 41, 694 - 695.
Doherty, M., Nobbs, L., & Noakes, T. D. (2003). Low frequency of the plateau
phenomenon during maximal exercise in british elite athletes. European Journal
of Applied Physiology 89(6), 619-623.
Eastwood, P. R., Hillman, D. R., & Finucane, K. E. (2001). Inspiratory muscle
performance in endurance athletes and sedentary subjects. Respirology, 6, 95-
194.
Edwards, A. M., & Cooke, C. B. (2004). Oxygen uptake kinetics and maximal aerobic
power are unaffected by inspiratory muscle training in healthy subjects where
time to exhaustion is extended. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 93,
139-144.
El-Manshawi, A., Killian, K. J., & Summers, E. (1986). Breathlessness during exercise
with and without resistive loading. Journal of Applied Physiology, 61(3), 896-
905.
Enorka, R. M. (1997). Neural adaptations with chronic physical activity. Journal of
Biomechanics, 30, 477-455.
Fabre, N., Perrey, S., Arbez, L., & Rouillon, J. D. (2007). Paced breathing in roller-ski
skating: effects on metabolic rate and poling forces. International Journal of
Sports Physiology and Performance, 2(46-57).
Fairburn, M. S., Coutts, K. C., Pardy, R. L., & McKenzie, D. C. (1991). Improved
respiratory muscle endurance of highly trained cyclists and the effect on
maximal exercise performance. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 12,
66-70.
Faithfull, D., Jones, J. G., & Jordan, C. (1979). Measurement of the relative
contributions of rib cage and abdomen / diaphragm to tidal breathing in man.
British Journal of Anaesthesia, 51(5), 391-398.
Falling, L. (1995). Principles and practices of pulmonary therapy. Philadelphia: W B
Saunders.
Fallon, K. E. (2004). Upper airway obstruction masquerading as exercise induced
bronchospasm in an elite road cyclist. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 38(e9).
Fallon, K. E. (2007). Commentary on inspiratory muscle training: a simple cost-
effective treatment for inspiratory stridor. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 41,
695.
Faria, E. W., Parker, D. L., & Faria, I. E. (2005). The science of cycling. Physiology
and training - part 1. Sports Medicine, 35(4), 285-312.
Faria, E. W., Parker, D. L., & Faria, I. E. (2005b). The science of cycling. Factors
affecting performance - part 2. Sports Medicine, 35(4), 313-337.
Folinsbee, L. J., Wallace, E. S., Bedi, J. F., & Horvath, S. M. (1983). Exercise
respiratory pattern in elite cyclists and sedentary subjects. Medicine and Science
in Sports and Exercise, 15(6), 503-509.
Frangolias, D., & Rhodes, E. (1996). Metabolic responses and mechanisms during water
immersion running exercise. Sports Medicine, 22(1), 38-53.
125
Frisbee, J. C., Murrant, C. L., Wilson, B. A., & Barclay, J. K. (1999). Polycythemia
decreases fatigue in tetanic contractions of canine skeletal muscle. Medicine &
Science in Sport & Exercise, 31(9), 1293-1298.
Gallagher, C. G., Brown, E., & Younes, M. (1987). Breathing pattern during maximal
exercise and during submaximal exercise with hypercapnia. Journal of Applied
Physiology, 63(1), 238-244.
Gallagher, C. G., & Younes, M. (1989). Effect of pressure assist on ventilation and
respiratory mechanics in heavy exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology, 66(4),
1824-1837.
Garssen, B., de Ruiter, C., & van Dyke, R. (1992). Breathing retraining: a rational
placebo? Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 141-153.
Gilbert, C. (2005). Better chemistry through breathing: the story of carbon dioxide and
how it can go wrong. Biofeedback, 33(3), 100-104.
Gnehm, P., Reichenbach, S., Altpeter, E., Widmer, H., & Hoppeler, H. (1997).
Influence of different racing positions on metabolic cost in elite athletes.
Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 29(6), 818-823.
Gosselink, R. A. A. M., Wagenaar, R. C., & Rijswijk, H. (1995). Diaphragmatic
breathing reduces efficiency of breathing in patients with chronic obstructive
pulmonary disease. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care
Medicine, 151, 1136-1142.
Grappe, F., Candau, R., & Belli, A. (1997). Aerodynamic drag in field cycling with
special reference to the obrees position. Ergonomics, 40, 1299-1322.
Grazzini, M., Stendardi, L., Gigliotti, F., & Scano, G. (2005). Pathophysiology of
exercise dyspnoea in healthy subjects and in patients with chronic obstructive
pulmonary disease. Respiratory Medicine, 99, 1403-1412.
Griffiths, L. A., & McConnell, A. K. (2007). The influence of inspiratory and expiratory
muscle training upon rowing performance. European Journal of Applied
Physiology, 99, 457-466.
Grossman, P., DeSwart, J. C. G., & Defares, P. B. (1985). A controlled study of a
breathing therapy for treatment of hyperventilation syndrome. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 29(1), 49-58.
Hamilton, A. L., Killian, K. J., Summers, E., & Jones, N. L. (1996). Symptom intensity
and subjective limitation to exercise in patients with cardiorespiratory disorders.
Chest, 110(5), 1255-1263.
Hamlin, M. J., & Hellemans, J. (2006). Effect of intermittent normobaric hypoxic
exposure at rest on haemotological, physiological, and performance parameters
in multi-sport athletes. Journal of Sports Sciences, 1-11.
Han, J. N., Stegen, K., De Valck, C., Clement, J., & Van de Woestijne, K. P. (1996).
Influence of breathing therapy on complaints, anxiety and breathing pattern in
patients with hyperventilation syndrome and anxiety disorders. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 41(5), 481-493.
Hanel, B., & Secher, N. H. (1991). Maximum oxygen uptake and work capacity after
inspiratory muscle training: a controlled study. International Journal of Sports
Medicine, 9, 43-52.
Hanson, P., Claremont, A., Dempsey, J. A., & Reddan, W. (1982). Determinants and
consequences of ventilatory responses to competitive endurance running.
Journal of Applied Physiology (Respiratory and Environmental Exercise
Physiology), 52, 615-623.
126
Harms, C. A., Babcock, M. A., McClaran, S. R., Pegelow, D. F., Nickele, G. A., Nelson,
W. B., et al. (1997). Respiratory muscle work compromises leg blood flow
during maximal exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology, 82(5), 1573-1583.
Harms, C. A., Wetter, T. J., McClaran, S. R., Pegelow, D. F., Nickele, G. A., Nelson, W.
B., et al. (1998). Effects of respiratory muscle work on cardiac output and its
distribution during maximal exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology, 85(2),
609-618.
Harms, C. A., Wetter, T. J., St Croix, C. M., Pegelow, D. F., & Dempsey, J. A. (2000).
Effects of respiratory muscle work on exercise performance. Journal of Applied
Physiology, 89(131-138).
Hershenson, M. B., Kikuchi, Y., & Loring, S. H. (1988). Relative strengths of the chest
wall muscles. Journal of Applied Physiology, 66(2), 750-754.
Hershenson, M. B., Kikuchi, Y., & Tzelepis, G. E. (1989). Preferential fatigue of the rib
cage muscles during inspiratory resistive loaded ventilation. Journal of Applied
Physiology, 66(2), 750-754.
Hey, E. N., Lloyd, B. B., Cunningham, D. J. C., Jukes, M. G. M., & Bolton, D. P. G.
(1966). Effects of various respiratory stimuli on depth and frequency of
breathing in man. Respiration Physiology, 1, 193-205.
Holm, P., Sattler, A., & Fregosi, R. F. (2004). Endurance training of respiratory muscles
improves cycling performance in fit young cyclists. BMC Physiology, 4(9), 1-14.
Inbar, O., Weiner, P., Azgad, Y., Rotstein, A., & Weinstein, Y. (2000). Specific
inspiratory muscle training in well-trained endurance athletes. Medicine &
Science in Sports & Exercise, 32(7), 1233-1237.
Innocenti, D. M. (1998). Hyperventilation. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.
Ito, M., Kakizaki, F., Tsuzura, Y., & Yamada, M. (1999). Immediate effect of
respiratory muscle stretch gymnastics and diaphragmatic breathing on
respiratory pattern. Internal Medicine, 38, 126-132.
Jeukendrup, A. E., & Martin, J. (2001). Improving cycling performance. How we
should spend our money. Sports Medicine, 31(7), 559-569.
Johnson, B. D., Aaron, E. A., Babcock, M. A., & Dempsey, J. A. (1996). Respiratory
muscle fatigue during exercise: implications for performance. Medicine &
Science in Sports & Exercise, 28(9), 1129-1137.
Johnson, B. D., Babcock, M. A., Suman, O. E., & Dempsey, J. A. (1993). Exercise
enduced diaphragm fatigue in healthy humans. Journal of Physiology, 460, 385-
405.
Johnson, B. D., Saupe, K. W., & Dempsey, J. A. (1992). Mechanical constraints on
exercise hyperpnea in endurance athletes. Journal of Applied Physiology, 73(3),
874-886.
Johnson, M. A., Sharpe, G. R., & Brown, P. I. (2007). Inspiratory muscle training
improves cycling time trial performance and anaerobic work capacity but not
critical power. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 15(Epub ahead of
print).
Jones, A. Y. M., Dean, E., & Chow, C. C. S. (2003). Comparison of the oxygen cost of
breathing exercises and spontaneous breathing in patients with stable chronic
obstructive pulmonary disease. Physical Therapy, 83(5), 424-431.
Kayser, B., Sliwinski, P., Yan, S., Tobiasz, M., & Macklem, P. T. (1997). Respiratory
effort sensations during exercise with induced expiratory-flow limitation in
healthy humans. Journal of Applied Physiology, 83(3), 936-947.
127
Kendrick, K., Baxi, S., & Smith, R. (2000). Usefulness of the modified 0-10 Borg scale
in assessing the degree of dyspnoea in patients with COPD and asthma. Journal
of Emergency Nursing, 26(3), 216-222.
Killian, K. J., Leblanc, P., Martin, D. H., Summers, E., Jones, N. L., & Campbell, E. J.
M. (1992). Exercise capacity and ventilatory, circulatory and symptom
limitation in patients with chronic airflow limitation. American Review of
Respiratory Disease, 146, 935-940.
Kostka, C. E., & Cafarelli, E. (1982). Effect of pH on sensation and vastus lateralis
electromyogram during cycling exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology, 52,
1181-1185.
Koulouris, N. G., & Kosmas, E. N. (2002). Negative expiratory pressure: a new
diagnostic in chest medicine. Pneumon, 15(3), 254-262.
Kowalchuk, J. M., Rossitter, H. B., Ward, S. A., & Whipp, B. J. (2002). The effect of
resistive breathing on leg muscle oxygenation using near-infrared spectroscopy
during exercise in man. Experimental Physiology, 87(5), 601-611.
Kraft, A. R., & Hoogduin, C. A. L. (1984). The hyperventilation syndrome. A pilot
study on the effectiveness of treatment. British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 538-
542.
Larsson, L., Hemmingsson, P., & Boethius, G. (1994). Self-reported obstructive airway
symptoms are common in young cross-country skiers. Scandinavian Journal of
Medicine and Science in Sports, 4, 124-127.
Larsson, L., Wadell, K. M., Jakobsson, E. J., Burlin, L. U., & Henriksson-Larsen, K. B.
(2004). Validation of the metamax II portable metabolic measurement system.
International Journal of Sports Medicine, 25(2), 115-123.
Laursen, P. B., Shing, C. M., & Jenkins, D. G. (2003). Reproducibility of a laboratory-
based 40-km cycle time trial on a stationary wind-trainer in highly trained
cyclists. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 24(7), 481-485.
Leddy, J. J., Limprasertkul, A., Patel, S., Modlich, F., Buyea, C., Pendergrast, D. R., et
al. (2007). Isocapnic hyperpnea training improves performance in competitive
male runners. European Journal of Applied Physiology, Epub ahead of print.
Levine, S., & Henson, D. (1988). Low-frequency diaphragmatic fatigue in
spontaneously breathing humans. . Journal of Applied Physiology, 64(2), 672-
680.
Ley, R., & Yelich, G. (1998). Fractional end-tidal CO2 as an index of the effects of
stress on math performance and verbal memory of test-anxious adolescents.
Biological Psychology, 49(1-2), 83-94.
Lomax, M. E., & McConnell, A. K. (2003). Inspiratory muscle fatigue in swimmers
after a single 200 m swim. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21, 659-664.
Lucia, A., Carvajal, A., Calderon, F. J., Alfonso, A., & Chicharro, J. L. (1999).
Breathing pattern in highly competitive cyclists during incremental exercise.
European Journal of Applied Physiology, 79, 512-521.
Lucia, A., Hoyos, J., Pardo, J., & Chicharro, J. L. (2001). Effects of endurance training
on the breathing pattern of professional cyclists. Japanese Journal of Physiology,
51, 133-141.
Lucia, A., Hoyos, J., & Perez, M. (2002). Inverse relationship between VO2max and
economy / efficiency in world-class cyclists. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 34, 2079-2084.
Lucia, A., Rabadan, M., Hoyos, J., Hernandez-Capilla, M., Perez, M., San Juan, A. F.,
et al. (2006). Frequency of the VO2 max plateau phenomenon in world-class
athletes. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 27(12), 984-992.
128
Lum, C. (1977). Breathing exercises in the treatment of hyperventilation and chronic
anxiety states. Chest, Heart and Stroke Journal, 2, 6-11.
Maclennan, S. E., Silvestri, G. A., Ward, J., & Mahler, D. A. (1994). Does entrained
breathing improve the economy of rowing. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 26(5), 610-614.
Mador, M. J., Magalang, U. J., Rodis, A., & Kufel, T. J. (1993). Diaphragmatic fatigue
after exercise in healthy human subjects. American Review of Respiratory
Disease, 148(6 part 1), 1571-1575.
Mahler, D. A., Shuhart, C. R., Brew, E., & Stukel, T. A. (1991). Ventilatory responses
and entrainment of breathing during rowing. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 23(2), 186-192.
Markov, G., Spengler, C. M., Knopfli-Lenzin, C., Stuessi, C., & Boutellier, U. (2001).
Respiratory muscle training increases cycling endurance without affecting
cardiovascular responses to exercise. European Journal of Applied Physiology,
85, 233-239.
Maxwell, N. S., Castle, P. C., & Spencer, M. (2007). Effect of recovery intensity on
peak power output and the development of heat strain during intermittent sprint
exercise while under stress. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 11(Epub
ahead of print).
McConnell, A. K. (2005). The role of inspiratory muscle function and training in the
genesis of dyspnoea in asthma and COPD. Primary Care Respiratory Journal,
14, 186-194.
McConnell, A. K., & Romer, L. M. (2004a). Dyspnoea in health and obstructive
pulmonary disease. The role of respiratory muscle function and training. Sports
Medicine, 34(2), 117-132.
McConnell, A. K., & Romer, L. M. (2004b). Respiratory muscle training in healthy
humans: resolving the controversy. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 25,
284-293.
McConnell, A. K., & Sharpe, G. R. (2005). The effect of inspiratory muscle training
upon maximum lactate steady-state and blood lactate concentration. European
Journal of Applied Physiology, 94, 277-284.
McDonald, W. (2007). Discussion on time trial criteria. In R. Vickery (Ed.). Sunshine
Coast.
McMahon, M. E., Boutellier, U., Smith, R. M., & Spengler, C. (2002). Hyperpnea
training attenuates peripheral chemosensitivity and improves cycling
performance. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 205, 3937-3943.
Mendelli, J., Lounana, J., Messan, F., Menuet, J. J., & Petitjean, M. (2006). Testing of
pulmonary function in a professional cycling team. Journal of Sports Medicine
and Physical Fitness, 46(2), 298-306.
Meyer, T., Faude, O., Urhausen, A., & Kindermann, W. (2004). Is lactic acidosis a
cause of exercise induced hyperventilation at the respiratory compensation point.
British Journal of Sports Medicine, 38, 622-625.
Milic-Emili, J., & Grunstein, M. M. (1976). Drive and timing components of ventilation.
Chest, 70 (suppl), 131-133.
Miller, M. R., Hankinson, J., Brusasco, V., Burgos, F., Casaburi, R., Coates, A., et al.
(2005). Standardisation of spirometry. European Respiratory Journal, 26, 319-
338.
Morgan, D. W., Kohrt, W. M., Bates, B. J., & Skinner, J. S. (1987). Effects of
respiratory muscle endurance training on ventilatory and endurance performance
129
of moderately trained cyclists. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 8, 88-
93.
Paton, C. D., & Hopkins, W. G. (2001). Tests of cycling performance. Sports Medicine,
31(7), 489-496.
Paton, C. D., & Hopkins, W. G. (2005). Combining explosive and high-resistance
training improves performance in competitive cyclists. Journal of Strength and
Conditioning Research, 19(4), 826-830.
Prabhu, B., Zintei, T., Marciniuk, D., Ciemens, R., & Gallagher, C. G. (1992). Lack of
effect of pedalling frequency on breathing pattern during bicycle ergometry.
American Review of Respiratory Disease, 145, A582.
Pyne, D. B., Boston, T., Martin, D. T., & Logan, A. (2000). Evaluation of the lactate
pro blood analyser. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 82(1-2), 1120116.
Rack, P. M. H., & Westbury, D. R. (1969). The effects of length and stimulus rate on
tension in the isometric cat soleus muscle. Journal of Physiology, 204, 443-460.
Ram, F. S. F., Holloway, E. A., & Jones, P. W. (2003). Breathing retraining for asthma.
Respiratory Medicine, 97, 501-507.
Rapee, R. M. (1985). A case of panic disorder treated with breathing retraining. Journal
of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 16, 63-65.
Richter, E. A., Kiens, B., Hargreaves, M., & Kjaer, M. (1992). Effect of arm-cranking
on leg blood flow and noradrenaline spillover during leg exercise in man. Acta
Physiologica Scandinavica, 144, 9-14.
Romer, L. M., & Dempsey, J. A. (2006). Effects of exercise-induced arterial
hypoxaemia on limb muscle fatigue and performance. Clinical and Experimental
Pharmacology and Physiology, 33(4), 391-394.
Romer, L. M., McConnell, A. K., & Jones, D. A. (2002a). Effects of inspiratory muscle
training on time-trial performance in trained cyclists. Journal of Sports Sciences,
20, 547-562.
Romer, L. M., McConnell, A. K., & Jones, D. A. (2002b). Effects of inspiratory muscle
training upon recovery time during high intensity, repetitive sprint activity.
International Journal of Sports Medicine, 23, 353-360.
Romer, L. M., McConnell, A. K., & Jones, D. A. (2002c). Inspiratory muscle fatigue in
trained cyclists: effects of inspiratory muscle training. Medicine & Science in
Sports & Exercise, 34, 785-792.
Ruiter, C. d., Rijken, H., Garssen, B., & Kraaimaat, F. (1989). Breathing retraining,
exposure, and a combination of both, in the treatment of panic disorder with
agoraphobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 647-655.
Rundell, K. W., & Spiering, M. S. (2003). Inspiratory stridor in elite athletes. Chest,
123(2), 468-474.
Saltin, B., & Astrand, P. O. (1967). Maximum oxygen uptake in athletes. Journal of
Applied Physiology, 23(3), 353-358.
Scheuermann, B. W., & Kowalchuk, J. M. (1999). Breathing patterns during slow and
fast ramp exercise in man. Experimental Physiology, 84, 109-120.
Secher, N. H., Clausen, J. P., Klausen, K., Nore, I., & Trap-Jensen, J. (1977). Central
and regional circulatory effects of adding arm exercise to les exercise. Acta
Physiologica Scandinavica, 100, 288-297.
Sharp, J., Drutz, W., & Moisan, T. (1980). Postural relief of dyspnoea in severe chronic
obstructive pulmonary disease. American Review of Respiratory Disease, 122,
201-211.
130
Sheel, A. W., Derchak, P. A., Morgan, B. J., Pegelow, D. F., Jacques, J. A., & Dempsey,
J. A. (2001). Fatigueing inspiratory muscle work causes reflex reduction in
resting leg blood flow in humans. Journal of Physiology, 537(1), 277-289.
Sheel, A. W., Koehle, M. S., Guenette, J. A., Foster, G. E., Sporer, B. C., Diep, T. T., et
al. (2006). Human ventilatory responsiveness to hypoxia is unrelated to maximal
aerobic capacity. Journal of Applied Physiology, 100(4), 1204-1209.
Sheel, A. W., Lama, I., Potvin, P., Coutts, K. D., & McKenzie, D. C. (1996).
Comparison of aero-bars versus traditional cycling postures on physiological
parameters during submaximal cycling. Canadian Journal of Applied
Physiology, 21, 16-22.
Shephard, R. J. (1998). Science and medicine of rowing: a review. Journal of Sports
Sciences, 16, 603-620.
Soley, M. H., & Shock, N. W. (1938). The etiology of effort syndrome. American
Journal of Medical Science, 196, 840-851.
Sonetti, D. A., Wetter, T. J., Pegelow, D. F., & Dempsey, J. A. (2001). Effects of
respiratory muscle training versus placebo on endurance exercise performance.
Respiration Physiology, 127, 185-199.
Sonnetti, D. A., Wetter, T. J., Pegelow, D. F., & Dempsey, J. A. (2001). Effects of respiratory
muscle training versus placebo on endurance exercise performance. Respiration
Physiology, 127, 185-199.
Spengler, C. M., Roos, M., Laube, S. M., & Boutellier, U. (1999). Decreased exercise
blood lactate concentrations after respiratory endurance training in humans.
European Journal of Applied Physiology, 79, 299-305.
Steinacker, J. M., Both, M., & Whipp, B. J. (1993). Pulmonary mechanics and
entrainment of respiration and stroke rate during rowing. International Journal
of Sports Medicine, 14, S15-S19.
Steussi, C., Spengler, C., Knopfli-Lenzin, C., Markov, G., & Boutellier, U. (2001).
Respiratory muscle endurance training in humans increases cycling endurance
without affecting blood gas concentrations. European Journal of Applied
Physiology, 84, 582-558.
Stickland, M. K., & Lovering, A. T. (2006). Exercise-induced intrapulmonary
arteriovenous shunting and pulmonary gas exchange. Exercise and Sport
Sciences Review, 34, 99-106.
Sutton, J. R. (1992). Limitations to maximal oxygen uptake. Sports Medicine, 13(2),
127-133.
Syabbalo, N. C., Krishnan, B., Zintel, T., & Gallagher, C. G. (1994). Differential
ventilatory control during constant work rate and incremental exercise.
Respiration Physiology, 97, 175-187.
Taylor, A., Rehder, K., Hyatt, R., & Parker, J. (1989). Clinical Respiratory Physiology.
Philadelphia: W B Saunders.
Thoman, R., Stoker, G., & Ross, J. (1966). The efficacy of pursed lips breathing in
patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. American Review of
Respiratory Disease, 93, 100-106.
Thomas, M., McKinley, R. K., Freeman, E., Foy, C., Prodger, P., & Price, D. (2003).
Breathing retraining for dysfunctional breathing in asthma: a randomised
controlled trial. Thorax, 58, 110-115.
Tisp, B., Burns, M., Kro, D., Kao, D., Madison, R., & Herrera, J. (1986). Pursed lip
breathing using ear oximetry. Chest, 90, 218-221.
131
Tucker, R., Bester, A., Lambert, E. V., Noakes, T. D., Vaughan, C. L., & St Clair
Gibson, A. (2006). Non-random fluctuations in power output during self-paced
exercise. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 40, 912-917.
Van Dixhoorn, J., & Duivenvoorden, H. J. (1984). Efficacy of nijmegan questionnaire
in recognition of the hyperventilation syndrome. Journal of Pscyhosomatic
Research, 29(2), 199-206.
van Doorn, P., Folgering, H., & Colla, P. (1982). Control of the end-tidal PCO2 in the
hyperventilation syndrome. Effects of biofeedback and breathing instructions
compared. Bulletin Europeen de Physiopathologie Respiratoire, 18(829-836).
Vaughan, C. L. (1984). Biomechanics of Sport. Florida: CRC Press, Inc.
Verges, S., Notter, D., & Spengler, C. (2006). Influence of diaphragm and rib cage
muscle fatigue in healthy humans. Respiratory Physiology and Neurobiology,
154(3), 431-442.
Volianitis, S., McConnell, A. K., Koutedakis, Y., McNaughton, L., Backx, K., & Jones,
D. A. (2001). Inspiratory muscle training improves rowing performance.
Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 33(5), 803-809.
Ward, M. E., Eidelman, D., & Stubbing, D. G. (1988). Respiratory sensation and pattern
of respiratory muscle activation during diaphragm fatigue. Journal of Applied
Physiology, 65(5), 2181-2189.
Ward, S. A. (2007). Ventilatory control in humans: constraints and limitations.
Experimental Physiology, 92(2), 357-366.
Webber, A. B., & Pryor, J. A. (1994). Physiotherapy for Respiratory and Cardiac
Problems. Singapore: Churchill-Livingston.
West, J. B. (1995). Respiratory Physiology - the essentials (5th ed.). Baltimore:
Williams & Wilkins.
Wetter, T. J., Harms, C. A., Nelson, W. B., Pegelow, D. F., & Dempsey, J. A. (1999).
Influence of respiratory muscle work on VO2 and leg blood flow during
submaximal exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology, 87(2), 643-651.
Wiles, J. D., Coleman, D., Tegerdine, M., & Swaine, I. L. (2006). The effects of
caffeine ingestion on performance time, speed and power during a laboratory-
based 1 km cycling time-trial. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24(11), 1165-1171.
Williams, J. S., Wongsathikun, J., Boon, S. M., & Acevedo, E. O. (2002). Inspiratory
muscle training fails to improve cadence capacity in athletes. Medicine and
Science in Sports and Exercise, 34(7), 1194-1198.
Wilmore, J. H., & Costill, D. L. (1999). Physiology of Sport and Exercise (2 ed.).
Auckland: Human Kinetics.
Wilson, R. C., & Jones, P. W. (1989). A comparison of the visual analogue scale and
modified borg scale for the measurement of dyspnoea during exercise. Clinical
Science, 76(3), 277-282.
Withers, R., & Hamdorf, P. (1989). Effect of immersion on lung capacities and
volumes: implications for the densitometric estimation of relative body fat.
Journal of Sports Sciences, 7, 21-30.
Witt, J. D., Guenette, J. A., Rupert, J. L., McKenzie, D. C., & Sheel, A. W. (2007).
Inspiratory muscle training attenuates the human respiratory metabolreflex.
Journal of Physiology, 13(Epub).
www.cyclingnews.com.
http://www.cyclingnews.com/road/2004/olympics04/?id=results/road_men_tt.
Wyelegala, J. A., Pendergrast, D. R., Gosselin, L. E., Warkander, D. E., & Lundgren, C.
E. G. (2007). Respiratory muscle training improves swimming endurance in
divers. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 99(4), 393-404.
132
Yoshida, T., Chida, M., Ichioka, M., & Suda, Y. (1987). Blood lactate parameters
related to aerobic capacity and endurance performance. European Journal of
Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 56(1), 7-11.
Zavorsky, G. S., Murias, J. M., Gow, J., Kim, D. J., Poulin-Harnois, C., Kubow, S., et al.
(2007). Laboratory 20-km cycle time trial reproducibility [Electronic Version].
International Journal of Sports Medicine.
133