Touch
Touch
Touch
TOUCH
AND
MASSAGE
in Early
Child
Development
EDITED BY
ISBN 0-931562-30-9
Printed in the United States of America. All rights reserved. Except as permitted under
the Copyright Act of 1976, no part of this publication may be reproduced or distributed
in any form or by any means or stored in a database or retrieval system without prior
written permission of the publisher.
The opinions and data presented by the authors are their own and are not necessarily
those of Johnson & Johnson Pediatric Institute, L.L.C. or the editors. Accurate referencing
is the responsibility of the writer and editor.
TOUCH
AND
MASSAGE
in Early
Child
Development
EDITED BY
C O N T R I B U T O R S . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv
P R E F A C E . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii
Johnson & Johnson Pediatric Institute, L.L.C.
I N T R O D U C T I O N : T H E I M P O R TA N C E O F T O U C H . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix
Tiffany Field, PhD
S E C T I O N I . M AT E R N A L T O U C H A N D T O U C H P E R C E P T I O N . . . . . 1
S E C T I O N I I . T H E R A P E U T I C A P P L I C AT I O N S O F T O U C H
I N P R E G N A N C Y, L A B O R A N D P O S T B I R T H . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97
S E C T I O N I V. E F F E C T S O F M A S S A G E O N S L E E P,
R E L A X AT I O N A N D W E L L - B E I N G . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181
Chapter 11: Massage Therapy and Sleep-Wake Rhythms in the Neonate . . . . . . . . . 183
Sari Goldstein Ferber, PhD
S E C T I O N V. M A S S A G E T H E R A P Y F O R O R P H A N S A N D
P E D I AT R I C P R O B L E M S . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 209
The Johnson & Johnson Pediatric Institute, L.L.C., is proud to sponsor this
groundbreaking compilation of studies on the power of touch in childhood growth
and development.
Astounding advances in health and medicine have improved the lives of millions
of children in the last century. Vaccines prevented childhood diseases; antibiotics
cured those who became ill; sophisticated new technologies and gleaming
incubators helped physicians keep tinier and younger premature infants alive.
Today, a growing body of research from around the world shows that human touch
is a powerful contributor to infants’ ability to thrive and grow. Rigorous scientific
studies show that
Each of the studies included in this book establishes a powerful case for touch.
In bringing this wide-ranging research together, the Johnson & Johnson Pediatric
Institute’s goal is to remind us that in this high technology world, there is still
room for low technology therapies that can increase the health and happiness
of infants and families around the world.
The Impo r t a nc e o f To u c h
The act of touch fulfills the basic human need to feel safe, comfortable and loved.
Touch is also an intrinsic factor in child development, but despite touch’s importance,
in recent years “touching” has been tabooed in the American school system because
of fears of sexual and physical abuse. Elementary and high school teachers have
been mandated not to touch children, and even children as young as preschoolers
cannot be touched in many private nursery schools because of potential accusations
of sexual abuse following increased publicity about abuse in schools.1 Although
the rates of sexual and physical abuse have climbed steadily despite this mandate,
teachers uphold this rule from fear of lawsuits by parents and losing their jobs. In
a study we conducted on the amount of teacher touching in preschool classrooms,
we observed significant decreases in teachers touching nursery school children as
the children became older (from infant to toddler to preschool age) including less
holding, hand-holding, stroking and carrying, and resulted in increased aggressive
behavior.2 Being concerned about the extremely low levels of touching, we then
provided demonstrations and instructions to the teachers in an attempt to increase
the levels of touching. Following this intervention, the teachers increased their level
of touching (including holding, hugging, kissing, hand-holding and caregiving
touch), although touching was still relatively infrequent following this intervention.
Others have tried to increase touching in nursery schools by having the children
give each other back rubs.
The implications of limited touch for children involve significant effects on their
growth, development and emotional well-being. Extreme cases can be seen in the
Romanian orphanages where children have achieved half their expected height due
to extreme forms of touch deprivation. Their cognitive and emotional development
has also been significantly delayed by the lack of physical stimulation. Other data
suggest that touch deprivation in early development may contribute to violence
in adults.
x Introduction
In a cross-cultural study, cultures in which there was more physical affection toward
infants had lower rates of adult physical violence and vice versa.3 The amount of
touching that occurs in different cultures is highly variable. For example, touching
behavior between couples was observed in cafes in several countries.4 Couples
were observed sitting at tables in cafes for 30-minute periods, and the amount
of touching between them was recorded. Among the highest touch cultures was
France (110 times per 30 minutes) and among the lowest was the United States
(2 times per 30 minutes). The high touch cultures had relatively low rates of
violence while the low touch cultures, in turn, had extremely high rates of youth
and adult violence. In a 1994 report by the Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, the homicide rate per 100,000 population was 1 in France and 22 in
the United States.5
The cafe data suggest that French families may provide more touch stimulation for
their children, and conversely, American families provide less. Studies we conducted
on Paris and Miami playgrounds suggested that these cultures differed in the
touching of preschoolers by parents, in peer touching among preschoolers and in
peer touching in young adolescents.6,7 For the Paris parents, touching occurred 43%
of the time as opposed to 11% of the time for Miami parents. Affectionate touch
between the preschool children occurred 23% of the time on the Paris playground
and only 3% of the time on the Miami playground. In contrast, aggressive touch
occurred 37% of the time on the Miami playgrounds and only 1% of the time on
the Paris playgrounds.
Because infants and toddlers are touched so infrequently in nursery school, we have
begun instructing teachers on how to massage infants and toddlers. In our studies,
we have noted that infants/toddlers who are massaged before naptime at preschool
are less irritable, go to sleep faster and sleep more soundly. Thus, teachers are very
happy massaging them. Parents have also been noted to help reduce irritability and
enhance sleep by massaging their infants before bedtime. These are potential ways
to reintroduce touch into our society.
This volume highlights the importance of touch across early development. Diego
et al present research data on how the fetus responds to tactile stimulation, while
Hernandez-Reif et al discuss the data on newborns’ responses to tactile stimulation.
In a chapter by Perez and Gewirtz, studies are presented on how young infants
respond to different types of touch. Infants’ responses to touch during early
Introduction xi
The power of touch as a therapeutic modality is highlighted by the data from these
studies. Many of them also suggest the importance of training parents to massage
their infants for growth and development and their chronically ill children for
helping treat their medical conditions. Methods to provide adequate touch could
effectively be introduced in delivery units, children’s hospitals and schools. They are
easy to learn and have been noted to help not only infants and children but also
reduce the stress levels and stress hormones of the parents and teachers providing
the stimulation. Touching and being touched are comforting, growth-producing
and healing experiences that need to be supported by our medical and educational
communities. The data presented in this volume are compelling and will help
advance touch in our world.
Ref ere n c e s
1. Mazur S, Pekor C. Can teachers touch children anymore: physical contact and its value in
child development. Young Children. 1985;40:10-12.
2. Field T, Harding J, Soliday B, Lasko D, Gonzalez N, Valdcon C. Touching in Infant, Toddler
and Preschool Nurseries. Early Child Development and Care. 1994;98:113-120.
3. Prescott JW. Affectional bonding for the prevention of violent behaviors: neurobiological,
psychological and religious/spiritual determinants. In: Hertzberg LJ, Ostrum GF, Field JR,
eds. Violent Behavior. Great Neck, NY: PMA Publishing; 1990:95-124.
4. Jourard SM. An exploratory study of body accessibility. British Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology. 1966;5:221-231.
5. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. National Center for Injury Prevention and
Control International Comparisons of Homocide Rates in Males 15-24 Years of Age, 1988-1991.
Atlanta, Ga: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 1994.
6. Field T. Preschoolers in America are touched less and are more aggressive than preschoolers
in France. Early Child Development and Care. 1999;151:11-17.
7. Field T. American adolescents touch each other less and are more aggressive toward their
peers as compared with French adolescents. Adolescence. 1999;34:753-758.
Touc h a n d Ma s s a g e i n Ea rl y Ch ild De ve lop me nt
SECTION I.
M AT E R N A L TO U C H A N D
TOUCH PERCEPTION
CHAPTER 1:
F E TA L R E S P O N S E S TO
OF PREGNANT WOMEN
Miguel Diego, MA
Tiffany Field, PhD
Maria Hernandez-Reif, PhD
Touch Research Institutes
University of Miami School of Medicine
Ab s t ra c t
The fetus responds to extrauterine stimuli depending on such factors as the nature
and intensity of the stimuli and gestational age. The study discussed in this chapter
assessed fetal responses to maternal foot and hand massages during ultrasound
examinations conducted midgestation (mean, 19.9 weeks). The study involved
80 women, divided equally into a foot-massage substudy and a hand-massage
substudy. Control subjects did not receive massages, but rather rested quietly during
the observation periods. Results indicated that foot massaging was associated with
a 16% increase in fetal activity. In contrast, hand massaging elicited only a 10%
rise in fetal activity, which did not significantly differ from the activity of the
control group. These results suggest that the hands may be less innervated than the
feet or that the foot- versus the hand-massage procedure may have triggered distinct
reflex arches and physiological responses. Further research is needed to explore the
mechanisms involved in the fetal response to maternal tactile stimulation.
4 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Introd uc t i o n
The fetus is often believed to develop in total isolation from the outside world.
Throughout gestation, the mother and fetus share a common environment, which
is affected continuously by the external world. Extrauterine light and sound cross
the maternal abdomen, uterus and embryonic membranes, thereby reaching the
amniotic fluid and the fetus.1,2 Furthermore, vibration applied to the mother’s
abdomen is transmitted mechanically across the intrauterine environment.1
Similarly, external tactile stimulation has been shown to produce changes in
the mother’s physiology3,4 and biochemistry5,6 that may affect the intrauterine
environment indirectly, even if such stimulation is not applied directly to the
mother’s abdomen.
Extrauterine stimulation has been used in the antepartum assessment of fetal well-
being.7,8 Vibratory stimulation has been used to test fetal hearing,9 and habituation
to vibratory stimulation has been used to test fetal function.7,10 Fetuses who respond
to vibratory stimulation have been reported to have better biophysical profiles.11,12
Fetal responses to stimulation depend largely on the properties of the stimuli and
the maturation state of the sensory system being tested. The fetus responds to
airborne auditory stimuli as early as 28 weeks gestation,13 which is only 4 weeks
after the development of the fetal auditory structures.14 Furthermore, the fetal
response depends largely on the intensity of the stimulus.15-17
Similarly, responses of the fetus to tactile stimulation also depend on the intensity
of the stimuli. Even though responses by the fetus to tactile stimuli have been noted
very early in gestation,18,19 fetal responses to vibratory stimulation are only apparent
after 26 weeks gestation.16,18,20 Taken together, these findings suggest that vibratory
stimulation applied to the mother’s abdomen may not be perceived because those
nerve endings have not yet been developed and myelinated.21 Alternatively, the
vibratory stimulation that is perceived by the fetus may be primarily auditory, and
not tactile in nature, and would be perceived only after sufficient development of
the fetal auditory system.
Other forms of external stimulation that may elicit fetal responses have also been
studied. Acupuncture delivered to the mother at acupoints SP-6 (located on the
Chapter 1: Fetal Responses to Foot and Hand Massage of Pregnant Women 5
tibia, above the tip of the medial malleolus) and LI-4 (located on the hand,
between the 1st and 2nd metacarpal bones) affect umbilical-artery blood flow to
the fetus during the third trimester.22 When delivered to the mother at accupoint
BL-67 (located on the fifth toe), acupuncture results in decreased fetal heart rate
(FHR)23 and increased movement (FM) in fetuses during the third trimester.23,24
The stimulation of specific acupuncture points is believed to activate specific
nervous system pathways.25
Massaging highly innervated areas, such as the feet and hands, may also stimulate
fetal activity. Midwives have reported anecdotally that massaging the Achilles tendon
of pregnant women during late gestation may induce contractions. Massaging the
feet and hands has been shown to reduce heart rate, respiration rate and systolic and
diastolic blood pressure,4,6 decrease levels of norepinephrine and epinephrine, “and
attenuate the response of cortisol to a stressful procedure.” These changes in the
mother’s physiology and biochemistry following massage therapy may contribute
to the increase in fetal activity. Data have also shown that massaging other highly
innervated areas of the mother’s body—nipples26 and breasts27—results in uterine
contractions during pregnancy.
Cur re n t St u d i e s
In this chapter, we review the studies we have done with several of our colleagues
on the effects of massaging the mother’s feet and hands on fetal activity.28 The
extremities were selected because the feet and hands are highly innervated and
because fetal responses to maternal foot acupuncture have been noted.23,24 These
studies were conducted midgestation, inasmuch as fetal responses to stimuli have
been noted as early as 14 weeks gestation,18,19 yet fetal activity responses to vibratory
stimuli occur only after 26 weeks gestation.16,18,20 Furthermore, the responses of
fetuses to acupuncture have been studied only after 33 weeks.23,24 As such, we and
our colleagues were interested in determining if foot and hand massage of the
pregnant woman would contribute to fetal activity. Finding increased fetal activity
would provide preliminary support for using massage as an assessment of fetal
responsivity prior to 26 weeks gestation.
6 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
METHODS
The fetal activity assessment and stimulation procedures occurred immediately prior
to a standard clinical ultrasound examination. The pregnant women completed the
State/Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) immediately before and immediately following
the ultrasound. These women were then monitored by ultrasound for baseline fetal
activity while lying on an examination table on their left side for 3 minutes. This was
followed by a 3-minute massage or control period and a 3-minute poststimulation
observation period. All participants were assessed between 10:00 am and 2:00 pm
in the same ultrasound examination office. No significant differences were noted on
assessment times or days.
FETAL-ACTIVITY ASSESSMENTS
Fetal activity was assessed using interval recordings on a real-time ultrasound scanner
fitted with a single Doppler transducer applied to the mother’s abdomen.28 Every
effort was made by the technician to visualize the entire fetus by manipulating the
transducer and adjusting the zoom control (Figure 1). At 20 weeks gestation, the
majority of fetuses were visualized fully. If the fetus could not be visualized fully, the
technician focused on the head, torso and arms, as well as on the upper-leg regions
to assess leg movement. Every 3 seconds, the researcher was prompted by a tape-
recorded cue (heard through an earphone) to record the type of fetal activity that
was occurring. All behaviors occurring between tones were ignored. The 3-second
interval was chosen arbitrarily for its ability to generate an easy-to-determine total
([60 seconds/3 seconds = 20 observations] x 3 minutes = 60 observations). The
researcher recorded the following: a single limb movement, characterized by any
movement of any one extremity; multiple limb movements; exclusive of torso
Chapter 1: Fetal Responses to Foot and Hand Massage of Pregnant Women 7
movements (i.e. more than one extremity, including the head, moving at any one
time); gross body movements with or without single or multiple limb movements;
or no movement at all. The percentage of time the fetus engaged in each
movement category was calculated by dividing the total number of movements by
the total number of observation units. Total amount of movement was calculated
by subtracting “1” from the percentage time that the fetus did not show any
movement. Interrater reliability, calculated on 10% of the samples from 2 of the
observers, revealed adequate kappa values ranging from 0.82 to 1.00.
F O OT- M A S S A G E P RO C E D U R E
of the feet; applying pressure with both thumbs to the arch of each foot (one foot
at a time), and then using the thumbs to rub between each toe using an up-and-
down motion (toward the web of the toe, and then toward the end of the toe) for
30 seconds on each foot; kneading the bottom of each foot for 20 seconds each,
and then the top of each foot for 20 seconds each, one foot at a time; flexing and
extending each foot, and then flexing and extending all toes on each foot, one foot
at a time for 3 seconds each; squeezing each foot for 5 seconds, moving from the
heel to the toes, one foot at a time; and, finger-stroking the entire top and bottom
of each foot, using all 5 fingers, one foot at a time, for 7 seconds.28
HAND-MASSAGE PROCEDURE
The 3-minute hand massage (Figure 2) was performed by the therapist as follows:
holding the wrist with one hand, and with the other hand pushing against the
fingers for 5 seconds each; with both hands, stroking the tops of the mother’s hands
briskly for 10 seconds each; with a thumb on the top of each hand and the other
fingers on the palms, compressing both hands intermittently, starting at the pinkies
and moving toward the thumbs for 10 seconds each; finger-stroking the entire back
of each hand, from the fingers to the wrists for 10 seconds each; using the thumbs
and forefingers to squeeze each finger on each hand for 20 seconds each; stretching
each finger by encircling each finger completely, gently pulling fingers away from
the hand, for 20 seconds each; applying pressure intermittently to one hand at a
time from the center to the edge of the bottom of the hand for 10 seconds each;
and, holding the wrist with the left hand, and with the right hand gently pulling
the hand away from the wrist for 5 seconds each.28
CONTROL-GROUP PROCEDURE
In the control procedure for the foot and hand massage the pregnant women were
asked to lie on their left side while resting quietly for 3 minutes.28
The mothers in the massage groups and in the control groups on average reported
feeling less anxiety following the massage or rest session.28 The reduced anxiety was
probably a function of the visit for ultrasound. Mothers in this study, each of
whom had normal, uncomplicated pregnancies, received immediate feedback from
the ultrasonographer on the health of their fetus. In an earlier study we had shown
that mothers who received feedback during ultrasound examinations reported less
anxiety than mothers who did not receive feedback.29 In addition, simply resting
on the left side for 12 minutes may lower anxiety levels.
Even though vibratory stimulation has failed to elicit fetal movement before 26
weeks gestation,16,18,20 the foot massage elicited fetal activity as early as 20 weeks
gestation.28 The fetuses of mothers who received a 3-minute foot massage showed
increased activity, whereas the fetuses of mothers in the control group did not
(Figure 3). The increased fetal activity is consistent with increased fetal movement
observed following foot acupuncture in older fetuses.23,24 Given that the feet are
highly innervated, stimulating the feet of a pregnant woman may trigger changes in
fetal activity via the activation of reflex arches, changes in maternal physiology and
other unknown mechanisms. These findings are consistent with research that show
10 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Figure 3. Percent time fetal movement occurred in response to foot and hand massage.
Fetuses of mothers receiving a 3-minute foot massage showed a significant increase in
fetal movement.28
innervated than the feet, which might explain why massaging the hands elicited
a less-intense response than massaging the feet. Alternatively, the foot-massage
procedure, versus the hand-massage procedure, may have triggered distinct reflex
arches and physiological responses. Using longer observation periods for the foot
and hand massages may have provided information on the onset and the duration of
the fetal responses to the foot-massage condition. Thus, the lesser effect of the hand
massage versus the foot massage may have been an artifact of the short observation
period. Further study may help elucidate the mechanisms whereby massaging the
feet of a pregnant woman results in increased fetal activity.
Conclusions
Ackn ow le d gm e nt s
We would like to thank the pregnant women who participated in these studies.
We would also like to thank Jean Pierre Lecanuet, John Dieter and Fawwaz Salman
for their help in designing the study and Julia Beutler, Shay Largie and Mercedes
Redzepi for their help in collecting the data. This research was supported by funding
provided by the Johnson & Johnson Pediatric Institute, L.L.C. and a National
Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Senior Research Scientist Award (#MH00331)
and an NIMH Merit Award (#MH46586) to Tiffany Field.
12 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Ref ere n c e s
1. Gerhardt KJ, Abrams RM. Fetal exposures to sound and vibroacoustic stimulation. Journal
of Perinatology. 2000;20(8, pt 2):S21-S30.
2. Kiuchi M, Nagata N, Ikeno S, Terakawa N. The relationship between the response to
external light stimulation and behavioral states in the human fetus: how it differs from
vibroacoustic stimulation. Early Human Development. 2000;58:153-165.
3. Delaney JP, Leong KS, Watkins A, Brodie D. The short-term effects of myofascial trigger
point massage therapy on cardiac autonomic tone in healthy subjects. Journal of Advanced
Nursing. 2002;37:364-371.
4. Hayes J, Cox C. Immediate effects of a five-minute foot massage on patients in critical care.
Intensive and Critical Care Nursing. 1999;15:77-82.
5. Field T. Maternal depression effects on infants and early interventions. Preventive Medicine.
1998;27:200-203.
6. Kim MS, Cho KS, Woo H, Kim JH. Effects of hand massage on anxiety in cataract surgery
using local anesthesia. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery. 2001;27:884-890.
7. Devoe LD. Nonstress testing and contraction stress testing. Obstetrics and Gynecology Clinics
of North America. 1999;26:535-556.
8. Smith CV. Vibroacoustic stimulation. Clinical Obstetrics and Gynecology. 1995;38:68-77.
9. Ke X, Gu Z, Wu R. Vibratory acoustic stimulation test in fetal hearing monitor [in Chinese].
Chinese Journal of Otorhinolaryngology. 1995;30:264-266.
10. Kuhlman KA, Burns KA, Depp R, Sabbagha RE. Ultrasonic imaging of normal fetal response
to external vibratory acoustic stimulation. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology.
1988;158:47-51.
11. Inglis SR, Druzin ML, Wagner WE, Kogut E. The use of vibroacoustic stimulation during
the abnormal or equivocal biophysical profile. Obstetrics and Gynecology. 1993;82:371-374.
12. Sarinoglu C, Dell J, Mercer BM, Sibai BM. Fetal startle response observed under
ultrasonography: a good predictor of a reassuring biophysical profile. Obstetrics and
Gynecology. 1996;88:599-602.
13. Querleu D, Renard X, Boutteville C, Crepin G. Hearing by the human fetus? Seminars
in Perinatology. 1989;13:403-420.
14. Pujol R, Lavigne-Rebillard M. Sensory and neural structures in the developing human
cochlea. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. 1995; 32:S177-S182.
15. Kisilevsky BS, Muir DW. Human fetal and subsequent newborn responses to sound and
vibration. Infant Behavior and Development. 1991;14:1-26.
16. Kisilevsky BS, Muir DW, Low JA. Maturation of responses elicited by a vibroacoustic
stimulus in a group of high-risk fetuses. Maternal/Child Nursing Journal. 1990;19:239-250.
Chapter 1: Fetal Responses to Foot and Hand Massage of Pregnant Women 13
17. Lecanuet JP, Granier-Deferre C, Busnel MC. Fetal cardiac and motor responses to octave-
band noises as a function of central frequency, intensity and heart rate variability. Early
Human Development . 1988;18:81-93.
18. Hepper PG, Shahidullah BS. Development of fetal hearing. Archives of Disease in Childhood.
1994;71:F81-F87.
19. Hooker D. The Prenatal Origin of Behavior. Lawrence, Kan: University of Kansas Press; 1952.
20. Kisilevsky BS, Muir DW, Low JA. Maturation of human fetal responses to vibroacoustic
stimulation. Child Development. 1992;63:1497-1508.
21. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessel TM. Principles of Neural Science. 4th ed. Columbus, Ohio:
McGraw-Hill/Appleton & Lange; 1993.
22. Zeisler H, Eppel W, Husslein P, Bernaschek G, Deutinger J. Influence of acupuncture on
Doppler ultrasound in pregnant women. Ultrasound in Obstetrics & Gynecology. 2001;17:
229-232.
23. Neri I, Fazzio M, Menghini S, Volpe A, Facchinetti F. Non-stress test changes during
acupuncture plus moxibustion on BL67 point in breech presentation. Journal of the Society
for Gynecologic Investigation. 2002;9:158-162.
26. Christensson K, Nilsson BA, Stock S, Matthiesen AS, Uvnäs-Moberg K. Effect of nipple
stimulation on uterine activity and on plasma levels of oxytocin in full term, healthy,
pregnant women. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica. 1989;68:205-210.
27. Gantes M, Kirchhoff KT, Work BA Jr. Breast massage to obtain contraction stress test.
Nursing Research. 1985;34:338-341.
28. Diego MA, Dieter JN, Field T, et al. Fetal activity following vibratory stimulation of the
mother’s abdomen. Developmental Psychobiology. 2002;41:396-406.
29. Field T, Sandberg D, Quetel TA, Garcia R, Rosario M. Effects of ultrasound feedback
on pregnancy anxiety, fetal activity, and neonatal outcome. Ultrasound Feedback.
1985;66:525-528.
30. Nolte J. The Human Brain: An Introduction to Its Functional Anatomy. 4th ed. St Louis, Mo:
Mosby; 1999.
31. DiPietro JA, Irizarry R, Hawkins M, Costigan K. The psychophysiology of the maternal-fetal
relationship. Paper presented at: The International Conference on Infant Studies; July 17,
2000; Brighton, England.
32. Manders MA, Sonder GJ, Mulder EJ, Visser GH. The effects of maternal exercise on fetal
heart rate and movement patterns. Early Human Development. 1997;48:237-247.
33. Marsal K, Lofgren O, Gennser G. Fetal breathing movements and maternal exercise. Acta
Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica. 1979;58:197-201.
CHAPTER 2:
TOUCH PERCEPTION
I N N E O N AT E S
Ab s t ra c t
Sucking and grasping behaviors occur in utero. This chapter presents data suggesting
that such behaviors exhibited by newborns are more than just reflexive responses.
Studies involving the handling and mouthing of objects by neonates indicate that
newborns clutch or grasp objects presented to them and suck on objects that provide
no nutritional value. Newborns, it has been determined, distinguish between
objects, perceiving differences in hardness, texture, weight, shape and temperature.
Thus, it appears that infants explore objects actively with their hands and mouths,
using behaviors similar to those exhibited by adults, attempting to perceive the
properties of specific objects. Future studies should explore further the touch and
perception abilities of neonates.
16 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Touch Pe rc e pt io n i n Ne o na t e s
The newborn baby invariably responds to touch. Stroking the newborn’s face often
leads to head turning. Mouth opening and closing (or “rooting”) may follow, and if
a finger or nipple touches the newborn’s lips, typical responding includes opening
the mouth, closing the lips around the object and sucking. These behaviors exhibited
by the neonate are observed routinely during feedings and in the course of neonatal
assessments, such as with the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale.1
Newborns also respond when their hands are touched. Placing a finger in the palm
of the newborn results in the fingers of the neonate closing or grasping. In a study
by Nagy and Molnar,2 decreased heart rate was observed following grasping, which
was interpreted as neonatal calming. However, heart-rate decelerations have also
been used as measures of “attending” in the very young infant.3,4 Therefore, perhaps
grasping serves multiple functions, as does sucking, the goals of which can be for
feeding, for calming (sucking on a pacifier or finger) or for exploring objects.
To date, these responses to touch by the neonate have been defined in the literature
as “reflexes,” or involuntary responses to stimulation,5,6 implying little or no
intentional behavior on the part of the newborn. Interestingly, examinations by
ultrasound have revealed grasping of the umbilical cord by the fetus at 18 weeks
gestation,7 and thumb-sucking at as early as 15 weeks gestation,8 suggesting that
sucking and grasping are not new behaviors for the neonate, or that the neonate
comes into the world with sucking and grasping experiences.
In this chapter, we question the traditional view that grasping and sucking responses
to touch are merely reflexive responses performed by the newborn. For example, if
sucking is reflexive then why does the neonate stop sucking when he or she is full
or suck more vigorously when hungry? That the responses are variable may suggest
that they are not completely reflexive. Perhaps reflexes exhibited by the neonate are
the precursors of voluntary, intentional “touching back.” Or, perhaps stimulating
the palm or mouth of the neonate elicits the respective reflexive response initially,
but the prolonged behaviors that follow are more “voluntary” by the neonate.
Another view that we question is that grasping and other limited motoric abilities,
such as lack of finger movement, impede newborns’ explorations of properties of
objects with their hands.9,10 A counterview—and the thesis of this chapter—is that
Chapter 2: Touch Perception in Neonates 17
neonates may explore many properties of objects through grasping and mouthing
because of one or more of the following:
• The nerve pathways in the hand and mouth are well-developed, lending
further support to the view that these sensory parts of the newborn are
capable of exploring and picking up information regarding touch.
In this chapter, data are presented regarding the differential grasping and sucking
responses of the neonate to varying object properties. Data are also presented that
document neonatal habituation (becoming less attentive to a familiar object) and
dishabituation (becoming more attentive to a novel object placed in their hands),
as well as behaviors that appear to be precursors of active touching or exploring
by the newborn.
The largest fetal “organ,” and the first to develop, is the sensory-rich skin, or the
sense of touch. By the 30th week of fetal development (10 weeks before birth),
neurological structures and pathways are developed sufficiently for the fetus to
perceive pain.11 Studies indicate that during painful or stressful procedures, such as
during intrauterine needling or surgery, fetuses and preterm infants exhibit elevated
levels of stress hormones and increased activity, which some interpret as pain
responses.12,13 However, because pain is a subjective experience, and because of
the accepted view that responses to touch by fetuses and neonates are “reflexive
responding,” the perception of pain by fetuses continues to be debated.14,15
Studies done in humans and animals also reveal the responding of neonates to heat
and to the skin being touched with fine-hair stimuli. (For a review, see Fitzgerald
18 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
and Jennings.16) Interestingly, these studies53,54 found that the younger the offspring,
the lower the threshold for responding and the more exaggerated the neonate’s
response is to stimulation, suggesting that the newborn is in a more excitable, or
perhaps more vigilant state initially than is the adult.16 Also noteworthy are the
facts that there are approximately 17,000 skin receptors in each hand17 and that the
hands and mouth have the highest concentrations of sensory receptors compared
with other skin areas, further supporting the view that the neonate is capable of
“feeling” in these areas.18 Taken together, the hypothesis that neonates “feel” or
perceive when their hands or mouths are stimulated appears more plausible than
that their responses to touch or pain are reflexive or nonvoluntary.
Touch Perception
When information is detected through the activity of the hands, fingers and/or
mouth, it is referred to as haptic (relating to or based on the sense of touch)
perception. Unlike the other sensory systems, the haptic system is unique in that
it can be used to explore and to alter the environment19: That is, the hands and
mouth can not only feel the properties of an object, but they can change them,
whereas the eyes can only see objects and the ears can only hear sounds, for
examples. Active touching assumes purposeful exploration and involves input from
skin receptors and kinesthetics (movement of muscles and joints),20 such as lifting
and holding, squeezing, finger tracing, prodding or poking. Finger tracing and
prodding are activities not typically performed by neonates.
Chapter 2: Touch Perception in Neonates 19
MOUTHING STUDIES
The infant’s mouth and tongue comprise a highly specialized system used to ingest
nutrients, express emotions and promote self-comfort, such as sucking on a breast,
pacifier, finger or thumb. A MEDLINE® search covering the last 35 years revealed
more than 1200 studies published on these topics in relation to infant sucking.
However, few studies were found regarding the mouthing of objects by infants for
the purposes of exploring, perceiving or learning about properties of objects (eg,
texture, hardness, temperature).
Fewer than one dozen studies emerged when the same literature search was
conducted for the perception of objects by newborns through mouthing or handling
activities. (However, more than 100 entries were found when the keyword included
“infants” rather than “neonates.”) We review the scant literature on neonatal haptic
perception in what follows immediately and in the next section.
20 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
One of the earliest studies of newborn “active oral touching,” conducted by Lipsitt
and Kaye (1965),24 revealed that newborns altered their sucking rate as a function
of the type of stimulus. Newborns were given a rubber nipple, a rubber tube or a
combination of rubber nipples and tubes on which to suck over consecutive trials.
Overall, the newborns sucked the nipples longer than they did the tubes, which
suggested that they preferred the nipples and, therefore, that they must have
perceived the nipples and tubes as different object properties. Almost 20 years later
(1983), using polygraph recordings, Rochat examined the sucking responses of
newborns and 1- and 4-month-old infants presented nipples that varied in shape
(elongated versus round) and material (rubber versus a brass component attached
to the surface of the rubber).25 His findings included increased scanning with the
infants’ tongues and lips and decreased sucking from the newborn period to
4 months of age, which suggested developmental or age differences in mouthing
(or exploratory) behaviors. Additionally, the newborns differentiated 2 nipple types
(round and round with a hole), as indicated by their sucking preferences, and the
1-month-old infants detected the material changes. These findings support the view
that from early in development infant sucking and mouthing behaviors serve
exploratory functions and facilitate the detection of object properties.
In the “texture” study, middle socioeconomic status pregnant women (mean age,
29 years) were assessed for depression until 12 depressed and 12 nondepressed
mothers agreed to participate.30 After delivery, the mothers were contacted and a
laboratory visit was scheduled. The newborns averaged 12 days of age at testing
(7 males/5 females in each group), and all were born full-term and medically
uncomplicated. Held by one of 4 “blinded” experimenters on his or her lap, the
neonates were presented with nubby- and smooth-textured fingertips to suck. The
“nubby” stimulus, a sterile rubber fingertip (Swingline® Rubber Fingertips) of the
kind worn by bankers and others for counting bills or turning pages, was covered
with .10 cm nubby protrusions spaced every .20 cm. The “smooth” stimulus was
simply the nubby fingertip turned inside out, which revealed a smooth, even
surface like that of a rubber glove. Each experimenter wore the nubby fingertip and
the smooth fingertip on the middle finger and index finger of his or her hand—
counterbalanced, so that sometimes the nubby fingertip was on the experimenter’s
middle finger and sometimes on the index finger.
The smooth and nubby fingertips were presented to the neonates for mouthing one
at a time, in an alternating fashion across 6 trials, so that each infant had 3 chances
to explore the texture of each fingertip orally. Each trial started with the newborn’s
first suck of the fingertip and ended when the infant released the sucking grip. The
amount of time (in seconds) spent sucking the smooth and nubby fingertips was
recorded and analyzed.
less time sucking the smooth fingertip compared with the newborns of
nondepressed mothers (Figure 1). These findings suggest the following:
Figure 1. Newborns of depressed mothers sucked the “nubby” and “smooth” objects for
shorter periods than did the neonates of nondepressed mothers.30
apparent that neonatal behaviors may have been influenced indirectly by maternal
depression via the mothers’ altered biochemistry during pregnancy. (See Field,31 for
a review of the effects of maternal depression on the newborn.)
Interestingly, unlike in the “texture alone” study,30 in this temperature study the
infants of depressed mothers sucked more frequently (mean, 117 sucks) than
the neonates of the nondepressed mothers (mean, 79 sucks). Also interesting was
that the duration of sucking was affected by the temperature of the nipples on the
first trial. When the cold nipples were presented first, infants sucked longer on all
subsequent presentations, regardless of the mothers’ depression scores. Why a cold
stimulus led to greater sucking is unclear: Perhaps sucking a cold nipple was more
stimulating or soothing, much like sucking a sweetened pacifier reduces crying in
newborns undergoing painful procedures.36 Nevertheless, this question warrants
further investigation.
Although the duration of sucking increased for all presentations, if the first trial was
done with a cold nipple, neither group showed a preference for sucking one nipple
over the other. This outcome was puzzling and suggested one of two conclusions:
Either the newborns did not discriminate the cold nipple from the warm nipple,
24 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
or perhaps they did perceive the temperature difference, but simply did not prefer
sucking one longer than the other. Also interesting was that mothers who suffered
from more severe depressions, as indicated by higher depression scores on the
Center for Epidemiological Studies of Depression (CES-D) scale,37 had infants
who sucked the nipples for longer durations (Figure 2). The possible reason: The
neonates of depressed mothers were sucking for self-comforting purposes, and not
for exploratory motives, in that both the cold and warm nipples were of a smooth
texture. As is known from at least 3 studies of neonates, infants prefer to suck on
smooth textures and nipple shapes.18,24,30 At birth, infants of depressed mothers are
reported to be more irritable38,39 and have higher cortisol (“stress-hormone”) levels
than do infants of nondepressed mothers.31,40 Thus, the “sucking-for-comforting”
hypothesis seems plausible.
The mothers in this study were also assessed on the Behavioral Inhibition Scale/
Behavioral Activation Scale (BIS/BAS).41 Commonly, individuals who exhibit
behavioral “inhibition” are characterized as anxiety-prone, and they often withdraw
Chapter 2: Touch Perception in Neonates 25
from novelty. Contrastingly, those who display behavioral “activation” are more
impulsive and inclined to approach or engage in activities.42,43 Differing biochemical
profiles have also been reported for infants of intrusive mothers versus infants
of withdrawn mothers, with the intrusive group showing higher dopamine and
serotonin levels than the withdrawn group, and the withdrawn group showing
higher cortisol levels than the intrusive group.44 However, to date, no studies of
perception have been conducted with neonates of inhibited or withdrawn mothers.
We hypothesized that, if the mothers’ biochemistry influences neonatal behavior,
different sucking responses would be evident for infants of intrusive mothers versus
withdrawn mothers. (M.H-R., T.F., M.D. and S. Largie, unpublished data, 2003)
Of the 42 mothers enrolled in this study, 12 met BIS/BAS criteria for behavioral
inhibition (withdrawn) and 12 met criteria for behavioral activation (intrusive).
Ten of these 24 women were deemed nondepressed (6 intrusive; 4 withdrawn):
Hence, this measure examined the influence of inhibited mothers’ versus withdrawn
mothers’ effects on newborn sucking only. Analyses of the neonates of the “intrusive”
mothers revealed that these infants sucked the cold nipple more than the warm
nipple (Figure 3), which suggested that this subgroup perceived the cold nipple
as different from the warm nipple. One explanation for this interesting finding
is that neonates of intrusive mothers may be more reactive to stimulation and/or
are more stimulus-seeking because of their higher dopamine levels, although the
infants’ biochemistry profiles were not measured for this study.
If the infants of the intrusive mothers found the cold nipple more stimulating,
this would support the “stimulus-seeking” hypothesis and explain the findings.
However, because these are post hoc analyses based on small sample sizes, replication
studies will be required before conclusions can be drawn about the interactions
with objects by neonates of intrusive mothers. In any event, the fact that differences
were found in sucking preferences between these 2 groups suggests the need for
studying the perceptual abilities of neonates who might be predisposed to varying
biochemical profiles or inherited maternal traits.
HAND STUDIES
Findings gleaned from studies of adults form the bases for studies of infants’ hand
explorations and perceptions. The literature on adults reveals that they use explicit
finger and hand movements to detect specific aspects of objects.20,45,46 For example,
adults rub their fingers repeatedly on objects’ surfaces when exploring their
textures.20,45,46 However, to detect hardness of a substance, they press or squeeze
objects.20,45,46 For the perception of temperature, adults simply make contact with
the surface of the object, such as by placing a hand on the object’s surface.20,45,46
To detect general information about an object’s shape or size, they enclose or mold
their fingers around the object, but to detect precise shape, they trace the object’s
edges with their fingers.20,45,46 They determine an object’s weight by displacing or
lifting the object with one of their hands.20,45,46
fourth and ninth months of life, as this is when infants are readily observed
scratching, rubbing, squeezing and waving objects. Importantly, at approximately
9 or 10 months of age, infants master the process of “sitting,” and thus are able to
support themselves while holding objects,47,48 which would allow them to hold an
object with one hand while the fingers of the other hand trace the object’s edges for
perception of exact shape.9 These studies suggest that infants may perceive some of
the properties of objects using different finger and hand movements than those
used by adults.
These findings suggest that the perception of hardness by hand may occur as early
as during the newborn period, and that infants show “exploratory” behaviors similar
to those shown by adults when detecting information about substance. However,
the fact that infants modulated their hand activities depending on the object’s
hardness suggests that newborns possess control of their hand behaviors. Moreover,
these findings contradict the view that neonatal grasping is a reflex response.
study by Rochat,18 these findings suggest that neonates modulated their hand
behaviors according to the property of the object.22 These findings22 also suggest
that the perception of texture may be achieved earlier than the 4-month time frame
noted by Bushnell and Boudreau,9 and that the neonates might have detected the
textures via squeezing the tubes, unlike the adults who used rubbing of their fingers
across the object’s surface. Perhaps in the absence of fingering activities, newborns
grasp and squeeze objects to explore and perceive their properties.
If the findings by Streri, Lhote and Dutilleul23 are conclusive, they refute the time
line for the detection of shape by infants, as well as suggest that neonates use
exploratory procedures that differ from those described for shape perception by
adults. You will recall that adults trace the edges of objects with their fingers to
perceive shape.20,45,46 This behavior is not evident in infants until late in the first
year of life.9 Streri, Lhote and Dutilleul23 also videotaped their neonates during
the procedure: Coding of these videotapes revealed the newborn infants bringing
the objects close to their faces or their other hand, squeezing the object and some
evidence of slow finger-stroking. Perhaps one or all of these behaviors facilitated
the perception of the objects’ shapes by the neonates. Additional trials with
neonates, which study their finger and hand activities with varying object shapes,
are necessary to determine the underlying mechanisms involved in shape perception
by newborns.
Chapter 2: Touch Perception in Neonates 29
1) No object in hand 2) Right hand with 3) Right hand with 4) Right hand with
object raised object lowered object raised again
30 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
2 test trials: For example, after holding the warm tube over numerous trials and
showing little interest in continuing to hold it, the infant was presented with the
novel cold tube and the duration of holding it was recorded. During the procedure,
the experimenters also coded the infants’ hand activities as “active exploring” (hand
opening, closing, squeezing, turning or lifting hand to mouth or face area) or
“passive exploring” (exhibiting only one of the “active-exploring” hand behaviors,
or if they displayed any of the behaviors ≤50% of the trials, or infants who showed
no hand activities during the procedure).
As expected, the amount of time spent holding the same-temperature tube declined
over repeated trials, but then, when presented with the alternate-temperature tube,
both groups of neonates (of depressed mothers and of nondepressed mothers)
held the novel-temperature tube longer, suggesting that these newborns detected
the different temperatures presented. It is important to note, however, that the
newborns of the depressed mothers differed in several important respects from
the neonates of the nondepressed mothers: First, the infants of the depressed
mothers took twice as long to habituate, which suggests that they were slower
in processing the temperature information by hand. In the literature on visual
habituation, a longer time to habituate has been associated with less optimal
cognitive functioning.50 Secondly, rather than showing a decline in the amount
of time they held the same-temperature tube during the first few trials, the
neonates of depressed mothers showed an increase in this amount of time. Again,
in the visual habituation literature, this is referred to as “sensitization.”50
Sensitization has been linked to an inability to control arousal, and has been
reported for infants who take longer to habituate.51,52 Sensitization may occur
because the infant finds the object stimulating or because the infant is already
aroused or excited and becomes more excited when stimulated. Infants who exhibit
this behavior may prove more reactive to stimulation, have poor self-regulatory
capacities and/or have difficulty focusing or attending. Another observed difference
was that the newborns of depressed mothers showed less-active touch, indicating
that perhaps they were less interested in exploring objects with their hands. Less
handling or manipulation of objects by infants “at risk” has been correlated with
longer-term cognitive deficits.35
Su mm a r y
Newborns clutch or grasp objects that are placed in their hands and they suck
objects that offer no nutritional value. For decades, these neonatal behaviors have
been defined as “reflexes,” or as “involuntary responses” to stimulation.
The studies reviewed in this chapter, to the contrary, suggest that grasping and
sucking objects appear to be “voluntary behaviors” of the newborn that serve as
exploratory behaviors for detecting the properties of the objects. From studies of
sucking and mouthing objects, newborns displayed perception of objects’ hardness,
texture and shape. Newborns also perceived temperature by mouth, but this was not
evident for newborns of nondepressed mothers. Studies of grasping and handling
revealed that newborns detected all properties of the objects presented to them,
including temperature and weight perception by hand. The behaviors of these
newborns that apparently were used to perceive the objects included squeezing,
grasping or clutching, slow finger stroking, hand turning and hand lifting of objects
to the mouth or face. Taken together, these findings suggest that neonates explore
objects actively with their hands and mouth, often using behaviors similar to those
applied by adults to detect or perceive specific properties of objects. Moreover, in
the absence of sophisticated fingering abilities, squeezing alone may enable the
detection of gross aspects of objects, such as shape information.
The less optimal mouthing and handling behaviors of the newborns of depressed
mothers were also reviewed. Their perception might have been impaired by poor
finger and hand movements or inactivity. Toys for the newborn should be developed
to foster mouth, hand and finger activity, especially for infants “at risk” who may
show delayed perception.
Chapter 2: Touch Perception in Neonates 33
Further study is needed on the sense of touch and the perception of touch by
neonates. Surprisingly, neonatal touch studies are rare, and touch perception
by neonates is still viewed as almost nonexistent. Touch is the most well-developed
sense organ at birth. Like the other modalities—smell, taste, vision and audition—
touch and touch perception need additional research.
Ackn ow le d gm e nt s
We wish to thank the neonates and mothers who participated in our studies.
We also thank Shay Largie for her assistance with recruitment and data collection.
This research was supported by funds provided to the Touch Research Institutes by
the Johnson & Johnson Pediatric Institute, L.L.C., as well as by a National
Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Senior Research Scientist Award (MH#00331)
and an NIMH Research Grant (#MH46586) awarded to Tiffany Field.
34 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Ref ere n c e s
1. Brazelton TB, Nugent JK. Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. 3rd ed. London, England:
Mac Keith Press; 1995.
2. Nagy E, Molnar P. Heart rate deceleration during the grasping reflex. European Journal
of Pediatrics. 1999;158:576-577.
3. Clifton RK, Meyers WJ. The heart-rate response of four-month-old infants to auditory
stimuli. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1969;7:122-135.
4. Pomerleau-Malcuit A, Clifton RK. Neonatal heart-rate response to tactile, auditory, and
vestibular stimulation in different states. Child Development. 1973;44:485-496.
5. Illingworth RS. The Development of the Infant and Young Child: Normal and Abnormal.
3rd ed. London, England: E & S Livingstone; 1966.
6. Prechtl HFR. The Neurological Examination of the Full-Term Newborn Infant. 2nd ed.
London, England: Heinermann; 1977.
7. Petrikovsky BM, Kaplan GP. Fetal grasping of the umbilical cord causing variable fetal heart
rate decelerations. Journal of Clinical Ultrasound: JCU. 1993;21:642-644.
8. de Vries JI, Visser GH, Prechtl HFR. The emergence of fetal behavior: I. Qualitative aspects.
Early Human Development. 1982;7:301-322.
9. Bushnell E, Boudreau JP. Exploring and exploiting objects with the hands during infancy.
In: Connolly KJ, ed. The Psychobiology of the Hand. London, England: Cambridge University
Press; 1998:144-161.
10. Morange F, Bloch H. Lateralization of the approach movement and the prehension
movement in infants from 4 to 7 months. Early Development and Parenting. 1996;5:81-92.
11. Walco GA, Cassidy RC, Schechter NL. Pain, hurt, and harm. The ethics of pain control
in infants and children. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1994;331:541-544.
12. Anand KJ, Hickey PR. Pain and its effects in the human neonate and fetus. The New
England Journal of Medicine. 1987;317:1321-1329.
13. Giannakoulopoulos X, Sepulveda W, Kourtis P, Glover V, Fisk NM. Fetal plasma cortisol
and beta-endorphin response to intrauterine needling. Lancet. 1994;344:77-81.
14. Glover V, Fisk N. Do fetuses feel pain? We don’t know; better to err on the safe side from
mid-gestation. BMJ: (Clinical Research Ed.). 1996;313:796.
15. Lloyd-Thomas AR, Fitzgerald M. Do fetuses feel pain? Reflex responses do not necessarily
signify pain. BMJ: (Clinical Research Ed.). 1996;313:797-798.
16. Fitzgerald M, Jennings E. The postnatal development of spinal sensory processing. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1999;96:7719-7722.
17. Forssberg H. The neurophysiology of manual skill development. In: Connolly KJ, ed.
The Psychobiology of the Hand. London, England: Cambridge University Press; 1998:97-122.
Chapter 2: Touch Perception in Neonates 35
18. Rochat R. Mouthing and grasping in neonates: evidence for the early detection of what hard
or soft substances afford for action. Infant Behavior and Development. 1987;10:435-449.
24. Lipsitt L, Kaye H. Change in neonatal response to optimizing and non-optimizing sucking
stimulation. Psychonomic Science. 1965;2:221-222.
25. Rochat P. Oral touch in young infants: responses to variations of nipple characteristics in
the first months of life. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1983;6:123-133.
26. Meltzoff AN, Borton RW. Intermodal matching by human neonates. Nature. 1979;282:
403-404.
27. Pecheux M, Lepecq J, Salzarulo P. Oral activity and exploration in 1-2 month old infants.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology.1988;6:245-256.
28. Gibson EJ, Walker AS. Development of knowledge of visual-tactual affordances of substance.
Child Development. 1984;55:453-460.
29. Kaye KL, Bower TGR. Learning and intermodal transfer of information in newborns.
Psychological Science. 1994;5:286-288.
30. Hernandez-Reif M, Field T, Del Pino N, Diego M. Less exploring by mouth occurs in
newborns of depressed mothers. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2000;21:204-210.
31. Field T. Infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development. 1995;18:1-13.
32. Campbell SB, Cohn JF. Prevalence and correlates of postpartum depression in first-time
mothers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1991;100:594-599.
33. Hart S, Jones NA, Field T, Lundy B. One-year-old infants of intrusive and withdrawn
depressed mothers. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 1999;30:111-120.
34. McCall RB. What process mediates predictions of childhood IQ from infant habituation
and recognition memory? Speculations on the roles of inhibition and rate of information
processing. Intelligence. 1994;18:107-125.
35. Ruff HA, McCarton C, Kurtzberg D, Vaughan HG Jr. Preterm infants’ manipulative
exploration of objects. Child Development. 1984;55:1166-1173.
36. Blass EM, Watt LB. Suckling- and sucrose-induced analgesia in human newborns. Pain.
1999;83:611-623.
36 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
37. Radloff L. The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general
population. Journal of Applied Psychological Measures. 1977;1:385-401.
38. Whiffen VE, Gotlib IH. Infants of postpartum depressed mothers: temperament and
cognitive status. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1989;98:274-279.
39. Zuckerman B, Bauchner H, Parker S, Cabral H. Maternal depressive symptoms during
pregnancy, and newborn irritability. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP.
1990;11:190-194.
40. Lundy BL, Jones NA, Field T, et al. Prenatal depression effects on neonates. Infant Behavior
and Development. 1999;22:119-129.
41. Carver C, White T. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation and affective responses
to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology. 1994;67:319-333.
43. Gray J. A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In: Eysenck HJ, ed. A Model for
Personality. Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag; 1981:246-276.
44. Field T, Diego MA, Dieter J, et al. Depressed withdrawn and intrusive mothers’ effects
on their fetuses and neonates. Infant Behavior and Development. 2001;24:27-39.
45. Klatzky RL, Lederman SJ, Metzger VA. Identifying objects by touch: an “expert system.”
Perception & Psychophysics. 1985;37:299-302.
46. Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL. Hand movements: a window into haptic object recognition.
Cognitive Psychology. 1987;19:342-368.
47. Fagard J, Jacquet A. Onset of bimanual coordination and symmetry versus asymmetry
of movement. Infant Behavior and Development. 1989;11:229-235.
48. Ramsey D, Weber S. Infants’ hand preference in a task involving complementary roles
for the two hands. Child Development. 1986;57:300-307.
54. Andrews K, Fitzgerald M. The cutaneous withdrawal reflex in human neonates: sensitization,
receptive field, and the effects of contralateral stimulation. Pain. 1994;56:95-101.
55. Klatzky R, Lederman S. Spatial and nonspatial avenues to object recognition by the human
haptic system. In: Eilan N, McCarthy R, Brewer B, eds. Spatial Representation. Cambridge,
Massachusetts: Blackwell Publishers; 1993:191-205.
CHAPTER 3:
M AT E R N A L
TOUCH EFFECTS ON
I N F A N T B E H AV I O R
Ab s t ra c t
Although the positive effects of massage therapy on infant behavior and growth
have been well documented, few studies have evaluated the reinforcing effects of
different types of tactile stimulation on infant behavior or the distinctions between
maternally delivered and infant-originated tactile stimulation. The study detailed
in this chapter assessed 3 types of tactile stimulation delivered at 2 levels of tactile
pressure. Results of this study revealed that the type of touch preferred most by
infants was “intense stroking.” Conversely, the type of touch preferred least by
infants was “intense poking.” The methods used in this study provide a reliable
means of quantifying tactile pressure.
40 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Introd uc t i o n
When the behavioral states of infants have been compared across several conditions:
alone, mother’s face only, mother’s voice only, mother’s touch only and a
combination of mother’s voice, touch and face,2 infants exhibit the lowest frequency
of fussing and crying during the “touch-only” condition. Furthermore, using touch
during the maternal “still-face” procedure has been shown to reduce the frequency
of fussing, crying and grimacing among 3-month-old to 5-month-old infants, as
well as to maintain more smiling and gazing.1 These effects have also been noted
in 3-month-old infants of depressed mothers.4
The positive effects of massage therapy (ie, rhythmic stroking) on infant behaviors
and growth rates have been documented in many studies.3-12 Massage has enhanced
growth rates among preterm, cocaine-exposed, HIV-exposed and full-term
neonates.5,6 Massage therapy has also reduced the signs of stress in normal,
preterm, cocaine-exposed and HIV-exposed neonates.13 In addition,
massaged preterm and cocaine-exposed neonates have received higher motor
scores on the Brazelton Neonatal Assessment Scale when compared with control
neonates who did not receive massage.14,15 Massage has also been associated with
improvements on “…emotionality, sociability, and soothability temperament
dimensions…”7 and, most especially, on sleep organization in full-term infants.8
Furthermore, 3-month-old infants who were massaged by their fathers greeted
them with more eye contact, smiling, vocalizing, reaching and orienting responses,
and showed fewer avoidance behaviors, than did those infants who were not
massaged by their fathers.9,16
Chapter 3: Maternal Touch Effects on Infant Behavior 41
Lastly, massage has been shown to affect infant learning: Infants who received
massage prior to an audiovisual habituation task performed better than those
infants who received either play or no stimulation before the audiovisual
habituation task.10
The role of touch as a reinforcer for various infant behaviors has also been
explored.11,12,17 One of the methods used to assess reinforcers is to follow the
behavior by an event and measure whether the behavior increases. If the behavior
increases from the event that followed, the event is termed a “positive reinforcer.”
Contingent tactile stimulation—alone or along with visual and/or auditory
stimulation—has been found to increase infant behaviors that produce it.11,12
In fact, contingent touching of the infant’s face, abdomen and/or limbs has been
found to increase vocalizations, smiles, eye contact and approach behaviors in
infants 1.5 months of age to 9 months of age.11,12,17,18
U N R E S O LV E D I S S U E S I N T H E L I T E R AT U R E
REGARDING INFANT TACTILE STIMULATION
Maternal stroking has been noted to occur very often during mother-infant
interactions.19 Maternal stroking seems to occur most frequently, followed by
maternal tickling, while maternal poking is rare. Some have suggested that each
of these forms of touch conveys a different message.2 The more negative types of
touch also occur more often during disturbed interaction: For example, compared
with nondepressed mothers, depressed mothers show more frequent jabbing and
poking of their infants.20 In turn, such jabbing and poking seem to be associated
with negative affect and gaze aversion exhibited by infants.21
Other relevant stimulus qualities, such as duration, pattern and pressure of touch
need further investigation. Specifying the pressure of tactile stimuli is important
because it may help determine the quality and/or quantity of behavioral effects.
Without accurate measures of the attributes of tactile stimuli, such as pressure,
it may prove difficult to predict the effects of these stimuli on infant behaviors.
Specifying and quantifying the qualities of the touch stimulation would advance
the sciences of touch measurably.
OVERVIEW
This study analyzed the effects of 3 different types of tactile stimulation at 2 levels
of tactile pressure. Stimuli were considered reinforcers if the infant’s target behavior,
leg kicking, increased. Secondly, the relative efficacies of different tactile stimuli as
positive reinforcers were evaluated.
SUMMARY OF METHODOLOGY
levels: for instance, mild stroking versus mild poking versus mild tickling; intense
stroking versus intense poking versus intense tickling. During the learning phases,
every leg kick produced the touch stimulus. The second and third A phases were
reversals of the phase that had preceded it immediately. During the reversal phases,
nontarget behaviors received contingent stimulation. For example, nonkicking
behaviors were followed by tactile stimulation. The frequency of stimulation was
equated with that of the previous treatment phase by ensuring that the mothers
provided the earlier contingent stimulus for their child’s nontarget (ie, nonkicking)
responses at least as often as they did for the earlier target (ie, kicking) response in
the corresponding time period of the previous treatment phase: For example, if a
mother delivered the touch contingent on the target behavior 20 times during the
first 30 seconds of the treatment phase, she was required to deliver the same touch
noncontingently at least 20 times during the first 30 seconds of the reversal phase.
The sample split equally along gender lines and included 10 Hispanic and
2 African American infants who were 2 months of age to 5 months of age (mean,
3.6 months). The experiment was conducted in the infants’ homes. Each child sat
on their infant seat 3 feet from a video camera, which recorded his or her behavior
as well as the touch stimulation. Each mother sat next to her child (Figure 1) so that
she could touch her infant according to the instructions given. The experimenter,
who was located on the other side of the room, scored the child’s behavior as shown
on the computer-monitor screen. The leg-kick measurement device consisted of an
infrared beam that was projected 5 inches above the child’s calves. When the child
kicked and either or both legs crossed the beam, a leg kick was registered. The
Figure 1. Representation of
positioning of mother and child
during experiment examining
maternal touch intensity
and infant response 21
44 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
microphone of the meter that measured sound level was positioned directly on the
infant’s skin. The mother was instructed to administer the touch within a 2-inch
radius of this very-sensitive microphone that detected the loudness (in decibels
[dBs]) of the sounds that were made as the mother’s skin and the infant’s skin
rubbed against each other during contact. More-intense touching produced higher
dB readings than did less-intense touching on the sound-level meter. The mother
could monitor the intensity of her touch via an analog display on the sound-level
meter. She was instructed to keep her touch intensity between –10 and –5 dB
for the mild-touch condition and between +5 and +10 dB for the intense-touch
condition.
The target behavior was an infant leg kick, defined as the leg going from a
bent-at-the-knee position to a straight position, or the reverse (straight to bent).
The leg-kick response was chosen because it occurs reliably in 2-month-old to
5-month-old infants. Occurrences of the leg-kick response, and of the touch
response and amplitude, were represented automatically by a highly visible blip
on a computer monitor.
Stroking, tickling and poking were produced by the infants’ leg kicks. Thus,
initially, the touch was used as a reinforcer for the infant response and directly
reflected infant preferences for the different types and intensities of touch. The
duration of every touch, 3 seconds, was measured by a timer operating within
the data-acquisition program. Stroking was defined as soft, rhythmic, continuous
touching of the infants’ limbs or abdomens using one hand. Tickling consisted of
arrhythmic touching of the infants’ limbs or abdomens using the fingertips of one
hand. Poking was comprised of a one-finger continuous touch of the infants’ limbs
or abdomens. Additionally, there were 2 levels of pressure for the stroking, tickling
and poking treatments.
SUMMARY OF FINDINGS
With the exceptions of mild poking and intense poking, which appeared only
occasionally, the other forms of touch proved reinforcing. The most-preferred type
of touch was intense stroking, while the least-preferred type of touch was intense
poking.
Chapter 3: Maternal Touch Effects on Infant Behavior 45
Results of this study indicated that there were numerous differences among the
treatments. Firstly, intense stroking proved to be a more-effective reinforcer than
did intense poking, mild poking, mild stroking and mild tickling. Additionally,
intense tickling proved more effective than intense poking and mild poking. Lastly,
mild stroking proved more effective than intense poking and mild poking, while
mild tickling proved more effective than intense poking and mild poking. These
results are depicted in Figure 2.
Conclusions
Results of the current experiment help expand our understanding of the effects
of maternal touch on infant behavior. Firstly, this study quantified the pressure
of maternal tactile stimulation. Finding a reliable way to quantify the pressure of
touch represents a milestone in the field of touch research, as the intensity of touch
46 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
can now be monitored with accuracy. Secondly, results of this study yielded
valuable information about the effects of different types of touch. While
tickling/tapping, stroking and occasionally poking can all be reinforcing to the
infant, intense stroking was more effective than tickling/tapping or poking as
a means of increasing the rate of infant leg kicks. Higher pressure stroking and
tickling/tapping appeared to be associated with more frequent kicking, while
higher pressure poking seemed to be associated with less frequent kicking. This
information can help parents understand the types and pressures of touch that
may be used most effectively during their interactions with their infants.
Chapter 3: Maternal Touch Effects on Infant Behavior 47
Re f e re n c e s
1. Stack DM, Muir DW. Adult tactile stimulation during face-to-face interactions modulates
five-month-olds’ affect and attention. Child Development. 1992;63:1509-1525.
2. Tronick EZ. Touch in mother-infant interaction. In: Field TM, ed. Touch in Early
Development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1995:53-65.
6. Field TM, Schanberg SM, Scafidi F, et al. Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation effects on preterm
neonates. Pediatrics. 1986;77:654-658.
7. Field T. Massage therapy effects. American Psychologist. 1998;53:1270-1281.
8. Field T, Grizzle N, Scafidi F, et al. Massage therapy for infants of depressed mothers. Infant
Behavior and Development. 1996;19:107-112.
9. Cullen C, Field T, Escalona A, Hartshorn K. Father-infant interactions are enhanced by
massage therapy. Early Child Development and Care. 2000;164:41-47.
10. Cigales M, Field T, Lundy B, Cuadra A, Hart S. Massage enhances recovery from habituation
in normal infants. Infant Behavior & Development. 1997;20:29-34.
11. Peláez-Nogueras M, Gewirtz JL, Field T, et al. Infants’ preference for touch stimulation in
face-to-face interactions. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1996;17:199-213.
12. Peláez-Nogueras M, Field T, Gewirtz JL, et al. The effects of systematic stroking versus
tickling and poking on infant behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology.
1997;18:169-178.
13. Wheeden A, Scafidi FA, Field T, Ironson G, Valdeon C, Bandstra E. Massage effects on
cocaine-exposed preterm neonates. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP.
1993;14:318-322.
14. Scafidi FA, Field TM, Schanberg SM, et al. Effects of tactile/kinesthetic stimulation on
the clinical course and sleep/wake behavior of preterm neonates. Infant Behavior and
Development. 1986;9:91-105.
15. Scafidi FA, Field T, Schanberg SM. Factors that predict which preterm infants benefit
most from massage therapy. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP.
1993;14:176-180.
48 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
16. Scholz K, Samuels CA. Neonatal bathing and massage intervention with fathers: the
behavioural effects 12 weeks after birth of the first baby. The Sunraysia Australia Intervention
Project. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1992;15:67-81.
17. Lum Lock KL. Infants’ Approach and Avoidance to Strangers Influenced by Maternal
Contingencies [master’s thesis]. Miami, Fla: Florida International University; 1997.
18. Weisberg P. Social and nonsocial conditioning of infant vocalizations. Child Development.
1963;34:377-388.
19. Eckerman CO, Rheingold HL. Infants’ exploratory responses to toys and people.
Developmental Psychology. 1974;10:255-259.
20. Field T. Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant
Behavior and Development. 1984;7:517-522.
21. Cohn JF, Tronick E. Specificity of infants’ response to mothers’ affective behavior. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1989;28:242-248.
CHAPTER 4:
TOUCHING DURING
MOTHER-INFANT
INTERACTIONS
Ab s t ra c t
Parental touching
and holding
are important in
early infant
development.
The Impo r t a n ce o f To u c h
Long-held evidence shows the importance of tactile stimulation for the normal
development of nonhuman species. Examples include the survival functions of
maternal washing of the young1 and the specific beneficial effects of handling,
licking and grooming on survival,2,3 growth, development and resistance to disease,4-7
as well as increased exploratory behavior.8 The classic body of work done by
Harlow on rhesus monkeys substantiated the importance of tactile stimulation by
demonstrating that the development of social attachment was more dependent on
physical contact than it was on reducing the feeding drive.9 Additional data from
primate and rodent models have implicated physical contact and touch (tactile
stimulation) as significant concomitants of the infant’s ability to regulate its own
responses to stress.10-12 More complete coverage of the animal research can be found
in a chapter by this author13 and in the texts written by Montagu14 and Field.15
While the importance of touch for the normal development of nonhuman species
is not in question, the importance of touch to human infants has also been
recognized. Classic observations regarding maternal deprivation of human infants
and the lack of tactile stimulation by human adults has underscored the value of
tactile contact including reports of infants who were deprived of mothering for
lengthy periods of time as well as institutionalized infants who were given only
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 51
essential care with no extra attention from the staff.16,17 This work, at least in part,
led to more systematic work on maternal attachment, bonding and deprivation.
Unfortunately, the literature on maternal deprivation is replete with methodological
problems, permitting only cautious interpretations. Recent research, however,
suggests that extreme forms of limited touch affect children’s growth and
development.18,19
The effects of touch/contact have also been seen in studies where touch has been
used to induce or change behavioral state. In many studies, touch is used between
intervals of a study to maintain alert state in babies, to calm them or as an
attention-getting stimulus.23,24 In the neonate, touch has been shown to reduce
stimulation, thereby acting like a control system to maintain state.25 According to
Brazelton, touch can also stimulate the infant, thereby illustrating how touch can
both instigate and maintain communication.25 Nonverbal maternal behaviors also
provide a means of modulating the overall level of stimulation to which the infant
is exposed, potentially facilitating regulation of its own state and level of arousal.26
Based on reviews of the literature, more emphasis has been placed on the physical
benefits of touch to high-risk infants and the development of the perception of
touch, while less attention has been devoted to touch as it relates to infants’ social
and emotional development.13 Moreover, there has been a general lack of research
on the tactile modality versus auditory and visual modalities in perceptual research
as well as facial and vocal channels in social-emotional research.
52 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Tronick has explored ideas (Figure 1) along the same lines, arguing that “…certain
forms of touch, such as gentle holding, might convey the message, you are safe,
whereas other forms of touch, such as poking or jabbing, may convey the message,
you are physically threatened.”28 Touching seems to communicate; to bring out
meaning: Montagu said, “Although touch is not itself an emotion, its sensory
elements induce those neural, glandular, muscular, and mental changes which in
combination we call an emotion.”14
Touch Communication
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 53
A typical
face-to-face
interaction
between a
mother and
her infant.
Photo by author.
As infants develop over the first few months of their lives, social interactions
become increasingly more frequent and represent a means of communicating and
learning. Face-to-face interactions are a frequent way that mothers and infants
interact. Commonly, researchers have focused on the mothers’ faces and voices as
means of communication during these interactions.52,61 However, while mothers’
facial and vocal expressions are important, they are only 2 of the many behaviors
that are used to express and communicate. Researchers have only recently begun
to explore the role of touch in early interactions.57,65-67,78 In the past, the focus was
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 55
on the more distal gaze and affect behaviors—to the relative exclusion of touch
and gesture.13 Yet, touch is employed by mothers quite commonly, along with
their vocal and visual expressions, during face-to-face interactions and during play:
For example, during face-to-face interactions, the infant and adult (primarily the
mother) are seated at eye-level to each other during a series of brief interaction
periods. The caregivers interact spontaneously, using their facial, vocal and tactile
expressions, while the infants respond to, and even initiate, interactions. Face-to-
face interactions have been one of the primary means used to study infants’ social
communications,50 emotional expressions and responses to stressful episodes,51
and the development of social expectations.52 However, typically, researchers have
analyzed maternal and infant facial and vocal behavior50,61 but not touch, although
incidental reports reveal that maternal touch occurs during 33% to 61% of brief
interaction periods.50,53,54 Other measures, such as posture,55 manual hand actions56
and gesture,57 have also been documented. Contextual features, such as location
during play, position, inclination of position and proximity of contact, are other
examples of important factors influencing infants during face-to-face play.58,59
Gusella, Muir and Tronick compared the responses during SF periods between
groups of infants wherein some received maternal touch in the preceding “normal”
period, while others received only maternal face and voice.61 These investigators
found that 3-month-old infants smiled and gazed less at their mothers during the
SF period than during the “normal” periods (compared with a “no-change” control
group who received 3 “normal” periods), but that their behavior was significantly
different from the control group only when maternal touch was permitted during
the “normal” period preceding the SF. That is, these 3-month-old infants exhibited
the SF effect only when maternal touching was part of the prior “normal” periods
and their attention declined over time without the tactile stimulation. This suggests,
according to Gusella, Muir and Tronick,61 that maternal touch during the “normal”
periods facilitated the maintenance of attention in these very young infants.
levels of gaze that are typical in “normal” interactions.65 This new role for touch in
moderating the SF effect has been replicated several times.33,66,67 Moreover, Muir
and I demonstrated that it was the tactile stimulation, not the visual stimulation,
from the adults’ hands that was responsible for the effects.33
Beyond the general issues of infants’ responsiveness and sensitivity, Muir and I went
on to address 2 additional questions: Are infants sensitive to subtle manipulations
of maternal touch? And, are mothers able to use touch to obtain specific responses
from their infants? Infants’ sensitivity to changes in their mothers’ touch has been
demonstrated through the SF procedure by providing a group of mothers with
differing verbal instructions.33,65,67 This is what might be referred to as “changes
in context”: For example, it has been shown that mothers can use touch to elicit
specific behaviors from their infants—such as maximizing their infants’ smiling67
and shifting their infants’ attention to their mothers’ hands.33 The resulting changes
in infants’ behaviors, as a function of the changes in the mothers’ verbal instructions,
implies that there were changes in maternal touch.
mothers used. Lastly, using sequential analyses to examine how touch contributes
to the reciprocal and contingent aspects of communicative exchange, LePage and
I were able to show that cyclical patterns of mother-and-infant behaviors were
demonstrated “around” maternal touching.67 Before their mothers touched them,
infants were likely to be gazing at either their mothers’ hands or their mothers’
faces. After their mothers touched them, however, the infants were more likely to
gaze at their mothers’ hands. Smiling was frequent and was always accompanied by
gazing at mothers’ faces or hands, rather than occurring simply in isolation. These
findings suggest that infants may have been directed to the active component of the
hands and that they were enjoying the tactile stimulation that they were receiving,
which was reflected in their smiling both before and after being touched.
It is important to note that for the previous 2 studies mentioned,57,67 the changes
in maternal touch were inferred on the bases of changes in the infants’ behaviors.
The actual patterns and types of touching were not measured directly. Therefore,
Arnold 73 and I13,72 have now examined some of the specific changes in touching
using the Caregiver-Infant Touch Scale.113,68 Some of these findings are described in
a following section that deals with patterns of touching during social interactions.
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 59
While facial, vocal and tactile components are used frequently during social
interactions, less is known about how they are used in combination, how they are
used to achieve goals, and, if they do convey messages, how this is accomplished.
Moreover, examination is warranted regarding how touching is integrated with the
other communication channels that are available to parents.13,71,72 Although it is
important to understand each component’s discrete and independent roles, the
context when much of early development occurs is social, and information is
typically specified multimodally for the developing infant: That is, touch is often
combined with other modalities of interaction. Consequently, how the modalities
are used in combination becomes an important issue: That is, do combinations
of modalities communicate more clearly? Are messages embedded in touch? After
examining infants’ sensitivity to subtle changes in maternal tactile behavior we
investigated the influence of “modality.” We studied this by comparing mother-
infant interactions in touch-only periods (ie, SF periods where touching only
was permitted) to conditions where mothers could use all modalities including
touch.57,71-73 We used changes in infants’ gaze and affect to imply responsiveness
to changes in their mothers’ behavior.
60 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Our general research objectives were threefold.71 If infants are sensitive to maternal
touching, we needed to answer the following questions:
Using the same interaction periods as used by Arnold and my lab,57 results
indicated that when mothers were restricted to using only touch, they employed
more touch to compensate for the absence of other modalities of interaction.
Contrastingly, when all modes of interaction were available, mothers employed
varied communication strategies. Importantly, while these infants smiled and gazed
at their mothers’ faces more often during interactions involving all modalities,
mothers were nonetheless successful in directing infant attention and eliciting
smiling using only touch and gesture.
Taken together with the studies that examined infants’ sensitivity to changes in
maternal touch, these 2 studies,71,73 examining the influence of modality, illustrate
that dramatic changes in infants’ responses to subtle perturbations can occur within
a restricted communicative context, underscoring the importance of nonverbal
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 61
3. Touch moderates still-face effect: Maintains and directs infant attention (gaze)
and elicits positive affect (smiling), influences negative affect
4. Across age
8. Infants are responsive to touch and are sensitive to subtle changes in maternal
tactile behavior
9. Modality: In general, infants smile and gaze at their mothers’ faces during interactions
involving all modalities, mothers are nonetheless successful in directing infant
gaze / attention and eliciting smiling using only touch and gesture
These findings underscore the value of studying touch and how it can illuminate our
understanding of communication
One adult (distal) smiled, talked, sang and made facial expressions. Another adult
(proximal) carried, rocked, bounced, patted and stroked the infant, but remained
silent with a neutral face. A third adult (neutral) was silent, unresponsive and made
no eye contact. Over a 3-week period, infants increased their time spent near the
distal adult, while the infants made no changes in their relationship to the proximal
and neutral adults. Moreover, infants chose to look more at the distal adult than at
the proximal adult. Unfortunately, in this trial there were no measures of affect, the
adult was not permitted to maintain eye contact in the proximal condition and the
infant did not need to establish eye contact with the proximal adult in order to
receive stimulation. This lack of “natural social interaction” may have contributed
to the poor responses elicited by the proximal adult, and may have driven the
infants to look away.65
Photo by author.
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 63
Rhythmic touch is also preferred over nonrhythmic touch in dyads,77 and infants
are more responsive when touch is added to face and voice.78 To reinforce the
position that touch can elicit specific responses, Wolff studied the development
of smiling and found that, between the fourth and sixth weeks of life, “patty-cake”
becomes an efficient stimulus for smiling.79 To ensure that it was the
proprioceptive-tactile stimuli rather than extraneous stimulation that elicited the
smile, Wolff played the game in such a way that the infant could not see or hear
the elicitor during the test.79 The smiles evoked in this manner were described as
broad, the intensity of smiling was high and it was difficult to habituate the
response with repeated stimulation. Patty-cake, of course, is not the only game that
elicits such positive responses from infants: There are other parent-infant games
that also involve much touching and physical contact—lap games, tickle games,
“I’m-gonna-get-you” games, finger-walking games and even bouncing games and
“horsey” games.
In a study that examined distinct types of infant smiles and their relationship to
the social context in which they typically occur, Dickson, Walker and Fogel81 coded
for basic, play and Duchenne smiles during parent-infant interactions. These
investigators found that it was the physical play that included tactile stimulation
that elicited the most “play smiles” (45% of the time), and that these play smiles
occurred less often during object play, vocal play and book reading.
only” condition.28 Interestingly, Tronick also showed a low level of scanning, high
levels of object attention and less smiling by the infants than during the “normal”
and “face-only” conditions. Consistent with the findings reported by my colleagues
and my lab,33,57,65,67 touching had calming effects on the infants, as reflected in
their decreased fretting, and seemed to permit an openness to the stimulation, as
reflected in the high levels of attention and continued smiling. On the bases of
these findings and others, Tronick suggested that touch is a component of the
mutual regulatory process of the caregiver-infant dyad, and he contends that touch
may serve a regulatory function.28,82
Despite the fact that the overall levels of touching are, in fact, important, they do
not inform us about the qualitative aspects of touching or how particular types
of touch may be used more or less often under specific circumstances. It makes a
difference, for example, whether one strokes, caresses, pats or pokes. Moreover, all
touch may not be used or interpreted similarly: Different types of touch and the
way the touch is applied may have different meanings. The way in which touch is
used, and how one is touched, reflects in some way how touch is communicated or
transmitted. Both the quality and quantity of tactile stimulation, as well as the
parameters, become important.68,86-88
Analyses revealed several important findings. Firstly, the natural and all SF
(touch-alone) periods were significantly different, and there were clear differences
among all SF periods. Specific patterns or profiles of touching were shown across
perturbation periods: For example, when asked to maximize infant smiling,
mothers used more-active types of touch (eg, lifting and tickling), more surface
area and greater intensity and speed. During the SF period when mothers were
asked to touch their babies in only one area, there was increased stroking and far
less shaking. Touching was also less intense, and most types of touch were judged
to be executed more slowly during this period. Thus, the more tactilely active
profile was revealed during the period when smiling was maximized. This finding
supports the notion of heightened activity during playful interactions. From these
results it is clear that mothers’ profiles of touching change during brief interactions,
as a function of experimenter instruction, suggesting that what was being
communicated through touch was different.
In her comparison of tactile and all-modes experimental and control groups in the
4 instructional contexts described previously, Arnold also examined these periods
for type of touch.73 Among her findings was that, within the tactile groups,
mothers of 3-month-old infants touched their babies more often than did mothers
of 5-month-old infants. Interestingly, there were also differences in the type of
touch used, as a function of instructional period: For example, relative to the
controls, more stroking was used in the excited and happy periods, and more
tickling and shaking were used in the attention-to-face period. Notable effects were
also found for the area of the body on which the touch occurred. Lastly, effects
were also demonstrated in comparisons across modalities.
12. Different types of touch are used in different contexts and for different purposes
These findings underscore the value of studying touch and how it can illuminate our
understanding of communication
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 67
Consistent with the theme that mothers use qualitatively different types of touch
during interactions with their babies, Tronick reported that mothers use affectively
positive types of touch with their 6-month-old infants.28 In this study, mothers
used stroking, rhythmic touching and holding with their 6-month-old babies for
the largest proportion of time, followed by tickling and kissing. “Negative” forms
of touch, such as pinching and poking, were rarely observed.
Together, these findings suggest that simple touch “duration” is not a sufficient
index with which to characterize adult behavior. Qualitative and quantitative
variations in touching occur for a variety of reasons and are important to measure
and describe. What is also clear is that mothers use different patterns of touching
for different functions.68
types of touch that are used by mothers with their infants, and subsequently
converge to suggest that touch might be an important parenting measure.
Sensory-impaired infants reveal yet another way with which touch can be used as
an important communicative channel. In these cases, whether the infant is visually
or hearing impaired, the tacto-gestural modality might be seen as assuming some
of the roles that vision or audition might have otherwise subsumed. Of course,
both the visual and tactile channels are of significance to the deaf infant.94 Yet,
according to Meadow-Orlans and Steinberg, when compared with mothers of
18-month-old hearing infants, mothers of same-age deaf infants used less-frequent
and positive touch, and they were less sensitive and more intrusive.95 Nonetheless,
as Koester, Papouek and Brooks discovered, when deaf mothers use these
more-intrusive touch behaviors with their infants a style of communication is
formed to which infants respond.98 MacTurk and coinvestigators also found that
maternal visual-tactile responsiveness was lower in mothers of deaf infants at
9 months: However, this level of visual-tactile responsiveness contributed to
positive interactions at 18 months.97
Although the majority of “touch” studies involving infants conducted to date have
involved mothers or female adults, there is an accumulating literature involving
fathers. It is known that fathers engage in more vigorous, physically stimulating
play with their infants,103-106 and it is believed by some that their style of play
serves to create a critical means for the development of attachment.107 According
to some research, fathers’ actual touching of their infants is rare immediately after
birth, while physical closeness and gazing at the infant are more typical.108 This
observation of less-frequent touching by fathers, as compared with mothers, has
also been noted in the neonatal intensive care unit.45
There have been only a scant few studies of fathers’ systematic touching of their
infants in different cultures: As an example, Hewlett observed the Aka pygmies
of the tropical forest region of the southern portion of the Central African
Republic.104 While Aka fathers held their infants substantially less often than did
their mothers, several “positive” patterns did emerge. The fathers who held their
infants often did so in a context-specific situation (eg, during leisure time).
Interestingly, however, it was the Aka fathers who were more likely to engage in
minor physical play, such as tickling and bouncing, with their infants. Aka fathers
did not engage in the vigorous types of play characteristic of American fathers.
70 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
The characteristics of holding and play in different cultures can also be a revealing
means of examining some aspects of physical touch and affection during parent-
infant interchanges. In a study describing Indian (New Delhi) mothers’ and fathers’
holding patterns, Roopnarine and colleagues found that mothers held their babies
more than did their fathers, and they were more likely to pick them up, feed them
and comfort them while holding and display affection while holding.110 However,
the overall duration of holding was reported as less than what is typically reported
for the North American family. This could be explained by the fact that, on a daily
basis, many other family members and friends hold the typical Indian baby. When
fathers were holding their infants, affection was commonly displayed. Tickling
and lap bouncing were found to be rare occurrences between Indian parents and
their infants. However, playing “peekaboo” was seen more commonly among the
mothers and their infants. Lastly, these infants themselves were more likely to
vocalize to, smile at and follow their mothers, compared with their fathers, but
there were no differences in the amounts of touch or approach behaviors between
the mothers and the fathers.
Taken together, findings from this brief overview of some of the father-infant
interaction studies where touch is considered have shown that touch is important—
and even intrinsic—to fathers and to other cultures. Touch can also be used
differently in some situations or be more frequent. These results converge to
suggest that touching is used to bring people together, for closeness and intimacy
and for proximity and play. More research with fathers is warranted, particularly
pertaining to fathers’ use and styles of touching during interactions.
Su mm a r y a n d Co nc l u s i o ns
Taken together, interaction studies have provided important insights and have
advanced our knowledge of the young infant’s sensitivity to manipulations in
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 71
facial, vocal and now tactile expressions. These findings emphasize the complexity
and sophistication of mother-infant dyadic interactions and the importance of
including measures of touch. Moreover, through these studies, an abundance of
new findings has been revealed: For example, mothers frequently use touch during
normal face-to-face interactions with their infants (65% of the time).54,65 For brief
periods of time, touch alone can maintain infants’ attention and elicit positive
affect at least as well as can vocal and facial expressions.111 In addition, infants are
sensitive to subtle changes in maternal touch,56,57 and therefore prefer stimulus
compounds that include touch.78 Results from these studies have also enlightened
us about infants’ sensitivity to maternal behavior; in particular, their sensitivity
to their mothers’ touch. Moreover, the influences of modality have been
demonstrated,57,71-73 and different patterns of touching are shown under different
instructional conditions.68,73 Most importantly, perhaps, these studies have provided
evidence for a functional context for touch that is not limited to the regulation
of distress. Rather, these findings indicate a role for touch in social-emotional
development, and they imply that touch serves an important communicative
function. However, the processes and mechanisms through which touch
communicates require additional research attention.
Touch is emerging as a diverse and adaptable modality that, although often used
alone, accompanies other modalities and channels of communication. Touch
is used frequently during the first year of life, serving a multitude of purposes:
• adjusting posture
Although facial and vocal expressions are important forms of communication, they
are only 2 of the many behaviors that are used to express and communicate. Far
72 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
less data exist regarding “tactile expressiveness” and communication through touch.
Field18 reaffirmed the observation that the communicative functions of touch have
been neglected relative to the other senses and to facial and vocal expressiveness—
points also made by Frank,112 myself13 and subsequently by Hertenstein.86
Moreover, in her article in Human Development, Field18 argues the important role
for touch in understanding and improving infants’ well-being, emphasizing that
significant effects on growth, development and emotional well-being are suggested
from numerous studies and many observations of extremes of limited touch in
infants and children. However, little is known about the long-term effects of
parental touch and touch patterns on the subsequent mental health, social and
emotional development and adjustment in healthy infants or more vulnerable
groups of children.
The findings discussed in this chapter emphasize the points that the tactile modality
provides an important means for parents and infants to develop and maintain a
connection with each other, as well as with the environment and to the self. They
also illustrate the flexibility and adaptability of touch, as well as the adaptability of
the communication system, such that both partners can modify their behaviors to
adjust and compensate for the situation: Therefore, both partners are responsive to
each other. It has also been made clear that patterns of touching may be different.
Given that the contexts within which much of early development occurs are social
and multimodal, how touching is integrated with other communication channels
that are available to parents is an important research pursuit in our search for
unraveling the mysteries and challenges of the sense of touch.
Beyond the aforementioned, several additional questions and pivotal issues are
prominent and have emerged from the extant literature.13 Firstly, what is being
communicated through touch? Secondly, assuming that communication is
occurring (based on the evidence to date), and that touch is serving a multitude of
important roles and functions, the next issue involves determining how to measure
this process. Establishing clear roles for, and contributions to, social and emotional
development are important. Thirdly, it is absolutely essential to address the
quantitative and qualitative characteristics (patterns) of touch and their salience
over age, as well as the changes that occur in infants’ and caregivers’ communicative
behaviors (eg, affective, gazing and touching behaviors) over time. Fourthly,
particularly throughout the first few years of life, the infant (and its parents) is
developing, changing and adjusting. It is important for future research to pursue
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 73
and be aware of how development itself plays a role in the changes we see related
to touch, physical contact and affection. Fifthly, relationships between patterns
of touching and/or discrete types of touch and emotional/affective displays are
required. Sixthly, the long-term implications for touch and its contributions to
development and adjustment are important endeavors, yet they present difficult
research challenges. The last 2 issues stress the importance of studying individual
differences and examining different trajectories. Lastly, it is essential that the
research be integrated into existing models and theory and that the development of
new, more comprehensive models be added to the mix as the research develops. The
value of such pursuits must not be overemphasized. Hertenstein has taken a positive
step in this direction by describing a general mode for tactile communication.86
Acknowledgments
This chapter was written with the support of the Social Sciences and Humanities
Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) and Fonds pour la Formation de
Chercheurs et l’Aide à la Recherche (FCAR). The author expresses her gratitude
to Yves Beaulieu for his detailed comments on an earlier version of this chapter.
Thanks are also extended to Julie Pepin and Laurie Gelfand for their help with
literature searches, library work and preparation of the final version of this chapter.
Some of the ideas for, and the content of, this chapter originated from the author’s
2001 studies on touch and physical contact during infancy.13 The publisher of this
handbook is also gratefully acknowledged.
74 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Ref ere n c e s
1. Montagu A. Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin. New York, NY: Columbia
University Press; 1971.
2. Hamnet FS. Studies in the thyroid apparatus. American Journal of Physiology. 1921;56:
196-204.
6. Kuhn CM, Schanberg SM. Responses to maternal separation: mechanisms and mediators.
International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the International
Society for Developmental Neuroscience. 1998;16:261-270.
7. Schanberg SM, Field TM. Sensory deprivation stress and supplemental stimulation in the rat
pup and preterm human neonate. Child Development. 1987;58:1431-1447.
8. Denenberg VH. The effects of early experience. In: Hafez ESE, ed. The Behavior of Domestic
Animals. 2nd ed. London, England: Baillure, Tindall and Cox; 1969:95-130.
9. Harlow HF. Love in infant monkeys. Scientific American. 1959;200:68.
10. Levine S. A further study of infantile handling and adult avoidance learning. Journal of
Personality. 1956;25:70-80.
13. Stack DM. The salience of touch and physical contact during infancy: unraveling some of the
mysteries of the somesthetic sense. In: Bremner G, Fogel A, eds. Blackwell Handbook of Infant
Development. Malden, Mass: Blackwell Publishers; 2001:351-378.
14. Montagu A. Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Harper
and Row Publishers; 1986.
17. Spitz RA, Wolf KM. Anaclitic depression. An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric
conditions in early childhood II. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child. 1946;2:313-342.
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 75
19. Rutter M. Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early
privation. English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 1998;39:465-476.
20. Brazelton TB. Touch as a touchstone: summary of the round table. In: Barnard KE, Brazelton
TB, eds. Touch: The Foundation of Experience. Vol 4. Madison, Wis: International Universities
Press; 1990:561-566.
21. Birns B, Blank M, Bridger WH. The effectiveness of various soothing techniques on human
neonates. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1966;28:316-322.
22. Korner AF, Thoman EB. The relative efficacy of contact and vestibular-proprioceptive
stimulation in soothing neonates. Child Development. 1972;43:443-453.
23. Barrera ME, Maurer D. The perception of facial expressions by the three-month-old. Child
Development. 1981;52:203-206.
24. Muir D, Field J. Newborn infants orient to sounds. Child Development. 1979;50:431-436.
25. Brazelton TB. Introduction. In: Brown CC, ed. The Many Facets of Touch. Skillman, NJ:
Johnson & Johnson Baby Products Co; 1984:xv-xviii. Pediatric Round Table Series, No. 10.
26. Koester LS, Papouek H, Papouek M. Patterns of rhythmic stimulation by mothers with
three-month-olds: a cross-modal comparison. International Journal of Behavioral Development.
1989;12:143-154.
27. Montagu A. Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Harper
and Row Publishers; 1986.
28. Tronick EZ. Touch in mother–infant interaction. In: Field TM, ed. Touch in Early
Development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1995:53-65.
29. Hertenstein MJ, Campos JJ. Emotion regulation via maternal touch. Infancy. 2001;2:
549-566.
30. Ainsworth MD, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study
of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1978.
31. Bowlby J. Attachment & Loss. Vol 1. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1969.
32. Landau R. Affect and attachment: kissing, hugging, and patting as attachment behaviors.
Infant Mental Health Journal. 1989;10:52-69.
33. Stack DM, Muir DW. Adult tactile stimulation during face-to-face interactions modulates
five-month-olds’ affect and attention. Child Development. 1992;63:1509-1525.
34. Isabella RA, Belsky J, von Eye A. Origins of infant-mother attachment: an examination
of international synchrony during the infant’s first year. Developmental Psychology.
1989;25:12-21.
35. Anisfeld E, Lipper E. Early contact, social support, and mother-infant bonding. Pediatrics.
1983;72:79-83.
76 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
36. Grossmann K, Thane K, Grossman KE. Maternal tactual contact of the newborn after
various postpartum conditions of mother-infant contact. Developmental Psychology.
1981;17:158-169.
37. Schaller J, Carlsson SG, Larsson K. Effects of extended post-partum mother-child contact
on the mother’s behavior during nursing. Infant Behavior & Development. 1979;2:319-324.
38. Cannon RB. The development of maternal touch during early mother-infant interaction.
Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing: JOGNN/NAACOG. 1977;6:28-33.
39. Carlsson SG, Fagerberg H, Horneman G, et al. Effects of amount of contact between mother
and child on the mother’s nursing behavior. Developmental Psychobiology. 1978;11:143-150.
40. de Chateau P. The influence of early contact on maternal and infant behaviour on
primiparae. Birth and the Family Journal. 1976-1997;3:149-155.
41. Klaus MH, Kennell JH, Plumb N, Zuehlke S. Human maternal behavior at the first contact
with her young. Pediatrics. 1970;46:187-192.
45. Harrison LL, Woods S. Early parental touch and preterm infants. Journal of Obstetric,
Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing: JOGNN/NAACOG. 1991;20:299-306.
46. Robin M. Neonate-mother interaction: tactile contacts in the days following birth. Early
Child Development and Care. 1982;9:221-236.
47. Field TM. Massage therapy effects. The American Psychologist. 1998;53:1270-1281.
48. Field T. Touch therapy effects on development. International Journal of Behavioral
Development. 1998;22:779-797.
49. Field T. Touch Therapy. New York, NY: Churchill Livingstone; 2000.
50. Kaye K, Fogel A. The temporal structure of face-to-face communication between mothers
and infants. Developmental Psychology. 1980;16:454-464.
51. Field TM, Vega-Lahr N, Scafidi F, Goldstein S. Effects of maternal unavailability on
mother-infant interactions. Infant Behavior & Development. 1986;9:473-478.
52. Cohn JF, Tronick EZ. Three-month-old infants’ reaction to simulated maternal depression.
Child Development. 1983;54:185-193.
53. Field TM. Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers.
Infant Behavior & Development. 1984;7:517-522.
54. Symons DK, Moran G. The behavioral dynamics of mutual responsiveness in early
face-to-face mother-infant interactions. Child Development. 1987;58:1488-1495.
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 77
55. Fogel A, Dedo JY, McEwen I. Effect of postural position and reaching on gaze during
mother-infant face-to-face interaction. Infant Behavior & Development. 1992;15:231-244.
56. Toda S, Fogel A. Infant response to the still-face situation at 3 and 6 months. Developmental
Psychology. 1993;29:532-538.
57. Stack DM, Arnold SL. Changes in mothers’ touch and hand gestures influence infant
behavior during face-to-face interchanges. Infant Behavior & Development. 1998;21:451-468.
58. Lavelli M, Fogel A. Developmental changes in early mother-infant face-to-face
communication (abstract). In: Program and abstracts of the Biennial Meeting of the
International Conference on Infant Studies; April 2-5, 1998; Atlanta, Ga.
59. Stack DM, Arnold SL, Girouard N, Welburn B. Infants’ reactions to maternal unavailability
in very low birth weight preterm and full-term infants. Paper presented at: the Biennial
Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; April, 1999; Albuquerque, NM.
60. Tronick EZ, Als H, Adamson L, Wise S, Brazelton TB. The infant’s response to entrapment
between contradictory messages in face-to-face interactions. Journal of the American Academy
of Child Psychiatry. 1978;17:1-13.
61. Gusella JL, Muir DW, Tronick EZ. The effect of manipulating maternal behavior during
an interaction on three- and six-month-olds’ affect and attention. Child Development.
1988;59:1111-1124.
62. Lamb ME, Morrison DC, Malkin CM. The development of infant social expectations in
face-to-face interaction: a longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 1987;33:241-254.
63. Mayes LC, Carter AS. Emerging social regulatory capacities as seen in the still-face situation.
Child Development. 1990;61:754-763.
64. Ellsworth CP, Muir DW, Hains SM. Social competence and person-object differentiation:
an analysis of the still-face effect. Developmental Psychology. 1993;29:63-73.
65. Stack DM, Muir DW. Tactile stimulation as a component of social interchange: new
interpretations for the still-face effect. British Journal of Developmental Psychology.
1990;8:131-145.
66. Peláez-Nogueras M, Field TM, Hossain Z, Pickens J. Depressed mothers’ touching increases
infants’ positive affect and attention in still-face interactions. Child Development.
1996;67:1780-1792.
67. Stack DM, LePage DE. Infants’ sensitivity to manipulations of maternal touch during
face-to-face interactions. Social Development. 1996;5:41-55.
68. Stack DM, LePage DE, Hains SM, Muir DW. Qualitative changes in maternal touch as a
function of instructional condition during face-to-face social interactions. Poster presented at
the 10th Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies; April 18-21, 1996; Providence,
RI.
69. LePage DE. Four- and 7-Month-Old Infants’ Sensitivities to Contingency During Face-to-Face
Social Interactions [dissertation]. Montréal, Québec, Canada: Concordia University; 1998.
78 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
70. LePage DE, Stack DM. Four- and 7-month-old infants’ abilities to detect tactile
contingencies in a face-to-face context (abstract). In: Program and abstracts of the Biennial
Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; April 3-6,1997; Washington,
DC.
71. Arnold SL, Brouillette J, Stack DM. Changes in maternal and infant behavior as a function
of instructional manipulations during unimodal and multimodal interactions (abstract).
In: Program and abstracts of the Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study
of Behavioral Development; August 12-16, 1996; Quebec City, Quebec, Canada.
72. Stack DM. Touching during mother-infant face-to-face interactions: influences of context
and modality. Paper presented at: The Touch Research Institutes Symposium Preconference,
XIIIth Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies; April 18-21, 2002; Toronto,
Ontario, Canada.
73. Arnold SL. Maternal Tactile-Gestural Simulation and Infants Nonverbal Behaviors During Early
Mother-Infant Face-to-Face Interactions: Contextual, Age, and Birth Status Effects [dissertation].
Montréal, Québec, Canada: Concordia University; 2002.
74. Roedell WC, Slaby RG. The role of distal and proximal interaction in infant social preference
formation. Developmental Psychology. 1977;13:266-273.
75. Leiba E. Maternal Nonverbal Behaviors and Infant Gaze During Triadic Play With Toys at
5 and 12 Months [master’s thesis]. Montréal, Québec, Canada: Concordia University; 2000.
76. Leiba E, Stack DM. Maternal nonverbal behaviors and affectionate displays during play at
5 and 12 months (abstract). In: Program and abstracts of the Canadian Psychological
Association Annual Convention; June 21-23, 2001; Sainte-Foy, Quebec, Canada.
77. Peláez-Nogueras M. Rhythmic and nonrhythmic touch during mother infant interactions.
(abstract). In: Program and abstracts of the Society for Research in Child Development;
March 30-April 2, 1995; Indianapolis, Ind.
78. Peláez-Nogueras M, Gewirtz JL, Field T, et al. Infants’ preference for touch stimulation
in face-to-face interactions. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1996;17:199-213.
79. Wolff PH. Observations on the early development of smiling. In: Foss BM, ed. Determinants
of Infant Behavior II. London, England: Methuen & Co Ltd; 1963:113-138.
80. Roggman LA, Woodson R. Touch and gaze in parent-infant play (abstract). In: Program and
abstracts of the Society for Research in Child Development Conference; April 27-30, 1989;
Kansas City, Kan.
81. Dickson KL, Walker H, Fogel A. The relationship between smile type and play type during
parent-infant play. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:925-933.
82. Tronick EZ. Emotions and emotional communication in infants. The American Psychologist.
1989;44:112-119.
83. Campos JJ, Mumme DL, Kermoian R, Campos RG. A functionalist perspective on the
nature of emotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1994;59:
284-303.
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 79
84. Thompson RA. Socioemotional development: enduring issues and new challenges.
Developmental Review. 1993;13:372-402.
85. Weinberg MK, Tronick EZ. Beyond the face: an empirical study of infant affective
configurations of facial, vocal, gestural, and regulatory behaviors. Child Development.
1994;65:1503-1515.
86. Hertenstein MJ. Touch: its communicative functions in infancy. Human Development.
2002;45:70-94.
87. Weiss SJ. Measurement of the sensory qualities in tactile interaction. Nursing Research.
1992;41:82-86.
88. Weiss SJ, Campos R. Touch. In: Lindeman CA, McAthie M, eds. Fundamentals of
Contemporary Nursing Practice. Philadelphia, Pa: WB Saunders Company; 1999:941-967.
89. Polan HJ, Ward MJ. Role of the mother’s touch in failure to thrive: a preliminary
investigation. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.
1994;33:1098-1105.
90. Fergus EL, Schmidt J, Pickens J. Touch during mother-infant interactions: the effects of
parenting stress, depression and anxiety (abstract). In: Program and abstracts of the Biennial
Meeting of the International Society of Infant Studies; April, 1998; Atlanta, Ga.
91. Cohn JF, Tronick E. Specificity of infants’ response to mothers’ affective behavior. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1989;28:242-248.
92. Malphurs JE, Raag T, Field T, Pickens J, Peláez-Nogueras M. Touch by intrusive and
withdrawn mothers with depressive symptoms. Early Development & Parenting. 1996;5:
111-115.
93. Lundy BL, Field T, Cuadra A, Nearing G, Cigales M, Hashimoto M. Mothers with
depressive symptoms touching newborns. Early Development & Parenting. 1996;5:129-134.
94. Koester LS. Effects of maternal or infant deafness on early interaction patterns (abstract).
In: Program and abstracts of the Biennial Meeting of the International Conference on Infant
Studies; May 6-10, 1992; Miami, Fla.
95. Meadow-Orlans KP, Steinberg AG. Effects of infant hearing loss and maternal support
on mother-infant interactions at 18 months. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology.
1993;14:407-426.
96. Koester LS, Papouek H, Brooks L. The role of tactile contact in deaf and hearing
mother-infant dyads (abstract). In: Program and abstracts of the Biennial Meeting of the
Society for Research in Child Development; March 2-April 2, 1995; Indianapolis, Ind.
97. MacTurk RH, Meadow-Orlans KP, Koester LS, Spencer PE. Social support, motivation,
language, and interaction. A longitudinal study of mothers and deaf infants. American Annals
of the Deaf. 1993;138:19-25.
98. Schwartzman AE, Ledingham JE, Serbin LA. Identification of children at risk for adult
schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. International Review of Applied Psychology. 1985;34:
363-380.
80 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
99. Serbin LA, Cooperman JM, Peters PL, Lehoux PM, Stack DM, Schwartzman AE.
Intergenerational transfer of psychosocial risk in women with childhood histories of
aggression, withdrawal, or aggression and withdrawal. Developmental Psychology.
1998;34:1246-1262.
100. Bentley VM. Maternal Childhood Risk as a Predictor of Emotional Availability and Physical
Contact in Mother-Child Interactions: an Intergenerational Study [master’s thesis]. Montréal,
Québec, Canada: Concordia University; 1997.
101. Weiss SJ, Wilson P, Hertenstein MJ, Campos R. The tactile context of a mother’s caregiving:
implications for attachment of low birth weight infants. Infant Behavior & Development.
2000;23:91-111.
102. Weiss SJ, Wilson P, St Jonn-Seed M, Paul SM. Early tactile experience of low birth weight
children: links to later mental health and social adaptation. Infant and Child Development.
2001;10:93-115.
103. Arco CM. Infant reactions to natural and manipulated temporal patterns of paternal
communication. Infant Behavior & Development. 1983;6:391-399.
104. Hewlett BS. Intimate fathers: patterns of paternal holding among Aka pygmies. In: Lamb
ME, ed. The Father’s Role: Cross-cultural Perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates; 1987:295-330.
105. Parke RD, O’Leary S. Family interaction in the newborn period: some findings, some
observations and some unresolved issues. In: Riegel KF, Meacham J, eds. The Developing
Individual in a Changing World. Vol 1. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton; 1976:
653-663.
106. Yogman MW. Development of the father-infant relationship. In: Fitzgerald HE, Lester BM,
Yogman MW, eds. Theory and Research in Behavioral Pediatrics. Vol 1. New York, NY:
Plenum Press; 1982:221-279.
107. Lamb ME. The Role of the Father in Child Development. New York, NY: John Wiley; 1981.
108. Tomlinson PS, Rothenberg MA, Carver LD. Behavioral interaction of fathers with infants
and mothers in the immediate postpartum period. Journal of Nurse-Midwifery. 1991;36:
232-239.
109. New RS, Benigni L. Italian fathers and infants: cultural constraints on paternal behavior.
In: Lamb ME, ed. The Father’s Role: Cross-cultural Perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates; 1987:139-167.
110. Roopnarine JL, Talukder E, Jain D, Joshi P, Srivastav P. Characteristics of holding, patterns
of play, and social behaviors between parents and infants in New Delhi, India.
Developmental Psychology. 1990;26:667-673.
111. LePage DE, Stack DM. Do manipulations of maternal face, voice and touch differentially
effect infant responses? (abstract). In: Program and abstracts of the Canadian Psychological
Association; June 11-13, 1992; Québec City, Canada.
112. Frank LK. Tactile communication. Genetic Psychology Monographs. 1957;56:209-255.
Chapter 4: Touching During Mother-Infant Interactions 81
113. Stack DM. Qualitative components of maternal touch during mother-infant interactions:
application of the Caregiver-Infant Touch Scale. Invited speaker at the Touch Research
Institutes Symposium Preconference at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research
in Human Development: April 24-27, 2003; Tampa, Fla.
114. Casler CR. Maternal deprivation: a critical review of the literature. Monographs of the Society
for Research in Child Development. 1961;26:2.
115. Casler CR. Perceptual deprivation in institutional settings. In: Newton G, Levine S, eds.
Early Experience and Behavior. New York, NY: Springer; 1968.
116. Ainsworth MDS. The effects of maternal deprivation: a review of findings and controversy
in the context of research strategy. Deprivation of maternal care: a reassessment of its effects.
Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1962.
117. Byrnes JM, Horowitz FD. Rocking as a soothing intervention: the influence of direction
and type of movement. Infant Behavior and Development. 1981;4:207-218.
CHAPTER 5:
DIFFERENCES ACROSS
C U LT U R A L G R O U P S
IN MOTHERS’ NONVERBAL
TEACHING METHODS
Ab s t ra c t
The primary purpose of the study described in this chapter was to determine how
mothers used touch to “teach” their infants; that is, to help direct their attention
and shape their actions. The study also examined the changes in mothers’ preferred
teaching methods from infancy through toddlerhood and described the cultural
differences in nonverbal teaching strategies. Mothers in 4 American cultural
groups—Caucasian, Japanese, Filipino and Hawaiian—participated in the
study, which assessed how the individuals in each of these groups touched, held,
repositioned and physically interacted with their infants and toddlers (3 months
to 36 months of age) during videotaped mealtimes and how they organized the
mealtime setting. The teaching methods used by the different groups and the
results of this study are explored in detail in this chapter.
84 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Introd uc t i o n
Parents teach children in many ways.1 They instruct children directly, they explain
to them how things work and they show them how to solve problems. Parents
naturally encourage their children to see and respond to reality in the same ways
that they do and to adopt the same “goals for action” that they pursue. Parents also
model actions that they want their children to copy. In addition, they structure
their children’s day-to-day lives so that they will engage in activities that the parents
feel are important and so that their children will come into contact with good
teachers and role models.
Bruner described carefully how parents teach their infants nonverbally.2 He noted
that they hold and move their babies’ limbs in ways that are valued culturally, and
that they direct their infants to attend only to certain objects and perform only
certain “goal-directed actions.” For example, mothers teach their babies that “bells
are for ringing” by wrapping the baby’s fingers around the stem of the bell, shaking
the bell and showing excitement when the bell rings. The baby then takes on this
goal for action and stops “mouthing” bells in order to ring them. Similarly, babies
learn to bang with mallets, to stir with spoons, to match shapes to slots and to say
“hi” and “bye-bye” into telephones.
Harkness and Super pointed out that parents teach their children by structuring
their environments.1 Different sets of parents in this study set up mealtimes quite
differently. Mealtimes differed in one or more of the following ways: whether
the family sat together at a table; whether adults and children ate together; how
many adults were present; how many other children were present; whether
infants/toddlers were constrained to high chairs, held on laps or allowed to roam;
whether mothers attended exclusively to infants; and whether the television was
turned on. Parents effectively convey the messages of their cultural and social groups
and their personal beliefs and habits by carefully (albeit, often subconsciously)
structuring their children’s environments and activity settings and by guiding their
children’s actions.
Chapter 5: Differences Across Cultural Groups in Mothers’ Nonverbal Teaching Methods 85
Cur re n t St u d y 3
METHOD
DATA COLLECTION
Families were recruited from 2 preschools and one elementary school in Honolulu,
Hawaii, for a study of dual-earner families. Parents set up a camera provided
to them, pointed it at their typical mealtime area and turned it on for at least
20 minutes during mealtimes. Videotapes from 80 families who had at least one
young child (ages 3 months to 36 months) were viewed and coded for this study.
PARTICIPANTS
Twenty families were studied from each of the following 4 American cultural
groups: Caucasian American, Japanese American, Filipino American and Hawaiian
American. The mothers in these families had stated on a prestudy questionnaire
that they identified most closely with that cultural group.
Within each cultural group in this sample, 10 families had an infant less than 23
months of age, while the other 10 families had an older toddler, from 24 months
to 36 months of age. Equal numbers of male and female infants/toddlers were
studied within each cultural group. The groups did not differ significantly in terms
of average age of the infants, gender of the infants, number of people present at
the meal, number of parents present at the meal and number of siblings present
at the meal.
CODING
After repeated viewings of a set of these videotapes, a coding system was developed
to define observed forms of touch, seating patterns for the infants and other setting
features. Two observers coded the mothers’ actions during 10 minutes of each
videotaped mealtime. They coded the 10 minutes that followed the first 3 minutes
of each videotape, thereby omitting coding on that part of the tapes during which
families typically adjusted to the camera while setting up their meals.
86 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
ANALYSIS
A one-way analysis of variance was used to compare age groups to each other,
cultural groups to each other and males to females. Data were analyzed to compare
the effects of mealtime setting features, the amounts of touch and the forms
of touch. Comparisons involved the extent to which mothers touched infants
in particular ways in these groups.
R E S U LT S
During meals, the mothers used the following 4 major forms of touch, which
reflect 4 forms of teaching:
It was determined that mothers touched their infants an average of 2.11 times per
minute. As Figure 1 shows, most instances of touch consisted of “instrumental
touching” (at an average rate of 1.33 instances/minute), during which the babies
experienced passively how feeding or cleaning should proceed. The second most
frequently used forms of touch were “supportive” (affectionate) and “responsive”
touch combined (at an average rate of 0.55 instances/minute). Touch was used least
frequently to “direct” or “restrict” infants (at an average of 0.24 instances/minute).
Figure 1. Observed forms of touch during mealtimes among mothers and their infants
in different cultural groups.3
Figure 2. The types of touch and the frequency with which mothers from different
cultural groups touched their infants versus their toddlers during mealtimes.3
Chapter 5: Differences Across Cultural Groups in Mothers’ Nonverbal Teaching Methods 89
The manners in which mothers from the various cultural groups involved in this
study structured their mealtime settings affected how they interacted with their
infants. The structuring of the activity setting and guiding children through the
activities are ways of conveying cultural preferences and messages.
Caucasian American mothers and Japanese American mothers tended to place their
infants and toddlers in high chairs in order to feed them (Figure 3). In addition,
Japanese American parents tended to provide their babies with play toys during
feeding. Caucasian American parents tended not to provide toys, explaining that
they thought the toys would distract their babies from eating.
Caucasian American parents tended to place the high chair at the periphery of
the larger family meal gathering. Cacausian American infants were commonly
given finger foods and were attended to sporadically. These parents attended most
consistently to their children who were verbal, as meals were seen as times for
exchanging information.
90 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Filipino American mothers tended to hold their infants and toddlers on their laps,
facing outward. From this position—being fed from behind—Filipino American
babies were touched much more frequently than were infants from the other
cultures studied. Since she could not see her child’s facial cues, the Filipino
American mother fed her child on her own schedule and not in relation to
readiness cues from her child.
Hawaiian American parents held their babies on their laps to feed them. They also
“passed” their infants to other adults and older children sitting around the meal
table. They allowed their toddlers to “roam” during meals (Figure 5), and they
were permitted to come and go to a central adult holding the toddler’s food bowl.
In this way, the child experienced maximum autonomy in deciding what and when
to eat. Roaming occurred more frequently in the Hawaiian American group than
in any of the other groups studied.
Japanese American: Japanese American mothers touched their infants the next
most frequently (mean, 2.57 touches/minute). During mealtimes, they placed their
infants in high chairs more frequently than did Filipino American or Hawaiian
American parents, and then attended to them carefully. Japanese American mothers
used the “responsive” form of teaching/touching more often than did mothers
in the other groups. They facilitated exploratory play and helped their children
complete self-initiated goals. And, although they did not block their children’s
unwanted actions, they reshaped them subtly until they were more acceptable.
92 Section I. Maternal Touch and Touch Perception
Cu ltura l Me ss a g e s Ab o u t To u c h
Parents in the various cultural groups studied in this study conveyed different
messages about togetherness and independence during mealtimes. Filipino
American mothers and Japanese American mothers were highly affectionate and
highly controlling toward their infants during mealtimes. They, in fact, taught their
infants that being a member of a tight-knit family group involves both the warmth
of belonging and the obligations of conforming to group ways.
Although Caucasian American mothers were more distant relationally, they allowed
their infants a greater sense of autonomy and choice. This autonomy involved
reduced closeness but increased individual self-expression through talk. Whereas,
even though Hawaiian American mothers enabled autonomy, they also allowed
their infants to seek more contact when their babies wanted such contact.
These groups of American parents also sent messages to their infants about the
appropriate ways to make contact with others. Caucasian American parents, in
particular, emphasized making contact through talking during mealtimes. They
effectively “separated” mealtime members into formal, boundaried spaces at the
dinner table, and then tried to close the distance with conversation. Therefore,
it is not surprising that Caucasian American children learn to talk early, loudly
and often.
Chapter 5: Differences Across Cultural Groups in Mothers’ Nonverbal Teaching Methods 95
Conclusions
In this study,3 the mean rate at which American mothers touched their infants and
toddlers during mealtimes was very low. Mealtimes may be settings during which
parents encourage independence and discourage touch. The low rate of touching,
the decrease in touch as children got older and, in particular, the decrease in
affectionate touch may reflect current American patterns of increasing physical
distance among people as they mature.
Touching their infants was most frequent among the most traditional mothers;
that is, those who held their infants on their laps. However, these were also the
most “directive” mothers. Touching was least frequent in mothers who rely on
baby furniture, such as high chairs and booster chairs: High chairs emphasize the
“separateness” of baby and mother, while they also constrain the infant. While they
allow the baby to sit and eat by himself/herself, freeing up the mother, they also
keep the infant from seeking more contact when he/she wants it.
Mothers who sat on the floor or on low chairs while their infants or toddlers
roamed touched their children more, in large part because the infants approached
the adults for more touch. Babies who roamed and “grazed” experienced autonomy
in choosing when to come for more food. In addition, such infants were free to
approach adults to request touch, which could not be done by the infants who
were constrained in high chairs.
Ackn owle d g m e nt s
Ref ere n c e s
1. Harkness, Super, 1989. Personal communication.
2. Bruner JS (1973). Organization of early skilled action. Child Development, 44, 1-11.
3. Martini M. Differences across cultural groups in mothers’ nonverbal teaching methods.
Paper presented at: Touch Research Symposium; April 19, 2001; Minneapolis, Minn.
Touc h a n d Ma s s a g e i n Ea rl y Ch ild De ve lop me nt
SECTION II.
T H E R A P E U T I C A P P L I C AT I O N S
O F T O U C H I N P R E G N A N C Y,
P R E G N A N C Y, L A B O R
Abstract
Massage therapy during pregnancy benefits the mother-to-be and the fetus.
Massage during labor also benefits the expectant mother, reducing her labor
pain and shortening her labor. Elderly volunteers who massaged infants
lowered their own stress hormone levels, as well as improved the health of
the newborns. In addition, depressed mothers who massaged their infants
experienced decreased depression. In this chapter studies are reviewed on
massage therapy during pregnancy and labor, as well as infant massage for
full-term and preterm infants, and the benefits experienced by both those
who give and those who receive the massages.
100 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
In our study on pregnancy massage, the massage therapy was expected to decrease
stress hormones and anxiety, leg pain and back pain. Improvements in mood and
sleep were also expected. In this study, 26 pregnant women were assigned to a
massage therapy or a relaxation therapy group for 5 weeks.1 The therapies consisted
of 20-minute sessions twice weekly. Both groups reported feeling less anxious after
the first session and less leg pain after the first and last sessions. Only the massage
therapy group, however, reported reduced anxiety, improved mood, better sleep
and less back pain by the last day of the study. In addition, urinary stress hormones
(ie, norepinephrine) decreased for the massage therapy group, these women
had fewer obstetric complications and their infants experienced fewer postnatal
complications, most especially a lower incidence of prematurity.
Prenatal depression has been noted to affect the developing fetus. In one of our
studies, depressed pregnant women who had elevated cortisol and norepinephrine
(stress hormones) gave birth to newborns who had depression-like symptoms2:
These newborns also had higher levels of cortisol and norepinephrine,3 as well
as greater relative right frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) activation, thereby
mimicking their depressed mothers’ stress hormone profile and EEG patterns.4
In an attempt to reduce maternal depression and its negative effects on the fetus
and neonate, we conducted a study on massage therapy with depressed pregnant
women (Field et al, 2003, unpublished data). These depressed women were
randomly assigned to a massage therapy, relaxation therapy or a standard
treatment control group and were compared with each other and with a group of
nondepressed women at the end of pregnancy. The massage and relaxation groups
received two 20-minute therapy sessions each week, given by their significant
others, during the last trimester of their pregnancies. Data analyses revealed the
following (Figure 1):
• Immediately after both types of therapy on the first and last days,
the women reported lower anxiety and reduced leg and back pain.
Figure 1. In depressed pregnant women, massage therapy during the last trimester
was associated with lower depression scores and decreased fetal movement.T.F., M.D., M.H-R.,
S. Schanberg, C. Kuhn, unpublished data, 2003
These data suggest that depressed pregnant women and their offspring can benefit
from these alternative therapies, particularly massage therapy.
Ma s sa g e Du r i ng L a b o r
Touch and massage have been used effectively during labor in nearly every culture
for hundreds of years.5 In contrast, physical support during delivery has been
available only recently to Western women.6 In the past, the massage and support
102 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
provided during labor were used to improve or correct the position of the fetus, to
stimulate uterine contractions, to “…prevent the fetus from rising back up in the
abdomen…” and to exert mechanical pressure to aid in the expulsion of the child.7
However, today the focus tends to center more on relaxation to reduce anxiety and
alleviate pain.5
This study compared significant others massaging their pregnant partners during
labor versus partners simply being present and doing what came naturally during
labor (typically, coaching the breathing exercises they had learned in prenatal
classes).8 Massage coupled with breathing exercises, versus breathing exercises alone,
was expected to reduce anxiety and pain, as well as the length of labor. Twenty-eight
women were recruited from prenatal classes and assigned randomly to receive
massage and coaching in breathing from their partners during labor, or to receive
coaching in breathing alone. The mothers who received massage reported decreases
in depressed mood, anxiety and pain, showed less agitated activity and anxiety,
and exhibited more positive affect following the first massage. In addition, the
mothers who were massaged had significantly shorter labors (Figure 2), less labor
medication, shorter hospital stays and less postpartum depression.
Figure 2. Mothers who received massage therapy from their partners during labor,
in addition to coached breathing exercises, had significantly shorter labors than those
who received coached breathing exercises alone.8
Chapter 6: Pregnancy, Labor and Infant Massage 103
Inf a n t Ma ss a ge
Infant massage is practiced in most countries of the world, especially in Africa and
Asia.9,10 In many countries, including Nigeria, Uganda, India, China, Bali, Fiji,
New Guinea, New Zealand (the Maiori), and the Soviet Union, infants are given
a massage with oil after the daily bath and before sleep time for the first several
months of their lives.
Infant massage has been discovered and researched only recently in the Western
world. Suddenly, in the United States, there are massage therapy schools in almost
every city teaching parents how to massage their infants. The techniques they are
learning and using are based on the teachings of 2 massage therapists who trained in
India.9,10 Indian infant massage involves a daily routine that begins during the first
days of life. The infant is laid on his or her stomach on the mother’s outstretched
legs, and each body part is stretched individually. Warm water and soap are applied
to the legs, arms, back, abdomen, neck and face. The massager looks like she is
scrubbing clothes on an old washboard and the process seems extremely rigorous.
After they are massaged and swaddled, the infants then sleep for prolonged periods.
Although data have not been collected on infant massage as it is practiced in India,
some infant massage therapists have attributed the precocious motor development
of these infants to their daily massages. Infant massage therapists have made
several claims based on anecdotal data, including that the massage provides
both stimulation and relaxation that helps respiration, circulation, digestion and
elimination.11 They have claimed that infants who are massaged sleep more soundly,
that the massage relieves gas and colic, and that it helps the healing process during
illness by easing congestion and pain.12 Furthermore, they assert that infant massage
helps enhance parent-infant bonding and warm, positive relationships, reduces
distress in the infant following painful procedures (such as inoculations), reduces
pain from teething and constipation, reduces sleep problems and makes parents
“feel good” while they are massaging their infants. Infant massage therapy groups
have also reported that infants who have special needs—such as those who are
blind and deaf or those who are paralyzed, have cerebral palsy or are premature—
seem to become more aware of their bodies, among receiving other benefits.
104 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
Figure 3. Premature babies do not like light touch, probably because it feels like
tickling. Babies who gained weight in the studies of infant massage were those who
received deeper-pressure massage that stimulated both tactile and pressure receptors.13
Figure 4. Massaged infants gained 47% more weight per day than control infants.14
Chapter 6: Pregnancy, Labor and Infant Massage 105
Pre t e r m In f a nt Ma s s a ge
Most data available on the effects of infant massage come from studies of preterm/
premature infants. During the last 2 decades, several studies were conducted on
what we called “tactile/kinesthetic stimulation” (due to the negative connotations
attached to the word “massage”). A meta-analysis of data from 19 of these studies
revealed that 72% of the massaged infants were affected positively.13 Most of
them experienced greater weight gain and better performance on developmental
assessments. In those studies that did not report significant weight gain, investigators
had used a light-stroking procedure. Babies do not like light touch (Figure 3),
probably because it feels like tickling. The babies who gained weight had been given
deeper-pressure massage, thereby stimulating both tactile and pressure receptors.
One of the studies used in this global analysis was conducted in our laboratory
at the Touch Research Institute in Miami.14 In that study, massage therapy was
given to preterm newborns 3 times per day, for 15 minutes per session, for 10 days.
As shown in Figure 4, the massaged infants averaged 47% greater weight gain
than infants who were not massaged, even though the groups consumed equal
amounts of formula. The massaged infants were awake and active more of the time,
even though we expected they would sleep more. They were also more alert and
responsive to the examiner’s face and voice, and they showed more organized limb
movements on the BNBAS. Finally, they were discharged from the hospital 6 days
sooner, saving approximately $3000 per infant in hospital costs. The comparable
cost savings today in the US would be $10,000 per infant. If every one of the
470,000 premature infants born each year in the US was massaged, the hospital
cost savings would approximate $4.7 billion per year. That dollar figure could
double based on more recent data, suggesting that the same weight gain can be
achieved in 5 days, versus 10 days, of massage therapy.15
Replication studies have been conducted in Israel16 and the Philippines.17 In the
study performed in the Philippines,17 which was done with an exact replication
of the Field et al methodology,18 preterm infants who were massaged gained 45%
more than infants who were not massaged. In the study conducted in Israel,16 a
31% greater weight gain and more organized sleep were reported for the massaged,
versus the control, preterm infants. In addition, the mothers who provided the
massage experienced less postpartum depression. These studies approximated
the weight gain data (47% and 31%, respectively) published by Field et al14 and
106 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
Scafidi et al.19 A recent study by Dieter et al15 suggests that a 46% greater weight
gain can be achieved in preterm infants following only 5 days of massage.
At approximately the same time that the previously mentioned premature infant
studies were being done,14 colleagues at Duke University Medical School were
conducting similar studies on rat pups.20 They separated rat pups from their
mothers to explore touch deprivation. The researchers stroked the deprived rat pups
with a paintbrush—much like the mother rat would tongue-lick them—so that the
pups would grow normally. In several studies, the team at Duke noted that growth
hormone decreased when the pups were separated from their mothers. This
decrease was noted in all body organs, including the heart, liver and brain. These
values, however, returned to normal once the pups were stroked with the paintbrush.
The more recent discovery of a growth gene that responds to touch suggests a
strong genetic influence on the relationship between touching and growth,
although the underlying mechanism is unclear at this time.21
This observation, plus the results of a study done in Sweden,22 led myself and
my colleagues to some theories about mechanisms that might explain the
touch/weight-gain relationship. The investigators in Sweden reported that
stimulating the mouths of the newborn, as well as the breasts of the breastfeeding
mothers, led to an increase in food absorption hormones, such as gastrin and
insulin.22 Schanberg and I argued that massage therapy delivered to several parts
of the body would lead to an even greater increase in food absorption hormones,
which could itself explain the weight gain.23 Assays on the insulin levels before,
during and after massage therapy suggested that insulin levels were elevated in
those preterm infants who received massage therapy versus those who did not
(a 61% increase versus a 4% decrease, respectively, in insulin levels).23 Of course,
future studies could assess the relative changes caused by several food absorption
hormones and other vagal stimulated changes, such as an increase in gastric
motility.
Mass a g i n g In f a nt s Wi t h Oi l
To determine whether massaging infants with oil enhanced the effects of massage, a
study was conducted in which 60, 1-month-old, full-term infants were randomized
Chapter 6: Pregnancy, Labor and Infant Massage 107
to one group that was massaged with oil, while the other group was massaged
without oil.24 Results of this trial showed that the infants massaged with oil
experienced greater soothing and calming effects. The infants who received massage
with oil were less active, showed fewer stress behaviors, demonstrated less head
averting and had greater declines in saliva cortisol levels. In addition, vagal activity
increased more following massage with oil versus massage without oil.
“Colic” and sleep problems are the complaints presented most frequently to
pediatricians by parents of infants. In a study on these problems, we taught parents,
whose infants attended a university nursery school, to massage their 3-month-old
to 6-month-old infants for 15-minute periods prior to bedtime.18 The massaged
infants, versus a group of control infants, became less irritable, fell asleep faster,
experienced fewer night awakenings and spent more time in quiet, alert states
during the daytime.
Parents who participated in this study, and who years later encountered the
investigators on campus, suggested that their marriages had been “saved” by
having received help to calm their infants and get them to sleep. Others expressed
chagrin that their children, now as old as 7 years, still needed a massage in order
to go to sleep.
De pre s se d Mo t h e r s Ma s s a gi ng T h e ir Infants
In our studies, we routinely teach parents to massage their infants so they can be
massaged on a daily basis at no cost. In addition, these parents are told that they,
too, might benefit from giving the massages. A study in which depressed mothers
were taught to massage their infants resulted in decreased maternal depression and
less distress behavior and disturbed sleep patterns in the infants.18 For this study,
the infants’ mothers were asked to perform 15-minute massages once daily for
2 weeks. At the end of the study period, the massaged infants fell asleep faster, slept
longer and were less fussy. The mothers enjoyed better face-to-face interactions
108 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
Fath e r s a s Ma s s a g e T h e ra p i s t s
In a study we conducted, infants were given massages by their fathers for 15 minutes
prior to their daily bedtimes over a 1-month period.26 By the end of the study,
the fathers who massaged their infants were more expressive and showed more
enjoyment and warmth during floor-play interactions with their infants than did
a control group.
that failure-to-thrive and depression are fairly common among the elderly, with a
frequency of 5% to 26%.28 Symptoms of depression in the elderly are similar to
those found in younger persons.29 In addition, the elderly can experience frequent
nighttime awakenings, increased levels of stress hormones and immune system
problems.30 Pet therapy (ie, having and holding pets) has been effective with
the elderly,31 and in our study massage therapy was also effective. In this study the
elderly volunteers were randomly assigned to either give infants massages or to
receive massage therapy themselves (Figure 6).27 At the end of the first month,
the volunteers then received the opposite treatment. Each of these sessions was
Figure 6. In the
grandparent-aged
volunteer study,
participants gave
massages to infants
and received massage
therapy themselves.27
15 minutes in length for the infant massages and 30 minutes for the grandparent-
volunteer massages. The latter sessions were longer simply because a full-body
table massage for an adult requires more time. Both the infant and the elderly
volunteer massages occurred twice per week for 4 weeks. Following the baseline
and end-of-study sessions, the grandparent volunteers reported lower anxiety levels,
fewer symptoms of depression and improved mood after both giving and receiving
massages. Their stress hormones also decreased after giving massages (Figure 7).
After only 1 month of giving or receiving massages, their scores on a lifestlye
questionnaire improved. They had more social contacts, made fewer trips to their
doctors’ offices and consumed less coffee. These changes probably helped improve
their sleep and their self-esteem. A somewhat surprising finding was that these
improvements were greater after 1 month of giving the infant massages than after
1 month of receiving massages. Thus, massage therapy was effective not only for
the infants receiving them but also for the adults who were giving these massages.
Su mm a r y
Ackn ow le d gm e nt s
The authors wish to thank the mothers, fathers, elderly volunteers and infants who
participated in these studies, as well as the research associates who assisted in the
data collecting and coding. This research was supported by a National Institute of
Mental Health (NIMH) Research Scientist Award (#MH00331) and an NIMH
Research Grant (#MH46586) given to Tiffany Field, as well as by funding
provided by the Johnson & Johnson Pediatric Institute, L.L.C. to the Touch
Research Institutes.
112 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
Ref ere n c e s
1. Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Hart S, Theakston H, Schanberg S, Kuhn C. Pregnant women
benefit from massage therapy. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynaecology. 1999;20:
31-38.
5. Hedstrom LW, Newton N. Touch in labor: a comparison of cultures and eras. Birth.
1986;13:181-186.
10. McClure VS. Infant Massage: A Handbook for Loving Parents. New York, NY: Bantam Books;
1989.
11. Grossman RL. Other Medicines. Garden City, NJ: Doubleday; 1985.
12. Eisenberg D, Wright TL. Encounters With Qi: Exploring Chinese Medicine. New York, NY:
WW Norton & Company; 1995.
13. Ottenbacher KJ, Muller L, Brandt D, Heintzelman A, Hojem P, Sharpe P. The effectiveness
of tactile stimulation as a form of early intervention: a quantitative evaluation. Journal of
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP. 1987;8:68-76.
14. Field TM, Schanberg SM, Scafidi F, et al. Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation effects on preterm
neonates. Pediatrics. 1986;77:654-658.
15. Dieter J, Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Emory E, Redzepi M. Stable preterm infants gain more
weight and sleep less after 5 days of massage therapy. Journal of Pediatric Psychology.
2002;28:403-411.
16. Goldstein-Ferber S. Massage in premature infants. Paper presented at: Child Development
Conference; 1998; Bar-Elon, Israel.
Chapter 6: Pregnancy, Labor and Infant Massage 113
17. Jinon S. The effect of infant massage on growth of the preterm infant. In: Yarbes-Almirante
C, De Luma M, eds. Increasing Safe and Successful Pregnancy. The Netherlands: Elsevier
Science, BZ; 1996:265-269.
18. Field T, Grizzle N, Scafidi F, Abrams S, Richardson S. Massage therapy for infants of
depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development. 1996;19:107-112.
19. Scafidi F, Field T, Schanberg S, et al. Massage stimulates growth in preterm infants: a
replication. Infant Behavior and Development. 1990;13:167-188.
20. Schanberg S, Field T. Maternal deprivation and supplemental stimulation. In: Field T,
McCabe PM, Schneiderman N, eds. Stress and Coping: Across Development. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988:3-25.
21. Schanberg S. The genetic basis for touch effects. In: Field TM, ed. Touch in Early
Development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1995:67-80.
28. Copeland JRM, Dewey ME, Wood N, Searle R, Davidson IA, McWilliam C. Range of
mental illness among the elderly in the community. Prevalence in Liverpool using the
GMS-AGECAT package. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the Journal of Mental Science.
1987;150:815-823.
29. Gaylord SA, Zung WWK. Affective disorders among the aging. In: Carstensen LL, Edelstein
BA, eds. Handbook of Clinical Gerontology. New York, NY: Allyn & Bacon; 1987:139-151.
30. Post F. Functional disorder II. Treatment and its relationship to causation. In: Levy R, ed.
Psychiatry of Late Life. London, England: Blackwell Scientific; 1982:213-229.
31. Grossberg JM, Alf EF Jr. Interaction with pet dogs: effects on human cardiovascular response.
The Journal of the Delta Society. 1985;32:518-524.
CHAPTER 7:
B R E A S T F E E D I N G D I F F I C U LT I E S
POSTBIRTH
University of Akron
Abstract
This chapter reports the results of a study evaluating the status of breastfeeding (BF)
for mother-infant dyads having BF difficulties postbirth and given skin-to-skin care
(SSC). The hypothesis was that SSC would facilitate successful BF. The focus was
on exclusive BF (only human milk) as well as on duration, because both measures
correlate positively with health benefits. The study involved 50 healthy, fullterm,
mother-newborn dyads experiencing BF difficulties between 11 hours and 24 hours
postbirth. Participants experienced “SSC with BF” (SB) for 3 consecutive BFs
(SB1–SB3) and one BF (SB4) 24 hours after SB1. Two dyads withdrew during the
hospital phase. At SB4, of the 48 remaining dyads, 39 (81.3%) were BF exclusively,
while 9 (18.7%) were BF partially. At 1-week follow-up, 35 dyads (72.9%) were BF
exclusively, 5 (10.4%) were BF partially, 6 (12.5%) were not BF and 2 (4.2%) were
lost to follow-up. At 1-month follow-up, 25 dyads (52.1%) were BF exclusively,
116 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
9 (18.8%) were BF partially, 13 (27.1%) were not BF and 1 (2.1%) was lost to
follow-up. These mother-infant dyads were a select group because they were already
experiencing BF difficulties. Thus, the fact that 81.3% of these dyads breastfed
exclusively during the early postpartum period is impressive, because this compares
favorably with 71.9% based on data from the Ross Mothers’ Survey,1 and the 75%
designated as Objective 16–19 for Healthy People 2010.2 Important insights were
also gained about ways to strengthen the intervention, including allowing infants
to have uninterrupted sleep and awaken spontaneously, placing infants in SSC at
first oral cues and protecting the BF process from interruptions.
Introd uc t i o n
In the United States (US), women have become increasingly motivated to breastfeed
their newborn infants. This motivation emanates from increasing evidence-based
information about the benefits of breastfeeding (BF) for their infants and even
themselves,3-7 as well as the recommendation that BF be exclusive (using human
milk only) for the first 6 months of the infant’s life, with the subsequent
introduction of complementary foods and continued BF thereafter through at
least the first year.8,9 Although the incidence of BF has increased gradually since
1984, exclusive BF, which yields the most benefits, decreased from 87.9% in 1971
to 71.9% in 1998.1
Stress occurring during the early postpartum period may present serious consequences
for newborn infants. In a randomized, controlled trial of 84 fullterm infants, salivary
cortisol reached inappropriate levels at 6 hours postbirth in the control infants who
were separated from their mothers and received standard procedural care in the
hospital nursery for hours 1 through 5.22 In contrast, the infants who remained
with their mothers, as compared with the control infants, exhibited significantly
lower levels of cortisol at 6 hours, indicating a healthy recovery from the high levels
of cortisol present at birth. Conceivably the inappropriate responses exhibited by
the control infants might result in persistent similar responses to stress across the
life span. This has been documented in animal models, wherein newborn rat pups
that had the least maternal contact (licking, grooming, and arched-back nursing
posture) suffered inappropriate stress responsivity. These inappropriate responses
to stress can lead to cognitive dysfunction,25,27 stress-related diseases23,24,26,27 and
susceptibility to addiction.28
Skin-to-skin
For dyads who have difficulties with BF, contact may
skin-to-skin contact (SSC)—otherwise be a useful,
known as “kangaroo care”—may be a useful, stress-reducing
stress-reducing intervention. With SSC, the intervention
mother holds her diaper-clad infant skin to for dyads
skin and underneath her clothing, using a who have
breastfeeding
blanket draped across her infant’s back for
difficulties.
warmth, if needed. SSC is recognized as an
118 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
been reported in which SSC was used as an intervention for mothers and their
fullterm infants who were identified as having difficulties with BF.
Harris reported that mothers and their fullterm infants who were having difficulties
with BF during the first few days postbirth often had a history of interruption of
peaceful mother-baby interactions during the immediate perinatal period (the first
2 hours postbirth).37 Harris then described how to treat these difficulties that the
mother-infant dyad were having with BF: She reported that if the mother and baby
are placed together, skin to skin, in a deep bathtub filled with water no warmer
than 38ºC (100.4ºF), even as late as 1 week postbirth, almost all infants will
move gradually to the breast, self-attach correctly with minimal assistance within
30 minutes and breastfeed successfully thereafter.
In the second study, a small pilot trial, 5 mothers and their fullterm infants who had
difficulties with BF were given SSC for 30 to 60 minutes.38 As a result, each infant
in 4 of these dyads not only located their mother’s breast during SSC and attached
independently, but they also attached correctly. The fifth dyad was not successful,
perhaps because at least 10 extended-family members were chatting loudly in the
room during this time. The first 3 dyads, who had not yet breastfed successfully by
18, 20 and 40 hours postbirth, were reported by Meyer and Anderson.38 However,
none of the 5 dyads was followed after their initial, successful BF.
birth and were allowed to remain there (ie, with SSC), they reached the breast, they
attached unaided and they began to nurse successfully within 30 to 60 minutes
postbirth.31,40 Importantly, one third of 70 fullterm infants born at home and given
unrestricted BFs and essentially continuous SSC from birth lost no weight.41 Even
when mothers and preterm infants, 34 to 36 weeks gestation, were separated during
the first 30 to 40 minutes postbirth and were then reunited for almost continuous
SSC thereafter, the infants achieved successful BF during the first few hours with
minimal assistance.33
• How often will each mother breastfeed between the end of SB3
and the beginning of SB4?
• At study entry, how much BF-related pain will mothers have already
experienced?
Metho d s
SAMPLE
INSTRUMENTS
Demographic and obstetrical data were collected from the medical records of the
mothers and their infants regarding the following: prenatal maternal behavior;
previous experience with BF; the birth experience; maternal and infant
demographics and medical condition; and any previous formula feedings and/or
experiences with SSC with the current infant.
Six instruments were used to measure difficulty and success of BF. The Mother-
Baby Assessment (MBA), a 10-point measuring tool of maternal and infant BF
behavior,45 was used to observe the progress of the mother-infant dyads as they
learned to breastfeed. The MBA has five sequential assessment steps: signaling,
positioning, fixing, milk transfer and ending. For this study, “difficulty” was
defined as a score of 7 or less. Three Visual Analogue Scales (VASs) were used in
the hospital for maternal self-reports of nipple pain, breast pain and BF success.
VASs are single-item indicators that ask respondents for their global ratings of
specific concepts based on factors salient to them.46 After the mothers went home
and because we were interviewing them by telephone, the VASs were converted to
Likert scoring systems. These interviews were done at 1 week and at 4 weeks. The
Index of Breastfeeding Scale (IBS) is a measure of duration but, more importantly
exclusivity, and was developed in 1988 by an interagency group of representatives
from organizations such as the World Health Organization (WHO) and the United
Chapter 7: Skin-to-Skin Care for Breastfeeding Difficulties Postbirth 121
VARIABLE N %†
Maternal race
African American 16 33.3
White 24 50.0
Asian/Pacific Islander 6 12.5
Other 2 4.2
Marital status
Married 32 66.7
Single 16 33.3
Employment status
Full time 29 60.4
Part time 7 14.6
Unemployed 8 16.7
Student 4 8.3
Delivery method
Vaginal 35 72.9
Cesarean section 10 20.8
Vacuum/forceps 3 6.3
Analgesia/anesthesia used
None 4 8.3
Pain medication 2 4.2
Epidural 30 62.5
Spinal 1 2.1
Pain medication and epidural 11 22.9
Smoking‡
Yes 2 4.2
No 46 95.8
Parity
Primipara 35 72.9
Multipara 13 27.1
Infant gender
Male 24 50.0
Female 24 50.0
Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF). These representatives were known for their
expertise in the area of BF measurement. This group referred to the IBS as a
“schema” for definitions of BF.47 We have since added the acronym and now
refer to the schema as “The IBS” in order to facilitate its clinical usefulness.
“Exclusivity” is defined as BF with no complementation and no supplementation.48
Complementation means BF plus formula.48 Supplementation means formula not
replacing a BF.48 The basic IBS is equivalent to the schema and has 6 categories:
exclusive; almost exclusive; high, medium, and low partial; and token.
For purposes of data collection in this study, the IBS was adapted to form 8
categories by subdividing the medium partial category into medium high and
medium low and adding a category called “none.” Therefore, the 8 newly formed
categories were called exclusive, almost exclusive, high partial, medium high partial,
medium low partial, low partial, token and none. BF exclusivity was coded on
a scale of 1 through 8, where “1 = exclusive” and “8 = none.” We also added
percentages to each category. During analyses for this report, these 8 categories
were collapsed into 4 categories: exclusive (including exclusive and almost exclusive)
100%, partial (including high, medium high, medium low, and low) (<100%
to >5%), token (5% to >0%), and none (0%). The almost exclusive category
essentially did not occur in this study.
The fourth instrument was the LATCH, a BF evaluation tool designed for
professional assessment based on either observations or questions to solicit responses
by the mothers.49 The LATCH evaluates the following 5 components of BF: how
well the infant latches correctly onto the breast; amount of audible swallowing
heard at the breast; type of nipple; comfort level of the mother regarding breasts,
nipples and position; and the amount of help required to position the infant.
Each of the 5 components is scored numerically as a 0, 1, or 2; total possible
points = 10. The higher the score, the greater the success and satisfaction with BF,
and the more likely the mother will continue with BF.50
PROCEDURE
Eligible dyads were enrolled in the study whenever research staffing appeared
adequate to cover all data-collection points. The original age for eligibility was
approximately 12 hours postbirth. However, because many of the enrolled infants
seemed very sleepy at 12 hours, eligibility was expanded after dyad 29 to include
infants who were between 12 and 24 hours, a time during which the effects of
obstetric medications may have diminished. Recruitment was accomplished in the
following manner. The researcher screened dyads for eligibility using the nurse
assignment sheet, on which BF difficulties were noted and in collaboration with the
nurses. Then, after selecting the mother whose infant’s age was closest to the target
postnatal age, the researcher approached the mother to explain the study, request
her participation and obtain informed consent. If this mother did not consent, the
same recruitment procedure was followed for the mother whose infant was next
closest to the target age.
Maternal saliva was collected first, before and after SB1 and SB4. Data were
collected for the VASs immediately before SB1 and after each SB. The MBA and
the LATCH were completed immediately after each SB. The IBS was used to
measure level of exclusivity before SB1 to learn what had occurred since birth,
immediately after each SB to measure each feeding and prior to SB2, SB3 and SB4
124 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
to measure relevant events that occurred during the interfeeding intervals (SB1 to
SB2 and SB2 to SB3) and during the longer period of time between SB3 and SB4.
If successful BF did not begin after 30 minutes of SSC, a nurse researcher provided
assistance. The end of each SB was defined in one of 3 ways: at the conclusion
of a successful BF; at the conclusion of a successful BF after assistance was given;
or at the conclusion of an unsuccessful BF despite assistance. In the latter case,
whenever possible, another SB opportunity was provided within 2 hours of the
end of the previous feeding. To avoid disturbing the infant and causing crying,
pre- and postfeeding weights were not done.
The MBFES was administered after SB4. In addition, the most recently recorded
infant weight was obtained. At 1 week and 1 month postnatal ages, mothers were
contacted by telephone to evaluate the following: BF exclusivity, using the IBS; BF
satisfaction, using the MBFES; and nipple pain, breast pain and BF success, using
the Likert scoring system. Data were analyzed using descriptive statistics. The
research protocol for each dyad lasted approximately 1 month. The data reported
here were collected from February 2002 to December 2002.
Result s
Using the last weight measurement before discharge, the average percent of infant
weight loss was 6% (SD = ± 3%; range = –2% to 21%). Three of these infants lost
more than 10% of body weight (11%, 14% and 21%). The infant who lost 21%
was feeding poorly because of “tongue thrust” and did not begin regaining weight
until after formula feeding by bottle was introduced. No obvious reasons for the
weight loss were identified in the other 2 infants. Forty-one mothers (85%) reported
a successful BF after SB1, 40 mothers (83%) reported the same after SB2 and 42
Chapter 7: Skin-to-Skin Care for Breastfeeding Difficulties Postbirth 125
Figure 1. Percentages of dyads who were exclusively BF, partially BF, formula fed and
lost to follow-up at discharge/B4 at 1 week and at 1 month follow-up (n = 48).
mothers (88%) reported a successful BF after SB3. Between the end of SB3 and
the start of SB4, 27 mothers (56%) breastfed their infants an average of 8.5 times
(SD = ± 4.42; range = 3 to 22).
Based on reports made by the mothers, breast pain was generally low, rose at week
1 and had begun to subside at month 1 (Figure 2). Nipple pain was only slightly
higher than breast pain, and had an essentially parallel curve. Mothers’ perceptions
of BF success increased rapidly with each SB experience and then decreased
slightly at week 1 and at month 1. Satisfaction with BF increased over time for the
28 mothers who completed the MBFES at SB4, week 1 and month 1 (120 ± 9.1,
121 ± 14.8 and 122 ± 14.6, respectively).
Saliva samples were collected before and after SB1 and SB4 from the last 20 dyads
enrolled. However, samples from only 5 mothers have been assayed to date. In
these 5 mothers, mean cortisol decreased from pre-SB to post-SB during each SB1
and each SB4, and was the lowest after SB4. Overall, pre-to-post change scores at
SB4 were greater than at SB1 (Table 2).
126 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
Figure 2. Mothers’ reports on visual analogue scales for BF success, nipple pain and
breast pain before SB1, after each SB (1–4) and at 1-week and 1-month follow-up
(n=24). Scores represent complete data sets at all 7 times.
Table 2. Maternal salivary cortisol levels(µg/dL) before and after SB1 and SB4 (n=48).
Mothers
1 2 3 4 5 Cortisol
SB H C H C H C H C H C Mean (SD±)
Pre-1 0920 .26 1000 .61 1100 .53 0900 .48 1010 .43 .46(.13)
Post-1 1100 .19 1130 .60 1213 .34 0935 .32 1420 .09 .31(.19)
Pre-4 0920 .20 0955 .56 1015 .82 1120 .75 1000 .44 .55(.25)
Post-4 1000 .15 1245 .26 1050 .63 1330 .22 1130 .19 .29(.19)
Discussion
These mother-infant dyads were a select group who were chosen for study because
they had been identified approximately 12 hours postbirth as being at greater risk
for BF failure than most. Nevertheless, 81% (n = 39) of these dyads breastfed
exclusively during the early postpartum period. This percentage compares favorably
with the 75% for any BF (Objective 16–19 for Healthy People 2010)2 and the
71.9%, also for any BF, for 1998 as calculated by Cadwell,1 using data from the
Ross Mothers’ Survey.53
Encouraging results were also observed on other measures: For example, 94%
(n = 45) of the infants lost ≤10% of their birth weight before hospital discharge.
SSC was successful for at least one of the first 3 SBs for 42 (88%) of the mothers.
On average, mothers breastfed 8.5 times between SB3 and SB4 (a period of
approximately 14 hours). This frequency of BF falls within the range recommended
in the guidelines of the Academy of Breastfeeding Medicine,54,55 which suggest
8 to 12 BFs during the first 24 hours to 48 hours postbirth. In addition, maternal
perception of success with BF doubled from baseline after the first skin-to-skin BF
(SB1) and remained high thereafter. Upon study enrollment, several mothers had
reddened nipples, small blisters and nipple cracks. By SB3, however, their ratings
of nipple pain had decreased, suggesting that the SB feedings were less injurious.
The perception of breast pain was generally rated low throughout the study
protocol. Although saliva from only 5 mothers has been assayed to date, 4 of these
mothers exhibited decreases in salivary cortisol levels from pre-SB to post-SB, with
the lowest levels at SB4, suggesting lower levels of stress related to SB.
Conclusions are tentative because of the small sample size (N = 48), the one-group
design and the brief follow-up period. However, in the process of conducting
this study, we have gained numerous insights. These insights may be helpful in
understanding some of the problems underlying poor BF outcomes in the US, as
well as some of the reasons why many mothers do not choose to initiate BF, do not
continue BF once begun, do not advise others to BF and do not plan to BF their
subsequent children.
Firstly, although our study was conducted in a women’s hospital that has an
excellent reputation for providing outstanding care, the BF attempts made by these
mothers and their infants were interrupted with astonishing frequency—as often as
128 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
Other interruptions resulted from choices that were made, sometimes reluctantly,
by the mothers: For example, some mothers allowed a BF to be interrupted so that
their infants could be photographed, or sometimes they did not begin a pending
BF if visitors arrived or were soon expected. They reported that they wanted to be
hospitable, yet they also desired privacy at times and were reluctant to say this to
their visitors. Mothers who have recently given birth have physical needs as well,
such as pain that makes moving about difficult and feelings of fatigue that make
them fearful that they will fall asleep if they continue to hold their infants.56
Thirdly, being gentle, considerate and respectful can make all the difference, not
only for the infants, but also for the mothers. Some mothers seem offended or
embarrassed by the way some health care personnel assist them with BF. This may
be one reason why many mothers who have chosen to breastfeed decide, while still
in the hospital, that “breastfeeding is not for me.” One common technique for
“getting the baby on” is to take hold of the mother’s breast with one hand and
the back of the baby’s head with the other hand, and, as the baby begins to open
his/her mouth, to push the baby’s mouth as far as possible onto the mother’s
breast.61 This technique is employed with the good intention of having the baby
latch onto the areola rather than the nipple. However, both the mother and her
infant become tense during this process, and the latch is usually neither ideal nor
fully effective. In the study by Mozingo and colleagues, mothers reported that this
kind of “insensitive care” by health care personnel was the chief reason they decided
to terminate BF within the first 2 weeks postbirth.62 One mother in the study
reported here referred to this as “aggressive treatment,” and soon thereafter decided
to feed with formula. Other mothers simply looked away and did not participate
actively in trying to learn how to position their infants most effectively.
130 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
Lastly, as time passes without successful BF, concern for hypoglycemia in the infant
intensifies, as do well-intended efforts by staff to rush the BF process or to resort to
supplementary or complementary feedings with formula. However, according to
the guidelines set by the Academy of Breastfeeding Medicine,54,55 and those set
forth by Eidelman,63 concern regarding hypoglycemia is unwarranted for healthy,
fullterm, appropriate-for-gestation infants who have no signs of illness. Eidelman
emphasized that his guidelines apply to infants who have been screened carefully
for risk and who are provided with what is generally considered ideal care, including
the initiation of SSC and BF within the first 30 to 60 minutes postbirth,(cf. 64) BF
that begins on cue before any crying, and BF that occurs every 1.5 to 2 hours in
the first 24 to 48 hours. This kind of care maintains normal temperature63 and
stimulates suckling and milk production.38 Similar care is also associated with
reduced breastfeeding jaundice,65 a later cause of mother-infant separation and even
rehospitalization. An important caveat is that this kind of care is difficult to achieve
consistently for all mother-infant dyads in current, typical hospital systems. Thus,
the concern regarding infant hypoglycemia remains a valid concern.
The first day, and especially the first few hours, postbirth is a crucial time for the
mother and her infant. Their future mutual comfort depends upon an investment
of time and effort by the mother during this brief window of developmental
opportunity when she and her infant are making major physiological adaptations—
the infant to extrauterine life and the mother to her postpregnant state. To
facilitate these processes, everything possible must be done to meet their needs in
a timely, self-regulatory, gentle and respectful way, including helping visitors to
understand. In so doing, hospital personnel can provide an additional benefit—
modeling these kinds of humane behaviors for mothers and their families.
Closing Thoughts
Ackn ow le d gm e nt s
The authors would like to thank all research staff (Susan Greene, Dianne Rafferty,
and Regina Tuma), hospital postpartum staff and all of the mothers, babies and
families who participated in the current study, as well as Medela, Inc., McHenry, IL
(breast pump accessories, and the loan of an electric Lactina breast pump) and
Exergen, Inc., Watertown, MA (Temporal ScannerTM, i.e., temporal artery
thermometry). This study was funded by the National Institutes of Health, the
National Institute of Nursing Research (#2R01 NR02444) and the General
Clinical Research Center (#M01 RR00080-36).
The Dr. Korones quotation has been used with permission of Ross Products
Division, Abbott Laboratories Inc., Columbus, OH 43215. From Korones SB.
Disturbance and infants’ rest. In: Iatrogenic Problems in Neonatal Intensive Care:
Report of the Sixty-Ninth Ross Conference on Pediatric Research (1976). Ross
Products Division, Abbott Laboratories Inc.60
132 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
Ref ere n c e s
1. Cadwell K. An international policy perspective on breastfeeding in the United States. In:
Cadwell K, ed. Reclaiming Breastfeeding for the United States: Protection, Promotion, and
Support. Sudbury, Mass: Jones & Bartlett; 2002:1-10.
2. US Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy People 2010. With Understanding
and Improving Health and Objectives for Improving Health. Vol 2. 2nd ed. Washington, DC:
US Government Printing Office; 2000.
3. Labbok MH. Effects of breastfeeding on the mother. In: Schanler RJ, ed. Breastfeeding 2001,
Part I: The evidence for breastfeeding. Pediatric Clinics of North America. 2001;48:143-158.
4. Lawrence RA, Lawrence RM. Breastfeeding: A Guide for the Medical Profession. 5th ed. New
York, NY: Mosby-Year Book; 1999.
5. Schanler R, ed. Breastfeeding 2001, Part I: The evidence for breastfeeding. The Pediatric
Clinics of North America. 2001;48:1-264.
6. United States Breastfeeding Committee. Benefits of Breastfeeding [issue paper]. Raleigh, NC:
United States Breastfeeding Committee; 2002.
8. American Academy of Pediatrics. Work Group on Breastfeeding. Breastfeeding and the use
of human milk. Pediatrics. 1997;100:1035-1039.
9. World Health Organization. The optimal duration of exclusive breastfeeding: results of a
WHO systematic review. Available at: http://www.who.int/inf-pr-2001/en/note2001-07.html.
Accessed September 12, 2003.
10. US Department of Health and Human Services. Report of the Surgeon General’s Workshop on
Breastfeeding and Human Lactation. Rockville, Md: US Dept of Health and Human Services;
1984. USDHHS publication HRS-D-MC-84-2.
11. UNICEF/WHO. The Innocenti Declaration. Meeting on Breastfeeding in the 1990s: A
global initiative hosted by UNICEF/WHO, co-sponsored by the United States Agency for
International Development (AID) and the Swedish International Development Authority
(SIDA), Spedale degli Innocenti; 1990; Florence, Italy.
12. US Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy People 2000: Conference Edition.
Vol 2. Washington, DC: US Dept of Health and Human Services, Public Health and Service,
Office of the Assistant Secretary for Health; 1990. USDHHS publication PHS 91-50213.
13. United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF)/World Health Organization (WHO). The
UNICEF/Baby Friendly Hospital Initiative: Ten Steps to Successful Breastfeeding. New York, NY:
United Nations Children’s Fund; 1992.
Chapter 7: Skin-to-Skin Care for Breastfeeding Difficulties Postbirth 133
14. Naylor AJ. Baby-Friendly Hospital Initiative. Protecting, promoting, and supporting
breastfeeding in the twenty-first century. In: Schanler RJ, ed. Breastfeeding 2001, Part II:
The management of breastfeeding. Pediatric Clinics of North America. 2001;48:475-483.
15. Turner-Maffei C. Using the Baby-Friendly Hospital Initiative to drive positive change. In:
Cadwell K, ed. Reclaiming Breastfeeding for the United States: Protection, Promotion, and
Support. Sudbury, Mass: Jones & Bartlett; 2002:23-33.
16. US Department of Health and Human Services. United States Breastfeeding Committee.
Breastfeeding in the United States: A National Agenda. Rockville, Md: US Department of
Health and Human Services, Health Resources and Services Administration, Maternal and
Child Health Bureau; 2001. USDHHS publication. Available at: http://www.usbreastfeed-
ing.org/USBC-Strategic-Plan-2001.pdf. Accessed September 12, 2003.
17. Riordan J, Auerbach KG. Breastfeeding and Human Lactation. 2nd ed. Boston, Mass: Jones
& Bartlett; 1999.
28. Piazza PV, Maccari S, Deminiere JM, Le Moal M, Mormede P, Simon H. Corticosterone
levels determine individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1991;88:2088-2092.
134 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
29. Christensson K, Siles C, Moreno L, et al. Temperature, metabolic adaptation and crying in
healthy fullterm newborns cared for skin-to-skin or in a cot. Acta Paediatrica (Oslo, Norway:
1992). 1992;81:488-493.
30. Field T. Touch. Cambridge, Mass: Massachusetts Institute of Technology; 2001.
34. Hurst NM, Valentine CJ, Renfro L, Burns P, Ferlic L. Skin-to-skin holding in the neonatal
intensive care unit influences maternal milk volume. Journal of Perinatology: Official Journal
of the California Perinatal Association. 1997;17:213-217.
36. Meier PP. Breastfeeding in the special care nursery. Prematures and infants with medical
problems. In: Schanler RJ, ed. Breastfeeding 2001, Part II: The management of breastfeeding.
Pediatric Clinics of North America. 2001;48:425-442.
37. Harris H. Remedial co-bathing for breastfeeding difficulties. Breastfeeding Review.
1994;2:465-468.
38. Meyer K, Anderson GC. Using kangaroo care in a clinical setting with fullterm infants
having breastfeeding difficulties. MCN. The American Journal of Maternal Child Nursing.
1999;24:190-192.
39. Anderson GC, Moore E, Hepworth J, Bergman N. Early skin-to-skin contact for mothers
and their healthy newborn infants (Cochrane Review). In: The Cochrane Library, Issue 2,
2003. Oxford: Update Software.
40. Righard L, Alade MO. Effect of delivery room routines on success of first breastfeed. Lancet.
1990;336:1105-1107.
41. Odent M. Newborn weight loss. Mothering. 1989;13:72-73.
42. Dodgson JE, Allard-Hale CJ, Bramscher A, Brown F, Duckett L. Adherence to the ten steps
of the Baby-Friendly Hospital Initiative in Minnesota hospitals. Birth (Berkeley, Calif ).
1999;26:239-247.
43. Kovach AC. A 5-year follow-up study of hospital breastfeeding policies in the Philadelphia
area: a comparison with the ten steps. Journal of Human Lactation: Official Journal of
International Lactation Consultant Association. 2002;18:144-154.
Chapter 7: Skin-to-Skin Care for Breastfeeding Difficulties Postbirth 135
44. Punthmatharith B. Randomized Controlled Trial of Early Kangaroo (Skin-to-Skin) Care: Effects
on Maternal Feelings, Maternal-Infant Interaction, and Breastfeeding Success in Thailand
[dissertation]. Cleveland, Ohio: Case Western Reserve University; 2000.
45. Mulford C. The Mother-Baby Assessment (MBA): an “Apgar score” for breastfeeding. Journal
of Human Lactation: Official Journal of International Lactation Consultant Association.
1992;8:79-82.
46. Youngblut JM, Casper GR. Single-item indicators in nursing research. Research in Nursing
& Health. 1993;16:459-465.
47. Labbok M, Krasovec K. Toward consistency in breastfeeding definitions. Studies in Family
Planning. 1990;21:226-230.
48. Cadwell CM. Defining breastfeeding in research. In: Cadwell K, ed. Reclaiming Breastfeeding
for the United States: Protection, Promotion, and Support. Sudbury, Mass: Jones & Bartlett;
2002:81-89.
49. Jensen D, Wallace S, Kelsay P. LATCH: a breastfeeding charting system and documentation
tool. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing: JOGNN/NAACOG. 1994;23:
27-32.
50. Schlomer JA, Kemmerer J, Twiss JJ. Evaluating the association of two breastfeeding
assessment tools with breastfeeding problems and breastfeeding satisfaction. Journal of
Human Lactation: Official Journal of International Lactation Consultant Association.
1999;15:35-39.
51. Leff EW, Jefferis SC, Gagne MP. The development of the Maternal Breastfeeding Evaluation
Scale. Journal of Human Lactation: Official Journal of International Lactation Consultant
Association. 1994;10:105-111.
52. Gray L, Watt L, Blass EM. Skin-to-skin contact is analgesic in healthy newborns. Pediatrics
(Online) [serial online]. 2000;105:1-6. Available at:
http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/105/1/e14. Accessed September 12, 2003.
53. Ryan AS. The resurgence of breastfeeding in the United States. Pediatrics. 1997;99:E12.
54. The Academy of Breastfeeding Medicine. ABM Protocols. Clinical protocol number 1 —
guidelines for glucose monitoring and treatment of hypoglycemia in term breastfed neonates.
November, 1999. Available at: http://www.bfmed.org. Accessed September 14, 2003.
55. The Academy of Breastfeeding Medicine. ABM Protocols. ABM clinical protocol number 3
— hospital guidelines for the use of supplementary feedings in the healthy term breastfed
neonate. 2001. Available at: http://www.bfmed.org. Accessed September 14, 2003.
56. Anderson GC, Chiu S-H, Dombrowski MAS, Swinth JT, Albert J, Wada N. Mother-
newborn contact in a randomized trial of kangaroo (skin-to-skin) care. Journal of Obstetric,
Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing: JOGNN/NAACOG. 2003;32:604-611.
57. Dennis CL. Breastfeeding initiation and duration: a 1990-2000 literature review. Journal
of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing: JOGNN/NAACOG. 2002;31:12-32.
136 Section II. Therapeutic Applications of Touch in Pregnancy, Labor and Postbirth
58. Anderson GC. The mother and her newborn: mutual caregivers. Journal of Obstetric,
Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing: JOGNN/NAACOG. 1977;6:50-57.
59. Duxbury ML, Henly SJ, Broz LJ, Armstrong GD, Wachdorf CM. Caregiver disruptions and
sleep of high-risk infants. Heart & Lung; the Journal of Critical Care. 1984;13:141-147.
60. Korones SB. Disturbance and infants’ rest. Paper presented in: Iatrogenic Problems in
Neonatal Intensive Care: Report of the Sixty-Ninth Ross [Laboratories] Conference on
Pediatric Research. February, 1976; Columbus, Ohio.
61. Auerbach KG. Evidence-based care and the breastfeeding couple: key concerns. Journal
of Midwifery & Women’s Health. 2000;45:205-211.
62. Mozingo JN, Davis MW, Droppleman PG, Merideth A. “It wasn’t working.” Women’s
experiences with short-term breastfeeding. MCN. The American Journal of Maternal Child
Nursing. 2000;25:120-126.
63. Eidelman AI. Hypoglycemia and the breastfed neonate. In: Schanler J, ed. Breastfeeding
2001, Part II: The management of breastfeeding. Pediatric Clinics of North America.
2001;48:377-387.
64. Durand R, Hodges S, LaRick S. The effect of skin-to-skin breastfeeding in the immediate
recovery period on newborn thermoregulation and blood glucose values. Neonatal Intensive
Care: The Journal of Perinatology-Neonatology. 1997;10:23-27.
65. Gartner LM, Herschel M. Jaundice and breastfeeding. In: Schanler RJ, ed. Breastfeeding
2001, Part II: The management of breastfeeding. Pediatric Clinics of North America.
2001;48:389-399.
Touc h a n d Ma s s a g e i n Ea rl y Ch ild De ve lop me nt
SECTION III.
TO U C H I N P R E M AT U R E I N FA N T S
CHAPTER 8:
TA C T I L E S T I M U L AT I O N
O F N E O N ATA L
PRETERM INFANTS
Ab s t ra c t
Introd uc t i o n
Annually, approximately 7.6% of live births in the United States are infants who
are born prematurely and weighing less than 2500 g.1 In the same 12-month period,
1.4% of live births are infants who weigh less than 1500 g, and therefore are
considered very-low-birth-weight (VLBW) infants.1 Goldson reported that 20%
to 60% of LBW infants have some developmental disability, and 10% to 20%
have significant adverse sequelae.2 Recently, there is increasing recognition of
the importance of providing appropriate types and amounts of environmental
stimulation to these fragile infants during the early weeks of life to reduce stress
and prevent complications, such as intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH), which
can increase the risk for subsequent neurological and developmental problems.
Although there has been debate over the years about whether preterm infants are
understimulated or overstimulated in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU),
there is now a consensus that both problems exist.3
In recent years, there has been increasing recognition of the value of providing
individualized, developmental care (DC) in the NICU as a strategy to reduce
NICU-induced stress and promote optimal health and developmental outcomes in
affected infants.4-9 Although providing appropriate handling is generally considered
an important component of DC, few studies have evaluated specific types of
handling and tactile stimulation provided as a component of DC. Several
researchers have suggested the need to identify the types of touch that might
be provided to fragile preterm infants to afford them comfort and minimize the
stresses associated with the NICU environment.10 Tactile interaction with their
infants is also important for parents of preterm infants, since the desire to maintain
contact between parent and infant is a primary component of positive parent-infant
attachment.11 Parents of preterm infants often have limited interactions with their
babies during the early weeks of life, and thus are at risk for problems in the
development of secure parent-infant attachment relationships.
Most of the touch received by preterm infants who are hospitalized in NICUs
consists of touch associated with medical or nursing procedures. These infants
receive very little comforting touch.12-18 Adverse effects of the touch associated with
medical and nursing procedures include hypoxia, bradycardia, sleep disruptions and
increased intracranial pressure (ICP).14,17,19-24 Repeated episodes of hypoxia and
increased ICP may place preterm infants at increased risk for complications, such
Chapter 8: Tactile Stimulation of Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Preterm Infants 141
Harrison and Woods examined the types of touch provided by parents and
grandparents during 3 visits to their infants in the NICU and found that the types
and amounts of touch varied considerably.29 The sample included 36 preterm
infants who were between 25 weeks and 33 weeks GA at birth. The mean total
duration of touch during the parental visits was 17.5 minutes, with a standard
deviation of 22.6 minutes. Infants less than 28 weeks GA received less touch than
did infants who were more than 28 weeks GA. Mothers touched more than did
grandmothers, who touched more than fathers. The most frequent types of touch
provided by the parents included holding, stroking, simple contact or rubbing. In
another report from the same study, Harrison, Leeper and Yoon noted that there
were no differences in infants’ mean heart rate (HR) or oxygen saturation (OS)
levels during parent touch, compared with baseline or posttouch periods, but there
was more variability in these measures during the touch periods.30 In addition,
there were significantly more episodes of abnormally low OS levels during periods
of parent touch than during baseline period.30
Findings from studies that have evaluated preterm infants’ responses to handling
associated with medical and nursing procedures have consistently demonstrated
adverse effects from such procedures, including hypoxia, bradycardia, sleep
disruptions, increased ICP and behavioral agitation.12-24,31 Long, Philip and Lucey
tracked continuous recordings of the heart rates, transcutaneous O2 (tcPO2) levels
and respiratory rates of 30 preterm infants for 20 hours per day during the first
5 days after birth.21 Half the infants were assigned to an experimental group and
half were assigned to a control group. Nurses and caregivers of control-group
infants were not allowed to observe the tcPO2 recordings, but nurses and caregivers
Chapter 8: Tactile Stimulation of Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Preterm Infants 143
St ud i e s Eva l u a t i ng Su p p l e m e nt al Tactile
St i mula t i o n Int e r ve nt i o ns i n t he NIC U
To reduce the stressors associated with the NICU environment and excessive
handling associated with medical and nursing procedures, several researchers over
the past 30 years have examined the effects of interventions designed to provide
preterm infants with supplemental comforting touch. These interventions have
included still, gentle touch, stroking/massage or stroking/massage combined with
vestibular stimulation.
144 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
GENTLE TOUCH
Because of concerns that in the early weeks of life many preterm infants are too
fragile to tolerate stroking or massage, several researchers have evaluated the effects
of still, gentle human touch (GHT) or touch without stroking or massage. Jay
provided gentle touch to 13 preterm infants ventilated mechanically for 12 minutes,
4 times a day, beginning when the infants were less than 96 hours old.34 Compared
with a matched control group, the experimental infants had higher hematocrits
and required less O2. Tribotti studied the responses of 4 stable preterm infants
(33 weeks to 35 weeks gestation) to gentle touch provided for 15 minutes, 3 times
daily, for 3 days.35 During the first session, infants demonstrated decreased tcPO2,
increased respiratory regularity and a slight decrease in motor activity. By the
third session, they exhibited increases in tcPO2 and respiratory regularity, and a
continued decrease in motor activity.
Findings from the studies of GHT suggest that this type of touch has no adverse
effects on mean HR or OS levels, but that such touch resulted in increased
respiratory regularity, decreased levels of active sleep, motor activity and behavioral
distress, and increased levels of quiet sleep during periods of gentle touch when
compared with B and PT periods.35-41 However, no differences were noted on
longer-term outcome measures, such as weight gain, morbidity scores or length of
hospital stay, in infants who received the GHT intervention when compared with
infants in the randomly assigned control groups.36,40,41
These findings from the studies of gentle touch suggest that nurses should
encourage parents and other caregivers to use GHT when handling physiologically
fragile preterm infants in the NICU. Gentle touch might be a useful intervention
to soothe infants who are agitated and stressed.
Daga, Ahuja and Lunkad studied the effects of maternal stroking of the backs of
7 preterm infants—during alternate gavage feedings—who were less than 32 weeks
Chapter 8: Tactile Stimulation of Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Preterm Infants 147
gestation and weighed less than 1600 g.48 O2 saturation levels were significantly
higher in infants who received the stroking at 20 minutes and 30 minutes after the
feeding, compared with a control group.
Solkoff, Yaffe, Weintraub and Blasé provided stroking to the neck, back and arms
for 5 minutes each hour during the first 10 days of life to 10 preterm infants,
each of whom weighed between 1190 g and 1590 g at birth.49 Compared with
a control group, infants who received the stroking were more active during the
hospitalization period, regained their birth weights faster and had fewer
developmental anomalies at 7 months to 8 months postdischarge. Solkoff and
Matuszak provided 7.5 minutes of stroking during each of 16 hours per day for
10 days to 6 preterm infants.50 The infants, who were an average of 14.2 days of
age when the interventions began, had a mean GA of 31.2 weeks. Infants in the
experimental group demonstrated more rapid habituation to light and sound,
improved body tone, increased alertness, more consolability, more state changes
and more rapid avoidance of noxious stimuli than did control-group infants.
Harrison conducted a pilot study to evaluate a 2-week GHT plus 2-week massage
intervention on 12 infants who were 27 weeks to 30 weeks GA at birth.55 Infants
were assigned randomly and equally to an intervention group and a control group.
Infants in the control group received standard care in the NICU. In addition to
receiving standard care in the NICU, infants in the intervention group received
10 days of GHT for 10 minutes twice each day, beginning when they were 6 days
to 9 days old. These infants then received 10 days of massage intervention for 10
minutes twice each day, beginning at age 20 days to 23 days. Screens were placed
around the infants’ beds during the interventions so that the research assistant (RA)
who collected observational data on infants’ behaviors and behavioral states would
not know whether the infant was in the touch or control group. Five times during
each 10-minute intervention phase, the nurse placed her hands on the infant’s
bed—whether the infant was in the touch group or control group—and the RA
then looked around the screen to observe and record infant behavioral state,
motor activity and behavioral distress. The GAs and birth weights of infants in the
Chapter 8: Tactile Stimulation of Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Preterm Infants 149
experimental and control groups were similar, although the mean weight of each
infant in the experimental group was 129 g more than each control-group infant.
Infants in the experimental group exhibited lower total and average daily morbidity
scores than those in the control group (total score, 25.8 versus 59.3, respectively),
fewer days in the hospital (44.0 versus 59.8 days) and on supplemental O2 (13.4
versus 15.0), lower neurobiological risk scores (0.6 versus 1.2), higher average daily
weight gain (18.2 g versus 16.9 g) and more optimal scores on the habituation,
orientation and autonomic stability Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment
Scale (BNBAS) subscales.55 Infants in the control group demonstrated slightly
higher scores on the motor, state regulation and range-of-state BNBAS subscales.
Experimental-group infants experienced slightly more quiet sleep and slightly
reduced levels of motor activity during the massage interventions, while control-
group infants experienced slightly less quiet sleep during the intervention (for
them, no touch) phase. Few changes were noted in O2 saturation comparing
baseline, touch and posttouch phases for experimental-group infants, but there
were slight increases in mean HR (approximately 6 BPM) during the massage
interventions. Because of the small sample size, it was not possible to assess the
statistical significance of these group differences. However, these findings suggest
that adding the 2-week massage component to the 2-week gentle touch intervention
resulted in positive middle-term outcomes that were not noted in previous studies
that included only a gentle touch intervention.
included infants who were physiologically stable at the time of the intervention.
Researchers have generally provided the interventions,56,57 although in at least one
study the intervention was provided by parents.58,59
The intervention protocols used have included stroking and massage combined
with passive range of motion of the limbs or rocking.56,58,61-71 Findings from these
studies suggest that providing supplemental tactile and kinesthetic or vestibular
stimulation offers middle- and long-term benefits, including the following:
increased daily weight gain63,64; increased secretion of urinary epinephrine and
norepinephrine62; increased levels of alertness, and more mature orientation,
motor skills and range of state61,63,71; and more mature habituation behaviors.61,73
Field and colleagues have conducted several studies evaluating the effects of a
15-minute massage protocol on stable preterm infants.61-64 This intervention
protocol consisted of 5 minutes of stroking, 5 minutes of kinesthetic stimulation
(passive flexion/extension movements) and then 5 minutes of stroking. The infants
were placed in a prone position during stroking and in a supine position during
kinesthetic stimulation. The initial study included 40 preterm infants, who had a
mean GA of 31 weeks and a mean birth weight of 1280 g and who were equally
assigned randomly to an experimental or control group.61 Experimental-group
infants received the intervention 3 times daily for 10 days, beginning when they
were considered medically stable and admitted to the “grower” nursery. These
infants averaged a 47% greater daily weight gain (mean, 25 g versus 17 g), were
more active and alert and had more mature habituation, orientation, motor and
range of state behavior on the BNBAS. In addition, they were discharged an
average of 6 days earlier than those in the control group, resulting in an average
hospital cost savings of nearly $3000 per infant.
In a second report from the same study, Scafidi, Field, Schanberg et al described
the responses of 14 of the experimental-group infants to periods of stimulation
and to no stimulation and to differences in responses to the tactile and kinesthetic
components of the intervention.63 During periods of stimulation, infants in the
experimental group spent more time in active sleep, less time in drowsy wakefulness
and more time exhibiting active limb movements. The infants were more active and
more likely to be in active sleep during tactile stimulation, compared with the
kinesthetic-stimulation phase of the intervention.
Chapter 8: Tactile Stimulation of Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Preterm Infants 151
Similar findings were published 4 years later from a replication study using similar
methods, but with a different sample of 40 stable preterm infants.64 In this study,
infants in the experimental group averaged a 21% greater daily weight gain (mean,
34 g versus 28 g), were discharged on average 5 days earlier and had more mature
performance on the habituation cluster of the BNBAS scale. In an associated study,
levels of urinary norepinephrine and epinephrine increased from Day 1 to Day 10
for the experimental group, but not for the control group.62 The authors concluded
that the intervention promoted greater maturity of the sympathetic nervous system,
as evidenced by the increased catecholamine levels for the experimental-group
infants.
4 sessions, mean TcPO2 levels decreased significantly from baseline to the first
tactile period and from baseline to the kinesthetic period, but increased from the
stimulation periods to the posttouch period. There were no significant effects
caused by the tactile or kinesthetic interventions on TcPO2 during the fourth
session. Most of the instances of decreased TcPO2 levels occurred during the
kinesthetic portion of the intervention, suggesting that movement and pressure
applied to the legs during the range of motion intervention may have resulted in
artifactual decreases in TcPO2.
Rose and colleagues assigned 60 preterm infants (28 weeks to 36 weeks GA)
randomly to either a control or an experimental group.74 Infants in the experimental
group received 3, 20-minute tactile interventions 5 days per week, beginning
within the first 2 weeks after birth and continuing until 1 day to 2 days prior to
hospital discharge (mean, 13 days). The tactile interventions consisted of massaging
the infant while the infant was in prone, supine and sitting positions. When the
infant was stable, one of the 3 sessions consisted of rocking the infant in a rocking
chair. Just prior to hospital discharge, experimental-group infants demonstrated
significantly greater cardiac responses to stimulation with a plastic filament.
The researchers also reported that these infants exhibited improved auditory
responsiveness during the neonatal period and improved visual recognition memory
at 6 months of age.
who were asked simply to talk or sing to their infants at comparable times, and
compared with mothers in a control group, mothers in the ATVV group enjoyed
more positive interactions with their infants, as measured by the Nursing Child
Assessment and Feeding Scale (NCAFS) at the time of discharge from the NICU.
• Hospital stays for infants receiving massage was 6 days less than for infants in control
groups. This finding has significant implications for healthcare costs, given the average
daily charges for an infant in a NICU. Additional research is needed to determine whether
such reduced lengths of stay can be replicated with larger and more-diverse samples.
• Infants receiving massage had more-optimal scores on the BNBAS for habituation,
motor maturity and range of state.
• Massage reduced postnatal complications, although 95% of the weighting for this
analysis came from a study that had a sample of only 30 infants.
• Massage, kinesthetic stimulation and gentle touch had no adverse effects on the
infants studied.
154 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
Vickers and colleagues, however, noted that there were methodological problems
in some of the reviewed studies, including failures to blind observers who assessed
outcomes and to ensure that infants in experimental and control groups were treated
similarly in all aspects other than the experimental intervention.10 Because of these
methodological problems, these investigators concluded that there was insufficient
evidence to determine whether massage should be provided to infants in the NICU.
Furthermore, they advocated additional research to assess the effects of massage
interventions on clinical outcomes, controlling for methodological problems by
concealing treatment allocation until the subject was entered into the trial, ensuring
that infants were treated similarly in all respects other than the experimental
intervention, blinding observers assessing outcomes to the treatment assignment,
assessing whether there are differences in the withdrawals from the study based on
treatment-group assignment and assessing longer-term developmental outcomes.
Studies of touch in preterm infants following discharge from the NICU have
focused on evaluating a massage intervention administered by mothers provided
in the home and examining relationships between characteristics of mothers’ touch
and the subsequent development of the parent-infant attachment relationship and
of the infant who is born prematurely. Rice studied the effects of an intervention
in which mothers were taught to provide their preterm infants a tactile/kinesthetic
intervention at home after discharge from the NICU.56,57 The sample included
30 infants born prematurely who were assigned randomly to an experimental or a
control group. Mothers of infants in the experimental group were asked to provide
their newborns the intervention for 15 minutes, 4 times each day, beginning on
the day when the infants came home from the hospital. Mothers held their infants
during the intervention, which consisted of a sequence of strokes using the
fingertips and palms and designed to provide more touching of the head than
other body parts. Following the stroking, mothers swaddled their infants and
rocked them for 5 minutes. Public health nurses visited the mothers in the
experimental group each day for the 30-day treatment period. Nurses visited
mothers in the control group only once per week, providing them with infant-care
information. Infants who received the massage by their mothers gained more
Chapter 8: Tactile Stimulation of Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Preterm Infants 155
Weiss and colleagues75 videotaped 131 mothers and their low-birth-weight infants
3 months after discharge from a NICU (as a component of a larger longitudinal
study). The videotapes were coded using the Tactile Interaction Indicator to
describe the types and amounts of touch provided during feedings. Nearly half
(47%) of the mothers’ touch was coded as “nurturing,” but 17% of the mothers
provided no nurturing touch. There was a positive relationship between the use of
nurturing touch and subsequent development of secure attachment (at 12 months
of age) for the less-vulnerable infants. However, more-vulnerable infants, who
received more-nurturing touch, were more likely to develop insecure attachments.
The investigators concluded that the more-vulnerable infants might be more
susceptible to overstimulation.
In a subsequent report from the same study, Weiss and coinvestigators examined
relationships between the qualities of mother touch provided during the feeding
at 3 months after NICU discharge and the child’s development at 2 years of age.76
Infants whose mothers used more-nurturing touch had fewer behavior problems,
such as depression and anxiety, at 2 years of age. The authors concluded that the
types of mother touch provided during the early months may influence subsequent
psychosocial development of low-birth-weight (LBW) infants, and that nurturing
touch may provide a protective effect for the infants by helping them develop self-
regulatory skills. The authors also concluded that there was a need for additional
studies of LBW infants to examine relationships between early mother touch and
subsequent child development, as well as to test the effects of different tactile
stimulation programs on parent-child interaction and on child development.
Su mma r y a nd D i s c u s s i o n
Findings from the studies reviewed suggest that infants in NICUs receive relatively
little nurturing touch.12,18 Most of the touch they receive is related to medical or
nursing procedures, and much of this touch has adverse effects, including hypoxia,
bradycardia, increased ICP and sleep disturbances.21,23,24 Gentle touch is safe for
physiologically fragile infants in the early weeks of life. It also has a soothing effect,
156 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
Re f e re n c e s
1. Martin JA, Hamilton BE, Ventura SJ, Menacker F, Park MM. Births: Final data for 2000.
National Vital Statistics Reports. National Center for Health Statistics. Vol 50. No. 5.
Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nvsr/nvsr50/nvsr50_05.pdf. Accessed August 27,
2003.
2. Goldson E. The developmental consequences of prematurity. In: Wolraich ML, ed. Disorders
of Development and Learning. 3rd ed. Hamilton, Ontario: BC Decker; 2003:345-360.
3. Goldson E. The environment of the neonatal intensive care unit. In: Goldson E, ed.
Nurturing the Premature Infant: Developmental Interventions in the Neonatal Intensive Care
Nursery. New York, NY: Oxford University Press;1999:3-17.
4. Als H. A synactive model of neonatal behavioral organization: framework for the assessment
and support of the neurobehavioral development of the premature infant and his parents
in the environment of the neonatal intensive care unit. Physical & Occupational Therapy in
Pediatrics. 1986;6:3-55.
5. Als H, Lawhon G, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Gibes-Grossman R, Blickman JG.
Individualized developmental care for the very low-birth-weight preterm infant: medical and
neurofunctional effects. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1994;272:853-858.
6. Ashbaugh JB, Leick-Rude MK, Kilbride HW. Developmental care teams in the neonatal
intensive care unit: survey on current status. Journal of Perinatology: Official Journal of the
California Perinatal Association. 1999;19:48-52.
7. Brown LD, Heermann JA. The effect of developmental care on preterm infant outcome.
Applied Nursing Research: ANR. 1997;10:190-197.
8. Buehler DM, Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Liederman J. Effectiveness of individualized
developmental care for low-risk preterm infants: behavioral and electrophysiologic evidence.
Pediatrics. 1995;96:923-932.
11. Beal JA. The Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale: a tool to enhance parental
attachment. Journal of Pediatric Nursing. 1986;1:170-177.
12. Blackburn S, Barnard K. Analysis of care giving events relating to preterm infants in the
special care unit. In: Gottfried AW, Gaiter JL, eds. Infant Stress Under Intensive Care:
Environmental Neonatology. Baltimore, Md: University Park Press; 1985:113-130.
13. Catlett AT, Holditch-Davis D. Environmental stimulation of the acutely ill premature infant:
physiological effects and nursing implications. Neonatal Network: NN. 1990;8:19-26.
14. Gottfried AW, Hodgman JE, Brown KW. How intensive is newborn intensive care? An
environmental analysis. Pediatrics. 1984;74:292-294.
158 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
16. Miller DB, Holditch-Davis D. Interactions of parents and nurses with high-risk preterm
infants. Research in Nursing & Health. 1992;15:187-197.
17. Murdoch DR, Darlow BA. Handling during neonatal intensive care. Archives of Disease
in Childhood. 1984;59:957-961.
18. Werner NP, Conway AE. Caregiver contacts experienced by premature infants in the neonatal
intensive care unit. Maternal-Child Nursing Journal. 1990;19:21-43.
19. Danford DA, Miske S, Headley J, Nelson RM. Effects of routine care procedures on
transcutaneous oxygen in neonates: a quantitative approach. Archives of Disease in Childhood.
1983;58:20-23.
20. Evans JC. Incidence of hypoxemia associated with caregiving in premature infants. Neonatal
Network: NN. 1991;10:17-24.
21. Long JG, Philip AG, Lucey JF. Excessive handling as a cause of hypoxemia. Pediatrics.
1980;65:203-207.
22. Norris S, Campbell LA, Brenkert S. Nursing procedures and alterations in transcutaneous
oxygen tension in premature infants. Nursing Research. 1982;31:330-336.
23. Peters KL. Does routine nursing care complicate the physiologic status of the premature
neonate with respiratory distress syndrome. The Journal of Perinatal & Neonatal Nursing.
1992;6:67-84.
24. Peters KL. Neonatal stress reactivity and cortisol. The Journal of Perinatal & Neonatal
Nursing. 1998;11:45-49.
25. Harrison L. Research utilization: handling preterm infants in the NICU. Neonatal Network:
NN. 1997;16:65-69.
26. Harrison L, Bodin MB. Effects of tactile stimulation on preterm infants: an integrative
review of the literature with nursing implications. Online Journal of Knowledge Synthesis
in Nursing [serial online]. 1994;1:6. Available at: http://www.stti.iupui.edu/
VirginiaHendersonLibrary/OJKSNMenu.aspx. Accessed August 29, 2003.
27. Linn PL, Horowitz FD, Buddin BJ, Leaker JC, Fox HA. An ecological description of a
neonatal intensive care unit. In: Gottfried AW, Gaiter JL, eds. Infant Stress Under Intensive
Care: Environmental Neonatology. Baltimore, Md: University Park Press; 1985:83-111.
28. Symon A, Cunningham S. Handling premature neonates: a study using time-lapse video.
Nursing Times. 1995;91:35-37.
29. Harrison L, Woods S. Early parental touch and preterm infants. Journal of Obstetric,
Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing: JOGNN/NAACOG. 1991;20:299-306.
30. Harrison L, Leeper JD, Yoon M. Effects of early parent touch on preterm infants’ heart rates
and arterial oxygen saturation levels. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 1990;15:877-885.
Chapter 8: Tactile Stimulation of Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Preterm Infants 159
31. Speidel BD. Adverse effects of routine procedures on preterm infants. Lancet. 1978;
1:864-866.
32. Corff KE. An effective comfort measure for minor pain and stress in preterm infants:
facilitated tucking. Neonatal Network: NN. 1993;12:74.
33. Fearon I, Kisilevsky BS, Hains SMJ, Muir DW, Tranmer J. Swaddling after heel lance:
age-specific effects on behavioral recovery in preterm infants. Journal of Developmental and
Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP. 1997;18:222-232.
34. Jay SS. The effects of gentle human touch on mechanically ventilated very-short-gestation
infants. Maternal-Child Nursing Journal. 1982;11:199-259.
35. Tribotti SJ. Effects of gentle touch on the premature infant. In: Gunzenhauser N, ed.
Advances in Touch: New Implications in Human Development. [Pediatric Round Table Series.
Vol 14]. Skillman, NJ: Johnson & Johnson Baby; 1990:80-89.
36. Harrison L, Olivet L, Cunningham K, Bodin MB, Hicks C. Effects of gentle human touch
on preterm infants: pilot study results. Neonatal Network: NN. 1996;15:35-42.
37. Harrison L, Groer M, Modrcin-McCarthy MA, Wilkinson J. Effects of gentle human touch
on preterm infants: results from a pilot study. Infant Behavior and Development. 1992;15:12.
38. Harrison L, Williams AK, Berbaum ML, Stem JT, Leeper J. Effects of developmental,
health status, behavioral, and environmental variables on preterm infants’ responses to a
gentle human touch intervention. International Journal of Prenatal and Perinatal Psychology
and Medicine. 2000;12:109-122.
39. Harrison L, Williams AK, Berbaum ML, Stem JT, Leeper J. Physiologic and behavioral
effects of gentle human touch on preterm infants. Research in Nursing & Health.
2000;23:435-446.
40. Modrcin-McCarthy MA. The Physiological and Behavioral Effects of a Gentle Human Touch
Nursing Intervention on Preterm Infants [dissertation]. Knoxville, Tenn: University of
Tennessee; 1992.
41. Modrcin-Talbott MA, Harrison LL, Groer MW, Younger MS. The behavioral effects of
gentle human touch on preterm infants. Nursing Science Quarterly. 2003;16:60-67.
42. Adamson-Macedo EN, Attree JLA. TAC-TIC therapy: the importance of systematic stroking.
British Journal of Midwifery. 1994;2:264-269.
43. Kattwinkel J, Nearman HS, Fanaroff AA, Katona PG, Klaus MH. Apnea of prematurity.
Comparative therapeutic effects of cutaneous stimulation and nasal continuous positive
airway pressure. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1975;86:588-592.
44. McGehee IJ, Eckerman CO. The preterm infant as social partner: responsive but unreadable.
Infant Behavior and Development. 1983;6:461-470.
45. Oehler JM. Examining the issue of tactile stimulation for preterm infants. Neonatal Network:
NN. 1985;4:25-33.
160 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
46. Oehler JM, Eckerman CO, Wilson WH. Social stimulation and the regulation of premature
infants; state prior to term age. Infant Behavior and Development. 1988;11:333-351.
47. Adamson-Macedo EN, de Roiste A, Wilson A, Hayes JA, Eaton B, de Carvalho FA.
Systematic gentle/light stroking and maternal random touching of ventilated preterms: a
preliminary study. International Journal of Prenatal and Perinatal Psychology and Medicine.
1997;9:17-31.
48. Daga SR, Ahuja VK, Lunkad NG. A warm touch improves oxygenation in newborn babies.
Journal of Tropical Pediatrics. 1998;44:170-172.
49. Solkoff N, Yaffe S, Weintraub D, Blasé B. Effects of handling and the subsequent
development of premature infants. Developmental Psychology. 1969;1:765-768.
50. Solkoff N, Matuszak D. Tactile stimulation and behavioral development among
low-birthweight infants. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 1975;6:33-37.
51. Kramer M, Chamorro I, Green D, Knudtson F. Extra tactile stimulation of the premature
infant. Nursing Research. 1975;24:324-334
52. Adamson-Macedo EN. Effects of tactile stimulation on low and very low birth weight infants
during the first week of life. Current Psychological Research and Reviews. 1985-1986;(winter
issue):305-308.
53. Adamson-Macedo EN, Dattani II, Wilson A, de Carvalho FA. A small sample follow-up
study of children who received tactile stimulation after pre-term birth: intelligence and
achievements. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. 1993;11:165-168.
54. de Róiste A, Bushnell IWR. Tactile stimulation: short- and long-term benefits for pre-term
infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1996;14:41-53.
55. Harrison L. Effects of a gentle touch and massage intervention on preterm infants: pilot
study results. Paper presented at: National Conference of the National Association of
Neonatal Nurses; October 3-6, 2001; Salt Lake City, Utah.
57. Rice RD. The effects of the Rice infant sensorimotor stimulation treatment on the
development of high-risk infants. Birth Defects: Original Article Series. 1979;15:7-26.
58. White-Traut RC, Nelson MN. Maternally administered tactile, auditory, visual, and
vestibular stimulation: relationship to later interactions between mothers and premature
infants. Research in Nursing & Health. 1988;11:31-39.
59. Whipple, J. The effect of parent training in music and multimodal stimulation on
parent-neonate interactions in the neonatal intensive care unit. Journal of Music Therapy.
2000;37:250-268.
60. Field TM, Schanberg SM, Scafidi F, et al. Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation effects on preterm
neonates. Pediatrics. 1986;77:654-658.
Chapter 8: Tactile Stimulation of Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Preterm Infants 161
61. Kuhn CM, Schanberg SM, Field T. Tactile-kinesthetic stimulation effects on sympathetic
and adrenocortical function in preterm infants. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1991;119:434-440.
62. Morrow CJ, Field TM, Scafidi FA, et al. Differential effects of massage and heel stick
procedures on transcutaneous oxygen tension in preterm neonates. Infant Behavior and
Development. 1991;14:397-414.
63. Scafidi FA, Field TM, Schanberg SM, et al. Effects of tactile/kinesthetic stimulation on the
clinical course and sleep/wake behavior of preterm neonates. Infant Behavior and
Development. 1986;9:91-105.
64. Scafidi FA, Field TM, Schanberg SM, et al. Massage stimulates growth in preterm infants:
a replication. Infant Behavior and Development. 1990;13:167-188.
65. White-Traut RC, Tubeszewski KA. Multimodal stimulation of the premature infant. Journal
of Pediatric Nursing. 1986;1:90-95.
66. White-Traut RC, Goldman MBC. Premature infant massage: is it safe? Pediatric Nursing.
1988;14:285-289.
67. White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, Patel MK, Kilgallon D. Patterns of physiologic
and behavioral response of intermediate care preterm infants to intervention. Pediatric
Nursing. 1993;19:625-629.
68. White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Burns K, Cunningham N. Environmental influences on the
developing premature infant: theoretical issues and applications to practice. Journal of
Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing. 1994;23:393-401.
69. White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, Cunningham N, Patel M. Responses of preterm
infants to unimodal and multimodal sensory intervention. Pediatric Nursing. 1997;23:
169-175, 193.
70. White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, et al. Developmental intervention for preterm
infants diagnosed with periventricular leukomalacia. Research in Nursing & Health.
1999;22:131-143.
71. White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, et al. Effect of auditory, tactile, visual, and
vestibular intervention on length of stay, alertness, and feeding progression in preterm infants.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2002;44:91-97.
72. Schanberg S, Kuhn CM, Field TM, Bartolome JV. Maternal deprivation and growth
suppression. In: Gunzenhauser N, ed. Advances in Touch: New Implications in Human
Development [Pediatric Round Table Series. Vol 14]. Skillman, NJ: Johnson & Johnson Baby;
1990:3-10.
73. Nelson MN, White-Traut RC, Vasan U, et al. One-year outcome of auditory-tactile-visual-
vestibular intervention in the neonatal intensive care unit: effects of severe prematurity and
central nervous system injury. Journal of Child Neurology. 2001;16:493-498.
74. Rose SA, Schmidt K, Riese ML, Bridger WH. Effects of prematurity and early intervention
on responsivity to tactual stimuli: a comparison of preterm and full-term infants. Child
Development. 1980;51:416-425.
162 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
75. Weiss SJ, Wilson P, Hertenstein MJ, Campos R. The tactile context of a mother’s care giving:
implications for attachment of low birth weight infants. Infant Behavior & Development.
2000;23:91-111.
76. Weiss SJ, Wilson P, Seed MS, Paul SM. Early tactile experience of low birth weight children:
links to later mental health and social adaptation. Infant and Child Development. 2001;10:
93-115.
CHAPTER 9:
PRETERM AND
FULL-TERM
INFANT MASSAGE
IN CHINA
Ab s t ra c t
During the current study, 405 healthy and sick term and preterm infants were
provided massage 3 times per day, for 15 minutes each, for 10 days. Three
different types of massage were compared; including the type discussed by Field
and colleagues in their article published in Pediatrics in 1986 that involves
massaging the entire body (overall body massage), a simpler massage covering
the head, abdomen, hands and feet, and a simple massage plus the rubbing of
acupressure points. Although no significant differences were noted in formula
intake, greater increases in daily weight were noted in the group that received
the overall body massage and the group that received massage with acupressure
points. No significant differences were noted in head circumference or body length.
Preterm sick babies also benefited from the overall massage by showing a lesser
decrease in their hemoglobin status than did infants in the other groups.
164 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
Introd uc t i o n
The C hi n a St u d y
The sample of 405 infants came from 6 neonatal clinics, including a large children’s
hospital and 2 municipal-level and 2 district-level maternal-infant medical care
centers. Three different types of massage were assessed, including the procedure
developed by Field and colleagues,2 currently being used in North America, the
Philippines, Korea and Israel. This involves massaging the infant’s entire body,
using moderate pressure. The second method that was tried is called the domestic
simple-touch massage, in which the head (namely the forehead and the face),
abdomen, hands, ankles and feet were massaged. The third method was the same
domestic simple-touch procedure plus the rubbing of acupressure points. Each
procedure was practiced for 15 minutes, 3 times per day, for 10 days. Of note
was that these massages were performed by specially trained physicians and nurses,
unlike most other massage therapy studies which have used parents or trained
research assistants.
The sample of babies was also different from most other massage therapy studies in
that it included normal term and preterm and sick term and preterm neonates. The
Chapter 9: Preterm and Full-term Infant Massage in China 165
infants labeled “sick” had been hospitalized with any of the following diseases to
a mild degree: pneumonia, apnea, hyperbilirubinemia and scleroderma without
complications. The test neonates and controls were assigned randomly to the
following 6 groups: normal term infants and controls who received the overall
body massage; sick term infants and controls who received the overall body
massage; sick preterm infants and controls who received the overall body massage;
normal term infants and controls who received the simple massage; normal term
infants and controls who received the simple massage plus the rubbing of
acupressure points; and sick preterm infants and controls who received the simple
massage plus the rubbing of acupressure points. Several measures were recorded:
growth items, hemoglobin status, formula intake and performance on a neonatal
behavioral and neurological assessment.
R E S U LT S
The data analyses suggested that the groups did not differ on the consumption
of formula. The groups did differ, however, on a number of growth measures.
The full-term infants who received the overall massage, the sick term infants
who received the overall massage and the normal preterm infants who received
the rubbing of acupressure points showed greater weight gain (49 versus 41 grams
per day for the term infants who received overall massage versus their controls;
35 versus 27 grams per day for the preterm infants who received overall massage
versus their controls; and 27 versus 22 grams per day for the preterm infants who
received the acupressure versus their controls). Body length and head circumference
were positively affected but only for the term group who received the overall body
massage. Similarly, hemoglobin status was positively affected but only for the sick
preterm group who received the overall body massage. This was indicated by a
lesser decrease than usually occurs and that occurred for the control group—1.98
versus 5.20 g/dL per 10 days. The cumulative scores on the neonatal behavioral
and neurological assessment were affected positively for the preterm infants who
received overall body massage (4.16 versus 2.63 for the controls), for the preterm
sick infants who received the overall body massage (2.36 versus 1.78 for the
controls) and for the term infants who received the simple massage plus acupressure
(6.30 versus 5.14 for the controls).
166 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
Conclusions
The results of this study, performed on healthy and sick term and preterm infants
in China, suggest that daily weight gain, as well as head circumference and body
length, is affected positively by massage therapy. The results also suggest that even
sick preterm infants can benefit from massage therapy: The infants exhibited a
lesser decrease in their hemoglobin status following the massage. Consistent with
many other studies, the groups who received overall body massage and massage
with acupressure showed significant weight gain. It is also notable that the only
procedures that appeared to benefit the infants were the methods that provide
moderate pressure to the whole body and the method that provided moderate
pressure to acupressure points on several parts of the body. As suggested by Field,4
the provision of moderate pressure, which appears to show superior results to
light pressure,5 may be stimulating an increase in vagal activity, which, in turn,
could be activating food absorption hormones. (Please also see Uvnäs-Moberg in
this volume.)
Ref ere n c e s
1. Ottenbacher KJ, Muller L, Brandt D, Heintzelman A, Hojem P, Sharpe P. The effectiveness
of tactile stimulation as a form of early intervention: a quantitative evaluation. J Dev Behav
Pediatr. 1987;8:68-76.
2. Field TM, Schanberg SM, Scafidi F, et al. Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation effects on preterm
neonates. Pediatrics. 1986;77:654-658.
3. Kuhn CM, Schanberg SM, Field T, et al. Tactile-kinesthetic stimulation effects on sympathet-
ic and adrenocortical function in preterm infants. J Pediatr. 1991;119:434-440.
4. Field T. Massage therapy facilitates weight gain in preterm infants. Psychol Sci. 2001;10:51-
54.
5. Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Diego M, Feijo L, Vera Y, Gil K. Massage therapy by parents
improves early growth and development. IBAD. In press.
CHAPTER 10:
O P T I M I Z I N G G ROW T H
P R E M AT U R E I N FA N T S :
Ab s t ra c t
Studies performed by my
colleagues and I have
examined the effects
of daily physical
movement (PM)
administered by
occupational therapists
(OT) and infants’
mothers (MT) on
growth and bone mass
in premature infants.
Introd uc t i o n
Premature infants (<37 weeks gestation), especially those who have gestational ages
(GAs) less than 33 weeks and/or birth weights of 1500 g or less, present numerous
physiological and developmental concerns, including those involving physical
growth and the provision of adequate nutrients to sustain growth. The ex-utero
growth rate in such infants often lags behind normal in-utero growth, especially in
very-low-birth-weight (VLBW) premature infants (<1000 g), despite the provision
of energy and nutrients at currently advised levels.1
Premature infants are also at risk of developing osteopenia (low bone mass) and
subsequent fractures due to the following: limited accretion of bone minerals
in-utero; a greater need for bone nutrients than infants who are delivered at
term gestation; and decreased ex-utero calcium retention.1-5 The provision of
bone-related nutrients at advised levels increases bone mineralization; however,
ex-utero mineralization rates do not equal in-utero rates, even with gains in normal
body weight.1,3 In addition, bone mineralization in premature infants does not
approach normal ranges until after the first year of life,6 and it may continue to
be inadequate into childhood, further increasing the child’s risk of fractures.7
Chapter 10: Optimizing Growth and Bone Mass in Premature Infants 169
Several interrelated extrinsic factors have been shown to influence ex-utero growth
and bone mineralization in premature infants, including the following: energy
and nutrient requirements to support the infant’s somatic growth rate; the type
and quality of dietary and nutrient intake; medications that may alter metabolism
or impair renal retention of calcium, such as glucocorticoids, furosemide diuretics
and thyroid-replacement preparations; and the multitude of morbidities commonly
associated with prematurity (Figure 1).8
Fig 1. Extrinsic factors that influence growth and bone mineralization in premature
infants.
Ex-Utero
Preterm Birth Growth & Bone Preterm Birth
Limited In-Utero Mineralization Ex-Utero
Lean & Fat Mass
in Preterm Nutrient Needs
& Mineral Accretion
Infants
Nutrient Intake
Source
Parenteral v. Enteral
Bioavailability
Related Morbidities
Physical Stress,Morbidities
Related Sepsis
Movement Malabsorption
Chronic lung disease
Medications
Diuretics
Medications
Glucocorticoids
Anti-convulsants
Thyroid replacement
170 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
Mechanical strain on bones and joints stimulates bone formation and growth.9
Osteoblasts, the cells responsible for bone formation, increase their activity in
response to mechanical strain in vitro.10 Weight-bearing physical activity has been
demonstrated to increase bone mass in children, young adults and older persons.11-13
Conversely, the absence of weight-bearing physical activity, such as that observed
in immobilized adults or astronauts during prolonged spaceflights, increases
bone resorption and renal excretion of calcium and decreases bone mass.14
Hypomineralized bone, such as that reported in newborn term infants who have
neuromuscular diseases, has been attributed to decreased movement in utero.16
Since standard care for hospitalized premature infants includes swaddling or
“nesting” and decreased sensory and physical stimulation,15 these infants receive
limited physical movement, which may increase bone resorption and
demineralization even when adequate dietary bone nutrients are provided.
Desi g n a n d Me t h o d s
The infants were matched by birth weight and GA and were fed either mother’s
milk with powdered fortification (Enfamil® Human Milk Fortifier; Mead Johnson®
Nutritionals, Evansville, Ind, USA) or premature infant formula (Enfamil®
Premature LIPIL®; Mead Johnson® Nutritionals) at an equal caloric density of
24 kcal/oz.
Chapter 10: Optimizing Growth and Bone Mass in Premature Infants 171
Figure 2A (left)
demonstrates extension
of infant’s arm by
occupational therapist
(OT) against infant’s
own resistance to the
movement.
Figure 2B (right)
shows extension
and stabilization of
infant’s leg as gentle
decompression is
applied by OT to ankle,
knee and hip joints.
172 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
Study 117 Randomized to physical movement • Daily body weight and nutrient intake
(PM; n=13) or tactile stimulation and output
(n=13) • Weekly body length and occipitofrontal
circumference (OFC)
• Baseline and at 4 weeks: bone mass of
distal one-third radius by single-photon
absorptiometry (SPA), and blood and
urine measures of bone mineral status
Study 218 Randomized to PM (n=16) or • Daily body weight and nutrient intake
tactile stimulation (n=16) and output
• Weekly body length and OFC
• Baseline and at 2-kg body weight: bone
mass of forearm by dual-energy X-ray
absorptiometry (DXA) and blood and
urine measures of bone mineral status
Results
There were no differences in mean baseline body weights at study entry or at study
completion among any of the infants in the 3 clinical studies (Table 2)17,18 (also
Moyer-Mileur, Ball, Brunstetter & Chan, unpublished data, 2003). The rates of
change for body length and head circumference were similar and indicated an
overall positive trend for all infants. Despite equal energy and nutrient intakes,
infants who received PM in both Study 117 and Study 218 had greater increases in
average daily weight than did the control infants. In Study 3 (Moyer-Mileur, Ball,
Brunstetter & Chan, unpublished data, 2003), infants who received daily PM by
the OT experienced greater rates of daily weight gain than did the control infants,
Chapter 10: Optimizing Growth and Bone Mass in Premature Infants 173
13
16
16
n=
n=
n=
6
13
16
16
1
rol
rol
rol
n=
n=
n=
n=
nt
nt
nt
T
PM
PM
OT
Co
Co
Co
M
Gestation (wk) 28.2 (1.3) 28.9 (1.5) 29.6 (1.6) 29.8 (1.5) 28.4 (1.0) 28.9 (1.5) 29.8 (1.5)
Birth weight (g) 1207 (172) 1240 (182) 1258 (230) 1283 (199) 1188 (276) 1252 (232) 1283 (199)
Adjusted age 30.0 (1.4) 31.3 (0.9) 31.9 (1.5) 31.9 (1.1) 31.6 (0.8) 32.3 (1.4) 31.9 (1.1)
study: Day 1 (wk)
Days on study 28 28 27 24 29 27 24
Energy (kcal/kg/d) 128 (6) 126 (8) 115 (12) 120 (7) 124 (10) 123 (11) 120 (7)
Protein (g/d) 3.4 (0.2) 3.4 (0.3) 3.3 (0.3) 3.5 (0.3) 3.7 (0.3) 3.7 (0.3) 3.5 (0.3)
Calcium (mg/d) 135 (7) 133 (7) 167 (17) 174 (18) 173 (15) 172 (15) 174 (18)
Vitamin D (IU/d) 380 (15) 377 (15) 312 (14) 323 (24) 368 (14) 367 (13) 323 (24)
Values presented as the mean (±SD). HMF = Human milk with fortifier. PTF = Premature infant formula fed
at 24 kcal/oz. There are no differences in the infants’ characteristics or nutrient intake within or between the
3 studies. In Study 3, however, all infants randomized to the MT group were fed HMF.
In addition to greater weight gains found in infants who received PM, significant
differences in forearm bone mass were found between Study group 1 and Study
group 2 (Figures 4, 5 and 6). At baseline, there were no between-group differences
in distal one-third radius or total forearm bone mineral content (BMC; mg), bone
area (BA; cm2) or bone mineral density (BMD; mg/cm2). After approximately 28
days of intervention, however, my colleagues and I observed significant gains over
174 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
Figure 3. Despite similar energy and nutrient intakes, rate of growth (g/kg body wt/d)
significantly greater in infants who received daily OT-administered PM vs those
randomized to control group (P≤.01, MANCOVA). No differences in growth rates
between maternal administered (MT) PM and OT or controls. (Moyer-Mileur, Ball,
Brunstetter & Chan, unpublished data, 2003).
Figure 4. Bone mineral content (BMC; g) change (%) in distal one-third radius,
as measured by single-photon absorptiometry (SPA; Study 117), and total forearm,
as measured by dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA; Studies 218 and 3[LJMM,
unpublished data, 2003]), significantly greater in infants who received daily PM administered
Figure 5. Bone area (BA; cm2) change (%) in distal one-third radius, as measured
by SPA; Study 117), and total forearm, as measaured by DXA; Studies 218 and 3.(LJMM,
unpublished data, 2003) Percent changes in BA were significant only for OT and MT infants
Figure 6. Bone mineral density (BMD; mg/cm2) change (%) in distal one-third
radius, as measured by SPA; Study 117, and total forearm, as measured by DXA;
Studies 218 and 3(LJMM, unpublished data, 2003) significantly greater in infants who received
daily PM administered by an OT or the MT versus control infants (P≤.03,
MANCOVA).
176 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
time for BMC (P<.02; Figure 4) and BMD. The type of feeding source—fortified
human milk or premature infant formula—was not found to be a significant
cofactor for changes in bone mass over time.
Serum and urine markers of bone activity did not differ between groups in Study 1.
However, levels of serum parathyroid hormone (PTH), responsible for regulation
of serum calcium homeostasis, were found to be related inversely to BMC at study
completion (r=–.83, P=.01). In Study 2, PTH levels were greater in infants
receiving PM (P=.03), although the change from study entry to study completion
did not differ statistically and were within normal limits for age. In Study 3, my
colleagues and I found that levels of bone alkaline phosphatase (BAP), a marker
of bone formation, increased approximately 15% from baseline in MT and OT
infants, but decreased approximately 20% from baseline in controls (P<.05). Urine
calcium to creatinine excretion was similar and within normal limits for all groups.
Discussion
In 1985, the American Academy of Pediatrics stated that the nutritional goal for
premature infants is to provide optimal nutrition to support growth equivalent to
in-utero gain during the third trimester.25 Nutritional intervention, while helping
to promote adequate weight gain, offers variable effects on postnatal bone
mineralization in premature infants. Multiple factors influence bone growth and
development including diet and PM. Although we have demonstrated the positive
effects of PM on bone mineralization in premature infants, a program of PM may
prove inappropriate for infants who have poor nutrient intake. Specker, Mulligan
and Ho26 reported on the effects of a 1-year program of PM on BMC in older
infants. They found that BMC is related to calcium intake. Randomized PM and
control infants who had moderately high calcium intakes had similar BMCs, but
low calcium intakes in infants in the PM program resulted in lower BMCs. These
investigators speculated that participation in a program of PM during rapid bone
growth may lead to reduced bone accretion in the presence of a low intake of
calcium. Thus, caution should be used before starting a program of PM in infants
who have limited nutrient intake; for example, premature infants fed unfortified
mother’s milk or standard infant formula, or infants who have a chronic illness or
are taking medication that interferes with the delivery of bone nutrients.
Conclusion
In the 3 studies reviewed, my colleagues and I have shown successfully that bone
mass indices increase when premature VLBW infants receive daily PM in addition
to recommended levels of energy and nutrients. More importantly, we have
demonstrated that a program of PM administered by the infant’s own mother
is equally as effective as a therapist-administered program of PM in promoting
greater bone mineralization in premature infants.
Chapter 10: Optimizing Growth and Bone Mass in Premature Infants 179
Re f e re n c e s
1. Tsang RC, Lucas A, Uauy R, Zlotkin S, eds. Nutritional Needs for Preterm Infants: Scientific
Basis and Practical Guidelines. Calcium, magnesium, phophorus, and vitamin D. Baltimore,
Md: Williams & Williams; 1993:1-10.
4. Widdowson EM, Southgate DAT, Hay E. Fetal growth and body composition. In: Landblad
BS, ed. Perinatal Nutrition. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1988:4-14.
5. Koo WWK, Massom LR, Walters J. Validation of accuracy and precision of dual energy X-ray
absorptiometry for infants. Journal of Bone and Mineral Research: the Official Journal of the
American Society for Bone and Mineral Research. 1995;10:1111-1115.
6. Rigo J, Nyamugabo K, Picaud JC, Gerard P, Pieltain C, De Curtis M. Reference values of
body composition obtained by dual energy X-ray absorptiometry in preterm and term
neonates. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition. 1998;27:184-190.
7. Abrams SA, Schanler RJ, Tsang RC, Garza C. Bone mineralization in former very low birth
weight infants fed either human milk or commercial formula: one-year follow-up observation.
The Journal of Pediatrics. 1989;114:1041-1044.
8. Krug SK. Osteopenia of prematurity. In: Groh-Wargo S, Thompson M, Hovasi Cox J, eds.
Nutritional Care for High-Risk Newborns. 3rd ed. Chicago, Ill: Precept Press; 2002:489-506.
9. Yeh JK, Liu CC, Aloia JF. Effects of exercise and immobilization on bone formation and
resorption in young rats. The American Journal of Physiology. 1993;264:E182-E189.
10. Schulthies L. The mechanical control system of bone in weightless spaceflight and in aging.
Experimental Gerontology. 1991;26:203-214.
11. Pirnay F, Bodeux M, Crielaard JM, Franchimont P. Bone mineral content and physical
activity. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 1987;8:331-335.
12. Pocock NA, Eisman JA, Yeates MG, Sambrook PN, Eberl S. Physical fitness is a major
determinant of femoral neck and lumbar spine bone mineral density. The Journal of Clinical
Investigation. 1986;78:618-621.
13. Davee AM, Rosen CJ, Adler RA. Exercise patterns and trabecular bone density in college
women. Journal of Bone and Mineral Research: the Official Journal of the American Society for
Bone and Mineral Research. 1990;5:245-250.
14. Mazess RB, Whedon GD. Immobilization and bone. Calcified Tissue International.
1983;35:265-267.
180 Section III. Touch in Premature Infants
15. Lickliter R. The role of sensory stimulation in perinatal development: insights from
comparative research for care of the high-risk infant. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
Pediatrics: JDBP. 2000;21:437-447.
16. Rodriguez JI, Garcia-Alix A, Palacios J, Paniagua R. Changes in the long bones due to fetal
immobility caused by neuromuscular disease. A radiographic and histological study. Journal
of Bone and Joint Surgery: American Volume. 1988; 70:1052-1060.
17. Moyer-Mileur L, Luetkemeier M, Boomer L, Chan GM. Effect of physical activity on bone
mineralization in premature infants. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1995;127:620-625.
18. Moyer-Mileur LJ, Brunstetter V, McNaught TP, Gill G, Chan GM. Daily physical activity
program increases bone mineralization and growth in preterm very low birth weight infants.
Pediatrics. 2000;106:1088-1092.
19. Chan GM, Mileur L, Hansen JW. Calcium and phosphorus requirements in bone
mineralization of preterm infants. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1988;113:225-229.
20. Greer FR, McCormick A. Bone growth with low bone mineral content in very low birth
weight premature infants. Pediatric Research. 1986;20:925-928.
21. Horsman A, Ryan SW, Congdon PJ, Truscott JG, Simpson M. Bone mineral accretion rate
and calcium intake in preterm infants. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 1989;64(spec no.
7):910-918.
22. Termine JD, Robey PG. Bone matrix proteins and the mineralization process. In: Favus MJ,
ed. Primer on the Metabolic Bone Diseases and Disorders of Mineral Metabolism. Philadelphia,
Pa: Lippincott-Raven; 1996:24-28.
23. Ljunghall S, Joborn H, Roxin LE, Skarfors ET, Wide LE, Lithell HO. Increase in serum
parathyroid hormone levels after prolonged physical exercise. Medicine and Science in Sports
and Exercise. 1988;20:122-125.
24. Miller SC, Hunziker J, Mecham M, Wronski TJ. Intermittent parathyroid hormone
administration stimulates bone formation in the mandibles of aged ovariectomized rats.
Journal of Dental Research. 1997;76:1471-1476.
S E C T I O N I V.
EFFECTS OF MASSAGE
O N S L E E P, R E L A X A T I O N
AND WELL-BEING
CHAPTER 11:
MASSAGE THERAPY
A N D S L E E P - WA K E R H Y T H M S
I N T H E N E O N AT E
Ab s t ra c t
The neonate spends most of the perinatal period sleeping. Whereas the
full-term neonate sleeps 70% of the 24-hour period, the preterm infant sleeps
up to 90% of a full day.1 Since self-regulation and growth occur during sleep,
enhancing sleep and assuring undisturbed sleep by the caregiver are of great
importance. In addition, one of the natural and immediate ways to comfort
an infant is by holding, touching, stroking and keeping the infant in close
physical contact with a caregiver. This chapter provides empirical evidence for
the benefits of touch by a caregiver on sleep. The infant’s sleep and adjustment
to the day-night cycle are described, and the effects of massage therapy are
illustrated by empirical data.
184 Section IV. Effects of Massage on Sleep, Relaxation and Well-Being
Neonates exhibit a greater amount of activity during the night than do older
infants.12-15 Therefore, it is not surprising that considerable maternal distress is
associated with infants’ disrupted night sleep.16 Consequently, practitioners have
sought methods to enhance the development of sleep-wake rhythms in infants.12,17
Mel a t o n i n a n d S o c i a l Cu e s
Although bright light is an important time cue, social cues are also important for
the entrainment of sleep-wake rhythms. Social cues—including social demands or
tasks, such as regularly timed meals and sleep-wake schedules—have been shown
to entrain sleep-wake rhythms.22
Support for the role of the caregiver in the infant’s modulation of states comes from
a study conducted by Thoman, Davis and Denenberg in which a group of full-term
infants were observed at home at 2 weeks to 5 weeks of age.23 Data were obtained
Chapter 11: Massage Therapy and Sleep-Wake Rhythms in the Neonate 185
in 2 social contexts: when the baby was alone and when the baby was with the
mother. Fewer state-modulated relationships were noted when the babies were with
their mothers than when they were alone. The investigators concluded that the
mother is probably acting as a social cue, influencing the infant’s adaptation to
the day-night environment.
When the infants were with their mothers they were awake most of the time.
Their waking states were more numerous and less sustained and the infants were
more susceptible to environmental influences than when they were in their sleeping
states.24 Thus, the mothers’ activities with their infants appeared to have more
effect on the infant during the daytime.
Mothers in the experimental group were instructed to prepare their babies for
sleep following the evening meal and bath, between 8:00 pm and 9:00 pm and to
provide 30 minutes daily massage therapy for their infants for 14 days, starting on
Day 10 of life. These mothers were instructed to relax, sit comfortably on a sofa
and hold their babies in their laps or lay them on the sofa next to them. Mothers
were then instructed to roll their infants on their sides and perform massage
therapy in a rhythm of 3 movements per second. This massage therapy involved
touching the infant’s head with one hand and lightly stroking its back in a circular
motion under the bedclothes with the other hand. Mothers in the control group
performed their usual bedtime activity, but without the massage therapy.
The massaged infants, versus the control infants, achieved more favorable
adjustments of their rest-activity cycles at the age of 8 weeks and a higher nocturnal
186 Section IV. Effects of Massage on Sleep, Relaxation and Well-Being
Figure 1. Rest-activity cycles during night and day comparing treatment and control
groups at age 8 weeks. Treated subjects displayed delayed primary and secondary peaks
of melatonin activity (P<.05).
Chapter 11: Massage Therapy and Sleep-Wake Rhythms in the Neonate 187
Ackn ow le d gm e nt s
I wish to thank Professors N. Zisapel, A. Weller and P. Lavie and Dr. M. Laudon
for their assistance in preparing this chapter. Special thanks also to the Sleep
Laboratory Library Team at the Technion Institute, Haifa, Israel.
188 Section IV. Effects of Massage on Sleep, Relaxation and Well-Being
Ref ere n c e s
1. Hutt C, von Bernuth H, Lenard HG, Hutt SJ, Prechtl HF. Habituation in relation to state
in the human neonate. Nature. 1968;220:618-620.
2. Feldman R, Weller A, Sirota L, Eidelman AI. Skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo care) promotes
self-regulation in premature infants: sleep-wake cyclicity, arousal modulation, and sustained
exploration. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:194-207.
3. Brazelton TB. Saving the bathwater. Child Development. 1990;61:1661-1671.
4. Rivkees SA, Hao H. Developing circadian rhythmicity. Seminars in Perinatology.
2000;24:232-242.
5. Cavallo A. The pineal gland in human beings: relevance to pediatrics. The Journal of
Pediatrics. 1993;123:843-851.
6. Kennaway DJ, Stamp GE, Goble FC. Development of melatonin production in infants and
the impact of prematurity. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism.
1992;75:367-369.
12. Weissbluth M, Weissbluth L. Colic, sleep inertia, melatonin and circannual rhythms. Medical
Hypotheses. 1992;38:224-228.
13. McGraw K, Hoffmann R, Harker C, Herman JH. The development of circadian rhythms
in a human infant. Sleep. 1999;22:303-310.
14. Mirmiran M, Lunshof S. Perinatal development of human circadian rhythms. Progress in
Brain Research. 1996;111:217-226.
15. Sadeh A, Lavie P, Scher A, Tirosh E, Epstein R. Actigraphic home-monitoring sleep-disturbed
and control infants and young children: a new method for pediatric assessment of sleep-wake
patterns. Pediatrics. 1991;87:494-499.
16. Guilleminault C, Leger D, Pelayo R, Gould S, Hayes B, Miles L. Development of circadian
rhythmicity of temperature in full-term normal infants. Neurophysiologie Clinique - Clinical
Neurophysiology. 1996;26:21-28.
Chapter 11: Massage Therapy and Sleep-Wake Rhythms in the Neonate 189
17. Nishihara K, Horiuchi S, Eto H, Uchida S. Mothers’ wakefulness at night in the post-partum
period is related to their infants’ circadian sleep-wake rhythm. Psychiatry and Clinical
Neurosciences. 2000;54:305-306.
18. Renfrew MJ, Lang S, Martin L, Woolridge M. Interventions for influencing sleep patterns
in exclusively breastfed infants. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2000;No.
2:CD000113.
20. Sandyk R. Melatonin and maturation of REM sleep. The International Journal of
Neuroscience. 1992;63:105-114.
21. Masana MI, Dubocovich ML. Melatonin receptor signaling: finding the path through the
dark. Science’s STKE [Electronic Resource]: Signal Transduction Knowledge Environment.
November 6, 2001:PE39.
22. Kennaway DJ, Goble FC, Stamp GE. Factors influencing the development of melatonin
rhythmicity in humans. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1996;81:
1525-1532.
23. Ehlers CL, Frank E, Kupfer DJ. Social zeitgebers and biological rhythms. A unified approach
to understanding the etiology of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1988;45:948-952.
24. Thoman EB, Davis DH, Denenberg VH. The sleeping and waking states of infants:
correlations across time and person. Physiology & Behavior. 1987;41:531-537.
25. Ferber SG, Laudon M, Kuint J, Weller A, Zisapel N. Massage therapy by mothers enhances
the adjustment of circadian rhythms to the nocturnal period in full-term infants. Journal of
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP. 2002;23:410-415.
CHAPTER 12:
M A S S A G E , R E L A X AT I O N
AND WELL-BEING:
A POSSIBLE ROLE
FOR OXYTOCIN AS AN
I N T E G R AT I V E P R I N C I P L E ?
Kerstin Uvnäs-Moberg, MD
Swedish University of Agricultural Science
Department of Animal Physiology
Karolinska Institute, Stockholm
Abstract
Massage has been shown to decrease cortisol and anxiety levels. Reduced anxiety
is accompanied by an increased sense of well-being. This chapter examines the
effects of massage on promoting relaxation, on enhanced sense of well-being and
positive social interaction. Oxytocin—a pituitary hormone that stimulates the
contraction of uterine muscle and the secretion of breast milk—may play a pivotal
role in producing these responses to massage. Studies are reviewed that show
increased oxytocin levels in animals, as well as in humans, in response to physical
contact between mother and child, such as skin-to-skin contact during
breastfeeding. Research has also exhibited decreased gastrin levels in response
to massage, suggesting a possible marker of increased social interests and skills.
192 Section IV. Effects of Massage on Sleep, Relaxation and Well-Being
Introd uc t i o n
Massage has been used since ancient times to relax muscles, cure disease and
promote general well-being. In spite of this, few studies exist that document the
positive effects of massage in a controlled scientific manner. In addition, there
are only a few studies that attempt to delineate the mechanisms underlying the
beneficial effects of massage. Field and colleagues,1 who represent an exception
in this regard, have performed extensive clinical studies on the effects of massage,
ranging from studies on premature infants to the effects on patients who have
various psychiatric disorders (also see chapter by Field et al, this volume). Most
of these studies have demonstrated that massage offers undeniable beneficial effects.
In addition to demonstrating the alleviation of specific symptoms, they have
typically found decreased cortisol levels. Anxiety levels have also been reduced,
and the resulting sense of well-being has increased.1-3 Massage has also been noted
to increase growth hormone, with positive effects on cellular growth.4
One of the aims of this chapter is not to summarize the clinical effects of massage,
but rather to suggest that massage, in part, exerts its beneficial effects by activating
a psychophysiological system that acts in opposition to the “fight-or-flight” system
and which therefore promotes relaxation, well-being and positive social interaction.
Oxytocin is suggested to be a major organizing principle of these effects at the
hypothalamic level.
In contrast to the rapidity with which the fight-or-flight response develops, the
state of relaxation and well-being develops slowly. The subjective signs of the latter
are subtle and sometimes more easily defined by their absence. Yet, relaxation and
well-being have distinct psychophysiological patterns, and their expressions can be
demonstrated experimentally in several behavioral and physiological model systems.
Pulse rate and blood pressure (BP) are kept at a low, healthy and balanced level,
and the GI tract, which is controlled by the vagus nerve, is activated, promoting
digestion and the storing of nutrients. Energy would rather be used for growth and
restorative processes than for muscular or thermogenic activity. Behaviorally,
reduced arousal and the development of calm prevail. Subjectively, this might be
related to a sense of well-being and relaxation. This state should not, however, be
confused with euphoria, which is a more intense feeling of joy and reward.6-8
Guido Reni’s painting of Madonna and Child (Figure 1) captures some of the
most significant features of the state of relaxation and well-being. The painting
illustrates that this state contains both an individual and an interactive component:
The Madonna appears relaxed, calm, content, happy, peaceful, warm, open,
generous, empathic and friendly. In the interaction with the child, she displays
closeness, trust, loyalty, giving and receiving, as well as love. Individual boundaries
are erased, and a sense of unity prevails. The Madonna and Child depicts just one
example of this state of relaxation and well-being, which can be experienced in a
variety of situations, independent of gender and age. Such situations range from
the well-being and physiological relaxation induced by a hot bath, by being part
of a social group or by receiving massage.
194 Section IV. Effects of Massage on Sleep, Relaxation and Well-Being
Figure 1. The Madonna with child as an expression of the individual and the
interactional aspects of eustasis (Madonna and Child by Guido Reni, 1575-1642).
relaxed closeness
calm trust
content loyalty
happy giving
peaceful receiving
warm love
open unity
generous
empathic
friendly
O x y t o c i n a s a n Age nt Be h i nd Re l axation
and Well-Be i n g : t h e Ev i d e nc e
For the purposes of this chapter, it is important to note that oxytocin is released
not only in response to suckling during breastfeeding and in response to labor,
but it is also released by nonnoxious stimulation—such as by touch, warmth and
stroking—applied to other parts of the body.14,18,32 Levels of oxytocin rise in plasma
as well as in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) in response to these stimuli. A release of
oxytocin in the amygdala has been demonstrated after suckling in sheep33 and
rats,34 which suggests that oxytocin is released from nerve terminals in the specific
brain regions receiving oxytocinergic nerve projections.
In their most basic forms, relaxation and well-being are induced by warmth, touch
and light pressure. This may occur in noninteractive situations, such as during
196 Section IV. Effects of Massage on Sleep, Relaxation and Well-Being
To elaborate the processes by which touch, warmth and stroking and oxytocin may
take part in inducing relaxation and well-being, their roles in the mother-infant
interaction are described. This is a situation during which mother and child, via
close physical contact, induce relaxation and well-being in each other. When the
mother and child are allowed skin-to-skin contact after birth, the newborn baby
exhibits a spontaneous breast-seeking behavior. Before he/she starts suckling, the
newborn infant massages the mother’s breasts with his/her hands.38 During this
period, a pulsatile release of oxytocin is induced in the mother. Interestingly, there
is a strong positive correlation between the maternal rise of oxytocin levels and the
number of infant hand movements, suggesting that it is the massage that induces
the release of oxytocin.39,40 Circulating oxytocin released from the mother’s posterior
pituitary during this period not only causes milk ejection, but it also induces
vasodilation in the skin of the mother’s chest.41 The mother experiences this
vasodilation as warmth, which enhances her sense of relaxation and well-being
and her interactions with her child. After birth, the child, in turn, responds to the
physical contact with the mother by becoming calmer, as expressed by less crying.42
The child also becomes more relaxed physically, which is evidenced by a higher foot
temperature as a result of decreased peripheral vasoconstriction.43 Interestingly,
there is a strong positive relationship between maternal breast temperature and the
baby’s foot temperature: The warmer the mother’s breasts, the warmer the baby’s
feet, which suggests that maternal temperature influences the sympathetic nervous
tone of the baby. In this way, the mother and child in our example are interlocked
in a feed-forward process of well-being and relaxation.
mother and the newborn. In addition to the ordinary sensory fibers that mediate the
senses of touch, light pressure and warmth, the cutaneous vagal afferents, which
project to the NTS, are activated. At the level of the NTS, sensory stimuli induce a
physiological relaxing effect by decreasing sympathetic nerve activity and increasing
parasympathetic tone. This results in a lowered BP, increased peripheral circulation
and activation of the endocrine system of the GI tract. In a second step, NA
pathways that project from the NTS to the hypothalamus are activated. As a
consequence, the HPA axis is inhibited, as evidenced by a decrease in plasma cortisol
levels. In addition, the secretion of oxytocin is stimulated not only from the pituitary
but also from the nerves that project to different areas of the brain. Oxytocin
has been shown to exert anxiolytic-like and sedative effects by way of central
mechanisms, in part by actions in the amygdala and in the LC. This may, therefore,
mediate the calming effect of physical contact. It also elevates the pain threshold by
an action in the periaqueductal gray and in the spinal cord (Figures 2A, 2B and 2C).
Figure 2A. Schematic illustration of nervous system connections between the skin and
the brain: A) vagal afferents.
Many different sources of information have shown that early skin-to-skin contact
between mothers and infants offers long-term benefits.46 If allowed skin-to-skin
contact after birth, mothers interact more frequently with their babies and have
more milk and lower gastrin levels 4 days later.47 In addition, they present a more
pulsatile pattern of oxytocin. The number of pulses is related to the sociability of
the mothers and how much milk they give their babies.39,48 In addition, continued
breastfeeding is known to be related to sustained beneficial psychological and
physiological effects.8 According to personality trait scales, these mothers are more
calm in the sense that they prefer a calm and regular lifestyle.8 They have also
proved to be more interested in social interactions,49,50 have lower BPs and react
with lesser levels of cortisol in response to physical stress.51
Taken together, these data indicate that mothers who are breastfeeding and who
are exposed to repeated releases of oxytocin develop a psychophysiological pattern
characterized by increased social competence, as well as by relaxation and calm—
exactly the effect pattern caused by repeated administrations of oxytocin in rats.15
Although not yet described in the literature, it is possible that endogenous
“opioidergic,” as well as α2-receptor, function has been increased as a result of
repeated treatments with oxytocin. In support of a role for oxytocin in creating
long-term antistress effects in mothers who are breastfeeding are the findings of
positive correlations between plasma oxytocin levels and calm personality traits,
socialization and social dependency in individual mothers.48-50
200 Section IV. Effects of Massage on Sleep, Relaxation and Well-Being
Clinical data suggest that massage given to humans produces many of the
physiological signs characteristic of relaxation and well-being, as seen in mothers
who are breastfeeding.8 Furthermore, experimental data obtained from rats
suggest a direct link between enhanced levels of oxytocin due to massage and
the psychophysiological state of relaxation and well-being.51 Each rat (both males
and females) was stroked on their ventral side for 5 minutes at a rate of 40 strokes
per minute, the frequency found to be the most effective.51 This treatment lowers
BP, causes sedation, elevates pain threshold and influences the release of GI
hormones.18,26,52 In support of a role for oxytocin in the coordination of the
massage-induced effects, levels of oxytocin were enhanced in the circulation, as
well as in the cerebrospinal fluid, in response to this treatment.51 Furthermore,
an oxytocin antagonist blocked some of the massage-induced effects, such as an
elevated pain threshold.18 Interestingly, a sustained anxiolytic-like effect enhanced
nociceptive threshold, lowered BP and decreased fasting levels of the vagally
controlled hormone gastrin in response to repeated massage-like treatments.
Recent experiments also showed that rats learned conditioned avoidance more
quickly and were more interactive socially after being given this massage-like
treatment.53 The rats treated with the massage-like stroking also gained more
weight than did the control rats.53 These effects on the rats are, in fact, exactly the
same as those described previously, which occur in response to repeated injections
of oxytocin.15 Also, the administration of an oxytocin antagonist counteracts most
of these effects. Taken together, these data support the idea that oxytocin released
by the massage-like stroking may be involved in the effects induced by this
treatment in adults.8
The fact that gastrin levels decreased in response to the massage-like treatment
warrants some comments.8 The hormone gastrin is well-known for its role as
a stimulator of secretion.13 It is released in response to the presence of food
constituents in the stomach and by vagal nerve activity. In fact, the vagal nerve
serves dual roles, as it may stimulate as well as inhibit gastrin release. The latter
“inhibitory” effect is mediated via a cholinergic mechanism.13 Oxytocinergic
neurons project from the paraventricular nucleus to the vagal motor nucleus9
and intracerebral administration of oxytocin may influence the release of gastrin,
suggesting that oxytocin may influence the levels of gastrin via central
Chapter 12: Massage, Relaxation and Well-Being: A Possible Role for Oxytocin 201
Eff e c t s o f Ma s s a ge o n Ho r m o ne Le ve ls in Humans
In the studies performed by Field and colleagues, massage was delivered to humans
of different ages who had different types of disease.1-3 In addition to a reduction
of the intensity of specific symptoms found in these studies, more general findings
were that massage reduced the levels of cortisol, it decreased BP and it induced an
anxiolytic-like effect. All of these effects are consistent with the hypothesis presented
previously in this chapter that nonnoxious sensory stimulation promotes social
interaction and reduces the levels of anxiety and stress.
We have suggested that oxytocin plays an integrative role in this system.14 Very
few studies, however, have actually demonstrated a release of oxytocin in response
to massage in humans. The study mentioned previously in this chapter, in which
infant massage released oxytocin in mothers, represents an exception in this
regard,40 as does a study by Turner and colleagues, who also demonstrated the
release of oxytocin in some adults following massage.55 In a recent study undertaken
with several of my colleagues, a clear rise was observed in the levels of oxytocin
in 10 humans following massage that involved stroking of the shoulders, chest and
the abdominal area (Uvnas-Moberg et al, unpublished data). The response—values
of oxytocin that were approximately doubled (Figure 3)—which was transient,
appeared 10 minutes or 20 minutes after the start of the massage. In another recent
study performed with 30 humans—in which no stroking was applied, but muscles
were relaxed by applying light pressure—my colleagues and I also observed a
significant release of oxytocin in the massage therapist, particularly at the beginning
of treatment (Uvnas-Moberg et al, unpublished data). Those who received massage
in this study displayed clear declines in the levels of GI hormones, especially
gastrin, but also in the levels of cholecystokinin (Figure 4). Thus, results of this
study indicate that the effects of massage on hormone release are bidirectional,
since both the massage therapist and those receiving massage were affected. In
addition, these results demonstrate that oxytocin is released following various types
202 Section IV. Effects of Massage on Sleep, Relaxation and Well-Being
of massage. In the studies described previously, the rise in the levels of oxytocin
was observed in response to stroking movements and following touch and light
pressure.40,55
Figure 3. Levels of oxytocin in 10 humans before and after receiving stroking and
light-pressure massage.57
Before Massage
After Massage
As discussed previously, a subset of sensory nerves, which join the sensory fibers
of the vagal nerve, can stimulate the CNS from the skin of the chest and the
204 Section IV. Effects of Massage on Sleep, Relaxation and Well-Being
Su mm a r y
Hormone levels are used to evaluate the effects of massage on humans. Cortisol
levels in humans have been noted to decrease following massage and are assumed to
reflect an antistress effect caused by the massage. Increased oxytocin levels following
massage may be related to antistress-like effects and sociability, whereas lowered lev-
els of gastrin following massage may be a marker of a massage-induced increase in
social interests and skills.
Chapter 12: Massage, Relaxation and Well-Being: A Possible Role for Oxytocin 205
Re f e re n c e s
1. Field T. Massage therapy for infants and children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
Pediatrics: JDBP. 1995;16:105-111.
2. Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Hart S, Theakston H, Schanberg S, Kuhn C. Pregnant women
benefit from massage therapy. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynaecology. 1999;20:
31-38.
3. Hernandez-Reif M, Field T, Diego M, Beutler J. Evidence-based medicine and massage.
Pediatrics. 2001;108:1053.
4. Schanberg S, Field TM. Maternal deprivation and supplemental stimulation. In: Field TM,
McCabe PM, Schneiderman N, eds. Stress and Coping: Across Development. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
5. Cannon WB. The Wisdom of the Body. New York, NY: Norton; 1932.
6. Uvnäs-Moberg K. Oxytocin-linked antistress effects — the relaxation and growth response.
Acta Physiologica Scandinavica Supplementum. 1997;161(suppl 640):38-42.
16. Windle RJ, Shanks N, Lightman SL, Ingram CD. Central oxytocin administration reduces
stress-induced corticosterone release and anxiety behavior in rats. Endocrinology.
1997;138:2829-2834.
17. McCarthy MM, Altemus M. Central nervous system actions of oxytocin and modulation
of behavior in humans. Molecular Medicine Today. 1997;3:269-275.
31. Rajkowski J, Kubiak P, Ivanova S, Aston Jones G. State related activity, reactivity of locus
ceruleus neurons in behaving monkeys. In: Richard P, Moos F, Freund-Mercier M-J. Central
effects of oxytocin. Physiological Reviews. 1991;71:331-370.
32. Stock S, Uvnäs-Moberg K. Increased plasma levels of oxytocin in response to afferent
electrical stimulation of the sciatic and vagal nerves and in response to touch and pinch in
anaesthetized rats. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica. 1988;132:29-34.
33. Kendrick KM, Keverne EB, Baldwin BA, Sharman DF. Cerebrospinal fluid levels of
acetylcholinesterase, monamines and oxytocin during labour, parturition, vaginocervical
stimulation, lamb separation and suckling in sheep. Neuroendocrinology. 1986;44:149-156.
34. Neumann ID, Krömer SA, Toschi N, Ebner K. Brain oxytocin inhibits the (re)activity of the
hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis in male rats: involvement of hypothalamic and limbic
brain regions. Regulatory Peptides. 2000;96:31-38.
35. Uvnäs-Moberg K. Physiological and endrocine effects of social contact. Annals of the New
York Academy of Sciences. 1997;807:146-163.
36. Vallbo AB, Olausson H, Wessberg J. Unmyelinated afferents constitute a second system
coding tactile stimuli of the human hairy skin. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;81:
2753-2763.
37. Eriksson M, Lundeberg T, Uvnäs-Moberg K. Studies on cutaneous blood flow in the
mammary gland of lactating rats. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica. 1996;158:1-6.
38. Widström AM, Ransjö-Arvidson AB, Christensson K, Matthiesen AS, Winberg J,
Unväs-Moberg K. Gastric suction in healthy newborn infants. Effects on circulation and
developing feeding behaviour. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica. 1987;76:566-572.
39. Nissen E, Uvnäs-Moberg K, Svensson K, Stock S, Widström AM, Winberg J. Different
patterns of oxytocin, prolactin but not cortisol release during breastfeeding in women
delivered by caesarean section or by the vaginal route. Early Human Development.
1996;45:103-118.
47. Widström AM, Wahlberg V, Matthiesen AS, et al. Short-term effects of early suckling and
touch of the nipple on maternal behavior. Early Human Development. 1990;21:153-163.
48. Uvnäs-Moberg K, Widström AM, Nissen E, Björvell H. Personality traits in women 4 days
postpartum and their correlation with plasma levels of oxytocin and prolactin. Journal of
Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynaecology. 1990;11:261-273.
49. Nissen E, Gustavsson P, Widström AM, Uvnäs-Moberg K. Oxytocin, prolactin, milk
production and their relationship with personality traits in women after vaginal delivery
or Cesarean section. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynaecology. 1998;19:49-58.
50. Altemus M, Redwine LS, Leong YM, Frye CA, Porges SW, Carter CS. Responses to
laboratory psychosocial stress in postpartum women. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2001;63:
814-821.
53. Lund I, Yu LC, Uvnäs-Moberg K, et al. Repeated massage-like stimulation induces long-term
effects on nociception: contribution of oxytocinergic mechanisms. The European Journal of
Neuroscience. 2002;16:330-338.
54. Björkstrand E, Ahlenius S, Smedh U, Uvnäs-Moberg K. The oxytocin receptor antagonist
1-deamino-2-D-Tyr-(OEt)-4-Thr-8-Orn-oxytocin inhibits effects of the 5-HT1A receptor
agonist 8-OH-DPAT on plasma levels of insulin, cholecystokinin and somatostatin.
Regulatory Peptides. 1996;63:47-52.
55. Turner RA, Altemus M, Enos T, Cooper B, McGuiness T. Preliminary research on plasma
oxytocin in normal cycling women: investigating emotion and interpersonal distress.
Psychiatry. 1999;62:97-113.
S E C T I O N V.
MASSAGE THERAPY
P E D I AT R I C P RO B L E M S
CHAPTER 13:
MASSAGE THERAPY
CHRONIC ILLNESSES
Ab s t ra c t
Research on massage in the Philippines has revealed its positive effects on many
functions in infants and children. These findings have led to widespread support
of massage therapy in the local pediatric community. This chapter focuses on the
results of studies from research in the Philippines on the effects of massage therapy
on infant behavior, weight gain, arterial oxygen tension, pain reduction, stress
hormones and immunoglobulins, asthma and labor. Massage therapy research
on neglected and abused children and burn management is also reviewed.
212 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
Introd uc t i o n
The massaging of infants provides a gentle and positive stimulus for newborns.
This touch therapy is a significant way to maintain and enhance the attachment
process between parents and baby. One of the major goals and concerns of
pediatricians in the Philippines has been to heighten the neurodevelopmental
outcome of children. The value of touch has been supported by several clinically
significant studies that highlight the benefits of this relatively inexpensive medical
intervention. In many countries, therapeutic massage has been supported by
numerous scientific studies, including those conducted at the Touch Research
Institute of the Philippine Children’s Medical Center. In these countries, massage
Chapter 13: Massage Therapy With Preterm Infants and Children With Chronic Illnesses 213
Weight Gain
Ar t e r i a l O x y ge n Te ns i o n
babies who were given 15-minute massages, once daily, for 3 consecutive days.
The functional, inspired O2 required to maintain optimal O2 saturation in
“graduates” of the neonatal intensive-care unit (NICU) was reduced notably.
The study also revealed that massaged babies experienced significantly higher
transcutaneous O2 levels and a lower fractional concentration of O2 in inspired
gas and pressure requirements compared with control babies who did not receive
massage therapy (Table 2).
Table 1. Mean and Standard Deviations for the Formula Intake and Weight Gain
Massage Group Control
(n = 20) (n = 20)
Measures M SD M SD P Value
Number of feeds/day 12 0 12 0 NS
NS = not significant
Pa i n Re d uc t i o n
As early as the moment of birth, the infant can already feel pain. Early studies of
neonatal development suggested that the neonate’s response to pain was decorticate
in nature, and that the perception or localization of pain was not present.11,12
This supposition was believed for many years and was applied particularly to
preterm infants who were thought to be incapable of feeling pain since they
lacked the fundamental neuronal pathways to transmit messages regarding painful
experiences. However, current evidence suggests that pain pathways and the
neurotransmitters involved in the transmission of impulses relating to noxious
stimuli are well developed at an early stage in gestation and that infants respond
to noxious stimuli in a variety of ways.13
Infants in the NICU are handled constantly, and the tactile stimuli they receive
are often painful and uncomfortable. Unfortunately, there are innumerable ways
in which a neonate experiences pain in a modern NICU: Pain-producing
interventions—from intravenous lines to nasogastric tubes, from nasal catheters
to indwelling cannulas, and from intubations to ventilations—cannot be avoided
completely if seriously ill babies are to survive. It would be beneficial for the
infants, of course, if the staff in the NICU was trained in massage therapy
techniques and allotted adequate time for this intervention.
Table 3. Effects of Massage Therapy and Sucrose on Reducing Pain Following Heel
Sticks in Infants16
Median n = 17 n = 17 n = 17 Results
Before Tx
O2 Saturation (%) 0 0 0 NS
NIPS 97 98 97 NS
After Tx
Cry Duration (min) 2.67 2.50 4.00 P<.05
St re s s Ho r m o ne s a nd Im m u no gl obulins
In a study by Serafica and Cifra involving critically ill infants and children,
massage therapy reduced the levels of many stress hormones, including ACTH,
cortisol, triiodothyronine (T3) and thyroxine (T4) (Table 4).20 It also increased
the levels of IgG, IgA and IgM in ill infants (median age, 4.1 months).
In these studies, which were conducted in the NICU and the pediatric ICU,
other positive effects were noted anecdotally: better mother-infant bonding and
nurse-patient-family interactions and greater family involvement in childcare.
In addition, the incidence of breastfeeding was greater and a better acceptance
of rooming-in practices was observed. Nurses also reported feeling greater job
satisfaction. Parents who were initially tired and hesitant to touch their babies
subsequently became relaxed and happy as a direct result of touching their infants,
as did surrogate parents and the extended family of grandparents, uncles, aunts
and older brothers and sisters who often substituted for the parents in applying
massage.
218 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
P value ≤.05
Chapter 13: Massage Therapy With Preterm Infants and Children With Chronic Illnesses 219
He a li n g
Moderate pressure massage can enhance the body’s energies to promote healing. In
addition to reducing the levels of certain types of pain, particularly acute pain, it
can alleviate the anxiety that often accompanies pain as well as alter the perception
of pain. The “gate control theory” of pain, a potential explanation for the alleviation
of pain,12 states that inhibitory interneurons that block input from the classic
nociceptors to the spinal cord are activated by mechanisms that decrease stress.
A st hma
Table 5. The Effect of Massage Therapy on Pulmonary Function Test and Patient
Satisfaction of Asthmatic Patients22
Pain Assessment
0.64 0.48 0.16 <.00001* 0.63 0.56 0.06 <.05†
Scale (Guilford, 1990)
FEF (25%-75%) 86.2 100.8 14.6 .02 * 98.9 102.7 3.8 <.05†
*Significant; †Not Significant; P-value = <.05; FVC= Forced Vital Capacity; FEV= Forced Expiratory Volume;
PEV= Peak Expiratory Velocity; FEF 25%-75%= Forced Expiratory Flow at 25%-75% of FVC; (% of Predicted)
220 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
Labor
Support during labor is centuries old. According to Klaus and colleagues,23 the
challenge is to use obstetric technology only when necessary, relying instead on
the practice of continuous support during labor to help the birth process follow its
natural course. This concept maximizes the importance of the doula (a woman who
gives care to a pregnant woman during labor). Studies worldwide have revealed that
support given by a doula reduces the use of epidural anesthesia by 60% and the
rate of C-sections by 30%.24 This intervention has been shown to play a significant
role in unwanted teenage pregnancies: Sarte and Cifra found that unmarried,
teenage mothers who were given care by a doula during labor experienced reduced
levels of anxiety and pain, shorter lengths of labor and less need for medication
(Table 6).25 Reduced uses of forceps and oxytocin were also noted.
*Statistically significant
Chapter 13: Massage Therapy With Preterm Infants and Children With Chronic Illnesses 221
Infants whose mothers received support from a doula have been found to receive
more care and touch in the first 20 minutes after birth.26 The incidences of
breastfeeding and demand-feeding were also higher in the group cared for by
a doula. Since health problems in these infants also appeared to decrease, the
pediatricians at the Philippine General Hospital are actively promoting the use
of doulas to their obstetricians.
Fu t ure Di re c t i o ns
Several studies involving touch and massage are in progress or are in the planning
stages. Ongoing studies are being conducted on neglected and sex-abused children.
It is expected that touch therapy will decrease victims’ anxiety, depression and
aversion to touch. It may also hasten their ability to establish new relationships.
In addition, children with autism are expected to benefit from massage therapy:
Massage therapy has improved relationships between these special pediatric patients
and their caregivers as well as their social environments.27
Touch therapy is being used currently to hasten the return of sensory perception
and responsiveness in comatose children (unpublished data). Massage may, in fact,
enhance circulation and improve mental functioning. Levels of cortisol have
decreased notably in comatose patients during massage therapy. This benefit
has been accompanied by heightened alertness, based on electroencephalograms
showing a decrease in alpha brain waves and increases in beta and theta brain
waves—alterations consistent with heightened alertness. These measures, it appears,
will continue to be used on comatose children.
Massage therapy has been shown to reduce the incidence and severity of headache
and abdominal colic.28 Infant colic is a very common complaint presented by
parents. Massage of the infant during episodes of colic and prior to bedtime may
enhance food absorption, increase positive feeding interactions with caregivers,
result in more-organized sleep/wake behaviors and reduce irritability and stress
behaviors.
Research shows that the management of burns may improve with massage by
raising the threshold of pain.29 Extensive physical handling of the burn patient
222 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
Massage therapy has been shown to alter the levels of stress hormones, such as
cortisol,32 and growth hormones, gastrin and insulin, in urine, saliva and blood.33
Gastrointestinal (GI) hormones, such as gastrin and insulin, are essential for the
process of food absorption, which, when enhanced, leads to weight gain. Previous
findings suggest that massage appears to increase vagal activity. This, in turn,
stimulates the production and release of the GI hormones.34 Studies conducted
at the Philippine General Hospital determined how touch can alter the level of
these hormones.35
Also being investigated is the best way to apply touch. Investigations are assessing
whether whole-body touch, touch of areas specifically related to the condition,
or procedure-related touch are indicated in specific situations. Studies are also
evaluating the use of oils and other materials as mediators to enhance the
applications of touch. Results of these various studies will soon be disseminated
to health workers and massage therapists in order to deliver this cost-effective
intervention most effectively.
Conclusions
Ackn ow le d gm e nt s
This research was supported by the National Institute of Health of the Philippines,
and by Johnson & Johnson Baby Company of the Philippines.
224 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
Ref ere n c e s
1. Ottenbacher KJ, Muller L, Brandt D, Heintzelman A, Hojem P, Sharpe P. The effectiveness
of the tactile stimulation as a form of early intervention: a quantitative evaluation. Journal
of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 1987;8:68-76.
2. White J, Labarba R. The effects of tactile and kinesthetic stimulation on neonatal
development in the premature infant. Developmental Pscyhobiology. 1976;9:569-577.
3. Kuhn C, Schanberg S, Field T, et al. Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation effects on sympathetic
and adrenocortical function in preterm infants. Journal of Pediatrics. 1991;119:434-440.
4. Scholz K, Samuels C. Neonatal bathing and massage intervention with fathers, behavioral
effects 12 weeks after birth of the first baby: the Sunrayasia Australia Intervention Project.
International Journal of Behavior Development. 1992;15:67-81.
5. Schanberg S, Field T. Sensory deprivation stress and supplemental stimulation in the rat pup
and preterm human neonate. Child Development. 1987; 5-8:1431-1447.
6. Klaus MH, Kennell JH, Plumb N, Zuehlke S. Human maternal behavior at the first contact
with her young. Pediatrics. 1970;46:187-192.
7. Righard L, Alade MO. Effects of delivery room routines on success of first breast-feed.
Lancet. 1990;77:1105-1107.
8. Jinon S, Yabes-Almirante C, de Luna MB, eds. The effect infant massage on growth of
pre-term infants. Increasingly Safe and Successful Pregnancies. Netherlands: Elsevier Science BV,
1996:264-269.
9. Wheeden A, Scafidi FA, Field T, Ironson G, Valdeon C, Bandstra E. Massage effects on
cocaine-exposed pre-term neonates. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics.
1994;14:318-322.
10. Tirador MH, Bote JR, Cifra H, Hernandez EA. The effect of massage therapy on arterial
oxygen and carbon dioxide tension of patients on continuous positive airway pressure
(CPAP). J&J Phil. Clinical Reprints. 1998.
11. Anand KJS, Hickey PR. Pain and its effects in the human neonate and fetus. The New
England Journal of Medicine. 1987;19:1321-1329.
12. Bogdeck N. Understanding pain pathways. Medical Progress. March 1989;38-48.
13. Rushfort JA, et al. Behavioral response to pain in healthy neonates. Archives of Diseases
in Childhood. 1994;70:174-176.
14. Tobias JD, Rasmussen GE. Pain management and sedation in pediatric intensive care unit.
Pediatric Clinics of North America. 1994;41:1269-1292.
15. Dimaano-Aliwalas, Cifra HL. Massage therapy as a modality of pain relief among neonates.
UP-PGH, Johnson & Johnson Clinical Reprints. 1998
16. Jarumahum J, Cifra HL. Sucrose and massage: modalities to pain relief in routine neonatal
procedures. UP-PGH, Johnson & Johnson Clinical Reprints. 1997.
Chapter 13: Massage Therapy With Preterm Infants and Children With Chronic Illnesses 225
17. Senturias JSN, Cifra HL, Ortiz EE, Angtuaco L, Camagay I. The use of touch therapy as
a modality to decrease the pain of intramascular injection of benzathine penicillin G among
pediatric patients. UP-PGH, Johnson & Johnson Clinical Reprints. 1998.
18. Uvnas-Moberg K, Windberg J. Release of AI hormones in mother and infant by sensory
stimulation. Aeta Paediatrica Scandinavica. 1987;76:851-860.
19. Acolet D, et al. Changes in plasma cortisol and catecholamine concentrations in response
to massage in preterm infants. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 1993;68:29-31.
20. Serafica EM, Cifra HL. The effect of massage on the level of serum stress hormones and
serum immunoglobulins in critically ill infants and children. UP-PGH, Johnson & Johnson
Clinical Reprints. 1999.
21. Field T, Henteleff T, Hernandez-Reif M, et al. Children with asthma have improved
pulmonary functions after massage therapy. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1998;132:854-858.
22. Balanag E, Alviado J, Cifra HL. The effect of massage therapy on pulmonary function test
and patient satisfaction of asthmatic patients. Pediatric Capsule. Vol 1 No. 1;2002.
23. Klaus MH, Kennell JH, Robertson SS. The effects of social support during parturition
on maternal and infant morbidity. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed). 1986;293:585-587.
24. Sosa R, Kennell JH, Robertsomn S, Urrutia J. The effect of a supportive companion on
perinatal problems, length of labor and mother-infant interaction. The New England Journal
of Medicine. 1980;303:597-600.
25. Sarte CL, Cifra HL. Doula support in unwanted pregnancy-effects on progress and
perceptions of labor. UP-PGH, Johnson & Johnson Clinical Reprints. 1995.
26. Hofmeyer GJ, Nikodem VC, Wolman WL. Companionship to modify the clinical birth
environment: effects on progress and perceptions of labour and breastfeeding. British Journal
of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 1991;98:756-764.
27. Field T, Lasko D, Mundy P, Henteleff T, Talpins S, Dowling M. Autistic children’s
attentiveness and responsitivity improved after touch therapy. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders. 1996;27:329-334.
28. Lester BM, Boukydis CFZ, Garcia-Coll CT, Hole WT. Colic for developmentalists. Infant
Mental Health Journal. 1990;11:4:321-333.
29. Field T, Peck M, Krugman S, et al. Burn injuries benefit from massage therapy. Journal of
Burn Care and Rehabilitation. 1998;19:241-244.
30. Field T, Cullen C, Diego M, et al. Leukemia immune changes following massage therapy.
2001;5:271-274.
31. Ferrel-Torry AT, Glick OJ. The use of therapeutic massage as a nursing intervention to
modify anxiety and the perception of cancer pain. Review Cancer Nursing. 1993;16:93-101.
33. Van Wyk JJ, Underwood LE. Growth hormone, somatomedins and growth failure. Hospital
Practice. 1978;13:57-67.
36. Spense JE, Olsem MA. The therapeutic touch 1975-1995; an integrated review of the
literature. Scand J Caring Sci. 1975;11:183-190.
CHAPTER 14:
ON ILLNESS SYMPTOMS
IN INFANTS LIVING IN
ECUADORIAN ORPHANAGES
Ab s t ra c t
Introd uc t i o n
Research involving young, nonhuman primates has indicated that being separated
from their mothers has a negative impact on their immune systems.2,3 An increase
in cortisol levels, as well as changes in other behavioral and physiological indicators,
accompanies this separation in nonhuman primates, which, in turn, negatively
affects the immune system.4 Some research has indicated prolonged negative effects
of maternal separation on the immune system of nonhuman primates.5 Worlein
and Laudenslager asserted that interactions between mother figures and their
peers are necessary for normal immunocompetence.4 They also stated that infant
monkeys reared in nurseries have different behavioral and immune tendencies
than young monkeys raised with their mothers.4 Even when infant monkeys were
raised by humans and were allowed to socialize with other monkeys, their immune
systems were compromised, although they did not experience behavioral deficits.6
These findings may transfer to the human population. Infants who are separated
from their parents, such as those living in orphanages where emotional and physical
care are often neglectful, may experience behavioral and health consequences.
Children adopted into the USA often arrive with very incomplete medical histories.
More complete health-status-related information often becomes available about
these children after their arrival into the USA. Johnson and colleagues found
that only 15% of children adopted from Romania were physically healthy and/or
developmentally normal when they were examined in the USA.8 Similarly, Albers
and colleagues reported that many children adopted into the USA from the
former Soviet Union and other eastern European countries were diagnosed with a
variety of conditions, the most common (51%) being intestinal pathogens.9 These
were retrospective studies that occurred after adoptions by families in the USA.
Even when health-status information exists from the native country, Albers and
colleagues stated that it tends to be sporadic and often incorrect.9 Although only
scant information is available regarding the health status of children housed in
Chapter 14: Massage Therapy Effects on Illness Symptoms in Infants 229
Cur re n t St u d y
METHOD/PARTICIPANTS
Group
Massage Therapy Play Control
(n = 14) (n = 16)
Variable Mean SD(±) Mean SD(±) P Value
INTERVENTION
Infants in the massage therapy group (n = 14) received one, 15-minute massage
daily, usually in the morning, delivered by orphanage volunteers or staff, all of
whom were trained in infant massage techniques endorsed by the American
Association of Infant Massage. Contrastingly, volunteers interacted with infants in
the play control group (n = 16) for 15 minutes daily in individual play sessions.
The first day of the intervention for the infants began after assignment to a group
and the collection of pretest information: As such, these infants began the study on
different days. Orphanage volunteers were instructed to record daily observations,
usually each morning, of all visible symptoms of illness experienced by the infants.
Symptoms noted included the following: upper respiratory symptoms, including
Chapter 14: Massage Therapy Effects on Illness Symptoms in Infants 231
The average duration of this experimental intervention was 53 days. The massage
therapy group infants received massage more often than the play control group
infants received play sessions during those 53 days (mean, 35.7 days versus 20.4
days, respectively; P<.001). The same was true during Day 15 to Day 45 of the
study period (mean, 20.7 days massage therapy sessions versus 13.7 days play
sessions, respectively; P<.001). This chapter reports data from Day 15 through
Day 45 of the study period. The first 14 days served as a baseline to establish the
typical infant-health status before using infant-illness data to determine whether
massage was correlated positively with a decrease in the incidence of illness.
SITE DESCRIPTIONS
Site 1 was a Catholic orphanage that, at the time, had 31 infants living in the
nursery. Infants were either brought in by their parents or found abandoned and
brought in by the police. The sanitary conditions were optimal, with an emphasis
placed on cleanliness in most areas. The floors in the nursery area, which included
6 rooms and one hallway, were mopped twice daily. Infants’ clothing was washed
on a daily basis, as were their crib sheets; a necessity, since the infants’ cloth diapers
often soiled both clothing and bed sheets. However, the toys that the infants
played with daily were washed on only a monthly basis. Normally, 17 to 26 infants
older than 6 months of age played in one large room where they were supervised
by 3 to 4 volunteers. On days when the volunteers were not in the orphanage
(approximately 1 of every 14 days), infants were often left to play unsupervised
in the large room, although nuns and orphanage staff were in the other rooms
in the nursery area. Infants younger than 6 months of age were kept in another
room and were taken out of their cribs for diaper changes and baths, but not for
individual stimulation.
232 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
Site 2 was a private orphanage that, at the time, had 8 infants and young children
in its care. Infants in this orphanage were brought in by their parents. Sanitary
conditions were similar to those found at Site 1. Normally, 2 volunteers supervised
6 to 8 infants and young children during their “playtime.” On days that volunteers
were not present, the children over the age of 6 months typically played by
themselves, although at least 2 staff members were always present. Similar to Site 1,
infants less than 6 months of age were normally taken out of their cribs for diaper
changes and baths, but not for individual stimulation.
Result s
Group
Data From Massage Therapy Play Control
Day 15 to Day 45 (n = 14) (n = 16)
of Intervention Mean SD(±) Mean SD(±) Effect Size P Value
Days without symptoms (n) 7.2 6.3 3.9 3.3 1.00 .08
Days data were missing (n) 8.4 5.8 8.3 5.4 .01 .98
group infants experienced diarrhea less frequently and exhibited a more positive
temperament than the play control group infants.
Group
Symptoms Experienced Massage Therapy Play Control
From Day 15 to Day 45 (n = 14) (n = 16)
of Intervention Mean SD(±) Mean SD(±) Effect Size P Value
Yellow/green-runny
1.8 2.1 1.1 1.5 .48 .28
or snotty nose
Whiny or Fussier Than Normal 0.8 1.2 1.8 2.3 .41 .16
Although these findings were not statistically significant at the P<.05 level, they
did approach statistical significance. Given that this was a pilot study on a small
sample, my colleagues and I opted to use “effect sizes” to determine the practical
significance of the results. Based on findings reported by Cohen, an effect size of
.40 or higher is considered indicative of “practical significance.”17 As can be seen
in Table 3, most effect sizes (8 of 11) exceeded this benchmark.
234 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
Im pli c a t i o n s
Results of this pilot project suggest that infant massage may reduce both the
number of days that children in orphanages experience symptoms of illness and the
types of symptoms they experience. The finding that infants in the massage therapy
group averaged more days with no symptoms of illness, despite being in the same
environment as infants in the play control group, warrants further research on the
effects of massage on decreasing infants’ susceptibility to illness. Data gathered on
the levels of urinary cortisol remain to be analyzed. Because there is an established
link between elevated levels of cortisol and compromised immune response,
infants who experienced fewer days of the symptoms of illness are likely to have
experienced decreased levels of cortisol over time.
With respect to the massage therapy group infants experiencing more yellow/green
runny noses and more incidents of crusty eyes (Table 3), a pediatrician colleague
(Odell, personal communication) indicated that this, in fact, could have been
an indication of the babies getting the infection or illness out of their bodies.
However, it would also be imperative to know whether the infants in the massage
therapy group experienced a shorter duration of illness each time. It was not
possible to conduct this analysis during this study because of the large number of
days (mean, 8.4 days) for which we had no data during the course of the illnesses
(Table 2). Future similar studies would need to have more reliable symptom
reporting to allow for this type of analysis.
Another interesting finding was that infants in the massage therapy group
experienced diarrhea less frequently than did infants in the play control group.
Annually, approximately 2 million children worldwide under the age of 5 die as
a result of diarrhea and associated dehydration.18 However, because this study was
a pilot project that enrolled relatively few infants, no conclusions can be drawn.
Nevertheless, the effect sizes obtained indicate that further research is needed on
the effectiveness of massage in preventing or ameliorating diarrhea.
My colleagues and I are planning to replicate this work in other orphanages, with
larger numbers of children, to better understand the effects of massage on children’s
immediate and long-term health outcomes. Several study improvements are
necessary, including stricter adherence to the intervention protocol and to daily
Chapter 14: Massage Therapy Effects on Illness Symptoms in Infants 235
Ackn owledgments
This project was made possible through a research grant awarded by the American
Massage Therapy Association Foundation to Vonda K. Jump, PhD, Senior Research
Associate at the Early Intervention Research Institute at Utah State University,
Logan, Utah. Special thanks go to the Ecuadorian orphanage staffs, the Orphanage
Support Services Organization and other volunteers, as well as to the babies for
allowing me to work with them. Thanks to Bianca for teaching me about massage.
236 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
Ref ere n c e s
1. Reite M. Effects of touch on the immune system. In: Gunzenhauser N, ed.; Advances in
Touch: New Implications in Human Development. USA: Johnson & Johnson Consumer
Products, Inc.; 1990.
2. Laudenslager ML, Reite M, Harbeck RJ. Suppressed immune response in infant monkeys
associated with maternal separation. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1982;36:40-48.
3. Reite M, Harbeck R, Hoffman A. Altered cellular immune response following peer
separation. Life Sciences. 1981;29:1333-1336.
4. Worlein JM, Laudenslager ML. Effects of early rearing experiences and social interactions
on immune function in nonhuman primates. In: Ader R, Felten DL, Cohen N, eds.
Psychoneuroimmunology. Vol 1. 3rd ed. Boston, Mass.: Academic Press; 2001:73-85.
5. Laudenslager ML, Capitanio JP, Reite ML. Possible effects of early separation experiences on
subsequent immune function in adult macaque monkeys. The American Journal of Psychiatry.
1985;142:862-864.
6. Worlein JM, Sackett, GP. Social development in nursery-reared pigtailed macaques (Macaca
nemestrina). American Journal of Primatology. 1997;41:23-35.
8. Johnson DE, Miller LC, Iverson S, et al. The health of children adopted from Romania.
JAMA. 1992;268:3446-3451.
9. Albers LH, Johnson DE, Hostetter MK, Iverson S, Miller LC. Health of children adopted
from the former Soviet Union and Eastern Europe: comparison with preadoptive medical
records. JAMA. 1997;278:922-924.
10. Diego MA, Hernandez-Reif M, Field T, Friedman L, Shaw K. HIV adolescents show
improved immune function following massage therapy. The International Journal of
Neuroscience. 2001;106:35-45.
11. Ironson G, Field T, Scafidi F, et al. Massage therapy is associated with enhancement of
the immune system’s cytotoxic capacity. The International Journal of Neuroscience.
1996;84:205-218.
12. Field T, Schanberg SM, Scafidi F, et al. Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation effects on preterm
neonates. Pediatrics. 1986;77:654-658.
13. Jinon S. The effect of infant massage on growth of the preterm infant. In: Yarbes-Almirante
C, De Luma M, eds. Increasing Safe and Successful Pregnancy. Amsterdam, Netherlands:
Elsevier Science; 1996:265-269.
14. Scafidi FA, Field T, Schanberg SM, et al. Massage stimulates growth in preterm infants:
a replication. Infant Behavior and Development. 1990;13:167-188.
Chapter 14: Massage Therapy Effects on Illness Symptoms in Infants 237
15. Field T. Infant massage therapy. In: Field T, ed. Touch in Early Development. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1995:105-114.
16. Field T, Henteleff T, Hernandez-Reif M, et al. Children with asthma have improved
pulmonary function after massage therapy. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1998;132:854-858.
17. Cohen J. Things I have learned (so far). The American Psychologist. 1990;45:1304-1312.
MASSAGE THERAPY
F O R P E D I AT R I C P RO B L E M S
Abstract
Numerous studies have reported the benefits associated with massage therapy
in children who have various illnesses and conditions. In this chapter studies
are reviewed demonstrating improvements in children with medical and
psychiatric disorders. The benefits of massage therapy have also extended to
those who performed the massages. Conditions discussed in this chapter include
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, autism, posttraumatic stress disorder,
depression, anorexia, bulimia, cerebral palsy, Down syndrome, various
autoimmune disorders, including asthma and diabetes, and immune disorders,
including human immunodeficiency virus and leukemia. Potential underlying
mechanisms that may contribute to the effects of massage are also discussed.
240 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
Atten t i o n a n d Be h a v i o r D i s o rd e r s
ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER
AUTISM
Autism affects 2 to 5 of every 10,000 children. Autism takes many forms with
several different degrees of severity and its causes are still unknown.2 Symptoms
of autism include limited language, social communication and ability to develop
relationships. Also children with autism show abnormal responses to sensory
stimuli (usually sound), atypical movements including immobility and
hyperactivity, a limited attention span and excessive off-task behaviors and an
aversion to touch.
A variety of therapies have been tried in children with autism, including behavior
modification and sensory-integrative approaches. In a recent study we reported
that children with autism who were provided with massage therapy showed
less inattentiveness (off-task behavior), reduced touch aversion and decreased
withdrawal (Figures 1 and 2).2
Chapter 15: Massage Therapy for Pediatric Problems 241
In another study we conducted,3 20 children with autism were given daily massages
by their parents. Ranging in age from 3 years to 6 years, these children were
randomly assigned to massage therapy and reading attention control groups. A
massage therapist trained the parents in the massage therapy group. Every night for
1 month, these parents massaged their children for 15 minutes prior to bedtime,
while the parents in the attention control group read stories by Dr. Seuss to their
children on the same time schedule. Sleep, which is often disturbed in children
with autism, was monitored. Teacher- and parent-rated scales, classroom and
242 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
playground observations and sleep diaries were used to assess the effects of massage
therapy on various behaviors, including hyperactivity, stereotypical and off-task
behaviors and sleep problems. The children in the massage therapy group showed
fewer stereotypical behaviors and more on-task and social-relatedness behaviors
during classroom observations. In addition, the massaged children experienced
fewer sleep problems at home.
Past studies have described posttraumatic responses in children that are similar
to those described for adults with the diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder
(PTSD).4 Symptoms include reduced responsiveness, increased arousal and conduct
problems that persist for several months following the traumatic event.
group who viewed relaxing videotapes, the massaged subjects were less depressed
and less anxious and had lower saliva cortisol levels after the massage. In addition,
nurses rated the massaged children as being more cooperative on the last day of
the study, and nighttime sleep increased over this 5-day period. Lastly, cortisol and
norepinephrine levels were decreased.
ANOREXIA
Anorexia, a body weight disorder affecting women almost exclusively, has increased
in incidence over the past 40 years.6 Symptoms of anorexia are extreme thinness
and poor body image. In a recent study we observed adolescent women with
anorexia who received massage therapy twice per week for 5 weeks and those
who received standard treatment alone.6 The women who received massage
reported lower stress levels and anxiety levels and had lower cortisol levels. Over
the 5-week treatment period, these women also reported improved body image
on the Eating Disorder Inventory and they experienced increased dopamine and
norepinephrine levels.
BULIMIA
AGGRESSIVE/VIOLENT ADOLESCENTS
In 1998 the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention published statistics
showing that the violence rate (ie, homicide rate) among adolescent and young
adult males in the US exceeded that of any other industrialized country. Excessive
violence has been noted in adolescents and adults in primitive countries where
244 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
young children receive minimal affection. Touch taboos and mandates not to touch
children in school may be contributing to the high rates of aggression/violence in
the US.
to less depressed affect, which, in turn, could feed back to produce less depressed
feelings.10 Increased vagal activity may also account for the enhanced attentiveness
in the children with ADHD1 and the children with autism.2
CEREBRAL PALSY
DOWN SYNDROME
ASTHMA
Complementary forms of therapy are being explored for children with asthma: For
example, a study of facial-relaxation training documented increases in immediate
peak expiratory flow rates in children.13 However, these increases fell short of the
standard criterion for clinical significance used in evaluating asthma medications—
a 15% increase in air-flow rates (Figure 4).
We evaluated massage therapy with children who have asthma because it has
lowered anxiety and cortisol levels in children who have other problems.14 Also,
massage therapy requires less compliance from children. Another reason for the
effectiveness of massage therapy is that the children’s parents provide the therapy.
Parents of children who have asthma have been shown to experience higher anxiety
Chapter 15: Massage Therapy for Pediatric Problems 247
levels compared with parents of children who do not have asthma—and increased
anxiety on the part of the parents can affect the children’s health.14 Giving parents
an active coping role in their child’s therapy may reduce their own anxiety. In this
study, 32 children with asthma were randomly assigned to receive either massage
therapy or relaxation therapy (control group).14 The children’s parents were taught
to provide the therapy, which was given for 20 minutes before bedtime each
night for 30 days. The children who received massage therapy showed immediate
decreases in anxiety and cortisol levels. Also, their attitudes toward asthma and
their peak air flow and other pulmonary functions improved over the course of
the study (Figure 5).
Figure 5. Effect on peak air-flow rates in children with asthma following 30 days
of massage therapy given by their parents.14
320
309
Peak Air Flow
300
279
280
260
First Day Last Day
CYSTIC FIBROSIS
control group. Parents in the treatment group were asked to give their children a
20-minute massage every night at bedtime for 1 month. Parents in the control
group were requested to read to their children for the same amount of time.
On day 1 and day 30, parents and children answered questions related to their
anxiety levels, and children answered questions related to their mood. In addition,
peak air flow readings were measured in the children. Following the first and last
massage sessions, the children and parents reported lower anxiety levels. Mood and
peak air flow readings also improved for children in the massage therapy group.
These findings suggest that the parents of children with CF may reduce their
anxiety levels by massaging their children, and that the children may benefit from
receiving the massage by having less anxiety and improved mood, which, in turn,
may facilitate breathing.
DIABETES
Parents’ involvement in the treatment of their children who have diabetes can be
a negative experience; for example, when monitoring dietary compliance, taking
blood samples and giving insulin injections. We tried to give parents of children
with diabetes a more positive role in their children’s treatment by being called on to
massage their children daily before bedtime for 1 month (Figure 6).18 Immediately
after each massage therapy session, anxiety and depression levels were lower in
both the parents and their children. At the end of the 1-month study period, the
children’s insulin and food-regulation scores improved, and the children’s blood
glucose levels decreased to the normal range (158 mg/dL to 118 mg/dL; Figure 7).
Figure 7. Effects on blood glucose levels in children with diabetes following 1 month of
nightly massage therapy provided by their parents.18
ATOPIC DERMATITIS
Atopic dermatitis (eczema) has been related to increased depression,19 stress and
anxiety,20 which, in turn, lead to increased cortisol levels that then dampen
immune function.21 In a study we conducted, children with atopic dermatitis were
treated with standard topical care and massaged by their parents for 20 minutes
daily for 1 month.22 A control group received standard topical care only. The
children’s affect and activity levels improved significantly, and their parents’ anxiety
levels decreased immediately after finishing each massage therapy session. Over the
span of the 1-month study period, parents of the massaged children reported lower
anxiety levels in their children, and the children improved significantly on all
clinical measures regarding their skin, including redness, scaling, lichenification
(violet patches caused by scratching), excoriation (removal of skin caused by
scratching) and pruritus (itching).
Juvenile rheumatoid arthritis (JRA), one of the most common chronic diseases
of childhood, is the most common rheumatic disease of this period of life.23 The
diagnosis of JRA is based largely on the observation of persistent arthritis (6 weeks
250 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
or more in duration) in one or more of the child’s joints. This disease manifests
itself typically before 16 years of age, with peak onset in the following age groups:
1- to 3-year-old children and 8- to 12-year-old adolescents.24 Common symptoms
of JRA include night pain and joint stiffness in the morning and following long
periods of inactivity.
POSSIBLE MECHANISMS
Im mun e Pro b le m s
to reduce the levels of anxiety and depression, factors associated with decreased
quality of life and diminished immune function.
The adolescents who received massage therapy, versus those who experienced
relaxation therapy, reported feeling less anxious. In addition, they were less
depressed and showed enhanced immune function by the end of the study.
Changes in immune function included increased numbers of natural killer cells
(cells that kill viral cells). In addition, the cells that are normally killed by the
HIV virus increased for the massage therapy group only.
LEUKEMIA
The outlook for children who have acute leukemia has improved steadily, so that
now at least 60% to 70% of children with leukemia achieve long-term cure and
survival.26 We postulated that massage therapy would be indicated for children
with leukemia because immune function (eg, natural killer cell number and natural
killer cell activity) has been enhanced following massage therapy in HIV-positive
adolescents, as discussed previously.25
POSSIBLE MECHANISMS
Stress hormones (eg, cortisol) have been noted to kill immune cells, particularly
natural killer cells (cells that kill viral and cancer cells).25 Since massage therapy
increases vagal activity, which, in turn, decreases cortisol, immune cells can survive.
Su mm a r y
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the children, adolescents and parents who participated in
these studies. This research was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health
(NIMH) Senior Research Scientist Award (#MH00331) and an NIMH Merit
Award (#MH46586) given to Tiffany Field, as well as by funding provided by the
Johnson & Johnson Pediatric Institute, L.L.C. to the Touch Research Institutes.
Chapter 15: Massage Therapy for Pediatric Problems 253
Re f e re n c e s
1. Field TM, Quintino O, Hernandez-Reif M, Koslovsky G. Adolescents with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder benefit from massage therapy. Adolescence. 1998;33:103-108.
2. Field T, Lasko D, Mundy P, et al. Brief report: autistic children’s attentiveness and
responsivity improve after touch therapy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
1997;27:333-338.
3. Escalona A, Field T, Singer-Strunck R, Cullen C, Hartshorn K. Brief report: improvements
in the behavior of children with autism following massage therapy. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders. 2001;31:513-516.
7. Field T, Schanberg S, Kuhn C, et al. Bulimic adolescents benefit from massage therapy.
Adolescence. 1998;33:555-563.
8. Diego MA, Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, et al. Aggressive adolescents benefit from massage
therapy. Adolescence. 2002;37:597-607.
9. Field T. Infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development. 1995;18:1-13.
10. Porges SW. Emotion: an evolutionary by-product of the neural regulation of the autonomic
nervous system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1997;807:62-77.
11. Hernandez-Reif M, Field T, Largie S, et al. Cerebral palsy symptoms in children decreased
following massage therapy. Early Child Care and Development. In review.
12. Hernandez-Reif M, Field T, Largie S, et al. Children with Down syndrome improved in
motor function and muscle tone following massage therapy. Early Child Care and
Development. In review.
13. Kotses H, Hindi-Alexander M, Creer TL. A reinterpretation of psychologically induced
airways changes. The Journal of Asthma: Official Journal of the Association for the Care of
Asthma. 1989;26:53-63.
14. Field T, Henteleff T, Hernandez-Reif M, et al. Children with asthma have improved
pulmonary functions after massage therapy. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1998;132:854-858.
15. Cystic Fibrosis Foundation. About cystic fibrosis: What is CF. Available at:
http://www.cff.org/about_cf/what_is_cf.cfm?CFID=1754499&CFTOKEN=98980601.
Accessed February 11, 2004.
254 Section V. Massage Therapy for Orphans and Pediatric Problems
16. Boucher RC. Cystic fibrosis. In: Isselbachner KJ, Martin JB, Braunwald E, Fauci AS, Wilson
JD, eds. Harrison’s Principles of Internal Medicine. 13th ed. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill;
1994:1194-1197.
17. Hernandez-Reif M, Field T, Krasnegor J, Martinez E, Schwartzman M, Mavunda K.
Children with cystic fibrosis benefit from massage therapy. Journal of Pediatric Psychology.
1999;24:175-181.
19. Gupta MA, Gupta AK, Schork NJ, Ellis CN. Depression modulates pruritus perception:
a study of pruritus in psoriasis, atopic dermatitis, and chronic idiopathic urticaria.
Psychosomatic Medicine. 1994;56:36-40.
20. Horne DJ, White AE, Varigos GA. A preliminary study of psychological therapy in the
management of atopic eczema. The British Journal of Medical Psychology. 1989;62:241-248.
21. Engels WD. Dermatologic disorders. Psychosomatics. 1982;23:1209-1211, 1214-1219.
22. Schachner L, Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Duarte AM, Krasnegor J. Atopic dermatitis
symptoms decreased in children following massage therapy. Pediatric Dermatology.
1998;15:390-395.
23. Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Seligman S, et al. Juvenile rheumatoid arthritis: benefits from
massage therapy. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1997;22:607-617.
24. Varni JW, Jay SM. Biobehavioral factors in juvenile rheumatoid arthritis: implications for
research and practice. Clinical Psychology Review. 1984;4:543-560.
25. Diego MA, Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Shaw K, Friedman L, Ironson G. HIV adolescents
show improved immune function following massage therapy. The International Journal of
Neuroscience. 2001;106:35-45.
26. Field T, Cullen C, Diego M, et al. Leukemia immune changes following massage therapy.
Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies. 2001;5:271-274.
Call 1-877-JNJ-LINK (565-5465)
Fax 1-877-JNJ-FAXX (565-3299)
Visit us at www.JJPI.com
TCH03-02