Behavioral and Emotional Profiles of Neglected Children: Marie-Eve Nadeau, Pierre Nolin, and Carine Chartrand
Behavioral and Emotional Profiles of Neglected Children: Marie-Eve Nadeau, Pierre Nolin, and Carine Chartrand
Behavioral and Emotional Profiles of Neglected Children: Marie-Eve Nadeau, Pierre Nolin, and Carine Chartrand
This study explored the emotional and behavioral profiles of 41 neglected children,
ages 6 to 12 years, who were compared with a control group of 41 children. The
Achenbach System was used in order to describe emotional and behavioral profiles
based on the Diagnostic Criteria of the DSM-IV [Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders, 4th ed.]). Neglected children had more symptoms on DSM-IV Scales
related to conduct or attention/hyperactivity problems. Based on the perceptions of
teachers, children exposed to neglect showed more externalized and internalized prob-
lems as well as symptoms on DSM-IV Scales. Results supported the relevance of using
the Diagnostic Criteria of the DSM-IV and the importance of getting the different per-
ceptions of respondents to better understand the emotional and behavioral portrait of
neglected children.
The relationship between abuse (all types) and the emergence of psychological, emotional,
and behavioral difficulties has been well documented. Indeed, various studies have shown
that abused children presented more psychopathologies (Cook et al., 2005, Ford, 2005)
including posttraumatic stress disorders (Saigh, Yasik, Sack, & Koplowicz, 1999), self-
mutilation behaviors, depression and anxiety problems (Kendall-Tackett, 2002), sexualized
behaviors (Hall, Mathews, & Pearce, 2002), oppositional problems and conduct problems
(Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, & Taylor, 2004), attachment problems (Schore, 2001), and eating
disorders (Johnson, Cohen, Kasen, & Brook, 2002). However, certain studies have pro-
posed that affective, social, and behavioral components were not affected in the same way,
depending on the type of maltreatment experienced by the child (Cicchetti, & Toth, 2000;
Trocmé et al., 2003).
The Canadian Incidence Study of Reported Child Abuse and Neglect carried out by
Trocmé et al. (2003) draws attention to four categories of abuse: (a) physical abuse, (b) sex-
ual abuse, (c) emotional abuse, and (d) neglect. Although neglect is the most frequent type
of maltreatment, this category of abuse is least studied (De Bellis, 2005). When neglected,
the child’s security and development are jeopardized by lack of attention or protection by
Submitted June 16, 2010; revised October 30, 2010; revised February 17, 2011; revised May 15,
2011; accepted May 17, 2011.
Address correspondence to Marie-Eve Nadeau, Clinique d’intervention des troubles anxieux,
Hôpital Rivière-des-Prairies, 7070, boulevard Perras, Montréal, Quebec, Canada H1E 1A4. E-mail:
marie-eve.nadeau@uqtr.ca
11
12 M.-E. Nadeau et al.
the caregiver (Trocmé et al., 2003). Parents show inability to provide the necessary care or
answer basic needs linked to health, hygiene, protection, education, and affective environ-
ment (Éthier, Lacharité, & Gagnier, 1994). Neglect not only affects the daily functioning of
the child, but also its entire development. In children particularly, this exposure may result
in delays or impairments in motor, language, cognitive, emotional, or behavioral develop-
ment and acquisition of social skills (De Bellis, Hooper, Spratt, & Woolley, 2009; Hildyard,
& Wolfe, 2002; Nolin & Ethier, 2007).
The theory of attachment and neurobiological development proposed by Schore (2001)
is certainly one of the explanatory models that can help to understand the affective and
behavioral manifestations of neglected children. This model allows viewing neglect as a
traumatizing situation, but also as a source for altering psychological or biological devel-
opment, as well as the abilities of interpersonal regulation, which can contribute to the
emergence of psychopathologies. Indeed, severe alterations in the attachment relationship
(e.g., parent’s responses are not adjusted to the child’s emotional needs, thus, the child
cannot use proper self-regulation abilities) increase sensitivity to stress (e.g., problems
focusing attention and modulating awareness level), and compromise socioemotional learn-
ing or self-regulation of emotions and behaviors (e.g., inability to regulate emotions without
external support, the sensation of being invaded by many intense emotions, excessive search
for help, social dependency or isolation, disengagement). In this connection, Schore (2001)
pointed out the importance of a stable attachment link in the psychological and neurode-
velopment of the child. Thus, it seems that trauma at a young age alters the development
of the brain regions associated with modulating emotions and stress responses (Cook et al.,
2005).
When faced with traumatizing events in relationships, neglected children find it dif-
ficult to adjust to situations that produce stress or assimilate new emotional experiences.
Thus, under these conditions, their analytical capacities tend to disintegrate; hence, the
emergence of their cognitive, emotional, and behavioral disorganization and their prone-
ness to react with extreme helplessness, confusion, withdrawal, or rage (Teicher, Anderson,
Polcari, Anderson, & Navalta, 2002).
dependent toward others (Egeland, Sroufe, & Erickson, 1983). Internalized symptoms have
been the main focus of studies for neglected children. However, researchers and practi-
tioners do not necessarily associate internalized symptoms with potential developmental
psychopathologies.
functioning. In consequence, this does not totally exclude the possibility of observing exter-
nalized symptoms in these children. Another possibility to explain this divergence consists
in the child’s own protection factors (e.g., his context of evolution, his temperament, and
his personality).
Therefore, the purpose of this study was to explore the role of the respondents
when they assess emotional and behavioral problems and investigate those internalized
symptoms and externalized symptoms of neglected children based on traditional scales
(Syndrome Scales) and clinical scales (DSM-IV [Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders, 4th ed.] Oriented Scales; American Psychiatric Association, 1994)
from the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment: School-Age Forms and
Profiles (ASEBA) in comparison to a control group. The DSM-IV Oriented Scales were
used to document the possible developmental psychopathologies based on diagnostic cat-
egories that could be associated with neglected children. To proceed, two versions of the
same questionnaire were used: one completed by the biological parent (Child Behavior
Checklist for Ages 6–18 [CBCL]) and one by the teacher (Teacher’s Report form [TRF]) of
the child.
Therefore, it has been suggested that: (a) neglected children will present more inter-
nalized symptoms (anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, thought problems), but also
present more externalized symptoms (rule-breaking behavior and aggressive behavior) in
comparison to the control group; (b) neglected children will also present more symptoms on
clinical scales (DSM-IV Oriented Scales) than the control group; and (c) different profiles
of children will be obtained depending on the two types of respondents.
Method
Participants
This study involved a total of 82 children ages 6 to 12, divided into two groups.
Demographic data relevant to both groups are presented in Table 1. Group 1 (neglected chil-
dren) included 41 children including 22 boys and 19 girls with an average age of 10.13 years
old. Neglected children were recruited by the Child Protection Services of Mauricie and
Centre-du-Québec (CJMCQ, Québec, Canada). This institution has the legal mandate to
identify cases of maltreatment (neglect or abuse). CJMCQ standard procedures require that
Table 1
Clinical characteristics for neglected children and nonmaltreated children
Tests
Control test: Intelligence. IQ was estimated using four subtests of the Canadian
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edition with the method developed by
Kaufman, Kaufman, Balgopal, and McLean (1996) and whose psychometric value has
been demonstrated. This shortened version is made up of two verbal subtests (Similarities
and Vocabulary) and two nonverbal subtests (Picture completion and Block design). The
average IQ is 100 with a standard deviation of 15 (Wechsler, 1991).
Instruments measuring behavioral and emotional problems. The ASEBA (CBCL and
TRF; Achenbach, & Rescorla, 1991) is a questionnaire frequently used in both research
and clinical settings for investigating internalized and externalized behavioral problems
16 M.-E. Nadeau et al.
in children. The ASEBA evaluates behavioral and emotional problems found in children
based on the Syndrome Scales: Anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, somatic com-
plaints, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, rule-breaking behavior,
and aggressive behavior. In addition to providing the scores of each of these tradi-
tional scales (Syndrome Scales), it allows the documentation of certain problems listed
based on the DSM-IV Oriented Scales: Affective problems, anxiety problems, somatic
problems, attention/hyperactivity problems, oppositional defiant problems, conduct prob-
lems, sluggish cognitive tempo, obsessive-compulsive problems, and posttraumatic stress
problems.
The DSM oriented scales were constructed by experts from many cultures who identi-
fied ASEBA items that they judged to be very consistent with particular DSM-IV diagnostic
categories.The questionnaire contains 113 items, and there is a version for the child’s par-
ent and his teacher. For each item that describes specific behavioral or emotional problems,
the parent or teacher must weigh the different expressions proposed based on a 3-point
Likert scale (0 [none]; 1 [sometimes]; 2 [often]). The correction program used in this study
was the ADM (Assistant Data Manager) system (Achenbach, 2007). The raw results were
converted into a T-score. A t-score between 65 and 70 is considered borderline versus a
T-score greater than 70 meaning a possible behavioral or emotional problem (Achenbach
& Rescorla, 2006). This instrument has good methodological properties (Achenbach &
Rescorla, 2006).
Procedures
The research project was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Québec in
Trois-Rivières. Parents signed an informed consent form before the children were assigned
to the research professional. After the consent is obtained, a research professional met with
biological parents in the family and with teachers in the school setting of the children in
order to evaluate behavioral and emotional problems using ASEBA. The children have been
assigned to the research professional by the researchers without knowing which group they
belonged to (blind to condition).
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analyzes were performed on each of the demographic variables and IQ, using chi-
square or ANOVA, as was a continuous variable (e.g., IQ) or dichotomous variable (e.g.,
gender). These analyzes were intended to ensure the homogeneity of the two groups on
these variables before comparing them on emotional and behavioral scales. Subsequently,
two multivariate analyses with covariance (MANCOVAs) were first carried out using all the
Syndrome Scales (anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, somatic complaints, social
problems, thought problems, attention problems, rule-breaking behavior and aggressive
behavior) based on the scores from the parents’ questionnaires, and then on the scores
of the teachers’ questionnaires, taking into account IQ and Hollingshead Index of SES
as covariables. Next, two MANCOVAs were carried out using the DSM-IV Oriented
Scales (affective problems, anxiety problems, somatic problems, attention/hyperactivity
problems, oppositional defiant problems, conduct problems, sluggish cognitive tempo,
obsessive-compulsive problems, and posttraumatic stress problems), taking into account
the Hollingshead Index of SES as a covariable, one with the scores of the questionnaires
completed by the parents and the other with the questionnaires completed by the teachers.
Behaviors, Emotions, and Neglect 17
Partial Eta Square (ηp 2 ) was reported to describe effect sizes for significant differences
(Small < 0.06; Medium = 0.12; Large > 0.16).
Results
Table 2
Parent syndrome scale scores for neglected children and nonmaltreated children
Traditional scales
Version completed by Neglect Controls Partial
the parent mean (SD) mean (SD) Statistic Eta2
IQ F(8, 71) = 0.49 .05
SES F(8, 71) = 0.63 .07
Group effect F(8, 71) = 1.31 .13
Anxious/Depressed 57.24 (9.05) 55.73 (5.73) F(1, 78) = 0.71 .01
Withdrawn/Depressed 59.44 (7.23) 57.95 (6.70) F(1, 78) = 0.68 .01
Somatic complaints 54.76 (6.09) 57.10 (6.34) F(1, 78) = 3.41 .04
Social problems 60.46 (8.10) 58.12 (5.89) F(1, 78) = 1.50 .02
Thought problems 56.76 (6.63) 57.24 (7.40) F(1, 78) = 0.03 .00
Attention problems 56.78 (6.68) 54.22 (3.48) F(1, 78) = 2.83 .04
Rule-breaking 60.07 (7.97) 57.07 (6.88) F(1, 78) = 2.46 .03
behavior
Aggressive behavior 62.73 (9.94) 59.44 (7.27) F(1, 78) = 2.85 .04
18 M.-E. Nadeau et al.
Table 3
Teacher syndrome scale scores for neglected children and nonmaltreated children
Traditional scales
Version completed by Neglect mean Controls Partial
the teacher (SD) mean (SD) Statistic Eta2
IQ F(8,71) = 0.87 .09
SES F(8,71) = 1.17 .12
Group effect F(8,71) = 2.78∗∗ .24
Anxious/Depressed 56.98 (7.29) 55.41 (6.21) F(1,78) = 2.35 .03
Withdrawn/Depressed 61.00 (8.73) 55.12 (5.68) F(1,78) = 13.61∗∗∗ .15
Somatic complaints 53.93 (5.87) 52.22 (4.89) F(1,78) = 1.67 .02
Social problems 61.63 (7.92) 57.32 (6.50) F(1,78) = 7.90∗∗ .09
Thought problems 57.15 (8.70) 52.73 (4.88) F(1,78) = 10.65∗∗ .12
Attention problems 60.44 (9.06) 54.05 (4.77) F(1,78) = 13.21∗∗∗ .15
Rule-breaking behavior 60.49 (9.13) 54.73 (5.88) F(1,78) = 9.05∗∗ .10
Aggressive behavior 63.71 (12.14) 56.34 (7.21) F(1,78) = 9.50∗∗ .11
∗∗
p < .01. ∗∗∗ p < .001.
Table 4
Parent DSM oriented scales scores for neglected children and nonmaltreated children
Discussion
The main goal of this study was to document emotional problems (internalized symptoms)
and behavioral problems (externalized symptoms) of neglected children on the Syndrome
Scales and DSM-IV Oriented Scales (ASEBA) based on the points of view of the two
respondents (biological parents and teacher), in comparison to a control group. The results
confirmed that neglected children had more internalized problems than children in the
control group, which is in line with the results of previous studies that have clearly demon-
strated this fact. Moreover, the results of this study support the suggestion that neglect is
also associated with externalized behaviors, a feature which seems to be less documented in
the literature (Bousha & Twentyman, 1984; Crittenden, 1992; Erickson et al., 1989; Haskett
& Kistner, 1991; Hildyard & Wolfe, 2002; Hoffman-Plotkin & Twentyman, 1984; Shields,
& Cicchetti, 1998). Furthermore, externalized behaviors are better observed in a school
setting than by family members.
As a matter of fact, the children had different emotional and behavioral profiles depend-
ing on the points of view of the respondents. Parents did not identify any problems in the
Syndrome Scales, whereas they pointed out externalized symptoms associated with con-
duct or attention/hyperactivity problems (DSM-IV Oriented Scales), in comparison with
children in the control group. In this way, parents tend to see their children as relatively nor-
mal in comparison to the perception of teachers, beside behaviors that needs more parental
management and are more disturbing for them.
On the other hand, based on the point of view of teachers, neglected chil-
dren had more symptoms on the Syndrome Scales, in particular internalized prob-
lems (withdrawn/depressed, social and thought problems), externalized problems
20 M.-E. Nadeau et al.
Table 5
Teacher DSM oriented scales scores for neglected children and nonmaltreated children
of respondent. However, few authors listed have noted a difference between the answers
provided by the parents and teachers (Culp, Howell, McDonald Culp, Blankemeyer, 2001).
Other authors have emphasized that maltreating parent’s may have biased perceptions of
their children’s behaviors (Jourdan-Ionescu & Palacio-Quintin, 1997) as their perceptions
could be tainted by subjectivity and be biased by multiple factors, including stress, conflicts,
and lack of parenting skills (Reid et al., 1987). Moreover, maltreating parents are more
insensitive and unresponsive to their children’s feelings (Lyons-Ruth et al., 1989). For these
reasons, certain authors proposed that the teacher’s perception is more objective (Lacharité,
1999). These findings underscore the importance of using the widest range of measure in
order to arrive at a more complete profile of the child’s affect and behavior. Clinically,
perceptions of different raters are essential to a successful treatment plan because different
raters may be observing different behaviors.
Overall, results of this study confirm the importance of using a wide variety of scales,
in particular the DSM Oriented Scales, to get a complete profile of the neglected chil-
dren. Until very recently, the questionnaires used in previous studies did not allow a
diagnostic clinical analysis based on the psychiatric model of the DSM-IV, but it is now
possible with the ADM (Achenbach, 2007) of the ASEBA (CBCL and TRF; Achenbach
& Rescorla, 1991). It seems to be an interesting route because it would provide healthcare
practitioners with profiles based on the diagnostic criteria of DSM-IV, thus contributing to
set-up treatments adapted to the child’s psychological situation and prevent development
of psychopathologies.
Finally, the results of this study support the idea put forth by Cicchetti (2004) who
stipulated that neglect has negative effects on the child’s development beyond the simple
fact of living in a poor socioeconomic environment. Indeed, covariance analysis showed
that neglected children were different from those in the control group with regard to emo-
tional and behavioral problems, even after having controlled the socioeconomic level. This
supports the fact that these internalized and externalized behaviors were associated with
neglect and not poverty.
Acknowledgments
Funding for this study was provided by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. The authors
would like to thank the Mauricie-Centre-du-Quebec Child Protection Services (Mauricie, Québec,
Canada) particularly Daniel Gagnon and Martin Dionne, who facilitated the recruitment of subjects.
The authors would also like to thank the personnel at the schools of the Commission Scolaire Chemin-
du-Roy (Mauricie, Québec, Canada). They would also like to thank Véronique Parent, Andrée-Anne
Durocher, Mylène Henry, Isabelle Frigon, Marie-France Gobeil, and Louise Bourassa who made an
invaluable contribution to this study.
References
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (1991). The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assement:
School age forms and profiles. Burlington: VT: University Associates Psychiatry.
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2006). The Achenbach System of Empirically Based
Assessment. In R. P. Archer (Ed.), Forensic uses of clinical assessment instruments (pp. 229–262).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Achenbach, T. M. (2007). Assistant data manager. Toronto: Psychological Corporation.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th
ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Bolger, K. E., Patterson, C. J., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1998). Peer relationships and self-esteem among
children who have been maltreated. Child Development, 69, 1171–1197.
Bousha, D. M., & Twentyman, C. T. (1984). Mother-child interaction style in abuse, neglect, and
control groups: Naturalistic observations in the home. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93,
106–114.
Camras, L. A., & Rappaport, S. (1993). Conflict behaviors of maltreated and non-maltreated children.
Child Abuse & Neglect, 17, 455–464.
Cicchetti, D. (2004). An odyssey of discovery: Lessons learned through three decades of research on
child maltreatment. American Psychologist, 59(8), 731–741.
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2000). Developmental processes in maltreated children. In D. J. Hansen
(Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Motivation and child maltreatment (pp. 85–160).
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Cook, A., Spinazzola, J., Ford, J., Lanktree, C., Blaustein M., Cloitre, M., et al. (2005). Complex
trauma in children and adolescents. Psychiatric Annals, 35(5), 390–398.
Crittenden, P. M. (1992). Children’s strategies for coping with adverse home environments: An
interpretation using attachment theory. Child Abuse & Neglect, 16, 329–343.
Culp, R. E., Howell, C. S., McDonald Culp, A., & Blankemeyer, M. (2001). Maltreated children’s
emotional and behavioral problems: Do teachers and parents see the same things? Journal of Child
and Family Studies, 10(1), 39–50.
De Bellis, M. D. (2005). The psychobiology of neglect. Child Maltreatment, 10(2), 150–172.
De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Spratt, E. G., & Woolley, D. P. (2009). Neuropsychological find-
ings in childhood neglect and their relationship to pediatric PTSD. Journal of the International
Neuropsychology Society, 15, 868–878.
Behaviors, Emotions, and Neglect 23
Egeland, B., Sroufe, A., & Erickson, M. (1983). The developmental consequences of different
patterns of maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 7, 459–469.
Erickson, M. F., & Egeland, B. (1996). Child neglect. In J. Briere, L. Berliner, J. A. Bulkley, C. Jenny,
& T. Reid (Eds.), The APSAC handbook on child maltreatment (pp. 4–20). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage Publications.
Erickson, M. F., Egeland, B., & Pianta, R. (1989). The effects of maltreatment on the development
of young children. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child maltreatment (pp. 647–684). New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Éthier, L. S., Lacharité, C., & Gagnier, J.-P. (1994). Prévenir la négligence parentale [Prevent parental
neglect]. Revue québécoise de psychologie, 15(3), 67–86.
Feldman, S. R., Salzinger, S., Rosario, M., Alvarado, L., Caraballo, L., & Hammer, M. (1995). Parent,
teacher, and peer ratings of physically abused and nonmaltreated children’s behavior. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 317–334.
Ford, J. D. (2005). Treatment implications of altered affect regulation and information processing
following child maltreatment. Psychiatric Annals, 35(5), 410–419.
Hall, D. K., Mathews, F., & Pearce, J. (2002). Sexual behavior problems in sexually abused children:
A preliminary typology. Child Abuse and Neglect, 2(10), 1045–1063.
Haskett, M. E., & Kistner, J. A. (1991). Social interactions and peer perceptions of young physically
abused children. Child Development, 62, 979–990.
Hildyard, K. L., & Wolfe, A. W. (2002). Child neglect: Developmental issues and outcomes. Child
Abuse and Neglect, 26, 679–695.
Hoffman-Plotkin, D., & Twentyman, C. T. (1984). A multimodal assessment of behavioral and
cognitive deficits in abused and neglected preschoolers. Child Development, 55, 794–802.
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Taylor, A. (2004). Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial
child: Evidence of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113,
44–55.
Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S., & Brook, J. S. (2002). Childhood adversities associated with
eating disorders and weight problems during adolescence and early adulthood. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 159, 394–400.
Jourdan-Ionescu, C., & Palacio-Quintin, E. (1997). Effets de la maltraitance sur les jeunes et nou-
velles perspectives d’intervention [Effects of maltreatment on children and new intervention
perspectives]. Psychologie française, 42, 217–228.
Kendall-Tackett, K. (2002). The health effects of childhood abuse: Four pathways by which abuse
can influence health. Child Abuse and Neglect, 6(7), 715–730.
Kaufman, A. S., Kaufman, J. C., Balgopal, R., & McLean, J. E. (1996). Comparison of three WISC-
III short forms: Weighing psychometric, clinical, and practical factors. Journal of Clinical Child
Psychology, 25(1), 97–105.
Lacharité, C. (1999). Typologie des problèmes comportementaux chez les enfants maltraités: descrip-
tion et implications pour l’intervention [Typology of behavioral problems in maltreatment
children: description and implications for intervention]. Revue québécoise de psychologie, 20,
127–139.
Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D., & Zoll, D. (1989). Patterns of maternal behavior among infants at risk
for abuse: Relations with infant attachment behavior and infant development at 12 months of
age. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carslon (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes
and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 464–493). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Nolin, P., & Éthier, L. (2007). Using neuropsychological profiles to classify neglected children with
or without physical abuse. Journal of Child Abuse and Neglect, 31, 631–643.
Perry, B. D., Pollard, R. A., Blakely, T. L., Baker, W. L., & Vigilante, D. (1995). Childhood trauma,
the neurobiology of adaptation, and “use-dependent” development of the brain. How “states”
become “traits.” Infant Mental Health Journal, 16, 271–291.
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces:
Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology, 36, 679–688.
24 M.-E. Nadeau et al.
Prino, T. C., & Peyrot, M. (1994). The effect of child physical abuse and neglect on aggressive,
withdrawn, and prosocial behavior. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 871–884.
Reid, B. J., Kavanagh, K., & Baldwin, V. D. (1987). Abusive parents’ perceptions of child problem
behaviors: An example of parental bias. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 457–466.
Saigh, P., Yasik, A., Sack, W., & Koplewicz, H. (1999). Child-adolescent posttraumatic stress dis-
order: Prevalence, risk factors, and comorbidity. In P. Saigh & J. Bremner (Eds.), Posttraumatic
stress disorder: A comprehensive text (pp. 18–43). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Schore, A. (2001). The early effects of trauma on right brain development, affect regulation and infant
health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, 201–269.
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated children: The con-
tributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27,
381–395.
Shonk, M. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Maltreatment, competency deficits, and risk for academic and
behavioral maladjustment. Developmental Psychology, 37, 3–17.
Teicher, M. H., Anderson, S. L., Polcari, A., Anderson, C. M., & Navalta, C. P. (2002). Developmental
neurobiology of childhood stress and trauma. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 25, 397–426.
Trocmé, N., Fallon, B., McLaurin, B., Daciuk, J., Felstiner, C., Black, T., et al. (2003). Étude
canadienne sur l’incidence des signalements de cas de violence et de négligence envers les
enfants–2003 [Canadian incidence study of reported child abuse and neglect–2003]. Ottawa:
Ministre de Travaux publics et Services gouvernementaux.
Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). San Antonio:
The Psychological Corporation.
Copyright of Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.