This document discusses phonemes and allophones. It begins by defining a phoneme as the smallest unit of sound that distinguishes meaning in a language. Phonemes are represented between slashes, while their specific pronunciations are represented in brackets. Allophones are variant pronunciations of the same phoneme that are conditioned by their phonetic context. The document then discusses methods for determining if two sounds are phonemes or allophones, including minimal pairs, which are pairs of words that differ in a single phoneme. It provides examples of minimal pairs in English and other languages. The final section defines an allophone as a phonetically distinct variant of a phoneme, and notes that the occurrence of one allophone over another
This document discusses phonemes and allophones. It begins by defining a phoneme as the smallest unit of sound that distinguishes meaning in a language. Phonemes are represented between slashes, while their specific pronunciations are represented in brackets. Allophones are variant pronunciations of the same phoneme that are conditioned by their phonetic context. The document then discusses methods for determining if two sounds are phonemes or allophones, including minimal pairs, which are pairs of words that differ in a single phoneme. It provides examples of minimal pairs in English and other languages. The final section defines an allophone as a phonetically distinct variant of a phoneme, and notes that the occurrence of one allophone over another
This document discusses phonemes and allophones. It begins by defining a phoneme as the smallest unit of sound that distinguishes meaning in a language. Phonemes are represented between slashes, while their specific pronunciations are represented in brackets. Allophones are variant pronunciations of the same phoneme that are conditioned by their phonetic context. The document then discusses methods for determining if two sounds are phonemes or allophones, including minimal pairs, which are pairs of words that differ in a single phoneme. It provides examples of minimal pairs in English and other languages. The final section defines an allophone as a phonetically distinct variant of a phoneme, and notes that the occurrence of one allophone over another
This document discusses phonemes and allophones. It begins by defining a phoneme as the smallest unit of sound that distinguishes meaning in a language. Phonemes are represented between slashes, while their specific pronunciations are represented in brackets. Allophones are variant pronunciations of the same phoneme that are conditioned by their phonetic context. The document then discusses methods for determining if two sounds are phonemes or allophones, including minimal pairs, which are pairs of words that differ in a single phoneme. It provides examples of minimal pairs in English and other languages. The final section defines an allophone as a phonetically distinct variant of a phoneme, and notes that the occurrence of one allophone over another
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online from Scribd
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 10
Seminar 2. Phoneme and its realization in speech. Allophone.
1. A phoneme as a unit of phonology.
A phoneme (/ˈfoʊniːm/) is a unit of sound that distinguishes one word from another in a particular language. For example, in most dialects of English, the sound patterns /sɪn/ (sin) and /sɪŋ/ (sing) are two separate words that are distinguished by the substitution of one phoneme, /n/, for another phoneme, /ŋ/. (Two words like this that differ in meaning through the contrast of a single phoneme form a minimal pair.) In many other languages, they would be interpreted as exactly the same set of phonemes, and so /n/ and /ŋ/ would be considered to be the same. In linguistics, phonemes that are usually established by the use of minimal pairs, such as kill vs kiss or pat vs bat, are written between slashes: /p/. To show pronunciation more precisely, linguists use square brackets: [pʰ] (indicating an aspirated p). Within linguistics, there are differing views as to exactly what phonemes are and how a given language should be analyzed in phonemic (or phonematic) terms. However, a phoneme is generally regarded as an abstraction of a set (or equivalence class) of speech sounds (phones) that are perceived as equivalent to each other in a given language. For example, the English k sounds in the words kill and skill are not identical (as described below), but they are distributional variants of a single phoneme /k/. Different speech sounds that are realizations of the same phoneme are known as allophones. Allophonic variation may be conditioned, in which case a certain phoneme is realized as a certain allophone in particular phonological environments, or it may otherwise be free, and may then vary randomly. Therefore, phonemes are often considered to constitute an abstract underlying representation for segments of words, while speech sounds make up the corresponding phonetic realization, or the surface form. 2. The method of minimal pairs. Assignment of speech sounds to phonemes A simplified procedure for determining whether two sounds represent the same or different phonemes A phoneme is a sound or a group of different sounds perceived to have the same function by speakers of the language or dialect in question. An example is the English phoneme /k/, which occurs in words such as cat, kit, scat, skit. Although most native speakers do not notice this, in most English dialects, the "c/k" sounds in these words are not identical: in About this soundkit (help·info) [kʰɪt], the sound is aspirated, but in About this soundskill (help·info) [skɪl], it is unaspirated. The words, therefore, contain different speech sounds, or phones, transcribed [kʰ] for the aspirated form and.[k] for the unaspirated one. These different sounds are nonetheless considered to belong to the same phoneme, because if a speaker used one instead of the other, the meaning of the word would not change: using the aspirated form [kʰ] in skill might sound odd, but the word would still be recognized. By contrast, some other sounds would cause a change in meaning if substituted: for example, substitution of the sound [t] would produce the different word still, and that sound must therefore be considered to represent a different phoneme (the phoneme /t/). The above shows that in English, [k] and [kʰ] are allophones of a single phoneme /k/. In some languages, however, [kʰ] and [k] are perceived by native speakers as different sounds, and substituting one for the other can change the meaning of a word. In those languages, therefore, the two sounds represent different phonemes. For example, in Icelandic, [kʰ] is the first sound of kátur, meaning "cheerful", but [k] is the first sound of gátur, meaning "riddles". Icelandic, therefore, has two separate phonemes /kʰ/ and /k/. Minimal pairs A pair of words like kátur and gátur (above) that differ only in one phone is called a minimal pair for the two alternative phones in question (in this case, [kʰ] and [k]). The existence of minimal pairs is a common test to decide whether two phones represent different phonemes or are allophones of the same phoneme. To take another example, the minimal pair tip and dip illustrates that in English, [t] and [d] belong to separate phonemes, /t/ and /d/; since both words have different meanings, English-speakers must be conscious of the distinction between the two sounds. In other languages, however, including Korean, both sounds [t] and [d] occur, but no such minimal pair exists. The lack of minimal pairs distinguishing [t] and [d] in Korean provides evidence that they are allophones of a single phoneme /t/. The word /tata/ is pronounced [tada], for example. That is, when they hear this word, Korean-speakers perceive the same sound in both the beginning and middle of the word, but English-speakers perceive different sounds in these two locations. Signed languages, such as American Sign Language (ASL), also have minimal pairs, differing only in (exactly) one of the signs' parameters: handshape, movement, location, palm orientation, and nonmanual signal or marker. A minimal pair may exist in the signed language if the basic sign stays the same, but one of the parameters changes. However, the absence of minimal pairs for a given pair of phones does not always mean that they belong to the same phoneme: they may be so dissimilar phonetically that it is unlikely for speakers to perceive them as the same sound. For example, English has no minimal pair for the sounds [h] (as in hat) and [ŋ] (as in bang), and the fact that they can be shown to be in complementary distribution could be used to argue for their being allophones of the same phoneme. However, they are so dissimilar phonetically that they are considered separate phonemes. Phonologists have sometimes had recourse to "near minimal pairs" to show that speakers of the language perceive two sounds as significantly different even if no exact minimal pair exists in the lexicon. It is virtually impossible to find a minimal pair to distinguish English /ʃ/ from /ʒ/, yet it seems uncontroversial to claim that the two consonants are distinct phonemes. The two words 'pressure' /ˈprɛʃər/ and 'pleasure' /ˈplɛʒər/ can serve as a near minimal pair. The aim of the phonological analysis is, firstly, to determine which differences of sounds are phonemic and which are non-phonemic and, secondly, to find the inventory of phonemes of the language. As it was mentioned above, phonology has its own methods of investigation. Semantic method is applied for phonological analysis of both unknown languages and languages already described. The method is based on a phonemic rule that phonemes can distinguish words and morphemes when opposed to one another. It consists in systematic substitution of one sound for another in order to find out in which cases where the phonetic context remains the same such replacing leads to a change of meaning. This procedure is called the commutation test. It consists in finding minimal pairs of words and their grammatical forms. For example: 1) pen [pen] 2) Ben [ben] 3) gain [gain] 4) cane [kain] 5) ten [ten] 6) den[den] Minimal pairs are useful for establishing the phonemes of the language. Thus, a phoneme can only perform its distinctive function if it is opposed to another phoneme in the same position. Such an opposition is called phonological. Let us consider the classification of phonological oppositions worked out by N.S. Trubetzkoy. It is based on the number of distinctive articulatory features underlying the opposition. 1. If the opposition is based on a single difference in the articulation of two speech sounds, it is a single phonological opposition, e.g. [p]-[t], as in [pen]-[ten]; bilabial vs. forelingual, all the other features are the same. 2. If the sounds in distinctive opposition have two differences in their articulation, the opposition is double one, or a sum of two single oppositions, e.g. [p]-[d], as in [pen]-[den], 1) bilabial vs. forelingual 2) voiceless-fortis vs. voiced-lenis 3. If there are three articulatory differences, the opposition is triple one, or a sum of three single oppositions, e.g. [p]- [ð], as in [pei]-[ ðei]: 1) bilabial vs. forelingual, 2) occlusive vs. constrictive, 3) voiceless-fortis vs. voiced-lenis. American descriptivists, whose most zealous representative is, perhaps, Zellig Harris, declare the distributional method to be the only scientific one. At the same time they declare the semantic method unscientific because they consider recourse to meaning external to linguistics. Descriptivists consider the phonemic analysis in terms of distribution. They consider it possible to discover the phonemes of a language by the rigid application of a distributional method. It means to group all the sounds pronounced by native speakers into phoneme according to the laws of phonemic and allophonic distribution: 1. Allophones of different phonemes occur in the same phonetic context. In this case their distribution is contrastive. 2. Allophones of the same phoneme(s) never occur in the same phonetic context. In this case their distribution is complementary. There is, however, a third possibility, namely, that the sounds both occur in a language but the speakers are inconsistent in the way they use them, for example, калоши-галоши, and [‘ei∫э - ‘егжэ]. In such cases we must take them as free variants of a single phoneme. We could explain the case on the basis of sociolinguistics. Thus, there are three types of distribution: contrastive, complementary and free variation.
3. Allophone. Types of allophones.
Allophone, one of the phonetically distinct variants of a phoneme (q.v.). The occurrence of one allophone rather than another is usually determined by its position in the word (initial, final, medial, etc.) or by its phonetic environment. Speakers of a language often have difficulty in hearing the phonetic differences between allophones of the same phoneme, because these differences do not serve to distinguish one word from another. In English the t sounds in the words “hit,” “tip,” and “little” are allophones; phonemically they are considered to be the same sound although they are different phonetically in terms of aspiration, voicing, and point of articulation. In Japanese and some dialects of Chinese, the sounds f and h are allophones. In phonology, an allophone (/ˈæləfoʊn/; from the Greek ἄλλος, állos, "other" and φωνή, phōnē, "voice, sound") is one of a set of multiple possible spoken sounds, or phones, or signs used to pronounce a single phoneme in a particular language.[1] For example, in English, [t] (as in stop [stɒp]) and the aspirated form [tʰ] (as in top [ˈtʰɒp]) are allophones for the phoneme /t/, while these two are considered to be different phonemes in some languages such as Thai and Hindi. On the other hand, in Spanish, [d] (as in dolor [doˈloɾ]) and [ð] (as in nada [ˈnaða]) are allophones for the phoneme /d/, while these two are considered to be different phonemes in English. The specific allophone selected in a given situation is often predictable from the phonetic context, with such allophones being called positional variants, but some allophones occur in free variation. Replacing a sound by another allophone of the same phoneme usually does not change the meaning of a word, but the result may sound non-native or even unintelligible. Native speakers of a given language perceive one phoneme in the language as a single distinctive sound and are "both unaware of and even shocked by" the allophone variations that are used to pronounce single phonemes. Language is an abstract category, it’s an abstraction from speech. Speech is the reality of a language, thus the phoneme as a language unit is materialized in speech sound. The phoneme is a sort of generalization (abstraction). Let us consider the English phoneme /d/. It is an occlusive plosive stop, forelingual, apical, alveolar, lenis consonant. This is how it sounds in isolation or in such words as door, darn, down, etc, when it retains its typical articulatory characteristics. In this case the consonant [d] is called principal allophone. The allophones which do not undergo any distinguishable changes in speech are called principal. Allophones that undergo quite predictable changes under the influence of the neighboring sounds in different phonetic situations are called subsidiary, e.g.: a) deal, did, did you - it is slightly palatalized before front vowels and [j]; b) bad pain, bedtime - it is pronounced without any plosion before another stop; с) sudden, admit - it is pronounced with nasal plosion before [n], [m]; d) dry - it becomes post-alveolar followed by [r]; e) middle - before [l] a literal plosion; f) breadth - before interdental sounds it becomes dental; g) dwell - when followed by [w] it becomes labialized; h) dead - in word final position it’s partly devoiced. Thus, we see that the allophones mentioned above are all fore-lingual lenis stops, but they show some differences. The allophones of the same phoneme never occur in the same phonetic context. Subsidiary allophones can be positional and combinatory. Positional allophones are used in certain positions traditionally. For example, the English /1/ is realized in actual speech as a positional allophone: it is clear in the initial position, and dark in the terminal position, compare light, let and hill, melt. Russian positional allophones can be observed in вопль, рубль where terminal /л/ is devoiced after voiceless /п, б/. Combinatory allophones appear in the process of speech and result from the influence of one phoneme upon another. Native speakers do not observe the difference between the allophones of the same phoneme. At the same time they realize that allophones of each phoneme possess a bundle of distinctive features that makes this phoneme functionally different from all other phonemes of the language. This functionally relevant bundle is called the invariant of the phoneme. All the allophones of the phoneme /d/ are occlusive, forelingual, lenis. If occlusive articulation is changed for constrictive one [d] will be replaced by [z]: e. g. breed - breeze, deal — zeal. The articulatory features which form the invariant of the phoneme are called distinctive or relevant. To extract relevant features of the phoneme we have to oppose it to some other phoneme in the phonetic context. If the opposed sounds differ in one articulatory feature and this difference brings about changes in the meaning this feature is called relevant: for example, port — court, [p] and [k] are consonants, occlusive, fortis; the only difference being that [p] is labial and [t] is lingual. The articulatory features which do not serve to distinguish meaning are called non- distinctive, irrelevant or redundant. For example, it is impossible to oppose an aspirated [ph] to a non-aspirated one in the same phonetic context to distinguish meaning. We know that anyone who studies a foreign language makes mistakes in the articulation of sounds. L.V. Shcherba classifies the pronunciation errors as phonological and phonetic. If an allophone is replaced by an allophone of a different phoneme the mistake is called phonological. If an allophone of the phoneme is replaced by another allophone of the same phoneme the mistake is called phonetic. 4. Variants of phonemes pronounced in actual speech 5. Types of phological opposition A phoneme can only perform its distinctive function if it is opposed to another phoneme (or to no sound) in the same position. Such an opposition is called distinctive, or phonological. There exist very interesting, but rather com-plicated classifications of phonological oppositions, such as the one worked out by M. S. Trubetzkoy in his book Grund- ziige der Phonologie, but, as it is applicable to an already established phonemic system, a different classi-fication is here proposed for an initial phonological analysis of a language. This classification is based on the number of dis-tinctive articulatory features underlying the opposition. Bilateral (одномерные) anil multilateral oppositions. In bilateral oppositions p || b, t || d, к || g members of the opposition possess common features which are characteristic only of the members of this opposition. In multilateral oppositions features characterizing 2 members of the opposition may be at the same time characteristic of other members of the system. In i: || ǽ as which is a multilateral opposition, in Тек || Tƒek, din || pin we see more than one distinctive feature. d - oral, apical, plosive, fortis, stop. \ p - oral, bilabial, plosive, fortis, stop, x - marks the distinctive features. X1= not bilabial; Х2 = not apical Multilateral oppositions may be homogenuous and heterogenuous. Homogenuousoppositions are represented by series of bilateral oppositions: i: || i || e || £ || as. A set of determining features is also peculiar to other members of the system. The extreme members of these oppositions are multilateral [i: || as]»between these members we can insert a series of bilateral oppositions. In к || g we may insert one more member: к [| g || rj = к || g, g j| ij. Heterogenuos oppositions cannot admit a series of bilateral oppositions between their extreme members. Bilateral and multilateral oppositions may be proportional (symmetrical) and isolated. A proportional opposition a set of oppositions which correspond in the same contrast: p : b, s : z, к : g, b : m, d : n, i|. -X -not lenis. In isolated oppositions (r : 1) we shall not find the same relations between members of other oppositions. The number of isolated oppositions in the phonological system prevails. 2. Characterizing oppositions as to the relations between the members of the opposition N. S. Trubetskoy distinguishes: a) Privative oppositions (one member of the opposition has a certain feature, the other — has not). The first member is called the marked member. That member of a bilateral opposition which is characterized by the presence of a relevant feature which marks the opposition is marked by the presence of that feature. The second member is the unmarked member of the opposition. In the opposition pin : bin /b/ is marked by the presence of lenis quality and absence of fortis quality. b) Gradual oppositions possess the same feature in a different degree (e.g. the difference in a degree of aperture (u : o, i : e). They may possess more than 2 members. c) Equipollent oppositions (равнозначные) are the oppositions where both members are equal logically; they are neither privative nor gradual (p : f, f: k). The number of these oppositions is great. They are not relevant for the investigation of the phonological system. At present R. Jacobson and Halle single out 14 binary distinctive features from binary oppositions. 3. Classification of oppositions with reference to the distinctive function of the members of the opposition. The oppositions are not always relevant. In some cases distinctive features relevant in one position are non-relevant in another and have no distinctive function. In the first case we deal with constant (permanent) opposition, in the second - neutralized. Пил : Бил — П || Б relevant in this position; ДуБ : Пила — Б || П phonologically irrelevant; П || Б is actual not in every position. 6. Different opinions on the nature of phoneme The phoneme theory came into being in Russia. It was founded by professor I.A. Baudoin de Courtenay (1845 – 1929), who was the founder of the Kazan linguistic school. The fundamental principles of the phoneme theory were worked out by him during the 1870s. His teaching eventually became known in Western Europe primarily through his disciples. He was a representer of the so-called psychological school of linguistics, which was current in his time. According to his theory a speech sound is a sound of a definite acoustic quality, produced by the organs of speech. Any given speech sound is incapable of variation. A Phoneme is defined by him as a group of related sounds of a given language, which are so used in connected speech, that no one of them ever occurs in positions, which any other can occupy. Speech sounds which belong to the same phoneme cannot distinguish one word from another, but phonemes distinguish one word from another in the same language. Baudoin de Courtenay's point of view was later developed by his disciple Lev Scherba (1880 – 1944). He separated phonetics from phonology and stated that sounds are not only articulatory and acoustic units, but they also possess functional qualities. Scherba noticed that in actual speech we pronounce a greater variety of sounds than we are aware of. In every language all these sounds are united in a comparatively small number of sound types. This sound types are capable of distinguishing meanings and forms of words. They serve the purpose of social intercourse. These sound types are called phonemes. And the actual speech sounds that we pronounce and the individual representing the universal are called phonemic variant or allophones. Scherba's conception is a truly materialistic theory of the phoneme. He was the first to define the phoneme as a real independent distinctive unit, which manifests itself in the form of its allophones. There are some other views on the nature of the phoneme. From physical point of view the phoneme is a family of related sounds in which various members of the family must show articulatory similarity to one another and at the same time no member of the family may occur in the same phonetic context. This approach overestimates the material aspect of the phoneme since it views the phoneme as a group of articulatory similar sounds without any regard to its functional and abstract aspects. The physical point of view was put forward by Daniel Johnes, he was the head of London school of phonology. A similar approach is expressed by the French linguist Ferdinant de Saussure. He viewed the phoneme as a sum of acoustic impressions and articulatory movements. The opposite approach is reflected in the so-called functional view. It regards the phoneme as the minimal sound unit by which meanings may be differenciated without much regard to actually pronounced speech sounds. According to this conception the phoneme is not a family of sounds since in every sound not all the articulatory features are involved in the differenciation of meanings. Only the features helping differenciate meanings are called distinctive and they make the phoneme corresponding to them. That's why the approach excludes non-distinctive features from the phoneme. The approach was first expressed by Nikolay Trubetskoy, the head of the Prague linguistic school. He defines phoneme as a unity of phonologically relevant features. He wrote that when the phoneme is neutralized it becomes an archiphoneme, which is an abstraction combining the distinctive features common to two phonemes (лук — луг, k and g are one archiphoneme). According to his view-point both 'k' and 'g' in the Russian words 'luk' and 'lug' represent the archiphoneme 'k', which is neither voiced, nor voiceless. A similar view is expressed by American phoneticians, who define the phoneme as a bunch of distinctive features and as an abstractional fiction.