rspb20080873 PDF
rspb20080873 PDF
rspb20080873 PDF
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use,
distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
2652 J. M. Carré & C. M. McCormick Facial metrics and aggression
around it, and increase the point counter total by one. It was Table 1. Mean (s.e.m.) for women (nZ51) and men (nZ37)
explained to participants that the point counter might for each of the variables measured in study 1.
flash several times with negative signs around it, resulting in
a one-point decrease in the point counter total. They were women men t (d.f.Z86) p-value
told that this meant that their partner (actually the computer
program) had stolen a point, and each stolen point would be face ratio 1.80 (0.10) 1.86 (0.13) 2.33 0.02
added to the partner’s counter. Participants could respond by trait domi- 5.8 (5.5) 8.35 (5.5) 2.15 0.04
nance
continuing to select option no. 1 (point reward) or could
PSAP
switch to option no. 2 or 3. Pressing option no. 2 ten times
responses
would steal a point from their partner; however, participants aggression 209 (151) 278 (145) 2.18 0.03
were instructed that they were randomly assigned to the reward 2486 (406) 2423 (302) K0.80 0.43
experimental condition whereby they, unlike their partner, protection 294 (164) 316 (127) 0.66 0.51
would not keep any points stolen. Pressing option no. 3 ten
times would protect their point counter against theft of points
for a brief time. Thus, the dependent variables from the PSAP
measure were option nos. 1 (reward earned), 2 (aggression) 600
aggressive behaviour
and 3 (protection). Selection of option no. 2 was considered
reactive aggression because the participants did not increase
reward, and in fact lost opportunity to increase reward, each 400
time option no. 2 was selected.
200
(e) Study 1 procedure
Participants arrived in groups of two or four and first
completed a demographic and trait dominance questionnaire. 0
1.50 1.65 1.80 1.95 2.10
Next, participants were photographed while in a seated
position and maintaining a neutral facial expression. Partici- face width-to-height ratio
pants were escorted to separate rooms for the PSAP Figure 2. Scatter plot depicting the relationship between face
procedure. The PSAP took approximately 40 min to width-to-height ratio and aggressive behaviour in under-
complete, after which they completed a brief questionnaire graduate men (nZ37, rZ0.38 and pZ0.02).
designed to assess whether they were aware of the deception
used in the experiment. Responses confirmed that partici- 3. RESULTS
pants believed that they were playing against another person. (a) Study 1
Tests for gender differences in trait dominance, facial ratio
and aggressive behaviour consisted of MANOVA and
(f ) Aggression measure in studies 2 and 3 follow-up t-tests. There was a main effect of gender
The penalty minutes that each player accrued per number of (F5,82Z3.04, pZ0.01): men had a greater facial ratio
games played during the 2007–2008 season (obtained from the (t86Z2.33, pZ0.02, Cohen’s dZ0.50); scored higher on
Ontario University Athletics website for study 2 and from trait dominance (t86Z2.15, pZ0.04, Cohen’s dZ0.46);
ESPN’s website for study 3) were used as the measure of and were more aggressive than women (t86Z2.18, pZ0.03,
aggression. Penalties included behaviours such as slashing, Cohen’s dZ0.47). Men and women did not differ in reward
cross-checking, high-sticking, boarding, elbowing, checking (t86ZK0.80, pZ0.43, Cohen’s dZ0.18) or protection
from behind, fighting and so on. These behaviours meet the (t86Z0.66, pZ0.51, Cohen’s dZ0.15) responses (table 1).
classic definition of aggressive behaviour as any act that is Separate regression analyses for men and women were
intended to harm another individual, who, in turn, is motivated computed with trait dominance and face ratio as predictors
to avoid the behaviour (Baron & Richardson 1994). of aggressive behaviour. For men, face ratio predicted
15 per cent of unique variance in aggressive behaviour
(g) Statistics (R 2Z0.18, F2,34Z3.60, pZ0.04; t36Z2.50, pZ0.02;
Gender differences in trait dominance and facial width- figure 2), but trait dominance was not a significant
to-height ratio were examined using multivariate analysis of predictor of aggression ( pZ0.27). Furthermore, the face
variance (MANOVA), with follow-up t-tests. Multiple linear ratio by trait dominance interaction was not significant
regression analysis was used to examine the relationship (R 2changeZ0.001, F1,33Z0.04, pZ0.84). For women, face
between predictor variables (trait dominance, facial width- ratio and trait dominance did not predict aggressive
to-height ratio and trait dominance by facial width-to-height behaviour (R 2Z0.03, F2,41Z0.66, pZ0.52), nor did the
ratio interaction) and aggressive behaviour as measured by interaction (R 2changeZ0.003, F1,40Z0.14, pZ0.72).
the PSAP. The main assumptions underlying linear
regression (e.g. outliers, linear relationship between pre- (b) Study 2
dictors and criterion, multicollinearity, independence of Individual differences in face ratio in male hockey players
observations, homoscedasticity and errors normally distrib- explained 29.2 per cent of the variance in penalty minutes
uted) were examined and were all met. Also, Pearson’s per game played (rZ0.54, pZ0.01; figure 3).
correlation coefficients were computed to examine the
bivariate association between the facial width-to-height ratio (c) Study 3
and aggressive behaviour in varsity and professional ice Individual differences in the face width-to-height
hockey players. All analyses conducted were two-tailed and explained a significant proportion of the variance in
the level of significance was set at p!0.05. aggressive behaviour (rZ0.30, pZ0.005) in NHL hockey
(a) 3.5
3.0
penalty minutes
2.5
2.0
1.5
1.0
0.5
0
1.5 1.7 1.9 2.1 2.3 2.5
face width-to-height ratio
(b) 3.5 (c)
penalty minutes
3.0
2.5
2.0
1.5
1.0
0.5
0
(d ) 3.5 (e)
3.0
penalty minutes
2.5
2.0
1.5
1.0
0.5
0
( f ) 3.5 ( g)
3.0
penalty minutes
2.5
2.0
1.5
1.0
0.5
0
1.5 1.7 1.9 2.1 2.3 2.5 1.5 1.7 1.9 2.1 2.3 2.5
face width-to-height ratio face width-to-height ratio
Figure 4. Scatter plots depicting the relationship between face width-to-height ratio and aggressive behaviour (number of
penalty minutes per game played) in male professional hockey players for the six Canadian teams in the NHL (nZ112) as a
group (a) and for each individual team (b–g): (a) all Canadian NHL teams (rZ0.30), (b) Calgary Flames (rZ0.17),
(c) Edmonton Oilers (rZ0.20), (d ) Montreal Canadiens (rZ0.39), (e) Ottawa Senators (rZ0.51), ( f ) Toronto Maple Leafs
(rZ0.37) and ( g) Vancouver Canucks (rZ0.24).
Cherek, D. R. 1981 Effects of smoking different doses of Peskin, M. & Newell, F. N. 2004 Familiarity breeds
nicotine on human aggressive behaviour. Psychopharma- attraction: effects of exposure on the attractiveness of
cology 75, 339–345. (doi:d10.1007/BF00435849) typical and distinctive faces. Perception 33, 147–157.
Costa, P., Terracciano, A. & McCrae, R. 2001 Gender (doi:10.1068/p5028)
differences in personality traits across cultures: robust and Rhodes, G. 2006 The evolutionary psychology of facial
surprising findings. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 322–331. beauty. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 199–226. (doi:10.1146/
(doi:10.1037//0022-3514.81.2.322) annurev.psych.57.102904.190208)
Danel, D. & Pawlowski, B. 2007 Eye-mouth-eye angle as a Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W. & Peters, M. 2005 Attractive-
good indicator of face masculinization, asymmetry, and ness and sexual behavior: does attractiveness enhance
attractiveness (Homo sapiens). J. Comp. Psychol. 121, mating success. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 186–201. (doi:10.
221–225. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.121.2.221) 1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.014)
Folstad, I. & Karter, A. J. 1992 Parasites, bright males, and Rhodes, G., Yoshikawa, Y., Palermo, R., Simmons, L. W.,
the immunocompetence handicap. Am. Nat. 139, Peters, M., Lee, K., Halberstadt, J. & Crawford, J. R. 2007
603–622. (doi:10.1086/285346) Perceived health contributes to the attractiveness of facial
Gerra, G., Zaimovic, A., Raggi, M. A., Moi, G., Branchi, B., symmetry, averageness, and sexual dimorphism. Perception
Moroni, M. & Brambilla, F. 2007 Experimentally induced 36, 1244–1252. (doi:10.1068/p5712)
aggressiveness in heroin-dependent patients treated with Roney, J. R., Hanson, K. N., Durante, K. M. & Maestripieri,
buprenorphine: comparison of patients receiving metha- D. 2006 Reading men’s faces: women’s mate attractive-
done and healthy subjects. Psychiatry Res. 149, 201–203. ness judgments track men’s testosterone and interest in
(doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2006.02.013) infant. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 2169–2175. (doi:10.1098/rspb.
Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., 2006.3569)
Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R. & Gough, H. 2006 The Rule, N. O. & Ambady, N. 2008 The face of success:
international personality item pool and the future of inferences from chief executive officers’ appearance
public-domain personality measures. J. Res. Pers. 40, predict company profits. Psychol. Sci. 19, 109–111.
84–96. (doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007) (doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02054.x)
Golomb, B. A., Cortez-Perez, M., Jaworski, B. A., Mednick, Sisk, C. L. & Zehr, J. L. 2005 Pubertal hormones organize the
S. & Dimsdale, J. 2007 Point Subtraction Aggression adolescent brain and behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 26,
Paradigm: validity of a brief schedule of use. Violence Vict. 163–174. (doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2005.10.003)
22, 95–103. (doi:10.1891/vv-v22i1a006) Slater, A., Bremner, G., Johnson, S. P., Sherwood, P.,
Johnson, T., Burk, J. & Kirkpatrick, L. 2007 Dominance Hayes, R. & Brown, E. 2000 Newborn infants’ preference
and prestige as differential predictors of aggression and for attractive faces: the role of internal and external facial
testosterone levels in men. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, features. Infancy 1, 265–274. (doi:10.1207/S15327078
345–351. (doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.04.003) 1N0102_8)
Laidre, M. E. & Vehrencamp, S. L. 2008 Is bird song a Swaddle, J. P. & Reierson, G. W. 2002 Testosterone increases
reliable signal of aggressive intent? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human
62, 1207–1211. (doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0539-3) males. Proc. R. Soc. B 269, 2285–2289. (doi:10.1098/rspb.
Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., Casey, R. J., Ritter, J. M., 2002.2165)
Rieser-Danner, L. A. & Jenkins, V. Y. 1987 Infant Thornhill, R. & Gangestad, S. W. 1999 Facial attractiveness.
preferences for attractive faces: rudiments of a stereo- Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 452–460. (doi:10.1016/S1364-
type. Dev. Psychol. 23, 363–369. (doi:10.1037/0012- 6613(99)01403-5)
1649.23.3.363) Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A. & Hall, C. C.
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Waitt, C., Tiddeman, B. P., 2005 Inferences of competence from faces predict election
Feinberg, D. R., Perrett, D. I., Apicella, C. L. & Marlow, outcomes. Science 308, 1623–1626. (doi:10.1126/science/
F. W. 2008 Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in 1110589)
faces: data across culture and species. PLoS ONE 3, 1–8. Vanpe, C. et al. 2007 Antler size provides an honest signal of
(doi:10.1371/journal.oone.0002106) male phenotypic quality in roe deer. Am. Nat. 169,
Mazur, A., Halpern, C. & Udry, J. R. 1994 Dominant looking 481–493. (doi:10.1086/512046)
male teenagers copulate earlier. Ethol. Sociobiol. 15, 87–94. Verdonck, A., Gaethofs, M., Carels, C. & de Zegher, F. 1999
(doi:10.1016/0162-3095(94)90019-1) Effect of low-dose testosterone treatment on craniofacial
McCormick, C. M. & Mathews, I. Z. 2007 HPA function in growth in boys with delayed puberty. Eur. J. Orthod. 21,
adolescence: role of sex hormones in its regulation and the 137–143. (doi:10.1093/ejo/21.2.137)
enduring consequences of exposure to stressors. Pharma- Verplaetse, J., Vanneste, S. & Braeckman, J. 2007 You can
col. Biochem. Behav. 86, 220–223. (doi:10.1016/j.pbb. judge a book by its cover: the sequel. A kernel of truth in
2006.07.012) predicting cheating detection. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28,
Morestz, J. A. & Morris, M. R. 2006 Phylogenetic analysis of 260–271. (doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehave.2007.04.006)
the evolution of a signal of aggressive intent in northern Weston, E. M., Friday, A. E., Johnstone, R. A. & Schrenk, F.
swordtail fishes. Am. Nat. 168, 336–349. (doi:10.1086/ 2004 Wide faces or large canines? The attractive versus the
506920) aggressive primate. Proc. R. Soc. B 271, S416–S419.
Mueller, U. & Mazur, A. 1996 Facial dominance of West (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0203)
Point cadets as a predictor of later military rank. Soc. Forces Weston, E. M., Friday, A. E. & Lio, P. 2007 Biometric
74, 823–850. (doi:10.2307/2580383) evidence that sexual selection has shaped the hominin
Penton-Voak, I. S. & Chen, J. Y. 2004 High salivary face. PLoS ONE. 2, 1–8. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.
testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance 0000710)
in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 229–241. (doi:10.1016/ Willis, J. & Todorov, A. 2006 First impressions: making up
j.evolhumbehav.2004.04.003) your mind after a 100-Ms exposure to a face. Psychol. Sci.
Penton-Voak, I. S., Pound, N., Little, A. C. & Perrett, D. I. 17, 592–598. (doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x)
2006 Personality judgments from natural and composite Zebrowitz, L. A., Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A. & Rhodes, G.
facial images: more evidence for a “kernel of truth” in 2002 Looking smart and looking good: facial cues to
social perception. Soc. Cogn. 24, 490–524. (doi:10.1521/ intelligence and their origins. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28,
soco.2006.24.5.607) 238–249. (doi:10.1177/0146167202282009)