Modes of Meaning in High School Physics
Modes of Meaning in High School Physics
Modes of Meaning in High School Physics
in
University of Wisconsin-Madison
1
Data for this paper come from the project, “The Socialization of Diverse Learners into Subject Matter
Discourse,” Jane Zuengler and Cecilia Ford, Principal Investigators. The project is part of the Center on English
Learning and Achievement (CELA), which is supported by the U.S. Department of Education’s Office of
Educational Research and Improvement (OERI Award #R305A60005). However, the views expressed herein are
those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the U.S. Department of Education, of CELA, or of
In schools around the world students are exposed to instructional input from a variety of sources.
In many situations, teachers provide a large part of the input by speaking and interacting with
their students but, in addition, instructional input is available from other sources that include
fellow students, textbooks, teacher-produced materials, and instructional media ranging from the
chalk board to the computer. Each of these various inputs to students has characteristic
situational and linguistic properties, such that we may regard each as a separate situationally
defined variety – or register – of the language of instruction. One example of this difference in
registers is the difference between the characteristics of the spoken English used in classroom
interaction between teacher and students and the written English of the students’ textbooks. In
this paper, we will be concerned to describe these differences and to draw implications from
them for the complementary and overlapping ways in which the two registers construct the
Thanks to a research tradition going back at least 30 years, we now know much about the
differences between speech and writing as modes of communication and the differences between
spoken and written English in particular (Biber, 1988; Chafe & Tannen, 1987; Halliday, 1989;
Kroch & Hindle, 1982; Tannen, 1982; and see Atkinson & Biber, 1994, for a review). The
functional and formal distinctions that have been identified between speech and writing are, of
course, applicable to a very wide range of specific instances of spoken and written texts. Thus
while this high level distinction will be useful in understanding major differences between
Variation across Speech and Writing 3
teacher talk and textbooks, there are also more specific differences due to the educational context
of these instances of speech and writing. Again, we have considerable knowledge about the
characteristics of spoken language used in classrooms (Barnes, Britton, & Rosen, 1971; Lemke,
1990; Mehan, 1979; Sinclair & Coulthard, 1992) and about the written registers of school
textbooks (Biber, 1991; Halliday & Martin, 1993; Martin, 1985; Taylor, 1983).
We are not aware, however, of any research that has been done to compare the
presentation of related instructional material by the teacher and in the textbook. To understand
the degrees to which textbook and teacher presentations of related subject matter overlap and
register variation. Such a study is also relevant to several other fields besides applied linguistics.
novices are socialized into both the written and spoken practices of the field. Second, in
educational practice, curricula may be better designed and teachers’ instructional practices may
be better informed if teachers and textbook authors understand the differences between how
knowledge of a field is constructed through live face-to-face interaction and through a textbook.
And then, in assessment, it is important to understand the degree to which the tests and quizzes,
which provide institutional evidence of student learning, reflect the ways that instruction is
provided by the teacher and the textbook. These three questions motivate the study that we
report on here.
Variation across Speech and Writing 4
The focus for the present study is a 12th-grade physics class that was videotaped regularly
during the spring 1997 semester at an inner-city high school in the American Midwest. A total
of 24 class periods were taped and field notes were made of the print materials that were used by
the students in and out of class. The data were collected as part of an ongoing longitudinal
project on academic language socialization (Ford, Zuengler, & Young, 1995). We report in this
paper on a study of the presentation of one topic from a class period about half way through the
semester and the presentation of the same topic in the textbook. The topic of the lesson and the
textbook is reflection in a plane mirror. The two texts on which we focus are presented below.
Figure 1 is the transcript of about one minute’s classroom interaction beginning with a student’s
question to the teacher and ending when the teacher overtly closes the topic by saying “But I
don’t wanna spend any more time on this” and then moves on to a different activity.2
The textbook assigned to the class is Conceptual Physics (Hewitt, 1992) and we show in
Figure 2 a passage two paragraphs (20 lines) long that begins immediately under the section
2
The oral interaction in the class is transcribed according to the conventions developed by Gail Jefferson
and published in Atkinson and Heritage (1984). Speakers are identified by initial only, and the teacher is “T.” If a
speaker cannot be identified, the label “?” is used. The instructional phases of the lesson are marked using the
heading “29.3 Mirrors” and ends just before the topic shifts in the third paragraph to reflection in
a curved mirror.
These two texts are tiny because this is an initial study carried out in order to generate
hypotheses about the differences among the text and the teacher talk. The conclusions that we
present are necessarily tentative because they are based on a small amount of data, which we
nonetheless believe to be representative of two much larger corpora of interactions and texts.
Before beginning our analysis, we also wish to distinguish our approach from that of others
working on similar problems of instructional discourse. First, we stress that in comparing these
two texts, we treat them as exemplars of two registers that interest us. We are not considering
them primarily as inputs to the students in this particular class, for while we can identify the
students that participated in the lesson, we cannot be sure which of them – if any – read the
passage in the textbook. Second, our analysis of the teacher talk makes only occasional
reference to the interactional nature of the discourse, and we carry out our analysis of the
textbook with barely a mention of the interaction in real time between reader and text. In other
words, we have severely limited our focus on interaction in this paper. We do this, not because
we believe that interaction is not important to the understanding of instruction – quite the reverse
is true, but rather because it is necessary to understand the two texts qua text before going on to
Because we are looking through these texts at a high school science lesson, it will be
important to understand the ways in which science – in particular physics – is done through
language. Here, again, there is an extensive literature on many aspects of language in scientific
activity (Bazerman, 1988; Elliot, 1975; Gunnarsson, 1993, 1997; Halliday, 1988; Halliday &
Martin, 1993; Harris, 1990; Jacoby, 1998; Jacoby & Gonzalez, 1991; Lemke, 1990; Myers,
1985, 1994; Ochs & Jacoby, 1997; Ochs, Jacoby, & Gonzalez, 1994; and see also the
bibliography of empirical studies of scientific register in Atkinson & Biber, 1994). The language
of school science textbooks has been a topic of particular interest in the United Kingdom and in
Australia, resulting in the U.K. schools-based Reading for Learning in the Sciences project and
the work carried out in systemic functional linguistic framework in Australia (Halliday & Martin,
1993; Martin, 1985). And it is this latter framework that we have adopted in our present
analysis.
Scholars studying variation across registers have adopted several different methods of analysis.
One approach (e.g., Biber, 1988) has been to analyze the formal properties of texts from different
registers and to compare the formal features of one register with another. Another approach
(e.g., Swales, 1990) has been to investigate the context of situation and the purposes associated
with certain texts and to compare these situational and functional parameters with those of other
texts. In our own analysis, we have chosen to follow, first, Biber (1994) by outlining and
comparing the situational and communicative parameters of the registers of teacher talk and
Variation across Speech and Writing 7
textbook. In comparing the two texts themselves, however, we have chosen not to limit our
analysis to the identification and comparison of lexical and grammatical features across texts, as
Biber and others have done in their multidimensional quantitative analyses of registers. Instead,
we attempt to understand how the two texts function as instances of communication and we have
carried out a systemic-functional analysis of the two texts following Halliday & Martin (1993).
purposes to which language is put in communication. In SFL, the functions of language are
central, that is to say, systemic functional linguists focus on what speakers do through the
medium of language and how they do it, in contrast to more structural approaches (including
Biber’s multidimensional analysis), which focus on the forms of language and their
combinations. SFL thus starts with the interpersonal context in which language is used and
Situational Parameters
Biber (1994) provides an explicit and detailed framework for describing the situational
parameters of registers. This is a useful basis for comparison of the two texts and is divided into
seven major headings, (1) the communicative characteristics of participants, (2) the relations
between addressor and addressee, (3) the setting, (4) the channel, (5) the relation of participants
to the text, (6) the purposes, intents, and goals of the communication, and (7) the topic or subject
Variation across Speech and Writing 8
with those of the textbook. Of the several differences that are apparent from this table we would
like to point out two. First, in Section 2 of the table, the roles of addressor and addressee differ
most markedly in the degree of shared personal knowledge and in the degree of interactiveness.
The teacher and the students in this class have been together for at least one semester before the
recording was made and know each other well, whereas the students have no personal
knowledge about the author of the textbook and he has no personal knowledge of them.
Moreover, the focal class involves extensive interaction among the teacher and the students,
whereas the interaction of the readers and the text is much more limited.
Another marked difference is in Section 5, which deals with the addressor’s attitudinal
stance toward the text. On the one hand, the teacher appears emotionally invested in his
interaction with the class. He uses wide pitch range and first and second person pronouns; he
also takes a humorous stance and makes jokes that are appreciated by the students such as his
imitation of a ray gun in turns 210 and 212. In contrast, the textbook author takes a more
removed stance toward his topic. His first paragraph contains only third-person references and
while he addresses his reader directly in the second paragraph beginning “Your eye cannot
ordinarily tell the difference,” there are no first-person references. There is a degree of humor in
the second illustration accompanying the text, but none in the text itself. We will explore the
consequences of these situational differences further as we analyze the semantic properties of the
texts themselves.
Variation across Speech and Writing 9
There are clear advantages to a systemic-functional analysis rather than a purely formal analysis
of these texts since we are concerned to elucidate how each works as an instance of
communication. In our analysis, we have followed quite closely the methods used by Halliday
and Martin in Writing science: Literacy and discursive power (Halliday & Martin, 1993). We
have done so because the systemic functional analysis that Halliday and Martin provide is more
empirically based than other SFL analyses we have seen and also because their analysis was
developed to deal with science texts similar to the written text that we are investigating here.
Our analysis focuses on three ways in which meaning is communicated through the two texts: (1)
the means by which physical and mental reality is construed in the texts, (2) the degree of
abstractness or concreteness of the texts, and (3) the rhetorical structure of scientific reasoning
that the texts demonstrate. In SFL terminology, these three features are referred to as
Halliday & Martin (1993) recognize three complementary ways in which texts have meaning:
texts are designed to represent the material processes of the physical and biological world, they
are also attempts to influence the social world through verbal and mental processes, and
moreover texts are organized through the relational processes of self-reference. Examples of
Material processes construct “a world of action in which physical and biological entities
interact, by themselves, or on other things” (Halliday & Martin, 1993, p. 27) and are generally
Verbal and mental processes construct “a world of semiotic activity in which typically
conscious entities negotiate meaning” (Halliday & Martin, 1993, p. 27) and are often realized by
which things can be without doing” (Halliday & Martin, 1993, p. 28) and are often represented
208)
Tables 2–4 allows us to compare the ways in which these two texts realize these three
modes of meaning, and we find similarities and differences between them. Comparing first
material processes in Table 2, we find that material processes are used by both the textbook
author and the teacher with high frequency, probably because of the expository nature of the two
texts. A marked difference, however, is that while the textbook uses passive voice for the two
action verbs “placed,” and “are reflected,” the teacher talk has no passive voice at all. The
teacher’s active action verbs are mostly first person and even when the verb is third person, the
referent is still the teacher, as in “does my height get different?” A consequence of this
difference is that the teacher talk places the teacher at the center of action – a participant in the
material process of reflection in a plane mirror – and indexes a direct and live relationship
between the speaker and his audience. A good example of the participation of the teacher in the
scientific process he describes is the way he casts himself in turns 210–212 as a cartoon
character, a role that may have emotional appeal to his teenage students.
?: Yeah.=
height.
In contrast, third-person verbs in the textbook cast the writer and the reader as observers,
and keep the reader at a distance from the process. In so doing, the textbook presents scientific
processes as observed objective entities independent of the reader and the writer, while the
teacher brings science into the established lived relationship between the speaker and the hearer.
Thus while the textbook casts the reader as an observer, the teacher casts himself – and by
Turning now to verbal and mental processes, as Table 3 shows, the textbook appears to
have a slightly higher density of verbs representing mental processes than appear in the teacher
talk. The textbook author tries by means of imperative mood to direct the reader’s mental
processes. For example, the textbook has “Consider a candle flame placed in front of a plane
(flat) mirror,” “Note that the rays diverge,” “Notice that the image is far behind the mirror,”
“Notice also that,” and “Figure 29-4 shows only two rays.” On the other hand, the teacher
invokes mental processes at the beginning of his exposition, which serves to focus his students’
attention and engage their cognition: “Well, think about it” (turn 202), and “Let’s say” (turn
Variation across Speech and Writing 13
202). And then once more in turn 210 to direct students’ attention to his nonverbal imitation of
The slight discrepancy between the textbook and the teacher talk may perhaps be
explained by the different modes of communication: The text is the only medium available to the
textbook writer to direct the reader to the points he wants them to focus on. The teacher, on the
other hand, can focus his students’ attention by his body movement, facial expression, gaze
direction, finger pointing, and the prosody of his speech as well as linguistically by means of
Finally, relational processes in both the textbook and the teacher talk are realized most
often by a copula “be.” As Table 4 shows, however, the textbook author uses a far wider range
of ways of expressing relational meaning in the textbook than does the teacher. The teacher talk
contains only the copula “be” and one other verb, “look” in “look smaller.” The textbook, on the
other hand, not only has copula “be” and verbs of near-being, it also has a wide range of less
obviously relational verbs such as “every one obeys the law of reflection,” “the image … is
called a virtual image,” “[light] … behaves virtually as if it did.” This greater range of
vocabulary for expressing relational processes in the textbook has the advantage of precision but,
at the same time, it may make it harder for the readers to comprehend that a relation between
Halliday & Martin (1993, p. 29) discuss several ways in which meaning is condensed in
scientific discourse. One way is by the use of technical terms, in which commonsense meanings
are compacted. Another way of condensing meaning frequently used in science discourse is by
meanings in each noun phrase and, in order to derive or “unpack” the commonsense meaning of
a nominalized phrase, the reader needs to use the co-text and the more general context of the
text. For example, a compact noun phrase such as “the law of reflection” is a grammatical
metaphor for the non-nominalized phrase “the law of how things reflect light.” The technical
term “reflect” also needs to be unpacked, in order to yield the commonsense meaning, “the law
Since these kinds of condensed meaning are characteristic of science texts, it is not
surprising that both technical terms and nominalizations are frequent in both the textbook and
teacher talk. The difference between the two texts lies in the degree to which the condensed
meanings are unpacked within the text itself. The textbook leaves the reader to do most of the
work of unpacking, while the teacher does all the work of unpacking himself in his subsequent
exposition, mainly by his gestures and body movement but also by answering his own rhetorical
This explicit visual unpacking that the teacher provides seems to indicate a marked
difference between the teacher talk and the textbook. Moreover the nominalizations in the
textbook require two stages of unpacking. First, the nominalization needs to be unpacked into a
verb indicating a process, and then the technical term needs to be unpacked into non-technical
language. Thus the condensed meanings in the textbook are removed from the base meaning by
at least two levels, while those in the teacher talk are removed by only one. In addition, the
teacher is able to make extensive use of nonverbal resources to unpack technical terms, while the
only nonverbal resource that is available in the textbook is a diagram. These differences
highlight the need for more complex cognitive processing by the reader of the textbook than by
introduction of a new technical term “virtual image.” This term is marked as new and technical
by its appearance in bold face and by the explicit packing of nontechnical terms in the definition
that precedes it: “The image of the candle the person sees in the mirror is called a virtual
image….” As we shall see in our analysis of assessment, this process of packing and unpacking
A third way of characterizing science texts according to Halliday and Martin (1993) is by setting
out the rhetorical structure of reasoning in scientific explanations. This can be done at two
Variation across Speech and Writing 16
levels: first, at a macro level, which shows the topical organization of the text as a whole, and
then at a micro level, which sets out the conjunctive relations between adjacent clauses in the
text.
Halliday & Martin (1993, p. 244) view texts as having different levels of thematic organization.
Above the level of clausal theme, there is a hyper-theme, which is a clause that can predict “a
kind of overarching theme, which serves the metacognitive function of aiding the comprehension
of the text as a whole. In this sense, it is similar to the topic sentence of a paragraph. Texts may
also have a hyper-new, which is an accumulated expression of the pattern of new selections that
make up the point of the text. Hyper-new is thus like a summary of the text’s point and also
helps the comprehension of text (Halliday & Martin, 1993, p. 247). On a yet broader level, there
are macro-themes and macro-news, which relate hyper-themes together and hyper-news together,
or more hyper-news” (ibid., p. 249). Table 6 compares the thematic organization of the textbook
In the textbook excerpt, the author has an advantage that the section title serves as the
macro-theme and signals the topic of the section. He also has two hyper-themes in the first
sentence of each paragraph to signal their topics. The teacher talk in contrast does not seem to
have an explicit macro-theme, except for the expectation, or schema, that the teacher and
students share of what will be done in this going-over-the-homework section of the lesson. Thus
an implicit macro-theme may be inferred from the sequential organization of the lesson. The
teacher talk hyper-theme, on the other hand, is provided interactionally by the student’s question
in turn 200, and the teacher’s orientation to answer that question over the next several turns.
Thus the teacher constructs the student’s question as the hyper-theme of his subsequent talk.
The thematic organization toward the end of each of the two texts differs markedly. The
author provides no explicit hyper-new in the textbook immediately following the presentation of
new information. No hyper-new is provided until the summary at the end of the chapter – 15
pages further on. This hyper-new is far removed from the text, and although a reader may turn to
3
In our analysis we will only look at topical theme since our concern is how the content of the argument is
laid out. Topical theme in English (following Fries, 1983, p. 116) “is realized as the initial constituent of the clause
or sentence.”
Variation across Speech and Writing 18
this summary, it is quite likely that a long time elapses before a reader reaches the chapter
review. In contrast, the teacher in turn 214 repeats his answer to the student’s question in turn
To summarize the thematic organization of these two texts, the textbook has an explicit
macro-theme and hyper-theme, and an explicit but distant hyper-new at the end of the chapter.
The teacher talk has an implicit macro-theme that may be deduced from the sequential
In addition to the macro-level thematic organization of a text, clauses in a text are linked together
by meaning relations that may be explicitly marked by conjunctions or, alternatively, may be
implicit, i.e., unmarked. These meaning relations, Martin (1983, p.1) has referred to as
conjunction, which he defines as “the semantic system by which speakers relate clauses in terms
relations may or may not be made explicit in a text, most clauses are connected to others through
one or more of these relations. Explicit conjunctions require only that the addressee notice and
understand the conjunction in the text in order to comprehend the connection between the ideas
expressed in clauses; implicit conjunctive relations, meanwhile, require the addressees to bring
Conjunction, as defined by Martin (1983) and other SFL linguists, is only one way in
which speakers create and structure text. Conjunction is a system by means of which speakers
connect the sense or meaning of clauses, using interclausal relations sometimes referred to as
“logical.” Other ways of creating textuality do so by means of relations of reference, that is,
relations between clausal constituents that have the property of referring to other constituents or
to elements outside the text itself. We have already presented one of these systems in our earlier
discussion of textual theme. There are also other systems, including referential relations between
clauses in a text realized by anaphor, lexical linkage, and structural parallelism, as well as
relations between a text and its nonlinguistic context realized by indexical expressions. We
focus in this paper on conjunction because it is the system by which speakers/writers reason
through language and particular patterns of reasoning have been shown to characterize particular
academic disciplines. In studying the ways in which novices are socialized into the discourse
practices of a particular field, it is therefore important to understand the how reasoning is done
The teacher talk is divided into clausal units below and its conjunctive structure is
4
The reticula presented in Figures 3 and 4 follow Martin (1983) and Halliday and Martin (1993).
Conjunctive relations are considered either internal or external. Internal conjunctive relations have an interpersonal
function and are those speech acts often realized in speech by discourse markers (Schiffrin, 1987) and are listed in
Variation across Speech and Writing 20
(a) Nuh- Well, think about it. (b) If I’m standing here, (c) and looking at my face,
(d) Okay here- here Let’s say this is maybe easier to see (e) I’m looking at my
eyeballs (f) and I’m gonna see the top of my head, (g) so I have to look up at an
angle about that much. Right? (h) Cuz I’m this height (i) As I move away, (j) does
my height get different? No. (k) I look smaller, (l) because I’m farther away,
right? (m) But really my- the height- my eye- (n) now look at the rays from my
eye, (o) They’re still going at the same angle, aren’t they. (p) So I must be the
same height. (q) I look smaller (r) cuz I’m farther away.
The textbook passage has been analyzed into clauses in a similar way below and its
(a) Consider a candle flame placed in front of a plane (flat) mirror. (b) Rays of
light are reflected from its surface in all directions. (c) The number of rays is
infinite, (d) and everyone obeys the law of reflection. (e) Figure 29-4 shows only
two rays that originate at the tip of the candle flame (f) and reflect from the mirror
the first column of the figures. External conjunctive relations, on the other hand, are meaning relations between
elements of the text itself and are listed in the third column. All conjunctive relations are classified, at a primary
to someone’s eye. (g) Note that the rays diverge (spread apart) from the tip of the
flame (h) and continue diverging from the mirror upon reflection. (i) These
divergent rays appear to originate from a point located behind the mirror. (j) The
image of the candle the person sees in the mirror is called a virtual image, (k)
because light does not actually pass through the image position (l) but behaves
virtually as if it did. (m) Your eye cannot ordinary tell the difference between an
object and its reflected image. (n) This is because the light that enters your eye is
entering in exactly the same manner, physically, as it would (o) if there really
were an object there. (p) Notice that the image is as far behind the mirror as the
object is in front of the mirror. (q) Notice also that the image and object have the
same size. (r) When you view yourself in a mirror, (s) your image is the same
size your identical twin would appear (t) if located as far behind the mirror as you
The reticula in Figures 3 and 4 reveal a number of similarities between the two texts. The
texts are very similar in length as measured by the number of clauses in each. Both the teacher
and textbook author use mostly explicit conjunctions and most conjunctive relations in both texts
are “external” – that is they relate events in the physical world of light rays, mirrors, and
observers that constitute the field of discourse. Most of the conjunctive relations expressed in
both texts are consequential (cause and effect) and temporal (before, after, and simultaneously),
Variation across Speech and Writing 22
reflecting the fact that both texts are explanations through descriptions of a process. These
relations – as well as comparative (contrast and similarity) and additive conjunctions – connect
most adjacent pairs of clauses in both texts. The teacher appears to use more “internal”
conjunctions (discourse markers like “okay” and “now”) than the textbook writer in organizing
the phases of his presentation for the students, which serve to make the rhetorical structure of the
discourse explicit. On the other hand, the layout of text in the textbook with its typographically
salient heading and its visual division into paragraphs serves the same function.
Although the conjunctive structures of the two texts appear quite similar, there is one
difference between the two texts that a conjunctive analysis reveals. The largest category of
conjunctions in the teacher talk comprises those consequential conjunctions expressing relations
of cause and effect and they realized by only two lexical items: “cuz” or “because” and “so.”
Most conjunctions in the textbook, by contrast, are temporal. This difference implies that while
the teacher describes relations of cause and effect explicitly, the textbook does so implicitly
through the description of a process in time. The difference between teacher talk and textbook
may be attributed to differences in the modality of communication. The textbook writer has no
alternative but to describe the process verbally, whereas the teacher is able to construct the
process in real time through gesture and movement and only has to resort to linguistic
Summary
In our attempt to understand how the teacher talk and textbook function as communication, we
have considered in turn the situational parameters of the two texts, the means through which
physical and mental reality is construed, their level of abstractness, and the rhetorical structure of
scientific reasoning that they embody. Those similarities and differences that we have identified
Conclusions
The purpose of the analysis that we have carried out is fourfold. Our basic aim is to conduct a
contrastive analysis of two registers – to understand the ways in which communication of related
subject matter by the teacher and the textbook overlap and complement each other. We are
equally concerned, however, with three educational issues. We wish to understand, first, how
the two different registers socialize students to ways of thinking and talking about science. We
also wish to draw practical implications for the design of textbooks and for teachers’
instructional practices. And we wish to relate assessment practices to the instructional input that
students receive from teacher and text. Each of these issues will be considered in turn below.
We have compared two very small sample texts from a single teacher and from a specific
textbook, and it would be unwise to generalize about the registers of teacher presentation and
Variation across Speech and Writing 24
textbook presentation of science from such limited data. There are certainly teachers whose style
of presentation is much closer to the discourse of the textbook and there may be textbooks that
attempt a more interactive style of presentation than this one. Nonetheless, we can say that a
basic structure of scientific presentation is common to both texts: They both explain a physical
phenomenon by means of describing a physical process. Both texts describe the process by
means of action verbs and meanings are condensed in technical terms and nominal groups. The
teacher and textbook writer both organize their explanations in similar ways, beginning with a
macro-theme to provide a frame for the topic to follow and a hyper-new to summarize it. And
Martin (1993) has shown, distinguish instructional discourse in science from instructional
The differences we find between the ways in which science is presented by the teacher
and the textbook result from the fact that the teacher has much better personal knowledge of his
students than the textbook writer has of his readers. One of his students comments that the
teacher is “down to earth, [he] puts everything on our level.” Another difference lies in this
teacher’s extensive use of gesture and body movement, which one of his students characterizes
as presenting “things you can see.” In our analysis, we have seen just how the teacher does this
by means of first person verbs and nonverbal communication that together cast him and his
students in the role of human participants in the physical process of reflection in a plane mirror.
The textbook, on the other hand, maintains a distance between the reader and the phenomenon
Variation across Speech and Writing 25
by casting the reader in the role of a third person observer. Once could say that the text helps
construct the reader as objective observer, while the teacher helps construct the students as
subjective participants.
organization of discourse.” When an expert in an academic discipline talks with a novice, the
organization that the expert imposes on the conversation is part of the knowledge of the field that
the novice must acquire in order to become a fully fledged member of the disciplinary discourse
community. In the situation we are describing in this paper, the teacher and textbook writer are
the experts and the students and readers are novices in the field of physics. We may then ask,
how does the organization of the textbook and teacher talk discourse socialize the students and
We have already answered this question in part by showing the specific patterns of
metafunction, stratification, and textuality that are common to both registers because of their
relations between clauses, and use of grammatical metaphor. There are differences, however, in
the degree to which the textbook and the teacher use grammatical metaphor, which are more
frequent in the textbook and which are not explicitly unpacked. We have also noted the
subjective versus objective roles in which the students are cast by the teacher and by the
Variation across Speech and Writing 26
textbook. Many previous analyses of written scientific register in English (e.g., those studies of
research articles summarized in Swales 1990, chapter 7) have identified the same features as
those found in this extract. Scientific objectivity is constructed in these texts by means of the use
of passives, third person verbs, use of nominalizations to encode processes, and high frequency
of technical vocabulary. More recent work on the spoken discourse of lab meetings, however,
has shown that in the course of making sense of scientific research, practicing scientists blur the
distinction between the scientist and the physical world to “construct a referential identity which
is both animate and inanimate, subject and object” (Ochs, Gonzalez, and Jacoby, 1996, p. 328).
In this respect, the teacher’s enactment of the physical process of light reflection (“I look smaller
cuz I’m farther away”) is a very similar referential strategy to that adopted by the practicing
physicists studied by Ochs et al. (“When I come down I’m in the domain state”). Thus the
teacher’s subjective, participatory approach, rather than being a popularization of the objectivity
What can be gleaned from this analysis for the design of more effective science curricula? A
more specific question is: What kind of instruction is more appropriate to the spoken or the
written modality? The students in this class appear to prefer the teacher’s presentations to the
textbook. One student comments that “[I] learn more from the teacher’s discussion than from the
textbook” and all students remark that they find the class fun and interesting. Because the
students were not asked to comment on the textbook, it is impossible to judge the extent to which
Variation across Speech and Writing 27
they used it and whether they considered it a success. We have remarked earlier on the
linguistic features such as passive verbs, more specific vocabulary for expressing relational
processes, absence of an accessible summary (hyper-new) at the end of the passage, and – most
significant perhaps – the lack of unpacking of nominalizations and technical terms. We have
also suggested that this greater cognitive complexity is typical of scientific writing and the
Because the setting of textbook communication is private and because readers do not have to
comprehend the text with the time constraints imposed by oral interaction, there is perhaps
greater probability of the reader comprehending more difficult material than in class.
In contrast, the teacher in his presentation to this class exploits the nonverbal media to
great advantage, using movement of his body to show the path of a ray of light and his voice in
comic imitation of a ray gun. Since almost all the students mention the fact that this class is
“fun,” we can conclude that the teacher is also highly effective in bringing humor and positive
affect into the classroom. These qualities of teacher talk are difficult to achieve in a textbook.
… And Tests
In order to complete the picture of the educational function of these two texts, we must finally
consider the ways in which each text relates to the institutional measures of student learning in
this class: the tests and quizzes administered and graded by the teacher. To do so we have
Variation across Speech and Writing 28
carried out a preliminary analysis of an in-class quiz. Because we have limited access to the
assessment instruments used in this class, we were not able to obtain the written tests themselves
but we are able to reconstruct the items from the videotape of a lesson that took place a few days
after the lesson reproduced in Figure 1. This subsequent lesson included a segment in which the
teacher showed ten test items related to reflection in a plane mirror on an overhead. The students
were given time to answer the items by themselves, after which the teacher went over the
questions and answers with the whole class. Figure 5 is our reconstruction of the items and
We wish to compare the meaning-making functions of language in the quiz with the
conducting a functional analysis of the quiz in a broadly similar way to the way we analyzed the
teacher talk and textbook. We consider the modes of meaning that are realized in the quiz and
then its levels of meaning. We have not analyzed the textuality of the quiz since it consists of ten
Within the quiz, relational verbs such as copula “be”, “is called”, and “appear”.are used
with very high frequency. In fact, nine out the ten items describe relational processes. Material
processes are signified infrequently in the quiz. Only three quiz items refer to material processes
through action verbs: item 1 (“bounce off”), item 2 (“made of,” “deposited”), and item 5 (“look,”
“touch”). Mental processes are not signified by any item in the quiz. Relational processes, are
Variation across Speech and Writing 29
thus the principal metafunction of the quiz, since almost all items require students to signify a
relationship between physical and/or verbal entities. Thus the discourse of the quiz differs from
both the textbook and the teacher talk since these texts make much greater reference to material
With regard to levels of meaning, half of the items in the quiz require students to move
either up or down levels. The students are required to pack commonsense knowledge into
scientific terms, such as in items 1 and 10 and they are also required to unpack scientific terms
into their commonsense meanings as in items 6, 7, and 8. These items ask directly what these
terms mean and thus the test is highly marked with technicality. The purpose of these items
seems to be to assess the students’ understanding of technical terms. The remaining items (2, 3,
4, 5, and 9) require students to correctly name the relationship between physical entities.
The main semiotic activities carried out by the students in answering the quiz involve
them in negotiating and naming relationships among physical entities and in negotiating levels of
meaning. They do the latter through processes of packing and unpacking technical terms such as
“protractor” or denominalizing processes such as transforming “incident ray” into “a ray that
strikes a surface.” Neither of these semiotic activities predominates in either the textbook or the
teacher talk, although a small number are found in both. The textbook contains more relational
processes than the teacher talk and the textbook contains the sole instance of explicit packing in
“The image of the candle the person sees in the mirror is called a virtual image….” Because of
the institutional value that is placed on the results of assessment instruments such as this quiz,
Variation across Speech and Writing 30
these particular kinds of meaning-making activities are privileged in this instructional setting. It
is a question for further research whether the privileging of certain kinds of semiosis over others
* * * * * *
out within the framework of systemic functional linguistics. We have analyzed in depth only
very small samples of teacher talk and textbook and we do not wish to generalize from these
samples until more data have been analyzed. Our preliminary results within this framework
show the contrasting ways in which teachers and textbooks make meaning within the academic
discipline of physics. And our analysis has several possible implications for education, including
the socialization of students to ways of thinking and talking about science, the meaning-making
activities that are most appropriate for live teacher presentation and those that work best in print,
and the relationship between institutional assessment practices and the curriculum.
References
Atkinson, D., & Biber, D. (1994). Register: A review of empirical research. In D. Biber
& E. Finegan (Eds.), Sociolinguistic perspectives on register (pp. 351–385). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Variation across Speech and Writing 31
Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis (pp. ix–xvi). New
Barnes, D., Britton, J., & Rosen, H. (1971). Language, the learner and the school.
Bazerman, C. (1988). Shaping written knowledge: The genre and activity of the
Biber, D. (1988). Variation across speech and writing. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Biber, D. (1991). Oral and literate characteristics of selected primary school reading
Finegan (Eds.), Sociolinguistic perspectives on register (pp. 31–56). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Chafe, W. L., & Tannen, D. (1987). The relation between written and spoken language.
Elliot, H. C. (1975). Similarities and differences between science and common sense. In
Middlesex: Penguin.
Ford, C., Zuengler, J., & Young, R. (1995). Socialization into subject matter discourse:
A longitudinal study of diverse learners, [Worldwide web page]. Proposal submitted to the
http://www.wcer.wisc.edu/CELA/Socialization_in2Subject_Matter/.
S. Petöfi & E. Sözer (Eds.), Micro and macro connexity of texts (pp. 116-152). Hamburg:
Gunnarsson, B.-L. (1993). Pragmatic and macrothematic patterns in science and popular
science: A diachronic study of articles from three fields. In M. Ghadessy (Ed.), Register
B.-L. Gunnarsson, P. Linell, & B. Nordberg (Eds.), The construction of professional discourse
Registers of written English: Situational factors and linguistic features (pp. 162–178). London
Halliday, M. A. K. (1989). Spoken and written language. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Halliday, M. A. K., & Martin, J. R. (Eds.). (1993). Writing science: Literacy and
science textbooks. In U. Connor & A. M. Johns (Eds.), Coherence in writing: Research and
Other Languages.
Hewitt, P. G. (1992). Conceptual physics: The high school science program. (2nd ed.).
Jacoby, S. (1998). Rethinking EST: What can “indigenous assessment” tell us about the
Kroch, A., & Hindle, D. (1982). A quantitative study of the syntax of speech and
Lemke, J. L. (1990). Talking science: Language, learning, and values. Norwood, NJ:
Ablex.
Variation across Speech and Writing 34
Martin, J. R. (1983). Conjunction: The logic of English text. In J. S. Petöfi & E. Sözer
(Eds.), Micro and macro connexity of texts (pp. 1–72). Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag.
Martin, J. R. (1985). Factual writing: Exploring and challenging social reality. New
Martin, J. R. (1993). Life as a noun: Arresting the universe in science and the
humanities. In M. A. K. Halliday & J. R. Martin (Eds.), Writing science: Literacy and discursive
Madden & C. L. Myers (Eds.), Discourse and performance of international teaching assistants
Myers, G. (1985). Texts as knowledge claims: The social construction of two biology
socialization across cultures (pp. 1-13). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ochs, E., Gonzalez, P., & Jacoby, S. (1996). “When I come down I'm in the domain
state”: Grammar and graphic representation in the interpretive activity of physicists. In E. Ochs,
Variation across Speech and Writing 35
E. A. Schegloff, & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Interaction and grammar. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Ochs, E., & Jacoby, S. (1997). Down to the wire: the cultural clock of physicists and the
Ochs, E., Jacoby, S., & Gonzalez, P. (1994). Interpretive journeys: How physicists talk
(Ed.), Advances in spoken discourse analysis (pp. 1–34). New York: Routledge.
Swales, J. M. (1990). Genre analysis: English in academic and research settings. New
Tannen, D. (Ed.). (1982). Spoken and written language: Exploring orality and literacy.
Taylor, C. V. (1983). Structure and theme in printed school text. Text, 3, 197–228.
Variation across Speech and Writing 36
Figure 1. Transcript of Part of a 12th-Grade Physics Lesson on the Topic of Reflection in a Plane
Mirror
Variation across Speech and Writing 37
Figure 2. Extract from a High School Physics Textbook on the Topic of Reflection in a Plane
Mirror. Source: Hewitt, P. G. (1992). Conceptual physics: The high school science program.
b
explicit/temporal “and”
c
explicit/additive “okay”
d
e
explicit/temporal “and”
f
explicit/consequential “so”
g
explicit/consequential “cuz”
h
i
explicit/temporal “as”
j
implicit/comparative (as if)
k
explicit/consequential “because”
l
explicit/comparative “but really”
m
explicit/additive “now”
n
o
explicit/consequential “so”
p
q
explicit/consequential “cuz”
r
Figure 3. Conjunctive Relations in the Teacher Talk (Conjunctions which may be used
a
implicit/temporal (at that time)
b
c
explicit/additive “and”
d
e
explicit/temporal “and”
f
g
explicit/temporal “and”
h
j
explicit/consequential “because”
k
explicit/comparative “but”
l
m
explicit/consequential “because”
n
explicit/consequential “if”
o
p
explicit/additive “also”
q
r
explicit/temporal “when”
s
explicit/consequential “if”
t
explicit/consequential “as long as”
u
Notes: “–” indicates parameter is undetermined. Table and categories based on Biber (1994).
Variation across Speech and Writing 42
Table 2. Material Processes Expressed by Action Verbs in the Teacher Talk and Textbook
“does my height get different?” (turn 206) [the rays] continue diverging
“they’re still going at the same angle” (turn light does not actually pass through
“Well, think about it” (turn 202) Consider a candle flame placed in front of
“Let’s say” (turn 202). Figure 29-4 shows only two rays
“I’m gonna see the top of my head “ (turn 204) Note that the rays diverge
“now look at the rays from my eyes” (turn the image of the candle the person sees in the
210). mirror
and Textbook
“this is maybe easier to see” (turn 202) The number of rays is infinite
“I’m this height” (turn 204) and every one obeys the law of reflection.
“I look smaller” (turn 208) The image of the candle the person sees in the
This is because
“I look smaller” (turn 214)
Table 5. Implicit and Explicit Unpacking and Packing of Condensed Meanings in the
Teacher Talk and Textbook
Hyper-Theme: If he’s three feet away, Hyper-Theme: Your eyes cannot ordinarily
reflected image.
Table 7. Summary of Similarities and Differences in the Teacher Talk and Textbook
SITUATIONAL PARAMETERS
Shared personal knowledge of teacher and students No shared personal knowledge
Teacher takes a humorous, emotionally involved Writer takes a removed, emotionally uninvolved
stance toward text stance toward the text