Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review: The International Journal of Indian Psychology July 2017

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/318585137

Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

Article  in  The International Journal of Indian Psychology · July 2017


DOI: 10.25215/0404.013

CITATIONS READS

3 3,299

2 authors:

Pradeep Gupta Vibha Sharma


Defence Research and Development Organisation Institute of Human Behaviour & Allied Sciences
11 PUBLICATIONS   4 CITATIONS    20 PUBLICATIONS   75 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Group CBT with Dhat syndrome pts. View project

Mental Health & Psychological well-being of Clinical Psychology Trainees View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Pradeep Gupta on 21 July 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


The International Journal of Indian Psychology
ISSN 2348-5396 (e) | ISSN: 2349-3429 (p)
Volume 4, Issue 4, DIP: 18.01.013/20170404
DOI: 10.25215/0404.013
http://www.ijip.in | July-September, 2017
Review

Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

Pradeep Kumar Gupta 1*, Dr. Vibha Sharma 2

ABSTRACT
Working Memory provides a mental work place to support everyday cognitive activities that
require both processing and storage. It has been found to be associated with a range of
cognitive functions. Recently, a strong link between working memory and learning has been
reported. This finding can be implicated in learning disabilities. It is likely that limited
working memory functioning of children with learning disabilities hampers their learning
skills; possibly because of the crucial role played by working memory in learning process.
The current paper reviews working memory functioning in subtypes of learning disabilities
(Dyslexia, Dysgraphia and Dyscalculia) and provides a detailed account of pattern of deficit
seen in working memory in the three subtypes of LD. Review of literature suggests that
working memory functions differently in children with LD than normal achieving children.
Children with LD often fail to learn because high working memory demand of the learning
task often exceeds their working memory capacity. Some researchers suggest that working
memory deficits are not entirely a capacity deficit, rather a strategy deficit. Evidences suggest
that learning performance of these children can be improved by teaching appropriate strategy
to reduce working memory load of the task, and through remedial training. Failure to identify
working memory problems at early stage and rectify resultant learning problems may leave
the child struggling with this invisible handicap and can even lead to dropout from school.

Keywords: Working Memory, Cognitive functions, Learning Disability (LD), Dyslexia,


Dysgraphia, Dyscalculia

Working memory is a system for the temporary holding and manipulation of information
during the performance of a range of cognitive tasks such as comprehension, learning, and
reasoning (Baddeley, 1986). It is an online memory configuration in which certain
information is held in awareness or span of attention for use in an ongoing information
processing. It’s a kind of mental workplace that we use for many aspects in our everyday life
including reading, mental arithmetic and planning a series of thought or actions. The primary
function of working memory is to facilitate and enhance the capacity of encoding, storage,

1
Clinical Psychologist, Deep Chand Bandhu Hospital, New Delhi, India
2
Associate Professor, Department of Clinical Psychology, Institute of Human Behaviour & Allied Sciences,
Delhi, India
*Responding Author
Received: June 17, 2017; Revision Received: July 2, 2017; Accepted: July 20, 2017
© 2017 Gupta P K, Sharma V; licensee IJIP. This is an Open Access Research distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License (www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any Medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

and retrieval functions that are essential for learning and higher level processing of
information.

Working memory tasks require one to hold on one important piece of information while
processing the other, for example - attempting to solve a mathematical problem (e.g.
multiplying two digit number) in mind requires one to remember the two numbers, apply
multiplication rules, store intermediate products simultaneously and proceed through next
stage of calculation. The task is possible only if we manage to meet both the storage and
processing demands of the activity. Carrying out such mental activities is an effortful process.
Even a minor distraction or an interruption by someone is likely to result in complete loss of
the stored information, and so in a failed calculation attempt. At this stage no amount of
effort will allow us to recall the lost information, the only course of action is to start the
calculation afresh. Our abilities to carry out such tasks are limited by the amount of
information we have to store and process (a working memory ability). We fail to perform
such tasks because the storage demands of the tasks exceed our working memory capacity.

The typical classroom learning requires one to meet both storage and processing demand of
the task (e.g., listening to teacher’s lecture and taking notes simultaneously). Unfortunately,
not all children are able to meet these demands and consequently they fail to learn. Among
these are children who are often referred as children with special educational needs and
children with learning disabilities.

Learning disability (LD) manifests as persistent difficulties in learning to efficiently read


(dyslexia), write (dysgraphia) or perform mathematical operations (dyscalculia) despite
normal intelligence, conventional schooling, intact sensory system, adequate motivation and
learning opportunity. LD results in academic underachievement that is unexpected based on
child’s potential as well as the opportunity to have learned more. Approximately 5-15%
school going children suffer from some form of learning disability (Krishnan, 2007; Krishna
Kumar, 1999; Mehta, 2003; Karande, S. E. & Kulkarni, M., 2005). The actual number can be
estimated to be higher as many children dropout from school at early stage and remain
unidentified.

Learning problems has often been seen to arise from subnormal intelligence. Many even
confuse a low verbal IQ with learning disability. Because intelligence includes verbal skills it
is likely that a child who has subnormal intelligence, will also show learning ability that is
somewhat below average. It is worth noticing that learning disabilities are specific, in the
sense that they affect specific processing areas (e.g. reading, writing and mathematics) as
opposed to global difficulties seen in children with compromised intelligence. Despite having
average or above average intellectual functioning, children with learning disabilities fail to
learn as quickly as others. This indicates that their problem can not be simply explained by
intellectual functioning and there could be more complex phenomena that should be looked
upon.

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 112
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

Working Memory and Learning process: How working memory is linked with learning
Since ages, intelligence is believed to determine one’s learning ability, academic achievement
and success in life. Recent researches have reported a strong link between working memory
and learning. Some researchers (e.g., Alloway, T. P. 2009b) on the basis of their research
findings suggest that working memory is even more important for learning than IQ (a
measure of intelligence). Findings of the study indicated that working memory was a better
predictor of learning than IQ even after two years. Some researchers have suggested that the
key factor underlying the relationship between working memory and learning is IQ (Nation,
Adams, Bowyer-Crane, & Snowling, 1999; Stothard & Hulme, 1992). However, contrasting
evidences suggest that working memory shares unique links with learning after statistically
accounting for IQ (e.g., Cain, Oakhill, & Bryant, 2004; Gathercole, Alloway, et al., 2006). IQ
is a measure of what a child has already learned whereas working memory measures one’s
ability to learn (the actual learning potential). Thus working memory seems to be a true
measure of one’s learning potential.

Academic learning requires children to meet both storage and processing demand of the
learning task (e.g., listening to teacher’s lecture and taking notes simultaneously, following a
set of instructions etc.). The process of reading sentences, holding them in mind, and
integrating the information to uncover the meaning relies heavily on one’s ability to
simultaneously process and store information over the short term. Similarly following a set of
complex instruction relies on the ability to remember the different parts of the instruction
while carrying out various steps to complete the action successfully. Working Memory
provides a mental workspace for tasks requiring both processing and storage. A successful
learning requires one to control attention to focus on relevant information while ignoring
irrelevant information. This function too is governed by working memory. Working memory
is required whenever anything must be learned because learning requires manipulation of
information, interaction with long-term memory, simultaneous processing and storage of
information. Nearly all of what must be learnt and remembered must pass through working
memory. Thus working memory seems to play a crucial role in learning.

Our working memory capacities are limited. For any person there is a limit to what can be
held in this mental workspace; if this limit is exceeded, information is lost. However, this
working memory capacity varies greatly between the individuals. Individual differences in
working memory are primarily determined by central executive processes. The typical
problem seen in working memory is difficulty to hold on to the first part of information while
processing the second. Working memory demand increases with task complexity. When
working memory demand of a given task exceeds children’s working memory capacity they
forget some important piece of information which leads to a failed learning attempt. Hence,
the capacity and effective functioning of working memory determines the rate and extent of
learning.

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 113
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

Learning Disabilities as a Working Memory deficit


Learning new material requires manipulation of information, interaction with long-term
memory, and simultaneous storage and processing of information. It also integrates new
knowledge with prior existing information in long term memory (LTM). Individuals with
learning disabilities are likely to have a deficiency in one or more cognitive processes
(Masoura, 2006), including phonological processing, long term retrieval, attention, short term
memory, and working memory. Out of several cognitive processes working memory has been
found to be strongly related to academic skills. Numerous studies have reported a strong
relationship between working memory performance, reading skills (Smith-Spark & Fisk,
2007; Swanson & Jerman, 2007), written expression (Kellogg, Olive, & Piolat 2007), and
mathematics performance (Hutton & Towse, 2001).

The typical learning environment requires one to simultaneously focus on instruction,


processing and storage of information in the presence of distracting stimuli making high
demand on working memory. Children who are capable of meeting those demands on
working memory learn the material easily but some children who have poor working memory
often fail to learn because they fail to meet high working memory demand of the task. In their
study Swanson & Berninger (1996) found that children with all types of learning disabilities
and difficulties display poor working memory performance, especially in verbal and
executive working memory. Executive working memory tasks require simultaneous
processing and storage of information. Children with learning disabilities often fail to meet
those demands and consequently fail to learn. The working memory load of learning task
often exceeds their working memory capacity.

Studies have indicated that deficits in working memory are a common feature of a wide range
of developmental disorders and specific learning difficulties, including ADHD, Dyslexia,
Specific language impairment and, reading and arithmetic difficulties (Arichbald &
Gathercole, 2007; Geary, Hoard, Byrd-Craven, Nugent & Numtee, 2007; Holmes, Hilton,
Place, Alloway, Elliot & Gathercole, 2014; Jeffries & Everatt, 2004; Swanson, Howard &
Sáez Lee, 2007).

Some investigators (e.g., Swanson & Siegel, 2001) believe that intrinsic working memory
limitations are the primary cause of learning disabilities. However, because most of the
research on working memory and learning disabilities is correlational, we cannot attribute
causality. It has also been argued that whether working memory deficit seen in children with
learning disabilities are a capacity deficit or strategy deficit. Some researchers (e.g.,
Swanson, 2000) theorize that a working memory deficit is not entirely a capacity deficit.
Rather, for some children with learning disabilities, a working memory problem is primarily
a strategy deficit. That is, children with a learning disability often possess sufficient working
memory resources but fail to apply effective strategies spontaneously or consistently,
resulting in learning failure. If working memory deficit is purely a strategy deficit, children
with learning disabilities can be helped by teaching them appropriate strategy to deal with

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 114
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

their working memory limitations. Once these children are able to overcome their working
memory limitations they would be able to learn as effectively as others.

Working memory has also been used as a measure to discriminate between children with
special educational needs and normal performing children with 81% accuracy (Gathercole
and Pickering, 2001). In their study Swanson et al., (1990) tested the assumption that children
of various academic abilities may be characterized by different patterns of working memory
function. To test this assumption, subgroups of children were identified through a hierarchical
cluster analysis based upon a test battery of sentence span, preload, and concurrent memory
demand tasks. One subtype presented a profile of children with learning disabilities showing
severe memory performance deficits, while another subgroup yielded high memory and high
academic performance. Results of study provided partial validation for the classification of
children with learning disabilities on psychometric measures according to patterns of working
memory performance. Findings of these studies indicate that working memory can be used
prospectively to identify children with learning disabilities. The empirical evidence also
indicate that working memory performance is one source of data that can reliably
differentiate between students with a learning disability and those who are slow learners
(Swanson et al., 1990).

In recent years, researchers have even identified specific components of working memory
associated with learning disabilities, for example – deficit in central executive functioning
has been found to be a characteristic feature of children with learning disabilities and children
with special educational needs (e.g., Gathercole and Pickering, 2001). Similarly, pattern of
deficit in different subtypes of learning disability have also been identified. To gain an
understanding of pattern of deficit in working memory in subtypes of learning disability,
studies have been presented separately.

Working Memory and Reading Disability (Dyslexia)


Reading skills are typically divided into two main categories—reading decoding, also known
as basic reading skills, and reading comprehension. Reading decoding is primarily dependent
on phonological processing—the ability to detect and manipulate the sound units (phonemes)
of oral language. Reading comprehension is more complex and involves several higher level
cognitive processes. Reading decoding is primarily related to phonological working memory
and verbal working memory, whereas reading comprehension is primarily related to verbal
working memory, executive working memory, and long-term memory (Swanson et al., 2006).

Studies have indicated that children with reading disability (dyslexia) have deficit in
“phonological awareness” which is dependent on phonological loop capacity. Phonological
loop plays an important role in language processing, literacy, and learning. Individuals with
longer phonological spans are better at vocabulary and language learning than those with
shorter spans (Baddeley, 2003a). Higher level processing of the verbal information, such as
putting the words together to form an idea, involves complex working memory functions that
are conducted by the central executive component of working memory.

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 115
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

Information encoded from verbal stimuli are also processed and stored in visuo-spatial
sketchpad (Baddeley, 1999; Salway & Logie, 1995), thus visuo-spatial sketchpad may serve
an important function in reading. Visuo-spatial sketchpad visually encodes printed letters and
words while maintaining a visuo-spatial frame of reference that allows the reader to
backtrack and keep his/her place in the text (Baddeley, 1986). Though visuospatial sketchpad
is also considered to play role in reading, major deficit in reading disability (dyslexia) has
been seen in phonological loop and central executive component only. For example,
Pickering and Gathercole (2001) studied working memory function in 15 dyslexic children
aged 7-14 years. Dyslexic children were compared with individually matched chronological
age (CA) and reading age (RA) control participants. Working Memory Test Battery for
Children (WMTB-C) was used to assess their working memory functions. Results showed
that dyslexic children performed significantly worse than non-dyslexic children of the same
age on a range of measures including; Word List Matching, Matrices static and Backward
Digit Recall. In many cases, the dyslexic children’s performance was similar to, if not worse
than that of the younger children of the same reading age. Interestingly however, with the
exception of Matrix Static Task, dyslexic children’s visuo-spatial working memory
performance was as good as non-dyslexic children of the same age. Findings suggest that the
visuo-spatial working memory skills of dyslexic children are largely intact, in contrast to
relatively poor phonological loop and central executive skills.

Gathercole, S. E. & Alloway, T. P. (2006) investigated associations between working


memory and both reading and mathematics abilities. Sample consisted of 46 children (13
females, 33 boys) aged between 6 and 11 years with reading disabilities. Participants were
assessed on a range of measures. Their reading and mathematical skills were assessed using
Wechsler Objective Reading Dimension (WORD) and Wechsler Objective Numerical
Dimensions (WOND). Working Memory Test Battery for Children (WMTB-C) was
administered to assess phonological short term memory, visuo-spatial memory and complex
memory (central executive component). Phonological processing skill was measured using
three subtests from the Phonological Assessment Battery. The sample was characterized by
deficits in complex memory (central executive functioning), visuo-spatial memory and by
low IQ scores, whereas language, phonological short-term memory and phonological
processing abilities fell in the low average range. Results indicated that the severity of
reading difficulties was significantly associated with working memory, language and
phonological processing abilities, whereas poor mathematics abilities were associated with
complex memory score, phonological short-term memory and phonological processing
scores. These findings suggest that working memory skills indexed by complex memory
(central executive component) represent an important constraint on the development of skill
and knowledge in the key domains of reading and mathematics in children with reading and
mathematics disability.

On the basis of above findings and available evidences it can be concluded that reading
disability (dyslexia) is associated with deficit in phonological working memory and central
executive functioning.

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 116
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

Working Memory and Writing Disability (Dysgraphia)


Similar to reading skills writing skill also comprises of a set of skills – transcription, word
and text generation, vocabulary, and text coherence. Working memory functioning in writing
disability is less researched. The available evidence indicate that cognitive processes involved
in written language are- working memory, executive processing, processing speed, and
planning.

In a study Swanson, H. L. & Berninger, V. W. (1996) studied individual differences in


children’s working memory and writing skill. The sample consisted of 300 participants (100
fourth graders, 100 fifth graders and 100 sixth graders). They were measured on phonological
working memory, verbal WM span, executive processing, and visuo–spatial working
memory. Findings indicated that WM measures contributed unique variance to writing that
was independent of reading skill, and STM measures best predicted transcription processes
and reading recognition, whereas WM measures best predicted text generation and reading
comprehension. Both verbal and visual–spatial working memory measures predicted reading
comprehension; whereas only WM measures that reflect executive processing significantly
predicted writing.

Another study conducted by Bourke, L. & Adams, M. (2003) investigated the role of working
memory in young children’s written language performance at word, sentence and text levels.
Sample consisted of 60 children aged between 6 and 7 years attending local schools. Their
non-verbal intellectual ability was assessed using Block Design Test of WISC-R.
Phonological working memory was assessed using Non-Word Repetition Test, Word Span
Test and Digit Span Test. Visuo-spatial working memory was assessed using Corsi Blocks
tapping test and Visual Pattern Test. Central executive component of working memory was
assessed by Verbal Fluency and Complex Span Test. Children’s written texts were analyzed
in terms of diversity of vocabulary, mean length of the sentences in morphemes, and overall
coherence. Children were also assigned an attainment level according to Key Stage 1 (KS1)
guidelines. Hierarchical regressions revealed that individual differences in central executive
functioning predicted vocabulary diversity, text coherence and KS1 attainment level.
Individual differences in working memory capacity were associated with poorer quality texts.

Findings of these studies indicate that simple writing skill (transcription process) is associated
with short term memory whereas text generation (a more complex process) is governed by
working memory capacity. Especially, central executive component of working memory
predicts vocabulary diversity, text coherence and quality of texts.

Working Memory and Mathematics Disability (Dyscalculia)


Mathematical tasks demand both storage and processing abilities. That is, one must
remember some piece of information in mind and while processing it further simultaneously.
The central executive is involved whenever an individual must simultaneously store and
process information (Tronsky, 2005). Mathematical skills have been found to be dependent
on working memory especially executive processing, fluid reasoning, visual processing,

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 117
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

processing speed, and planning. Here we present some studies for a better understanding of
the role of working memory in dyscalculia.

In a study Kyttälä et al. (2003) studied the relationship between visuo-spatial working
memory and early numeracy in preschoolers. A total of 46 preschoolers (26 girls and 20
boys) aged between 5 yr 3 months to 6 yr 10 months were selected for this study. Their
Intelligence was measured with two subtests (vocabulary and block design) of Wechsler
intelligence scale for children, third edition (WISC-III). Visuo-spatial working memory was
measured by a visuo-spatial test battery comprising two VSWM storage tasks (static visuo-
spatial working memory and dynamic visuo spatial working memory) and one mental
rotation task as a processing task. The early numeracy test for toddlers (ENT) was used to
assess mathematical pre-skills. The results suggested that score on early numeracy test and
counting tasks correlated with VSWM capacity as well as with intelligence (Block design
test) whereas relational tasks correlated only with general intelligence. The study suggests
that visuo-spatial working memory plays role in mathematical pre-skills (e.g. counting).

Another study conducted by Meyer, M. L. et al. (2010) investigated the relation between
specific WM components and mathematics achievement in 2nd grade (n=20) and 3rd grade
(50) children. Their IQ was assessed using Wechsler abbreviated Scale of Intelligence.
Children with full-scale IQ between 80 and 120 were selected in the sample. Participant’s
mathematical ability was assessed using WIAT-II and working memory was assessed using
four subtests of WMTB-C namely- Counting recall, Backward digit recall, Digit recall and
Block recall. Results of the study indicated that for 2nd graders, the central executive and
phonological components predicted Mathematical Reasoning skills; whereas the visuo-spatial
component predicted both Mathematical Reasoning and Numerical Operations skills in 3rd
graders. This pattern suggests that the central executive and phonological loop facilitate
performance during early stages of mathematical learning whereas visuo-spatial
representations play an increasingly important role during later stages.

A large sample study conducted by McLean, J. F. & Hitch, J. G. (1999) investigated working
memory functioning in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Sample
consisted of 122 children aged 9-year (64 females and 58 males) with difficulties specific to
arithmetic (as indicated by normal reading). They were compared with both age-matched and
ability-matched controls. All children were administered the Graded Arithmetic–Mathematics
Test, the Primary Reading Test and written tests of speeded calculation. A battery of 10 tasks
was used to assess different aspects of working memory, including subtypes of executive
function. Findings indicated that relative to age-matched controls, children with poor
arithmetic had normal phonological working memory but were impaired on spatial working
memory and some aspects of executive processing. Compared to ability-matched controls,
they were impaired only on one task designed to assess executive processes for holding and
manipulating information in long-term memory. Deficits in executive and spatial aspects of
working memory seemed likely to be important factors in poor arithmetical attainment.

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 118
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

Findings of these studies indicate that visuo-spatial working memory plays role in acquisition
of mathematical pre-skills (e.g. counting) in early childhood while central executive and
phonological components facilitate mathematical reasoning skills. In later stages,
mathematical reasoning and numerical operations skills seem to be governed by visuo-spatial
working memory. In children with mathematics disability (dyscalculia) deficit in visuo-
spatial working memory and central executive functioning has been observed while
phonological working memory remains preserved.

Some studies have also investigated working memory in mixed/multiple learning disabilities
and have reported that children with multiple learning disabilities (literacy and mathematics)
perform poorly in all aspects of working memory. In contrast, children with only one specific
learning disability demonstrate fairly distinctive working memory profiles, with deficits
limited to one or two components. For example, children with a specific reading disability
frequently have impairments in phonological short-term memory and verbal working memory
(Pickering & Gathercole, 2004), whereas children with a specific mathematics disability tend
to have deficits in visuo-spatial and executive working memory.

CONCLUSION
The role of working memory in academic learning and learning disabilities are discussed.
Pattern of deficit in working memory differs in subtypes of learning disabilities. Reading
disabilities (dyslexia) are characterized by deficit in phonological working memory and
central executive functioning. Simple writing skills (e.g., transcription) are dependent on
short term memory whereas as complex writing skills (e.g., vocabulary and text generation)
are governed by central executive component of working memory. Thus major deficit in
writing disabilities (dysgraphia) are seen in central executive functioning. Mathematical tasks
demand simultaneous processing and storage and so rely heavily on central executive
functioning. Thus deficit in central executive functioning seem to be a characteristic feature
of mathematics disability (dyscalculia). It is believed by some researchers that working
memory deficit in children with learning disabilities are not entirely a capacity deficit, rather
for some children it is a strategy deficit. That is, children with learning disabilities fail to
apply appropriate strategies which limit their working memory functioning and leads to
learning failure. This view creates hope for these children as they can be taught appropriate
strategies to overcome working memory limitations (e.g., simplifying instructions and
presenting information sequentially to reduce working memory load) and learn effectively.

Acknowledgments
The author appreciates all those who provided their valuable inputs and helped to facilitate
the research process.

Conflict of Interests: The author declared no conflict of interests.

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 119
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

REFERENCES
Alloway, T. P. (2009). Working memory, but not IQ, predicts subsequent learning in children
with learning difficulties. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25(2), 92-98.
Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition in specific language
impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic
bulletin & review, 14(5), 919-924.
Baddeley, A. (2003a). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of
communication disorders, 36(3), 189-208.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1999). Essentials of human memory. Psychology Press.
Bourke, L., & Adams, A. M. (2003). The relationship between working memory and early
writing assessed at the word, sentence and text level. Educational and Child
Psychology, 20(3), 19-36.
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability:
Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component
skills. Journal of educational psychology, 96(1), 31.
Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2006). Working memory in
children with reading disabilities. Journal of experimental child psychology, 93(3),
265-281.
Gathercole, S., & Pickering, S. (2001). Research Section: Working memory deficits in
children with special educational needs. British Journal of Special Education, 28(2),
89-97.
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd‐Craven, J., Nugent, L., & Numtee, C. (2007). Cognitive
mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning
disability. Child development, 78(4), 1343-1359.
Holmes, J., Hilton, K. A., Place, M., Alloway, T. P., Elliott, J. G., & Gathercole, S. E. (2014).
Children with low working memory and children with ADHD: same or different?.
Hutton, U. M., & Towse, J. N. (2001). Short-term memory and working memory as indices
of children's cognitive skills. Memory, 9(4-6), 383-394.
Jeffries, S., & Everatt, J. (2004). Working memory: its role in dyslexia and other specific
learning difficulties. Dyslexia, 10(3), 196-214.
Karande, S., & Kulkarni, M. (2005). Specific learning disability: the invisible
handicap. Indian Pediatrics, 42(31), 5-9.
Kellogg, R. T., Olive, T., & Piolat, A. (2007). Verbal, visual, and spatial working memory in
written language production. Acta Psychologica, 124(3), 382-397.
Krishnakumar, A. (1999). Coping with a disability. Frontline, 19(8).
Krishnan, B. (2007). Effective special needs education. Education World (March). Retrieved
March 23, 2012, from www.educationworldonline.net/index.php/page-article-choice-
more-id-834
Kyttälä, M., Aunio, P., Lehto, J. E., Van Luit, J., & Hautamäki, J. (2003). Visuospatial
working memory and early numeracy. Educational and Child Psychology, 20(3), 65-76.
Masoura, E. V. (2006). Establishing the link between working memory function and learning
disabilities. Learning disabilities: A contemporary journal, 4(2), 29-41.

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 120
Working Memory and Learning Disabilities: A Review

McLean, J. F., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific
arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of experimental child psychology, 74(3), 240-260.
Mehta, M. (2003). Learning disabilities in India: Willing the mind to learn. Retrieved April
12, 2012, from http://http://www.highbeam.com/Search.aspx
Meyer, M. L., Salimpoor, V. N., Wu, S. S., Geary, D. C., & Menon, V. (2010). Differential
contribution of specific working memory components to mathematics achievement in
2nd and 3rd graders. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(2), 101-109.
Nation, K., Adams, J. W., Bowyer-Crane, C. A., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Working memory
deficits in poor comprehenders reflect underlying language impairments. Journal of
experimental child psychology, 73(2), 139-158.
Pickering, S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Working memory test battery for children (WMTB-
C). Psychological Corporation.
Salway, A. F., & Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuospatial working memory, movement control and
executive demands. British Journal of Psychology, 86(2), 253-269.
Smith-Spark, J. H., & Fisk, J. E. (2007). Working memory functioning in developmental
dyslexia. Memory, 15(1), 34-56.
Stothard, S. E., & Hulme, C. (1992). Reading comprehension difficulties in children. Reading
and writing, 4(3), 245-256.
Swanson, H. L. (2000). Are working memory deficits in readers with learning disabilities
hard to change?. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(6), 551-566.
Swanson, H. L., & Berninger, V. W. (1996). Individual differences in children's working
memory and writing skill. Journal of experimental child psychology, 63(2), 358-385.
Swanson, H. L., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working memory on reading growth
in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 96(4), 249-283.
Swanson, H. L., & Siegel, L. (2001). Learning disabilities as a working memory
deficit. Issues in Education, 7(1), 1-48.
Swanson, H. L., Cochran, K. F., & Ewers, C. A. (1990). Can learning disabilities be
determined from working memory performance?. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 23(1), 59-67.
Swanson, H. L., Howard, C. B., & Saez, L. (2006). Do different components of working
memory underlie different subgroups of reading disabilities?. Journal of learning
disabilities, 39(3), 252-269.
Swanson, H. L., Howard, C. B., & Sáez, L. (2007). Reading comprehension and working
memory in children with learning disabilities in reading. Children’s comprehension
problems in oral and written language: A cognitive perspective, 157-185.
Tronsky, L. N. (2005). Strategy use, the development of automaticity, and working memory
involvement in complex multiplication. Memory & Cognition,33(5), 927-940.

How to cite this article: Gupta P K, & Sharma V (2017). Working Memory and Learning
Disabilities: A Review. International Journal of Indian Psychology, Vol. 4, (4),
DIP:18.01.013/20170404, DOI:10.25215/0404.013

© The International Journal of Indian Psychology, ISSN 2348-5396 (e)| ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) | 121

View publication stats

You might also like