This Content Downloaded From 186.195.42.1 On Fri, 10 Jul 2020 04:26:02 UTC
This Content Downloaded From 186.195.42.1 On Fri, 10 Jul 2020 04:26:02 UTC
This Content Downloaded From 186.195.42.1 On Fri, 10 Jul 2020 04:26:02 UTC
Human
Author(s): Anne Christine Taylor
Source: The Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute , Jun., 1996, Vol. 2, No. 2
(Jun., 1996), pp. 201-215
Published by: Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Wiley and Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland are collaborating with
JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Journal of the Royal Anthropological
Institute
CNRS, Paris
This article begins by exploring some of the premisses concerning personhood, sociality and
mortality underlying the experience and representation ofselfinJivaroan Achuar culture. Although
inexplicit and seemingly contradictory, these assumptions combine to produce an intricate though
unspoken theory of what is implied in being a true, live human. Jivaroan sense of self is rooted
in the progressive fusion of a generic, given bodily form and of attributed perception of this same
bodily form; the initially anonymous body image is thus progressively singularized by the memory
of the affective moods experienced in daily social interaction. Achuar selfhood is therefore
susceptible to states ofweakness and uncertainty, categorized as induced illness, as well as to states
of enhancement brought on by communication with a certain category of spirits. The interactive
basis of the set of representations concerning selfhood leads the author to discuss traditional
anthropological ways of dealing with indigenous ideas, and to suggest an approach more attentive
to the contextualization of knowledge.
Among the many 'things that go without saying'1 is how very honoured I feel
by your invitation to deliver this lecture. Honoured, but also, in equal if not
greater measure, horrified. First, because the occasion evokes a host of father
figures, dead or alive, on whom we have all sharpened our teeth in our youth,
and whose benevolence is therefore open to doubt; secondly, because I am a
French anthropologist, and in the skirmishing which is one of the great pleas-
ures of the long relationship between our two scientific communities, I find the
stakes have suddenly been drastically raised. Thirdly, because picking one's way
through the minefield of conflicting paradigms that is today's anthropology is
an inherently stressful affair.
The problem I want to consider here, one of central importance for Malinowski,
is that of the relation between social environment and individual psychology.
Indeed, this lecture is a kind of response to the call he made in the introductory
chapter of Argonauts to study 'what concerns man most intimately, that is the
hold which life has on him', in so far as I shall attempt here to define what is
implied for an individual in being alive and experiencing the selfhood of a socially
constructed body in an Amazonian culture. Much research has been devoted
recently to issues of this sort, so much so that the body has, to a large extent,
come to replace society as our discipline's major focus of analytic exploration;
being, but attributable in given contexts to many different sorts of things, in-
cluding inanimate ones; life, in short, is a postulated state of mind rather than a
state of matter.
It follows from this that being a real, live human implies displaying a special
type of bodily appearance, practising certain types of communicative and social
behaviour, and possessing certain states of consciousness. In order to specify
this combination which defines real, live humanness, we may begin by taking a
closer look at bodily appearance and explore some Achuar notions concerning
the body. The salient features of the mental model that shapes these ideas are as
follows. First, and most surprisingly, the Achuar have remarkably unelaborated
theories of procreation, and they have in fact very little to say about the concep-
tion and formation of a child; questions about these matters clearly strike them
as irrelevant. Further, pregnancy and birth are not ritualized and there are no
myths explicitly concerned with conception and procreation.8 Secondly, if we
examine prohibitions and observances linked to bodily substances and functions
- a set of practices often considered of particular importance for understanding
indigenous ideas about the body's formation and the shaping of the person -
some interesting properties begin to emerge. Prohibitions are most numerous
and stringently observed precisely in those practices and situations which, from
the indigenous point of view, involve a process of transformation: making a
canoe, preparing curare poison, suffering from snake-bite. Thus, the relative
scarcity and laxity of prohibitions attendant on pregnancy and childbirth sup-
port the view that these processes are not seen as similar to other culturally
stressed metamorphoses. In other words, and contrary to death as viewed in
certain contexts, birth is never thought to be a process of transformation, and
there is therefore no parallelism between entry to, and exit from, the state of
being a real live human. It should also be stressed that, with regard to their
effects, bodily substances do not form a class separate from other, non-bodily,
substances: semen, for example has the same kind of properties as curare, as
snake-poison, or as the burning sensation of red pepper; and menstrual blood is
just blood, or if it has any power this comes from non-specific attributes such
as the potency of red, or that of being heavy. Finally, things posited as alive, that
is to say credited with intentionality and consciousness, are all fundamentally
the same in terms of organic attributes and physiological mechanisms: a bat or
a dog, or for that matter a manioc plant, are all believed to be organized in the
same way. They function according to identical biological processes, and their
bodily stuff - appearance apart - is the same. If we humans are not normally
aware of this fact it is for epistemological reasons - because we do not ordinarily
communicate with them - and not because these metabolisms are ontologically
distinct.
We are thus led to the conclusion that what differentiates species is essentially
shape or, more accurately, appearance. As I have shown elsewhere (Taylor 1993),
from a Jivaroan point of view perceived shape refers to a set of differentiated
bodily forms, particularly faces, specific to each class of animate beings. These
outward forms exist in limited numbers and are endlessly recycled, which ex-
plains why there is no natural creation, and why birth is not viewed as a process
of transformation or 'making' that adds something new to the world. Birth is
reappearance, and the Achuar person thus comes ready-made in terms of bodily
image of the shamanic cure. I refer here to the quest for so-called arutam
visions, whereby a person may, in the course of a private ritual involving isola-
tion, rigorous fasting and the ingestion of large doses of hallucinogenic drugs,
receive a message or vision relative to his or her future existence. The spirit
responsible for this prophecy, the arutam or 'ancient thing', takes the shape of a
dead Jivaro, who, after a complicated and frightening series of metamorphoses,
briefly appears in person to the seeker and addresses him or her. In the case of
men, this message usually concerns the outcome of an act of war or a revenge
killing, which they then, of course, feel compelled to carry out. The arutam
experience is thus directly linked to those situations and interpersonal relation-
ships most heavily fraught with unpredictability, and it rests on the same logic
that underlies the resort to shamanic healing. I need not add that the pragmatic
structure of the interaction between seeker and spirit is just as complex as that
implied in shamanic curing, indeed in many ways more so (I refer here to the
way in which the stereotyped circumstances surrounding the ritual encounter
are constitutive of the event's meaning). I will therefore limit myself to under-
scoring two salient features of the arutam quest.
First, it centres on the ritual framing of the normal interactions on which
subjectivity is built, as perceived by the Jivaroans. Thus, when the Achuar speak
of the arutam's message as a kind of 'soul' which will become henceforth a part
of themselves, they are evoking a reification, projected into the future, of an
image of self rooted in a special kind of intersubjective relation, that between
themselves - more accurately a modified state of their consciousness - and the
arutam. This hypostasis is modelled on the introjection of an attributed image
of self that underlies normal states of subjectivity; thus, just as the wakan - the
body's soul - briefly survives the recently deceased as a substantivization of the
memory that surviving kin hold of it, so the arutam vision he or she has received
encapsulates the spirit's description, or image, of their future selves. The wakan,
in short, is a reification of attributed memory, while the arutam 'soul' is a reifi-
cation of projected selfhiood. Paradoxically, this is in fact all that ultimately
remains of people, in the guise of the 'ancient thing' or arutam they in turn
eventually become. In other words, the arutam ritual is not linked to an elabo-
rate cosmology or ontological theory. It is based on the same perception of
subjectivity and intersubjectivity that informs the notion of wakan, and its spe-
cific meanings are rooted in the ritual construction of a particular context of
interaction, rather than on an elaboration of content. In the second place, the
effect or result of the arutam quest springs from an event of hypercommunica-
tion, a kind of saturation of certainty and unambiguous meaning. Yet this
glimpse of a destiny cleansed of unpredictability must remain indescribable, in
so far as it is strictly prohibited to speak about the message received from the
arutam. Were one to do so, one would immediately lose the benefit of the vision,
indeed the vision or message itself as a kind of soul-stuff whereby the sense of
self is fortified. And that, after all, is the prime motivation for undergoing the
mystical experience, as well as its final outcome: the acquisition of invulnerabil-
ity, made manifest by forcefulness of speech and manner, face painting of a
certain kind, and heightened anger; that is to say, intensified homicidal drive.14
In sum, just as illness leads to a loss of the capacity to communicate except
through the mute language of symptoms, the state of super well-being brought
other words, an indigenous concept. Being a live human person is not a state
defined as such - there is no canonical discourse about 'the person', and nobody
will ever state 'this is our idea of what a man or a woman is' - yet it is nonetheless
precisely circumscribed by the articulation of a set of non-explicit premisses.
Being a person is thus an array or cline of relational configurations, a set of links
in a chain of metamorphoses simultaneously open and bounded. The chain is
open because death itself is an endless process, as is the shift from 'we' to 'they',
from Jivaro to foreigner; it is bounded, nevertheless, because being a live self
can be defined only by contrast to either a state of being less than alive, in
illness, or a state of being more than alive, through the acquisition of arutam.
And this is why we find both the stark problematic oppositions set up in mythic
narratives - life versus death - and, in ritual and in other fields of practice, life
and death as a continuous process. This is the key to the paradox I started out
with, concerning the coexistence of two seemingly contradictory notions of
death. These are not, in fact, two distinct compartmentalized conceptions of
mortality, one naturalistic, the other persecutory, but rather two mutually impli-
cated perspectives, one focusing on the terms rather than on their relation, the
other focusing on the relation while bracketing the terms. Further, the actuali-
zation of the different occasions in which a notion of the self is evoked forms a
chain: the arutam quest leads to killing, aggression and suspicion, which is the
cause of sickness and disorientation, which in turn requires either shamanic
healing or further arutam quests, and so on. This means that the different types
of relationships and self-creations discussed here are not only related structur-
ally, but also practically
The approach to the question of personhood outlined above has some wider
implications which I would, by way of conclusion, draw to your attention. Most
Jivarologists, myself included, have tended to consider the arutam complex,
because it is intellectually spectacular, and also because of its esoteric aspects, as
the heart of Jivaroan culture, the very basis of its identity for Indians and
ethnographers alike. And yet, while it is true that until recently the vast majority
of Achuar men had experienced arutam encounters, it is equally true that these
mystical quests only concern an extreme state of Jivaroan personhood, not
'Jivaroanness' as such, and most informants would state that one could live a
normal Jivaroan life, and be a Jivaro (though admittedly a second-rate one)
without ever experiencing an arutam encounter. Thus, by positing that the aru-
tam complex lies at the heart, rather than on the boundaries, of Achuar culture,
we have misrepresented not only the arutam complex itself but Jivaroan culture
in general. Nor is this optical skewing a particularity ofJivarology. I suspect that
a great many of our ethnographic accounts are in fact based on a similar confla-
tion between 'culture' and 'extreme states'. For example, any account that
purports to characterize the symbolic culture of this or that society by general-
izing conceptions deduced from any kind of specialized discourse can only be
wrong: wrong because it is blind to the importance of practice and contextuali-
zation, and assumes that culture is a system of language and thought shared,
with due allowance for sex and generation, by all. Semantic premisses may
indeed be shared by all, but pragmatics most certainly are not: in so far as the
conditions of use determine how these premisses are elaborated, expressed and
experienced, it is surely unsustainable to claim that a given representation is
sense mirror our informants' experiential and mental universe, but I do not see
how we could even imagine this universe unless we invent for ourselves and
our readers a kind of discursive 'stand-in' for a culture we have not been
socialized in, and thereby give ourselves the means of empathizing to some
degree with the lives and thoughts of the people we are studying. We must, in
other words, treat the net of often inexplicit and unelaborate assumptions that
is constitutive of culture as tf it were a metaphysics, because this fictional con-
struct is of necessity our essential, indeed our only, procedure for bringing to
light and verifying the necessary circularity of the combination of premisses
found in any given culture, as well as our only way of allowing for the exercise
of that 'analogical introspection' that lies at the heart of our discipline. And this
means that we can go on happily working away at our socio-cosmological
monographs, provided we respect two sets of conditions. First, we have to pay
much closer attention than previously to the contextual aspects of discourse and
communication, cease assuming that culture is a collective text, and be more
realistic in our description of who thinks what and how in which circum-
stances. Second, we should accept that our ethnographic accounts are elaborate
thought experiments rather than accurate renditions of indigenous systems of
thought, and view them as a conceptual tool inherent to the practice and writ-
ing of anthropology. Cumbersome this tool may be; yet if we take into account
the complexity of the phenomena that, by common consent, anthropology is
supposed to deal with, our monographs must rate as highly economical instru-
ments (in every sense of the term) and the disproportion between our means -
an elaborate controlled fiction with no pretence at realistic description of men-
tal phenomena - and our ends - the discovery and verification of the nature of
the links between mental models - is not as large as might seem at first glance.
The shift of perspective I have suggested is not much to ask for, and it implies
no major overhaul of object or method. Yet it does seem to offer anthropology
some kind of future as a worthwhile intellectual pursuit, which is surely more
than can be said either for the bland objectivism we have long practised, or for the
self-destruct vehicle devised by the more zealous militants of post-modernism.
Moreover, the sort of minor displacement of point of view I am advocating is
entirely typical of our discipline's intellectual tradition; indeed the occurrence
of such slight and often unnoticed shifts is precisely what makes anthropology
appear to outsiders as the endless rehashing of the same problems, while its
practitioners feel it is constantly progressing. Thus, I would like to think that
the banality of my conclusion is proof of sorts that the approach I am arguing
for is not entirely alien to that great canon so signally shaped and illustrated by
the scientist whose memory we honour today.
NOTES
I The expression refers to a paper by M. Bloch published in Kuper 1992, under the title
'What goes without saying. The conceptualization of Zafimaniry society'. Throughout this lec-
ture, I will often refer implicitly to the hypotheses presented in that contribution. I would like
to thank Bloch for his illuminating comments on an earlier draft of this work, and for his help
in preparing this paper for publication.
2 This is a crude simplification. It would be more accurate to say that the intentiqnally evil
agency that causes death is usually anthropomorphized, but not necessarily human. For illustra-
tions of the view of strictly human agency, see for example Capistrano de Abreu (1941: 140-1)
on the Cashinahua, and Harner (1972: 152-3) and Descola (1994: 257-70) on the Jivaroan
Shuar and Achuar. Among the Piaroa, the agent may be a divinity, an animal or a foreign sor-
cerer (Overing 1985), among the Yagua, it may be human or vegetal (Chaumeil 1983: 264-311);
among the Guajiro (Perrin 1992: 209-12) and the Tukano (Reichel-Dolmatoff 1971: 80-6), it
may be animal (qua animal or under the guise of the Master of animals).
3 A sample of these myths is analysed by Levi-Strauss (1967).
4 Two points are at issue here. The first is the problem of the integration of mental models.
Among anthropologists, discussion of this question, in so far as it exists, has mainly centred on
the way domain-specific cognition might, through the capacity for meta-representation, be cul-
turally elaborated (Atran 1993 [following Sperber 1990]; Bloch 1993; Sperber 1993). As Bloch
points out, this theme, which is obviously of vital interest to anthropologists, is still very little
explored. The second point is the question of the unity of a given culture. 'Cognitivist' anthro-
pologists who, in the name of psychological realism, refuse to consider culture as the manifes-
tation of an underlying shared script or, afortiori, as the expression of unconscious structures,
find it very difficult to account for this aspect of their object of study. They tend to sidestep the
problem by claiming that coherence or systematicity is simply an artefact of ethnography, or
even that the very notion of culture is meaningless. Culture may be little more than 'a second
rate orchestra playing a half remembered tune' without the benefit of a conductor (Lawson
1993: 206), yet we must still explain how and why everyone remembers the same tune, however
inaccurately, and why the level of cacophony in any given 'culture' is in fact surprisingly low.
5 This lecture is based on data collected by P Descola and myself during 26 months of field-
work between 1976 and 1979 and in 1984 and 1992. It also draws, of course, on the extensive
literature concerning the Jivaro, particularly on the work of Brown (1985), Harner (1972), Kar-
sten (1935) and Pellizzarro (1978; 1980).
6 For some published versions of this myth as it is told among the Shuar of Ecuador, see
Pellizzarro 1980.
7 Implicit in this formulation is the idea that the difference between mythical and ritual dis-
course is inherent in their respective meaning; i.e, part of the sense of a mythical narrative
derives from the fact that it is implicitly contrasted to a ritual pronouncement, and vice-versa.
Although it is perfectly legitimate to isolate such bodies of discourse for analytic purposes, at
some point their interconnexion, even if it appears to be purely negative, must be taken into
account.
8 With the exception of a body of myths relating to a switch from lethal caesarian to 'natural'
childbirth. These myths are common at least to the Shuar, Achuar and Aguaruna. According to
these narratives, the birth of a child used in illo tempore to imply the mother's death, since ba-
bies could only be delivered by cutting open their mothers' bellies. Rats eventually took pity on
women, and struck a deal with them whereby they taught them to give birth normally in ex-
change for a share of their peanut harvest (see Pellizzarro 1980 for some Shuar versions of this
myth).
9 These songs belong to a class of utterances named anent, a kind of soul-speech that tran-
scends normal channels of communication. A brief example of one such invocation, silently
addressed by a woman to her absent husband:
'Go flock to my little father's heart/ make him return to me crying pitifully/ go flock to his
thoughts (and make him cry) 'why does this feeling come to me?'/ fly to his thoughts and
make him awaken in tears/ (saying) 'why do I awaken thus?/ Oh, she's angry at me/ she is
going to leave me!'/ make him awaken with this thought/ crying, crying, go flock to himn my
little wakan, go flock to him' (my translation; the vernacular version, along with linguistic data,
may be read in Taylor & Chau 1983: 118-119).
10 The irenic perspective of some of the British Americanists is linked to their emphasis on
morality, i.e. normative values of sociality, as expressed by their informants and embodied in
their practice; see for example Belaunde 1992; Gow 1991; McCallum 1989; Santos Granero
1991. By contrast, French Americanists, who generally adhere to a structural-Durkheimian
approach, attempt to build up a model of observed social relations highlighting the 'construc-
tive' (in the sociological sense) aspect of conflict. For examples of this, see Albert 1985; Clastres
1972; Carneiro da Cunha & Viveiros de Castro 1985; Combes & Saignes 1991; Erikson 1986
(two are French only by intellectual filiation).
11 Virtually the only close kinsperson always to escape suspicion is one's mother. However,
one should not imagine that family life among the Jivaro is persistently conflict-ridden and
fraught with uncertainty. In normal circumstances, relations among members of a household or
local group are easy-going and often tender. Still, no one is surprised or particularly indignant -
as opposed to angry - when close members of a same family fall out and become involved in a
feud. In such cases the group splits and one or the other party joins a different territorial unit.
12 I think it also goes some way to explain the importance in this culture of forms of magical
discourse, such as the anent, meant to shape or modify others' affects.
13 The Achuar distinguish and name a variety of illnesses. Some - usually epidemic patholo-
gies - are labelled as 'white sickness', others are considered 'endogenous' and are initially dealt
with by resorting to plant medicine or to domestic forms of 'magical' curing. These afflictions
are thought of as 'accidental' only in the sense that the agent responsible for inflicting them
may have done so involuntarily, but of course the unintentionality of this imputed intentional-
ity is inherently suspect; if an illness lingers or worsens suspicion soon fades into the certainty
of deliberate mischief Judgements concerning the 'health status' of individuals or indeed whole
communities are thus highly dependent on the perceived shape of social relations: in times of
imminent or open conflict, not only do people tend to become unusually prone to sickness, but
their illness, whatever its taxonomic status, is immediately attributed to shamanic aggression.
14 This is true of men only Women do not systematically seek to encounter arutam, and they
experience them more rarely than men. The hidden 'strength' (kakarma-) - rather than anger -
they gain thereby is generally described in terms of a longer life and greater well-being, privi-
leged relations to the entities that control the appropriated fertility of garden plants, domestic
animals and humans, and secure affective relations to kin. It is also worth noting that female
arutam encounters occur in women's gardens, rather than in the forest, and that they seem to
happen only in times of acute emotional crises.
REFERENCES
Resume'
Cet article s'attache tout d'abord a explorer certaines des premisses relatives a la personne,
au lien social et a la mortalite sous-jacentes a l'exp6rience et a la repr6sentation du soi dans
la culture jivaro-achuar. Ces pr6suppos6s, quoique non explicites et apparemment contra-
dictoires entre eux, se combinent pour former une th6orie implicite pr6cise et complexe de
l'etre humain vivant. La conscience jivaro du soi s'enracine dans la fusion d'une forme
corporelle generique et d'une introjection de la perception d'autrui de cette forme-corps;
ainsi, l'image d'un corps initialement anonyme s'individualise progressivement en s'impr6-
gnant de la memoire des 6motions li6es aux rapports sociaux quotidiens. La perception du
soi est donc sujette tant a des etats d'affaiblissement et d'incertitude - cat6goris6s comme
maladies induites - et a des 6tats d"hypersubjectivite' engendres par la communication
rituelle avec des esprits. Les fondements interrelationnels des representations du soi menent
l'auteur a s'interroger sur l'approche traditionnelle en anthropologie des faits de conscience
et a proposer une perspective d'analyse plus attentive aux contextes et aux conditions
d'enonciation des savoirs traditionnels.