Phonetics II
Phonetics II
• Nouns
• Verbs Stressed
• Adjectives
• Adverbs
Structural words:
• Determiners
• Pronouns
• Prepositions Usually not stressed
• Conjunctions
• Auxiliaries
• Cited
• Emphasis
• Demonstrative
• Negatives
• Foregrounding
• Comparative
• Final prepositions and auxiliary verbs
• Different grammatical functions
Weak forms
Prepositions:
• At: /ət/
• For: /fə(r)/
• From: /frəm/
• Of: /əv/
• To: /tə/
Auxiliary verbs:
• Am: /əm/
• Are: /ə(r)/
• Can: /kən/
• Do: /də/
• Does: /dəz/
• Had: /həd/ /əd/
• Has: /həz/ /əz/
• Have: /həv/ /əv/
• Must: /məst/
• Shall: /ʃəl/
• Should: /ʃəd/
• Was: /wəz/
• Were: /wə(r)/
• Will: /wəl/
• Would: /wəd/ /əd/
Conjunction articles
• A: /ə/
• An: /ən/
• As: /əz/
• But: /bət/
• Some: /səm/
• Than: /ðən/
• That: /ðət/
• The: /ðə/
Pronouns:
Auxiliary verbs
Pronouns
• Be: /bi/
• Been: /bɪn/
• He: /hi/
• Him: /hɪm/
• She: /ʃi/
• We: /wi/
I think he knows.
aɪ θɪŋk i nəʊz
aɪ θɪŋk ʃi nəʊz
I think we know.
aɪ θɪŋk wi nəʊ
B.C B.V
Add -r For /fə/ /fər/ fə ˈtəʊni
fər ˈælɪs
Her /(h)ə/ /(h)ər/ hə ˈmʌðə
hər ˈʌŋkl
There /ðə/ /ðər/ ðə kæn´tiː bi
ðər ɪz nɒt ˈɛni
Are /ə/ /ər/ ə ðeɪ ɔːl
ər ɔːl əv ðəm
Were /wə/ /wər/ wə ðeɪ ɔːl
wər ɔːl əv ðəm
Your /jə/ /jər/ jə ˈmʌðə
jər ɑːnt
H dropping
Weak forms which drop their initial H except at the beginning of utterances
• Had: /əd/
• Has: /əz/
• Have: /əv/
• He: /iː/
• Her: /ə(r)/
• Him: /ɪm/
• His: /ɪz/
Assimilation
Assimilation of place
when a sound changes its place of articulation to another place, it is called assimilation of
place.
progressive assimilation
could become velar /ŋ/ if preceded by a velar plosive in the same word and followed by a
consonant in the same or next word or by a pause
assimilation of manner refers to two neighbouring sounds becoming similar in their manner of
articulation.
An example the progressive could be in shut your mouth when pronounced rapidly. Here, the
approximant /j/ can be articulated with a narrow gap between the speech organs under the
influence of the preceding /t/.
An example of the regressive could be in that side /ðæs saɪd/ and in good night /gʊn naɪt/. In
/ðæs saɪd/, the plosive /t/ becomes fricative /s/, and in /gʊn naɪt/, the plosive /d/ becomes
nasal /n/.
Assimilation of voice
This type of assimilation is represented in noun plural marker, the possessive and the singular
present tense which always agree in voicing with the preceding obstruent consonant
(regressive).
Examples:
[s] becomes
/s/ if preceded by /z/ if preceded by /ɪz/ if preceded by
a voiceless non-sibilant a voiced non-sibilant sibilants
[p], [t], [k], [f], [θ] [b], [d], [g], [l], [m], [n], [r], [s], [z], [ʃ], [ʒ], [tʃ ], [dʒ]
[v], [j]
Elision
Some of the sounds that are heard if words are pronounced slowly and clearly appear not to
be pronounced when the same words are produced in a rapid, colloquial style, or when the
words occur in a different context.
Elision of vowels
It usually happens when a short, unstressed vowel occurs between voiceless consonants, e.g.
in the first syllable of 'potato', the second syllable of 'bicycle', or the third syllable of
'philosophy'.
Elision also occurs when a vowel occurs between an obstruent consonant and a sonorant
consonant such as a nasal or a lateral this process leads to syllabic consonants, as in 'sudden' /
sʌdn /, 'awful‘ /ɔ: fl / (where a vowel is only heard in the second syllable in slow, careful
speech).
Elision of consonants
It happens most commonly when a speaker "simplifies" a complex consonant cluster: 'acts'
becomes / aks / rather than / akts /, 'twelfth night' becomes / twelθ naɪt / or /twelf naɪt /
rather than / twelfθ naɪt /. It seems much less likely that any of the other consonants could be
left out: the /l/ and the /n/ seem to be unelidable.
Liaison (Linking)
In general this is not something that speakers need to do anything active about - we produce
the phonemes that belong to the words we are using in a more or less continuous stream, and
the listener recognises them (or most of them) and receives the message.
linking r
This is the best-known case of liaison in English. Many words in English ('car', 'here', 'tyre') are
pronounced with a final / r / in rhotic accents such as General American or Scots but which in
RP end in a vowel when they are pronounced before a pause or before a consonant.
However, when they are followed by a vowel, RP speakers pronounce /r/ at the end ('the car
is‘ /ðəkɑ: rɪz /)
Another aspect of liaison in English is the movement of a single consonant at the end of an
unstressed word to the beginning of the next if that is strongly stressed.
A well-known example is 'not at all', where the / t / of 'at' becomes initial (and therefore
strongly aspirated) in the final syllable for many speakers.
Linking
when two vowel sounds meet, speakers often link them in various ways.
Linking j
Linking w
In English phonetics Yod coalescence is a form of assimilation –it is aphenomenon which takes
place when /j/ is preceded by certain consonants most commonly /t/ and /d/:
/wɒtʃu ni:d/
Intrusion
Where two vowel sounds meet and there is no written letter r, speakers tend to introduce the
/r/ phoneme in order to ease transition (intrusive r). This happens when the first word ends in /ə/
/ a:/ / or /ɔ:/
e.g.
The differences in the pronunciation of the underlined words, despite the fact that the phonemes
are the same, are differences of juncture.
The clock keeps ticking. /ki:ps tɪkɪŋ/
/ki:p stɪkɪŋ/
Contractions
Contractions occur where two word combine to the extent that the two are pronounced as one
word, or one syllable.
Examples: