Amy Neale Final Thesis
Amy Neale Final Thesis
Amy Neale Final Thesis
Amy C. Neale
Cardiff University
PhD
March 2002
Summary
i
Acknowledgements
ii
Summary....................................................................................................................... I
Acknowledgements .....................................................................................................II
1: Introduction .............................................................................................................1
iii
3.1.5 Transitivity in Halliday’s ‘Notes on Transitivity and Theme, Part 1’ (1967) ................ 49
3.1.6 Transitivity In Halliday’s ‘Notes on Transitivity and Theme, Part 3’ (1968) ................ 57
3.1.7 Halliday (1970) ‘Language Structure and Language Function’ ..................................... 60
3.1.8 Muir’s A Modern Approach to English Grammar (1972).............................................. 65
3.1.9 Berry Introduction to Systemic Linguistics, Volume 1: Structures And Systems (1975) 67
3.1.10 Hudson’s English Complex Sentences (1971) .............................................................. 69
3.1.11 Fawcett (1973/81), ‘Generating a Sentence in a Systemic Functional Grammar’ ....... 71
3.1.12 Halliday (1977) ‘Text as Semantic Choice in Social Contexts’ ................................... 74
3.1.13 Fawcett (1980), Cognitive Linguistics and Social Interaction ..................................... 77
3.1.14 Halliday (1985/94), Introduction to Functional Grammar .......................................... 82
3.2 More Recent Developments in TRANSITIVITY in the SF Framework........................... 88
3.2.1 Berry’s (1977) and Fawcett’s (1980) Early Networks For ‘Lexical Delicacy’ .............. 88
3.2.2 Hasan’s Approach to Modelling ‘Lexis as Most Delicate Grammar’ (1987)................. 90
3.2.3 Tucker’s (1998) ‘Lexis as Most Delicate Grammar’...................................................... 96
3.2.4 Davidse’s Descriptions of ‘Experiential Grammar’ ....................................................... 97
3.2.5 Matthiessen (1995) Lexicogrammatical Cartography: English Systems ..................... 106
5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and some new
developments ............................................................................................................137
iv
5.4 ‘Action’ Processes ........................................................................................................... 148
5.4.1 Why there is no system of [Agent-Centred] versus [Affected-Centred] in the current CG
............................................................................................................................................... 150
5.4.2 Further changes to the ‘Action’ Process system network............................................. 153
5.5 Relational Processes ........................................................................................................ 155
5.5.1 The introduction of the category ‘Directional’ Processes ............................................ 155
5.5.2 Matching Processes ...................................................................................................... 159
5.5.3 The treatment of ‘Matching’ Processes in other grammars.......................................... 160
5.5.4 Summary of ‘Relational’ Processes.............................................................................. 164
5.6 Mental Processes ............................................................................................................. 164
5.6.1 Overview ...................................................................................................................... 164
5.6.2 Emotion Processes........................................................................................................ 166
5.6.3 Processes of Perception ................................................................................................ 168
5.6.4 Cognition Processes ..................................................................................................... 169
5.7 Environmental Processes................................................................................................. 170
5.8 Influential Processes........................................................................................................ 171
5.9 Event-Relating Processes ................................................................................................ 175
5.10 Conclusion..................................................................................................................... 176
7: The Process Type Database (PTDB): its development and application .........193
v
7.3.1.3 The use of Francis, Hunston And Manning (1996), Cobuild Grammar Patterns 1:
Verbs...................................................................................................................................... 206
7.4 Everyday text observation and native language user intuition as supplementary sources for
the PTDB............................................................................................................................... 209
7.5 Multi-Word Verbs ........................................................................................................... 209
7.5.1 Treatment of Multi-Word Verbs in the Cardiff Grammar............................................ 210
7.5.2 Obtaining Multi-Word Verbs from corpora ................................................................. 213
7.5.3 Incorporating Multi-Word Verbs in the PTDB ............................................................ 215
7.6 Assigning Process types and Participant Role configurations to the verb senses in the
PTDB..................................................................................................................................... 219
7.6.1 The methodology for analyzing the verb senses in the PTDB ..................................... 221
7.6.1.2 Using Levin for categorization .................................................................................. 222
7.6.2 Using the Cardiff Grammar Participant Role tests for TRANSITIVITY analysis....... 223
7.6.3 Specific problem areas in analyzing the PTDB............................................................ 224
7.6.3.1 The treatment of metaphor and grammatical metaphor in the PTDB ....................... 224
7.6.3.2 An example of interrogating corpora in the case of problematic senses ................... 228
7.7 Conclusions ..................................................................................................................... 231
vi
8.3.2.1.12 ‘Change in fullness’ Processes ............................................................................. 256
8.3.2.1.13 ‘Change developmentally’ Processes ................................................................... 256
8.3.2.1.14 ‘Evaluative change’ Processes.............................................................................. 256
8.3.3 ‘Involuntary behaviour’ Processes ............................................................................... 257
8.3.3.1 ‘Specific physiological’ Processes ............................................................................ 257
8.3.3.2 ‘Reactive physiological’ Processes ........................................................................... 259
8.3.3.3 ‘Psychological’ Processes ......................................................................................... 259
8.3.3.4 ‘Internal bodily movement’ Processes ...................................................................... 259
8.3.3.5 ‘Change in awakeness’ Processes.............................................................................. 260
8.3.3.6 ‘Change in consciousness’ Processes ........................................................................ 261
8.3.3.7 ‘Suffocating’ Processes ............................................................................................. 261
8.3.4 ‘Emission’ Processes .................................................................................................... 262
8.4 The system network for ‘two-role’, ‘agent plus affected’, ‘action’ Processes .......... 263
8.4.1 ‘Affecting as such’ Processes....................................................................................... 265
8.4.2 ‘Not affecting’ Processes.............................................................................................. 265
8.4.3 ‘Social action’ Processes .............................................................................................. 266
8.4.3.1 ‘Social action involving two parties’ Processes ........................................................ 266
8.4.3.2 ‘General social action’ Processes .............................................................................. 268
8.4.3.2.1 ‘Visiting’ Processes ................................................................................................ 269
8.4.3.2.2 ‘Social encountering’ Processes ............................................................................. 270
8.4.3.2.3 ‘Social action by empowered person’ Processes .................................................... 270
8.4.4 ‘Material action’ Processes........................................................................................... 273
8.4.4.1 ‘Affecting by contact’ Processes ............................................................................... 274
8.4.4.2 ‘Affecting by lack of contact’ Processes ................................................................... 280
8.4.4.3 ‘Change of state’ Processes ....................................................................................... 280
8.4.4.3.1 ‘Change as such’ Processes .................................................................................... 281
8.4.4.3.2 ‘Evaluative change of state’ Processes................................................................... 281
8.4.4.3.3 ‘Cooking’ Processes ............................................................................................... 282
8.4.4.3.4 ‘Changing consistency’ Processes.......................................................................... 283
8.4.4.3.5 ‘Changing quality’ Processes ................................................................................. 283
8.4.4.3.6 ‘Changing size’ Processes ...................................................................................... 284
8.4.4.3.7 ‘Changing number’ Processes ................................................................................ 284
8.4.4.3.8 ‘Changing temperature’ Processes ......................................................................... 284
8.4.4.3.9 ‘Changing speed’ Processes ................................................................................... 284
8.4.4.3.10 ‘Changing strength’ Processes ............................................................................. 285
8.4.4.3.11 ‘Changing fullness’ Processes .............................................................................. 285
8.4.4.3.12 ‘Changing appearance’ Processes ........................................................................ 285
vii
8.4.4.3.13 ‘Changing consciousness’ Processes.................................................................... 285
8.4.4.3.14 ‘Changing dryness’ Processes .............................................................................. 285
8.4.4.3.15 ‘Shaping’ Processes.............................................................................................. 286
8.4.4.3.16 ‘Developing’ Processes ........................................................................................ 286
8.4.4.4 ‘Preparation’ Processes ............................................................................................. 286
8.4.4.5 ‘Ingestion’ Processes ................................................................................................. 289
8.4.4.6 ‘Concealment’ Processes........................................................................................... 289
8.4.4.7 ‘Changing position’ Processes .................................................................................. 289
8.4.4.8 ‘Using’ Processes ...................................................................................................... 291
8.5 The system network for ‘three role’, ‘directional’, ‘relational’ Processes ............... 292
8.5.1 The initial choices in the system network for generating ‘three-role’, ‘directional’,
‘relational’ Processes............................................................................................................. 294
8.5.1.1 The system network for ‘agent not accompanying affected carrier’ ......................... 296
8.5.1.2 The system network for ‘agent accompanying affected carrier’ ............................... 300
8.6 Conclusion....................................................................................................................... 302
9: Conclusions...........................................................................................................304
Bibliography .............................................................................................................310
Appendix B1: The System Network for One-Role, Affected Only Action
Processes ...................................................................................................................324
Appendix B2: The System Network for Two-Role, Agent Plus Affected Action
Processes ...................................................................................................................334
Appendix B3: The System Network for Three-Role Directional Processes .......351
viii
Chapter 1: Introduction
1: Introduction
1
In Chapter 3 – ‘Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG’ – a full description of what is meant by both
TRANSITIVITY and ‘delicacy’ is given, as well as a full history of approaches to TRANSITIVITY in
SFG. Chapter 3 also provides a description of the development of ‘system’ in SFG.
2
Chapter 8 – ‘PROCESS TYPE: The delicate system networks for TRANSITIVITY’.
3
Chapter 6 – ‘PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative lexicogrammar’.
1
Chapter 1: Introduction
diverse patterns found in the complementation of verbs, this being the most complex
aspect of the ‘patterns’ into which verbs enter (e.g. as described in Francis, Hunston and
Manning 1996). The work presented here is therefore complementary to these other
very ‘delicate’ descriptions of TRANSITIVITY, while at the same time taking the study
of the semantic classification of Process types to a new level of detail. So, this thesis
also describes More Delicate TRANSITIVITY in the second of the two senses recognised
above.
The rest of this chapter has four main sections: Section 1.2 sets the scene, and
places the thesis in the general areas of (a) SFL as a means of generating language and
(b) explorations in the modelling of TRANSITIVITY. Section 1.3 discusses the
motivation behind the work from the perspective of theory and description, of
application and of methodological considerations. Section 1.4 presents the aims and
scope of the thesis. And finally, Section 1.5 presents an overview of the rest of the
thesis.
‘the grammarian’s dream is (and must be, such is the nature of grammar) of constant
territorial expansion. He would like to turn the whole of linguistic form into grammar,
hoping to show that lexis can be defined as “most delicate grammar”. The exit to lexis
would then be closed, and all exponents ranged in systems.’ (1961/76:69).
These words are taken from Halliday’s seminal paper, ‘Categories of the Theory
of Grammar’ (1961), which was the basis for the theory that came to be known as
Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL). This theory has its basis in Firthian linguistics
2
Chapter 1: Introduction
4
Dale, Mellish and Zock (1990:5) state that ‘Systemic Functional Grammar … emphasize(s) the set of
independent choices that a language generator has to make’, and Fawcett (1992:630) suggests that
‘systemic functional grammars lend themselves naturally to incorporation in computer models of natural
language generation.’
3
Chapter 1: Introduction
description of language from that propounded by Halliday, and the other users of the
Sydney ‘dialect’ of SFG.
Fawcett (1980) sets out a combinatory approach that does not separate the
cognitive from the interactional, and so he proposes a ‘psychosociolinguistic’ model
(1980:1). This approach has led naturally to the realization that the model was
potentially relevant to the field of computational linguistics, and so the COMMUNAL
project came into being. In particular, this thesis will be concerned with GENESYS:
the sentence generation component of the project, which consists of the system
networks and the realization rules of the lexicogrammar5.
One important feature of the CG approach to the modelling of language is that it
is not grammaticality that is paramount, but rather frequency of occurrence. In the
generative grammar this is modelled by the probability of one feature in a system being
chosen rather than another. These probabilities are based on (a) frequencies of
occurrence as observed in large corpora, and (b) likelihood of occurrence, at specific
points in the planning of the production of text sentences. It is the first that is the
concern of this thesis.
This thesis is potentially concerned with modelling the frequency of occurrence
of meanings realised through the TRANSITIVITY system. Halliday (1994:106) states
that this system allows for ‘constru(ing) the world of experience into a manageable set
of process types’ (my emphasis), and so it is through this system that a central portion
of the ‘experiential’ metafunction is realized. Fawcett (1980:134) points out that in the
experiential component ‘the referent situation that has been formulated by the
performer’s problem solver for transmission to the addressee is viewed as “process”,
and that the term “process” is to be interpreted in a sense that includes “relationships”
and “states” as well as “actions” and “changes of relationship and state”.’
In terms of the work reported in this thesis, creating the system network for the
‘Processes’ has required a number of prior stages: the production of an unstructured list
of frequently occurring verb senses in English; analysing each verb sense in the list so
that it can initially be classified as belonging to one broad class of PROCESS TYPE in
terms of its Participant Roles, and then further sub-classified into specific semantic
5
This aspect of the COMMUNAL system is called GENESYS because it GENErates SYStematically.
4
Chapter 1: Introduction
categories that represent the scale of delicacy, and that thus constitute a semantic system
network.
5
Chapter 1: Introduction
saw in Section 1.1, when implemented computationally it also serves as what Tucker
(1996b) calls a ‘powerful generative device, based upon the notion of choice’ (Tucker,
1996b:150). This research is devoted to building a generative system network, in
contrast with one that can only be used as a guide in, for example, manual text analysis.
The specific goal is to model this aspect of TRANSITIVITY in a Natural Language
Generation system in a computer. The thesis is strongly motivated by a need for a
delicate TRANSITIVITY generation component in the COMMUNAL project. I will
not go into further detail about COMMUNAL at this point (see Chapter 4) other than to
say that one of its main aims is to build system networks that are ‘pushed sufficiently
far towards meaning to constitute the semantics, and that there is consequently no need
for a higher level network that is even more ‘semantic’.’ (Fawcett, Tucker and Lin,
(1993:123)). So the practical motivation of this thesis is to achieve this for the system
networks for TRANSITIVITY, which can thus fit this ‘holistic’ program. (See Chapter
4 for an overview of COMMUNAL).
6
Chapter 1: Introduction
greatly extending the existing system networks for TRANSTIVITY, and so working
towards the fulfilment of the ‘grammarian’s dream’, as described above.
The achievement of this goal has in practice involved a number of sub-goals.
The first is to ensure that the product of the research is able to be used as a contribution
to an existing project so that it will not only be applicable but will be applied.
The second sub-goal is methodological, and it is to make use of natural language
corpora in order to ensure that all conclusions are based on real language use, and so
that the system networks that are produced are representative of real language in use.
Following from this, the third goal is to produce a description of this particular
area of meaning that gives coverage on an unrivalled scale. It is only through aiming at
a wide breadth of coverage that the research can test the robustness of the descriptive
framework that it seeks to extend – and, as we shall see in Chapter 5, this work has
contributed to the development of the overall framework as well as to the more delicate
parts of the network. This work therefore contributes to the goals of the COMMUNAL
Project, one of which is to test the model through implementation in a computer system
of the CG description of English. One of the specific contributions of this research has
been to test the existing configurations of Process type and associated Participant Roles
(PRs) and add to them.
The goal of breadth of coverage leads logically to a final aim, which is to give a
central place in the study to multi-word verbs as highly frequent items that should be
central to a full model of language.
One theme of this thesis, therefore, is breadth of coverage. This necessarily
leads to limitations of various types – primarily, of course, on what can be said about
each verb sense or even each class of verb senses that is included. But it also means
that the research is limited to the PROCESS TYPE system – so that it does not seek to
contribute new ideas to the complementation patterns associated with each verb sense,
(for which see Fawcett 1996), nor the other aspects of TRANSITIVITY mentioned in
Section 1.1. Nor does it discuss the question of ‘aspectual type’ for groupings of verbs,
(part of which is derivable from the broad semantic classes reorganized here but part of
which is regarded in the CG as being attached to the ‘event’ that is realized in the clause
rather than in the ‘verb senses’).
A final and more general point that should be made on the scope of this research
is that it concerns English, and my claims do not aim to be universal.
7
Chapter 1: Introduction
8
Chapter 1: Introduction
Chapter 4 provides an overview of the CG. The Cardiff Grammar (CG) is the
‘dialect’ of SFG developed at Cardiff by Fawcett, Tucker, Tench, Young and others.
Since it is the context for the present research, this overview provides a framework in
which to place the chapters that follow. This chapter describes the main characteristics
of the CG and its relationships with the COMMUNAL project, and it emphasizes the
fact that the CG provides for both the generation and the analysis of language. The first
half of the chapter describes the generative component of the COMMUNAL project,
and the second half describes how the grammar can be used as a tool by the text analyst.
Chapter 5 provides a description of the current model of TRANSITIVITY in the
CG. While this description starts from Fawcett’s earlier work (specifically, Fawcett
(1980) and (1987)), the main focus is on the descriptive framework as it currently
stands. The chapter describes the six Process Type categories in the CG: action,
relational, mental, influential, event-relating and environmental, and it points out the
contribution of the present research to the current TRANSITIVITY model in the CG.
Chapter 6 provides the first full description of the computational method for
generating the Subject and Complement of a clause from the sub-networks in the
TRANSTIVITY of PARTICIPANT ROLES. The chapter describes in detail the early
part of the TRANSITIVITY system network into which the system networks for
PROCESS TYPE presented in Chapter 8 connect. The description also draws on the
products of the networks presented in Chapter 8 for the likelihood of changing the
probabilities and for the realisation rules in the PROCESS TYPE system network. Thus
some aspects of the description of the lexicogrammar described in this chapter have
been influenced by the research presented in this thesis.
As stated in Section 1.3.3, the first stage of the methodology established in the
present research was to establish a body of data as the basis for the further development
of the system networks. This involves not merely data collection but also data creation,
and the result is the PTDB. Chapter 7 describes the creation and development of this
database, and discusses the theoretical issues involved in (a) using corpora to determine
which verb senses in English we should include in a generative grammar, (b) how to
ensure that an appropriate proportion of these verbs are multi-word verbs, (c) the
methodology for analysing very large numbers of verb senses (close to 5,400 so far) in
terms of the Process type and their associated Participant Roles, and (d) how to deal
with specific problem areas that arise in this body of data.
9
Chapter 1: Introduction
While the PTDB stands as a useful research tool in its own right, that can be
used, as it stands, as an aid to text analysis, the original purpose of its creation was to be
a resource for building the system networks presented here. Chapter 8 describes the
method employed to create full system networks of Process types by recognising further
semantic classifications within each. The chapter then presents a full description of the
large and delicate system networks for (1) one-role, affected only, action Processes, (2)
two-role, agent plus affected, action Processes and (3) three role, directional Processes.
Finally, chapter 9 provides a discussion and conclusion for the whole thesis.
This chapter summarizes what the thesis demonstrates, what the products and
applications of the thesis are, what the limitations of the thesis are, and it suggests some
pointers for future work.
10
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
2.1 Introduction
The purpose of this chapter is to provide a brief overview of some of the well
established and interesting approaches to TRANSITIVITY of the last forty years. All
the alternative theories considered here are concerned with the semantic classification
of verbs and/or the associated semantic roles in a clause. The purpose of this Chapter
is to provide some criteria for the decision to adopt Systemic Functional Grammar
(SFG) as the most suitable theory for achieving the goals presented in Chapter 1.
One reason might be that, unlike the theories considered in Section 2.2, the SF
approach is not interested in determining linguistic universals for the phenomenon,
and is thus not constrained by exploratory margins that are ‘based on a not necessarily
linguistic intuition’ (Starosta (1988:115-117), as quoted in Halliday and Matthiessen
(1999:442)). Another reason for working in a SF framework is that the goal of my
work is to provide an account that will fit into an existing computationally generative
grammar of English that uses a SF framework. Moreover, while the theories
considered in Section 2.3 provide an interesting and coherent description of the
phenomena in language, they are ultimately descriptive rather than generative
grammars. Finally, the studies presented in Section 2.4 and 2.5 also offer descriptive
accounts of TRANSITIVITY. Unlike the big Grammars provided by Quirk et al and
COBUILD, the studies presented in these sections are concerned with one area of
language. They have, as we shall see, influenced the research presented in this thesis,
despite being conducted in different frameworks (Chomskian, Lexical Semantics,
Functional Grammar, etc.). This chapter, therefore, will provide a backdrop with
which to compare the approach that this thesis takes.
Each of the scholars whose work is presented in this chapter considers
possible classifications of verbs and the possible semantic classification of the
‘arguments’ that each verb can take. The works that I will be concerned with are
described in such a way as to give an overview of key points of the key studies in the
area rather than as an exhaustive historical description.
11
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
12
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
forms’, and which involve a determination of the semantic function. He draws the
conclusion that ‘all semantically relevant syntactic relations between NPs and the
structures which contain them must be of the ‘labelled’ type’ (1968:17).
Fillmore uses the term ‘case’ in the sense first introduced by Blake (1930) ‘to
identify the underlying syntactic-semantic relationship, and the term case form to
mean the expression of the case relationship’ (1968:21). We can recognise from his
much quoted position on the phenomenon that even though he departs from Chomsky
into the realms of the ‘semantic’, his fundamental principles are the same:
‘The case notions comprise a set of universal, presumably innate, concepts which
identify certain types of judgements human beings are capable of making about the
events going on around them’ (1968:24).
If we compare this quotation from Fillmore with the following quotation from
Fawcett, (which illustrates a central notion in this thesis), we can see that the two
frameworks are comparable in defining the area of language. However, these two
paragraphs are also insightful in presenting the difference between Fillmore’s
framework and that of Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL).
‘The analysis of Participant Roles is one of the most insightful ways of understanding
the view of the world that the person producing a text holds. The ‘author’ of a
spoken or written text often reveals quite unconsciously his or her “world view”’
(Fawcett, forthcoming a).
To explain further: while Fillmore is concerned with innate universals, and the
nature of human beings, SF linguists are also interested in what happens in instances
of text produced by individuals.
This section will now look in detail at the main criteria used in Fillmore’s
‘Case Grammar’, to ascertain the extent to which his ‘Case for Case’ publication
recognises the importance of considering the ‘semantic’ function of the grammatical
functions of the sentence.
Fillmore proposes that the NPs associated with a verb are in a ‘case’
relationship with that verb, at the deep structure level. Interestingly, he recognises
that ‘the arrays of cases defining the sentence types of a language have the effect of
imposing a classification on the verb in the language.’ (1968:21), but he does not take
13
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
the further step (as Halliday does, as others whose work is described in this chapter
do, and as is attempted by this thesis) of suggesting what such classifications might
be. Instead, the only semantic classification that he offers is the class of the case
relation, which are comprised of the following set, and have the following labels:
Agentive (A)
Instrumental (I)
Dative (D)
Factitive (F)
Locative (L)
Objective (O)
--
Benefactive (B)
Time (T)
Comitative (C)
6
I have introduced a division between the first six case relations and the last three because these last
three are not included in his initial description of the ‘set’, but are introduced later within the
description of ‘Case Grammar’.
14
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
frame’ information, he is paving the way for this type of information to be included in
a word’s lexical entry in a lexicon. Fillmore suggests that the configurations of case
relations that can occur with a given word constitute that verb’s ‘case frames’, and
each verb in the language needs to be marked for which case frame type it can be
present in. To illustrate, Fillmore presents the lexical entry for ‘open’, and the
different possible case frames for this verb form are presented in Figure 2.2.
Figure 2.2 shows the possible case frames for ‘open’, and also the ‘frame
feature’, (the last in the list) which serves as a summary for the lexical entry for
‘open’ by capturing all the possible case relations, and also capturing the optionality
of cases with bracketing.
So we see that the ‘case frames’, and ultimately the ‘frame feature’ of each
verb occurs in the deep structure. Fillmore explains what he means by deep structure
by stating that:
15
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
proposes rules for English prepositions, as Figure 2.3 shows and which stands as a
suggested classification rather than an absolute classification. Fillmore tells us that
the prepositions that are allied with the various case forms are in fact realizations of
the same underlying element, which he terms ‘Kasus’ or ‘K’.
Agentive: by
Instrumental: by, with
Objective: zero
Factitive: zero
Benefactive: for
Dative: to
Locative: on, at, in, etc.
Figure 2.3 Fillmore’s proposal for the preposition types that each case typically takes
7
In SFL, the equivalent of the verb at the level of semantics is the Process.
16
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
deep structures’ into surface forms of sentences, then it is likely that the syntactic
deep structure of the type that has been made familiar from the work of Chomsky and
his students is going to go the way of the phoneme. It is an artificial intermediate
level between the empirically discoverable ‘semantic deep structure’ and the
observationally accessible surface structure, a level the properties of which have more
to do with the methodological commitments of grammarians than with the nature of
human languages.’ (1968:88).
17
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
However, another type of information is also involved, which states that each
argument of a predicate has a thematic role (or ‘theta role’, or ‘Ø role’), and ‘the set
of thematic functions which arguments can fulfil are drawn from a highly restricted,
finite, universal set’ (Radford, 1988:373). We can see that this claim is very similar
to the claim made by Fillmore, as described in Section 2.2.1. Radford also states that
18
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
the set of thematic functions varies from author to author, but he gives us some idea
of a potential set, which clearly reflects the influence of Fillmore as well as Gruber.
19
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
what theta role (if any) it assigns compositionally to its Subject. This … is surely the
kind of ambitious goal which any serious Theory of Language should set itself’
(1988:389). This is primarily the goal to which the present research is intended to
make a contribution – though in a different theoretical framework.
20
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
And the subject may also have what they term an ‘eventive’ role, where the
noun is deverbal, as in Example (7). In Hallidayan terms this is an example of
Grammatical Metaphor, with the Process of ‘invading’ being nominalized.
Other roles which they recognise that the direct object can function as are
‘locative’, as with the verbs walk, swim, pass, jump, etc, as in Example (9), and
‘resultant’ (or ‘object of result’), whose referent exists only because of the activity
indicated by the verb, as in Example (10), where the cake is created by the process of
21
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
baking. Quirk et al point out that this example should be contrasted with (10a), which
demonstrates a different sense of ‘bake’, with ‘some potatoes’ being affected rather
than created.
They also suggest that the direct object can have a ‘cognate’ role, which, as
Example (11) shows, refers to the event indicated by the verb, where the object is
morphologically related to the verb. And finally, they state that the direct object can
have an ‘eventive’ role, which, in the same way as the ‘eventive’ subject role, takes
the form of a deverbal noun, and bears the major part of the meaning, as in Example
(12). We shall see in Section 7.5.1 of Chapter 7 that in the Cardiff Grammar (CG),
this construction is considered to be a ‘reified process’8.
8
This means that the process is realized as a ‘thing’, which is not the direct object, but instead is the
‘MEx’ (‘Main Verb Extention’).
22
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
23
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
derive meaning groups from the patterns they recognise, these groups do not include
all the verbs of one meaning type. Further, while they make the claim that ‘given a
list of words occurring with a particular pattern, the majority will be divisible by most
observers into reasonably coherent meaning groups’ (2000:86), this is intended to be
merely an observation and NOT a theoretical claim. In other words, they do NOT claim
that ‘a word has a particular pattern because it has a particular meaning’ (2000:86).
This contrasts, as we shall see, with the position taken by Levin (1993).
What their research shows, however, is that by working from the pattern type,
i.e. a group of similarly behaving words, it is possible to derive subclassified meaning
groups. Interestingly, these have many parallels with the product of the research to be
presented in this thesis, i.e. the semantically based system networks that I have
developed that are presented in Chapter 8.
Hunston and Francis argue that their observations about patterns associated
with verb senses ‘blur the distinctions between lexis and grammar (following
Sinclair)’ (2000:250 their bracketing). The blurring of this distinction is also at the
heart of the SF approach to language, where grammatical and lexical items are located
on a continuum – especially as implemented in the version that we shall distinguish in
Chapter 5 as the Cardiff Grammar. However, Hunston and Francis distinguish their
approach from that of SFG, stating that they are not concerned with a
lexicogrammatical continuum, where lexis and grammar are ‘essentially the same
phenomenon’ (2000:251), and where the continuum is realized through the system
network.
Hunston and Francis suggest that there are difficulties with the SFG
framework for modelling lexis. Firstly, they state that a system network involves
directionality, and this, in their view, downgrades lexis ‘to the end-point of grammar’
(2000:251). They state that if you take lexis as the starting point (i.e. as they do) you
arrive at ‘a very different kind of grammar’. This is a fair judgement of the Sydney
version of SFG, but I should point out that in the CG the features in the networks from
which lexical items are generated – prototypically lexical verbs, nouns and adjectives
– often lead on to further systems that are realized in further structures. It seems
possible that the CG, which keeps syntax and lexis in balance, already incorporates
some of the characteristics that Hunston and Francis consider necessary in an
adequate theory of language. Secondly, Hunston and Francis suggest that in the SFG
framework there is no place for the ‘unit of meaning’, or ‘the co-selection of a whole
24
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
phraseology’ (2000:251). While this may be true of Halliday’s model, it is not true
of the CG, as Tucker’s ‘So Grammarians Haven’t the Faintest Idea’ (1996)
demonstrates.
Francis et al’s (1996) approach includes a ‘meaning finder’, which is an index
that lists what Hunston and Francis (2000:109) term ‘notional groups’. These
‘notional groups’ are derived from the verbs that take the same pattern type. For
example, the ‘eating, drinking and smoking’ notional group can occur with the pattern
V into n, as in example (1). So while they are hesitant to make any strong theoretical
claims about the relation between form and meaning, their data provides evidence that
the patterns they recognise can be related groups of verbs that share the same
meaning.
Hunston and Francis recognise that meanings can be mapped onto patterns,
and that Halliday’s terminology of ‘process’, ‘participant’ and ‘circumstance’ allows
us to implement such a mapping. They note that ‘the participant roles proposed by
Halliday (1994), represent the most effective way of dealing with this kind of
variation in mapping. … the role realised by a noun group (in a clause) depends
entirely on the choice of verb’ (2000:127). Furthermore, they point out that two verbs
with different patterns may take the same roles, and also that two verbs with the same
pattern may demand different roles (2000:125) – and in this respect there appears to
be common ground with the position taken in this thesis.
A criticism they make of Halliday’s approach to this area is that, in the case of
metaphorical processes, he provides a dual analysis – one congruent analysis and one
metaphorical analysis. The present thesis also recognises this as a problem, and a
description of the treatment of metaphor in this thesis is provided in Section 7.6.3.1 of
Chapter 7. These present an alternative approach to dealing with metaphor that does
not generalise between ‘congruent’ and the ‘metaphorical’ usage, preferring instead to
model what we might call ‘intended’ meaning. It greatly reduces the number of cases
of metaphor, but it does not directly – and should not – eliminate metaphor entirely.
Hunston and Francis state that in their work they ‘associate roles not with
process types but with notional groups, or meanings’ (2000:129), but the position that
I shall take in this thesis is that a ‘process-type’ is defined in terms of its associated
25
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
configuration of PRs. The question is: Do the Process types, as so defined, coincide
with the groupings by ‘verb sense’ (a concept that seems to be close to Hunston and
Francis ‘notional group’). As we shall see in Chapter 8, there is not a simple answer
to this question.
In describing language as patterns of co-occurring items, Hunston and Francis
incorporate Sinclair’s ‘idiom principle’, which allows for the recognition of frequent
collocations as key in the description. The concept behind a theory that is based on
language use and thus centralizes frequently occurring patterns is of particular interest
to this thesis. And, as we shall see in Chapter 7, the results of their work have proved
to be invaluable for my research. However, the CG also has the means for dealing
with frequently occurring ‘units of meaning’, such as analysis of Main Verb plus
Main Verb Extension, and ‘reified Processes’, as we shall see in Chapter 7.
Moreover, the CG has the means for generating how such ‘units of meaning’ are fitted
together. In addition, the concept of ‘re-entry’ in the system network approach means
that we can specify predetermined and partly predetermined choices on re-entry to the
network, and so the units of meaning can be realized in the lower units in a complex
structure, and so fit together with the other units of meaning to realize pattern types
such as those of Hunston and Francis that involve more than one layer of structure
(see Fawcett, Tucker and Lin, (1993) and Tucker (1996) and the explanation of how
the Cardiff version of SFG works).
2.3.3. Biber et al’s (1999) The Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written
English
Biber et al’s Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English (LGSWE) is
also a grammar based on the analysis of a large corpus. Using essentially the same
categories as Quirk et al (1985), they provide a detailed description, with many
statements of frequencies and their variations according to certain major registers.
Reflecting a similar approach to that of Hunston and Francis (2000), Biber et
al state that ‘syntax and lexicon are often treated as independent components in
English. Analysis of real texts shows, however, that most frequent syntactic
structures tend to have an associated set of words or phrases that are frequently used
with them’. (1999:13).
26
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
Their treatment of verbs considers the ‘major verb functions and classes’.
Their approach is different from the COBUILD approach, in that they recognise
‘semantic domains’ which involve hierarchies. The COBUILD researchers do not do
this explicitly, but, as we have seen, they look at meaning groups, as recognised from
the patterns, and these groups do not have sub-types. Biber et al recognise seven
major semantic domains: ‘activity verbs, communication verbs, mental verbs,
causative verbs, verbs of simple occurrence, verbs of existence or relationship and
aspectual verbs’ (1999:360), all of which can be subclassified. They recognise that
each of these classes has a ‘core’ meaning, which relates to the notion of prototypes.
But they also recognise that many verbs have multiple meanings that must be
accounted for, and that may be from different semantic domains.
Like COBUILD, they recognise the patterns that verbs can occur in. They use
traditional terminology to describe these patterns (intransitive, monotransitive,
ditranstive and complex transitive). They suggest which semantic domains occur with
which patterns, and they describe this across registerial domains. However, they
make no attempt to describe the semantic features of the patterns themselves, and nor
does the description attempt to provide an account of semantic roles associated with
each verb type.
Their description provides useful corpus based statistics of the occurrences of
verbs in real language, and this is the main value of Biber et al to this thesis. In
particular, they detail the occurrence of multi word verbs in real language, and, as we
shall see in Chapter 7, this was of real value in developing the Process Type Database.
27
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
‘class types’ fits with the ‘Process types’ and ‘Participant Role (PR) configurations’
that this thesis is concerned with.
Levin’s hypothesis is that a verb’s alternations and its internal structures are
determined by its meaning. She states that her work:
‘is guided by the assumption that the behaviour of a verb, particularly with respect to
the expression and interpretation of its arguments, is to a large extent determined by
its meaning’ (1993:1)
28
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
Levin recognises a set of alternations that, she implies, can account for all the
meanings she is concerned with. I should emphasize that, while she implies that the
set of alternations is finite, she points out that her offering does not attempt to be an
exhaustive account of such a finite set. She suggests that any new verbs that come
into the language will behave in the pattern of the established alternation types, and
she proposes that the alternation type that a new verb will behave in is predicted by
meaning type. If we look at the example of the verb ‘fax’ we note that this verb has
come into usage fairly recently, and, interestingly, that it behaves in the same way as
the other verbs of the type ‘instrument of communication’ (e.g. ‘cable’, ‘wire’,
‘radio’, etc). So, ‘fax’ comes into the language, and the speakers of the language
‘instinctively’ know that this new verb fits into a class which Levin terms ‘Verbs of
Instrument of Communication’, and, as such, the verb ‘fax’ will participate in the
‘dative alternation’, as illustrated in Examples (2a) and (2b).
Levin asserts that a new verb fits into the semantically related class, and it fits
into the alternation type that this class takes. This serves as support for her hypothesis
that there is link between meaning and syntactic behaviour.
The aspect of Levin’s work that is most interesting to this thesis is the
categorisation of verb classes. I studied her classes carefully and was sometimes able
to adopt her ideas for my classification, as we shall see in Chapter 8. However,
despite my original hope that Levin’s work would play a large part in this thesis, the
experience of looking closely at the large number of verb senses that I have included
in the Process Type Database (as described in Chapter 7), I found several
incompatibilities that made it difficult to incorporate as much as I had expected of her
work in the research presented here.
In this section I will concentrate on an evaluation of Levin’s proposals, and
then in Section 7.6.1.2 in Chapter 7 I will provide an account of how I attempted to
use her description of verb classes as a resource for classification.
The first aspect of Levin’s work that is incompatible with a central point of
this thesis is that her classification does not include any multi-word verbs. As we
shall see, the multi-word verbs occur in English with a very high frequency, and they
29
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
are used to convey a large number of meanings in the language. However, Levin does
not deal with more than a very few cases.
However, she does account for the possibility of an alternation that introduces
a ‘preposition’ into the clause. An example of this is her ‘Preposition Drop
Alternation’. She describes this transitivity alternation as ‘involving a verb found
either in an intransitive frame with a prepositional phrase complement or else in a
transitive frame.’ (1993:43)9. This is manifested in the difference between a ‘single
word verb sense’ and an associated ‘prepositional verb sense’, as Examples 3a and 3b
show.
While it appears that an alternation such as this accounts for the introduction
of a preposition into the clause, and so for the possible recognition of a verb class that
take a prepositional phrase as its Complement. However, this is not a standard
prepositional verb, such as look at or dispose of are. It is interesting that the ‘peer
verbs’ class, which includes the most frequently occurring prepositional verb (Biber et
al, 1999) look at, does not enter into any alternation, but is a semantically coherent
group that take a prepositional phrase complement.
Furthermore, Levin does not include any ‘phrasal verbs’ in her classification.
Yet it is the frequently used phrasal verbs in the language that often have individual
meanings, unrelated to the single word verb form that they include. For example, she
does not include the phrasal verb ‘get up’, which is semantically unrelated to the
single verb ‘get’. The nearest verb classes for this phrasal verb to occur in are ‘verbs
of assuming a position’ (bend, bow, crouch, flop, hang, kneel, lean, lie …) and ‘verbs
of inherently directed motion’ (advance, arrive, ascend, come, depart …). In fact, ‘get
up’ does not fit into these classes because it does not participate in the relevant
alternations. It would therefore be interesting to add to Levin’s work by determining
the place for the many frequently used multi-word verbs.
9
The approach taken in the present thesis would not recognise (3b) as an intransitive verb.
30
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
10
My own detailed study of ‘open’ as it occurs in the COBUILD corpus is presented in Section 7.6.3.2
of Chapter 7.
31
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
given class can be transitive while some cannot, it would perhaps have been more
insightful to provide a gloss of the sense type, which could then be referred to as a
separate sense11. Just as the verbs ‘kill’ and ‘die’ refer to related activities and are
nonetheless recognised by Levin as separate senses (and so occur in separate
classes), we should also treat different senses of the forms such as open
separately12.
4. ‘Verbs of spatial configuration’. It is simply not clear which sense of ‘open’
Levin includes in this class. The example she provides for this class is ‘a statue
stood on the corner’, and so we can recognise it to be an intransitive clause, which
lacks an agentive argument13.
Rather than recognising the different senses a form might have, Levin explains
that the verb form has ‘extended meaning’. She claims that ‘when a verb has more
than one meaning, one of its meanings is basic, and the others are systematically
related to it, that is, they are instances of extended meaning’ (1993:22). Here I shall
take a different position, namely that most of Levin’s ‘extended meanings’ are
separate ‘senses’ and the term ‘extended meanings’ is more appropriately used to
refer to metaphor.
This discussion of alternative viewpoints on this matter highlights how
problematical it is to determine which verb sense she is referring to, at any given point
– especially since she does not provide examples for each verb listed in each class.
She merely points out that there are exceptions, without fully illustrating their
implications. For example, for open in the class described in 3 above she states that
verbs in this class cannot occur in a transitive construction EXCEPT FOR break and
open. Kohl et al (1998) justifiably state that ‘since [Levin] gives only one example
sentence per verb class, significant effort was required to reach our goal of producing
natural sounding example sentences for all verbs.’ They cite, as an example, the class
of verbs ‘Motion around an Axis’, for which Levin provides an example sentence of
11
The hypothesis of treating all verb senses as individual and unrelated items will be discussed further
in this thesis, particularly in Section 5.4.1 in Chapter 5, where an argument for not generalising
between ‘transitive’ and ‘intransitive’ constructions is presented.
12
Levin includes ‘kill’ in the ‘murder verbs’ class, and ‘die’ in the ‘verbs of disappearance’ class.
13
As we shall see in Chapter 5, in the Cardiff Grammar treats this example as an ‘action’ Process, with
‘one-role’, and that role is a ‘carrier’.
32
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
‘Janet turned the cup’ and the alternation ‘the cup turned’. Kohl et al point out that
‘when other verbs in the class are mechanically substituted for turning the example
sentences, some of them sound odd: for example, ‘Janet wound the cup’.
Furthermore, Levin does not cover all the senses that a verb form can have,
but seems only to cover the most central sense – i.e. what we might refer to as the
‘prototype’14. Thus, she recognises the verb ‘make’ as occurring in the verb classes
‘build verbs’, for which the semantic specification is that something is created, and
‘dub verbs’, for which the semantic specification is that the verb ‘relates to the
bestowing of names’ (1993:182). But from my trawling through occurrences of
different senses of the verb form I was able to recognise a further sense that is not
included in any of her verb class. This is the sense of ‘make’ as a synonym of the
verbs ‘amounts to’ and ‘equals’. In fact, she does not include this area of meaning at
all, which leads us to a further consideration.15
Since the semantic coverage of each of the verb forms included in Levin
(1993) is not overtly defined, it is not possible to even begin to discover how she
arrived at the areas of meaning that she recognises (in contrast with the clear account
of the methodology used by Hunston and Francis (2000)). We know that her main
goal is to show the impressive extent to which meanings and the syntactic patterns
that a verb can occur in (or rather, the alternations a verb can occur in), match up, and
that concurrently her aim is to ascertain how the semantic relations she recognises
correlate with semantic classes. But in attempting to achieve this goal there seem to
be many semantic fields, or ‘areas of experience’ that are missing (such as the area of
meaning which is realised by the verbs ‘makes’, ‘equals’ and ‘amounts to’), and from
the viewpoint of this thesis it is regrettable that there are such holes in the coverage of
the semantic domains.
A further failing in this study is that her coverage does not include some of the
most frequently used verbs. Indeed, their inclusion would itself be a useful key to a
full coverage of many semantic domains. As we shall see in Section 7.6.1.2 of
14
I return to the notion of ‘prototypes’ in verb categories in Section 7.6.1 of Chapter 7.
15
Hunston and Francis (2000) recognise a similar argument in relation to their own description of the
‘patterns’ a verb can occur in. They argue that ‘the semantic set that die belongs to changes according
to the pattern with which the verb is used. Die, in fact, belongs to several sets: one with the meaning
‘disappear’, one where it is connected with live, one with the meaning ‘finish’ and one with the
meaning ‘suffer’.’ (2000:146). A more detailed description of Hunston and Francis (2000) was
provided in Section 2.3.2 of this Chapter.
33
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
Chapter 7, Levin fails to deal with 10% of the verbs that COBUILD recognises to be
in the top-most bracket of frequently occurring verbs (those with five diamonds in
Francis et al (1996)).
Finally, and perhaps most importantly, Levin’s verb classes do not always
comprise a semantically coherent group, despite her assertion that ‘any class of verbs
whose members pattern together with respect to diathesis alternations should be a
semantically coherent class: its members should share at least some aspect of
meaning’. (1993:14).
It would seem that while a group of verbs that fit the same alternation types
leads one to recognise some kind of relation, it is not necessarily the case that this
relation is a semantic one. For example, her verb class of ‘amalgamate verbs’ are
grouped purely on the grounds of the alternations they can enter into, as she states:
‘the most salient property of the amalgamate verbs is that they undergo the simple
reciprocal alternation, but they are set apart from the other verbs of combining and
attaching in not being found in the together reciprocal alternation.’ (1993:161). But
when we look closely at this verb class, we can see that they do not belong
semantically with the ‘verbs of combining and attaching’, and indeed, that they are
not all semantically related to the verb ‘amalgamate’. For example, she includes the
verbs ‘alternate’, ‘compare’ and ‘confuse’ in this class, which seem to the reader at
least to be semantically different. Another large verb class which consists of
semantically diverse verbs is the ‘alternating verbs of change of state’ (1993:244),
which was mentioned above as having open as a member. The verbs in this class
have a relationship to each other in so much as they all involve a ‘change of state’,
but, as we shall see in Chapter 8, my classification of this highly generalized category
necessarily involves several stages of subcategorisation to reach some semantic
cohesion.
I will conclude this summary of the work of Levin (1993) by quoting her
honest statement that ‘the scope of this book is likely to contain inconsistencies,
omissions and inaccuracies, which reflect the practical difficulties that face attempts
to accurately and exhaustively carry out hypothesis checking over a large number of
English verbs’ (1993:19). And, although I have tried to repair some of the
inconsistencies in Levin’s semantic classification, I would like to adopt her cautionary
words as ones that can also be applied to the work presented in this thesis.
34
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
However, Levin also states that ‘what is important is the existence of core sets
of verbs with specific sets of properties that can provide the basis for the later
identification of meaning components’ (1993:19) (my emphasis). What my
research aims to do is to provide a set of system networks for ‘most delicate
TRANSITIVITY’ that has a place in a generative model of language. And since it is
to be a semantic classification of verbs that can also handle ‘sets of verbs with
specific sets of [syntactic] properties’ I am, I would like to think, carrying out some of
the work of the ‘later development of many concepts’ that she anticipates. Moreover,
my aim is that the results will be sufficiently explicit to be tested in a computer model
of language generation.
35
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
semantic difference appears elsewhere in the clause, that difference will also show (a)
to be higher in Transitivity’. And they make the point – familiar from studies
summarized earlier – that these parameters are ‘structural-semantic’ features.
They argue that high transitivity and low transitivity correlate respectively
with ‘foregrounding’ and ‘backgrounding’ in discourse. In line with this, ‘two
participant’ clauses most frequently serve a foregrounding function, while ‘one
participant’ clauses serve a backgrounding function. This assertion, they state, is
borne out by the fact that ‘two participant’ clauses typically involve actions and ‘one
participant’ clauses typically involve states. And in the case where a backgrounding
function has ‘two participants’, the Agent will often be covert.
Hopper and Thompson claim that this is a correlation between grammar and
discourse, and they conclude that one necessarily influences the other. This is, of
course, a functionally based approach, and while their categorization is along different
lines from that in a SFG, both adopt a functional standpoint.
They conclude: “while we claim that the discourse distinction between
foregrounding and backgrounding provides the key to understanding the grammatical
and semantic facts we have been discussing, we also explicitly recognize that
grounding itself reflects a deeper set of principles – relating to decisions which
speakers make, on the basis of their assessment of their hearer’s situation, about how
to present what they have to say.’ (1980:295).
While there are no concepts in their work that I need to incorporate in the
present research, it is interesting to find an approach to transitivity that is from a
DISCOURSE perspective and to think about the possible future applications.
36
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
37
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
is therefore semantically based, and the first split was between what they termed
‘actions’, ‘events’, and ‘states’.
Within these initial categories, actions and events are split into the following
‘files’: motion, perception, contact, communication, competition, change, cognition,
consumption, creation, emotion, possession, bodily care and functions, and social
behaviour and interactions. These ‘files’ are partly based on Miller and Johnson-
Laird (1976).
States are split into the following: ‘be’, ‘resemble’, ‘belong’ and ‘suffice’,
each of which they recognise to be a heterogeneous class that does not constitute a
semantic domain, and also the auxiliary verbs, control verbs and aspectual verbs.
So, as with Fillmore and Levin, the goal for WordNet is to carve up the ‘verb
lexicon’ into semantically related fields. This is, for the present research, roughly
equivalent to dividing up the relevant parts of the experiential strand of meaning into
Process types.
They then rebuilt these domains into groups of synonyms, which they call
‘synsets’. These sets are constituted of verbs that can be substituted for each, i.e. that
are claimed to be synonyms of each other, such as ‘shut’ and ‘close’. They recognise
that the synonyms in a synset can be of different registers in the language, and thus a
speaker has some basis for knowing which verb is appropriate in which context. They
mark such register differences with a bracketed comment to state this fact. They also
recognise that there are selectional restrictions that will determine which verb is
appropriate in a context (i.e. what can co-occur with each verb) and that this will also
differentiate apparent synonyms. In the case where verbs are differentiated by their
selectional restrictions, they choose to avoid putting such verbs in the same synset.
In this publication, Fellbaum suggests that future versions of WordNet may
well incorporate syntactic information. They may attempt to map thematic roles onto
each verb entry, and thus be useful for Natural Language Processing applications.
The importance for this thesis is that this was not their starting point, as they used a
pre-existing inventory of semantic classes, and then fitted the lexemes into these
classes. The Process Type Database, to be presented in Chapter 7, has the advantage
of being created from a ‘bottom-up’ perspective, i.e. working from the words upwards
to the categorial information, which is thus suitable for interpreting in system network
terms and thus for becoming part of an integrated, generative lexicogrammar.
38
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
2.5.3 Faber and Mairal Uson (1999) Constructing a Lexicon of English Verbs
Faber and Mairal Uson work in the framework of Dik’s Functional Grammar
(FG), and they seek to develop his proposals. These suggest the value of organizing
predicates into coherent semantic classes, encoding both syntactic and semantic
regularities. They have developed a framework that they call the ‘Functional-
Lexamatic Model’ (FLM), and their intention is to expand the FG lexicon, particularly
in the area of verbs in the belief that a verb’s semantic components can reflect their
syntactic properties. Thus, their starting point is similar to Levin’s. However, they
are working in a different framework from Levin, and they aim to map both
paradigmatic and syntagmatic information onto each verb entry in the lexicon.
Faber and Mairal Uson aim to include information in their lexicon of how
lexemes are related to other lexemes. They state that:
‘microstructurally, this means examining the role meaning definitions play in the
development of the interface between syntax and semantics. Macrostructurally, this
signifies situating lexemes within the larger context of their lexical domain, and
specifying their relations with lexemes in other areas of meaning. In this way,
lexemes are not conceived as a frozen list of items, but rather as dynamic
representations within a conceptual network.’ (1999:3)
And the fact that they take this step of aiming to realize a dynamic
representation with a conceptual network brings them closer than some other studies
to the aims of the present research. They move away from a flat taxonomy towards
the ‘system network’ model of SF, which they term a ‘conceptual network’. It seems
that their network is essentially like a system network, because their lexicon also
incorporates a hierarchical structure, with classifications and further
subclassifications. They classify by constructing ‘lexical hierarchies’, and a lexical
hierarchy includes a ‘genus’, which they also refer to as the ‘prototype. For example,
in the lexical domain of possession, the genus is ‘have’, with all the other verbs in this
domain structured around this genus. Moreover, they state that the relation between
‘have’ and the other related verbs ‘is conceived as a structural relation in the global
context of an entire lexical domain, and not merely pertaining to individual lexemes.’
(1999:102). The ‘genus’ is like the ‘x_as_such’ type of feature that we shall meet in
Chapter 8.
39
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
2.6 Conclusion
All of the approaches that have been considered in this Chapter have one key
assumption in common, with the exception of the COBUILD model, as they are all
concerned with describing a ‘lexicon’, while, at the same time, seeking to include
syntactic/structural information about each item. Clearly, the way forward is through
the concept of a ‘lexicogrammar’, which includes both the syntactic and the lexical
semantic. And this is precisely the type of model that the rest of this thesis will be
concerned with.
An overview of all the work conducted in this area could have included
summaries of research on many further theories and, in particular, we could consider
in more detail Gruber (1965), Fillmore’s later work including his ‘Frame Net’,
Anderson’s work, and also that of Chafe, Cook, and Dik. But for the purposes of this
thesis I have chosen to concentrate on another line of thinking that also began in the
40
Chapter 2: An Overview of Selected Approaches to TRANSITIVITY
late sixties. The next chapter will therefore present a detailed examination of the SFL
approach, starting with Halliday’s 1961 paper, ‘Categories of the Theory of
Grammar’.
41
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
16
A detailed description of the present CG account of TRANSITIVITY will be presented in Chapter 5.
42
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
these important papers that Halliday’s view on TRANSITIVITY is first presented and
that the move away from traditional views of grammar towards a more semantic
approach can be detected.
Having determined Halliday’s early standpoint, the next section of the chapter
will turn to other scholars who took these works and produced summaries of, or
arguments to, Halliday’s proposals, namely Muir (1972), Berry (1975) and Hudson
(1971). The chapter will also examine further contributions by Halliday, in particular
1970 and 1977. Also included, as stated above, are important publications by Fawcett
– 1973 and 1980 – as precursors to the branch of the theory known as the Cardiff
Grammar. The first half of the chapter will conclude with a discussion of Halliday’s
most recent and explicit statement on TRANSITIVITY – his 1985 handbook, ‘An
Introduction to Functional Grammar’.
After IFG, which serves as the handbook to Halliday’s theory, we find further
work by scholars who are either taking on board the theory and adapting as they see
fit for their specific uses, or using the theory as a tool and conducting research which
extends the theory into further domains. The second half of this chapter, which is
concerned with various other approaches to TRANSITIVITY in Systemic Functional
Linguistics, will essentially be split into three main parts, each concerned with one
major theme.
The first section of part two will consider a concept which is a major concern
of a Systemic Functional grammar and concerns the area of TRANSITIVITY first
introduced by Halliday in 1961; the notion of ‘lexis as most delicate grammar’.
Tucker (1998) provides a description of approaches to lexis in a SFL
framework, and, as might be expected, this description includes accounts of the ‘lexis
as most delicate grammar’ view, which was described in Section 3.1.3. Tucker
recognizes the works of Berry (1977), Fawcett (1980) and Hasan (1987) as being the
most important explorations of the ‘lexis as most delicate grammar’ notion, and this
chapter will examine these scholar’s approaches to modelling a ‘lexicogrammar’.
Section 3.2.1 will consider Berry and Fawcett’s proposals, and then Section 3.2.2 will
look at Hasan’s (1987) treatment of a small area of semantically related verbs. The
‘delicacy’ theme has been recurrent in the first half of this chapter, and it is of
particular relevance to the present research, which is concerned with creating delicate
system networks.
43
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
The next major section of this half of the chapter is concerned with another
theme that has appeared through the first half of this chapter. This is the notion of
‘ergativity’ in TRANSITIVITY, and Section 3.2.4 will explore Davidse’s work in this
area. The notion of causation is central in classifying the functioning of Processes
and Participants in the experiential metafunction, and Davidse’s extensive work on
the area provides us with some possible extensions of Halliday’s work.
Finally, the last section of this chapter of TRANSITIVITY is concerned with a
third recurrent theme – the progression of the description of TRANSITIVITY in SFG.
This final section will be concerned with Matthiessen (1995) as a current description
of TRANSITIVITY for the purposes of generation, and so including the presentation
of the system networks for TRANSITIVITY that are missing in Halliday (1985/94).
It should be noted that the further developments in the framework of the
Cardiff Grammar are described fully in Chapter 5.
44
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
45
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
17
See also the networks presented in Chapter 8.
46
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
English clause structure’. And he continues: ‘all clause structures can then be stated
as combinations of these four in different places: SAPA, ASP, SPC, ASPCC, etc’
(1961/76:61). There is no explicit statement about TRANSITIVITY, but at this point
Halliday is in effect saying that a clause can have a subject and zero, one or two
complements.
To summarize ‘Categories’, we may say that Halliday’s main concern here is
with what the clause might ‘do’ syntagmatically. He isn’t yet raising
TRANSITIVITY from its traditional domain of the unit of ‘word’ (verb) to the unit of
clause and, although he introduces the idea of system and delicacy, he does not relate
this to TRANSITIVITY, and there is, therefore, no statement about TRANSITIVITY
in paradigmatic terms.
47
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
effectiv e
2
ÌP g o al-
in tran s itiv e
ext
Ì -C
g o al- non-
tran s itiv e b en efactiv e
Through this explicit network, combined with the realizations given below
each feature, we are able to envisage exactly how a traversal of the network, making
more delicate choices, might enable ‘lexis’ to be incorporated into a single system
with grammar, and Halliday states that ‘The selection of any one feature specifies,
through an accompanying realization statement, how, and by what item, the feature is
to be realized in structure.’ (Halliday 1966/76).
Further explanation of this 1964 network will be delayed until Section 3.1.5,
as it is not until 1967 that Halliday provides a clear description, and the networks of
1967 are almost identical to that in Figure 3.1.
18
Halliday adopts the term ‘realization’ in his paper ‘Deep Grammar’ (1966), and from this point it
takes the place of the earlier ‘exponence’. The term ‘realization’ was originally Lamb’s term, and, as
we shall see in due course, it was to come to be used in conjunction with the description of system
networks throughout the theory.
48
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
49
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
iii
effective
iv
i descriptive
extensive
v
operative
major clause vi
middle
vii
receptive
ii
intensive
Figure 3.2 from Halliday, 1967:43
This network allows for the generation of the following. If (i) [extensive]
Process is chosen, then the Process will be of ‘action’, e.g. the verb ‘to wash’ and the
clause in example (1):
If (ii) [intensive] Process is chosen, then the Process will express the
‘ascription’ of some ‘attribute’ to a given Participant, e.g. ‘seem’, as in (2):
50
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
and in (6):
If [descriptive] and [operative] (iv/v) are chosen, then the Subject will
represent the ‘Initiator’ of a non-directed action, performed by another Participant,
e.g. ‘he’ in example (7):
If [effective] and [receptive] (iii/vii) are chosen, then the Subject will represent
the Goal e.g. ‘the clothes’ in example (8):
If [descriptive] and [receptive] (iv/vii) are chosen, then the Subject will
represent the Actor who performs a non-direct action, e.g. ‘the prisoner’ in example
(9):
19
This is an odd example, because it involves a third Participant Role that is omitted in this case, i.e.
‘marching’ inherently involves a Participant Role that is a direction. This will be addressed later in the
present section.
51
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
If [effective] and [middle] (iii/vi) are chosen, then the Actor and Goal
represent the same thing, thus producing a reflexive clause, e.g. ‘she’ and ‘herself’ in
example (10):
If [descriptive] and [middle] (iv/vi) are chosen, then the Actor and Initiator
represent the same thing, e.g. ‘the prisoners’ in example (11):
Halliday is concerned with one further role at this point: that of ‘Attribuant’,
which occurs in a clause of ascription, i.e. ‘intensive’ Process, indicated by (ii) in the
network.
An important factor about TRANSITIVITY, as Halliday describes it in this
paper, is that a Participant may be covert. That is to say, a function or role can be
obligatory to the Process, but not overtly realized in the structure and so ‘the
expression of any given Participant Role is not obligatory’ (1967:44).
This leads to a further connected network (Figure 3.2.1), with choices between
‘the presence and the absence of a complement as goal’ (1967:46), and also the
distinction between [process-oriented] and [agent-oriented].
52
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
effective
goal-transitive
descriptive
goal-intransitive
extensive
operative
agent-oriented
middle
process-oriented
receptive
intensive
Having discussed what features might occur in the clause for the types
explored so far, Halliday suggests further possible Participant Role types. He calls
these ‘circumstantials’, and although in this thesis we are not concerned with
Circumstances,20 these further roles need to be assessed with regard to the fact that
they have been traditionally given a place in ‘transitivity’, especially by those scholars
concerned with ‘case’ - see the discussion in Chapter 2 of Fillmore (1968a) and Lyons
(1968).
20
At least not in the Cardiff Grammar framework’s definition of Circumstances.
53
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
And Halliday notes that this is a Circumstantial Role because ‘structurally, (it)
may be realized by the clause element “complement”’ (1967:53). His description of
the ‘beneficiary’ states that it is not usual for an ‘inanimate’ Participant to occur as
‘beneficiary’ unless in a quasi-metaphorical usage, as in examples (15a) and (15b):
54
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
TYPE. As we shall see later in this chapter (Section 3.1.14), this semantic set (plus
other verbs of a similar type) will be classed by Halliday as ‘relational’ Processes of
‘possession’.
The next ‘circumstantial’ PR described by Halliday is that of ‘range’. ‘Range’
is described as ‘specify(ing) the extent of (the process’s) scope or relevance’
(1967:58). Unlike the ‘beneficiary’, the ‘range’ will typically be ‘inanimate’.
Halliday states that the distinction between ‘range’ and ‘goal’ is not clear cut. The
‘beneficiary’ function is an easily recognizable element of the clause, and is a tangible
concept, and a semantic label describing the function of the role. However, the
‘range’ is different in that it is – like the ‘goal’ – an extension of the ‘actor’ and
‘process’ relationship.
The ‘range’ is typically found to occur with the Process type ‘descriptive’, i.e.
non-directed action. In a lot of cases it can actually be a nominalization of the
process, as in example (19):
In such cases then, there is an expectancy of collocation between the verb and
the noun.
The description of ‘range’ so far is as an optional role in the clause (typical of
descriptive processes). However, Halliday suggests that an obligatory ‘range’ occurs
in the clause in example (20), ‘in which the process … is entirely expressed in the
nominal event, the verb merely specifying that there is a process involved.’ (1967:60).
55
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Halliday points out that with this example the role would be more explicit with the
insertion of ‘if’, as in example (23a):
Both of these last two roles to be described can only occur at the position of
Complement in the clause (unlike the other roles which we have looked at, which may
occur at the position of Subject). Moreover, they cannot be nominal groups with a
proper noun, a pronoun or a determiner at the head.
In this section I have described the features of the system network in Halliday
1967, and also the possible functions that can occur with the various clause types that
their features define. Coming just six years after ‘Categories’ (1961), where Halliday
simply specified that the clause can have zero, one or two complements, this
represents a great advance. However, as we shall see in the rest of the chapter, further
changes to and expansions of the system network will occur.
21
Halliday 1967 states that the Range cannot be considered a full participant, but he does not give a full
explanation for why this should be so; ‘while the goal is treated by the language as a full participant in
the process, the Range is still only a pseudo-participant.’ (Halliday 1967:61)
56
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
22
(Fillmore 1966:4-5): “It seems to me that … there is a semantically relevant relation between ‘the
door’ and ‘open’ that is the same in the two sentences ‘the door will open’ and ‘the janitor will open
the door’, in spite of the fact that ‘the door’ is the Subject of the so-called intransitive verb and the
Object of the so-called transitive verb … It is this function for which I used the term ‘affected’”.
23
For the Cardiff Grammar’s use of this term, see Chapter 5.
57
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Halliday further suggests that there are instances ‘where there is no external
causer’ (1968:188), and so the obligatory ‘affected’ entity and the optional ‘causer’
combine with the result that two roles are realized as one Participant. To exemplify
this he gives the example (3) where Mary is a ‘causer/affected’:
A further distinction that Halliday makes between the transitive and the
ergative is that transitive functions are ‘fundamentally those of action clauses’
(1968:188), whereas ergative functions ‘seem to be common to all types of process
and, in fact, to all clause types, including relations and mental processes.’ (1968:189).
Interestingly, this is the first time that the ergative/transitive distinction has
come into Halliday’s discussion. He goes on to recognize that the structure of
‘action’ clauses in terms of ergative patterning suggests some ‘tentative observations
concerning other clause types’ (1968:190).
I will now examine Halliday’s description of these other Process types in his
1968 paper. Essentially, it signals a further stage in the development of his concept of
TRANSITIVITY towards a more ‘semantic’ system network.
The first type to be discussed is the Process that prototypically uses the verb
be. It relates to the description of ‘intensive’ clauses, (see the discussion of Halliday
1967 above), which he recognizes as a Process of ‘ascription’, assigning an
‘attribute’, as in example (4):
58
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
It appears (although it is not made explicit at this point) that the ‘intensive’
and the ‘equative’ are sub-classes of what will later come to be known as a
‘relational’ Process, and so would be modelled in a network as in Figure 3.3.
intensive
relational process
equative
59
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
60
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
‘in serving this function language also gives structure to experience, and helps to
determine our way of looking at things, so that it requires some intellectual effort to
see them in any other way than that which our language suggests to us’. (Halliday,
1970:143).
The three major Process types that Halliday recognizes at this stage are
‘action’, ‘mental’ and ‘relational’, and he considers these types to be classifications in
terms of semantic roles. As we saw with his treatment of ‘mental’ Processes above
(Section 3.1.6), the roles of ‘actor’ and ‘goal’ do not encompass everything that needs
to be captured semantically. In this paper he introduces a number of new, more
semantically based roles as a departure from simply recognizing a clause such as (1)
as being ‘transitive’, and thus involving a ‘Process’ plus an ‘Actor’ plus a ‘Goal’:
He links this notion of ‘logical categories’ with his proposed roles (or
‘participant functions’, as he also refers to them), so that the roles of ‘Actor’, ‘Goal’
and ‘Beneficiary’ relate to ‘logical subject’, ‘logical direct object’ and ‘logical
61
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
indirect object’ respectively. The implication is that these concepts of ‘Actor’, ‘Goal’
and ‘Beneficiary’ are in line with Sweet’s ‘logical categories’, and that the elements
of structure in the clause (i.e. Subject, Object, Complement) are in line with Sweet’s
‘grammatical categories’. We saw in Section 3.1.4 how Halliday conflates these in
the analyses of clauses by the use of superscripts.
The description of TRANSITIVITY in this paper highlights the
subclassification of these ‘logical categories’, or ‘Participant Roles/Functions’ for
further semantic specification of what is ‘going on’ in the clause.
The additional roles to ‘actor’, ‘goal’ and ‘beneficiary’ that Halliday proposes
are ‘instrument’, ‘force’ and two types of ‘recipient’. We will first consider the two
types of ‘recipient’. In Halliday’s description he suggests that the role ‘beneficiary’
can be split into two types of ‘recipient’: ‘recipient of an object’, as in (3), and
‘recipient of a service’, as in (4):
However, it is interesting that he does not distinguish these two types in terms
of ‘inner’ and ‘outer’ roles, or ‘participants’ and ‘circumstances’. In the Cardiff
Grammar it is proposed that in these two examples, (3) involves three Participants
while (4) involves two Participants plus one Circumstance. Thus, a split can be
detected in the functioning of the role that Halliday terms ‘beneficiary’.
We shall secondly consider his introduction of the roles ‘instrument’ and
‘force’. The role of ‘instrument’ is found in an INTENTIONAL action, as in example
(5):
62
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
63
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
the ‘equative’ Process is reversible, as in (11), and the ‘attributive’ Process is non-
reversible, as in (12):
With regard to the roles inherent in these two Process types, ‘attributive’
Processes have an ‘attribute’ associated with them, and ‘equative’ Processes have an
‘identifier’.
As well as ‘action’ and ‘relational’, the further major Process type he
describes is ‘mental’. In this paper he recognizes semantically defined labels for the
roles involved in ‘mental’ Processes. He proposes first the role of ‘processor’, and
second some entity that is being ‘perceived’, ‘reacted to’, ‘cognized’ or ‘verbalized’,
and for this he continues to use the label ‘phenomenon’ (as in 1968).
In his discussion of ‘mental’ Processes he gives an early indication of the
importance of probability, i.e. the likelihood of occurrence of features. He highlights
the fact that the ‘non-middle’ clauses either have the ‘phenomenon’ or the ‘processor’
as subject, and that the probability of a passive construction is higher with the type
with the ‘phenomenon’ as subject type; compare (13) and (14)
64
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
presented introductions to his ideas. Here I shall take the presentations in Muir
(1972), and Berry (1975 and 1977), as useful interpretations of Halliday’s concept of
TRANSITIVITY. It should be noted that these two presentations are chiefly based on
Halliday’s descriptions prior to this 1970 paper, and therefore do not include the
overtly semantic PR labels.
65
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Muir also emphasises that semantic roles are produced by specific choices in
the network. But he does not attempt the conflation of these roles/functions with the
structure, as Halliday does in 1967 with the use of superscripts. Whereas Halliday
states that elements can be ‘further specified by the addition of superscripts: e.g. Pact
(active predicator), Cint (intensive complement).’ (1967:39), Muir keeps the two types
of structure separate. This follows naturally from the structure of the book, with its
separation of surface grammar and deep grammar. In his section on TRANSITIVTY,
the only statement on structure is the assertion that ‘we can predict structure from
system, but not system from structure’ (1972:115). It will be interesting to see in
further accounts of TRANSITIVITY, whether this is still considered to be the case.
In his account of TRANSITIVITY Muir foregrounds two main premises.
Firstly he highlights the importance of the deep level system as that which defines the
structure, and he states that ‘if we consider structure as the way in which systemic
features are realized we can assign unambiguous descriptions to each clause’
(1972:115). Secondly he emphasizes the importance of the relation between ‘Process’
and ‘Participant’, and how Participants are not ‘tied to any one element in clause
structure’ (1972:106). Ultimately however, as we have seen with Halliday’s elevation
of the notion of system in language, the system is realized in structure.24
24
Huddleston highlights the fact that in systemic grammar realization rules are represented by the
insertion of superscripts into the structural representation; ‘the addition to a function label of a
superscript defining the syntagmatic function more delicately.’ (Huddleston, 1966/81:65).
66
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
invention
action
material supervention
event
perception
relational
67
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
two aspects of the network - simultaneity and complex dependency - display the
complexities of language that the theory can model.
causative
unrestricted active
process
non-causative
passive
typical
middle middle
untypical
middle
restricted
process transitive
non-middle
intransitive
material action
process process
intention
process
event
process
supervention
typical process
animacy
untypical perception
animacy process
internalized reaction
major process process
mental
process
externalized cognition
process process
T (&V)
relational
minor process
Figure 3.5 Berry’s System Network for TRANSITIVITY and VOICE (T & V) (1975:189)
68
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
groupings for Processes, but the only such differences introduced thus far are between
‘action’ and ‘ascription’, or ‘doing’ and ‘being’ (Halliday 1967:39). What can be
seen in Halliday (1970) and thus Berry (1975) is a description of TRANSITIVITY
that is a further step towards a semantically oriented system network, where further
Process types are proposed.
25
At this stage in his theory at least – in later developments of ‘Daughter Dependency’ (1976), his
‘classification rule’ is paramount.
69
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
70
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
be most useful for our present purposes, rather than in order to reflect the suggestions
of Halliday (1967, 1968) and Fillmore (1968) on “transitivity” and voice’ (1971:247).
This claim suggests that he is not concerned with concepts such as ‘Participant
Role’ or ‘Case’26. It is therefore interesting to discover that Hudson does recognize
the roles of ‘actor’, ‘goal’, ‘attribuant’, and what he terms ‘completer’ in the clause,
and how these can be among the functions of the noun-clause, and are conflated with
an element in the structure – i.e. Subject or Complement. The function of the noun-
clause (i.e. the role that it will take) will be determined in the system for the ‘matrix
clause’, and this will have been determined previously in the grammar to his system
for noun-clauses.
In summary, we can determine that although Hudson’s work centres on noun-
clauses in complex sentences, these too involve options from the TRANSITIVITY
system and he therefore takes the position, like Halliday, that the ‘functions’ or ‘roles’
of a clause are an important aspect of its realization. However, he does not relate this
to semantics, and does not recognize the different Process types to which different
configurations of roles belong.
26
He calls these roles ‘transitivity functions’.
71
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
generated can then be conflated with the elements of structure that are generated for
the clause.
attribuant
action affected
plus
agent range VOICE
same
TRANSITIVTY plus referent REFLEXIVIZATION
affected
other
referent VOICE
relational
mental
72
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Although Fawcett 1973/81 does not present full system networks for all the
Process types (as we can see in the reproduction of his system network in Figure 3.6)
he describes the referent-situations of ‘mental’ and ‘relational’ as yielding further
subclassifications, and we might model these systemically as follows:
perception
affective reaction
mental recognition
verbalization
equative
class inclusive
relational locative
associative
M = open Ag Af (Main verb is expounded by ‘open’, and has the inherent roles
Agent and Affected)
and the other being ‘to become open’, which has the realization:
M = open Af (Main verb is expounded by ‘open’, and has the inherent role
Affected).
73
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Fawcett concludes this section of the paper by stating that ‘any adequate
explanation of the lexicon will be a task of such complexity as to make any account of
more than a limited area beyond our reach for some time to come.’ (1973/81:167). At
this point, Fawcett has taken up the challenge of Halliday’s earlier (1961) indication
of generating lexis through a system network. However, it is interesting that the
means he provides for doing this do not involve the merging of grammar and lexis
into the continuum of a single network, but rather as two networks equally dependent
on each other, which are entered simultaneously. He states that in this paper he has
‘tried to show how it is only by taking account of options in all the components
simultaneously that the complex relationships between them can be modelled in a
satisfactory manner’ (1973/81:181). However, what this simultaneity does not
encompass is how to account for the fact that choices made in the TRANSITIVITY
network (i.e. the type of Process / the type of Role to be generated) may
influence/direct the choices to be made in the lexicon27.
In summary, we can say that the focus of what Fawcett is proposing in this
early paper is a cognitive model of language, or ‘a treatment of a grammar in which it
is seen not as an entity on its own, but part of a mentality.’ (1973/81:181). His
network for TRANSITIVITY is SEMANTICALLY based, and has the semantic roles as
features in the system, as well as functions to be mapped on to the structure, and it is
on this that the current Cardiff Grammar framework for TRANSITIVITY is based (as
shall be demonstrated in Chapters 4 and 5).
27
We shall in Chapter 4/5 that provision is made in the current framework of the Cardiff Grammar for
this such problem of generation.
74
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Here Halliday describes the ‘situations’ that occur in social contexts, and these
include the ‘social action’ which he considers to be the ‘field’ aspect of his tripartite
system of register – of ‘field’, ‘mode’ and ‘tenor’. Specifically, Halliday relates the
‘semiotic structure of situation’ (1977:201) of field to the experiential component of
semantics.
In describing a Systemic Functional grammar as a means for producing a text
– which is what he is doing in this paper – Halliday compiles a set of systems that
become ‘a network that extends from the social system, as its upper bound, through
the linguistic system on the one hand and the social context on the other, down to the
‘wording’, which is the text in its lexicogrammatical realization.’ (1977:207). This
notion of ‘realization’ has occurred a number of times in this historical description of
TRANSITIVITY. At this point, in combining realization with the ‘wording’, it is
possible for the first time in Halliday’s work to ascertain exactly how he proposes that
the worded realization might be generated by the system.
75
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
material
mental
personal (Speaker: individual human)
intensive extent
location
circumstantial manner
relational cause
acccompaniment
(Process: ’be with’)
possessive matter
existential
One interesting feature of this network is that it includes a new Process type,
not encountered before – that of ‘verbal’ Processes. Through this subsystem a level
of delicacy is reached that indicates the generation of the lexical verb. Thus, if
[verbal process] is chosen, and this is followed through, making the relevant choices
to reach ‘statement’, the network indicates here that the eventual lexical verb might be
‘say’. And Halliday demonstrates how vocabulary is ‘most delicate grammar’ by
stating that ‘lexical items appear as one form of the realization of systemic options,
typically as the last step in subcategorization.’ (1977:223).
The Process type ‘relational’ is given its fullest treatment so far in this 1977
system, presented in Figure 3.7. Up to this point ‘relational’ Processes have led to the
76
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
77
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
For Fawcett, however, this role would be not a Circumstance but an Inherent Role of
the Process.
Furthermore, Fawcett’s system networks do not generate functions as an
intermediate stage in the structure, as Halliday’s (and also Berry’s (1975)) do.
To illustrate, Berry (1975:77) seems to suggest that the roles come
intermediately, with ‘functions’ (as she recognizes them) coming between networks
of meaning options and form. She shows this diagramatically on a continuum:
Berry makes this explicit by stating that ‘each element of structure has certain
functions associated with it. The element S [subject], for instance, usually has the
function of expressing the actor of an action’ (Berry, 1975:77).
At this point it will be useful to examine a version of his system network for
TRANSITIVITY, as presented in Figure 3.8, to demonstrate exactly how for Fawcett
the roles are features in the system.
78
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
others
relational stative
locational
agent only
affected only
attribuant only
simple
agent-
centred
plus other inherent roles
mental
According to Fawcett (1980:136), Inherent Roles are not directly part of the
grammar, but ‘phenomena that are referred to in the features that are selected for a
referent situation’ (Fawcett 1980:136). However, this was a temporary position and
in his later work PRs have a place in the structure of the clause that is as well
established as concepts such as ‘Subject’. In any case, we can see that the roles are
important for defining the Process type, as his network shows, with the detailed
description of the system for ‘action’ Processes being defined by the roles chosen in
the network. The question of whether the roles are at the level of grammar or of
meaning seems to arise from Halliday’s addition of a ‘socio-semantic’ level, with the
concepts of ‘subject’ and ‘agent’ being considered at the same level as each other, i.e.
the lower level of the lexicogrammar.
Fawcett discusses what the network should be concerned with, and his
conclusions are as follows:
79
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Fawcett’s analysis classes the ball as an ‘affected’, and not ‘goal’, as he points
out that the use of the term ‘goal’ implies the involvement of an ‘actor’. This
suggests, if we consider once again Halliday’s 1968 descriptions of ergativity (see
Section 3.1.6), that the Fawcettian approach to TRANSITIVITY is essentially
ergative, in its recognition of ‘affected’ entities as being evident in most Process
types.
With regard to Fawcett’s treatment of the Process types, at this point he still
uses Halliday’s original term ‘action’, whereas Halliday has by this time changed to
using the term ‘material’. Fawcett’s reason for this is that not all ‘actions’ are
‘material’, and therefore it is not a useful term. The verbs marry and attack are given
as examples of ‘non-material actions’. We shall return to this point in Chapter 5, in
the presentation of Fawcett (in forthcoming a).
Fawcett’s classifies ‘mental’ Processes into the following sub-categories;
‘emotion’ (Halliday’s 1968 ‘reaction’); ‘perception’; ‘cognition’; and
80
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
81
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
82
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
different meanings. He states that the present continuous, or ‘the “present in present”
is more focused in time; hence it occurs with Processes that have clear beginnings and
endings, as is typical with ‘material’ Processes. ‘Mental’ Processes, which are in
general not clearly bounded in time, are associated with the less focused tense form,
the simple tense.’ (IFG:116). Thirdly, and importantly from the viewpoint of
semantics, he distinguishes ‘mental’ Processes as they are not Processes of ‘doing’.
Therefore, the roles of ‘actor’ and ‘goal’ are abandoned in favour of the functions
‘senser’ – the Participant who ‘senses’, and ‘phenomenon’ – the Participant that is
‘sensed’.
Finally, he recognizes further major semantic subclassifications of ‘mental’
Processes, i.e. features rather than names of networks, e.g. ‘perception’, ‘affection’
and ‘cognition’.
Halliday’s presentation of ‘relational’ Processes is somewhat changed in this
description. One approach to them was presented in a system network in Halliday
1977 (Section 3.12 above), but in IFG there is a different and more detailed account.
Perhaps the most useful explanation of the functioning of Processes of the ‘relational’
type can be deduced from the following table that Halliday presents:
Table 3.2 ‘The principal types of relational process’ (from Halliday, 1994:113)
As this table shows, three main types of ‘relational’ Process are identified:
1.‘intensive’ (‘x is a’), 2. ‘circumstantial’ (‘x’ is at a’), and 3. ‘possessive’ (‘x has a’).
Furthermore, each type is cross-classified with two other types of meaning; either
‘attributive’ or ‘identifying’.
83
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Figure 3.10 The potential system network for Relational Processes as they are
presented in Halliday (1994:121 – 138)
84
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Process types seem to be Processes of ‘action’, and furthermore, ‘action’ that typically
occurs with one Participant only, as we shall see in Chapter 5.
The ‘verbal’ Process type relates to ‘any kind of symbolic exchange of
meaning’ (1985:129), as in example (1):
Interestingly, Halliday indicates that this Process does not require a conscious
Participant. It is possible to have a Participant as ‘a watch’, as in example (2).
In the case of this Process type, the Subject there ‘has no representational
function’. Typically, this Process type will take the verb ‘be’, and in this way is
similar to a ‘relational’ Process type. Also, this type of clause will often contain a
distinct Circumstantial element. In the Cardiff Grammar, Halliday’s ‘existential’
28
See Chapter 4 for a more detailed account of the notion of probability in SFL.
29
The concept of, and treatment of, metaphorical TRANSITIVITY is discussed in Chapter 7.
85
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
86
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
The notion central to Halliday’s IFG description is one of ‘cause and effect’.
This is described in terms of the notion that every process has one Participant, and
that this Participant is the ‘Medium’ through which the ‘Process’ is actualized – that
is, in all processes except those in the ‘mediopassive voice’, as in example (5).
Halliday suggests that the Process and the Medium ‘together form the nucleus
of an English clause; and this nucleus then determines the range of options that are
available to the rest of the clause’ (1994:164).
The Medium functions as a PR in the clause, and it functions differently
according to the Process:
Thus, he states that ‘the Medium is the nodal Participant throughout: not the
doer, or the causer, but the one that is critically involved’ (1985:147).
In conclusion, Halliday’s discussion of TRANSITIVITY in IFG consolidates
the views explored in earlier discussions on TRANSITIVITY. It concludes the
developments of Halliday’s influential work on TRANSITIVITY in the framework of
SFL over nearly quarter of a century. Next we shall consider the related work by
other scholars who use what has been described up to this point as its basis.
87
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
3.2.1 Berry’s (1977) and Fawcett’s (1980) early networks for ‘lexical delicacy’
Whilst the works that I have examined in this chapter have been concerned
with the initial systems for TRANSITIVITY, the papers that I will consider next
attempt to extend the system networks towards a deeper level of delicacy. In Section
3.1.3 we considered Halliday’s proposals for a single system for language generation,
and a unified ‘lexicogrammar’. According to Tucker (1998:14), Berry’s (1977)
introductory SFL text, ‘constitutes the first published attempt to implement Halliday’s
“most delicate grammar” approach to lexis by setting out a system network which
represents the meaning distinctions which are carried by individual lexical items’.
The system network that Berry proposes is for the choice of ‘animal type’,
and, as Tucker (1998) points out, what is interesting about her network is that, rather
than being a means for modelling lexis as part of the lexicogrammar, it is more a
system for the subcategorization of a phenomenon through semantic relations. The
initial systems are parallel systems, with the system of ANIMALHOOD being choice
between [human] and [non-human], and the system of MATURITY being choice
between [adult] and [youthful]. Thus these choices are based on a semantic ‘polarity’
– i.e. they are antonyms. The more delicate choices are hyponymic, where choices of
[non-human] plus [tame] plus [domestic] lead to a ‘type of’ relation, with choice
between [feline], [canine], etc.
While Berry’s network demonstrates how it is possible to reach lexis through
delicate choices in a network – in this case, by semantic subclassification – what it
88
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
does not demonstrate is how grammar and lexis can come together in a unified
network. Tucker highlights the fact that Berry, at this stage, is not necessarily striving
towards this goal, and that, moreover, she sees the two as being related on a cline,
rather than with lexis being ‘subsumed under grammar’ (Berry, cited in Tucker,
1998:15).
unmarked change
of position ……………………. M < move
change of physical
CULTURAL state …..
30 CLASSIFICATION
I will give a full description of ‘realization rules’ and ‘filling’ in Chapter 4.
OF PROCESSES
change by cooking …..
Figure 3.11 Adapted from Fawcett’s ‘A very tentative and partial system network and
realisation rules for the cultural classification of ‘affected-centred’ processes in
English’ (1980:153).
90
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
‘(1) Language consists of three strata: semantics, lexicogrammar and phonology. (2)
These strata are related by ‘realization’: meanings are coded as wordings, wordings
are coded as sound patterns. (3) Each stratum is describable as a network of options;
the description is, therefore, paradigmatic, with environments for options also being
defined paradigmatically’ (Hasan, 1987:184).
Hasan (1987) takes this view of language as her basis, and so we can detect
from early on in the paper that semantic delicacy is paramount to her ‘lexis as most
delicate grammar’.
Hasan is concerned with a particular small group of semantically related
lexical verbs, which are subsumed under the heading ‘disposal’. She recognizes these
Processes to be subcategories of the Process type ‘action’. The notion of
subcategorisation is key to her technique for the extension of a network, with the
‘subcategorisation’ of features enabling more and more delicate semantic distinctions
to be made. Hasan states that her sub-categorisation ‘should not be confused with
Chomsky’s selection restriction rules (1965)’ (Hasan, 1987:188). However, she does
not define clearly what her sub-categorisation involves. In my view her sense of
subcategorisation is like this: ‘subcategorise by choosing these features from a
network of possibilities’ (see below for examples).
In producing delicate networks, Hasan’s starting point in the system for
TRANSITIVITY is [action] Process types31, through which she pursues a path to
reach [disposal] Processes, which then leads to the choice of [acquisition] or
[deprivation], and it is the extension of these two networks with which she is
concerned. This is presented in Figure 3.12.
31
It is interesting that her starting point is [action], rather than [material] which would be in keeping
with the Hallidayan TRANSITIVITY system. Having [action] as the entry condition for the network is
the same as it is in the Cardiff Grammar system for TRANSITIVITY.
91
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
acquisition
disposal
92
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Typically, for instance, in example (2), a lot of leaves are easily recognisable as the
‘medium’:
However, Hasan proposes that in the case of (1), the objectified role of her
thoughts is not the ‘medium’, but ‘collect + … thoughts must be seen as a unit’
(1987:188). It seems to me that in creating a delicate system network, this example of
‘collecting’ is still a Process of ‘acquisition’, and in fact the section of the network in
which choices are made for Participant Role type should provide for occurrences such
as her thoughts occurring in the clause.32
Her system for Processes of ‘acquisition’ leads to the subcategories of not just
‘collect’, but also ‘gather’ and ‘accumulate’ and the eventual realization of them as
lexical items. We shall now consider more of the system to discover how Hasan
defines the delicate properties of these items to enable the modelling of their semantic
differences as a system of choices between each.
32
In the Cardiff Grammar this would be the case. See Chapters 4 and 5 below for how the network for
TRANSITIVITY handles the areas of PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES.
93
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
acquisition unitary
+vast
neutral
ACCESS unmarked
random
deprivation planned
disposal
ACT transform
iterative
CHARACTER
non-iterative
action
non-reflexive
beneficile inherent
+benefactive
BENEFACTION potential reflexive
-benefactive
+liquid
non-beneficile independent
+solid
discrete
cooperative
fused
Hasan’s description of this system begins with how [disposal] Processes can
be generated. She introduces the use of three further systems which must be
considered for further specification of a lexical item to be reached through the
[disposal] network. These systems are [access], [character] and [benefaction].
The systems [access] and [character] enable further semantic specification of
the Processes involved in [disposal], and are also dependent on the choice [disposal]
being made. ‘Access’ is concerned with whether the ‘activity’, or Process, leads to
the [acquisition] or [deprivation] of the Medium. ‘Character’ is concerned with ‘the
nature of the activity’, and leads to the choice of the activity being either ‘iterative’ or
‘non-iterative’ – that is, a repetitive activity or a non-repetitive activity.
‘Benefaction’ is a larger system to be considered, and this classification comes
from the Hallidayan TRANSITIVITY classification, in which the Circumstantial Role
of ‘Beneficiary’ is recognized. As can be seen in Figure 3.13, the immediate choice
94
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
It is at this point in the network that specifications are made for the ‘most
delicate’ semantic distinctions for the three Processes.
The network then goes on to enable very fine distinctions to be recognised
between the three verbs, which are very closely semantically related. On choosing
[iterative], the next choice to be made in the network, as shown in Figure 3.13, is
between [unitary] and [neutral]. The choice of [unitary] will lead to the generation of
a ‘Medium (that) is constrained to be “plural”. In English the only linguistic form that
can meet all these requirements is gather’ (Hasan 1987:194). And the choice of
‘neutral’ will lead to a further system of choice between [+ vast] which will
95
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
semantically define and therefore generate the item ‘accumulate’, and [unmarked]
which will generate the item ‘collect’.
Through this section of the system it is possible to determine exactly how a
lexical item’s semantic properties can be framed in and generated through a
grammatical system.
Hasan 1987 also describes ‘the lexicogrammar for deprivation’, which is also
contained within the network shown at Figure 3.8 and which generates the lexical
verbs ‘scatter’, ‘divide’, ‘distribute’, ‘strew’, ‘spill’ and ‘share’. However, her
description will not be discussed here. The most important concept proposed in this
paper is that ‘it upholds the systemic functional view of an uninterrupted continuity
between grammar and lexis’ (Hasan 1987:208), and also it indicates how the concept
may be actualized. By modelling semantic differences between lexical items in this
manner one is ultimately able to model an important aspect of the semantics.
However, I should also indicate that Hasan mentions that one of the
limitations of her work is that her network ‘is simply the output of one meta-function
– the experiential’ (1987:207), and she states that the choice between examples of
synonymously related items such as ‘ask’ / ‘enquire’, and ‘buy’ / ‘purchase’ might be
generated through the interpersonal system. I would like to point out that this too can
be accounted for in the network through the assignment of rules that specify
preferences in the light of ‘register’ features, and which consequently alter
probability. This notion will be discussed in later chapters, and indeed its
implementation in certain of the networks will be illustrated.
96
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
In his 1998 publication, Tucker suggests that other work conducted on areas of
lexis in a SF framework have only ever been concerned with ‘modelling fragments of
lexicogrammar’ (1998:34). What he is proposing however is that, working within the
Cardiff Grammar, it is possible to realize lexis and structure in a ‘single unified
system network’, providing for not only the adjectival items themselves, but also the
structures that occur around them.
I will say no more about Tucker’s provision for this area of the
lexicogrammar, which is separate to the concern of this thesis, other than to state that
his proposals enable language generation through a system that does not require a
separate lexicon component, but is in fact the realization of ‘lexis as most delicate
grammar’.
33
For her treatment of Relational and Mental processes see Davidse 1991.
97
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
types and new Participant Roles. She investigates a distinction in the area of
‘material’ Processes between transitive and ergative by exploring how the Participants
are configured and she is concerned with ‘the multivariate relations that exist between
the elements of structure “process” and “participant”’ (1991:7).
Further to this, she states that she is working ‘at the more delicate
“cryptotypical” level’ (1991:20), and so is concerned with the covert grammatical
categories that ‘Process type’ might be seen to classify.
The term ‘cryptotype’ was coined by Whorf (1945/56) to describe the
grammatical categories that are found in language that are what he calls ‘covert’
categories’. Whorf distinguishes between ‘overt’ and ‘covert’ categories, with the
‘overt’ categories in language being those that ‘have a formal mark which is present
in every sentence containing a member of that category’ (1945/56:87). An example
of such a category is the marking of plurality of many of the nouns in English with the
suffix ‘s’. In contrast, a covert category, or a cryptotype, does not display an overt
marker in every occurrence of the cryptotype. The example that Whorf gives of a
covert category is that of gender assignment in the English language. He states that
‘each common noun and personal given name belongs to a certain gender class, but a
characteristic overt mark appears only when there is occasion to refer to the noun by a
personal pronoun in the singular number’ (1945/56:90). To illustrate he states that a
person without cultural knowledge of Western European use of English would not be
able to know:
‘that the names of biological classes themselves are ‘it’; that smaller animals are
usually ‘it’; larger animals often ‘he’; dogs, eagles and turkeys usually ‘he’; cats and
wrens usually ‘she’; body parts and the whole botanical world ‘it’; countries and
states as fictive persons ‘she’ … etc’ (1945/56:90).
Further, Whorf describes that the ‘covert’ markers which identify these
cryptotypes are ‘reactances’. And so, the ‘reactance’ for the cryptotype of English
gender is the use of personal pronoun.
Davidse recognizes the occurrence of these covert categories in language. She
sees Halliday’s TRANSITIVITY categories as cryptotypical (1991:15), and she
asserts that the means for identifying TRANSITIVITY is through ‘reactances’, which
make it possible for one to reach the covert categories such as Halliday’s that are
98
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
‘there’ in the grammar. The reactances she recognizes for reaching TRANSITIVITY
categories are the configurations of Participant Roles.
This use of Process and Participant configuration at some cryptotypical level
of the grammar is similar to the way in which Fawcett (1980) models different
‘material’ Process types as ‘agent-centred’ Processes and ‘affected-centred’
Processes. As we shall see, this concept is very relevant to Davidse’s argument.
Essentially, this distinction enables the modelling of causation and non-causation of
the Process, and for Davidse this idea of ‘centre-ing’ is pivotal in her description. The
main thrust of her description is that the ‘transitive’ is ‘Actor-centred’ and the
‘ergative’ is ‘Medium-centred’ (1991:27), and both can be either Middle (one
Participant) or Effective (two Participants). The similarities of Davidse’s work to
Fawcett’s earlier framework for TRANSITIVITY in Fawcett (1980) are consequently
very relevant to my research. These similarities are striking and will be further
discussed in Chapter 5, where I shall make the case for abandoning Fawcett’s
distinction between ‘agent-centred’ and ‘affected-centred’ Processes.
The present discussion of Davidse’s work will firstly detail the phenomena
that she is describing. She presents the differences between the ‘transitive’ and the
‘ergative’ in terms of how they function in the grammar. In this description I will
consider how she distinguishes transitivity and ergativity in terms of their functioning
as ‘effective’ constructions and as ‘middle’ constructions, and also as ‘pseudo-
effective’ constructions. ‘Pseudo-effective’ are a new type of structure that she
recognizes, and we will determine below what construction type she uses this term to
describe.
In line with Halliday 1968, Davidse recognizes the ‘transitive’ to be a
construction involving a ‘deed’ and a possible ‘extension’. The transitive is realized
by an Actor who performs the deed, and a possible Goal to whom the deed might be
extended. The ergative construction, she states, realizes the ‘Instigation of Process’
(1992:109), with the role of ‘Medium’ being crucially involved, but with the
potentiality for a further role that she classes as ‘Instigator’: the Participant that might
INSTIGATE the process. Figure 3.14 presents her system network for the transitive and
the ergative, and we shall see in the description below how she considers these
features to be realized.
99
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
100
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Interestingly, however, these three construction types are not features in her
‘material’ Process system network, as shown in Figure 3.10.
Davidse describes a reactance which provides evidence for the
transitive/ergative split through the notion of transitive intentionality. This reactance
concerns the ‘absolute construction’ of intentional transitive clauses, for example the
absolute construction of (4) and (4a).
34
Although in the Cardiff Grammar ‘absolute construction’ will not be used to describe such a
construction.
101
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
(9) the fact that boiling water was poured into it broke the glass
(10) *the fact that he aimed hit the target.
What Davidse’s descriptions show is that the transitive construction is, ‘actor-
centred’ with transitive examples such as (4) involving the ‘actor-centred’ Process of
hitting. As stated above however, Davidse recognizes that the ergative construction is
‘medium-centred’, and thus examples such as (7a) (as an attempt at providing a single
Participant alternation of (7) in the same way as (4a)) cannot be related to an event
where John is the Medium, because we try to interpret John as Actor.
(7a)*John broke.
102
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
(1) He died.
(2) The door is opening.
(2a) Who’s opening it?
The current version of the Cardiff Grammar does not recognize this split
between an intransitive which cannot have an Agent ADDED to it and a non-ergative
which can. The position taken in the generation of these types in the CG
TRANSITIVITY system is that there are different verb senses – either one or two role
– and it is not necessary to generalise between the structures.
103
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Davidse indicates that in her model these are not transitive, but are
intransitives with the Range functioning as a Circumstance. However, she also notes
that the Range can become the Subject in a passive construction, and that this would
typically be, certainly for Halliday (in IFG), the test for defining a role as a Participant
in the Process rather than a Circumstance. Therefore she recognizes, like Halliday,
that Ranges are not ‘true participants’, but are somewhere between Participant and
Circumstance, and so she concludes that it is a ‘pseudo participant’ (1992:125).
The case of ergative pseudo-effective structures are more troublesome than the
transitive type. She gives (2a), (2b) and (2c) as examples:
She states that the second role in each of these examples cannot be passivized,
nor does it pass her ‘do to’ test (*an arm was fractured by him in the accident; *what
he did to an arm was fracture it in the accident). This ‘do to’ test is a reactance that
indicates that the second role is ‘affected’ by the Process, and is thus an ergative-
effective structure. Therefore, failure in this test indicates that the two Participant
clauses she suggests are not ‘effective’, and so must be categorised as ‘pseudo-
effective’.
Interestingly, it is a test similar to this that Fawcett (in preparation a) uses to
detect the roles of Agent and Affected in the clause. To detect whether a role is an
Agent, Fawcett applies to the Process the statement ‘what x did was to …’, and to
detect whether a role is an Affected entity in the clause, the statement ‘what happened
to y was that x …ed him/her’. Fawcett’s test is different to Davidse’s, however,
because the ‘do to’ test only identifies Affected entities in two role Processes. His
preferred tests allow for testing one role Processes as well.
Davidse follows Langacker’s classification and calls the first role in her
pseudo-effective constructions a ‘Setting’, which, like the Range, she recognizes as a
104
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
Having considered the three main construction types we can say that
Davidse’s 1992 paper takes the well-established distinction between transitives and
ergatives, and examines certain fine distinctions within each. In the ‘transitive’ type
the Goal is an ‘inert affected’ and ‘the process is being done to it, but the Goal itself
does not ‘do’ the process’ (1992:118). In the ‘ergative’ type the Process HAPPENS to
the Medium. Through the investigation of the cryptotypes at work in the system of
TRANSITIVITY, she suggests a number of distinctions within each of the transitive
and the ergative in order to produce a full picture of TRANSITIVITY. She develops
a system network of TRANSITIVITY, (in this description, the area of ‘material’
Processes) that incorporates these important distinctions. And, perhaps most
importantly for the present work, she identifies certain TRANSITIVITY types that are
not found in Halliday’s network and which need to be recognized as stated here. In
doing this, she extends the system of TRANSITIVITY that Halliday’s work (1967,
1968, 1985/94) proposes by allowing for the transitive and ergative distinctions to be
105
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
106
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
transitivity, since it is this area that covers the Processes and Participants in the clause,
and since it is with these aspects that my research is concerned. His description of
‘circumstantial transitivity’ – which is concerned with the circumstantial roles in the
clause – is outside of the scope of the research to be presented here.35
Matthiessen suggests that the ‘instantiation’ of the experiential in the clause
has a predictable structure, with the elements of Process, Participants, and any
Circumstances being typically realized through the units of verbal group, nominal
group and prepositional group respectively. In order to illustrate how Participants and
Circumstances can be recognized in this way, he offers Examples (1a) and (1b):
I suggest that defining the Circumstance in this way is not useful to the
analysis. In these examples it is not the status of the duckling that changes. An
‘ergative’ transitivity analysis recognizes that the duckling is the ‘Affected entity’ in
both examples – and so a Participant in both examples. What I propose is that the two
examples involve two different Processes, with the lexical simple verb shoot having a
partly different meaning from the lexical multi-word verb shoot at. In his description
of participation in and circumstantiation of the Process, Matthiessen suggests
(following Halliday 1985/4) that ‘the difference between Participants and
Circumstances is a cline (scale), reflecting the degree of involvement in the process’
(1995:196). However, in the Examples (1a) and (1b), the involvement of the entity
duckling in the Process does not alter, and so does not ‘move’ on the cline of
participation/circumstantiation. In fact, what alters is the type of Process taking place,
with a relatively un-subtle semantic difference between the action of shooting and the
action of shooting at.
Matthiessen, like Davidse, does recognize the importance of the differences
between Process types, and the sometimes very fine distinctions between semantic
categories. He states that, as with all categorization, ‘there are prototypical cases with
all four process types, but there are also more intermediate, borderline cases’
35
See Ball, (forthcoming), and Ball and Tucker (2000) for a description of the Cardiff Grammar
approach to circumstantial transitivity.
107
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
(1995:204). These are peripheral senses which, if set on a cline as he suggests, might
arguably fall into either of two categories. This notion of fuzzy edged categories is
exemplified diagrammatically in the well recognized representation given on the
cover of the second edition of Halliday’s IFG (1994). It is therefore interesting that
Matthiessen does not recognize in his description of ‘nuclear transitivity’ Halliday’s
‘behavioural’ and ‘existential’ ‘SECONDARY’ Process types as fuzzy edged categories.
Rather, Matthiessen recognizes these Process types as ‘subcategories’ in the network.
In classifying Process type, Matthiessen states that what determines the
position of a Process in a ‘space’ of experience ‘include[s] the degree of ‘potency’ of
the participants (animacy, volitionality, etc), the degree of their affectedness (state
change), the degree to which the process can project another process, and so on’
(1995:221). This indicates that in his view a Process should be classified not
according to its ‘stand-alone’ meaning, but is largely measurable according to the
Participants associated with it. Therefore, TRANSITIVITY is discernible from the
configuration of Process and Participants. And so the framework for
TRANSITIVITY that Matthiessen describes includes simultaneous systems of
PROCESS TYPE (and so choice between ‘material’, ‘mental’, ‘verbal’ and
‘relational’ Processes) and AGENCY, which enables choice of Participant Role
configuration, in terms of whether the clause is to be ‘effective’ or ‘middle’.
The most useful approach for the present description of Matthiessen 1995 is to
present his system networks for each Process type, and to then consider his modelling
of the associated lexical verbs.
We shall initially consider how his description splits grammar and lexis. He
provides a full network for the grammatical end of each Process type system, i.e. the
least delicate section of the system network. He also acknowledges the ‘lexis as most
delicate grammar’ approach, and presents a table indicating the range of subtypes that
can be produced at a level of further delicacy than the four major Process types, using
the sub-categorization from Roget’s Thesaurus as his basis (1995:217).
In the light of his acceptance of ‘lexis as most delicate grammar’, it is
interesting that he deals with the lexis for each Process type as a section that is
separate from the grammar. Throughout the description he uses a ‘sub-categorisation’
method for producing further delicacy to the point of lexical output that is similar to
that of Hasan 1987 (Section 3.2.1). However, he does not provide the delicate system
network that will generate the lexical item. He presents his subcategories in a ‘table’
108
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
format which stops one step short of modelling them in a network. It is not clear why
he does not take this additional step.
In this description I will show how his table format can be reorganised as a
system network. I will initially consider each Process type in turn, and I will then
present his description of ‘material’ lexis as a system.36
His description of ‘effective’ clauses is that they involve a Goal and ‘represent
doings’ (1995:240). He states that ‘from the ergative point of view, an Agent that
brings about actualization of the Process is actualized through the Medium; and from
36
For reasons of space, only Material lexis can be considered in this description.
109
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
the transitive point of view, the Actor’s involvement in the Process extends to impact
on another Participant, the Goal.’ (1995:240). Therefore, all effective material
clauses are ‘Actor/Agent + Process + Goal/Medium’. It is interesting that he uses
this double labelling, and, as we shall see in Chapter 5, the Cardiff Grammar does not
use these two sets of labels, but uses the essentially ergative terms ‘Agent’ and
‘Affected’ as features in the system network for generating PROCESS TYPES.
The choice of ‘effective material process’ leads to a system that chooses the
‘type of doing’, and the choice is between ‘dispositive’ Process and ‘creative’
Process. A ‘dispositive’ Process involves an already existing Goal as in Example (5),
whereas a creative process brings the Goal into existence, as in Example (6).
We will see in Section 3.2.3.5 below how he extends the system for ‘creative’
Processes towards lexical delicacy.
110
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
‘verbal-like’ Processes, and some ‘activity-like’ Processes, for example ‘chatting’ and
‘fighting’.
Chapter 5 will show how in the Cardiff Grammar these Processes are either
‘mental’ Processes (‘feeling’ and ‘pondering’), or ‘one role’, ‘action’ Processes
(‘chatting’ and ‘fighting’). Although the Cardiff Grammar recognizes that fuzzy
edges occur in categories, this is not modelled through the introduction of new
Process types, but in a similar manner to Matthiessen, as subcategories in the system
network.
111
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
(1a) I understand.
(1b) I grieve.
These examples would be dealt with in the Cardiff Grammar as being ‘mental’
Processes involving two roles, with one being COVERT. Matthiessen makes no
mention at this point of the ‘overtness’ or ‘covertness’ of roles, and it is only if
‘phenomenalization’ is chosen that a Participant of the type ‘phenomenon’ will be
included in the clause, as in examples (2) and (3):
112
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
37
However, see the description of the Cardiff Grammar in Chapter 5 for Fawcett’s (forthcoming a)
treatment of ‘desiderative’ Processes.
113
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
114
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
115
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
ascriptive identifying
intensive non-phase: unmarked be be
non-phase: other make, constitute, form, symbolize, represent, mean,
produce, signify,
weigh, cost, measure, define, constitute, realize,
number… translate,
exceed, outnumber, spell, imply, sum up,
matter, hurt, suffice, add up to …
abound, figure, differ…
phase: reality seem, appear, sound, look, seem
taste, smell
phase: time become, go, turn, grow, become
remain, keep, stay
remain, stay
assigned: expanding make, render, turn, keep appoint, choose, elect,
ensure, verify,
brand, call brand, call, name, christen
assigned: projecting declare, decree, proclaim, declare, decree, proclaim
assume, believe, think assume, believe, think
intend, want, wish
possessive non-benefactive have, lack, need, deserve, possess, own
feature, boast
contain, house, include
benefactive provide, afford, owe
other extending combine, join accompany, combine with,
contrast with, distinguish
replace, substitute for
circumstantial spatio-temporal last, take, date (from), cross, circle, surround,
range (from/to) span, dominate, support,
face, parallel, take up
manner: comparative become (‘suit’), suit resemble, match, fit,
exceed, outnumber
matter concern, deal with, treat, cover, touch upon, discuss,
go into review, summarize
cause depend on, hinge on bring about, cause,
produce
condition
conflict with, contradict,
preclude, prevent
116
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
material ->
creative/
dispositive.
creative ->
event (occurrence)/
thing.
general ->
perform (0.001)/
do (0.002)/
make (0.003)/
take (0.004).
expansion ->
phase_time/
conation/
causation.
phase_time ->
begin (0.005)/
start (0.006)/
open (0.007)/
continue (0.008)/
stop (0.009)/
discontinue (0.010).
conation ->
fail (0.011)/
succeed (0.012)/
attempt (0.013)/
try (0.014).
causation ->
cause (0.015)/
bring about (0.016).
117
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
thing ->
neutral/
build/
symbol/
culinary/
cloth/
hole/
other.
neutral ->
create (0.017)/
make (0.018)/
prepare (0.019)
build ->
assemble (0.020)/
build (0.021)/
construct (0.022).
symbol ->
design (0.023)/
draft (0.024)/
draw (0.025)/
forge (0.026)/
paint (0.027)/
sketch (0.028)/
write (0.029).
culinary ->
bake (0.030)/
brew (0.031)/
cook (0.032)/
mix (0.033).
cloth ->
knit (0.034)/
sew (0.035)/
weave (0.036).
hole ->
dig (0.037)/
drill (0.038).
other->
establish (0.039)/
open (0.040).
118
Chapter 3: Approaches to TRANSITIVITY in SFG
119
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
38
COMMUNAL stands for Convivial Man-Machine Understanding through Natural Language.
120
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
networks, COMMUNAL also includes richer and more powerful realization rules,
which function to specify the actual lexicogrammatical output. Halliday states that
the computer implementation of CG and the NIGEL grammar are ‘among the largest
grammars existing anywhere in computational form’ (1994:xii).
This chapter has two main sections. The CG is a grammar that aims to provide
for both the generation and the analysis of language. The concern of those developing
CG has been the provision of a descriptive AND a generative grammar, and thus the
modelling of the generation and the understanding of language. And so the present
chapter will reflect this division of labour. The first section will be concerned with a
description of the generative component of the COMMUNAL project and the
important features of the grammar that make it so useful for language generation.
And the second section will describe how the grammar can be used as a tool for the
text analyst – that is, the descriptive use of CG. While the two are developed
together, the text description work on an area of the grammar is typically a little ahead
of the full computer implementation.
Of particular interest to the present research is the CG development of
semantic system networks, or, more specifically, the modelling of the meaning
potential of language to the point of ‘lexis as most delicate grammar’ (Halliday,
1961), which was described in Chapter 3. This is one of several significant advances
that CG offers over other SF grammars – i.e. the system networks are both semantic,
and they model ‘lexis as most delicate grammar’39. The NIGEL grammar is described
in Tucker 1996 as having been unable to implement this, and so as having had to use a
separate lexicon, and as far as I am aware this situation has not changed in the
intervening years. Halliday states that ‘there is no clear line between semantics and
grammar, and a functional grammar is one that is pushed in the direction of the
semantics’ (1994: xix), and the goal of the CG (and so COMMUNAL) is to push the
grammar all the way to the semantics.
39
Although, as we saw in Section 3.2 of Chapter3, other system networks have been produced which
display ‘lexis as most delicate grammar’ (eg. Hasan 1987), CG is the only grammar at the present time
to be actively working towards the semanticization of the whole grammar.
121
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
potential instance
122
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
This recognition of ‘potential’ and ‘instance’, and the ‘meaning’ and ‘form’ of
language is a fundamental aspect of Systemic Functional Grammar, and is in contrast
with Chomsky’s notion of ‘competence’ and ‘performance’. By viewing language
from an alternative position to Chomsky’s – that of Halliday’s meaning ‘potential’
and its instantiation – one is able to build a model in which all instances of language
can be described and classified. Thus, rather than concentrating on the syntactic rules
of language ‘competence’, SF linguists assert that instead ‘in these terms it is the role
of the system networks to specify what a user of a language ‘can mean’.’ (Fawcett
1980:55).
Halliday summarises this notion by stating that:
‘The system network is a theory about language as a resource for making meaning.
Each system in the network represents a choice: not a conscious decision made in real
time but a set of possible alternatives, like ‘statement/question’ or ‘singular/plural’.”
(Halliday, 1994:xxvi).
A full pass through a network gives a set of options that have been chosen on
that pass, and these form a ‘selection expression’. This is a bundle of features that
will then be taken to the point of realisation. However, the selection expressions
alone cannot be the specification of the grammar. As Tucker (1996) points out, the
important thing about a SF approach is that the contrasts are shown in a network, i.e.
what is not chosen is also available for the user to contrast with what IS chosen.
The present research models verbs, using the CG approach to develop a
semantic classification using system networks. As we have seen in Chapter 2, other
large examinations of verbs include the treatment in WordNet (Fellbaum 1998) and
Levin’s classification of their alternations they take (Levin, 1993). These are not
concerned with verb senses as choices in the context of related choices in the
lexicogrammar, but with verbal entries in a lexicon. Rather than ‘dividing up the
lexicon into semantic fields’ (Fellbaum, 1988:69), my research presents the separate
semantic fields as being reached by a process of choice between alternatives.40
It is useful to acknowledge not only the network’s ability to present
contrastive choice, but also the presentation of ‘dependency’. In Fawcett’s words
40
This notion will illustrated be fully defined in Chapter 8.
123
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
(1980:19) ‘a system makes available a choice between two or more features, but the
availability of such a choice is always dependent on the selection of a logically prior
feature’. By recognising the important function that the network serves in offering
contrasts between features in the network, we can also recognise that the network can
be interpreted from two directions – the direction of choice and language generation,
and the direction of dependency and so of language understanding.
Any network begins with an ‘entry condition’, and it is this feature on which
the subsequent features depend. The entry condition, in fact, defines the network –
the choices to be made are made to express meaning in the general area that is
identified by the entry condition. Thus, in the CG network for ‘Process type’ in
Figure 4.2 below, the entry condition ‘relational’ defines the fact that in this network
we will be choosing a ‘relational’ Process type. And the choice of [possessive],
[attributive], [locational] or [directional] can only be made BECAUSE we are in a
network for ‘relational’ Processes, and so these choices are dependent on the entry
condition.
attributive
locational
relational
directional
possessive
Figure 4.2: The early portion of the system for ‘relational’ Processes.
Thus we use the entry condition to enter a network, and we then make choices
within the network guided by higher planning decisions and our beliefs about the
world, and ultimately we produce a ‘selection expression’ of features.
124
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
been chosen in the networks come to be expressed as specific items, structures, and
intonation or punctuation’. Chapter 8, Section 8.3.1 will present an example of a full,
working realization rule.
In order to produce the appropriate output, the realization rule uses certain
‘realization operators’ (Fawcett et al 1993:131), and these operators are of two types:
Operators (i) to (iv) below build structures and produce descriptions, and Operators
(v) to (vii) below predetermine, either absolutely or probabilistically, the features to
be chosen on subsequent journeys through the network, i.e. the generation of further
features for another unit in the developing tree structure. The most detailed
description of how these realization operators function is Fawcett et al (1993), and it
is on this that the following description is based.
i. Unit insertion
This operation adds a unit to the structure. For example, the feature
[situation] is realized by inserting ‘Cl’ (for clause). The important factor that must be
incorporated into the sequence in which the operations apply is that an element of a
unit can only be added to the structure when the unit it will depend on has been
inserted.
ii. Componence
This operation puts the elements of structure in the right ‘Places’. The
concept of ‘Place’ is an important one in GENESYS, and numbered places are
assigned within a unit. In the largest version of the grammar for example, the unit
‘clause’ has 251 assigned places, and the ‘componence rule’ might place the element
of structure ‘Subject’ at ‘Place 35’. This rule would be represented as ‘S @ 35’. It is
interesting to note that the Subject is in fact the most stable element of the clause, and
will always occur at Place 35 and a Participant Role will still be conflated with it (for
‘conflation’ see below). Even if the Subject that has been generated is to be realized
covertly, it will still be assigned Place 35, and a Participant Role will still be conflated
with it. As we shall see in the second half of this chapter, the relationship between the
unit and the composite elements of which it is made up is componence.
iii. Conflation
This operation conflates two elements. In particular, it conflates a Participant
Role with an element of structure. Thus, having placed the element [Subject], the
realization rule will conflate the appropriate Participant Role with it – for example,
very often the role Agent will be conflated with the Subject. Another example of
125
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
conflation occurs when the Main Verb and the Operator are realized by the same
lexical item: in example (1) the Main Verb and the Operator are both realised by the
item is, and so must be conflated for the structural output.
iv. Exponence
This operation indicates when an element should be expounded by an item. In
example (1) above, the element Main Verb was expounded by the item is. Fawcett et
al (1993:132) state that ‘it is the relationship of exponence that takes the
representation out of the abstract categories of syntax to items (whether lexical or
grammatical), i.e. to phenomena that have a phonological or graphological form’.
By adding the operation for exponence to the preceding operations, we are able to
detect exactly how GENESYS generates text sentences, combining syntax and lexis
and the equivalents in semantics.
v. Re-entry
Typically, the Agent that we have generated (which has been conflated with
the Subject) will be filled with a ‘nominal group’. So far, the present description of a
pass through the network has chosen [situation] and so, by using a ‘unit insertion
rule’, it has added the unit ‘clause’ to the structure. In order to generate the nominal
group that will fill the Agent, we need to re-enter the network and choose [thing]
rather than [situation]. And the ‘re-entry’ operation will determine that this happens.
The operator would state ‘for Agent re-enter at [thing]’. Then, after re-entry and the
selection of another set of features, the types of operator met above would insert a
nominal group and build its structure.
vi. Fetch Operators
This operation concerns proper names. It is straight forward, and means that
an entity must be fetched from memory. For example, if a ‘familiar’ name (‘Connor’)
is required for the Subject of the clause – i.e. the [thing] to fill the Agent – then the
fetch rule would specify that it must be fetched from memory.
vii. Preference (including Preselection)
This operation resets probabilities on features for re-entry into the network.
What this entails will become clear below, in a description of the necessity for both
probabilities and preselection, or sp rules in COMMUNAL.
126
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
4.2.3 Probabilities
In order for choices to be made in the system network – particularly when a
random generation is being executed by GENESYS – there needs to be some basis on
which to establish a reason for each choice to be made. It has long been a guiding
principle in SFG that choices should be based on evidence of usage, and that some
‘probability’ of occurrence should be captured as guidance for choice.
Even in Halliday (1961) – before the concept of system had been fully
explored – Halliday recognised the notion of probability as important to a model of
language, stating that ‘the interaction of criteria makes the relation between
categories, and between category and exponent, increasingly become one of
“more/less” rather than “either/or”. It becomes necessary to weight criteria and to
make statements in terms of probabilities.’ (Halliday 1961/76:63).
Matthiessen (1999:1) explores the ‘quantitative profile of the system of
transitivity’, giving frequencies of occurrence of different Process types based on
actual corpus evidence. From this he is able to determine some probabilities. His
corpus sample is very small, consisting of only 2,072 clauses (14,500 words), with the
examples coming from mostly written sources. So it is – compared to the other major
corpora on which various linguistic evidence is based – very small and limited
registerially. However, the hand analysis of a small corpus is the only realistic means
for looking at the quantitative nature of TRANSITIVITY, since it cannot (yet) be
automated.
Matthiessen states that ‘we can interpret the relative frequencies as indicative
of systemic probabilities’ (1999:46), and by determining the frequencies of the
Process types from his sample, he is able to establish some probabilities for the
system for PROCESS TYPE as shown in the network presented in Figure 4.3.
127
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
material
(51.0%)
relational
(24.0%)
mental
PROCESS TYPE (9.1%)
verbal
(10.0%)
behavioural
(3.9%)
existential
(2.0%)
Figure 4.3: Matthiessen’s (1999) probabilities for the system of PROCESS TYPE
These probabilities then guide the choice-making in the system, and enable
‘likely’ stretches of language to be generated.41
The network itself is a system of possibilities. Through discovering
likelihoods of occurrence in language, Figure 4.3 demonstrates how we are able to
further enrich the network through the application of probabilities. In the next
section we shall discover how it is possible for these probabilities to change according
to other choices in a network through the application of ‘same pass’ rules.
41
The initial system for TRANSITIVITY in CG is different to that described by Matthiessen, and so
the probabilities are different too. The probabilities for the equivalent system in the CG are presented
in Figure 6.1 of Chapter 6.
42
It is because the realization rules work in this way that Tucker 2000 (the 13th EISFW, Glasgow, July
2000) has suggested that it is not in fact ‘lexis’ that is most delicate grammar, but whatever it is that the
realization rule states, as this happens at a more delicate point than the realization of lexis.
128
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
SP rule is the ability to specify changes that must be made on the same pass through
the network (as their name suggests).
Thus, the great extension to SF theory that SP rules provide us with is the
ability to make a choice at a point in the network which can alter the options available
at a later stage in the network, thus making the system a dynamic phenomenon. I will
now explain exactly what this means, using an example from the TRANSITIVITY
network.
In the TRANSITIVITY network, once the choices in the initial network have
been made – i.e. once we have chosen [action], [two-role process], [plus affected] –
we then move into the following section of the network presented in Figure 4.443.
PROCESS TYPE
… plus affected
PARTICIPANT ROLE
This left ‘curly’ bracket is the convention used to convey diagrammatically the
connection in the network of simultaneity. Thus, these two systems of PROCESS
TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLE are entered simultaneously.
Having reached this point in the network, we will now look at a section of the
‘material action’ network, which is located within the ‘action’ Processes network, as
presented in Figure 4.544.
43
The current network for TRANSITIVITY in CG will be presented in Chapter 6.
44
The full and delicate network for TRANSITIVITY from which this section is taken will be presented
in Chapter 8.
129
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
preparation_as_such …
bathing (6.004610)
washing (6.004611)
shaving (6.004614)
domestic_preparation …
Figure 4.5: The system for [bodily preparation] with ‘same pass’ rule.
Figure 4.5 includes both realization rules (e.g. 6.004610, which specifies that
the Main Verb will be expounded by the item ‘bath’) and same pass rules. Thus, on
the feature [bodily preparation], we find the SP rule (sp300). This rule states that the
probabilities must be changed in the PARTICIPANT ROLES part of the network,
which, as illustrated above, is entered simultaneously with the PROCESS TYPE
network. The probabilities can be changed in the PARTICIPANT ROLE network
because in the PROCESS TYPE section of the network is ABOVE the PARTICIPANT
ROLES section. Therefore, for the purposes of computational generation the
PROCESS TYPE system is entered before the PARTICIPANT ROLE system.
Therefore, the same pass rule ‘sp300’ in Figure 4.5 changes the probabilities
for the system [affected role type], which offers a choice between [affected
unmarked], [affected sought], [affected relating out], [affected exclaimed at] and
[affected covert].
The reason for the SP rule on [bodily preparation] is that in these types of
Process the probabilities for how the Affected entity in the clause will be realised
need to be changed from the default. This is in order to assign a higher probability to
the likelihood of the Affected entity being covert, for Processes such as ‘washing’,
etc.
The reason why the default settings45 in the GENESYS network offer a very
low probability for ‘affected covert’ (there is a 0.01% probability of the Affected
entity being covert) is that they are set to be relevant to Processes such as ‘hitting’
45
The default settings in GENESYS are for the ‘unmarked’ option of discourse that is [spoken],
[consultative] and [non-technical]. It is because of the SP rules that there CAN be default settings, i.e.
because the system CAN change.
130
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
etc., where a covert Affected entity is very much less likely to occur.46 However, in
the case of a ‘bodily preparation’ Process a covert Affected entity is more likely. The
SP rule therefore resets the probability to roughly 50% likelihood for ‘bodily
preparation’ Processes in order to generate Example (2), where the Affected, which is
covert, would be myself.
Fawcett and Tucker (2000) emphasize that the SP rule is a tool not only for
making the network DYNAMIC, but also for making it more ECONOMICAL. They point
out that within the overall network there are a huge number of possible unique
networks which represent all possibilities for the language, and which could all be
individually described (but this would be an immeasurable job to undertake).
However, it is both practical and more economical to capture them all in a
‘superordinate’ network which, through the use of SP rules is an active system, can
change its probabilities to accommodate all of the possible choices.
46
It is possible, though unusual, for a clause with a Process of ‘hitting’ to have a covert Affected entity,
as in he hits really hard.
131
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
Let us now look a little more clearly at some of the types of higher planning
involved in COMMUNAL. In his argument for viewing language as a ‘program’,
Fawcett (1992) proposes that some major components are necessarily involved in the
building of a computational model of language. These are as follows:
1. the Belief System, which determines decisions affected by such things as culture,
social situation, register, context of co-text, etc.
2. the reasoning and higher planning component, which ‘enables (the system) to
recognize a need for a reply’ (1992:634).
3. algorithms that draw upon aspects of the Belief System that are needed in
linguistic stages of planning. For example, in the choice of theme, aspectual type,
tense, etc.
4. the Discourse Planner, which makes choices in genre grammars and exchange
structure grammar.
5. the lexicogrammar/sentence generator – or GENESYS. It is with this part of the
model that this research is concerned.
Of specific relevance to this research is the fact that within the belief system
there is an ontology of ‘predicate types’ (Fawcett, 1996), which will feed directly into
the decision making which affects choices made in the TRANSITIVITY network of
the lexicogrammar. In this framework, much of the ‘definition of word sense’ aspect
of a dictionary entry finds its place not in the system networks that are used in the
generation of language, but in the belief system. As Fawcett says, ‘it is in this higher
component – in one that is at TWO removes from the word-form – that the ‘dictionary
meaning’ of, say, [knowing situation] is spelled out.’(1996:328).
All the characteristics of the model described in the first half of the chapter go
towards ensuring that the output from generation is an acceptable stretch of language.
The characteristics are designed to enable the modelling of the meaning potential of
the language by making the network (1) dynamic, (2) more delicate, and (3) more
sensitive to the likelihood of occurrence of features.
In the first part of this chapter I have surveyed the generative aspect of the
model: its ‘meaning potential’ (the system networks) and its ‘form potential’ (the
realization rules).
132
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
4.3.1 Categories
Halliday’s proposal for categories in a rank scale has remained unchanged
since it first appeared in 1961, with a top to bottom relationship of the ranks of unit:
clause; group; phrase; word; morpheme.
The CG approach differs to this. Fawcett does not recognise a ‘rank’ of units,
but a set of three units which are: clause (which is defined by having one Main Verb);
group (which can be of the type nominal group; prepositional group; quality
group; or quantity group) and cluster (which expresses the complex meanings
associated with two elements of nominal group, (i) genitive cluster, which fills either
the deictic determiner or the head of the nominal group, and (ii) proper name
cluster, which fills the head of the nominal group).
The reason for abolishing the rank scale is partially because Fawcett believes
that ‘the expectation that every element of the clause would be filled by a group’
(Fawcett, 2000b:42) is untenable. Part of the rationale for this view is that the
elements of the ‘verbal group’ are better handled as direct elements of the clause
(Fawcett 2000b, and forthcoming). Because of this, Fawcett sees the grammar
working WITHOUT the notion of the rank scale, i.e. a ‘framework of sentence, clause,
group, word and morpheme as a strict hierarchy of constituents, each one being
related by constituency to the next.’(Halliday, 1994: 23). Instead, Fawcett proposes
that the best means for modelling constituency is by indicating the probability of one
unit filling an element of another unit, thereby focussing on ‘likelihood’ for
determining how the three types of unit relate to each other. He states that:
‘we replace the ‘rank scale’ claim by the statement that (1) the five major classes of
unit (i.e., the clause and the four classes of group) all occur quite frequently at a
number of different elements of structure within a number of different classes of unit;
133
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
(2) that they do so with varying degrees of probability, and (3) these probabilities
(and others) need to be represented in the grammar.’ (Fawcett 2000b:283).
Fawcett (2000b) suggests that this difference in the CG from Halliday’s theory
is helpful to the text analyst, not least because it removes the problem of treating an
element of the verbal group as an element in the clause (the ‘Finite’). For Halliday
these two elements are at different ranks, and so, according to the need for ‘total
accountability’ in the rank scale (Halliday 1966), these two elements should not be
conflated.
In CG the elements of structure are the components of the units, and each
unit is composed of a different set of elements. For example, the main elements of
structure within the clause are Main Verb (M), Main Verb Extention, Subject (S),
Operator (O), Auxiliary (X), Complement (C) and Adjunct (A), whereas the main
elements of structure within the nominal group are head (h), modifier (m), qualifier
(q), deictic determiner (dd) and quantifying determiner (qd).
134
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
Σ
filling
Cl
componence
componence
h qd m h
filling
qlgp
componence
a
exponence
Figure 4.6: Text analysis showing the relations at each level of ‘rank’
The relationships are indicated down the left hand side of the tree diagram,
with the first one being that of filling. This is a relationship between an element and
the unit below it, and the first example we see is between the element ‘sentence’
(signified by the symbol for ‘sigma’) and the unit ‘clause’, with the next example
being between the element ‘Subject’ and the unit ‘nominal group’.
The next relationship in the diagram is componence, which is a part-whole
relationship between the unit and its elements, thus the relationship between the unit
‘clause’, and the composite elements of S, O/M and C.
The relationship of conflation is shown in the diagram with the conflation of
the two elements Operator and Main Verb, and of the elements Subject and
Complement with their respective Participant Roles – as we saw in Section 4.2.2
above.
The final relationship exemplified in the diagram is that of exponence. This
relationship takes the categories out of the abstractions of syntax, and to the point of
phonological or graphological representation. In this example, the Subject is
expounded by ‘Connor’.
The relationships shown in the tree diagram indicate how this type of analysis
is useful to the applied linguist. The tree diagram is a method of analysing
135
Chapter 4: An Overview of the Cardiff Grammar
syntagmatic relations in a way that is most insightful as the first stage of an analysis,
as it is useful for showing the level of complexity of a stretch of language. The more
complex the sentence – in terms of the co-ordination and the embedding of units
within units – the more detailed and multi-layered the analysis will be47. For
example, in Figure 4.6 the complexity within the Subject and the Complement is
shown by the level of detail required for their analysis, as compared to the element
Main Verb/Operator.
Now that we have an overview of the Cardiff Grammar, from the viewpoints
of both generation and description, we are in a position to locate within this model the
area of particular interest to this research – TRANSITIVITY in English.
47
Co-ordination and embedding in fact represent another relationship that can occur in a stretch of
language – namely that of ‘recursion’.
136
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
137
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
‘directional’ (a change which occurred before the present research) but also the
addition of the ‘matching’ relationships alongside the ‘relational’ subsystems of
‘attributive’, ‘directional’, ‘locational’ and ‘possessive’ (a change which was directly
prompted by the present research).
The third main Process type is described in Section 5.6, and this is the
‘mental’ Process system. This area of the grammar has not been described in any
detail so far in this thesis, and so Section 5.8 presents the various sub-types of
‘mental’ Process, with a brief account of important changes that have been made to
Fawcett’s system of ‘mental’ Processes.
And finally in Sections 5.7, 5.8 and 5.9 the ‘environmental’ Processes, the
‘influential’ Processes and the very new ‘event relating’ Processes are described.
This chapter therefore highlights the changes that have been made to the
theory since Fawcett 1980, and provides the reader with a fairly detailed overview of
both the Process types and the Participant Roles involved in the CG system network
for TRANSITIVITY.
The aim of this research is to provide delicate semantic system networks for
the part of the grammar that generates the Main Verb in the clause, or, in Systemic
Functional terms, the Process in the ‘situation’48. The concern is with modelling the
‘experiential’ component of language, which Fawcett describes as being where ‘we
find the meanings through which language reflects the objects, qualities and
relationships that a person finds in the world around him’ (1980:134). The system
networks that are the end product of this research enable the generation of this
component of the language for a very large number of Processes.
I consider that the CG is the most appropriate framework for the present
research, because the CG places emphasis on networks that model meaning at the
level of semantics. The CG already has in place a very large system network for the
generation of language, and my research has developed further the system network for
TRANSITIVITY to allow the generation of a large number of frequent lexical verbs.
48
In the Cardiff Grammar, the process in the clause is realized not only by the Main Verb, but also in
some cases by one (and occasionally more than one) Main Verb Extension (MEx), and sometimes also
a preposition. These occur when the clause contains a multi-word verb, where the Process is extended
beyond a single item. A full description of the treatment of multi-word verbs in the Cardiff Grammar
is given in Section 7.3.3 below.
138
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
5.2 The TRANSITIVITY system in the previous Cardiff Grammar
In the following two sections I will give brief historical accounts of the CG
approach to TRANSITIVITY. TRANSITIVITY in Fawcett (1980) was described in
Section 3.1.1.2 in considerable detail, and so only a brief account is presented here.
However, Fawcett (1987), which concerns ‘relational’ Processes, has not yet been
considered, and Section 5.2.2 therefore presents a comprehensive description of this
area of the grammar. For the full description of the CG presented in later sections I
will also draw upon the computer implemented networks (Fawcett and Tucker 2000)
and on Fawcett (forthcoming).
We shall see below exactly how these TRANSITIVITY choices have been
changed to produce the current CG TRANSITIVITY framework.
5.2.2 Fawcett (1987) ‘The semantics of clause and verb for relational processes in
English’
This (1987) paper modifies and expands Fawcett’s statement on
TRANSITIVITY for the area of ‘relational’ Processes, and holds an intermediate
position between his 1980 description and the CG position today. We will examine
139
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
the important aspects of this version of the model before considering the current
TRANSITIVITY system.
Fawcett’s ‘relational Process’ system in 1987 is rather different from his
initial proposals in 1980 and presents a more semantically based classification, as can
be seen in Figure 5.1:
attributive
locational
possessive
relational
simple carrier
affected carrrier
agent carrier
140
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
choice of Process type. In the ‘relational’ Process network all three semantic
subclasses of Process type (‘attributive’, ‘locational’ and ‘possessive’) are seen as
being able to take the same PR configurations and this generalisation is captured
elegantly, with the left hand ‘curly’ bracket signifying simultaneity. Note too that, as
Figure 5.1 shows, Fawcett allows for the generation of compound roles; thus an Agent
and an Affected can each be conflated with a Carrier to specify precisely the semantic
function of the role.
Particularly important to his ‘relational’ Process system is the combination of
an Agent with the compound role of Affected-Carrier, which is central to the three
role Processes that he describes. He terms these ‘third party agent’ Processes,
because a ‘third party’ is introduced to what would otherwise be a two-role Process.
This configuration facilitates the generation of ‘possessive’ Processes such as
Example (1):
(2) What happened to Sebastian was that Belle gave him the key.
141
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
Secondly, it is related to the ‘Possessed’ entity of ‘the key’ by also functioning
as a Carrier, i.e. it passes the re-expression test for a Carrier in a ‘possessive’ Process
in (3):
Fawcett further illustrates the ‘third party agent’ pattern by using two
contrasting examples (both taken from Halliday 1970):
142
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
‘Possessive’ Processes occur with the following patterns: (a) ‘simple carrier’
(Carrier plus Process plus Possessed), (b) and (c) ‘compound carrier’
(Affected/Agent-Carrier plus Process plus Possessed) or (d) ‘third party agent’ (Agent
plus Process plus Affected-Carrier plus Possessed). In all of these patternings
Fawcett recognizes ‘possessive’ Processes as being concerned with both ‘having’ and
‘lacking’, and so ‘possessive’ is the initial choice in the network. Further, he
introduces a distinction between a ‘possessive’ Process being either a ‘changing’
relation, with someone causing a change in possession (e.g. ‘getting’ something), or a
‘maintaining’ relation, with the possessive state remaining (e.g. ‘keeping’ something).
If the options [having] and [changing] have been chosen in the network, then
the next step in delicacy involves a choice between a [permanent] and [temporary]
‘possessive’ Process, with the final choice of [for money] or [free]. In this way, the
delicate semantic differences between the lexical verbs giving, selling, lending and
hiring are captured. From this progression in delicacy, it is possible to see how the
lexis may be seen as ‘most delicate grammar’ (Halliday, 1961/76:69) through its
generation from the system network for TRANSITIVITY. Through more and more
delicate sub-specifications, the very fine semantic nuances of each verb sense can be
modelled, so generating lexical items.
Fawcett provides a similar treatment for the area of the network that covers
‘attributive’ Processes. This area of ‘relational’ Processes presents an interesting
difference to Halliday’s system. In Fawcett 1997, the ‘identifying’ Processes and the
‘attributive’ Processes that Halliday distinguishes are subsumed into one system of
‘attributive processes’.
In Halliday’s differentiation between these two types, one of the key aspects
that he highlights is the reversibility of ‘identifying’ clauses and the non-reversibility
of ‘attributive’ clauses. The examples that Halliday uses are:
143
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
Halliday states that (9) is not an ‘agnate’ form of (8) (Halliday 1994:120).
However, Fawcett asserts that this non-reversibility is not a reason to set up the two
types as different Processes, but that ‘the reversibility [of pairs of examples such as
(6) and (7)] is handled in terms of thematic choice (so that it does not appear in the
relational process network).’ (1987:175). Moreover, there are Processes that Halliday
states to be identifying which are not reversible (1985/94:122), such as (6a), which
can be compared with (7a), which is ungrammatical in this sense.
For Fawcett, the means for modelling the semantic difference of ‘attributive’
and ‘identifying’ comes at a more delicate point in the system network for
‘attributive’ Processes. It is a choice of whether the ‘attribute’ is a ‘thing’, a ‘quality’,
a ‘situation’, etc. In arguing against making the distinction between ‘identifying’ and
‘attributive’ in a primary system, Fawcett is able to quote Halliday’s statement that
“identity may be merely the limiting case of inclusion”’. And Fawcett then goes on to
state that ‘the essence of what is proposed here, then, is that what makes certain
clauses identifying (or “equative”) is not the process realized in the clause, but the
equativeness between two nominal groups.’ (Fawcett 1987:138).
In Fawcett’s description of ‘attributive’ Processes, the compound role of
Affected-Carrier occurs again in the ‘third party agent’ pattern, and he explains that
his reason for treating ‘attributive’ Processes in the same way as ‘possessive’
Processes is because of ‘the striking semantic parallels’ (1987:151) between two pairs
of clauses. Thus (10) is to (11), as (12) is to (13).
144
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
this Process type, choices are made in Fawcett’s ‘attributive’ system network that
enable the generation of lexical items. And the differentiation between Halliday’s
‘identifying’ and ‘attributive’ is made possible, in the case of a doctor or the doctor,
through re-entry to the ‘thing’ network, where the nominal group will be generated.
The third type of ‘relational’ Process that Fawcett considers in this 1987
description is the ‘locational’. This category is not recognized by Halliday; instead
his third ‘relational’ Process type is classified as ‘circumstantial’. In Fawcett (1987)
the Process type of ‘locational’ follows the same patterning as the other ‘relational’
Process types, and, as with the ‘attributive’ Processes, the PR of Affected-Carrier is
involved. It would be illogical for the role of ‘Beneficiary’ to occur with a
‘locational’ process, e.g. for ‘her books’ in (14) to be described as a ‘Beneficiary’.
49
However, some of these verbs of motion also occur as ‘action’ Processes, of the type ‘agent-only’.
This is found in the use of the verb that indicates habitual activity, for example, ‘Sebastian jogs (every
morning’).
145
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
(17) He (Agent) marched (Process) the prisoners (Affected-Carrier) (to the
barracks) (Location).
For Fawcett, the most important reason for treating these Processes as
‘locational’ and not ‘circumstantial’ is that the two roles in examples such as (18) i.e.
Ivy and in Peru, are equally inherent in the Process, and in Peru is thus a Participant
Role and not a Circumstantial Role.
146
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
attributive
locational
possessive
relational
simple carrier
affected-carrier
agent-carrier
compound carrier
change
maintain
third party agent
Figure 5.2 Fawcett 1987:161, ‘A maximally economical system network for relational
processes in English’.
This summary of Fawcett’s 50-page paper makes clear that his emphasis is on
the importance of semantic system networks from which the TRANSITIVITY
structures and the lexical verbs are generated. His proposals for a system of
‘relational’ Processes have been formulated with this premise in mind. Further, the
system captures the important generalisation that all three ‘relational’ Process sub-
types function with the same Participant Role configurations. Therefore, Fawcett’s
network for ‘relational’ Processes fulfils two Systemic Functional goals of producing
grammars that are both elegant, and ‘systemic semantic’ (1987:179).
Nevertheless, this network can be, and is, expanded upon. As a result of
having produced a large list of frequently occurring Processes, there are many verbs
that suggest the desirability of change to this part of the TRANSITIVITY network,
and, as we shall see in the next few sections, these have now been made.
147
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
in the system network for TRANSITIVITY are presented Figure 5.3, which is
presented as a table rather than as a system network because the percentages included
are not probabilities that will occur in the system network, but instead are the figures
for the number of ‘types’ found to occur in the Process Type Database (PTDB). The
initial choices in the system network are presented in Figure 6.1 in Chapter 6, and the
PTDB is described in Chapter 7.
Process type Sub-Process type
54% action 37% one role
63% two role
31% relational 8% attributive
49% directional
20% locational
15% possessive
8% matching
10% mental 56% cognition
16% perception
28% emotion
2.6% influential
2.3% event relating
0.1% environmental
Figure 5.3 The initial features in the system network for Process Type and the
frequency of occurrence of each type in the PTDB.
The three main categories – ‘action’, ‘relational’ and ‘mental’ – are broadly
similar to Halliday’s main Process type categories. However, Halliday’s description
includes three further categories that he considers to be located at the ‘boundary’
areas, sharing features of the main categories. In the rest of this chapter we shall see
the reasons why these further Process types, of ‘behavioural’, ‘verbal’ and
‘existential’ Processes, do not need to be assigned separate subnetworks in the CG
framework.
148
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
reason for this is that there are a very large number of what might be termed ‘non-
material’ action Processes. There are many ‘social action’ processes that pass the
requisite tests for Processes of this type but which, I will argue, do not involve any
material action. This point will be expanded upon in Chapter 8, where I describe the
proposed new system network for ‘action’ Processes in full detail. As we shall see, a
division will be made in the ‘action’ network between [social action] and [material
action].
In this chapter we shall only cover the initial systems of the ‘action’ sub-
network, i.e. those that determine which Participant Roles are inherently involved in
the Process. The more delicate options, i.e. their ‘overtness’ and ‘covertness’ etc.,
will be introduced in Chapter 7.
The first portion of the action process network is presented in Figure 5.4.
agent only
affected only
carrier only
plus affected
plus range
plus manner
Figure 5.4 The current System Network for Action Processes in the Cardiff Grammar
Some significant changes can be detected when Figure 5.4 is compared to the
previous descriptions of the ‘action’ Process network in the CG – specifically that of
Fawcett (1980) (as described in Section 3.1.12 of Chapter 3). The first and most
obvious change is that the primary distinction is now in the number of Participant
Roles. In Fawcett (1980), the entry condition [action process] led into a single system
in which both ‘one-role’ and ‘two-role’ Processes could be chosen. This is no longer
the case. The entry condition [action process] now leads to the choice between a
149
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
system for [one-role process], a system for [two-role process] and a system for one
very infrequent type of [three-role process], which will be discussed below.
This new aspect of the network leads to another major change in the network,
which is the abandonment of the system that offers a choice between [agent-centred]
and [affected-centred]. In the next section we will look closely at this change, which
has in part been made as a result of the evidence accumulated in the research reported
here.
Levin (1993) would regard these two as ‘alternations’ of each other, as would
many linguists, and this viewpoint was reflected in Fawcett’s network (1980:137) for
‘action’ Processes50. Having made the choice [action process] in the system network,
and on having reached the entry condition [affected-centred] the 1980 system used to
lead to a possible ‘simple’ Process, which would be a ‘one-role’, ‘affected only’
Process, OR a [plus agent] Process, which would be a ‘two-role’ Process. Either of
these choices would lead to the same network for choice of Process type, with
realizations such as change, break, cook, melt, etc.
Fawcett’s terms ‘agent-centred’ and ‘affected-centred’ are an attempt to
provide more explicit labels for what are often called ‘ergative’ and ‘transitive’
clauses. Halliday (1967 and 85/94) distinguishes these two types by using different
Participant Roles for each; transitive clauses involve ‘Actors’ and ‘Goals’, and
ergative clauses involve ‘Agents’ and, by 1985, ‘Mediums’. Fawcett (1980) departs
from the use of the terms ‘transitive’ and ‘ergative’, and from the two sets of PR’s,
and proposes a means for distinguishing the two types in which ‘the terms used …
50
The term ‘alternation’ is used based on Levin 1993; the example given here would be, for Levin, a
causative/inchoative alternation (Levin, 1993:2/3).
150
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
have the advantage of reflecting the fact that it is the “centrality” of a particular
inherent role in each process that determines its nature’ (Fawcett, 1980:140).
One of the innovations resulting from the present research has been that this
distinction is no longer made in the current CG system network for TRANSITIVITY,
which models the difference through other means. At the centre of the argument lies
the distinction between a ‘one role’ ‘ergative’ example such as (1) and a ‘two role’
‘ergative’ example such as (2). Here I wish to suggest that the two examples
exemplify different senses of the verb ‘break’ rather than a single verb with two
different associated patterns of PRs. I suggest that what is referred to in these
examples are two separate Processes: in the glass broke (1), an object changes its state
into the state of being ‘broken’, while in Sebastian broke the glass (2), an agent either
intentionally or unintentionally CAUSES an object to change its state into the state of
being broken. And these two meanings can be modelled successfully using the
current CG framework for TRANSITIVITY. Indeed, we will see in Chapter 8 the
way in which these two situation types are now modelled at different places in the
system network of TRANSITIVITY, and I will there give further reasons for having
two features in the network rather than one to generate these two Processes of
‘breaking’.
The question that must be addressed at this point is whether this proposal loses
an important generalisation. My opinion is that it does not. The grammar still shows
that the two senses of ‘break’ share a single form, through the fact that they share the
same realization rule in the system network for the purposes of generation. And the
common ground between the two senses of ‘break’ is similar to that between ‘rise’
and ‘raise’, and ‘die’ and ‘kill’, etc. Indeed it would be inconsistent to treat the two
senses of ‘break’ in a way that is different from the distinction between ‘die’ and
‘kill’.
I would also point out that a generalisation which captures (1) and (2) seems
very like a ‘transformation’, as in a TG grammar, with one type (probably the two
role, causative example) being presented as the ‘norm’ and the other as a
‘transformation’ of it. The SF perspective does not aim to generate norms and
transformations, but to generate directly the most appropriate instance of language for
a given context, based not only on form but also on function. A move away from a
formal ‘alternation’ such as this one is therefore in keeping with the general SF
approach to language.
151
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
In further support for the decision to dispose of the ‘agent-centred’ versus
‘affected-centred’ distinction, I will return briefly to Davidse’s work on ‘transitivity’
and ‘ergativity’(which was considered in Section 3.2.2 of Chapter 3). In her paper
‘Transitivity/Ergativity: the Janus-headed grammar of actions and events’, Davidse
considers example (1) to be an ‘ergative middle’, which is equivalent to the CG
analysis of it being ‘one role’ and ‘affected only’. She states that ‘the ergative middle
leaves it open whether the action was self-instigated or external’ (1992:114). This
statement, in my view, provides support for my earlier reason for not generalising the
two separate senses of ‘break’ in the grammar. In the construction of the ‘one role’ or
‘middle’ type (1), there is no implied, recoverable ‘agent’, and so this should not be
modelled in the system.
It is proposed here that the terminology used for introducing PRs involved in
‘action’ Processes provides a means for describing all meaning types, and while the
use of Agent and Affected may colour the system to make it seem more causative –
and therefore more ergative – the presence (overt or covert) or absence of these PR’s
provides a useful means for analyzing ALL types of ‘action’ Process.
Further, it is interesting to consider Davidse and Geysken’s (1998) paper, ‘The
ergative causativization of intransitives’, (which is shortened to ‘ECI’). Here they
look at the introduction of the role ‘instigator’ to the non-instigatable structure of the
intransitive:
As Davidse and Geyskens point out, this recognizes a pair that seems to be
neither ‘inergative-ergative’ nor ‘intransitive-transitive’, but ‘intransitive-ergative’.
The CG analysis of this pair would be:
or more probably
152
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
or more probably
51
This example is not the most obvious for illustrating this point, as typically we do not talk of ‘dogs’
undertaking exercise on their own, and thus they do not typically function as Agent in a ‘one role’,
‘agent only’ process. However, the example is used here so that the discussion is parallel to Davidse
and Geysken’s (1998).
153
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
because the role ‘carrier’ transcends all the Process type systems, i.e. it occurs with
‘action’ Processes, ‘relational’ Processes and ‘mental’ Processes, whereas
‘attribution’ is a feature that is restricted to the ‘relational’ Process system.
One other change that should be highlighted is that Fawcett has introduced a
further option in the ‘action’ Process system, and this for a small semantic type that
makes use of the PR ‘manner’. This is used in the generation of both ‘two role’,
‘action’ Processes as in (1) and the only ‘action’ Process in the grammar that involves
three PRs as in (2):
This section, which has considered the current framework for ‘action’
Processes in the CG, can be linked with Chapter 8, where the delicate system
networks for the area of ‘action’, ‘affected only’ and ‘action’, ‘two-role’, ‘agent plus
affected’ will be presented.
154
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
5.5 Relational Processes
Section 5.2.2 gave a detailed description of Fawcett’s (1987) treatment of
‘relational’ Processes. In this section I will present two major alterations to this that
are found in the current CG description. The first is the introduction of a further
‘relational’ Process type, or, rather, the division of the ‘locational’ Process type into
two systems. The second alteration is the introduction of a completely new
‘relational’ Process type – that of ‘matching’ – which I will describe in Section 5.6.2.
52
In Fawcett (1980) the terms used to describe this choice were ‘stative’ and ‘dynamic’.
155
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
attributive
relational possessive
maintain
locational
change
attributive
possessive
relational
locational
directional
By introducing a further Process type, Fawcett seems at first to lose the neat
patterning of the system for ‘relational’ Processes. In the 1987 system network, each
type has not only the same possible PR configuration but also the same initial choice
for the further semantic specification of [change] and [maintain]. Essentially, Fawcett
moves this [change] versus [maintain] option from the ‘locational’ Process type
network to become part of the primary system in ‘relational’ Processes, and he does
not use it with either the ‘locational’ or the ‘directional’ Process types.
The four Process types still have the same possible configurations of PRs, so
that the generalisation captured in Figure 5.2 (in Section 5.2.2) is still maintained.
However, the probabilities are different. Most ‘locational’ Processes choose ‘Simple
Carrier’, and most ‘directional’ Processes choose one of the other options.
At this point it will be interesting to consider Halliday’s treatment of this area
of the grammar. Unlike Fawcett, he regards the roles involved in Processes such as
the ones we are presently concerned with as ‘circumstantial’ elements, i.e. ‘associated
with or attendant on the process’ (Halliday, 1994:150), rather than as roles that are
INHERENT in the Process.
Halliday’s system of ‘circumstantial’ Processes includes seven major types of
circumstance, one of which is ‘location’. This leads to a system that contrasts ‘rest’
and ‘motion’, as is shown in the system network-like presentation of his
‘circumstances of location’ in Figure 5.6, where the choice of [motion] leads to a
156
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
‘directional’ (though Halliday does not use this term) choice between [towards] and
[away from].
Spatial Temporal
‘Path’ is the PR that conveys where the movement takes place, on the way
from the ‘source’ to the ‘destination’, and it is found with Processes such as pass as in
(2):
53
This is based on conclusions drawn from the analysis of the Process Type Database.
Overwhelmingly, the most frequent ‘directional’ Processes in the data involve a ‘destination’.
157
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
(4) Belle went from her house (So), past the lake (Pa), to the bus stop (Des).
158
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
drawing ‘economical’ and ‘elegant’ networks is a useful goal, the network writer must
always allow for the exceptions that occur in language for a fully generatable
grammar.
159
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
Fawcett (forthcoming) describes and justifies our system network for this new
‘relational’ Process type, and in this section I will draw on that publication. The
system network is shown in Figure 5.7:
plus third party agent Ag + Pro + Af-Ca + Mtch e.g. match (up) with
marry to, join to,
combine with,
separate from
Figure 5.7 ‘The major options in the ‘matching’ part of the TRANSITIVITY network
(Fawcett, forthcoming a, Chapter 3, Section 3.4.5)
As Figure 5.7 shows, the possible PR configurations of this new Process type
follow the same pattern as that of the other ‘relational’ Processes, of ‘Carrier plus
Matchee’, ‘Affected-Carrier plus Matchee’ (which is very infrequent), ‘Agent-Carrier
plus Matchee’, and the three role option of ‘Agent plus Affected-Carrier plus
Matchee’.
The Processes that this new category models can be divided into two broad
areas. Firstly, there are those which involve ‘matching’: either with two roles, as in a
matches b, or with three roles, as in a matches b with c, and secondly those which
involve ‘joining’: either with two roles, as in a joins b, or with three roles, as in a
joins b with/to c.
160
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
Francis, Hunston and Manning (1996), where the two-role ‘matching’ type Processes
are described as ‘ergative reciprocals’.
Of these ‘ergative reciprocal’ verbs, the pattern combinations that we should
be concerned with (i.e. the semantic group with which we are concerned) are as
follows. Firstly, they describe ‘pattern combination 1’ (1996:510) which is:
There are five verbs that COBUILD recognize as having this patterning:
match; overlap; marry; touch and wed.
The verb groups that COBUILD associate with these patternings are described
in the COBUILD grammar, and from these descriptions we can see that the verbs in
these groups are all sub-types of what the CG would now analyze as ‘matching’
Processes.
The “combine” and “separate” groups are concerned with the ‘joining’ or ‘un-
joining’ of two or more things, either physically or metaphorically, and include verbs
such as amalgamate; blend; combine; conjoin; connect; decouple; dovetail; entwine;
161
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
fuse; integrate; interlink; interlock; intertwine; interweave; join; lace; link; merge;
mix; overlap; separate; tie; touch; unify; add up; line up; link up; mix up.
The “compare” group is concerned with ‘seeing a similarity, difference, or
connection between two or more things’ (1996:61), and includes the following verbs:
compare; conflate; connect; contrast; distinguish; equate; juxtapose; match;
mismatch; muddle; reconcile; relate; separate.
The “link” group is concerned with the linking of two or more things, or the
making of a connection, and includes the verbs: anchor; compare; connect; correlate;
index; liken; link; match; relate; tie.
These COBUILD lists provide us with classifications of verbs that are grouped
according to systemic relationships. These groupings serve as useful ‘checklists’
when considering which ‘matching’ type verbs we must model in the grammar.
162
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
5.5.3.3 Matthiessen’s Lexicogrammatical Cartography (1995)
Within a SF context, Matthiessen (1995) gives an account of some of the
Processes of this type. He treats these Processes as ‘relational’, but gives
consideration to the following verbs only:
From Table 5.1 we can see that Matthiessen treats nearly all of these verbs as
‘circumstantials’, which is a category that is semantically very diverse.
From this brief investigation of other scholar’s treatment of this area, we can
see that other scholars recognize that these verbs function in a semantically related
way, and as such they must be dealt with by the grammar, and are worthy of treatment
as a distinct Process type. Therefore, another new type of ‘relational’ Process is
developed.
163
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
5.5.4 Summary of ‘relational’ Processes
To conclude this section on ‘relational’ Processes, Figure 5.8 is the current
‘relational’ Process system network in the CG, and it includes the introduction of the
two new ‘relational’ Process types:
simple carrier
affected-carrier
agent-carrier
relational
attributive
locational
source
one direction path
destination
directional source-destination
two directions path-destination
source-path
possessive source-path-destination
matching
Figure 5.8 The current System Network for Relational Processes in the Cardiff
Grammar
5.6.1 Overview
Prior to Fawcett’s current statement on ‘mental’ Processes, this area of
TRANSITIVITY had been given minimal treatment in the CG literature. In his 1980
publication, Fawcett suggested that the choice [mental] in the TRANSITIVITY
system would lead to a system with the following choices:
164
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
emotion
perception
mental
cognition
communication
Figure 5.9 Fawcett’s 1980 proposal for the system for Mental Processes54
emoter-oriented like
emotive
emotion phenomenon- amuse
oriented
desiderative want
affected-cognizant realise
cognition
agent-cognizant study
Figure 5.10 ‘The major options in the ‘mental’ part of the TRANSITIVITY network’
(Fawcett, forthcoming a, Section 3.5).
54
His 1973/81 proposal was slightly different to this, with choices of perception, affective reaction,
recognition and verbalization (Fawcett, 1973/81).
55
The description of ‘mental processes’ in this Section will be based on Chapter 2, Section 3.5 of
Fawcett (forthcoming).
165
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
The later parts of this section will describe each subsystem. Firstly, however,
since no description of the ‘mental’ Processes in CG was provided in Chapter 3, it
will be useful to indicate how the CG system differs from Halliday (1968, 1970 and
1985/94).
For Halliday, all ‘mental’ Processes occur with the PR configuration of
‘sensor’ and ‘phenomenon’. The CG, however, proposes that different PRs should be
associated with each ‘mental’ Process type, and offers four separate single PRs;
Emoter, Perceiver and Cognizant. The last two can function as compound roles; the
Cognizant being conflated with either Agent or Affected and the Perceiver being
conflated with Agent. All of these can occur with the fourth PR of Phenomenon.
This set of PR configurations allows for a far more delicate semantic specification
than Halliday’s, and so the analysis is more revealing.
Another difference between Halliday’s treatment of this area of the
TRANSITIVITY system and the CG is that Fawcett subsumes his own earlier
‘communication’ Processes (and so Halliday’s ‘verbal processes’) into the CG
‘cognition’ sub-system. Halliday locates the ‘verbal’ Processes (e.g. ‘say’, ‘tell’,
‘demand’, ‘ask’, etc.) in the TRANSITIVITY system at the boundary of two
‘principal’ Process types. In other words, for Halliday ‘verbal’ Processes contain
aspects of both ‘mental’ and ‘relational’ Processes – and he suggests that this is a
‘subsidiary’ Process type (1995:138).
Halliday also introduces two other ‘subsidiary’ Process types: the first is the
‘behavioural’ Processes, and some of the Processes that he analyses as ‘behavioural’
are considered to be of the type ‘mental’ in the CG framework. We will discover in
Sections 5.6.3 how Fawcett treats ‘behavioural’ Processes and in Section 5.6.4 what
he used to call ‘verbalization’ (1973/81) or ‘communication’ (1980) Processes.
166
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
56
See Fawcett, forthcoming, Chapter 2 for his discussion of ‘realms’.
167
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
The option [phenomenon-oriented] describes Processes where an event or an
object provokes certain feelings in an Emoter. For example:
The ‘phenomenon oriented’ Processes also involve the PRs ‘Phenomenon’ and
‘Emoter’.
A rare type of ‘emotion’ Process that is not included in Figure 5.8 is an
[emoter-oriented] type. This has the PR’s ‘affected-emoter’ plus ‘phenomenon’, as in
example (9):
168
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
The CG ‘mental’ Process system network also models ‘three role’ Processes
of ‘perception’, in which someone ‘causes someone to perceive something’. The
most obvious example of a ‘three role’, ‘perception’ Process is ‘showing’, as in (14)
where the PR’s are ‘agent’ plus ‘affected-perceiver’ plus ‘phenomenon’:
169
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
of (5) and (6), which involve an Affected-Cognizant plus a Phenomenon, and the
Processes which involve actively going about ‘knowing’, and involve an Agent plus a
Phenomenon, as in (7) and (8).
Another possible ‘cognition’ PR configuration involves the role ‘Matchee’,
which we first met in Section 5.5.2, and (9) and (10) provide examples of the use of
this PR in a mental Process.
(1) Belle asked Sebastian if he loved her.
(2) She told him to go.
(3) I know the area.
(4) Sebastian forgot my birthday again.
(5) I didn't realize you two lived so close.
(6) We learned that he had left.
(7) She studied French.
(8) We will plan a trip abroad.
(9) She matched the material to the colour chart.
(10) He likened the book to his own experience.
170
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
as process rain
environmental
as attribute be sunny
The first choice of [as process] enables the generation of weather related verb
senses that function as the Process in the clause. The two most obvious examples are
rain and snow, but there are others that provide more specific descriptions, for
example, for describing light rain we can say it’s spitting.
The most interesting point about this Process type is that the Subject has no
real world referent. Thus the item it is referentially empty. This choice is available in
the network and is the only option in the whole TRANSITIVITY system network that
involves NO PR’s, and allows for the generation of example (1):
The second choice in the network also involves an empty Subject. However,
this choice does involve one PR: that of Attribute. This option allows for clauses such
as example (2), where the Process is very like a ‘relational’ Process of ‘attribution’.
(2) It is sunny.
57
As will be described in Chapter 7, the PTDB is the base of data for the research presented in this
thesis.
171
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
Fawcett has formulated the ‘influential’ Process type to model a set of
Processes in the grammar that are not accounted for elsewhere in the
TRANSITIVITY system. Processes of this type all include an embedded event in the
matrix clause that is somehow ‘influenced’ in one way or another by the Process.
Figure 5.10 presents the initial choices in the system network.
Figure 5.12 ‘Some major options in the ‘influential’ part of the TRANSITIVITY
network’ (Fawcett, forthcoming a, Chapter 2, Section 3.7)
As Figure 5.12 shows, there are twelve possible types of ‘influential’ Process.
The first choice is between those Processes whose first PR is an Agent and those
Processes whose first PR is an Affected. This is an interesting distinction because it
means that not all ‘influential’ Processes are ‘causative’ (or rather, ‘instigated’ – i.e.
they do not all involve an Agent). For example, in ‘succeeding’ or ‘failing’ in
something, the Process is not one that is DONE, but one that HAPPENS TO YOU, and thus
the typically first role in Example (1) is an Affected entity.
172
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
(1) He failed to hit it.
The second distinction between the twelve possible Processes is whether the
typically second PR is a Created or a Range. The PR Created occurs in clauses where
the embedded event did not exist until the ‘influence’ occurred. We met this PR in
the ‘action’ Process network, where it functions with an Agent for a ‘thing’ that is
created, as was shown in Figure 5.4, and is illustrated in example (2):
It also functions, though only very rarely, as a ‘one role’, ‘created only’
Process as in example (3):
In the ‘influential’ Process system network there is a third use for the PR
Created, and this type allows for events to be created, as in example (4):
There are two senses of the PR Created that are associated with ‘influential’
Processes. The first sense allows for CAUSATION. ‘Causation’ in the ‘influential’
system network may be of the overt type, for Processes such as ‘making’, or it may be
used in a weaker sense for Processes of ‘permitting’ and ‘allowing’ an event. It may
also be used for Processes that are the converse of ‘making something happen’; i.e.
for ‘preventing’ an event. The second sense of ‘creation’ is more closely related to
the created entity in the ‘action’ Process system, and allows for ‘bringing something
into being’, i.e. Processes such as ‘starting’ an event.
The PR Range occurs where the embedded event extends for the same period
of time as the matrix event, and therefore it cannot be AFFECTED by the matrix event
(and so is not an ‘affected’ PR). It is clear that it is not CREATED by the matrix event
(and so it is not a ‘created’ PR), so the CG uses the ‘minimally involved’ PR of Range
for it.
173
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
The Range also occurs in the ‘action’ Process network as presented in Figure
5.4. In the ‘influential’ Process system network, the Range functions as the semantic
label for the embedded event, in an example such as (5):
174
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
the ‘influential’ Processes contain aspects of both ‘action’ and ‘mental’ Processes, I
suggest that we could introduce a different PR configuration to capture this, with the
typically second PR in the clause being analyzed as either a ‘Phenomenon (Ph)
instead of a Range, or a ‘Created-Phenomenon (Cre-Ph)’ instead of a Created. This
would avoid the use of the same PR configuration that is used in the ‘action’ Process
system. Further, the introduction of a new compound PR of ‘Created-Phenomenon’
can be usefully extended to the ‘mental’ Process system for Processes such as
‘thinking up’ and ‘devising’, where something is brought into being as the result of a
‘mental’ Process.
(1) Belle (Ag) led (Pro) Sebastian (Af-Ca) to the river (Des).
The sense of ‘lead to’ as ‘event relating’ Process, however, must be analyzed
differently from this. The reasons that Fawcett proposes for this are (i) that the ‘event
relating’ type has only two PRs and (ii) that the PRs do not pass the tests for the ‘three
175
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
role’, ‘directional’ type. These reasons justify recognizing a new Process type
category of ‘event relating’ Processes, rather than these senses being metaphorised
senses of a ‘congruent’ sense.
Figure 5.13 presents the full system network that Fawcett (forthcoming)
proposes for this new type of Process type.
causing effect
requiring effect
preventing effect
ending effect
effect-from-cause
event-
relating premise-to-inference
inference-to-premise
earlier-to-later
temporal
later-to-earlier
identity
compar-
ison similarity
difference
Figure 5.13 ‘Some major options in the ‘event-relating’ part of the TRANSITIVITY
network.’ (Fawcett, forthcoming a, Chapter 2, Section 3.8)
5.10 Conclusion
This chapter has shown how each of the main areas of TRANSITIVITY have
been given detailed consideration in the CG, in an attempt to account satisfactorily for
all the relevant aspects of meaning. In particular, we have seen that each area of
semantically related Processes is distinguishable according to its PR configurations
and how, therefore, the notion of semantic features in the system network that reflect
the names of the PRs that are generated when these are chosen is very important as
well as the classification of verb senses. In Chapter 7 I will demonstrate the way in
which certain tests for recognising PRs, can be applied to an element in the clause to
176
Chapter 5: TRANSITIVITY in the Cardiff Grammar: the recent model and
some new developments
determine its PR type, and so give us more confidence than is otherwise possible that
we have identified a particular PR.
This chapter has been concerned with the current system for TRANSITIVITY
in the Cardiff Grammar, and has provided an introductory description of each Process
Type. This has illustrated the major changes that have taken place in the network as a
whole, and the need for these changes in modelling the output of the experiential
strand of meaning in language. This level of detail in the description is needed in
order to understand the work on modelling verb senses in the TRANSITIVITY
network that is to be presented in Chapters 7 and 8.
Having considered the initial system network for each Process Type in the
CG, in Chapter 6 I will describe the generation of (a) the PROCESS TYPE and (b) the
PARTICIPANT ROLES and their structural relations to each other in a fully explicit
model that can be implemented in a computer.
177
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
6.1 Introduction
The TRANSITIVITY system network component in the COMMUNAL
project is such that it is able to generate a limited number of lexical verbs, and the
present thesis builds from this basis. Indeed, one of the goals of the present thesis is to
provide the system networks for COMMUNAL to generate a comprehensive
TRANSITIVITY component. I will therefore use this Chapter to describe what has
already been implemented in the computer, so that the system networks proposed in
Chapter 8 can be placed within the existing framework. This Chapter will
demonstrate firstly how the system network for TRANSITIVITY is modelled in
COMMUNAL, and secondly how the operations that were described in Chapter 4 are
used to make the system more efficient. Specifically, the focus is on the functioning
of the ‘same pass’ rules and how they enable the modelling of BORDERLINE
GRAMMATICALITY.
For each major Process type, the TRANSITIVITY system involves three
major subsystems, and this section will examine them in turn. These subsystems are
1) the system of PROCESS TYPE, 2) the system of PARTICIPANT ROLES, and 3)
the system of SUBJECT THEME. I shall show, through the presentation of these
systems, how it is possible to generate the Main Verb of the clause, the types of
associated Participant Role and their realizations, and select a PR to function as the
Subject Theme.
This description will takes us through the system network for
TRANSITIVITY that will generate ‘action’ Processes. This has traditionally been
seen as the ‘central’ Process type, and the system presented here will indicate how
both the lexical verb and the appropriate Participant Roles are generated.
In Chapter 4 I described how the CG system networks function, and how the
various devices enable it to generate appropriately. As you will recall, most devices
depend on the use of probabilities. The present description of part of the ‘action’
area of TRANSITIVITY will present examples of the probabilities of one feature
occurring over another in the system. This allows the user of the grammar to make a
178
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
more informed choice when selecting options, but, more importantly, it produces
REALISTIC text-sentences when the grammar generates randomly. The features in the
system network have numbers next to them that are expressed in percentages to
reflect these probabilities of occurrence. On entry into the TRANSITIVITY network,
the first portion is as follows:
carrier only 0%
action 25% to Figure
6.2
The first choices to be made are [action] Process, as opposed to any of the
other Process type choices, and then within [action], [two-role] Process rather than
[one-role] Process. This means that at a later stage in the network the Process to be
generated will require two PRs, which will also need to be generated. The next
system network choice determines what ‘type’ the second role in the clause will be,
and here we will choose [plus affected]. The second role will therefore be an
Affected, i.e. an entity that will be affected by the Agent ‘doing’ the Process.
The choice [plus affected] is an entry condition to two sub-networks, and these
are entered simultaneously. These are PROCESS TYPE, where the options are
realised as lexical verbs, and PARTICIPANT ROLES, where the features generate the
semantic roles.
179
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
PROCESS TYPE …
PARTICIPANT ROLES …
58
It was, in fact, this part of the network that was used in Section 4.2.4 to illustrate the function of
Same Pass rules.
180
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
6.2 The system of PROCESS TYPE
Our next logical move through the network therefore concerns the PROCESS
TYPE network.
preparation as such …
20% ingestion …
PROCESS TYPE
Figure 6.3 The PROCESS TYPE system in the CG TRANSITIVITY system network
This section of the system provides the possibility for generating ‘meaning
realized in lexis’ – i.e. the lexical verb in the clause. In the most recent version of the
COMMUNAL grammar the possibilities for choice of lexical verb are still very
limited, because the emphasis has been in fine-tuning the effects on structure of a
small set of verb senses. Figure 6.3 shows three possible ‘material action’ types. The
choice [preparation] has been added to the network as a result of the present research,
and Chapter 8 will show how the area of the network dependent on this feature
includes a very large number of frequent ‘material action’ Processes, and the delicate
semantic specification for their generation through the grammar.
For the current purposes, we will make the choice [bodily preparation], which
will enable us to make interesting illustrative choices at a later point. On this feature,
we find our first SP rule, the function of which was described fully in Section 4.2.4 in
Chapter 4. To summarize, this rule specifies a change in probability for the type of
Affected role to be generated in terms of whether it is overt or covert. This SP rule is
required for [bodily preparation] Processes, because the probabilities for the ‘affected’
entity need to reflect that there is a higher probability of the ‘affected’ entity being
181
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
COVERT, as in Example (1); i.e. the Affected is generated, but not realised at the level
of form.
The second reason for having a SP rule here is because there is a higher
chance (than there is with other Process types) that the outcome will be a ‘reflexive’
clause, with the Affected being the same referent as the Agent, and new probabilities
need to be assigned to reflect the likelihood of this happening. If [affected is other] is
chosen, then the probabilities will remain the same. However, if [affected is self] is
selected, then the probabilities must be changed to allow for reflexivity.
This possibility is realised in the network by inserting the following condition
into the SP rule59:
if bodily preparation process is chosen, then for the same pass through the network,
prefer...
the features as follows : 20% affected is other, 80% affected is self
and if [affected is self] is chosen, then for the same pass through the network, prefer
the choices:
10% self is overt, 90% self is covert
Thus the new probabilities on the features result in the following different
probabilities in clause types:
The next stage of the network simply provides the choice of types of [bodily
preparation].
59
The condensed form of this rule in the grammar itself is expressed here in natural language.
182
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
preparing_as_such …
bathing (6.004610)
washing (6.004611)
shaving (6.004614)
domestic_preparation …
Here, we will choose [washing]. This means that the realization of the Process
type will be the Main Verb expounded by the item ‘wash’.
60
See Chapter 8 for a description of this system network as extended by this research, and see
Appendix B for the full system network.
183
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
The examples following each feature illustrate the realization of each choice in
the system. And the probabilities are set for the default choice, i.e. [information
giver], as we shall see shortly. One of the most influential SP rules (sp6_2) in the
network is found in this system. The main purpose of this rule is to model the very
different probabilities of whether the ‘agent’ is ‘unmarked’ or ‘sought’. The
probabilities in this system have already been heavily skewed through the choices
made in the MOOD network ([information giver] in the case of Figure 6.5). To make
this clear we must consider choices that would have been made at an earlier point in
the system, and to do this we need to step outside our current pass through the
network and CONCEIVE OF this pass as being a later stage in a much fuller traversal of
the larger system network – i.e. of the grammar as a whole. Prior to entering the
TRANSITIVITY section of the network, choices were made in the MOOD network,
and these choices will affect the probabilities in the part of the TRANSITIVITY
network being considered here.
The MOOD network is concerned with ‘interpersonal’ meaning, and so it
offers choices on what TYPE of information is to be communicated. The initial
probabilities in the present network - the ‘default’ probabilities - are set for
‘information giver’ (what is traditionally termed ‘declarative’) to be the clause type
most likely to occur. For the grammar to produce plausible sentences when
generating randomly, a SP rule will reset the relevant probabilities throughout the
network for whichever choice has been made in MOOD system.
So, at the point we have reached in the TRANSITIVITY system, the SP rule
6_2 on the feature [agent_and_affected] directs us to re-evaluate in the following way.
If the default choice of [information giver] has been chosen in the MOOD system,
then the probabilities for what type of Agent will be generated are given in Figure 6.5
184
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
Three of the options in the network are set at 0% probability, and this is because they
cannot be chosen if an ‘information giver’ is being generated. But if, for example, the
clause type [relative situation] has been selected, then [agent relating out] would have
become a high probability and the others would have been adapted accordingly. Of
course, it is still possible that it is the Affected or a Circumstance that is the ‘sought’
element.
The choice [agent covert] will ensure the generation of a passive clause. The
way in which the lexicogrammar allows us to make this inherent PR covert is by
creating an ‘agent-less passive’ construction. Thus, in this grammar there is only a
very low probability of choosing [agent covert] for ‘proposals for action’, which
would typically require an ‘agent’ and thus an ‘active’ clause form.
The SP rule re-evaluates possible changes in the probabilities in the following
way. If [new content seeker], for example, has been chosen in the MOOD system,
then the clause will be, in traditional terms, a wh- ‘interrogative’ (i.e. seeking new
content that will contribute to performer’s ‘knowledge’ about the event), and the
probabilities for the agent-type are:
If [proposal for action] has been chosen in the MOOD system, then the final
clause will be some kind of ‘offer’ – and the probabilities for what the agent-type will
be are:
In the current pass through the network, we shall assume that [information
giver] has been chosen in the MOOD system, and so we shall leave the default
probabilities in Figure 6.5 as they are. We shall thus choose [agent unmarked].
So, the function of rule ‘sp6_2’ is to govern the type of Agent, in terms of both
its role in the structure of the clause (as we shall shortly see) and in terms of its
185
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
internal semantics and structure. These will be realized only on re-entry to the
network to generate the nominal group that fills the Agent.
Rule ‘sp6_2’ has equivalent SP rules that are attached to each different
possible PR-type (‘unmarked’, ‘sought’, etc), as shown in Figure 6.5, and they operate
to guide the generation of an appropriate Affected on the same principles.
In Figure 6.5, the choice [agent unmarked] has the rule ‘sp6_23’ attached to it.
As stated above, the choice [agent unmarked] is most likely to occur if [information
giver] has been chosen in the MOOD system. ‘Sp6_23’ determines that if, however,
the choice [new content seeker] has been made (in the MOOD system), and [agent
unmarked] is also chosen, then the probability for choosing [affected unmarked] will
in fact be only 0.001% likely to occur. The preferred choice will therefore be
[affected sought], and so this choice is marked 99.998% probable. Thus, rule
‘sp6_23’ ensures that if, under random generation, the ‘agent’ is not ‘sought’, then the
‘affected’ is very likely to be.
In this manner the expectations set up by the choice in the MOOD network of
[new content seeker] are met. But these new probabilities stop short of making it an
ABSOLUTE requirement. This is because the ‘sought’ element could be neither the
‘agent’ nor the ‘affected’, but a Circumstance, as in example (12):
The next SP rule in the network – sp6_24 – applies if we make the choice
[agent sought]. This ensures that if the Agent is ‘sought’ then the Affected is unlikely
to be – though it might be, as in who hit who?. More explicitly, this rule determines
that if [new content seeker] has been chosen in the MOOD system, the most likely
choice for the Affected will be [affected unmarked], with the probabilities on this
feature set at 99.989%. Thus the likelihood of occurrence of [affected sought] or
[affected covert] is 0.001% and 0.01%, respectively.
The next SP rule is on the choice [agent covert], and is rule ‘sp6_27’. This
rule determines that if any of the choices [information giver], [polarity seeker] or
[confirmation seeker] have been chosen in the MOOD system, then the ‘affected’
must be unmarked, as in example (13):
186
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
(13a) The jeans were washed. (information giver)
(13b) Were the jeans washed? (polarity seeker)
(13c) Weren’t the jeans were washed? (confirmation seeker)
If, however, [new content seeker] has been chosen in the MOOD system then
the choice of feature will be either ‘0.001% affected unmarked’ or ‘99.999% affected
sought’, and so [affected sought] is the most probable, and this will produce a clause
such as (14):
However, although given a very low probability, we must still account for the
choice [affected unmarked] where the ‘sought’ element is a circumstance, so that
example (15) might be generated:
With these rules in mind, we move on to the system for choice of ‘affected’
entity in the clause. All the possible choices of ‘agent’ (‘unmarked’, ‘sought’,
‘relating out’, ‘exclaimed at’ and ‘covert’) lead into the system in which we choose
‘affected’ entity. The default probabilities for this system are as follows:
The probabilities for [affected covert] are set at 0.01% to allow for the
generation of examples such as (16), which might be said of a boxer:
187
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
TYPE system. Some Process types, such as the choice of [bodily preparation]
Processes (‘washing’ and also ‘shaving’) are more likely than others (such as ‘hitting’
and ‘kissing’) to pre-select a COVERT ‘affected’ entity. Thus the grammar currently
provides different probabilities for (17) (in the sense of 17a and b) and (18) (in the
sense of 18a).
(17) He washed.
(17a) He washed himself.
(17b) He washed the dishes.
(18) He hits hard.
(18a) He hits his opponents hard.
Now let us return to the choice to be made for our example. Even though we
are generating a Process of ‘washing’, where the chance of [affected covert] is more
probable than with another type of Process, we will let ourselves be governed by the
probabilities, and choose the more likely [affected unmarked].
The next type of SP rule is the last to be considered here. It covers the
possibility for variation in the probabilities on features in the next system that we shall
enter – that of SUBJECT THEME. This rule determines whether the Agent or the
Affected will be the Subject Theme. In this pass through the network there is a
choice, but if either [agent covert] or [affected covert] had been chosen then this
system would not be entered at all, since the overt role will be the Subject Theme by
default.
188
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
At the point of [affected unmarked], if the choice [proposal for action] and
[positive] have been chosen previously, then the probabilities for Subject Theme will
be:
And if [proposal for action] and [negative] have been chosen, then the rule
will set the probabilities to:
This gives a higher likelihood to the Affected being the Subject Theme, thus
allowing a passive construction to be generated. The SUBJECT THEME system is,
in a sense, the pivotal system of the PARTICIPANT ROLES sub-network. Choice in
the SUBJECT THEME system fixes the PLACE in the clause structure of the PR that is
conflated with the Subject, and the ‘realization rules’ attached to the features in this
system PLACE these PRs.
Realization rules were discussed in Section 4.2.2 in Chapter 4. Each
‘realization rule’ governs the outcome of the pass through the network. A ‘realization
rule’ in the SUBJECT THEME system will specify the two different sets of
preferences that are to apply on re-entry to the network depending on whether the PR
is to be ‘unmarked’ or ‘sought’.
All the choices made in the PARTICIPANT ROLE system lead to the
SUBJECT THEME system, which must be entered next. The SUBJECT THEME
system is thus said to have multiple entry conditions. Within this system there is a
‘SP rule’ that illustrates how it is possible to provide for cases where the
thematization of the Affected must not be allowed. The rule entails that if any of
[seeker], [confirmation seeker], or [exclamation] have been chosen and if one of
[affected exclaimed at], [proposal for action], [relative situation], [situation with role
sought], or [partial dependent situation] are NOT chosen, then [affected not as marked
theme] must be chosen. This necessarily complex condition determines that in such
189
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
cases the Affected that we have generated will not be the Subject Theme in the clause
that is eventually produced.
The second section of the rule provides for the interesting fact that there is one
case where the thematization of the Affected is obligatory. This is when
[exclamation] has been chosen in the MOOD network and [affected exclaimed at] has
been chosen in TRANSITIVITY (as described above). This is so that a clause such as
(2) can be generated.
This clause type is actually far more frequent when the Process is one of
‘being’, which is a ‘relational’ Process type, as in example (3):
The next SP rule in the SUBJECT THEME system provides for cases where
the thematization of the Agent in a passive construction must not be allowed. If any
of [seeker], [confirmation seeker], or [exclamation] are chosen and if any of [agent
exclaimed at], [proposal for action], [relative situation], or [situation with role sought]
are also chosen, then the ‘agent’ cannot be the marked theme in the clause.
As with the Affected, the second section of this rule provides for the one case
where the thematization of the Agent is obligatory. As before, this is when
[exclamation] and [agent exclaimed at] have been chosen. If these conditions are met,
then the Agent will be the marked theme in the clause.
In this case, unlike other cases where there is a ‘wh’-element, there is no
possibility, however formal the register, of choosing a passive marker, plus having the
‘agent’ as theme. This means that the grammar has been set to generate (4) but not
(5):
190
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
6.5 Conclusion
In this chapter we have worked through the system network for
TRANSITIVITY to produce the Main Verb in the clause, the associated Participant
Roles and their realizations, and to select the Subject Theme, and so determine the
structural relationships of PRs to each other and to the Process. In traversing the
network for TRANSITIVITY we have recognised how valuable the ‘same pass’ rules
are for generation through a complex lexicogrammatical system, and how these rules
allow for the dynamism needed in a network in order to produce ‘natural’ language.
For each area of the lexicogrammar we have to seek out, often by trial and
error, the best possible combination of (1) system network conventions, (2) same pass
preference resetting rules, and (3) realization rules, to try to meet three demands that
Fawcett (personal communication) has suggested are to be met in model-building of
‘elegance, coverage and perspicuity’.
We have also explored the way in which setting probabilities on features in the
network enables us to generate various possibilities of language which are ‘possible’
within the constraints of the language. The great advantage of introducing
probabilities to a grammar is that infrequent instances of language are also allowed
for, and that what we might call ‘dubious grammaticality’ is also modelled. In this
approach there is no need for the definite exclusion or inclusion of constructions, and
the grammar can give guidelines as to the likelihood of occurrence of each possible
structure – in relation, where appropriate, to any one lexical item or class of lexical
items.
This Chapter has described a pass through the system network for one sub-
area of TRANSITIVITY, and this has illustrated how we can capture all the delicate
possibilities of this area of language within a single network – so demonstrating
economy, perspicuity and elegance, whilst all the time striving towards ‘the
grammarian’s dream: lexis as most delicate grammar’ (Halliday, 1961/76:69). This
aim is realized by generating the lexis as we work our way through the network.
The two chapters that follow will take up this already very delicate system for
TRANSITIVITY and show how I have greatly expanded the delicacy of the system
networks that model verb senses – and so their realization in lexical verb forms. It is
by building on the framework described in this chapter that I have been able to
191
Chapter 6: PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLES in a generative
lexicogrammar
propose how it is POSSIBLE – and NECESSARY – to extend the TRANSITIVITY
networks to incorporate a very large number of verb senses.
In Chapter 7, I will show how this framework has been used for the current
research – for the classification of approximately 5,400 verb senses. We shall see that
the CG framework has proved to be adequate as a basis for the analysis of this large
number of verbs.
192
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
7.1 Introduction
The previous chapters have been concerned with earlier research on
TRANSITIVITY, and in particular with the Cardiff Grammar (CG) approach, since it
is the approach adopted for the present research. This chapter will explore the
methodology used for creating a new type of database that will ultimately contribute
to the CG approach to TRANSITIVITY.
One of the major products of the present research is the resource that has come
to be known as the Process Type Database (PTDB). This body of data, which
currently models almost 5,400 Process types (i.e. verb senses), is not only the basis
for the other aspects of the present research, but it is also a resource that will be
available for further development and for consultation by other grammarians and text
analysts. The PTDB as it currently stands is presented in Appendix A, but it is a
living document, which can be altered as changes in usage occur, and to which new
entries can be made. A static description of TRANSITIVITY is only of use to a
research project that aims to model a limited representation of language, and this is
not the aim of the COMMUNAL project (as described in Chapter 4).
The creation of a database of almost 5,400 verb senses is a considerable task,
and this task has occupied a large part of my total research time. It is intended to be a
representative list of the most frequently used, and therefore most useful, Process
types and their associated Participant Roles (PRs) and, as this chapter will
demonstrate, its basis is in text corpora.
This chapter has two parts. The first half will describe the sources and
resources used for building the list of Processes included in the PTDB. The second
half will then describe how the PTDB has been designed to be a source that can be
drawn on for creating the delicate system networks which are presented in Chapter 8.
While the PTDB draws significantly on Francis, Hunston and Manning (1996) – the
major published description of verbs in recent years – it is essentially complementary
to their description in the aspects of verbs that it seeks to cover. The PTDB is
concerned primarily with verb senses and only partially, as their realization, with verb
forms. The database is concerned with the number and type of associated Participant
193
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
Role and the degrees of likelihood that they will be overtly realized and less so with
the internal syntax of their PRs. Francis, Hunston and Manning (1996), on the other
hand, are more concerned with the patterns that a verb occurs in, and so the internal
structure of the arguments that the verb takes rather than the semantics of the
argument.
194
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
61
His results are shown in Figure 4.3 in Chapter 4.
195
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
62
It is possible to search for a lemma occurring with another word either side, but this does not produce
‘meaningful’ results.
63
The clearest case is the omission of such verbs where the Main Verb is a form of be in the
COBUILD Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs (1989).
196
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
197
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
198
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
main limitation of West (1953) for the purposes of the present study is its sometimes
unclear sense distinctions – a matter that I shall return to in the next section64.
Figure 7.1 shows an example of what West recognizes to be one sense of the
verb ‘accept’:
Figure 7.1 The entry for one sense of accept in West 1953.
This entry tells us that in 5 million words of source material, the word ‘accept’
occurred 732 times, and in 50% of these occurrences it meant ‘taking a thing, person,
offer, office’.
The information about ‘frequency of occurrence of the different meanings of
all multi-meaning words’ (West, 1953:xi) comes from Lorge and Thorndike’s
Semantic Count, whose differentiation between meanings is based on the distinctions
made in the Oxford English Dictionary (OED). The OED was split into sections of
thirty-two pages, and an individual researcher was assigned to each section. The
source material for the Semantic Count came from ‘encyclopedias, magazines,
textbooks, novels, essays, biographies, books about science, poetry and the like’
(Lorge, cited in West 1953:xi), and the total of the sample was 5 million words. The
source material was limited to written texts only, and these written texts are limited
registerially.
The methodology used for the production of the Semantic Count list of forms
and meanings involved each researcher reading the source material and recording
every occurrence of any word from their section of the OED. Each record included
64
It would have been interesting to have access to the original Semantic Word Count and the Interim
Report to discover what their original sense distinctions were, but it was not possible to obtain copies
of these.
199
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
the item’s source, and also its sense according to the OED. Thus, if we refer back to
Figure 7.1, we can see that a researcher will have examined all 732 occurrences of
‘accept’ in the source material, and determined how many occurrences fell into each
sense distinction category recognized in the OED.
This human-analytical method is of great interest in the light of the current
situation in corpus linguistics, where the analysis of most corpora is automated.
Despite the limitations of the data (i.e. the age of the source material, the lack of
spoken material, and the sometimes unclear sense distinctions), it has the great
advantage over all modern computer based corpora that it provides information about
the MEANINGS of the words. A body of data giving semantic frequencies in this way
would be even more valuable if it could be updated to include the technological
advances that benefit current corpora (i.e. the ability to handle very much larger
bodies of text). Unfortunately at the present time judgments about semantic
specifications can only be made by a human language user who has experience of
contextual knowledge of a lexical item, and can therefore make a decision for each
occurrence.
The concept of using the semantic frequencies of lexical items in writing for
language teaching materials was embraced by researchers in language teaching. The
work that is the most relevant to the present project is Hindmarsh’s Cambridge
English Lexicon (1980) in which the author based his data on West’s General Service
List. At the start of the present research I considered using Hindmarsh as a
supplementary source for the classification of word senses, but since its distinctions
between meanings appear to be based directly on those in West (1953), it seems clear
that this publication has no significant role to play in building the PTDB. In general,
the inadequacies in West’s categories were echoed in those of Hindmarsh.
The development of the PTDB itself went through several stages. The first
version involved a revision of West’s data and reworking the verb forms and senses
into a spreadsheet. As mentioned above, the Semantic Count’s OED-based
distinctions (and thus those given in West) did not always seem appropriate for
modern spoken and written English, and so the transfer of the data into the PTDB was
problematic. This led me to explore alternative, more modern sources for semantic
distinctions, and so to the corpus-based Collins COBUILD English Language
Dictionary. The important role of this work in the present project will be described in
Section 7.3.1.2.
200
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
Nevertheless, West’s categories provide the broad base for the PTDB, and I
shall now explain how I adapted the figures given in West’s General Service List for
use in the PTDB.
As we have seen, West’s data provide percentages for the occurrence of each
sense where the percentage is a proportion of occurrence in 5 million words of source
material. However, since the database is required (1) to reflect meanings rather than
forms and (2) to be as intelligible as possible, I decided that it would be more
beneficial for the database user to be able to compare the figures for each word
MEANING with each other directly – rather than as a percentage of a word FORM. In
the PTDB, therefore, I present first the number of occurrences of the verb FORM in 5
million (the total number of words in the source corpus), and then secondly – and
more importantly – the figures for the number of occurrences of each verb SENSE in 5
million words. These figures were calculated from the percentage representation for
each word sense. The result of this is that a verb SENSE can be consulted directly in
the PTDB. The PTDB is therefore, so far as I am aware, the first database, in either
electronic or printed format, that gives as much prominence to meaning as to form.
To summarize: in its original form, West’s General Service List has a number
of problems that make it difficult to use directly as a data resource in this research.
However, it was this work that inspired the development of the PTDB in the first
place, and wherever possible its semantic distinctions have been adopted in the PTDB
– with the result that its figures can be used as the basis for the semantic probabilities
assigned in the system networks65. However, it has been supplemented and adapted
through the employment of further resources, as we shall now see.
65
However, I should point out that, in practice, drawing probabilities from West’s work was not an
easy task.
201
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
based on the Bank of English corpus, which, at the time of the (1995) publication of
the dictionary, included 250 million words of English usage taken from various
spoken and written sources. It is a particularly useful source for the clarification of
word senses, as it claims to ‘specialize in presenting the words and phrases that are
frequent in everyday use’, and it goes on to state that ‘everything in the book is worth
learning for mastery of contemporary English’ (1995:viii). The dictionary therefore
contains much useful information for creating a contemporary Systemic Functional
model of TRANSITIVITY.
The version of the COBUILD dictionary that I initially used for the purpose of
clarifying word senses was the (1987) version, Collins COBUILD English Language
Dictionary. This dictionary is based on a corpus of 20 million words, and the
definitions included are therefore based on a significantly smaller body of data than
the 250 million word Bank of English. Furthermore – and importantly for the present
research – this (1987) version does not include any frequency information for the
word forms included other than what can be inferred from the order of presentation.
The more recent (1995) version, Collins COBUILD English Dictionary, does include
frequency information. More detail of this is provided in Section 7.3.2, as this is the
point in the PTDB methodology at which the COBUILD frequency information is
utilized. For the purposes of this section of the description, the focus is on the
methodology used for supplementing the distinctions between verb senses found in
West (1953) and for including examples of usage for each sense.
One might consider that basing the definitions of verb senses on one source
(or two if West is included) would provide a one- (or two-) dimensional view of how
verbs function in English, and that perhaps we would gain a more comprehensive
picture if we were to use sense distinctions from various dictionaries. However, this
research project gives greater weight to the corpus-based COBUILD dictionaries than
to dictionaries whose distinctions are made according to the judgment (or ‘intuition’)
of the lexicographer. In any case, such an approach would run the risk of introducing
too many conflicting categorizations. And, in fact, the detailed practical work of the
project has shown that the categories in West and COBUILD can be amalgamated
relatively easily – thus providing an informal mutual evaluation that is nonetheless
impressive.
202
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
To explain how the COBUILD dictionary was used, we shall return to the
example of the item accept (as shown in Figure 7.1 above), but with its second sense
also indicated this time.
66
See Table 7.2 for the figures.
67
Here accept functions not as a Main Verb in a full Clause, but as a highly restricted clause (termed a
‘truncated’ clause in Fawcett 2001a) that fills a modifier in a nominal group.
203
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
4. People will accept suffering that can be shown to lead to a greater good.
5. Cheques can only be accepted up to the value guaranteed on the card.
6. Should the British army accept gays?
7. Stephen Smith was accepted into the family like an adopted brother.
8. The company cannot accept responsibility for loss or damage.
9. An older man would never accept orders from a younger woman.
10. …drugs which will fool the body into accepting transplants.
11. The telephone booths accept 10 and 20 pence coins.
This set of eleven supposedly different senses of ‘accept’ illustrates the very
detailed treatment of lexical items given in COBUILD, based on how a word behaves
in a large body of texts. The next stage in developing the PTDB involved a close
inspection of the COBUILD treatment, in order to determine how many senses of
accept need to be modelled in the representation of the TRANSITIVITY system.68
In the case of the verb accept, the final decision for what should be included in
the PTDB was as follows:
FORM Occurrence MEANING Occurrence Cardiff Participant Role
of form in 5 Grammar Configuration
million Feature
Accept 732 receive (a gift/ offer/ advice/ 366 possessive, Ag-Ca + Pos + Af-Ca
story/ fact; your clothes will agent carrier
be gratefully accepted by
jumble sale organisers )
68
Interestingly in the present example (as in many others), a detailed examination of the COBUILD
dictionary examples indicated that a lot of the examples are passive. The editors of COBUILD state in
their introduction that the examples are ‘chosen carefully to show the patterns that are frequently found
alongside a word or phrase’ (1995:ix). This in turn suggests that passive constructions are more
frequent in the corpus evidence of language use drawn on in the COBUILD project than is usually
thought, and although this finding is not immediately relevant to this research, it is worth noting.
204
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
In Table 7.2 the eleven senses proposed in the COBUILD dictionary have
been condensed into just three separate verb senses, each corresponding to a different
TRANSITIVITY analysis. It may appear strange that the large number of senses
proposed by COBUILD have been reduced in this way, when the purpose of
consulting the dictionary was precisely to determine the number of sense distinctions
that is needed. But most of the eleven COBUILD senses reflect very fine semantic
distinctions that relate more to the internal characteristics of the Participants involved
in the Process than to the Process itself. For example, the first sense of ‘accept’ in
Table 7.2 corresponds to Examples (1), (3), (4), (5), (9), (10) and (11) in Table 7.1.
The same Participant Roles are needed, whether the acceptance occurs in the
‘physical’ realm as in (1) and (5), or in the ‘psychological’ realm as in (4), or in the
‘social’ realm as in (9). The value of consulting COBUILD is shown by the addition
of a THIRD sense in Table 7.2 over the senses in West, i.e. those that correspond to
Examples (6) and (7)69.
As you can see, the PTDB includes both a ‘gloss’ of the meaning of the verb
AND an example of its usage. An example illustrates the main senses as they are used
in context, and this proves to be important as an aid when analyzing the Process type
and Participant Role configuration for each sense (which is described in Section 7.6).
It should be emphasized that the reason for the use of a dictionary for exploring the
various word senses was not to enable me to base the classification on the dictionary
DEFINITIONS, although some lexical research has attempted to do this (e.g. Faber and
Mairal Uson (1999)), in order to construct lexical hierarchies. My purpose was to
ensure that I considered all possible meanings that might be relevant to modelling a
system network for TRANSITIVITY – a network that is defined primarily by the
Participant Roles that occur with each Process type. The method of working involved
69
This goes to show just how troublesome word sense disambiguation is, and how we have to rely on
fairly informal criteria to determine word senses.
205
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
a great deal of individual work on my part, with regular sessions with Professor
Fawcett to discuss problem cases.
By applying this method of using the COBUILD dictionary to all the verbs, I
have been able to develop the PTDB to the point where it contains a very large
number of verb senses, with a particularly thorough coverage of frequent verbs (i.e.
those that receive a four and five diamond rating in Francis, Hunston and Manning
(1996), for which see the next section). However, Section 7.5 describes how we need
to consider not only simple, one-item verbs that predominate in most studies to date
but also the senses of multi-word verbs. The inclusion of frequent multi-word verbs
as well as one-word verbs brings the total number of word senses in the PTDB at the
time of writing to a little under 5,400.
7.3.1.3 The use of Francis, Hunston and Manning (1996), COBUILD Grammar
Patterns 1: Verbs
As we have seen, West (1953) is unique in providing information relating to
the occurrence of different word senses in a body of data. However, in the course of
my research a further source of information on the frequency of verbs became
available. This is the Francis, Hunston and Manning (1996). Its main advantage is
that it reflects late 20th century usage, but it has the disadvantage for my goals of all
corpus-based work so far, in that the frequencies it provides are at the level of form,
not meaning.
As I have said, the method for research that I have termed the ‘second level
use of corpora’ has not been widely used so far. For research that requires a large
body of evidence this method is particularly beneficial, since it enables the researcher
to use corpus based results without having to trawl through a large corpus for every
item to make judgments on. For research such as that presented in this thesis – which
seeks to model a very large number of verbs – accessing data directly from a corpus is
impractical, and also unnecessary as the required information is attainable through the
second level use of corpora.
As one of the main corpus projects at the present time, COBUILD has been
the basis of a number of recent publications that are based on the analysis of a very
large corpus. These publications provide information which not only serves as a large
206
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
body of manuals for language learning and teaching but also as a source of
information which can be used for the ‘second level use of corpora’.
Francis, Hunston and Manning (1996)’s Grammar Patterns is intended as an
aid for the teaching and learning of the verb patterns that occur in English, and it
describes the patterning of over 4,000 verb forms. It assumes a minimal description
of grammar, centred around word-classes, but extending in places to the co-
occurrency of classes of groups and of clauses. This resource provides a verb index
with a frequency classification, and it is this that gives us the frequencies of verbs in
this very large corpus. The classification consists of six bands, and each verb that is
considered falls into one of the band areas. Susan Hunston has provided me with the
figure boundaries for each band (personal communication), and this in turn has
enabled me to determine some real figures for the verb forms70. The frequency
classification is given in the form of an index, and the band boundaries are as follows:
I added this information to the PTDB so that each entry for a verb form
includes a COBUILD ‘band’ reference that indicates its frequency of occurrence in
the COBUILD corpus. I also added the actual figure of occurrence for all of the band
five (thus, high frequency) verb forms71. Thus the PTDB has not only the West
figures for occurrence, but also frequencies from the more recently produced
COBUILD documentation.
70
I am indebted to Dr Hunston at Birmingham University for this information.
71
This information was provided by Ball (forthcoming).
207
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
The main limitation of the information obtained from this COBUILD source is
that these figures refer only to occurrences of verb FORMS. Whereas West’s data,
which is based on a relatively small body of source material, was able to provide
frequencies for word senses by using human analysts, this much larger and
statistically significant corpus of language is necessarily limited to automated analytic
tools that cannot recognize semantic differences.
This continues to be a problem of using corpus evidence for research that is at
the level of meaning, and the determination of MEANINGFUL frequencies is a task that,
at the present time, can still only be reliably undertaken by a human analyst. Corpus
searches conducted as a pilot to the present research have proved that the corpus-
based differentiation of verb senses for even the most frequent verb forms – those in
band five – would be too large a project for a single researcher working on a three
year project.
The information on frequency given in the Francis, Hunston and Manning
(1996) differs from that given in the Collins COBUILD English Dictionary (1995) (as
discussed in Section 7.2), in that the banding in the Grammar Patterns publication is
concerned only with VERB forms, whilst the bands given in the dictionary incorporate
ALL word forms. Thus, the frequency classification provided in the dictionary does
not distinguish between the number of nouns and the number of verbs. For example,
in the dictionary the entry for ‘act’ is placed in band five (most frequent), but in
addition to its seven verbal senses it also has five nominal senses. In Francis,
Hunston and Manning, however, the frequency classification is altered to reflect its
verbal usage only, and so ‘act’ is given only a band four classification. Thus the
Grammar Patterns gives more relevant information for the present project.
The COBUILD classification indicates that there are 134 verbs in band five.
These 134 verbs are therefore the most frequent verbs – at least, the most frequent
one-word verbs at the level of form – in a very large corpus. Interestingly, the
majority of these verb forms were already in place in the PTDB before consultation of
Francis, Hunston and Manning, having been included as a result of consulting West’s
General Service List (which is based, as we have seen, on a much smaller corpus of 5
million words of early 20th century written material). It is interesting to note, then,
that the most frequently used one-word verbs in English appear to have remained
constant for the last fifty years.
208
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
209
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
‘the main categories of multi-word verbs consist of such combinations as drink up,
dispose of and get away with which … come under the headings of PHRASAL VERB,
210
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
functioning as the Main verb plus ‘Main Verb Extension’, Fawcett (forthcoming)
demonstrates how the MEx can be realized by a nominal group, as in Example (3):
The ‘reified process’ may at first appear to be in some respects like the
Participant Role of ‘Range’, as it is an ‘event thing’, and also it can become the
Subject Theme. This is the analysis that Halliday (1985/94) provides. He in fact uses
the term ‘Range’ to describe two different types of phenomena in the clause. Firstly,
he uses the term ‘Range’ to describe a Participant such as the mountain in Example
(5), which is what the CG also recognizes to be a Range. As Halliday states, the
mountain is ‘an entity which exists independently of the process … Mountains exist
whether anyone climbs them or not; but the mountain specifies the range of Mary’s
climbing’ (1994:146).
Secondly, he uses the term Range to describe something that is part of the
Process. For example, playing tennis, singing a song, etc. Again, this is the same
analysis as the CG.
However, Halliday extends the use of ‘Range’ even further to describe
occurrences of nominal groups with ‘lexically empty’ verbs in clauses such as
Example (6):
211
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
He rightly states that ‘the process of the clause is expressed only by the noun
functioning as Range.’ (1994:147).
But Fawcett argues – and I tend to agree – that it is advisable to limit the use
of the terms ‘Range’ to phenomena which exist in their own right in the world, i.e.
‘[A Range] has an existence outside the particular event that is being reported’
(Fawcett, forthcoming, Chapter 2, Section 1.5). And instead, Fawcett recognizes that
a ‘reified process’ – such as a kiss in the Example (3), a bath in Example (6), or a
glimpse in Example (7) – actually IS the Process.
This idiomatic use of ‘taking’ might at first lead one to believe that the Process
involved three roles. However, (8) is not equivalent to the three role directional
process in example (10):
212
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
Table 7.4 The analysis of the items ‘pull’ and ‘push’ in a sample of 50 lines from the
BNC
The results of this corpus search show how a random selection of occurrences
of these high frequency items provides evidence that they are much more likely to
occur as multi-word verbs than as single word verbs. It is consequently important that
the multi-word verb senses of push and pull are given a central place in a model of
TRANSITIVITY.
While these results are based on a very limited source, the message is
startlingly clear. Moreover, this study brings out a second major problem in accessing
information about verb senses from large bodies of corpora. Fifty lines of text are
relatively easy to search through to draw out examples, whereas a search of the
individual functioning of lexical items in a much larger corpus – which would provide
much more reliable results – would be an impossible task to undertake. This
72
Fifty is the number of lines of random corpora that can be accessed from the BNC online.
213
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
73
No figures are given by Biber et al for this multi-word verb.
214
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
Ideally, the last categories in this entry, i.e. those functioning as multi-word
verbs, would each be entered as separate verb types.
To obtain the true figures for the multi-word verbs included in West, I
developed the following method. All the multi-word verbs to which West assigns a
figure of over 10% are included in the PTDB as separate entries. However, this did
not provide the PTDB with a very high number of multi-word verbs – certainly not
enough for a representative account of TRANSITIVITY. I therefore consulted a
number of other sources, from which I developed a list of sixty-eight single word
Processes which frequently combine with items such as in, out, on, off, etc, to form
multi-word verbs. The three major sources for these single word Processes were
215
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
West (1953), the COBUILD Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs (1989) and Fawcett
(forthcoming).
From West’s General Service List (1953) I took the thirty-eight multi-word
verbs in the category which had been assigned a figure of 10% or over. This 10% is
an arbitrary figure, but using this category provides us with a list of frequently
occurring multi-word verbs in the West corpus.
By coincidence, the COBUILD Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs (1989) authors
also provide a list of thirty-eight common verbs that they maintain occur in a large
number of combinations with different ‘particles’ (1989:vi). Unlike the list in West
(1953), this list is not based on the frequency of occurrence of each multi-word verb
sense, but instead the list is made up of those one-word verbs that are EXPECTED to
frequently occur as the Main Verb in a multi-word verb.
The final source for Processes included in the list of one-word verbs that
frequently occur with other lexical items to form multi-word verbs is Fawcett
(forthcoming). This source includes what Fawcett recognizes to be highly occurring
verbs. His list includes sub-lists of both ‘one-role’ and ‘two-role’ Processes that are
multi-word verbs. Fawcett’s main source is the COBUILD Grammar (Sinclair et al
1990). The Processes he proposes seem to concur with those suggested by West and
COBUILD, except that the latter excludes be. The full list is as follows:
216
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
be lay stand
break leave stay
bring lie stick
burn live sweep
buy look take
call make talk
carry mix tear
cast move throw
come pass tie
do play touch
draw pull turn
drop push use
fall put wash
fight run wear
fill send weigh
get set wipe
give shut
go shoot
hammer sit
hand slip
hang slow
hold smile
hit speak
keep split
kick spread
knock spring
Figure 7.3 A list of sixty-eight single word verbs which regularly combine to be multi-
word verbs.
This list combines with the list of frequent words that can occur as MEx in the
clause. This list of frequent MExs is as follows: ‘up’, ‘down’, ‘in’, ‘out’, ‘on’, ‘off’,
‘about’, ‘(a)round’, and additionally, with ‘movement’ Processes, ‘inside’ and
‘outside’74.
I added this list of sixty-eight single word Processes which frequently combine
with MExs and / or prepositions to form multi-word verbs to the PTDB, and I then
looked each in turn up in the COBUILD Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs to ensure full
coverage of the sense types. I used this dictionary in the same way that I used the
COBUILD English Dictionary (1995) (as described in Section 7.3.2) in order to
supplement the single word verb senses. I entered into the PTDB all of the MExs and
74
The source of this list of frequent MExs is Fawcett, in press, Chapter 5.
217
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
prepositions that COBUILD recognizes each Process to occur with, plus an example
of usage for each. This has meant that close to 800 new multi-word verb senses have
been added to the database.
There is one major problem with these new additions. This is that they do not
give a true representation of the high frequencies of the multi-word verbs. An
example of this is the single word verb give. This is very high frequency item,
occurring 63,524 in the COBUILD corpus. But the multi-word verb give off is
(probably) low frequency compared with, for example, the multi-word verb ‘come
on’, which was cited, as we have seen, in Biber et al (1999) as high frequency.
Despite the difficulty in assigning probabilities to such verbs, the advantage of
including this list in the PTDB is that the database now has a high coverage of multi-
word verbs.
The main disadvantage of the COBUILD Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs (1989)
as a source is that is does not recognize ‘be’ as a Process that can function as a multi-
word verb. Examples such as ‘be born’, and ‘be up’, in Examples (1) and (2), suggest
that ‘be’ occurs frequently in conjunction with other words as a multi-word verb, and
therefore it should be included in the PTDB.
Instead I consulted both the Longman Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs (1983) and
the Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English (1999) to determine the multi-
word verb senses of ‘be’ that should be included in the PTDB, as both of these
sources provide detailed information for the functioning of ‘be’ as a multi-word verb.
To summarize this section on multi-word verbs, I have recognized in my
research both the need to include multi-word verbs and the fact that it is a major
problem to obtain frequencies for them from corpus-based studies. The weaknesses
of the ‘second level’ corpus data described above confirms this. However, it is very
important to include them in a system network for TRANSITIVITY that indicates
frequencies, as they certainly do occur frequently in language. In order to establish a
relatively full coverage in the PTDB I have drawn on the sense distinctions of the
COBUILD Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs for each multi-word verb derived from the list
of sixty-eight single word verbs, and the Longman Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs to
218
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
supplement this in order to provide the multi-word verb occurrences of be. Finally,
Biber et al’s Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English is the only source that
publishes lists of multi-word verb frequencies taken from corpus evidence, and I
included this frequency information in the PTDB. But these frequencies are at the
level of form rather than meaning.
7.6 Assigning Process types and Participant Role configurations to the verb
senses in the PTDB
We come now to the most challenging stage in the construction of the PTDB.
Since the purpose of producing the PTDB is to formulate a picture of what should be
modelled in the production of a representative system network, we need to provide a
TRANSITIVITY analysis for each verb sense. A network can then be created from
this analysis, into which each verb sense can be placed. In other words, having
established a method for acquiring what I believe to be the most frequent verb senses
in English, each verb sense in the list must be analyzed according to its Process type
and its configuration Participant Roles.
The description of a system network provided in Chapter 3 demonstrated how
a system network involves subclassification, and, as we saw in Chapter 5, the first
subclassification in the TRANSITIVITY system is the types of Process, as presented
in Figure 7.4:
action
relational
TRANSITIVITY mental
influential
environmental
Figure 7.4 The initial subclassification in the system network for TRANSITIVITY
219
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
Figure 7.5 The likelihood of the overt or covert realization of a PR in the text.
220
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
7.6.1 The methodology for analyzing the verb senses in the PTDB
Having determined the frequency information for a great many of the verbs in
the PTDB, the most productive method for analysis is to consider the most frequently
occurring verbs first. This is because, as Sinclair et al (1995:xiii) state, ‘the words in
the top two bands [in the COBUILD English Dictionary] account for approximately
75% of English usage – so their importance is obvious’. The PTDB therefore
includes an analysis of all of the verbs that fall into band five and band four, but also a
great many others.
I decided that the analysis would require consultation with others involved in
the CG, as a single perspective would not produce the most conclusive analysis. One
particular problem in analysis is that of determining the sometimes fine boundary
between Participant Roles and Circumstantial Roles. Section 5.2.2 presented a
discussion of how Halliday’s framework differs from the Cardiff Grammar in what is
considered to be Participant and what is considered to be Circumstance, and it has
become clear from my large scale analysis that this is a ‘grey’ area, and problematic
to determine. However, a combination of Fawcett’s tests for Participant Roles (see
Section 7.6.2), and consultation with Ball (forthcoming) – whose research seeks to
provide criteria for recognizing Circumstantial Role types – aided in the
determination of Participant Roles.
A further feature of the PTDB is that it includes real text examples, (or
examples shortened from real text examples) which proved to be a valuable guide in
the analysis. The examples, taken from the COBUILD Dictionary (1995), provide a
context in which the Process is involved. It is only possible to determine the
TRANSITIVITY analysis of a verb by assessing its occurrence in a number of
contexts and with a variety of collocates. Furthermore, by analyzing real corpus
examples taken from COBUILD, I was able to test – and in a few cases expand – the
Cardiff Grammar as a tool for analyzing the TRANSITIVITY of real instances of
language.
221
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
I hypothesized that, once the analysis of the most frequent verbs was
completed, these frequent types might serve as ‘prototypes’ for each Process type
category, as in Rosch’s (1978) theory of categorization for cognitive categories
(despite the fact that her theory concerned ‘things’ rather than ‘processes’). I hoped
that this might facilitate analysis of further verb senses through the cross-referencing
of verbs associated with a prototype. However, this approach to analysis needed to be
used with care since the Process type categories do not involve a single superordinate
verb sense.
Having analyzed the most frequently occurring verbs in the PTDB, I found
that it was not the case that these verbs fulfill a central, or prototypical, criterion for a
Process type category. In fact, the most frequently occurring verbs tended to be often
the hardest to analyze because (a) the present research aims to model verb SENSES,
and it is generally the most frequently occurring verb FORMS that seem to have the
most senses, and (b) the most frequently occurring verb forms and senses have the
most extended meanings, or metaphorical uses, so that they must be ‘unpacked’ to
reach a core meaning75.
Nevertheless, I found that words from semantically similar domains often
involved the same TRANSITIVITY analysis, and this therefore assisted the overall
analysis of the PTDB.
75
I will return to this notion in Section 7.6.3.1, where I will consider specific problems in analysis.
222
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
that were found to be high frequency in the present research. Of the 134 verbs that
COBUILD considers to be highest frequency (band five) there are 13 verbs that Levin
does not include76, and of the 231 verbs that COBUILD considers to be in the next
band of frequency (band four), Levin omits 49. The conclusion must therefore be that
although she at first appears to have a very broad coverage, with her 3262 verb forms,
many of these are low frequency, and the list appears not to be based on a corpus of
an adequate size.
Further, on examining Levin’s verb classes, I found that various groups of
verbs that have the same alternation are not as semantically similar as she claims. At
least, they do not have a semantically close fit with the classes of Process type in our
system networks, as defined by configurations of PR. It follows, therefore, that her
semantic groupings have proved to be much less useful than was hoped, as a source of
‘intermediate’ features in the system network of Process types.
7.6.2 Using the Cardiff Grammar Participant Role tests for TRANSITIVITY
analysis
The most important of the ‘tools’ provided by Fawcett for analyzing
Participant Role types – and thus Process types – is the set of ‘re-expression tests’ for
testing each element of structure in the clause to be analyzed. The full set of tests is
given in Fawcett, forthcoming a, Chapter 2, Section 4, and is provided in this thesis at
Appendix C. Fawcett provides a test for each possible Participant Role. It is
important to note that the tests are intended to be guidelines for the analyst, not as
absolute rules.
The tests are ways that the clause can be re-expressed that will enlighten the
analyst as to the function that is taking place. Fawcett also provides supplementary
criteria for recognizing PR types; for example, he states that ‘an Agent is typically
animate and usually human – but not necessarily, because a wide range of objects
have ‘creature-like’ qualities’.
Fawcett’s tests are similar to Halliday’s ‘probes’, which for his Material
Processes are the same tests as the CG tests for Agent and Affected, and these are
‘What x did to y was to …’ and ‘What happened to y was that …’. One difference
76
These 13 are: base; be; become; decide; expect; fail; force; involve; let; lose; plan; seem and spend.
223
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
between the two frameworks is that Halliday’s set of probes is incomplete, while
Fawcett provides a full set of tests for the semantic PRs.
224
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
225
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
For the purposes of creating data base that can be used as the basis for a
generative Systemic Functional Grammar I suggest that this approach does not benefit
us. Rather, what is needed is a grammar that can provide directly for metaphorical
usage. And the Cardiff Grammar can indeed provide for this by analyzing the
‘protests’ in Example (1) as the ‘Affected-Carrier’ in a ‘relational’ Process of
‘direction’ (flooding in), with a covert direction, i.e. the protests are ‘affected’ by
something external to the text, about which we need not speculate.
Halliday proposes examples of grammatical metaphor such as ‘have a bath’
and ‘make a mistake’, and, as was shown in Section 7.5.1, these are also allowed for
in the CG. Halliday’s description of grammatical metaphor states that ‘a semantic
configuration that would be represented congruently (non-metaphorically) by one
type of clause is represented metaphorically by another’ (1994:57). So, in the
analysis in Example (4) what would congruently be realized as a PROCESS of bathing
is, in Halliday’s terms, realized metaphorically as a NOMINAL GROUP, and he analyses
these occurrences as Process plus Range. The CG analysis, however, differs from this
by treating the whole as part of the process, recognizing the nominal group as a
Process Extensions, as in Example (4):
Halliday asserts that ‘nominalizing is the single most powerful resource for
creating grammatical metaphor’ (1995:352). In treating nominalization as a type of
‘grammatical metaphor’, Halliday is implying that the analyses of such a phenomenon
must provide both a congruent and an incongruent form. I see parallels between
Halliday’s notion of ‘unpacking’ and the approach to meaning of Wierzbicka, who
states that ‘meaning cannot be described without a set of semantic primitives’
(1996:11) – which indeed it cannot. But in applying an ‘unpacking’ approach the
‘meaning’ is ‘stripped away’ to reveal ‘primitives’ by which a more complex term can
be defined. Wierzbicka’s approach to semantics is enlightening in its way, but should
these semantic primitives be part of a model of language for language GENERATION?
If a historically metaphorised use of language – such as flooding in in Example (1) –
is ‘stripped’ enough, it is reducible to a set of semantic primitives, as in Example (3).
In the COMMUNAL project the nearest equivalent to this level of representation is to
226
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
be found in the logical form in the ‘Belief System’ (see Chapter 4 for a brief
description of this). In modelling the level of meaning within language – which is our
goal here – stripping back layers to this level does not necessarily produce a
‘congruent’ form.
Halliday and Matthiessen (1999)’s discussion of lexical and grammatical
metaphor centres around ‘phylogenetic’, ‘ontogenetic’ and ‘logogenetic’ congruency,
and they state that:
‘on all these grounds we have to acknowledge that the metaphorical relationship is
not a symmetrical one: there is a definite directionality to it such that one end of the
continuum is metaphorical and the other is what we shall call congruent.’
(1999:235).
They are suggesting that what comes first – historically, developmentally and
textually – is the congruent meaning, and they state that it is necessary to recognize
this congruent meaning and to signal when a metaphorical meaning is used.
In partial support of the position that I am taking, we can note that in
Halliday’s discussion of metaphor and how the metaphorical is ‘in some respect
“transferred”’ (1994:342), he states that ‘this is not to say that the congruent
realization is better … or even that it functions as the norm’. And he also says that a
congruent form and a metaphorical form may not be synonymous; ‘the selection of
metaphor is itself a meaningful choice, and the particular metaphor selected adds
further semantic features’.
This points precisely to the problem we have when generating ‘lexis as most
delicate grammar’; how do we choose between two truly synonymous items? There
will be some semantic reasoning to set them apart, and this is the very reason why
metaphorical uses come into being – because there is a gap in the meanings of the
congruent forms that are captured by the metaphor.
The topic of grammatical metaphor is extremely broad, and a great many of its
concepts are open to discussion. A great deal of further work could have been
conducted in this general area77, and in deciding the best approach to dealing with
metaphorical TRANSITIVITY. However, for the purposes of this research, where the
77
See in particular Halliday and Matthiessen (1999) and Ravelli (1988).
227
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
focus is on a broad coverage of verb senses used in English, I have taken what might
be described from some theoretical viewpoints as a rather weak stance, in which
Process types whose meanings originate historically in a metaphorical use of
language, such as flooding in in (1) and upsetting in that upsets me are treated as
Processes in their own right. Examples have been analyzed in a ‘combinatory’
manner that makes for less use of the type of ‘dual analysis’ approach that is implied
in Halliday’s concept of grammatical metaphor.
The stance that I am taking, therefore, is that verb senses in which the
metaphor is still ‘alive’ are ‘unpacked’ to their congruent meanings, and these will be
classified under the congruent Process type. But verb senses in which the metaphor is
considered ‘dead’ are analyzed as an extended meaning of the original verb sense that
is now recognized as a verb sense in its own right. Thus, if a metaphorical extension
of a verb sense is discovered that has been created for a specific occasion, its meaning
will be ‘unpacked’ and its ‘literal’ meaning will be used, since it does not warrant a
place in the network, and it will have been tagged as ‘metaphorical’ in the PTDB.
However, if an occurrence of what may be termed ‘dead’, (or ‘fading’) metaphor
(Fawcett, forthcoming a, Chapter 2), is found, it needs to be modelled as an
established part of the language, and it is analyzed for its intended meaning.
For example, the multi-word verb catch a glimpse of is entered in the PTDB as
a verb sense, and is recognized as a mental process of perception, just as glimpse is.
This is an instance of grammatical metaphor where the verb ‘glimpse’ has been
nominalized, and is an event noun in the Process of ‘catching’. Therefore, the
congruent meaning of ‘catch’ (which would be a ‘relational’ Process of ‘possession’)
is no longer recoverable in this example.
228
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
78
The corpus used for this random selection was the COBUILD Bank of English.
229
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
6. The next sense with two roles is that which is again related to making something
available, but is not a contained space, e.g. he opened the heavy Bible. Again the
PR configuration is ‘Agent plus Affected’, and the Affected may be one of a wide
range of objects, e.g. newspapers, magazines, cervix, champagne, her nightgown,
eyes, heart, mouth, old wounds, parachute, the border, its ranks, its markets
7. Finally, the last detectable two-role sense is the opening of a closing device –
particularly the opening of a lid, e.g. opening a can of beans. This sense too has
the concept of making something available, as with the sense described in (3)
above, but here it is not container, but the lid itself. Further, sense (3) is likely to
have a one-role equivalent, whereas this sense will typically require an Agent.
230
Chapter 7: The Process Type Database (PTDB)
this was found not to be sufficient when assigning to the verb form open its various
Process Types and Participant Role configurations.
7.7 Conclusions
This chapter has been concerned with the production and analysis of a very
large body of data about verb forms and their extensions. This body of data stands at
around 5,400 entries, each of which includes an example and a gloss to identify and
illustrate the verb sense, and a full CG analysis in terms of both Process type and
Participant Role configuration.
As Chapter 8 will show, this database provides an invaluable source. With its
5,400 entries it is unique as a resource that may be consulted by those analyzing texts.
But the main purposes for which it has been developed is for the creation of delicate
system networks for TRANSITIVITY, and we shall see how this is achieved in
Chapter 8.
231
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
8.1 Introduction
This chapter is the culmination of work presented in the previous two
chapters. I have previously stated that my goal in creating a very large data base of
frequently occurring verbs, and then analysing these verb senses according to their
Process type and Participant Role configuration (as was described in Chapter 7), is to
extend the existing system networks and to create new ones so that these can
implemented in GENESYS – the sentence generation component of the
COMMUNAL project79. What is presented here, then, is the set of fully delicate
system networks that I have developed, and these illustrate the applications in
grammar-building of the data collection and analysis in the database described in
Chapter 7.
To produce system networks that reach a more delicate level than those that
already exist for the system of TRANSITIVITY, one must recognise that each lexical
verb can be specified by a detailed semantic classification, i.e. its associated semantic
features. And this, in the SF perspective, exemplifies Halliday’s (1961)
‘grammarian’s dream’ of treating lexis as ‘most delicate grammar’. As we saw in
Chapter 3, various scholars have addressed this proposal of Halliday’s. Of these
publications the broadest and deepest coverage of delicate system networks is
Tucker’s (1996a) presentation of his system networks for QUALITY. In his work
Tucker usefully surveys the positions on system networks for lexis presented by other
scholars within SFL over the years – e.g. Berry (1975), Fawcett (1980), Hasan (1987)
and Cross (1992) – and he builds on these contributions to provide full and delicate
system networks for the area of language which, in the CG, is known as QUALITY.
In this publication Tucker demonstrates the way forward for SFL to succeed in
creating delicate system networks for all areas of the language and for generating all
lexical items. Reiterating Halliday’s 1961 proposals, he states that
79
The COMMUNAL project was described in Chapter 4.
232
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
‘the goal is … to treat lexis in the same way as structure, that is, as part of one unified
system network representing the meaning potential of the language. Such a network,
with its rightmost systems concerning the realization of the more delicate distinctions
in meaning in individual lexical items, begins, I believe, to reflect Halliday’s original
notion of “most delicate grammar”.’ (Tucker, 1996:539).
In Chapter 3, Part 2 we saw Hasan’s detailed system network for one small
area of TRANSITIVITY. Hasan’s work addressed the notion of creating delicate
system networks to the point of realizing lexical items, and her work shows that the
key to producing delicate networks is establishing groups of semantically similar
verbs. The main problem in establishing such groups, however, is recognising what
types of semantic relations occur between verb senses. In the traditional lexical
semantic literature most of the recognised relations are for NOUNS. For example,
Tucker (1996:550) discusses the ISAKINDOF and ISAPARTOF relations of
hyponymy and meronymy: relations that typically hold between noun types. Here,
we are concerned with relations between Process types, and for VERBS the most
widely referred to semantic relation is the dubious relation of ‘synonymy’, as we have
seen in Fellbaum’s (1998) description of the ‘synsets’ in WordNet (Section 2.5.2 of
Chapter 2).
Another semantic relation that holds between verb senses is ‘troponymy’.
This is a relation originally recognised by Fellbaum and Miller (1990), and their
intention is that this relation should ‘distinguish a “verb hyponym” from its
superordinate’ (1990:79). In a detailed corpus study of the verb forms ‘crush’ and
‘squeeze’ (Neale 1997), I found this to be the best lexical semantic description for
semantically related verbs. This relation can be specified by the formula To V1 is to
V2 in some particular manner (Fellbaum, 1998:79). However, arising from my study
of ‘crush’ and ‘squeeze’, I suggested the usefulness of recognising a further relation
which we might call ‘bi-troponymy’, where TO ‘CRUSH’ IS TO ‘SQUEEZE’ IN SOME
233
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
marching entails walking, but walking does not entail marching. We will find the ‘bi-
troponymous’ relation proposed here holding between various pairs of verbs in the
system networks presented in this chapter.
In many others of the groupings to come out of the PTDB there seems to be
some hyponymic type of relationship between the categories, i.e. there is often a
‘superordinate’ verb sense that defines the semantic group. The CG incorporates this
relation into the system networks by using it as the entry condition to the system, and
then adding an ‘as such’ feature to generate the item itself. We shall see examples in
the full description of the system network below, e.g. in Section 8.3.2. We can also
relate some of the hypernymic features to Wierzbicka’s (1996) semantic
‘primitives’80. In a system network, however, we are not treating a hyponymic or
superordinate verb sense as a ‘primitive’, but instead as a semantic feature for
defining a group of related verb senses. The hypernym serves as a sign that leads us
to recognise a certain group-type.
Before I begin the full description of the main system networks that I have
created through this study, I would like to comment briefly on how I propose to set
out this description. The method for subcategorising Processes leads to systemic
distinctions that are very similar across different system networks, especially as the
meaning differentiations in the systems get narrower. In the description that follows
there is therefore a lot of repetition of similar system networks. For example, in the
‘action’ networks, many of the systems have an [increase] vs. [decrease] distinction,
e.g. for speed, temperature, size, etc. The verbal description of the various system
networks given in this chapter therefore attempts to avoid the repetition of too much
detail. To provide as clear and concise a description as possible I have included
diagrams for each system network section, and these are the focus of the text
description. And wherever possible I have provided references to more detailed
descriptions by other scholars.
A second point to be made before the description begins is that the
classification presented here does not claim to be entirely ‘new’. The groupings are
the way they are because this is how I – together with others who have contributed to
the classifications through published work and/or personal communication – see this
80
Wierzbicka’s (1996) primitives were previously referred to in Chapter 7.
234
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
area of language on the basis of the criteria set out in Chapters 4 and 581. There will
inevitably be some similarities to other classifications.
What I wish to highlight as new about the system networks that I am
presenting here is that they are sufficiently full to provide the relevant part of a
computational implementation of a natural language generator, i.e. the Cardiff
Grammar. And for each lexical verb that is generated from the system networks that
follow there is an associated ‘realization rule’. The role that realization rules play in
the Cardiff Grammar was introduced in Chapter 4, where I described the way that
each realization rule specifies the actual lexicogrammatical output for each lexical
item. What this then does is to specify the structural requirements of the abstract
features that have been chosen in the system network, so that these lexical items can
be expressed in the clause. The way in which they operate in these system networks
will be explained in Section 8.3.1, where we reach a suitable set of specific cases for a
regular verb, and then later in Section 8.4.4.1 we shall see how a realization rule
operates slightly differently for an irregular verb.
A second major novel aspect of these system networks is that there are no
others, so far as I am aware, that include such a high proportion of ‘multi-word verbs’
of all types. And a third novel feature of these networks is that they include estimates
of the appropriate probabilities of each feature in a system being chosen.
81
I am particularly indebted to Robin Fawcett for working with me in the early stages of developing
these classifications, and Fiona Ball and Gordon Tucker have also had a valuable input.
235
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
and then within each Process type I used the Participant Role configuration type
(Column H) as a basis for the next stage of ‘subclassification’.
This PR configuration provides no less than sixty broad categories. These
‘Process type plus PR configuration’ categories constitute the existing framework for
TRANSITIVITY in the CG, as set out in Chapter 5, which is in turn taken from
Chapter 2 of Fawcett (in preparation). My task in this chapter is to discriminate
further semantic subcategories, and so to produce delicate system networks for
generating lexical items, and this has involved teasing out the many further semantic
differences that are required to model the relations between verb senses in each of the
Process type groupings.
I used several sources that deal with similar relations to establish possible
methods for classification. My starting point was Levin (1993)82. She provides a
categorization in terms of ‘alternations’ for 3262 verb forms. I hoped that this might
serve as a guide for further categorisation within the PR configuration groups. As
described in Chapter 7, Section 7.6.1.2, I attempted to apply her ‘verb classes’ to each
verb sense in the PTDB groupings of PR configuration type. However, it transpired
that Levin’s classes, which are largely governed by the syntactic alternations into
which they enter, do not correspond anything like as closely to semantic classes as she
claims – at least not as defined in terms of their PR configurations. In other words,
Levin’s supposedly semantic classes do not match closely with the Process type and
PR type groupings of the CG, which has rendered her classifications much less useful
than I had originally hoped. Nevertheless, I inevitably found that SOME of her broad
categories match SOME of my Process type categories – for example, the well-
documented area of ‘change of state’ type verbs (see Sections 8.3.2 and 8.4.4.3 for
‘change of state’ system networks).
The other sources that I used for potential classification of the verbs in the
PTDB were Francis, Hunston and Manning’s (1996) COBUILD publication, which
classifies verbs according to the pattern of formal units they occur in; Faber and
Mairel-Uson’s (1999) Constructing a Lexicon of English Verbs, which classifies verbs
in a ‘semantic network’, based on ‘synsem (syntactic-semantic) parameters’
(1999:144); Matthiessen’s (1995) Lexicogrammatical Cartography, which includes
82
Levin’s approach to verb classification is described in more detail in Chapter 2.
236
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
tables for the categorization of verbs – though, interestingly, he does not transfer them
to system networks83, and also Halliday and Matthiessen (1999), who provide a
limited number of classification possibilities that they assign to their level of
semantics, which – for them – occurs ‘above’ the lexicogrammar. And, finally, I
consulted Roget’s Thesaurus, which, as a semantically based classification, is a
further source of useful ideas for categorization. The ‘thesaurus’ model has been
recognised in various Systemic Functional writings as a useful model, from Halliday
(1961) to the present. As Tucker (1996:536) states, ‘one interesting observation on
thesauri … is that they are organized on a basis of related meanings, a type of
organization with which a systemic functional approach to lexis has much in
common.’
The full TRANSITIVITY networks, which are presented in Appendix B, are
written according to the conventions of ‘Poplog’. Note that they are complemented
by the essential ‘realisation rules’, and that ‘probabilities’ are assigned to each
feature84. As we saw in Chapter 7, the PTDB itself contains a great deal of valuable
frequency information (though not for every entry), since it is based on the ‘second
level’ use of corpora, and from this I was able to derive useful information on
probabilities.
We can be relatively confident about the ‘guesstimate’ probabilities for the
134 most frequent verbs that COBUILD recognises, as these have an actual figure of
occurrence assigned to them in the PTDB – but within the limitations of the facts that
(1) the COBUILD figures are based on occurrences at the level of form, and (2) the
frequency information does not include occurrences of multi-word verbs. Despite
these limitations, it is clear that any category that includes one of these 134 verbs will
have a high probability.
I should point out that, while the assignment of probabilities is central for a
systemic functional model of language – and is something that the CG is striving to
realize – the task of developing a probabilistic grammar based on corpus frequencies
is very difficult, especially for the fairly full coverage that I am presenting here.
83
I say ‘interestingly’, because he is working in the systemic tradition, and so we would expect him to
present his classification in a system network.
84
The concepts of ‘realization rules’ and ‘probabilities’ in system networks were introduced in Chapter
4.
237
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Whilst the system networks presented both in this chapter and in Appendix B contain
probabilities for each system, the figures included can only be based in part on real
language examples. This is particularly clear in assigning probabilities for the non-
terminal systems.
In principle, assuming a system network of simple dependency relations, as
the system network for PROCESS TYPE largely is, it is possible to take the
frequencies of an item that expounds each terminal feature in a pathway through the
network (if these are known) and to derive from these the probabilities of the
penultimate features, and so on back to the primary system in the network. However,
at this stage of the research, when the status of the intermediate features cannot be
considered to be finalized it has been decided not to undertake this massive task.
What matters is the principle involved and where possible I have made, in
consultation with Fawcett and other colleagues in COMMUNAL, ‘guesstimates’ in
assigning probabilities, and incorporating COBUILD and other sources that include
high frequency information.
We are now ready to examine the three major Process types for which I have
produced delicate system networks. These are (1) the ‘one-role’, ‘affected only’,
‘action’ Processes, (2) the ‘two-role’, ‘agent plus affected’, ‘action’ Processes and (3)
the ‘three role’, ‘directional’, ‘relational’ Processes. Together they account for about
one third of the 5,400 verb senses in the current version of the PTDB, so the system
networks to be described here are a very large sample of the full set required in a
large, generative systemic functional grammar.
I will begin by describing the system network for ‘one-role’, ‘affected only’
‘action’ Processes.
8.3 The system network for ‘one-role’, ‘affected only’, ‘action’ Processes
Our first task is to locate this system network within the overall system
network for TRANSITIVITY in English. It is a continuation of the system network
that we saw in Chapter 6 at Figure 6.1, and it is entered by choosing ‘action’, ‘one-
role’, ‘affected only’ in that network. At this point, there are simultaneous choices in
the systems for PROCESS TYPE and PARTICIPANT ROLE. What I am presenting
here, then, is the subnetwork for the PROCESS TYPE choices.
238
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
This description will consider the whole of the system network for this Process
type, from the entry condition to the most delicate feature, i.e. to the feature from
which the lexical item itself is generated (which is always marked by the presence of
a realization rule number). As you will see, many of the verbs of this type also occur
as ‘two-role’, ‘action’ Processes (of the type ‘agent plus affected’). However, for the
reasons explained in Section 5.4.1 of Chapter 5, the two-role verb senses are modelled
in a separate system network, and we shall be reminded of these reasons by the
detailed description in Section 8.4 below. The verb forms that can occur as both one-
role and two-role senses are precisely those which are involved in what are
traditionally termed ‘ergative’ constructions, and which Fawcett, in his earlier work
(Fawcett, 1980), termed ‘affected-centred’. However, arising from work in the
current project, Fawcett has changed the way in which the CG models
TRANSITIVITY, treating these ‘one-role’ and ‘two-role’ verbs AS SEPARATE SENSES,
which must therefore be modelled in different system networks. The initial section of
the system network is presented in Figure 8.3.
239
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
5% stopping being …
90% changing (6.002323)
20% change of state 9% altering (6.002324)
as such 0.9% transforming (6.002325)
0.1% metamorphosing (6.002326)
33% of substance …
10% emission 33% of light …
34% of sound …
Figure 8.3 The early system for ‘one-role’, ‘affected only’ ‘action’ Processes
I will now take the four initial categories of this system network as typical
examples of the problem of establishing highly generalized semantic features, and
then I will describe the further classifications in the more delicate subsystems in the
system network, and ultimately the way in which they generate lexical items.
The categories in this system are arrived at by working backwards in the data
from delicacy in the categories of lexical verbs, that come at a later part of the system
network, and that this description will deal with as it progresses.
This description will now be described in turn each subsystem in this system
network for ‘one-role’, ‘affected only’ ‘action’ Processes, starting with the system for
‘stopping being’ Processes. One might at first think that ‘stopping being’ is simply
another type of ‘change of state’, but the fact that the change is to a state of ‘not
240
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
being’ warrants a separate category. And it is clear that the other two categories,
‘involuntary behaviour’ and ‘emission’ are not changes of state at all.
1% perishing (6.002224)
In this system network we can see that each terminal feature has an associated
realization rule. To exemplify how the realization rules function, we will look at the
rule 6.002221, which is attached to the feature ‘dying’. On reaching this feature in the
network, we find that its realization rule is as presented in Figure 8.3.2:
241
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
95% name of person thing / 4.999% name of social group thing / 0.001% name of place thing
Figure 8.3.2 The full realization rule for ‘dying’ (6.002221), taken from Fawcett,
Tucker and Lin (1996).
To explain further, rule 6.002221 states that the selection of the Process of
‘dying’ causes the Main Verb, which is already in the structure being generated, to be
expounded by ‘die’, and it states that this is a regular verb (‘r’)85. The rule also
specifies that if, in the same pass through the network, the choice ‘affected unmarked’
has been made, then the following preferences should be applied to all choices. The
rule also states what type of ‘thing’ can be expected to occur with the Process ‘die’,
and this will govern the choices to be made on the re-entry to the system network for
the generation of ‘thing’ to be realized in the clause.
I should perhaps add that the verb senses in the system network for ‘stopping
being’ Processes might seem at first to contrast directly with a group that might have
as their entry condition ‘starting being’, (for example ‘being born’, ‘arising’,
‘emerging’, ‘erupting’, ‘evolving’, ‘forming’, ‘materializing’, ‘resulting’, ‘appearing’,
‘blowing up’ and ‘breaking out’).
However, the ‘starting being’ group of Processes are in fact ‘created only’
Processes, because they all pass the test for a Created PR (see Appendix C), which is
‘What came into being was x’. And the ‘stopping being’ Processes pass the test for
85
The realization rule for an irregular verb is slightly different, and an example of such a rule is given
in Section 8.4.4.1.
242
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
an Affected PR, which is ‘what happened to x was that …’, and are thus generated in
this system network.
86
Francis, Hunston and Manning (1996:5) has a category called the ‘Change Group’, which involves
the verb change and some others that you will find in the present system network, but it has a less
broad coverage than that found here. Levin (1993:240) uses the terms ‘change of state’ for one of her
categories of verbs, and within this she and I have several similar sub-category types (for example,
‘cooking’ and ‘breaking’). However I arrived at these category types not by simply borrowing from
243
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
very tentative and partial system network … for the cultural classification of
‘affected-centred’ processes in English’ (1980:153). While his proposals may have
been tentative, they have provided a useful basic system network for the present
development of this area of TRANSITIVITY.
The first subcategory in the ‘change of state’ section of the network involves a
distinction between ‘change of state as such’ verbs and ‘change of state specified’.
This type of ‘as such’ feature is used throughout the CG system networks for ‘cultural
classification’; that is, the system in the grammar that leads to the term that is
accepted by the culture to classify a ‘Thing’, a ‘Process’ or a ‘Quality’. 87 The ‘as
such’ wording is used as part of the name for the features in the network that must be
included in order to both (a) generate equivalent lexical items, and (b) serve as a
superordinate category for generating further, more semantically delicate items.
Thus, the feature ‘change of state as such’ enables the ‘superordinate’ Processes of
change to be generated: ‘changing’, ‘altering’, ‘transforming’ and ‘metamorphosing’.
In contrast, the feature ‘change of state specified’ leads to further systems for choices
between more semantically specific and so more delicate Processes of ‘change’ – as
we shall see in the next system.
Levin (1993), but through the data and through the division of the CG Process type categories. So, for
example, I have two cooking categories to represent the division of Process type categories: one in this
‘one-role’, ‘affected only’ Process type, and one in the ‘two-role’, ‘agent plus affected’ Process type.
87
See Fawcett 1980:151f, 217f for his early system networks for the cultural classification of
Processes.
244
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Once again we find the use of ‘as such’ in a feature. The feature ‘moving as
such’ has a realization rule attached to it, which specifies that if this choice is made
then the Main Verb in the clause will be expounded by the item ‘move’.
The four other subsystems in this system are for a choice between various
verbs of ‘one-role’, ‘change by moving’. These verbs typically have another sense
which involves two roles, but the ‘one-role’, ‘affected only’ sense of these verbs
allows for the expression of movement without an Agent, and so involves the type of
verb sense expressed in Example (1):
Figure 8.3.4 The system network for ‘moving relative to opening in enclosure’
Processes
The ‘moving relative to upright state’ Processes are presented in Figure 8.3.5,
and they allow for examples such as (2).
245
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
1% capsizing (6.002415)
moving relative to upright state 40% overturning (6.002416)
40% toppling (6.002417)
19% overbalancing (6.002419)
Figure 8.3.5 The system network for ‘moving relative to upright state’ Processes
The ‘moving relative to straight line’ Processes are presented in Figure 8.3.6,
and they allow for examples such as (3).
246
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Many of the verb forms generated through this section of the ‘one-role’,
‘affected only’ system network will also appear in the ‘directional’ Processes system
network, and we shall see part of this network in Section 8.5.
88
While they claim that they are describing a ‘semantic’ level that is higher than the ‘meaning
potential’ of the lexicogrammar, it is hard to see why this additional layer of networks is required.
247
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
89
Indeed, it has two ‘affected only’ senses, as we shall see in 8.3.2.1.4.
248
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
‘caving in’ and ‘decomposing’. But there is no sense of ‘falling apart’ that has two
roles, as Example 1 shows.
249
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Figure 8.3.8.1 The system network for ‘change resulting in disintegration’ Processes.
250
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
90
From here on, I shall not reproduce the final system networks unless they involve a point on which I
wish to comment, because they supply duplicate systems given in Appendix B.
251
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
moving’ (40%) and ‘change resulting in disintegration’ (15%) systems. But it is not
as low a frequency as some of the others we will encounter below, since it contains
some fairly high frequency verb senses. For example, Francis, Hunston and Manning
(1996) class the verb burn (which here is in the system network for ‘change from
solid to gas’) as a ‘4 diamond’ frequency. This means that the verb FORM occurs
between 11,216 and 32,885 times in a corpus of 250 million words91, which is their
second most frequent class.
Unfortunately, it is not possible to assess from Francis, Hunston and
Manning’s (1996) documentation what proportion of these occurrences would be the
‘one-role’, ‘affected only’ sense.
91
See Section 6.6.3 of Chapter 6 for the whole of the COBUILD classification band frequency
information, which I obtained from Dr Susan Hunston (personal communication).
252
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
253
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
92
See Section 8.4.3.2.3 for an example of a SP rule.
254
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Processes ‘cooling down’ and ‘chilling’, and so to a choice between the equivalent
lexical verbs.
255
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
256
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
49.5% improving (6.002823)
50% favourable 50% getting better (6.002824)
0.5% ameliorating (6.002825)
evaluative change
50% worsening (6.002826)
50% unfavourable
50% deteriorating (6.002827)
20% psychological …
10% suffocating af …
257
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
5% sneezing (6.002834)
2% sniffling (6.002835)
2% snoring (6.002836)
2% wheezing (6.002837)
25% respiratory 2% yawning (6.002838)
40% breathing (6.002841)
specific physiological 40% coughing (6.002842)
2% exhaling (6.002848)
2% inhaling (6.002849)
The first sub-system allows a choice between ‘digestion’ verb senses belch,
burp, hiccup, fart, blow off and let off. The second sub-system allows a choice
between ‘respiratory’ verb senses sneeze, sniffle, snore, yawn, wheeze, breathe,
cough, exhale and inhale. The third sub-system leads to ‘outward appearance’ verb
senses blush, flush, sweat and perspire. And the fourth sub-system leads to ‘bodily
substance emission’ verb senses dribble, drool, bleed, vomit and puke. As stated in
the introduction to this chapter, the probabilities are set for a ‘casual’, ‘spoken’
register, and thus the last two Processes in the bodily substance emission system
network – ‘puking’ and ‘vomiting’ – are equally probable. However, the feature
‘bodily substance emission’ has a ‘same pass’ rule attached to it that states that if on
the same pass, the register preference of ‘formal’, or ‘written’ has been chosen, then
the probabilities would be changed to make ‘puking’ very unlikely to be chosen.
258
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Figure 8.3. 16 The system network for ‘internal bodily movement’ Processes.
The next system generates Processes that are semantically related in that they
all pertain to involuntary ‘internal bodily movement’. This system leads to quake,
quiver, shake, shiver, shudder, tremble, writhe and twitch, and (though it probably
259
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
occurs more frequently in a two-role Process) convulse. Figure 8.3.16 shows this
system network.
In this system network, the probabilities are set to prefer ‘shaking’, and then
‘trembling’ over all the other Processes. This is an example of where the probabilities
directly reflect the frequency information provided by Francis et al (1996). Counter
to our intuitions, which might suggest that ‘shiver’ occurs more frequently that
‘tremble’, the bands of frequency that Francis et al assign on the basis of the
COBUILD corpus are as follows: ‘convulsing’: no band; ‘quaking’: no band;
‘quivering’: band 1; ‘shaking’: band 4; ‘shivering’: band 1; ‘shuddering’: band 1;
‘trembling’: band 2; ‘writhing’: band 1; ‘twitching’: band 193.
93
For the actual figures for each COBUILD band, see Section 7.3.3 of Chapter 7
260
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
One might at first expect that this progression towards ‘sleeping’ should
logically be followed by the Processes that describe ‘state of sleeping’. However, the
PR involved in such Processes is not in fact an Affected entity, because the Process
does not ‘happen to’ the PR, but instead described the state in which the PR is. The
Processes ‘sleeping’, ‘catnapping’, ‘dozing’, ‘drowsing’, ‘napping’, ‘slumbering’ and
‘snoozing’ will all be found, therefore, in the system network for ‘one role’, ‘carrier
only’ Processes (which is not described in detail in this thesis).
261
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
of substance …
emission
of sound …
262
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
8.4 The system network for ‘two-role’, ‘agent plus affected’, ‘action’ Processes
The next system network for TRANSITIVITY to be described now is also
located within the ‘action’ Process network – but it generates ‘two-role’ Processes
that have both an Agent and an Affected. A very large number of Processes are
generated through this network, and it is the most frequent Process type in database.
Of the 5000 verb senses in the current version of the PTDB almost 1300 of them are
‘action’ Processes that have an Agent and an Affected94. Thus, approaching one
quarter of the entire verb senses in the data are generated through this system
network, so that the task of creating a system network that accommodates them all in
94
I should point out that I cannot claim that 25% of Processes in English are of this type, because the
development of system networks for any area of meaning leads naturally to the recognition of other
systemically related Process types than those we initially found in the PTDB.
263
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
appropriate relationships to each other has been by far the biggest challenge of the
present research.
In the PTDB there is a clear split between two major semantic types within
this large number of ‘two-role’ Processes with the features ‘agent’ plus ‘affected’.
This split is between (1) ‘social action’ type verbs and (2) ‘material action’ type
verbs, and it is represented by two of the features in the initial system for this
network. However, two other choices allow for a smaller number of verb senses.
These two other features are separate from ‘social action’ and ‘material action’
because they are non-specific to these two types of classification. The initial system
is as presented in Figure 8.4.1.
Figure 8.4.1 The initial system network for ‘two-role’, ‘agent plus affected’ Processes
It would, of course, have been possible to have a system in which the entry
condition [two role, agent plus affected] led to a choice between [affecting as such]
and [affecting specified], with [affecting as such] leading to a further choice between
[affecting general] and [not affecting], and with [affecting specified] leading to a
choice between [social action] and [material action]. However, as at other points in
the TRANSITIVITY network, I have opted to cut out this possible initial system, so
reducing the number of features generated on each pass through the network.
I will describe in turn the subnetwork to which each feature leads, and, as we
shall see, the ‘social action’ and the ‘material action’ systems will include by far the
largest number of verb senses.
264
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
I should point out that the sense of ‘hit’ generated in this system network is
not the main sense of ‘hit’ (which is a two-role, agent plus affected Process, with
intention, as in Section 8.4.4.1, Figure 8.4.5).
265
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
35% visiting …
5% social encountering …
social action 35% social action by empowered person
10% abusing …
2% interrupting event …
5% supporting …
0.5% playing with (6.004201)
0.5% using (6.004202)
80% general social action 0.5% cursing (6.004203)
3% blaming (6.004204)
0.5% guiding (6.004205)
0.5% taming (6.004206)
0.5% sticking_up (6.004207)
0.5% holding_up (6.004208)
0.5% spoiling (6.004209)
0.5% giving_away (6.004210)
0.5% calming (6.004211)
266
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
‘Starting couple’ Processes include ‘asking out’, ‘proposing to’ and ‘proposing
marriage to’. All of these Processes are realized by multi-word verbs, and these
therefore require more complex realization rules. For example, the realization rule
attached to the feature proposing marriage to will specify (1) that the Main Verb will
be expounded by propose, (2) that a Main Verb Extension (MEx) will be generated
which will be filled by a nominal group (ngp) with only a head, i.e. marriage, and (3)
that a Complement will be generated and filled by a prepositional group in which the
preposition is to, the completive of which has the PR of Affected conflated with it, as
in Figure 8.4.495.
Cl
S/Ag M MEx C
ngp pgp
h p cv/Af
ngp
The system for ‘within couple’ leads both to verb senses and to further
subsystems. The Processes that are generated include ‘embracing’, ‘getting off with’
and ‘going with’. And the further systems are for the verbs of ‘hugging’, ‘cuddling’,
‘kissing’ and ‘having sex with’. The systems for ‘hugging’, ‘cuddling’ and ‘kissing’
all lead to further systems for (1) the verb senses ‘as such’ (i.e. the meaning of the
feature with no further specification), (2) the other verb senses that refer to the action
in a narrower way (e.g. necking, snogging). These include the multi-word verb senses
that are realized as the reified Processes of giving a hug/cuddle/kiss. The realization
rules for each of these multi-word verbs will be roughly similar to the realization rule
for proposing marriage to, i.e. they generate (1) a Main Verb and (2) a Main Verb
Extension (MEx), filled by a nominal group that expresses a reified (and so
95
The composition of such realization rules are provided for in COMMUNAL in the complete version
of the Cardiff Grammar. See Tucker (1996b) for a recent account of how they work.
267
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
268
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
35% visiting …
5% social encountering …
2%religious domain …
5% criminal domain …
Figure 8.4.3.1 The system network for ‘General Social Action’ type verbs
269
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
This is a semantic grouping of verb senses in which the uttering of the speech
act (within certain constraints) actually BRINGS ABOUT real world happenings, and the
title of Austin’s 1962 publication, ‘How to do things with words’, reflects this. The
notion of performative verbs led me to recognise that there are social domains within
which an authorised person may ‘bring about’ a happening through language that is
referred to by a verb sense. These areas function as subsystems in the network and
are presented in Figure 8.4.3.1.
The ‘religious domain’ system is for the Processes ‘blessing’, ‘christening’,
‘baptizing’, ‘confirming’, ‘marrying’ and ‘crowning’.
The ‘employment domain’ system leads to the Processes ‘sacking’, ‘letting
go’, ‘laying off’ and ‘making redundant’. The realization rule for ‘sacking’ states that
if ‘American English’ has been chosen in the system for DIALECT, then the Main
270
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Verb should be realized as fire, but if ‘British English’ has been chosen in the system
for DIALECT, then the Main Verb should be realized as sack. This reflects the view
that there is no difference in meaning between ‘sacking someone’ and ‘firing
someone’. But if it was later found that there was some minor difference, two verb
senses (‘sacking’ and ‘firing’) could be reorganised in the system, and a ‘same pass’
conditional rule could be attached to ‘employment domain’ stating ‘If on same pass
American English then M < fire, else M < sack’.
The system for ‘legal domain’ includes the Processes ‘policing’, ‘arresting’,
‘pulling in’, ‘cautioning’, ‘charging’, ‘trying’, ‘fining’, ‘framing’ and ‘shutting up’,
and the system also includes a further feature which is ‘imprisoning as such’. This
leads to a sub-system that allows for the Processes ‘sending to prison’, ‘sending
down’ and ‘jailing’.
The Process ‘sending to prison’ is a ‘reified process’, and is thus generated as
a multi-word verb in this system. This contrasts with the most typical sense of the
verb form ‘send’, which is as a ‘directional’ Process with three PRs (someone sends
something somewhere). However, this ‘imprisoning’ verb sense is a multi word verb,
and is thus a ‘two-role’, ‘Agent plus Affected’, as the analysis in Example (3) shows:
(3) The judge (Agent) sent (Main verb) Lord Archer (Affected) to prison
(Main Verb Extension).
The next sub-system choice is ‘medical domain’ verbs. These Processes are
‘attending’, ‘treating’, ‘curing’ and ‘healing’.
The ‘military domain’ system leads to the Processes ‘attacking’, ‘defending’,
‘opening fire on’, ‘besieging’, ‘invading’, ‘occupying’, ‘seizing’, ‘taking’, ‘taking
prisoner’, ‘conquering’ and ‘defeating’.
The system for ‘leadership domain’ leads to the Processes ‘commanding’,
‘leading’, ‘governing’, ‘taxing’, ‘punishing’ and ‘rewarding’. The Processes
‘civilizing’ and ‘modernizing’ can also be placed here.
The ‘criminal domain’ system leads to the Processes ‘stealing’, ‘robbing’,
‘holding up’, ‘doing over’ and ‘burgling’.
The final sub-system in the ‘social action by empowered person’ system
network is that of ‘abusing’, and this leads to a choice between ‘physically abusing’
271
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
and ‘verbally abusing’. The ‘physically abusing’ system leads to the Processes
‘bothering’, ‘stalking’, ‘molesting’, ‘raping’, ‘assaulting’ and ‘abusing’.
The ‘verbally abusing’ system leads to the Processes ‘teasing’, ‘taking the
mickey out of’, ‘abusing’, ‘scolding’, ‘threatening’, ‘criticizing’, ‘tearing into’,
‘putting down’, ‘talking down to’, ‘running down’, ‘laying into’, ‘knocking’, ‘getting
on at’, ‘getting at’, ‘getting back at’, ‘doing down’, ‘being on at’, ‘being hard on’,
‘being at’ and ‘standing up to’. It is interesting to note how many of these frequently
used Processes are multi-word verbs, and thus, how important it is for the model to be
able to account for these such Processes, as was described in Section 7.5 of Chapter 7.
The Processes that this system generates are mostly the metaphorised use of a
verb that has another sense. Compare, for example (5) and (6).
(5) They accidentally knocked the doorframe when moving the wardrobe.
(6) I’m not knocking him for doing it.
96
This was described in Chapter 5, Section 5.6.4.
272
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
The next major subsystem of the ‘general social action’ system network is the
system for generating ‘interrupting event’ type Processes. This system leads to three
choices, and these are ‘interrupting as such’ (which generates interrupt as the Main
verb); break off (which is then generated as the Main verb) and a further system of
‘interrupting discourse’. Like the ‘verbal abuse’ Processes above, the Processes
generated in this system involve verbal communication – but again they are not
‘mental’ Processes, since the PRs involved pass the tests for Agent and Affected. The
Processes that the system for ‘interrupting discourse’ generates are ‘breaking in on’;
‘breaking into’; ‘cutting into’; ‘cutting off’ and ‘cutting short’.
Finally, the last subsystem in the ‘general social action’ system is the
‘supporting’ system. This leads to a choice between ‘physically supporting’ and
‘verbally supporting’.
The Processes included in the ‘verbally supporting’ system are ‘speaking up
for’, ‘speaking for’ and ‘encouraging’.
The ‘physically supporting’ Processes are ‘supporting’, ‘backing up’,
‘standing up’, ‘standing up for’, ‘sticking by’, ‘sticking to’, ‘sticking up for’, ‘getting
behind’, ‘accommodating’, ‘looking after’, ‘bringing up’, ‘caring for’, ‘catering for’,
‘taking care of’, ‘tending’ and ‘nursing’.
Some of these verb senses stem from a concrete and very physical action, and
thus the verb form support will also occur in the system network for ‘material action’,
‘agent plus range’97. But the metaphorical extensions of these verb senses have
become more current in everyday usage for describing social situations, and so we can
recognise a separate semantic group and generate them through a separate system
network. Nevertheless, we should not forget that because the FORMS are the same, the
same realization rules will be used in both this system and where they occur in the
‘material action’ system for ‘agent plus range’.
97
In a typical example such as The single pillar supported the whole roof, ‘the roof’ does not pass the
Affected test, and is therefore a Range.
273
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
semantic group that Halliday (1994) terms ‘material’ Processes. However, as we have
seen, the Cardiff Grammar replaces Halliday’s term ‘material’ with ‘action’ as the
superordinate term, because the same Participant Roles of Agent and Affected are
found in both ‘social action’ and ‘material action’.
Figure 8.4.4 presents the initial choices in the ‘material action’ system
network. The features in this initial system for ‘material action’ emerged from the
grouping of Processes of this type into semantically coherent groups, and therefore
these categories emerged from the bottom-up description that was described in
Chapter 7.
10% preparing …
material action
5% ingestion …
2.5% concealment …
5% using …
Figure 8.4.4 The initial choices in the system network for ‘material action’.
274
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
20% destroying …
20% hitting …
20% killing …
5% hurting …
affecting by contact
10% touching …
10% breaking …
2% mending …
8% cutting …
5% material encountering …
We can compare the subcategories in this system network with four of Levin’s
(1993) Verb Classes: ‘break’ verbs, ‘cut’ verbs, ‘touch’ verbs and ‘hit’ verbs. She
recognises these separate groups according to the different alternations they take,
which can be expressed in the matrix in Figure 8.4.5.1.
Figure 8.4.5.1 Levin’s classes and alternations for a set of verb types.
275
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
network, in the sub-system of ‘affecting by contact’ that has ‘cutting’ as its entry
condition.
The subsystem for generating ‘destroying’ verb senses contrasts with the ‘one-
role’, ‘affected only’ subsystem of ‘total disintegration/non-total disintegration’,
which we saw in Section 8.3.2.1.3. There we noted that Processes of the type ‘total
disintegration’ can ONLY occur with one PR, whereas the Processes in the ‘non-total
disintegration’ system can occur with both a single PR sense (‘affected only’) and a
two PR sense (‘agent plus affected’). It is here, among the ‘destroying’ Processes that
we find the two-role equivalents of ‘non-total disintegration’.
In the group of ‘destroying’ verbs, many of the verb forms function with two
separate PR configurations, whereas some verb forms can ONLY function with ‘two-
roles’ of ‘Agent plus Affected’. To illustrate the point, consider the verb destroy. It
can only function as in (1) and not as in (2). This Process necessarily requires an
Agent to bring about the happening that is ‘destroying’.
As I pointed out in Section 5.4.1 of Chapter 5, we have decided, in the CG, not
to try to capture the generalisation that Levin terms the ‘causative/inchoative’
alternation (1993:27) as a single system, and thus we no longer make the agent-
centred / affected-centred distinction that Fawcett made in his 1980 network.
However, the fact that some of these verbs (such as destroy) occur only with two PRs
and some with one or two (such as shatter) is still reflected in the grammar through
the fact that the two features that generate the two Process types share the same
realization rule number.
The Processes in this system are ‘destroying’, ‘kicking down’, ‘kicking in’,
‘knocking down’, ‘knocking out’, ‘pulling down’, ‘wrecking’, ‘ruining’, ‘spoiling’,
‘tearing down’, ‘wasting’, ‘tearing apart’, ‘blowing up’, ‘breaking down’, ‘breaking
up’, ‘burning’, ‘crushing’, ‘exploding’, ‘bursting’, ‘sinking’, ‘washing away’ and
‘washing down’.
The next system for generating ‘affecting by contact’ verbs is the ‘hitting’
system. This leads to a number of verb senses, which can be described as
‘troponyms’ (Fellbaum and Miller, 1990), of ‘hitting’, in line with the discussion in
276
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
MANNER. This system could perhaps be taken to a further level of delicacy in which
each verb sense is generated in terms of the manner in which the ‘hitting’ takes place.
Examples of the Processes generated in this system are ‘hitting’, ‘hitting at’, ‘hitting
out at’, ‘beating’, ‘biting’, ‘cutting at’, ‘crashing against’, ‘hammering at’, ‘knocking
about’, ‘pushing’, ‘pinching’, ‘running through’, ‘shooting away’, ‘striking’ and
‘whipping’.
As this system network includes the irregular verb ‘beat’, this seems to be an
appropriate point to provide an example realization rule for an irregular verb. The
realization rule is as follows:
In Section 8.3.1, the realization rule ‘6.002221’ was presented, and here we
saw that BASIC TYPICALLY LIVING THING PREF BLOCK and TYPICALLY
LIVING THING CC PREF BLOCK are a means for specifying what ‘thing’ or
‘things’ will typically occur with the Process. This difference with this realization
rule for ‘beating’ is that it specifies ‘irr’, and so is marked as ‘irregular’, and it leads
us to the general ‘irregular verb subrule table’. This, in turn, leads us to the entry for
the irregular verb ‘beat’, as displayed in Figure 8.4.5.2 (which is taken from a table of
many hundreds of such entries, these being derived from Quirk et al, 1985).
The next subsystem is for ‘killing’ type verbs. For Francis, Hunston and
Manning (1996:19), the ‘kill’ group of verbs include ‘verbs concerned with harming,
breaking, attacking, or destroying something or someone’. So for Francis, Hunston
and Manning (1996), the ‘kill’ group is a large and semantically diverse group. This
reinforces the sense that among the very large number of Processes which are
277
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
generated in the system of ‘affecting by contact’, the ‘kill’ type Processes constitutes
a subsystem. I am introducing a smaller group than Francis, Hunston and Manning
suggest for those Processes that specifically refer to ‘killing’. And this subsystem is
for verbs that denote particular ways of ‘ending life’ by ‘affecting by contact’. Some
might perhaps argue that, if the ‘physical abuse’ type verbs are generated through the
system of social action (as we saw in Section 8.4.3.2), then the ‘killing’ type verbs
should be as well. But Francis, Hunston and Manning’s (1996) ‘kill’ group of verbs
are clearly, like mine, ‘material action’ (e.g. break, hit, cut, etc.). In contrast they do
not include the senses that I class as ‘physical abuse’ (e.g. bother, stalk, molest, rape,
assault, abuse and set on) as part of this group98.
The verb senses that realize the Process of ‘killing’, and which are generated
through this subsystem, are as follows: assassinate, choke, drown, hang, kill, kill off,
knock off, murder, shoot, suffocate, do in, do away with, bump off, burn to death,
take out and put down. Within this group there is a further division between a first
group of verb senses that are troponyms of ‘killing’, and a second group of verb
senses that are euphemisms for an event that it is culturally ‘difficult’ to refer to.
The next subsystem of ‘two-role’ ‘agent plus affected’ verb senses is that of
‘hurting’. One might be tempted to think that these Processes involve some degree of
‘perception’, on the grounds that an action can only be defined as ‘hurting’ if the
Affected entity perceives it as such. However, this ‘perception’ – if that is what it is –
is caused by a ‘material action’ by an Agent, and the PRs involved pass the Agent and
the Affected tests. For example, ‘What X did was to hurt/injure/wound the child’ and
‘What happened to the child was that X hurt/injured/wounded it.’
The Processes that are generated through this system are ‘burning’, ‘cutting’,
‘harming’, ‘hurting’, ‘injuring’, ‘putting out’, ‘dislocating’, ‘skinning’, ‘blinding’,
‘rubbing’, ‘scratching’, ‘scraping’, ‘stinging’, ‘wounding’, ‘twisting’ and ‘winding’.
Next we come to the subsystem for ‘touching’ verb senses. So far, the
subsystems that we have seen in the system network for ‘affecting by contact’ have all
involved the semantics of violence, for example, ‘destroying’, ‘hitting’, ‘killing’. The
‘touching’ subsystem leads to verb senses that are typically non-violent but still
involve some contact. Examples of ‘touching’ Processes are: ‘being at’ (in the sense
98
In fact, it is not clear how they would classify this group of semantically similar verb senses.
278
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
279
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
However, even though the tests sound strange, it is difficult to suggest what
this Process might be in not ‘action’ and ‘agent plus affected’. There is another sense
of ‘missing’ that is a ‘mental’ Process with the roles ‘emoter’ and a ‘phenomenon’.
Examples (1) and (2) are much more acceptable if they are interpreted in a context
where they would be analysed as realizing the ‘emotion’ sense of the verb.
280
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
4% change by cooking …
2% changing consistency …
6% changing quality …
2% changing number …
2% changing temperature …
change of state
2% changing speed …
2% changing strength …
2% changing fullness …
2% changing appearance …
2% changing consciousness …
2% changing dryness …
6% shaping …
10% developing …
281
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Figure 8.4.7 The system network for ‘evaluative change of state’ Processes
99
Note here that the realization rule for ‘cooking’ is the same as the realization rule in the ‘one role’,
‘affected only’ system.
282
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
283
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
The next system for ‘changing flavour’ leads to the Processes ‘salting’,
‘sweetening’, ‘browning’ and ‘flavouring’.
And the last ‘changing quality’ subsystem is for generating two verb senses of
the type ‘changing sharpness’, and these are Processes are ‘sharpening’ and
‘blunting’.
284
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
‘hastening’ and ‘hurrying’, and the system for ‘decreasing speed’ leading to the
Processes ‘slowing down’, ‘decelerating’ and ‘slowing up’.
285
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
286
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
2% bathing (6.004610)
2% batheing (6.004611)
58% washing (6.004612)
30% bodily preparing 2% dressing (6.004613)
2% undressing (6.004614)
2% shaving (6.004615)
20% showering (6.004616)
5% skinning (6.004631)
5% beating (6.004632)
5% bottling (6.004633)
5% buttering (6.004634)
5% dressing (6.004635)
10% food preparing 10% grating (6.004636)
10% peeling (6.004637)
10% slicing (6.004638)
5% smoking (6.004639)
5% shelling (6.0046310)
10% whipping (6.0046311)
10% whisking (6.0046312)
5% liquidizing (6.0046313)
5% stuffing (6.0046314)
5% mincing (6.0046315)
287
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
The first subsystem is for the various general senses of ‘preparing’. These
Processes are ‘preparing’, ‘binding’, ‘breaking in’, ‘composing’, ‘curing’, ‘dressing’,
‘setting out’, ‘laying out’ and ‘putting out’.
The second subsystem is for verb senses that are specific, and refer to
preparing types that involve the body. The feature for ‘bodily preparing’ has a ‘same
pass’ rule attached to it that changes probabilities, as described in Section 4.2.4 of
Chapter 4. This rule changes the probabilities in the PARTICIPANT ROLE system to
allow for the likelihood of the ‘Affected’ entity typically being covert when it refers
to the same entity as the Subject/Agent, as in He hasn’t showered yet.
The Processes in this system are ‘bathing’, ‘batheing’, ‘washing’, ‘dressing’,
‘undressing’, ‘shaving’, and ‘showering’.
This system also has a subsystem which leads to ‘body-part specified’
Processes, and these Processes are ‘brushing’, ‘combing’ and ‘flossing’, which
necessarily involve either ‘hair’ or ‘teeth’ as the Affected entity.
The third subsystem is that of ‘domestic preparation’, which includes some of
the same verb FORMS as the ‘bodily preparing’ system. However, this system leads to
a different SENSE of the form. Thus, the verb wash has the same realization rule in
both systems, but must be generated in both systems because there are two senses of
wash. This is born out by the fact that the ‘domestic preparing’ sense of wash does
not need a SP rule attached to it in the system, as the default probabilities for the
Affected entity are suitable for this system.
The Processes generated through this system are ‘brushing’, ‘cleaning’,
‘cleaning up’, ‘clearing’, ‘clearing out’, ‘clearing up’, ‘doing out’, ‘hoovering’,
‘polishing’, ‘rubbing’, ‘shining’, ‘washing’, ‘dusting’, ‘wiping’, ‘wiping out’, ‘wiping
up’, ‘wiping down’, ‘washing out’, ‘washing up’, ‘washing down’, ‘sweeping’,
‘sweeping out’, and ‘sweeping up’.
The final ‘preparing’ subsystem is ‘food preparing’. These Processes form a
specific set because they all have very specific meanings pertaining to food
preparation. And while a verb such as slicing may be used in other domains, the
decision was taken that its core sense is most typically some kind of food preparation,
and the other uses are semi-metaphorical extensions of this.
The Processes generated through this system are ‘skinning’, ‘beating’,
‘bottling’, ‘buttering’, ‘dressing’, ‘grating’, ‘peeling’, ‘slicing’, ‘smoking’, ‘shelling’,
‘whipping’, ‘whisking’, ‘liquidizing’, ‘stuffing’ and ‘mincing’.
288
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
289
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
express a ‘direction’, of the type Destination, Source or Path. It will become clear as
we explore the system for ‘changing position’ verb senses that the roles involved with
these Processes are Agent and Affected, and not ‘direction’.
Figure 8.4.10 The system network for generating ‘changing position’ Processes.
290
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
And the final subsystem in the ‘change position’ system network is ‘with
inherent direction’, which leads to a choice between ‘typically vehicle wise’ and
‘typically pursuit wise’. The system for ‘typically vehicle wise’ leads directly to the
single verb sense back up, which is the sense of this verb that is used exclusively for
vehicle-like Affected entities. The system for ‘typically pursuit wise’ leads to the
Processes ‘pursuing as such’, ‘getting after’, ‘following’, ‘going after’, ‘tracking’,
‘running after’, ‘coming after’, ‘coming for’ ‘making after’ and perhaps ‘being after’.
The system ‘beginning to use’ currently leads to only one Process; ‘breaking
into’, as in He broke into the sugar, so may well be redundant. The system of ‘using
as such’ leads to the Processes of ‘using’, ‘drawing on’, ‘running down’, ‘running
off’, ‘running on’ and ‘swallowing up’. And the system of ‘using completely’ leads
to ‘using up’, ‘getting through’, ‘finishing’, ‘finishing up’, ‘finishing off’, ‘being
through’ and ‘running out of’.
100
Another valuable source of frequency information is Leech, Rayson and Wilson (2001), whose
publication of word frequency lists is based on the British National Corpus, and which records the verb
‘use’ to occur 1071 times per 1 million words, which is a much larger figure than found in the
COBUILD corpus.
291
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
8.5 The system network for ‘three role’, ‘directional’, ‘relational’ Processes
In Chapter 5, Section 5.5, I introduced the relatively recent addition to the
Cardiff Grammar system for TRANSITIVITY that the next section will present in
detail: the ‘directional’ Process type. This Process type has so far not been publicly
documented in the literature about the Cardiff Grammar101, however, I should state
that the system network described in this section is based on the system network
developed by Fawcett for the GENESYS lexicogrammar in the COMMUNAL project
in 1989. This unpublished network is the implementation of Fawcett’s 1987 model,
and therefore does not include ‘directional’ Processes, but instead includes ‘change of
location’ type Processes. However, despite the change in terminology, the same
semantic space is covered in the present system network.
While the system network that I will describe in this section builds on
Fawcett’s original system network, it extends it significantly, so demonstrating how
corpus based data that represents a full coverage of this frequently used area of
meaning enables us to produce a much more delicate classification, and so a more
complete system network.
101
The most in-depth description of the Cardiff Grammar model for ‘relational’ Processes is Fawcett
(1987), which includes ‘directional’ Processes with ‘locational’ ones; however, this is soon to be
replaced with Fawcett (forthcoming a).
292
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
In the CG, the PRs are generated in a separate system network to the
PROCESS TYPE network102, with which we have so far been concerned in Sections
8.3 and 8.4. For the three-role Process that we are now considering, we will for a
moment step outside the PROCESS TYPE system network (i.e. generating meaning
realized in the lexis) to consider how the third PR of ‘direction’ is to be generated (i.e.
generating meaning realized in the structure).
Figure 8.5 reminds us which part of the system network for TRANSITIVITY
we are currently considering. The choices we have made to reach this point are
[relational], [directional] and [directional plus third party agent].
directional,
plus third agent affected-carrier
party agent unmarked unmarked …
agent affected-carrier
sought sought …
source-destination
two directions path-destination
source-path
source-path-destination
Figure 8.5 The system network for generating the Participant Role of ‘direction’.
Figure 8.5 illustrates how the lower part of the PARTICIPANT ROLES
system network provides for the PR type ‘direction’. What is not specified in Figure
8.5, however, is that each choice of direction type (either one, two or three directions,
of the three types as shown in the network) leads to a further network where ‘overt
direction’ or ‘covert direction’ must be chosen. And if ‘overt direction’ is chosen,
then the next choice is between ‘deictically recoverable place’, which leads to re-entry
102
The overall system for TRANSITIVITY in the CG is presented in Chapter 6, and this area of the
network for PR and PROCESS TYPE is covered there.
293
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
to the system network to generate here, over there, in the corner, etc, (which are
treated as a type of NOMINAL GROUP), or ‘direction specified’, which leads to re-entry
to the system network to generate a PREPOSITIONAL GROUP such as to London. As you
can see, the upper part of the Participant Roles system network consists of a system
that is essentially the same as the one for Agent and Affected that we encountered in
Section 6.3 of Chapter 6. Here, however, the choices between ‘sought’, ‘referring
out’ and ‘covert’ relate to an Agent and Affected-Carrier, so that a typically product
of this system network is Example 1.
Our focus here, however, is on the network for PROCESS TYPE, so I shall
not discuss the PARTICIPANT ROLES further.
We can now turn to the system for generating Processes of the type
‘directional plus third party agent’.
8.5.1 The initial choices in the system network for generating ‘three-role’,
‘directional’, ‘relational’ Processes
The system begins with a simultaneous choice, as shown in Figure 8.5.1.
Figure 8.5.1 The initial choices in the ‘three role, directional’ Process system
network
294
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Let us consider the lower two systems first. The purpose of the system ‘return
to original location’ vs. ‘no return to original location’ is to generate ‘back’ as a Main
Verb Extension (MEx), as in example (1).
103
See Chapter 6, Section 6.1.2 for a description of simultaneity and dependency in the system
network.
295
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
We turn now to the modelling of preferences for the higher network, i.e. the
one that specifies the Main Verb. Nearly all the options in this network require a SP
rule that states what the preferences in the lower networks should be. For example,
we know from corpus studies that push and pull occur frequently with associated
MEx’s of direction, as in He pushed me over104. However, the networks must also
account for the fact that not all Main Verbs occur with all MEx’s. For example, there
must be a rule to specify that if put is to be generated as the Main Verb, then the MEx
along must not be allowed to occur. The alternative to this would be to specify the
array of possible types of ‘subsidiary direction’ for each Process type instead. It is not
clear which would be the simpler/most fruitful method, but from the viewpoint of
both the developer of the grammar and the user of the system networks for text
analysis, the approach described here
8.5.1.1 The system network for ‘agent not accompanying affected carrier’
The choice between ‘agent not accompanying the affected carrier’ and ‘agent
accompanying the affected carrier’ is again a choice between two semantic types. I
will first present the system for ‘agent not accompanying affected carrier’.
The first choice in this system is between ‘distance implied’ and ‘no distance
implied’. ‘Distance’ implied’ allows for the Processes which only occur over a large
distance, for example, as Figure 8.5.2 shows, ‘mailing’ and ‘shipping’. This group
contrasts with ‘no distance implied’, which we shall come to shortly.
104
Table 7.4 in Chapter 7 demonstrate the results of the search results for push and pull in a sample of
50 lines of corpus examples from the British National Corpus (BNC) online.
296
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Figure 8.5.2 shows that the system for ‘sending as such’ leads to sending and
getting off to. And the choice of ‘getting off to’ requires an attached SP rule that will
state that, if this choice is made, then the choice of ‘subsidiary direction’ should not
be allowed.
The system for ‘sending via post office’ leads to the verb senses ‘mailing’ and
‘posting’, and this system should have a ‘preference rule’ stating that if ‘American
English’ has been chosen in the system for DIALECT, then the probabilities for the
choice of Main Verb should prefer mail, but if ‘British English’ has been chosen, then
the probabilities for the choice of Main Verb should prefer post.
If the choice ‘no distance implied’ has been chosen, then a subsystem is
entered, as shown in Figure 8.5.3.
297
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
56% propelling the affected carrier using a
body part …
Within the system for ‘movement with force’, if ‘propelling the affected
carrier using a body part’ is chosen, then another system will be entered for the
specification of the body part. The choices in this system are ‘typically using arm’,
‘typically using foot’, and ‘typically using any body part’.
‘Typically using arm’ leads to a choice between ‘affected carrier controlled
throughout’, which will allow for ‘pushing’ to be generated as the Main Verb, and
‘affected carrier initially controlled’, which will allow for ‘throwing’ to be generated
as the Main Verb. ‘Typically using foot’ will allow for ‘kicking’ as the Main Verb.
And ‘typically using any body part’ will allow for ‘pressing’ as the Main Verb.
The choice ‘propelling affected carrier using equipment’ will allow for
‘pumping’ as the Main Verb. And the other choices in the system lead directly to the
‘movement with force’ verb senses ‘shooting’ and ‘firing’.
The choice of ‘movement without force’ leads to the system network
presented in Figure 8.5.4:
298
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
90% agent has 10% full control of dispersible af-ca … 30% of air …
full control
of movement 8% full control of af-ca into container…
25% inherently
downwards …
10% agent has 20% manner specified …
initial control 25% inherently
of transference out of …
80% manner unspecified …
299
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
And finally, the subsystem choices for ‘transference with inherent direction’
leads to the system presented in Figure 8.5.5.
Figure 8.5.5 The system network for ‘transference with inherent direction’ Processes.
The system for ‘manner specified’ leads to the Processes ‘twisting’, ‘rolling’,
‘threading’, ‘spinning’, ‘shaking’ and ‘sliding’, and the system for ‘manner
unspecified’ leads to the Processes ‘passing’, ‘casting’ and ‘turning’.
300
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
Figure 8.5.6 The system network for ‘agent accompanying affected carrier’
In the first system in Figure 8.5.6 the choice ‘towards performer viewpoint’
leads to the very high frequency verb sense ‘bringing’, so introducing a deictic
element to the semantics of the verb. The choice ‘not towards performer viewpoint’
leads to a further choice between ‘unimpeded movement’ and ‘impeded movement’.
The system for ‘impeded movement’ leads to the Processes ‘getting’ and ‘picking out
of’. And the system for ‘unimpeded movement’ leads to the very high frequency
Process ‘taking’, and also ‘delivering’, ‘tearing’, ‘extracting’, ‘removing’ and ‘fishing
out’. These last four verb senses require an associated SP rule to specify that in the
‘same pass’, but in the system for Participant Role, the probabilities for these Main
Verbs should prefer the PR involved to be Source (rather than Destination or Path).
The system network for ‘direction’ requires SP rules similar to this to occur at various
places, since this Process type allows these three different types of PR to occur as the
third PR in the Process.
As with the choice ‘not towards performer viewpoint’, the choice ‘performer
support specified’ leads to a choice between ‘non impeded support’ and ‘impeded
support’. And the choice ‘non impeded support’ leads to the Process ‘carrying’, while
the choice ‘impeded support’ leads to the Processes ‘dragging’ and ‘pulling’.
The final choice in the ‘agent accompanying affected carrier’ system is for
‘performer leadership specified’. This leads to a choice between ‘performer initiated
pls (performer leadership specified)’ and ‘not performer initiated pls’. The choice of
301
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
‘not performer initiated pls’ leads to the Processes ‘heading’, ‘leading’, ‘guiding’,
‘drawing’ and ‘hurrying’. And the choice of ‘performer initiated pls’ leads to the
Processes ‘marching’, ‘running’, ‘rushing’, ‘steering’, ‘walking’, ‘flying’, ‘driving’,
‘riding’, ‘seeing’, ‘showing’, ‘galloping’, ‘trotting’, ‘backing up’, ‘reversing’ and
‘sailing’. I should point out that it is in this system network that the verb ‘march’ is
generated, because ‘march’ is the Main Verb in Halliday’s well-known example in his
paper ‘Notes on Transitivity and Theme Part 1’ (1967) of an ‘action’ Process, as
Example (1) shows105.
From the viewpoint of the CG model, Halliday’s example sounds rather odd,
and it can only be interpreted as having a covert third role of ‘direction’. In other
words verb sense is generated in this system for ‘agent accompanying affected carrier,
‘performer leadership specified’, rather than being a two-role Process, as Halliday
suggests.
8.6 Conclusion
In this Chapter I have provided a broad overview of three very large areas of
the system for TRANSITIVITY, moving from the relatively general ‘Process type’
categories to the most delicate of choices between meanings realized as lexical items.
As a conclusion to this chapter I would like to reiterate that what this chapter
provides is an illustration of how it is possible to move from an unstructured body of
lexical data to a semi-structured classification of Processes according to Process Type
and Participant Role configuration, to ultimately reach a fully semantically structured
system network for the realization of ‘lexis as most delicate grammar’.
This chapter therefore serves as an illustration of how the data that I have
collected and compiled in the form of the PTDB contributes significantly to the
development of a model of language for generating clauses. By using such data, I
have been able to produce very delicate system networks for generating the Main
Verb (and its Extensions, etc.) based not on introspection and guesswork, but on real
105
Halliday 1967 was described in detail in Chapter 3, Part 1.
302
Chapter 8: The Delicate System Networks for TRANSITIVITY
corpus evidence that is derived from real language use. Thus, the system networks
presented in this Chapter make a considerable contribution towards the massive task
of building a full generative model that represents real language use.
303
Chapter 9: Conclusions
9: Conclusions
The final chapter of the thesis provides a summary and an evaluation of the
research. The first section focuses on the contribution of this research to the Cardiff
Grammar, and on the way in which it has demonstrated the value of using corpora in
extending its scope. The second section describes the products of the research and
their applications, namely the Process Type DataBase (PTDB) described in Chapter 7
and presented in full in Appendix A, and the three major system networks, described
in Chapter 8 and presented in Appendix B. The third section discusses some of the
limitations of the thesis – primarily limitations of time in the research and space in
presenting it in thesis form, but also methodological and theoretical limitations. The
final section of the chapter suggests some implications for future work.
304
Chapter 9: Conclusions
language] from the grammatical end we often fail to take into account what is
revealed by coming at it from the lexical end’ (1996b:163).
This thesis has also demonstrated the value of the notions ‘systemic’ and
‘functional’ in a SFG approach to language description, and to the analysis and
generation of texts. The value of a systemic approach is that it enables us to model
language on a cline of delicacy. And by using this model we can derive a functional
description of each item in the language. As argued in Chapter 7, the most fruitful
method for arriving at a functional description is by observing function in use: for
example, in a corpus. And it is for the reasons presented in that chapter that, for
example, a ‘part-of-speech’ analysis of a corpus, which is based on traditional
grammatical classifications in terms of classes at the level of form, is less useful
method for this kind of model building. This thesis has demonstrated that the
semantic category of ‘Process’, rather than the traditional grammatical category of
‘verb’, allows us to describe the meaning and function of the item. It is the meanings
of verbs rather than the forms of verbs that enter into specific patterns, such as
complementation, and this is why word SENSE is paramount. For similar reasons, this
is why West’s (1953) information on the frequency of words, as described in Chapter
7, is still an important study fifty years on. It is still the only ‘second level’ corpus
study that provides frequency information for disambiguated word senses – rather
than just for grammatical categories such as ‘verb’, ‘noun’ and ‘adjective’.
Tucker (1996b:175) makes the important statement that ‘when evidence from
data cannot be explained or modelled within the theory [then] alternative theories
need to be set up’. This statement leads us to a further conclusion that can be drawn
from the work presented in this thesis. The Cardiff Grammar is tested in the computer
implementation of the COMMUNAL project, and this testing ensures that the CG
model attends to many aspects of language which are required for the generation of
language, but which might otherwise be unnoticed. But by using real data from a
corpus for creating the model, we are testing the model in a different way and to an
even greater degree, because we are testing its ability to be extended to the full
coverage of language. This thesis has largely seen how the CG has the means to
accommodate ‘peculiarities’ in the data. Chapter 7 demonstrated how the creation of
a large and representative database can serve to draw attention to some important
features in the language that must be accounted for. Firstly, the data highlighted the
importance of allowing a central place for multi-word verbs in the system networks
305
Chapter 9: Conclusions
for PROCESS TYPE106. These highly frequent items must have an equal place in the
resulting system networks to the single-word verbs, despite the well-known difficulty
in accessing information about their functions and occurrences in a corpus. The
second problematic area that Chapter 7 discusses is metaphor. Chapter 7 describes
how instances of metaphor found in the PTDB can be handled in the system networks,
either by accounting for them as new senses of a verb form (i.e. ‘dead’ metaphor), or
as the ‘extended usage’ of a core verb sense.
We are thus able to conclude that this thesis has demonstrated the desirability
of using corpora both to test and expand a model of language. Moreover, the thesis
has demonstrated the value of the ‘second level’ use of corpora – that is, taking the
wealth of published corpus observations, such as the COBUILD grammar, and
making use of it for further language description and modelling.
106
It did so, ironically, through its failure to identify them: in current work by Michael Day at Cardiff,
this problem is being attended to.
306
Chapter 9: Conclusions
type ‘one-role’, ‘affected only’; ‘action’ Processes of the type ‘two-role’, ‘agent plus
affected’; and ‘relational’ Processes, of the type ‘three-role’, ‘directional’. These
system networks include the realization rule numbers and probability figures for each
system and Process, and so the mechanisms for making them operational as part of
the wider generative grammar that is GENESYS (the sentence generation component
of the COMMUNAL project.)
While the system networks are capable of being implemented in the bigger
system, we should not lose sight of the fact that they also stand as a product in their
own right. They constitute a fuller description than any available elsewhere, so far as
I am aware, of an aspect of English that has, I would hope, implications both for (1)
systemic theory, and (2) other approaches to the area of language that we term
TRANSITIVITY.
307
Chapter 9: Conclusions
308
Chapter 9: Conclusions
for appropriate choices in the PARTICIPANT ROLES part of the network, in which
option in reflexive form and ‘covert’ roles, etc, are selected.
Other possible work that may be undertaken as a result of this thesis involves
the extending and updating of the PTDB, and consequently the updating of the system
networks, ad infinitum. As Chapter 8 demonstrated, the semantic nature of the system
networks means that it is straightforward to make additions. In Chapter 2 we saw that
Levin (1993) recognises the way in which new words in the language slot into the
existing semantic and syntactic structure. In this thesis, it is the case that once the
Process type and PR configurations for a new verb sense have been determined, the
verb sense can be slotted into the appropriate system network by locating the
appropriate sub-system in that system network. As we saw in Chapter 7, the Process
type and PR configurations are derivable from the context (and/or co-text), and this is
why the examples of use from the COBUILD Dictionary were so important for
analysis, and why they have been included for each verb sense in the PTDB.
A final pointer for future work to come out of this thesis is the need to obtain
further frequency information for the verb senses. If we had, for example, a modern
version of the work of West (1953), it would enable us to derive semantic
probabilities for modern English. However, as Chapter 7 showed, obtaining
frequencies from a corpus for the senses of verbs is not an easy task, and it is not easy
to see how it can be fully automated. I anticipate that within the next five years this
will be more of a possibility than it has been during this research. The reason for this
is the emergence of corpus tools for providing coherent and thorough analyses.
309
Bibliography
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Aijmer, K. and Altenberg, B. (1991). English Corpus Linguistics. London: Longman.
Bach, E. and Harms, R.T. (1968). Universals in Linguistic Theory. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Ball, F. and Tucker, G. (2000). ‘On the Preferential Co-occurrence of Processes and
Circumstantial Adjuncts: Some Corpus Evidence’. Presented at 2000 ISFC Singapore.
Benson, J.D. and Greaves, W.S., (eds), (1985). Systemic Perspectives on Discourse,
Volume 1: Selected Theoretical Papers from the Ninth International Systemic
Workshop. Norwood: Ablex.
Benson, J.D., Cumming, M.J. and Greaves, W.S. (eds) (1988). Linguistics in a
Systemic Perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
310
Bibliography
Berry, M., Butler, C., Fawcett, R. and Huang, G. (eds) (1996). Meaning and Form:
Systemic Functional Interpretations. Norwood: Ablex.
Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S. and E. Finegan (1999). The Longman
Grammar Of Spoken And Written English. London: Longman.
Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: its nature, origins and use. Praeger:
New York.
311
Bibliography
Davidse, K. (1996) ‘Ditransitivity and Possession’. In Hasan, R., Butt, D. & Cloran,
C. (eds) (1996). Functional Descriptions: Language, Form and Linguistic Theory.
Amsterdam: Benjamin.
Davidse, K. and Geyskens, S. (1998). ‘‘Have you walked the dog yet?’ The ergative
causativization of intransitives’. In Word 48.2.
Fawcett, R.P. (1987). ‘The semantics of clause and verb for relational processes in
English’. In Halliday, M.A.K and Fawcett, R.P. (eds) New Developments in Systemic
Linguistics, Vol. 1: Theory and Description. London: Frances Pinter.
312
Bibliography
Fawcett, R.P. (2000b). ‘In place of Halliday’s ‘verbal group’, Part 1: Evidence from
the problems of Halliday’s representations and the relative simplicity of the proposed
alternative’. In Word 51.2. 157-203.
Fawcett, R.P. (2001). The Functional Syntax Handbook: Analyzing English at the
level of form. London: Continuum.
Fawcett, R.P. (in preparation). ‘Two positions on meaning and levels in a systemic
functional grammar’. (To be offered to Functions of Language).
Fawcett, R.P., Tucker, G.H. and Lin, Y.Q. (1993). ‘How a systemic functional
grammar works: the role of realization in realization’. In Horacek, H. and Zock, M.
(eds) (1993) New Concepts in Natural Language Generation. London: Pinter.
Fawcett, R.P., Tucker, G.H. and Lin, Y.Q. (1996) The GENESYS Lexicogrammar
Version 5.3. (software). Computational Linguistics Unit, Cardiff University, Cardiff.
Fawcett, R.P. and Tucker, G.H. (2000). The System Network and Realization Rules
for Meanings Realized in the Clause. In COMMUNAL Working Papers, No. 19.
Computational Linguistics Unit, University of Cardiff, Cardiff.
313
Bibliography
Fillmore, C.J., (1966) ‘Toward a modern theory of case’. The Ohio State University
project on linguistic analysis, Report No. 13.
Fillmore, C.J., (1968), ‘The Case for Case’. In Bach, E. and Harms, R.T. (1968).
Universals in Linguistic Theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Ghadessy, M. (ed) (1993) Register analysis: Theory and practice. London: Pinter.
Halliday, M.A.K. (1967) ‘Notes on Transitivity and Theme: Part 1’. In Journal of
Linguistics. 3.1.
Halliday, M.A.K. (1968) ‘Notes on Transitivity and Theme: Part 3’. In Journal of
Linguistics. 4.2.
314
Bibliography
Halliday, M.A.K. (1976). System and function in language: selected papers by MAK
Halliday. London: Oxford University Press.
Halliday, M.A.K. (1977). ‘Text as semantic choice in social contexts’. In van Dijk, T.,
& Petöfi, J. (eds.) Grammars and descriptions. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
Halliday, M.A.K and Fawcett, R.P. (eds) (1987). New Developments in Systemic
Linguistics, Vol. 1: Theory and Description. London: Frances Pinter.
315
Bibliography
Hasan, R., Butt, D. and Cloran, C. (eds) (1996). Functional Descriptions: Language,
Form and Linguistic Theory. Amsterdam: Benjamin.
Huang, G.W. (1995). Enhanced Theme in English: its structures and functions. PhD
Thesis, Cardiff: University of Wales, Cardiff, School of English Studies,
Communication and Philosophy.
Huddleston, R. (1981). ‘Systemic features and their realization’. In Halliday & Martin
(eds.) Readings in Systemic Linguistics. London: Batsford.
316
Bibliography
Kohl, K., Jones, D., Berwick, R. and N. Nomura (1998). ‘Representing Verb
Alternations in WordNet’. In Fellbaum, C. (1998) (ed). WordNet: An Electronic
Lexical Database. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Levin, B. (1985). Lexical Semantics in Review. Lexicon Project Working Papers No.
1. Massachusetts: MIT.
Lorge, I. and Thorndike, E.L. (1938). A semantic count of English words. New York:
Columbia Univ. Press.
317
Bibliography
Martin, J.R. (1992) English Text: System and Structure. Amsterdam: J.Benjamins.
Neale, A. (1997). A Study of the Behaviour of the Verbs ‘Crush’ and ‘Squeeze’ in
Use. PG Dip Dissertation. Cardiff: School of English, Communication and
Philosophy, Cardiff University
Quirk R., Greenbaum S., Leech G., and Svartvik J. (1972). A Grammar of
Contemporary English. Longman, London.
318
Bibliography
Rosch, E., and Lloyd, B.B., (eds.), 1978. Cognition and Categorization. New
Jersey: Erlbaum.
Steiner, E. and Veltman, R. (1988) (eds). Pragmatics, Discourse & Text: some
systemically inspired approaches. London: Pinter.
319
Bibliography
Tucker, G. (1996b), ‘‘So grammarians haven’t the faintest idea’: reconciling lexis-
oriented and grammar-oriented approaches to language’. In Hasan, R., Butt, D. and
Cloran, C. (eds) (1996). Functional Descriptions: Language, Form and Linguistic
Theory. Amsterdam: Benjamin.
van Dijk, T. & Petöfi, J. (eds.) (1977). Grammars and descriptions. Berlin: Walter de
Gruyter.
320
Appendix A2: Guidelines for using the Process Type DataBase
The Process Type Database, which was fully described in Chapter 7 of this
thesis, is supplied as an Appendix that can be viewed and searched107.
The database is in the form of an alphabetical list. The entry for each verb
sense is on a separate row in the spreadsheet, and the degrees of analysis are provided
in each column. Each column provides the following:
Column A:
The first column is an alphabetical list of verb forms. This ordering enables
ease of searching a particular verb sense.
Column B:
Column B provides figures of the occurrence of the verb form, as reported by
West. For a more detailed description of the use of West in creating this DataBase,
refer to Chapter 7. The ‘occurrences of form’ figures provided in this list show how
frequently the verb form occurred in a corpus of 5 million words.
Column C:
The information in this column is taken from two sources. The first is from
Francis et al (1996) Collins COBUILD Grammar Patterns 1: Verbs, and is given as
either ‘C0’, ‘C1’, ‘C2’, ‘C3’, ‘C4’ or ‘C5’, which represents the Band distinctions that
Francis et al recognised in their ‘verb index’ (see Section 7.3.1.3 of Chapter 7 for
more detail), or as ‘-c’ if the verb form does not occur in Francis et al (1996). All of
107
The Process Type DataBase is presented as Appendix A1, and is supplied on CD-ROM, on the
inside cover of this thesis. The PTDB is compiled in Microsoft Excel, and also included on the CD-
ROM is a version of Microsoft Excel Viewer, which will allow those users who do not have Excel
installed to view and explore the PTDB.
321
Appendix A2: Guidelines for using the Process Type DataBase
the C5 entries in this column also include a figure, which indicates the total number of
occurrences of that verb form in the Bank of English108.
The other source of information given in this column is Biber et al (1999), The
Longman Grammar Of Spoken And Written English. This publication provides
frequency information for 130 multi-word verb forms as they occur in a corpus of 1
million words, and all of this frequency information is given in Column C as ‘Lo.n’.
Column D:
Column D provides the verb SENSE information in the form of a gloss of the
meaning, and an example of use (taken from Collins COBUILD English dictionary -
2nd ed). This is the important information for determining how many different senses
of each verb form are recognised.
Column E:
The next column provides figures for the occurrences of the verb senses.
These figures are adapted from West (1953), as described in Chapter 7, and show how
many times the particular verb sense occurred in his 5 million word corpus.
Column F:
Column F provides the main analysis for each verb sense. This is the most
useful column for the research presented in this thesis, because it is possible to search
for Process types. By using the filter facility in Excel it is possible to compile lists of
all the verb senses in the PTDB that are a particular Process type. Excel ‘AutoFilter’
has been set up in the Database. To use AutoFilter, click on the AutoFilter arrows at
the very top of the PTDB, on the right of Column F’s column label. A drop down list
will then appear with every option that occurs in that column. Column F starts as
follows:
‘all
top 10
custom
?
attributive, affected carrier
attributive, agent carrier
108
These figures were provided Ball (work in progress).
322
Appendix A2: Guidelines for using the Process Type DataBase
attributive, plus 3 p Ag
attributive, simple carrier
etc…’
Clicking on an entry on this list will provide all, and only, the occurrences of
that particular entry in the entire PTDB. Therefore, clicking ‘two role plus affected’
will provide a list of all the verb senses which formed the basis for the system
network presented in Appendix B2.
Column G:
This column provides Levin’s (1993) approximate analysis for all of the verb
senses in the PTDB that she too considers.
Column H:
Column H provides the Participant Role (PR) configuration for each verb
sense. This configuration can also be determined from the information in Column F:
Cardiff Grammar Feature. However, Column H provides additional information
about the likelihood of the covertness and overtness of the PRs, as described in
Section 7.6 of Chapter 7.
Column I:
The final column, Column I, provides a place for any notes that needed to be
recorded in the compilation and analysis of the PTDB.
323
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
affected_only ->
5# stopping_being_af /
60# change_of_state_af /
25# involuntary_behaviour_af /
10# emission_af.
/*NB the ‘stop_being’ verbs contrast with ‘start_being’ verbs, which are ‘created only’.*/
stop_being ->
60# dying (6.002221) /
10# vanishing (6.002222) /
1# expiring (6.002223) /
1# perishing (6.002224) /
28# disappearing (6.002225).
change_of_state->
20# change_of_state_as_such /
80# change_of_state_specified.
change_of_state_as_such ->
90# changing (6.002323) /
9# altering (6.002324) /
0.9# transforming (6.002325) /
0.1# metamorphosing (6.002326).
change_of_state_specified ->
40# change_by_moving /
5# change_by_cooking /
30# change_resulting_in_disintegration /
4# change_in_basic_consistency /
1# change_in_vision /
1# change_in_quality /
4# change_in_size /
1# change_in_number /
1# change_in_temperature /
1# change_in_speed /
1# change_in_strength /
1# change_in_fullness /
2# change_developmentally /
8# evaluative_change.
change_by_moving ->
30# moving_as_such (6.002327) /
30# moving_relative_to_opening_an_enclosure /
1# moving_relative_to_upright_state /
1# moving_relative_to_linear_state /
38# inherently_directional.
moving_relative_to_opening_an_enclosure ->
45# opening (6.002411)/
5# shutting (6.002412) /
324
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
moving_relative_to_upright_state ->
1# capsizing (6.002415) /
30# overturning (6.002416) /
30# toppling (6.002417) /
19# overbalancing (6.002419).
moving_relative_to_straight_line ->
30# bending (6.002421) /
50# turning (6.002422) /
2# curving (6.002423) /
2# swerving (6.002424) /
2# looping (6.002425) /
6# winding (6.002426) /
8# twisting (6.002427).
inherently_directional ->
30# upward_id /
70# downward_id.
upward_id ->
50# coming_up (6.002436) /
50# rising (6.002444).
downward_id ->
10# falling (6.002428) /
7# falling_away (6.002429) /
7# falling_back (6.002431) /
10# falling_down (6.002432) /
10# falling_off (6.002433) /
7# descending (6.002434) /
10# going_down (6.002435) /
6# plummeting (6.002437) /
7# dropping (6.002438) /
6# sinking (6.002439) /
6# nosediving (6.002441) /
6# dipping (6.002442) /
6# plunging (6.002443) /
7# lowering (6.002445).
change_by_cooking ->
50# cooking_as_such (6.002511) /
20# cooking_in_liquid /
25# cooking_in_oven /
5# cooking_by_direct_heat.
cooking_in_liquid ->
50# boiling (6.002512) /
2# hardboiling (6.002513) /
2# softboiling (6.002514) /
35# frying (6.002515) /
5# simmering (6.002516) /
2# poaching (6.002517) /
2# stewing (6.002518) /
2# saute_ing (6.002519).
cooking_in_oven ->
50# roasting (6.002521) /
325
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
cooking_by_direct_heat ->
50# grilling (6.002523) /
25# barbeque-ing (6.002524) /
25# toasting (6.002525).
change_resulting_in_disintergration ->
30# total_disintergration /
69# non-total_disintergration /
1# breaking_down (6.002526).
total_disintergration ->
20# falling_apart (6.002527) /
20# falling_down (6.002528) /
3# falling_in (6.002529) /
3# disintergrating (6.002531) /
20# collapsing (6.002532) /
3# caving_in (6.002533) /
3# decomposing (6.002534) /
10# smashing (6.002535) /
3# busting (6.002536) /
5# snapping (6.002537) /
5# bursting (6.002538) /
5# shattering (6.002539).
non-total_disintergration ->
50# breaking (6.002541) /
1# breaking_up (6.002542) /
1# unravelling (6.002543) /
1# degenerating (6.002544) /
1# fragmenting (6.002545) /
1# fracturing (6.002546) /
1# rupturing (6.002547) /
8# splitting (6.002548) /
1# splintering (6.002549) /
3# crumbling (6.002551) /
8# cracking (6.002552) /
1# buckling (6.002553) /
1# warping (6.002554) /
1# chipping (6.002555) /
8# crushing (6.002556) /
4# ripping (6.002558) /
8# tearing (6.002559) /
1# dissipating (6.002561).
change_in_basic_consistency ->
20# change_from_liquid_to_solid /
10# change_from_solid_to_liquid /
10# change_from_liquid_to_gas /
10# change_from_solid_to_solid /
40# change_from_solid_to_gas /
10# non-specific_cibc.
change_from_liquid_to_solid ->
20# solidifying (6.002611) /
70# freezing (6.002612) /
5# setting (6.002613) /
0.4# congealing (6.002614) /
0.4# clotting (6.002616) /
326
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
1# curdling (6.002617) /
1# crystallizing (6.002618) /
0.2# caramelizing (6.002619).
change_from_solid_to_liquid ->
48# melting (6.002621) /
7# liquefying (6.002622) /
45# dissolving (6.002623).
change_from_liquid_to_gas ->
0.4# vaporizing (6.002624) /
15# evaporating (6.002625) /
55# drying (6.002626) /
0.4# dehydrating (6.002627) /
0.2# dehumidifying (6.002629) /
29# boiling (6.002512).
change_from_solid_to_solid ->
49# thawing (6.002632) /
49# defrosting (6.002633) /
1# tenderizing (6.002634) /
0.5# fossilizing (6.002635) /
0.5# ossifying (6.002636).
change_from_solid_to_gas->
95# burning (6.002637) /
5# incinerating (6.002638).
non-specific_cibc ->
30# hardening (6.002639) /
1# acidifying (6.002641) /
1# oxidizing (6.002642) /
1# emulsifying (6.002643) /
4# corroding (6.002644) /
15# firming (6.002645) /
30# softening (6.002646) /
15# stiffening (6.002647) /
1# depressurizing (6.002648) /
1# magnetizing (6.002649) /
1# ulcerating (6.002651).
change_in_vision ->
2# blurring (6.002652) /
60# clearing (6.002653) /
2# dimming (6.002654) /
20# fading (6.002655) /
10# fading_away (6.002656) /
4# fading_out (6.002657) /
2# fogging (6.002658).
change_in_quality ->
5# warping (6.002711) /
10# evening_out (6.002712) /
15# levelling_out (6.002713) /
5# mellowing (6.002714) /
5# roughening (6.002715) /
10# sharpening (6.002716) /
5# slackening (6.002717) /
5# steepening (6.002718) /
10# straightening (6.002719) /
327
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
5# sweetening (6.002721) /
5# tautening (6.002722) /
15# tightening (6.002723) /
5# reddening (6.002724).
change_in_size ->
50# increasing_size /
50# decreasing_size.
increasing_size ->
2# enlarging (6.002725) /
10# expanding (6.002726) /
20# growing (6.002727) /
20# increasing (6.002728) /
1# inflating (6.002729) /
1# broadening (6.002731) /
0.5# fattening (6.002732) /
5# heightening (6.002733) /
1.5# lengthening (6.002734) /
1# thickening (6.002735) /
5# widening (6.002736) /
1# snowballing (6.002737) /
1# mushrooming (6.002738) /
1# ballooning (6.002739) /
15# spreading (6.002741) /
5# swelling (6.002742).
decreasing_size->
10# compressing (6.002743) /
10# decreasing (6.002744) /
10# contracting (6.002745) /
10# shrinking (6.002746) /
10# narrowing (6.002747) /
10# thinning (6.002748) /
10# lessening (6.002749) /
10# shortening (6.002751) /
10# reducing (6.002752) /
10# diminishing (6.002753).
change_in_number ->
40# number_specified /
60# number_unspecified.
number_specified ->
80# doubling (6.002759) /
10# by_three /
10# quadrupling (6.002761).
by_three ->
50# tripling (6.002762) /
50# trebling (6.002763) /
number_unspecified ->
20# multiplying (6.002764) /
20# reproducing (6.002765) /
60# dividing (6.002766).
change_in_temperature ->
50# increasing_temperature /
50# decreasing_temperature.
328
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
increasing_temperature ->
15# warming_through (6.002767) /
20# warming_up (6.002768) /
8# heating (6.002769) /
20# heating_up (6.002771) /
30# overheating (6.002772) /
7# roasting (6.002521).
decreasing_temperature ->
80# cooling_down (6.002774) /
20# chilling (6.002775).
change_in_speed ->
50# increasing_speed /
50# decreasing_speed.
increasing_speed ->
2# quickening (6.002776) /
18# accelerating (6.002777) /
80# speeding_up (6.002778).
decreasing_speed ->
7# slowing (6.002779) /
20# slowing_up (6.002781) /
70# slowing_down (6.002782) /
3# decelerating (6.002783).
change_in_strength ->
50# increasing_strength /
50# weakening (6.002784).
increasing_strength ->
60# strengthening (6.002785) /
40# toughening (6.002786).
change_in_fullness ->
50# increasing_fullness /
50# decreasing_fullness.
increasing_fullness ->
10# filling (6.002787) /
50# filling_up (6.002788) /
40# flooding (6.002789).
decreasing_fullness ->
50# draining (6.002791) /
50# emptying (6.002792).
change_developmentally ->
40# change_developmentally_as_such /
30# change_developmentally_typically_creature /
30# change_developmentally_typically_plant.
change_developmentally_as_such ->
45# developing (6.002811) /
60# growing (6.002727) /
5# progressing (6.002813).
change_developmentally_typically_creature ->
329
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
change_developmentally_typically_plant ->
49# sprouting (6.002819) /
49# ripening (6.002821) /
2# germinating (6.002822).
evalutive_change ->
50# favourable_ec /
50# unfavourable_ec.
favourable_ec ->
49.5# improving (6.002823) /
50# getting_better (6.002824) /
0.5# ameliorating (6.002825).
unfavourable_ec ->
50# worsening (6.002826) /
50# deteriorating (6.002827).
involuntary_behaviour_af ->
15# specific_physiological /
15# reactive_physiological /
20# psychological_af /
10# internal_bodily_movement /
20# change_in_awakeness /
10# change_in_conciousness /
10# suffocating_af.
specific_physiological ->
25# digestion_sphys /
25# respiratory_sphys /
25# outward_appearance_sphys /
25# bodily_substance_emission_sphys.
digestion_sphys ->
15# belching (6.002828) /
20# burping (6.002831) /
20# hiccuping (6.002833) /
11# farting (6.002852) /
11# blowing_off (6.002853) /
11# letting_off (6.002854) /
12# passing wind (6.002855.
respiratory_sphys ->
5# sneezing (6.002834) /
2# sniffling (6.002835) /
2# snoring (6.002836) /
2# wheezing (6.002837) /
5# yawning (6.002838) /
40# breathing (6.002841) /
40# coughing (6.002842) /
2# exhaling (6.002848) /
2# inhaling (6.002849) /
outward_appearance_sphys ->
330
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
bodily_substance_emission_sphys ->
70# bleeding (6.002839) /
9# dribbling (6.002843) /
3# drooling (6.002844) /
9# puking (6.002845) /
9# vomiting (6.002847) /
reactive_physiological ->
15# cowering (6.002855) /
17# cringing (6.002856) /
17# flinching (6.002857) /
17# recoiling (6.002858) /
17# shrinking (6.002747) /
17# wincing (6.002861).
psychological_af->
45# breaking_down (6.002526) /
45# cracking_up (6.0028612) /
10# snapping (6.002537).
internal_bodily_movement ->
2.5# convulsing (6.002862) /
2.5# quaking (6.002863) /
7# quivering (6.002864) /
40# shaking (6.002865) /
7# shivering (6.002866) /
7# shuddering (6.002867) /
20# trembling (6.002868) /
7# writhing (6.002869) /
7# twitching (6.002871).
change_in_awakeness ->
50# to_waking_state /
50# to_sleeping_state.
to_waking_state ->
10# awakening (6.002872) /
90# waking_up (6.002873).
towards_sleeping_state ->
10# tiring (6.0028761).
90# complete_tss.
complete_tss ->
40# falling_asleep (6.002874) /
40# going_to_sleep (6.002875) /
17# dropping_off (6.002876) /
change_in_conciousness ->
50# to_unconciousness /
50# coming_round (6.002885).
to_unconciousness ->
35# fainting (6.002886) /
35# passing_out (6.002887) /
331
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
suffocate ->
50# suffocate_as_such (6.002891) /
50# suffocate_specified.
suffocate_specified ->
10# asphyxiating (6.002892) /
45# choking (6.002893) /
45# drowning (6.002894).
emission_af ->
45# of_substance /
55# of_sound.
of_substance ->
13# bleeding (6.002911) /
12# bubbling (6.002912) /
13# dribbling (6.002913) /
13# dripping (6.002914) /
12# foaming (6.002915) /
12# spilling_out (6.002916) /
13# squirting (6.002917) /
12# streaming (6.002918).
of_sound ->
3# banging (6.002937) /
1# beating (6.002938) /
2# beeping (6.002939) /
2# bellowing (6.002941) /
2# blaring (6.002942) /
2# booming (6.002943) /
2# burbling (6.002944) /
3# buzzing (6.002945) /
3# chiming (6.002946) /
1# chinking (6.002947) /
1# clacking (6.002948) /
1# clanging (6.002949) /
1# clanking (6.002951) /
2# clicking (6.002952) /
1# clinking (6.002953) /
1# clunking (6.002954) /
3# groaning (6.002955) /
3# growling (6.002956) /
3# gurgling (6.002957) /
3# hissing (6.002958) /
2# hooting (6.002959) /
3# howling (6.002961) /
3# humming (6.002962) /
3# jangling (6.002963) /
2# jingling (6.002964) /
3# moaning (6.002966) /
2# murmuring (6.002967) /
1# pealing (6.002968) /
1# pinging (6.002969) /
3# popping (6.002972) /
3# shrieking (6.002973) /
3# singing (6.002974) /
332
Appendix B1: The system network for one-role, affected only action Processes
1# sizzling (6.002975) /
2# spluttering (6.002976) /
1# sputtering (6.002977) /
1# squawking (6.002978) /
3# squeaking (6.002979) /
3# squealing (6.002981) /
1# thudding (6.002982) /
3# ticking (6.002983) /
1# tolling (6.002984) /
1# tooting (6.002985) /
2# trumpeting (6.002986) /
1# twanging (6.002987) /
3# wailing (6.002988) /
3# whining (6.002989) /
1# whirring (6.002991) /
3# whistling (6.002992) /
1# zinging (6.002993).
333
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
agent_plus_affected ->
10# affecting_as_such /
0.5# not affecting
20# social_action /
69.5# material_action.
affecting_as_such ->
70# affecting (6.004101) /
5# acting_on (6.004102) /
5# colouring (6.004103) /
5# disturbing (6.004104) /
5# hitting (6.004105) /
5# influencing (6.004106) /
5# interferring_with (6.004107).
not_affecting ->
10# sparing (6.004111) /
90# leaving_alone (6.004112).
social_action ->
20# social_action_involving_two_parties /
80# general_social_action.
social_action_involving_two_parties ->
15# starting_couple /
70# within_couple /
15# ending_couple.
starting_couple ->
60# asking_out (6.004152) /
30# proposing_to (6.004153) /
10# proposing_marriage_to (6.004154).
within_couple ->
0.5# embracing (6.004161) /
20# hugging /
20# cuddling /
30# kissing /
28.5# having_sex_with /
0.5# getting_off_with (6.004162) /
0.5# going_with (6.004163).
hugging ->
40# hugging_as_such (6.0041611) /
60# giving_a_hug (6.0041612).
cuddling ->
30# cuddling_as_such (6.0041621) /
60# giving_a_cuddle (6.0041622) /
10# having_a_cuddle (6.0041623).
334
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
kissing ->
60# kissing_as_such (6.0041631) /
30# giving_a_kiss (6.0041632) /
3# necking (6.0041633) /
4# snogging (6.0041634) /
3# pecking (6.0041635).
having_sex ->
50# making_love_to (6.0041641) /
30# fucking (6.0041642) /
20# screwing (6.0041643).
ending_couple ->
3# throwing_over (6.004171) /
3# dumping (6.004172) /
25# finishing_with (6.004173) /
25# breaking_up_with (6.004174) /
25# splitting_up_with (6.004175) /
4# running_out_on (6.004176) /
15# leaving (6.004177) /
general_social_action ->
35# visiting /
5# encountering_ag_af /
35# social_action_by_empowered_person /
10# abusing_ag_af /
2# interrupting_event /
5# supporting_ag_af /
0.5# playing_with (6.004201) /
0.5# using (6.004202).
0.5# cursing (6.004203) /
3# blaming (6.004204) /
0.5# guiding (6.004205) /
0.5# taming (6.004206) /
0.5# sticking_up (6.004207) /
0.5# holding_up (6.004208) /
0.5# spoiling (6.004209) /
0.5# giving_away (6.004210) / by father of daughter
0.5# calming (6.004211).
visiting ->
70# visiting_as_such (6.004221) /
5# calling_on (6.004222) /
5# calling_in_on (6.00423) /
5# seeing (6.004224) /
5# meeting_up_with (6.004225) /
5# getting_together_with (6.004226) /
5# looking_up (6.004227).
encountering_ag_af ->
80# encountering /
20# not_encountering.
encountering ->
1# encountering_as_such (6.004231) /
90# meeting (6.004232) /
5# bumping_into (6.004233) /
2# running_across (6.004234) /
2# running_into (6.004235).
335
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
not_encountering ->
20# avoiding (6.004236) /
4# passing (6.004237) /
4# passing_over (6.004238) /
60# ignoring (6.004239) /
4# neglecting (6.004240) /
4# standing_up (6.004241) /
4# staying_away_from (6.004242).
social_action_by_empowered_person ->
2# religious_domain /
20# employment_domain /
30# legal_domain /
28# medical_domain /
7# military_domain /
7# leadership_domain /
5# criminal_domain /
0.5# punishing (6.0042011) /
0.5# rewarding (6.0042012).
religious_domain ->
70# blessing (6.0042013) /
10# christening (6.004214) /
5# baptizing (6.004215) /
1# confirming (6.004216) /
13# marrying (6.004217) /
1# crowning (6.004218).
employment_domain ->
60# sacking (6.004220) /
20# letting_go (6.004221) /
10# laying_off (6.004222) /
10# making_redundant (6.004223) /
legal_domain ->
2# policing (6.0042231) /
50# arresting (6.0042232) /
2# pulling_in (6.0042233) /
10# cautioning (6.0042234) /
10# charging (6.0042235) /
1# trying (6.0042236) /
1# fining (6.0042237) /
23# imprisoning /
0.5# framing (6.0042239) /
0.5# shutting_up (6.0042240).
imprisoning ->
25# imprisoning_as_such (6.00422381) /
25# sending_to_prison (6.00422382) /
25# sending_down (6.00422383) /
25# jailing (6.00422384).
medical_domain ->
25# attending (6.0042241) /
336
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
25# treating (6.0042242) /
25# curing (6.0042243) /
25# healing (6.0042244).
military_domain ->
25# attacking (6.0042251) /
25# defending (6.0042252) /
6# opening_fire_on (6.0042253) /
1# beseiging (6.0042254) /
20# invading (6.0042255) /
1# occupying (6.0042256) /
6# seizing (6.0042257) /
6# taking (6.0042258) /
5# conquering (6.0042260) /
6# defeating (6.0042261).
leadership_domain ->
10# civilizing (6.0042271) /
10# commanding (6.0042272) /
20# leading (6.0042273) /
10# governing (6.0042274) /
10# taxing (6.0042275) /
10# punishing (6.0042276) /
10# rewarding (6.0042277) /
10# modernizing (6.0042278) /
10# freeing (6.0042279) /
criminal_domain ->
40# stealing (6.0042281) /
30# robbing (6.0042282) /
5# holding_up (6.0042283) /
5# doing_over (6.0042284) /
20# burgling (6.0042285).
abusing_ag_af ->
60# physically_abusing_ag_af /
40# verbally_abusing_ag_af.
physically_abusing_ag_af ->
10# bothering (6.004241) /
5# stalking (6.004242) /
5# molesting_as_such (6.004243) /
10# raping (6.004244) /
10# assaulting (6.004245) /
40# attacking (6.004246) /
15# abusing (6.004247) /
5# setting_on (6.004248).
verbally_abusing_ag_af ->
10# teasing (6.004251) /
4# taking_the_micky_out_of (6.004252) /
10# abusing (6.004253) /
4# scolding (6.004254) /
10# threatening (6.004255) /
11# criticizing (6.004256) /
3# tearing_into (6.004257) /
5# putting_down (6.004258) /
3# talking_down_to (6.004259) /
3# running_down (6.004260) /
5# laying_into (6.004261) /
337
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
5# knocking (6.004262) /
3# getting_on_at (6.004263) /
5# getting_at (6.004264) /
5# getting_back_at (6.004265) /
3# doing_down (6.004266) /
3# being_on_at (6.004267) /
5# being_hard_on (6.004268) /
3# being_at (6.004269).
interrupting_event ->
45# interrupting (6.004280) /
45# interrupting_discourse /
10# breaking_off (6.004281).
interrupting_discourse ->
20# breaking_in_on/upon (6.004282) /
20# breaking_into (6.004283) /
20# cutting_in (6.004284) /
20# cutting_off (6.004285) /
20# cutting_short (6.004286).
supporting_ag_af ->
70# physically_supporting_ag_af /
30# verbally_supporting_ag_af.
physically_supporting_ag_af ->
20# supporting_as_such (6.004290) /
10# backing_up (6.004291) /
5# standing_by (6.004292) /
10# standing_up_for (6.004293) /
4.375# sticking_by (6.004294) /
4.375# sticking_to (6.004295) /
4.375# sticking_up_for (6.004296) /
4.375# getting_behind (6.004297) /
4.375# accomodating (6.004298) /
5# looking_after (6.004299) /
4.375# bringing_up (6.0042911) /
5# caring_for (6.0042912) /
4.375# catering_for (6.0042913) /
5# taking_care_of (6.0042914) /
4.375# tending (6.0042915) /
5# nursing (6.0042916).
verbally_supporting_ag_af ->
40# speaking_up_for (6.0042917) /
30# speaking_for (6.0042919) /
30# encouraging (6.0042920).
material_action ->
35# affecting_by_contact /
5# affecting_by_lack_of_contact /
35# change_of_state_ag_af /
10# preparing /
5# ingestion /
2.5# concealment_ag_af /
2.5# change_position_ag_af /
5# using.
affecting_by_contact ->
20# destroying /
338
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
20# hitting /
20# killing /
10# hurting /
10# touching /
10# breaking /
2# mending /
8# cutting.
destroying->
6# blowing_up (6.004301) /
6# breaking_down (6.002526) /
4# breaking_up (6.002542) /
10# burning (6.002637) /
6# burning_down (6.004305) /
4# crushing (6.004306) /
10# destroying_as_such (6.004307) /
6# exploding (6.004308) /
4# kicking_down (6.004309) /
4# kicking_in (6.0043010) /
6# knocking_down (6.0043011) /
1# knocking_out (6.0043012) /
4# pulling_down (6.0043013) /
3# bursting (6.002536) /
3# spoiling (6.0043015) /
4# sinking (6.002439) /
4# wrecking (6.0043017) /
7# ruining (6.0043019) /
2# tearing_down (6.0043021) /
0.5# wasting (6.0043022) /
0.5# washing_away (6.0043023) /
5# tearing_apart (6.0043025).
hitting->
9# beating (6.004311) /
10# beating_up (6.004312) /
2# biting (6.004313) /
1# crashing_against (6.004315) /
1# doing_over (6.004316) /
0.2# hammering (6.004317) /
0.2# hammering_down (6.004318) /
0.2# hammering_at (6.004319) /
25# hitting_as_such (6.0043110) /
7# hitting_at (6.0043111) /
7# hitting_out_at (6.0043112) /
7# kicking (6.0043113) /
4# knocking_about (6.0043114) /
4# laying_into (6.0043115) /
4# pushing (6.0043116) /
4# poking (6.0043117) /
4# pinching (6.0043118) /
0.2# running_down (6.0043119) /
0.2# running_over (6.0043120) /
1# running_through (6.0043121) /
4# shaking (6.002865) /
0.2# shelling (6.0043123) /
0.2# shooting_off (6.0043124) /
0.2# shooting_away (6.0043125) /
4# striking (6.0043126) /
1# striking_against (6.0043127) /
0.2# swinging_at (6.0043128) /
339
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
0.2# tearing_at (6.0043129) /
1# whipping (6.0043130).
killing->
2# assasinating (6.004321) /
5# choking (6.004322) /
5# drowning (6.004323) /
5# hanging (6.004324) /
37# killing_as_such (6.004325) /
5# knocking_off (6.004326) /
20# murdering (6.004327) /
5# shooting (6.004328) /
5# suffocating (6.004329) /
1# doing_in (6.0043210) /
1# doing_away_with (6.0043211) /
2# bumping_off (6.0043212) /
1# asphyxiating (6.0043213) /
1# burning_to_death (6.0043214) /
1# taking_out (6.0043215) /
2# putting_down (6.0043216) /
2# executing (6.0043217).
hurting->
20# burning (6.002637) /
20# cutting (6.004332) /
5# harming (6.004333) /
20# hurting_as_such (6.004334) /
5# injuring (6.004335) /
0.5# putting_out (6.004336) /
0.5# dislocating (6.004337) /
0.5# skinning (6.004338) /
0.5# blinding (6.004339) /
4# rubbing (6.0043310) /
1.5# scratching (6.0043311) /
0.5# scraping (6.0043312) /
1# stinging (6.0043313) /
20# wounding (6.0043314) /
0.25# twisting (6.0043315) /
0.25# (6.002426) /
0.5# tearing (6.0043317).
touching->
2.5 being_at (6.004341) /
5# tapping (6.004342) /
5# brushing (6.004343) /
2.5# fingering (6.004344) /
30# pressing (6.004345) /
5# rubbing (6.004346) /
50# touching_as_such (6.004347).
breaking->
40# breaking_as_such (6.002541) /
2# bursting (6.004352) /
0.4# chipping (6.002555) /
3# cracking (6.002552) /
3# crashing (6.004355) /
3# crushing (6.002556) /
0.4# fracture (6.002545) /
3# ripping (6.002558) /
3# shattering (6.002539) /
340
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
7# smashing (6.002535) /
3# snapping (6.002537) /
0.2# splintering (6.002549) /
7# splitting (6.002548) /
2# splitting_open (6.0043514) /
7# tearing (6.002559) /
3# tearing_up (6.0043516) /
7# collapsing (6.002532) /
7# damaging (6.0043518).
mending ->
30# mending_as_such (6.004361) /
34# fixing (6.004362) /
30# repairing (6.004363) /
2# touching_up (6.004364) /
2# taking_in (6.004365) /
2# screwing_down (6.004366).
//**if casual then M < ‘fix’, if consultative then M < ‘mend’, if formal then M < ‘repair’**//
cutting ->
1# sawing (6.004371) /
85# cutting_as_such (6.004372) /
5# cutting_into (6.004373) /
5# cutting_off (6.004374) /
5# cutting_up (6.004375) /
5# cutting_at (6.004314) /
1# snipping (6.004376) /
1# trimming (6.004377) /
2# carving (6.004378) /
0.1# dicing (6.004379) /
0.8# filing (6.0043710) /
0.1# shredding (6.0043711) /
2# tearing (6.0043712) /
2# shaving (6.0043713).
not_affecting_by_contact ->
100# missing (6.004381).
change_of_state_ag_af ->
40# changing_as_such /
6# evaluative_change_of_state_ag_af /
6# cooking /
2# changing_consistency /
6# changing_quality /
10# changing_size /
2# changing_number /
2# changing_temperature /
2# changing_strength /
2# changing_fullness /
2# changing_appearance /
2# changing_consciousness /
2# changing_dryness /
6# shaping /
10# developing.
change_as_such ->
80# changing (6.002323) /
5# altering (6.002324) /
5# transforming (6.002325) /
341
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
5# turning_around (6.004404) /
5# moving (6.002327).
evaluative_change_of_state_ag_af ->
70# change_for_better_ag_af /
30# worsening (6.002827) /
change_for_better_ag_af ->
50# improving (6.002823) /
50# making_better (6.004407).
cooking ->
70# cooking_as_such_ag_af (6.002511) /
0.1# overcooking (6.004411) /
4.9# cooking_in_liquid_ag_af /
17# cooking_in_oven_ag_af /
8# cooking_by_direct heat_ag_af.
cooking_in_liquid_ag_af ->
95# cooking_in_water_ag_af /
2.5# cooking_in_oil_ag_af /
2.5# cooking_in_juices_ag_af.
cooking_in_water_ag_af /
0.5# blanching (6.0044101) /
90# boiling (6.002512) /
0.5# coddling (6.0044103) /
0.5# parboiling (6.0044104) /
3.5# poaching (6.002517) /
5# simmering (6.002516) /
cooking_in_oil_ag_af /
5# deep-frying (6.0044107) /
80# frying (6.002515) /
5# pan-frying (6.0044109) /
5# saute-ing (6.002519) /
5# stir-frying (6.0044111) /
cooking_in_juices_ag_af.
70# stewing (6.002518) /
30# pot-roasting (6.0044113).
cooking_in_oven_ag_af ->
47# baking (6.002522) /
2# braising (6.0044115) /
2# mircrowaving (6.0044116) /
2# oven-frying (6.0044117) /
47# roasting (6.002521) /
change_consistency ->
20# eating_away_at (6.004420) /
342
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
15# reducing (6.004421) /
15# firing (6.004422) /
25# dissolving (6.004423) /
25# freezing (6.002612).
change_quality ->
15# change_smoothness_ag_af /
15# change_hardness_ag_af /
15# change_tightness_ag_af /
15# change_flavour_ag_af /
15# change_sharpness_ag_af /
5# levelling (6.004425) /
5# dirtying (6.004426) /
5# steadying (6.004427) /
5# standardizing (6.004428) /
5# steepening (6.002718).
change_smoothness_ag_af ->
70# making_smooth /
30# roughening (6.002715).
making_smooth ->
55# ironing (6.004431) /
15# smoothing (6.004432) /
15# levelling (6.004433) /
15# rolling (6.004434) /
change_hardness_ag_af ->
50# hardening_as_such (6.002639) /
50# softening_as_such (6.002646).
change_tightness_ag_af ->
40# making_slack /
60# tightening_as_such (6.002723).
making_slack ->
25# slackening (6.004438) /
75# loosening (6.004439).
change_flavour_ag_af ->
5# salting (6.004440) /
45# sweetening (6.002721) /
5# browning (6.004442) /
45# flavouring (6.004443).
change_sharpness_ag_af ->
65# sharpening_as_such (6.002716) /
35# blunting (6.004445).
change_size ->
70# increasing_size_ag_af /
30# decreasing_size_ag_af.
increasing_size_ag_af ->
2# enlargening (6.002725) /
15# extending (6.004447) /
40# increasing (6.002728) /
1# inflating (6.0044497) /
2# putting_up (6.0044410) /
2# lengthening (6.002734) /
343
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
5# lifting (6.0044412) /
1# broadening (6.002731) /
1# fattening (6.002732) /
5# widening (6.002736) /
5# heightening (6.002733) /
5# deepening (6.0044417) /
1# thickening (6.002735) /
15# developing (6.0044419).
decreasing_size_ag_af ->
5# bringing_down (6.004450) /
5# contracting (6.002745) /
10# cutting (6.004452) /
15# cutting_back (6.004453) /
15# cutting_down (6.004454) /
5# decreasing (6.002744) /
15# knocking_down (6.004456) /
15# reducing (6.002752) /
2# lessening (6.002749) /
2# lightening (6.004459) /
2# shortening (6.002751) /
2# compressing (6.002743) /
2# thinning (6.002748) /
2# shrinking (6.0044513).
change_number ->
20# splitting (6.004460) /
20# dividing (6.002766) /
20# breaking _up (6.002542) /
20# dividing_up (6.004463) /
8# doubling (6.002759) /
4# tripling (6.002762) /
4# treble (6.002763) /
4# quadruple (6.002761).
change_temperature ->
50# increasing_temperature /
50# decreasing_temperature.
increasing_temperature ->
25# heating (6.002769) /
25# heating_up (6.002771) /
5# overheating (6.002772) /
10# warming (6.004473) /
25# warming_up (6.002768) /
10# warming_through (6.002767).
decreasing_temperature ->
30# cooling (6.004476) /
35# cooling_down (6.002774) /
35# chilling (6.002775).
change_speed ->
50# increasing_speed /
50# decreasing_speed.
increasing_speed ->
70# speeding_up (6.002778) /
10# quickening (6.002776) /
10# hastening (6.004482) /
344
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
10# hurrying (6.004483).
decreasing_speed ->
70# slowing_down (6.002782) /
15# decelerating (6.002783) /
15# slowing_up (6.002781).
change_strength ->
50# increasing_strength /
50# decreasing_strength.
increasing_strength ->
70# strengthening (6.002785) /
30# toughening (6.002786).
decreasing_strength ->
30# dulling (6.004492) /
35# killing (6.004493) /
35# weakening (6.002784).
change_fullness ->
50# increasing_fullness /
50# decreasing_fullness.
increasing_fullness ->
20# filling (6.002787) /
20# filling_in (6.004501) /
20# filling_out (6.004502) /
40# filling_up (6.002788) /
decreasing_fullness ->
50# emptying (6.002792) /
50# draining (6.002791).
change_appearance ->
10# blurring (6.002652) /
10# colouring (6.004511) /
10# colouring_in (6.004512) /
40# fading (6.002655) /
10# inking (6.004514) /
10# lightening (6.004515) /
10# shading_in (6.004516).
change_in_consciousness ->
70# to_conscious /
30# to_unconscious.
to_conscious ->
5# awakening (6.004520) /
85# waking (6.004521) /
5# bringing_round (6.004527) /
5# bringing_to (6.004528).
to_unconscious ->
40# sending_to_sleep (6.004523) /
30# knocking_out (6.004524) /
30# laying_out (6.004526) /
change_dryness ->
10# wetting (6.004530) /
345
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
60# drying (6.002626) /
10# drying_up (6.004532) /
10# watering (6.004533) /
5# damping (6.004534)/
5# dampening (6.004535).
shaping ->
3# arching (6.004540) /
3# coiling (6.004541) /
3# curling (6.004542) /
3# curving (6.002423) /
30# bending (6.002421) /
15# shaping (6.004546) /
3# working (6.004547) /
3# creasing (6.004548) /
3# crinkling (6.004549) /
3# folding (6.0045410) /
5# flattening (6.0045411) /
5# forming (6.0045412) /
5# pulling_apart (6.0045413) /
10# straightening (6.002718) /
3# twisting (6.002426) /
3# wrinkling (6.0045416).
developing ->
30# aging (6.002815) /
25# bringing_on (6.004551) /
5# building_up (6.004552) /
30# developing_as_such (6.002811) /
30# growing (6.002727).
preparing ->
30# preparing_as_such /
30# bodily_preparing /
30# domestic_preparing /
10# food_preparing /
preparing_as_such ->
80# preparing (6.004601)
0.5# binding (6.004602) /
0.5# breaking_in (6.004603) /
0.5# composing (6.004604) /
0.5# curing (6.004605) /
8# setting_out (6.004606) /
5# laying_out (6.004607) /
5# putting_out (6.004608).
bodily_preparing ->
2# bathing (6.004610) /
60# washing (6.004611) /
2# dressing (6.004612) /
2# undressing (6.004613) /
2# shaving (6.004614) /
20# showering (6.004615) /
2# bp_body_part_specified.
bp_body_part_specified ->
70# brushing (6.0046101)
25# combing (6.0046102)
5# flossing (6.0046103)
346
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
domestic_preparing->
2.5# brushing (6.004620) /
15# cleaning (6.004621) /
5# cleaning_up (6.004622) /
5# clearing (6.002653) /
2.5# clearing_out (6.004624) /
2.5# clearing_up (6.004625) /
2.5# doing_out (6.004626) /
5# dusting (6.004627)
10# hoovering (6.004628) /
2.5# polishing (6.004629) /
2.5# rubbing (6.0046210) /
2.5# shining (6.0046211) /
15# washing (6.0046212) /
2.5# wiping (6.0046213) /
2.5# wiping_out (6.0046214) /
2.5# wiping_up (6.0046215) /
2.5# wiping_down (6.0046216) /
2.5# washing_out (6.0046217) /
5# washing_up (6.0046218) /
2.5# washing_down (6.0046219) /
2.5# washing_away (6.0046220) /
10# sweeping (6.0046221) /
2.5# sweeping_out (6.0046222) /
2.5# sweeping_up (6.0046223).
food_preparing ->
5# skinning (6.004631) /
5# beating (6.004632) /
5# bottling (6.004633) /
5# buttering (6.004634) /
5# dressing (6.004635) /
10# grating (6.004636) /
10# peeling (6.004637) /
10# slicing (6.004638) /
5# smoking (6.004639) /
5# shelling (6.0046310) /
10# whipping (6.0046311) /
10# whisking (6.0046312) /
5# liquidizing (6.0046313) /
5# stuffing (6.0046314) /
5# mincing (6.0046315).
ingesting->
10# generic_ingestion /
50# ingesting_food /
40# ingesting_liquid.
generic_ingestion ->
10# swallowing (6.004641) /
10# ingesting_as_such (6.004642) /
10# digesting (6.004643) /
10# going_through (6.004644) /
10# taking_in (6.004645) /
10# throwing_down (6.004646) /
10# putting_away (6.004647) /
10# bringing_up (6.004648) /
10# holding_down (6.004649) /
10# keeping_down (6.0046410).
347
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
ingesting_food ->
93# eating (6.0046411) /
2# chewing (6.0046412) /
1# masticating (6.0046413) /
1# devouring (6.0046414) /
1# feeding_on (6.0046415) /
1# nibbling (6.0046416) /
1# picking_at (6.0046417).
ingesting_liquid ->
84# drinking (6.0046418) /
1# quaffing (6.0046419) /
2# sipping (6.0046420) /
1# sucking (6.0046421) /
2# lapping_up (6.0046422) /
2# washing_down (6.0046423) /
2# throwing_back (6.0046424) /
2# knocking_back (6.0046425) /
2# swigging (6.0046426).
concealment ->
50# concealing /
50# revealing.
concealing->
3# concealing_as_such (6.004651) /
2# barring (6.004652) /
5# blocking (6.004653) /
2# bottling_up (6.004654) /
5# burying (6.004655) /
30# closing (6.002413) /
30# covering (6.004657) /
2# cutting_out (6.004658) /
2# harbouring (6.004659) /
10# hiding (6.0046510) /
2# shading (6.0046511) /
2# screening (6.0046512) /
2# stopping (6.0046513) /
2# veiling (6.0046514).
revealing->
10# blowing (6.004661) /
10# showing (6.004662) /
70# revealing_as_such (6.004663) /
10# baring (6.004664).
change_position_ag_af ->
40# relative_to_opening_in_enclosure /
40# relative_to_upright_state /
10# removal_of_closing_device /
9# with_inherent_direction.
0.5# stirring (6.004672) /
0.5# swinging (6.004673) /
relative_to_opening_in_enclosure ->
60# opening_ag_af /
40# closing.
opening_ag_af ->
348
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
95# opening_as_such (6.002411) /
2.5# forcing_open (6.0046741) /
2.5# throwing_open (6.0046742).
closing ->
46# closing_as_such (6.002413) /
46# shutting (6.002142) /
2# drawing (6.0046745) /
2# shutting_up (6.0046746) /
2# putting_to (6.0046747) /
2# pulling_to (6.0046748).
relative_to_upright_state_ag_af ->
80# knocking_over (6.0046749) /
5# upsetting (6.0046750) /
5# felling (6.0046751) /
5# upending (6.0046752) /
5# kicking_over (6.0046753).
removal_of_closing_device ->
90# opening (6.002411) /
2.5# cracking_open (6.0046754) /
2.5# twisting_off (6.0046755).
5# removing (6.0046756) /
with_inherent_direction ->
5# vehicle_wise /
95# pursuit_wise /
vehicle_wise ->
backing_up (6.0046757).
pursuit_wise ->
20# pursuing_as_such (6.0046761) /
3# getting_after (6.0046762) /
20# following (6.0046763) /
3# creeping_up_on (6.0046764) /
3# being_after (6.0046765) /
20# going_after (6.0046766) /
3# tracking (6.0046767) /
7# running_after (6.0046768) /
7# coming_after (6.0046769) /
7# coming_for (6.0046770) /
7# making_after (6.0046771).
using ->
0.5# beginning_to_use_ag_af /
90# using_as_such_ag_af /
9.5# using_completely_ag_af.
beginning_to_use_ag_af ->
100# breaking_into (6.004681).
using_as_such_ag_af ->
90# using_as_such (6.004682)/
2# drawing_on (6.004683) /
2# running_down (6.004684) /
2# running_off (6.004685) /
2# running_on (6.004686) /
2# swallowing_up (6.004687).
349
Appendix B2: The system network for two-role, agent plus affected action
Processes
using_completely_ag_af ->
10# using_up (6.004688) /
5# getting_through (6.004689) /
50# finishing (6.0046810) /
10# finishing_up (6.0046811) /
10# finishing_off (6.0046812) /
5# being_through (6.0046813) /
10# running_out_of (6.0046814).
350
Appendix B3: The system network for three-role, directional Processes
&
2# return_to_original_location /
98# no_return_to_original_location.
&
60# no_subsidiary_direction /
40# plus_subsidiary_direction.
agent_not_accompanying_af_ca ->
40# distance_implied /
60# no_distance_implied.
distance_implied ->
50# sending_as_such /
5# electronic_sending /
35# sending_via_post_office /
10# sending_by_vehicle.
sending_as_such ->
98# sending (3.0041101) /
2# getting_off_to (3.0041102). Sp rule – no subsidiary direction
electronic_sending ->
60# emailing (3.0041103) /
39# faxing (3.0041104) /
1# beaming (3.0041105).
sending_via_post_office ->
50# mailing (3.0041106) /
50# posting (3.0041107).
sending_by_vehicle ->
20# shipping (3.0041108) /
40# flying_in (3.0041109) /
40# flying_over (3.0041110).
movement_with_force ->
56# propelling_af_ca_using_body_part /
16# propelling_af_ca_using_equipment /
14# shooting (3.0041111) /
14# firing (3.0041112).
propelling_af_ca_using_body_part ->
35# typically_using_arm /
32# typically_using_foot (3.0041114)./
33# using_any_body_part (3.0041115)..
typically_using_arm ->
50# tua_af_ca_controlled_throughout (3.00411131)/
50# tua_af_ca_initially_controlled (3.00411132).
movement_without_force ->
90# agent_has_full_control _of_movement /
10# agent_has_initial_control_of_movement.
agent_has_full_control_of_movement ->
50# unmarked_full_control /
10# substance_specific_full_control /
10# full_control_involving_dispersable_af_ca /
8# full_control_of_af_ca_into_container /
1# full_control_of_af_ca_into_earth (3.0042122) /
1# full_control_using_implement /
20# full_control_with_inherent_direction.
unmarked_full_control ->
40# putting (3.0042101) /
5# placing (3.0042102) /
10# setting (3.0042103) /
40# moving (3.0042104) /
1# sticking (3.0042105) /
2# spreading (3.0042106) /
2# transferring (3.0042107).
substance_specific_twf ->
40# sst_involving_liquid /
30# sst_involving_light /
30# sst_involving_air.
sst_involving_liquid ->
98# pouring (3.0042108) /
0.5# flooding (3.0042109) /
1# squirting (3.0042110) /
0.5# spurting (3.0042111).
sst_involving_light ->
29# beaming (3.0042112) /
70# shining (3.0042113) /
1# glaring (3.0042114).
sst_involving_air ->
70# blowing (3.0042115) /
352
Appendix B3: The system network for three-role, directional Processes
full_control_of_af_ca_into_container ->
50# packing (3.0042120) /
50# packing_up (3.0042121). Sp rule – no specified direction.
full_control_using_implement ->
40# pinning (3.0042123) /
20# nailing (3.0042124) /
40# sweeping (3.0042125).
full_control_with_inherent_direction ->
25# full_control_inherently_in_relation_to_upright_position /
25# full_control_inherently_upwards /
25# full_control_inherently_downwards /
25# full_control_inherently_out_of.
full_control_inherently_in_relation_to_upright_position ->
25# laying (3.0042126) /
25# leaning (3.0042127) /
25# sitting (3.0042128) /
25# seating (3.0042129).
full_control_inherently_upwards ->
35# lifting (3.0043101) /
30# raising (3.0043102) /
35# picking_up (3.0043103). Sp rule – no subsidiary direction
full_control_inherently_downwards ->
35# dropping (3.0043104) /
35# lowering (3.0043105) /
30# sinking (3.0043106).
full_control_inherently_out_of ->
30# withdrawing (3.0043107) /
40# removing (3.0043108) /
30# sending_down_from (3.0043109). Sp rule – no subsidiary direction.
agent_has_initial_control_over_movement ->
20# manner_of_movement _specified /
80# manner_of_movement _unspecified.
353
Appendix B3: The system network for three-role, directional Processes
agent_accompanying_af_ca ->
60# performer_viewpoint_specified /
20# performer_support_specified /
20# performer_leadership_specified.
performer_viewpoint_specified ->
50# towards_performer_viewpoint (3.0043119)/
50# not_towards_performer_viewpoint.
not_towards_performers_viewpoint ->
90# unimpeded_movement /
10# impeded_movement.
unimpeded_movement ->
80# taking (3.0043120) /
5# delivering (3.0043121) / sp rule – no subsidiary direction
5# tearing (3.0043122) / Sp rule – prefers source
5# extracting (3.0043123) / sp rule – prefers source
10# removing (3.0043124) / sp rule – prefers source
5# fishing_out (3.0043125). Sp rule – no subsidiary direction.
impeded_movement ->
60# getting (3.0043126) /
40# picking_out_of (3.0043127). Sp rule – no subsidiary direction
performer_support_specified ->
60# non_impeded_support (3.0043128) /
40# impeded_support.
impeded_support ->
30# dragging (3.0043129) /
70# pulling (3.0043130) /
performer_leadership_specified ->
80# performer_initiated_pls /
20# unmarked_pls.
performer_initiated_pls ->
1# marching (3.0044101) /
10# running (3.0044102) /
1# rushing (3.0044103) /
1# steering (3.0044104) /
8# walking (3.0044105) /
0.6# flying (3.0044106) / sp rule – don’t prefer ‘in’ in subsidiary direction
10# driving (3.0044107) /
1# riding (3.0044108) /
25# seeing (3.0044109) /
25# showing (3.0044110) /
0.2# galloping (3.0044111) /
0.2# trotting (3.0044112) /
8# backing_up (3.0044113) /
8# reversing (3.0044114) /
354
Appendix B3: The system network for three-role, directional Processes
1# sailing (3.0044115).
not_performer_initiated_pls ->
7# heading (3.0044116) /
40# leading (3.0044117) /
40# guiding (3.0044118) /
6# drawing (3.0044119) /
7# hurrying (3.0044120).
355
Appendix B3: The system network for three-role, directional Processes
356
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
Example: ‘Ike washed the dog’ to ‘What Ike did was to wash the dog’.
‘The sun melted the snow’ to What the sun did was to melt the snow.’
Thus an Agent is typically animate and usually human - but not necessarily,
because a wide range of objects have ‘creature-like’ qualities - from robots and
computers to the sun and even a pillar, as in That pillar holds up / supports the floor
above.
T1a for Agent (Ag) preceded by from (only in 3-role ‘cognition’ Processes)
Example: ‘Ivy found out the answer from Fred’ to ‘What Fred did was to cause Ivy
to find out the answer’. (See Example 3 in the worked examples at the end
357
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
of this section.)
Examples:
‘Ike washed the dog’ to ‘What happened to the dog was that Ike washed it’.109
‘The snow melted’ to ‘What happened to the snow was that it melted’.
‘What was happening to the bacon was that it was sizzling.’
The Created is typically used in the type of two-role ‘action’ Process which
leads to ‘bringing objects into being’, i.e. the ‘creation’ of an object. Examples are
‘painting a picture (but not a wall)’, ‘writing a letter / book’ or ‘building a palace’ , as
in the first example below.
But it is also used, by extension, for ‘influential’ Processes in which an Agent
brings into being an event - and events, as we have seen, may be expressed either in a
full clause or in a nominalization, as in the examples given below under ‘influential’
Processes. If the event is not already a nominalization, it may help the re-expressed
109
Alternatively, if you prefer, it could be re-expressed as ‘What happened to the dog was that it was
washed by Ike’.
358
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
version to sound more natural if you first nominalize the embedded clause, e.g. by
turning me to go in He allowed me to go into my going. However, it has to be
admitted that even when this is done these re-expressions still sound rather less
completely natural. (We will come in a moment to the reason for deciding, on
balance, to treat this role as a Created rather than a Range.)
Less frequently, the Created is found in a one-role process, as in a fight
erupted and a fire started. It is because the PR is clearly the same in The race started
and He started the race that the decision came down on the side of treating both He
started the race and He started running in the same manner. See the second ‘action
Process example below.110
Examples:
‘action’ Process:
‘John painted a picture’ to ‘What came into being was a picture’.
‘A fire / the race started’ to ‘What came into being was a fire / the race’.
‘influential’ processes:
‘He made Ivy cry’ to ‘What came into being was Ivy’s crying’.
‘He started (me) swimming’ to ‘what came into being was (me) swimming’.
‘He started the race’ to ‘What came into being was the race’.
‘He allowed me to go’ to ‘What came into being was my going’.
‘He stopped me from crying’ to ‘What didn’t come into being was my crying’.
The Range is the PR for which it is hardest to provide a positive test. Its main
use is in two-role ‘action’ clauses, where it typically occurs with an Agent, and where
the entity being tested is not an Affected or a Created. In other words, while (in an
110
It might seem that an alternative test for the Created could be ‘What was brought into being was X’,
rather than ‘What came into being was X’. This works well for the two role Processes, but since we
want one test that covers both two-role and one-role Processes ‘What came into being was X’ is
preferable.
359
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
111
Halliday (1994:146-9) does not make this distinction, which I see as an important one. He treats
both cases as Ranges. The need for the distinction become clear when one adds a cognitive dimension
to one’s model of language and its use, so that one has to decide whether or not a given nominal group
in a text has a referent (and so represents an ‘object’ in the belief system). See the discussion in
Section 1.5.
360
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
Please also note that in the re-expression test for ‘influential’ Processes below
we sometimes need to add a form of the ‘pro-verb’ do (as underlined in the examples)
112
Another test that may at first seem tempting is the passivisation test, e.g. re-expressing Whymper
climbed the Matterhorn as The Matterhorn was climbed by Whymper. (PLUS failure in the Affected
and Created tests). But when the Range is the type that occurs in an ‘influential’ or ‘event-relating’
Process this test can lead to misleading results, when used by an inexperienced analyst. This is
because, while it is possible to ‘passivize’ many such examples, e.g. by re-expressing Ivy stopped me
reading it as My reading it was stopped by Ivy, the more natural ‘passive’ equivalent of this example is
I was stopped reading it by Ivy - a construction which involves the ‘raising’ of a element of the
embedded clause to function as an element of the matrix clause (for which see Chapter 22 of Fawcett
2001). A second problem is that there are no passive equivalents of some ‘event-relating’ Processes.
361
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
‘He stopped me reading it’ to ‘What he stopped me (from) doing was reading
it’.
‘He succeeded in hitting it’ to ‘What he succeeded in doing was hitting it’.
‘He failed to hit it’ to ‘What he failed to do was to hit it’.
‘It went on bending’ to ‘What it went on doing was bending’.
In this last set of examples it has been necessary to use the alternative test.
Using the first test would have resulted in some very clumsy sentences. They are quite
complex before we apply any tests, with two embedded events, and the standard test
makes them even more complex - but they are not impossible.
362
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
This test must be carried out before any of T7-T11, because the latter all
assume that the Carrier has been identified. The Carrier occurs both as a simple PR
and as an Agent-Carrier (Ag-Ca) or and Affected Carrier (Af-Ca) See Section 6
below for compound roles.
363
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
See the last two examples below for this re-expression test.
Examples:
‘Ivy was happy’ to ‘The thing about Ivy was that she was happy’.
‘Ivy was the boss’ to ‘The thing about Ivy was that she was the boss’.
‘Ivy was in Rome’ to ‘The thing about Ivy was that she was in Rome’.
‘Ivy went to Rome’ to ‘The thing about Ivy was that she went to in Rome’
(where Ivy is an Agent as well as a Carrier).
‘Ivy had fair hair’ to ‘The thing about Ivy was that she had fair hair’.
‘The sun was shining’ to ‘The thing about the sun was that it was shining’.
‘For Ike to hit Fred was bad’ to
‘The thing about Ike’s hitting Fred was that it was bad’.
Examples:
‘Ivy was happy’ to ‘Happy is what / how Ivy was’.
‘Ivy was a year tutor’ to ‘A year tutor is what Ivy was’.
‘Ivy became / got rich’ to ‘Rich is what / how Ivy was as a result.’
‘Ivy became a year tutor’ to ‘A year tutor is what Ivy was as a result.’
‘Ivy became the boss’ to ‘The boss is what Ivy was as a result’.
‘Ike / the decision made Ivy happy’ to
‘Happy is what / how Ivy was as a result.’
‘They elected Ivy (as) the boss’ to ‘The boss is what Ivy was as a result’.
364
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
‘Two and two make four’ to ‘Four is what two and two are’.
‘Ivy weighed / was 60 kilos’ to ‘60 kilos is what Ivy was’.
‘Yesterday was like my birthday’ to
‘Like my birthday is what / how yesterday was’.
‘Ike was in a temper / heaven’ to ‘In a temper / heaven is how Ike was’.
Interestingly, the above clause fails the Location test (T8); i.e. we do not say
*’In a temper / heaven is where Ike was’.
Examples:
‘Ivy lived in Cardiff in 1992’ to ‘(In) Cardiff is where Ivy was in 1992’.
‘Their wedding was in 1935’ to ‘(In) 1935 was when their wedding was’.
‘Ike was with Ivy’ to ‘With Ivy was where Ike was’.
Examples:
‘Ivy went to Peru / IBM’ to ‘Ivy went to Peru / IBM’.
‘Fred put it on the desk’ to ‘It went to the desk’.
‘He threw the stone at the wall’ to ‘the stone went towards the wall’.
365
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
This role (Destination) and the next (Source) are NOT used in any of the
Processes of (a) ‘X giving Y Z’ (or ‘X giving Z to Y’), (b) ‘X taking Z from Y’, (c) X
getting Y’ or (d) ‘X losing Y’, which are all types of ‘possession’ Process.
Recognizing this enables a wider range of generalizations to be captured in a more
economical manner than is done in some other approaches.
Examples:
‘Ivy left Peru/IBM’ to ‘Ivy went away from Peru/IBM’.
‘Fred took it off the desk’ to ‘it went away from the desk’.
‘He threw the out of the yard’ to ‘the stone went away from the yard’.
Examples:
‘Ivy drove past / passed the cottage’ to ‘Ivy went via the cottage’.
‘Ivy went across / crossed the field’ to ‘Ivy went via the field’.
‘Fred pushed it through the hole’ to ‘it went via the hole’.
366
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
Examples:
‘Ivy had a car / problem’ to ‘A car/ problem was what Ivy had’.
‘The car needs a new tyre’ to ‘A new tyre was what the car lacked’.
‘Ivy got/ caught a cold’ to ‘A cold was what Ivy had as a result’.
‘Ike gave Ivy a car / a cold’ to‘ A car / a cold was what Ivy had (as a result).
‘Ivy lost the key’ to ‘The key was what Ivy lacked (as a result)’.
Examples:
‘Ike liked Ivy’ to
‘Ike had a good feeling about Ivy’.
‘Ivy loathed caviar’ to
‘Ivy had a bad feeling about caviar’.
367
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
The term ‘physically’ is needed, because many verbs (such as see and perceive
itself) are also used in ‘cognition’ Processes. The expression ‘as a result’ is needed
when the verb is show, display, etc.
Examples:
‘Ike saw the dog’ to
‘Ike physically perceived the dog’.
‘Ike saw it gnawing the bone’ to
‘Ike physically perceived it gnawing the bone’.
‘Ike showed Ivy his tonsils’ to ‘
‘Ivy physically perceived his tonsils (as a result)’.
368
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
Examples:
‘Ike knew / remembered / forgot (about) Fred’ to
‘Ike knew Fred’.
‘Ivy realised that it was late’ to
‘Ivy knew that it was late (as a result)’.
‘Ike forgot the answer’ to
‘Ike didn’t know the answer’.
‘Ike told Ivy the answer’ to
‘Ivy knew the answer as a result.
‘Ivy told / said to Ike to buy it’ to
‘Ike knew to buy it as a result’.
‘Ike remembered eating it’ to
He knew that he had eaten it (as a result)’.
‘Ike forgot to buy the bread’ to
Ike didn’t know to buy the bread (as a result)’.
‘Ike thought / imagined / believed that Ivy was there’ to
‘Ike thought that Ivy was there’.
‘It seemed / appeared to Ike that Ivy was there’ to
‘Ike thought that Ivy was there’.
While most PRs are typically filled by a ‘thing’, the Phenomenon is typically
filled by an ‘event’. It is therefore typically filled by a clause, though it may also be
369
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
The unusual wording ‘as a precondition for asking’ is needed ONLY when the
clause reports a question. (An example in the ‘three-role cognition’ Processes below
illustrates this point.)
Examples:
emotion Processes:
‘Ike enjoyed his lunch’ to ‘Ike had a good feeling about his lunch’.
‘Ike wanted to visit Rome’ to ‘Ike had a good feeling about visiting Rome’.
‘The noise annoyed Ivy’ to ‘Ivy had a bad feeling about the noise’.
perception Processes:
‘Ike saw the dog’ to ‘Ike physically perceived the dog’.
113
The last part of the test is to enable it to identify the type of Phenomenon that occurs when the claue
reports an ‘information seeker’ or a ‘proposal for action’.
370
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
cognition Processes:
‘Ike discovered that he was late’ to ‘Ike knew that he was late (as a result)’.
‘Ike planned to visit Rome’ to ‘Ike thought to visit Rome’.
‘Ike told Ivy "Fred’ll be there"‘ so’ Ivy knew / thought "Fred’ll be there" as a
result’.
‘Ike told Ivy "I’ll be there"‘ so ‘Ivy knew / thought "Ike’ll be there" as a
result’.
114
Strictly speaking, the test for questions used in this example would be more accurate if we added at
the beginning ‘Z thought ...’ (where Z is the referent of the possible Agent in the original clause), e.g.
like this: ‘Ike thought Ivy knew whether Fred was there (as a pre-condition for asking)’. In other
words, it is a precondition of asking an ‘information seeking’ question (though not a ‘test’ question)
that the performer thinks that the Addressee may know the answer.
371
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
Examples:
‘Her shirt goes with her jeans’ to
115
At first, then, it may seem that two different roles are involved, which we might call a ‘Matchee’
and a ‘Combinee’. It makes little difference to the overall model whether we set up two different PRs
with more closely defined criteria, or one PR with a broader set of criteria. The reason why it is
possible to handle the two together is that the two broad Process types of ‘matching’ and ‘joining’ often
occur together. In other words, while a process of ‘matching’ does not necessarily lead on to a process
of ‘joining’, a process of ‘joining’ is typically preceded a ‘matching’ process. Here we have decided to
avoid adding another PR to the list of seventeen simple PRs by using the term ‘Matchee’ for the two
types, in part because of the relative infrequency of both.
372
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
373
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
In Relational Processes:
In Mental Processes:
374
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
We will work through the analysis of four examples involving compound PRs.
The first two are ‘relational’ Processes (with two and three PRs respectively) and the
third is a ‘mental’ Process with three PRs. The fourth is an example of how to tackle a
problem case where the tests do NOT provide a clear answer.
375
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
(Recall that, in all types of ‘relational’ Processes, the Carrier must be tested first, i.e.
before the Attribute, Location, Possessed or Matchee.)
But note the condition on T6: ‘PLUS, for a SIMPLE Carrier (i.e. not a
COMPOUND Carrier), failure in the Agent AND Affected tests’. We must therefore
test next to see whether ‘Ivy’ is also an Agent or an Affected.
NOTE: Interestingly, then, the test shows that ‘remaining somewhere’ counts as
an act of ‘doing’, even though it consists of NOT moving. This suggests that the
concepts of ‘planning’ and ‘deciding’, and so taking responsibility’ are more
central to the concept of an Agent than ‘physical movement’. Note too that, if the
Carrier had been the parcel instead of Ivy, it would not have passed the test for
Agent. On the other hand, it would have passed the test for the Affected (‘What
happened to the parcel was that it remained in Rome’), so that it would be an
Affected-Carrier, not a simple Carrier.
Thus the PRs are an Agent-Carrier and a Location. The Process is therefore
the ‘locational’ type of ‘relational’ Process.
376
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
377
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
How would we analyze the following related clauses: ‘Ivy lent the key to Ike’,
‘Ivy was lent the key by Ike’, and ‘Lend the key to Ivy’? Even though the sequence
of the PRs is different, exactly the same set of tests apply. Remember the guidelines
for preparing a clause for testing. You may wish to try out the test on these examples,
to check that they work. But be sure to apply the Carrier test before the Possessed
test.
You might also like to analyze ‘Ike picked up the book’. This is a Process of
‘someone causing themselves to have something’, i.e. Ag-Ca + Pro + Af-Pos.
378
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
NOTE: Optimists in matters of human relations will accept knew in this test; but
pessimists may not, and may even feel that thought is too positive. (All such tests are
a matter of experience and/or faith and/or taste.)
NOTE: If the clause was ‘Fred asked Fiona whether Ike loved Ivy’, the test for the
Phenomenon would be to re-express it as ‘Fiona knew whether Ike loved Ivy’ (as a
precondition for asking)’ - i.e a precondition of Fred’s asking the question was that he
thought that Fiona knew whether Ike loved Ivy. If the clause was ‘Ivy told Ike to go’,
the re-expression would simply be ‘Ike knew to go (as a result)’.
The problem is that this is NOT the usual sense of hear - where the PRs would
be a Perceiver and a Phenomenon. Here there are three PRs. (If you think from Ivy is
NOT a PR, you are faced with the problem of identifying what type of Adjunct it is.
And there is none in Chapter 3 that fits.)
When faced with a difficult case, such as this, the best strategy is (1) to think
paradigmatically, and then (2) to try to match the PRs to a well-established set of PRs.
So we ask: " What other lexical verbs can occur in a similar structure?" The answer is
that quite a few can, e.g. discover, learn, realize, etc. (These can all also occur as
simply an Affected-Cognizant + Process + Phenomenon.) It seems clear that a
Process of ‘someone hearing something from someone’ has the same PRs as
‘someone telling someone something’ - and so it is equivalent to ‘someone causing
379
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
NOTE: This is the special case when the normal version of the test for Agent (T1)
need to be supplemented.
All of the other compound roles can all be handled in a similar way.
Remember that, when there is a covert role, you should give it a temporary formal
representation in the clause before applying the tests, e.g. by supplying ‘someone’,
something’, ‘somewhere’ or ‘some time’.
380
Appendix C: The Re-Expression Tests for Participant Roles
381