Assessment of Early Parent Child Relationships
Assessment of Early Parent Child Relationships
Assessment of Early Parent Child Relationships
Keywords: Parent-infant observation, parent–child observation, parent–child interaction, parent–child early rela
tional assessment, relationship assessment, parent–child relationship assessment tools
Page 1 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
vention plan and in evaluating progress during the therapeutic process. Sources of infor
mation should include the parents and other caregivers, the child, the extended family
when indicated, day-care providers, and the primary health care provider. If the family is
involved in other services, such as social services, mental health services, or early inter
vention services, information should also be obtained from these collateral sources.
Through an interview process with the parent, such as the ERA Video Replay Interview
(Clark, 1985, 2010, 2015), the Working Model of the Child Interview (WMCI, Zeanah &
Benoit, 1995), or the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; George, Kaplan, & Main, 1996),
identification of a parent’s internal working model of relationships, or “ghosts in the
nursery” (Fraiberg, Adelson, & Shapiro, 1980), is better understood. Parental attitudes
and perceptions of their infant or child, the meaning of the child’s behavior, and their in
ternal working model (Bretherton, 1985) of what can be expected in a relationship devel
op within their own past experiences in early attachment (p. 48) relationships. Disturbed
or adverse past relationship experiences may result in a parent misinterpreting and pro
jecting negative attributions or feelings onto the infant; for example, the parent may mis
interpret certain behaviors of the infant as demanding, negative, or attacking (Zero to
Three, 1994).
The University of Wisconsin Parent–Infant and Early Childhood Program’s Relational As
sessment Model uses a multimodal approach and actively involves the parents through in
terviews, observations, and parent report assessment instruments (Clark, Seidl, & Paul
son, 1997; Clark, Burk, Hewitt, & Hipke, 2006). Observations of parent–child interactions
are conducted across developmentally salient situations. Assessment procedures are
structured to address particular domains appropriate to the child’s level of development.
For example, infants need emotionally available parents who are capable of reading their
cues and responding in a sensitive and timely fashion (Ainsworth, 1969; Stern, 2002),
whereas toddlers need caregiving that is respectful of their emerging autonomy and pro
vides cognitive and emotional scaffolding, clear expectations, consistent limit setting, and
assistance in managing transitions, with affective and behavioral regulation. Note that an
observation of interactions should be considered just one snapshot in time, whereas the
parent–child relationship represents the child’s and parent’s “sense and quality of con
nectedness” over time and across settings (Clark, 1985, 2010, 2015). The parent’s mood
and parenting capacities, the family’s stress, and the family’s access to and need for so
cial supports and resources should also be assessed through the interview as part of the
parent–child relationship assessment. Assessment approaches in this chapter include in
terview, self-report, and observational measures.
Developing a trusting relationship with the parents is critical to the assessment process.
Taking a collaborative approach with parents throughout the assessment may build such
an alliance. At the onset of the interview, parents should be asked about their concerns
about their child and what they would like to get out of the assessment. By empathically
listening to parents’ experiences of their child and their struggles in parenting, the clini
cian is providing a parallel process of attunement and responsiveness with the parents
Page 2 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
that they can then, with thoughtful exploration and support, provide for their child. Ele
ments of the parent interview that are particularly salient to an assessment of the parent–
child relationship include the following:
1. Demystify the assessment process by explaining the multimodal nature of the as
sessment procedures and the parents’ significant role in the assessment process.
2. Ask parents what their hope is regarding what they will receive from the evalua
tion process.
3. Provide a safe, comfortable, developmentally appropriate environment for the
child/children and parents. Ideally, all members of the family household as defined by
the parents should attend the initial evaluation. Having the whole family present pro
vides the clinician information about family dynamics, including sibling relationships
and cross-generational alliances.
4. Assess the parents’ optimal parenting capacities across several developmentally
salient situations (e.g., routine tasks of daily living such as feeding, diapering, teach
ing and setting limits, play, separations/reunions).
5. Involve the parents in assessing their child’s regulatory capacities and behaviors
and their capacity to see their child as a separate individual by observing the child
together and discussing what you and they are observing.
6. Inquire about presenting problems and parents’ perspectives regarding the mean
ing of the child and his or her behaviors by asking parents to describe their child and
their impressions about the source or cause of the concerns.
7. Include a perinatal history about the pregnancy, labor, and delivery. This time rep
resents the critical beginnings of the child’s relationship with each parent. An un
planned or medically high-risk or stressful pregnancy or a complicated labor or deliv
ery may have profound implications for the parent–child relationship. Ask open-end
ed questions (e.g., What was the pregnancy like for you?) to allow parents to share
those aspects of the experience that are important to them.
8. Involve parents in assessing their relationship by reviewing a video recording of
the parent and child interacting together. Help them focus on strengths, notice their
child’s cues, and validate parents’ concerns. “Wonder along with” the parents about
who their child reminds them of in general and when the problem behaviors are
present. This information may help to elucidate parents’ projections of negative in
tentionality attributed to their child.
9. Assess the sociocultural context of the parent–child relationship, respecting and
appreciating the family’s beliefs and values. Recognize the parents as the experts in
their (p. 49) personal sociocultural environment and ask them to educate you about
their life experiences and worldviews. Seek additional consultation from cultural ex
perts to address the clinician’s cultural knowledge deficits or biases.
10. Provide parents with feedback about the assessment findings with a caring, non
judgmental attitude about the parents or their parenting style. This approach will fa
cilitate a therapeutic joining with the parents vital to the development of a collabora
tive therapeutic relationship.
Page 3 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
When conducting an assessment of the quality of the parent-child relationship using ob
servational methods, there are several key points to remember:
1. Note the intensity, frequency, and duration of the affect and behavior exchanged
between parent and child. This information may differentiate normal interactions
from disturbed interactions and assist the clinician in determining the seriousness of
a relationship problem. For example, the Diagnostic Classification of Mental Health
and Developmental Disorders of Infancy and Early Childhood (DC:0–5) Axis II uses
this information to categorize caregiver–child relationships as levels of quality to dis
tinguish well-adapted, at-risk, compromised, and disordered relationships (Zero to
Three, 2016).
2. Assess the quality of interaction within the context of the situation. For example,
differentiate parental directives or conversation related to structured tasks from play
interactions in which the parent is following the child’s lead or engaging the child in
a mutually enjoyable social interaction.
3. Consider parents’ responses relative to the child’s age and developmental level.
Examples of reading cues and responding sensitively include a mother who adjusts
the way she holds her infant after noticing the child’s discomfort in a particular posi
tion or the father who responds to his toddler tugging at his arm by a caring touch,
talking to, or picking up his child.
4. When the child engages in behavior to seek the parent’s attention, negative test
ing or oppositional behavior, note whether the parent responds to the child verbally
or behaviorally in a way that suggests he or she experiences the child’s behavior as
resistant or “bad.”
5. Note whether the rapidity and regularity with which the parent responds to the
child are contingent on the child’s cues, requests, or needs and helps the child feel
that his or her actions are effective.
6. Differentiate a genuine sense of connectedness from “going through the motions.”
An emotionally connected parent is aware of and involved with the child even when
not actively interacting with the child. The parent is attentive to the child, subtly
monitoring the child with an empathic awareness of the child’s emotional state. Con
nectedness may also include seeing the child as a separate individual.
7. Assess the parent’s capacity to reflect the child’s affect and/or behavior through
echoing (with infants), gazing, confirmation of affect, behavior, approval, encourage
ment, and praise, as well as labeling the child’s internal feeling states. This process
of mirroring represents the parent’s emotional availability and affective attunement
to the infant or young child.
8. Assess the parent’s capacity for scaffolding by looking at the amount and way in
which the parent gains, helps to focus, and sustains the child’s attention to the rele
vant aspects of the situation. Scaffolding is a process in which parents recognize
their child’s developmental capacities and provide a physical and socioemotional en
vironment that gives the child an opportunity to expand his or her capacities (Vygot
sky, 1978). Just as a metal scaffold allows construction workers to build taller build
Page 4 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
ings, parents’ emotional and cognitive scaffolding helps their child reach higher lev
els of cognitive, social, and motor skills as well as emotional and behavioral regula
tion. Scaffolding with a younger infant may be manifested by protective caregiving.
With an older child, this process may include assistance such as teaching, demon
strating, stating expectations clearly, and setting limits with a sensitivity to the
child’s affective and cognitive functioning.
9. Note the parents’ consistency and predictability in their interactions with their
child as well as the parents’ capacity to follow their child’s lead versus directive or
intrusive behavior.
10. Observe from the child’s perspective as well. Ask the question, If I were this
child, what would I see/experience when I look up at my mother or father?
11. Observe the child’s affect, mood, emotional lability, temperament, activity level,
attentional capacities, persistence, quality of play, social initiative and responsive
ness, compliance, (p. 50) communicative and motor competence, visual contact, and
assertion or aggressivity.
12. After the observations, ask the parent(s) how typical the interaction was. If the
parents indicate that it was different from usual, inquire about how it was different
and what the parent(s) attributes this to. For example, parent(s) may state that the
child was much more cooperative than usual and that they rarely have the opportuni
ty to play with their child one on one. Such information informs the diagnostic
process and the planning for therapeutic intervention.
13. Observe the dyadic organization and regulation in the interaction and parent’s
capacity to pace and coordinate with the child’s needs and actions, as well as the af
fective tone of the dyad and capacity for mutual enjoyment, joint attention, reci
procity, and goodness of fit.
Page 5 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 6 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Insightfulness As Parents of toddlers Parent domains: Studies examining Well suited to re
sessment (IA; Ko and preschoolers Insight into child’s the utility of the IA search and clinical
ren-Karie, Oppen motives reveal significant purposes.
heim, Dolev, Sher, & Openness relationships be Promising utility in
Etzion-Carasso, Complexity in de tween IA categories work with both low-
2002; Oppenheim & scription of child and child attach and high-risk chil
Koren-Karie, 2002, Maintenance of fo ment classifications dren and growing
2009; Oppenheim, cus on child and child behavior. evidence that in
Koren-Karie, & Sa Richness of descrip There is growing sightfulness may in
gi, 2001) tion of child evidence of concur crease the impact of
Coherence of rent and predictive intervention.
thought validity (e.g., Lau Video replay proce
Acceptance sanne Trilogue Play dure.
Anger procedure) with co Interviews are tran
Worry ordination and co scribed and coded.
Separateness from operation.
child IA post–Circle of Se
curity Intervention
predicted child se
curity of attach
ment at follow-up.
Page 7 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Parent Develop Parents of infants Parental affective High interrater reli 90-minute semi
ment Interview and young children experiences ability (intraclass structured inter
(PDI)–R (Slade, Degree, acknowl correlation = .78 view with the par
Aber, Bresgi, Berg edgment, and mod to .95). ent (brief version is
er, & Kaplan, 2003); ulation of anger Construct validity 45 minutes).
Reflective function Neediness with the AAI—Re Revised version as
ing codes: Slade, Degree, acknowl flective functioning sesses for reflective
Aber, Bresgi, Berg edgment, and mod on the PDI was functioning.
er, & Kaplan, 2004. ulation of separa shown to be corre 45 items
tion distress lated with reflective
Degree and ac functioning on the
knowledgment of AAI (Slade, 2005;
guilt Slade et al., 2005).
Experience of joy
and pleasure
Sense of competen
cy and efficacy
Child affective ex
periences
Representation of
child anger
Child separation
distress
Child dependence–
independence
State of mind
Coherence
Page 8 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Richness of percep
tion
Working Model of Parents of infants Richness of percep WMCI has demon Grounded in attach
the Child Interview and toddlers tion strated strong inter ment theory.
(WMCI; Zeanah & Openness to change rater reliability for Well suited to re
Barton, 1989; Coherence classification scor search and clinical
Zeanah, Benoit, Intensity of involve ing (k = .76–.79). purposes
Barton, & Hirsh ment Strong concurrent Time intensive; re
berg, 1996) Caregiving sensitiv validity with the quires training and
ity Parent–Child Early coding from an au
Acceptance/rejec Relational Assess dio recording.
tion ment and predictive Structured inter
Infant difficulty validity with the view
Fear of loss Strange Situation. 60 minutes to ad
Affective tone minister
Narrative organiza
tion
Page 9 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 10 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Atypical Maternal Infancy Affective communi Concurrent validity Useful for high-risk
Behavior Instru cation errors with the Strange Si samples.
ment for Assess Role/boundary con tuation has been es
ment and Classifica fusion tablished (intra
tion (Bronfman, Frightened/disori class correlation
Madigan, & Lyons- ented behavior and [ICC] = .75–.84)
Ruth, 1992–2009) voices and the Frighten
Intrusiveness/nega ing, Frightened,
tivity Dissociated or Dis
Withdrawal organized Behavior
on the Part of the
Parent Scale (total
atypical behavior
score, ICC = .67)
Coding Interactive Newborns, infants, Parental sensitivity Validated across a Utility in studies of
Behavior (CIB; Feld toddlers, preschool Parental intrusive wide range of ages both at-risk and
man, 1998) ers, school-age chil ness and cultures. healthy dyads.
dren, adolescents, Parental limit set 43 codes rated on a
and adults ting 5-point Likert scale
Child involvement Prerecorded video
Child withdrawal tape is coded.
Child compliance
Dyadic reciprocity
Page 11 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 12 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Clinical Problem- 24–54 months (with Parent: High internal con Nine episodes—
Solving Procedure modifications as Emotional availabil sistency of items (Ω range of activities is
Rating Scale (Crow young as 12 ity = .88 child items well suited for clini
ell & Feldman, months, see Nurturance/em and .84 for caregiv cal observations.
1988) Zeanah, Larrieu, pathic responsive er items) and corre Parents’ interview
Heller, & Valliere, ness lates with other would be helpful to
2000) Protection measures of parent– establish ecological
Comforting/re child relationship validity.
sponse to distress quality (r = .51–.56,
Teaching p = .001).
Play
Discipline/limit set
ting
Instrumental care/
structure/routines
Infant:
Emotion regulation
Security/trust/self-
esteem
Vigilance/self-pro
tection/safety
Comfort-seeking
Learning/curiosity/
mastery
Play/imagination
Page 13 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Self-control/cooper
ation
Self-regulation/pre
dictability
Emotional Availabil Infancy/early child Parent: Low to high inter Grounded in attach
ity Scales (Biringen, hood version: birth– Sensitivity rater reliability ment and emotional
2000, 2008; Birin 5 years old. Structuring when measuring at availability theo
gen et al., 2005; Nonintrusiveness tachment (k = .24– ries; although pri
Biringen, Robinson, Nonhostility 1.0) and parent– marily used in re
& Emde, 1998) Child: child relationship search, may be use
Responsiveness to quality (k = .70–. ful for assessing in
the parent 98). tervention pro
Involvement of the Strong cross-con grams. Observation
parent text reliability when sessions >15 min
comparing scores utes yield stronger
from a laboratory predictive value.
setting and home Used in over 20
setting (Bornstein countries
et al., 2006).
Nursing Child As Infancy (including Mother: High interrater reli Ratings of home ob
sessment Satellite premature infants) Sensitivity to the ability (k = .89–.92) servations widely
Training (NCAST) through 3 years for child’s cues and internal consis used for clinical and
the Teaching Scale. Response to the tency (α = .80–.82). research purposes.
Birth to 12 months child’s distress NCAST is primarily
for the Feeding Fostering social- used by nurses.
Scale. emotional growth
Page 14 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 15 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Parent–Child Early Birth to 5 years old Parental factors: High interrater reli Theoretically and
Relational Assess Positive affective in ability (.85) and in empirically derived
ment (PCERA) volvement and ver ternal consistency scales rated from
(Clark, 1985, 2010, balization (α = .78–.94). video-recorded ob
2015) Negative affect and Discriminant validi servations to identi
behavior ty has been estab fy areas of strength
Intrusiveness, in lished differentiat and concern. The
sensitivity, and in ing high-risk from PCERA includes a
consistency normative dyads video replay inter
Infant or child fac (parents with de view to engage
tors: pression, other psy parent(s) in assess
Positive affect, com chiatric and alcohol ing their relation
municative and so and other drug ship with their child
cial skills abuse disorders, and for collabora
Quality of play, in premature infants tive goal setting in
terest, and atten and infants with clinical settings.
tional skills other medical con Widely used for re
Dysregulation and ditions) search and clinical
irritability purposes.
Parent–child dyadic 29 parental items,
factors: 28 infant or child
Mutuality and reci items, and 8 dyadic
procity items.
Disorganization and
tension
Video reply and in
terview:
Page 16 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 17 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Parenting Interac 10 to 47 months Affection High interrater reli For use with di
tions with Children: Responsiveness ability (k = .69–.80), verse ethnic
Checklist of Obser Encouragement internal consistency groups, particularly
vations Linked to Teaching (α = .68–.79), and for practitioners
Outcomes (Rog moderate to high working with at-risk
gman et al., 2013) confirmatory facto families.
ry analysis (r = . Checklist of 29 ob
43–.86). servable behaviors.
Content validity
was strong (i.e., av
erage rating of 2.6
of 3).
Construct validity
ranged from low to
moderate at 14, 24,
and 36 months of
age (r = .13–.65).
Predictive validity
was strong for later
child language, cog
nitive, and social-
emotional out
comes.
Page 18 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 19 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Parenting Stress In 1 month to 12 years Parent domains: High internal con Self-report paper-
dex (PSI; Abidin, Competence sistency for the and-pencil instru
1986, 2012) Isolation child (α = .78–.88), ment used clinically
Attachment parent (α = .75–. and in research
Health 87), and total score The fourth edition
Role restriction (α = .98) domains. has been translated
Depression Test–retest reliabili into other lan
Spouse ty was moderate to guages and used in
Child domains: high (.63, .91, and . ternationally
Distractibility/hy 96 for child, parent, Approximately 20
peractivity and total stress minutes to adminis
Adaptability scores, respective ter
Reinforces parent ly). 120 items
Demandingness High internal con The PSI Short Form
Mood sistency on PSI, is useful for clini
Acceptability fourth edition, cians because of its
Life Stress Short Form (α = . ease of use.
88–.90) for parent
domain subscales
and child domain
subscales.
Page 20 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individ
ual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Ratings are made on a 5-point Likert scale for 29 domains of parental functioning, 30 do
mains of infant/child functioning, and 8 domains of dyadic functioning. The amount, dura
tion, and intensity of affect and behavior exhibited by the parent, the infant or child, and
the dyad are rated:
1. Items assessing aspects of parental behavior and affect include parental positive
and negative affect, mood, sensitivity and contingent responsivity to the infant or
child’s cues, flexibility/rigidity, capacity to structure and mediate the environment,
genuine visual regard, connectedness, mirroring, and creativity/resourcefulness, etc.
2. Infant/child items include positive and negative affect, somber/serious mood, irri
tability, social initiative and responsiveness, interest/gaze aversion, assertion/aggres
sivity, persistence, impulsivity, emotional regulation, etc.
3. Dyadic items include mutual enjoyment, tension, reciprocity, joint attention, good
ness of fit, etc.
(p. 51) (p. 52) (p. 53) (p. 54)The parent, infant/child, and dyadic scales were
(p. 55) (p. 56)
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
child relationship (Clark, 1983). The PCERA has been further developed for use with nor
mative and other at-risk populations (Clark, 1999; Clark, Hyde, Essex, & Klein, 1997;
Clark, Paulson, & Conlin, 1993; Clark, Tluczek, & Brown, 2008). Clark (1983) and Good
man and Brumley (1990) found that PCERA scores differentiated patterns of mothers
with depression and mothers with schizophrenic disorders from the patterns of well-func
tioning mothers. In studies of mother–infant interaction with mothers with substance
abuse problems, mothers with a range of psychiatric problems, and well-functioning
mothers, Siqveland, Haabrekke, Wentzel-Larsen, and Moe (2014) found that mother–in
fant interaction quality as measured by the PCERA improved from 3 months to 12 months
for the well-baby group and the group with psychiatric problems; however, the mother–in
fant interaction quality worsened across time for the group with maternal substance
abuse problems. The PCERA has also been found to differentiate positive and less sensi
tive mother–infant interactions with babies in the neonatal intensive care unit (Gerstein,
Poehlmann-Tynan, & Clark, 2015; Korja et al., 2008; Poehlmann, Burnson, & Weymouth,
2014; Weber & Harrison, 2014). When assessing babies with very low birth weights, Stolt
et al. (2014) found that the PCERA factors of maternal positive affective involvement (at 6
months) and maternal communication (at 12 months) were associated with child lan
guage skills at 2 years. The PCERA has excellent internal consistency and concurrent and
predictive validity (Clark, 1999) as well as concurrent validity, specifically with the WMCI
(Korja et al., 2010). The PCERA ratings of early mother–infant interaction have been cor
related with both concurrent measures of child temperament, behavior, and parenting
stress and later quality of mother–child interactions and security of attachment behaviors
at 12 months (Mothander, 1990; Teti, Nakagawa, Das, & Wirth, 1991) and has been found
to document change following therapeutic intervention (Clark, 1999; Minde, Faucon, &
Falkner, 1994; St. Petersburg–USA Orphanage Research Team, 2009). With clinical inves
tigators choosing the PCERA for use in a variety of clinical programs and research
projects internationally, studying high-risk and normative populations, the PCERA has be
come one of the most widely used clinical research measures.
Each of the four situations in the PCERA provides a window for understanding what has
been shared in the early parent–child relationship. Each situation may be experienced dif
ferently by parents, with some eliciting conflictual feelings and others allowing for feel
ings of competence in the parenting role. After the interaction procedure has been ex
plained to parents, written consent is obtained for video recording. The clinician explains
that the video recording is for the family’s benefit so that the parent and clinician can
view the video together in (p. 57) a reflective manner during a subsequent session. The
following explanation is provided to parents: “We understand that this is a snapshot of
one point in time. We’ll be interested in your sharing with us afterwards how the interac
tion is alike or different from how things usually go.” The video-recording procedure in
volves placing the camera at a 45-degree angle to the parent and infant or child, who are
seated together, and using a medium shot to capture the facial expressions, behavior, and
dyadic interactions of the parent and the child.
Page 22 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
During the feeding situation, the parent and young child are provided a snack of juice and
crackers and told, “We are interested in seeing [child’s name] and you during a feeding or
a snack time together. Please be with [child’s name] as you usually would.” If the mother
is breastfeeding, she is asked whether she would be comfortable being video recorded
during a feeding time with her baby. The feeding situation allows for an assessment of the
parent’s capacity for nurturance and social interaction as well as sensitivity to the child’s
cues and need for regulation. The child’s readability, affect regulation, social initiative,
and responsivity during feeding may also be assessed. Comfort, tension, and regulation of
the dyad in this situation are observed.
The instructions and nature of the structured task are determined by the age of the infant
or child. For example, parents of infants under 7 months are asked to change their baby’s
diaper and attempt to get the baby interested in shaking a rattle. Parents of children be
tween 8 and 12 months are given two cups and a toy and asked to hide a toy under one
cup and alternately hide it under the second cup within the child’s sight and have the
child try to find it. It is also suggested that, if time permits, they may read a book togeth
er. For children 13 months and older, parents are asked to build a tower of three cubes
and have the child do the same. With a child of 19 months and older, the task includes
building a tower with more cubes, building a bridge of blocks, and having the child make
a design with colored blocks that matches the increasingly challenging block design
cards. This task is always a little too difficult for the child to complete on his or her own.
The structured task situation allows for an assessment of the parents’ capacity to struc
ture and mediate the environment according to the child’s developmental and individual
needs. Some of the tasks tap the child’s emerging abilities and require adult cognitive
scaffolding as well as emotional availability for the child to complete the task successful
ly. The child’s attentional skills, persistence, and interest in complying with parental ex
pectations in a structured situation are observed. The dyad’s capacity for joint attention
to an activity, reciprocity in negotiations, and mutuality may be assessed.
Instructions for the free play situation are as follows: “This is a free play time with your
child. Please play with your child as you normally would.” For infants under 6 months, the
instructions include: “Here are some toys you may use if you choose.” For children over 6
months, the instructions are: “You or [child’s name] may chose the toy(s) that you would
like to play with together.” The standard toy list for the PCERA includes rattles, plastic
keys, a busy box, two toy telephones, a ball, two puppets, a doll, a bottle, a blanket, small
cars or trucks, bristle blocks, crayons and paper, and plastic animal and human family fig
ures. The free play situation allows for an assessment of the parents’ capacity to be play
ful with and enjoy their child as well as to follow their child’s lead in play. The child’s ca
pacity for exploratory and representational play and the dyad’s capacity for social interac
tion, mutuality, and reciprocity can be observed.
At the end of the instructions for the free play, parents are also given instructions for the
separation/reunion episode. They are told, “We’d also like to see how things go for
[child’s name] when you leave the room. After 5 minutes of play I’ll knock on the door but
won’t come in. Let [infant/child’s name] know that you’re going to be leaving the room
Page 23 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
briefly to talk with the assessor. Then please come stand outside the room for a few min
utes with me.” To ensure the child’s safety, this episode is only conducted when there is a
one-way mirror or video camera or when another adult is in the room the child. Is in This
situation allows assessment of the parents’ ability and level of comfort in preparing the
child for a brief separation. The child’s capacity for self-regulation and quality of mood
and exploratory play during the parents’ absence are assessed. The dyad’s quality of af
fect and engagement at reunion may also be observed.
Confirmatory factor analyses of 12-month free play interactions revealed eight factors:
Parent Factor I, Parental Positive Affective Involvement and Verbalization; Parent Factor
II, Parental Negative Affect and Behavior; Parent Factor III, Parental Intrusiveness, Insen
sitivity, and Inconsistency; Infant Factor IV, Infant Positive Affect, Communicative and So
cial Skills; Infant Factor V, Infant Quality of Play, Interest, and Attentional Skills; Infant
Factor VI, Infant Dysregulation and Irritability; Dyadic Factor VII, Dyadic Mutuality and
Reciprocity; and Dyadic (p. 58) Factor VIII, Dyadic Disorganization and Tension. High in
ternal consistency of factors at 4, 12, 24 months and 4.5 years has been determined, in
terrater reliability, and predictive and discriminant validity have been established for the
PCERA (Clark, 1983, 1999; Clark, Hyde, et al., 1997; Clark et al., 1993) in numerous stud
ies with normative and high-risk populations. Training is highly recommended and con
sists of 40 hours of didactic lectures, video ratings, and consensus discussion prior to rat
ing of four standardized videos and achievement of exact interrater agreement at or
above 0.85 and/or within one scale point agreement of 0.90 or above.
A video replay interview, in which brief segments of the video-recorded interactions are
played back and reviewed with the parents, is an important part of the assessment
process. In a semistructured interview, the parents’ perceptions, attitudes, and goals dur
ing the interactions with the infant or child are explored. Objective assessments often fail
to answer questions about what parents are experiencing with their infant or child. The
Video Replay Interview allows parents to share what they were seeing, doing, and feeling
in relation to their infant or child as well as their perceptions of their infant or child and
themselves in the parenting role. By wondering along with the parents about their per
ceptions, attitudes, and feelings about their infant or child, the clinician can gain insight
about parents’ phenomenological experience of the parent–child relationship that influ
ences their behavioral interactions with their infant or child. Before the video is re
viewed, parents are asked, “How was this interaction like or different from how things
usually go at home for [child’s name] and you together?”; “What was the most enjoyable
part of this session for you?”; “What part was the most difficult or did you like the least?”;
and “Do you have any questions or comments about the video recording?”
Video segments to review with parents are selected prior to the Video Replay interview.
In the first segment, the video is paused at a point when the infant or child’s face can be
seen clearly on the screen. The meaning of the infant or child to the parent is assessed
with questions such as, “I wonder who [child’s name] looks like or reminds you of [physi
cal features, temperament, behavior]?” “How did you select your child’s name?” and
“How would you describe that person and your relationship with them?” The next seg
Page 24 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
ment shown is when the parent is not responding to the child’s cues. The parent’s capaci
ty to read their infant or child’s cues may be assessed. In addition, parents’ capacity for
reflective functioning is assessed, that is, the ability to reflect on the internal affective ex
perience of their child and themselves (Fonagy & Target, 1998; Slade, 1999). The clini
cian asks parents, “How were you feeling during that interaction?” “What do you imagine
was going on for your child?” and “What do you imagine your child may have been feeling
then?” This process helps the clinician to assess the parents’ capacity to see the child as a
separate individual and their ability to empathically read their child’s cues or misinter
pret their child’s cues or attribute negative intentionality to their child’s behavior, making
it difficult to respond empathically. In addition, the clinician can also help parents to ex
pand their perceptions of their child and his or her behavior and increase their ways of
being with their child by wondering with them about alternative explanations for the in
fant or child’s behavior.
To assess the reinforcement value of the infant or child and parents’ sense of competence
in the parenting role, the clinician stops the video at a point when parent and child are
experiencing a mutually satisfying interaction and wonders, “How do you imagine [child’s
name] felt at that moment?” Then the clinician asks parents what they think they did to
elicit this positive response from their child. The clinician offers additional observations
that amplify parents’ strengths in reading their child’s needs and responding in a devel
opmentally sensitive manner. Additional questions in the video replay interview inform
the clinician about parents’ experience of parenting this child and include, “How would
you describe yourself as a parent?” “In general, what have you found most difficult or
frustrating about being a parent of [child’s name]?” “What have you found most enjoyable
about being a parent of [child’s name]?” “When do you feel best, or that you have done
well as a parent?” and “Did becoming a parent change you as a person in any way? If so,
how?”
Finally, it is extremely valuable to obtain a relational history from each parent to better
understand his or her own template of being parented or his or her internal working mod
el of relationships. The following questions may be asked during the video replay inter
view or at a subsequent session: “How would you compare yourself to your own parents?”
“What do you remember about being parented by your mother or father when you were
young?” “What three words would you use to describe your mother or father? Please give
specific examples that illustrate these adjectives.” “How was discipline (p. 59) handled in
your family?” “Who was available/responsive to you?” and “Who kept you safe, physically
and emotionally (or did not)?” (George et al., 1996).
The use of video replay in clinical interventions with parent–child dyads has been de
scribed as a powerful tool to enable therapeutic change in the client (Clark & Metcoff,
1983; Musick et al., 1981; Steele et al., 2014). Steele and colleagues (2014) described the
use of video as a way to enhance the therapeutic alliance by watching and experiencing
the interaction together. Steele et al. suggested that watching the video together can re
duce distortions of the experience and requires the therapist to be sensitive to the
caregiver’s experience while reviewing the footage and emphasized the importance of
Page 25 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
empowering and supporting the caregiver during this time. Video review also allows the
caregiver to reflect on the interaction, contributing to reflective functioning.
Page 26 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Parental affect
Parental characteristic
mood
Anxious mood X
Displeasure, disap X
proval, criticism
Enjoyment, pleasure X
Amount of verbalization X
Page 27 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Quality of verbalizations X
Contingent responsivity X
to child’s positive or age-
appropriate behavior
Contingent responsivity X
to child’s perceived nega
tive and/or unresponsive
behavior
Parental style
Intrusiveness X
Consistency/predictabil X
ity
Child mood/affect
Apathetic/withdrawn/ X
depressed mood
Anxious/tense/fearful X
mood
Irritable/frustrated/an X
gry mood
Sober/serious mood X
Child behavior/adaptive
abilities
Avoiding/averting/resis X
tance
Compliance/noncompli X
ance
Page 28 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Assertion/aggressivity X
Self-regulation/organi X
zational capacities
Consolability/soothabili X
ty
Child communication
Visual contact X
Communicative compe X
tence
Reciprocity X
Organization/regulation X
of interactions
To understand better the meaning of Josie’s ambivalent behavior and verbalizations with
her daughter, the video was reviewed to select segments to replay with Josie, including a
segment in which she could see Kira’s face large on the screen, one in which Josie missed
or did not respond sensitively to Kira’s cues, and another of a positive interaction in
which Kira responded to Josie. Josie shared that how they were together the week before
was how they are usually, except that they do not play together very much because she
Page 29 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
works nights and Kira stays with relatives, and when Josie picks Kira up, she herself is
usually too tired to play.
In the part of the Video Replay that focused on her own experience of being parented,
Josie shared that her father “wasn’t around at all” and that her mother was beaten and
killed in a drug-related incident when Josie was a toddler. Josie was raised by her grand
mother, who abused her both verbally and physically. She was also “beaten up” in middle
school and sent to live with an aunt in another city. She described multiple losses and
traumatic experiences and shared that her auntie had been there for her with love and
consistency. Her sisters helped her but she could not count on them regularly because of
their own struggles. Josie shared that she has struggled with anxiety, depression, and im
pulsive behavior that has gotten her in trouble with her siblings, who she was both close
with and “at war with,” other family members, and at work, resulting in her being fired
from two cashier jobs. This young woman expressed distrust of her own capabilities as
well as what she could expect from others, which contributed to the unpredictability of
her being home when her home visitor came. She seemed to have internalized the nega
tive attitude and extreme frustration that her grandmother expressed toward her with
her own child. When asked who Kira looks like or reminds her of, Josie responded that
“She looks like her Daddy … his mouth … especially when she is angry.” She talked about
Kira’s temperament by describing her as “a sweet, cuddly baby.” In a segment of her
reading a board book to Kira, Josie taught her the sound that a cow makes and how to
turn the pages of the book. She was effective in gaining and focusing her daughter’s at
tention on the book and she saw that Kira responded to her. This was reinforcing for
Josie. In the next segment, Kira turned one of the pages quickly and the force of the
board book page on her mother’s hand was responded to with Josie yelling, “Ouch! Why
you hurting me?” Kira cried and her mother responded, “You look like a fish.” Kira looked
away and at the (p. 60) other toys on the floor that were brought for the assessment and
found some small cars. Kira expressed enthusiasm in seeing what she could make happen
in pushing the cars across the floor and one of them hit her mother in the leg, to which
her mother responded, “Why you have to be so mean?” She brought out a puppet and
said, “Hey, what’s up?” “Do you like me?” (three times) and then she picked up a toy
phone and said, “Hey girlfriend, where you at? You’re late … you’re grounded!” She then
took the pop-up toy and demonstrated to Kira how to play with it, saying “You do it!”
There was some teasing with a rattle, at first not letting Kira reach it, and then Josie
threw it at her. Then Josie ran a car (p. 61) over Kira’s foot and then apologized and kissed
her daughter’s foot, saying “I’m sorry, little girl.” Kira cried again. When asked in the
Video Replay what her daughter might be feeling, Josie laughed and said, “I threw the
rattle at her … she saw it so she wanted it so I threw it at her. Maybe ‘cause I ran over
her foot with the car … she hit my foot with it before!” With empathy and support, Josie
evidenced insight and began to make some connections with how her reactions to Kira,
such as attributing negative intentionality to developmentally appropriate behaviors, may
be related to hurtful behaviors she experienced when she was young. When asked how
she would describe her relationship with her grandma, who she called Ma, she exclaimed
“either she didn’t pay any attention to me or she was Mean! … She hit me with a belt,
Page 30 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
broom, extension cord, switch and shoes.” This was noted compassionately by the inter
viewer as likely being very frightening and confusing for a child. When wondered with
about how she compared herself to her Ma, she said “I see everything in myself.” Her
concern about being like her Ma was reflected by the interviewer as she wondered with
Josie whether she would appreciate some support in learning other ways of being with
her child. Josie responded with a nod of her head and added, “As a mom I am still learn
ing. The more they grow, the more you get more reactions. You don’t know what to expect
… she better not get into my hair stuff!” When wondered with about what she found most
enjoyable about being a parent, Josie responded, “I miss being pregnant, I got a lot of at
tention.” When asked when she feels best or feels like she is doing well as a parent, she
responded, “When I can provide for my baby.” Josie expressed some distrust and ambiva
lence, but also some motivation to participate in the mother–infant therapy group be
cause she said she wanted to be a “good mom.” The PCERA Video Replay interview con
cludes with collaborative goal setting with the parent and the development of a Parent–
Child Relationship Development Plan. Josie was engaged, with the support of her home
visitor and the psychologist, in identifying relationship goals for their work together. Ta
ble 3.5 presents an example of a Parent–Child Relationship Development Plan.
Page 31 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
• Provide nonjudg
mental developmental
guidance regarding
her 12-month-old’s
emerging autonomy
and interest in explor
ing her environment.
Page 32 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
• Wondering with
Josie about what Kira
is feeling and needing
in the moment.
Josie would like to con Josie will make • Josie will engage in
tinue to grow in aware initial phone individual and dyadic
ness of, and capacity to call to set ap psychotherapy to ad
tolerate exploring, the pointment for dress her own trauma
loss and trauma that individual ther and loss as well as
she experienced as a apy. dyadic support for her
young child and how relationship with Kira.
that may contribute to
her experience of her
daughter’s lack of re
sponsiveness at times
and age-appropriate
physicality.
Page 33 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Encourage
Josie to consid
er trauma-in
formed child–
parent psy
chotherapy.
Page 34 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
H. Steele and Steele (2008) suggested that beyond its use in research, the AAI is helpful
in clinical work. For example, when this interview is conducted at the beginning of thera
py, themes that emerge regarding the parents’ early attachment relationships can help
establish treatment goals and a therapeutic alliance. Another important clinical use of the
AAI is learning about a client’s experience of trauma and loss, defensive processes, repre
sentations of self and other, and ghosts and angels in the nursery. The AAI can also be
used to observe the client’s reflective functioning (H. Steele & Steele, 2008).
Page 35 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
to events as well as the child’s response to the interactions described by the parent. Reli
ability is measured using intraclass correlations, which range from .78 to .95 (Slade et al.,
2005). The construct validity of the PDI is determined by looking at the association with
the AAI. Reflective functioning on the PDI is correlated with reflective functioning in the
AAI (Slade, 2005; Slade et al., 2005).
The PDI has been used in several clinical research studies, including with a population of
substance-abusing mothers. Suchman and colleagues used the PDI to assess maternal re
flective functioning before and after a 20-week mother–toddler therapy intervention and
found that the intervention did produce more maternal reflective functioning (medium to
large effect sizes) (Suchman, DeCoste, Castiglioni, Legow, & Mayes, 2008). Emotionally
avoidant language (use of more positive and less frequent negative feeling words) was al
so analyzed in the PDI with substance-abusing mothers. Borelli, West, DeCoste, and Such
man (2012) found that use of positive, but not negative, feeling words during the PDI was
correlated with lower reflective functioning, recent self-reported substance abuse, and
poorer maternal sensitivity.
Page 36 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
tion. Role boundary confusion is defined as the extent to which a parent elicits affection
from his or her baby or draws the baby’s attention to him- or herself rather than following
the baby’s cues. In early studies of the AMBIANCE, maternal role boundary confusion in
infancy was found to be highly correlated with negative intrusive behavior toward the in
fant, as evidenced by hostility or unnecessary involvement with the baby’s ongoing activi
ty. Together, role confusion and negative intrusive behaviors were predictive of infant dis
organized attachment behavior during the Strange Situation Procedure (Lyons-Ruth et
al., 1999; Vulliez-Coady, Obsuth, Torreiro-Casal, Ellertsdottir, & Lyons-Ruth, 2013). Over
all, as a measure, the AMBIANCE has demonstrated adequate construct and discriminant
validity (Lyons-Ruth et al., 1999; Lyons-Ruth, Bureau, Holmes, Easterbrooks, & Brooks,
2012).
Page 37 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 38 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 39 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
scored on a 5-point scale (from 1 = strongly agree to 5 = strongly disagree). For the Life
Stress items, the respondent simply indicates whether a specific life event (e.g., divorce,
loss of job, and pregnancy) has occurred within the past 12 months. The PSI-4 has
demonstrated high internal consistency and test–retest reliability and has maintained its
factor structure, reliability, and validity across numerous translations (Abidin, 2012). The
PSI-4 also comes in a short-form version, which was derived from the long version and
takes approximately 10 minutes to complete (PSI-4-SF; Abidin, 2012). The PSI-4-SF con
tains three subscales, including Parental Distress, Parent–Child Dysfunctional Interaction,
and Difficult Child, and the sum of these domains allows the examiner to obtain a Total
Stress score. The PSI-4-SF has also demonstrated high internal consistency across items.
The PSI-4 has been revised to improve cultural sensitivity of language, while also includ
ing fathers in the standardization sample. Additionally, the normative pool was updated to
match the demographic composition of the 2007 U.S. Census (Abidin, 2012).
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5; Ameri
can Psychiatric Association, 2013), is a dimensional classification system developed to as
sist clinicians and researchers in the identification, study, and/or treatment of individuals
with mental health problems. Several diagnoses address disturbances in the parent–child
relationship. Reactive attachment disorder is defined as “a pattern of markedly disturbed
and developmentally inappropriate attachment behaviors, in which a child rarely or mini
mally turns preferentially to an attachment figure for comfort, support, protecting, and
nurturance” (p. 266). To receive this diagnosis, the child must have an underdeveloped
attachment relationship with his or her caregiver and show related symptoms before age
5. Specifically, this diagnosis requires emotionally withdrawn behavior toward caregivers,
social and emotional disturbances, and insufficient care of the child. Disinhibited social
engagement disorder is another diagnosis in the DSM-5 that describes a child’s response
to insufficient care. Children with disinhibited social engagement disorder may have no
or reduced inhibitions and approach and interact with unfamiliar adults in overly familiar
verbal or physical ways. This may include the child leaving his or her caregiver in unfa
miliar settings with no hesitation and rarely or never checking back with the caregiver.
Insufficient care (i.e., neglect, repeated changes in caregivers, unusual rearing settings)
Page 40 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
is a criterion for disinhibited social engagement disorder, and the insufficient care is
thought to be the cause of the disinhibited behavior.
The DSM-5 also includes codes for psychosocial and environmental conditions that should
be addressed by a clinician. The category of relational problems includes a code for par
ent–child relational problems: “the main focus of clinical attention is to address the quali
ty of the parent–child relationship or when the quality of the parent–child relationship is
affecting the course, prognosis, or treatment of a mental or other medical disorder” (p.
715). It is important to note that the parent in this definition is the primary caregiver of
the child, not necessarily (p. 67) the biological parent. This parent–child relational prob
lem is usually accompanied by behavioral, cognitive, or affective impaired functioning. Al
though not found in the DSM-5 system, the medical diagnosis of a nonorganic failure to
thrive has been associated with a disturbance in the parent–child relationship. This disor
der is usually recognized by the child’s pediatrician when a child who shows no other
signs of illness demonstrates poor growth patterns that cannot be accounted for by
parental growth patterns or further medical testing. Such patterns have been associated
with caregiver deprivation and neglect. These children often manifest other signs of child
neglect or maltreatment, such as poor hygiene and/or frequent accidental injuries (Tun
nessen, 1999). Table 3.6 illustrates several classification systems used for describing at
tachment disorders. Although these systems vary in their classification, each system de
scribes a disturbance in the balance between the child’s proximity-seeking and explo
ration of the environment.
Page 41 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 42 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 43 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Parent–
adopted
child con
flict
Parent–fos
ter child
conflict
Page 44 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Page 45 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
the ICD-10-CM, three codes are given under the category of parent–child conflict: parent–
biological child conflict, parent–adopted child conflict, and parent–foster child conflict.
The parent–child conflict section falls under the larger category of problems related to
upbringing (World Health Organization, 2016).
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
caregivers” (Zero to Three, 2016, p. 126). The child’s withdrawn and inhibited behavior
must be characterized by no or minimal developmentally appropriate comfort-seeking
and social engagement (Zero to Three, 2016). To receive (p. 68) (p. 69) (p. 70) this diagno
sis, a child must have experienced insufficient care (neglect) and/or repeated change of
caregivers (limiting the child’s ability to form secure attachments), and these experiences
must be believed to have caused the child’s behaviors. Another criterion for this diagno
sis is that the child’s symptoms or the caregiver’s response to these symptoms are impair
ing and significantly impact the child’s and/or family’s functioning (Zero to Three, 2016).
Page 47 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Because relationships are central to a young child’s health and development, it is impor
tant to note that if a child’s relationships have been determined to have an Axis II rating
of Level 2, 3, or 4 on Part A or B but the child is currently asymptomatic, that child may
be considered at risk for psychopathologies described in Axis I in the future (Zeanah &
Lieberman, 2016).
Page 48 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Additionally, interactions will look different depending on a family’s community and cul
tural context. For instance, parents in Western cultures tend to vocalize to their infant,
gaze at their infant, and display objects to their child more frequently than parents in
African, Middle Eastern, and Far Eastern cultures. However, parents from these Eastern
cultures provide their infants with more physical contact than parents from Western cul
tures. Parents in Eastern cultures more commonly place their infants on their laps, invit
ing physical contact but no gaze, whereas parents in Western cultures are less likely to
have their children on laps, which provides less physical contact but more opportunities
for eye contact (Feldman, 2007). Even when comparing mothers of subcultures within the
larger Western culture, different parenting beliefs arise, which lead to differing parenting
practices (Senese, Bornstein, Haynes, Rossi, & Venuti, 2012). Some cultures value inde
pendence in the infant and toddler, while others value interdependence. Families in some
cultures must focus on the survival of their infants, while others are able to focus on the
achievements of their infants. The parents’ beliefs about infant development and the
forces that support infant learning and development may also differ and result in various
parenting styles across cultures (McCollum & McBride, 1997). Beliefs about successful
infant adjustment, age of reaching various milestones in early childhood, and when and in
what ways to care for the child depend on cultural beliefs and affect parenting strategies
(Bornstein, 2012). For example, Japanese families tend to want their children to be emo
tionally mature, showing self-control, good manners, and interdependence, while Ameri
can families tend to want their children to be independent, assertive, verbally competent,
and self-actualized (Bornstein, 2012). When assessing parenting approaches across indus
trialized cultures, Bornstein (2012) saw that mothers who focused on the infant’s physical
development had infants who became more motorically competent, and mothers who fo
cused on social interaction with their infant had infants who were more interactive with
their mothers.
Page 49 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
The Diversity-Informed Infant Mental Health Tenets were developed by the Irving Harris
(p. 72) Foundation Professional Development Network (Irving Harris Foundation, 2012).
These tenets include the importance of self-awareness in regard to personal values and
beliefs, a diversity-informed stance toward infants and families (i.e., work to acknowledge
privilege and combat discrimination, recognize and respect nondominant bodies of knowl
edge, honor diverse family structures), practice/research field participants (i.e., under
stand that language can be used to hurt or heal, support families in their preferred lan
guage, allocate resources to systems change, make space and open pathways for diverse
professionals), and broader advocacy (i.e., advance policy that supports all families).
These diversity tenets are important in promotion of the professional’s self-reflection and
sensitive and respectful consideration of family values when conducting parent–child rela
tionship assessments.
Conclusion
In conclusion, assessment of the parent–child relationship and, when indicated, diagnosis
of a relationship disturbance or disorder are important components, if not the center
pieces, of a clinical assessment of mental health in the infant or young child. Actively en
gaging parents in the assessment process is respectful and fosters a partnership between
clinician and family in development of the therapeutic intervention. When parents take an
active role in the assessment process and feel their concerns, strengths, and motivations
have been heard and understood by the clinician, they are more likely to engage in the
assessment and follow-through with therapeutic intervention. By conducting assessments
of each member of the parent–child dyad as well as the dyadic and family unit, the clini
cian obtains information that is essential to developing treatment goals, ports of entry
and intervention strategies. Goals should be developed collaboratively with parents for
the child and each parent–child relationship. When assessing the infant or young child in
the context of his or her caregiving environment, it is critical that the process include the
acquisition of information from others such as day-care providers, pediatricians, mental
health providers, early intervention and home visiting programs, social services, alcohol
and other drug addiction treatment providers, and the court system. In our experience,
the most successful interventions involve coordination of services across settings with
regular communication. Empathically listening to the parents’ struggles and reflecting
their feelings and concerns enhances the therapeutic relationship between clinician and
parent. Finally, assessing the quality of the parent–child relationship should be an ongo
ing process because early childhood is a time of rapid change and parents are often faced
with increased challenges as well as opportunities for growth and development in the
parenting role, and the strengths and needs of the parent–child dyad will change as the
relationship evolves.
References
Abidin, R. R. (1986). Parenting stress index manual (2nd ed.). Charlottesville, CA: Pedi
atric Psychology Press.
Page 50 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Abidin, R. R. (2012). Parenting stress index (4th ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessments
Resources.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., & Waters, E. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psycho
logical study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental dis
orders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
Barnard, K. E. (1979). Instructor’s learning resource manual. Seattle, WA: NCAST Publi
cations.
Barnard, K. E. (1994). What the Feeding Scale measures. In G. S. Sumner & A. Spietz
(Eds.), NCAST: Caregiver/parent–interaction feeding manual (pp. 98–121). Seattle, WA:
NCAST Publications.
Benoit, D., Parker, K. C. H., & Zeanah, C. H. (1997). Mothers’ representations of their in
fants assessed prenatally: Stability and association with infants’ attachment classifica
tions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 38, 307–313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01515.x
Bernstein, V. J., Harris, E. J., Long, C. W., Iida, E., & Hans, S. L. (2005). Issues in the multi-
cultural assessment of parent–child interaction: An exploratory study from the Starting
Early Starting Smart collaboration. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26,
241–275. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2005.02.002
Biringen, Z. (2008). The Emotional Availability (EA) scales: Infancy/early childhood ver
sion (4th ed.). Boulder: Colorado State University.
Biringen, Z., Damon, J., Grigg, W., Mone, J., Pipp-Siegel, S., Skillern, S., & Stratton, J.
(2005). Emotional availability: Differential predictions to infant attachment and kinder
garten adjustment based on observation time and context. Infant Mental Health Journal,
26, 295–308. http://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20054
Biringen, Z., Derscheid, D., Vliegen, N., Closson, L., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (2014). Emo
tional availability (EA): (p. 73) Theoretical background, empirical research using the EA
Page 51 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Biringen, Z., Robinson, J., & Emde, R. N. (1998). The Emotional Availability Scales (3rd
ed.). Fort Collins: Colorado State University.
Borelli, J. L., West, J. L., DeCoste, C., & Suchman, N. E. (2012). Emotionally avoidant lan
guage in the parenting interviews of substance-dependent mothers: Associations with re
flective functioning, recent substance use, and parenting behavior. Infant Mental Health
Journal, 33, 506–519. http://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21340
Bornstein, M., Gini, M., Putnick, D. L., Haynes, O. M., Painter, K. M., & Suwalsky, J. T. D.
(2006). Short-term reliability and continuity of emotional availability in mother–child
dyads across contexts of observation. Infancy, 10, 1–16. http://doi.org/10.1207/
s15327078in1001_1
Britton, J. R., Britton, H. L., & Gronwaldt, V. (2006). Breastfeeding, sensitivity, and attach
ment. Pediatrics, 118, e1436–e1443. http://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-2916
Bronfman, E., Parsons, E., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (1992–2004). Atypical Maternal Behavior In
strument for Assessment and Classification (AMBIANCE): Manual for coding affective
communication (2nd ed.). Unpublished manual. Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Med
ical School, Cambridge, MA., 181–184.
Bronfman, E., Madigan, S., Lyons-Ruth, K. (2009). Atypical Maternal Behavior Instrument
for Assessment and Classification (AMBIANCE). Manual for Coding Disrupted Affective
Communication. Version 2.0. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University.
Clark, R. (1983). Interactions of psychiatrically ill and well mothers and their young chil
dren: Quality of maternal care and child competence (Doctoral dissertation). Northwest
ern University, Evanston, IL.
Clark, R. (1985). The Parent–Child Early Relational Assessment: Instrument and manual.
University of Wisconsin Medical School, Madison.
Clark, R. (2010). The Parent–Child Early Relational Assessment: Instrument and manual.
University of Wisconsin Medical School, Madison.
Page 52 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Clark, R. (2015). The Parent–Child Early Relational Assessment: Instrument and manual.
University of Wisconsin Medical School, Madison.
Clark, R. (1999). The parent–child early relational assessment: A factorial validity study.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 59, 821–846. http://doi.org/
10.1177/00131649921970161
Clark, R., Burk, L., Hewitt, E., & Hipke, K. (2006, July). Multi-method assessment of the
quality of the mother–infant relationships pre and post-treatment for postpartum depres
sion: Associations between observational (PCERA), self-report (PSI), and interview (AAI)
measures. Paper presented at the World Association for Infant Mental Health, Paris,
France.
Clark, R., Hyde, J. S., Essex, M. J., & Klein, M. H. (1997). Length of maternity leave and
quality of mother–infant interactions. Child Development, 68, 364–383. http://doi.org/
10.2307/1131855
Clark, R., Paulson, A., & Conlin, S. (1993). Assessment of developmental status and par
ent–infant relationships: The therapeutic process of evaluation. In C. H. Zeanah Jr. (Ed.),
Handbook of infant mental health. New York, NY: Guilford Press. 191–209.
Clark, R., Seidl, M., & Paulson, A. (1997). Mixed and 902. Under involved. In A. Liber
man, S. Wider, & E. Fenichel (Eds.), The DC:0–3 casebook: A guide to the use of Zero to
Three’s Diagnostic Classification of Mental Health and Developmental Disorders of Infan
cy and Early Childhood in assessment and treatment planning (pp. 359–388). Washington,
DC: Zero to Three.
Clark, R., Tluczek, A., & Brown, R. (2008). A mother–infant therapy group model for post
partum depression. Infant Mental Health Journal, 29, 514–536.
Clark, R., Tluczek, A., & Gallagher, K. C. (2004). Assessment of parent–child early rela
tional disturbances. In R. DelCarmen-Wiggins & A. Carter (Eds.), Handbook of infant, tod
dler, and preschool mental health assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Clark, R., Tluczek, A., Moore, E. C., & Evenson, A. L. (2019). Theoretical and empirical
foundations for early relationship assessment in evaluating infant and toddler mental
health. In A. Carter & R. DelCarmen-Wiggins (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of infant, tod
dler, and preschool mental health assessment (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Crowell, J. A., & Feldman, S. S. (1988). Mothers’ internal models of relationships and
children’s behavioral and developmental status: A study of mother–child interaction. Child
Development, 59, 1273–1285. http://doi.org/10.2307/1130490
Page 53 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Day, M., & Parlakian, R. (2003). How culture shapes social-emotional development: Impli
cations for practice in infant–family programs. Washington, DC: Zero to Three.
Dayton, C. J., Levendosky, A. A., Davidson, W. S., & Bogat, G. A. (2010). The child as held
in the mind of the mother: The influence of prenatal maternal representations on parent
ing behaviors. Infant Mental Health Journal, 31, 220–241. http://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.
20253
Emde, R. N. (1992). The infant’s relationship experience: Developmental and affective as
pects. In A. J. Sameroff & R. N. Emde (Eds.), Relationship disturbances in early childhood:
A developmental approach. New York, NY: Basic Books. 33–51.
Farran, D., Clark, K., & Ray, A. (1990). Measures of parent–child interaction. In E. Gibbs
& D. Teti (Eds.), (p. 74) Interdisciplinary assessment of infants: A guide for early interven
tion professionals (pp. 227–247). Baltimore, MD: Brooks.
Feldman, R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physi
ological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. Journal of Child Psy
chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 48, 329–354. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
1469-7610.2006.01701.x
Feldman, R., Dollberg, D., & Nadam, R. (2011). The expression and regulation of anger in
toddlers: Relations to maternal behavior and mental representations. Infant Behavior and
Development, 34, 310–320. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.02.001
Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2009). Biological and environmental initial conditions
shape the trajectories of cognitive and social-emotional development across the first
years of life. Developmental Science, 12, 194–200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
1467-7687.2008.00761.x
Feldman, R., Weller, A., Eidelman, A. I., & Sirota, L. (2003). Testing a family intervention
hypothesis: The contribution of mother–infant skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) to fam
ily interaction and touch. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 94–107. http://doi.org/
10.1037/0893-3200.17.1.94
Page 54 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Fisher, C. B., Hoagwood, K., Boyce, C., Duster, T., Frank, D. A., Grisso, T., … Zayas, L. H.
(2002). Research ethics for mental health science involving ethnic minority children and
youths. American Psychologist, 57, 1024–1040. http://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.
57.12.1024
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1998). Mentalization and the changing aims of child psycho
analysis. Psychoanalytic Dialogues: The International Journal of Relational Perspectives,
8, 87–114. http://doi.org/10.1080/10481889809539235
Fraiberg, S., Adelson, E., & Shapiro, V. (1980). Ghosts in the nursery: A psychoanalytic
approach of impaired infant–mother relationships. In S. Fraiberg & L. Fraiberg (Eds.),
Clinical studies in infant mental health: The first year of life (pp. 164–196). New York, NY:
Basic Books. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0002-7138(09)61442-4
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). Adult attachment interview protocol (3rd ed.).
Berkeley: University of California.
Gerstein, E. D., Poehlmann-Tynan, J., & Clark, R. (2015). Mother–child interactions in the
NICU: Relevance and implications for later parenting. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 40,
33–44. http://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsu064
Goodman, S. H., & Brumley, E. H. (1990). Schizophrenic and depressed mothers: Rela
tional deficits in parenting. Developmental Psychology, 26, 31–39. http://doi.org/
10.1037//0012-1649.26.1.31
Gray, S. A. O., Forbes, D., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., & Carter, A. S. (2015). Caregiver insight
fulness and young children’s violence exposure: Testing a relational model of risk and re
silience. Attachment & Human Development, 17, 615–634. http://doi.org/
10.1080/14616734.2015.1100207
Hesse, E. (1999). Adult attachment interview. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Hand
book of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 395–433). New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Irving Harris Foundation. (2012). Diversity-informed infant mental health tenets. Re
trieved from http://www.imhdivtenets.org
Keren, M., Feldman, R., & Tyano, S. (2001). Diagnoses and interactive patterns of infants
referred to a community-based infant mental health clinic. Journal of the American Acade
Page 55 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Klotz, M., & Canter, A. (2006). Culturally competent assessment and consultation. Re
trieved from http://www.nasponline.org/resources/principals/
CulturallyCompetentAssessmentandConsultationNASSP.pdf
Koren-Karie, N., Oppenheim, D., Dolev, S., Sher, E., & Etzion-Carasso, A. (2002). Mother’s
insightfulness regarding their infants’ internal experience: Relations with maternal sensi
tivity and infant attachment. Developmental Psychology, 38, 534–542. http://doi.org/
10.1037/0012-1649.38.4.534
Korja, R., Ahlqvist-Björkroth, S., Savonlahti, E., Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., …
Lehtonen, L. (2010). Relations between maternal attachment representations and the
quality of mother–infant interaction in preterm and full-term infants. Infant Behavior &
Development, 33, 330–336. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.010
Korja, R., Savonlahti, E., Ahlqvist-Björkroth, S., Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., …
Lehtonen, L. (2008). Maternal depression is associated with mother–infant interaction in
preterm infants. Acta Paediatrica, 97, 724–730. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
1651-2227.2008.00733.x
Landorph, S. L., & Clark, R. (2013). Parent child early relational assessment–PCERA eller
ERA. In R. Schwartz & S. H. H. Reitzel (Eds.), Barnet og dets relationelle milj Om tilknyt
ningsbaseret undersgelsesmetodik (pp. 149–176). Copenhagen, Denmark: Hans Reitzels.
Larrieu, J. A., & Bellow, S. M. (2004). Relationship assessment for young traumatized chil
dren. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Young children and trauma: Intervention and treatment (pp.
155–172). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Lor, M., Crooks, N., & Tluczek, A. (2016). A proposed model of person-, family-, and cul
ture-centered nursing care. Nursing Outlook, 64, 352–366. http://doi.org/10.1016/
j.outlook.2016.02.006
Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfman, E., & Parsons, E. (1999). Maternal frightened, frightening, or
atypical behavior and disorganized infant attachment patterns. Monographs of the Soci
ety for Research in Child Development, 64, 67–96.
Lyons-Ruth, K., Bureau, J.-F., Easterbrooks, M. A., Obsuth, I., Hennighausen, K., & Vulliez-
Coady, L. (2013). Parsing the construct of maternal insensitivity: Distinct longitudinal
pathways associated with early maternal withdrawal. Attachment & Human Development,
15, 562–582. http://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2013.841051
Lyons-Ruth, K., Bureau, J. F., Holmes, B., Easterbrooks, A., & Brooks, N. H. (2012). Bor
derline symptoms and suicidality/self-injury in late adolescence: Prospectively observed
relationship correlates in infancy and childhood. (p. 75) Psychiatry Research, 206, 273–
281. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.09.030
Page 56 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Main, M., Goldwyn, R., Hesse, E. (1984). Adult attachment scoring and classification sys
tem. Unpublished Manuscript. University of California at Berkeley.
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1992). Frightening, frightened, dissociated, or disorganized be
havior on the part of the parent: A coding system for parent–infant interactions (4th ed).
Unpublished manuscript. University of California-Berkeley
Matas, L., Arend, R. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the second
year: The relationship between quality of attachment and later competence. Child Devel
opment, 49, 547–556. http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004
Minde, K., Faucon, A., & Falkner, S. (1994). Sleep problems in toddlers: Effects of treat
ment on their daytime behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 33, 1114–1121. http://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199410000-00007
Miron, D., Lewis, M. L., & Zeanah, C. H. (2009). Clinical use of observational procedures
in early childhood relationship assessment. In C. H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant
mental health (3rd ed., pp. 252–265). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Mothander, P. R. (1990). The first year of life: Predictive patterns of infant development,
maternal adjustment, and mother–infant interaction (Doctoral dissertation). Uppsala Uni
versity, Uppsala, Sweden.
Murphy, A., Steele, M., Dube, S. R., Bate, J., Bonuck, K., Meissner, P., … Steele, H. (2014).
Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) questionnaire and adult attachment interview.
Child Abuse & Neglect, 38, 224–233. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.09.004
Musick, J. S., Clark, R., & Cohler, B. J. (1981). The mothers’ project: A clinical research
program for mentally ill mothers and their young children. In B. Weissbourd & J. Musick
(Eds.), The social and caregiving environments of infants. National Association for the Ed
ucation of Young Children (pp. 111–127). Washington, DC.
Norman, V. J., & Christiansen, K. (2013). Validity of the PICCOLO tool in child care set
tings: Can it assess caregiver interaction behaviors? Infant Mental Health Journal, 34,
319–329. http://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21391
Oppenheim, D., Goldsmith, D., & Koren-Karie, N. (2004). Maternal insightfulness and
preschoolers’ emotion and behavior problems: Reciprocal influences in a therapeutic
preschool program. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 352–367. http://doi.org/10.1002/
imhj.20010
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Oppenheim, D., & Koren-Karie, N. (2013). The insightfulness assessment: Measuring the
internal processes underlying maternal sensitivity. Attachment & Human Development,
15, 545–561. http://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2013.820901
Oppenheim, D., Koren-Karie, N., & Sagi, A. (2001). Mothers’ empathic understanding of
their preschoolers’ internal experience: Relations with early attachment. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 16–26. http://doi.org/
10.1080/01650250042000096
Out, D., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2009). The role of dis
connected and extremely insensitive parenting in the development of disorganized attach
ment: Validation of a new measure. Attachment & Human Development, 11, 419–443.
http://doi.org/10.1080/14616730903132289
Poehlmann, J., Burnson, C., & Weymouth, L. A. (2014). Early parenting, represented fami
ly relationships, and externalizing behavior problems in children born preterm. Attach
ment & Human Development, 16, 271–291. http://doi.org/
10.1080/14616734.2014.884610
Ravitz, P., Maunder, R., Hunter, J., Sthankiya, B., & Lancee, W. (2010). Adult attachment
measures: A 25-year review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 69, 419–432. http://
doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.08.006
Roggman, L. A., Cook, G. A., Innocenti, M. S., Jump Norman, V., & Christiansen, K.
(2013). Parenting interactions with children: Checklist of observations linked to outcomes
(PICCOLO) in diverse ethnic groups. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34, 290–306.
Rosenblum, K. L., Zeanah, C. H., McDonough, S., & Muzik, M. (2004). Video-taped coding
of working model of the child interviews: A viable and useful alternative to verbatim tran
scripts? Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 544–549. http://doi.org/10.1016/
j.infbeh.2004.04.001
Sameroff, A. J., & Emde, R. (1989). Relationship disturbances in early childhood: A devel
opmental approach. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Sameroff, A. J., McDonough, S. C., & Rosenblum, K. (2004). Treating parent–infant rela
tionship problems: Strategies for intervention. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Page 58 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Senese, V. P., Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Rossi, G., & Venuti, P. (2012). A cross-cul
tural comparison of mothers’ beliefs about their parenting very young children. Infant Be
havior and Development, 35, 479–488. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.02.006
Siqveland, T. S., Haabrekke, K., Wentzel-Larsen, T., & Moe, V. (2014). Patterns of mother–
infant interaction from 3 to 12 months among dyads with substance abuse and psychi
atric problems. Infant Behavior and Development, 37, 772–786. http://doi.org/10.1016/
j.infbeh.2014.09.003
Slade, A. (1999). Attachment theory and research: Implications for the theory and prac
tice of individual psychotherapy with adults. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook
of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
575–594.
Slade, A., A., Aber, J. L., Bresgi, I., Berger, B., & Kaplan, M. (2003). The parent de
(p. 76)
Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., & Locker, A. (2005). Maternal reflec
tive functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: A preliminary study. Attachment
& Human Development, 7, 283–298. http://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245880
Sprang, G., & Craig, C. (2015). Crowell problem solving procedure: A psychometric analy
sis of a laboratory measure of the caregiver–child relationship. Child and Adolescent Men
tal Health, 20, 202–209. http://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12082
St. Petersburg–USA Orphanage Research Team. (2009). The effects of early social-emo
tional and relationship experience on the development of young orphanage children.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 73(3), vii–295. http://
doi.org/10.1002/9781444309683
Stack, D. M., Serbin, L. A., Girouard, N., Enns, L. N., Bentley, V. M. N., Ledingham, J. E., &
Schwartzman, A. E. (2012). The quality of the mother–child relationship in high-risk
dyads: Application of the Emotional Availability Scales in an intergenerational, longitudi
nal study. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 93–105. http://doi.org/10.1017/
S095457941100068X
Steele, H., & Steele, M. (2008). Ten clinical uses of the Adult Attachment Interview. In H.
Steele & M. Steele (Eds.), Clinical applications of the Adult Attachment Interview (pp. 3–
32). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Steele, M., Steele, H., Bate, J., Knafo, H., Kinsey, M., Bonuck, K., … Murphy, A. (2014).
Looking from the outside in: The use of video in attachment-based interventions. Attach
ment & Human Development, 16, 402–415. http://doi.org/
10.1080/14616734.2014.912491
Page 59 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Stern, D. (2002). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni
versity Press.
Stolt, S., Korja, R., Matomäki, J., Lapinleimu, H., Haataja, L., & Lehtonen, L. (2014). Early
relations between language development and the quality of mother–child interaction in
very-low-birth-weight children. Early Human Development, 90, 219–225. http://doi.org/
10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.02.007
Suchman, N., DeCoste, C., Castiglioni, N., Legow, N., & Mayes, L. (2008). The Mothers
and Toddlers Program: Preliminary findings from an attachment-based parenting inter
vention for substance-abusing mothers. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 25, 499–517. http://
doi.org/10.1037/0736-9735.25.3.499
Tamminen, T. (2006). How does culture promote the early development of identity? Infant
Mental Health Journal, 27, 603–605. http://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20109
Teti, D. M., Nakagawa, M., Das, R., & Wirth, O. (1991). Security of attachment between
preschoolers and their mothers: Relations among social interaction, parenting stress, and
mother’s sorts of the Attachment Q-Set. Developmental Psychology, 27, 440–447. http://
doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.440
Thomas, J. M., Benham, A. L., Gean, M., Luby, J., Minde, K., Turner, S., & Wright, H. H.
(1997). Practice parameters for the psychiatric assessment of infants and toddlers (0–36
months). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 21S–36S.
http://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199710001-00003
Tunnessen, W. (1999). Signs and symptoms in pediatrics (3rd ed.) New York, NY: Lippin
cott Williams & Wilkins.
Page 60 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Vulliez-Coady, L., Obsuth, I., Torreiro-Casal, M., Ellertsdottir, L., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2013).
Maternal role confusion: Relations to maternal attachment and mother–child interaction
from infancy to adolescence. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34, 117–131. http://doi.org/
10.1002/imhj.21379
Weber, A. M., & Harrison, T. M. (2014). Maternal behavior and infant physiology during
feeding in premature and term infants over the first year of life. Research in Nursing and
Health, 37, 478–489. http://doi.org/10.1002/nur.21618
Weston, D. R., Thomas, J. M., Barnard, K. E., Wieder, S., Clark, R., Carter, A. S., &
Fenichel, E. (2003). DC:0–3 assessment protocol project: Defining a comprehensive infor
mation set to support DC:0–3 diagnostic formulation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24,
410–427. http://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10065
Wheeler, R., Ludtke, M., Helmer, J., Barna, N., Wilson, K., & Oleksiak, C. (2013). Imple
mentation of the PICCOLO in infant mental health practice: A case study. Infant Mental
Health Journal, 34, 352–358. http://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21395
Zeanah, C. H., & Barton, M. (1989). Internal representations and parent–infant relation
ships. Infant Mental Health Journal, 10, 135–141. http://doi.org/
10.1002/1097-0355(198923)10:3<135::aid-imhj2280100302>3.0.co;2-h
Zeanah, C. H., & Benoit, D. (1995). Clinical applications of a parent perception interview.
In K. Minde (Ed.), Infant psychiatry: Child and adolescent psychiatric clinics of North
America (pp. 539–554). Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.
Zeanah, C. H., Benoit, D., Barton, M. L., & Hirshberg, L. (1996). Working Model of the
Child Interview coding manual. Unpublished manuscript.
Zeanah, C. H., Benoit, D., Hirshberg, L., Barton, M. L., & Regan, C. (1994). Moth
(p. 77)
ers’ representations of their infants are concordant with infant attachment classifications.
Developmental Issues in Psychiatry and Psychology, 1, 9–18.
Page 61 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
Zeanah, C. H., Larrieu, J. A., Heller, S. S., & Valliere, J. (2000). Infant–parent relationship
assessment. In C. H. Zeanah Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (2nd ed., pp. 222–
235). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Zeanah, C. H., & Lieberman, A. F. (2016). Defining relational pathology in early child
hood: The Diagnostic Classification of Mental Health and Developmental Disorders of In
fancy and Early Childhood DC:0–5 approach. Infant Mental Health Journal, 37, 509–520.
http://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21590
Zero to Three. (1994). Zero to Three (DC:0–3). National Center for Clinical Infant Pro
grams. Diagnostic classification of mental health and developmental disorders of infancy
and early childhood. Arlington, VA: Zero to Three.
Zero to Three. (2016). DC:0–5: Diagnostic classification of mental health and developmen
tal disorders of infancy and early childhood. Washington, DC: Zero to Three.
Roseanne Clark
Audrey Tluczek
Elizabeth C. Moore
Amber L. Evenson
Page 62 of 62
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). © Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).