Understanding Humor Throu Communication
Understanding Humor Throu Communication
Understanding Humor Throu Communication
through Communication
Understanding Humor through
Communication
John Meyer
LEXINGTON BOOKS
Lanham • Boulder • New York • London
Published by Lexington Books
An imprint of The Rowman & Littlefield Publishing Group, Inc.
4501 Forbes Boulevard, Suite 200, Lanham, Maryland 20706
www.rowman.com
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form or by any
electronic or mechanical means, including information storage and retrieval systems,
without written permission from the publisher, except by a reviewer who may quote
passages in a review.
TM
The paper used in this publication meets the minimum requirements of American
National Standard for Information Sciences Permanence of Paper for Printed Library
Materials, ANSI/NISO Z39.48-1992.
Acknowledgments vii
Bibliography 121
Index 131
v
Acknowledgments
vii
ONE
Communication and Theories of
Humor Origin
1
2 Chapter 1
humor evolved into its status as a desirable and entertaining part of all
society. People came to seek out opportunities to laugh.
Humor can be manifested by external indicators such as laughter or
smiling, but is not always so. Physiological studies of humor focus on its
effects on the human body, including its tendency to relax the individual
and cause sudden recurring exhalations of breath. The human face is also
prone to “true” or “deep” expressions of humor contrasted with partial
or “imitated” signs—polite smiling, for example. Such studies begin to
approach the psychological and health benefits of the humor experience,
which are many but are often difficult to pin down or quantify (Martin,
2007). Yet, for most, there is no denying the pleasant experience the expe-
rience of humor gives.
The humor experience is more than physical manifestations or symp-
toms. Its origins are cognitive and symbolic. People see humor in a varie-
ty of stories, events, actions, pictures, and symbols. They then respond in
a wide variety of ways, internal and invisible or external and socially
shared. We may be able to study their physical responses, including
laughter, to assess the humor response. However, one may find some-
thing amusing while giving little outward sign of doing so. The cognitive
response is key to humor—what each individual perceives. Humor re-
searchers in the past two centuries have moved beyond the idea that
“humors” are physical or social characteristics of people, to seeking the
source of humor—what makes possible those physical manifestations,
the most common of which is laughter?
In essence, the human mind chooses to and is enabled to find humor
in any situation. The events or actions that spark humorous responses are
studied, yet also key characteristics of those events or actions that lead to
a humorous experience are sought. Each individual determines what is
funny, yet humor is also a strong relational or group phenomenon. A
variety of theories have been put forward to explain how humor exists,
how it came to be, or how it is created. All theories cluster around some
kind of change in circumstances or perceptions; in essence some kind of
pleasant psychological alteration (Morreall, 1983). Later chapters of this
book will explore some of these theories in detail, to gather ideas of what
we believe causes humor to set the scene for the pursuit of humor as it
functions in communication as the overarching goal.
When two or more people communicate, occurrence of shared humor
suggests that a common path for understanding a topic and a common
choice for a closer relationship exist. The humor experience involves rec-
ognition of patterns and their violation, and a shared understanding of
such patterns and violations leads to a strong sense of sharing in a rela-
tionship. If I come into work one morning and say to a colleague, “that
team needs to get itself a defense,” the response depends upon my col-
league’s recognition of the team I refer to and the game in which the team
lacked a defense. Laughter may also ensue at the mutual thought that
6 Chapter 1
although, of course, the team did have a defense it seemed during the
game like it did not. Even if an argument ensues, our mutual recognition
of those shared sports patterns reinforces a commonality in our relation-
ship. Through its dramatic emotional evidence of shared perspectives,
humor can build unity in a relationship.
When examining thoroughly our daily communication, the pervasive
invocations of humor become compelling. During a conversation, when I
laugh at something the other says, that shows I heard the message, made
some connection to context with the message, and indicates to the other
some appreciation for the message as phrased. Humor indicates an emo-
tional response to a message along with acknowledging receipt of that
message. With shared and understood responses to increasing numbers
of messages, communicators become more confident and willing to share
more information, work together, and develop potentially more mean-
ingful relationships. Humor in communication then becomes one key
aspect of developing closer interactions with family members, friends,
and members of shared social groups.
Organizations thus serve as natural homes for humor—as people
working together develop patterns of communication and respond to
variations or violations of those patterns. Studies of communication at
work, to be explored later, show how humor pervades the daily commu-
nication of work groups. The ability to joke with coworkers emerges as a
crucial indicator of satisfaction with work life. People are, in general,
much happier with room to joke around at work, at least with some
people. In the more personal venue of family life, too, “inside jokes” are
developed which are often seen as evidence of positive family develop-
ment and desired unity. When we feel comfortable enough with a group
to readily experience or create humor as a part of it, we experience more
cohesiveness and integration into that group. Thus, many of us observing
a family or work group laughing together may experience a sense of
wistful envy—“it sure looks like they’re having fun”—and we would like
to be a part of it or part of a similar cohort.
Humor provides one key benefit for humanity in that its perception
stimulates consideration of new or alternative concepts or conclusions
based on actions or patterns observed. For individuals, the clash of con-
cepts necessary for humor provokes thought. In social groups, the com-
fort of sharing humor leads to a willingness to explore through commu-
nication. Fittingly, Koestler (1964) situated humor as a central component
of creativity deeply set in the physical and psychological nature of each
person. Humor stimulates alternative thoughts through deviations from
social norms that allow for creative manipulation of concepts. Humor fits
logically as a central component of creativity because it is an adaptable
and unrestrictive aspect of human symbolic behavior. Playing with famil-
iar concepts and creating new ones through humor spark creative
thought. Those first creative acts or thoughts then become a standard part
Communication and Theories of Humor Origin 7
THEORIES OF HUMOR
Humor has long been considered a key part of human personality, life,
and communication. Much study has explored its origins. Humor is alter-
natively explained as an evolved victory yell, a way to release nervous
energy, or a means to deal with and adapt to change. The superiority or
victory perspective appears to be the oldest. The sense of “sudden glory”
referred to by Hobbes matches the sense of triumph one feels when an
opponent is the butt of a memorable gibe. Humor may have evolved as a
way to communicate “safety” to others, because, at least for now, hu-
mans have “won” and are in control of the situation (Martin, 2007). A
successful hunt, a rich harvest, a victory in a contest, or a win in a debate
lead to joyful emotions that closely parallel the mirth experience. This
sense of superiority serves, for some, as foundational for humor.
Yet a “win” or a victory may not be necessary, as the venting of any
nervous energy may make humor possible. Concentration on a task, or
conforming to social norms, necessitates holding back and refraining
from indulging in human subconscious impulses, thereby building up
nervous energy. Experiencing humor serves to dissipate that energy. Fa-
mous psychoanalyst Sigmund Freud held that “humor and laughter per-
10 Chapter 1
mitted individuals to deal with nervous energy and social taboos such as
sexuality, death, fear, embarrassment, and aggression” (Rancer & Gra-
ham, 2012). Humor allows for socially shared and acceptable ways to
deal with such uncomfortable or socially taboo issues. Humor releases or
relieves such tensions, in a generally safe and somewhat socially unifying
way. The sense of relief or relaxation is taken as central to humor by
others.
However, explaining the cognitive element of humor remains. One
must recognize a certain pattern and perceive its alteration before one can
understand humor (Morreall, 1983). Some level of cognitive development
must occur that makes possible abstract thought; capable of invoking
ongoing patterns and entertaining possibilities of implications or evolu-
tions of them. Here is where the “surprise” or “twist” that seems essential
to humor emerges. Humor appreciation seems to require a highly devel-
oped consciousness with matching brain development. The cognitive re-
quirements include the ability to retain knowledge of abstract patterns
and to understand potential alternatives to them. Concepts and patterns
may even “clash” in the brain for the humor experience. Experiencing
humor indicates perception of a change, or that “things are not what they
initially seem to be,” thus requiring some evolved mental development to
appreciate multiple perspectives. A well-developed cognitive capacity is
thus necessary for full invocation and appreciation of humor in situa-
tions, making it a primarily human characteristic Such extensive cogni-
tive ability allows for thinking of alternative or contradictory concepts
that are held by some as necessary for humor, leading to the incongruity
approach. Before exploring details of the three major theoretical ap-
proach to humor creation, however, several efforts at universal theories
that encompass them all are worth attention.
for the fun of saying them, or do things for the enjoyment of them. This
alternative, doing something or talking with no concern for consequences
or the literal truth value of statements, suggests a paratelic frivolity of
talk or action. This game playing can still be taken seriously, though, as
some people focus tightly or get wrapped up in a task in order to win or
because they feel a passion for it. When such a paratelic state is combined
with a feeling of mirth, one has found humor.
The mental separation into a paratelic as opposed to a telic state
makes humor an “escape” through which one can entertain varied per-
spectives regardless of potentially dangerous implications in reality. This
explains the creative potential in humor, as one can “joke around” with-
out having to immediately account for how the ideas will impact or im-
prove one’s practical reality. When the conceptual manipulation with no
practical import elicits a mirth experience, one has created humor. Once
one mentally or verbally returns to practical implications or necessities
arising out of an experience, even if it was initially viewed as humorous
in the paratelic mode, one returns to a “serious” or telic mode. Some
advanced cognitive development would be necessary, then, for such
“flipping” to a mode of amusement, back to seriousness, and back and
forth as people find mirth in communicative situations or cognitively and
mirthfully “play.”
Consider if I am meeting with a group to decide on entertainment
programming for a college over the next academic year, I can suggest
ideas that will solve the group’s problem and provide such entertain-
ment. I want to get the job done by coming up with some neat ideas for
entertainment and set up a plan to enact them. I may get impatient when
other group members start to joke around, because I am in a telic mode
and they are in a paratelic mode, appreciating humor. However, if I
respond to humor with humor, joining fellow group members in a para-
telic mode, an excellent idea for entertainment may emerge that I or other
group members would not have thought of by staying in a serious, prac-
tical, telic mode. One member may joke, for instance, suggesting that “I’d
like to make Professor Smith disappear. Now that would be entertain-
ing!” This idea, laughed at in paratelic mode by the group since all would
realize none would actually want to carry out such a plan, may lead
someone to bring the idea of a magic show up for discussion as the group
returns to a telic mode. Through such cognitive alterations from the expe-
rience of humor to practical and serious considerations, outstanding new
ideas can take shape. A similar development may occur even within the
mind of one person, as amusement by a situation follows and precedes
practical thoughts about it.
Another overarching perspective encompasses key elements that uni-
versal humor theories all seek to include; holding that three components
emerge in successful humor (Smith, 1993). First, one finds an element of
surprise, also referred to as a sudden switch of perspective. Second
12 Chapter 1
Mr. Brown: This is disgusting. I just found out that the superintendent
has made love to every woman in this building except one.
Mrs. Brown: Oh, it must be that stuck-up Mrs. Johnson on the third
floor.
Relief theory would note the violation of expected sexual norms, and
hold that the humor perceived is a result of that violation, perhaps in the
sense that someone actually can violate that norm, or perhaps that it was
not the hearer that violated it! Superiority is invoked due to Mr. Brown’s
“victimhood”; his “loss” by being cheated upon is viewed as essential to
the humor. Incongruity theory would hold that the simple unexpected
Communication and Theories of Humor Origin 13
fact that Mrs. Brown has implicitly confessed to illicitly making love in a
subtle way leads to humor for all those who pick up on that. According to
Veatch’s (1998) theory, the expected social norm of monogamous sexual
relationships is juxtaposed with Mrs. Brown’s confession of her violation
of it, leading to humor. Each theory can explain the instance, but each
theory also highlights different aspects of the humor.
Perspectives on humor origins affect our understanding of how hu-
mor functions in communication. Even if a theory fails in its goal of
explaining all humor instances, its unique approach elicits and clarifies
key strategies that people try to enact using humor. All theories put
forward to explain humor thus have merit for helping to understand its
foundations and uses in communication. The three classic theories, then,
relief, superiority, and incongruity, deserve further exploration.
Relief Humor
According to the relief theory, humor results from reducing stress or
tension, often suddenly, and allows release of nervous energy. Physiolog-
ical symptoms are the most important to look at; the emotional reaction is
the key (Berlyne, 1972; Meyer, 2000; Morreall, 1983). Tensions (or “arou-
sal”) either exist previously (jag theory) or are built up (boost theory) in a
person before sudden release. A psychoanalytic perspective holds that
people generally restrain themselves from violating social norms to avoid
the negative consequences of doing so. Humans’ normal state in society,
then, finds them repressing certain desires and longings so as not to
express them in behavior. Humorous events allow a sudden violation or
enjoyment of the idea of violating social norms. Thus, some of our exist-
ing tensions, aroused earlier, are released, and this feeling of relief creates
the humor. This is the jag theory, holding that humorous events are
“jags” in that they jettison previously aroused social tensions. The boost
theory holds that humor events first raise tensions before releasing
them—as in the classic joke told before the punchline reveals the “an-
swer,” or some specific situation causes tension that a humorous event
then relieves. The joke to begin a speech, especially in a controversial or
awkward situation, is a common example of enactment of the boost theo-
ry of relief humor. However the tension is created, however, relief theory
holds that its release or relaxation is essential to humor. Humor is held as
primarily a physiological effect accompanying some mental events. In
essence, the mirth is key to relief, however it came to be. What makes it
humor is its relief for the human organism.
The more tension relief found, the more humor experienced. This ba-
sic precept of relief theory has actually provided for some objective test-
ing. One memorable study that sought to prove the relief theory is valid
involved bringing students to an apparent biology lab, where they were
told they were to help with a biology experiment. Researchers warned
14 Chapter 1
participants that they would have to either hold a rat, hold a vicious rat
that could bite, or take a blood sample from a vicious rat. In each case, the
rat they were actually handed was plastic (Shurcliff, 1968). Sure enough,
the most humor was experienced by those thought to be in the more
dangerous situations. The plastic rat provoked the most laughter and
mirth in those that thought they would need to inject or hold a vicious
rat, respectively. The more apprehension aroused by the stated planned
task, the more humor participants experienced when the task turned out
to be not the threat they had expected.
Relief theory depends least upon the ability to reason—it allows for
happy laughter with no clear cause. Getting one’s “funnybone tickled”
and moments where “you had to be there” could stem from simple relief
at being surrounded by family or friends, with little (or less) to worry
about at the moment. Nervousness relieved somehow may lead to mutu-
al laughter in situations where there was no joke or one-liner that was
obviously laughable. Laughter at the expense of no one or where any
incongruity is difficult to pinpoint thrives on the relief explanation. A
sense of comfort and safety in the setting and with the social group leads
to humor. A sense of security may be required for humor appreciation in
the first place (Miczo, 2004). The physiological response to humor is pri-
mary for the relief approach. It feels good to laugh, and we enjoy humor
so much because nervous energy is jettisoned or released.
The release of nervous energy has definite implications for human
wellness. Multiple health benefits of humor have clearly emerged from a
variety of research projects, indicating psychological and physical heal-
ing enhanced (DuPre, 1998; Moody, 1978), which sustains explanation via
the relief theory of humor. Such release of tension clearly can have bene-
ficial physical consequences, including lower blood pressure and strong-
er functioning immune systems, protecting humans against disease and
decay. Hence there exists some scientific backing for the cliché that
laughter is the best medicine. Yet more recent research calls into question
the direct physiological evidence of humor experiences’ health benefits. It
turns out the positive effects of humor may be found more in the area of
social support and emotional positivity rather than direct physiological
effects (Martin, 2007). It is natural, however, for humans to seek such a
pleasant way to ease tensions and relieve nervous energy.
Relief theory also focuses on how humor results from releases of ener-
gy due to subconscious overcoming of sociocultural inhibitions. Freud
(1911) held this to be the essential source of humor—venting nervous
energy resulting from subtle social inhibitions through conscious jokes or
actions, or experiencing reduction of social pressure through laughter.
Lewd or scatological humor thus may serve as venues for comic resis-
tance to social norms while in practice preserving them intact. The relief
involved with discussing or playfully violating a basic social norm pro-
vides a feeling of mirth and a humor experience. Consider for the relief
Communication and Theories of Humor Origin 15
Superiority Humor
The superiority theory holds that humor results from a sense of tri-
umph or winning (Feinberg, 1978; Gruner, 1997; Morreall, 1983). Humor
may have evolved from success in the hunt or in battle to success with
verbal jousting, and then to a success of understanding (Ziv, 1984). A
sense of relief and pleasure became humor from a literal victory—a suc-
cessful hunt or defeat of another human group—ensuring safety for a
period of time. Primitive exhilaration over a physical victory became
supplanted through the centuries by a successful verbal riposte, which
today can emerge through understanding a joke about others or seeing
sophisticated satire making light of politicians or commentators one does
not like. As a child (and some may still be overgrown children) one may
find something hilarious about the three stooges, Larry, Curly, and Moe,
slamming each other about and getting clunked on the head or knocked
clear off their feet. After developing more abstract levels of cognitive
ammunition, however, adults can appreciate subtle mockery or put-
downs that require understanding of ongoing events along with social
norms or political preferences that get violated.
Superiority humor also serves to explain the attraction of pleasant
mirth responses as a common individual and social reaction to stupid or
ignorant actions. Laughter follows descriptions of people acting in ignor-
ant ways, as one comments on how “these people are too stupid to live.”
The violation or contradiction endemic to humor is held to have an edge
to it—the violation indicates the superiority of those who perceive it and
presumably would not engage in the violation themselves. This theory
easily explains how laughter is used as a social corrective to enforce
social norms (Ziv, 1984). It is not pleasant to be “laughed at,” though we
love to laugh “with” others. Being laughed at threatens our social stand-
16 Chapter 1
Incongruity Humor
The incongruity theory holds that humor results from a mental reac-
tion to something unexpected, unusual, or odd in a nonthreatening way
when an accepted norm or pattern is violated. This difference must be
close enough to the norm to be nonthreatening, but different enough to
be remarkable (Berger, 1976; Deckers & Divine, 1981; McGhee, 1979;
Schaeffer, 1981). Such difference provokes perception of humor in one’s
mind along with an emotional experience of mirth. This theory empha-
sizes cognition, though–individuals must rationally understand patterns
of reality to recognize differences. Appreciating humor requires the men-
tal capacity to understand and categorize expected patterns and mild
deviations from them, including an unexpected event or object, a physi-
cal or moral defect, an odd or disproportionate object, or an observable
deviation from an implied standard. A key aspect of human survival is
identifying patterns and responding to them. Humor winds up inter-
twined with this capacity, as sudden alterations or violations of patterns
may be viewed as funny. A “safe” pattern change may then be contrasted
with a “dangerous” pattern change, the former calling for mirth with the
latter calling for serious action. Humor then becomes a way to recognize
and deal with nonthreatening changes or surprises.
Incongruity theory relies most on people’s mental juxtapositions,
comparisons of pattern perceptions, and explorations of multiple abstract
concepts needed to appreciate humor. Once people recognize patterns in
life, humor becomes a way to play in a paratelic manner with them. One
classic test of this theory asked individuals to move books, one at a time,
from one table to another. After participants had moved several, a book
was placed in the set that looked like any other but was lined with lead
filling, making it very heavy. After following the initial pattern of book
moving, participants responded with laughter upon lifting the dramati-
cally heavy book (Deckers & Kizer, 1975). Even the brief development of
an understood pattern was enough for its violation to elicit a humor
response. Humor thus stems from cognitive ability to detect and under-
stand patterns, and then perceive and incorporate violations of those
patterns.
Humor development has indeed been found to occur in correlation
with cognitive development in children (McGhee, 1979), which reinforces
the cognitive perspective of the incongruity theory. As children grow,
they gain more ability to manipulate abstract thoughts and understand
more sophisticated patterns of thought and observation. Some actions or
sayings that are funny to children may be funny in additional ways to
adults. A preschool child playing “teacher” may tell his friends to “Get
18 Chapter 1
out your mat and get on it,” and have them play along, while an adult
laughs at the additional notion of how apt an imitation of a common
teacher saying that phrase it was. The adult has the mental capacity to
add the abstract “teacher script” and “imitation teacher script” to the
basic idea of playing school that engages the children. This ability to hold
and compare abstract thoughts in mind seems essential to humor.
An element of surprise is also a key ingredient in humor, but not
essential. When an incongruity—a violation of expected norms or mental
reasoning patterns—suddenly and dramatically presents itself, a person
seeks to resolve the incongruity. A child, for instance, telling another
child to “Get out your mat and get on it,” is not a normal pattern, even if a
teacher does it regularly. The surprise of hearing it from a child can be
part of the humor experience. Even if the event itself is not a surprise,
some resolution or integration with expected patterns is necessary for
understanding humor. Humor appreciation requires a dramatic rear-
rangement of concepts in the mind (Maase, Fink, & Kaplowitz, 1984).
Such a change in concepts, as the mind adjusts to a pattern violation and
a possible new pattern, may result in humor.
A humor-triggering divergence from the norm also must be moder-
ate—neither so minutely different as to be unnoticed, nor so shockingly
out of place as to provoke fear. If one is unaware of a pattern, or only
aware of the basics, a minor violation may not even be perceived, pre-
venting humor appreciation. Conversely, a dramatic violation that pierc-
ingly strikes one’s notice may engage emotions other than mirth due to
feelings in response to threat or danger. Strong emotions can overwhelm
humor–anger, fear, outrage, or disgust can prevent its appreciation.
Consider, for instance, two recommendations for “maintaining a
healthy level of insanity”: When the money comes out of the ATM,
scream, “I won! I won!” Alternatively, when leaving the zoo, start run-
ning toward the parking lot, yelling, “Run for your lives! They’re loose!”
We can immediately picture such typical situations of modern life, but
can then appreciate the sudden violation of the expected pattern. In the
first situation, the juxtaposition of getting cash with successful gambling
at a machine can be two abstract concepts that blend in the mind to
produce laughter. If one did not understand the routine of getting cash
from an ATM, or how gambling machines worked, the situation might
not be funny. The second situation may be taken seriously, and humor
might not be present—a sincere threat from loose animals might be per-
ceived, as the second person joins in the sprint. Too much deviation from
an expected pattern may shock and alarm, preventing humor apprecia-
tion. Thus, the alteration of the norm must be noticeable, sudden, and
require adjustments of expected thought patterns. Therein lies the hu-
mor, according to the incongruity perspective.
Incongruity theory focuses on what needs too happen conceptually
for people to laugh or “get a joke.” Pattern recognition is key (Nowgen,
Communication and Theories of Humor Origin 19
ing another alternative. The excitement and advanced creativity that re-
sults in social groups engaged in such a comic perspective, as opposed to
the despair and oppression (even in the face of possible efficiency) one
associates with tragic-oriented groups, suggests humor may serve a use-
ful social function indeed.
Within any conversation, such laughter as occurs can communicate
positively about relationships, or unify a group through its contagious
spread sparked by evaluating procedures or events (Keyton & Beck,
2010). Since laughter most often signifies humor to a lesser or greater
extent, an overarching “comic” perspective is evident when humor is
experienced. As humor unites and divides people, through doing so so-
cial groups are constituted, relationships grow or die, and social boun-
daries are established. Thus, simply by invoking any kind of wit or joke
in conversation, humor serves key social functions. Humor use promotes
a “sharedness” that can enhance relationships and further interaction,
even at the expense of dividing from or alienating some others.
in more recent times, this is even encouraged. One of the most famous
and popular episodes of the 1970s situation comedy, The Mary Tyler
Moore Show, dealt with how to deal with the death of a clown—when was
laughter appropriate? It turned out, in very humorous fashion, even at
his funeral! Although humor use in certain social circumstances normally
may be viewed as “socially unacceptable,” people find ways to use it
anyway to cope with loss and change. This experience actually seems to
enhance the human ability to adapt and cope, in defiance of expected
social norms. Humor’s capacity to disrupt events perceptually and social-
ly is valued even in ultimately tragic situations where one would expect
to humor use to be inappropriate.
Although humor is disruptive, people desire it to be so, as one of the
most pleasurable human experiences. People go out of their way to seek
humor, as popular movies, television shows, comedians, and joke-telling
indicate. Such disruptions in the form of humor lead to a contagious
humor state, spread to others by facial expressions and especially by the
sound of laughter (Chafe, 1987). That sound becomes more than just a
byproduct of the human body’s expulsion of air, as it makes known to
others that one is experiencing humor, which often infects them too. An
individual disruption thus becomes a social disruption, affecting a rela-
tionship or entire group. Laughter may be thought of as a signal indicat-
ing safety in the face of a social disruption (Morreall, 1983). Laughter,
through indicating lack of seriousness or threatening violence, can reas-
sure individual social survival intact even through a disruption. Humor
thus changes human lives through social interaction as well as individual
perceptions and thinking.
Humor produces arousal in social beings because it represents a rup-
ture in, or a temporary subversion of, the routine character of social life.
Laughter is then a spontaneous physiological response to the violation of
deeply embedded expectations about “what kind of thing should be hap-
pening here.” Examining jokes as apparent violations of a commonly
understood social order can reveal a lot about that very social order. A
humorous treatment of a topic, or laughter at it, can serve to mitigate a
threat or communicate its absence in a situation. Humor use seems to
provide morally, politically, or relationally safe ways to express ideas in
spite of the restrictions that any kind of order—even an order voluntarily
produced and reproduced—will inexorably impose. Jokes may also, for
both tellers and recipients, jointly produce an intelligible social order.
Gary Fine (1976) explored how obscene humor provided “safe” ways to
deal with sexual or unhygienic threats to society by laughing at playful
violations of them. American “bachelor parties” on the eve of a wedding,
in one instance, allows laughter at sexually explicit behavior in the face of
ongoing and soon reinforced social restrictions on sexual activity, espe-
cially within a marriage. The object or cause of the humor will reinforce
desired social patterns.
Functions of Humor in Communication 27
One approach to humor interprets all of its indicators and its sharing
as manifestations of disablement (Chafe, 1987). Since the situation is play-
ful or “safe,” a response that leads to laughter physiologically reinforces
reduction of any ability to respond seriously or in any way that could
create serious consequences. A sense of “nonseriousness” or not being
concerned with immediate consequences may be communicated by
laughter between people (Chafe, 2007), and also experienced by individu-
als who may be viewed as disabled through humor experience. One can-
not fight when one is doubled over in laughter. The experience of humor
keeps people from acting, partly by physically disabling them, partly
through pleasure that distracts them from performing any serious task.
Laughing hard makes it difficult to get anything accomplished, as trying
to lift something while doing so indicates, or trying to make a decision as
a group in the face of strong laughter. Consider U. S. Vice President Joe
Biden, introducing as part of a campaign address a state senator who was
in attendance but who also was in a wheelchair. Not seeing the latter,
after introducing the man Mr. Biden intoned, “Stand up, Chuck, let eve-
rybody see you.” This was followed by uncertain laughter, as Biden then
noticed the situation, and said, “Well, there he is . . .” After that point, it
became difficult for a time to continue the speech with any intended
serious political topics. Similar frustrations are experienced by group
members wanting a serious discussion from a group that is distracted by
laughter at some humorous diversion. Such disablement or nonserious-
ness apparently helped human survival enough that it spreads easily
from an individual to others in a group. Laughter can be a signal for
safety or lack of concern that can transcend visual presence (Morreall,
2009). Serious, possibly regretted actions were avoided by communicat-
ing a sense of safety in the face of a noted disruption. Thus were humans
prevented from continuing to act on an ineffective perception or follow a
damaging course of action.
The key to humor as a desirable disruption emerges when the humor
experience prevents humans from taking things seriously, either physi-
cally or mentally. Humor especially keeps one from taking seriously
things one ought not to take seriously (Chafe, 1987). Everyday life often
leads to circumstances that might suggest taking a particular action; yet,
with a broader perspective, one understands that it should not be en-
acted. Jokes and humor shared therefore often suggest what NOT to do,
reinforcing social norms by calling attention to their violation. Chafe pro-
vided an example: A New Yorker cartoon shows a man beginning to
walk across a street when the sign says “WALK.” When he is in the
middle of the street, the sign changes to “DON'T WALK” and he stops,
even though cars are bearing down on him. This cartoon created a world
that has a certain kind of plausibility; a world where one follows instruc-
tions unthinkingly. Although we can understand such a world, a broader
perspective recognizes its counterproductive nature, and instead of tak-
28 Chapter 2
ing it seriously we fall into the humor state, keeping us from actually
enacting such behavior (although often vicariously). Instead of acting in
conformity with that funny world, we expel air from our lungs, our mus-
cles weaken, we feel pleased, and we let others know about it. We also
refrain from acting, as we do not take the situation seriously.
Humor itself, like any social activity, falls into patterns, and thus may
be viewed formulaically: Berger (1997) held that a limited number of
techniques exist to elicit humor, categorized by issues of language, logic,
identity, and action. Within these categories, he found 45 techniques that
create humor, suggesting that the particular changes created by humor in
a person are limited and potentially may be isolated. Language tech-
niques involve invocation of puns, irony, or sarcasm; humor that
emerged from word play or language manipulation. Logic techniques
involve clashes of logic, including absurdity, coincidence, and the com-
mon laughter at mistakes or at accidents, along with overly rigid individ-
uals or repetition. Identity techniques provide humor though character
traits, especially through stereotypes, parody, and imitations. Action hu-
mor involves slapstick or clumsy physical behaviors. Berger’s was the
most comprehensive attempt to classify these specific changes that hu-
mans encounter that produce humor, although others also have at-
tempted this (Chiaro, 1992). Such categorizations of humor forms show
that even pattern violations can potentially be patterned. Still, the wide
variety of potential sources of humor suggests a great number of creative
avenues for creativity through invoking it.
In natural speech, laughter often ensues not in response to jokes but
simply as an accompaniment to what someone is saying. The contagious
nature of humor is regularly used to improve social interaction
(O’Donnell-Trujillo & Adams, 1983), reducing a seriousness that might be
threatening. Laughter’s message implying “safety” may be taken advan-
tage of even if nothing specific can be pointed to as “funny.” One or both
parties to a conversation may laugh without seeking details on what the
other person specifically found funny—even if that person could explain
it. Humor thus serves, like politeness and smiling, as a social lubricant
enhancing interaction. Any need that one person may perceive to take
corrective action against perceived slights or violations by the other is
“put on hold” through such laughter indicating mutual ongoing regard
during a conversation. This allows the conversation to proceed, and for
both parties to risk some creative license in expressing views and bring-
ing up other topics.
Humor’s disabling process actually transforms it into a socially ena-
bling mechanism. Any human social group is in danger of falling into
rigid patterns of social expectations, or routines. Some sort of social dis-
ruption is therefore needed to enhance interest, rejuvenation, and creativ-
ity. Seriously getting things done needs to be balanced by a “play ethic.”
Play, like sleep, is a mentally disabling mechanism that allows people to
Functions of Humor in Communication 29
social creatures who want to be a part of social events, and humor experi-
ences are no different. Thus, we often laugh along and reinforce humor’s
power as a social unifier.
The combination of these humor qualities leading to a more cohesive
group can be illustrated by studying humor in the workplace. When
members of an organization were interviewed individually to elicit nar-
ratives that encapsulated values shared in their work lives, only the hu-
morous stories were duplicated by multiple interviewees. Such stories,
shared with each other as well as with the interviewer, clearly demon-
strated work values held in common (in one specific case, valuing a sense
of fun, clear communication, and teaching children—Meyer, 1997). The
understood patterns that sparked humor were definitely among the
strongest held in common by people communicating at their workplace;
each similarly chose to find such incidents funny, and sharing these sto-
ries with one another had made them seem even funnier (or, at least, their
outward reactions to humor to be more pronounced). Humor was also
used as social discipline by workers, however—those who violated
norms were called to account by their actions being laughed at in stories.
Clearly, humor divided or united through subjective choices shared by
group members communicating.
Unifying humor has been referred to as affiliative humor while divi-
sive humor has been called aggressive humor (Miczo, 2004; Miczo, Aver-
beck, & Mariani, 2009). The former draws out positive feelings in a group
and strengthens relationships; the latter creates distance between com-
municators and may lower morale between them. A playful frame of
mind makes affiliative humor more likely and creativity follows. As hu-
mans develop cognitively the ability to understand and use humor, they
begin by comprehending a unifying or relaxing function first and then
evolve a more refined appreciation of humor capable of enacting social
divisions. Children’s humor, for instance, develops from a more proso-
cial, silly form in the preschool years to a more antisocial, norm-violating
form during grade school years (Socha & Kelly, 1994). The unifying func-
tions seem to be learned first, before the divisive yet playful forms be-
come invoked. At age 10 or so, politeness violations become funnier than
simple object or language violations. People start with affiliative humor,
then, and later gradually learn to apply its converse, the aggressive per-
spective.
As humans move from a seeking for comfort and security to under-
standing social groupings, humor is used to discover and then enforce
each. Humor creation is facilitated by a sense of security, safety, and
control (Miczo, 2004). Secure attachment to another enhances and is en-
hanced by humor use. Laughter can signal a play frame that promotes a
humorous message—the initiation of one by the sender and acceptance of
one by the receiver of the message. Yet humor can also attack—it can
tease or ridicule or set apart. A violation can be pointed out or a differ-
Functions of Humor in Communication 33
er at one’s expense, while highway patrol officers may not enjoy that
story at all. In any given situation, a person may choose to unite in appre-
ciation of humor or divide from others by not appreciating it. These
contrasting aspects of humor may be laid out and diagrammed in rela-
tion to one another in order to ascertain humor’s key social functions.
Identification
First, enhancing unity with an audience occurs most strongly through
identification, where a message invokes and stresses shared context or
meaning. Humor that strongly elicits laughter reinforces the meaning of a
message about “us,” showing that “we share this”—whatever the topic
may be. Identification humor builds speaker credibility (Gruner, 1985;
Malone, 1980) and group cohesiveness (Graham, Papa, & Brooks, 1992).
Clarification
Second, clarification humor captures an opinion or belief in a memor-
able short phrase or anecdote. With humor, audiences recall such clarifi-
cations of issues (Goldstein, 1976; Gruner, 1967) and media often replay
them. In the past such quips might make the newspapers, magazines, or a
quick actuality or “sound bite” on the radio or television news. Now they
might spread via email, social media posts, or by views on YouTube.
When the stress of the remark remains on the expected norm rather than
the potential or actual violation of such a norm, such humor use clarifies
beliefs or social norms. A memorable or apt story or phrase emerges that
puts the issue advocated into words in a compelling way. The laughter is
not primarily sparked by a norm violation, but upon some reinterpreta-
tion of the desired social norm itself.
For instance, jokes, quips, and teases have long been noted and exam-
ined in the workplace; there, receiving new information often sparks hu-
mor use serving to clarify how the group will respond to potential
change (Ullian, 1976). The awkward situation of being told to change
something the group is used to or values can be managed by a humorous
remark. Discomfort and uncertainty about change are channeled through
remarks that make light of the need for altering routines or the social
order. Implicit in such remarks is often the notion that change is unwel-
come and will require some adjustment, but will eventually occur. When
an instance of humor emphasizes the expected norm rather than a per-
ceived violation of such a norm, it serves to clarify beliefs or social norms.
People enhance relationships through laughing together at such remarks
that effectively reinforce social norms—by showing that the background
norms and expectations still endure.
Errors and mistakes in messages can be treated humorously, allowing
messages that seek to transmit information yet go awry to actually rein-
force their message through humor. For instance, one female employee
called the office to make some arrangements for procedures after her
baby was born. The next day, she came into the office, only to have
multiple employees do a double take and ask variants of “What are you
doing here?” It turned out word had spread that she had her baby, so she
was involved in many humorous conversations with people letting them
Functions of Humor in Communication 37
know that, no, she had not had the baby yet (Meyer, 1997). The fact that
she did need to make arrangements for after the baby was born was
reinforced by her clarifying messages, even as much humor was per-
ceived. Thus can mistakes humorously clarify a norm or intended goal.
Messages seeking to transmit information that go awry can actually rein-
force their meaning through humor. Such a message becomes more no-
ticed than it otherwise might have been. Messages that humorously vio-
late work norms may thus reinforce them, as regarding bill-paying: “If
you think nobody cares, try missing a couple of payments,” as well as the
work ethic itself: “Hard work pays off in the future. Laziness pays off
now.” The incongruity of a message’s violation of expectations makes it
stand out all the more due to its error, yet its intended message is still
implied. The expected pattern of messages is reinforced by laughter at an
“exception” to the norm or rule.
The key effect of the clarification function lets one encapsulate a posi-
tion on an issue through a humorous remark. Rapier wit can help per-
suade through a memorable quip or one-liner. Humor that occurs in a
pointed or memorable phrase with which one agrees but may spark
thought is typical of the clarification function. As the humor use captures
attention and stimulates memory, receivers recall the position advocated
with extra clarity (Goldstein, 1976; Gruner, 1967). Normally, such humor
reinforces relationships or group cohesion. For instance, people of a cer-
tain age might relate to the aphorism, “[age] 35 is when you finally get
your head together and your body starts falling apart.” Such crisp,
pointed comments reinforce a commonly-experienced social norm.
Essential to the clarification function is memorably expressing a posi-
tion on an issue through a humorous remark. One can thus express a
position through a memorable quip or one-liner. Consider the advice:
“always remember that you are unique. Just like everyone else.” Such a
dramatic clash of concepts may be funny, but also thought-provoking.
Humor allows a sudden and dramatic clash of a perceived violation with
an expected norm, with sender and message receivers sharing that norm
and presuming that it will emerge triumphant after the interaction.
Enforcement
Third, moving toward the more divisive end of the humor function
continuum, enforcement humor allows for the potentially friendly criti-
cism of a social norm violator. Duncan (1962) noted that humor can en-
force social norms through “discipline by laughter.” Humor can be a
“safety valve” for laughing at violations of norms and thus strengthen
the effectiveness of the norm. Teases can gently correct while maintaining
some level of concern for another party (Alberts, Kellar-Gunther, & Cor-
man, 1996; Graham, Papa, & Brooks, 1992; Young & Bippus, 2001). One
can laugh at the ignorance of an outsider to the group, or a child still
38 Chapter 2
learning social norms and knowledge, without malice but clearly sug-
gesting that the person needs to learn the norm or knowledge. Humor
serving this function does stress the violation of the norm, indicating that
it may be funny for now but needs to be corrected.
Enforcement humor enacts criticism of one violating social expecta-
tions. This could be done in the form of teases, or more forthright ridi-
cule. Although similar to the “friendlier” clarification function where
one’s position can be encapsulated in a humorous phrase, enforcement
adds a critical or attacking element. This humor “calls to account” a per-
son or group found to be violating an expected norm, or a found incon-
gruity that needs correction. One husband, for instance, was reported to
say that “I haven’t spoken to my wife in 18 months. I don’t like to inter-
rupt her.” The surprise here serves as a reminder that one should not talk
too much, or that both parties in a relationship should talk more equally.
In such instances of teasing, the humor comes from a violation of a seri-
ous social norm that would often be considered unacceptable. Invoking a
humorous remark serves as a reminder of the norm and the act that
needs correcting. How the other party responds to such a remark can
make a crucial difference in relationships or social arrangements, as dis-
cussed below.
Duncan (1962) explored norm enforcement through humor use, call-
ing it “discipline by laughter.” The desire not to be the subject of humor
or jokes can be strong, and people will take pains to avoid it. Thus, being
teased about something lets that person and other witnesses know that
such divergences from the norm will be noted, pointed out, perhaps ridi-
culed. Paradoxically, even as such enforcement humor may spur some-
one to take steps to avoid violating the norm invoked, relationships are
often strengthened along with social norms through such “corrective
teasing.” Humor often serves to reinforce differences as well as norms
found in work organizations (Lynch, 2007; 2009; 2010). One humorous
question that people have asked can illustrate this: “Why do they lock
those exterior gas station bathrooms? Are they afraid someone will clean
them?” Clearly, what should be done (clean the bathrooms) is not being
done, and this can be expressed in a surprising humorous way. Often, the
source of such humor comes from contradicting norms that many would
never seriously consider removing or altering. The attention and laughter
that such violations receive reinforce the notion that here is an important
social norm that should not be changed. After all, if the norm was trivial
or of little concern, violating it would be a non-noteworthy event. Viola-
tions of a norm that matters, however, prove to be funny because the
norm in fact does matter.
Teases occur in relationships that have grown somewhat close (Al-
berts, Kellar-Gunther, & Corman, 1996; Young & Bippus, 2001). Enough
contact has preceded the interaction that shared norms make possible
mutual understanding of humor “scripts,” and relational norms are fa-
Functions of Humor in Communication 39
Differentiation
Fourth, the most divisive function of humor, differentiation, contrasts
dramatically one group with another or one speaker with another. One is
being laughed at by those laughing with the humorous messenger. Such
humor exposes social alliances and divisions. Ridicule can reinforce polit-
ical unity among one group’s members as it stresses contradictions and
differences with others (Schutz, 1977). As the harshest function of humor,
its audience may be very familiar with the subject, but are expected to
completely disagree with the humor’s target. This function of humor has
been explored through hundreds of years through theories of superiority,
long viewed as a key element of humor (Gruner, 1997). The strong effects
40 Chapter 2
43
44 Chapter 3
The comic mode implies a playful situation that makes the emotion of
mirth possible. Pleasure is taken in a pattern violation or a relief of some
stressor. Contrasting perspectives mesh and humor is perceived. There is
more objectivizing of whatever is perceived (Grimes, 1955), and consider-
ations of alternatives are readily undertaken, yet without immediate con-
cern for possible consequences or action to be taken. The tragic mode, on
the other hand, focuses directly on potentially dangerous consequences
to a situation and often on an immediate action that must be taken. Mak-
ing a decision about what is to be done is of immediate moment, and
potentially bad consequences loom from practically any alternative. In-
stead of “playing” with possible juxtapositions of perceptions, immediate
and clear perception of the “truth” is desired, because a decision fraught
with implications is necessary. The tragic mode involves only bona-fide
communication in a telic mode, where consequences matter and mes-
sages are to be taken literally and seriously as given. A sense of “entrap-
ment” is pervasive in the tragic mode, while the comic mode has a sense
of liberation from “requirements” or immediate consequences. Thus, tol-
erance and creativity—and allowance for mistakes—are more fit for the
comic mode, while errors can be damaging or even fatal when one oper-
ates in the tragic mode.
Once an event, phrase, or more generically labeled life pattern disrup-
tion that may lead to humor occurs, there can be wide variation in human
responses to it. Some find it funny, and experience humor, but others do
not. Explaining these idiosyncratic differences has been a key barrier to
developing a comprehensive theory of humor in communication. There is
a clear element of intentionality in humor, as individuals choose whether
or not to find an event or perception funny (Attardo, 1992). This individ-
ual choice is at the heart of humor creation and appreciation: before it can
be social, it must be an individual choice to initiate or experience some-
thing as funny. So we begin with a potential humor event: something that
an individual might choose to find funny. Should one take it seriously,
and treat it as necessitating thought and action that leads to direct conse-
quences to the self? Or should one play with it mentally, seeing alterna-
tive perspectives simultaneously, and taking pleasure in unusual impli-
cations or juxtapositions of thought and symbolism? One can sense a
difference as mirthful, funny, delightful, or uncertain, dangerous, requir-
ing consequential thought or action.
This key choice illustrated in the model (figure 3.1), when a pattern
violation in the form of a disrupted norm or a psychological shift occurs,
involves whether or not to find it funny and experience humor. Even if
humans do not consciously make such a choice, mood and previous ex-
periences set the stage for the split second in which one experiences
humor or does not (Carrell, 1992). One can view the event in a playful,
non-serious, paratelic or comic mode, taking it lightly and not showing
concern for serious consequences. Alternatively, one can view the same
A Model of Individual Humor Choice 45
poses of positive affect, expressiveness, and negative affect. The first pur-
pose, positive affect, involved unifying or ingratiating humor, or the like-
lihood of telling a joke or making a humorous remark to show similarity
or affiliation with others. Expressiveness involved self-disclosure and ex-
pression of feelings. Self-deprecating humor and sarcasm or irony about
one’s personal failures fit here. Finally, negative affect turned out to be
instances of humor that served differentiation functions, including de-
meaning and belittling others. Interpersonal competence was found to be
greater with those who used more positive affect humor, unsurprisingly.
More negative uses of humor tended to be viewed as means of control of
others. Humor emerged clearly as one crucial tool in competent commu-
nication and relationship-building.
A newer measure highlighting humor assessment created by Wrench
and McCroskey (2001) moved beyond the dimensions of joke-telling and
storytelling to include contributions of funny sayings and generally “be-
ing funny” in social situations. An added survey for those receiving hu-
morous messages from the target communicator included dimensions of
humor appreciation in communication as well as that of humor initiation
studied in earlier measures. The humor assessment instrument was more
sensitive to how much one “got” a joke, as in how much of the script did
one know, as well as how much one “approved” of the humor, as in
found it funny (Richmond, Wrench, & Gorham, 2001). The researchers
sought a global measure of one’s sense of humor as expressed through
communication. The humor assessment instrument also found level of
humor initiation in communication correlated with elements of sociabil-
ity and communication satisfaction, and communication competence.
An entire research program has followed from the presentations of
these scales, focusing on how individuals handle humor and sets of com-
munication variables that correlate with those measures. Individuals
high in humor orientation use humor more spontaneously and have
more flexibility in their communication (Wanzer, Booth-Butterfield, &
Booth-Butterfield, 1995). They more readily comment in fun and can
spark humor in a variety of situations. They also will explore a great
variety of topics in communication, and are more likely to “go with the
flow” in conversation. They also can initiate conversation and joking
around on unique or unusual topics, They find humor in more situations
than those low in humor orientation, and they also like to create positive
impressions with communicators and likely work harder at it.
Those with high humor orientation, thus, are perceived as funnier and
more entertaining by others. These people are “the life of the party” and
are fun to talk to on a personal level. They tend to brighten one’s day, as
they can be fairly counted upon the get others to laugh, smile, or appre-
ciate a creative thought in conversation. People seek them out for conver-
sation and to develop relationships. Thus, more humor-oriented individ-
uals are less lonely and have enhanced social attractiveness (Wanzer,
54 Chapter 3
prove one’s own life as well as that of those with whom one shares
communication. Higher levels of self-reported sense of humor associate
with higher levels of extraversion and social adaptability. These are the
people more willing to communicate in various social settings and will
change messages—and types of humorous stories and remarks—to fit the
social situation. Retaining such a flexible and happy approach to life
throughout one’s life cycle, especially as one ages, is a worthy goal, and
suggests that cultivating humor appreciation and sharing that with other
can contribute greatly to life satisfaction.
The propensity to use humor may actually be partly biologically de-
termined and partly culturally learned (Wrench & McCroskey, 2001). To
some degree, then, “we cannot help it.” We are funny or we’re not. Yet
enough research shows that we are not “locked into” whatever level of
the trait of humor orientation that may be measured at one time. The
stage is set by biology and genes, but we can influence our own humor
orientation by choosing a comic mode more often, seeing the humor in
situations, and then choosing to communicate that humor in some way
through messages. The health and social benefits of humor use to indi-
viduals may translate into longer, healthier life spans and more ability to
adapt to changing natural and social environments. The evolutionary
benefits to humor in individuals follow, as greater levels of humor appre-
ciation lead, arguably, to better abilities to survive and cope with health
and social difficulties—to enhance one’s life overall. This may explain
why the capacity for humor seems “built-in” for humans and is biologi-
cally useful in an evolutionary way.
As these individual tests of humor have developed, two dimensions
of individual senses of humor have been measured. One is the ability to
create humor; to see incongruities and readily communicate them to oth-
ers. The second dimension involves humor appreciation, or the ability to
understand and enjoy messages invoking certain incongruities (Ziv,
1984). In spite of the rich quantity of research involving the initiation of
humor in conversations, most people appreciate humor and engage in
laughing but are less readily able to create laughter in others. Thus, the
second dimension seems more readily developed than the first. Many
individuals can laugh together readily, and appreciate humor in a variety
of ways without necessarily even laughing out loud. Yet, fewer people in
any social group show great ability to create humor, whether through
stories or thoughtful sayings. The uniqueness of “class clowns” comes to
mind; these were almost always a small minority of any social group. The
fact that comedians can make a living being funny does suggest that their
abilities are relatively rare and prized highly. Most individual measures
collapse both aspects of sense of humor together, but the measures dis-
cussed here of humor creation can elaborate on unique comic production
capability, but there is no pretending that a strong ability to initiate and
create humor in conversation is truly widespread in the population. Even
56 Chapter 3
us, for instance, early individual studies of humor found higher apprecia-
tion for jokes and stories that divided women from men (Cantor, 1976).
Aggressive humor reifying social divisions of gender would explain find-
ings in earlier years of a preference across both genders for humor at the
expense of females. Fortunately, more recent studies find much less of
this phenomenon. Many other individual differences can be subject to
humorous aggression, though, and humans have been masterful through
the centuries at group divisions, into an “us” versus “them.” Humor
through aggression can paradoxically divide those subject to the aggres-
sion while unifying those appreciating or sharing the humor. Even here,
this may serve individuals well who find themselves in debates and need
a memorable way to “attack” or “disarm” an argumentative opponent.
One might argue that aggressive humor is far more socially acceptable
and useful than serious violence, or even than more serious, harsh words
that remain in a telic mode. Humor, in the end, is far more civilized as
aggression than is violence.
An additional way to measure an individual’s humor experience is to
take note of nonverbal behaviors. One can smile, laugh briefly or ner-
vously, or laugh extendedly engaging one’s diaphragm. Smiles can also
be distinguished by depth of experience or engagement. A smile associat-
ed with genuine enjoyment or amusement involves the eye muscles as
well as the mouth. This is referred to as the Duchenne smile, named for
the French doctor who identified the unique smile in 1862 (Martin, 2007).
A Duchenne smile and genuine laughter, then, can show a deep level of
humor experience, while nervous laughter and a smile not involving the
eye muscles but instead others around the face may indicate a more
“social” smile, or respect for relational or group experiences without a
strong personal humor appreciation. Nonverbal indicators have a reputa-
tion as more genuine measures of personal experiences, especially of
emotions. Individual experiences of mirth then relate to similar findings
by the measures discussed above where humor experience provides for
more creative problem solving by individuals as well as accumulates
social and physical support for coping with life (Martin, 2007).
These primarily quantitative lines of ongoing research on humor
show how central it is to individual communication. All individuals can
be assessed for humor production or humor appreciation rates, with vari-
ous combinations of those possible. Each individual humor rating is
found to correlate with all sorts of important social and communication
variables. Each person’s unique approaches to humor dramatically affect
one’s unique approaches to communication with others, which in turn
lead to desirable development of relationships. Following the human
need to relate to one another and reach out for social solidarity to get
through life, much fruitful research has occurred exploring humor’s place
in establishing and affecting human relationships.
FOUR
Humor and Persuasion
59
60 Chapter 4
drawing power and greater attention can provide more opportunities for
persuasion. Increased interest may lead to persuasion, rather than humor
use directly persuading listeners. Still, that boost in focus that may be
captured by using humor could be crucial for persuasion. Persuaders
need all the tools they can get, and enough evidence has mounted that
humor is one key tool that can pierce the fog of information overload and
myriad persuasive attempts to reach audiences for potential persuasion.
Attracting interest may be called the first necessary step toward persua-
sion, and humor use can do just that.
Interest may not only be gained by using humor, it may also distract
listeners from the persuasive message. The distraction of a mirth re-
sponse from the specific persuasive appeals can also increase their effects,
but humor also spurs some discounting of the message because it is treat-
ed as more entertaining than influential (Skalski, Tamborini, Glazer, &
Smith, 2009). A hearer may enjoy the humor so much that entertainment
and enjoyment become primary goals, and exploring arguments or alter-
natives on an issue get pushed aside as goals. Humor may serve as a
distraction, which during receiving of a persuasive message is associated
with less mental production of counterarguments and increasing levels of
communication acceptance (Osterhouse & Brock, 1970). The one plus to
this state of affairs, then, is that one who is distracted during the course of
a persuasive message is more subject to being influenced by it. So gaining
a listener’s interest along with distracting through humor can lead to
effective persuasion after all. In the end, though, relying on humor alone
to persuade can be dangerous because possibly ineffective, but using it to
interest the audience in a presentation to set the stage for additional
persuasive appeals through logic, emotion, and testimony can enhance
persuasion.
Humor does serve as a distraction that can enhance persuasion. The
effort made to understand the humor may lower the mental time and
energy spent considering the persuasive message. Emotions also affect
individual readiness for persuasion, as they act as part of the filter per-
suasive messages must pass through. Individuals who are in a good
mood will less often disagree with a persuasive message. By manipulat-
ing concepts incongruously for the sake of humor, persuaders provide a
building-up and release of tension, providing for audience relief and
mood enhancement (Maase, Fink, & Kaplowitz, 1984). A distraction that
improves mood, then, can enhance persuasion through decreased resis-
tance. There is also some evidence of a “sleeper effect” where humorous
messages may be more persuasive over the long term than immediately
(Nabi, Moyer-Guse, & Byrne, 2007). Playing with concepts in a persua-
sive talk can, through humor, get people interested, distracted, and in a
better mood where further thinking about the topic at hand will perhaps
persuade them.
62 Chapter 4
humor can bolster speaker credibility with audiences and hold their at-
tention (Gruner, 1967; 1970; Duncan & Nelson, 1985). Reagan would find
a way to tell a story that would reach out to his audiences and showed
them that he could relate to their lives. Through adding this element of
entertainment for audiences, Reagan successfully bolstered his credibil-
ity. In addition, Reagan would find a way to mock “inferior” politicians
who were favoring all sorts of ridiculous measures, persuasively uniting
his audience with him in opposition to “them.” He once noted that “bu-
reaucracy has a built-in instinct for preservation and reproduction of its
own kind. A federal program, once started, is the nearest thing to eternal
life you’ll ever see on this earth” (Reagan, 1976). He would also tell hu-
morous stories to memorably make campaign points. Some could be en-
capsulated in turns of phrase like: “if the President wants a definition of
recession, I’ll give him one. Recession is when your neighbor loses his
job, depression is when you lose yours, and recovery will be when Jimmy
Carter loses his“ (Boller, 1982, p. 354). Reagan enacted three key strate-
gies involving humor as he boosted his credibility and audience good
will toward him, entertained audiences with memorable quips and sto-
ries to make persuasive points, and staged a “jolly rebellion” against his
opponents without becoming harsh, slashing, or negative. Thus, humor
was his “velvet weapon” (Meyer, 1990). Politicians since that time have
tried to use humor as effectively, or have suffered in comparison.
Humor, as Reagan found repeatedly, can show a politician’s ability to
relate to and establish a similar social level for addressing an audience.
Self-deprecatory humor can be used sparingly to effectively enhance a
powerful politician’s credibility, as President Gerald Ford often did
(Chapel, 1978). Yet candidates still must beware putting in too much
humor lest they be thought of as “un-presidential” or not seriously able
to handle the nation’s problems (Levasseur & Dean, 1996). Doses of hu-
mor in political rhetoric can help to persuade, even though an overdose
will reduce credibility due to perceived lack of concern for conse-
quences—the telic mode is considered most requisite for serious political
candidates, and persuaders, after all. Once one relates to an audience and
boosts credibility, other means of persuasion are clearly called for.
Scholars have also begun to explore the growing humorous counter-
weight to politicians—the nightly news/entertainment blend that not
only reviews events of the day, but mocks them and tells humorous
stories or enacts funny skits about them. In more and more instances,
younger people have replaced the old standard view of the persuasive
political world that included news accounts punctuated by a rare humor-
ous one-liner or story with nightly entertainment at the expense of politi-
cians and newsmakers. “In short, fewer Americans are depending on
traditional news outlets for their understanding of the political world,
relying to some degree on late night talk and political comedy shows
instead” (Lichter, Baumgartner, & Morris, 2015, p. 32). The most interest-
Humor and Persuasion 65
Through many years, even in the printed press in the days before late
night political television comedy, much political argument has involved
satire. It has a decidedly mixed persuasive record, however. Satire is
presented on the “edge” of the telic and paratelic divide: a serious mes-
sage is being conveyed, yet it is conveyed in a humorous way. Since
mockery and imitation are part of satire, however, to understand it one
must have a well-developed sense of expected patterns and their viola-
tion. Viewers of The Colbert Report over the past decade, for instance, may
tune in to hear a conservative view of the news, and come away perceiv-
ing to have done so. However, many others may enjoy the spoofing or
mockery of conservative commentators (uses and gratifications theory
reinforces explanations of either tendency). Similarly, the compelling
portrayal of Alaska Governor Sarah Palin by Tina Fey on Saturday Night
Live in 2008 caused many to actually believe the Governor said some of
what was actually said by Fey during the show’s comedy bits. The “Fey
effect” may have caused people to lower their opinions of Sarah Palin’s
credibility in order to share in enjoyment of the compelling Tina Fey
characterizations of her (Esralew & Young, 2012). The attention drawn to
such satire by the entertaining humor can thus actually affect people’s
views of the politician mocked through agenda-setting and priming of
certain perspectives through a popular humor-filled approach. Yet the
uses and gratifications approach to media use reinforces the typical
strengthening of one’s political views based on the humor one seeks out
and enjoys. For instance, those who dislike Sarah Palin likely find much
more humor in the satire, while others may be entertained by her mimic-
ry while even being reinforced in their liking for and support of her.
Currently, politicians are subject to a continual bombardment of
satire. Popular late-night and round-the-clock comedy shows parade
skits and bits that mock politicians, and these are now posted online for
multiple repeated viewings. One study taking stock of the most popular
hosted late-night talk shows found that over the course of the recent 20
years, one out of seven jokes were directed at current presidents of the
United States (Lichter, Baumgartner, & Morris, 2015). That excluded
jokes about former presidents, candidates for president, and other politi-
cians. Creating satirical mocking of current politicians has become a con-
tinually productive industry. People clearly find this entertaining and
enjoy it, as such humor has become pervasive in United States culture.
But its persuasive effects are less clear. Although Lichter et al. found a
stronger tendency to joke at the expense of Republicans than Democrats,
no case can be made that all of this satire is having major persuasive
effects—especially since all politicians are fair game. Also, the subject of
the most jokes during the studied period was a Democrat, Bill Clinton,
even for a time after his presidency had ended. Constant ridicule of all
politicians may indeed have persuaded people that they are less than
credible as a group. The reduced regard in which most hold all presi-
Humor and Persuasion 67
ery, those they oppose. Even a hint of disagreement with a power struc-
ture or authority expressed through humor may arouse interest in an
audience through potential conflict. Effective politicians can use ridicule
as a powerful rhetorical weapon to point out the faulty policies and ig-
norance of opponents. Similarly, people regularly joke about politicians,
policies, or positions with which they disagree. Humor may be viewed as
subversive or as a form of rebellion, due to its implicit disagreement with
prevailing arguments or norms and the clashing perceptions or violations
of expectations necessary for humor appreciation (Lynch, 2007). Persuad-
ers at any level of society can give vent to criticism by using humor.
Consider a more recent example of a pollster questioning a constituent:
“What should we do with people who want to get government handouts
and are too lazy to work?” The answer came back: “Kick them out of
congress!” The twist, or incongruity in response, can suggest alternative
perspectives and perhaps reinforce one’s argument. Appreciating the hu-
mor in arguments that are thereby made more compelling may open the
audience to entertain alternative arguments through enforcement or dif-
ferentiation functions.
Overuse of humor, however, damages credibility (Gruner, 1985). Poli-
ticians and campaigners are liable to suffer from this if they try too hard
to engage audiences through humor. An additional difficulty is encoun-
tered when a politician or persuader makes light of a person or issue
using satire. A common danger of using satire becomes clear when it
primarily reinforces the views of those who already agree rather than
changing anyone’s mind (Gruner, 1965; Bloom & Bloom, 1979). Consider
one definition of diplomacy: the art of saying “nice doggie” until one can
find a stick. Those already skeptical about the effectiveness of diplomacy
may perceive the humor, while those highly ego-involved or serious
about the profession of diplomacy could find the statement inappropriate
or out of place. People may similarly choose to be entertained by satire,
or take it literally thus needing to be forthrightly told the persuasive
claim advocated, which spoils the humor effect of satire. People who find
satire funny find it hugely persuasive, mostly because they already agree
with the premise of the satire. Those who do not may take the satire
literally, not seeing its humor, or become generally outraged and angry
rather than being persuaded. Satire seems to be a more effective reinforc-
ing mechanism for persuaders rather than a tool to change minds.
The limits of humor as a persuasive tool were demonstrated by the
Center for Disease Control and Prevention which promoted a “zombie
apocalypse” preparedness campaign, intending through humor to pro-
mote actual preparedness efforts by individuals and households. On the
one hand, the campaign generated a lot of attention through social media
hits, even causing the site’s server to crash initially. Social media posting
and spreading netted thousands of views of the campaign, but a study
showed that in spite of the publicity, those reached by the zombie mes-
Humor and Persuasion 69
sages were no more likely than anyone to actually take the recommended
preparedness actions (Fraustino & Ma, 2015). Including humor to address
a serious topic was fantastically successful at garnering attention, but
hardly successful at all in terms of changing to recommended behaviors.
Even as humor may capture attention and provide a distraction from the
persuasive message, it may also be treated as entertainment and not
worthy of considering for telic, or serious, consequences (Moyer-Guse,
Mahood, & Brookes, 2011). So viewers were entertained by the idea of
preparing for a zombie apocalypse, but did not proceed to actually pre-
pare their households for a disaster, as advocated by the campaign. Simi-
lar reactions have been found to health campaigns that have added hu-
mor providing entertainment value but not necessarily affecting advocat-
ed behaviors.
Overall, humor seems to have greater effect as an attention-getter,
credibility booster, and memory spark than as a persuasive appeal itself.
A dash of humor can stimulate interest and potentially open an audience
to further persuasion, but within limits. College students, for instance,
gravitated toward texts with humor when they were exploring relatively
simple concepts, but found lower credibility when humor use tried to
supplement difficult material (Bryant, Brown, Silverberg, & Elliott, 1981).
The positive identification and clarification functions of humor serve per-
suaders well to set the scene for persuasion, but the enforcement or diffe-
rentiation functions must be used with caution. Prosocial behaviors in-
volving humor increase compliance-gaining in the college classroom
(Punyanunt, 2000). Negative humor that puts down an audience or its
members serves, not surprisingly, to decrease the credibility or persua-
siveness of the message (Frymier, Wanzer, & Wojtaszczyk, 2008). Divi-
sive humor may compellingly put down an opponent or opposing idea,
but may pass unappreciated by those who disagree with the premises on
which the humor is based. Adding humor to a speech can only enhance
interest or credibility so much, and self-deprecatory humor can enhance
undamaged credibility (Chang & Gruner, 1981). Too much use of humor,
though, seems to backfire in terms of credibility and “overselling” inter-
est in a topic. Humor is best used as an ingredient to set the stage for
persuasion, rather than counted upon as an essential persuasive tool.
FIVE
Humor in Organizations and Cultures
Organizations are social groups united for a common purpose, and may
be studied from the level of an immediate family all the way to that of a
geographical region of the world. Each organization can be said to be
constituted as a unique culture, possessing its own norms, roles, and
expectations. A culture is a web of meaning created by its members
through communication (Frost, Moore, Louis, Lundberg, & Martin, 1991;
Mohan, 1993; Pacanowsky & O’Donnell-Trujillo, 1982; Sackmann, 1990).
Culture can be observed and studied as a set of patterns of communica-
tion behavior. When these patterns are violated by choice-making hu-
mans, or inherent contradictions arise in the pattern, the strength and
desirability of the patterns becomes an issue. Whether responded to as
tragic violations or comic incongruities, the social structure may be called
into question through communication. Questions arise as to how valued
the patterns of interaction really are, and whether or not they are worth
keeping. Values are at the heart of a culture and must be unearthed and
studied to understand an organization’s culture (Meyer, 1995; Schein,
1985). These patterns exist at an abstract level and often go unnoticed.
Understanding organizational culture can be practically difficult, since
we are relatively unaware of or take for granted cultural norms and
beliefs until we note their violation. Often, such violations are the focus of
humorous messages. Through such violations of communication norms
or routines we often inductively or intuitively learn about a culture.
Humor depends on and traffics in violations of norms, or unusual
events. Throughout history, “tricksters’ tales” have put norms and expec-
tations into practical effect by describing their violation in memorable,
humorous ways (Apte, 1985). One key aspect of humor is its origin in
perceived incongruity. A norm must be expected, and a violation of that
norm must also be perceived (Meyer, 2000; Veatch, 1998). Seeing the
71
72 Chapter 5
shown to exclude those higher in the hierarchy as not in the know and
worthy of being mocked (Korczynski, 2011; Lynch, 2007). Yet, humor use
reinforces the social structure by making light of needed changes or serv-
ing as “non-serious” (as in not actionable or true) complaints (Graham,
Papa, & Brooks). One way of making sense of events for 911 call takers
involves joking about events in ways those not part of the organization
might find inappropriate. Once, when a call about a cat that a dog had
chased into a tree was followed not long after by a report that the cat had
walked onto an electric line and been electrocuted, jokes about “fried
feline” followed (Tracy, Myers, & Scott, 2006, p. 298). Such references
would never make it into official reports, of course, and supervisors or
bosses may not be included in them, but they were regular ways for
organization members to make sense of their work life.
Workplace humor serves as one key element in group cohesiveness
(Holmes & Marra, 2002). Sharing humor involves a group understanding
of set up patterns of expectation along with similar perceptions of their
violation. Such experiences together and sharing a particular value set or
attitude about those patterns leads to group consubstantiality (Burke,
1984). Shared humorous experiences then come to characterize the group
culture. Each may be only one event, but their retelling makes them
fraught with symbolic meanings of what constitutes the identity of cul-
ture members. Events as simple—or alarming—as a dumpster fire or
electrocuted cat become markers for how organizational members should
react, whether protecting one another, ensuring safety, pursuing quality,
or putting some objective distance between workers and traumatic events
so that all can do their jobs. Being part of or understanding such humor-
ous events becomes one requirement for assuming organizational mem-
bership as part of one’s identity.
Organizational cultures not only find humor as key to group unity,
but also to maintain hierarchy and cultural norms. Ben-Ari and Sion
(2005) emphasized that humor “should be analyzed not only as a means
of maintaining social order, but also as mechanisms by which patterns of
domination are sustained in everyday life” (p. 667). In joking about norm
or pattern violations, the existing hierarchy and power structure is re-
ified. Laughing at those who violate norms motivates all to conform to
them. Those in power also have more leeway to tell jokes (Martin, Rich, &
Gayle, 2004), and those joked about may certainly take note. People then
learn what behaviors are held up to ridicule, and what behaviors mem-
bers presume to be acceptable. As hierarchies become solidified, humor
use becomes more publicly acceptable for those at elevated levels. Organ-
ization members of higher status have been found to joke around more,
often at the expense of those with lower status (Duncan, 1982). Feeling
more “in control” in an organization enhances the use of humor. Consid-
er the manager who noted, “teamwork is a lot of people doing what I
say.” Those in charge may thus through humor define their power and
74 Chapter 5
lowed up with “it’s just a joke” or “I only meant it in fun.” Yet the
differentiation was still accomplished, defusing resistance by putting dis-
senters in the positon of rejecting the humor and being “no fun.” Humor
also allows a speaker to mock the other group’s beliefs and individuals
while reproducing stereotypes of that group (Barnes, Palmary, & Dur-
rheim, 2001). Through such mockery, those in the group who share such
perceptions or beliefs are united in laughter. Terrion and Ashforth (2002)
found that put-down humor influences group cohesiveness, enforces
group norms, and increases identification levels. However, Terrion and
Ashforth specified that the interpretation of put-down humor may vary
from person to person. Not all who seem to enjoy it actually do so. When
a put-down is made, many laugh along with those who genuinely find
humor in the message, for the sake of social cohesion. Thus the divisive
effects of humor on the group remain influential despite those who may
not share fully in the appreciation of put-down humor.
As a key aspect of social interaction, humor has been found to crucial-
ly inhere in several organizational or cultural functions. Humor use rein-
forces essential or taken-for granted values while also helping in manag-
ing clashing values. Humor disciplines organizational members per-
ceived as violating norms, and reinforces group cohesiveness by
strengthening the acceptance of norms and values. Humor helps to ease
tension and adapt to sudden or ongoing changes while also enhancing
member identification with the organization or culture. Humor use helps
to educate new members, while also establishing and reinforcing rela-
tionships. Managers and leaders enhance their effectiveness by invoking
humor, yet members can resist supervisor power and directives through
humor. Negatively, humor can divide sub-groups one from another and
oppress groups through expectations of enjoying humor that puts others
down. With humor emerging as such a central and crucial organizational
tool, it not surprisingly has been researched extensively in organizational
settings. This chapter has attempted to summarize major findings in
terms of humor found in organizations on a group and cultural level, but
questions persist about how unique individuals may respond to all of
these humor attempts by one other person. It is to those questions of
humor in relationships that we now turn
SIX
Humor in Personal Relationships
Relationships get their start, in part, through some sort of humor shared
with another person. When will the other person smile, or laugh? This
becomes a key question after meeting someone. A gauge of initial friend-
liness may depend in part on a mutual humor experience. An assessment
of the potential of a future relationship may also depend on humor. Hu-
mor is thus held by many to be central to the human interaction experi-
ence, and as relationships develop humorous events come to characterize
and represent one unique relationship. Two persons in a new relation-
ship begin learning about one another and each other’s perceptions of life
and its expected social patterns. Multiple potential sources of humor be-
come manifest. One is a sense of shared scripts for understanding a cultu-
ral violation together; another is mutual recognition of an awkward or
strange situation—any “departure” from a culturally or normatively ex-
pected script. Humor is regularly inserted into communication to test the
views of others along with the potential of a developing relationship
(Davis & Farina, 1970). Uncertainty is reduced, one way or another, by
the responses to the humor attempts or perceptions, and sharing in hu-
mor builds a unity that strengthens a relationship.
Solid, logical reasons exist for the cliché that people look for a sense of
humor in others before forming a relationship. As two parties grow clos-
er, uncertainty reduction is expected to occur through the couples’ com-
munication (Berger & Calabrese, 1975). Humor use serves to reveal
shared understanding of life patterns. Reactions to divergences from
those patterns teach partners about one another and shared humorous
responses strengthen the bonds formed through communication. Seeking
humor experiences can help to overcome even great social differences, as
higher levels of humor orientation correlate with less apprehension for
communicating with someone from another culture (Miczo & Welter,
81
82 Chapter 6
2006). Shared humor responses help to bridge the gaps between our solo
human uniqueness. The effectiveness of humor use to build relationships
is widely known, as those individuals with a clear sense of humor are
more valued as potential relationship partners (Graham, 1995). The re-
duction of social distance readily accomplished through humor experi-
ences makes a growing relationship more probable.
Even as a couple, whether romantic or friendly, grows closer through
humor, there may be limits to the speed of closer growth. People seek to
learn about the other through communication, and much communication
can be attributed to this motive (Berger & Calabrese, 1975). Humor is a
powerful tool for such learning. Yet, seldom do we seek to learn every-
thing about another person through a few interactions. Humans appre-
ciate the communication process as well as gaining knowledge about
another. There is a sense of fun that is maintained through exploring the
perspectives of each party in the relationship. Thus humor use may be
part of the “game” of relationship development, and more than simply a
practical tool for learning about another. Humor reduces the social dis-
tance between people not only by reducing uncertainty about others, but
simply through making them feel good through laughter (Ziv, 1984). A
sense of mystery infuses mutual experiences of humor with adventure
and fun. Resistance exists toward the idea of knowing everything about
the other person in a relationship. Even those together for years can en-
joy—and find funny—instances of surprise and continued learning about
one another. Not only do life situations provide new revelations, individ-
ual reactions can also surprise even after two people have learned a lot
about each other.
Following up on research that shows a higher degree of humor appre-
ciation relates to communication successes, logically a more developed
sense of humor fits with more success in relationships. People studied in
initial interactions showed that those who initiated more humor use were
highly desired for potential future relationships (Graham, 1995). Even if
one is not naturally a comedian, attempts at being funny will likely be
appreciated for promoting ingratiation. The fun inherent in humor and
the friendliness it enhances work together to build relationships. Studies
of couples show that those who respond positively to sharing the other’s
humor in their communication report higher levels of relationship satis-
faction (Honeycutt & Brown, 1998). One may initiate humor yet also
alternatively appreciate humor from the other. A higher ability to initiate
humor relates to handling stress better as well as effective expression of
emotions to the other (Wanzer, Booth-Butterfield, & Booth-Butterfield,
2005). Instead of letting a tragic mode of thinking dominate, raising stress
levels and potential negative emotions, a comic mode can help channel
stress through mocking a situation or help all realize that things may not
be so bad after all and situations can be handled together. Humor clearly
emerges as a key ingredient for healthy relationships.
Humor in Personal Relationships 83
sees me in flannel pajamas, he response is, “Oh no! You’ve got your
armor on tonight!” (Meyer, 2012, p. 172).
Issues including taking note of activities during daily life all the way to
potentially controversial topics in a relationship like sexuality were han-
dled by couples in a mutually inclusive way through humor.
ing and the other facing the costs. A comic mode can encourage brain-
storming and creative problem solving in the face of conflict.
Potential threats to relationships can, at times, be mitigated through
humor use. People who can retain that sense of fun and creativity at least
in part through humor will tend to be more open to options for overcom-
ing obstacles and forgiving mistakes. In relationships, the ability to cope
with relational transgressions is increased by one’s level of humor appre-
ciation (Labelle, Booth-Butterfield, & Weber, 2013). The playful, open,
creative communication made possible through humor use can defuse
serious conflict and can open parties up to communication leading to
conflict management or resolution. Humor is used by couples and friends
to show affection, enjoy events together, and avoid or let go of conflicts,
all of which enhance relationship satisfaction (Hall, 2013). One party may
refuse to see the humor in a situation or comments about it, and this will
prevent humor’s salutary effects. As explored in the next chapter, how
one party reacts to the other’s attempts at humor may signal the positive
or negative future of the relationship.
Humor’s reinforcing of similarities between people can both over-
come conflict and reduce uncertainty, thereby strengthening relation-
ships. The shared appreciation of social patterns indicated—and perhaps
violated—by humor use reinforces the strength of personal bonds and
the ability to relax and play with the other. These effects can be seen in
research showing that those of similar races and classes employ humor
with one another more (Smith, Harrington, & Neck, 2000). The familiarity
of those who can appreciate humor similarly enhances cultural scripts or
norms held in common—along with violations of them that can be
laughed at together and then built upon to improve relationships. Humor
can help parties in relationships remain together and cope through stress-
ful times by enhancing a sense of shared control and reinforcement of
shared values—a strength that enhances couples’ ability to overcome dif-
ficult situations (Horan, Bochantin, & Booth-Butterfield, 2012). Indeed, in
relationships, a sense of humor can enhance the flexibility and creativity
of both parties as they experience life events together. Humor’s capacity
to illuminate violations and contrasts between expectations and alternate
patterns allows for relational “play” and mutual growth.
Hierarchical Relationships
Humor can also overcome personal differences that initially may seem
rather daunting. A serious social distance can lead to communication
being overly formal or scripted, as when one talks to someone with a
much higher degree of perceived power. A formal organizational rela-
tionship or intimidating power difference can be overcome by introduc-
tion of humor. One relationship fraught with contrasts that need bridg-
ing—mainly knowledge and power differences—is that formed between
Humor in Personal Relationships 91
We look for a sense of humor when we meet another person, but what
about humor at one’s own or others’ expense? We learn about relation-
95
96 Chapter 7
ships through humor, but we may also learn about disagreements, and
varied perspectives, by seeing what that other person finds to be funny.
The sense humor gives of “being on the same page” lets couples reinforce
the relationship and reassure one another that future events can be man-
aged comfortably. Such a reduction in uncertainty fits theoretical expec-
tations for a positively developing relationship (Berger & Calabrese,
1975). Yet, looking for similarities in a relationship, we may uncover
some dramatic differences, some even calling the relationship itself into
question. What if the other person jokes about a group that is considered
important? What if a topic of high ego involvement is seen to be taken
lightly or even an opposite way as revealed through humor use? For
instance, one of the most moving and inspiring movies one has ever seen
is ridiculed as idiotic or foolish. This can put a damper on a relationship
and even on further communication. Although communication with hu-
mor may fulfill a basic need for uncertainty reduction (Berger & Bradac,
1982), it may bring into certainty dramatic differences, outrages, and in-
sults.
One instance of revealing differences was enacted when a manager of
a local sales force reported that an employee’s company credit card was
stolen, but the manager chose not to pursue the crime because there were
fewer charges on the credit card after it was stolen than before. This twist,
to a listener, could be humorous, yet at the same time slams the employee
as a big spender. When an employee who thinks highly of that person
hears that line, even if seen as funny, it reveals a fairly low opinion of that
worker. Such information, perhaps not known before, becomes known
through humor. Yet difficulties in the work relationship may well follow.
Humor reduces uncertainty in promotion of unity, for sure, but it also
does so as an instigator of conflict.
Relational Partners Must Choose to Share Humor for the Relationship to Grow
When humor functions positively in a relationship, the flexibility and
openness involved in taking humorous approaches to events reinforces
the parties’ commitments to the other and to the relationship. Humor
could be used to avoid painful confrontations with the other, but more
often humor serves to approach or broach a topic that, taken in all seri-
ousness, could be threatening to one or the other party’s identity or to the
relationship as a whole (Meyer, 2012). Yet the context for the humor and
preceding events in relationships affect the choice each party makes to
acknowledge humor or not. Humorous incidents are judged based upon
both affective and cognitive impressions, so that both experience and
situation are involved in the presence of humor (Leventhal & Cupchik,
1976). Humor meant by its producer to unify, for instance, might be
considered the height of differentiation to an offended party. Also, a basic
choice is made by both senders and receivers of humor whether to expe-
rience humor or not (Attardo & Raskin, 1993). Humorous messages, tak-
en in a “bona-fide” spirit, may communicate basic information only or
simply nonsense without any perception of humor by another party (Ra-
skin, 1985). The “non-bona-fide” playful aspect of humor is key to experi-
encing the mental duality that allows its experience. Depending on the
context of the situation, though, one may choose to “play” or not.
Negative uses of humor and dysfunctional relationships are con-
nected (Alberts, 1990). With humor, a partner’s response to the humor
attempt is as important as the attempt itself. Well-adjusted couples accept
the other’s attempts at humor and respond positively (Alberts). Humor
use by highly humor-oriented individuals involves more flexibility and
attractiveness by them, but those individuals also high in verbal aggres-
Dangers of Humor for Relationships 99
siveness would aim that humor at someone else through ridicule or sar-
casm more (Wanzer, Booth-Butterfield, & Booth-Butterfield, 1996). One
woman described a potentially “violent” event for a couple that both
parties accepted as humorous, yet one could well imagine if one or both
of the parties had taken the event seriously, conflict could have sparked
and grown:
We had just emptied some boxes from my mom’s house that contained
some of my childhood keepsakes. Among the items was a box of my
old Barbie dolls. Lilly [the three-year-old daughter] promptly tore into
the box, helping herself to the dolls. I watched as she dressed one of the
dolls in a wedding dress. She was playing with the bride and a Ken
doll in a tuxedo. I told Parker to watch her because it was so cute. But
as soon as Parker turned around to look at her, she ripped Ken’s head
off. Parker and I started to laugh hard and Lilly just looked up and
gave us a huge grin. In her childish Lilly-speak she said, “Do you think
I broke it?”
This instance of humor helped our relationship because we were
both stressed from visiting my mother. (A visit to her place is always
stressful.) The innocence in Lilly’s actions combined with the “vio-
lence” of ripping the doll’s head off gave us an opportunity to laugh as
a couple and as a family, even though Lilly probably didn’t really
know why she was laughing.
In this instance, the couple both found the event funny. If one or the other
became outraged enough, though, and chose the tragic or serious per-
spective, it could lead to escalating conflict even as the other party may
attempt to joke about the situation. Refusal to accept the humor could
maintain a serious communication concerned with consequences, but
could also raise tension and exacerbate conflict.
The choice to appreciate humor has major relational implications. Be-
ing in “play” mode, ready to accept humor as such, portends a growing,
positive, relationship. Extensive research has shown that people who
show a higher degree of humor appreciation or sense of humor are more
successful in their relationships. In interpersonal relationships, humor
has been shown to enhance intimacy (Hampes, 1992; 1994), empathy
(Hampes, 2001), assertiveness (Bell, McGhee, & Duffey, 1986) and trust
(Hampes, 1999). Studies pairing communicators measured to have a
highly developed sense of humor with those demonstrating a less devel-
oped sense of humor show the power of a sense of humor within inter-
personal encounters—those invoking humor are much more desired as
potential future partners (Graham, 1995). Also, couples that shared and
accepted one another’s humor showed more satisfaction in their marriage
(Honeycutt & Brown, 1998).
There is far less research, however, on those couples that decline to
accept humor in statements by the partner. Often, it may be due to the
fact that they do not remain couples for long (Alberts, 1990). The decision
100 Chapter 7
and jokes about the partner in a negative way. Teases about a person’s
identity and appearance have been identified as most frequent, but the
topic of a tease does not primarily determine the response--background
knowledge of the relationship is the major factor in how one chooses to
take a tease, along with the context of the tease, nonverbal messages, and
one’s own mood (Alberts, Kellar-Gunther, & Corman, 1996). Teasing that
becomes “contaminated” by aggression can become simply another form
of attack. Satisfied couples in conflict accept humor attempts by the part-
ner, lightening the discussion and mood. Dissatisfied couples, on the
other hand, treat humor attempts as one further attack (Alberts, 1990).
mor can dramatize one’s conflicts with others and clarify perceived social
boundaries, as well as reinforce bonds and shared meaning with empath-
ic recipients. One couple used humor to characterize their situation as
laughable rather than truly tragic and worth fighting over:
I was watching an episode of The Middle where the characters discuss
celebrating their 16th and 17th anniversaries together because they had
postponed the 16th for so long, they had reached the 17th without
celebrating. I turned to my husband and explained the set-up. “That
definitely sounds like something we would do,” he said.
“So our life is a sitcom?” I questioned. “I don’t know if that is funny
or depressing.”
“Our life is full of sad humor,” he replied.
This instance of humor was a little on the sad side. It wasn’t a
“funny haha” moment. But at the place in life much of our life is shad-
owed by stress and strain. The funny part of this instance was that
television was so reminiscent of our real lives.
By putting their relationship in context of society, and seeing it reflected
on a television show and laughing at it, the couple could see how they
can deal with such challenges. An alternative, of course, would be offense
at the depiction followed by a resolve never to allow such a situation to
happen again—or worse, anger at the partner for the situation transpir-
ing. For some endangered couples, even an attempt at a humorous depic-
tion could be viewed as offensive and unacceptable—worth fighting
over.
Shared humor has been found to back up strengthening relationships,
and long term relationships almost always have elements of shared hu-
mor that are key parts of them. “A sense of humor” is a key characteristic
of a desired friend or romantic partner. Yet, when humor is not shared,
when most attempts at teases or jokes are taken in a telic or consequential
manner, the relationship is likely in trouble. Deterioration of some sort
will likely follow. Consider, for instance, my own choice necessary in the
past year when a seven-year-old boy I had gotten to know over several
years approached me during playtime at his after-school program and
said, “I’m your chicken finger!” I tended to find this funny, as it refer-
enced a repeated game that he had initiated earlier, where I chased him
because I was hungry for chicken fingers. So I laughed, and responded
with a chase, and our relationship was maintained and arguably
strengthened. I could, however, have taken it seriously and stayed in a
telic or tragic mode—saying something like, “No, you’re not a chicken
finger,” with no smile, no indication of humor in it. In remaining in a telic
or serious mode, I would cause a more distant or formal relationship. At
the very least, my seven-year-old friend would likely question the close-
ness of our friendship.
One can even chart social groupings by who shares and understands
instances of humor in common. We can also laugh at the odd or ignorant
104 Chapter 7
behaviors of different social groups, that do not “get” the inside appre-
ciated jokes or language or understand the commonly accepted norms
that may be violated or “played with.” Humor appreciation thus rein-
forces and delineates human social groupings. Back to the chicken finger
example, sometimes other children at the after school program would say
the same thing and join in the game. It violates multiple social scripts for
children to be saying, to an adult “homework helper” volunteer, “I’m a
chicken finger!” Yet, as other children said it, too, they joined in the
game, however funny it was. Other children who did not “get” the
phrase or tried to figure it out seriously, either were not interested in the
game or were not comfortable joining it. So, even if just for a short time
during a chase game, appreciating the humor of “I’m a chicken finger”
sorted us into groups of those playing the game, who understood the
rules, and those who did not. More permanent social groups are even
more defined by the topics and types of humor mutually appreciated.
Children’s laughter provides early opportunities for understanding
humor use. Some of their laughter seems to stem from the joy of play, or
even the delight of existence. Other laughter comes from a more concrete
communicated situation; a funny word, story, or joke, for instance. Chil-
dren do know that making others laugh can encourage liking by the other
person, and can help avoid conflict later (Bigelow, Tesson, & Lewko,
1996). They also learn even in preschool the difference between laughing
with someone and laughing at someone. Even in early childhood, the
humor mode becomes a combination of aesthetic, enjoyable moments
and practical, useful communication purposes. There is a genuineness to
children’s emotions and statements that can be alternatively refreshing
and harsh, and this includes humor. The delight and joy in children’s
humor can seem innocent and contagious, while some apparent laughing
at others’ expense can contribute to children’s reputation for cruelty. Still,
young children’s propensity for humor shows how essential—and de-
sired—the comic perspective is for the human life.
One can see how humor, so pervasively embedded in human commu-
nication, can have ill effects as well as the often lauded benefits. Through
uncertainty reduction, it can highlight dramatic differences between peo-
ple and groups. Control of others can be asserted through humor’s para-
doxical differentiating and uniting characteristics. People can refuse to
perceive remarks as humorous, making the choice against “play” or unity
and responding with seriousness or even aggression to perceived divi-
sion. Humor can simply be one more weapon in asserting divisions and
differences in the conflicts that people experience. Finally, humor can
serve to illustrate the separations and boundaries that characterize social
life. Instances of humor can potentially be dark, negative experiences
evoking serious responses. In spite of these drawbacks, however, humor
for most turns out to be a highly positive aspect of life and communica-
tion.
EIGHT
A Social Model of Humor
After detailing the individual model of the choice for or against humor,
and then exploring the implications of humor in the vast array of human
communication situations, one cannot escape the idea that the humor
experience is clearly social. The paradox between the individual appreci-
ation of humor and the social nature of it is difficult to characterize.
Humor is individual, yet humor is social. What about a situation, for
instance, makes things funnier or elicits more laughter when one is with a
group than when alone—even about the same initial message or event?
Humor appreciation between individuals suggests shared meaning on
multiple levels, showing its capacity as a social medium (Leventhal &
Cupchik, 1976). Martineau (1972) described how humor could unite
groups together and divide members from others. His sociological model
reinforces discussions in earlier chapters about how humor use within
and between group members could fortify group unity, or cause conflict
and divide or reorganize relationships between members and groups.
Ambiguity seems to be one key to humor, as people can see humor from
a variety of differing pattern violations in the same event. Even in one’s
own mind, multiple perceptions of an event that violates an expected or
moral order seem necessary to experience humor. This effect is likely
multiplied among individuals sharing in laughter. Even if two people
share humor over an event or message, no one can be sure they are
experiencing humor for exactly the same reasons.
Humor as a social marker, however, invites a paratelic perspective on
reality—indicating that things are not so serious, consequences do not
matter, for now, and thus creativity and playfulness are encouraged. Any
sense of threat is mitigated by sharing in humor with others. Even if one
does not experience humor for the same reasons or based on exactly the
same choices based on the same perceptions as others, sharing enough to
105
106 Chapter 8
ideal or desired state. Indeed, since we never fully reach the ideal in our
imperfect reality, humor and the comic mode may be the only way to
escape the rigid perspective of the tragic mode during our lives.
Clearly, humor is one of the essential tools for social cohesion. The
comic mode is valued so highly that all look for people who will share it.
Thus is humor such a strong factor in relationship formation and growth.
Seeing another respond with humor, where we choose to see it as well,
reinforces our own beliefs and thought processes. Such personal rein-
forcement from another enhances the desire to be with and communicate
with that person. Humor’s relationship-enhancing capability served as a
key spark to the author’s own study of humor in interpersonal situations.
The power of a smile, and the even greater exhilaration of laughing to-
gether with someone, seemed magical for powering relationship growth.
In all of the many venues of humor research, its capacity to enhance
relationships through unity gives humor an essential place throughout.
The source of this power was worth exploring. Behind my interest in
humor, there always has lurked the question of “why?” Why pursue
research on humor at all? Part of its reward as a research topic—studying
humor in a variety of settings including organizations, persuasive cam-
paigns, and personal relationships—has been shedding light on humor’s
strengthening of the bonds between people. In spite of potential differen-
tiation and divisiveness from humor that inheres in any human interac-
tion, the process through invoking humor of reducing uncertainty, ex-
ploring personal relationships, and “playing together” turns out to be a
major mover of human relationships and understanding one another.
It turns out, upon reflection, that humor helps in many ways to turn
“I-it” relationships into “I-Thou” relationships, to invoke Martin Buber
(1958). Instead of interacting with others as if they were other movable
benefits or impediments to our existence, we get to know them as people
related in their conscious existence to ourselves. Choosing to experience
humor and share it together is one essential aspect of positive communi-
cation, and therefore a positive life. Humor should be added to any list of
positive communication behaviors, as sharing humor indicates strong
listening to others, disclosing aspects of the self, and inspiring others to
creatively interact or work together (Mirivel, 2014). Humor use is part of
what binds humans together into groups and societies. It also links us as
existentially lonely individuals to those human groups and societies. The
essential individual humanness of someone can often be exposed or
shared, however briefly, through humor. Its treatment as essential to
relationships therefore makes sense. Its place as an essential social cata-
lyst is also clear. One foundation for individual learning, dyadic relation-
ships, and social grouping, it turns out, is the choice to experience and
use humor in communication.
112 Chapter 8
necessary to promote learning, and get involved with the subject matter
and relating it to students, I inevitably leave with a better attitude. There
are other alternative perspectives on the world and events than that frus-
tration, tiredness, or irritation which was dragged to class. Being willing
to laugh, at myself or events, provides a sense of control or perspective
that overpowers the negativity that comes with the sense of being
trapped by fate that fits the tragic mode. The choice remains for every-
one, even instant by instant, whether to view events comically or tragical-
ly, and that choice can dramatically affect attitude and individual morale.
The social nature of humor can be individually tested by taking note
of times where one finds something so much funnier with other people
responding similarly to it than when one is alone. Even if humor is expe-
rienced by oneself, it also takes on a more social character when experi-
enced in a relationship or group. Laughter becomes louder or more like-
ly, smiles grow, and are shared through greater eye contact. Here is why
people enjoy watching comedies or going to comedy shows together.
People also remember such experiences—they talk about them later as
touchstones of group or relationship unity. An essential, unique, individ-
ual humor experience becomes shared and symbolic of a social group.
Conversely, people may feel uncomfortable expressing a genuine humor
response to something when it appears that no one else perceived it that
way. A sincere choice for humor may not be validated by others present,
snuffing out the social spread of humor. Most probably feel, though, that
if one could only find others who “get it,” who think similarly, they too
would share in the humor.
Humor’s emergence in early childhood and development throughout
one’s growth shows its inherency in human life. This crucial place of
humor can be taken to stem from contradiction or discrepancy. Human
ability to recognize patterns leads to a similar recognition of divergences
from or violations of them. One key aspect of mentally processing contra-
diction or discrepancy involves humor. A mentally comic response to
violations opens one up to experiencing humor. As one end of an inher-
ent perception continuum, the comic is also made possible by the tragic.
If there were no tragedy, there would be no comedy, and vice versa.
Similar juxtapositions include order and chaos and the ongoing contrasts
between what we believe ought to be and what actually is (Burke, 1984;
Eckardt, 1992). We would not recognize chaos without order, and order
is made possible by its distinction from chaos. Encountering one or the
other in an unexpected way can be funny—viewed in the comic mode.
One can find meaning in the arbitrary events of life, and then through
their chosen meanings find them tragic or comic. Are humans spiritual
beings, that strive toward eternity, or are humans extra-perceptive ani-
mals, still bound by all the same physiological limitations? Both! One
may say. Yet then the clashes between these perspectives will very likely
present humorous contradictions. A tragic perspective finds fate in
A Social Model of Humor 115
charge, with humans subject to the whims of the universe, while a comic
perspective opens up freedom, creativity, a variety of responses to situa-
tions (Eckardt, 1992). We can mourn situations tragically, or accept
them—and perhaps manage them in some way—comically. A comic per-
spective approaches a “truth” as changeable, and depending upon per-
spective. One rational expectation must be contradicted by another or by
an observed reality. These clashing realities allow for creativity that a
tragic perspective does not—in the latter, what is, simply is. The vise or
trap that the universe can have in store for us can be very tragic indeed.
Yet, in the multiple acts of fate that lead any of us to a particular situa-
tion, it can be funny, too. Humor pervades life along with tragedy.
An example of one life can show the effects of this ongoing choice for
the comic or for the tragic. A dear friend suffered the loss of her father
one March. He was very elderly, but had not been sick and the death was
sudden. What made this sad situation worse was the death of her mother,
who had been in the hospital with pneumonia, a few days later. She was
beside herself with grief. One would expect this, but her incapacity to
cope was notable. It was as if her own life was ending, as well. Friends
and acquaintances jumped in to help, but were not only appreciated but
expected to do everything. This went on for weeks. What made this situa-
tion even more memorable was the friend’s extended reaction to this.
Even years later, the simplest things in life were found to be fraught with
an air of tragedy. The loss of her parents had reinforced or elicited a
tragic perspective on almost all aspects of life. One seldom saw the friend
laughing or smiling anymore; things were too tragic for that. Simple
things like shopping or attending events were found to be difficult, sad;
hard to bear. One recognized the symptoms of depression. Indeed, one
key element of depression is the lack of humor or a comic perspective in
most areas of life. Some sense of the comic seems necessary for our men-
tal health. Living life tragically brings one down physiologically and so-
cially. My friend lost many of her friends, as they felt strongly for her in
her loss, but as the months and years passed felt inadequate to share in or
mitigate the depth of her tragic perspective. This led to even more sense
of tragedy, of course, as loneliness set in. It was a growing spiral of tragic
perspective—one tragedy eliciting another, as each event was viewed in
only a tragic way.
This life perspective, of course, can be contrasted with others who
suffer similar losses and yet are able to laugh—at remembered happy
times with the deceased person, or at situations that the person, if still
alive, would have enjoyed and found humorous. Even while there are
tears, there can be laughter at times. The comic perspective can be taken.
A break from the sad, determined, fateful view of things from the tragic
perspective is possible. The sense of balance that each perspective pro-
vides for the other seems to lead to mental and even physical health.
Most of us can think of sad or angry times when, all of a sudden, we have
116 Chapter 8
“under the sun” imperfections are inevitable and always with us. Humor
is one key way we can deal with such ongoing imperfections. By laugh-
ing at them we can defer stress and worry about them, and have leave to
think of possible alternative ways of dealing with inherent imperfections.
An additional extreme may be imagined, also. What about someone
who will never get serious about anything? A person may seek to banish
tragedy to the point that nothing is sad; a humorous angle is always
found and communicated. One can take a cynical route and see the “fak-
ery” or illusion possible in human perception of anything. Similarly, any-
thing can be mocked. How far, indeed, can satire be pushed? If nothing is
serious, everything is humorous. The non-bona-fide nature of most hu-
morous communication suggests no need for concern for consequences.
One probably could not survive taking that perspective totally for long.
At some point, one must take actions that improve one’s chances for
survival, rather than simply laughing at a situation.
As shown by the personal humor model described earlier, that key
choice—for the comic mode or the tragic mode—can be made at every
moment of life. Moods and perspectives can change even over seconds.
Each situation can be viewed and responded to either way, which will
then influence the following perceptions—and communication about
them. There is some evidence that humans are genetically “pro-
grammed” to more often respond to either the comic or the tragic in
situations (Martin, 2007). Yet if the choice of one or the other predomi-
nates too much, mental dysfunction is likely to follow, whether chronic
depression or manic failure to take anything seriously. Clearly, one
should not always be choosing the same one—comic or tragic—but a
good mix seems to lead to a balanced life in many aspects both mental
and physiological.
The comic and the tragic can alternate with one another and even be
blended in many aspects of life. Consider one of the most famous works
of music, Mozart’s Requiem. Some parts are known as among the saddest
music ever written, while others soar with exuberance and praise and joy,
all within the space of a few minutes using the same voices and instru-
ments. Human lives are beset with opportunities to find tragedy and
comedy in evolving situations, and music like Mozart’s gives voice to
such feelings by eliciting both passionate sorrow and passionate joy. Mu-
sically, note combinations, tones, and volume contrast turn music from
sad to happy. At one moment emotions of despair predominate, and a
few moments later emotions of exhilaration and glee emerge. In life,
mental perspectives and even slight adjustments can change the tone of
our perceptions and reactions to situations. In any aspect of life, then, we
may find both tragedy and comedy.
How can something so contradictory and wide-ranging in its social
and communicative effects as humor be so valued yet be so taken-for-
granted as part of our lives and interactions? Its pervasiveness leads to its
A Social Model of Humor 119
potential—for both good and bad effects. Some shared “twist” of interac-
tion or a surprising event can be discussed, shared, and laughed at. A
memorable comment leads to a clearer expression of opinion on an issue.
A cutting remark can set the tone for a vigorous debate on an issue.
Teases can critique with a telic or practical point while attempting to
maintain a paratelic mode of having fun. Even differentiation through
put-downs can be fun for the people appreciating them, even if not for
the people who are the subject of them. The pleasure humor engenders,
along with its diverse and multiple communicative functions, makes it a
desired and flexible tool for communicators along with a welcome im-
provement to one’s life.
For this author, the effects of humor on personal closeness mentioned
earlier make it one of the most valuable tools for human interaction and
influence. A connection forms between the self and those who express or
initiate humor that is appreciated. Whether mass mediated in a politi-
cian’s speech or actor’s portrayal, or enacted by someone addressing one
directly in life, humor appreciation leads to a response involving cogni-
tion as ideas and concepts connect mentally. Social issues can be ad-
dressed and new alternatives tried. At the same time, relationships are
dramatically affected, as sharing humor leads to growing closer in a more
secure or more clearly defined relationship. Interpersonal relationships
thrive on mutual humor appreciation to the point that it is treated almost
as a necessary ingredient for them. In leading toward “I-Thou” relation-
ships and enhancing their possibility and growth, humor provides a com-
prehensive tool ready to change perspectives and grow relationships. For
its huge potential to do so, and its wide availability as a communicative
choice, I am eternally grateful.
Bibliography
121
122 Bibliography
Berger, C. R., & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and
beyond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human
Communication Research, 1, 99–112.
Bigelow, B. J., Tesson, G., & Lewko, J. H. (1996). Learning the rules: The anatomy of
children’s relationships. New York: Guilford.
Bippus, A. M. (2000). Humor usage in comforting episodes: Factors predicting out-
comes. Western Journal of Communication, 64, 359–384.
———. (2003). Humor motives, qualities and reactions in recalled conflict episodes.
Western Journal of Communication, 67, 413–426.
Bloom, E. A., & Bloom, L. D. (1979). Satire’s persuasive voice. Ithaca, NY: Cornell.
Boland, R. J., & Hoffman, R. (1983). Humor in a machine shop: An interpretation of
symbolic action. In L. R. Pondy, P. J. Frost, G. Morgan, & T. C. Dandridge (Eds.),
Organizational symbolism (pp. 187–198). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Bolkan, S., & Goodboy, A. K. (2011). Behavioral indicators of transformational leader-
ship in the college classroom. Qualitative Research Reports in Communication, 12,
10–18.
Boller, P. F. Jr. (1982). Presidential anecdotes. New York: Penguin Books.
Booth-Butterfield, M., & Booth-Butterfield, S. (1991). Individual differences in the com-
munication of humorous messages. Southern Communication Journal, 56, 43–60.
Booth-Butterfield, M., Booth-Butterfield, S., & Wanzer, M. (2007). Funny students cope
better: Patterns of humor enactment and coping effectiveness. Communication Quar-
terly, 55, 299–315.
Booth-Butterfield, M.; Wanzer, M. B.; Weil, N., & Krezmien, E. (2014). Communication
of humor during bereavement: Intrapersonal and interpersonal emotion manage-
ment strategies. Communication Quarterly, 62, 436–454.
Bryant, J., Brown, D., Silverberg, A. R., & Elliott, S. M. (1981). Effects of humorous
illustrations in college textbooks. Human Communication Research, 8, 43–57.
Brooker, G. W. (1981). A comparison of the persuasive effects of mild humor and mild
fear appeals. Journal of Advertising, 10, 29–40.
Buber, M. (1958). I and thou, 2nd Ed. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.
Burke, K. (1984). Permanence and change. Los Angeles: University of California Press.
(Originally published in 1934)
Campbell, K. L., Martin, M. M., & Wanzer, M. B. (2001). Employee perceptions of
manager humor orientation, assertiveness, responsiveness, approach/avoidance
strategies, and satisfaction. Communication Research Reports, 18, 67–74.
Cann, A., Zapata, C. L., & Davis, H. B. (2009). Positive and negative styles of humor in
communication: Evidence for the importance of considering both styles. Communi-
cation Quarterly, 57, 452–468.
Cantor, J. R. (1976). What is funny to whom? The role of gender. Journal of Communica-
tion, 26, 164–172.
Carrell, A. (1992). The need to incorporate audience and situation into a theory of
humor. Paper presented at the Speech Communication Association annual confer-
ence, Chicago, IL, November.
Chafe, W. (1987). Humor as a disabling mechanism. American Behavioral Scientist 30,
16–26.
———. (2007). The importance of not being earnest: The feeling behind laughter and humor.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.
Chang, M. J., & Gruner, C. R. (1981). Audience reaction to self-disparaging humor.
Southern Speech Communication Journal, 46, 419–426.
Chapel, G. W. (1978). Humor in the White House: An interview with presidential
speechwriter Robert Orben. Communication Quarterly, 26, 44–49.
Chapman, A. J. (1975). Humorous laughter in children. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 31, 42–49.
Chapman, A. J., & Foot, H. C. (1996). Humor and laughter: Theory, research, and applica-
tions. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Chiaro, D. (1992). The language of jokes: Analysing verbal play. London: Routledge.
Bibliography 123
Horan, S. M., Bochantin, J., & Booth-Butterfield, M. (2012). Humor in high-stress rela-
tionships: Understanding communication in police officers’ romantic relationships.
Communication Studies, 63, 554–573.
Howland, M., & Simpson, J. (2014). Attachment orientations and reactivity to humor
in a social support context. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 31, 114–137.
Kangasharju, H., & Nikko, T. (2009). Emotions in organizations: Joint laughter in
workplace meetings. Journal of Business Communication, 46, 100–119.
Keltner, D. (2008). In defense of teasing. The New York Times, December 7.
Keyton, J., & Beck, S. J. (2010). Examining laughter functionality in jury deliberations.
Small Group Research, 41, 386–407.
Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. New York: Macmillan.
Korczynski, M. (2011). The dialectical sense of humour: Routine joking in a Taylorized
factory. Organization Studies, 32, 1421–1439.
La Fave, L., & Mannell, R. (1976). Laughing matter: Does ethnic humor serve preju-
dice? Journal of Communication, 26, 116–123.
LaBelle, S., Booth-Butterfield, M., & Weber, K. (2013). Humorous communication and
its effectiveness in coping with interpersonal transgressions. Communication Re-
search Reports, 30, 221–229.
Lefcourt, H. M. (2001). Humor: The psychology of living buoyantly. New York: Kluwer
Academic/Plenum.
Levasseur, D. G., & Dean, K. W. (1996). The Dole humor myth and the risks of recon-
textualizing rhetoric. Southern Communication Journal, 62, 56–72.
Leventhal, H., & Cupchik, G. C. (1976). A process model of humor judgment. Journal of
Communication, 26, 190–205.
Lichter, S. R., Baumgartner, J. C., & Morris, J. S. (2015). Politics is a joke! How TV
comedians are remaking political life. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Lorenz, K. (1963). On aggression. New York: Harcourt.
Lull, P. E. (1940). The effectiveness of humor in persuasive speech. Speech Monographs,
7, 26–40.
Lutgen-Sandvik, P., Riforgiate, S., & Fletcher, C. (2011). Work as a source of positive
emotional experiences and the discourses informing positive assessment. Western
Journal of Communication, 75, 2–27.
Lynch, O. H. (2002). Humorous communication: Finding a place for humor in commu-
nication research. Communication Theory, 12, 423–445.
———. (2007). Humorous organizing: Revealing the organization as a social process. Saar-
brucken, Germany: VDM Verlag Dr. Muller.
———. (2009). Kitchen antics: The importance of humor and maintaining profession-
alism at work. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 37, 444–464.
———. (2010). Cooking with humor: In-group humor as social organization. Humor:
An International Journal of Humor Research, 23 (2), 127–160.
Maase, S. W., Fink, E. K., & Kaplowitz, S. A. (1984). Incongruity in humor: The cogni-
tive dynamics. In Bostrom, R. N., & Westley, B. H. (Eds.), Communication Yearbook 8.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Malone, P. B. (1980). Humor: A double-edged tool for today’s managers. Academy of
Management Review, 5, 357–360.
Markiewicz, D. (1974). Effects of humor on persuasion. Sociometry, 37, 407–422.
Martin, D. M., Rich, C. O., & Gayle, B. M. (2004). Humor works: Communication style
and humor functions in manager/subordinate relationships. Southern Communica-
tion Journal, 69, 206–222.
Martin, R. A. (2007). The psychology of humor: An integrative approach. Burlington, MA:
Elsevier.
Martin, R. A., Puhlik-Doris, P., Larsen, G., Gray, J., & Weir, K. (2003). Individual
differences in the uses of humor and their relation to psychological well-being:
Development of the Humor Styles Questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality,
37, 48–75.
126 Bibliography
131
132 Index
moral order violations, 12, 41, 43, 105 moderation in, 18, 24; in
Mozart, Wolfgang, 118 organizations, 71; script awareness
Mr. Brown, 12–13 in, 84–85
music, comic and tragic modes of, 118 personal relationships: attachment in,
88–89; comic and tragic modes in,
National Press Club dinner, 63 82, 111; conflict, 89–90;
nervous energy, 9–10, 13, 14–15 development of, 82, 111;
9/11, 25 differentiation function in, 89;
911 call operators, 73 dividing humor in, 88–89;
Nixon, Richard, 65 enforcement function in, 89;
nonverbal behavior, in measuring hierarchy in, 90–92; ingratiation in,
humor, 58 82; inside jokes in, 6; moderation in,
94; pet humor in, 83; relief humor
objectivity, 59–60 in, 15; security and trust, 88–89;
organizations and cultures sexual humor in, 83, 83–84; shared
(organizations): clarification scripts in, 81; sharing in, 8, 81,
function in, 36–37; concertive 103–104; success based on humor in,
control in, 74; controlling others in, 82; superiority humor in, 97; teasing
98; defined, 71; differentiation in, 8, 38–39, 86–87, 92–94, 100, 102;
function in, 79–80; enforcement tragic mode in, 82; uncertainty
function in, 72; hierarchical reduction in, 81, 83, 84–94. See also
relationships in, 72–75, 78–79; dangers for relationships
humor orientation in, 54; managers persuasion, 7; audience interest for,
in, 73–74, 79, 80, 85; patterns of 60–61, 69; credibility for, 8, 41, 54,
communication in, 6; pattern 59, 62–66, 68, 69, 106; distraction
violation in, 71; production control and, 61; effectiveness, 59, 60, 68–69;
in, 74; resistance humor in, 74–75, ego-involvement and, 60;
78, 79, 80, 106; sharing in, 72, 73; ingratiation in, 65; late night
symbolism within, 73; uniting and political television comedy for,
dividing humor in, 32, 72, 79–80; 64–66, 67; moderation for, 62, 62–63,
violations of norms in, 72, 73 68, 69; objectivity in, 59–60;
origin theories, 1 politicians’ use of, 62–68; satire in,
66–68; sharing for, 62; sleeper effect,
Palin, Sarah, 66 61
Papa, M. J., 52–53 pet humor, 83
paradoxes, 29, 41, 58, 105, 112, 117 philosophers, 117
paratelic mode, 10–11, 15, 16, 17; in late physicians, 90–91
night political television comedy, physiology, 5, 13, 14
66; in social model, 105–106 playful and nonplayful humor, 33
patterns: adaptability and change in, play state, 1, 8, 99, 110. See also paratelic
23–24; cognitive development of, 10; mode
in communication, 2, 5–6, 7; in life Poduri, Chalapathi, 35
pattern disruption, 23–29; in political commentary: late night
organizations, 6; symbolic, 2; in political television comedy for,
symbolic interactionism theory, 3 64–66, 67; superiority humor, 65;
pattern violations: alternative concepts tragic mode in, 45–46
discovered by, 71–72; ambiguity in, politicians: differentiation function
105; in communication, 3, 5, 71; in used by, 41; ingratiation used by, 8,
incongruity humor, 17–19, 71; 62, 63, 65; moderation for, 62–63, 68;
Index 135
persuasion used by, 62–68. See also disablement in, 109; “I-Thou”
specific politicians relationships in, 111, 119; liberation
production control, 74 in, 44, 107–109, 110; paradoxes in,
public address, 1, 8, 13, 35, 62, 106. See 29, 41, 58, 105, 112, 117; paratelic
also persuasion mode in, 105–106; sharing in, 106,
punishment. See enforcement function 114; social boundaries in, 109–110;
teasing in, 110
Reagan, Ronald, 63–64 social norms: enforcement function for
recognition, 11–12 violation of, 37–39, 72; facilitation
reification, 3, 31, 41, 58, 74, 78 of, 30–31; uniting humor stressing,
relationships. See personal 42
relationships students, 91–92
relief humor: nervous energy released subjectivity, 31
in, 13, 14–15; in personal superiority humor: for controlling
relationships, 15; physiology in, 13, others, 97; as danger for
14; theories of, 12–15, 19; violations relationships, 95, 97; differentiation
in, 13, 14 function in, 39–41; edge in, 15;
religion, 117 enforcement function of, 15–16;
Requiem (Mozart), 118 laughing ‘at’ not ‘with’ in, 15–16; in
resistance humor, 74–75, 78, 79, 80, 106 personal relationships, 97; in
reversals, 113 political commentary, 65; theories
road rage, 108 of, 12–13, 15–17, 19; victory yell in,
Romney, Mitt, 65 16; violation in, 15; winning in, 16
royal fool, 108 surprise, 10, 11, 18, 102
Swift, Jonathan, 67
safety, 26–27, 28, 110. See also symbolic interactionism theory, 3
uncertainty reduction symbolic patterns, 2
satire, 15, 49, 66–68, 118 symbolism, 5, 6, 31, 47, 85; in
satisfaction, of couples, 82, 87, 90, organizations, 73; in violations, 112
97–100, 101–102, 103
Saturday Night Live, 65, 66 teachers, 91–92, 113–114
script awareness, 47–48, 81, 84–85 teasing: as dividing, 100; as
security and trust, 88–89, 102 enforcement, 37–39, 39; moderation
self-deprecation, 16, 35, 53, 62, 64, 69 in, 94; in personal relationships, 8,
sense of control, 48–49 38–39, 86–87, 87, 92–94, 100, 102; in
sexual humor, 12–13, 26, 52, 76, 83, social model, 110; as telic, 119; in
83–84 uncertainty reduction, 86–87; as
sharing: in communication, 2, 5–6; uniting humor, 8
inside jokes, 7; in personal techniques, 28
relationships, 8, 81, 103–104; television remote control, 100–101
persuasion by, 62; of scripts, 81; in telic mode, 10–11, 66, 103, 119
social model, 106, 114; uniting Terrion, J. L., 80
humor as, 72, 73 terrorists, 107
Sion, L., 73 theories of humor, 5, 9–10; boost theory
sleeper effect, 61 in, 13; on incongruity humor, 12–13,
social boundaries, 109–110 17–19; jag, 13; on relief humor,
social dilemmas, 112–119 12–15, 19; on superiority humor,
social model, 9, 107; comic and tragic 12–13, 15–17, 19; universal, 10–19
choices in, 106–107, 111, 112–114; tragedy, 25–26
136 Index
tragic mode: choosing of, 43–51, 46; 72, 79–80; playful and nonplayful,
comic mode compared to, 44; comic 33; as sharing, 72, 73; social norms
mode dependent on, 114; of death, stressed in, 42; teasing as, 8
115; ego-involvement in, 47; evil universal theories of humor, 10–19
instead of error in, 45–46; as
individual choice, 43–51, 46, vacation e-mails, 116
106–107, 111, 112–114, 118; Veatch, T. C., 12, 13, 41
loneliness linked to, 88, 115; in victory yell, 9, 15, 16, 19
music, 118; in personal Vietnam, 25
relationships, 82; in political violations: dividing humor stressing,
commentary, 45–46; road rage and, 42; enforcement function for, 37–39,
108; in social model, 106–107, 111, 72; of moral order, 12, 41, 43, 105; in
112–114 organizations, 72, 73; in relief
tricksters’ tales, 71 humor, 13, 14; safe, of social order,
26; script awareness in, 47–48; in
uncertainty reduction: dangers for superiority humor, 15; symbolism
relationships and, 95–104; in in, 112; tricksters’ tales as, 71. See
personal relationships, 81, 83, 84–94; also pattern violations
script sharing in, 84–85; teasing in,
86–87 weaponized humor, 41, 57–58, 64,
United States, 45–46, 66, 78, 107–108, 100–102, 104
116–117 weather, and criticism, 15
uniting humor: affiliative humor as, 32, winning, 15–16
56–57, 100, 102; aggressive humor workplace. See organizations and
as, 32, 57–58; clarification function cultures
as, 7, 34; in communication, 21, World War II, 25, 108
29–34; dividing compared to, 29–34; Wrench, J. S., 53
ethics of care in, 30; identification
function for, 7; identification Zapata, C. L., 57
function in, 7, 34; ingratiation as, 7, zombies, 68–69
42, 53, 62, 82; in organizations, 32,