Emotion, Personality, and The Frontal Lobe: Satoshi Umeda
Emotion, Personality, and The Frontal Lobe: Satoshi Umeda
Emotion, Personality, and The Frontal Lobe: Satoshi Umeda
Satoshi Umeda
S. Umeda (*)
Department of Psychology, Keio University, Mita 2-15-45,
Minato-ku, Tokyo 108-8345, Japan
e-mail: umeda@flet.keio.ac.jp
Research on the neural basis of emotion and self has come to be the major topic in
social cognitive neuroscience. Many cognitive neuroscientists in this research area
have focused in particular on the functional neuroanatomy involved in the cortical
midline structures. Numerous different domains in self-processing, including moni-
toring, evaluation, agency, and theory of mind, are all more or less involved in mid-
line structures, including the medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex
(Northoff and Bermpohll 2004).
Over the past several years, a number of neuroimaging studies have tried to
clarify the neural mechanisms of “theory of mind,” and have demonstrated that the
medial prefrontal cortex is activated when subjects are engaged in various kinds of
10 Emotion, Personality, and the Frontal Lobe 225
mind-reading or mentalizing tasks (Frith and Frith 1999). Several recent studies in
developmental cognitive neuroscience have reported some evidence that people
with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism show weak activations in the
medial prefrontal cortex in comparison with control subjects (Di Martino et al.
2009; Happé et al. 1996; Nieminen-von Wendt et al. 2003). These findings are con-
sidered fundamental evidence for deficits in theory-of-mind abilities among indi-
viduals with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism (Frith 2001).
Concerning types of emotion, no universally accepted typology has been defined,
but one widely used division separates basic emotions from more advanced emo-
tions. The emotions of happiness, surprise, sadness, fear, disgust, and anger have
been identified as the six basic emotions (Ekman 1992). By contrast, complex
advanced emotions such as friendliness, pensiveness, thoughtfulness, and melan-
choly are all required to modulate social communication with others or to state
one’s own inner complex mental states (Shaw et al. 2005). In fact, these kinds of
advanced emotions are highly associated with mind-reading abilities, because
understanding advanced emotions in another is a basis for realizing appropriate
theory-of-mind reasoning or empathy-processing for social communications with
others. The “basic-advanced” distinction of emotion is also supported by several
other findings. For instance, Castelli (2005) reported that people with autism under-
stand the basic emotions but fail to recognize some complex emotions.
Previous functional neuroimaging studies on human emotion over the past decade
have focused primarily on basic emotions, and several neuroimaging studies of
advanced emotion have been reported to date. For instance, previous studies sug-
gested that increasing regret in a gambling task enhanced activity in some brain
areas including the medial orbitofrontal region (Coricelli et al. 2007). However, the
question of how humans learn advanced emotions remains poorly understood at both
behavioral and neural levels. The goal of the first study was to address the question
using a functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) method. Illustrated facial
expressions with unknown novel types of emotions were used to examine the learn-
ing processes of advanced emotions at the behavioral and neural levels.
In this study, subjects comprised 18 healthy adults (9 men, 9 women; mean age,
22.2 years; mean duration of education, 14.6 years). As materials for this experi-
ment, six figures (two positive-based, two negative-based, and two neutral-based
learning conditions) were prepared, consisting of one physical situation with time
series and one complex facial expression (Fig. 10.1).
In the learning (MRI scanning) phase, subjects were instructed to lie on the scanner
bed and asked to learn the relationships between the physical situations displayed
above and the facial expressions displayed below. Each stimulus was displayed for
10 s, followed by the cross-hair presentation. Stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA)
was 50 s. The subjects were also later required to undertake an acquisition test outside
226 S. Umeda
Positive-based condition
Negative-based condition
Neutral-based condition
Fig. 10.1 Experimental stimuli used in the positive-, negative- and neutral-based learning conditions
the scanner. In the behavioral test phase after the MRI scanning, subjects were asked to
make recognition judgments for figures consisting of one physical situation and one
facial expression projected on the computer screen. The recognition test comprised
36 figures, including 12 correct and 24 incorrect combinations.
For acquisition, a 3-T Allegra system (Siemens, Germany) was used to acquire
high-resolution T1-weighted anatomic images and single-shot gradient-echo echo planar
images (EPIs) with blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) contrast of 35 axial
slices with the following parameters: cubic resolution, 3.5 mm; repetition time, 2 s;
echo time, 30 ms; and flip angle, 90°. Image preprocessing and data analyses were
performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM2; http://www.fil.
ion.ucl.ac.uk/~spm/) on a Matlab platform.
For the behavioral results, the mean proportion of correct responses was 0.86 for
the positive-based condition, 0.81 for the negative-based condition, and 0.83 for the
neutral-based condition. Mean reaction time was 4,394 ms for the positive-based con-
dition, 3,678 ms for the negative-based condition, and 3,991 ms for the neutral-based
condition. No significant differences were apparent among these three conditions.
As imaging results, several substantial findings were shown in the time-course
data for activated brain areas during the three conditions, as follows: (1) the poste-
rior medial prefrontal cortex (BA8) and anterior cingulate cortex (BA32) were
strongly activated in the negative-based learning condition (Fig. 10.2); (2) the right
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA9/46) was strongly activated in both positive- and
negative-based learning conditions (Fig. 10.3); (3) bilateral parahippocampal areas
were strongly activated in the positive-based learning condition (Fig. 10.4).
10 Emotion, Personality, and the Frontal Lobe 227
Fig. 10.2 Time course of signal changes for positive-, negative- and neutral-based learning condi-
tions in the posterior medial prefrontal (BA8) and anterior cingulate cortex (BA32)
0.7
0.6
Signal Change (%)
0.5
0.4 Positive
0.3
Negative
0.2
0.1 Neutral
0
T value
-0.1
-0.2
0 6 12 18 24 30 36 42 48
Fig. 10.3 Time course of signal changes for positive-, negative- and neutral-based conditions in
the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA9/46)
The next question is whether any difference in brain activation patterns exists
between learning of simple emotion and complex advanced emotion. To answer
this question, activation patterns were compared between simple and complex
emotion conditions. Simple emotion means stimuli including facial expression
with happy eyebrows and happy mouths, or with fearful eyebrows and fearful mouths.
228 S. Umeda
-4
0.4 0.4
Signal Change (%)
Fig. 10.4 Time course of signal changes for positive-, negative- and neutral-based conditions in
right and left parahippocampal areas
These examples are displayed on the top and middle left panels of Fig. 10.1.
Complex emotion means stimuli including facial expression with happy eyebrows
and fearful mouths, or with fearful eyebrows and happy mouths. These examples
are displayed on the top and middle right panels of Fig. 10.1.
The contrast between brain activations during simple and complex emotion
learning indicated that bilateral superior parietal lobules (BA7), bilateral dorsal pre-
motor areas, and the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) were greatly acti-
vated in complex emotion conditions rather than in simple emotion conditions
(Fig. 10.5). Interestingly, the superior parietal lobule and dorsal premotor area are
both highly involved in imitation and have been identified as parts of the mirror
neuron system (Brass and Heyes 2005).
These overall results suggest that the prefrontal and medial temporal areas are
essential for acquiring relationships between situations and facial expressions, and
that those areas make independent contributions to learning specific advanced emo-
tions. The findings of this study show that the medial prefrontal cortex (BA8) and
anterior cingulate cortex (BA32) are strongly activated in negative-based conditions
compared with the other two conditions. This suggests that these areas are critical
for acquiring negative-based advanced emotions, which are supposed to be highly
involved in empathetic processing (Singer et al. 2004, 2006). As mentioned before,
the medial prefrontal cortex has been identified as an area with weak activation
among individuals with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. The present
result that this area is involved mostly in negative-based conditions could represent
further evidence for a lack of theory-of-mind ability in people with Asperger
syndrome or high-functioning autism.
10 Emotion, Personality, and the Frontal Lobe 229
Bilateral Superiror
Parietal Lobules (BA 7)
T value
+58
Another important finding was that activations in the right dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex were greater in positive- and negative-based conditions than in neutral-based
conditions. This finding is consistent with previous results that activation in this
area correlates strongly with autonomic activity and activities in the anterior cingu-
late cortex (Critchley et al. 2001, 2003; Critchley 2004; Fukushima et al. 2010).
The present results suggest that bodily responses under the control of the autonomic
nervous system accompany learning associations between physical situations and
facial expressions with emotional valence.
Moreover, bilateral hippocampal areas showed greater activation in positive-
based learning conditions than in the other two conditions. Several previous studies
have reported that the hippocampus is greatly activated during the processing of
positive emotional stimuli, rather than negative emotional stimuli (Britton et al.
2006; Kuchinke et al. 2005; Prohovnik et al. 2004). Recent morphometric studies
using MRI have reported smaller hippocampus volumes in patients with major
depression (for a review, see Davidson et al. 2002). The present result showing that
the hippocampus is greatly involved in positive-based emotion learning is consistent
with the idea that the hippocampus supports positive-based emotional processing.
The contrast between brain activations during simple and complex emotion
learning shows that parts of the mirror neuron system are greatly involved during
complex emotion learning. Activations in these areas suggest that when people
encounter unknown facial expression with a certain situation, parts of the mirror
neuron system are used to understand what the facial expression means by actual
facial imitations (Leslie et al. 2004). Concerning the greater activation found in the
left VLPFC, previous functional imaging data obtained from a false recognition
230 S. Umeda
task showed that this area is highly involved when people recognize the presented
word as a new word that has not previously been encountered (Umeda et al. 2005).
Activation in this area during complex emotion learning thus suggests that under-
standing of unknown facial expression is based on a saliency detection process for
identification as a novel emotion.
The overall findings from the first study provide some evidence that advanced
emotion learning is realized by a combination of neural activities for learning, for
social and empathetic processing, and for the mirror neuron system.
Some previous studies have actually examined the performance of patients with
selective damage to the medial prefrontal cortex. Baird et al. (2006) tested two
patients and reported intact intellectual, memory, and language abilities, and visuo-
perceptual functions, but showed weak or impaired performance on selective execu-
tive function tests. No theory-of-mind performance was tested in this study. Another
neuropsychological study addressed the question of theory of mind impairment by
testing a patient with a selective lesion in the medial prefrontal cortex (Bird et al.
2004). They carefully examined performance of the patient on various kinds of
theory-of-mind tests, but found no significant impairment on tests, and thus stated
that extensive medial frontal regions are not necessary for theory-of-mind perfor-
mance. The findings from both studies have some important implications for our
understanding of the effects of damage to the medial prefrontal cortex. Most inter-
esting was the finding that following the damage to this area, patients did not show
any severe impairment on test performance, including theory-of-mind performance.
However, the data are currently too limited to reach solid conclusions on the effects
of medial prefrontal damage.
The two patients had medial prefrontal damage (Umeda et al. 2010). The first case,
T.O., was a 31-year-old man. A full-time employee of a big electronic company in
Japan, he had undergone neurosurgery for brain tumor. Magnetic resonance imag-
ing revealed that damage extended through the left-dominant medial prefrontal and
anterior cingulate cortices, reaching the left supplementary motor area. The right
hand and leg were moderately paralyzed for a few months after surgery, but these
symptoms later resolved.
10 Emotion, Personality, and the Frontal Lobe 233
The most striking aspect of T.O. was a reported change in personality. According
to his self-report, he noticed that his sense of reality was attenuated after surgery,
leading him to feel detached from the world despite being sure of his location, and
this feeling often occurred in a manner similar to a panic attack. These symptoms
partly resembled the characteristics of depersonalization. He also mentioned that
surgery had made him feel depressed, anxious, and withdrawn from everything.
The second patient, H.C., was a 56-year-old man. An employer of a small private
company in Japan, he had undergone surgery following rupture of a right peri-
callosal artery aneurysm. Magnetic resonance imaging revealed that the area of
damage included the right-dominant medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices,
extending slightly into the right supplementary motor area.
According to self-reports, he noticed that his memory had deteriorated after
surgery, with a feeling that most daily episodes could not be clearly remembered.
He reported difficulty doing two things simultaneously, and became aware that
everything needed a strong effort to be done. He also mentioned that his personality
had changed after surgery, leading him to notice that feelings of sadness and anger
had been dimmed and that he had become much more depressive, anxious, and
withdrawn compared to his previous personality.
Three months after surgery, six of the more frequently used neuropsychological
assessments were conducted to examine higher-order cognitive functions in these
patients. (1) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) for general intel-
ligence, (2) Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) for memory and attention,
(3) Rey Auditory–Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) for verbal recall ability, (4) Rey–
Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCFT) for visuoconstructive skills and visual
memory, (5) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) for abstract reasoning and ability
to appropriately shift cognitive strategies, and (6) Stroop Test for selective attention
and inhibition.
On the intelligence test, T.O., with left-sided damage, showed dissociation
between verbal and performance intelligence quotient (IQ), with an inferior score on
performance IQ. In the IQ test, he showed difficulty in performing subtests of block
design, object assembly, and digit symbols. In terms of memory performance, T.O.
showed lower scores on some measures for identifying delayed recall performance
(e.g., delayed recall on WMS-R, RAVLT, and ROCFT) compared with his relatively
higher scores on other measures. The results of mild amnesia were consistent with
his self-report regarding daily activities. For instance, he reported often becoming
confused in remembering whether he had taken his medication. T.O.’s performances
on tests for executive function were all within the normal range. Performance on the
standardized aphasic test did not show any difficulties in language activities.
The second patient, H.C. with right-sided damage, showed an intellectual perfor-
mance within the normal range. In terms of memory performance, he exhibited nor-
mal scores on WMS-R, although scores on RAVLT and ROCFT were somewhat
lower. In fact, in terms of daily activities he showed difficulty with temporal-order
judgments for everyday episodes within a time range of a few days. H.C.’s perfor-
mance on tests for executive function was also within the normal range. Performance
on the standardized aphasic test did not show any difficulties in language activities.
234 S. Umeda
To clarify theory-of-mind performance, patients T.O. and H.C. were tested using
four types of story comprehension task: (1) first-order false-belief test (Baron-Cohen
et al. 1985; Frith and Frith 1999), (2) second-order false-belief test (Baron-Cohen 1989),
(3) strange stories test (Happé 1994), and (4) faux-pas recognition test (Baron-
Cohen et al. 1999).
The first-order false-belief test is one of the most famous tests for theory-of-mind
reasoning. This test assesses the ability to recognize that others can have false beliefs
about the world that can differ from reality, and that people’s behaviors can be
predicted by the representation of others’ mental states. The more complex second-
order false-belief test requires participants to understand a second person’s concerns
about the world, based on social interactions of minds in which people are con-
cerned about each other’s mental states.
The last two tests were used to examine the more advanced theory-of-mind rea-
soning ability in the patients. The strange stories test assesses the ability to infer
mental states in a story context for social understanding. A previous study reported
that subjects with autism-spectrum disorders show impaired provision of context-
appropriate mental state explanations for strange stories, in comparison with normal
control subjects (Happé 1994).
As well as the strange stories test, the faux-pas recognition test was used to
assess the ability to recognize inappropriate statements in a story context (Baron-
Cohen et al. 1999). Subjects were presented with each story and asked whether a
faux pas was contained. If a faux pas was detected, they were then asked for an
explanation of it. Each subject was also requested to answer two additional ques-
tions to test story comprehension in each story, to see whether a comprehensive
understanding of each story was achieved. Baron-Cohen et al. (1999) reported that
subjects with autism-spectrum disorders show impaired detection of faux pas on the
faux-pas recognition test compared with normal control subjects, despite intact
story comprehension.
In addition, T.O. and H.C. were required to complete all 50 items in the Autism-
Spectrum Quotient (AQ) questionnaire (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). This question-
naire was developed as a self-administered method of screening for adults with
normal intelligence and traits associated with autism-spectrum disorders. Score
ranges from 0 to 50 in the questionnaire. Adults with Asperger syndrome or high-
functioning autism show a mean score of 35.8, significantly higher than controls
with a mean score of 16.4 (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). Another recent study has
shown that the threshold score for suspected Asperger syndrome or high-functioning
autism is 26.0 (Woodbury-Smith et al. 2005).
Both patients passed the first- and second-order false-belief tests, providing
expected answers suggesting a proper understanding of each story. In terms of
advanced theory-of-mind tests, both patients showed good performance on the strange
stories test. Overall percentage of providing appropriate explanations for given stories
was 85.7% for T.O. and 100% for H.C. on the strange stories test. On the faux-pas
10 Emotion, Personality, and the Frontal Lobe 235
recognition test, the percentage of detecting faux pas and having appropriate
explanations was 60.0% for T.O. and 100% for H.C. for the provided faux-pas stories.
By contrast, the percentage of detecting “no” faux pas was 70.0% for T.O. and
57.1% for H.C. for the provided control stories. Both patients thus reported faux pas
even in control stories without any faux pas. In addition, both patients showed
higher scores on the two questions for story comprehension (92.5% for T.O. and
89.3% for H.C.).
In terms of the AQ, scores were 31 for T.O. and 29 for H.C., above the threshold
score for Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism of 26 as defined by
Woodbury-Smith et al. (2005). Interestingly, both patients spontaneously reported
just after completing the questionnaire that they were sure that some of the person-
ality traits focused upon in the questionnaire identified the actual personality changes
they felt. They were then asked to complete the AQ again for what they supposed
their original personality was before surgery. AQ scores for purported presurgical
state of the two patients were 13 for T.O. and 23 for H.C., much lower than the initial
scores and below the threshold score for Asperger syndrome or high-functioning
autism. Both patients thus appear to have developed some autistic personality traits
following surgical operations.
A general finding was that the two patients had developed some characteristics
of autism after surgical operations. To specify these characteristics in greater detail,
these items were compared with items identified in a two-factor structure model
(Hoekstra et al. 2008). Two factors were identified among all 50 items in the AQ,
namely “social interaction” and “attention to detail”. For T.O., all 18 items fell into
the “social interaction” factor while for H.C. six of seven items fell into the “social
interaction” factor. The only item falling into the “attention to detail” factor for H.C.
was the item that showed he had become fascinated by dates.
In another opportunity separate from this study, 11 patients with damage to other
parts of the brain (orbitofrontal lesion, basal forebrain lesion, dorsolateral prefrontal
lesion, medial temporal lesion, amygdala lesion, and traumatic brain injury) were
asked to complete the AQ, to compare scores and possible personality changes
detected by the AQ. Mean score for the 11 patients was 17.0 (range, 9–25), and
median score was 17.0. All patients declared that personality traits identified on the
AQ were unchanged after surgical operations or closed-head injuries.
The two patients displayed damage basically limited to the medial prefrontal cortex
and showed mild difficulties in memory performance in daily activities, but no serious
problems in language activities and executive functions. These patterns of results
are basically consistent with previous case studies regarding damage to the same
area of the brain (Baird et al. 2006; Bird et al. 2004). Concerning theory-of-mind
tests, performance in the first- and second-order false-belief test was perfect in both
cases. Performance in the advanced theory-of-mind tests by T.O. was slightly
236 S. Umeda