ADOS - Toddler Module
ADOS - Toddler Module
ADOS - Toddler Module
Author manuscript
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Author Manuscript
CA 94158
Whitney Guthrie,
Autism Institute, Department of Psychology, Florida State University, 1940 N. Monroe Street Suite
72, Tallahassee, FL 32303
Amy Wetherby,
Autism Institute, Department of Psychology, Florida State University, 1940 N. Monroe Street Suite
72, Tallahassee, FL 32303
Catherine Lord
Author Manuscript
Center for Autism and the Developing Brain, Weill Cornell Medical College, 21 Bloomingdale Rd.,
White Plains, NY 10605
Abstract
Standardized calibrated severity scores (CSS) have been created for Autism Diagnostic
Observation Schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2) Modules 1–4 as a metric of the relative severity of
autism-specific behaviors. Total and domain CSS were created for the Toddler Module to facilitate
comparison to other modules. Analyses included 388 children with ASD age 12 to 30 months and
were replicated on 435 repeated assessments from 127 children with ASD. Compared to raw
scores, associations between total and domain CSS and participant characteristics were reduced in
the original sample. Verbal IQ effects on Social Affect-CSS were not reduced in the replication
sample. Toddler Module CSS increases comparability of ADOS-2 scores across modules and
Author Manuscript
Keywords
Autism spectrum disorder; Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; Severity; Toddlers; Social
Affect; Restricted and Repetitive Behavior
Corresponding Author: Amy N. Esler, 717 Delaware St SE, Minneapolis, MN 55414, Office: 612-626-6340, Fax: 612-625-3261,
esle0007@umn.edu.
Esler et al. Page 2
The development of early screening and diagnostic tools for autism spectrum disorder
Author Manuscript
(ASD) has allowed diagnoses to occur at younger ages (Dawson & Bernier, 2014; Guthrie,
Swineford, Nottke, & Wetherby, 2012; Woolfenden, Sarkozy, Ridley, & Williams, 2012).
The Toddler Module of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2;
Lord, Luyster, Gotham, & Guthrie, 2012a) has demonstrated high levels of reliability and
validity as a diagnostic tool for ASD in children age 12 to 30 months (Guthrie et al., 2012;
Luyster et al., 2009). However, social communication and behavioral patterns in children
who develop ASD can be variable early in the second year of life (Bryson et al., 2007;
Landa, Holman, & Garrett-Meyer, 2007; Ozonoff et al., 2010; Werner & Dawson, 2005). An
important clinical use of the Toddler Module of the ADOS-2 is to identify concerns in need
of continued monitoring (Lord, Luyster, Guthrie, & Pickles, 2012b). At the research level,
the Toddler Module may aid in understanding ASD symptom trajectories beginning as soon
as children are walking independently (Lord et al., 2012a).
Author Manuscript
Over the past decade, much research has focused on the development of younger siblings of
children with ASD (“infant siblings”) to better understand onset, risk, and underlying
biological mechanisms (e.g., Ozonoff et al., 2011). Studies of infant siblings suggest
variable and complicated developmental trajectories (Bryson et al., 2007; Landa et al., 2007;
Messinger et al., 2013; Ozonoff et al., 2014). Regardless of diagnostic outcome, infant
siblings tend to score higher (i.e., more symptomatic) on the ADOS-2 and lower on
measures of developmental skills, compared to infants who do not have a family history of
ASD (Messinger et al., 2013; Ozonoff et al., 2014).
It is well known that ASD is a highly heterogeneous disorder, and this hinders our
understanding of its causes and recommended courses of treatment (State & Levitt, 2011).
The behavioral heterogeneity of ASD is in part due to differences in level of intellectual
Author Manuscript
In research and in practice, the ADOS-2 frequently is used to diagnose and describe ASD
symptoms. A calibrated severity score (CSS) was created for Modules 1 through 4 to
estimate overall level of ASD symptoms relative to others with ASD of the same age and
language level (Gotham, Pickles, & Lord, 2009; Hus & Lord, 2014). The CSS was created in
response to the need for a metric of severity that is as independent as possible of participant
variables of intellectual ability, language, and age. Compared to raw total scores, the CSS
Author Manuscript
was less influenced by verbal language level, especially for Modules 1–3—where verbal IQ
accounted for 43% of the variance in raw scores, it accounted for only 10% of the variance
in the CSS. The CSS also has more uniform distributions across age/language level groups.
These results were replicated by de Bildt et al. (2011) and Shumway et al. (2012) in
independent samples, with a similar pattern of reduced association with verbal IQ for the
CSS.
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 3
On the other hand, ASD symptoms may best be measured by domain rather than in
Author Manuscript
aggregate (Shumway et al., 2012). Separate calibrated severity scores were developed for
Social Affect and Restricted, Repetitive Behavior domains of the ADOS-2 to provide a
clearer picture of ASD symptom severity (Hus, Gotham, & Lord, 2012; Hus & Lord, 2014).
Several potential uses for domain CSS have been identified, including studying whether the
two domains have distinct trajectories or respond differently to intervention; increasing
phenotypic homogeneity by clustering individuals according to similar levels of severity in
each domain; and using a CSS to control statistically for differences in one domain while
focusing on the other. There is a need for standardized tools to further define and
characterize severity, to improve reliability of ratings across sites and clinicians, and to
increase comparability across research samples (Weitlauf, Gotham, Vehorn, & Warren,
2014).
At the time that overall and domain CSS were created, large datasets using the Toddler
Author Manuscript
Module of the ADOS-2 were not available to be included in analyses. Thus, a CSS could not
be calculated for children who received the Toddler Module. Researchers have tried to
overcome this limitation in various ways. For example, a CSS could not be generated in
infant sibling and intervention studies until 36 months for many children (e.g., Messinger et
al., 2013; Ozonoff et al., 2014). Other studies (Guthrie et al., 2012; Venker, Ray-
Subramanian, Ellis Weismer, & Bolt, 2013) attempted to capture symptom severity by
applying Module 1 CSS to the Toddler Module. However, as the authors acknowledged, the
CSS developed for Module 1 cannot be directly applicable to the Toddler Module due to
differences in coding criteria and items comprising the algorithms for the respective
modules. Application of CSS for the ages addressed by the Toddler Module, 12 to 30
months, may help us better understand developmental trajectories indicative of risk,
especially because they provide a continuous scale of presence and severity of ASD
Author Manuscript
symptoms across development, into the other four modules. A Toddler Module CSS would
allow longitudinal comparisons of symptom severity potentially from the earliest point of
concern, and may improve understanding of how ASD symptoms emerge, relatively
independent of language abilities.
A note on terminology: in the recently revised ADOS-2 (Lord et al., 2012d), the CSS was
renamed the Comparison Score. However, here, we maintain use of the term “CSS” to refer
to the standardized severity scores to facilitate comparisons to the studies by Gotham et al.
(2009), Hus et al. (2014), and Hus & Lord (2014), which this manuscript seeks to replicate.
The purpose of the present research is to develop ADOS-2 Toddler Module total and domain
CSS to expand the continuous metric of ASD symptom severity to younger ages. We
Author Manuscript
hypothesize that the Toddler Module CSS will be less affected by child characteristics and
demographics than raw scores. However, because the Toddler Module covers a more
restricted age and IQ range than Modules 1–4, we were interested to see whether the CSS
would result in reductions in the influence of age and IQ to the extent demonstrated in
Modules 1–4. To achieve this aim, this study employed methods from Gotham and
colleagues’ (2009) development of the total CSS for modules 1–3 and from Hus et al.’s
(2012) and Hus & Lord’s (2014) development of calibrated domain scores for Social Affect
and Restricted, Repetitive Behaviors.
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 4
Methods
Author Manuscript
Participants
The sample consisted of 388 individual children eventually diagnosed with ASD. Repeated
assessments were performed on 127 children, yielding a total of 823 assessments, where
“assessment” is defined as contemporaneous Toddler Module data and a best estimate
clinical diagnosis. The child’s most recent diagnosis was used for the purposes of the current
study. Mothers in the repeated assessments group had more education (χ2=15.19, p<.001).
Although, at the group level, children with one assessment did not differ significantly from
children with repeated assessments in age, gender, race, verbal IQ, or nonverbal IQ,
significant differences in these variables emerged when children were grouped based on the
Toddler Module algorithm received. Among children who used fewer than five words during
the Toddler Module or were between the ages of 12 and 20 months (i.e., 12–20/Nonverbal
Author Manuscript
algorithm), children in the repeated assessment group tended to be slightly younger than the
single assessment group (21.52 versus 22.65 months, p<.01), and they had higher nonverbal
IQs (86.65 versus 79.12, p<.001). Children between age 21 and 30 months who used five or
more single words during the Toddler Module (i.e., Some Words 21–30 algorithm) showed
more differences: children with repeated assessments were slightly older than children with
one assessment (25.78 versus 24.82 months, p <.01) and had higher verbal mental ages
(23.78 versus 19.35, p <.001), verbal IQ (88.87 versus 78.84, p <.01), nonverbal mental ages
(25.66 versus 22.84, p <.001), and nonverbal IQ (96.42 versus 89.27, p <.01). These
differences were likely due to referral biases; for example, children with repeated
assessments were more likely to be infant siblings (28% of the repeated assessment group
versus 10% of the single assessment group, χ2=27.90) who may have enrolled in a research
study prior to showing developmental concerns.
Author Manuscript
In creating the CSS for Modules 1–4, repeated assessment data were retained in the analyses
(Gotham et al., 2009; Hus & Lord, 2014). However, because of the differences between
children seen once and children seen longitudinally described above, a subsample was used
for standardization of calibrated severity scores that eliminated repeated assessment data.
This subsample of 388 children with ASD (hereafter termed “original sample”) contained
data from all children with one assessment, and one assessment was randomly selected for
children with repeated assessments. A replication sample then was created using children
with ASD with repeated assessments, excluding the 388 children in the original sample, to
further validate calibrated severity scores.
the dataset included 8% African American (N=30), 2% Asian American (N=7), 71%
Caucasian (N=276), 6% Hispanic (N=24), 0.3% Native American (N=1), and 9% Biracial
(N=36). Males comprised 83% of the dataset (N=323) and females comprised 17% of the
sample (N=65). Thirteen percent reported maternal education at the graduate or professional
level, 52% had a bachelor’s degree or some college, and 30% reported completing high
school or less (4% did not report education level). Contemporaneous verbal IQ data was
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 5
available for 274 children (71% of the original sample) and nonverbal IQ for 329 (85%) (see
Author Manuscript
The dataset represented combined data from four sites: the University of Michigan Autism
and Communication Disorders Center, Florida State University (FSU), the University of
Minnesota, and the University of Wisconsin-Madison. The majority (N=211) came from
FSU and were recruited from pediatric primary care physicians through the FIRST
WORDS® Project, a prospective, longitudinal study of a general population screening
program for communication delays and ASD. Children from the University of Michigan
(N=84) consisted of (a) consecutive referrals of children from 12 to 30 months of age to the
clinic, (b) children from University of Michigan projects studying early development of
children with communication delays and/or at risk for ASD, and (c) children participating in
various treatment studies. The original validation sample for the Toddler Module (Luyster et
al., 2009) was included in this dataset. Children from the University of Wisconsin (N=58)
Author Manuscript
Of the 127 children with ASD with repeated assessments, 46 had two or three Toddler
Module assessments, 52 had four or five assessments, 26 had between six and 10
assessments, and three had between 11 and 15 assessments. Children with four or more
assessments tended to be participants who were showing communication and/or ASD
concerns and were participating in treatment studies at FSU or the University of Michigan,
or children participating in a study of early diagnosis of ASD at the University of Michigan
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 6
where participants were seen on a monthly basis. Participants in the monthly study at the
Author Manuscript
University of Michigan were showing communication delays and/or risk for ASD, or were
infant siblings. As a side note, we were not concerned about practice effects for children
with repeated assessments, because although children may become familiar with particular
tasks (e.g., the bath routine), ADOS-2 scores and classifications are based on spontaneous
initiations and responses rather than performance on specific tasks (Lord et al., 2012d).
children age 12 to 30 months with language skills ranging from no verbal language to single
words and simple phrases. Toddlers must be walking independently, and a nonverbal mental
age of at least 12 months is recommended. The Toddler Module follows the structure of
Module 1, which is designed for language levels ranging from nonverbal to single words and
simple phrases. Module 1 activities, child behavioral descriptions, and scoring criteria were
modified based on developmental expectations for toddlers.
The Toddler Module algorithm contains separate domain categories of Social Affect and
Restricted, Repetitive Behaviors and a single total score to determine classification. Separate
algorithms are provided based on age and language level: all children age 12 to 20 months,
and children age 21–30 months who produce fewer than five words during the ADOS-2,
receive the 12–20/Nonverbal 21–30 algorithm, and children age 21–30 months who produce
five or more words during the ADOS-2 receive the Some Words 21–30 months algorithm.
Author Manuscript
Clinical cut-off scores are grouped within levels of concern for ASD, acknowledging the
diagnostic uncertainty inherent in very young children due to significant developmental
variability or confounding conditions (e.g., intellectual disability, language impairment).
Research classifications with cut-points for ASD and nonspectrum also are available
(Luyster et al., 2009).
We examined the sensitivity of Toddler Module research classifications and concern ranges
for our samples, and results were similar to those reported in the original validation study
(Luyster et al., 2009). Using the research cutoffs of a total score of 12 for 12–20/Nonverbal
and 10 for Some Words 21–30, sensitivity in the original sample was .94 for children who
received the 12–20/Nonverbal 21–30 algorithm and .88 for children receiving the Some
Words 21–30 algorithm. Sensitivity in our replication sample was 0.88 for the 12–20/
Author Manuscript
Nonverbal 21–30 group and 0.71 for the Some Words 21–30 group. In the original sample,
82.2% fell within the moderate-to-severe concern range, 14.4% fell into the mild-to-
moderate range, and 3.4% fell into the little-to-no concern range. In the replication sample
here, 72.2% fell within the moderate-to-severe range, 19.1% were in the mild-to-moderate
range, and 8.7% were in the little-to-no concern range.
In the current study, the ADOS-2 Toddler Module was conducted as part of a clinic or
research evaluation. A similar battery of assessment measures was used across sites and
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 7
and children meeting criteria for DSM-IV diagnoses of Autistic Disorder, PDD-NOS, or
Asperger’s Disorder were given a best estimate diagnosis of ASD. To be consistent with
DSM-5 (APA, 2013), and because clinical subcategories have been found to be unstable over
time (e.g., Lord et al., 2006), unreliable across clinicians, and not representative of
meaningful differences in symptom presentation (Lord et al., 2012c), children with any
autism spectrum diagnosis were grouped into one ASD category for the present analyses.
Clinic-referred patients received oral feedback and a written report without financial
compensation. Participants recruited only for the purpose of research received financial
compensation and a written summary of evaluation results. Institutional Review Boards at
the University of Michigan, FSU, University of Minnesota, and University of Wisconsin-
Madison approved all procedures related to this project.
Author Manuscript
Site differences emerged in demographic and child variables. Differences in child variables
across sites were expected due to differences in recruitment patterns and study design across
sites. We viewed these site differences as beneficial to the purpose of this study, as we
sought to include children with varied levels of impairment and symptom characteristics.
The University of Wisconsin sample generally was older, had lower verbal skills, and
showed greater impairment in IQ and ADOS-2 scores than children from other sites.
Families in the FSU sample self-identified as more racially and ethnically diverse than
families from other sites. See Supplemental Tables 1 and 2 for further details on site
differences.
severity scores for ADOS-2 modules 1 through 4 (Gotham et al., 2009; Hus et al., 2012; Hus
& Lord, 2014). Calibrated severity scores were created by dividing the pool of children with
ASD into narrowly defined age and language cells, and standardizing raw total scores from
the Toddler Module algorithms within these cells.
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 8
few children age 21 to 30 months who had a large single word vocabulary who were
Author Manuscript
eventually diagnosed with ASD. Within the two developmental cells, distributions of Total,
Social Affect (SA) domain, and Restricted, Repetitive Behavior (RRB) domain scores were
generated separately for every one-month age group. Next, age groups with similar score
distributions were collapsed to create the fewest number of age- and language-level-
determined “calibration cells.” In the end, distributions were highly similar across ages
within the 12–20/Nonverbal group and the Some Words 21–30 group. Thus, two calibration
cells resulted, corresponding to the Toddler Module algorithms.
In creating the CSS for Modules 1–4 (Gotham et al., 2009; Hus & Lord, 2014), Total-raw
scores within calibration cells were mapped onto a 10-point severity rating scale based on
percentiles of Total-raw scores corresponding to each ADOS-2 diagnostic classification.
Lower calibrated severity scores were associated with fewer social communication and
repetitive behavior concerns. Scores 1–3 represented nonspectrum classifications, 4–5
Author Manuscript
represented ASD classifications, and 6–10 represented autism classifications. Similarly, for
the Toddler Module, a CSS of 1–3 was set to represent Total-raw scores falling within the
little-to-no concern range, scores of 4–5 represented scores in the mild-to-moderate concern
range, and 6–10 represented scores falling within the moderate-to-severe concern range.
Toddler Module concern range thresholds were determined by the algorithm relevant to each
calibration cell. The range of Total-raw scores corresponding to each point on the CSS
metric was determined by percentiles of available data associated with each CSS point
within a concern range, resulting in the Total-CSS.
Development of domain CSS—Because there are not separate SA and RRB cut-offs for
ADOS-2 classifications, the percentiles used for mapping the overall Total scores were used
to inform mapping of raw SA and RRB totals to each respective domain CSS. As with
Author Manuscript
Modules 1–4, raw RRB domain scores were mapped onto CSS values of 5–10, due to the
limited range of the RRB raw total (Hus et al. 2012; Hus & Lord, 2014). Because concern
ranges were not available to anchor CSS for SA and RRB domains, mappings were adjusted
for the SA-CSS so that, for each of the algorithm groups, at least 90% of children in the
moderate-to-severe concern range received an SA-CSS greater than or equal to 6. For
children in the 12–20/Nonverbal group, sensitivity was 94.8%; in the Some Words 21–30
group, sensitivity was 90%. Also, 100% of children in the mild-to-moderate concern range
in both groups received an SA-CSS of 4 or higher, and none of these children received an
SA-CSS score above 7. As with Modules 1–4, a goal of 80% sensitivity was set for the
RRB-CSS, due to expected lower sensitivity in detecting repetitive behaviors within the
limited time and contexts of an ADOS-2 administration (Hus et al., 2012). This goal was
attained for each algorithm group: 85.7% of children in the moderate-to-severe range in the
Author Manuscript
12–20/Nonverbal group, and 88.8% of children in the moderate-to-severe range in the Some
Words 21–30 group, received an RRB-CSS of 6 or higher. Similarly, over 80% of children in
the mild-to-moderate concern range received an RRB-CSS of 5 or higher across both
algorithm groups. Table 3 shows the raw score range corresponding to each CSS point
within each calibration cell.
To ensure that scores 6–10 correspond to approximate fifths of the ASD participants who
scored in the moderate-to-severe concern range, roughly 20% of participants in the
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 9
moderate-to-severe group should receive any individual score from 6 to 10. This was
Author Manuscript
generally the case in our dataset: for the Total-CSS, percentages across scores 6 through 10
ranged from 18.5 to 22.3%, SA-CSS ranged from 14.1% to 21.9%; and RRB-CSS ranged
from 15.4 to 20.1%.
Analyses conducted by Gotham et al. (2009), Hus et al. (2012), and Hus & Lord (2014)
were repeated with this Toddler Module dataset. Distributions of raw and calibrated severity
scores were compared to assess whether CSS distributions across age/language cells were
more uniform than raw score distributions. Linear regression models were analyzed to
compare the relative independence of CSS and raw totals from child characteristics.
Potential predictors were entered into a structured hierarchical regression model, in which
Block 1 included verbal and nonverbal IQs and mental ages (which are known to affect the
expression of ASD symptoms and for which we hoped to limit the effect on ADOS-2 scores
through the CSS; Bishop, Richler, & Lord, 2006; Lord & Spence, 2006), and Block 2
Author Manuscript
included gender, maternal education, and race (variables that could affect ASD symptoms
but that often have had non-significant effects when IQ and mental age variables are
controlled; Gotham et al., 2009). Only model R2 are reported, because interpretation of the
meaning of these individual coefficients is limited by multicollinearity. For all regression
models, Cohen’s f2 was computed; f2 of .02, .15, and .35 reflect small, medium, and large
effect sizes, respectively (Cohen, 1988). Significant predictors were then entered into
Forward Stepwise models to determine the relative contributions of these individual
variables to raw scores and CSS. These analyses then were replicated using Toddler Module
non-overlapping assessments from children with repeated measure data to further validate
the CSS. Finally, several assessments with longitudinal data were chosen to exemplify
various patterns of severity change over time across diagnostic groups. Analyses were
completed using SPSS Version 21 and 22.
Author Manuscript
Results
Comparing Distributions of Raw Totals and CSS
Distributions of Toddler Module raw Total, Social Affect, and Restricted, Repetitive
Behavior scores were generated for each age/language cell (Fig. 1 a, c, e) and compared to
the distributions of CSS for each cell (Fig. 1 b, d, f). Distributions of CSS showed increased
comparability across the two groups. There was a non-significant trend for children in the
older, verbal group to have lower Total-CSS compared to the nonverbal and younger group
(t=1.90, p<.058); the difference between groups is within 0.5 point and similar to mean CSS
distributions for Modules 1–4 (Gotham et al., 2009; Hus & Lord, 2014). Children in the
Some Words 21–30 group had lower SA-CSS than children in the Nonverbal/12–20 group
Author Manuscript
(t=4.40, p<.001). We tolerated this difference, because Toddler ADI-R scores and IQ scores
suggested a level of greater impairment in children in the Nonverbal/12–20 group. Adjusting
the SA-CSS to be more equal between groups could have misrepresented true differences in
severity. Differences in RRB-CSS were not significant. Means and standard deviations of
CSS and raw scores are listed by age/language cell in Table 4.
As expected, site differences in CSS were present. No significant differences were found for
children who used five or more words during the ADOS-2. Among nonverbal children, the
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 10
University of Wisconsin sample had significantly higher Total-CSS (F=12.31, p<.001), SA-
Author Manuscript
CSS (F=5.86, p<.001), and RRB-CSS (F=17.15, p<.001) than children from the University
of Michigan or FSU. Children from the University of Minnesota also had higher RRB-CSS
than children from the University of Michigan (p<.01) (Supplemental Table 2).
Predictors of Total-raw and Total-CSS—Using the full model, 30.5% of the variance
in Toddler Module Total-raw was explained. No individual predictor was statistically
significant, but multicollinearity was high for IQ and mental age variables. Verbal IQ
showed a trend (p = .063) as a predictor of Total-raw scores. For Total-CSS, the full model
accounted for 20.1% of the variance, and no variables emerged as significant predictors.
This represents a reduction in the influence of child characteristics from an f2 of .44 for
Total-raw to an f2 of .25 for Total-CSS.
Author Manuscript
Although no single predictor was statistically significant, because the models were
significant, individual predictors were entered into Forward Stepwise models to assess the
individual contribution of each variable (see Supplemental Table 3). For Total-raw scores,
verbal IQ accounted for the majority of the variance (26.4%), while nonverbal mental age
contributed an additional 3.0%. All other variables were excluded from the model, indicating
they were not significant. In the Forward model predicting Total-CSS, verbal IQ again
accounted for the majority of the variance (15.7%), and nonverbal IQ explained 3.1%. These
results reflect a reduction in the influence of verbal IQ from a large effect size (f2=.36) for
Total-raw to a medium effect size (f2 =.19) for Total-CSS.
full model accounted for 23.6% of the variance in SA-raw scores, and again, only verbal IQ
showed a trend for significance (p = .063). In contrast, 19.3% of the variance in SA-CSS was
explained by child characteristics, with verbal IQ showing a trend for significance (p = .
077). Thus, the influence of child characteristics was reduced from an f2 of .31 for SA-raw
to an f2 of .24 for SA-CSS.
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 11
Again, because the models were significant, individual predictors were entered into Forward
Author Manuscript
Stepwise models. For SA-raw, verbal IQ contributed the greatest proportion of the variance,
(19.3%), while nonverbal mental age accounted for 3.5%. For SA-CSS, verbal IQ explained
16.2% of the variance, while nonverbal mental age explained 2.1%. All other variables were
excluded from both models. The CSS for SA modestly reduced the influence of verbal IQ
from an f2 of .21 for SA-raw to an f2 of .19.
All predictors were entered into Forward Stepwise models, and only verbal IQ emerged as a
predictor of RRB-raw, accounting for 15.4% of the variance. For RRB-CSS, verbal IQ and
nonverbal IQ were statistically significant but explained small proportions of the variance in
RRB-CSS (7.1% and 1.8%, respectively). Thus, the influence of verbal IQ was reduced from
an f2 of .18 for RRB-raw to an f2 of .08 for RRB-CSS.
corresponded to raw total scores of 19–21 for the Nonverbal/12–20 group in the original
sample, the range was 19–20 for the replication sample). The original CSS map was
therefore used for analyses with the replication sample.
Distributions of total and domain raw scores and CSS are presented in Figure 2.
Distributions of Total-CSS showed increased comparability across the two groups in the
replication sample in contrast to raw total scores. However, the trend of the Some Words 21–
30 group having lower CSS than the Nonverbal/12–20 group was exaggerated and more
significant in this replication sample. Children in the Some Words 21–30 group had
significantly lower Total-CSS (t=3.71, p<.001), SA-CSS (t=6.46, p<.001), and SA-RRB
(t=2.19, p=.029) compared to the Nonverbal/12–20 group. In general, mean CSS were lower
in the replication sample than in the original sample (see Table 5). This difference is likely
Author Manuscript
due to recruitment effects and the fact that the University of Wisconsin sample, which was
generally older and less cognitively able, was not included in the replication sample. As a
result, the repeated assessment sample had higher verbal and nonverbal skills and included a
higher proportion of children who were in treatment studies and/or assessed prior to
developing clear ASD concerns compared to the original sample.
Linear regression analyses were repeated with the replication sample, with Forward
Stepwise models performed where appropriate. Results of Forward Stepwise regressions are
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 12
presented in Supplemental Table 4. The full model accounted for 41.8% of the variance in
Author Manuscript
Total-raw scores, and verbal IQ emerged as a significant predictor. The same model
accounted for 30.6% of the variance in Total-CSS, and verbal IQ remained a significant
predictor. This represents a reduction in the influence of child characteristics from an f2 of .
72 for Total-raw to an f2 of .44 for Total-CSS. Because there was only one significant
predictor of Total-raw and Total-CSS, Forward Stepwise models were not run.
For Social Affect, the full model accounted for 40.2% of the variance in SA-raw, and verbal
IQ and maternal education level emerged as significant predictors. The same model
accounted for 38.0% of the variance in SA-CSS. Verbal IQ was a significant predictor of
SA-CSS, and maternal education level showed a trend for significance (p = .052). The
influence of child characteristics was slightly reduced from an f2 of .67 for SA-raw to an f2
of .61 for SA-CSS. Next, verbal IQ and maternal education level were entered into Forward
Stepwise models. For SA-raw, verbal IQ explained 36.7% of the variance, and maternal
Author Manuscript
education was excluded from the model, indicating it was not significant. For SA-CSS,
verbal IQ explained 34.5% of the variance, and maternal education again was excluded.
Effect sizes remained large (f2=.58 for SA-raw and f2=.53 for SA-CSS).
For Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors, the full model accounted for 16.3% of the variance
in RRB-raw and 12.2% of the variance in RRB-CSS. In this case, gender emerged as a small
but statistically significant predictor of RRB-raw with slightly higher scores for males; no
variable was a significant predictor of RRB-CSS. The influence of child variables showed a
small reduction from an f2 of .19 for RRB-raw to an f2 of .14 for RRB-CSS. As only one
variable emerged as a predictor of RRB-raw, Forward Stepwise models were not performed.
Case Summaries
Author Manuscript
Four children with longitudinal data were selected to illustrate the utility of the Toddler
Module CSS for examining early patterns of ASD symptoms and their trajectories over time.
CSS by chronological age are plotted in Figure 3, with ADOS-2 module and raw score
displayed for each time point. See Table 6 for child characteristics at first and last
assessment.
Case 1—‘Henry’ is a clinic-referred male who showed a stable and severe pattern of ASD
symptoms. Henry was diagnosed with ASD at 17 months and enrolled in full-time applied
behavior analysis (ABA) intervention at 18 months. At 17 months, he rarely initiated social
interaction, rarely vocalized, and typically communicated using physical means (use of
other’s body, giving objects). He engaged in frequent complex mannerisms, visual sensory
exploration, and repetitive spinning of objects. After entering ABA, Henry markedly
Author Manuscript
improved in structural communication and began using vocalizations and words to request.
His relatively lower SA-CSS after initiating intervention reflected improvements in pairing
eye contact with requests, using words and phrases for a variety of pragmatic purposes, and
initiating and responding to social interactions more frequently, albeit still inconsistently.
His RRB-CSS showed a stable pattern of frequent engagement in repetitive sensory and
motor behaviors, stereotyped speech, and repetitive uses of objects. Difficulties interrupting
these behaviors affected interaction quality and rapport.
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 13
Case 2—‘Kyle,’ who was seen as part of a clinical research study on early diagnosis,
Author Manuscript
showed a pattern of moderate and stable severity. Kyle was given a best estimate diagnosis
of Autistic Disorder at 14 months. He produced five or more words during each Toddler
Module administration. In social communication on the Toddler Module, Kyle showed
persistent atypical use of eye contact, facial expressions, and gestures, although he did
initiate joint attention, point to, and show objects to some degree. He also engaged in
unusual sensory interests and exhibited preoccupations/repetitive uses of objects; however,
he showed fluctuations in restricted/repetitive behaviors between 19 and 34 months of age,
and his sensory interests decreased over time. Kyle experienced a significant increase in
verbal skills starting at 22 months, and these skills remained above average from this point.
Case 3—‘Roman’ is a male with an older brother with ASD, seen as part of a study on
early diagnosis. Roman showed a less severe pattern of ASD symptoms overall and
ultimately was assigned a DSM-IV diagnosis of PDD-NOS at 24 months. Roman showed
Author Manuscript
few deficits in social interaction and communication early on, although he consistently
engaged in mild preoccupations and repetitive uses of objects, reflected in his RRB-CSS
trajectory. His Toddler Module scores were mainly in the little-to-no concern range until he
developed phrase speech. At that time, mild deficits in social overtures and responses and
inconsistent use of eye contact and gestures were observed. After age 24 months, he also
began engaging in complex mannerisms. These behaviors led to corresponding increases in
domain and total CSS. He scored just under the ASD range at his final appointment, when
he showed fewer complex mannerisms and improvements in use of facial expressions,
gestures, and showing. Best estimate diagnosis remained PDD-NOS.
Case 4—‘Lydia’ is a clinic-referred female with an older sister with ASD. Her parents
sought an evaluation at 14 months due to concerns about social communication and motor
Author Manuscript
development (an ADOS-2 was not given until she was walking, which occurred at 17
months). Lydia was diagnosed with ASD at 17 months and immediately enrolled in full-time
ABA intervention. She was diagnosed with absence seizures at 28 months. She showed a
moderate and stable pattern of ASD symptoms over time. Throughout her assessments,
Lydia showed deficits in social communication involving limited eye contact and use of
gestures or pointing. She consistently shared enjoyment but in a limited number of ways; for
example, she frequently smiled and brought toys over to her parents’ laps but did not orient
the toys or initiate joint attention to distal objects. After initiating intervention,
improvements were observed in Lydia’s structural language and use of words for a variety of
pragmatic purposes, pairing eye contact with social overtures, and participating in structured
play. However, she continued to show a high level of repetitive uses of objects and
stereotyped speech after starting intervention. Separating the SA-CSS and RRB-CSS
Author Manuscript
Discussion
As with the CSS for Modules 1–4, the Toddler Module CSS resulted in more uniform
distributions across age and language level compared to raw total and domain scores. The
CSS was less influenced by child characteristics not specific to ASD, including verbal IQ,
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 14
than raw total and domain scores. In the original sample, verbal IQ was a significant
Author Manuscript
predictor of raw and domain scores; however, its influence was reduced for CSS compared
to raw scores. For Total scores, verbal IQ was reduced from accounting for 26.4% of the
variance in Total-raw to 15.7% of the variance in Total-CSS. For SA scores, verbal IQ
explained 19.3% of SA-raw, and this was modestly reduced to 16.2% for SA-CSS. In the
case of RRB, verbal IQ accounted for 15.4% of the variance in RRB-raw and 7.1% of RRB-
CSS. Nonverbal mental age exerted a small but statistically significant influence on Total
and SA raw scores and CSS, and nonverbal IQ emerged as statistically significant,
accounting for small amounts of the variance in RRB-raw and RRB-CSS. The amount of
variance explained by these nonverbal cognitive variables was reduced for RRB-CSS and
SA-CSS, but not for Total-CSS. Furthermore, mean Toddler CSS were comparable across
Toddler algorithms and to CSS means for Modules 1–4 (de Bildt et al., 2011, Gotham et al.,
2009, Hus et al., 2012; Hus & Lord, 2014; Shumway et al., 2012), supporting the utility of
Author Manuscript
using these scores for comparisons of children with ASD across modules using cross-
sectional data.
Total and domain CSS decreased the influence of verbal IQ less for the Toddler Module than
they had for Modules 1–3 (Gotham et al., 2009; Hus et al., 2012). However, it is likely that
the behaviors measured by the Toddler Module are less separable from developmental and
verbal levels than those measured by later modules. Early measures of verbal skills (such as
the Mullen Scales) include items which overlap with ADOS-2 SA items. Thus, the fact that
the influence of child characteristics was not substantially reduced for the Toddler Module,
particularly for SA-CSS, is not surprising.
Our replication sample, which included only repeated assessment data, yielded slightly less
encouraging results. We observed a similar pattern of reduced influence of verbal IQ on
Author Manuscript
Total-CSS and RRB-CSS compared to raw scores, with the influence of verbal IQ not
substantially reduced for SA-CSS compared to SA-raw. Furthermore, children in the Some
Words 21–30 group had significantly lower raw scores and CSS than children in the 12–20/
Nonverbal group, which was not the case in the original sample. This result could be related
to sampling; a larger proportion of the replication dataset consisted of children from
prospective studies. Infant siblings accounted for 29% of the replication sample overall and
31% of children in the Some Words 21–30 group, compared to 14% and 12% for children in
the original sample. It is reasonable to assume the prospective nature of the studies involving
repeated assessments led to some children being seen while ASD symptoms were first
emerging. Our original sample included children seen for a single assessment, including the
older, more severely affected Wisconsin sample and a higher proportion of children who
were clinic-referred. It will be important to replicate these findings in samples with a variety
Author Manuscript
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 15
Toddler Module calibrated severity scores should be especially useful in studies examining
Author Manuscript
changes in the behavioral phenotype of ASD over time. Domain CSS may contribute to
studies seeking to identify early behavioral patterns that predict ASD risk prior to the
emergence of the full disorder. For example, the presence of repetitive behaviors at 12
months has been identified as a key predictor of diagnosis (Ozonoff et al., 2008; Wolff et al.,
2014), and changes in repetitive behaviors between age 2 and 3 were a predictor of adult
outcomes in a longitudinal study of individuals starting at age 2 through adulthood
(Anderson, Liang, & Lord, 2014). The Toddler Module RRB-CSS is now available to
examine ASD symptoms independent of social communication symptoms. As with other
modules, Toddler calibrated severity scores may also be especially useful for studies that
examine relationships between genetic or neurobiological markers and dimensional
behavioral features of ASD.
There is an emerging evidence base for preventative intervention programs for infants at risk
Author Manuscript
for ASD (Green et al., 2013; Steiner, Gengoux, Klin, & Chawarska, 2013). These programs
enroll children as young as 8 months of age due to their risk status as younger siblings of
children with diagnosed ASD. There is a need for objective measures of changes and
improvements in ASD symptoms for very young children in response to intervention.
Toddler Module calibrated severity scores provide a means to track ASD symptoms starting
as soon as children are walking, allowing for examination of long-term outcomes for
children. However, researchers should be cautioned that the ADOS-2 is a diagnostic
measure, and its purpose is to detect core symptoms in ASD in social communication, play,
and repetitive behaviors. If children truly move out of a diagnosis of ASD, then the CSS
should reflect this trajectory. However, for children with established diagnoses of ASD,
calibrated severity scores designed to capture severity of core symptoms may not be
expected to abate in the same way that measures of anxiety or ADHD symptoms may show
Author Manuscript
improvement in response to treatment (Hus & Lord, 2014). The two children in our case
examples who initiated full-time ABA intervention prior to 18 months showed a pattern of
some reduction in Social Affect severity but little reduction in severity of repetitive
behaviors. However, conclusions cannot be drawn from these anecdotal examples, and more
work is needed to examine the utility of the CSS for measuring an individual’s response to
intervention. Although there is a practical need for tools to measure progress in core
symptoms of ASD, it is not recommended that the CSS be used in isolation in making
funding or eligibility decisions for intervention.
We reiterate the caution stated in previous studies in which calibrated severity scores for the
ADOS-2 were developed (Gotham et al., 2009; Hus et al., 2012; Hus & Lord, 2014) and
described within the ADOS-2 manual: Toddler Module calibrated severity scores should not
Author Manuscript
be interpreted as an overall measure of a child’s level of impairment. These scores are one
marker of severity of ASD symptoms, as measured by the ADOS-2, relative to other
children with ASD at the same age and language level. Calibrated severity scores provide
one piece of information in determining a child’s need for supports. Additional assessment
of cognitive development, language, adaptive skills, and internalizing and externalizing
behaviors is needed to develop a comprehensive picture of a child’s needs.
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 16
Limitations
Author Manuscript
As stated earlier, due to the variability in sample sources for this dataset, results may be
influenced by recruitment effects. In order to achieve a dataset of very young children large
enough to conduct our analyses, data from several different studies with different
recruitment patterns were combined. Our dataset consists of consecutive clinic referrals,
community-based samples, and participants recruited for a variety of treatment studies and
studies specific to high-risk infants. Clinic-referred samples contain potential bias, in that
there is evidence that young children with significant delays in developmental skills are
more likely to be referred for diagnostic evaluation (De Giacomo & Fombonne, 1998; Stone,
Hoffman, Lewis, & Ousley, 1994). Moreover, clinic-referred patients under 30 months who
are not language delayed may be more likely to have significant ASD symptoms, accounting
for their early referral (Luyster et al., 2009). Both of these issues may result in a score
distribution at the higher end of the range of ADOS-2 scores. On the other hand, children
Author Manuscript
followed prospectively may have initiated research participation before symptoms had
clearly manifested, which may have resulted in lower scores on the ADOS-2 compared to a
clinic-referred group. An acknowledged limitation of our replication sample is that it was
not independent from our original sample; one assessment from children with repeated
assessments was randomly selected for inclusion in the original sample, and the replication
sample consisted of the remaining assessments from children with multiple assessments
only. Results from analyses with the replication sample also should be interpreted with
caution due to the known differences in our sample in characteristics of children seen
multiple times compared to children seen once.
Toddler Module calibrated severity scores show promise as a tool for behavioral
phenotyping of ASD in very young children. Our analyses did not include an examination of
patterns of total and domain CSS for children who received nonspectrum diagnoses or for
Author Manuscript
Conclusion
The current study extends findings of calibrated severity scores for the ADOS-2 Modules 1–
4 to the Toddler Module to increase comparability of scores across time, age, and module.
Toddler calibrated severity scores are less influenced by verbal level and thus should provide
a better metric of ASD symptom severity than raw total and domain scores. However,
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 17
although this effect was reduced, it was not eliminated, and researchers and clinicians will
Author Manuscript
need to be aware that scores on the Toddler Module are likely to be higher for children with
significant language delays. As with Module 1–4 calibrated severity scores, Toddler
calibrated severity scores should be replicated in large independent samples to further
explore their reliability and clinical utility.
Supplementary Material
Refer to Web version on PubMed Central for supplementary material.
References
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th.
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.
Anderson DK, Liang JW, Lord C. Predicting young adult outcome among more and less cognitively
Author Manuscript
able individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2014;
55(5):485–494. [PubMed: 24313878]
Bishop SL, Richler J, Lord C. The structure of autism symptoms as measured by the autism diagnostic
observation schedule. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in
Childhood and Adolescence. 2006; 12(4–5):247–267. [PubMed: 16911971]
Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L, Brian J, Roberts W, Szatmari P, Rombough V, McDermott C. A
prospective case series of high-risk infants who developed autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders. 2007; 37(1):12–24. [PubMed: 17211728]
Chawarska K, Klin A, Paul R, Macari S, Volkmar F. A prospective study of toddlers with ASD: Short-
term diagnostic and cognitive outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009; 50(10):
1235–1245. [PubMed: 19594835]
Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Psychology Press; 1988.
Dawson G, Bernier R. A quarter century of progress on the early detection and treatment of autism
spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 2013; 25(4):1455–1472. [PubMed:
Author Manuscript
24342850]
de Bildt A, Oosterling IJ, van Lang NDJ, Sytema S, Minderaa RB, van Engeland H, de Jonge MV.
Standardized ADOS scores: Measuring severity of autism spectrum disorders in a Dutch sample.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2011; 41(3):311–319. [PubMed: 20617374]
De Giacomo A, Fombonne E. Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism.
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998; 7(3):131–136. [PubMed: 9826299]
Fenson, L.; Resznick, S.; Thal, D.; Bates, E.; Hartung, J.; Pethick, S., et al. The MacArthur
Communicative Development Inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. San Diego: Singular
Publishing Group; 1993.
Gnaulati, E. Back to normal: Why ordinary childhood behavior is mistaken for ADHD, bipolar
disorder, and autism spectrum disorder. Beacon Press; 2013.
Gotham K, Pickles A, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders. 2009; 39(5):693–705.
[PubMed: 19082876]
Author Manuscript
Green J, Wan MW, Guiraud J, Holsgrove S, McNally J, Slonims V, Johnson M. Intervention for infants
at risk of developing autism: a case series. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;
43(11):2502–2514. [PubMed: 23532347]
Guthrie W, Swineford LB, Nottke C, Wetherby AM. Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder:
stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry. 2013; 54(5):582–590. [PubMed: 23078094]
Hus V, Gotham K, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS domain scores: Separating severity of social affect
and restricted and repetitive behaviors. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012:1–
13. [PubMed: 21360019]
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 18
Hus V, Lord C. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Module 4: Revised algorithm and
standardized severity scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014:1–17.
Author Manuscript
Kim SH, Lord C. New autism diagnostic interview-revised algorithms for toddlers and young
preschoolers from 12 to 47 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;
42(1):82–93. [PubMed: 21384244]
Landa RJ, Holman KC, Garrett-Mayer E. Social and communication development in toddlers with
early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;
64(7):853–864. [PubMed: 17606819]
Lord, C.; Luyster, R.; Gotham, K.; Guthrie, W. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. 2nd.
Torrence, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2012a. (ADOS-2) Manual (Part II): Toddler
Module
Lord C, Luyster R, Guthrie W, Pickles A. Patterns of developmental trajectories in toddlers with
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012b; 80(3):477–489.
[PubMed: 22506796]
Lord C, Petkova E, Hus V, Gan WJ, Lu FH, Martin DM, Risi S. A multisite study of the clinical
diagnosis of different autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012c; 69(3):
Author Manuscript
[PubMed: 18805942]
Ozonoff S, Young GS, Belding A, Hill M, Hill A, Hutman T, Iosif AM. The broader autism phenotype
in infancy: When does it emerge? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry. 2014; 53(4):398–407. [PubMed: 24655649]
Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, Messinger D, Yirmiya N, Zwaigenbaum L, Stone WL. Recurrence
risk for autism spectrum disorders: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics. 2011;
128(3):E488–E495. [PubMed: 21844053]
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 19
Shumway S, Farmer C, Thurm A, Joseph L, Black D, Golden C. The ADOS calibrated severity score:
Relationship to phenotypic variables and stability over time. Autism Research. 2012; 5(4):267–
Author Manuscript
Weitlauf AS, Gotham KO, Vehorn AC, Warren ZE. Brief Report: DSM-5 "Levels of Support:" A
comment on discrepant conceptualizations of severity in ASD. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders. 2014; 44(2):471–476. [PubMed: 23812664]
Werner E, Dawson G. Validation of the phenomenon of autistic regression using home videotapes.
Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005; 62(8):889–895. [PubMed: 16061766]
Wolff JJ, Botteron KN, Dager SR, Elison JT, Estes AM, Gu H, Piven J. Longitudinal patterns of
repetitive behavior in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2014
Woolfenden S, Sarkozy V, Ridley G, Williams K. A systematic review of the diagnostic stability of
Autism Spectrum Disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2012; 6(1):345–354.
Zimmerman, IL.; Steiner, VG.; Pond, RE. Preschool Language Scale. Fourth. San Antonio, TX:
Harcourt Assessment; 2002. (PLS-4)
Zimmerman, IL.; Steiner, VG.; Pond, RE. Preschool Language Scale, Fifth Edition (PLS-5). San
Antonio, TX: Pearson; 2011.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 20
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 1.
Author Manuscript
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 21
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 2.
Author Manuscript
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Esler et al. Page 22
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 3.
Case summaries of longitudinal total and domain calibrated severity scores.
Author Manuscript
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Table 1
Note. VIQ = verbal IQ; NVIQ = nonverbal IQ; VMA = verbal mental age; NVMA = nonverbal mental age; ADI-R = Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised; SA = Social Affect; RRB = Restricted,
Repetitive Behavior; RPI = Reciprocal Peer Interaction; ADOS = Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule.
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Page 23
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Table 2
Note. VIQ = verbal IQ; NVIQ = nonverbal IQ; VMA = verbal mental age; NVMA = nonverbal mental age; ADI-R = Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised; SA = Social Affect; RRB = Restricted,
Repetitive Behavior; RPI = Reciprocal Peer Interaction; ADOS = Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule.
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Page 24
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Table 3
Raw Totals
Esler et al.
Note. CSS = calibrated severity score; 12–20/NV = Toddler Module algorithm for children age 12–20 and nonverbal children; SW 21–30 = Toddler Module algorithm for children age 21–30 months who
used single words; SA = Social Affect; RRB = Restricted, Repetitive Behavior
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Page 25
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Table 4
Raw Score and CSS Means and Standard Deviations by Age/Language Cell (ASD Assessments Only)
N M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
12–20/NV 272 18.68 4.66 7.44 1.86 14.47 3.54 7.44 1.84 4.21 2.06 7.16 2.04
SW 21–30 116 14.83 4.79 7.03 2.09 11.94 4.25 6.51 2.07 2.89 1.59 6.76 1.91
Note. CSS = calibrated severity score; 12–20/NV = Toddler Module algorithm for children age 12–20 and nonverbal children; SW 21–30 = Toddler Module algorithm for children age 21–30 months who
used single words; SA = Social Affect; RRB = Restricted, Repetitive Behavior
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Page 26
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Table 5
Raw Score and CSS Means and Standard Deviations by Age/Language Cell (Replication Sample ASD Assessments Only)
N M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
12–20/NV 285 17.36 4.92 6.94 2.02 13.74 3.99 7.08 2.04 3.62 2.00 6.61 1.99
SW 21–30 150 12.89 5.26 6.12 2.46 10.49 4.70 5.70 2.28 2.39 1.40 6.17 1.91
Note. CSS = calibrated severity score; 12–20/NV = Toddler Module algorithm for children age 12–20 and nonverbal children; SW 21–30 = Toddler Module algorithm for children age 21–30 months who
used single words; SA = Social Affect; RRB = Restricted, Repetitive Behavior
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Page 27
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Table 6
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 June 08.
Page 28